Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 15

Music Therapy Perspectives Advance Access published June 27, 2016

Investigating the Effectiveness of a Developmental,


Individual Difference, Relationship-Based (DIR)
Improvisational Music Therapy Program on Social
Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum
Disorder
JOHN A. CARPENTE, PHD, MT-BC, LCAT Associate Professor, Molloy College, Rockville Centre, NY
Founder/Director, Rebecca Center for Music Therapy at
Molloy College, Rockville Centre, NY

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


ABSTRACT:  The purpose of this study was to examine the effec- strategies within research protocols such as following the child’s
tiveness of improvisational music therapy carried out within a lead and contingent imitation while considering their develop-
DIRFloortime framework in addressing the individual social com- mental capacities, and teaching communication skills within
munication needs of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).
Participants included four children enrolled in a therapeutic day
a social context (natural environment) (Dawson et  al., 2010;
school, 4–8 years of age, and diagnosed with ASD. Each child partici- Schreibman et al., 2015) as a means of improving communica-
pated in twenty-four 30-minute individual DIR-based improvisational tion (Casenhiser, Shanker, & Stieben, 2013; Dawson et al., 2010;
music therapy sessions over the course of 13 weeks. The Functional Green et al., 2010; Hwang & Hughes, 2000; Ingersoll, Dvortcsak,
Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) was used to evaluate changes in Whalen, & Sikora, 2005; Kasari, Gulsrud, Wong, Kwon, & Locke,
social communication skills. Results indicated improvements in areas 2010; Prizant, Wetherby, Rubin, & Laurent, 2003).
of self-regulation, engagement, behavioral organization, and two-way
purposeful communication. The DIRFloortime Model
The Developmental Individual Difference Relationship-
based model (DIRFloortime) is one of several DSP models.
Background DIRFloortime is a caregiver-mediated home-based interven-
Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) display tion that involves training parents to maximize interactions
challenges related to social communication skills (American with their children to improve social reciprocity and functional
Psychiatric Association, 2013). These challenges generally impact pragmatic communication (Greenspan & Wieder, 2006a;
the individual’s ability to experience shared attention, express Simpson, 2005; Solomon, Van Egeren, Mahoney, Huber, &
and understand nonverbal and verbal communication, maintain Zimmerman, 2014). The DIRFloortime model involves the
peer relationships, and engage in social reciprocity. Treatment implementation of child-led strategies within a developmen-
programs based on the behavioral model constitute the predomi- tal approach in order to foster communication skills within
nant therapeutic approach (Schreibman, 2005). According to this a social context (Greenspan, 1992; Greenspan & Wieder,
model, ASD is a learning difficulty that can be addressed with 1997, 2006b; Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers, 2011, 2012;
operant conditioning strategies using discrete behavioral trials Solomon, Necheles, Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007; Casenhiser,
to increase language and socialization and decrease repetition. Shanker, & Stieben, 2013). The model seeks to facilitate social
The literature, however, indicates limitations regarding the use communication skills via affective-relational experiences that
of highly structured therapist-led behavioral interventions. For are based on the child’s interests in order to foster engagement,
example, behavioral gains did not typically generalize to new relatedness, communication, and high-level thinking, that is,
settings or maintain over time (Schreibman, 2005; Harris et al., symbolism and abstraction, within a social context. Thus, the
2015; Ingersoll, Lewis, & Kroman, 2007; Paul, 2008). Results also task of the therapist is to provide developmentally appropriate
indicated a lack of spontaneity and overdependence on prompts relational experiences that are based on the child’s lead, all
(Ingersoll, 2008; Schreibman, 2005; Schreibman et al., 2015). within the context of the child’s relationship with the therapist.
As a result of these findings, interventionists are now con- The DIR model provides clinicians and parents with a frame-
sistently incorporating Developmental Social Pragmatic (DSP) work for assessing and conceptualizing the needs of individuals.
The “D” is concerned with the child’s developmental capacities.
John A.  Carpente, Associate Professor of Music Therapy at Molloy College, is the
The “I” deals with the child’s individual differences, and the “R”
founder and director of the Rebecca Center for Music Therapy and Center for describes his/her learning relationships with others. Thus, the
Autism at Molloy College. model provides therapists with a guide for creating an intervention
Address correspondence concerning this article to John A. Carpente, PhD, MT-BC,
LCAT, The Rebecca Center for Music Therapy at Molloy College, 1000 Hempstead plan that takes into consideration each child’s unique differences
Ave., NY Rockville Centre 11571. E-mail: jcarpente@molloy.edu and strengths in order to foster social, emotional, and intellectual
© the American Music Therapy Association 2016. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com development rather than simply focusing on isolated behaviors
doi:10.1093/mtp/miw013 (Greenspan, 1992; Greenspan & Wieder, 2006a, 2006b).
1
2 Music Therapy Perspectives

The “D” refers to six levels of development that define the Carpente, Elefant, & Kim, 2015; Holck, 2004a; Kim, Wigram,
child’s fundamental capacities for 1) shared attention and self- & Gold, 2008; Nordoff & Robbins, 2007). Child-led IMT may
regulation, 2)  relatedness and engagement, 3)  two-way pur- be viewed as a developmental approach noted for providing
poseful communication, 4)  shared problem-solving, 5)  sym- a meaningful framework, similar to early mother-infant inter-
bolic thinking, and 6) bridging ideas. According to Greenspan action, which is used to promote shared focus of attention,
(1992), these capacities are the building blocks and founda- turn-taking, and emotional attunement (Holck, 2004b; Kim,
tion for higher levels of thinking and relating such as the ability Wigram, & Gold, 2008). When working within a child-led
to sustain long chains of communication in a back-and-forth framework, the therapist may improvise music that generally
purposeful manner, create and share ideas, and think symboli- follows the child’s focus of attention and interests in order to
cally and abstractly. Table 1 illustrates the six developmental establish a relationship while fostering engagement, related-
levels of social-emotional functioning. ness, and communication (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Carpente,
The “I” represents Individual differences and refers to how 2013; Geretsegger, Holck, Carpente, Elefant, & Kim, 2015;
the child processes information such as receptive and expres- Holck, 2004b; Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2008; Nordoff &
sive language, motor and sensory stimuli (e.g., touch, sound, Robbins, 2007). The process of tuning into the child’s musi-
and other sensations), auditory input, visual-spatial informa- cal and non-musical expression has been an integral feature
tion; and motor-planning and sequencing abilities. For each of of clinical practice and is an essential skill of an improvisa-

