Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/321496935

I believe, therefore I achieve (and vice versa): A meta-analytic cross-


lagged panel analysis of self-efficacy and academic performance

Article  in  Learning and Individual Differences · January 2018


DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.015

CITATIONS READS

0 563

4 authors:

Kate Talsma Benjamin Schüz


University of Tasmania Universität Bremen
1 PUBLICATION   0 CITATIONS    106 PUBLICATIONS   2,668 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Ralf Schwarzer Kimberley Norris


Freie Universität Berlin University of Tasmania
413 PUBLICATIONS   23,056 CITATIONS    25 PUBLICATIONS   84 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Academic achievement behaviours View project

Adaptation and resilience in extreme environments View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Kimberley Norris on 04 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

I believe, therefore I achieve (and vice versa): A meta-analytic cross-lagged T


panel analysis of self-efficacy and academic performance

Kate Talsmaa, , Benjamin Schüza,b, Ralf Schwarzerc,d, Kimberley Norrisa
a
University of Tasmania, School of Medicine (Psychology), Tasmania, Australia
b
Universität Bremen, Department of Human and Health Sciences, Bremen, Germany
c
Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Psychology, Berlin, Germany
d
SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Clinical, Health, and Rehabilitation Psychology, Wroclaw, Poland

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Self-efficacy has long been viewed as an important determinant of academic performance. A counter-position is
Self-efficacy that self-efficacy is merely a reflection of past performance. Research in the area is limited by unidirectional
Academic performance designs which cannot address reciprocity or the comparative strength of directional effects. This systematic
Reciprocal effects review and meta-analysis considered both directions of the relationship simultaneously, pooling data from
Cross-lagged panel analysis
longitudinal studies measuring both academic self-efficacy and academic performance over two waves. Pooled
Meta-analysis
correlations (k = 11, N = 2688) were subjected to cross-lagged path analysis that provided support for a re-
ciprocal effects model. Performance had a net positive effect on subsequent self-efficacy (β = 0.205,
p < 0.001), significantly larger than the effect of self-efficacy on performance (β = 0.071, p < 0.001).
Moderator analyses indicated that reciprocity holds for adults, but not for children (in whom performance
uniquely impacts subsequent self-efficacy beliefs, but not the reverse). Cross-lagged effects were stronger in
studies which used methodologies consistent with recommendations of self-efficacy theorists.

1. Introduction 1.1. Self-efficacy → academic performance (I believe; therefore I achieve)

The non-intellective antecedents of student performance are of A vast body of research has explored the idea that self-efficacy is the
great interest to educators and education researchers (Robbins et al., antecedent in the relationship and exerts a positive motivational in-
2004; Stankov & Lee, 2014), and research in this area is an important fluence on performance (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; Vancouver,
determinant of education policy (Bong, 2012; Pajares & Usher, Thompson, & Williams, 2001). Such research takes its lead from early
2008). One construct which has received a great deal of research studies by Bandura and colleagues (see Pajares, 1997; Zimmerman,
attention is perceived self-efficacy – a core dimension of human 2000 for review), which demonstrated that self-efficacy influenced
agency widely believed to be positively related to academic success subsequent behaviour. Self-efficacy → performance research also draws
(Bandura, 1977, 1997; Bong, 2012; Pajares & Schunk, 2001). Self- on the definition of self-efficacy as a future-oriented, predictive con-
efficacy refers to an individual's perception of their own capability to struct: measures of self-efficacy involve statements of confidence in
organise and execute required courses of action to achieve particular ability to achieve a future performance goal (Bong, 2012). From an
outcomes (Bandura, 1977, 1997). Self-efficacy is believed to enhance applied perspective, research on this relationship draws impetus from
performance through a range of mechanisms: individuals with high (and feeds into) educational settings, in which interventions are sought
levels of self-efficacy set more difficult goals, expend more effort, to improve performance.
persist for longer with challenges, and show resilience in the face of In a review of self-efficacy → performance research conducted over
adversity (Klassen & Usher, 2010). These achievements in turn are the past three decades, Klassen and Usher (2010) describe self-efficacy
assumed to increase self-efficacy, which results in a self-fulfilling as a crucial and powerful influence on academic performance, ac-
prophecy process (Bandura, 1977, 1997). counting for approximately a quarter of the variance in outcomes. Self-


Corresponding author at: University of Tasmania, School of Medicine, Psychology, Private Bag 30, Hobart, TAS 7001, Australia.
E-mail address: kate.talsma@utas.edu.au (K. Talsma).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.015
Received 6 April 2017; Received in revised form 9 October 2017; Accepted 22 November 2017
1041-6080/ © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

efficacy is argued to rival previous performance and mental ability in its was incremental to that of socioeconomic status, standardised
power to predict academic performance (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995). achievement measures, and high school GPA (β = 0.20, k = 18).
Meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies consistently point to self-effi- The research summarised above provides evidence of both a self-
cacy as one of the strongest correlates of academic performance. efficacy → performance relationship and a performance → self-efficacy
Richardson, Abraham, and Bond (2012) found that self-efficacy was the relationship; it also suggests that self-efficacy has an effect on sub-
strongest correlate of tertiary GPA (ρ = 0.59), exceeding high school sequent performance incremental to that of previous performance
GPA (ρ = 0.41), scholastic aptitude tests (ρ = 0.31–0.33), and in- alone. While this longitudinal research extends considerably on pre-
telligence (ρ = 0.21). Similar relationships are reported by Multon, vious cross-sectional findings, it does not address the possibility that
Brown, and Lent (1991) and Honicke and Broadbent (2016). self-efficacy and performance are reciprocally related – nor does it
Longitudinal research in the self-efficacy → academic performance elucidate the relative strength of directional effects.
paradigm is comparatively sparse (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016).
Nonetheless, findings are consistent across school-aged and tertiary 1.3. A chicken-and-egg conundrum
samples, in that self-efficacy positively predicts subsequent academic
performance, over periods ranging from a single semester, to courses The question of the direction of causality in the relationship be-
over several years (rs = 0.37–0.52; Chiang & Lin, 2014; Garriott & tween self-beliefs and academic performance has been described as one
Flores, 2013; Majer, 2009; Parker, Marsh, Ciarrochi, Marshall, & of “thorniest issues” in this area (Pajares & Schunk, 2001). According to
Abduljabbar, 2014; Phan & Ngu, 2016). social cognitive theory, self-efficacy exists in a framework of reciprocal
Based on such findings, direct manipulation of self-efficacy has long determinism: behaviour both shapes, and is shaped by a range of in-
been recommended as an intervention strategy in learning settings teracting factors (Bandura, 1977, 1997). In this model, self-efficacy and
(e.g., Bong, 2012; Pajares & Usher, 2008; Zimmerman & Bandura, performance modify each other iteratively within a constant feedback
1995). Given the practical implications for educational reforms and loop (Multon et al., 1991). In educational settings, learners reflect on
interventions, as well as the implications for theory and research, it is their performance and use this information when formulating their self-
important to be confident that the relationship between self-efficacy efficacy beliefs, which then influence subsequent performance (Phan,
and academic performance is being interpreted accurately (Valentine, 2012).
DuBois, & Cooper, 2004). Several recent studies demonstrate increased interest in the mutual
influences of self-efficacy and academic performance over time. In
1.2. Academic performance → self-efficacy (I achieve; therefore I believe) longitudinal studies of both high school and university students in
which multiple measurements of self-efficacy and performance are
As the bulk of research on the relationship between self-efficacy and staggered over several years, self-efficacy and performance predict each
performance is cross-sectional, inferences about the direction of influ- other, either in a self-efficacy → performance → self-efficacy pattern, or
ence are impossible (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; Pajares & Usher, in a performance → self-efficacy → performance pattern (e.g., Caprara,
2008). Some researchers argue that cross-sectional relationships reflect Vecchione, Alessandri, Gerbino, & Barbaranelli, 2011; Hwang, Choi,
the influence of performance on subsequent self-efficacy, not the re- Lee, Culver, & Hutchison, 2016). These types of designs provide evi-
verse (Shea & Howell, 2000; Vancouver et al., 2001). Heggestad and dence of positive mutual temporal effects, but by staggering data col-
Kanfer (2005) argue that self-efficacy is simply a proxy for past per- lection over time they do not enable the modelling of simultaneous
formance with no unique predictive power. reciprocal effects (Rogosa, 1988).
There is little doubt that performance outcomes influence self-effi- Thus, while reciprocal determinism between academic self-efficacy
cacy. Previous mathematics performance positively predicts mathe- and academic performance may be considered a fait accompli
matics self-efficacy in both school-aged and tertiary samples (e.g., (Lindsley, Brass, & Thomas, 1995), there is little direct empirical evi-
Klassen, 2004; Matsui, Matsui, & Ohnishi, 1990). Chin and Kameoka dence which supports this proposition (Williams & Williams, 2010).
(2002) reported that reading scores predicted the educational self-ef- This gap in the literature is likely due, in part, to the paucity of long-
ficacy of high school students (β = 0.32) after accounting for a range of itudinal studies in the area, a problem which is compounded because
demographic and psychosocial predictors. In a recent study, both the two unidirectional research paradigms in this area are pursued
standardised test scores and first semester GPA correlated with self-ef- largely independently of each other (Shea & Howell, 2000). In the most
ficacy 12 months later, r = 0.30 (Lee, Flores, Navarro, & Kanagui- recent self-efficacy → performance meta-analysis, Honicke and
Munoz, 2015). Broadbent (2016) suggest a reciprocal relationship exists and re-
Such findings are not inconsistent with self-efficacy theory; in fact, commend that this be investigated directly.
investigations of the academic performance → self-efficacy relationship In the case of self-efficacy and performance, the issue of how to
often grow out of self-efficacy theory directly. Mastery experience (an assess the unique influence of one variable on the other has been
individual's experience of performance success) is one of four posited characterised by debate. Controlling for raw past performance (e.g.,
sources of self-efficacy beliefs, along with vicarious experience, verbal Feltz, 1982; Mitchell, Hopper, Daniels, George-Falvy, & James, 1994) is
social persuasion, and emotional physiological arousal (Bandura, argued to be an over-correction (Bandura & Locke, 2003), while re-
1997). Research shows that, of these four, mastery experience is the sidualising self-efficacy from past performance (Bandura & Locke,
strongest (if not the only) predictor of self-efficacy (Britner & Pajares, 2003) is argued to lead to statistically artefactual results (Heggestad &
2006; Lent, Lopez, & Bieschke, 1991; Usher & Pajares, 2008). Kanfer, 2005). In fact, as both past self-efficacy and past performance
While self-efficacy theorists do not discount the influence of per- are expected to be covarying common-cause variables (Bandura, 2012;
formance on self-efficacy, they refute the argument that self-efficacy Feltz, Chow, & Hepler, 2008; Heggestad & Kanfer, 2005; Vancouver
has no unique impact on performance (Bandura, 2012; cf. Heggestad & et al., 2001), any unidirectional approach will result in inflated esti-
Kanfer, 2005). Some researchers have attempted to rule out the hy- mates of the influence of these variables on each other (Brown et al.,
pothesis that self-efficacy provides no incremental prediction of per- 2008). An approach is required where both self-efficacy and perfor-
formance beyond that accounted for by previous performance, by meta- mance can be controlled at time 1 (Bandura, 2012; Feltz et al., 2008).
analysing the self-efficacy → performance relationship, controlling for One approach that may overcome these limitations is cross-lagged
previous performance. Valentine et al. (2004) used this approach, in- panel analysis (CLPA; see Fig. 1); a uniquely powerful approach to
terpreting the unique self-efficacy → performance effect (β = 0.10, chicken-and-egg questions (Singh & Tyagi, 2014) which has been
k = 9) as “small but noteworthy” (p. 127). Robbins et al. (2004) found gaining traction in the behavioral sciences literature (e.g., Riketta,
that the prediction of academic achievement by academic self-efficacy 2008). CLPA provides more robust evidence of potential reciprocal

