Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319

Review

Reduction of alcohol-related harm on United States college campuses:


The use of personal feedback interventions
Helene Raskin White ∗
Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies, 607 Allison Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854-8001, USA

Received 7 June 2005; received in revised form 11 October 2005; accepted 6 February 2006

Abstract

This paper reviews research evaluating personalised feedback interventions (PFIs) for reducing the harms associated with alcohol abuse
among US college students. PFIs provide students with feedback about their own alcohol use relative to college norms, as well as information
about other aspects of their drinking behaviours, related problems and/or perceived risks. Studies conducted in the United States using
randomised designs indicate that PFIs are efficacious for reducing various aspects of alcohol use and/or related negative consequences for
both high-risk volunteer and mandated college students. To date, these studies have demonstrated that written-feedback-only PFIs are as
efficacious as brief in-person PFIs, at least on a short-term duration. Therefore, college administrators should be encouraged to develop
interventions to screen students and provide written personal feedback in order to reduce high-risk drinking patterns among college students.
Web-based approaches might prove to be a very cost-effective strategy, although more research is needed to determine their efficacy, as well
as what aspects of the feedback are the most effective.
© 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Drinking; Alcohol; College students; Interventions; Feedback

Introduction alcohol to cope with this stress and associate with peers who
also use alcohol to cope and who reinforce such behaviour
In the United States (US), significant increases in the (Paschall & Flewelling, 2002). In addition, first-year stu-
frequency and quantity of alcohol use occur during the tran- dents may perceive heavy drinking as normative behaviour
sition from high school to college (Bachman, Wadsworth, among their peers and may come to college with positive
O’Malley, Johnston, & Schulenberg, 1997). Many young alcohol expectancies (Maggs, 1997; Paschall & Flewelling,
people leave their homes, parents, and old friends when they 2002). Finally, alcohol use may facilitate the making of new
enter college. These changes lead to new freedoms and reduc- friendships (Schulenburg & Maggs, 2002; White & Jackson,
tions of informal social controls. In addition, first-year college 2004/2005). Therefore, increases in drinking as young peo-
students have more free time than they did while attend- ple enter college may be for positive reasons (for example,
ing high school. All of these new freedoms contribute to to be social) and for negative reasons (to cope with stress
an increase in alcohol use among first-year college students or to conform to misperceived norms). Cooper, Agocha, and
(Arnett, 2005; Maggs, 1997; White & Jackson, 2004/2005). Sheldon (2000) found that drinking to cope with negative
Young people also face more challenges during their first circumstances predicted heavy drinking among college stu-
year of college than they did in high school, such as pay- dents. However, research suggests that college students are
ing for school and balancing social pressures with academic more likely to drink for positive or celebratory reasons than to
demands, new roles and the need to develop new friendships, cope with negative feelings (Read, Wood, Kahler, Maddock,
all of which may create stress. Some students may turn to & Palfai, 2003).
Whereas there may be some benefits of drinking for
∗ Tel.: +1 732 445 3579; fax: +1 732 445 3500. college students (such as socialising), excessive drinking
E-mail address: hewhite@rci.rutgers.edu. among this group is associated with negative consequences

0955-3959/$ – see front matter © 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.drugpo.2006.02.006
H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319 311

that can have long-term repercussions (White & Jackson, Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2002). The authors con-
2004/2005). In 2001, more than 1700 US college student cluded that the most effective individual approaches for alco-
deaths and over 500,000 unintentional injuries were alcohol- hol prevention on college campuses were: (1) interventions
related (Hingson, Heeren, Winter, & Wechsler, 2005). Exces- that combined brief cognitive-behavioural skills with norms
sive drinking by college students increases the risks of fatal clarification and motivational enhancement; (2) brief motiva-
and non-fatal injuries, academic failure, violence and other tional enhancement interventions; and (3) alcohol expectancy
crime and unsafe sexual behaviour (Goldman, 2002; Presley, challenges (Larimer & Cronce, 2002). Brief interventions
Meilman, & Cashin, 1996; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Lee, are needed because most multi-component programs are
2000; Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, & Lee, 2001). In addition, resource-intensive, making them difficult to implement on
there are second-hand negative consequences from exces- a large scale (Boyd & Faden, 2002).
sive drinking by students that are experienced by others on Brief interventions have been used in a variety
campus and in local neighbourhoods (for example, physical of settings—including college health centres, emergency
and sexual assaults, vandalism, insults and humiliation, sleep rooms, crisis centres and physicians’ offices—and are usu-
disturbances and so on) (Wechsler et al., 2001). Therefore, ally defined as minimal contact with a medical or mental
interventions are needed to reduce the harms associated with health professional that ranges from several minutes to sev-
excessive drinking among college students. eral sessions in length (usually one or two sessions, but almost
Although 31% of US college students report symptoms always less than four) (Barnett et al., 2001; Bien, Miller, &
of alcohol abuse and 6% report symptoms of dependence Tonigan, 1993; Dimeff et al., 1999). Brief and inexpensive
(Knight et al., 2002), most will outgrow heavy drinking and interventions have demonstrated effectiveness with individ-
alcohol problems on their own and without treatment (Marlatt uals who are not seeking help (Heather, 1998). Studies have
et al., 1998; Schulenberg, O’Malley, Bachman, Wadsworth, also shown a positive effect of brief interventions in health
& Johnston, 1996; Weingardt et al., 1998). However, young care settings with adult problem drinkers (Babor & Grant,
people are still vulnerable to harmful consequences during 1992; Ballesteros, Duffy, Querejeta, Ariño, & González-
their peak using years. Therefore, more effective prevention Pinto, 2004; Barnett et al., 2001). A major advantage of
programs are needed to help students safely move through brief interventions is that they are cost-effective and, thus,
this risky developmental period (Dimeff, Baer, Kivlahan, & can reach large numbers of people. Therefore, even pro-
Marlatt, 1999). Furthermore, although most heavy drinkers grams with small individual effects can have an overall large
mature out, there exists a subset that maintains heavy drinking effect on society. The purpose of this paper is to describe the
and goes on to develop more serious problems with alcohol rationale for brief personal feedback interventions for college
after college (O’Neill, Parra, & Sher, 2001). Unfortunately, students and to summarise evaluations of these interventions.
it is difficult to predict in advance who will and who will
not mature out. Therefore, heavy-drinking college students
represent an important target group for prevention efforts. Brief personal feedback interventions
It is especially important to reach at-risk students who do
not typically seek help. College campuses are settings where One type of brief intervention that has been particularly
young people with problems can be identified and treated effective with college students is brief personal feedback
(Barnett, Monti, & Wood, 2001). Campuses need to imple- interventions (PFIs). PFIs assume that by receiving infor-
ment new strategies for screening and early identification of mation about one’s own drinking patterns in relation to peers
high-risk youths and ensure that treatment is readily available and personal risk factors, the individual will be motivated
to those in need (Knight et al., 2002, p. 263). to change, develop a strategy for change and implement
Over the last decade, and especially the last several years, change (Miller, Toscova, Miller, & Sanchez, 2000). PFIs are
there has been massive growth in college prevention pro- designed, therefore, to heighten the participant’s awareness
grams, although few have been evaluated properly (Anderson of personal patterns of use, peer norms, risks related to use
& Milgram, 1996, 2001). Furthermore, the proliferation of and the experience of negative consequences under certain
college prevention programs has not led to a reduction in drinking conditions. The increased awareness and salience
problems related to alcohol use among college students of personal risk factors is a key component of theories of
(Wechsler et al., 2002). For the most part, universal programs behavioural change (Larimer et al., 2001, p. 372). Further-
have been the least successful (Chiauzzi, Green, Lord, Thum, more, because students tend to over-estimate the acceptability
& Goldstein, 2005; Moskowitz, 1989). Targeted and indi- of heavy drinking by peers and the amounts of alcohol that
cated programs may be especially cost-effective because they other students drink (Borsari & Carey, 2001, 2003; Perkins,
reach students who could potentially benefit the most from 2002), individualised feedback to counteract these misper-
the intervention (Larimer et al., 2001). A thorough review ceptions is hypothesised to reduce harmful drinking.
of individually oriented interventions with adequate evalu- PFIs are recommended for people who do not exhibit
ation designs was conducted for the US National Institute severe substance use dependence but, rather, have mild to
on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Task Force on College moderate problems or who use substances in harmful ways
Drinking (Task Force of the National Advisory Council on (Dimeff et al., 1999). Such interventions seem particularly
312 H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319