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


the six developmental levels described in Table 1, the thera- tional music therapist (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Bruscia, 1987;
pist is required to understand the individual differences of the Geretsegger, Holck, Carpente, Elefant, & Kim, 2015; Nordoff
child, and determine how they interfere with his/her ability to & Robbins, 2007).
move up the developmental sequence (Greenspan & Wieder, Working in music as a therapeutic intervention has shown
2006a, 2006b). to be potentially more effective than other mediums to engage
The term “Relationships” pertains to how the child inter- children with ASD, and may provide unique opportunities
acts with others (e.g., family members, teachers, therapists, for them to interact nonverbally compared with play-based
and caregivers) in order to inform the therapist as to the pat- interactions (Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2008). The structure and
terns and modes of interaction that should be included in the predictability found in music provides a context and vehicle
therapeutic program to support development. Relationships for reciprocal interactions and promotes flexibility and social
and the learning experiences that occur in them are an essen- engagement from which relationships emerge (Bruscia, 1987,
tial component of the DIRFloortime model. According to 2014; Geretsegger, Holck, Carpente, Elefant, & Kim, 2015;
Greenspan and Shanker (2004), relationships are the vehicle Nordoff & Robbins, 2007). While children with ASD are gen-
for affect-based developmentally appropriate interactions that erally found to have impaired perception of linguistic and
are necessary for healthy development. social auditory stimuli (Boddaert et  al., 2004), they are also
Each facet of the DIRFloortime model complements the reported to possess either intact or sometimes superior musi-
other. First, it is essential to understand the child’s level of cal perception regarding pitch perception (Bonnel et al., 2003;
developmental functioning (see Table 1). Second, it is impor- Heaton, 2005), melody discrimination (Applebaum, Egel,
tant to understand the child’s individual differences and how Koegel, & Imhoff, 1979; Mottron, Peretz, & Menard, 2000),
they may be interfering with his/her development. Finally, it is pitch recall (Heaton, Hermelin, & Pring, 1998), the ability to
imperative to assess and understand the child’s mode of relat- disembed notes within chords (Heaton, 2003), and impro-
ing and managing relationships with others. Once there is a vising melodies (Thaut, 1988). Studies have also indicated
developmental and sensory portrait of the child, it is the thera- that individuals with ASD have a strong affinity for musical
pist’s task to support the individual differences while providing stimuli when compared to other auditory and visual stimuli
the child with relational experiences that will help guide him/ (Blackstock, 1978; Thaut, 1987), as well as for displaying the
her to achieve the highest potential within the six develop- ability to understand affective connotations in music (Heaton,
mental levels. Hermelin, & Pring, 1999).
Floortime is an intervention method that is integral to the
DIR therapeutic process. It is a clearly defined five-step skill DIRFloortime and Client-Led Improvisational Music Therapy
sequence that guides the therapist or parents to follow affec- Floortime and client-led improvisational music therapy
tively toned interactions through gestures and words in order share many similarities in terms of their philosophy and
to help move the child up the developmental ladder by first approach. Both are child-led, relying on the therapist’s ability
establishing a foundation of shared attention. Table  2 illus- to be creative, flexible, spontaneous, and emotionally attuned
trates the five steps that make up the Floortime method. Thus, with the child. In addition, both highly value the child–ther-
the DIR portion of the model helps conceptualize the child, apist relationship as being the vehicle for development and
helping the therapist in drawing up a comprehensive assess- clinical progress (Carpente, 2011, 2014). Finally, the aim of
ment, while Floortime is the treatment intervention that is both methods is to engage the child in affective back-and-forth
guided by the child’s profile. reciprocal experiences within a social context to foster social
communication capacities. However, they differ in mediation,
Improvisational Music Therapy setting, duration/intensity, and evidence base.
Similar to the DIRFloortime model, improvisational music Family involvement and caregiver delivery are an essential
therapy (IMT) may also adopt a client-led relationship-based aspect of most DSP interventions. Several randomized control
framework when working with children with ASD (Alvin trials have evaluated the effectiveness of caregiver mediated
& Warwick, 1991; Carpente, 2012; Geretsegger, Holck, DSP interventions (Aldred, Green, & Adams, 2004; Green et al.,
Table 1.
Six Developmental Levels of Social–Emotional Functioning

Developmental Chronological
milestones age Description At risk for ASD
Level I: Shared attention and regulation Occurs 0–3 months of age Internal emotional regulation and homeostasis. An infant at risk for ASD may exhibit challenges
Integrates and utilizes sensory stimuli, i.e., sight, in sustaining attention to sensory stimulation, e.g.,
smell, sound, touch, and taste to self-regulate; sights or sounds, and may prefer to engage in
maintain availability for interaction while perseverative behaviors.
stabilizing awareness of sensations to remain calm
and alert
Level II: Attachment and engagement in Occurs 2–5 months Forming a special relationship with a parent An infant at risk for ASD may display challenges
relationship or caregiver; builds a foundation for future in maintaining engagement, and may withdraw
relationships from interaction and become self-absorbed.
Level III: Two-way purposeful Occurs 4–10 months Purposeful and meaningful communication An infant at risk for ASD may display a lack of
communication using gestures, vocalizations, facial expressions interest in others, or engage in brief back-and-
in order to open and close 5 or more circles of forth exchanges with very little initiative, and may
communication (pre-verbal communication, engage in random behaviors.
reading and processing gestural cues)
Level IV: Behavioral organization, Occurs 10–18 months Developing a complex sense of self. Engages A child at risk for ASD will exhibit challenges
problem-solving, and internalization in a continuous flow of back-and-forth in initiating and sustaining back-and-forth
interactions; engaging is shared problem-solving interactions of emotional signals (e.g., showing
while opening and closing at least 10 or more mom or dad a toy) and may engage in
circles of communication; experiencing and perseverative behavior patterns.
comprehending range of emotions, e.g., pleasure,
assertiveness, curiosity, intimacy, fear, anger
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Level V: Representation capacities Occurs 18–30 months of age Internal representation (symbolic thinking). Learns A child at risk for ASD will display challenges
to represent events, things, feelings symbolically; using words and/or phrases meaningfully and
engage in pretend (symbolic) play; functional engaging in pretend play; he/she may repeat
speech continues to develop words (echolalia).
Level VI: Representation differentiation Occurs 30–42 months Bridges between ideas and feelings and connects A child at risk for ASD will engage in memorized
ideas scripts with random ideas; or use words and ideas
Logically; developing abstract thinking and able out of context.
to answer questions dealing with what, when,
how, and why questions
*All information within this table is from Greenspan & Wieder (2006a, 2006b).
3

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


4 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 2.
Six Steps to Administering Floortime™

Floortime technique Procedures


Observation Listening to and watching the child’s facial expressions, tone of voice, gestures, body
posture, use of or lack of words, how he/she navigated around the room, separates
from caregiver)
Approach Once assessing the child’s mode of interacting or responding, the therapist can
approach the child with clinically appropriate music, words/lyrics, gestures, and affect.
He/she can open the circle of communication with the child by acknowledging the
child’s emotionality, then elaborating and building on whatever interests the child at
the moment
Follow the child’s lead After the initial approach, therapist follows the child’s lead by joining the child in
whatever they are doing while being a supportive play partner, creating music that
supports, reflects, and/or enhances what the child is playing
Extend and expand play As therapist follows the child’s lead, he/she extends and expands the play-making,

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


providing supportive music and comments about the child’s play without being
intrusive; providing opportunities for the child to express ideas while guiding them into
various musical directions to foster problem-solving and reciprocity
Child closes the circle of communication as When child is approached, he/she closes the circle when he/she builds on the
the therapist opens the circle therapist’s comments with comments and/or gestures, and/or ideas of his/her own.
One circle flows into another, and many circles may be opened and closed in
quick succession as one interacts with the child. By building on each other’s ideas
and gestures, the child begins to appreciate and understand the value of two-way
communication
*All information within this table is from Greenspan & Wieder, 2006a, 2006b).

2010; Kasari, Gulsrud, Wong, Kwon, & Locke, 2010; Ingersoll, (IMT) intervention in addressing individual social communi-
2008, Wetherby & Woods, 2006). Results indicated significant cation skills of children with ASD. The guiding research ques-
improvement in the children’s ability to share attention, and tion was: Do children with ASD receiving DIR-based IMT
engage with parents and communicate reciprocally after seven improve their social communication functioning, as assessed
months to one year of monthly three-hour parent training visits by the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS)?
to the home, and six to ten twenty-minute caregiver-mediated
Floortime sessions at home (Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers, Method
2012, Casenhiser, Shanker, & Stieben, 2013; Solomon, Van
Egeren, Mahoney, Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014). Participants
IMT is typically delivered by a highly skilled music therapist in Four participants enrolled in a therapeutic day-school were
a clinical setting. The two RCTs that have assessed the effective- selected by the school psychologist for participation in the study
ness of IMT for children with ASD have found improvements in based on the following criteria: 1) a diagnosis of ASD, 2) newly
joint attention and affective sharing after delivering 30–45-min- enrolled at the therapeutic day-school, 3) 4–8 years of age, and
ute IMT sessions once a week for 12 to 16 weeks (Kim, Wigram, 3)  no prior experience in music therapy. Each parent of the
& Gold, 2008; Gattino et al., 2011). While caregiver interven- participants was asked to sign a consent form for their child’s
tions are now considered evidence-based (Wheeler, Williams, participation in the study. All personal information and data
Seida, & Ospina, 2008; Wong et  al., 2015) music therapy, was kept strictly confidential (pseudonyms are used throughout
including IMT is regarded as promising but not sufficiently evi- this paper to identify the subjects). The study was reviewed and
denced for improving social interaction in children with ASD approved by an East Coast University and the therapeutic day-
(Rossignol, 2009). Several randomized control trials suggest school’s Institutional Review Board (IRB).
that IMT as an intervention for young children with ASD can
improve responding to joint attention and some forms of ini- Measure
tiating joint attention (Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2008), affective The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS)
sharing and initiating behavior (Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2009), (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Wieder, 2001) was used as a pre- and
nonverbal communication skills (Gattino, Riesgo, Longo, Leite, post-test to measure each child’s progress toward social com-
& Faccini, 2011), and the parent–child relationship (Thompson, munication development. The FEAS, a play-based assessment
McKerran, & Gold, 2013; Thompson, 2012; Oldfield, 2001). instrument, is a valid and reliable, age-normed, observational
This study will (1) translate and apply DIRFloortime to IMT instrument that has been used in several DIRFloortime studies
principles as a means for improving core features of ASD (Solomon, Necheles, Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007; Solomon, Van
based on a standardized, criterion-referenced rating scale Egeren, Mahoney, Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014; Liao et al., 2014;
developed to evaluate social communication via play context Dionne & Martini, 2011; Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers,
(Functional Emotional Assessment Scale) and (2) examine the 2011, 2012). The FEAS was designed to determine a child’s social
effectiveness of a DIR-based improvisational music therapy communication capacities based on the six developmental
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 5