137
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Fig. 1. Conceptual diagram of the cross-lagged path


model.

causality within dynamic relationships than other models, by meeting likely than children to use meta-cognitive strategies (Flavell, 1979) and
the requirement for temporal precedence, and demonstrating the un- reflect on self-constructs (Harter, 1999). Therefore, it is anticipated that
ique effect of variable X on variable Y, and vice versa, controlling for the modelled relationships will be stronger for adults than for children.
other model paths (Selig & Little, 2012).
1.5.2. Sex
1.4. Main aims of the present study Participant sex may also moderate the reciprocal relationship.
Theoretically, differences may grow out of self-fulfilling prophecies
This study aims to explore the reciprocity and comparative strength whereby females are socialised to view themselves as less capable than
of net directional effects in the self-efficacy/academic performance re- males (Williams & Williams, 2010). Characteristics of educational set-
lationship by meta-analysing longitudinal panel studies (two-variable, tings may be implicated in this process, with associated differences in
two-wave designs). Pooled correlations will be fit to a CLPA model competence perceptions and performance (Eccles & Blumenfeld, 1985).
(Fig. 1) comprising two auto-correlations/stability coefficients (βSE1SE2 Meta-analyses have reported sex differences in regard to academic
and βP1P2), two synchronous/cross-sectional correlations (βSE1P1 and performance (Richardson et al., 2012) and self-efficacy (Huang, 2013).
βSE2P2), and two cross-lagged correlations (βSE1P2 and βP1SE2). A re- However, Valentine et al. (2004) found that sex was not a moderator
ciprocal relationship is demonstrated by significant positive cross- when self-efficacy was used to predict academic performance. Given the
lagged paths; the stronger antecedent, if any, is marked by a larger lack of consistency in findings, this moderator analysis will be ex-
coefficient. ploratory in nature.
A further aim of the present review is to examine variables which
moderate the self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship. Moderator 1.5.3. Self-efficacy scale type
analyses are critical given the consistent finding of substantial hetero- The type of scale most appropriate for self-efficacy measurement is
geneity in the self-efficacy/performance relationship (Honicke & the subject of debate, and it is anticipated that scale differences will
Broadbent, 2016; Multon et al., 1991; Richardson et al., 2012; moderate findings. Bandura (2006) recommends the use of unipolar
Valentine et al., 2004). Further to accounting for heterogeneity, these scales, with participants providing ratings of their degree of confidence
analyses contribute to our understanding of the conditions under which to perform a task, from 0 to 100%. Unipolar scales are argued to be
self-efficacy and academic performance are most strongly related – more sensitive and reliable (Bandura, 1986). While the use of Likert
taking previous findings within a unidirectional self-efficacy → perfor- scales is criticised (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Lee & Bobko, 1994) it re-
mance paradigm and extending these to the reciprocal self-efficacy ↔ mains very common (see Table 2). Likert scales are argued to be in-
performance relationship framework. appropriate for a variable which does not have a positive/negative
In summary, the main aims of this study are: to conduct a meta- valence with a neutral midpoint (Bandura, 2012). Pajares, Hartley, and
analytic CLPA on studies presenting panel data regarding self-efficacy Valiante (2001) compared unipolar and Likert scales and found uni-
and academic performance, to test whether the data are consistent with polar measures to be more psychometrically sound. Thus, it is hy-
reciprocal effects, to explore the relative antecedent strength of the two pothesised that models based on studies using unipolar scales will de-
variables, and to test whether the relationship varies as a function of monstrate stronger cross-lagged paths than those using Likert scales.
participant- and study-level variables identified as potential moderators
in the literature. 1.5.4. Lag between measurement waves
Variation in lag time between panel measurements is also con-
1.5. Moderators of the self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship sidered as a moderating factor. In educational research, decisions about
measurement timing often lack a strong rationale, resting instead on
1.5.1. Age pragmatic considerations such as key academic dates (e.g., Phan,
Participant age has been found to lead to differences in self-efficacy 2012), or conventions for ease of interpretation (e.g., Valentine et al.,
levels and variations in the relationship between self-efficacy and per- 2004). However, shorter lags between measurement waves are likely to
formance (e.g., Davis-Kean et al., 2008). The earliest meta-analysis of be associated with stronger effects (Bandura, 1997; Gist & Mitchell,
the relationship between self-efficacy and academic performance 1992; Moriarty, 2014), potentially owing to intervening variables
(Multon et al., 1991) reported a stronger correlation for college stu- ‘washing out’ the relationship over time (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016;
dents (r = 0.35) than elementary students (r = 0.21). This difference Valentine et al., 2004). Recent research findings appear to be consistent
likely relates to different levels of cognitive maturity, with adults more with this position (Galyon, Blondin, Yaw, Nalls, & Williams, 2012; Gore

138
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Jr, 2006; Obrentz, 2012; Zusho, Pintrich, & Coppola, 2003). Thus, we to ensure adequate construct validity and obtain a sample of more
hypothesise that shorter lags will be associated with stronger effects homogeneous studies that will allow testing the proposed relationships
than longer lags. with more reliability than previous studies.
A better understanding of the complexities of the relationship be-
1.5.5. Specificity of measurement/match between specificity of self-efficacy tween self-efficacy and academic performance will provide valuable
and performance input into theory development and research design, and will also help
Measurement issues regarding the degree of specificity of the self- to inform education policy and teacher training programs.
efficacy measure and the degree of match between the self-efficacy and
performance measures have been discussed for many years (Bandura, 2. Method
1986). Further, these issues have been demonstrated to be critical to the
accuracy of prediction of behaviour (Pajares & Usher, 2008). Domain- This systematic review and meta-analysis was undertaken in ac-
based self-efficacy measures reflect confidence in one's ability to per- cordance with the PRISMA guidelines (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, &
form academically in a subject or particular course; e.g., I'm sure I can do Altman, 2009).
even the hardest work in my math class (Lewis et al., 2012). Domain-based
self-efficacy can also be measured by asking individuals to rate their 2.1. Inclusion criteria
levels of confidence in achieving a range of grades on a subject-specific
course (e.g., Beck & Schmidt, 2013). Task-based self-efficacy measures 2.1.1. Operationalisation of self-efficacy
reflect confidence in one's ability to perform particular academic tasks; There is evidence that measurement issues pose a genuine threat to
often, this will be answering specific questions correctly (e.g., Bonne & the validity of research in this area (Bandura, 2012; Klassen & Usher,
Johnston, 2016) or attaining a particular grade on an isolated perfor- 2010). Previous reviews report that differences in the operationalisa-
mance task such as an exam (e.g., Mone, Baker, & Jeffries, 1995). It is tion of both self-efficacy and performance account for heterogeneity
generally reported that more specific self-efficacy measures are more across studies (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; Multon et al., 1991;
strongly correlated with performance than less specific measures, and Richardson et al., 2012; Valentine et al., 2004). Self-efficacy is often not
more closely matched measures of self-efficacy and performance are appropriately measured, nor differentiated from other constructs such
more strongly correlated with each other (Bong, 2001; Pajares & Miller, as self-esteem and self-concept (Bandura, 2012; Klassen & Usher, 2010;
1995; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1995). In addition, reviewers report that Lee & Bobko, 1994; Pajares, 1996). This problem is compounded when
measurement specificity (Multon et al., 1991) and match (Valentine reviews cast a wide net, because very broad definitions of variables may
et al., 2004) moderate the unidirectional relationship between self-ef- result in the conglomeration of distinct constructs (Bandura, 2012). To
ficacy and performance in meta-analyses. Thus, it is hypothesised that address these issues, it has been recommended that researchers use
models based on studies using more specific self-efficacy measures, and more narrowly defined and theoretically grounded measures – which
matched specificity of self-efficacy and performance, will demonstrate we will apply in this present review (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Robbins
stronger model paths than those with less specific measures and non- et al., 2004; Valentine et al., 2004). We include studies when scales
matched specificity. reflect the three key components of self-efficacy: a personal judgement
of ability (internal attribution) to prospectively (future orientation)
1.5.6. Cohort effects perform academically (a behaviour, as opposed to an attitude or per-
We note that, as no restrictions were put in place in terms of date of sonal characteristic) (Bandura, 2006; Schwarzer & McAuley, 2016).
article publication, it is possible that cohort effects may be found in the Global/generalised/“trait” measures will not be included, owing to
sample. Broad changes in educational policies and assessment practices theoretical concerns about their applicability and their limited power to
over time may affect the self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship predict academic performance (Bandura, 2012; Klassen & Usher, 2010;
(Goldstein, 1983). An exploratory moderator analysis using time since Pajares, 1997). Furthermore, this review does not include efficacy for
publication as a variable will be undertaken to test this possibility. self-regulated learning, e.g., How well can you arrange a place to study
without distractions? (Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons, 1992). In
1.6. Summary sum, we seek measures which reflect a can do judgement for future
academic performance (Table 1).
By using meta-analytic cross-lagged panel analysis, this systematic
review addresses the chicken-and-egg question of self-efficacy and 2.1.2. Operationalisation of academic performance
performance, bringing together two avenues of enquiry which have so Previous reviews have also suffered from inconsistent oper-
far been largely isolated (self-efficacy → performance versus perfor- ationalisations of academic performance (Robbins et al., 2004). Multon
mance → self-efficacy). To our knowledge, a meta-analytic cross-lagged et al. (1991) and Honicke and Broadbent (2016) used a wide range of
panel analysis is yet to be conducted with the constructs of self-efficacy performance metrics in their reviews, and both reported that perfor-
and academic performance.1 The present review builds on others (e.g., mance specification moderated the relationships found. A more precise
Richardson et al., 2012; Valentine et al., 2004) in several important definition of academic performance would contribute to the validity of
ways. Firstly, the present analysis assesses the net effect of self-efficacy analyses. While GPA is a readily accessible measure of academic per-
on performance while holding all CLPA model paths constant, rather formance, aggregation potentially blurs performance across subject
than just the effect of previous performance. Secondly, this is the first areas, task types, and – most importantly for the present analysis –
analysis, to our knowledge, of the net effect of academic performance across time. To provide measurement precision, we define academic
on subsequent academic self-efficacy, accounting for other model paths performance as objective scores on individual performance tasks, such
including initial levels of self-efficacy. Finally, our approach applies as tests or exams. Such a definition is also consistent with the re-
stricter inclusion and exclusion criteria than previous reviews in order commendations of self-efficacy theorists. With these points in mind,
studies were included if they:

1
It should be noted that, in a meta-analysis of cross-lagged panel studies of the broader 1. Presented data regarding academic self-efficacy, measured in ac-
self-efficacy/performance relationship by Sitzmann and Yeo (2013), a subset of studies
cordance with self-efficacy theory, at any level of specificity (see
were related to self-efficacy and academic performance. However, the methodological
rigour of this study and its overall contribution to the literature has been questioned
Table 1).
(Bandura, 2015). For example, it has been noted that some included studies did not 2. Collected data regarding academic performance on a specific occa-
measure one of the two key constructs (Bandura, 2015). sion.

139
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Table 1
Examples of self-efficacy measures: specificity and inclusion/exclusion.

Excluded Included - task Included - domain Included - academic

“I am confident of my own Confidence ratings for ability to achieve a “I'm sure I can do even the hardest work in my “I'm confident I can master the courses I'm
decision.” (Hwang et al., range of grades on an impending exam. math class.” (Lewis et al., 2012) taking this semester.” (Bong, 2001)
2016) (Mone et al., 1995)
“I am a fast learner” Confidence ratings for ability to correctly “I can explain the facts, concepts and “I'm certain I can figure out how to do the
(Richardson et al., 2012) answer specific questions on an impending arguments covered in this course to others in most difficult class work.” (Hornstra, Van Der
exam (Bonne & Johnston, 2016) my own words” (Galyon et al., 2012) Veen, Peetsma, & Volman, 2013)
“I am good at maths.” (Scott, Confidence ratings for ability to perform Confidence ratings for achieving a range of “How well can you pass all of your subjects in
2000) particular writing tasks (Meier, McCarthy, & course grades (Beck & Schmidt, 2013) school?” (Galla et al., 2014)
Schmeck, 1984)

3. Collected data on self-efficacy and performance using a panel design inclusion criteria. If there was any uncertainty as to whether studies
in accordance with the path model shown in Fig. 1, providing a set met inclusion criteria, authors were contacted.
of six correlations. Twenty-three records were excluded because the required data were
4. Included participants in traditional primary, secondary and tertiary not available after multiple attempts to contact the authors (about half
educational settings. of these were dissertations). The vast majority of remaining exclusions
5. No interventions were conducted between the measurement waves, were made because studies did not measure self-efficacy or perfor-
as interventions may obscure the effects of self-efficacy and aca- mance (either at all, or in accordance with the specifications above) or
demic performance on each other. Control group data were eligible because studies did not collect data in a panel design. Some studies
for inclusion if other inclusion criteria were met. superficially appeared to be panel designs (from their use of terms such
as “time 1” and “time 2” or “wave 1” and “wave 2”), but on consulta-
We note that, in a single measurement wave, some researchers tion with authors, the variables in a given wave were actually measured
measure self-efficacy immediately prior to performance (e.g., Galyon some months apart. Three studies measured self-efficacy subsequent to
et al., 2012), while some measure self-efficacy in the days or weeks performance at each measurement wave; these were excluded as this
before the performance measurement (e.g., Richard, Diefendorff, & approach is not consistent with recommendations of self-efficacy the-
Martin, 2006). Often, this is for practical reasons: to increase partici- orists to measure self-efficacy prior to performance (Moriarty, 2014).
pation (Beck & Schmidt, 2013), or to reduce effects of exposure to ac- Ultimately, 11 studies were identified which met the inclusion cri-
tual test questions in a self-efficacy measure (Bonne & Johnston, 2016). teria.
Research indicates that there is little difference in the prediction of
subsequent performance whether self-efficacy is measured immediately 2.3. Data extraction
prior or up to two weeks prior to performance (Beck & Schmidt, 2013;
Mone et al., 1995). Thus, studies in which self-efficacy is measured The first author used coding sheets to extract the required data from
within two weeks of performance were included, with degrees of con- included studies. Correlation matrices were taken from articles where
currency analysed as a risk of bias (Section 2.6). available (k = 4), or obtained from authors (k = 7). In order to ensure
independence of data points, and to avoid difficulties interpreting three
2.2. Literature search or more waves of data in panel analysis (Rogosa, 1988), each study
provided a single set of six correlations as per Fig. 1. Several studies
Electronic searches were conducted in Scopus, PsycINFO, the reported additional measurement waves; in order to fit data to the CLPA
Education Resources Information Centre (ERIC) and Web of Science. model, only the first two waves were included. Several studies mea-
Three search fields covered terms relating to self-efficacy, academic sured self-efficacy at multiple levels of specificity, in which case, the
performance (e.g., school, education, test), and methodological terms measure which most closely matched the performance measure was
(e.g., panel, longitudinal, lag). Full search parameters are available as used, as such a match is believed to result in the greatest predictive
an online Supplementary file. In order to reduce the risk of publication power (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Pajares, 1997). Coding also covered
bias, unpublished papers such as dissertations and theses were eligible sample size, publication status, study location, participant variables and
for inclusion (Smith, 1980). In addition, there were no restrictions on study characteristics relating to moderator analyses (Section 2.5) and
year of publication or language, as long as abstracts were available in factors potentially associated with risk of bias (Section 2.6). A subset of
English. included studies was coded by KN and inter-rater agreement was 100%,
A flow chart of the search is available as on online Supplementary owing to the transparency of coding categories.
file. The final search was conducted in April 2016. In total, 5487 studies
were located in the database search, 3177 remained with duplicates 2.4. Overall analysis
removed. After screening titles and abstracts for relevance, 197 articles
remained. We manually searched the reference lists of known extant Using Metafor for R (Viechtbauer, 2010), six pooled correlations
meta-analyses which included analyses of the relationship between self- were calculated from the included studies' zero-order correlations. The
efficacy and academic performance. Where articles provided summaries raw correlations were subjected to a Fisher's z transformation, and
of included studies (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; Robbins et al., 2004; random effects modelling was used as the included studies are believed
Sitzmann & Yeo, 2013; Valentine et al., 2004), these were used to to represent a distribution of true effects (Borenstein, Hedges, &
identify studies which measured both self-efficacy and performance and Rothstein, 2007). Restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used
had a longitudinal design, with 36 studies identified for potential in- to ensure accurate estimation of variance (Thompson & Sharp, 1999),
clusion. In other cases (Multon et al., 1991; Richardson et al., 2012), all and study weighting was based on the inverse of study-specific var-
titles of included studies were screened for relevance (n = 252) and a iance, giving more weight to studies with greater precision
further 114 potentially relevant studies were identified. Full texts of (Viechtbauer, 2010). Analyses were conducted on the zero-order cor-
these 347 articles were obtained and screened according to the relations using p-curve 4.05 (Simonsohn, Nelson, & Simmons, 2015) to

140
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

calculate the underlying statistical power for the estimation of each of scale was considered to be psychometrically less sound than a unipolar
the six pooled correlations. The matrix of pooled correlations resulting scale (Bandura, 2012), this study was grouped with the Likert scales.
from the above analysis served as input for the CLPA (Fig. 1) using
MPLUS 7 (Muthen & Muthen, 1998–2012). 2.5.1.3. Specificity/match. Self-efficacy was coded as either domain-
based or task-based. In the included studies, all measures of
2.5. Moderator analyses performance were incidentally task-based (e.g., exams, tests). This
rendered coding for both self-efficacy specificity and match between
The overall approach to moderator analyses was to test whether any self-efficacy and performance measures unnecessary, because these
of the six pooled correlations forming the basis of the CLPA varied as a groups overlapped completely. That is, because performance was
function of the identified moderator variables. For moderators identi- task-based, studies with task-based self-efficacy were characterised
fied as significant, separate path models for subgroups of the moderator both as being matched to performance and as having a more specific
variable were estimated using the same procedure as for the overall level of self-efficacy, whereas studies with domain-based self-efficacy
analysis (Section 2.4). Subsequent to the estimation of the separate path measures were characterised both as being unmatched to performance
models for moderator subgroups, the cross-lagged path coefficients for and as having a less specific level of self-efficacy. Therefore, only one
contrasting subgroups were tested for significant differences using the moderator analysis was required, and for ease of interpretation, we
pooled standard deviation method (Kock, 2014). refer hereafter to the moderator categories as “matched” or “non-
Tests of the effects of proposed moderators were undertaken using matched”.
meta-regression analyses in R using the metafor package (Viechtbauer,
2010). Within the framework of random-effects meta-analysis, meta- 2.6. Assessment of risk of bias
regression is an analysis of the relationship between study-level vari-
ables and effect size – analogous to multiple regression in primary Risk of bias was assessed taking into account the reliability of self-
studies assessing the relationship between subject-level variables and efficacy and performance measurements, the concurrency issue men-
an outcome (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). As a tioned in Section 2.1, publication/selective reporting bias, and author
significant moderator effect for any individual pooled correlation could bias. We note that some potential sources of bias (e.g., atheoretical or
result in changes to the overall meta-analytic path analysis, moderator imprecise operationalisations of constructs) are minimised through the
analyses were conducted for any moderator that showed significant Q application of the specific inclusion criteria outlined in Section 2.1.
statistics for any of the six individual model paths. R2 values, which
indicate the amount of variance in the pooled correlations accounted 2.6.1. Reliability of self-efficacy measurement
for by the moderator variable, were also calculated. All reported Cronbach's alpha reliability coefficients for self-efficacy
measures were acceptable at > 0.70, with all but one exceeding
2.5.1. Moderator specification α = 0.83 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Nonetheless, self-efficacy re-
Continuous values for moderators were available for sex (proportion liability was analysed as a potential continuous moderator of the six
of males; k = 9), cohort (years since publication; the specific timing of pooled correlations, using the same meta-regression approach as was
data collection was not published in most cases), and time lag between taken with the moderators (Section 2.5). None of the model paths were
waves (in weeks). Categorical moderator variables were necessary in significantly moderated by reliability (all Q statistics, p > 0.05);
several cases as follows. therefore, reliability of the self-efficacy measure is not deemed to pre-
sent a risk of bias in the present study.
2.5.1.1. Age. Average participant age was not specified in more than
half of the studies included in this review, so a categorical moderator 2.6.2. Reliability of performance measurement
(adults/children) was constructed using age data, where available, or Only three studies provided reliability information regarding per-
participant education level information (e.g., grade 5 versus tertiary formance; therefore, a moderator analysis was not conducted. All per-
education). In this way, all studies could be included in the moderator formance measures were taken from academic records and are there-
analysis. fore deemed to be at low risk of bias.

