Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

Child Development, July/August 2009, Volume 80, Number 4, Pages 952–967

Developmental Social Cognitive Neuroscience: Insights From Deafness


David Corina Jenny Singleton
University of California, Davis University of Illinois
at Urbana-Champaign

The condition of deafness presents a developmental context that provides insight into the biological, cultural,
and linguistic factors underlying the development of neural systems that impact social cognition. Studies of
visual attention, behavioral regulation, language development, and face and human action perception are
discussed. Visually based culture and language provides deaf children with affordances that promote
resiliency and optimization in their development of visual engagement, executive functions, and theory of
mind. These experiences promote neural adaptations permitting nuanced perception of classes of linguistic
and emotional-social behaviors. Studies of deafness provide examples of how interactions and contributions
of biological predispositions and genetic phenotypes with environmental and cultural factors including child-
hood experiences and actions of caregivers shape developmental trajectories (M. I. Posner & M. K. Rothbart,
2007).

The field of social cognitive neuroscience has been caregivers that may shape developmental trajecto-
instrumental in bridging formal psychological con- ries (Posner & Rothbart, 2007).
structs derived from social psychology with emerg- Both adult and developmental psychological
ing understanding of neurofunctional properties of research literatures have benefited from under-
the human brain. Growing evidence from this work standing how early childhood experiences may
supports the notion of specialized neural regions alter the typical developmental time course of
that are preferentially involved in the processing of sociocognitive functions, as in cases of acquired
social information. As social phenomena inherently brain damage (Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 1998;
cuts a broad path through human experience, rang- Damasio, 1995; Koenigs et al., 2007), developmental
ing from understanding oneself to the appreciation disorders such as autism (Amaral, Schumann, &
of and interactions with others, it should not be Nordahl, 2008; Bachevalier & Loveland, 2006;
surprising that the neural systems implicated in the Baron-Cohen & Belmonte, 2005; Frith, 2001;
mediation of social behaviors are multipurpose and Oberman & Ramachandran, 2007) and Williams
highly intertwined with more basic sensory-percep- syndrome (Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai, &
tual, emotional, linguistic, and cognitive compo- St. George, 2000; Karmiloff-Smith, 2007; Meyer-
nents (Beer & Ochsner, 2006; Lieberman, 2006). Lindenberg et al., 2005) or language deprivation
In the quest to understand these neural systems, (Curtiss, 1977; Newport, 1990). These accidents of
recent work has begun to explore their develop- nature often put into relief ‘‘seams’’ within and
ment, striving to explicate the interactions and con- between complex interacting systems, and thus
tributions of biological predispositions and genetic provide clues to neural differentiation of processing
phenotypes with environmental and cultural factors systems as a function of early experience.
including childhood experiences and actions of In the present case, we consider aspects of
social cognition within a relatively understudied
domain, childhood deafness. As we hope to show,
This work is supported by National Science Foundation (NSF)
the context of deafness presents highly unique
Grant SBE-0541953 awarded to Gallaudet University and sup- developmental situations that offer great potential
ports Drs. Corina and Singleton and National Institutes of for providing critical insights into the biological,
Health–National Institute on Deafness and Other Communica-
tion Disorders Grant 2ROI-DC03099 to D. Corina. We thank
cultural, and linguistic factors that underlie the
Mary C. Mendoza for her help in the preparation of this manu- development of neural systems that impact social
script.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
to David Corina, NSF Science of Learning Center for Visual
Language and Visual Learning (VL2), Gallaudet University,  2009, Copyright the Author(s)
Washington, DC 20002. Electronic mail may be sent to Journal Compilation  2009, Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
corina@ucdavis.edu. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2009/8004-0003
Insights From Deafness 953

cognition. While we cannot hope to cover all mately 9% of the population (Ries, 1994; this figure
issues in an exhaustive fashion, we consider sev- includes those who lose their hearing due to
eral pertinent examples in hopes that our modest advancing age). The prevalence of profound con-
efforts may encourage others to consider this genital deafness is in the range of 0.5–1 per 1,000
unique human condition. The present review con- births and these infants experience reduced or no
siders deafness within a broad perspective. We access to the spoken language used by their family.
hope to demonstrate that insights regarding the An even smaller percentage of these deaf individu-
development of social cognition may be gleaned als are born to deaf parents and acquire a signed
not only from understanding the adaptations of a language, such as ASL, as their native language
cognitive system in the face of sensory depriva- (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004).
tion, but also from understanding deafness itself By some estimates, 96% (Mitchell & Karchmer,
as a social experience, in which cultural and lin- 2004) of deaf children are raised in households
guistic traditions are adaptive and provide deaf with parents who are hearing. There is continuing
children with unique developmental niches that controversy over the best strategies to promote
could perhaps contribute to differentiated process- linguistic competencies in children who are unable
ing. We hope to show how multifaceted conditions to process spoken language as efficiently as their
of deafness puts into relief differential contribu- normally hearing counterparts. Parents must weigh
tions of sensory experience, cultural practices, and a wide range of factors in their decisions in the
linguistic influences that factor into social cognitive face of often conflicting information from medical,
development. social services, educators, and community advo-
Our review will discuss emerging findings cates. While technological advances in assistive
from cognitive and neural studies that highlight hearing devices, including digital hearing aids and
three main areas of development (visual attention, cochlear implantations may provide improved
high-level visual processing, and language and access to auditory information, linguistic develop-
communication) that are known to contribute to ment in deaf children raised in hearing households
social cognitive development (Grossman & is often compromised. Language deficits are often
Johnson, 2007). In these areas, deaf individuals present regardless of whether such children are
have shown particular behavioral or neural organi- learning ASL (Schick & Hoffmeister, 2001; Strong &
zational patterns that are distinguishable from their Prinz, 1997), English-based signing (Geers, Moog, &
hearing counterparts. Most research in this area has Schick, 1984; Schick & Moeller, 1992) or spoken
focused on how factors of sensory and ⁄ or linguistic English (Geers et al., 1984; de Villiers, 2003). In
deprivation have contributed to these differences. contrast, deaf children in deaf signing families,
In the present review, we further consider who are exposed to sign language from birth,
how behavioral and neural changes may also be acquire sign language just as hearing children
shaped by environmental experiences, such as the effortlessly acquire spoken languages, along a
cultural and linguistic practices of families and similar developmental time course, and influenced
communities. A review of these data highlights how by linguistic complexity and cognitive development
dorsal–parietal, frontal-executive, and temporal– (Lillo-Martin, 1999; Newport & Meier, 1985). As
ventral cortical systems may be differentially we will discuss, the linguistic and sociocultural
sensitive to these factors. We begin with a brief context of deaf children raised by deaf caregivers
overview of childhood deafness, American Sign (in comparison to those raised by hearing care-
Language (ASL), and the American Deaf givers) has important implications for social
community. cognitive development.
ASL is an indigenous language that evolved
within North America and is used by members
who self-identify with the Deaf community
Deafness, ASL and Deaf Culture
and who are part of ‘‘Deaf Culture’’ (Ladd, 2003;
Acquired deafness may result from infection or Padden & Humphries, 2005). The capitalization
drugs administered during pregnancy. Hereditary of the word deaf in this context serves to sig-
deafness is associated with over 350 genetic condi- nify recognition of deaf individuals as a distinct
tions (Martini, Mazzoli, & Kimberling, 1997), and cultural group, who are considered a minority
about one third of these genetic conditions are asso- community and whose primary language is
ciated with syndromes (Petit, 1996). The prevalence signed (for discussions, see Lane, 1984; Padden &
of hearing loss (from mild to profound) is approxi- Humphries, 1988).
954 Corina and Singleton

