Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 6

Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Criminal Justice


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jcrimjus

Race and (antisocial) personality



Matt DeLisi
Iowa State University, 203A East Hall 510 Farm House Lane Ames, Iowa 50011-1070, United States

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Background: Race differences in crime and violence are dramatic in the United States but criminology has
Race avoided offering explanations due mostly to political correctness.
Personality Method: The current study draws on research on personality, race differences in personality, race and self-
Self-control control, and race and psychopathy to examine whether personality is potentially useful for articulating race
Psychopathy
differences in offending.
Crime
Results: Findings on race differences in personality, self-control, and psychopathy are mixed and effect sizes
among significant findings are small. Relying on the street code, one of the few theories that explicitly attempts
to explain criminal violence perpetrated by African Americans, it is hypothesized that personality pathology
potentially drives the violent adaptations that are attributed to the code.
Conclusion: The race-crime-personality linkages are equivocal at best and more research is needed to gauge the
utility of personality as a basis for understanding race differences in crime and violence, particularly homicide.

1. Introduction age-crime curve. For 30 to 34 year olds, among whites, the highest rate
was 6.3 and the lowest rate was 5.1. Among blacks, the highest black
In a recent book, DeLisi, 2015) showcased how glaringly different rate was 64.4 and the lowest rate was 44.0. In other words, the lowest
racial groups are on measures of crime—in this example homicide—by black rate was nearly seven times higher than the highest white rate.
reviewing research based on data from the Centers for Disease Control For 35 to 39 year olds, among whites, the highest rate was 4.9 and the
and Prevention. In their study, Hu, Webster, and Baker (2008) analyzed lowest rate was 4.1. Among blacks, the highest black rate was 35.8 and
trends from 1999 to 2005 using CDC data and disaggregated the firearm the lowest rate was 27.3. In other words, the lowest black rate was
homicide mortality rate per 100,000 by race and age group. For 15 to nearly six times higher than the highest white rate. For 40 to 44 year
19 year olds, among whites, the highest rate was 7.0 and the lowest rate olds, among whites, the highest rate was 4.0 and the lowest rate was
was 6.2. Among blacks, the highest rate was 56.4 and the lowest rate 3.4. Among blacks, the highest black rate was 26.2 and the lowest rate
was 48.5. In other words, the lowest black rate was nearly seven times was 20.3. In other words, the lowest black rate was five times higher
higher than the highest white rate. For 20 to 24 year olds, among than the highest white rate.
whites, the highest rate was 10.7 and the lowest rate was 9.0. Among Such extraordinary race differences in homicide mortality and by
blacks, the highest black rate was 106.1 and the lowest rate was 94.2. In extension, homicide offending and victimization demand an explana-
other words, the lowest black rate was nearly nine times higher than the tion.1 But on this and many issues relating to race, criminology has been
highest white rate. For 25 to 29 year olds, among whites, the highest mostly silent due to political correctness and fear of being labeled as
rate was 8.4 and the lowest rate was 6.7. Among blacks, the highest racist for offering explanations for black-white differences (Felson,
black rate was 93.1 and the lowest rate was 74.4. In other words, the Deane, & Armstrong, 2008; Sampson and Wilson, 2005). A notable
lowest black rate was nearly nine times higher than the highest white exception is Anderson, 1999) code of the street theory which attributes
rate. black disproportionate involvement in violent crime to subcultural
These trends continued even in the less crime-prone years of the adaptations where various interpersonal signs of disrespect are

This article was processed by Drs. Boutwell and Wright.



Corresponding author.
E-mail address: delisi@iastate.edu.
1
Other than heart disease, homicide is the main reason for life expectancy differences between whites and blacks, and blacks are between 5 and 10 times more likely than whites to kill
or be killed (O'Flaherty & Sethi, 2010). Much of the race differential in homicide relates to other involvement in antisocial behavior (which would potentially implicate differential
personality functioning). For instance, the black to white ratio for homicide involving other felonies, such as robbery is more than 10 to 1. For drug crimes, the ratio is nearly 8 to 1, and
for other arguments and assaults the ratio has ranged between 4 and 1 and 9 to 1 historically (O'Flaherty and Sethi, 2010).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2018.05.004
Received 6 January 2018; Accepted 2 May 2018
0047-2352/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: DeLisi, M., Journal of Criminal Justice (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2018.05.004
M. DeLisi Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