milestones listed in Table 1. The FEAS includes six subtests that stages to achieve each phase, always beginning and reverting
relate directly to Greenspan’s six functional developmental back to following the client’s musical–emotional lead (Phase 1).
levels and the ages at which typically developing children are IMT was employed within the context of a child-led DIR-based
expected to attain them (Greenspan, DeGangi, & Wieder, 2001): approach. Thus, the therapist created music based on the child’s
1) self-regulation and in shared attention (0–3 months); 2) attach- musical responses, and/or movements, and/or emotionality, and
ment and engagement (2–5  months); 3)  two-way, purposeful inclinations or tendencies to foster engagement, relatedness,
communication (10–12 months); 4) behavioral organization and attunement, and social communication. The music therapist cre-
problem-solving (18–30  months); representational capacities ated music that met and followed the child’s musical–emotional
(create ideas, use words or phrases meaningfully, engage in pre- lead in order to foster engagement, relatedness, social interac-
tend play, and think symbolically [18–30 months]); and (6) rep- tion, and communication. The clinically improvised music was
resentational differentiation (build bridges between ideas, think based on the child’s reactions, responses, and emotional state
logically and sequentially, answer “why” questions, thinking as a means to join the child’s play and foster relationship and
abstractly, that is, representation differentiation (30–42 months). shared attention. Hence, the task of the therapist was to pro-
The FEAS scoring system is based on a three-point scale for vide music that deepened the child’s experience in play and fos-
each of six levels of emotional capacity. Items are rated as 0– tered a continuous flow of affective back-and-forth interactions
not at all or very brief; 1–present some of the time, observed though a range of musical contexts and frameworks. The course

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


several times,or consistently present many times. Thus, high of improvising musical experiences included a process of three
scores on the FEAS indicate a higher developmental level and phases: 1) following the child’s musical–emotional lead, 2) two-
in turn signify greater social communication. The ratings can way purposeful musical-play, and 3) affect synchrony in musical
be summed to obtain subtest scores as well as total scores play. See Table 3 for an illustration of the sequence and opera-
(Greenspan, DeGangi, & Wieder, 2001). Cutoff scores, illus- tional definitions for each of the three phases.
trated in Table 8, are used to determine whether the child is
classified as “deficient,” “at risk,” or “normal.” Phase 1: Following the Child’s Musical–Emotional Lead
The FEAS pre- and post-test ratings were summed to obtain Phase 1, “following the child’s musical–emotional lead,”
subtest and total scores. FEAS scores were compared to the cut- involved the therapist observing the child while creating music
off scores to determine the child’s classification, in each devel- around his/her natural inclinations, emotional interests, and
opmental level, as “deficient,” “at risk,” and “normal.” Pre- and musical (e.g., instrument, vocal, and/or movement) and non-
post-test comparisons of classifications were the primary out- musical responses (e.g., gestural and/or facial expressions).
come measure and were compared for each child to determine Clinical techniques such as reflecting, synchronizing, and/or
if progress was made on their overall score and on each subtest. enhancing (Bruscia, 1987) are implemented within musical
frameworks in order to meet the child’s affect and to foster
Procedures engagement and joint attention.
Each child participated in a total of 26 DIR-based IMT ses-
sions over the course of 13 weeks. Each treatment session Phase 2: Two-Way Purposeful Musical Play
lasted 15–30 minutes, depending upon the child’s tolerance, Phase 2, “two-way purposeful musical play,” included
and was given twice per week. The first and last sessions the therapist transitioning from a child-led to a therapist-led
involved pre- and post-testing administered by a psycholo- interaction by providing musical experiences that sought
gist who is DIRFloortime certified and highly experienced in out a response from the child. The therapist-led experience
administering the FEAS. The other 24 sessions consisted of indi- was focused on redirecting the child’s attention and music in
vidual DIR-based improvisational music therapy conducted order to elicit a musical response that in turn closed or com-
in the school’s music therapy treatment room. This room was pleted a circle of communication (e.g., punctuating the end
equipped with a video recorder that was used to record all of a phrase, vocally or via cymbal play; gesturally responding
sessions. Various instruments, which require no prior skills or to the therapist’s music; adjusting to the therapist’s change in
experience, were available to the children, including a snare music). This phase included the therapist incorporating elicita-
drum, 12-inch crash cymbal, 12-inch tambourine and buf- tion and redirection techniques (Bruscia, 1987) as a means of
falo drum, 24-inch floor tom, chromatic set of resonator bells, fostering reciprocal musical play (two-way purposeful musi-
pentatonic xylymba, chromatic xylophone, two pitched reed cal play). The child’s responses may have been expressed via
horns, an acoustic piano, and an acoustic and electric guitar. a glance, and/or instrument play, and/or vocalization, and/
In addition, a variety of sizes of drumsticks and mallets with or movement, and/or facial expression. At any time during or
various textured handles were made available. after the transition into Phase 2, the therapist may revert back
to Phase 1 if the child demonstrated difficulty transitioning
Intervention and/or withdrawing from the interaction.
The DIR-based IMT involved the therapist implementing
three procedural phases (see Table 3) in tandem with Floortime Phase 3: Affect Synchrony in Musical Play
techniques illustrated in Table  2. Each phase is identified by “Affect synchrony,” Phase 3, involved the therapist provid-
its own objectives and musical–clinical techniques, and each ing music experiences that included a range of elements, that
requires different developmental capacities on the part of the is, tempo and dynamics, within a consistent predictable musi-
child. Therefore, the child’s capacity to engage in musical play cal structure while incorporating a range of clinical techniques
will determine the working phase or phases of the session. such as empathy, structuring, elicitation, redirection, and leav-
Hence, the therapist follows and moves through a sequence of ing spaces (Bruscia, 1987). The task of the therapist in this phase
6 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 3.
Musical–Clinical Intervention Procedural Phases

Phases Clinical techniques Procedure Outcome


I. Following the child’s musical– Empathy techniques: reflecting, Therapist observes, listens, and Build rapport with the child by
emotional lead synchronizing creates musical experiences creating an accepting musical
, and/or enhancing based on child’s reactions, environment
(Bruscia, 1987) responses, and initiated that is respectful of his/her
behaviors; music is focused differences, reactions, and
on meeting the child’s affect; responses; guides therapist in
therapist may focus on the understanding child’s musical
client’s sense of dynamics, tendencies
tempo, rhythm, and pace , preferences, sensitivities, and
of motor movements and preferred musical media (2013);
utilization of the instruments, and helps foster self-regulation
while being attentive to any and joint attention as the child

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


vocal sounds being expressed is available for back-and-forth
(Carpente, 2013) interaction
II. Two-way purposeful musical Elicitation and redirection Musically, the therapist Child to engage in back-and-
play (1987) improvises and initiates music forth musical play in which the
that offers or expresses a interaction shifts between child-
musical question, statement, and therapist-led (2013)
and/or partial statement seeking
a musical response (2013)
III. Affect synchrony in Empathy, elicitation, structuring Therapist creates musical Child initiates and responds
musical play and redirection (1987) (through experiences incorporating to musical ideas and cues via
a range of tempo and dynamics) a range of elements and a range of musical elements;
contexts that provide child engaging in long chains of
with opportunities to initiate, back-and-forth reciprocal
respond, and engage in a affective musical interactions
continuous flow exchanging roles leading and
of affective musical interactions following in play (2013)
(2013)

was to create opportunities for the client to initiate and respond


while exchanging leadership and followership in musical play.