2.5.1.2. Scale. Unipolar scales were compared with Likert scales 2.6.3. Concurrency of measurement
categorically. In one case, the scale could not be identified as either As mentioned above, studies were defined as meeting the require-
unipolar or Likert (Vancouver & Kendall, 2006). As this single-item ments for panel measurement when self-efficacy and performance were

Table 2
Features of included studies including zero-order correlations and moderators.

Study n Age %m SE α CC Lag Match Scale SE1P1 SE1P2 SE2P1 SE2P2 SE1SE2 P1P2

Beck et al., 2013 69 A ns 0.94 No S No U 0.333 0.292 0.488 0.313 0.736 0.697
Bonne et al., 2016 50 C 36 ns No L Yes L 0.570 ns 0.370 0.310 0.300 0.390
Daniels et al., 2010 111 A ns 0.83 Yes S No U 0.201 0.229 0.429 0.219 0.676 0.541
Finney et al., 2003 103 21 32 0.91 Yes L Yes L 0.480 0.224 0.404 0.441 0.314 0.340
Galyon et al., 2012 213 A 26 0.92 Yes S Yes L 0.079 0.026 0.239 0.213 0.547 0.570
Lewis et al., 2012 1456 C 47 0.84 No L No L 0.318 0.272 0.285 0.353 0.399 0.680
Meier et al., 1984 71 A 57 0.85 Yes L Yes U 0.361 0.370 0.210 0.090 0.690 0.460
Mone, 1994 252 21.6 59 0.87 Yes S Yes U 0.310 0.290 0.480 0.320 0.670 0.490
Mone et al., 1995 215 A 53 0.85 No S Yes U 0.320 0.220 0.450 0.240 0.650 0.530
Richard et al., 2006 83 19.9 21 0.83 No S No L 0.340 0.340 0.480 0.390 0.760 0.630
Vancouver et al., 2006 65 21 21 0.83 No S Yes O 0.108 0.253 0.405 0.319 0.630 0.189

Note: mean age is provided where reported, otherwise, age group (adults = A, children = C) is provided based on sample information. SE α = Cronbach's alpha for self-efficacy; %
m = proportion males; CC = concurrency; L = long, S = Short; U = unipolar, L = Likert, O = Other; ns = not specified; SE1P1 = self-efficacy time 1 with performance time 1, etc.

141
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Table 3
Pooled correlations and tests of heterogeneity, publication bias and observed power: overall analysis.

k n SE1P1 SE1P2 SE2P1 SE2P2 SE1SE2 P1P2

Pooled correlation (SE) 11 2688 0.316⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.248⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.402⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.312⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.689⁎⁎⁎ (0.07) 0.583⁎⁎⁎ (0.06)
95% CI 0.231, 0.401 0.182, 0.313 0.328, 0.477 0.255, 0.369 0.551, 0.827 0.468, 0.697
I2 68.15 45.32 58.61 30.95 88.89 83.42
Q 26.007⁎⁎ 14.777 26.5777⁎⁎ 14.1855 191.9659⁎⁎⁎ 71.3827⁎⁎⁎
Failsafe N 749 431 1144 735 3144 2979
p-Curve, full (Z) − 9.56⁎⁎⁎ − 6.02⁎⁎⁎ − 13.31⁎⁎⁎ − 7.98⁎⁎⁎ − 19.99⁎⁎⁎ − 17.58⁎⁎⁎
p-Curve, half (Z) − 9.63⁎⁎⁎ − 5.26⁎⁎⁎ − 12.65⁎⁎⁎ − 7.28⁎⁎⁎ − 20.68⁎⁎⁎ − 17.08⁎⁎⁎
Observed power [90% CI] 99% [98%, 99%] 94% [77%, 99%] 99% [99%, 99%] 91% [91%, 99%] 99% [99%, 99%] 99% [99%, 99%]

Note: correlations are Fisher's Z transformations. SE1P1 = self-efficacy at time 1 with performance at time 1; SE1P2 = self-efficacy at time 1 with performance at time 2; SE2P1 = self-
efficacy at time 2 with performance at time 1; SE2P2 = self-efficacy at time 2 with performance at time 2; SE1SE2 = self-efficacy at time 1 with self-efficacy at time 2;
P1P2 = performance at time 1 with performance at time 2.
⁎⁎
p < 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

measured within two weeks of each other at any given measurement 2015).2 Observed power estimates, ranging from 91% to 99%, are also
wave. In order to assess the validity of this approach to inclusion, a reported in Table 3. The I2 and Q homogeneity test values suggest
categorical moderator variable was coded such that those studies in sampling error alone is unlikely to account for the differences in effect
which all measures were taken in the same sitting were compared with sizes. Forest and funnel plots are available as online Supplementary
all other studies. This variable did not significantly moderate any of the material. Overall, the pooled correlations are consistent with previous
pooled correlations that served as input for the path model (all Q sta- findings, with moderate-to-strong positive auto-correlations, and small-
tistics, p > 0.05). Therefore, differences in concurrency of measure- to-moderate positive synchronous and cross-lagged correlations.
ment are not deemed to present a risk of bias in the current study. Fig. 2 displays the meta-analytic path model for the overall analysis,
estimated from the pooled correlations in Table 3. All of the paths are
2.6.4. Selective publication/reporting bias positive and statistically significant, p < 0.001. Focusing on the cross-
We screened both published and unpublished papers in the litera- lagged paths, the performance → self-efficacy effect was β = 0.205,
ture search process, and conducted both traditional (Failsafe N) and whereas the self-efficacy → performance effect was β = 0.071. These
contemporary (p-curve) analyses of the risk of selective publication/ estimates are based on a fully saturated model. A model was also tested
reporting bias. Results (see Section 3) suggest minimal risk of bias based in which the cross-lagged paths were constrained to be equal, indicating
on selective publication/reporting. an equivalent reciprocal relationship. A Chi-squared test of the con-
strained model was interpreted as a Chi-squared difference test, com-
2.6.5. Author bias paring whether the model constraining the cross-lagged paths to be
Inclusion criteria and coding procedures were transparent and un- equal resulted in a loss of fit compared to the model allowing them to
ambiguous, resulting in 100% inter-rater agreement on coding in a vary: if this test was significant, the constrained model provided a
subset of studies; thus, the current meta-analysis is deemed to be at low significantly poorer fit than the saturated model (Menard, 2007). In this
risk of author bias. case, the constrained model provided a significantly poorer fit to the
data, χ2 = 37.47, p < 0.0001; thus, the cross-lagged paths are sig-
3. Results nificantly different from each other.
Overall, these results indicate reciprocal effects: self-efficacy un-
Included studies (k = 11) were published between 1984 and 2016. iquely influences subsequent performance, and vice versa. Performance
Study sample sizes ranged from 50 to 1456 (M = 244). Ten studies is a significantly stronger antecedent of self-efficacy than the reverse:
were conducted in North America, with one study conducted in New the performance → self-efficacy effect is almost three times the size of
Zealand. There were ten published articles and one conference paper. the self-efficacy → performance effect.
Ten studies comprised mostly white participants; one study comprised
mostly Hispanic participants. Six studies considered mathematical do- 3.2. Moderator analyses
mains, while the remainder considered management, writing, human
development, and psychology. Further details of included studies, in- The relationship between academic self-efficacy and academic
cluding zero-order correlations and moderator information, are in performance was significantly moderated by sample age, length of the
Table 2. time lag, the degree of match between the self-efficacy measure and the
performance measure, and the type of self-efficacy scale used. The
3.1. Overall analysis proportion of male participants and elapsed time since publication did
not moderate the relationship. Complete meta-regression analyses are
Table 3 shows the pooled estimates of the six correlations in the available in Supplementary material.
cross-lagged panel model, along with standard errors, and 95% con- Path analyses were conducted on eight separate pooled correlation
fidence intervals. Also in Table 3 are the results of tests of hetero- matrices, with two subgroups for each of the four significant moderator
geneity, selective publication/reporting bias, and observed power. The variables. For those variables which were already categorical, the
failsafe N values indicate that substantial numbers of additional null- subgroups as specified in Section 2.5.1 were used for the separate path
effect studies would be needed to increase the p-value for each path
to > 0.05 (Rosenthal, 1979). Data sets which are subject to selective
2
The p-curve user guide directs authors to present a p-curve disclosure table. Such a
reporting or p-hacking are likely to show a left-skewed p distribution.
table is most applicable to syntheses of varied experimental data. In the present case,
The full- and half- p-curve analyses all show right-skewed distributions, much of the information that would be presented in such a table is identical across studies
indicating that the data included in the calculation of each of the six (e.g., all designs and statistics are correlational). Other relevant information which would
model paths have evidential value (Simonsohn, Simmons, & Nelson, form part of the disclosure table (e.g., statistical values) is in Table 2.

142
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Fig. 2. Cross-lagged panel model: overall analysis.