appropriate with college students who often demonstrate negative consequences of use. The BASICS model, thus,
risky use of substances or who use in combination with at-risk relies on motivational enhancement techniques to increase
situations (for example, alcohol-impaired driving). PFIs are clients’ readiness for change and to help guide them through
often conducted in person within the context of a brief motiva- the change process (Dimeff et al., 1999). BASICS has been
tional interview (BMI), a technique that focuses on increasing tested on several college campuses throughout the United
clients’ motivations to change (Miller & Rollnick, 2002). A States. Below, results from these studies, as well as other stud-
major goal of BMIs is to make clients aware of the discrep- ies that have evaluated PFIs for college students, are reviewed.
ancy between their actual and desired behaviour. BMIs have
the potential to be especially effective with young people
because they are tailored for individuals who may not be inter- Evaluations of personal feedback interventions
ested in changing their behaviour and utilise strategies that are
appropriate to individuals’ readiness to change (Barnett et al., Some of the studies evaluating PFIs with college students
2001). Marlatt, Larimer, Baer, & Quigley (1993) identified have incorporated a PFI within the context of an in-person
numerous advantages of BMIs for college students. First, the intervention, and others have used written feedback only.
non-confrontational and non-judgemental style of a BMI is Most of the evaluations have used samples of student vol-
appropriate for college students who are generally defensive unteers, generally screened for high risk, and most of these
about their drinking and do not respond positively to being studies were included in a recent review by Walters and
lectured. Second, this technique avoids labelling young peo- Neighbors (2005). A few studies have also evaluated PFIs for
ple as having a problem or as being a substance abuser. Third, mandated students. This paper extends Walters and Neigh-
the technique is based on each individual’s specific history bors’ review by adding four studies of mandated students
and risk factors. Therefore, it addresses the highly variable (Barnett, Colby, & Monti, 2004; Borsari & Carey, 2005;
nature of college drinking behaviour. Finally, because BMIs Fromme & Corbin, 2004; White et al., 2006), which were
put the responsibility on the individual to recognise his or her not published at the time that the authors wrote their review,
own need to change, the student is treated as a ‘thoughtful as well as one new Web-based study (Chiauzzi et al., 2005)
adult’. and another study that was not included in their review (Nye,
Many PFIs for college students have been modelled on the Agnostinelli, & Smith, 1999). Two studies included in the
Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Stu- Walters and Neighbors’ review (Dimeff & McNeely, 2000;
dents (BASICS) model, which was designed as a model BMI Walters, 2000) are not summarised here, because the cited
for alcohol prevention among this population (Dimeff et al., references did not provide enough information about their
1999). The BASICS program combines Miller & Rollnick’s design and/or findings to be able to adequately summarise
(2002) concept of motivational enhancement with cognitive- them. Only studies that have used randomised designs are
behavioural skills training (Baer et al., 1992) within a harm included in this review.
reduction perspective (Marlatt, Baer, & Larimer, 1995). The
primary goal is to move students to reduce risky behaviours Interventions with student volunteers
and avoid the harmful effects from drinking rather than focus
on a specific drinking target, such as abstinence (Dimeff et al., Baer et al. (1992) randomised students to either a class-
1999, p. 5). BASICS is conducted in two sessions. In the first room format, which entailed six 90-min weekly meetings
session, assessments of drinking are made, and information including an alcohol challenge, or to an individualised feed-
is presented about alcohol use, such as the effects of varying back and advice format, which included an assessment and
blood alcohol concentrations (BACs). In the second session, 1 h of personalised feedback in the context of a motiva-
the student is given feedback about his or her drinking in tional interview. Follow-ups were conducted at 3, 6, 12 and
relation to other students, and then strategies to reduce risk 24 months. Both groups (approximate N = 69 at follow-up)
are discussed. The feedback sheet contains information on a reduced their frequency and volume of drinking by about 40%
student’s drinking pattern relative to other college students, from baseline to follow-up, and reductions were maintained
his or her peak BAC level, alcohol-related problems and per- over time. There were no significant differences between
sonal risk factors (for example, dependence symptoms and individuals who attended the classes and those who received
family history of alcoholism; see Dimeff et al., 1999; Murphy the brief feedback intervention, although students were more
et al., 2001). favourable about the classroom intervention. This study was
The theoretical model for BASICS assumes that, upon one of the earliest to demonstrate that a brief intervention for
receiving feedback on the extent of one’s personal risk, alco- college students could be successful in modifying drinking
hol use and expectancies in relation to peers, the student behaviours. However, the relatively small N and lack of a
will increase his or her readiness to change his or her drink- no-treatment control group were limitations.
ing behaviours (Miller & Rollnick, 2002). In addition, the Marlatt et al. (1998) evaluated the efficacy of the BASICS
students’ perceptions about risk, peer use and alcohol/drug model with 348 high-risk student volunteers who were
expectancies will change (Dimeff et al., 1999). These changes randomly assigned to the intervention condition or to an
will lead to reduced drinking, which in turn should reduce assessment-only control group. During the winter of their
H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319 313