expected to be responded to with force. Recently, DeLisi, 2014) chal- Openness to experience involves the appreciation and seeking of ex-
lenged the veracity of the street code by hypothesizing that the adap- periences. Open individuals are characterized as curious, imaginative,
tations that Anderson describes—street adaptation among offenders willing to have novel experiences, and open to varied emotional ex-
and decent adaptation among non-offenders—are themselves relatively periences. Closed individuals are characterized as conventional, con-
coherent groups based on likely personality profiles. From this per- servative, dogmatic, rigid, and behaviorally set in their ways. It in-
spective, an individual's personality drives social interaction patterns cludes facet scales for fantasy, aesthetics, feelings, actions, ideas, and
and behavioral responses. With this idea in mind, the current article values. Agreeableness refers to the kinds of interactions a person has
explores the role of personality as a potentially useful construct for along a continuum from compassion to antagonism. High scorers on
understanding racial and ethnic differences in antisocial behavior by agreeableness are good natured, trusting, soft-hearted, helping, and
utilizing theory and research on (1) personality and antisociality, (2) altruistic. Low scorers tend to be cynical, rude, uncooperative, irritable,
race and personality, (3) race and self-control, and (4) race and psy- and manipulative. It includes facet scales for trust, straightforwardness,
chopathy. Suggestions for using personality-based research to under- altruism, compliance, modesty, and tender-mindedness. Con-
stand race differences in criminal offending are offered. scientiousness captures the degree of organization, persistence, control,
and motivation in goal-directed behavior. High scorers are organized,
reliable, hardworking, self-directed, ambitious, and persevering. Low
1.1. Personality and Antisociality scorers are aimless, unreliable, lazy, careless, negligent, and hedonistic.
It includes facet scales for competence, order, dutifulness, achievement
Personality functioning is critical for understanding behavior in striving, self-discipline, and deliberation.
most domains of life.2 Several conceptual models for personality exist. Within the Five Factor Model of Personality, a coherent profile of
One of the earliest and most frequently studied is Eysenck's PEN model personality traits is commonly seen among antisocial individuals.
which articulates psychoticism, extraversion, and neuroticism as the Within Neuroticism, there is a tendency toward high anger and hosti-
“Big 3” components of personality (Eysenck, 1964, 1996; Eysenck, lity, low self-consciousness, and high impulsiveness. Within
1977; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985; also see, Zuckerman, 1997). Extra- Extraversion, there is a tendency toward low warmth, low positive
version is theorized to typify individuals with low cortical arousal who emotions, and high excitement-seeking. There are not expected asso-
seek stimuli from the environment to increase their arousal level. Given ciations within Openness to Experience. In structural models of per-
their reduced arousal, Eysenck suggested that extraverts require higher sonality, and strongest effects are expected for Agreeableness and
levels of stimulation to affect learning and are more challenging to Conscientiousness. For Agreeableness, there is a tendency toward low
condition. Extraverts are typified as lively, active, assertive, sensation trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, and tender-
seeking, carefree, dominant, surgent, and venturesome. Neuroticism is mindedness. For Conscientiousness, there is a tendency toward low
characterized by a tendency toward negative emotionality that is the- competence, order, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline,
orized to result from a hyperactive autonomic nervous system and in- and deliberation. These profiles are supported by multiple meta-ana-
creased emotional drives. Neurotics are typified as anxious, depressed, lytic studies (Jones, Miller, & Lynam, 2011; Miller & Lynam, 2001;
guilty, tense, moody, emotional, and having low self-esteem. Psycho- Samuel and Widiger, 2008).
ticism is the dimension that most explicitly comports with antisocial Indeed, numerous empirical linkages have been established between
traits. It characterizes a person who is tough-minded, aggressive, cold, personality and antisocial behavior. Drawing on data from the Dunedin
egocentric, impersonal, impulsive, antisocial, unempathic, and creative. Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, Krueger, Schmutte,
Tellegen, 1982) developed the Multidimensional Personality Ques- Caspi, Moffitt, Campbell, and Silva (1994) examined the linkages be-
tionnaire to assess his three dimensional model of personality that in- tween personality and criminal offending among 862 male and female
cluded positive emotionality, negative emotionality, and constraint. 18 year-olds using the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire
Positive emotionality is the tendency to experience positive affective (MPQ). Their findings were striking. The MPQ contains three super-
experiences and is comprised of lower-order factors of wellbeing, ordinate factors which are constraint, negative emotionality, and po-
achievement, social potency, and social closeness. Negative emotion- sitive emotionality. Constraint was negatively correlated with self-re-
ality is the tendency to experience negative affective experiences and is ports of delinquency, informant reports of delinquency, and court
comprised of lower-order factors of alienation, aggression, and stress convictions for both men and women. Constraint was not significantly
reaction. Constraint is characterized by self-restraint and the inhibition correlated with police contacts for either gender. Negative emotionality
of emotional and behavioral responses and is comprised of lower-order was positively correlated with self- and informant-rated delinquency,
factors of harm avoidance, traditionalism, and control. police contacts, and court convictions (with the exception of court
The Five Factor Model is a structural model of personality that convictions among women). Positive emotionality was not significantly
contains five dimensions: openness to experience, conscientiousness, correlated with any measure of antisocial conduct. The most versatile
extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism and its predominant and severe offenders differed significantly in personality from norma-
measures are the NEO-PI (Costa Jr. and McCrae, 1985) and NEO-PI-R tive delinquents. The versatile offenders were characterized by a re-
(Costa Jr. and McCrae, 1992). Neuroticism refers to the chronic level of jection of traditional values, thrill seeking, impulsivity, aggressiveness,
emotional adjustment and instability and includes facet scales for an- alienation, and lack of sociability. They scored extremely low on harm
xiety, angry hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, avoidance and social closeness and uniquely high on alienation.
and vulnerability. Extraversion refers to the quantity and intensity of Cale, 2006) performed a meta-analysis of 52 studies using 97 sam-
preferred interpersonal interactions, activity level, need for stimulation, ples containing 15,403 participants to review the association between
and capacity for joy. It includes facet scales for warmth, gregariousness, the three commonalities of Eysenck's and Tellegen's models, namely
assertiveness, activity, excitement seeking, and positive emotions. personality features of extraversion/sociability, neuroticism/emotion-
ality, and impulsivity/disinhibition. These studies spanned an extra-
2
Criminology has historically eschewed the use of personality to understand criminal ordinary array of humanity and included schoolchildren, nonclinical
behavior despite evidence which indicated that various personality features were sig- participants, prison inmates, children with Conduct Disorder, ag-
nificant in distinguishing offenders from non-offenders (cf., Andrews and Wormith, 1989; gressive delinquents, non-aggressive delinquents, participants who en-
Cochrane, 1974; DeLisi et al., 2010; Ellis, 1987; Furnham and Thompson, 1991;
joyed risky jobs, participants who enjoyed risky sporting activities,
Hindelang, 1972; Romero et al., 2003; Tennenbaum, 1977; Waldo and Dinitz, 1967). This
curious history was recently reviewed by Wright and DeLisi, 2015) who suggested that
criminal drug addicts, college students, twins, middle school students,
criminology disavowed personality research on ideological grounds in favor of theoretical bullies, alcoholics, high school students, university undergraduates, and
explanations that were structural or sociological in scope. diverse samples of criminals or persons with externalizing symptoms.

2
M. DeLisi Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Cale reported significant relations between these three broadband and aversive attitudes than whites among probation officers (Bridges &
dimensions of personality and antisocial conduct. Among 94 samples, Steen, 1998). Cooper, Wood, Orcutt, and Albino (2003) reported racial
73% of the samples had positive effect sizes for extraversion/sociability similarities between whites and blacks in a sample of ~2000 adoles-
and the mean effect size was r = 0.10. Among 90 samples, 84% of the cents whereby a general syndrome toward behavioral disinhibition was
samples had positive effect sizes for neuroticism/emotionality and the associated with diverse antisocial and delinquent acts. However, they
mean effect size was r = 0.18. Among 96 samples, 98% of the samples also found that thrill-seeking was more strongly associated with de-
had positive effect sizes for impulsivity/disinhibition and the mean viance among white than black youth.
effect size was r = 0.37. In other words, impulsivity/disinhibition was Some criminological models expressly or indirectly suggest that race
clearly the strongest personality link to antisocial behavior. differences (particularly black-white differences) in personality func-
These associations are not limited to normal personality, but are tioning are an important source of variation for race differences in
also seen among the personality disordered. Decuyper, De Pauw, De delinquency and violent crime. In social information processing theory,
Fruyt, De Bolle, and De Clercq (2009) performed a meta-analysis of 26 hostile attribution bias is more predictive of violence among black of-
independent samples of 6913 participants for psychopathy and 57 in- fenders than white offenders (Vitale et al., 2005). In general strain
dependent samples of 16,424 participants for Antisocial Personality theory African Americans are theorized to experience more and greater
Disorder. What did their findings reveal? In terms of findings with an types of strain that produce higher levels of negative emotionality,
effect size of r ≥│0.20│, persons with Antisocial Personality Disorder especially anger and hostility than whites. These differences in strain,
were angry, hostile, impulsive, excitement-seeking, distrustful, devious, anger, and hostility manifest in greater violence among blacks
egocentric, tough-minded, incompetent, hasty, disloyal, and lacking in (Kaufman, Rebellon, Thaxton, & Agnew, 2008; also see, Jang &
self-discipline. The effects among psychopathic persons were similar Johnson, 2003; Nyborg & Curry, 2003). In the previously mentioned
although psychopaths were significantly more antagonistic in addition code of the street, Anderson, 1999) developed a theory specifically for
to being colder interpersonally and negatively affected by anxiety. urban African Americans that attempts to explain their voluminous
involvement in violent crime. If one were to translate the street code
1.2. Race and personality into personality parlance, it would be characterized by high anger and
hostility, low altruism, high sensation seeking, high assertiveness, and
Against this backdrop of antisocial personality features, it is im- low scores on most facets of Agreeableness and Conscientiousness.
portant to examine race differences in personality to see if anything can These personality features lend themselves to impulsive overreactions
be useful for understanding race differences in offending. Using a na- to perceived threats.
tionally presentative sample of Americans, Goldberg, Sweeney,
Merenda, and Hughes (1998) found that blacks and Hispanics had 1.3. Race and self-control
significantly lower scores on Conscientiousness than whites, but no
other significant differences were found. Based on data from the Na- Although self-control as it is understood in criminology is believed
tional Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, Blum et al. (2000) to be disjointed from personality, it is not disjointed substantively. At
found that black and Hispanic youth were more likely than white youth the most basic level, self-control is indicative of the core personality
to engage in violent conduct whereas white youth were more prone to construct of Conscientiousness, and the constitutive elements of self-
internalizing symptoms including suicide attempts. Using data from control in Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) are effectively personality
middle schoolers in the United States, McLaughlin, Hilt, and Nolen- constructs. Unfortunately, the literature on race differences in self-
Hoeksema (2007) found that Hispanic females reported experiencing control is disparate and mixed with some research indicating null race
greater levels of anxiety, depression, and aggression than white or black differences, others suggesting slight race differences, and still other
girls. Black males reported the highest levels of overtly aggressive be- research provided evidence of acute race differences in self-control. For
haviors, anxiety, and disordered eating than white or Hispanic males. instance, research using exclusively black samples has shown that self-
Foldes, Duehr, and Ones (2008) conducted a large-scale meta-ana- control operates as theorized by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) and is
lysis of group differences in personality among whites, blacks, His- associated with problem behaviors (Vazsonyi and Crosswhite, 2004)
panics, Asians, and American Indians. Several important findings were just as has been shown in majority white samples. A study of jail in-
produced. They found small to moderate black-white differences in mates found that white inmates had the lowest self-control followed by
personality with whites scoring higher than blacks on emotional sta- Hispanic inmates and black inmates. In other words, blacks had the best
bility, Extraversion and within Extraversion, sociability. Hispanic-white self-control. However, the effects of self-control on crime in this crim-
comparisons were similarly small, and white-Asian differences were inal sample were consistent across racial groupings (De Li, 2005).
slight with the exception of Conscientiousness factors which favored Others have theorized that blacks have significantly worse self-
Asians. More moderate findings were found in white-American Indian control than whites based on blacks' disproportionate involvement in
comparisons where whites had greater emotional stability, Extraver- motor vehicle accidents, failure to wear a seat belt, marijuana use, risky
sion, and Agreeableness and American Indians scored higher on Con- sexual behaviors, and contracting sexually transmitted diseases
scientiousness. The greatest contrasts were seen in comparisons of (Greenberg, 2008). In this way, race differences in mortality and mor-
personality between blacks and Asians. bidity are partially understood as reflecting race differences in pre-
Research of race differences in personality among general popula- ventive behaviors that require planning, a long time horizon, and the
tion samples is not very revealing and indicates few differences, and delay of gratification.
where there are differences, they tend to be small in their effects. Studies of the constituent parts of self-control as conceptualized by
Research of race differences in personality among antisocial samples Gottfredson and Hirschi indicate that core self-regulation deficits by
reveals a different picture. Cross and Tracy (1971) found that black race are a potentially fruitful explanation of race differences in of-
delinquents had a shorter time horizon for gratification delay and a fending. In a study using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study,
more external locus of control than white delinquents. In a study of Kindergarten Class of 1998–1999, Beaver, Wright, and DeLisi (2011)
black and white males who were participants in a domestic violence reported dramatic race differences in cognitive skills and self-control
abatement counseling program, Hamberger and Hastings (1992) re- with blacks evincing lower cognitive skills and lower self-control. The
ported large differences. Black males scored significantly higher than self-control differences were based on teacher assessments of the stu-
whites on measures of narcissism, paranoia, hypomania, drug use, and dent's self-control, teacher assessments of the student's approach to
psychotic delusions. Blacks were also more aggressive than whites. learning, ability to concentrate, and remain focused on difficult tasks,
Blacks have been rated as having more antisocial personality features and the student's externalizing behaviors including the frequency with

3
M. DeLisi Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

which the child argues, fights, acts impulsively, and gets angry. to families with two parents, blacks would a priori have lower self-
These race differences are enduring and result in race differences in control than whites.
school sanctions which are commonly attributable to discriminatory
enforcement practices. However, race differences in prior antisocial 1.4. Race and psychopathy
behavior have been shown to completely account for race differences in
school suspensions (Wright, Morgan, Coyne, Beaver, and Barnes, 2014). A relatively narrow literature has investigated the potential of
Subsequent research using these data also found that a severe subgroup psychopathy—a particularly pernicious form of personality dysfunc-
of children characterized by low self-control was the only subgroup tion—as an explanation for race differences in crime and violent con-
where nonwhite children were the majority (Vaughn, DeLisi, Beaver, duct. Again, the literature is mostly equivocal about race differences in
and Wright, 2008). Children with severe self-control impairments had psychopathic personality features. In the manual for the Psychopathy
lower interpersonal skills, lower social skills, greater parental physical Checklist: Youth Version (PCL:YV; Forth, Kosson, & Hare, 2003), blacks
punishment, lower approaches to learning, and reduced cognitive skills. are significantly more psychopathic than whites with an effect size of
In a study of 164 male murderers imprisoned in Georgia and their Cohen's d = 0.61 which is moderate in size. Skeem, Edens, Camp, and
subsequent parole risk, Heilbrun, Heilbrun, and Heilbrun (1978) found Colwell, 2004) performed a meta-analysis of 21 studies that en-
that those who committed impulsive murders also were most likely to compassed 8890 participants from correctional, substance abusing, and
fail on parole because of their low self-control. Moreover, blacks were psychiatric samples. They found significant race differences in psy-
more likely than whites to commit impulsive homicides, and their chopathy although the effect size was quite small. Blacks averaged
greater parole failure reflected a basic deficit in complying with the about 0.7 points higher than whites on the Psychopathy Checklist-Re-
conditions of parole (also see, Heilbrun & Heilbrun, 1977).3 In other vised (PCL-R) and the size of this effect was small (Cohen's d = 0.11).
words, racial differences were seen in impulsivity, impulsive lethal Perhaps due to the smallness of this effect, the authors concluded that
violence, and self-control capability that is needed for complying with “research to date indicates no reliable, meaningful differences between
parole. Other studies have noted racial differences in temperamental Blacks and Whites on the most widely accepted measure of psychopathy
features that relate to self-regulation. Using data from the Child De- (Skeem, Edens, Camp, and Colwell, 2004, p. 520).
velopment Project, a longitudinal, multi-site study of children's ad- McCoy and Edens (2006) conducted a meta-analysis of 16 studies
justment from ages 5 to 16, Lansford, Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, spanning 2199 participants. These studies included samples of diverse
and Pettit (2004) reported that blacks scored higher than whites on groups including adjudicated delinquents, incarcerated adolescents,
measures of difficult temperament, unadaptable, and resistance to juvenile probationers, juvenile prisoners, juveniles charged with of-
control. Their study focused on the effects of parental physical dis- fenses in adult/criminal court, adjudicated substance-abusing youth,
cipline and subsequent externalizing behaviors. Whereas physical dis- and incarcerated juvenile sex offenders. In terms of race, the samples
cipline was associated with greater externalizing behaviors among ranged from 17% Black to 83% Black. The effect size differences in
whites, it was associated with reduced externalizing behaviors among psychopathy by race were variable ranging from Cohen's d = −0.40
blacks. Based on the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth De- (where whites were more psychopathic than blacks) to Cohen's
velopment, Miner and Clarke-Stewart (2008) examined temperamental d = 0.61. Overall, McCoy and Edens (2006) found that blacks had
features and trajectories of externalizing behaviors among children ages higher psychopathy scores than whites although the effect was small
2, 3, 4, 7, and 9 years. They found that caregivers and teachers alike (Cohen's d = 0.20).
rated African American children as having greater externalizing fea- In a paper on racial and ethnic differences in psychopathic per-
tures than whites at all ages and the effects got stronger over time sonality, Lynn, 2002) examines many content areas that bear not only
(mothers' ratings indicated the opposite effect). These externalizing on psychopathy but also directly relate to self-control as conceptualized
behaviors included a range of aversive tendencies including displays of in criminology by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990). Lynn draws on dis-
defiance, disobedience, being touchy or petulant, having a hot temper, parate sources of data that are inclusive as possible of the various racial
stubbornness, sullenness, and irritability, demandingness, and sudden and ethnic groups in the United States. In a review of studies of the
changes in mood. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) that used data
If one takes a strictly criminological etiological account of self- from the United States, Japan, and Nigeria, significant racial and ethnic
control, then sharp race differences in self-control should be expected. differences were found. Relative to whites, Native Americans or
Data from the Pittsburgh Youth Study, a prospective longitudinal study American Indians had the highest scores on the Psychopathic deviate
of youth, Peeples and Loeber, 1994) found that black youth were sig- scale of the MMPI followed by Hispanics and blacks. All three of these
nificantly more likely to be reared in a single-parent home, had less groups had significantly higher psychopathy scores than whites. In all
parental supervision, and had less parental monitoring than white studies that included data on East Asians, the latter had significantly
youth. These characteristics are theorized to produce low self-control lower psychopathy scores than whites. Data on Conduct Disorder pro-
(Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Moreover, blacks were more hyperactive duced similar results. Based on studies employing samples from the
than whites although the differences were not significant. Blacks are United States, United Kingdom, Hong Kong, Mauritius, and The Neth-
also more likely to be subject to parental corporal punishment and these erlands, significant racial and ethnic differences were once again found.
differences also have resulted in increased negative affect, impulsivity, Native Americans, blacks, and Hispanics had significantly higher scores
and discipline-related anger (Lorber, O'Leary, and Smith Slep, 2011). on Conduct Disorder than whites based on prevalence estimates and
Given the race differences in exposure to single-parent homes and effect sizes. East Asians had lower scores than whites. For ADHD, dif-
concomitant reductions in parental supervision and monitoring relative ferent patters emerged with blacks significantly exceeding whites.
However, whites had greater ADHD symptoms and prevalence than
3
Hispanics and Eats Asians.
A potential explanation for race differences in homicide offending/victimization
Lynn, 2002) also reviewed several forms of externalizing behaviors
generally and impulsive homicide offending/victimization specifically centers on the
putative warrior gene monoamine oxidase A or MAOA. Low-activity variants of MAOA including school suspensions and exclusions, moral values, responsi-
are the risk alleles and have been linked to antisocial and violent behaviors. Moreover, bility with honoring financial obligations, and diverse forms of crime
the rare 2-repeat allele of the MAOA gene is associated with delinquency and violence at spanning homicide, rape, robbery, burglary, auto theft, assault, do-
levels that are twice as high as carriers of other MAOA variants (Guo et al., 2008). mestic abuse/violence, indices of violent crime, recidivism, and im-
However, only 0.1% of whites carry the 2-repeat allele of MAOA, but it more prevalent
among African Americans and has been linked to a bevy of antisocial behaviors and
prisonment. Among these data, a clear gradient was found with blacks
subsequent criminal justice system statuses (Beaver et al., 2013; Beaver, Barnes, & having the greatest involvement followed by Hispanics, whites, and
Boutwell, 2014). Asians. In studies that included them, Native Americans were either the

4
M. DeLisi Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

most severe group or second only to blacks. Moreover, individuals with these personality features are also precisely
Other behavioral outcomes that relate to gratification delay, tena- the kind of individuals who would respond to innocuous forms of social
city and capacity for long-term investments, recklessness, responsi- interaction (e.g., direct eye contact, inadvertent contact, and other
bility, and impulsivity were also reviewed. Although these relate to trivial behaviors that initiate street code responses) with violence and
psychopathy, they also relate expressly to self-control theory within even lethal violence. My confidence in this assertion is bolstered by
criminology and define an individual whose global deficits in self-reg- qualitative research findings on criminal offenders particularly research
ulation produce maladaptive behaviors in a host of domains. On these, that illuminates violent offenders' accounts for why they respond to
the results were again mixed with Hispanics engaging in behaviors that various normative situations with violence (e.g., Brookman, Bennett,
conveyed the greatest recklessness followed by blacks and whites. One Hochstetler, and Copes, 2011; Copes & Hochstetler, 2003; Gunter,
conclusion to draw from Lynn, 2002) review is that core differences in 2008). Although the offenders' accounts are framed as highlighting the
self-regulation deficits and antisocial personality/psychopathic traits salience of the code of the street, it is my contention that the street code
are an important source of variation that likely produce some of the is derivative of higher order constructs in the personality domain. Street
sharp behavioral differences seen among whites, blacks, Hispanics, and code rationalizations for violence and lethal violence are also seen
Asians in criminal behavior. On this point and larger issues, there was among criminal and inmate samples in cultures outside the United
sharp disagreement with Lynn's work (Skeem, Edens, Sanford, and States and among participants other than African Americans. This
Colwell, 2003; Zuckerman, 2003; also see, Lynn, 2003). suggests the causal force is not cultural or subcultural, but individual-
level.4 Indeed, an investigation of the code of the street with geneti-
2. Discussion cally-sensitive data found that youth with the greatest number of ge-
netic risk factors were most likely to subscribe to the street code
Equivocation is seemingly the best way to characterize the research (Simons, Lei, Beach, Brody, Philibert, and Gibbons, 2012).
on race differences in personality functioning, race differences in self- To date, no study of the street code has adequately controlled for
control, and race differences in psychopathy. Many studies produce null race differences in personality functioning. This means that we do not
to significant effects albeit with miniscule effect sizes. But other studies know whether the street code would survive the competing effects of
particularly those that examine self-regulation and its correlated neu- personality dimensions and whether race differences on these dimen-
rocognitive features show sharp racial differences. This is important sions are also salient for understanding race differences in violence.
because early-emerging temperamental and personality functioning Moreover, it is generally unknown whether youth who ascribe to the
that centers on behavioral regulation, emotional regulation, and cog- street code are characterized by low self-control or are psychopathic
nition are signal variables for predicting subsequent delinquency, although both characterizations seem likely.
crime, violence, and socially maladaptive behaviors (DeLisi & Vaughn, It is not surprising that the dominant explanation for race differ-
2014; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Thus they are potentially im- ences in criminal violence attributes those differences to structural and
portant variables for understanding the severe race differences in subcultural forces for the tendency to skirt examinations of individual-
criminal offending and violence perpetration as implicated by theory level constructs to understand ethnic differences in violence is common
(e.g., Anderson, 1999) and the CDC results described at the introduc- in criminology. Unfortunately, the research reviewed here on person-
tion. ality, self-control, and psychopathy does not shed powerful light on
Of the features of personality, Conscientiousness and Agreeableness race differences either. However, many types of data can be utilized to
are the dimensions with the most thorough association with antisocial see whether personality—which is an enduring and significant pre-
behavior. They are also personality features that are thoroughly lacking dictor of so many life outcomes and behaviors—is also responsible for
if one considers theoretical work (e.g., Anderson, 1999) that was de- the various racial and ethnic differences that are seen in offending,
signed to specifically explain interpersonal violence such as impulsive victimization, and criminal justice system involvement.
homicide among the African American male population. The facets of
Conscientiousness are competence, order, dutifulness, achievement- References
striving, self-discipline, and deliberation. Low scorers on competence
are unable or unwilling to complete tasks and responsibilities and tend Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral Life of the Inner
to misjudge situations. Low scorers on order are haphazard and tolerate City. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Andrews, D. A., & Wormith, J. S. (1989). Personality and crime: Knowledge destruction and,
disorder in their lives and in their home and social environments. Low construction in criminology. Justice Quarterly. 6(3), 289–309.
scorers on dutifulness see little value in complying with rules and ex- Beaver, K. M., Barnes, J. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2014). The 2-repeat allele of the MAOA
pectations and thus are rule-breaking in their conduct. Low scorers on gene confers an increased risk for shooting and stabbing behaviors. Psychiatric
Quarterly, 85(3), 257–265.
achievement-striving extend little effort, do just enough to get by in Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., Boutwell, B. B., Barnes, J. C., DeLisi, M., & Vaughn, M. G.
situations, or simply fail. Low scorers on self-discipline are lazy and (2013). Exploring the association between the 2-repeat allele of the MAOA gene
waste their time with unproductive activities. Low scorers on delib- promoter polymorphism and psychopathic personality traits, arrests, incarceration,
and lifetime antisocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(2),
eration are not cautious and instead rush into situations or behavioral
164–168.
responses without appropriately thinking through the potential rami- Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & DeLisi, M. (2011). The racist teacher revisited: Race and
fications. social skills in a nationally representative sample of American children. In R. J.
Newley (Ed.). Classrooms: Management, Effectiveness, and Challenges (pp. 115–132).
The facets of Agreeableness are trust, compliance, altruism,
New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
straightforwardness, modesty, and tender-mindedness. Low scorers on Blum, R. W., Beuhring, T., Shew, M. L., Bearinger, L. H., Sieving, R. E., & Resnick, M. D.
trust are distrustful of other people and see them as adversaries or (2000). The effects of race/ethnicity, income, and family structure on adolescent risk
threats. Low scorers on compliance fail to follow through with re- behaviors. American Journal of Public Health, 90(12), 1879–1884.
Bridges, G. S., & Steen, S. (1998). Racial disparities in official assessments of juvenile
sponsibilities. Low scorers on altruism are inconsiderate of others and offenders: Attributional stereotypes as mediating mechanisms. American Sociological
focused on the self. They are oblivious to their broader connection to Review, 63, 554–570.
humanity. Low scorers on straightforwardness are devious and conning. Brookman, F., Bennett, T., Hochstetler, A., & Copes, H. (2011). The ‘code of the Street'and

Low scorers on modesty are braggarts who have an excessive sense of


entitlement or self-worth. Low scorers on tender-mindedness are cun-
4
Identical behavioral responses to putative threats to self or signs of disrespect are seen
ning, harsh, and retributive.
internationally (Brookman et al., 2011; Dalrymple, 2001; Gunter, 2008), among diverse
Taken together, low scorers on Conscientiousness and racial and ethnic groups (Brookman, Bennett, Hochstetler, & Copes, 2011; Dalrymple,
Agreeableness are exceedingly likely to be involved in crime as in- 2001; Gunter, 2008; Taylor, Esbensen, Brick, and Freng, 2010) and in prison cultures that
dicated by meta-analyses (Jones et al., 2011; Miller & Lynam, 2001). are not urban settings theorized by Anderson

5
M. DeLisi Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

the generation of street violence in the UK. European Journal of Criminology, 8(1), Kaufman, J. M., Rebellon, C. J., Thaxton, S., & Agnew, R. (2008). A general strain theory
17–31. of racial differences in criminal offending. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Cale, E. M. (2006). A quantitative review of the relations between the “big 3” higher order Criminology, 41(3), 421–437.
personality dimensions and antisocial behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, Krueger, R. F., Schmutte, P. S., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Campbell, K., & Silva, P. A. (1994).
40(3), 250–284. Personality traits are linked to crime among men and women: Evidence from a birth
Cochrane, R. (1974). Crime and personality: Theory and evidence. Bulletin of the British cohort. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(2), 328–338.
Psychological Society, 27, 19–22. Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Ethnic
Cooper, M. L., Wood, P. K., Orcutt, H. K., & Albino, A. (2003). Personality and the pre- differences in the link between physical discipline and later adolescent externalizing
disposition to engage in risky or problem behaviors during adolescence. Journal of behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 801–812.
Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 390–410. Lorber, M. F., O'Leary, S. G., & Smith Slep, A. M. (2011). An initial evaluation of the role
Copes, H., & Hochstetler, A. (2003). Situational construction of masculinity among male of emotion and impulsivity in explaining racial/ethnic differences in the use of cor-
street thieves. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 32(3), 279–304. poral punishment. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1744–1749.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Lynn, R. (2002). Racial and ethnic differences in psychopathic personality. Personality and
Psychological Assessment Resources. Individual Differences, 32(2), 273–316.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and Lynn, R. (2003). Race differences in psychopathic personality: A reply to Skeem and
NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Zuckerman. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6), 1471–1477.
Assessment Resources. McCoy, W. K., & Edens, J. F. (2006). Do black and white youths differ in levels of psy-
Cross, H. J., & Tracy, J. J. (1971). Personality factors in delinquent boys: Differences chopathic traits? A meta-analysis of the psychopathy checklist measures. Journal of
between blacks and whites. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 8(1), 10–22. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(2), 386.
Dalrymple, T. (2001). Life at the Bottom: The Worldview that makes the Underclass. Chicago, McLaughlin, K. A., Hilt, L. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in
IL: Ivan R. Dee. internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child
De Li, S. (2005). Race, self-control, and drug problems among jail inmates. Journal of Drug Psychology, 35(5), 801–816.
Issues, 35(4), 645–663. Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). Structural models of personality and their relation to
Decuyper, M., De Pauw, S., De Fruyt, F., De Bolle, M., & De Clercq, B. J. (2009). A meta- antisocial behavior: A meta-analytic review. Criminology, 39, 765–798.
analysis of psychopathy-, antisocial PD-and FFM associations. European Journal of Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from
Personality, 23(7), 531–565. age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater.
DeLisi, M. (2014). Antisocial traits murdered the code of the street in a battle for respect. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 771–786.
Journal of Criminal Justice, 42(6), 431–432. Nyborg, V. M., & Curry, J. F. (2003). The impact of perceived racism: Psychological
DeLisi, M. (2015). Homicide. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt. symptoms among African American boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
DeLisi, M., Beaver, K. M., Vaughn, M. G., Trulson, C. R., Kosloski, A. E., Drury, A. J., ... Psychology, 32(2), 258–266.
Wright, J. P. (2010). Personality, gender, and self-control theory revisited: Results O'Flaherty, B., & Sethi, R. (2010). Homicide in black and white. Journal of Urban
from a sample of institutionalized juvenile delinquents. Applied Psychology in Criminal Economics, 68(3), 215–230.
Justice, 6(1), 31–46. Peeples, F., & Loeber, R. (1994). Do individual factors and neighborhood context explain
DeLisi, M., & Vaughn, M. G. (2014). Foundation for a temperament-based theory of an- ethnic differences in juvenile delinquency? Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 10(2),
tisocial behavior and criminal justice system involvement. Journal of Criminal Justice, 141–157.
42(1), 10–25. Romero, E., Gomez-Fraguela, A., Luengo, A. N., & Sobral, J. (2003). The self-control
Ellis, L. (1987). Relationships of criminality and psychopathy with eight other apparent construct in the general theory of crime: An investigation in terms of personality
behavioral manifestations of sub-optimal arousal. Personality and Individual psychology. Psychology, Crime & Law, 9(1), 61–86.
Differences, 8(6), 905–925. Sampson, R. J., & Wilson, W. J. (2005). Toward a theory of race, crime, and urban in-
Eysenck, H. J. (1964). Crime and Personality. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. equality. In S. L. Gabbidon, & H. T. Greene (Eds.). Race, Crime, and Justice: A Reader
Eysenck, H. J. (1977). Crime and Personality (3rd Ed.). London: Routledge & Keegan Paul. (pp. 177–190). New York, NY: Routledge.
Eysenck, H. J. (1996). Personality and crime: Where do we stand? Psychology, Crime & Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2008). A meta-analytic review of the relationships be-
Law, 2, 143–152. tween the five-factor model and DSM-IV-TR personality disorders: A facet level
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and Individual Differences. New York: analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1326–1342.
Plenum Press. Simons, R. L., Lei, M. K., Beach, S. R., Brody, G. H., Philibert, R. A., & Gibbons, F. X.
Felson, R. B., Deane, G., & Armstrong, D. P. (2008). Do theories of crime or violence (2012). Social adversity, genetic variation, street code, and aggression: A genetically
explain race differences in delinquency? Social Science Research, 37(2), 624–641. informed model of violent behavior. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 10(1), 3–24.
Foldes, H. J., Duehr, E. E., & Ones, D. S. (2008). Group differences in personality: Meta- Skeem, J. L., Edens, J. F., Camp, J., & Colwell, L. H. (2004). Are there ethnic differences in
analyses comparing five U.S. racial groups. Personnel Psychology, 61(3), 579–616. levels of psychopathy? A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 28(5), 505–527.
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare PCL:YV technical manual. Toronto, Skeem, J. L., Edens, J. F., Sanford, G. M., & Colwell, L. H. (2003). Psychopathic per-
Ontario: Multi-Health Systems. sonality and racial/ethnic differences reconsidered: A reply to Lynn (2002).
Furnham, A., & Thompson, J. (1991). Personality and self-reported delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6), 1439–1462.
Personality and Individual Differences, 12(6), 585–593. Taylor, T. J., Esbensen, F. A., Brick, B. T., & Freng, A. (2010). Exploring the measurement
Goldberg, L. R., Sweeney, D., Merenda, P. F., & Hughes, J. E. (1998). Demographic quality of an attitudinal scale of street code-related violence: Similarities and dif-
variables and personality: The effects of gender, age, education, and ethnic/racial ferences across groups and contexts. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 8(3),
status on self-descriptions of personality attributes. Personality and Individual 187–212.
Differences, 24(3), 393–403. Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief Manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire.
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
University Press. Tennenbaum, D. J. (1977). Personality and criminality a summary and implications of the
Greenberg, D. F. (2008). Age, sex, and racial distributions of crime. In E. Goode (Ed.). Out literature. Journal of Criminal Justice, 5(3), 225–235.
of Control: Assessing the General Theory of Crime (pp. 38–48). Stanford, CA: Stanford Vaughn, M. G., DeLisi, M., Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2008). Identifying latent classes
University Press. of behavioral risk based on early childhood manifestations of self-control. Youth
Gunter, A. (2008). Growing up bad: Black youth road culture and badness in an East Violence and Juvenile Justice, 7(1), 16–31.
London neighbourhood. Crime, Media, Culture, 4(3), 349–366. Vazsonyi, A. T., & Crosswhite, J. M. (2004). A test of Gottfredson and Hirschi's general
Guo, G., Ou, X. M., Roettger, M., & Shih, J. C. (2008). The VNTR 2 repeat in MAOA and theory of crime in African American adolescents. Journal of Research in Crime and
delinquent behavior in adolescence and young adulthood: associations and MAOA Delinquency, 41(4), 407–432.
promoter activity. European Journal of Human Genetics, 16(5), 626–634. Vitale, J. E., Newman, J. P., Serin, R. C., & Bolt, D. M. (2005). Hostile attributions in
Hamberger, L. K., & Hastings, J. E. (1992). Racial differences on the MCMI in an out- incarcerated adult male offenders: An exploration of diverse pathways. Aggressive
patient clinical sample. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58(1), 90–95. Behavior, 31(2), 99–115.
Heilbrun, A. B., & Heilbrun, K. S. (1977). The black minority criminal and violent crime: Waldo, G. P., & Dinitz, S. (1967). Personality attributes of the criminal: An analysis of
The role of self-control. The British Journal of Criminology, 370–377. research studies, 1950-65. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 4(2),
Heilbrun, A. B., Heilbrun, L. C., & Heilbrun, K. L. (1978). Impulsive and premeditated 185–202.
homicide: An analysis of subsequent parole risk of the murderer. The Journal of Wright, J. P., & DeLisi, M. (2015). Conservative Criminology: A Call to Restore Balance to the
Criminal Law and Criminology, 69, 108–114. Social Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.
Hindelang, M. J. (1972). Relationship of self-reported delinquency to scales of the CPI and Wright, J. P., Morgan, M. A., Coyne, M. A., Beaver, K. M., & Barnes, J. C. (2014). Prior
the MMPI. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology & Police Science, 63, 75–81. problem behavior accounts for the racial gap in school suspensions. Journal of
Hu, G., Webster, D., & Baker, S. P. (2008). Hidden homicide increases in the USA, Criminal Justice, 42(3), 257–266.
1999–2005. Journal of Urban Health, 85(4), 597–606. Zuckerman, M. (1997). The psychobiological basis of personality. In H. Nyborg (Ed.). The
Jang, S. J., & Johnson, B. R. (2003). Strain, negative emotions, and deviant coping among Scientific Study of Human Nature: Tribute to Hans J. Eysenck at 80 (pp. 3–16). Oxford,
African Americans: A test of general strain theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, England: Pergamon.
19(1), 79–105. Zuckerman, M. (2003). Are there racial and ethnic differences in psychopathic person-
Jones, S. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2011). Personality, antisocial behavior, and ality? A critique of Lynn's (2002) racial and ethnic differences in psychopathic per-
aggression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 329–337. sonality. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6), 1463–1469.

Вам также может понравиться