Note: figures are standardised path coefficients. A model in which the cross-lagged paths were constrained to be equal provided a significantly poorer fit to the data (see Section 3.1).
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

Table 4
Pooled correlations: moderator analyses.

n SE1P1 SE1P2 SE2P1 SE2P2 SE1SE2 P1P2

⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎


Age group Adults 1182 r (SE) 0.290 (0.05) 0.240 (0.04) 0.427 (0.04) 0.288 (0.03) 0.758 (0.06) 0.562⁎⁎⁎ (0.06)
95% CI 0.196, 0.383 0.161, 0.320 0.345, 0.509 0.226, 0.351 0.631, 0.885 0.448, 0.676
Children 1506 r (SE) 0.456⁎⁎ (0.16) 0.279⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.296⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.367⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.419⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.645⁎⁎ (0.21)
95% CI 0.151, 0.761 0.228, 0.330 0.245, 0.347 0.317, 0.418 0.368, 0.470 0.239, 1.051
Lag Short 1008 r (SE) 0.249⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.229⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.448⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.282⁎⁎⁎ (0.03) 0.797⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.598⁎⁎⁎ (0.07)
95% CI 0.158, 0.341 0.137, 0.320 0.357, 0.539 0.220, 0.345 0.704, 0.889 0.470, 0.726
Long 1680 r (SE) 0.433⁎⁎⁎ (0.07) 0.281⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.301⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.333⁎⁎⁎ (0.07) 0.473⁎⁎⁎ (0.12) 0.544⁎⁎⁎ (0.12)
95% CI 0.295, 0.571 0.232, 0.329 0.253, 0.349 0.195, 0.470 0.241, 0.705 0.309, 0.778
Matched specificity Yes 969 r (SE) 0.328⁎⁎⁎ (0.07) 0.223⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.397⁎⁎⁎ (0.05) 0.285⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.643⁎⁎⁎ (0.08) 0.487⁎⁎⁎ (0.06)
95% CI 0.187, 0.469 0.120, 0.327 0.298, 0.495 0.210, 0.359 0.484, 0.802 0.377, 0.597
No 1719 r (SE) 0.323⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.281⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.420⁎⁎⁎ (0.07) 0.360⁎⁎⁎ (0.02) 0.779⁎⁎⁎ (0.14) 0.773⁎⁎⁎ (0.06)
95% CI 0.276, 0.371 0.233, 0.328 0.285, 0.554 0.312, 0.407 0.509, 1.05 0.664, 0.882
Scale Uni-polar 718 r (SE) 0.314⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.275⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.473⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.265⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.818⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.592⁎⁎⁎ (0.04)
95% CI 0.240, 0.388 0.201, 0.349 0.399, 0.547 0.191, 0.339 0.744, 0.892 0.514, 0.671
Likert 1970 r (SE) 0.328⁎⁎⁎ (0.09) 0.221⁎⁎⁎ (0.06) 0.346⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.349⁎⁎⁎ (0.04) 0.566⁎⁎⁎ (0.11) 0.546⁎⁎⁎ (0.10)
95% CI 0.160, 0.496 0.104, 0.338 0.261, 0.431 0.276, 0.422 0.358, 0.774 0.346, 0.747

Note: r = pooled correlations after Fisher's Z transformations; SE = self-efficacy; SE1P1 = self-efficacy at time 1 with performance at time 1; SE1P2 = self-efficacy at time 1 with
performance at time 2; SE2P1 = self-efficacy at time 2 with performance at time 1; SE2P2 = self-efficacy at time 2 with performance at time 2; SE1SE2 = self-efficacy at time 1 with self-
efficacy at time 2; P1P2 = performance at time 1 with performance at time 2.
⁎⁎
p < 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

analyses (i.e., adults and children, matched and non-matched specifi- Figs. 3 to 6 show the path models for the subgroups of moderator
city, and Likert and unipolar scales). Time lag had been analysed as a variables found to impact significantly on the relationship between self-
continuous moderator; thus, we formed short and long categories for efficacy and academic performance, estimated on the basis of the
comparison.3 pooled correlations in Table 4. These models were largely consistent in
Pooled correlations, standard errors and 95% confidence intervals that most paths were again positive and statistically significant. One
for the moderator categories are shown in Table 4, with a similar pat- key exception is that, for children, the βSE1P2 cross-lagged path was
tern of correlations to the overall model. Homogeneity test values, near-zero and did not reach statistical significance. In all cases, the
observed power, forest plots and funnel plots for pooled correlations for performance → self-efficacy paths were stronger than the corre-
moderator subgroups are available as online Supplementary material. sponding self-efficacy → performance paths.
As with the overall analyses, Chi-squared tests were conducted to
ascertain whether improved model-fit compared to saturated models
3
For this categorisation, we note that recent findings in the literature indicate that self-
was obtained with cross-lagged paths constrained to be equal. With the
efficacy is more strongly related to performance when measured up to 2 months prior to
the performance measure, with weaker relationships associated with longer lags (from exception of long time lags (p = 0.07), models in which the cross-
3 months upwards) (Gore Jr, 2006; Obrentz, 2012; Phan, 2012; Zusho et al., 2003). In the lagged paths were constrained to be equal provided a significantly
included data set, this lag range coincided with a natural division in lag times: 7 studies poorer fit to the data (p < 0.001). Full details of these tests are
had lag times of up to 7 weeks, and 4 studies had longer lags of 3 to 12 months, while no available as Supplementary material.
studies had a lag of between 7 weeks and 3 months. This categorisation therefore pro-
Table 5 shows the results of tests of differences between cross-
vided theoretically grounded as well as readily interpretable differentiation of studies
with a reasonable number of studies in each category. lagged path coefficients for moderator subgroups. The reciprocal

143
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

relationship was significantly stronger for adults than for children, for The effect sizes for performance → self-efficacy are also similar in
both of the cross-lagged paths. Studies with shorter lags showed magnitude. On first observation, it may be argued that these effects,
stronger reciprocal effects than those with longer delays between which, by convention are small-to-medium (Cohen, 1988), are unlikely
measurement waves; this difference was statistically significant for the to have far-reaching implications. However, academic achievement
βP1SE2 path. Reciprocal effects were stronger for studies in which the exists within a very intricate framework (Pajares, 2007), involving
specificity of the self-efficacy measure matched the performance mea- factors both internal to the student; e.g., personality, coping styles
sure, though the differences did not reach statistical significance. Stu- (Komarraju, Karau, Schmeck, & Avdic, 2011; MacCann, Fogarty,
dies with unipolar self-efficacy scales showed stronger reciprocal effects Zeidner, & Roberts, 2011) and external to the student, e.g., parental
than those with Likert scales; this difference was statistically significant engagement, student-teacher relationships (Dotterer & Lowe, 2011;
for the βP1SE2 path. Moreira, Dias, Vaz, & Vaz, 2013). In such a complex context, the con-
sistency and size of the present effects provide some evidence that they
4. Discussion are meaningful. The potency of the effect of self-efficacy on academic
performance may have been overstated in the literature because of
4.1. Overall analysis: I believe therefore I achieve, and vice versa reliance on unidirectional findings. However, in the context of re-
ciprocal effects, there is cause for optimism that interventions targeting
This study explored the chicken-and-egg conundrum in the re- either self-efficacy or performance will have flow-on effects to the other
lationship between academic self-efficacy and academic performance variable – and interventions which target both constructs will have
by means of a systematic review and meta-analytic CLPA. The meta- synergistic effects. Conversely, considering either self-efficacy or per-
analysis provided support for a reciprocal effects model: both self-effi- formance in a vacuum potentially means ignoring half of the equation.
cacy and performance had statistically significant, unique positive in-
fluences on each other over time (Fig. 2). 4.1.2. Research implications
The finding of a reciprocal self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship
4.1.1. Interpretation suggests that carefully timed measurement of both constructs will en-
The finding of reciprocal effects is consistent with the conception of hance research in this area. It is common in the literature to measure
reciprocal determinism in social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977, self-efficacy on multiple occasions followed by a single performance
1997; Pajares & Usher, 2008). Reciprocity aligns with notions of the measurement, or to measure self-efficacy once, followed by staggered
self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship as a feedback loop, cycle or measures of performance. Researchers also measure self-variables on
spiral (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Lindsley et al., 1995; Multon et al., 1991; multiple occasions and then average the measurements. Such ap-
Shea & Howell, 2000). The net performance → self-efficacy effect proaches are unlikely to provide accurate insights into how the two
highlights the important role of performance experience in the forma- variables relate over time. Of course, research must be designed ac-
tion of self-efficacy beliefs (Britner & Pajares, 2006; Usher & Pajares, cording to the specific research questions under consideration.
2008). The finding of a unique positive self-efficacy → performance Generally, however, researchers should consider reciprocity when at-
influence while controlling for the opposite pathway in the CLPA casts tempting to isolate temporally dynamic features of the relationship
doubt on the claim that self-efficacy is merely reflective of past per- (Bandura & Jourden, 1991).
formance (Bandura, 2012; Feltz et al., 2008; cf. Heggestad & Kanfer,
2005; Vancouver et al., 2001). Instead, the unique influence of self- 4.1.3. Practical implications
efficacy on performance highlights the generative or mobilising power The finding of a reciprocal self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship
of self-efficacy, which reflects what you do with what you've got (Gist & suggests that students would benefit when interventions to enhance
Mitchell, 1992). Consistent with this, a range of studies demonstrate self-efficacy are combined with regular opportunities to experience
that self-efficacy affects goals, effort, resourcefulness, persistence, use performance success. Approaches to enhancing academic self-efficacy
of cognitive strategies and choice of activities (see Bandura, 1997, are detailed elsewhere (Pajares, 2006; Siegle & McCoach, 2007). We
2012; Klassen & Usher, 2010; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Schunk, 2001). provide several examples which are likely to have synergistic effects
In gauging the practical significance of these results, we note that owing to their anticipated concurrent impact on self-efficacy and per-
the effect sizes for self-efficacy → performance are in the range pre- formance. Educators may observe that there is nothing novel here –
viously found by Valentine et al. (2004) and Richardson et al. (2012). what is suggested is that these approaches will be of most benefit when

Fig. 3. Cross-lagged panel models for adults/chil-


dren respectively.
Note: figures are standardised path coefficients. For
both adults and children, a model in which the
cross-lagged paths were constrained to be equal
did not provide a significantly poorer fit to the data
(see online Supplementary material).
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

144
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Fig. 4. Cross-lagged panel models for matched/non-


matched specificity respectively.
Note: figures are standardised path coefficients. For
both matched and non-matched specificity, a model in
which the cross-lagged paths were constrained to be
equal provided a significantly poorer fit to the data (see
online Supplementary material). ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001,
⁎⁎
p < 0.01, ⁎p < 0.05.