freshman year, students in the PFI met with an interviewer and a clinician and discussed his or her personal feedback sheet
received personal feedback about their drinking in relation to (N = 30); (2) an alcohol education condition, consisting of a
college averages, risk behaviours, personal risks for problems 30-min video on consequences of drinking and then a 20-
and beliefs about alcohol effects. Each student was given min individual session with a clinician to discuss the video,
a written personalised feedback profile along with generic as well as a generic information sheet on risks of drinking
information about alcohol and strategies for reducing risk. for college students (N = 29); or (3) a no-treatment control
At the 6-month follow-up, those in the PFI reported less fre- group (N = 25). Students were followed up at 3 and 9 months
quent drinking, lower quantities and lower peak quantities after the intervention. Given the small sample size, there were
over time compared to the controls. Effect sizes were rel- no statistically significant differences among the groups on
atively small (standardised effect sizes of about 0.15). The the drinking variables. However, at the 3-month follow-up,
following year, members of the PFI group were sent written effect sizes indicated that BASICS students reported greater
feedback regarding their responses to the baseline, 6-month reductions in frequency of drinking than the participants of
and 12-month assessments and some (especially those at risk) the education (between group effect size = 0.46) and control
received a second BMI in person or over the telephone. At the (between group effect size = 0.46) groups and greater reduc-
2-year follow-up, there were significant reductions in drink- tions in alcohol problems than the control students (between
ing and related problems for all students; however, those in the group effect size = 0.33). At the 9-month follow-up, there
PFI group displayed significantly greater reduction in drink- were no significant differences among groups for the drinking
ing rates (effect sizes ranged from 0.14 to 0.20) and problems variables. The study also found that the PFI was more effi-
(effect size of 0.32) than those in the control group (Marlatt et cacious with heavier than lighter drinkers. Finally, students
al., 1998). The results were maintained when outcomes were in the BASICS group rated the intervention more favourably
compared by gender, parental history of alcoholism, conduct than those in the education group.
problems and whether or not students lived in fraternities or Using a small sample of 72 high-risk drinker volunteers,
sororities. A 4-year follow-up indicated that these PFI effects Nye et al. (1999) evaluated the efficacy of two types of writ-
were sustained over time (Baer, Kivlahan, Blume, McKnight, ten personal feedback (self-focusing and normative) deliv-
& Marlatt, 2001). ered in person. The self-focusing feedback provided students
At the same school, Larimer et al. (2001) administered a with information about their own drinking patterns, whereas
similar intervention to fraternity members. Fraternities were the normative feedback provided students with information
randomly assigned to a PFI (N = 77) or an assessment-only about drinking patterns of college students of their same
control group (N = 82). Students in the PFI received a 1-h indi- sex. Study participants were randomly assigned to one of
vidually tailored feedback session, presented according to the four groups (self-focusing-only, normative-only, both self-
principles of motivational interviewing. PFI students reported focusing and normative, and a control group that received
greater reductions in the number of drinks per week (effect printed material on a health issue unrelated to alcohol); group
size = 0.42) and typical peak BAC (effect size = 0.38) com- sample sizes for this study were relatively small. Students
pared to controls. There were no significant group differences who received either the normative information alone or the
in volume of alcohol consumed per occasion, frequency of self-focusing information alone perceived the greatest per-
drinking or alcohol-related problems. Students who received sonal alcohol problem recognition and provided the most
their intervention from peers (trained undergraduates) rather negative evaluations of their own drinking. The group that
than from professionals (trained psychology graduate stu- received both types of feedback decreased problem recogni-
dents) reported significantly greater reductions in peak BAC tion, possibly because the combined feedback created more
(effect size = 0.58). defensiveness. Effects sizes were not presented.
At another university, Borsari and Carey (2000) evaluated Neal and Carey (2004) compared two types of feedback
the efficacy of a PFI within a BMI, based on the BASICS interventions delivered in a small group context for the major-
model. They randomly assigned 29 students to the PFI and ity of participants (five students received the session indi-
31 students to a no-treatment control group. At the 6-week vidually). High-risk volunteers were randomly assigned to
follow-up, the brief intervention group reported significantly a PFI delivered in a small group (N = 31), a personal striv-
greater reductions in frequency of overall drinking (effect ing assessment (PSA) group (N = 31) or an attention control
size = 0.28), frequency of heavy drinking (effect size = 0.12) group (N = 30). Students in the PFI received personal feed-
and number of drinks per week (effect size = 0.21) compared back on their own drinking and normative comparison data, as
to controls. There were no group differences in alcohol- well as feedback on their frequency of alcohol-related prob-
related problems. The authors found that perceptions of typi- lems. Students in the PSA group participated in an exercise
cal student drinking mediated the effects of the intervention. in which they rated how each of their 10 most important
Results should be interpreted with caution, given the rela- personal strivings (presented to them on a form) would be
tively small sample size. affected by various levels of alcohol consumption. The atten-
Murphy et al. (2001) also evaluated the BASICS model. tion control group received a handout on alcohol absorption
High-risk student volunteers were randomly assigned to: (1) and metabolism, which was explained in a lecture format. Fol-
a 50-min session, in which the student met individually with lowing the intervention, PFI students showed a significantly
314 H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319