Data Analysis
The FEAS pre- and post-test ratings for each child were
summed to obtain subtest scores that were totaled to obtain
the total score and compared to the cutoff scores to classify the
child as either “deficient,” “at risk,” or “normal” (Greenspan,
DeGangi, & Wieder, 2001) (see Figure 1). Pre- and post-sub-
test classification levels at pre- and post-test were compared Figure 1. Percentage of children who advanced at least one
for each child in order to assess progress on overall social classification level on the FEAS. * 50% of the children scored
communication and developmental capacities. “normal” at pre-test in area IV and maintained a score of
“normal” at pre-test Thus, all of children (100%) scored “nor-
Results mal” at post-test in level IV.
A comparison of pre- and post-test classification levels on
the FEAS will be presented followed by a description of the
client process and therapist method for each of the four cases for each developmental area in the form of raw scores and
and an integration of the quantitative and qualitative data. subscores, level of functioning, and number of functioning
levels changed (see tables 4–7).
FEAS Scores All participants (100%) were classified as “deficient” on
Comparisons of pre- and post-test scores on the FEAS for overall social communication scores at pre-test. At post-test,
each case (see Figures 2–5) will be discussed in terms of clas- two of the four (50%) advanced two classification levels from
sification (i.e., “deficient,” “at risk,” or “normal”) according to “deficient” to “normal.”
cutoff scores (see Table 8) and clinical descriptions. Figure 1 At pretest all four (100%) participants were scored as “deficient”
illustrates a comparison of the four participants pre- and post- on four of the six subscales: attachment and engagement (level II),
test scores on the FEAS. Each participant’s score is displayed two-way purposeful communication (level III), representational
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 7

Changes in Developmental Levels illustrates the percentage of children who advanced at least one
Pre Post
classification level on the FEAS. (Note that at pre-test two chil-
2
dren [50%] scored “normal” on behavioral organization [level
IV] and maintained this score following treatment. Therefore, at
L
e 1
post-test all children scored “normal” on level IV.)
v
e
l 0 Case Studies
s
Case 1: Kyle
Area I Area Area III Area IV Area V Area Kyle was deficient in all developmental areas at pre-test
Figure 2. Kyle’s changes in developmental levels. and made no functional progress on any of the areas except
for behavioral organization and problem-solving (level IV),
Changes in Developmental Levels in which he advanced two levels to “normal.” See Table  4
Pr Post for Kyle’s pre- and post-test raw scores and changes in
2
functioning level.
Generally, during Kyle’s first six treatment sessions, he

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


L
e 1
exhibited difficulty adapting to the musical environment due
v to his complex sensory system, for example craving vestibular
e
l 0
and proprioceptive inputs. He presented with a mixed-reac-
s
tivity sensory system and challenges in self-regulation. Thus, it
was difficult to engage him musically as he consistently with-
Area I Area Area III Area IV Area V Area VI drew, emotionally and/or sensorily, from any attempts to join
Figure 3. Elaine’s changes in developmental levels. him in play. Following session six, the therapist shortened the
length of sessions, and provided various sensory stimuli (e.g.,
Changes in Developmental Levels deep pressure on his arms and legs, clay for tactile input, and
Pre Post
a rocking game chair for vestibular input) within the musi-
2
cal experiences that catered to Kyle’s individual differences in
order to foster self-regulation and shared attention.
L
e 1
During this time, the therapist provided Kyle with his
v
e
required sensory diet within musical-play experiences. While
l 0 his ability to maintain self-regulation and engage in vocal play
s
began to emerge, interactions were fragmented, brief, and
lacked a continuous flow.
Area I Area II Area III Area IV Area V Area VI During sessions 13 through 16, Kyle began to display the
Figure 4. Anthony’s changes in developmental levels. ability to engage in music experiences, via vocal play, for longer
periods (3–4 measures of 4/4 at a time). In addition, his ability to
Changes in Developmental Levels adapt and problem-solve in musical play began to emerge in the
Pre Post
form of turn-taking, predicting, and imitating brief melodies and
2
short melodic rhythms. During these experiences, the therapist
began interactions by musically following Kyle’s lead (treatment
L
e 1
phase 1) by creating music that mirrored and reflected his play
v and emotionality, while incorporating spaces into the music for
e
l 0
Kyle to fill in. In addition, short repeated melodic phrases were
s
created within simple harmonic frameworks, that is, ii–V–I, that
included simple rhythms to create call-and-response (turn-tak-
Area I Area Area III Area IV Area V Area VI ing) and imitation opportunities.
Figure 5. Michele’s changes in developmental levels. As treatment continued, from sessions 16 to 25, Kyle con-
tinued to demonstrate the ability to problem-solve by joining
capacity (level V), and representational differentiation (level VI). into musical play, rhythmically and tonally, as well as read-
Two (50%) of the four participants scored “normal” on behavioral ing and responding to musical cues that created imitation and
organization and problem-solving (level IV) at pre-test, and two turn-taking experiences. Generally, interactions continued to
were rated as “deficient.” In addition, two participants scored “at be brief and lacked a continuous flow of back-and-forth play.
risk” on self-regulation and shared attention (level I). It appeared that the musical conditions needed to maintain
At post-test three of the four (75%) were considered “nor- sameness in terms of staccato phrasing, short motifs, and fixed
mal” on self-regulation and shared attention, engagement, and dynamics. The therapist provided musical experiences that gen-
behavioral organization. In the area of two-way purposeful erally shifted between treatment Phases 1 and 2 (following and
communication, two of the four participants (50%) had pro- redirecting for two-way purposeful play), always reverting back
gressed one level to “at risk” and one had progressed two levels to following his lead in order to help Kyle re-engage in play.
to “normal.” All of the participants had progressed one or two In summary, as reflected in the Kyle’s FEAS post-test, his
levels on at least one of the six developmental levels. Figure 1 challenges in the ability to sustain self-regulation (level I)
8 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 4.
Comparison of Kyle’s Pre- and Post-FEAS Scores

Pre-test Post-test
Change
Raw scores Raw scores
in functioning
Areas Subscores Subscores level
I. Self-regulation 8 (Deficient) 7 (Deficient) 0
Attentive to play with toys 2 2
Explores objects/toys freely 2 2
Remains calm during play 2 0
Touching textured toys/caregiver 2 2
Shows content affect 1 1
Focused without distraction 0 1
Appears over aroused 0 0
II. Forming relationships 6 (Deficient) 7 (Deficient) 0

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


and engagement
Emotional interest in caregiver 2 2
Relaxed when near caregiver 0 1
Anticipates with curiosity 0 1
Uncomfortable with caregiver 2 2
Initiates closeness to caregiver 0 0
Avoids caregiver 2 0
Socially references caregiver 0 1
Communicates from across space 0 0
III. Two-way purposeful 5 (Deficient) 6 (Deficient) 0
communication
Opens circles of communication 1 1
Initiates intentional actions 1 1
Closes circles of communication 1 2
Uses words, gestures, or sounds 2 2
IV. Behavioral organization 0 (Deficient) 2 (Normal) +2
and problem-solving
Communicates in several modes 0 0
Copies caregiver & incorporates 0 2
V. Representational 1 (Deficient) 0 (Deficient) 0
capacity
Engages in symbolic play 0 0
Engages in pretend play 0 0
Communicates intentions 0 0
Expresses dependency 1 0
Expresses pleasure/excitement 0 0
Expresses assertiveness 0 0
Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas 0 0
VI. Representational 0 (Deficient) 0 (Deficient) 0
differentiation
Bridges 2 unrealistic idea 0 0
Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 0
Use pretend to express dependency 0 0
Use pretend to express pleasure 0 0
Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0 0

made it difficult for him to engage (level II) in continuous two- Case 2: Elaine
way purposeful communication (level III). His ability to adapt Elaine’s pre-test FEAS scores indicated “normal” on behav-
to musical experience in the areas of joining into play interac- ioral organization and problem-solving (level IV), “at risk” on
tions, turn-taking, and imitation appeared to be reflected in self-regulation and shared attention (level I), and “deficient”
his gains in level IV of the FEAS (behavioral organization and in all other developmental areas, that is, engagement, two-
problem-solving). Figure 2 illustrates Kyle’s changes in devel- way purposeful communication, representational capacities,
opmental levels. and representation differentiation. Following treatment, Elaine
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 9