With a structure distinct from English, ASL is a the evidence does indicate that for higher level pro-
fully complex, natural human language. Although cessing tasks, compensatory differences may be
ASL is only one of many autonomous sign lan- observed in restricted domains.
guages of the world, it is the one that has been Research indicates specific enhancements for
studied most extensively. Its linguistic history processing of the visual periphery and the detection
involves a creolization of Old French Sign Lan- of movement in peripheral vision in the deaf
guage with local signed languages and homesign (Bavelier et al., 2001; Neville & Lawson, 1987a,
forms that existed in the United States in the early 1987b, 1987c; Parasnis & Samar, 1985; Proksch &
1800s (Woodward, 1976). Bavelier, 2002). There is growing consensus that the
In the 1960s, linguists began to document the visual skills for which deaf individuals exhibit
distinct grammatical structure of ASL (Stokoe, heightened performance (compared with hearing
1960 ⁄ 2005). Since then, the field of signed language individuals) appear under specific conditions,
linguistics has prospered, including studies of other namely, those that engage spatial attention (for
signed languages of the world. This linguistic recent views, see Bavelier, Dye, & Hauser, 2006).
research has demonstrated patterns that are shared Moreover, these studies suggest that it is auditory
with spoken languages and revealed unique lin- deprivation, rather than sign language experience
guistic structures that capitalize on the visual–ges- per se, that brings about these observed attention
tural modality (for recent discussions, see differences (Bavelier et al., 2001; Bosworth & Dob-
Emmorey, 2002; Sandler & Lillo-Martin, 2006). With kins, 2002; Neville & Lawson, 1987a, 1987b, 1987c;
the emergence of Deaf Studies and the legitimatiza- Proksch & Bavelier, 2002). Specifically, several stud-
tion of ASL as a U.S. minority language in its own ies have examined the performance of deaf native
right, with an estimated number of 100,000–300,000 signers to hearing native signers. This latter group
users (Padden & Humphries, 2005), the language is composed of normally hearing individuals raised
and culture of the Deaf community have thrived in deaf signing households. The comparison of
and gained increased attention from multiple these groups provides an opportunity to systemati-
research disciplines (e.g., anthropology, psychol- cally examine the contributions of language experi-
ogy, and education). ence (shared between these two groups) from
auditory experience (which differs between these
groups). In these studies, it was only deaf subjects
who showed evidence of enhancement, suggesting
Visual Attention: Biological and Social
that the lack of auditory input may be more impor-
Implications
tant than early sign language experience in altering
There is a common assumption that deficits in a spatial attention. Neuroimaging and electrophysiol-
sensory modality provide increased sensitivities in ogy data indicate functional changes in dorsal
remaining modalities: in the case of individuals visual pathways among deaf subjects in response to
with early blindness, for example, that their hearing visual attention tasks (Armstrong, Neville, Hillyard,
is superior to that of sighted individuals. Similarly, & Mitchell, 2002; Bavelier et al., 2000, 2001; Neville
in the case of early profound deafness, enhance- & Lawson, 1987a, 1987b, 1987c). These data, cou-
ments in visual processing could be presumed. pled with animal studies of sensory deprivation,
However, careful research in this domain has dem- provide a compelling case for changes to dorsal–
onstrated that primary sensory abilities such as visual processing streams as a function of auditory
auditory detection thresholds, intensity discrimina- deprivation (Bavelier & Neville, 2002; Bavelier
tion (Niemeyer & Starlinger, 1981), and auditory et al., 2006; Neville & Bavelier, 2002).
gap detection (Weaver & Stevens, 2006) in the case Differences in visual attention that develop dur-
of the blind, and brightness discrimination (Bross, ing a deaf person’s childhood may impact aspects
1979), visual contrast sensitivity (Finney & Dobkins, of social interaction. There is a controversial record
2001), temporal discrimination thresholds (Mills, of research that has suggested deafness is associ-
1985), temporal resolution (Bross & Sauerwein, ated with social-emotional disturbances. However,
1980; Poizner & Tallal, 1987), and discrimination many of these early studies of deaf children’s
thresholds for motion direction (Bosworth & behavior were often flawed by a failure to appreci-
Dobkins, 1999) in the case of the deaf, are not ate the heterogeneity within the deaf population
significantly altered as a result of loss of a being tested. A common claim is that deaf chil-
competing modality (see Bavelier & Neville, 2002; dren’s behavioral problems are related to impulsiv-
Niemeyer & Starlinger, 1981, for discussion). Yet, ity and distractibility. Reivich and Rothrock (1972)
Insights From Deafness 955

suggested that impulsivity and lack of inhibition they argue, results from redistribution of attention
accounted for a significant amount of the problem away from the center and toward peripheral vision.
behavior in deaf pupils reported by teachers. As A similar reallocation view was proposed earlier by
reported in Dye, Hauser, and Bavelier (2008), Neville and collaborators (Neville & Lawson,
mothers rate deaf children as having greater 1987a, 1987b, 1987c; Neville, Schmidt, & Kutas,
distractibility-hyperactivity problems than hearing 1983; see Dye et al., 2008, for a more extensive dis-
children (Quittner, Glueckauf, & Jackson, 1990), cussion of these issues).
while other studies suggest little evidence of A different perspective is offered by Hauser,
deaf–hearing differences in attention span Lukomski, and Hillman (2008) who consider execu-
(Meadow, 1976). More recently, researchers have tive function (EF) skills rather than selective visual
used manipulations of continuous performance attention as a central factor in the reports of impul-
tasks (CPT) with deaf subjects to differentially sivity and distractibility in the deaf children. They
assess variables such as impulsivity, persistence, note that response inhibition is the prerequisite to
and distractibility (Mitchell & Quittner, 1996; self-regulation, which is instrumental for purposive
Quittner, Smith, Osberger, Mitchell, & Katz, 1994; intentional behavior (Barkley, 2001). Hauser et al.
Smith, Quittner, Osberger, & Miyamoto, 1998). (2008) note that while several studies (e.g., Gioia,
According to these studies, deaf children have been Isquith, Guy, & Kentworth, 2000; Roth, Isquith, &
found to exhibit more impulsivity and experience Gioia, 2005) have reported deaf and hearing differ-
increased distractibility. In a different CPT study, ences on clinical tests and rating scales of EF such
Parasnis, Samar, and Berent (2003) reported that as the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
deaf college students with hearing parents had Functions, other studies have failed to find such
increased impulsivity when selecting the appropri- differences (Everhart & Marschark, 1997). Studies
ate response accompanied by decreased perceptual comparing the impulsivity rates of deaf children
sensitivity (they found it harder to distinguish born to deaf parents versus those born to hearing
between targets and nontargets) compared with parents, found that deaf-of-deaf children have been
hearing college students. Some have interpreted rated more positively, indicating lower impulsivity
such differences to reflect a deficit in visual rates (Harris, 1978; Hauser, Lukomski, & Isquith,
selective attention stemming from poor multimodal 2009; Hauser, Wills, & Isquith, 2006; Oberg, 2007).
sensory integration as a result of early, profound Taken together, these studies reveal an intriguing
hearing loss. Smith et al. (1998), for example, interplay between brain adaptations resulting from
suggest that sound is an enabling condition for the altered sensory experience and potential behavioral
development of selective attention. In this deficiency sequelae that, in particular contexts, maybe consid-
hypothesis view (Smith et al., 1998), the integration ered maladaptive. Reports of increased impulsivity
of information from the different senses is and distractibility have been considered in relation
proposed to be an essential part of the development to visual attention and EF. Researchers such as
of normal attention functioning within each Smith et al. (1998) view the lack of hearing as a cau-
individual sensory modality. Thus, the lack of sal factor in resulting in deficit selective attention
access to sound results in underdeveloped selective processes, whereas Parasnis et al. (2003) suggest
attention capacities in deaf children. Hearing the behavior is not deficient per se but rather an
individuals can attend selectively to a narrow adaptive response in the face of increased reliance
visual field and still monitor the broader environ- on visual information monitoring. In this view,
ment through sounds. This multimodal contact there is no explicit claim regarding the role of
with the world permits an efficient division of audition in the development of selective visual
labor. Deaf individuals, by contrast, must simul- attention. Instead, these researchers attribute the
taneously use vision both to accomplish the specific behavior to adaptations in the distribution of
tasks and to monitor the broader environment attention and focus on functional-anatomical
(Mitchell, 1996; Smith et al., 1998). accounts that implicate parietal attention networks
An alternative interpretation of the behavior is in this population (Dye, Baril, & Bavelier, 2007; Dye
offered by Parasnis et al. (2003) who argue that the et al., 2008; Parasnis et al., 2003). Alternatively,
apparent impulsivity exhibited by deaf subjects Hauser et al. (2008) implicate differences in EF,
may be an adaptive strategy. In the absence of which may manifest in terms of differences in
auditory input, deaf individuals must have greater response inhibition. Functional-anatomical models
reliance upon vision for being alerted. The which implicate the dorsolateral prefrontal area
decreased perceptual sensitivity on the CPT task, in EF and working memory and the ventromedial
956 Corina and Singleton