higher intention to reduce their drinking than the PSA and size = 0.33) than controls; however, these significant differ-
control groups and higher discrepancy compared to the con- ences disappeared at the 6-month follow-up. The intervention
trol group, although the authors provided no effect sizes. The did not affect peak BAC or alcohol-related consequences. The
three groups did not differ in terms of reported negative affect researchers suggested that future PFIs should include infor-
or any drinking outcome measured the week following the mation about negative consequences on the feedback sheet.
intervention. Although the sample for this study was larger than the pre-
Murphy et al. (2004) randomly assigned high-risk stu- vious two evaluations of mailed interventions, the follow-up
dent volunteers to either receive written personal feedback sample was still relatively small.
and a harm reduction sheet only (N = 28) or to receive writ- In addition to mailed feedback, a couple of recent studies
ten personal feedback and a harm reduction sheet within the have evaluated computer-delivered feedback. In a study by
context of a BMI (N = 26). At the 6-month follow-up, stu- Neighbors, Larimer, and Lewis (2004), heavy drinkers (based
dents in both conditions reduced their frequency of drinking, on a large screening) completed a baseline assessment and
number of drinks per week and frequency of heavy drinking were randomly assigned to receive a PFI delivered imme-
with medium effects sizes (mean within group effect size was diately by computer and then printed out (N = 126) or to a
0.42 for the feedback-only group and 0.48 for the feedback no-treatment control group (N = 126). The feedback profile
plus BMI group), although there were no significant reduc- included only information about the amount the participant
tions in alcohol-related problems. There were no significant consumed, his or her perceptions about other students’ drink-
differences between intervention conditions, demonstrating ing, and other students’ actual drinking. The results indicated
that written PFIs can be as efficacious as in-person PFIs. that there were significant reductions in drinking behaviour
The small sample size may have contributed to the inabil- for the intervention group relative to the control group at
ity to detect group differences. Furthermore, there was no both the 3- and 6-month follow-ups (effect sizes = 0.35 and
no-treatment control group. 0.36, respectively). In addition, at both follow-ups, there were
A few studies have evaluated mailed PFIs. Agostinelli, significant differences in perception of peer norms (effect
Brown, and Miller (1995) evaluated the efficacy of a mailed sizes = 0.61 and 0.63, respectively). Consistent with the find-
PFI with a very small sample of high-risk student volunteers ings of the Borsari and Carey (2000) study, the reduction in
(N = 26). After completing a baseline assessment, students drinking was mediated by the reduction in perceptions of peer
were randomly assigned to receive written feedback by mail use.
or to a no-contact control group. Six weeks later, those in the In another study, Chiauzzi et al. (2005) evaluated the effi-
PFI significantly reduced their alcohol consumption (effect cacy of a Web-based PFI. Heavy drinking college students
size of 1.14) and average BAC (effect size = 1.00) compared were randomised by sex to either an interactive Web site that
to the control group, but not their peak BAC. offered motivational feedback (known as ‘My Student Body:
Using a small sample of high-risk volunteers (N = 37), Alcohol’) (N = 105) or to a control group that received alcohol
Walters, Bennett, and Miller (2000) also compared a 2-h education on a Web site (N = 110). Both groups were expected
small group intervention with mailed personal feedback to to access the Web site for four weekly 20-min sessions. Stu-
a mailed PFI-only condition and a no-contact control condi- dents were followed up approximately 1 month after baseline
tion. At the 6-week follow-up, the mailed PFI significantly and 3 months post-intervention. Both groups showed signifi-
decreased the number of drinks per month (within group cant decreases in alcohol frequency, occasions of binge drink-
effect size = 1.01) relative to controls (within group effect ing and alcohol quantity over time. At the 1-month follow-up,
size = 0.04). The group intervention plus feedback group did PFI students decreased their maximum number of drinks con-
not differ significantly from the other two groups (within sumed on a drinking day more quickly (from logged 2.0 to
group effect size = 0.60). There were no significant differ- 1.56) than controls (from logged 1.8 to 1.6) and decreased
ences among groups in terms of peak BAC, alcohol-related their overall drinking composite measure (from z-score −.09
consequences and alcohol expectancies, although the small to −.32), whereas controls increased their overall drinking
sample size may have affected these results. Overall, the (from z-score .23 to .39). At the 3-month follow-up, there
results indicated that a feedback-only intervention was as effi- were no significant differences between groups on any alco-
cacious as a more intense in-person intervention in reducing hol use measure. However, significant differences between
drinking behaviour over a short time period. the PFI and control groups were found for women, persistent
Collins, Carey, and Sliwinski (2002) expanded upon these heavy drinkers and drinkers with low motivation to change.
studies of mailed feedback and evaluated the efficacy of Within each of these subgroups, students in the PFI condi-
a mailed PFI for at-risk college student volunteers. Stu- tion reduced their alcohol consumption rates faster than those
dents were randomly assigned to the PFI group (N = 47) in the control condition. Furthermore, non-White students
or an attention-control group (which received a psycho- reduced their drinking more quickly than White students.
educational brochure about alcohol, N = 48). At the 6-week Finally, PFI students reported more favourable evaluations
follow-up, students in the PFI reported fewer drinks per of the intervention site than education students. The PFI con-
heaviest drinking week (between group effect size = 0.28) dition included educational, motivational and skill building
and fewer heavy-drinking episodes (between group effect components, and it is not possible, therefore, to attribute the
H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319 315

positive effects of the intervention specifically to the feedback ence between intervention groups. As found in the Murphy
component. et al. (2004) study, there were no significant differences in
In sum, the above studies have demonstrated that in- any outcome (or mediator) between those who received the
person, mailed and computer-generated PFIs are effective in in-person intervention and those who received only written
reducing alcohol use and/or related problems among heavy feedback.
drinking college student volunteers. Nevertheless, the results Barnett et al. (2004) compared a one-session BMI with
should be interpreted cautiously due to the small sample sizes personal feedback to a standard alcohol education interven-
and short-term follow-ups in many of the studies. tion (using the CD-ROM, Alcohol 101). After the interven-
tion, students were randomly assigned to receive a booster
Interventions with mandated students session 1 month later. The booster session was the same for-
mat as their original intervention, although somewhat shorter.
A few recent studies have evaluated PFIs for students Students with and without the booster were followed up 3
mandated to receive an intervention because of infractions months after the baseline intervention. Preliminary findings
of university rules concerning alcohol use. Such students for 117 students indicated that both intervention groups (BMI
are an important target group for interventions (Larimer & and education) significantly reduced their frequency of drink-
Cronce, 2002) and generally report higher drinking rates, ing, frequency of heavy drinking and the number of drinks
more alcohol-related problems and lower grades than other per week, although no between group differences were found
students (Fromme & Corbin, 2004; Tevyaw, Monti, & Colby, for these outcomes (effect sizes were not presented). Students
2004). in the alcohol education group reduced their typical BAC sig-
Borsari and Carey (2005) compared high-risk mandated nificantly more than those in the BMI (possibly because the
students in an in-person PFI (N = 34) to those receiving an CD-ROM focused on BAC levels). There were no changes
alcohol education intervention (N = 30). Both interventions in the number of alcohol-related negative consequences. Stu-
were conducted one-on-one. The PFI was provided within the dents in the BMI group with the booster session were more
context of a BMI, using the principles of motivational inter- likely to seek further counseling compared to those in the
viewing (Miller & Rollnick, 2002). At the 6-month follow- other three groups.
up, both groups significantly reduced their heavy drinking Fromme and Corbin (2004) evaluated the Lifestyle Man-
episodes (PFI within group effect size = 0.39; alcohol educa- agement Class (LMC) using 403 high-risk volunteers and 113
tion group = 0.52). The PFI intervention group (within group mandated college students. Students were randomly assigned
effect size = 1.11) showed significantly greater reductions to either a peer-led LMC (N = 193), a professionally led LMC
in the alcohol-related problems than the alcohol education (N = 159) or to a control group (wait-list control group for
group (within group effect size = 0.07) (between group effect mandated students or assessment-only control group for vol-
size = 0.39). Although there was no significant time by group unteers) (N = 164). The LMC involved two 2-h classes with
interaction effect for typical BAC, the reduction was larger about 10 students in each group. In the first session, students
for the PFI (within group effect size = 0.67) than for the alco- completed an assessment of their drinking behaviour and then
hol education group (within group effect size = 0.19). discussed alcohol use, behavioural change, self-management
White et al. (2006) (see also White, Labouvie, Morgan, and moderation. In the second session they were provided
Pugh, & Celinska, 2005) evaluated a brief PFI for mandated with graphic feedback about their drinking in comparison to
college students. The program was modelled after BASICS. peer use and there was more discussion about alcohol use,
After the initial assessment session, students were randomly peer norms and harm reduction strategies. Students were
assigned to either receive a written personal feedback pro- followed up approximately 6 weeks after the pre-test and
file only (N = 104) or to go over their personal profile with 6 months post-intervention. All groups, including the con-
a counselor within the context of a BMI (N = 118). At the trol group, reduced their drinking over time. At the 6 week
3-month follow-up, students in both conditions significantly post-test, the only alcohol-related outcome that differentiated
reduced their number of drinks per week (effect size = 0.32), between treatment and control groups was that the treatment
peak BAC in a typical week (effect size = 0.46), occasions of groups reported a greater decrease in instances of alcohol-
high-volume drinking (effect size = 0.14) and alcohol-related impaired driving (within group effect sizes for mandated
problems (effect size = 0.58). In addition, there was a sig- students ranged from 0.03 to 0.22 and for volunteer students
nificant decrease in the percent of students who smoked from 0.17 to 0.20) than controls (within group effect size
cigarettes (24% decrease) and used cannabis (20% decrease), ranged from −0.09 to 0.00). At the 6-month follow-up, there
as well as in the number of drug-related problems (effect were no significant effects involving treatment condition. The
size = 0.19). The authors also found that both PFIs had a sig- results indicated that there were no differences in alcohol use
nificant effect on proposed mediators; participants in both outcomes among students in the peer-led and professionally
conditions significantly reduced their perception of the num- led LMCs.
ber of students who use cannabis (effect size = 0.26) and In sum, all studies evaluating the efficacy of interventions
other drugs (effect size = 0.27) and increased their readiness with mandated students showed declines in alcohol use out-
to change (effect size = 0.19), although there was no differ- comes, although the type of intervention did not appear to
316 H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319