used in conjunction with each other in a cyclical fashion. explored with both contextual factors and the individual students in
Guided mastery, which exists at the junction between self-efficacy mind (Klassen & Usher, 2010).
and performance, involves assisting students to navigate through in-
cremental authentic performance experiences, with scaffolding from
teachers being reduced over time (Bong, 2012; Pajares & Schunk, 4.2. Moderator effects: When believing leads to achieving, and vice versa
2001). It is recommended that tasks are graded to individual learners so
that they are moderately challenging but accomplishable, and that 4.2.1. Age
teachers help students set incremental goals that are specific and As anticipated, the reciprocal relationship between self-efficacy and
proximal (Bong, 2012; Pajares, 2006). To provide evidence to students academic performance varied depending on participant age, with sig-
of their growing mastery, progress toward goals should be made ex- nificantly stronger cross lagged effects for adults than for children.
plicit, feedback should be timely and accurate, and success attributed to Importantly, the βSE1P2 cross-lagged path was not statistically sig-
internal, stable student characteristics/behaviors (Bong, 2012; Pajares, nificant for children: self-efficacy did not have a net effect on sub-
2006; Siegle & McCoach, 2007). sequent performance for children in this sample. That is, the relation-
An adaptive interpretation of performance experience may also ship between self-efficacy and academic performance appears to be
leverage reciprocal self-efficacy ↔ performance effects. Performance unidirectional for children, but reciprocal for adults. This suggests that,
should be framed in terms of progress rather than shortfalls, using in- in children, self-efficacy may be detached from the generative processes
ternal rather than comparative standards, and showing how the ex- generally ascribed to it, such as mobilisation of resources and strategies
perience can improve future performance (Bandura, 2012; Lindsley which lead to better performance, which are argued to exist in adults.
et al., 1995; Pajares, 2006). Where performance is interpreted as a This may be because the capacity to work with abstractions regarding
failure, attributing this to factors under the student's control (e.g., in- the self is cultivated as cognitive maturity develops (Harter, 1999). Self-
sufficient effort or ineffective strategy use) can provide a buffer for self- efficacy exists within a system of self-regulation, and individuals' self-
efficacy beliefs (Bonne & Johnston, 2016; Pajares, 2006). regulatory capacities are known to develop over time (Bandura, 1981;
It must be noted that there is no single guaranteed way to enhance Heckhausen & Dweck, 1998). Self-efficacy is influenced by a range of
self-efficacy beliefs or performance outcomes, and it has been suggested sources of information, in conjunction with a cognitive appraisal of that
that an assumption that it is always possible to generate lasting, information (Bandura, 1997). It follows that, with greater cognitive
adaptive changes to self-efficacy beliefs is neither accurate nor helpful maturity and more educational experience, adults generate different
(Gist & Mitchell, 1992; Klassen & Usher, 2010). Approaches should be and, in particular, more precise appraisals of previous performance
outcomes or future task difficulty, with subsequent differences in self-

Fig. 5. Cross-lagged panel models for unipolar/Likert


self-efficacy scale respectively.
Note: figures are standardised path coefficients. For
both unipolar and Likert scales, a model in which the
cross-lagged paths were constrained to be equal pro-
vided a significantly poorer fit to the data (see online
Supplementary material). ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001, ⁎⁎p < 0.01,

p < 0.05.

145
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Fig. 6. Cross-lagged panel models for short/long lag


respectively.
Note: figures are standardised path coefficients. For the
short lag, a model in which the cross-lagged paths were
constrained to be equal provided a significantly poorer
fit to the data (see online Supplementary material). For
the long lag, a model in which the cross-lagged paths
were constrained to be equal did not provide a sig-
nificantly poorer fit to the data (see online
Supplementary material). ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001, ⁎⁎p < 0.01,

p < 0.05.

efficacy levels (Phan, 2012; Phan & Ngu, 2016; Zimmerman, 1989). The model; it follows that there would be more intervening variables over a
differences between adults and children may also reflect a lack of ca- longer period of time, and also that these would impart a greater degree
libration between self-efficacy and performance for children, which is of influence over that period (Valentine et al., 2004). Considering the
also likely to develop with an accumulation of task experience and dearth of research regarding the impact of different time lags on the
feedback (Artino, 2012; Bandura, 2012). relationship between self-efficacy and performance, it may be that the
Other differences between adults and children in each of the paths longer time lags exceed the period during which the effects of interest
forming the overall model may also contribute; for example, self-effi- are at their maximum levels (Clegg, Jackson, & Wall, 1977). We note
cacy appears to be more consistent over time for adults, which is con- that the reciprocal influences of self-efficacy and performance did not
sistent with the notion that self-efficacy stabilises as individuals ap- differ significantly from each other in studies conducted over a longer
proach the middle life span (Oettingen & Gollwitzer, 2015). On the term. We speculate that over the course of repeated exposure to per-
other hand, performance is comparatively less stable over time for formance-feedback cycles, the dual influences of self-efficacy and per-
adults than children, likely due to the tendency for performance tasks to formance may converge to a point of equilibrium.
increase in difficulty more dramatically in adult educational settings Consistent with our hypothesis, effect sizes were larger when the
(Pokay & Blumenfeld, 1990). In the same vein, it is also possible that specificity of the self-efficacy and performance measures were matched
age functions as a proxy for the type of academic tasks undertaken, with than when they were not matched. This is consistent with both the
simpler and more defined tasks in childhood, and more complex and theoretical position and recommendation for a micro-analytic research
abstract tasks in adulthood. strategy (Bandura, 1977, 2012) and with empirical evidence in this
One may be tempted to ascribe the lack of a significant finding for regard (Bong, 2001; Pajares & Miller, 1995). Also as anticipated, effect
the βSE1P2 path to a lack of power to detect the effect, given that there sizes were larger for studies which used unipolar scales than for those
are only two studies comprising children. However, there are several which used Likert scales. This finding is consistent with commentary
indications that this is unlikely to be the case. When considering the above regarding the relative psychometric properties of different types
number of participants in each group, the sizes are roughly equal of scales when measuring self-efficacy (Bandura, 2006, 2012; Klassen &
(adults, n = 1182; children, n = 1506). In addition, the observed Usher, 2010).
power for the underlying pooled correlations is strong. Furthermore, we Overall, the moderator analyses found that measurement ap-
note that all other paths in the model comprising children are statisti- proaches congruent with those recommended by self-efficacy theorists
cally significant, suggesting that the lack of a significant βSE1P2 path is a resulted in stronger effects. While these differences did not consistently
valid result. reach statistical significance, the self-efficacy → performance effect
These findings suggest that educators of adults should consider sizes for those studies using theory-consistent approaches were con-
leveraging the synergies inherent in the self-efficacy ↔ performance sistently approximately twice the size of those found in other studies. It
relationship by using interventions which may enhance self-efficacy appears that criticisms of self-efficacy measurement are going unheard
directly, while also providing regular opportunities for students to by the research community, with negative ramifications for the quality
perform academic tasks successfully. Children may benefit from early of self-efficacy research (Klassen & Usher, 2010). Especially as long-
opportunities to learn about and engage in metacognitive functions itudinal research exploring self-efficacy continues to develop, with in-
(Nietfeld, Cao, & Osborne, 2006), and to increase their understanding creased need to detect more subtle changes over time, researchers are
of, and exposure to, performance-feedback cycles (Artino, 2012; Gist & urged to make well-informed decisions about the self-efficacy scales
Mitchell, 1992; Gore Jr, 2006). used (Bandura, 2006; Bong, 2006; Lee & Bobko, 1994).

4.2.2. Methodological moderators: lag, specificity match, self-efficacy scale 4.2.3. Sex and cohort effects
type In this data set, sex was not identified as a significant moderator.
Studies that used relatively short time lags between first and second While Huang (2013) found a gender difference in self-efficacy in a
assessments (7 weeks or less) showed stronger cross-lagged relation- meta-analysis, the effect size was very small (g = 0.08), and was cal-
ships than studies with longer lag times (3–12 months), as expected. culated from 247 samples which were found to be highly hetero-
This is consistent with Bandura's (1997) position regarding temporal geneous, with individual effects ranging from g = −1.60 to g = 1.40.
disparity, which suggests that self-efficacy should be a more accurate Furthermore, findings of sex differences in self-beliefs and achievement
predictor if assessed more proximally to behaviour. In addition, it is are not consistent across meta-analyses, with Hansford and Hattie
likely that a range of unmeasured variables influence the paths in the (1982) and Valentine et al. (2004) reporting no effect. It is possible that

146
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Table 5 Within self-efficacy theory, it is an individual's interpretation of their