Table 5.
Comparison of Elaine’s Pre- and Post-FEAS Scores

Pre-test Post-test
Change
Raw scores Raw scores
in functioning
Areas Subscores Subscores level
I. Self-regulation 11 (At risk) 12 (Normal) +1
Attentive to play with toys 2 2
Explores objects/toys freely 2 2
Remains calm during play 2 2
Touching textured toys/caregiver 2 2
Shows content affect 1 1
Focused without distraction 1 2
Appears over aroused 1 1
II. Forming relationships 11 (Deficient) 16 (Normal) +2

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


and engagement
Emotional interest in caregiver 2 2
Relaxed when near caregiver 1 2
Anticipates with curiosity 1 2
Uncomfortable with caregiver 2 2
Initiates closeness to caregiver 2 2
Avoids caregiver 1 2
Socially references caregiver 2 2
Communicates from across space 1 2
III. Two-way purposeful 5 (Deficient) 7 (At risk) +1
communication
Opens circles of communication 0 2
Initiates intentional actions 1 1
Closes circles of communication 2 2
Uses words, or sounds, or gestures 2 2
IV. Behavioral organization 3 (Normal) 4 (Normal) 0
and problem-solving
Communicates in several modes 1 2
Copies caregiver & incorporates 2 2
V. Representational 3 (Deficient) 7 (At risk) +1
capacity
Engages in symbolic play 0 1
Engages in pretend play 2 2
Communicates intentions 0 2
Expresses dependency 0 1
Expresses pleasure/excitement 0 1
Expresses assertiveness 0 0
Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas 1 0
VI. Representational 1 (Deficient) 4 (Normal) +2
differentiation
Bridges 2 unrealistic idea 1 2
Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 2
Use pretend to express dependency 0 0
Use pretend to express pleasure 0 0
Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0 0

had advanced to “normal” on all developmental areas except she appeared to become easily dysregulated and unengaged
for representational (level V), in which she improved to “at- while moving and displayed difficulty self-regulating. When
risk.” See Table 5 for Elaine’s pre- and post-test raw scores and engaged in instrument play, she exhibited challenges main-
changes in functioning level. taining engagement and relatedness, as well as difficulty
Treatment sessions 1 through 5 included Elaine craving adapting to dynamic and tempo changes.
motion as she moved aimlessly around the music room while Sessions 6 through 10 consisted of the therapist attempt-
playing each instrument in an unrelated manner. Generally, ing to guide Elaine’s perseverative movements into interactive
10 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 6.
Comparison of Anthony’s Pre- and Post-FEAS Scores

Pre-test Post-test
Change
Raw scores Raw scores
in functioning
Areas Subscores Subscores level
I. Self-regulation 10 (Deficient) 13 (Normal) +2
Attentive to play with toys 2 2
Explores objects/toys freely 2 2
Remains calm during play 2 2
Touching textured toys/caregiver 0 2
Shows content affect 1 1
Focused without distraction 2 2
Appears withdrawn/sluggish 1 2
II. Forming relationships 12 (Deficient) 16 (Normal) +2

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


and engagement
Emotional interest in caregiver 2 2
Relaxed when near caregiver 2 2
Anticipates with curiosity 0 2
Uncomfortable with caregiver 2 2
Initiates closeness to caregiver 2 2
Avoids caregiver 1 2
Socially references caregiver 1 2
Communicates from across space 2 2
III. Two-way purposeful 5 (Deficient) 8 (Normal) +2
Communication
Opens circles of communication 1 2
Initiates intentional actions 1 2
Closes circles of communication 1 2
Uses sounds/words/gestures 2 2
IV. Behavioral organization 3 (Normal) 4 (Normal) 0
and problem-solving
Communicates in several modes 1 1
Copies caregiver & incorporates 1 1
V. Representational 5 (Deficient) 14 (Normal) +2
capacity
Engages in symbolic play 1 2
Engages in pretend play 2 2
Communicates intentions 0 2
Expresses dependency 0 2
Expresses pleasure/excitement 0 2
Expresses assertiveness 2 2
Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas 0 2
VI. Representational 0 (Deficient) 7 (Normal) +2
differentiation
Bridges 2 unrealistic ideas 0 2
Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 2
Use pretend to express dependency 0 2
Use pretend to express pleasure 0 0
Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0 1

dance experiences, following her lead of movement in order periods during musical play. This was evident by her ability
to foster self-regulation, engagement, and relatedness. Her to sustain musical interaction via instrument play and move-
dance-like interactions were generally accompanied by the ment to the waltz-like music being presented. When engaged
therapist providing legato and lyrical singing, playing the in instrument play, she demonstrated the ability to interact
piano in ¾ tempo, and incorporating words that Elaine offered in a related manner on the drum and cymbal by playing the
via her repetitive and echolalic vocalizations. basic beat and engaging in call-and-response play, punctuat-
During sessions 11 through 15, Elaine demonstrated a sig- ing ends of phrases on the cymbal. In addition, while engaged
nificant increase in her ability to self-regulate for extended in these robust musical interactions, Elaine exhibited affective
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 11

Table 7.
Comparison of Michele’s Pre- and Post-FEAS Scores

Pre-test Post-test
Change
Raw scores Raw scores
in functioning
Areas Subscores Subscores level
I. Self-regulation 11 (At risk) 13 (Normal) +1
Attentive to play with toys 2 2
Explores objects/toys freely 2 2
Remains calm during play 2 2
Touching textured toys/caregiver 2 2
Shows content affect 1 2
Focused without distraction 1 2
Appears over aroused 1 1
II. Forming relationships 6 (Deficient) 16 (Normal) +2

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


and engagement
Emotional interest in caregiver 0 2
Relaxed when near caregiver 2 2
Anticipates with curiosity 0 2
Uncomfortable with caregiver 2 2
Initiates closeness to caregiver 2 2
Avoids caregiver 0 2
Socially references caregiver 0 2
Communicates from across space 0 2
III. 2-way purposeful 4 (Deficient) 7 (At risk) +1
communication
Opens circles of communication 0 2
Initiates intentional actions 1 1
Closes circles of communication 1 2
Uses sounds/words/gestures 2 2
IV. Behavioral organization 0 (Deficient) 4 (Normal) +2
and problem-solving
Communicates in several modes 0 2
Copies caregiver & incorporates 0 2
V. Representational 0 (Deficient) 2 (Deficient) 0
capacity
Engages in symbolic play 0 0
Engages in pretend play 0 2
Communicates intentions 0 0
Expresses dependency 0 0
Expresses pleasure/excitement 0 0
Expresses assertiveness 0 0
Creates 2 or more unrelated ideas 0 0
VI. Representational 0 (Deficient) 0 (Deficient) 0
differentiation
Bridges 2 unrealistic ideas 0 0
Bridges 2 realistic ideas 0 0
Use pretend to express dependency 0 0
Use pretend to express pleasure 0 0
Expresses assertiveness in pretend 0 0

extra-musical responses such as smiling and socially referenc- the music while implementing musical spaces and exaggera-
ing the therapist with eye glances and nonverbal vocalizations. tion of Elaine’s responses in order to foster two-way purposeful
Her challenges related to postural control and motor plan- play and reciprocal interactions (treatment phase 2).
ning made it difficult for her to sustain a continuous flow Musical styles and frameworks, such as flamenco and Latin,
of basic beating; however, she maintained engagement and were used during instrument play because of their boldness,
relatedness through facial expressions and social referencing. clarity of tempo, rhythmic nature (included syncopation), and
The therapist incorporated these extra musical responses into emphasis on a strong downbeat. These musical interventions
12 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 8. I), engagement (level II), two-way purposeful communication


Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (level III), behavioral organization and problem-solving (level
Profile Form (Cutoff Scores) IV), and creating, that is, representational capacities (level V)
and bridging ideas with the therapist, that is, representational
Subtest Normal At Risk Deficient differentiation (level VI). Figure 3 illustrates Elaine’s changes in
developmental levels.
Self-regulation & interest in the 12–14 11 0–10
world
Forming relationships and 14–16 13 0–12 Case 3: Anthony
engagement At pretest Anthony scored “normal” on behavioral organ-
2-way purposeful communication 8–10 7 0–6 ization (level IV) but “deficient” in all other developmental
Behavioral organization and 2–4 0–1 areas. At post-test he had advanced two levels to “normal” in
problem-solving the areas of self regulation (level I), engagement (level II), two-
Representational capacity 8–14 8–14 7 way purposeful communication (level III), representational
Representational differentiation 2–10 0–1 capacities (level IV), and representational differentiation (level
Total Score 48–96 46–47 0–45 VI). See Table 6 for Anthony’s pre- and post-test raw scores and
(Greenspan, DeGangi, & Wieder, 2001) changes in functioning level.

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


During sessions 1 through 10, Anthony presented with chal-
lenges in self-regulation as well as the ability to engage in
(i.e., styles) were implemented to foster Elaine’s beating on the relational musical play. He consistently withdrew from musi-
drum and the cymbal. In addition, these styles provided expe- cal interactions, physically and/or emotionally. Generally,
riences that contained drastic contrast between staccato and Anthony presented with a flat affect and typically engaged in
legato articulation, as well as harmonic tension and resolution instrument play in a one-dimensional manner (playing only
points in the music. Furthermore, these musical interventions loud and fast) and unrelated to the therapist’s music. Anthony
reflected Elaine’s affect, and incorporated opportunities for her appeared to have difficulty understanding and/or being aware
to predict and lead musical play interactions. of the therapist and therapist’s music.
Her musical responses were mostly expressed via instru- Sessions 11 through 18 featured a combination of child-
ment play as well as through facial expressions, gestural led (treatment phase 1) and therapist-led (phase 2) treatment
cues, and vocalizations. As the sessions progressed, her phases that consisted of the therapist implementing predict-
responses increased relationally and communicatively. able musical structures and familiar songs. The rational of this
Thus, there was an increase in spontaneous language dur- strategy was to cater to Anthony’s strong memory skills as a
ing improvised song making. She also began to seek out means to foster self-regulation and longer periods of relat-
the therapist and initiate play interactions and ideas. During edness. Repetition and predictability from session to session
these experiences, in which she displayed an increase in appeared to assist Anthony in engaging in related play for a
self-regulation, engagement, and two-way purposeful com- sustained period of time.
munication, the treatment phases shifted from phase 1, fol- Pre-composed songs that required specific musical
lowing her lead, into phase 2 (two-way purposeful musical- responses helped develop Anthony’s musical resources such
play), and at times venturing into phase 3 (affect synchrony). as auditory discrimination, musical range (dynamics and
Thus, musical-play interactions varied between therapist-led tempo) related to changes in the therapist’s music, and ges-
and child-led experiences tural/affective cues related to musical responses. More impor-
During sessions 16 through 19, musical-play interactions tantly, however, Anthony’s improved ability to respond to the
continued to increase in robustness, range, and continuity as therapist’s music illustrated his awareness of another person
the therapist continued to embrace and respect Elaine’s self- as well as his ability to comprehend and respond to musical
stimulatory behaviors via musical and movement experiences. changes as they occurred in time.
Following her lead appeared to foster sustained self-regulation During sessions 18 through 25, as sessions progressed,
and longer periods of engagement and enabled the interaction the familiar songs began to expand and include a variety of
to move into treatment phases 2 and 3. spontaneous musical changes in tempo, dynamics, tonal-
During sessions 20 through 25, Elaine showed more ini- ity, phrasing, and articulation that called out for Anthony’s
tiation in continuously seeking out play interactions with the participation. As his involvement in play increased and
therapist. She also began to lead interactions and require less musical-relational capacities expanded, improvisation was
support in the form of therapist following her lead. incorporated within the familiar songs on a regular basis.
Toward the end of treatment, Elaine began to display capac- During improvisational experiences, treatment phases easily
ities in her ability to create and connect ideas (musical play, transitioned from child- to therapist-led and back to child-
words, gestures, etc.) with the therapist’s while sustaining led and so forth as the therapist implemented techniques of
joint attention and relatedness in a continuous flow of back- elicitation and redirecting to foster Anthony’s ability to regu-
and-forth interactions. The robustness of the interactions pro- late, relate, and musically adapt through a range of musical
vided opportunities for her to explore and experience a range contexts.
of musical play that included non-referential and referential His emerging musical–social resources appeared to be
improvisations as well as improvised songwriting. Her capaci- assimilated into more spontaneous music-making experiences.
ties in musical play appeared to be reflected in her progress Anthony initiated lyrical content, which included singing to
to “normal” FEAS scores in the areas of self-regulation (level his favorite doll, as well as requesting specific instruments and
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 13

offering song ideas. To that end, he engaged in higher levels back-and-forth musical pauses. Her ability to maintain self-
of musical interaction such initiating and assimilating and dif- regulation and engage via a range of musical experiences
ferentiating musical ideas, via a range of affect and musical had expanded. In addition, because of the increase in atten-
expressivity. These capacities seemed to be indicated in his tion and engagement, Michele’s ability to engage in two-
post-test FEAS scores by advances from “deficient” to “nor- way purposeful musical play had emerged. These capaci-
mal” in all areas. Figure  4 illustrates Anthony’s changes in ties demonstrated in musical play appeared to be reflected
developmental levels. in her improved FEAS post-test scores. Her FEAS scores
indicated “normal” on self-regulation (level I), engagement
Case 4: Michele (level II), and behavior organization (level IV). In addition,
At pretest, Michele scored “deficient” on all developmental she improved to “at-risk” in the areas of two-way purpose-
areas except for self- regulation (level I), in which she scored ful communication (level III). Figure  5 illustrates Michele’s
“at-risk.” Following treatment, her post-test indicated “nor- changes in developmental levels.
mal” on self-regulation (level I), engagement (level II), and
behavioral organization (level IV). She scored “at-risk” on Discussion
two-way purposeful communication (level II) and maintained The results of this case series of DIR-based IMT are con-
a score of “deficient” on representational differentiation (level sistent with evidence from other IMT studies showing its

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


V) and representational differentiation (level VI). See Table 7 effectiveness for improving social communication skills in
for Michele’s pre- and post-test raw scores and changes in children with ASD (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Edgerton, 1994;
functioning level. Holck, 2004b; Gattino et  al., 2011; Kim, Wigram, & Gold,
During sessions 1 through 9, Michele exhibited difficulty 2008, 2009; Robbins & Robbins, 1991; Nordoff & Robbins,
with engaging in musical play due to challenges in her abil- 2007; Thompson, McFerran, & Gold, 2013). In addition, the
ity to sustain self-regulation, attention, motor plan, and pro- results align with non-music-therapy DSP approaches such
cess sensory information. These differences consistently as DIRFloortime that emphasize the importance of following
interfered with her ability to interact. In addition, her sensory the child’s lead while considering developmental capacities
system appeared to easily overload, in which she withdrew within the context of a relationship to improve social com-
from musical interactions in an under-reactive manner. She munication (Casenhiser, Shanker, & Stieben, 2013; Dionne
appeared unaware of the musical surroundings and presented & Martini, 2011; Ingersoll, Dvortcsak, A., Whalen, C., &
with challenges related to understanding the functional pur- Sikora, 2005; Mahoney & Perales, 2003, 2005; Pajareya
pose of the instruments. These difficulties impacted her ability & Nopmaneejumruslers, 2011, 2012; Solomon, Necheles,
to comprehend basic social dynamics of relating and commu- Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007; Solomon, Van Egeren, Mahoney,
nicating in musical play. Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014).
During sessions 10 through 18, she began to display islands This study is one of the first in music therapy to demonstrate
of capacity of self-regulation and shared attention while improvement on social communication among children with
engaged in instrument play with the therapist. The therapist autism using a standardized, criterion play-based observation
continuously followed her lead by improvising music that tool as an outcome measure. While the sample in this study is
reflected her play and emotionality while incorporating sim- small, these results suggest that gains in social communication,
ple short melodic phrases, vocally while utilizing percussion, after receiving IMT delivered by a music therapist, generalized
that contained clear cadences. These phrases were repeated in to a toy play-based context in an environment not associated
order for her to become familiar with the musical motifs, thus with music therapy. Therefore, the data suggest that the prin-
helping her engage and attend to musical play. She began to ciples underlying DIR-based IMT may produce improvements
display an increase in self-regulation and appeared to become in social communication of children with ASD despite signifi-
more affectively connected to musical play by smiling and cant differences in setting, mediator, and medium. However,
offering nonverbal vocalizations that displayed prosody and without a control group, it is difficult to know whether the
musical contours. She also began to exhibit the ability to changes in the FEAS scores were directly attributable to the
engage in brief turn-taking experiences and occasionally com- IMT intervention. Furthermore, additional in-depth, repeated,
pleted musical phrases, in a related manner, when playing and objective measures of child development, for example IQ,
percussion and pitched reed horns while therapist accompa- language, and so forth, could have been added to improve the
nied vocally and with various non-pitched percussive instru- measurement of the outcomes.
ments. Although these new social–musical capacities began The difference in the duration and intensity of the DIR-
to emerge, Michele displayed difficulty sustaining the play based IMT intervention used in this study and the standard
interactions whereby they were brief, fragmented, and lacked DIRFloortime intervention is significant. Children in this
a continuous flow back-and-forth interaction. During these study received a total of 13 hours of IMT, one hour a week
musical experiences, the therapist generally reverted back to over 13 weeks. This is consistent with other IMT studies that
treatment phase 1, following her lead in order to re-engage have yielded significant improvement in social communi-
Michele in musical play. cation with children with ASD (Robbins & Robbins, 1991;
During the final sessions of treatment, Michele became Nordoff & Robbins, 2007; Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2008, 2009;
increasingly responsive to musical cues (e.g., dynamics, Gattino et al., 2011). DIRFloortime studies, however, ranged
tempo, and affect) in the form of relational and communica- from seven weeks (Dionne & Martini, 2011) to 12  months
tive instrument play as well as adaption, such as joining into (Solomon, Necheles, Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007; Solomon, Van
play, turn-taking, and cause-and-effect relationships during Egeren, Mahoney, Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014) and involved
14 Music Therapy Perspectives

monthly home visits to train parents as well as a minimum 2007; Geretsegger, Holck, Carpente, Elefant, & Kim, 2015;
of two hours a day of parent-mediated treatment (Solomon, Kim, Wigram, & Gold, 2008, 2009; Thompson, McFerran, &
Necheles, Ferch, & Bruckman, 2007; Solomon, Van Egeren, Gold, 2013).
Mahoney, Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014; Dionne & Martini,
2011; Liao et  al., 2014). Whether IMT offers advantages for
improving social communication in children with ASD is a References

question that deserves further research using larger samples Aigen, K. (2005). Music-centered music therapy. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers
and rigorous experimental methods. Aldred, C., Green, J., & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication inter-
vention for children with autism: Pilot randomised controlled treatment study
In addition, the development of a valid and reliable music-
suggesting effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(8),
therapy-based assessment instrument is needed to objectively
1420–1430.
compare gains in social communication in an IMT context Alvin, J., & Warwick, A. (1991). Music therapy for the autistic child (2nd ed.).
with those made in a toy play-based context. An observational Oxford: Oxford University Press.
tool validated against the FEAS as well as an ASD diagnos- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
tic instrument such as the Autistic Diagnostic Observational tal disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Schedule (ADOS) would also allow for a comparison of the Applebaum, E., Egel, A. L., Koegel, R. L., & Imhoff, B. (1979). Measuring musical
abilities of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
DIRFloortime and DIR-based IMT.
9(3), 279–285.

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


The single group design of this study cannot rule out the Blackstock, E. G. (1978). Cerebral asymmetry and the development of early infantile
possible effect of maturation and curriculum of the thera- autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 339–353.
peutic day-school in which the children in this study were Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervais, H., Good, C. D., Bourgeois, M., Plumet, M.
enrolled. Future studies are needed that will use sample sizes H.… Brunelle, F. (2004). Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in
large enough to randomly assign participants to conditions childhood autism: A voxel-based morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage, 23(1),
and identify pretreatment characteristics expected to effect 364–369.
Bonnel, A., Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Trudel, M., Gallun, E. & Bonnel, A. M. (2003).
results. In addition, future studies investigating the effective-
Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analy-
ness of IMT should include follow-up data to assess the main- sis. Journal of Cognition Neuroscience, 15(2), 226–235.
tenance of improvements in social communication over time Bruscia, K. E. (1987). Improvisational models of music therapy. Springfield, IL: C.
and an “interaction only” condition to determine whether it Thomas.
is the one-one interaction or the intervention provided that is Bruscia, K. E. (2014). Defining music therapy (3rd ed.). Gilsum, NH: Barcelona
responsible for the results. Publishers.
Carpente, J. A. (2011). Addressing core features of autism: Integrating Nordoff-
Implications for Clinical Practice Robbins music therapy within the developmental, individual-difference, rela-
tionship-based (DIR®)/FloortimeTM model. In A. Meadows (Ed.), Developments
Implications of this study for IMT are closely related to in music therapy practice: Case study perspectives. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona
clinical practice. Although it is imperative to understand a Publishers.
child’s musical responses and create musical goals based on Carpente, J. A. (2012). DIR®/Floortime™ Model: Introduction and considerations for
his/her musical relatedness, it is equally important to under- improvisational music therapy. In P. Kern & M. Humpal (Eds.), Early childhood
stand biological factors that may impinge upon a child’s abil- music therapy and autism spectrum disorders: Developing potential in young
children and their families. Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
ity to engage in musical play. Thus, as indicated in this study,
Carpente, J. A. (2013). The Individual Music-Centered Assessment Profile for
a child who has difficulty engaging in conventional musical Neurodevelopmental Disorders (IMCAP-ND): A clinical manual. Baldwin, NY:
play may be experiencing factors completely unrelated to Regina Publishers.
music (e.g., difficulties with motor-planning, postural con- Carpente, J. A. (2014). Individual Music-Centered Assessment Profile for
trol, auditory and/or visual processing, sensory modulation Neurodevelopmental Disorders (IMCAP-ND): New developments in music-
and integration, expressive or receptive language, and/or centered evaluation. Music Therapy Perspectives, 32(1), 56–60.
self-regulation). Casenhiser, D. M., Shanker, S. G., & Stieben, J. (2013). Learning through interaction
in children with autism: Preliminary data from asocial-communication-based
Furthermore, because treatment focuses primarily on
intervention. Autism, 17(2), 220–241.
facilitating relatedness and communication in children with Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., et al. (2010).
ASD, it is important to look for these qualities both within Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The
and outside music, such as in a smile, a gaze, or a hug. Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125, 17–23.
Of course, these responses can be accepted and processed Dionne, M., & Martini, R. (2011). Floor time play with a child with autism: A single-
within the context of musical play, or may be a result of subject study. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 78(3), 196–203.
Edgerton, C. L. (1994). The effect of improvisational music therapy on the commu-
a musical interaction. However, in all of their forms, these
nicative behaviors of autistic children. Journal of Music Therapy, 31(1), 31–62.
responses should be regarded as communicative and related
Gattino, G. S., Riesgo, R. D. S., Longo, D., Leite, J. C. L., & Faccini, L. S. (2011).
responses. Effects of relational music therapy on communication of children with autism:
Current trends in ASD research indicate the increased use A  randomized controlled study. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 20(2),
and effectiveness of developmentally based strategies on social 142–154.
communication of children with ASD (Casenhiser, Shanker, & Geretsegger, M., Holck, U., Carpente, J., Elefant, C., & Kim, J. (2015). Common char-
Stieben, 2013; Dawson et al., 2010, Ingersol, 2008; Ingersoll, acteristics of improvisational music therapy with children with autism spectrum
disorder: Developing a treatment guide. Journal of Music Therapy, retrieved
Dvortcsak, Whalen, & Sikora, 2005; Prizant, Wetherby, Rubin,
from http://oxfordjournals.org/
& Laurent, 2003; Schreibman, 2005; Schreibman et al., 2015). Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., &
This research seeks to follow in the direction of these new Pickles, A. (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in chil-
developments and adds to the existing body of IMT and ASD dren with autism (PACT): A randomized controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732),
literature (Aigen, 2005; Edgerton, 1994; Nordoff & Robbins, 2152–2160.
Music Therapy Program on Social Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 15

Greenspan, S. I. (1992). Infancy and early childhood: The practice of clinical assess- A comparative study. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(2),
ment and intervention with emotional and developmental challenges. Madison, 77–85.
CT: International Universities Press. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music
Greenspan, S. I., DeGangi, G., & Wieder, S. (2001). The functional emotional in highfunctioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of
assessment scale (FEAS) for infancy and early childhood: Clinical and research Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 1057–1065.
applications. Bethesda, MD: Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental and Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (2007). Creative music therapy: A guide to fostering clini-
Learning Disorders. cal musicianship. Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers.
Greenspan, S. I., & Shanker, S. G. (2004). The first idea: How symbols, language, and Oldfield, A. (2001). Music therapy with young children with autism and their par-
intelligence evolved from our primate ancestors to modern humans. Cambridge, ents: Developing communications through playful musical interactions specific
MA: Da Capo Lifelong Books. to each child. Music Therapy in Europe, 47–62.
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1997). Developmental patterns and outcomes in Pajareya, K., & Nopmaneejumruslers, K. (2011). A pilot randomized controlled trial
infants and children with disorders in relating and communicating: A  chart of DIR/Floortime™ parent training intervention for pre-school children with
review of 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. Journal of autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(5), 563–577.
Developmental and Learning Disorders, 1, 87–142. Pajareya, K., & Nopmaneejumruslers, K. (2012). A one-year prospective follow-up
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2006a). Engaging autism: Using the Floortime study of a DIR/Floortime™ parent training intervention for preschool children
approach to help children relate, communicate, and think. Cambridge, MA: Da with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand,
Capo Lifelong Books. 95(9), 1184.
Greenspan, S. I., Wieder, S. (2006b). Infant and early childhood mental health: Paul, R. (2008). Interventions to improve communication in autism. Child and
A  comprehensive developmental approach to assessment and intervention. Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(4), 835–856.

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on June 29, 2016


American Psychiatric Association. Prizant, B. M., Wetherby, A. M., Rubin, E., & Laurent, A. C. (2003). The SCERTS
Harris, H., Israeli, D., Minshew, N., Bonneh, Y., Heeger, D. J., Behrmann, M., & Model: A  transactional, family-centered approach to enhancing communica-
Sagi, D. (2015). Perceptual learning in autism: Over-specificity and possible tion and socioemotional abilities of children with autism spectrum disorder.
remedies. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 1574–1576. Infants & Young Children, 16(4), 296–316.
Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labeling and disembedding in autism. Journal of Robbins, C., & Robbins, C. (1991). Self-communication in creative music therapy.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 543–551. In K. Bruscia (ed.), Case Studies in Music Therapy (pp. 55–72). Gilsum, NH:
Heaton, P. (2005). Interval and contour processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Barcelona Publishers.
Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 787–793. Rossignol, D. A. (2009). Novel and emerging treatments for autism spectrum disor-
Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch processing: A precur- ders: A systematic review. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 21(4), 213–36.
sor for savant musical ability. Music Perception, 15(3), 291–305. Schreibman, L. E. (2005). The science and fiction of autism. New York: Harvard
Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1999). Can children with autistic spectrum University Press.
disorders perceive affect in music? An experimental investigation. Psychological Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Stahmer, A. C., Landa, R., Rogers, S. J., McGee, G.
Medicine, 29(6), 1405–1410. G.& Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions:
Holck, U. (2004a). Interaction themes in music therapy: Definition and delimitation. Empirically validated treatments for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 13(1), 3–19. and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2407-8
Holck, U. (2004b). Turn-taking in music therapy with children with communication Simpson, R. L. (2005). Evidence-based practices and students with autism spec-
disorders. British Journal of Music Therapy, 18(2), 45–54. trum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 20(3),
Hwang, B., & Hughes, C. (2000). Increasing early social-communicative skills of 140–149.
preverbal preschool children with autism through social interactive training. Solomon, R., Necheles, J., Ferch, C., & Bruckman, D. (2007). Evaluation of a par-
Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 25(1), 18–28. ent training program for young children with autism: The PLAY Project Home
Ingersoll, B. (2008). The social role of imitation in autism: Implications for the treat- Consulting Program, Autism, 11:205–24.
ment of imitation deficits. Infants & Young Children, 21(2), 107–119. Solomon, R., Van Egeren, L. A., Mahoney, G., Huber, M. S. Q., & Zimmerman, P.
Ingersoll, B., Dvortcsak, A., Whalen, C., & Sikora, D. (2005). The effects of a devel- (2014). PLAY Project Home Consultation intervention program for young chil-
opmental, social–pragmatic language intervention on rate of expressive lan- dren with autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of
guage production in young children with autistic spectrum disorders. Focus on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(8), 475.
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 20(4), 213–222. Thaut, M. (1987). Visual versus auditory (musical) stimulus preferences in autistic chil-
Ingersoll, B., Lewis, E., & Kroman, E. (2007). Teaching the imitation and spontaneous use dren: A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 17, 425–432.
of descriptive gestures in young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral Thaut, M. (1988). Measuring musical responsiveness in autistic children: A  com-
intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(8), 1446–1456. parative analysis of improvised musical tone sequences of autistic, normal, and
Kaiser, A. P., Hancock, T. B., & Nietfield, J. (2000). The effects of parent-implemented mentally retarded individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
enhanced milieu teaching on social communication of children who have 18(4), 561–571.
autism. Early Education and Development, 11(4), 423–426. Thompson, G. (2012). Family-centered music therapy in the home environment:
Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A. C., Wong, C., Kwon, S., & Locke, J. (2010). Randomized Promoting interpersonal engagement between children with autism spectrum
controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with disorder and their parents. Music Therapy Perspectives, 30(2), 109–116.
autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 40(9), 1045–1056. Thompson, G. A., McFerran, K. S., & Gold, C. (2013). Family-centered music therapy
Kim, J., Wigram, T., & Gold. (2008). The effects of improvisational music therapy on to promote social engagement in young children with severe autism spectrum
joint attention behaviors in autistic children: A  randomized controlled study. disorder: A randomized controlled study. Child: Care, Health and Development,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1758–1766. 40(6), 840–852.
Kim, J., Wigram, T., & Gold, C. (2009). Emotional, motivational, and interpersonal Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social interaction project for children
responsiveness of children with autism in improvisational music therapy. with autism spectrum disorders beginning in the second year of life: A prelimi-
Autism, 13(4), 389–409. nary study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26(2), 67–82.
Liao, S. T., Hwang, Y. S., Chen, Y. J., Lee, P., Chen, S. J., & Lin, L. Y. (2014). Home- Wheeler, D., Williams, K., Seida, J., & Ospina, M. (2008). The Cochrane Library
based DIR/Floortime™ intervention program for preschool children with autism and autism spectrum disorder: An overview of reviews. Evidence-Based Child
spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Health: A Cochrane Review Journal, 3(1), 3–15.
Pediatrics, 34(4), 356–367. Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig, A., Kucharczyk, S....Schultz,
Mahoney, G., & Perales, F. (2003). Using relationship-focused intervention to T. R. (2015). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and young adults
enhance the social–emotional functioning of young children with autism spec- with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehensive review. Journal of Autism and
trum disorders. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23(2), 74–86. Developmental Disorders, 45(7), 1951–1966.
Mahoney, G., & Perales, F. (2005). Relationship-focused early intervention with Woodward, A. (2004). Music therapy for autistic children and their families: A crea-
children with pervasive developmental disorders and other disabilities: tive spectrum. British Journal of Music Therapy, 18(1), 8–14.

Вам также может понравиться