affect most outcomes. Because of ethical reasons, it is often treatment controls (for example, Agostinelli et al., 1995;
difficult to assign mandated students to a no-treatment con- Collins et al., 2002; Walters et al., 2000), and in all of these
trol group. Therefore, only the Fromme and Corbin (2004) studies, the control group completed a baseline assessment.
study had a no-treatment (wait list) control group, and for Therefore, to date, no studies have demonstrated that in-
most of the drinking variables, these students improved sim- person PFIs are superior to written feedback alone (mailed
ilarly to those receiving the intervention. The absence of a or off the Web), and existing studies have suggested that
no-treatment control group in the other three studies of man- feedback-alone is associated with reductions in drinking
dated students makes it difficult to determine the specific compared to assessment-only. Group formats for providing
effects of the intervention relative to being reprimanded or feedback have not proven to be as efficacious as these other
simply being assessed. modes (Walters & Neighbors, 2005). Longer term follow-ups
are needed to demonstrate that these reductions are sustained
over time. Given the existing evidence, however, it is reason-
Discussion able to hypothesise that written feedback can be an efficacious
approach for reducing the harms related to alcohol use among
The results of rigorous evaluations of PFIs with US college college students. Murphy et al. (2004) suggested that written
students have been quite promising. Students receiving per- PFIs may be particularly effective with ambivalent students
sonal feedback regarding their own drinking and peer norms who might increase resistance in the presence of a counselor.
reduce drinking and related problems more than those who Electronic assessment and feedback may be an especially
do not receive feedback, and, thus far, this finding holds cost-effective strategy for providing PFIs to college students.
regardless of whether the feedback is presented in person or In focus groups conducted in New Zealand, high-risk college
not. Similar findings have been reported outside the United student drinkers expressed an interest in electronic assess-
States. For example, university students in New Zealand ment and feedback in contrast to a reluctance to discuss
were randomly assigned to a computerised assessment and their drinking with a doctor or another health professional
PFI in the context of a BMI or to a leaflet-only control (Kypri, 2004). Furthermore, an anonymous survey of New
condition. Students in the PFI significantly reduced their Zealand university students found that electronic screening
drinking and related problems relative to controls (Kypri, and brief intervention was the most popular intervention
2004). strategy endorsed by students (Kypri, 2004). Students in the
Therefore, the efficacy of BMIs for college students may US also prefer unassisted, self-directed or minimal-contact
not depend on personal contact, but instead may be the result alcohol use interventions over other types of interventions
of the feedback provided (Larimer & Cronce, 2002). In fact, (Chiauzzi et al., 2005).
both Murphy et al. (2004) and White et al. (2006) compared Furthermore, personal computer (for example, Neighbors
in-person to written feedback-only PFIs and demonstrated et al., 2004) and Web-based (for example, Chiauzzi et al.,
that written feedback alone was as effective as in-person 2005) feedbacks have been shown to be efficacious in reduc-
feedback to reduce harmful drinking behaviours, at least on a ing alcohol consumption among heavy-drinking college stu-
short-term basis. The former study used high-risk college stu- dents when compared to no intervention and an alternative
dent volunteers and the latter was conducted with mandated intervention, respectively. Thus, recent research supports
students. However, the first study was based on a relatively the feasibility of providing motivational interventions on a
small sample and neither study utilised a no-treatment con- large-scale anonymous basis using computerised technolo-
trol group. Therefore, it is possible that assessment alone may gies (Chiauzzi et al., 2005, p. 272). In other words, Web-
have had a positive effect on reducing drinking behaviour based approaches offer a promising non-intrusive way to
(Fromme & Corbin, 2004; White et al., 2006). However, provide incentive to change risky drinking among college stu-
White et al. (2006) compared their mandated students to a dents (Koski-Jannes & Cunningham, 2001), although more
representative sample of college students who were assessed research is needed with larger samples to establish the effi-
twice—2 months apart—as a control group for a different cacy of such interventions.
study (i.e., they served as a quasi-control group for the PFI It is, therefore, reasonable to recommend that college
students). The analyses indicated that the observed changes administrators begin to implement screening and feedback
from baseline to follow-up among the mandated students interventions for incoming students. In a recent review arti-
receiving the PFI were not due to normal developmental cle, Larimer, Cronce, Lee, and Kilmer (2004/2005) identified
reductions in substance use over the course of a college several opportunities to screen young people for high-risk
year. White et al. (2006) found that the quasi-control group drinking on college campuses. They also discussed the ben-
increased some aspects of their drinking (such as the num- efits and costs of universal screening compared to screening
ber of drinks per week and peak BAC) after a baseline students in local emergency rooms or student health centres,
assessment, whereas the mandated students receiving the PFI and to screening mandated students. The authors identified
decreased in these same drinking measures. Furthermore, several issues that must be considered when initiating a large-
several short-term follow-up studies have demonstrated that scale screening and intervention program, including who
mailed PFIs lead to reductions in drinking compared to no- should deliver the intervention, how to encourage students to
H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319 317

participate in these programs, how to increase student trust Anderson, D. S., & Milgram, G. G. (1996). Promising practices source-
and accuracy of responses, institutional liability and con- book: Campus alcohol strategies. Fairfax, VA: George Mason Uni-
fidentiality of the data (see Larimer et al., 2004/2005, for versity.
Anderson, D. S., & Milgram, G. G. (2001). Promising practices source-
greater details on recommendations for college administra- book: Campus alcohol strategies (2nd ed.). Fairfax, VA: George
tors). Larimer et al. concluded, and I agree, that regardless of Mason University.
how measures are implemented, the intervention content and Arnett, J. J. (2005). The developmental context of substance use in emerg-
process should be based on interventions that have already ing adulthood. Journal of Drug Issues, 35, 235–254.
been proven to be efficacious.Although a large amount of Babor, T. F., & Grant, M. (1992). Project on identification and manage-
ment of alcohol related problems. Report on phase II: A randomized
evidence supports the efficacy of PFIs, more research is still clinical trial of brief interventions in primary health care. Geneva,
needed before universal screening and feedback interven- Switzerland: World Health Organization.
tions are initiated. Walters and Neighbors (2005) highlighted Bachman, J. G., Wadsworth, K. N., O’Malley, P. M., Johnston, L. D.,
several gaps in the existing literature. First, it is necessary & Schulenberg, J. (1997). Smoking, drinking and drug use in young
to identify what components of feedback intervention are adulthood: The impacts of new freedoms and new responsibilities.
Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
necessary and sufficient, as well as whether there are addi- Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., Blume, A. W., McKnight, P., & Marlatt,
tive effects of various feedback components. Furthermore, G. A. (2001). Brief intervention for heavy drinking college students:
research is needed to determine which components are effec- Four-year follow-up and natural history. American Journal of Public
tive for which types of students. There is also a need to Health, 91(8), 1310–1316.
determine the appropriate reference group when providing Baer, J. S., Marlatt, G. A., Kivlahan, D. R., Fromme, K., Larimer, M. E.,
& Williams, E. (1992). Experimental test of three methods of alcohol
normative feedback to various groups of students. Finally, risk reduction with young adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
more research is needed to evaluate potential mechanisms of Psychology, 60(6), 974–979.
intervention efficacy. Ballesteros, J., Duffy, J. C., Querejeta, I., Ariño, J., & Gonzalez-Pinto, A.
Given that most college students, at least in the US, have (2004). Efficacy of brief interventions for hazardous drinkers in pri-
access to the Internet (Chiauzzi et al., 2005), it would be mary care: Systematic review and meta-analyses. Alcoholism: Clinical
and Experimental Research, 28(4), 608–618.
possible to deliver Web-based interventions to the entire stu- Barnett, N., Monti, P. M., & Wood, M. (2001). Motivational interview-
dent body and, thereby, to provide much needed screening ing for alcohol-involved adolescents in the emergency room. In E.
and intervention services (Knight et al., 2002). Several of the F. Wagner & H. B. Waldron (Eds.), Innovations in adolescent sub-
studies reviewed above demonstrated that PFIs were more stance abuse interventions (pp. 143–168). Amsterdam, Netherlands:
efficacious with higher than lower risk students (for example, Pergamon/Elsevier Science.
Barnett, N. P., Colby, S. M., & Monti, P. M. (2004). Brief motivational
Chiauzzi et al., 2005; Murphy et al., 2001). Therefore, Web- intervention with college students following medical treatment or dis-
based PFIs potentially might be used to supplement or replace cipline for alcohol. In N. P. Barnett, T. O. Tevyaw, K. Fromme, B.
more labour-intensive, in-person interventions for high-risk Borsari, K. B. Carey, W. R. Corbin, et al. (Eds.), Brief alcohol inter-
students. More research is needed to determine what com- ventions with mandated or adjudicated college students (pp. 971–974).
ponents of Web-based feedback are beneficial and for what Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 28(6), 966–
975.
types of college students Web-based PFIs are and are not Bien, T. H., Miller, W. R., & Tonigan, J. S. (1993). Brief interventions
effective. for alcohol problems: A review. Addiction, 88(3), 315–336.
Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2000). Effects of a brief motivational inter-
vention with college student drinkers. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 728–733.
Acknowledgements Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2001). Peer influences on college drinking: A
review of the research. Journal of Substance Abuse, 13(4), 391–424.
The writing of this paper was supported by the National Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2003). Descriptive and injunctive norms in
Institute on Drug Abuse (DA 17552) as part of the Rutgers college drinking: A meta-analytic integration. Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 64(3), 331–341.
Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center. An earlier ver-
Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2005). Two brief alcohol interventions for
sion of the paper was presented at the 16th International mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19(3),
Conference on the Reduction of Drug Related Harm, Jan- 296–302.
uary 25, 2005, in Belfast, Ireland. The author thanks Valerie Boyd, G. M., & Faden, V. (2002). Overview. Journal of Studies on Alco-
Johnson, Erich Labouvie, Mary Larimer, Thomas Morgan, hol, (Suppl. 14), 6–13.
Chiauzzi, E., Green, T. C., Lord, S., Thum, C., & Goldstein, M. (2005).
Robert Pandina, Lisa Pugh and two anonymous reviewers
My student body: A high-risk drinking prevention Web site for college
for their comments and contributions and Lisa Metzger and students. Journal of American College Health, 53(6), 263–274.
Grace Yan for assistance with manuscript preparation. Collins, S., Carey, K., & Sliwinski, M. (2002). Mailed personalized nor-
mative feedback as a brief intervention for at risk college drinkers.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(5), 559–567.
Cooper, M. L., Agocha, V. B., & Sheldon, M. S. (2000). A motivational
References perspective on risky behaviors: The role of personality and affect
regulatory processes. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 1059–1088.
Agostinelli, G., Brown, J. M., & Miller, W. R. (1995). Effects of norma- Dimeff, L. A., Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., & Marlatt, G. A. (1999). Brief
tive feedback on consumption among heavy drinking college students. Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS): A
Journal of Drug Education, 25(1), 31–40. harm reduction approach. New York: Guilford Press.
318 H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319

Dimeff, L. A., & McNeely, M. (2000). Computer-enhanced primary care motivational interview. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65(2), 200–
practitioner advice for high-risk college drinkers in a student primary 203.
health-care setting. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 7(1), 82–100. Murphy, J. G., Duchnick, J. J., Vuchinich, R. E., Davison, J. W., Karg, R.
Fromme, K., & Corbin, W. (2004). Prevention of heavy drinking and S., Olson, A. M., et al. (2001). Relative efficacy of a brief motivational
associated negative consequences among mandated and voluntary col- intervention for college student drinkers. Psychology of Addictive
lege students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(6), Behaviors, 15(4), 373–379.
1038–1049. Neal, D. J., & Carey, K. B. (2004). Developing discrepancy within
Goldman, M. S. (2002). Introduction. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, self-regulation theory: Use of personalized normative feedback and
(Suppl. 14), 5. personal strivings with heavy-drinking college students. Addictive
Heather, N. (1998). Using brief opportunities for change in medical Behaviors, 29(2), 281–297.
settings. In W. R. Miller & N. Heather (Eds.), Treating addictive Neighbors, C., Larimer, M. E., & Lewis, M. A. (2004). Target-
behaviours (2nd ed., pp. 133–147). New York: Plenum Press. ing misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: Efficacy of
Hingson, R., Heeren, T., Winter, M., & Wechsler, H. (2005). Magnitude a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback interven-
of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among US college students tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(3), 434–
ages 18-24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public 447.
Health, 26, 259–279. Nye, E. C., Agostinelli, G., & Smith, J. E. (1999). Enhancing alcohol
Knight, J., Wechsler, H., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Weitzman, E., & problem recognition: A self-regulation model for the effects of self-
Schuckit, M. (2002). Alcohol abuse and dependence among US col- focusing and normative information. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
lege students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(3), 263–270. 60(5), 685–693.
Koski-Jannes, A., & Cunningham, J. (2001). Interest in different forms O’Neill, S. E., Parra, G. R., & Sher, K. J. (2001). Clinical relevance of
of self-help in a general population sample of drinkers. Addictive heavy drinking during the college years: Cross-sectional and prospec-
Behaviors, 26(1), 91–99. tive perspectives. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(4), 350–
Kypri, K. (2004). College student hazardous drinking in New Zealand, the 359.
USA, UK, and Australia: Implications for research, policy, and inter- Paschall, M., & Flewelling, R. L. (2002). Postsecondary education and
vention. In J. B. Saunders, K. Kypri, S. T. Walters, R. G. Laforge, & heavy drinking by young adults: The moderating effect of race. Jour-
M. E. Larimer (Eds.), Approaches to brief intervention for hazardous nal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(4), 447–455.
drinking in young people (pp. 324–325). Alcoholism: Clinical and Perkins, H. W. (2002). Social norms and the prevention of alcohol misuse
Experimental Research, 28(2), 322–329. in collegiate contexts. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, (Suppl. 14),
Larimer, M. E., & Cronce, J. M. (2002). Identification, prevention and 164–172.
treatment: A review of individual-focused strategies to reduce prob- Presley, C. A., Meilman, P. W., & Cashin, J. R. (1996). Alcohol and drugs
lematic alcohol consumption by college students. Journal of Studies on American college campuses: Use, consequences, and perceptions
on Alcohol, (Suppl. 14), 148–163. of the campus environment: Vol. 4. Carbondale, IL: CORE Institute,
Larimer, M. E., Cronce, J. M., Lee, C. M., & Kilmer, J. R. (2004/2005). Southern Illinois University, 1992-1994.
Brief intervention in college settings. Alcohol Research and Health, Read, J. P., Wood, M. D., Kahler, C. W., Maddock, J. E., & Palfai, T.
28(2), 94–104. P. (2003). Examining the role of drinking motives in college student
Larimer, M. E., Turner, A. P., Anderson, B. K., Fader, J. S., Kilmer, J. alcohol use and problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(1),
R., Palmer, R. S., et al. (2001). Evaluating a brief intervention with 13–23.
fraternities. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62(3), 370–380. Schulenburg, J. E., & Maggs, J. L. (2002). A developmental perspective
Maggs, J. L. (1997). Alcohol use and binge drinking as goal-directed on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transi-
action during the transition to postsecondary education. In J. Schu- tion to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, (Suppl. 14),
lenberg, J. L. Maggs, & K. Hurrelmann (Eds.), Health risks and 54–70.
developmental transitions during adolescence (pp. 345–371). New Schulenberg, J. E., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Wadsworth, K. N., &
York: Cambridge University Press. Johnston, L. D. (1996). Getting drunk and growing up: Trajectories of
Marlatt, G. A., Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., Dimeff, L. A., Larimer, M. frequent binge drinking in the transition to young adulthood. Journal
A., Quigley, L. A., et al. (1998). Screening and brief intervention for of Studies on Alcohol, 57(3), 289–304.
high-risk college student drinkers: Results from a 2-year follow-up Task Force of the National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and
assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(4), Alcoholism. (2002). A call to action: Changing the culture of drinking
604–615. at US colleges. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse
Marlatt, G. A., Baer, J. S., & Larimer, M. E. (1995). Preventing alco- and Alcoholism (NIAAA).
hol abuse in college students: A harm-reduction approach. In G. M. Tevyaw, T. O., Monti, P. M., & Colby, S. M. (2004). Differences between
Boyd, J. Howard, & R. A. Zucker (Eds.), Alcohol problems among mandated college students and their peers on alcohol use and readiness
adolescents: Current directions in prevention research (pp. 147–172). to change. In N. P. Barnett, T. O. Tevyaw, K. Fromme, B. Borsari, K.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. B. Carey, W. Corbin, S. M. Colby, & P. M. Monti (Eds.), Brief alco-
Marlatt, G. A., Larimer, M. E., Baer, J. S., & Quigley, L. A. (1993). hol interventions with mandated or adjudicated college students (pp.
Harm reduction for alcohol problems: Moving beyond the controlled 966–967). Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 28(6),
drinking controversy. Behavior Therapy, 24(4), 461–504. 966–975.
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing: Prepar- Walters, S. T. (2000). In praise of feedback: Effective intervention for
ing people for change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. college students who are heavy drinkers. Journal of American College
Miller, W. R., Toscova, R. T., Miller, J. H., & Sanchez, V. (2000). Theory- Health, 48(5), 235–238.
based motivational approach for reducing alcohol/drug problems in Walters, S. T., Bennett, M. E., & Miller, J. H. (2000). Reducing alcohol
college. Health Education and Behavior, 27(6), 744–759. use in college students: A controlled trial of two brief interventions.
Moskowitz, J. M. (1989). The primary prevention of alcohol problems: A Journal of Drug Education, 30(3), 361–372.
critical review of the research literature. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Walters, S. T., & Neighbors, C. (2005). Feedback interventions for college
50(1), 54–88. alcohol misuse: What, why and for whom? Addictive Behaviors, 30,
Murphy, J. G., Benson, T. A., Vuchinich, R. E., Deskins, M. M., Eakin, 1168–1182.
D., Flood, A. M., et al. (2004). A comparison of personalized Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., & Lee, H. (2000). College binge
feedback for college student drinkers delivered with and without a drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem: Results of the Har-
H.R. White / International Journal of Drug Policy 17 (2006) 310–319 319

vard School of Public Health 1999 College Alcohol Study. Journal students: Issues and longitudinal perspectives. Psychology of Addictive
of American College Health, 48(5), 199–210. Behaviors, 12(3), 155–167.
Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson, T. F., & Lee, H. White, H. R., & Jackson, K. M. (2004/2005). Social and psychological
(2002). Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased influences on emerging adult drinking behavior. Alcohol Health and
prevention efforts: Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health Research, 28(4), 182–190.
College Alcohol Study surveys: 1993–2001. Journal of American Col- White, H. R., Labouvie, E. W., Morgan, T. J., Pugh, L., & Celinska, K.
lege Health, 50(5), 203–217. (2005, May). Evaluating two brief personal feedback interventions for
Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Nelson, T. F., & Lee, H. (2001). Drinking mandated college students. Invited paper presented at the Society for
levels, alcohol problems, and secondhand effects in substance-free Prevention Research, Washington, DC.
college residences: Results of a national study. Journal of Studies on White, H. R., Morgan, T. J., Pugh, L., Celinska, K., Labouvie, E. W., &
Alcohol, 62(1), 23–31. Pandina, R. J. (2006). Evaluating two brief substance use interventions
Weingardt, K. R., Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., Roberts, L. J., Miller, E. for mandated college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(2),
T., & Marlatt, G. A. (1998). Episodic heavy drinking among college 309–317.

Вам также может понравиться