Tests of differences between cross-lagged path coefficients for moderator subgroups. performance which constitutes mastery experience (Klassen & Usher,
2010; Usher & Pajares, 2008). A B grade is likely to be interpreted as a
Path t p
success for a C-average student, but as a failure for a straight-A student
Age group SE1P2 3.13 0.0018 (Pajares, 2006). By focusing on objective performance, the inter-
P1SE2 3.71 0.0004 pretative mechanism linking performance to self-efficacy may be ob-
Lag SE1P2 0.78 0.4200
scured in some cases.
P1SE2 4.33 < 0.0001
Match SE1P2 1.19 0.2327 With the search strategy we implemented, we were able to exclude
P1SE2 0.77 0.4435 studies which called a construct self-efficacy but measured it in a way
Scale SE1P2 1.37 0.1717 inconsistent with social cognitive theory, but we were not able to in-
P1SE2 1.77 0.0342 clude studies which measured a construct consistent with social cogni-
tive theory but called something else, rendering our search potentially
incomplete. However, an approach which would have ensured the in-
there was no such effect in the samples reviewed here, or that the
clusion of theoretically consistent but unlabelled self-efficacy studies
number of included studies provided insufficient power to detect a very
seems unfeasible, and researchers conducting studies within a social
small effect.
cognitive framework seem likely to use theory-consistent terminology.
Time since publication was not identified as a significant moderator,
The present review, being based on non-experimental data, pre-
suggesting that cohort effects are not responsible for the patterns of
cludes firm conclusions about causality. In this case, the concern is that
variance observed in the included data.
the model is not fully specified, owing to the abundance of factors af-
fecting human behaviour (Rogosa, 1988; Selig & Little, 2012). A finding
4.3. Limitations of reciprocal effects may point to a blurring of the underlying processes
which lead to self-efficacy and then to academic performance, or vice
Given the narrow inclusion criteria of this study, a small number of versa. Experiments conducted at a micro-analytic level are required
studies was uncovered. This may be considered a limitation of the when the aim is to detect causality, and further experimental research
present analyses. However, we note that no formal minimum number of in academic settings is recommended. That being said, it has been noted
studies exists in order for meta-analysis to be useful – results from as that self-beliefs do not readily lend themselves to experimental ma-
few as two studies can be fruitfully synthesised (Valentine, Pigott, & nipulation: successful manipulation is likely to concurrently modify
Rothstein, 2010). Meta-analysis may not be suitable for a small group of other, potentially unmeasured, mental processes (Valentine et al.,
studies showing wildly different effects. In this case, however, a range 2004). In this context, cross-lagged panel analysis is considered to be
of assessments of the nature of the included data provide support for the “an important tool for building an argument for the causal effect of one
validity of the present findings. For example, there are several indica- variable on another” (Selig & Little, 2012, p. 271). In an area where
tions that the correlation matrices used as input for the path analyses complete experimental control is unlikely to be achieved, these types of
are stable: consistent effects across studies are shown in forest plots analyses are of considerable value.
(online Supplementary material), failsafe N values are substantial
(Table 3), and there is strong underlying statistical power for the esti- 4.4. Directions for future research
mation of each of the model paths (Table 3). Furthermore, the lower
bounds of the 95% confidence intervals for the pooled correlations not While all included studies contained measures of both self-efficacy
only confirm the statistical significance of the effects, but also suggest and performance on two occasions, in most cases (k = 7), full corre-
that the smallest possible estimates of effects based on the included data lation matrices were not published and were sought from the authors.
would still be deemed meaningful in the social sciences (Cohen, 1988). Therefore, not only are there very few studies that collect panel data on
Nonetheless, the small number of studies does pose limitations in terms these constructs, there are even fewer that leverage this data to explore
of interpretation: in particular, when interpreting the moderator ana- reciprocity and the strength of directional effects. The lack of available
lysis regarding age, we note that the studies focusing on children con- information regarding SES of participants precluded a moderator ana-
sidered only the mathematical domain, and therefore we caution that lysis on that basis, and the lack of variability in terms of participant race
this finding may not generalise to other areas of study. and location meant that we were unable to undertake moderator ana-
Consistent with previous reports (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016; lyses for these variables. Most participants were white North-
Multon et al., 1991; Richardson et al., 2012; Robbins et al., 2004; Americans; this may affect the generalisability of our findings. These
Valentine et al., 2004), significant heterogeneity across studies was issues point to the need for further longitudinal research generally, and
observed in the present review (Table 3). However, the I2 value of 45% particularly with socially and culturally diverse participants.
for the self-efficacy → performance path for the SE-first studies (the Children were also under-represented in included studies. Given the
path that is comparable to previous unidirectional meta-analyses) contrasting findings for adults and children, the literature would be
compares favourably to the values of ~70–90% reported in the only enhanced by an understanding of how the self-efficacy ↔ performance
review which published comparable statistics (Richardson et al., 2012). relationship changes over the course of childhood and adolescence. It
By convention, an I2 of 45% is indicative of moderate heterogeneity; appears that the relationship may change from unidirectional to re-
values of 70–90% indicate substantial-to-considerable heterogeneity ciprocal over this period. In the present analysis, where a gross dis-
(Higgins & Green, 2008). Thus, we venture that the fine-grained ap- tinction had to be made between adults and children, it was not pos-
proach taken has resulted in a more homogeneous group of studies for sible to explore this.
analysis. Even so, we note that the strict methodological inclusion cri- We note that additional waves of data were available in some in-
teria left little room to restrict the sample further based on other con- cluded studies, but could not be incorporated into the current analyses.
textual factors, such as domain of study, and that included studies may Further meta-analytic research using change modelling approaches is
therefore vary in ways that were not addressed by our moderator warranted when sufficient primary studies become available.
analyses.
A further limitation is that the pooled correlations in the path 4.5. Conclusion
analyses are subject to measurement error, whereas the path analysis
assumes no such error (Selig & Little, 2012). However, given the sta- In this systematic review with meta-analytic cross-lagged panel
bility of the input data as described above, this issue is mitigated. analysis, a reciprocal relationship between self-efficacy and academic

147
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

performance was found, consistent with Bandura's view of self-efficacy cognitive predictors of college students' academic performance and persistence: A
within a framework of reciprocal determinism (Bandura, 1977, 1997). meta-analytic path analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72(3), 298–308. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2007.09.003.
This suggests that interventions designed to influence either or both Caprara, G. V., Vecchione, M., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2011). The
constructs may be fruitfully applied. To make a financial analogy, in- contribution of personality traits and self-efficacy beliefs to academic achievement: A
vestment in either self-efficacy or performance improvements is likely longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(1), 78–96.
Chiang, Y. T., & Lin, S. S. J. (2014). The measurement structure, stability and mediating
to yield compound interest. In developing curricula, combining direct effects of achievement goals in math with middle-school student data. Scandinavian
enhancement of self-efficacy with regular opportunities for perfor- Journal of Educational Research, 58(5), 513–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
mance success is likely to take best advantage of the identified re- 00313831.2012.732607.
Chin, D., & Kameoka, V. A. (2002). Psychosocial and contextual predictors of educational
ciprocal effects. and occupational self-efficacy among Hispanic inner-city adolescents. Hispanic
The self-efficacy ↔ performance relationship varied as a function of Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 24(4), 448–464.
participant age, self-efficacy scale type, measurement lag and match of Clegg, C., Jackson, P., & Wall, T. (1977). The potential of cross-lagged correlation analysis
in field research. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 50(3), 177–196.
specificity between measures. A reciprocal relationship emerged for
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ:
adults, but this did not hold for children. Across methodological mod- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
erators, those studies with approaches consistent with self-efficacy *Daniels, M. A., Kain, J. M., Gillespie, J. Z., & Schmidt, A. M. (2010). Latent profiles of goal
theory consistently showed stronger reciprocal effects. orientation and motivational outcomes. Paper presented at the 70th annual Academy of
Management meeting, Montreal, Canada.
In closing, we note that the effects reported here, while modest, are Davis-Kean, P. E., Huesmann, L. R., Jager, J., Collins, W. A., Bates, J. E., & Lansford, J. E.
embedded within a complex system involving various influences on (2008). Changes in the relation of self-efficacy beliefs and behaviors across devel-
self-efficacy and academic performance in isolation, as well as the opment. Child Development, 79(5), 1257–1269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2008.01187.x.
subtleties of their interrelation over time. A deeper understanding of Dotterer, A., & Lowe, K. (2011). Classroom context, school engagement, and academic
the reciprocal nature of the relationship between the two, and the achievement in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(12),
comparative strength of directional effects, is likely to be of value to 1649–1660. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9647-5.
Eccles, J. S., & Blumenfeld, P. (1985). Classroom experiences and student gender: Are
both researchers and educational practitioners. there differences and do they matter. Gender Influences in Classroom Interaction,
79–114.
Appendix A. Supplementary data Feltz, D. L. (1982). Path analysis of the causal elements in Bandura's theory of self-efficacy
and an anxiety-based model of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 42(4), 764–781.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https:// Feltz, D. L., Chow, G. M., & Hepler, T. J. (2008). Path analysis of self-efficacy and diving
doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.015. performance revisited. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30(3), 401.
*Finney, S. J., & Schraw, G. (2003). Self-efficacy beliefs in college statistics courses.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(2), 161–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
References4 S0361-476X(02)00015-2.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cogniti-
Artino, A. R. (2012). Academic self-efficacy: From educational theory to instructional ve–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906.
practice. Perspectives on Medical Education, 1(2), 76–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ Galla, B. M., Wood, J. J., Tsukayama, E., Har, K., Chiu, A. W., & Langer, D. A. (2014). A
s40037-012-0012-5. longitudinal multilevel model analysis of the within-person and between-person ef-
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. fect of effortful engagement and academic self-efficacy on academic performance.
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191. Journal of School Psychology, 52(3), 295–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2014.
Bandura, A. (1981). Self-referent thought: A developmental analysis of self-efficacy. Social 04.001.
Cognitive Development: Frontiers and Possible Futures, 200–239. *Galyon, C. E., Blondin, C. A., Yaw, J. S., Nalls, M. L., & Williams, R. L. (2012). The
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. relationship of academic self-efficacy to class participation and exam performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Social Psychology of Education, 15(2), 233–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Macmillan. 011-9175-x.
Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. Self-efficacy Beliefs of Garriott, P. O., & Flores, L. Y. (2013). The role of social cognitive factors in Mexican
Adolescents, 5, 307–337. American students' educational goals and performance: A longitudinal analysis.
Bandura, A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy revisited. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 1(2), 85–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032378.
Journal of Management, 38(1), 9–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206311410606. Gist, M. E., & Mitchell, T. R. (1992). Self-efficacy: A theoretical analysis of its determi-
Bandura, A. (2015). On deconstructing commentaries regarding alternative theories of nants and malleability. Academy of Management Review, 17(2), 183–211.
self-regulation. Journal of Management, 41(4), 1025–1044. Goldstein, H. (1983). Measuring changes in educational attainment over time: Problems
Bandura, A., & Jourden, F. J. (1991). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing the impact of and possibilities. Journal of Educational Measurement, 20(4), 369–377.
social comparison on complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Gore Jr, P. A. (2006). Academic self-efficacy as a predictor of college outcomes: Two
Psychology, 60(6), 941. incremental validity studies. Journal of Career Assessment, 14(1), 92–115. http://dx.
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. doi.org/10.1177/1069072705281367.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 87–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010. Hansford, B. C., & Hattie, J. A. (1982). The relationship between self and achievement/
88.1.87. performance measures. Review of Educational Research, 52(1), 123–142. http://dx.
*Beck, J. W., & Schmidt, A. M. (2013). State-level goal orientations as mediators of the doi.org/10.3102/00346543052001123.
relationship between time pressure and performance: A longitudinal study. Journal of Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. Guilford Press.
Applied Psychology, 98(2), 354–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031145. Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Motivation and self-regulation across the life span.
Bong, M. (2001). Role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college students' course Cambridge University Press.
performance and future enrollment intentions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, Heggestad, E. D., & Kanfer, R. (2005). The predictive validity of self-efficacy in training
26(4), 553–570. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2000.1048. performance: Little more than past performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Bong, M. (2006). Asking the right question: How confident are you that you could suc- Applied, 11(2), 84–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.11.2.84.
cessfully perform these tasks. In F. Pajares, & T. Urdan (Eds.). Self-efficacy beliefs of Higgins, J. P., & Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions.
adolescents (pp. 287–305). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Vol. 5. Wiley Online Library.
Bong, M. (2012). Self-efficacy. International guide to student achievement. 64–66. Honicke, T., & Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on academic
*Bonne, L., & Johnston, M. (2016). Students' beliefs about themselves as mathematics performance: A systematic review. Educational Research Review, 17, 63–84. http://dx.
learners. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 20, 17–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc. doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.11.002.
2016.02.001. Hornstra, L., Van Der Veen, I., Peetsma, T., & Volman, M. (2013). Developments in
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., & Rothstein, H. (2007). Meta-analysis: Fixed effect vs. random motivation and achievement during primary school: A longitudinal study on group-
effects. Meta-analysis.com. specific differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 23(1), 195–204. http://dx.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Meta-regression. doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.09.004.
Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.187–203. Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Vol. 28, 233
Britner, S. L., & Pajares, F. (2006). Sources of science self-efficacy beliefs of middle school Spring Street, New York, NY 10013: Springer1–35.
students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(5), 485–499. http://dx.doi.org/ Hwang, M. H., Choi, H. C., Lee, A., Culver, J. D., & Hutchison, B. (2016). The relationship
10.1002/tea.20131. between self-efficacy and academic achievement: A 5-year panel analysis. Asia-Pacific
Brown, S. D., Tramayne, S., Hoxha, D., Telander, K., Fan, X., & Lent, R. W. (2008). Social Education Researcher, 25(1), 89–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0236-3.
Klassen, R. M. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of the efficacy beliefs of South Asian
immigrant and Anglo Canadian nonimmigrant early adolescents. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 96(4), 731–742. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.
4
References marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis. 731.