prefrontal areas with emotional and social decision countries and using natural signed languages), a
making (Anderson, 1998; Eslinger & Grattan, 1991; repertoire of ‘‘visually responsive’’ culturally
MacPherson, Phillips, & Della Sela, 2002) factor embedded behaviors has been documented among
significantly in this account. caregivers (Erting, Prezioso, & Hynes, 1990; Harris,
At this time, it is difficult to adjudicate between 2000; Koester, Brooks, & Traci, 2000; Waxman &
these accounts. One notes, for example, that the Spencer, 1997). To summarize, deaf caregivers: (a)
measures that have been used to establish these use visual or tactile signals to elicit their child’s
effects range widely in methodology and ecological visual attention (through a hand-wave or physical
validity, from well-controlled psychophysical mea- touch), (b) displace their signing to be within the
sures with highly constrained visual stimuli to sub- child’s line of sight (as they look at the object),
jective rating scales of human behaviors. While (c) use longer wait times before producing child-
researchers are increasingly cognizant of the hetero- directed language (i.e., waiting until the child inde-
geneity of deafness and the importance of evaluat- pendently looks to the caregiver before starting to
ing these variables in the interpretation of results, sign), and (d) use greater persistence to successfully
further attention to this issue is greatly needed. gain and redirect the child’s attention (see Loots &
Additional work is needed to explicate the causal Devise, 2003, for a more detailed summary of deaf
and adaptive factors that may underlie the reported caregiver–infant dyad behaviors).
behavioral differences in impulsivity, distractibility, Deaf caregivers’ intuitive communicative inter-
and self-regulation of this population. One factor to action strategies appear to socialize deaf infants
be considered is a deaf child’s early socialization and toddlers into coordinated and complex visual
experiences.These practices may build resilience to engagement. Indeed, Harris and Mohay (1997) and
the possible effects of sound deprivation. Koester (1995) observed that deaf infants of deaf
The development of visual attention and gaze caregivers consistently look to their caregivers, and
following is seen as an important milestone for all their caregivers intuitively respond with appropri-
children (Brooks & Meltzoff, 2008; Meltzoff & ate linguistic and affective input. In contrast, the
Brooks, 2009). Brooks and Meltzoff (2002) explain deaf infants of hearing caregivers in their studies
that ‘‘gaze following is an entrée into the psycholog- demonstrated low rates of spontaneously looking
ical world in which things are important not only to their caregivers.
because of their physical properties, but because Loots, Devise, and Jacquet (2005) suggest that
they are referred to by others’’ (p. 965). In hearing ‘‘once the attention-switching strategy and sus-
caregiver–child dyads, the infant follows the gaze of tained looking are developed, the deaf child’s spon-
their caregiver and subsequently engages in joint taneous and active use of visual attention becomes
visual attention toward an object while their care- more important in initiating and continuing
giver simultaneously produces a linguistic label (or moments of intersubjectivity than the continuous
an emotional reaction) that they can hear while use of visual–tactile strategies’’ (p. 372). Thus, the
visually exploring the object. Young infants, below initial visual–tactile scaffolding produced by care-
12 months, may use head turning by the caregiver givers appears to provide the needed supports for
as the primary directional cue to elicit directional the development of self-regulation of visual atten-
attention, but by 12–18 months, infants ‘‘grant tion (e.g., attention switching, monitoring visual
special status to human eyes accompanied by head access, and following caregiver head position and
movement’’ and treat the adults’ gaze as object eyegaze direction). Moreover, observational studies
directed (Brooks & Meltzoff, 2002, p. 965). ‘‘The of native-signing deaf preschoolers engaged in mul-
processes that begin with joint visual attention skills tiparty discourse within visually complex environ-
in infancy are thought to be important to the sub- ments show that they can smoothly gaze-follow
sequent capacity for social learning throughout the their teacher and correctly use discourse cues
lifespan’’ (Vaughan van Hecke et al., 2007, p. 54). in ASL to anticipate visual turn taking (Crume &
If one adopts this standard model for the deve- Singleton, 2008). Yet, in this classroom context, deaf
lopment of joint visual attention, a deaf child could teachers continued to use explicit visual and tactile
be seen as facing a tremendous perceptual and strategies to support the development of visual
cognitive challenge. A deaf child needs to look at attention self-regulation, presumably for deaf pre-
their caregiver to get linguistic input, but they also schoolers with hearing parents who would not
need to look at objects to learn about the world. have experienced such caregiver strategies at home.
Across a number of studies observing deaf care- The skills involved in following a caregiver’s
giver–deaf infant dyads (based in several different direction of eyegaze and pointing gestures,
Insights From Deafness 957

and child-initiated coordinated attention such as of caregiver–infant interaction provides the basis
pointing to or showing objects to the caregiver, are for developing a shared understanding of events.
argued to involve aspects of the brain’s EF such as For example, in their longitudinal study of 11 hear-
attention regulation, inhibitory control, and ing caregivers (5 English-speaking Euro-American
self-monitoring (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008; Mundy middle class and 6 Spanish-speaking Latino work-
& Acra, 2006). Thus, in the context of deaf chil- ing class) and their hearing infants aged 6–
dren’s family and school settings, we would argue 26 months, Zukow-Goldring and Arbib found that
that deaf caregiver and teachers’ cultural and when these caregivers accompanied their participa-
linguistic practices that structure visual attention tion or imitation invitations to their infants with
may even enhance a deaf child’s EF—or at the very attention-directing gestures, the infants’ ability to
least builds in resilience, or protection from the engage or reach new understandings was
possible disadvantages of sound deprivation. increased.
This view of the cultural structuring of devel- Therefore, given the importance of hearing
opmental processes is consistent with work con- infants’ development of eyegaze following for
ducted in cross-cultural investigations of their cognitive and linguistic development and
caregiver–child interaction. For example, Rogoff the apparent privileged role of caregivers’ atten-
and her colleagues (Chavajay & Rogoff, 1999; tion-directing gestures in this process, we would
Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu, & Moisier, 1993) discuss expect that the highly demanding and gesturally
cultural variation in the ways that caregivers complex context of visual language interaction
attend to their children and how they encourage (i.e., the experience of deaf children raised by
their infants and children to attend to objects deaf parents or having daily interaction with
and events. For example, Chavajay and Rogoff ASL-fluent early childhood teachers) could pro-
(1999) observed that middle-class European mote early and advanced development of eyegaze
American children had a tendency to engage in following and attention shifting. Future studies
a kind of ‘‘fixed-capacity’’ focus on single objects are needed to determine whether this prediction
or events, and then alternated their attention to would be supported. The fact that the develop-
new objects or events. Their parents expressed ment of visual attention can be differentially
pride when the child could sustain their attention shaped across cultures suggests the possibility of
on a singular point of focus. In contrast, Guate- an intimate relation between cultural practices
malan Mayan toddlers and their caregivers and modifications of the neural systems associ-
seemed to have a cultural preference for simulta- ated with social-attention and EF.
neously attending to multiple sources of focus. In
discussing these cultural differences, Chavajay
and Rogoff noted that the social worlds of the
Higher Visual Processing: Facial Expressions and
Mayan children included large families often in
Human Actions
each other’s presence, maintaining proximity
often by sitting in a semicircle, which could The perception of facial expressions and the dis-
thereby increase the child’s opportunity for expo- cernments of human actions serve a critical role in
sure to competing attentional events and multi- achieving successful social interaction. Research on
party conversations. these high-level visual processing domains has
An important question to address, then, is begun to shed new light into neural systems that lie
whether particular neural systems (e.g., prefrontal at the interface between visual perception and
systems) could be susceptible to modification as a social function. Studies of higher level visual pro-
result of such cultural practices and social interac- cessing skills have also reported differences
tions. In the simplest sense, these cultural contexts between deaf and hearing individuals. These
can be seen as contributing a framework for how include studies of mental rotation, image genera-
adults structure the everyday experiences of a tion, memory for spatial location and face process-
child, defining opportunities for a child to engage ing (Bettger, Emmorey, McCullough, & Bellugi,
in complex, culturally meaningful interactions. 1997; Emmorey, Kosslyn, & Bellugi, 1993; McKee,
Along similar lines, Zukow-Goldring and Arbib 1987), and action recognition (Corina et al., 2007).
(2007) discuss how caregivers ‘‘bracket ongoing In contrast to the effects of visual attention noted
actions with gestures that direct the infant’s atten- above, enhancements in these domains are linked
tion to perceptual information embodied in action to experience with signing rather than auditory
sequences’’ (p. 2181). Their position is that this kind deprivation per se.
958 Corina and Singleton

when they first acquire the manual signs for nega-


Understanding Faces
tion, they appear without a headshake (which is
In the context of social cognition, studies of facial ungrammatical in ASL). The appearance of the cor-
expression recognition are of particular interest. rect accompanying headshake lags behind the
The capacity to extract socially relevant information appearance of the manual sign by 1–8 months.
from faces is fundamental to normal reciprocal These findings indicate a separation between acqui-
social interactions and interpersonal communica- sition of a social-cultural communicative behavior
tion (Pelphrey, Singerman, Allison, & McCarthy, (headshake) and the acquisition of grammatical
2003). For deaf signers, facial expression serves marker (obligatory linguistic headshake) to signal
dual roles; in addition to the universal emblems negation.
of emotionality (Ekman, 1992), a separate class In behavioral studies, judgments of linguistic
of facial expressions serves to indicate linguistic and emotional facial expressions produced by
contrasts in signed language, including language- fluent adult signers indicate differential right and
specific syntactic and morphological distinctions left sided expressivity, which has been argued to
(Baker-Shenk & Cokely, 1980). Deaf and hearing reflect underlying neural control (Campbell, 1978;
people alike use the face to express emotion. How- Corina, Bellugi, & Reilly, 1999). This finding is
ever, ASL signers also use facial expression and also corroborated by neuropsychological studies of
changes in head and body position to convey lin- facial expression production by left- and right-
guistic contrasts (Liddell, 1980). The production of hemisphere-damaged signers. Corina et al. (1999)
linguistic and emotional facial expression differ in reported a double dissociation in which a right-
timing and scope. Facial expressions that serve lin- hemisphere-damaged signer exhibited greatly redu-
guistic functions have a clear onset and offset, and ced facial emotionality with well-preserved produc-
follow the clausal and lexical properties of ASL tion of linguistic expressions; the second case study,
grammar. In contrast, emotional expressions are of the left-hemisphere-damaged signer, showed
more variable in execution, may differ in intensity the opposite—exuberant affective expressions with
and scope, and are not tied to the presence of spe- severely limited linguistic facial expression.
cific linguistic forms. Researchers have reported Recent data from functional MRI (fMRI) have
that categorical processing of facial displays can be provided new evidence of differential speciali-
demonstrated for sign but may be grounded in uni- zation for facial expression recognition in deaf
versally perceived distinctions between communi- signers (McCullough, Emmorey, & Sereno, 2005) in
cative face actions (Campbell, Woll, Benson, & response to static faces. Studies of deaf signers and
Wallace, 1999). hearing nonsigners have reported bilateral superior
In language acquisition studies, Reilly and her temporal sulcus (STS) activation for emotional
colleagues (Reilly & Bellugi, 1996; Reilly, McIntire, facial expression in deaf subjects and right hemi-
& Bellugi, 1990a, 1990b) have shown different tim- sphere dominance for hearing subjects. In addition,
ing between deaf children’s development of facial activation in the fusiform face area (FFA) was left
expressions for emotion and for linguistic contrasts. lateralized for deaf signers for both emotional and
As with hearing children, emotion expressions are linguistic forms of expression, whereas activation
produced by deaf children consistently by 1 year of was bilateral for both expression types in the hear-
age. However, linguistic facial expressions show a ing nonsigning subjects. fMRI studies of facial
protracted development, appearing only after the information processing in nonsigners have sought
acquisition of manual linguistic signs. A striking to differentiate these functions within the posterior
example is the development of headshake as a STS and FFA. Numerous studies report a role of
negation marker in ASL grammar. The physical bilateral STS in detection of eyegaze (Allison, Puce,
form of this linguistic behavior is identical to the & McCarthy, 2000; Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore, &
common headshake that hearing and deaf children McCarthy, 1998). Recent work indicates that STS
in the United States use to mean ‘‘no’’ (this often activity is modulated by the context within which
develops before the age of 2 years). In the adult eyegaze shifts occur, suggesting that this region
form of ASL, negative manual signs such as no, not, is involved in social perception via its role in
or can’t, must be accompanied with the linguistic the analysis of the intentions of observed actions
negation marker: headshake. Interestingly, Reilly (Materna, Dicke, & Thier, 2008; Mosconi, Mack,
et al.’s developmental studies report that deaf chil- McCarthy, & Pelphrey, 2005). It is interesting to
dren first begin to produce isolated headshakes in note that group differences in STS activation
response to questions or to reject requests. But between deaf and hearing subjects in the
Insights From Deafness 959

McCullough et al. (2005) study emerged only when mical regions that factor prominently in this
the linguistic expressions were presented within network include ventral premotor cortex and
the context of linguistic manual sign. This is con- inferior parietal lobule, the STS, and intraparietal
sistent with the growing evidence that posterior sulcus.
STS regions involved in facial processing may Thus, studies of deaf signers may provide an
serve interpretative functions. This may be an important way to help constrain the investigation
indication that this neural region (i.e., STS) in the of the broad neurofunctional scope of the hMNS.
deaf subjects is modulated by linguistic communi- For example, to the extent that an hMNS underlies
cative intent. In contrast, the activation in the FFA, linguistic capacity, we might predict that deaf
though hemispherically distinct from hearing signers, whose language is expressed largely by
subjects, was not modulated by the contextual manual actions would strongly engage the hMNS.
manual sign cues. This is consistent with reports In a recent review of the neuroimaging findings
that indicate that FFA may serve more founda- in signers, Corina and Knapp (2006) evaluated
tional roles in structural encoding of face specific whether frontal and parietal lobe bilaterally
information (Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, showed activations during the perception and
1997; Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006; McCarthy, 1999). production of signing as is predicted by an hMNS
McCullough et al. suggest that hemispheric differ- account of language. The findings showed only
ences observed in these studies may reflect percep- partial support for this hypothesis and indicated
tual differences that invoke greater local featural that only left hemisphere regions satisfied the
processing (as opposed to global-configural pro- necessary condition of cojoint activation. In addi-
cessing) in the deaf subjects. tion, left frontal activations were most commonly
It is unknown whether there is a relation reported in Broca’s area (BA 45) rather than the
between the staggered production of affective and frontal-ventral regions (44 ⁄ 6) that are often asso-
linguistic facial expressions in deaf signing children ciated with hMNS activations. These data thus
and the appearance of the differential hemispheric suggest a full hMNS account of language must
asymmetries for these classes of expression. What acknowledge the left hemisphere specialization for
is noteworthy is that in these cases, the demands human languages and the possibility for regional
of language processing in the visual-gestural shifts in anatomical representations.
modality may be driving the differential speciali- Studies of nonlinguistic human action processing
zation and reorganization within temporal lobe provides further evidence that current hMNS
regions. accounts of action understanding are likely too
simplistic. In a recent PET study, Corina et al.
(2007) examined neural activation in response to
the perception of object-oriented human action
Understanding Human Actions
(e.g., drinking from a glass) and intransitive actions
Since the discovery of ‘‘mirror neurons’’ in Macaque (e.g., rubbing your eyes) in deaf and hearing
primates (Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, subjects. While hearing subjects evidenced a
1996; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, & Fogassi, 1996), pattern of bilateral inferior-frontal and parietal
there has been renewed interest in the neural activations commonly associated with hMNS, deaf
basis of human action understanding. A human subjects exhibited qualitatively different patterns
mirror neuron system (hMNS) has been evoked as of activation with prominent bilateral ventral-
a neural basis for a wide range of social behaviors occipito-temporal activations.
including imitation, empathy, and language, and These human action processing data are impor-
by some accounts even provided a foundation for tant in the context of social cognition as they sug-
human culture (Gallese, 2007). Dysfunction within gest that the hMNS may not be an inevitable
a human mirror neuron has been argued to under- processing system for human action understanding.
lie developmental deficits such as autism (Iacoboni Indeed, data from language and gesture studies in
& Mazziotta, 2007; Oberman & Ramachandran, the deaf subjects suggest that the hMNS may be
2007; Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, & Perrett, mutable and subject to reorganization. The engage-
2001). Data from functional neuroimaging have ment of the temporal-ventral region may reflect
been used as evidence for a human homolog of a active monitoring of featural properties of human
mirror neuron system characterized as a bilateral actions that are necessary for differentiation of
fronto-parietal network capable of representations linguistic and nonlinguistic human action. Thus, as
of action–perception pairings (Miall, 2003). Anato- with facial expression processing (as discussed
960 Corina and Singleton

earlier), there is growing evidence for functional vation for these studies is derived from longstand-
specialization of temporal-ventral systems that may ing findings that hearing infants show attention
underlie the careful analysis of human forms and preferences for speech over other forms of complex
actions. acoustic stimuli (Colombo & Bundy, 1981; Glenn,
What is striking is that in each of these cases, Cunningham, & Joyce, 1981; Vouloumanos &
face and human action recognition, deaf individu- Werker, 2004). Previous speech-based studies have
als are confronted with a constrained signaling been taken as evidence for an innate bias to human
medium (i.e., visually perceptible human manual language. However, it is unclear whether this
and facial actions) that must take on additional looking preference reflects a bias for speech or
functionality (to convey not only emotion and human language more generally. In this study,
environmental interaction but language as well). 6-month-old hearing infants looked longer at sign
Each of these cases reveals further specialization stimuli compared to complex pantomimes.
within temporal lobe and temporal-ventral Interestingly, the preference for sign language was
regions, perhaps in response to this added visual not observed in 10-month-old hearing infants; in
signaling complexity. Hearing children bring both fact, there were no differences in looking times for
auditory and visual capacities in assimilating their pantomime or signs at this later age. This change
environment, which, as previously noted, affords in looking behavior between 6 and 10 months may
an efficient division of labor that is not available reflect hearing infants’ gradual loss of sensitivity to
to the deaf child. Therefore, a hearing child’s task linguistic forms that are not encountered in their
does not require resolving competing linguistic home environment. This result mirrors infants’ loss
and nonlinguistic input within the visual modality of detection of linguistic contrasts for spoken
to the same degree as deaf, sign-exposed children language (Best, McRoberts, & Sithole, 1988; Eimas,
must do. One might expect that in addition to 1975; Werker & Tees, 1984). This attunement may
developing a temporal-ventral processing system have functional origins that extend beyond
that might attune the deaf child to differentiate language per se: It is known that infants are close
subtle visual signals, the deaf child must further monitors of psychosocial content available from
develop frontal-executive control mechanisms to the eye, mouth, and head movements of adults
modulate responses to these signals. Taken (Meltzoff & Brooks, 2007). Thus, infants’ discrimi-
together, one may interpret these ‘‘competing’’ nation of linguistic from nonlinguistic motion may
modality interactions of language and non- be an even more sophisticated instantiation of their
language input as an example of complex on-line attempts to learn whether the actions of the
human–environment interaction that demands a viewed adult have emotional-communicative rele-
particular kind of attentional and higher order vance (Knapp, Cho, & Corina, 2008). The specula-
processing that supports the unlayering or disen- tion is that this timeframe may be a period in
tangling of perceptually similar, but functionally which infants are beginning to show a specializa-
different, input. The result of this specialized tion for human actions that are linguistic in nature
processing may lead to enhanced parietal- versus those which are not. Preliminary evidence
attentional and occipito-temporal functioning. suggests that deaf sign-exposed infants maintain
Whether there are specialized frontal-executive their interest in signs over pantomime during this
functionalities that arise in concert with or in time (Klarman, Krentz, Brinkley, Corina, & Kuhl,
response to these processing demands is not yet 2008).
known. A question of interest is whether deaf The appreciation of facial expression and inter-
caregivers may be using communicative strategies pretation of human actions are of course critical to
to uniquely demarcate these complex layers, all social interactions and there is no evidence to
which has the effect of supporting the child’s indicate that deaf individuals possess differences in
process of resolving this task. responsivity to human emotions or actions. In the
Whether there are strict timetables or critical present case, however, acquisition of a visual–
periods for such reorganization is unknown (but gestural language appears to place requirements
see Newman, Bavelier, Corina, Jezzard, & Neville, for the development of specialized mechanisms
2002); however, a recent developmental study pro- for processing this information, which often is
vides some insights. Krentz and Corina (2008) used highly similar in form to nonlinguistic, but socially
a looking time paradigm to explore nonsigning meaningful counterparts. We maintain that the
hearing infants’ sensitivities for distinguishing sign brain is capable of accommodating these new
language from complex pantomime. The moti- functions and does so by engendering increased
Insights From Deafness 961

bilateral hemispheric participation and specializa- nevertheless exhibit social responsiveness in con-
tion within the inferior temporal lobes. trast to autistic children. Furthermore, while devel-
opment of ToM is developmentally delayed, the
DoH children nevertheless advance through the
eventual progression in their attainment of ToM. In
The Role of Language in Social Cognition: Deaf
contrast, autistic children demonstrate a different
Children and Theory of Mind Studies
sequence of understanding in the later stages of the
Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the awareness of progression. Thus, it is argued that individuals
how mental states such as memories, beliefs, with autism may have a distinctive, autism-specific
desires, and intentions govern the behaviors of self difficulty with the sort of mental state understand-
and others (Baron-Cohen, 2000). ToM is considered ing that is needed for false-belief tasks above and
a cornerstone of social intelligence and satisfying beyond other sorts of mental state understanding
social interaction and develops rapidly during the (Peterson et al., 2005).
preschool years (Peterson, Wellman, & Liu, 2005). Recent work by Meristo et al. (2007) has
There is ongoing controversy as to the causal prin- extended earlier investigations of the development
ciples that underlie belief attribution and ToM (for of ToM in deaf children by comparing native sign-
a recent review of pertinent issues and evidence, ing children in Italy and Estonia in their respective
see Saxe, Carey, & Kanwisher, 2004). The relation signed languages as a function of school environ-
of language to the development of ToM is one of ment. In this study, deaf children’s communicative
many prominent issues raised in these debates. experiences in the classroom, native (DoD) signing
Studies of deaf children have contributed signifi- deaf children attending bilingual-(sign and spoken
cantly to our understanding of the relation between language) based programs outperformed native
language and ToM. As noted above, by some esti- signers attending oral programs that emphasized
mates, 96% (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004) of deaf speech only. This pattern was observed in both the
children are raised in households with parents who Italian and Estonian samples. In addition, these
are hearing. For a child with auditory challenges, studies replicated the well-known finding of overall
this often results in a lack of full access to a consis- poorer performance of ToM tasks in deaf children
tent adult language model and can lead to delays from hearing homes in Italy, Estonia, and Sweden
in language development at critical periods. (Meristo et al., 2007). These findings suggest that
The selective deficits in language ability the expression of ToM in native signing deaf chil-
observed in some cases of deafness provide an dren may depend not only on early language expe-
opportunity to tease apart effects of cognitive matu- rience but also on children’s continuing exposure to
ration and engagement in social interaction at least opportunities for monitoring conversational input
to the extent that the latter do not themselves about mental states. Opportunities for exposure to
depend on language acquisition (Schick, de Villiers, contexts in which discourse of mental causes is
de Villiers, & Hoffmeister, 2007). Studies of deaf present is known to be correlated with the develop-
children with hearing parents report significant ment of false belief (Cutting & Dunn, 1999).
delays in the mastery of verbal and nonverbal ToM Data from deaf signing children have been used
tasks (Marschark, 1993; Peterson & Siegal, 1995; to argue that specific formal properties of language,
J. de Villiers, 2005; de Villiers & de Villiers, 2000). for example, the understanding of syntactic com-
The case for linguistic influences as a factor comes plements (J. de Villiers, 2005; P. de Villiers, 2005),
from the comparisons of deaf children of hearing underlie the appearance of ToM abilities. Yet
parents (DoH) with deaf children who are raised whether these specific constructions are causal is a
in households with deaf signing parents (DoD). matter of debate (see Saxe, 2006; Saxe et al., 2004).
DoD children acquire sign language naturally as Cross-linguistic studies can provide a powerful test
a primary language and exhibit linguistic com- of the generality of such claims. As reported by
petencies on par with hearing children raised in Schick et al. (2007), deaf children’s understanding
spoken language environments. The DoD children of syntactic complements in either ASL or English
do not evidence delays on ToM tasks (Schick et al., were shown to be independent predictors of suc-
2007). cess on verbal and low-verbal ToM tasks. The data
Additional studies have compared the perfor- from Meristo et al. (2007) reported above are sur-
mance of DoH to autistic children, who are also prising in this regard as they report differences in
known to display deficits on ToM tasks (Peterson ToM for native deaf signing Italian and Estonian
et al., 2005). Deaf children with linguistic deficits children. Unless we assume that the acquisition of
962 Corina and Singleton

these critical syntactic complement constructions is interdependencies that are present. We hope to
gradient in nature, it is difficult to reconcile these have shown how the study of deafness and the cul-
claims. tural structuring of attention and visual engage-
ment highlight the potential contributions of
biological, environmental, and cultural factors,
especially during childhood. This case reveals
Conclusion
unique cultural, cognitive, and biological affor-
In this review, we have examined how the human dances that may enable deaf children to develop
condition of deafness can provide avenues for resiliency, and protect them from the potential neg-
understanding the development of neural systems ative consequences of sensory deprivation.
that impact social cognitive processing. Our inte-
grated approach echoes Posner and Rothbart (2000)
who emphasize that developmental pathways for
References
sociocognitive processes are influenced by ‘‘complex
interaction effects of early temperament predisposi- Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). The
tions, socialization processes, relationships, and cul- human amygdala in social judgment. Nature, 393(6684),
ture’’ (p. 438). We have reviewed evidence that 470–474.
auditory deprivation may engender changes within Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social per-
ception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends
dorsal-visual pathways that affect spatial-visual
in Cognitive Science, 4(7), 267–278.
attention in a highly specific fashion. We explored
Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W.
claims that these attentional differences may impact (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends in Neurosciences,
aspects of social interaction, especially with respect 3, 137–145.
to reports of distractibility and impulsivity of deaf Anderson, V. (1998). Assessing executive functions in
individuals by some clinical and educational litera- children: Biological, psychological, and developmental
tures. We contrasted functional-anatomical-based considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(3),
explanations of these data, which emphasized dif- 319–349.
ferential roles of parietal-attentional and frontal- Armstrong, B. A., Neville, H. J., Hillyard, S. A., & Mitchell,
executive systems in the manifestation of these T. V. (2002). Auditory deprivation affects processing of
behaviors. We noted how exposure to a visually motion, but not color. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain
Research, 14(3), 422–434.
based culture and language may provide a deaf
Bachevalier, J., & Loveland, K. A. (2006). The orbitofron-
child with certain cognitive affordances that pro-
tal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emo-
mote resiliency and optimization in their develop- tional behavior in autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
ment of visual engagement, other EFs, and Reviews, 30(1), 97–117.
sociocognitive skills such as ToM. These studies Baker-Shenk, C., & Cokely, D. (1980). American Sign Lan-
raise important questions concerning the mecha- guage: A teacher’s resource text on grammar and culture.
nisms by which culture and socialization may Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
impact social cognitive development and provide Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-
clues to neural systems mediating these changes. regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological per-
We discussed how deafness and the acquisition spective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 1–29.
of a visually based language may promote neural Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A
fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, D. Cohen, &
adaptations that permit the more nuanced percep-
H. Tager-Flusberg (Eds.), Understanding other minds:
tion required for distinguishing classes of social
Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp.
behaviors (whether linguistic or emotional). These 3–20). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
data can inform debates regarding the role of Baron-Cohen, S., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Autism: A
hMNSs in the perception of human actions and lan- window onto the development of the social and the ana-
guage. These studies can provide insight into how lytic brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 109–126.
these specialized adaptations are realized, in the Bavelier, D., Brozinsky, C., Tomann, A., Mitchell, T.,
present case, by engendering differential hemi- Neville, H., & Liu, G. (2001). Impact of early deafness
spheric specialization and engagement of STS and and early exposure to sign language on the cerebral
temporal ventral regions. organization for motion processing. Journal of Neuro-
In this review, while we have often discussed science, 21(22), 8931–8942.
Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W., & Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf
various factors and domains implicated in the
individuals see better? Trends in Cognitive Science,
developmental social cognition of deafness in
10(11), 512–518.
isolation, we readily acknowledge the intricate
Insights From Deafness 963

Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plastic- Colombo, J., & Bundy, R. S. (1981). A method for the
ity: Where and how? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(6), measurement of infant auditory selectivity. Infant
443–452. Behavior and Development, 4, 219–223.
Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Corina, Corina, D. P., Bellugi, U., & Reilly, J. (1999). Neuro-
D., Liu, G., et al. (2000). Visual attention to the periph- psychological studies of linguistic and affective facial
ery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals. Jour- expressions in deaf signers. Language and Speech,
nal of Neuroscience, 20(17) RC93, 1–6. 42(2–3), 307–331.
Beer, J. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: A Corina, D., Chiu, Y. S., Knapp, H., Greenwald, R., San
multi level analysis. Brain Research, 1079(1), 98–105. Jose-Robertson, L., & Braun, A. (2007). Neural corre-
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & lates of human action observation in hearing and deaf
St. George, M. (2000). I. The neurocognitive profile of subjects. Brain Research, 1152, 111–129.
Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths Corina, D. P., & Knapp, H. (2006). Sign language process-
and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, ing and the mirror neuron system. Cortex, 42(4), 529–
12(Suppl. 1), 7–29. 539.
Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Crume, P., & Singleton, J. L. (2008). Teacher practices for
Examination of the perceptual reorganization for promoting visual engagement of deaf children in a bilingual
speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English- preschool. Paper presented at the Association of College
speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Educators of the Deaf ⁄ Hard of Hearing, Monterey, CA.
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a mod-
345–360. ern day ‘‘wild child’’. New York: Academic Press.
Bettger, J., Emmorey, K., McCullough, S., & Bellugi, U. Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion
(1997). Enhanced facial discrimination: Effects of expe- understanding, language and family background: Indi-
rience with American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf vidual differences and inter-relations. Child Develop-
Studies and Deaf Education, 2(4), 223–233. ment, 70, 853–865.
Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (1999). Left-hemisphere Damasio, A. R. (1995). On some functions of the human
dominance for motion processing in deaf signers. Psy- prefrontal cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
chological Science, 10(3), 256–262. ences, 769, 241–251.
Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002). The effects of Dye, M. W. G., Baril, D. E., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Which
spatial attention on motion processing in deaf signers, aspects of visual attention are changed by deafness?
hearing signers, and hearing nonsigners. Brain and Cog- The case of the attentional network task. Neuropsycholo-
nition, 49(1), 152–169. gia, 48(8), 1801–1811.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of Dye, M. W. G., Hauser, P. C., & Bavelier, D. (2008).
eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Visual attention in deaf children and adults: Impli-
Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 958–966. cations for learning environments. In M. Marschark & P.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following C. Hauser (Eds.), Deaf cognition: Foundations and outcomes
and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth (pp. 250–263). New York: Oxford University Press.
through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve Eimas, P. D. (1975). Auditory and phonetic coding of the
modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35, 207–220. cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r–l] distinction
Bross, M. (1979). Residual sensory capacities of the deaf: by young infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 341–
A signal detection analysis of a visual discrimination 347.
task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48(1), 187–194. Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion: An old
Bross, M., & Sauerwein, H. (1980). Signal detection analy- controversy and new findings. Philosophical transactions
sis of visual flicker in deaf and hearing individuals. of the Royal Society of London: Series B. Biological Sciences,
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51(3), 839–843. 335(1273), 63–69.
Campbell, R. (1978). Asymmetries in interpreting and Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain:
expressing a posed facial expression. Cortex, 14(3), 327– Insights from Sign Language Research. Mahwah, NJ:
342. Erlbaum.
Campbell, R., Woll, B., Benson, P.J., & Wallace, S.B. Emmorey, K., Kosslyn, S. M., & Bellugi, U. (1993). Visual
(1999). Categorical perception of face actions: Their role imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imag-
in sign language and in communicative facial displays. ery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers. Cognition,
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A(1), 67– 46(2), 139–181.
95. Erting, C. J., Prezioso, C., & Hynes, M. (1990). The inter-
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experi- actional content of deaf mother–infant communication.
ence and executive functioning in young children. In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to
Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 97–106). New
Chavajay, P., & Rogoff, B. (1999). Cultural variation in York: Springer-Verlag.
management of attention by children and their care- Eslinger, P. J., & Grattan, L. M. (1991). Perspectives on
givers. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 1079–1090. the developmental consequences of early frontal lobe
964 Corina and Singleton

damage: Introduction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, opmental disabilities: Clinical research and practice (pp.
257–260. 119–131). New York: Guilford.
Everhart, V. S., & Marschark, M. (1997). Models, modules, Iacoboni, M., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2007). Mirror neuron sys-
and modality. In M. Marschark, P. Siple, D. Lillo-Martin, tem: Basic findings and clinical applications. Annals of
R. Campbell, & V. S. Everhart (Eds.), Relations of language Neurology, 62(3), 213–218.
and thought: The view from sign language and deaf children Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The
(pp. 172–184). New York: Oxford University Press. fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate
Finney, E. M., & Dobkins, K. R. (2001). Visual contrast cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuro-
sensitivity in deaf versus hearing populations: Explo- science, 17(11), 4302–4311.
ring the perceptual consequences of auditory depriva- Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area:
tion and experience with a visual language. Cognitive A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces.
Brain Research, 11(1), 171–183. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London:
Frith, U. (2001). Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Series B. Biological Sciences, 361(1476), 2109–2128.
Neuron, 32(6), 969–979. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Williams syndrome. Current
Gallese, V. (2007). Before and below ‘theory of mind’: Biology, 17(24), R1035–R1036.
Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social Klarman, L., Krentz, U., Brinkley, J., Corina, D. P., &
cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Kuhl, P. (2008). Deaf and hearing infants’ preference for
of London: Series B. Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 659– American Sign Language. Poster presented at American
669. Psychological Association Conference, Boston.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Knapp, H. P., Cho, H., & Corina, D. P. (2008). Perception
Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, of sign language and human actions. In M. R. de Quad-
119(Pt. 2), 593–609. ros (Ed.), TISLR 9: Theoretical Issues in Sign Language
Geers, A., Moog, J., & Schick, B. (1984). Acquisition of spo- Research 9. 9 Congreso International de Aspectos Teóricos
ken and signed English by profoundly deaf children. das Pesquisas nas Linguas de Sinais. December 6 to 9, 2006
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(4), 378–388. Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina Florianópolis, SC
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kentworth, L. Brasil. Florianópolis: Lagoa Editora (2006).
(2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cush-
(BRIEF) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological man, F., Hauser, M., et al. (2007). Damage to the pre-
Assessment Resources. frontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgments.
Glenn, S. M., Cunningham, C. C., & Joyce, P. F. (1981). A Nature, 446(7138), 908–911.
study of auditory preferences in nonhandicapped Koester, L. S. (1995). Face-to-face interactions between
infants and infants with Down’s Syndrome. Child hearing mothers and their deaf or hearing infants.
Development, 52(4), 1303–1307. Infant Behavior and Development, 18(2), 145–153.
Grossman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The development Koester, L. S., Brooks, L., & Traci, M. A. (2000). Tactile
of the social brain in human infancy. European Journal contact by deaf and hearing mothers during face-
of Neuroscience, 25(4), 909–919. to-face interactions with their infants. Journal of Deaf
Harris, R. I. (1978). The relationship of impulse control to Studies and Deaf Education, 5(2), 2127–2139.
parent hearing status, manual communication, and aca- Krentz, U. C., & Corina, D. P. (2008). Preference for lan-
demic achievement in deaf children. American Annals of guage in early infancy: The human language bias is not
the Deaf, 123(1), 52–67. speech specific. Developmental Science, 11(1), 1–9.
Harris, M. (2000). Social interaction and early language Ladd, P. (2003). Understanding deaf culture. In search of
development in deaf children. Deafness and Education deafhood. Toronto, ON: Multilingual Matters.
International, 2(1), 1–11. Lane, H. (1984). When the mind hears: A history of the deaf.
Harris, M., & Mohay, H. (1997). Learning to look in the New York: Random House.
right place: A comparison of attentional behavior in Liddell, S. K. (1980). American Sign Language syntax. The
deaf children with deaf and hearing mothers. Journal of Hague: Mouton.
Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(2), 95–103. Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Social cognitive neuroscience: A
Hauser, P. C., Lukomski, J., & Hillman, T. (2008). Devel- review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology,
opment of deaf and hard of hearing students’ executive 58, 259–289.
function. In M. Marschark & P. C. Hauser (Eds.), Deaf Lillo-Martin, D. (1999). Modality effects and modularity
cognition: Foundations and outcomes (pp. 286–308). New in language acquisition: The acquisition of American
York: Oxford University Press. Sign Language. In T. K. Bhatia & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.),
Hauser, P. C., Lukomski, J., & Isquith, P. (2009). Deaf col- Handbook of language acquisition (pp. 531–567). San
lege students’ performance on the BRIEF-A and Connors. Diego: Academic Press.
Manuscript in preparation. Loots, G., & Devise, I. (2003). The use of visual-tactile
Hauser, P. C., Wills, K., & Isquith, P. K. (2006). Hard of communication strategies by deaf and hearing fathers
hearing, deafness, and being deaf. In J. E. Farmer, J. and mothers of deaf infants. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Donders, & S. Warschausky (Eds.), Treating neurodevel- Deaf Education, 8(1), 31–42.
Insights From Deafness 965

Loots, G., Devise, I., & Jacquet, W. (2005). The impact of Miall, R. C. (2003). Connecting mirror neurons and for-
visual communication on the intersubjective develop- ward models. Neuroreport, 14(17), 2135–2137.
ment of early parent–child interaction with 18- to 24- Mills, C. B. (1985). Perception of visual temporal patterns
month-old deaf toddlers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf by deaf and hearing adults. Bulletin of the Psychonomic
Education, 10(4), 357–375. Society, 23, 483–486.
MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sela, S. (2002). Mitchell, T.V. (1996). How audition shapes visual attention.
Age, executive function and social decision making: A Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University,
dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psy- Bloomington.
chology and Aging, 17(4), 598–609. Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). When parents
Marschark, M. (1993). Psychological development of deaf chil- are deaf versus hard of hearing: Patterns of sign use
dren. New York: Oxford University Press. and school placement of deaf and hard-of-hearing chil-
Martini, A., Mazzoli, M., & Kimberling, W. (1997). An dren. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9(2),
introduction to the genetics of normal and defective 133–152.
hearing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 830, Mitchell, T. V., & Quittner, A. L. (1996). Multimethod
361–374. study of attention and behavior problems in hearing-
Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). Dissociable impaired children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intrapari- 25(1), 83–96.
etal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic res- Mosconi, M. W., Mack, P. B., McCarthy, G., & Pelphrey,
onance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, K. A. (2005). Taking an ‘‘intentional stance’’ on eye-
20(1), 108–119. gaze shifts: A functional neuroimaging study of social
McCarthy, G. (1999). Physiological studies of face pro- perception in children. Neuroimage, 27(1), 247–252.
cessing in humans. In M. S. Gazzaniga & E. Bizzi Mundy, P. C., & Acra, C. F. (2006). Joint attention, social
(Eds.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 393–410). engagement, and the development of social compe-
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. tence. In P. J. Marshall & N. A. Fox (Eds.), The develop-
McCullough, S., Emmorey, K., & Sereno, M. (2005). Neu- ment of social engagement: Neurobiological perspectives (pp.
ral organization for recognition of grammatical and 81–117). New York: Oxford University Press.
emotional facial expressions in deaf ASL signers and Neville, H., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Human brain plasticity:
hearing nonsigners. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Evidence from sensory deprivation and altered lan-
Research, 22(2), 193–203. guage experience. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 177–
McKee, D. E. (1987). An analysis of specialized cognitive 188.
functions in deaf and hearing signers. Unpublished doc- Neville, H. J., & Lawson, D. (1987a). Attention to central
toral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh. and peripheral visual space in a movement detection
Meadow, K. P. (1976). Behavioral problems of deaf chil- task: An event-related potential and behavioral study.
dren. In H. S. Schlesinger & K. P. Meadow (Eds.), Stud- I. Normal hearing adults. Brain Research, 405, 253–267.
ies of family interaction, language acquisition, and deafness Neville, H. J., & Lawson, D. (1987b). Attention to central
(pp. 257–293). San Francisco: San Francisco Final and peripheral visual space in a movement detection
Report, Office of Maternal and Child Health, Bureau of task: An event-related potential and behavioral study. II.
Community Health Services. Congenitally deaf adults. Brain Research, 405, 268–283.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2007). Intersubjectivity Neville, H. J., & Lawson, D. (1987c). Attention to central
before language: Three windows on preverbal sharing. and peripheral visual space in a movement detection
In S. Bråten (Ed.), On being moved: From mirror task. III. Separate effects of auditory deprivation and
neurons to empathy (pp. 149–174). Philadelphia: John acquisition of a visual language. Brain Research, 405(2),
Benjamins. 284–294.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2009). Social cognition and Neville, H. J., Schmidt, A., & Kutas, M. (1983). Altered
language: The role of gaze following in early word visual-evoked potentials in congenitally deaf adults.
learning. In J. Colombo, P. McCardle, & L. Freund Brain Research, 266(1), 127–132.
(Eds.), Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and Newman, A. J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., &
research disorders (pp. 169–194). New York: Psychology Neville, H. J. (2002). A critical period for right hemi-
Press. sphere recruitment in American Sign Language pro-
Meristo, M., Falkman, K. W., Hjelmquist, E., Tedoldi, M., cessing. Nature Neuroscience, 5(1), 76–80.
Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2007). Language access and Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints of lan-
theory of mind reasoning: Evidence from deaf children guage learning. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 11–28.
in bilingual and oralist environments. Developmental Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acquisition of
Psychology, 43(5), 1156–1169. American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Hariri, A. R., Munoz, K. E., Mervis, cross-linguistic study of language acquisition (pp. 881–
C. B., Mattay, V. S., Morris, C. A., et al. (2005). Neural 938). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
correlates of genetically abnormal social cognition in Niemeyer, W., & Starlinger, I. (1981). Do the blind hear
Williams syndrome. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 991–993. better? Investigations on auditory processing in
966 Corina and Singleton

congenital or early acquired blindness. II. Central func- development of visual attention. Psychological Science,
tions. Audiology, 20(6), 510–515. 5(6), 347–353.
Oberg, E. (2007). Assessing executive functioning in children Reilly, J. S., & Bellugi, U. (1996). Competition on the face:
with a hearing loss. Unpublished master’s thesis, Roches- Affect and language in ASL motherese. Journal of Child
ter Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY. Language, 23(1), 219–239.
Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The sim- Reilly, J. S., McIntire, M., & Bellugi, U. (1990a). The acqui-
ulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron sys- sition of conditionals in American Sign Language:
tem and simulation in the social and communicative Grammaticized facial expressions. Applied Psycholin-
deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological guistics, 11(4), 369–392.
Bulletin, 133(2), 310–327. Reilly, J. S., McIntire, M., & Bellugi, U. (1990b). Faces:
Padden, C. A., & Humphries, T. L. (1988). Deaf in The relationship between language and affect. In
America: Voices from a culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to langu-
University Press. age in hearing and deaf children (pp. 128–141). New York:
Padden, C., & Humphries, T. L. (2005). Inside deaf culture. Springer-Verlag.
New York: Harvard University Press. Reivich, R. S., & Rothrock, I. A. (1972). Behavior problems
Parasnis, I., & Samar, V. J. (1985). Parafoveal attention in of deaf children and adolescents: A factor-analytic
congenitally deaf and hearing young adults. Brain and study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Cognition, 4(3), 313–327. 15(1), 93–104.
Parasnis, I., Samar, V. J., & Berent, G. P. (2003). Deaf Ries, P. W. (1994). Prevalence and characteristics of per-
adults without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder sons with hearing trouble: United States, 1990-91. Vital
display reduced perceptual sensitivity and elevated and Health Statistics, 10(188), 1–75.
impulsivity on the Test of Variables of Attention Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L.
(T.O.V.A.). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor
Research, 46(5), 1166–1183. actions. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2),
Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, 131–141.
G. (2003). Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Moisier, C. (1993).
shifts: The influence of context. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers
156–170. and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation Child Development, 58(8, Serial No. 236).
and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology & Psy- Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. I., & Gioia, G. A. (2005). Behavior
chiatry, 36, 459–474. Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version.
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
in theory-of-mind development for children with deaf- Sandler, W., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2006). Sign language and
ness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517. linguistic universals. New York: Cambridge University
Petit, C. (1996). Genes responsible for human hereditary Press.
deafness: Symphony of a thousand. Nature Genetics, Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current
14(4), 385–391. Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 235–239.
Poizner, H., & Tallal, P. (1987). Temporal processing in Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understand-
deaf signers. Brain and Language, 30(1), 52–62. ing other minds: Linking developmental psychology
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mech- and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychol-
anisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathol- ogy, 55, 87–124.
ogy, 12(3), 427–441. Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on atten- (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf
tion networks as a model for the integration of psycho- children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396.
logical science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23. Schick, B., & Hoffmeister, R. (2001). ASL skills in deaf chil-
Proksch, J., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Changes in the spatial dren of deaf parents and of hearing parents. Paper pre-
distribution of visual attention after early deafness. sented at the, Society of Research on Child
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 687–701. Development International Conference, Minneapolis,
Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, MN.
G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans view- Schick, B., & Moeller, M. P. (1992). What is learnable in
ing eye and mouth movements. Journal of Neuroscience, manually coded English sign systems? Applied Psycho-
18(6), 2188–2199. linguistics, 13(3), 313–340.
Quittner, A. L., Glueckauf, R. L., & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Smith, L. B., Quittner, A. L., Osberger, M. J., &
Chronic parenting stress: Moderating versus mediating Miyamoto, R. (1998). Audition and visual attention:
effects of social support. Journal of Personality and Social The developmental trajectory in deaf and hearing pop-
Psychology, 59(6), 1266–1278. ulations. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 840–850.
Quittner, A. L., Smith, L. B., Osberger, M. J., Mitchell, T. Stokoe, W. (2005). Sign language structure: An outline of
V., & Katz, D. B. (1994). The impact of audition on the the visual communication systems of the American
Insights From Deafness 967

Deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10, 3–37 for theory of mind (pp. 266–297). Oxford, UK: Oxford
(Original work published 1960). University Press.
Strong, M., & Prinz, P. M. (1997). A study of the relation- Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Tuned to the
ship between American Sign Language and English signal: The privileged status of speech for young
literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, infants. Developmental Science, 7(3), 270–276.
37–46. Waxman, R. P., & Spencer, P. E. (1997). What mothers do
Vaughan van Hecke, A., Mundy, P., Acra, C. F., Block, to support infant visual attention: Sensitivities to age
J., Delgado, C., Parlade, M., et al. (2007). Infant and hearing status. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Edu-
joint attention, temperament, and social com- cation, 2(2), 104–114.
petence in preschool children. Child Development, Weaver, K. E., & Stevens, A. A. (2006). Auditory gap detec-
78(1), 53–69. tion in the early blind. Hearing Research, 211(1–2), 1–6.
de Villiers, J. (2005). Can language acquisition give chil- Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech
dren a point of view? In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization dur-
Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 186–219). ing the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development,
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 25(1), 49–63.
de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (2000). Linguistic deter- Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D.
minism and the understanding of false beliefs. In I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuro-
P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and science and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–295.
the mind (pp. 189–226). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Woodward, J. (1976). Historical Bases of American Sign
de Villiers, P. (2003). Language of the deaf-acquisition of Language. In P. Siple (Ed.), Understanding language
English. In R. D. Kent (Ed.), The MIT encyclopedia of through Sign language research (pp. 333–348). New York:
communicative disorders (pp. 336–338). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.
MIT Press. Zukow-Goldring, P., & Arbib, M. (2007). Affordances
de Villiers, P. (2005). The role of language in theory effectiveness and assisted imitation: Caregivers and the
of mind development: What deaf children tell us. In directing of attention. Neurocomputing, 70(13–15), 2181–
J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters 2193.

Вам также может понравиться