148
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Klassen, R. M., & Usher, E. L. (2010). Self-efficacy in educational settings: Recent research Pajares, F., & Kranzler, J. (1995). Self-efficacy beliefs and general mental ability in
and emerging directions. 16(Part A), 1–33. mathematical problem-solving. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(4),
Kock, N. (2014). Advanced mediating effects tests, multi-group analyses, and measure- 426–443.
ment model assessments in PLS-based SEM. International Journal of e-Collaboration Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1995). Mathematics self-efficacy and mathematics perfor-
(IJeC), 10(1), 1–13. mances: The need for specificity of assessment. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
Komarraju, M., Karau, S. J., Schmeck, R. R., & Avdic, A. (2011). The big five personality 42(2), 190–198.
traits, learning styles, and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-beliefs and school success: Self-efficacy, self-
Differences, 51(4), 472–477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. concept, and school achievement. In R. Riding, & S. Rayner (Eds.). Perception (pp.
Lee, C., & Bobko, P. (1994). Self-efficacy beliefs: Comparison of five measures. Journal of 239–266). London: Ablex Publishing.
Applied Psychology, 79(3), 364. Pajares, F., & Usher, E. L. (2008). Self-efficacy, motivation, and achievement in school
Lee, H. S., Flores, L. Y., Navarro, R. L., & Kanagui-Munoz, M. (2015). A longitudinal test of from the perspective of reciprocal determinism. Advances in Motivation and
social cognitive career theory's academic persistence model among Latino/a and Achievement, 15, 391–423.
White men and women engineering students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 88, Parker, P. D., Marsh, H. W., Ciarrochi, J., Marshall, S., & Abduljabbar, A. S. (2014).
95–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2015.02.003. Juxtaposing math self-efficacy and self-concept as predictors of long-term achieve-
Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., & Bieschke, K. J. (1991). Mathematics self-efficacy: Sources and ment outcomes. Educational Psychology, 34(1), 29–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
relation to science-based career choice. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(4), 01443410.2013.797339.
424–430. Phan, H. P. (2012). Informational sources, self-efficacy and achievement: A temporally
*Lewis, J. L., Ream, R. K., Bocian, K. M., Cardullo, R. A., Hammond, K. A., & Fast, L. A. displaced approach. Educational Psychology, 32(6), 699–726. http://dx.doi.org/10.
(2012). Con Cariño: Teacher caring, math self-efficacy, and math achievement among 1080/01443410.2012.708320.
Hispanic English learners. Teachers College Record, 114(7), 1–42. Phan, H. P., & Ngu, B. H. (2016). Sources of self-efficacy in academic contexts: A long-
Lindsley, D. H., Brass, D. J., & Thomas, J. B. (1995). Efficacy-performing spirals: A itudinal perspective. School Psychology Quarterly. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
multilevel perspective. Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 645–678. http://dx. spq0000151.
doi.org/10.5465/amr.1995.9508080333. Pokay, P., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1990). Predicting achievement early and late in the se-
MacCann, C., Fogarty, G. J., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2011). Coping mediates the mester: The role of motivation and use of learning strategies. Journal of Educational
relationship between emotional intelligence (EI) and academic achievement. Psychology, 82(1), 41–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.41.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 60–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. *Richard, E. M., Diefendorff, J. M., & Martin, J. H. (2006). Revisiting the within-person
cedpsych.2010.11.002. self-efficacy and performance relation. Human Performance, 19(1), 67–87. http://dx.
Majer, J. M. (2009). Self-efficacy and academic success among ethnically diverse first- doi.org/10.1207/s15327043hup1901_4.
generation community college students. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 2(4), Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university
243–250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017852. students' academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Matsui, T., Matsui, K., & Ohnishi, R. (1990). Mechanisms underlying math self-efficacy Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 353–387.
learning of college students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 37(2), 225–238. http:// Riketta, M. (2008). The causal relation between job attitudes and performance: A meta-
dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(90)90042-Z. analysis of panel studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 472.
*Meier, S., McCarthy, P. R., & Schmeck, R. R. (1984). Validity of self-efficacy as a pre- Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do
dictor of writing performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(2), 107–120. http:// psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis.
dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01173038. Psychological Bulletin, 130(2), 261–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.
Menard, S. (2007). Handbook of longitudinal research: Design, measurement, and analysis. 2.261.
Elsevier. Rogosa, D. (1988). Analysis of reciprocal effects. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.). Educational re-
Mitchell, T. R., Hopper, H., Daniels, D., George-Falvy, J., & James, L. R. (1994). search, methodology and measurement: An international handbook. Elsevier Science &
Predicting self-efficacy and performance during skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Technology Books.
Psychology, 79(4), 506–517. Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for Bulletin, 86(3), 638.
systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Schwarzer, R., & McAuley, E. (2016). The world is confounded: A comment on Williams
Medicine, 151(4), 264–269. and Rhodes (2016). Health Psychology Review, 10(2), 133–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.
*Mone, M. A. (1994). Comparative validity of two measures of self-efficacy in predicting 1080/17437199.2016.1162667.
academic goals and performance. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54(2), Scott, L. A. (2000). A matter of confidence? A new (old) perspective on sex differences in
516–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164494054002026. mathematics achievement. (AAI9955419). PsycINFO database.
*Mone, M. A., Baker, D. D., & Jeffries, F. (1995). Predictive validity and time dependency Selig, J. P., & Little, T. P. (2012). Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for long-
of self-efficacy, self-esteem, personal goals, and academic performance. Educational itudinal data. Handbook of developmental research methods. 265.
and Psychological Measurement, 55(5), 716–727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Shea, C. M., & Howell, J. M. (2000). Efficacy-performance spirals: An empirical test.
0013164495055005002. Journal of Management, 26(4), 791–812. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Moreira, P. A. S., Dias, P., Vaz, F. M., & Vaz, J. M. (2013). Predictors of academic per- 014920630002600409.
formance and school engagement — Integrating persistence, motivation and study Siegle, D., & McCoach, D. B. (2007). Increasing student mathematics self-efficacy through
skills perspectives using person-centered and variable-centered approaches. Learning teacher training. Journal of Advanced Academics, 18(2), 278–312.
and Individual Differences, 24, 117–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10. Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2015). Official user-guide to the P-curve.
016. Retrieved from www.p-curve.com/guide.pdf.
Moriarty, B. (2014). Research design and the predictive power of measures of self-effi- Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2015). Better P-curves: Making p-curve
cacy. Issues in Educational Research, 24(1), 55–66. analysis more robust to errors, fraud, and ambitious p-hacking, a reply to Ulrich and
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to Miller (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(6), 1146–1152.
academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000104.
38(1), 30–38. Singh, B., & Tyagi, T. K. (2014). The application of cross-lagged panel analysis in edu-
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user's guide (Seventh Edition). Los cational research. FACTA UNIVERSITATIS-Series Philosophy, Sociology, Psychology and
Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. History, 2, 39–51.
Nietfeld, J. L., Cao, L., & Osborne, J. W. (2006). The effect of distributed monitoring Sitzmann, T., & Yeo, G. (2013). A meta-analytic investigation of the within-person self-
exercises and feedback on performance, monitoring accuracy, and self-efficacy. efficacy domain: Is self-efficacy a product of past performance or a driver of future
Metacognition and Learning, 1(2), 159–179. performance? Personnel Psychology, 66(3), 531–568.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). The assessment of reliability. In I. H. Bernstein, Smith, M. L. (1980). Publication bias and meta-analysis. Evaluation in Education, 4, 22–24.
& J. C. Nunnally (Vol. Eds.), Psychometric theory. Vol. 3. Psychometric theory (pp. 248– http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-765X(80)90004-X.
292). Stankov, L., & Lee, J. (2014). Quest for the best non-cognitive predictor of academic
Obrentz, S. B. (2012). Predictors of science success: The impact of motivation and learning achievement. Educational Psychology, 34(1), 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
strategies on college chemistry performance. 01443410.2013.858908.
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). Self-regulation in adolescence. Cambridge Thompson, S. G., & Sharp, S. J. (1999). Explaining heterogeneity in meta-analysis: A
University Press. comparison of methods. Statistics in Medicine, 18(20), 2693–2708.
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the
Research, 66(4), 543–578. literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 751–796.
Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy research. Advances in Motivation and Valentine, J. C., DuBois, D. L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relation between self-beliefs and
Achievement, 10(149). academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 39(2),
Pajares, F. (2006). Self-efficacy during childhood and adolescence: Implications for tea- 111–133.
chers and parents. In F. Pajares, & T. Urdan (Eds.). Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). How many studies do you need?:
(pp. 339–368). Greenwich, CT: Information Age. A primer on statistical power for meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral
Pajares, F. (2007). Culturalizing educational psychology. In F. Salili, & R. Hoosain (Eds.). Statistics, 35(2), 215–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/1076998609346961.
Culture, motivation, and learning: A multicultural perspective (pp. 19–42). Charlotte, NC: *Vancouver, J. B., & Kendall, L. N. (2006). When self-efficacy negatively relates to mo-
IAP. tivation and performance in a learning context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(5),
Pajares, F., Hartley, J., & Valiante, G. (2001). Response format in writing self-efficacy 1146–1153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.1146.
assessment: Greater discrimination increases prediction. Measurement and Evaluation Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., & Williams, A. A. (2001). The changing signs in the
in Counseling and Development, 33(4), 214. relationships among self-efficacy, personal goals, and performance. Journal of Applied

149
K. Talsma et al. Learning and Individual Differences 61 (2018) 136–150

Psychology, 86(4), 605. Educational Psychology, 25(1), 82–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1016.


Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1995). Self-efficacy and educational development. Self-
Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03. Efficacy in Changing Societies, 202–231.
Williams, T., & Williams, K. (2010). Self-efficacy and performance in mathematics: Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for academic
Reciprocal determinism in 33 nations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. American
453–466. Educational Research Journal, 29(3), 663–676. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1163261.
Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning. Zusho, A., Pintrich, P. R., & Coppola, B. (2003). Skill and will: The role of motivation and
Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 329. cognition in the learning of college chemistry. International Journal of Science
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn. Contemporary Education, 25(9), 1081–1094.

150

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться