Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 788

Handbook of Autism and

Pervasive Developmental Disorders


Third Edition

Volume 1: Diagnosis, Development, Neurobiology, and Behavior

Edited by
Fred R. Volkmar
Rhea Paul
Ami Klin
Donald Cohen

JOHN WILEY & SONS, INC.


HANDBOOK OF AUTISM AND
PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS
Handbook of Autism and
Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Third Edition

Volume 1: Diagnosis, Development, Neurobiology, and Behavior

Edited by
Fred R. Volkmar
Rhea Paul
Ami Klin
Donald Cohen

JOHN WILEY & SONS, INC.



This book is printed on acid-free paper.

Copyright © 2005 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.

Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey.


Published simultaneously in Canada.

MCMI-III is a trademark of DICANDRIEN, Inc.

No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any
means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section
107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher,
or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222
Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com.
Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons,
Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008.

Limit of Liability/ Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in
preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of
the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a
particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials.
The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a
professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other
commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.

This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter
covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If
legal, accounting, medical, psychological or any other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent
professional person should be sought.

Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. In all instances
where John Wiley & Sons, Inc. is aware of a claim, the product names appear in initial capital or all capital
letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate companies for more complete information regarding
trademarks and registration.

For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care Department
within the United States at (800) 762-2974, outside the United States at (317) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.

Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be
available in electronic books. For more information about Wiley products, visit our web site at www.wiley.com.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data:

Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders / edited by Fred R. Volkmar . . .


[et al.].—3rd ed.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Contents: V. 1. Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior—v. 2. Assessment,
interventions, and policy.
ISBN 0-471-71696-0 (cloth : v. 1)—ISBN 0-471-71697-9 (cloth : v. 2)—ISBN
0-471-71698-7 (set)
1. Autism in children. 2. Developmental disabilities. 3. Autistic children—Services for. 4.
Developmentally disabled children—Services for. I. Volkmar, Fred R.
RJ506.A9H26 2005
618.92′85882—dc22
2004059091

Printed in the United States of America.

10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
To the Memory of Donald Cohen

At the time of his death, Donald Cohen was actively involved in the planning of this
edition of the Handbook. His untimely passing made it impossible for him to see the
final product. We are deeply grateful to him for his thoughtful counsel and mentorship
as well as the truly impressive example he presented as a clinician-researcher. We hope
that this Handbook is a testament to his vision and a fitting tribute to his memory.

Photo: Michael Marsland, Yale University


Contributors

M. CHERRO AGUERRE, MD MARGARET L. BAUMAN, MD


University of the Republic Harvard Medical School
School of Medicine Massachusetts Hospital
Cavia Boston, Massachusetts
Montevideo, Uruguay
JAC BILLINGTON, BSC
GEORGE M. ANDERSON, PHD Autism Research Centre
Child Study Center University of Cambridge
Yale University School of Medicine Cambridge, England
New Haven, Connecticut
JAMES W. BODFISH, PHD
JOEL R. ARICK, PHD Department of Psychiatry
Special Education University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Portland State University Chapel Hill, North Carolina
Portland, Oregon
JOEL D. BREGMAN, MD
CHRIS ASHWIN, PHD Center for Autism
Autism Research Centre North Shore Long Island Jewish Health
University of Cambridge System
Departments of Experimental Psychology Bethpage, New York
and Psychiatry
Cambridge, England COURTNEY BURNETTE, MS
Department of Psychology
GRACE T. BARANEK, PHD, OTR/L University of Miami
Division of Occupational Science Coral Gables, Florida
Department of Allied Health Sciences
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill ALICE S. CARTER, PHD
Chapel Hill, North Carolina Department of Psychology
University of Massachusetts Boston
SIMON BARON-COHEN, PHD Boston, Massachusetts
Autism Research Centre
University of Cambridge BHISMADEV CHAKRABARTI, BA, BSC
Departments of Experimental Psychology Autism Research Centre
and Psychiatry University of Cambridge
Cambridge, England Cambridge, England

vii
viii Contributors

KATARZYNA CHAWARSKA, PHD JOAQUIN FUENTES, MD


Child Study Center GUATENA
Yale University School of Medicine San Sebastian, Spain
New Haven, Connecticut

SOO CHURL CHO, MD ANN FULLERTON, PHD


Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Special Education
Seoul National University Hospital Portland State University
Seoul, Korea Portland, Oregon

IAN COOK, MD
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
JOHN GERDTZ, PHD
Sciences
Saint Mary’s College of California
David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA
Moraga, California
Los Angeles, California

ELAINE E. COONROD, MS
Department of Psychology and Human PETER F. GERHARDT, EDD
Development Gerhardt Autism /Aspergers Consultation
Vanderbilt University Group, LLC
Nashville, Tennessee Baltimore, Maryland

CHRISTINA CORSELLO, PHD


Autism and Communication Disorders Center TEMPLE GRANDIN, PHD
University of Michigan Department of Animal Science
Ann Arbor, Michigan Colorado State University
Fort Collins, Colorado
NAOMI ORNSTEIN DAVIS, MA
Boston University School of Medicine
Boston, Massachusetts RICHARD GRIFFIN, BA
Autism Research Centre
RUTH FALCO, PHD University of Cambridge
Special Education Cambridge, England
Portland State University
Portland, Oregon
JAN S. HANDLEMAN, EDD
PIERRE FERRARI, MD Douglas Developmental Disabilities Center
Centre Hospitalier Public De Psychiatrie Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey
De L’Enfant Et De L’Adolescent New Brunswick, New Jersey
Foundation Vallee
Gentilly Cedex, France
FRANCESCA HAPPÉ, PHD (ALSO BA HONS
PAULINE A. FILIPEK, MD OXFORD)
Department of Pediatrics and Neurology Social, Genetic and Developmental
University of California Psychiatry Centre
Irvine College of Medicine Institute of Psychiatry
Orange, California King’s College, London

ERIC FOMBONNE, MD
McGill University SANDRA L. HARRIS, PHD
Department of Psychiatry at the Montreal Douglas Developmental Disabilities Center
Children’s Hospital Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey
Montreal, Quebec, Canada New Brunswick, New Jersey
Contributors ix

PETER HOBSON, MD LINDA J. KUNCE, PHD


The Tavistock Clinic Department of Psychology
Adult Department Illinois Wesleyan University
London, United Kingdom Bloomington, Illinois

DAVID L. HOLMES, EDD AMY LAURENT, OTR/L


Lifespan Services, LLC Communication Crossroads
Princeton, New Jersey North Kingstown, Rhode Island
YOSHIHIKO HOSHINO, MD
Department of Neuropsychiatry JOHN LAWSON, PHD
Hikarigaoka Autism Research Centre
Fukushima-shi, Japan University of Cambridge
Cambridge, England
PATRICIA HOWLIN, MD
St. George’s Hospital Medical School GABRIEL LEVI, MD
Cranmer Terrace Departimento di Scienze Neurologische e
London, United Kingdom Psichiatriche dell’eta Evolutina
Rome, Italy
BROOKE INGERSOLL, PHD
Oregon Institute on Disability and
Development Child Development and JENNIFER A. LONCOLA, PHD
Rehabilitation DePaul University
Center Oregon Health and Science University School of Education
Portland, Oregon Chicago, Illinois

HEATHER K. JENNETT, MS LAUREN LOOS, MS


Douglas Developmental Disabilities Center Autism Specialist
Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Oregon Department of Education
New Brunswick, New Jersey Salem, Oregon

WARREN JONES, BA CATHERINE LORD, PHD


Child Study Center UMACC
Yale University School of Medicine University of Michigan
New Haven, Connecticut Ann Arbor, Michigan

AMI KLIN, PHD


KATHERINE A. LOVELAND, PHD
Child Study Center
University of Texas Health Sciences Center
Yale University School of Medicine
at Houston
New Haven, Connecticut
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences
KATHY KOENIG , MSN
Houston, Texas
Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine
New Haven, Connecticut MYRNA R. MANDLAWITZ, BA, MED, JD
MRM Associates
JASON B. KONIDARIS Washington, DC
Norwalk, Connecticut
WENDY D. MARANS, MS, CCC/SLP
DAVID A. KRUG, PHD Child Study Center
Special Education Yale University School of Medicine
Portland State University Private Practice
Portland, Oregon New Haven, Connecticut
x Contributors

LEE M. MARCUS, PHD PETER MUNDY, PHD


Division TEACCH Department of Psychology
Department of Psychiatry University of Miami
University of North Carolina School Coral Gables, Florida
of Medicine
Chapel Hill, North Carolina J. GREGORY OLLEY, PHD
Clinical Center for the Study of Development
ANDREŚ MARTIN, MD and Learning
Child Study Center University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Yale University School of Medicine Chapel Hill, North Carolina
New Haven, Connecticut
SALLY OZONOFF, PHD
MEGAN P. MARTINS, BA M.I.N.D. Institute
Douglas Developmental Disabilities Center U.C. Davis Medical Center
Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Sacramento, California
New Brunswick, New Jersey
VAYA PAPAGEORGIOU, MD
GAIL G. MCGEE, PHD Medical Psychopedagogical Center of
Emory University School of Medicine North Greece
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Greece
Sciences
Atlanta, Georgia L. DIANE PARHAM, PHD, OTR, FAOTA
Department of Occupational Science and
JAMES MCPARTLAND, MS Occupational Therapy
Child Study Center University of Southern California
Yale University School of Medicine Los Angeles, California
New Haven, Connecticut
RHEA PAUL, PHD, CCC-SLP
ADRIENNE MERYL, BA Department of Communication Disorders
M.I.N.D. Institute Southern Connecticut State University
U.C. Davis Medical Center New Haven, Connecticut
Sacramento, California
MICHAEL D. POWERS, MD
GARY B. MESIBOV, PHD Center for Children with Special Needs
Division TEACCH Glastonbury, Connecticut
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and
Chapel Hill, North Carolina Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine
RICHARD MILLS, CQSW, RMPA, MA, FRSA New Haven, Connecticut
NAS Southern Region Office
Church House, Church Road BARRY M. PRIZANT, PHD
Filton, United Kingdom Childhood Communication Services
Cranston, Rhode Island
NANCY J. MINSHEW, MD and
Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic Center for the Study of Human Development
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Brown University
Providence, Rhode Island
MICHAEL J. MORRIER, MA
Emory University School of Medicine SHERRI PROVENCAL, PHD
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Department of Psychology
Sciences University of Utah
Atlanta, Georgia Salt Lake City, Utah
Contributors xi

ISABELLE RAPIN ERIC SCHOPLER, PHD


Albert Einstein College of Medicine Division TEACCH
Bronx, New York Department of Psychiatry
University of North Carolina School
of Medicine
DIANA L. ROBINS, PHD Chapel Hill, North Carolina
Department of Psychology
Georgia State University
LAURA SCHREIBMAN, PHD
Atlanta, Georgia
Department of Psychology
University of California, San Diego
SALLY J. ROGERS, PHD La Jolla, California
M.I.N.D. Institute
U.C. Davis Medical Center ROBERT T. SCHULTZ, PHD
Sacramento, California Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine
EMILY RUBIN, MS, CCC/SLP New Haven, Connecticut
Communication Crossroads
Carmel, California VICTORIA SHEA, PHD
Division TEACCH
The University of North Carolina at
MICHAEL RUTTER, CBE, MD, FRCP, Chapel Hill
FRCPSYCH, FRS Chapel Hill, North Carolina
Social, Genetic and Developmental
Psychiatry Centre
Institute of Psychiatry MIKLE SOUTH, MS
DeCrespigny Park Department of Psychology
Denmark Hill University of Utah
King’s College, London Salt Lake City, Utah

VIRGINIA WALKER SPERRY, MA


ANDERS RYDELIUS, MD, PHD
Child Study Center
Karolinska Institute
Yale University School of Medicine
Department of Woman and Child Health
New Haven, Connecticut
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Unit
St. Goran’s Children’s Hospital
Stockholm, Sweden MATTHEW STATE, MD, PHD
Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine
CELINE SAULNIER, PHD New Haven, Connecticut
Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine
WENDY L. STONE, PHD
New Haven, Connecticut
Vanderbilt Children’s Hospital
Nashville, Tennessee
LAWRENCE SCAHILL, MSN, PHD
Child Study Center RUTH CHRIST SULLIVAN, PHD
Yale University School of Medicine Autism Services Center
New Haven, Connecticut Huntington, West Virginia

MARTIN SCHMIDT, MD DEAN SUTHERLAND, MS


Kinder Jundenpsychiatrische Klinik Department of Speech Therapy
Zentralinstitute fur Seelische Genundheit Canterbury University
Mannheim, Germany Christchurch, New Zealand
xii Contributors

JOHN A. SWEENEY, PHD ERYN Y. VAN ACKER


University of Pittsburgh College of Education
Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic University of Illinois at Chicago
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Chicago, Illinois

PETER SZATMARI, MD RICHARD VAN ACKER, PHD


McMaster University College of Education
Department of Psychiatry University of Illinois at Chicago
Faculty Health Sciences Chicago, Illinois
Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
FRED R. VOLKMAR, MD
HELEN TAGER-FLUSBERG, PHD Child Study Center
Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology Yale University
Boston University School of Medicine New Haven, Connecticut
Boston, Massachusetts
HERMAN VAN ENGELAND, MD
KUO-TAI TAO, MD Divisie Psychiatrie Kinder en
Division of Nanging Jeugdpsychiatrie
Child Mental Health Research Center Utrecht, The Netherlands
Nanging, China
SARA JANE WEBB, PHD
BRUCE TONGE, MD Center for Human Development and Disability
Centre for Developmental Psychiatry Autism Center Psychophysiology Laboratories
Monash Medical Center University of Washington
Australia Seattle, Washington

KENNETH E. TOWBIN, MD AMY M. WETHERBY, PHD


Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program Department of Communication Disorders
National Institute of Mental Health Executive Director, Center for Autism and
Bethesda, Maryland Related Disorders
Florida State University
KATHERINE D. TSATSANIS, PHD Tallahassee, Florida
Child Study Center
Yale University School of Medicine SALLY WHEELWRIGHT, MA
New Haven, Connecticut Autism Research Centre
University of Cambridge
BELGIN TUNALI-KOTOSKI, PHD Cambridge, England
Center for Human Development Research
University of Texas Health Sciences Center LORNA WING, MD
at Houston National Autistic Society Centre for Social
Houston, Texas and Communication Disorders
Bromley, Kent, United Kingdom
SAM TYANO
The Geha Psychiatric Hospital DIANNE ZAGER, PHD
The Beilinson Medical Center Pace University
Tel Aviv University Medical School New York, New York
Tel Aviv, Israel
Editorial Board

MIRIAM BERKMAN, JD, MSW CHRISTOPHER GILLBERG, MD, PHD


Child Study Center Department of Child and Adolescent
Yale University School of Medicine Psychiatry
New Haven, Connecticut Göteborg University
Göteborg, Sweden
ALICE S. CARTER, PHD
Department of Psychology LYNN KERN KOEGEL, PHD
University of Massachusetts Boston Autism Research and Training Center
Boston, Massachusetts University of California
Santa Barbara, California
EDWIN COOK JR., MD
Department of Psychiatry
KATHERINE A. LOVELAND, PHD
University of Chicago
Mental Science Institute
Chicago, Illinois
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences
PETER DOEHRING, PHD University of Texas Health Sciences Center
Delaware Autism Program Houston, Texas
Newark, Deleware
GARY MESIBOV, PHD
ELISABETH M. DYKENS, PHD
Division TEACCH
Kennedy Center for Research on Human
University of North Carolina
Development
Chapel Hill, North Carolina
Vanderbilt University
Nashville, Tennesse
MICHAEL D. POWERS, PSYD
B. J. FREEMAN, PHD The Center for Children with Special Needs
Department of Psychiatry Glastonbury, Connecticut and
University of California Yale Child Study Center
Los Angeles, California New Haven, Connecticut

ERIC FOMBONNE, MD PATRICIA A. PRELOCK, PHD, CCC-SLP


Department of Psychiatry Department of Communication Sciences
McGill University University of Vermont
Montreal, Canada Burlington, Vermont

xiii
xiv Editorial Board

SALLY J. ROGERS, PHD MARIAN SIGMAN, PHD


The M.I.N.D. Institute Center for Autism Research and Treatment
University of California Davis Medical Center UCLA School of Medicine
Sacramento, California Los Angeles, California

LAURA SCHREIBMAN, PHD MATTHEW STATE, MD, PHD


Department of Psychology Child Study Center
University of California Yale University School of Medicine
San Diego, California New Haven, Connecticut

ROBERT SCHULTZ, PHD PETER SZATMARI, MD


Child Study Center Department of Psychiatry
Yale University School of Medicine McMaster University
New Haven, Connecticut Hamilton, Ontario

BRYNA SIEGEL, PHD LARRY WOOD


Director, Autism Clinic Benhaven
Children’s Center at Langley Porter East Haven, Connecticut
University of California
San Francisco, California
Preface

A comprehensive Handbook devoted to autism that allow a child to become a family member
and pervasive developmental disorders testifies and social being.
to the volume of research, services, theory, This Handbook is guided by a developmen-
and advocacy related to children and adults tal psychopathological orientation (Cicchetti
with the most severe disorders of development. & Cohen, 1995). Within this framework, prin-
Indeed, the third edition of this work is now ciples and findings about normal development
literally two books. The expansion in size and are used to illuminate how development may
sophistication reflects substantial advances in become derailed and lead to pathological con-
knowledge during the one decade that sepa- ditions, and, conversely, studies of disorders
rates it from its predecessor published in 1997. such as autism are used to cast light on normal
Autism has attracted remarkable interest developmental processes. Autism and similar
and concern of clinicians and researchers from developmental disorders may serve as “experi-
the time of its first scientific description over ments of nature.” Their underlying biology and
60 years ago by Leo Kanner (1943). As a disor- psychology, as well as the types of adaptations
der that afflicts the core of socialization, it that individuals can use to compensate for
has posed scientific challenges to theories of their difficulties, may reveal mechanisms and
developmental psychology and neurobiology as processes that are otherwise concealed from
well as therapy and education. Virtually every awareness or scientific scrutiny.
type of theory relating to child development— As a serious, generally lifelong condition,
cognitive, social, behavioral, affective, neuro- autism has generated important challenges to
biological—has been applied to understanding the systems that relate to individuals with dis-
the enigmatic impairments and competencies abilities, including educational, vocational,
of autistic individuals. And the results of em- medical, and psychiatric systems, as well as to
pirical studies inspired by these diverse theo- social policy, legislation, and the legal systems.
retical perspectives have enriched not only the Because of its multifaceted impact on develop-
field of autism but also the broad field of de- ment, autism also has focused the attention of
velopmental psychopathology. Indeed, autism all the professions concerned with children and
has served as a paradigmatic disorder for adults with difficulties, including psychology,
theory testing and research on the essential education, psychiatry, physical rehabilitation,
preconditions for normal social-cognitive mat- recreational therapy, speech and language,
uration—expression and recognition of emo- nursing, pediatrics, neurology, occupational
tions, intersubjectivity, sharing a focus of therapy, genetics, social work, law, neuroradi-
interest with other people, the meaning and ology, pharmacology—indeed, virtually every
uses of language, forming first attachments caring profession. By drawing these disci-
and falling in love, empathy, the nuanced un- plines together in the clinic and laboratory,
derstanding of the minds of others—indeed, autism has helped forge the multidisciplinary
the whole set of competencies and motivations approach to developmental disabilities. One

xv
xvi Preface

goal of this Handbook is to provide an orienta- family is experienced by parents, siblings, and
tion of shared concepts and knowledge to fa- extended family as profoundly painful. There
cilitate the future collaboration among the can, of course, be consolations in dealing well
disciplines and professionals who work with with adversity; yet, however well a family and
autistic individuals and their families. individual cope, a lifetime with autism brings
Nothing strikes more at the core of a fam- with it more than a fair share of disappoint-
ily’s functioning than the birth of a child with ment, sadness, and emotional scarring for all
a serious disability. Kanner recognized the involved. Only with scientific advances that
central involvement of families in his first re- will prevent, greatly ameliorate, or even cure
ports when he described the peculiarities of these conditions will this pain be fully eased.
social relations in families who came for his Clinicians and researchers have been drawn to
consultation and care. In his first accounts, he autism in the hope of achieving this result, and
misread the data presented to him and postu- their remarkable commitments are also re-
lated an etiologic role of parental behavior in flected in this Handbook and in services
the pathogenesis of autism. This mistake throughout the world.
haunted the field and pained families for many At times, however, therapeutic zeal has ex-
years; it still may arise in certain places, as ceeded the knowledge available. The Hand-
ghosts tend to do. However, Kanner soon book aims at providing authentic knowledge,
righted his theory and emphasized the central broadly accepted by experts. Yet, we recog-
message of his initial report that autism is es- nize that there are sometimes sharp differ-
sentially a reflection of an inborn dysfunction ences of opinion and theoretical perspective
underlying affective engagement. Because so- and that today’s wisdom may be tomorrow’s
cial interaction is a two-way street, parents delusion. Thus, it is important to foster diver-
and others who spend time with an autistic sity while encouraging everyone to pursue rig-
child will no doubt relate differently than with orous, empirical research that will improve
his or her socially engaged, ebullient, linguis- future treatments. Scientific progress oddly
tically gifted siblings. Of interest, more recent leads to many divergent ideas and findings for
genetic information about autism and As- a long time before a deeper level of clarity is
perger syndrome, discussed in the Handbook, achieved.
returns us to Kanner’s observations about so- While we encourage tolerance of differing
cial variations and impairments running scientific views, we do not think that “any-
within families. New findings of aggregation thing goes.” Virtually every month or two,
of autism, cognitive problems, and social dif- parents and others who care for autistic chil-
ficulties within families suggest that an under- dren and adults are likely to hear announce-
lying vulnerability may be transmitted from ments of new, miraculous treatments. They
one generation to the next. If so, explicating may be confused by the options and feel guilty
the interaction between genetic and environ- for not making the sacrifices necessary to try
mental factors in the course of these disorders still another approach. Today, within a stone’s
will bring us back to questions not too far from throw of our own university, parents are en-
where Kanner started his speculations. gaged in a medley of divergent treatments. As
The impact of autistic individuals on family the recent review by the National Research
life has changed with the creation of more ade- Council (2001) has shown, a variety of treat-
quate services. Burdens on families have been ments have now been shown to be effective for
eased by early identification, initiation of edu- individuals with autism. The efficacy of a host
cational and other treatments during the first of other treatments, commonly referred to as
years of life, suitable family guidance and sup- complementary or alternative treatments, re-
port, high-quality educational and other pro- mains to be scientifically well established.
grams, respite care, supportive living and other Often, such treatments compete with more tra-
arrangements for adults with autism, effective ditional ones. Parents, and sometimes profes-
pharmacological treatments, and knowledge sionals, may feel at a loss in terms of
that can guide lifetime planning. Yet, with per- evaluating such treatments and making sound,
haps rare exception, an autistic child in the empirically based decisions about which treat-
Preface xvii

ment(s) should be pursued with respect to an a major impact on the care and treatment of in-
individual child. Occasionally, differences be- dividuals with autism, as well. Far more than
tween advocates and skeptics in relation to most experts believed possible 20 or even 10
treatment ethics and efficacy arouse passions, years ago, many individuals with autism have
including legal proceedings and splits between not only the right but also the capacities to par-
professionals or within the family. How are ticipate within their communities—to study,
parents and professionals best able to make in- work, live, recreate, and share in family life.
formed decisions? The Handbook reflects this important educa-
Like other areas of science, the field of tional and cultural evolution in which a philos-
autism will advance when we adopt, whenever ophy of despair has given way to one of hope.
possible, the rigorous standards of scientific We also appreciate that there are enormous
research. Indeed, our own work as clinician- differences among individuals with autism
researchers has led us to the conclusion that and related conditions in their abilities and
we should offer no less. Thus, in the Handbook needs, among families in their strengths and
we have attempted to provide a comprehensive resources, and among communities and na-
account of current, scientific thinking and tions in their own viewpoints and histories.
findings and to mark out speculation and the- These differences should be respected, and
ory for what these are. We also have eschewed policy and discussion should recognize that
accounts of ideas and treatments, however fas- “autistic people” do not form a homogeneous
cinating they might be, that are too far from class. Clinicians and practitioners generally
the mainstream of scientific research and em- are able to keep the individual at the focus of
pirically guided practice. Such decisions are concern, as we do when we think together with
our responsibility and may leave some advo- families about their unique child or with an
cates feeling shortchanged or even angry; they adult with autism about his or her special life
retain their right to free speech and, who situation. At such times, broader issues of so-
knows, may yet be vindicated. cial policy recede into the background as the
In underlining the importance of data in fullness of the individual’s needs and interests
guiding decisions about treatment, we also rec- are paramount. In shaping social policy and
ognize that clinical care always occurs within planning regional and national systems, how-
a social context and is shaped by beliefs, val- ever, there is a clear consensus for the ap-
ues, and other historical and cultural values. proach to treatment and lifetime planning
Prevailing views about the rights of individu- captured by the ideology of autonomy and
als with disabilities and their role in society community-based living and working. We hope
have changed dramatically over the past that this orientation is conveyed by this Hand-
decades. Embodied in legislation and judicial book. At the same time, there is no single, right
decision, the emergent viewpoints about rights formula for every child or adult with autism: A
to education, services, access, job opportuni- community and nation should strive to have
ties—to basic human respect—have shaped available a spectrum of services to satisfy the
services and improved the quality of the lives varied and changing needs and values of indi-
of individuals who would only decades ago viduals with autism and their families.
have been subject to abuses of various types Clearly defined concepts are essential for
that limited freedom, stigmatized, or dehu- communication among scientists, especially
manized. We have been delighted to see this for interdisciplinary and international collabo-
view gaining increasingly wide acceptance ration. In the field of autism and other behav-
around the world. ioral disorders, there has been substantial
Parents and individuals with disabilities progress in nosology and diagnosis. This prog-
have been effective advocates. Communities ress has enhanced discussion, research, and
and professionals have been sensitized to the cross-disciplinary exchange. It had the merit of
subtle ways in which individuals with disabili- underlining the concept of developmental dis-
ties may be deprived of autonomy and are order and the breadth of dysfunctions in social,
made to be more handicapped by lack of provi- cognitive, language, and other domains. Simi-
sion for their special needs. This trend has had larly, the introduction of multiaxial diagnosis
xviii Preface

underscored the need for patients to be seen the Handbook began in 2000 with an expansion
from varied points of view and the need to of the number of editors in light of the increas-
supplement “categorical disorders” (e.g., ingly diverse and sophisticated body of re-
autism) with knowledge about other aspects of search that was becoming available.
functioning, including medical status and In this edition, we have retained the best
adaptive abilities. As we discuss in the first features of the second edition with expanded
section of this Handbook, advances in classifi- coverage in selected areas. In many instances,
cation have led new knowledge and increas- authors have kindly revised earlier contribu-
ingly focused and refined research. The tions in light of current research; in other
consensus exemplified in Diagnostic and Sta- cases, we have solicited new contributors and
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth chapters. As a result of the expanded coverage,
edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Asso- the book has expanded into two volumes with a
ciation, 1994), and International Classifica- total of nine sections. This more extensive
tion of Diseases, 10th edition, (ICD-10; World coverage reflects the increasing depth and
Health Organization, 1992), has stimulated a breadth of work within the field.
tremendous increase in research over the past In creating this Handbook, we invited chap-
decade. Today the two internationally recog- ters from recognized scholars. The responses
nized systems provide a consistent approach to to the invitations were gratifying. Each com-
the diagnosis of the most severe disorders of pleted chapter was reviewed by the editors and
early onset. While there are still some regional by two members of a distinguished editorial
or national diagnostic alternatives, the trend committee. The use of peer review is not typi-
is, fortunately, toward consensus. At the same cal for volumes such as this, and we are grate-
time, the universal acceptance of a standard ful that all authors of chapters welcomed this
meter and of Greenwich time does not ensure process. The reviewers wrote careful critiques,
great science or lack of debate and much work sometimes many pages in length; these reviews
remains to be done, but the current approach were provided to the authors for their consider-
has helped provide a solid framework on which ation during revision. The interactive process
future refinements can sensibly be made. of revising chapters has helped ensure that the
The thousands of publications—scientific contributions are as good as the field allows.
papers, monographs, chapters, books—about The past several years have seen a major in-
autism and pervasive developmental disorder crease in the funding of research on autism.
are evidence of its intrinsic interest to re- While we are gratified by this increased sup-
searchers and clinicians and to the human im- port, we hope for even more because only
portance of these disorders for those who through research will we be able to change in-
suffer from them and their families. The grow- cidence and alter the natural history of autistic
ing body of books and resources specifically and other pervasive disorders. The cost of car-
designed for parents and family members has ing for one autistic individual over a lifetime
been a noteworthy achievement of the past sev- may be more than any single investigator will
eral years. At the same time, you could reason- ever have to spend during a career of research.
ably ask why a revision of the Handbook is Many hundreds of millions of dollars are spent
needed now. internationally on direct services; only a tiny
This third edition of the Handbook of percentage of this expenditure is devoted to
Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders any type of formal research. It is as if the
is the second revision of a book that first ap- United States committed all of its funding to
peared in 1987. This edition quickly became building iron lungs and considered virology to
established as an important scholarly resource. be a secondary concern in relation to polio. To
Within a decade much had changed, and the fully exploit the many new methods for study-
second edition of this volume appeared. The ing brain development and brain-behavior rela-
rapid pace of scientific progress was reflected tions and to attempt to translate biological and
in the second edition, which was expanded to behavioral research findings into treatments
increase coverage of new research and treat- will require substantial investment of research
ment methods. Preparations for this version of funds. The recent network of federal centers
Preface xix

through the Collaborative Program of Excel- A Handbook portrays what is known and re-
lence in Autism (CPEA) and the Studies to veals what is poorly understood. Although
Advance Autism Research and Treatment many studies have been conducted and areas
(STAART) as well as through the Research explored, there is no hard biological or behav-
Units on Psychopharmacology (RUPP) and the ioral finding that can serve as a reliable com-
Centers for Disease Control (CDC) have al- pass point to guide research; in spite of great
ready had major benefits. These benefits will efforts and decades of commitment by re-
eventually include not only a reduction in suf- searchers and clinicians, the fate of many
fering and in costs for those with autism, but autistic individuals remains cloudy; and even
also important knowledge that will benefit a with new knowledge, there are still too many
far larger group of children and adults with areas of controversy. That investigators and
other serious neuropsychiatric and develop- clinicians, working alongside families and ad-
mental disorders. We hope that one contribu- vocates, have learned so much, often with very
tion of the Handbook will be to underscore the tight resources, speaks to their commitment to
gains from systematic research and the impor- understanding and caring for autistic children
tance of sustained support for multidiscipli- and adults. The goal of this Handbook is to
nary clinical research groups. document their achievements and inspire their
We wish to recognize the support that has future efforts.
been provided over the decades to our own clin- FRED R. VOLKMAR, MD
ical and research program by the National Insti- AMI KLIN, PHD
tute of Child Health and Human Development, RHEA PAUL, PHD
National Institute of Deafness and Communica- Yale Child Study Center
tion Disorders, and the National Institute of New Haven, Connecticut
Mental Health, as well as by the Korczak Foun- November, 2004
dation, the W. T. Grant Foundation, the Doris
Duke Foundation, the Simon’s Foundation, REFERENCES
Cure Autism Now, the National Alliance for American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag-
Autism Research, and private donors. nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
We thank the members of our editorial ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
board for their excellent contributions to this American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
process and Lori Klein, who helped us coor- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
dinate this effort, as well as the wonderful ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
editorial staff at Wiley, who have consis- Cohen, D. J., & Donnellan, A. M. (1987). Hand-
tently sought to help us deliver the best possi- book of Autism and Pervasive Developmental
ble work. We have been very fortunate in Disorders. New York: Wiley.
Cicchetti D., & Cohen D. J. (1995). Developmental
being able to work within the scholarly envi-
Psychopathology. (Vols. 1–2). New York: Wiley.
ronment provided by the Yale School of Medi-
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec-
cine and the Child Study Center. The unique tive contact. Nervous Child 2, 217–250.
qualities of the Child Study Center reflect the Volkmar, F., Klin, A., Siegel, B., et al. (1994). Field
contributions of generations of faculty who trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American
have committed themselves to clinical schol- Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367.
arship, teaching, and service. We particularly World Health Organization. (1977). Manual of the
wish to acknowledge the guidance and sup- international statistical classification of dis-
port of senior mentors—Albert J. Solnit, eases, injuries and causes of death (9th ed.,
Sally Provence, Sam Ritvo, Sara Sparrow, Vol. 1). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
and Edward Zigler—as well as many col- World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10
classification of mental and behavioral disor-
leagues and collaborators in this work, in-
ders. Clinical descriptions and diagnostic
cluding Robert Schultz, Cheryl Klaiman, guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Larry Scahill, Matt State, Elenga Grigorenko, World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10
George Anderson, James Leckman, Kasia classification of mental and behavioral disor-
Chawarska, Katherine Tsatsanis, Wendy ders. Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva,
Marans, and Emily Rubin. Switzerland: Author.
Contents

VOLUME 1: DIAGNOSIS, DEVELOPMENT,


NEUROBIOLOGY, AND BEHAVIOR

SECTION I
DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION

Chapter 1. Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 5


Fred R. Volkmar and Ami Klin

Chapter 2. Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 42


Eric Fombonne

Chapter 3. Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 70


Fred R. Volkmar, Kathy Koenig, and Matthew State

Chapter 4. Asperger Syndrome 88


Ami Klin, James McPartland, and Fred R. Volkmar

Chapter 5. Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 126


Richard Van Acker, Jennifer A. Loncola, and Eryn Y. Van Acker

Chapter 6. Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 165


Kenneth E. Towbin

Chapter 7. Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 201


Patricia Howlin

SECTION II
DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR

Chapter 8. Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 223


Katarzyna Chawarska and Fred R. Volkmar

Chapter 9. The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 247


Katherine A. Loveland and Belgin Tunali-Kotoski

xxi
xxii Contents

Chapter 10. Adolescents and Adults with Autism 288


Victoria Shea and Gary B. Mesibov

Chapter 11. Social Development in Autism 312


Alice S. Carter, Naomi Ornstein Davis, Ami Klin, and Fred R. Volkmar

Chapter 12. Language and Communication in Autism 335


Helen Tager-Flusberg, Rhea Paul, and Catherine Lord

Chapter 13. Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 365


Katherine D. Tsatsanis

Chapter 14. Imitation and Play in Autism 382


Sally J. Rogers, Ian Cook, and Adrienne Meryl

Chapter 15. Autism and Emotion 406


Peter Hobson

SECTION III
NEUROLOGICAL AND MEDICAL ISSUES

Chapter 16. Genetic Influences and Autism 425


Michael Rutter

Chapter 17. Neurochemical Studies of Autism 453


George M. Anderson and Yoshihiko Hoshino

Chapter 18. Neurologic Aspects of Autism 473


Nancy J. Minshew, John A. Sweeney, Margaret L . Bauman,
and Sara Jane Webb

Chapter 19. Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 515


Robert T. Schultz and Diana L . Robins

Chapter 20. Medical Aspects of Autism 534


Pauline A. Filipek

SECTION IV
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES

Chapter 21. Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 583


Lorna Wing

Chapter 22. Executive Functions 606


Sally Ozonof f, Mikle South, and Sherri Provencal

Chapter 23. Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 628


Simon Baron-Cohen, Sally Wheelwright, John Lawson, Richard Grif fin,
Chris Ashwin, Jac Billington, and Bhismadev Chakrabarti
Contents xxiii

Chapter 24. The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 640


Francesca Happé

Chapter 25. Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 650


Peter Mundy and Courtney Burnette

Chapter 26. The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 682
Ami Klin, Warren Jones, Robert T. Schultz, and Fred R. Volkmar

Author Index I•1

Subject Index I•39

VOLUME 2: ASSESSMENT, INTERVENTIONS,


AND POLICY

SECTION V
ASSESSMENT

Chapter 27. Screening for Autism in Young Children 707


Elaine E. Coonrod and Wendy L . Stone

Chapter 28. Diagnostic Instruments in Autistic Spectrum Disorders 730


Catherine Lord and Christina Corsello

Chapter 29. Clinical Evaluation in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Psychological


Assessment within a Transdisciplinary Framework 772
Ami Klin, Celine Saulnier, Katherine Tsatsanis, and Fred R. Volkmar

Chapter 30. Assessing Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders 799


Rhea Paul

Chapter 31. Behavioral Assessment of Individuals with Autism: A Functional


Ecological Approach 817
Michael D. Powers

Chapter 32. Sensory and Motor Features in Autism: Assessment and Intervention 831
Grace T. Baranek, L . Diane Parham, and James W. Bodfish

SECTION VI
INTERVENTIONS

Chapter 33. Curriculum and Classroom Structure 863


J. Gregory Olley

Chapter 34. Behavioral Interventions to Promote Learning in Individuals with Autism 882
Laura Schreibman and Brooke Ingersoll

Chapter 35. Behavioral Interventions 897


Joel D. Bregman, Dianne Zager, and John Gerdtz
xxiv Contents

Chapter 36. Critical Issues in Enhancing Communication Abilities for Persons with
Autism Spectrum Disorders 925
Barry M. Prizant and Amy M. Wetherby

Chapter 37. Enhancing Early Language in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders 946
Rhea Paul and Dean Sutherland

Chapter 38. Addressing Social Communication Skills in Individuals with


High-Functioning Autism and Asperger Syndrome: Critical Priorities
in Educational Programming 977
Wendy D. Marans, Emily Rubin, and Amy Laurent

Chapter 39. School-Based Programs 1003


Joel R. Arick, David A. Krug, Ann Fullerton, Lauren Loos, and Ruth Falco

Chapter 40. Helping Children with Autism Enter the Mainstream 1029
Jan S. Handleman, Sandra L . Harris, and Megan P. Martins

Chapter 41. Models of Educational Intervention for Students with Autism: Home, Center,
and School-Based Programming 1043
Sandra L . Harris, Jan S. Handleman, and Heather K. Jennett

Chapter 42. Working with Families 1055


Lee M. Marcus, Linda J. Kunce, and Eric Schopler

Chapter 43. Employment: Options and Issues for Adolescents and Adults with
Autism Spectrum Disorders 1087
Peter F. Gerhardt and David L . Holmes

Chapter 44. Psychopharmacology 1102


Lawrence Scahill and Andrés Martin

SECTION VII
PUBLIC POLICY PERSPECTIVES

Chapter 45. Preparation of Autism Specialists 1123


Gail G. McGee and Michael J. Morrier

Chapter 46. Educating Children with Autism: Current Legal Issues 1161
Myrna R. Mandlawitz

Chapter 47. Cross-Cultural Program Priorities and Reclassification of Outcome


Research Methods 1174
Eric Schopler

SECTION VIII
INTERNATIONAL PERSPECTIVES

Chapter 48. International Perspectives 1193


Fred R. Volkmar
Contents xxv

SECTION IX
PERSONAL PERSPECTIVES

Chapter 49. Community-Integrated Residential Services for Adults with Autism:


A Working Model (Based on a Mother’s Odyssey) 1255
Ruth Christ Sullivan

Chapter 50. A Sibling’s Perspective on Autism 1265


Jason B. Konidaris

Chapter 51. A Personal Perspective of Autism 1276


Temple Grandin

Chapter 52. A Teacher’s Perspective: Adult Outcomes 1287


Virginia Walker Sperry

Chapter 53. Autism: Where We Have Been, Where We Are Going 1304
Isabelle Rapin

Author Index I•1

Subject Index I•39


SECTION I

DIAGNOSIS AND
CLASSIFICATION

The paired processes of diagnosis and classifi- unique signs and symptoms are provided a
cation are fundamental to research and inter- context. They are given a more general mean-
vention. The diagnostic process includes all of ing. For example, the clinician will assign the
the activities in which a clinician engages in patient’s coughing and fever to the category
trying to understand the nature of an individ- pneumonia. This categorical diagnosis is
ual’s difficulty. The result of this process is placed within the narrative of the patient’s life
often a narrative account—a portrait of the in- and current problems. It may be related to the
dividual’s past, the current problems, and the patient’s family or genetic background, expe-
ways in which these problems can be related to riences, exposures, vulnerabilities, and the
each other and to possible, underlying causes. like, and it will be used to explain why the pa-
A useful diagnostic process also suggests tient has come for help and what type of treat-
methods for being helpful, including specific ment may be useful.
treatments. In the course of the diagnostic pro- The diagnostic process is based on current
cess, a clinician will learn about the patient’s knowledge, technologies, and skills; it can
history, talk to others about the patient, ob- sometimes be quite brief (as in the diagnostic
serve the patient, engage in specialized exami- processes for an earache) or remarkably exten-
nations, and use laboratory and other methods sive (as in the diagnostic process for autism).
for helping define patients’ problems and their Diagnostic classifications, also, are based on
causes. The clinician will integrate the find- available knowledge and laboratory methods;
ings from these activities, based on special- they also embody conventions, the consensus
ized, scientific knowledge. Often, a patient among clinicians and experts about a useful
will have several types of problems; the diag- way for sorting illnesses and troubles.
nostic process may lead to a narrative that New knowledge and methodologies change
links these to an underlying, common cause or the diagnostic process as well as the classifica-
may separate the problems on the basis of their tion system. The advent of methods such as mo-
differing causes or treatments. Often, more lecular genetic testing, magnetic resonance
than one clinician may be involved in the diag- imaging of the brain, and structured, formal as-
nostic process; then, the final clinical, diag- sessment of cognitive processes have changed
nostic formulation will integrate the pooled the diagnostic process and classification and
information into a coherent and consensual will continue to do so in the future.
narrative that reflects the varied information. The skillful diagnostic process, and the re-
One component of the diagnostic process is sultant account about the patient and his ill-
the assignment of the patient’s difficulties—his ness, often is broad-based, nuanced, and
or her signs, symptoms, pains, troubles, worries, individualized. The clinical formulation, the
dysfunctions, abnormal tests—to a specific full statement of findings, may capture the
class or category of illness or disorder. Through many dimensions of a person’s life, including
classification, the patient’s individualized, his or her competencies as well as specific

1
2 Diagnosis and Classification

impairments and difficulties. However, a diag- criteria were provided for PDD, but the clini-
nostic categorization—a label or classification cal description conveyed a sense of the contour
of specific troubles and their designation as a of its clinical territory. To be a citizen of this
syndrome, disorder or disease—-is delimited. territory, a child had to exhibit difficulties
Providing the label of a specific disease delim- from the first several years of life involving
its individuality for the sake of being able to several domains (social, language, emotional,
utilize general knowledge gained from scien- cognitive) and with significant impairment of
tific study and experience with others with functioning. In 1980, and again when DSM-III
similar problems. In this important respect, it was revised in 1987 (DSM-III-R), the only ex-
is useful to think that individuals are engaged ample of a specifically defined example of
in the process of diagnosis and symptoms and PDD was autism. Indeed, autism remains the
signs are classified and labeled. A diagnostic paradigm or model form of PDD. From 1980 to
label is not able or meant to capture the full- 1994, other children whose difficulties were
ness of an individual. Diagnostic classification captured by the sense of PDD, but who were
systems and specific assignment to a disease not diagnosed as having autism, were de-
or disorder category are tools, which when scribed as having “pervasive developmental
combined with other tools should lead to help- disorder that is not otherwise specified”
ful understanding and treatment. (PDD-NOS). Although not an official diagnos-
The newer methods of classification of de- tic term, the phrase autism spectrum disorder
velopmental, psychiatric, behavioral, or mental (ASD) is now in widespread use and is synony-
disorders respect the distinction between diag- mous with the term PDD.
nosing an individual and classifying his or her The 1994 edition of the Manual of Mental
problems. They are also multidimensional and Disorders (DSM-IV), based on new evidence
elicit information about other domains of the and international field testing, refined the di-
patient’s life, in addition to areas of leading agnostic criteria for autism and formalized
impairment. This approach shapes and has three new classes or types of pervasive devel-
been shaped by the two international systems opmental disorders: childhood disintegrative
of classification in which autism and perva- disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and Rett’s dis-
sive developmental disorders are included: the order. Also, a consensus was reached between
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental the two major systems, DSM and ICD, for the
Disorders of the American Psychiatric Associ- system of classification and specific diagnos-
ation and the International Statistical Classifi- tic criteria. Thus, for the first time, there is
cation of Diseases and Related Health Problems happily an internationally accepted, field-
of the World Health Organization (WHO). The tested, diagnostic system for the most severe
introductions to the recent editions of these disorders of development. The DSM-IV and
two systems (DSM-IV, American Psychiatric ICD-10 systems form the epistemological
Association, 1994; and ICD-10, WHO, 1992) backbone of this Handbook.
provide helpful overviews of the goals of clas- The chapters in this section of the Handbook
sification and the roles of diagnostic cate- describe current frameworks for classification,
gories in clinical understanding. the four forms of pervasive developmental dis-
A new diagnostic term was introduced in the orders for which specific criteria are provided
DSM-III in 1980: the concept of pervasive de- in DSM-IV, and the kinds of disturbances that
velopmental disorder (PDD). The umbrella remain within the territory of pervasive devel-
term PDD gained broad popularity among pro- opmental disorders that are not further classi-
fessionals from various disciplines as well as fied. This section also provides a review of
with parents and advocates. Without a previous studies of natural history and outcome.
history in psychiatry, psychology, or neurology, It is our expectation that advances in under-
the novel term PDD had the advantage of not standing the pathogenesis of pervasive develop-
carrying excessive theoretical baggage or con- mental disorders will continue to have a major
troversy. It also had a broad inter-disciplinary impact on the diagnostic and classification
appeal and a nice emphasis on development and processes. Thus, in any discussion about diag-
disorders of development. No specific diagnostic nosis and nosology, it is important to recognize
Diagnosis and Classification 3

their provisional nature. Advances in knowl- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
edge may lead to changes in diagnostic ap- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
proaches. It is also critical to remember the ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
importance of balancing categorical ap- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
proaches to diagnosis with a fuller understand- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
ing of the many dimensions of individual
World Health Organization. (1992). International
children and adults, that is, as whole people. classification of diseases (10th ed.). Geneva,
Switzerland: Author.
REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag-


nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
CHAPTER 1

Issues in the Classification of Autism and


Related Conditions

FRED R. VOLKMAR AND AMI KLIN

Clinicians and researchers have achieved con- et al., 2000; Piven, Palmer, Jacobi, Childress,
sensus on the validity of autism as a diagnostic & Arndt, 1997; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey,
category and the many features central to its Schultz, & Klin, 2004).
definition (Rutter, 1996). This has made pos- Today, autism is probably the complex psy-
sible the convergence of the two major diag- chiatric or developmental disorder with the
nostic systems: the fourth edition of the best empirically based, cross-national diag-
American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnos- nostic criteria. Data from a number of re-
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders search groups from around the world have
(DSM-IV, 1994) and the 10th edition of the In- confirmed the usefulness of current diagnostic
ternational Classification of Diseases (ICD- approaches, and, even more importantly, the
10; World Health Organization [WHO], 1992). availability of a shared clinical concept and
Although some differences remain, these language for differential diagnosis is a great
major diagnostic systems have become much asset for clear communication among clini-
more alike than different; this has facilitated cians, researchers, and advocates alike (Buite-
the development of diagnostic assessments laar, Van der Gaag, Klin, & Volkmar, 1999;
“ keyed” to broadly accepted, internationally Magnusson & Saemundsen, 2001; Sponheim,
recognized guidelines (Rutter, Le Couteur, & 1996; Sponheim & Skjeldal, 1998). In the fu-
Lord, 2003; see Chapter 28, this Handbook, ture, the discovery of biological correlates,
Volume 2). It is somewhat surprising that, as causes, and pathogenic pathways will, no
greater consensus has been achieved on the doubt, change the ways in which autism is
definition of strictly defined autism, an inter- diagnosed and may well lead to new nosologi-
esting and helpful discussion on issues of cal approaches that, in turn, will facilitate fur-
“ broader phenotype” or potential variants of ther scientific progress (Rutter, 2000).
autism has begun (Bailey, Palferman, Heavey, Simultaneously, considerable progress has
& Le Couteur, 1998; Dawson et al., 2002; been made on understanding the broader range
Pickles, Starr, Kazak, Bolton, Papanikolaou, of difficulties included within the autism

The authors acknowledge the support of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(CPEA program project grant 1PO1HD3548201, grant 5-P01-HD03008, and grant R01-HD042127-02), the
National Institute of Mental Health (STAART grant U54-MH066494), the Yale Children’s Clinical Re-
search Center, and of the National Alliance of Autism Research, Cure Autism Now, and the Doris Duke
Foundation as well as the Simons Foundation. We also gratefully acknowledge the helpful comments of Pro-
fessor Michael Rutter on an earlier version of this manuscript.

5
6 Diagnosis and Classification

spectrum; that is, as our knowledge of autism DEVELOPMENT OF AUTISM AS A


has advanced, so has our understanding of DIAGNOSTIC CONCEPT
a broader range of conditions with some
similarities to it. Table 1.1 lists categories of Although children with what we now would
pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs) as describe as autism had probably been de-
classified by ICD-10 and DSM-IV. scribed much earlier as so called wild or feral
In addition to the international and cross- children (Candland, 1993; Simon, 1978) it was
disciplinary agreement about diagnostic crite- Leo Kanner who first elaborated what today
ria for autism, a consensus has emerged about would be termed the syndrome of childhood
other issues that were once debated. Today, autism.
there is broad agreement that autism is a
developmental disorder, that autism and asso- Kanner’s Description—Early
ciated disorders represent the behavioral Controversies
manifestations of underlying dysfunctions in
the functioning of the central nervous system, Kanner’s (1943) seminal clinical description
and that sustained educational and behavioral of 11 children with “autistic disturbances of
interventions are useful and constitute the affective contact ” has endured in many ways.
core of treatment (National Research Coun- His description of the children was grounded
cil, 2001). in data and theory of child development, par-
In this chapter, we summarize the develop- ticularly the work of Gesell, who demon-
ment of current diagnostic concepts with a strated that normal infants exhibit marked
particular focus on autism and on the empiri- interest in social interaction from early in life.
cal basis for its current official definition. We Kanner suggested that early infantile autism
address the rationale for inclusion of other was an inborn, constitutional disorder in which
nonautistic PDDs/autism spectrum disorders children were born lacking the typical motiva-
(ASDs), which are discussed in detail in other tion for social interaction and affective com-
chapters in this section. We also note areas in ments. Using the model of inborn errors of
which knowledge is lacking, such as the rela- metabolism, Kanner felt that individuals with
tionships of autism to other comorbid condi- autism were born without the biological pre-
tions and the ongoing efforts to provide conditions for psychologically metabolizing
alternative approaches to subtyping these the social world. He used the word autism to
conditions. convey this self-contained quality. The term

TABLE 1.1 Conditions Currently Classified as Pervasive Developmental Disorders Correspondence


of ICD-10 and DSM-IV Categories
ICD-10 DSM-IV
Childhood autism Autistic disorder
Atypical autism Pevasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
(PDD-NOS)
Rett syndrome Rett’s disorder
Other childhood disintegrative disorder Childhood disintegrative disorder
Overactive disorder with mental retardation No corresponding category with stereotyped movements
Asperger syndrome Asperger’s disorder
Other pervasive developmental disorder PDD-NOS
Pervasive developmental disorder, unspecified PDD-NOS
Sources: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, by American Psychiatric Associa-
tion, 1994, Washington, DC: Author; and International Classification of Diseases: Diagnostic Criteria for Research,
tenth edition, by the World Health Organization, 1992, Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 7

was borrowed from Bleuler (1911/1950), who children were born long after the theory was
used autism to describe idiosyncratic, self- dead; unfortunately, this notion still prevails in
centered thinking. Autism for Kanner was in- some countries.
tended to suggest that autistic children, too, Two types of information went against the
live in their own world. Yet, the autism of indi- psychogenic theories. It is now known that
viduals with autism is distinct from that of children with autism are found in families
schizophrenia: It represents a failure of devel- from all social classes if studies control for
opment, not a regression, and fantasy is impov- possible factors that might bias case ascertain-
erished if present at all. The sharing of the ment (e.g., Wing, 1980); while additional data
term increased early confusion about the rela- on this topic are needed, more recent and rig-
tionship of the conditions. orous research has failed to demonstrate asso-
In addition to the remarkable social failure ciations with social class (see Chapter 2, this
of autistic individuals, Kanner observed other Handbook, this volume, for a review). A more
unusual features in the clinical histories of the central issue relevant to psychogenic etiology
children. Kanner described the profound dis- concerns the unusual patterns of interaction
turbances in communication. In the original that children with autism and related condi-
cohort, three of the children were mute. The tions have with their parents (and other people
language of the others was marked by as well). The interactional problems of autistic
echolalia and literalness, as well as a fascinat- individuals clearly can be seen to arise from
ing difficulty with acquiring the use of the the side of the child and not the parents
first person, personal pronoun (“I”), and refer- (Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, 1986)
ral to self in the third person (“ he” or by first although parents may be at risk for various
name). Another intriguing feature was the problems (see Chapter 15, this Handbook, this
children’s unusual responses to the inanimate volume). Probably most important, data sup-
environment; for example, a child might be un- port the role of dysfunction in basic brain sys-
responsive to parents, yet overly sensitive to tems in the pathogenesis of the disorder (see
sounds or to small changes in daily routine. Volkmar et al., 2004). Today, the data appear
While Kanner’s brilliant clinical accounts to support the concept that biological factors,
of the unusual social isolation, resistance to particularly genetic ones, convey a vulnerabil-
change, and dysfunction in communication ity to autism; as Rutter (1999) has noted, the
have stood the test of time, other aspects of the issue of interaction between genetic and envi-
original report have been refined or refuted by ronmental vulnerabilities of all types remains
further research. an important one relevant to a host of disorders
A contentious issue early in the history of in addition to autism.
autism research concerned the role of parents Kanner speculated that autism was not re-
in pathogenesis. Kanner observed that parents lated to other medical conditions. Subsequent
of the initial cases were often remarkably suc- research has shown that various medical con-
cessful educationally or professionally; he also ditions can be associated with autism (see
appreciated that there were major problems in Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume) and,
the relations between these parents and their most importantly, that approximately 25% of
child. In his initial paper, he indicated that he persons with autism develop a seizure disorder
believed autism to be congenital, but the issue (Rutter, 1970; Volkmar & Nelson, 1990; see
of potential psychological factors in causing also Chapters 18 & 20, this Handbook, this
autism was taken up by a number of individu- volume). With the recognition of the preva-
als; this issue plagued the history of the field lence of medical problems, some investigators
for many years. From the 1960s, however, it proposed a distinction between “primary” and
has been recognized that parental behavior as “secondary” autism depending on whether as-
such played no role in pathogenesis. Yet, the sociated medical conditions, for example, con-
pain of parents having been blamed for a genital rubella (Chess, Fernandez, & Korn,
child’s devastating disorder tended to linger in 1978), could be demonstrated. As time went
the memories of families, even those whose on, it became apparent that, in some basic
8 Diagnosis and Classification

sense, all cases were “organic,” and designa- Other Diagnostic Concepts
tions such as primary and secondary autism
are no longer generally made. In contrast to autism, the definition of autistic-
Kanner also misconstrued the relation be- like conditions remains in need of more clarifi-
tween autism and intellectual disability. His cation (Rutter, 1996; Szatmari, 2000; Szat-
first cases were attractive youngsters without mari, Volkmar, & Walther, 1995). Although
unusual physical features, who performed well the available research is less extensive than
on some parts of IQ tests (particularly those that on autism, several of these autistic-like
that test rote memory and copying, such as conditions were well enough studied, broadly
block design, rather than comprehension of ab- recognized, and clinically important enough to
stract, verbal concepts). Kanner felt that autis- be included in DSM-IV and ICD-10. We antic-
tic children were not mentally retarded, and he, ipate that further studies will improve the def-
and many psychologists after him, invoked mo- inition of these conditions and that new
tivational factors to explain poor performance. disorders may well be delineated within the
Autistic individuals were called “ functionally broad and heterogeneous class of PDD.
retarded.” Decades of research have now shown Diagnostic concepts with similarities to
that when developmentally appropriate tests are autism were proposed before and after Kan-
given in their entirety, full-scale intelligence ner’s clinical research. Shortly after the turn
and developmental scores (IQ and DQ scores) of the century, Heller, a special educator in
are in the mentally retarded range for the ma- Vienna, described an unusual condition in
jority of individuals with autism (Rutter, Bai- which children appeared normal for a few
ley, Bolton, & Le Couter, 1994) and maintain years and then suffered a profound regression
stability over time (Lockyer & Rutter, 1969, in their functioning and a derailment of future
1970). Kanner’s impression of potentially nor- development (Heller, 1908). This condition
mal intelligence, even in the face of apparent was originally known as dementia infantilis or
retardation, was based on what has proven to disintegrative psychosis; it now has official
be a consistent finding on psychological test- status in DSM-IV as childhood disintegrative
ing. Children with autism often have unusu- disorder (see Chapter 3, this Handbook, this
ally scattered abilities, with nonverbal skills volume). Similarly, the year after Kanner’s
often significantly advanced over more ver- original paper, Hans Asperger, a young physi-
bally mediated ones (see Chapter 29, this cian in Vienna, proposed the concept of autis-
Handbook, Volume 2); at the same time, chil- tic psychopathy or, as it is now known,
dren with autism differ in their pattern of Asperger’s disorder (Asperger, 1944; see
behavior and cognitive development from chil- Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume). Al-
dren with severe language disorders (Bartak, though Asperger apparently was not aware of
Rutter, & Cox, 1977). On the other hand, Kanner’s paper or his use of the word autism,
when the focus shifts from autism, strictly de- Asperger used this same term in his descrip-
fined, to the broader autistic spectrum, a tion of the marked social problems in a group
much broader range of IQ scores is observed of boys he had worked with. Asperger’s con-
(Bailey et al., 1998). cept was not widely recognized for many
The severity of the autistic syndrome led years, but it has recently received much
some clinicians in the 1950s to speculate that greater attention and is now included in both
autism was the earliest form of schizophrenia DSM-IV and ICD-10. Another clinician, An-
(Bender, 1946). Clinicians during the first dreas Rett, observed an unusual developmental
decades of the study of autism tended to at- disorder in girls (Rett, 1966) characterized by
tribute complex mental phenomena such as hal- a short period of normal development and then
lucinations and delusions to children who were, a multifaceted form of intellectual and motor
and remained, entirely mute (Volkmar & Cohen, deterioration. Rett’s disorder is also now offi-
1991a). In the 1970s, research findings began to cially included in the PDD class (see Chapter 5,
show that these two conditions are quite dis- this Handbook, this volume).
parate in terms of onset patterns, course, and The descriptions proposed by some other
family genetics (Kolvin, 1971; Rutter, 1972). clinicians have not fared as well. For example,
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 9

Mahler, a child psychoanalyst, proposed the studies, to share knowledge among investiga-
concept of symbiotic psychosis (Mahler, 1952) tors, and to encourage the development of a
for children who seemed to fail in the task of body of knowledge. For clinicians and educa-
separating their psychological selves from the tors, classification helps guide selection of
hypothesized early fusion with their mothers. treatments for an individual and the evaluation
This concept now has only historical interest, of the benefits of an intervention for groups of
as does her view of a “normal autistic phase” individuals with shared problems (Cantwell,
of development. In contrast, Rank (1949), also 1996). For the legal system, government regu-
working from the framework of psychoanaly- lation, insurance programs, and advocates,
sis, suggested that there is a spectrum of dys- classification systems define individuals with
functions in early development that affects special entitlements. If a diagnostic classifica-
children’s social relations and their modula- tion system is to be effective in these varied
tion of anxiety. Her detailed descriptions of domains, the system must be clear, broadly ac-
atypical personality development are of con- cepted, and relatively easy to use. Diagnostic
tinuing interest in relation to the large number stability is an important goal; difficulties
of children with serious, early-onset distur- arise if diagnostic systems are changed too
bances in development who are not autistic. rapidly, for example, interpretation of previous
These ideas were developed by Provence in her research becomes a problem. A classification
studies of young children with atypical devel- system should provide descriptions that allow
opment (Provence & Dahl, 1987; see also disorders to be differentiated from one another
Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). in significant ways, for example, in course or
In the first (1952) and second (1968) edi- associated features (Rutter, 1996). Official
tions of the American Psychiatric Associa- classification systems must be applicable to
tion’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals only conditions that afflict individuals of both
the term childhood schizophrenia was offi- sexes and of different ages; at different devel-
cially available to describe autistic children. opmental levels; and from different ethnic, so-
Much of the early work on autism and related cial, and geographical backgrounds. Finally, a
conditions is, therefore, difficult to interpret system must be logically consistent and com-
because it is unclear exactly what was being prehensive (Rutter & Gould, 1985). Achieving
studied. As information on life course and these divergent goals is not always easy (Volk-
family history became available (Kolvin, mar & Schwab-Stone, 1996).
1971; Rutter, 1970), it became clear that The clinical provision of a diagnosis or mul-
autism could not simply be considered an early tiple diagnoses is only one part of the diagnos-
form of schizophrenia, that most autistic indi- tic process (Cohen, 1976). The diagnostic
viduals were retarded, that the final behavioral process provides a richer description of a child
expression of the autistic syndrome was poten- or adult as a full person; it includes a historical
tially the result of several factors, and that the account of the origins of the difficulties and
disorder was not the result of deviant parent- changes over time, along with other relevant
child interaction (Cantwell, Baker, & Rutter, information about the individual’s develop-
1979; DeMyer, Hingtgen, & Jackson, 1981). ment, life course, and social situation. The
These findings greatly influenced the inclu- diagnostic process highlights areas of compe-
sion of autism in the third edition of DSM tence, as well as difficulties and symptoms; it
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980), to notes the ways the individual has adapted; it
which we return later. describes previous treatments, available re-
sources, and other information that will allow
ISSUES IN CLASSIFICATION a fuller understanding of the individual and his
or her problems. Also, the diagnostic process
Systems for classification exist for many dif- may suggest or delineate biological, psycholog-
ferent reasons, but a fundamental purpose is to ical, and social factors that may have placed
enhance communication (Rutter, 2002). For the individual at risk, led to the disorder,
researchers, this is essential to achieve relia- changed its severity, or modified the symp-
bility and validity of findings from research toms and course. The result of the diagnostic
10 Diagnosis and Classification

process should be a rich formulation—an ac- may be tied to specific diagnostic categories
count that will be elaborated with new knowl- (Rutter & Schopler, 1992). Such an approach
edge, including the response of the individual tends, unfortunately, to emphasize the diag-
to intervention. It cannot be overemphasized nostic label, rather than the diagnostic process.
that while the diagnostic label or labels pro- On the other hand, if a governmental body
vide important and helpful information, they adopts a broad diagnostic concept, the avail-
do not substitute for a full and rich under- able resources may be diluted and individuals
standing of the individual’s strengths and most in need of intensive treatment may be de-
weaknesses and life circumstances. Thus, pro- prived while those with less clearly definable
grams should be designed around individuals service requirements are included in programs
rather than labels. (Rutter & Schopler, 1992).
A diagnostic formulation, based on an ex- There are many misconceptions about diag-
tended diagnostic process, is provisional and nosis and classification (see Rutter, 1996;
subject to change with new information and Volkmar & Schwab-Stone, 1996; Volkmar,
experience. In this sense, it is a continuing ac- Schwab-Stone, & First, 2002). For example,
tivity involving the individual, family, clini- DSM-IV and similar systems of classification
cians, and educators. The diagnostic process, are organized around dichotomous categories;
as a clinical activity, depends on a body of sci- in these systems, an individual either has or
entific knowledge and is enriched when there does not have a disorder. Yet, classification
is a common diagnostic language used for clin- can also be dimensional, in which an individ-
ical and research purposes. Information pro- ual has a problem, group of problems, or dys-
vided by this process is useful at the level of function to a certain degree. Dimensional
the individual case but also has important pub- approaches offer many advantages, as exempli-
lic health and social policy implications, for fied by the use of standard tests of intelli-
example, in formulating intervention strate- gence, adaptive behavior, or communication;
gies and allocating resources. in many ways, such approaches have domi-
Diagnostic systems lose value if they are nated in other branches of medicine and fre-
either overly broad or overly narrow. The clas- quently coexist with categorical ones (see
sification system must provide sufficient Rutter, 2002, for a review). Not only can the
detail to be used consistently and reliably by disease process (e.g., hypertension) be dimen-
clinicians and researchers across settings. sional but also various risk factors may be
When they achieve “official” status, as is the dimensional, and a dimensional focus has im-
case for ICD and DSM, classification schemes portant advantages for advancing knowledge in
have important regulatory and policy implica- this regard. On the other hand, at some point
tions. Sometimes, there may be conflicts be- qualitative and dimensional changes (as in
tween scientific and clinical needs, on one blood pressure) may lead either to functional
hand, and the impact of definitions on policy, impairment or specific symptoms (e.g., a high
on the other. For example, there may be good blood pressure can lead to angina), and the
scientific reasons for a narrowly defined cate- categorical approach is needed to address this
gorical diagnosis that includes only individuals important implication of what is basically a di-
who definitely and clearly have a specifically mensional phenomenon. Depression is a rele-
defined condition and excludes individuals vant example from psychiatry; for example, all
where there is less certainty. From the point of of us have the experience of mood fluctuations
view of service provision, however, broader di- during the course of our daily lives, but when
agnostic concepts may be most appropriate. depression becomes so significant that it be-
Unfortunately, there has often been a failure to gins to interfere with functioning or causes
recognize the validity of these two tensions impairment in other ways, we can consider use
around aspects of diagnosis. of specific treatments for depression.
Classification schemes of an “official” na- Dimensional and categorical classification
ture may have unintended, but important, im- systems are not incompatible. It is possible to
plications, for example, in terms of legal set a boundary point along a dimension that
mandates for services; this is particularly true can be used to define when a disorder is diag-
in the United States where federal regulations nosed. This boundary can be determined by
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 11

empirical studies that indicate that an important most important thing to convey about a phe-
threshold has been crossed that will in- nomenon or set of observations. Such notions
fluence functional status or impairment; or provide us with a sense of orderliness or narra-
the boundary can be defined by convention tive coherence. However, there is no truly
reached by clinicians, researchers, those who naive form of description or a naive descrip-
establish policy, or some combination of fac- tion of what clinicians and researchers mean
tors. For example, disorders such as depression by symptoms of a disorder. Even the decision
are readily amenable to dimensional defini- about what to consider a disorder of an individ-
tions. To some extent, all of us have experience ual presupposes a theory of what should be
of the symptoms of depression, yet, for the considered a disorder or dysfunction.
clinical syndrome of depression, a threshold The boundaries of the nosology for DSM-
must be surpassed: There must be a sufficient IV and ICD reflect a history of the profes-
number and range of symptoms that cause suf- sions of neurology, psychiatry, and general
fering, interfere with daily functioning, and medicine as well as preconceptions of where
persist (see Rutter, 2002; Chapter 28, this the current lines should be drawn. For exam-
Handbook, Volume 2). ple, the inclusion of Rett’s disorder in DSM-
For studies of autism and associated condi- IV raised the question of why a disorder with
tions, various dimensional approaches have such clear neurological aspects should be
been employed. Some instruments used for classified within the PDDs (Gillberg, 1994).
purposes of screening or diagnostic assessment However, neurological factors play a strong
focus on behaviors or historical features (or role in many disorders (including autism), but
both) that may be highly suggestive of a diag- that does not mean that they are only neuro-
nosis of autism. Such approaches have not logical. Much of the issue of where disorders
(with some notable exceptions—see Chapter such as autism or Rett’s are placed has to do
28, this Handbook, Volume 2) typically tried to with a practical issue of usage (see Rutter,
relate in a straightforward way with categori- 1994, for a discussion). A similar argument
cal approaches. Given the issues of focusing on could be had about Alzheimer’s disease,
highly unusual behaviors, other problems are which clearly falls within the professional
posed in the development and standardization purview of both psychiatrists and neurolo-
of such instruments. At the same time, such in- gists. One important effect of the decision to
struments have had a very significant role in include Rett’s disorder has been the ability to
research as well as clinical work, for example, focus specifically on this group in terms of
in screening for persons likely to have autism genetic mechanisms (see Chapter 5, this
(see Chapter 27, this Handbook, Volume 2). Handbook, this volume).
Another example of the dimensional ap- No nosology, including DSM-IV or ICD-10,
proach is embodied in the use of traditional can be totally free of theory, although there
tests of intelligence or communicative ability are good reasons for current psychiatric sys-
(see Chapters 29 & 30, this Handbook, Volume tems to aspire to be as atheoretical and de-
2). For such instruments, the provision of good scriptive as possible. This is illustrated in the
normative data is an important benefit. A earlier versions of DSM (American Psychiatric
growing body of work has focused on the di- Association, 1952, 1968) where theory was so
mensional metrification of social competence much part of definition that research work was
using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales impeded. Theoretically oriented classification
(see Chapter 29, this Handbook, Volume 2). systems often are difficult to use since there
The role of theory in guiding development may be differences even among those who
of classification systems is a source of confu- share a theoretical perspective. Since 1980,
sion. Many assume that a classification system the trend in psychiatry has been toward de-
must be based on a theoretical model. To some scriptive, operational definitions that empha-
degree, all accounts of an event, process, clini- size observable behaviors and discrete clinical
cal set of findings, or disorder relate to a “ the- findings (Frances, Widiger, & Pincus, 1989);
ory” (or what more probably might be called a indeed, such an approach is represented, in
hypothesis or theory in the making). Such pro- many respects, by Kanner’s original descrip-
totheories focus on what to the viewer is the tion of autism. Such an approach to diagnosis
12 Diagnosis and Classification

is often called phenomenological although this findings during the next years may make this
term is confusing, since phenomenology is a more feasible in diagnosing and subtyping
branch of philosophy that concerns the under- autism.
lying structures of experience and the modes Like other human constructions, classifica-
of learning about mental and psychological tion systems can be misused (Hobbs, 1975).
phenomena (including the use of introspection One misuse is to confuse the person with the
and dense description). Phenomenology repre- diagnostic label. A person with a disorder is a
sents a theoretical approach to diagnosis that person first: An individual with autism is not
has an important history in psychology and psy- an “autistic.” A label does not capture the full-
chiatry. When contemporary researchers and ness of the person, nor his or her humanity.
clinicians speak of phenomenological systems, There is a risk that categorical terms may min-
they usually mean something quite different: imize the tremendous differences among per-
descriptions of the surface (signs and symp- sons who have a particular condition. The very
toms) or accounts of observable phenomena. In broad range of syndrome expression in autism
any event, DSM-IV and ICD-10 attempt to avoid requires the provision of multiple kinds of in-
all encompassing, grand theories of pathogene- formation in addition to the categorical diag-
sis and concepts that require adherence to a nosis, for example, level of communicative
particular viewpoint about the functioning of speech, intellectual abilities, interests, and ca-
the mind or the origins of psychopathology. In pacity for independent living.
this sense, they attempt to provide a relatively Another misuse of a categorical diagnosis
common language and framework that can be occurs when it is elevated to the status of
used by adherents of different theoretical being an explanation or when its use obscures
points of view. lack of knowledge. In Moliere’s plays, the
Another misunderstanding is that classifi- physician would mystify and impress the pa-
cation systems require etiologies and causes. tients with long Latin terms that were offered
Here, too, the trend within psychiatry has been as explanations but were merely redescriptions
toward systems that recognize that the causes of the patient’s symptoms. For many diag-
of most psychiatric, developmental, and emo- noses, this is still the case. For example, it is
tional disorders remain uncertain and complex helpful to parents to know that their 2-year-old
(Rutter, 1996). Also, there is a realization that child is not talking because he or she has a
many different causes may lead to the appar- disorder. However, it is different when this
ently very similar clinical condition while one disorder is deafness—which may explain the
specific cause may be associated with various muteness, at some interesting level of under-
conditions. Scientific studies will reveal new standing—than when the disorder is autism.
causes for old diseases, and there often are The diagnosis of autism clarifies some aspects
surprises as different underlying factors are of the nature of an individual child’s muteness
revealed for what has appeared to be a simple, by placing this child within a class of individu-
homogeneous clinical condition. The increas- als about whom a great deal of valuable infor-
ing knowledge and the disparity between mation about treatment and course has been
genotype (underlying cause) and phenotype learned. But the classification does not really
(clinical presentation) indicate the importance explain the language disorder any more than
of not basing a classification system only on the diagnosis of attention deficit / hyperactiv-
purported causes. However, as etiologies are ity disorder explains a child’s overactivity and
elucidated, it makes sense to consider includ- frustration intolerance. When a label is mis-
ing them within a diagnostic framework. In taken for an explanation, areas of ignorance
DSM-IV, a causal framework is most clear in may be covered over and the search for under-
the definition of posttraumatic stress disorder lying causes may end prematurely.
(PTSD), a condition in which a clear precipi- The final misuse of classification is the
tant (a traumatic experience) is related to a potential for stigmatization. Parents and
range of persistent symptoms. For autism, a advocates are anxious about the ways in
causal nosology is not yet available, although which classification may negatively skew how
genetic, neuroimaging, behavioral, or other the child or adult is seen by others or the
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 13

limitations and adversities that may follow diagnostic process. They may also be misused.
upon being labeled. Unfortunately, this danger However, they can be helpful in clarifying the
is real. When a child has been classified as nature of an individual’s difficulties and thus
mentally retarded or intellectually disabled, suggest care and indicate course.
this has sometimes meant removal from the
mainstream of education and a lifelong reduc- THE ROLE OF RESEARCH
tion of opportunity. The diagnosis of schizo-
phrenia has had negative connotations Initial descriptions of disorders such as autism
associated with madness and danger. Autism, and related conditions were invariably made
too, has had its social disadvantages; for exam- by a clinician-investigator who noticed some
ple, at one time it may have implied a particu- seeming element(s) of commonality among
lar view of etiology in which parents were children with very complex developmental
placed at fault. A diagnostic label may exclude difficulties. Although modifications in early
individuals from programs or reduce chances descriptions of these conditions have, not sur-
in purchasing insurance. For these reasons, prisingly, often been made over time, there
parents and advocates have sometimes felt that usually has been a fundamental continuity of
inclusion of autism as a mental disorder may basic aspects of definitions with the historical
imply that autism is the result of some type of definition. Over the past several decades, em-
emotional upset within the child or family— pirical research has assumed a progressively
when it clearly is not—or that it stigmatizes greater role in refining diagnostic criteria and
the child. Dealing with these issues is a contin- categories. In this regard, even when empirical
uing process, and there have been major ad- research suggests that some feature or features
vances in destigmatization over the past years. are central to the definition, these need not,
Public education, professional awareness of necessarily, have a central etiological role.
the potential abuse of diagnostic labels, and Conversely, features less critical for purposes
legal imperatives are all important in reducing of definition may have major importance for
prejudice against individuals with handicaps intervention. In autism, the unusual pattern of
and disabilities. These issues also have had social deficit originally described by Kanner
important implications for studies of epidemi- (1943) remains the central defining core of the
ology and service planning, particularly when condition (Klin, Jones, Schultz, & Volkmar,
the available data related to labels are used for 2003); stereotyped motor mannerisms, on the
educational or intervention purposes; in such other hand, do not as clearly separate autism
contexts, parents might, for example, chose to from other conditions with severe and pro-
utilize the term autism to entitle their child to found mental handicap (Volkmar, Klin, Siegel,
additional services even if full criteria for Szatmari, Lord, et al., 1994). Similarly, un-
autism are not met or when the child might just usual sensory experiences are commonly ob-
as readily receive another label for service served in individuals with autism; they, too,
provision (a problem referred to as diagnostic may be a focus of intervention, but they are not
substitution—see Chapter 2, this Handbook, a robust, defining feature of the condition (see
this volume). Conversely, the well-intentioned Chapter 32, this Handbook, Volume 2, and
attempt to destigmatize a child by describing Rogers & Ozonoff, in press, for reviews).
his or her disability simply as a different style Other symptoms may be highly predictive of
of learning or being has the potential to reduce the presence of autism, but they are of such
entitlements and services and opportunities low frequency that they are not included in
for the gains associated with treatment (Na- usual definitions. For example, a child’s un-
tional Research Council, 2001). usual attachment to a physical object—such as
In summary, categorical diagnoses organize a string or a frying pan—is highly suggestive
professional experience and data, promote of the diagnosis of autism, but this preoccupa-
communication, and facilitate the provision of tion is not included in official diagnostic crite-
suitable treatments and interventions. They are ria because the behavior is not invariably
always open to improvement. They derive their present and even when present tends to be ob-
full meaning within the context of a continuing served only in younger individuals.
14 Diagnosis and Classification

Developmental aspects of syndrome expres- child’s vulnerability to other difficulties


sion are particularly important in autism and (Rutter, Shaffer, & Shepherd, 1975). Multiax-
related conditions. A developmental approach ial systems help to ensure that in the search
to classification views specific behaviors for a single, encompassing, categorical diag-
within the context of normative development. nosis, the rich and multifaceted diagnostic
For example, the echolalia of autistic individu- process is not undervalued.
als is similar in some respects to the repeti-
tions observed in the speech of typically APPROACHES TO CATEGORICAL
developing 2- and 3-year-olds (see Chapter 30, DEFINITIONS OF AUTISM
this Handbook, Volume 2). From this perspec-
tive, echolalia is not simply a symptom but also In contrast to many conditions in child psychi-
is seen among typical children at a particular atry, strictly defined autism does not “shade
phase of development; when an older, mute, off ” into normalcy in the usual sense (Rutter
autistic child begins to use echolalia, it may be & Garmezy, 1983) and thus represents one of
a sign of progress in language development. On the more robust disorders for purposes of cate-
the other hand, as originally noted by Kanner, gorical diagnosis; at the same time, the body of
some aspects of the functioning of individuals genetic research has raised the important issue
with autism are fundamentally not develop- of a “ broader ” phenotype, that is, of a contin-
mentally appropriate at any age (see Chapters uum of social and related vulnerabilities
28, 30, & 32, this Handbook, Volume 2). This (Volkmar et al., 2004).
is specifically true of the social dysfunction Even for strictly defined autism, there are
and lack of engagement. Even infants are en- problems in the development of explicit defini-
gaged socially. The typical aloofness of autism tions. These include the tremendous range in
and lack of reciprocity are distinctly abnormal syndrome expression and change in symptoms
at any age and appear especially so when these over the course of development. Since the per-
social disabilities are far out of proportion to son with autism may not always be able to pro-
the individual’s functioning in other domains vide a direct, verbal report, the reports of
of daily living (see Chapter 11, this Handbook, parents or caregivers must be relied on, as
this volume). with very young children, raising other poten-
Behavioral deviance, such as lack of social tial problems including reliability and validity
reciprocity or abnormal preoccupations, is of historical information. Methods have been
often the focus of the criteria used in defining proposed for diagnosis that focus on very early
a categorical diagnosis. Such deviance is also development. These methods, which some-
a focus of rating scales and other assessment times use dimensional ratings scales (see
instruments used in relation to autism. This Chapter 28, this Handbook, Volume 2), may
diagnostic approach may be combined with an be problematic in relation to providing a cate-
assessment of how the individual compares to gorical diagnosis for an adolescent or adult
typical children and adults, for example, in re- with autism. In the absence of an accepted
lation to language use. The multiaxial system measure of diagnostic pathophysiology, one
of DSM-IV is an attempt to systematically would wish to consider both the historical in-
convey the value of considering an individual formation as well as course and current func-
from multiple perspectives. This includes tioning in conferring a diagnosis of a severe
assessment of the individual’s personality, developmental or psychiatric disorder. Yet,
educational and social resources, ongoing the use of development and history raises
stresses, medical problems and diseases, and practical problems for categorical diagnostic
adaptive functioning as well as impairment systems. In general, history has been over-
(Rutter & Schopler, 1992). Multiaxial diag- looked in the current official nosologies (with
nostic approaches are especially helpful in un- the exception of noting the age of onset)—a
derstanding individuals who have disorders topic to which we return later.
that start during childhood and are persistent, There are interesting and relevant ques-
like autism, and have major impact on all tions, too, about what should be included in a
spheres of development and increase the categorical diagnostic set of criteria. Should
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 15

such a set emphasize only those symptoms and DSM-III


signs that most clearly differentiate one condi-
tion from another, or should the set of criteria DSM-III (1980) was a landmark in the devel-
also include important symptoms (e.g., rushes opment of psychiatric taxonomy based on
of panic and anxiety or overactivity and im- research findings and emphasizing valid, reli-
pulsiveness) that are also found among other able descriptions of complex clinical phenom-
conditions? Should the criteria capture the ena. Autism was included along with several
largest number of children who may have the other disorders in a newly designated class
condition or be more selective? What about of childhood onset disorders, Pervasive Devel-
symptoms that may be infrequent but of great opmental Disorders (PDD). Other disorders
clinical importance when they occur, such as included residual infantile autism, child-
self-injurious behavior? To what degree should hood onset pervasive developmental disorder
diagnostic criteria also be fuller descriptions (COPDD), and residual COPDD. A subthresh-
of the condition? old condition was included as well, atypical
Investigators began to propose more ex- PDD. The class name pervasive developmental
plicit categorical definitions of autism in the disorder was newly coined and was meant to
1970s as a consensus on the validity of autism convey that individuals with these conditions
emerged. This was parallel to attempts in adult suffered from impairment in the development
psychiatry to provide better definitions of and unfolding of multiple areas of function-
psychiatric disorders for research purposes ing. The term also was meant to avoid a theo-
(Spitzer, Endicott, & Robins, 1978). The im- retical presupposition about etiology, and it
portance of a multiaxial or multidimensional quickly achieved broad acceptance. Subse-
approach to diagnosis became increasingly ap- quently, the choice of the term PDD has been
preciated (Rutter et al., 1975). Rutter (1978) debated (see Gillberg, 1991; Volkmar &
synthesized Kanner’s original report and Cohen, 1991b), and other terms, for example,
subsequent research in a highly influential def- autism spectrum disorder (ASD), have also
inition of autism as having four essential fea- come into common usage; the two terms are
tures: (1) early onset by age 22 years, (2) used synonymously here.
impaired social development, (3) impaired The DSM-III system was a major advance.
communication, and (4) unusual behaviors It extended official recognition to autism, dis-
consistent in many ways with Kanner’s con- carded the earlier presumption of a relation
cept of “insistence on sameness” (resistance to between autism and childhood schizophrenia,
change, idiosyncratic responses to the environ- and provided a useful definition largely re-
ment, motor mannerisms and stereotypes, flecting Rutter’s (1978) approach. The use of a
etc.). Rutter specified that the social and com- multiaxial system also facilitated research.
munication impairments were distinctive and However, some shortcomings with this system
not just a function of associated mental retar- were relatively quickly apparent. The rationale
dation. In contrast, the National Society for for the inclusion of COPDD was apparently to
Autistic Children (NSAC; Ritvo, 1978) in the account for those relatively rare children who
United States proposed a definition that in- developed an autistic-like disorder after age
cluded disturbances in (1) rates and sequences 30 months (Kolvin, 1971); this disorder was
of development, (2) responses to sensory stim- not, however, meant to be analogous with the
uli, (3) speech, language-cognition, and non- concept of Heller’s syndrome (disintegrative
verbal communication, and (4) the capacity to psychosis) since it was assumed (incorrectly)
relate appropriately to people, events, and ob- that the latter was invariably a function of
jects. This definition also emphasized the neu- some related general medical condition (Volk-
robiological basis of autism. While clinically mar, 1992). The definition of autism itself was
providing more detail, the Ritvo-NSAC defini- rather sparse and tended, perhaps not surpris-
tion proved rather less influential than the ingly given the official name of the disorder
Rutter synthesis, probably because the latter (infantile autism), to focus very much on
seemed conceptually clearer and closer to autism as it is exhibited in younger children.
Kanner’s original description. The use of the term residual autism was
16 Diagnosis and Classification

included to account for cases where the child tive impairment in verbal and nonverbal com-
once met the criteria for infantile autism but munication and in imagination, and restricted
no longer did so; this seemed, at some level, to repertoire of activities and interests.
imply that the individual no longer had autism. A small national field trial was conducted
The term atypical PDD was used for sub- to finalize scoring rules for the DSM-III-R
threshold conditions, that is, for a constella- definition of autism (Spitzer & Siegel, 1990).
tion of difficulties that appeared to most Sixteen proposed criteria for autistic disorder
appropriately be placed within the PDD class were grouped into the three broad categories.
but which did not meet criteria for infantile Based on this field trial, the diagnosis of
autism or another explicitly defined condition, autism required that an individual child or
unintentionally suggesting Rank’s earlier adult had to exhibit at least 8 of these 16 crite-
(1949) concept. Individuals with hallucina- ria, in total, with a specified distribution over
tions and delusions were specifically excluded the three areas of disturbance. This require-
from the PDD diagnoses. While it is unlikely ment for an early onset of the condition was
that many persons with autism will develop dropped in DSM-III-R because of the wish to
schizophrenia, it might be anticipated that in- provide a generally applicable criterion set, re-
dividuals with autism would develop schizo- gardless of age, and partly for the philosophi-
phrenia at least as often as other individuals in cal reason that the age of onset should not be
the general population, a hypothesis that considered a diagnostic feature, that is, that
seems to be sustained by available evidence clinicians should rely on present examination
(Volkmar & Cohen, 1991a). rather than history in making the diagnosis.
The multiaxial placement of disorders in This change would make it possible to diagnose
DSM-III also was a source of controversy; that autism in children who, for example, appeared
is, autism and other PDDs were placed on Axis to develop autism or something suggestive of it
I as was mental retardation although other spe- much later in development (Weir & Salisbury,
cific developmental disorders were listed on 1980); such cases have never, however, been
Axis II of the multiaxial system. The problems very common and it seemed problematic that
with DSM-III were widely recognized, and a their uniqueness was not flagged in some way
major revision was undertaken for DSM-III-R (e.g., through diagnostic coding).
(American Psychiatric Association, 1987). DSM III-R was attentive to changes in the
expression of autism with age and developmen-
DSM-III-R tal level. This represented a clear improvement
over DSM-III (Volkmar, Cicchetti, Cohen, &
Preparations for the revision of DSM-III began Bregman, 1992) where the concept of residual
soon after it appeared. What started as revi- autism had been an unsatisfactory attempt to
sion soon became a major renovation. Radical deal with this issue. Criteria in DSM-III-R
changes were introduced into the concept of were offered for autistic disorder and were ap-
autism in DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric plicable to the entire range of the expression of
Association, 1987; see Waterhouse, Wing, the syndrome. Thus, an individual could retain
Spitzer, & Siegel, 1993, for discussion of these the diagnosis of autism even if he or she was
changes). The rapid revision of the official functioning at a higher developmental level or
nosology posed problems for researchers who had experienced an amelioration of symptoms
were required to rediagnose their patients if with age, perhaps as a result of educational in-
they wished to remain au courant. tervention or maturation. The name of the
The definition of autistic disorder in DSM- condition was changed from infantile autism
III-R was more consistent with that of Wing to reflect these changes. Finally, in DSM-III-
(Wing & Gould, 1979) and others who advo- R, the problematic COPDD category was
cated a somewhat broader view of the diagnos- dropped, leaving those children who had car-
tic concept (see Chapter 21, this Handbook, ried this diagnosis suspended in limbo or, in
this volume). Three major domains of dysfunc- practice, placed within the PDD-not other-
tion were still included, with specific criteria wise specified (NOS) category. The term for
provided for each domain: qualitative impair- all subthreshold categories was changed to
ment in reciprocal social interaction, qualita- “Not otherwise specified” (NOS) throughout
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 17

DSM. Individuals with autism were no longer, ICD, which appeared in 1968 (see Rutter et al.,
by definition, excluded from also exhibiting 1975; Spitzer & Williams, 1980). At the same
schizophrenia. time, there was general agreement that future
The ambitious goal of a heuristic definition refinement would be needed and, over the next
in DSM-III-R was a conceptual advance over decade, a series of steps were undertaken to
DSM-III, but carried unforeseen consequences. improve the ICD system (Sartorius, 1988).
DSM-III-R criteria expanded the diagnostic One important aspect was the development of a
concept (Factor, Freeman, & Kardash, 1989; multiaxial system for the psychiatric disorders
Hertzig, Snow, New, & Shapiro, 1990; Szat- of childhood (Rutter et al., 1975). By 1978, the
mari, 1992a; Volkmar et al., 1992). The rate of ninth edition of ICD appeared and plans for a
false-positive cases (if clinician judgment is revision were put into place. The ICD-9 ac-
taken as the standard) diagnosed according to corded official recognition to infantile autism
DSM-III-R was nearly 40% (Rutter & as well as disintegrative psychosis (or what
Schopler, 1992; Spitzer & Siegel, 1990). This would now be termed childhood disintegrative
tendency to overdiagnose autism in more intel- disorders); both conditions were included in a
lectual handicapped individuals likely also had category of childhood psychotic conditions—a
the inadvertent effect of diverting clinical at- category that also included other specific psy-
tention from autism as it appeared in intellec- chotic conditions of childhood and unspecified
tually more able individuals. psychotic conditions. This approach reflected
Other problems with DSM-III-R also were the historical view (then beginning to change)
noted. First, the criteria set was more complex that autism represented one of the first mani-
and detailed, and the inclusion of specific ex- festations of childhood psychosis.
amples within the actual criteria seemed to The plan for revision of ICD-10 was well
limit clinician judgment. The elimination of underway at the time that DSM-IV was being
age of onset as a central diagnostic feature was developed. An important aspect of ICD-10 has
not consistent with Kanner’s original report been its conceptualization as a group of docu-
(1943) nor subsequent research that firmly es- ments written specifically for different users;
tablished that autism was an early-onset dis- for example, in contrast to the DSM-IV ap-
order (e.g., Harper & Williams, 1975; Kolvin, proach, research criteria for disorders are pro-
1971; Short & Schopler, 1988; Volkmar, vided separately from clinical guidelines for
Cohen, Hoshino, Rende, & Paul, 1988; Volk- primary health care providers. ICD-10 offers
mar, Stier, & Cohen, 1985). Probably the main comprehensive descriptions of clinical con-
issue with DSM-III-R, however, was the major cepts underlying the disorder, followed by
changes introduced in the diagnostic concept. points of differential diagnosis, and then pre-
These changes severely complicated the inter- sents the main symptoms that should be pres-
pretation of studies that used different diag- ent for a diagnosis. As a result, the ICD-10
nostic criteria. This issue was particularly system offers, in some important respects,
acute relative to the pending changes in the more flexibility to the clinician; this is partic-
classification of autism and similar conditions ularly valuable given the intended interna-
in the 10th edition of the ICD-10 (WHO, tional and cross-cultural use of the system.
1992), since it appeared that DSM-III-R
markedly overdiagnosed autism relative to the DSM-IV AND ICD-10
draft ICD-10 definition (Volkmar, Cicchetti,
Bregman, & Cohen, 1992). The process of revision in the ICD-10 was
closely related to the development of the
FROM ICD-9 TO ICD-10 DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association,
1994). The International (ICD) and American
Since it was first introduced toward the end of (DSM) systems are fundamentally related, and
the nineteenth century, the ICD has under- by formal agreements must share, to some de-
gone many revisions (Kramer, 1968). The gree, a common approach to diagnostic coding.
limitations of the psychiatric section were There are, however, important general and
increasingly recognized, and extensive revi- specific differences between the two major di-
sion was undertaken in the eighth edition of agnostic systems (Volkmar & Schwab-Stone,
18 Diagnosis and Classification

1996). For example, the ICD-10 system high- PDD class; there was also agreement about the
lighted the importance of an individual’s his- desirability of compatibility of DSM-IV and
tory in making a diagnosis while DSM-III-R ICD-10 (Rutter & Schopler, 1992).
relied on contemporaneous examination. Also In addition to these literature reviews, a
in contrast to DSM-IV, ICD-10 was specifi- series of data reanalyses were undertaken
cally designed to have one set of research diag- with regard to autism. These reanalyses used
nostic criteria and a separate set of clinical previously collected data and indicated that
guidelines. The American and International the DSM-III-R definition of autistic disorder
approaches would probably have resulted in was overly broad (Volkmar, Cicchetti, &
very different patterns of diagnosis. Bregman, 1992). Several issues were identi-
Preparations for the creation of the new, fied during this process of analysis of the lit-
fourth edition of DSM began very shortly after erature and of available data that needed
DSM-III-R appeared, partly due to the pending clarification for DSM-IV, including issues of
changes in the ICD-10. As part of the revision overdiagnosis in the more intellectually
process, work groups reviewed the current challenged and underdiagnosis in more able
classification systems in light of existing re- individuals. Consistent with the empirical
search and identified areas both of consensus principles guiding the creation of DSM-IV,
and controversy. They considered various is- the working group decided that the clarifica-
sues, including clinical utility, reliability, and tion of these and other issues would be based
descriptive validity of categories and criteria on the findings from a large, multinational
as well as coordination with the ICD-10 revi- field trial (Volkmar, Klin, Siegel, Szatmari,
sion (Frances et al., 1991). As part of the pro- Lord, et al., 1994).
cess of creating DSM-IV, clinical investigators
conducted literature reviews for each of the DSM-IV Field Trial
potential diagnostic categories. These reviews
were particularly helpful for some of the new As part of the DSM-IV field trial for autism,
diagnostic categories. For example, although 21 sites and 125 raters participated from the
childhood disintegrative disorder (Heller’s United States and around the world. By de-
syndrome) is apparently much less common sign, the raters had a range of experience in
than autism, the data supported the view that it the diagnosis of autism and a range of profes-
differed from autism in a number of important sional backgrounds. The field trial included
ways (Volkmar, 1992; Volkmar & Cohen, information on nearly 1,000 cases seen by
1989). Asperger’s disorder was included in one or more raters. In cases where the same
ICD-10, but the text indicated that the validity case was rated by multiple raters to assess re-
of the syndrome as a disorder, distinct from liability, the rating by one clinician was cho-
autism, was not yet fully established (Rutter & sen at random to be included in the main
Schopler, 1992; Szatmari, 1992a, 1992b). The database. The preference for the entire field
absence of official or other generally agreed trial was for cases rated on the basis of con-
upon definitions for Asperger’s disorder had temporaneous examination and not just on re-
contributed to markedly different uses of the view of records. By design, five contributing
term in clinical and research work (see Chap- sites provided ratings on approximately 100
ter 4, this Handbook, this volume). With Rett’s consecutive cases of individuals either with
disorder, the issues revealed by the review pro- autism or other disorders in which the diagno-
cess had less to do with the validity of the di- sis of autism would reasonably be included in
agnostic concept and more with the question of the differential diagnosis while the other 16
whether Rett’s should be included in the PDD sites provided ratings of a minimum of about
class rather than as a neurological disorder 20 cases. Cases were included only if it ap-
(Gillberg, 1994; Rutter, 1994; Tsai, 1992). Al- peared that the case exhibited difficulties
though the literature identified major gaps in that would reasonably include autism in the
knowledge and persistent issues, the consensus differential diagnosis. The availability of
of workers in the field favored the inclusion of clinical ratings of cases seen at clinical cen-
additional diagnostic categories within the ters around the world was of interest in terms
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 19

TABLE 1.2 DSM-IV Autistic Disorder Field Trial


Group Characteristics
Clinically Autistic Other PDDs Non-PDD
(N = 454) (N = 240) (N = 283)
Sex Ratio (MF) 4.491 3.711 2.291
Mute 54% 35% 33%
Age 8.99 9.68 9.72
IQ 58.1 77.2 66.9
Notes: Cases grouped by clinical diagnosis. Diagnoses of the “other PDD”
cases included: Rett syndrome (13 cases), childhood disintegrative disorder
(16 cases), Asperger syndrome (48 cases), PPD-NOS (116 cases), and atypical
autism (47 cases). Diagnoses of the non-PDD cases included mental retardation
(132 cases), language disorder (88 cases), childhood schizophrenia (9 cases),
other disorders (54 cases).

of issues of compatibility between DSM-IV designed to be a research diagnostic system,


and ICD-10. Characteristics of the field trial had, as expected, higher specificity.
sample are presented in Table 1.2. As mentioned earlier, one of the major dif-
Typically, multiple sources of information ferences between DSM-III-R and both DSM-III
were available to the rater, and the quality of and ICD-10 was the failure to include history
the information available to the rater was in the diagnostic process, for example, early
judged to be excellent or good in about 75% of age of onset as an explicit diagnostic feature.
cases. Individuals from a variety of ethnic Reported age of onset of autism was examined.
backgrounds and in various educational set- The mean reported age at onset for autism was
tings were included. This approach differed in early. The data on reported age of onset are
important respects from that employed in presented in Figure 1.1.
DSM-III-R where, for example, children with Age at onset had a modest, positive rela-
conduct disorders (without development disor- tionship with measured intelligence. Individu-
der) were included in the comparison group. als with slightly later onset were more likely
A standard system of coding was used to to have higher IQ scores. If onset by 36
elicit information on basic characteristics of months was added as an essential feature to
the case (age, IQ, communicative ability, edu- DSM-III-R, the sensitivity of that system was
cational placement), the rater, and various di- increased. Thus, inclusion of age of onset as
agnostic criteria. The coding form also
provided possible criteria for Asperger’s dis-
order, Rett’s disorder, and childhood disinte- TABLE 1.3 Table IV-2: Sensitivity
grative disorder, based on the draft ICD-10 (Se)/Specificity (Sp) by IQ Level
definitions. DSM-IIIa DSM-III-R ICD-10b
The field trial provided data for studying
By IQ Level N Se Sp Se Sp Se Sp
the patterns of agreement among the various
diagnostic systems. These results are pre- <25 64 .90 .76 .84 .39 .74 .88
sented in Table 1.3. As shown, the DSM-III 25–39 148 .88 .76 .90 .60 .88 .92
40–54 191 .79 .76 .93 .74 .84 .83
diagnoses of infantile autism and residual 55– 69 167 .86 .78 .84 .77 .78 .89
autism had a reasonable balance of sensitivity 70–85 152 .79 .81 .88 .81 .74 .96
and specificity; the use of the residual autism >85 218 .78 .83 .78 .78 .78 .91
category in DSM-III was associated with Overall .82 .80 .86 .83 .79 .89
other problems. In contrast, DSM-III-R crite- a
“Lifetime” diagnosis (current IA or “residual” IA).
ria had a higher sensitivity but lower speci- b
Original ICD-10 criteria and scoring.
ficity and a relatively high rate of Adapted from “Field Trial for Autistic Disorder
false-positive cases, especially among indi- in DSM-IV,” by F. R. Volkmar et al., 1994, American
viduals with retardation where the rate Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367. Used with
reached 60%. The ICD-10 draft definition, permission.
20 Diagnosis and Classification

120 individuals who were assigned a diagnosis of


autism only by DSM-III-R. This instability of
100 diagnostic classification was most apparent for
younger children and for individuals with
Number of Cases

80 lower IQ.
The field trial data were also analyzed
60
using signal detection methods and principal
components analyses. The various approaches
40
to the data suggested that certain items could
20
be eliminated from the ICD-10 definition,
particularly items with low base rates or
0
strong developmental associations (see later
6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48 54 60 discussion). Before final decisions could be
Report Onset (months) made on the DSM-IV definition, it was neces-
Figure 1.1 Age of onset: Cases with clinical diag-
sary to address the broader issue of whether
nosis of autism. other explicitly defined disorders would be in-
cluded in the PDD class in DSM-IV. While the
DSM-IV autism field trial was not primarily
focused on the definition (much less the valid-
an essential diagnostic feature for autism was ity) of these conditions, the issues of the defi-
supported and was consistent with the ICD-10 nition and validity were relevant to the
draft criteria. DSM-IV and ICD-10 definitions of autism.
Aspects of the reliability of criteria and of The boundaries for autism and the nonautistic
diagnoses made by the various diagnostic sys- PDD were mutually related: A narrow defini-
tems were examined using chance corrected tion of autism would force some cases into the
statistics. Since raters with a range of experi- nonautistic PDD group. The broad definition
ence had participated in the field trial, it was of autism in DSM-III-R had certain advan-
possible to address rater experience in relation tages, for example, in ensuring access to ser-
to reliability. In general, the interrater reliabil- vices; but a narrower definition might be
ity of individual diagnostic criteria was in the important for research studies that require
good to excellent range. Only one criterion had greater homogeneity.
poor interrater reliability. Typically, the more
Definition of Autism in DSM-IV and ICD-10
detailed ICD-10 criteria had, as expected,
greater reliability. Also as expected, experi- The field trial data provided an important em-
enced evaluators usually had excellent agree- pirical basis for constructing the definition of
ment among themselves and were more likely autism for DSM-IV. The data showed that the
to agree with one another than with less expe- DSM-III-R definition could be substantially
rienced raters. The experience of the raters improved by addition of a criterion relating to
rather than their professional discipline had age of onset and by raising the diagnostic
the greatest impact on reliability (Klin, Lang, threshold. Similarly, various combinations of
Cicchetti, & Volkmar, 2000). DSM-III, DSM-III-R, and new criteria all
The temporal stability of ratings was as- could have been used to provide a reasonably
sessed in two ways. A small number of cases balanced diagnostic system. Given the concern
for test-retest reliability were collected as part about the importance of compatibility with
of the field trial; in addition, follow-up infor- ICD-10 and the implications for research of a
mation was available on the cohort of 114 universally accepted definition, the working
cases originally reported earlier (Volkmar, group of DSM-IV considered the benefits of
Bregman, Cohen, & Cicchetti, 1988). Criteria the ICD-10 system. Possible modifications in
and diagnostic assignments were highly stable the ICD-10 system were examined. The goal
over relatively short periods of time in the was to establish a definition for DSM-IV that
range of less than one year. Findings with the balanced clinical and research needs, was rea-
cases followed up by Volkmar et al. (1988) sonably concise and easy to use, provided rea-
suggested more diagnostic instability for those sonable coverage over the range of syndrome
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 21

expression in autism, and was applicable over gesture. The domain of restricted patterns of
the full life span, from early childhood behavior, interests, and activities includes en-
through adulthood. compassing preoccupations that are abnormal
Of the original 20 ICD-10 criteria, four either in focus or intensity, adherence to non-
were identified for possible elimination. Alter- functional routines or rituals, stereotyped
natives to specific criteria were examined, and motor movements, and persistent preoccupa-
a modified definition was developed. This tion with parts of objects.
modified definition worked well both overall
and over different levels of age and associated The Definition of the Nonautistic PDDs
mental retardation; it also could be readily
used by less experienced examiners. In contrast to DSM-III-R, a number of condi-
Diagnostic criteria for autism in DSM-IV tions other than autism and subthreshold
and ICD-10 are presented in Table 1.4. autism (i.e., PDD-NOS) are now officially
For the diagnosis of autism, at least six cri- recognized in both DSM-IV and ICD-10.
teria must be exhibited, including at least two Given that these are newer disorders (at least
criteria relating to social abnormalities (group in terms of their official recognition), it is not
one) and one each relating to impaired commu- surprising that the substantive body of work
nication (group 2) and range of interests and on their definitions is less extensive than that
activities (group 3). In addition, the onset of for autism.
the condition must have been prior to age 3
Rett’s Disorder
years as evidenced by delay or abnormal func-
tioning in social interaction, language as used There were few concerns about the validity of
in social interaction, and symbolic/imagina- the entity explicated by Rett. It was clear that
tive play. In addition, DSM-IV accepted the di- the transient, autistic-like phase of social with-
agnostic convention that the disorder could not drawal occurred early in the child’s develop-
better be accounted for by the diagnosis of ment and presented the primary problem for
Rett’s disorder or childhood disintegrative dis- differentiation from autism (and one of the
order (the definitions of these concepts are main arguments for its placement in the PDD
discussed subsequently). class). However, there were some objections to
Qualitative impairment in social interaction including it in the PDD class (Gillberg, 1994)
can take the form of markedly impaired non- although it was also clear that it should be in-
verbal behaviors, failure in developmentally cluded somewhere (Rutter, 1994). The impor-
expectable peer relationships, lack of shared tance of its inclusion has been underscored by
enjoyment or pleasure, or lack of social- the subsequent discovery of a gene involved in
emotional reciprocity. The stronger weighting the pathogenesis of the disorder (Amir, Van den
of the impairments in socialization was noted Veyver, Wan, Tran, Francke, et al., 1999; also
during the field trial to be important in avoid- see Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume).
ing overdiagnosis of autism in more intellec-
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder
tually handicapped persons. This is also
consistent with extensive previous clinical Although this condition had been included in
work, from the time of Kanner onward (e.g., ICD-9 the presumption in DSM-III-R was that
Rutter, 1978; D. Cohen, 1980; Siegel, Vukice- individuals with childhood disintegrative dis-
vic, Elliott, & Kraemer, 1989) that high- order (also known as Heller’s syndrome or
lighted social dysfunction as the critical disintegrative psychosis) usually suffered
domain of impairment in autism. from a neurological or other progressive pro-
Impairments in communication can take cess that accounted for their marked behav-
the form of delay or lack of spoken language, ioral and developmental deterioration. The
impairment in conversational ability, stereo- literature, however, did not support this associ-
typed language use, and deficits in imagina- ation (Volkmar, 1992). While rare, childhood
tive play. For persons with autism, the delay disintegrative disorder appeared to be a disor-
or lack of spoken language must not be ac- der that could be distinguished from autism
companied by compensations through other and that was, like autism, of generally un-
communicative means, for example, the use of known etiology. The rationale for including
TABLE 1.4 ICD-10 Criteria for Autism
Childhood Autism (F84.0)
A. Abnormal or impaired development is evident before the age of 3 years in at least one of the following areas:
(1) receptive or expressive language as used in social communication;
(2) the development of selective social attachments or of reciprocal social interaction;
(3) functional or symbolic play.
B. A total of at least six symptoms from (1), (2) and (3) must be present, with at least two from (1) and at least
one from each of (2) and (3)
(1) Qualitative impairment in social interaction are manifest in at least two of the following areas:
(a) failure adequately to use eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate
social interaction;
( b) failure to develop (in a manner appropriate to mental age, and despite ample opportunities) peer
relationships that involve a mutual sharing of interests, activities and emotions;
(c) lack of socio-emotional reciprocity as shown by an impaired or deviant response to other people’s
emotions; or lack of modulation of behaviour according to social context; or a weak integration of
social, emotional, and communicative behaviors;
(d) lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., a
lack of showing, bringing, or point out to other people objects of interest to the individual).
(2) Qualitative abnormalities communication as manifest in at least one of the following areas:
(a) delay in or total lack of, development of spoken language that is not accompanied by an attempt to
compensate through the use of gestures or mime as an alternative mode of communication (often
preceded by a lack of communicative babbling);
( b) relative failure to initiate or sustain conversational interchange (at whatever level of language skill
is present), in which there is reciprocal responsiveness to the communications of the other person;
(c) stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic use of words or phrases;
(d) lack of varied spontaneous make-believe play or (when young) social imitative play.
(3) Restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behaviour, interests, and activities are manifested in
at least one of the following:
(a) an encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that
are abnormal in content or focus; or one or more interests that are abnormal in their intensity and
circumscribed nature though not in their content or focus;
( b) apparently compulsive adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals;
(c) stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms that involve either hand or finger f lapping or twisting
or complex whole body movements;
(d) preoccupations with part-objects or non-functional elements of play materials (such as their odour,
the feel of their surface, or the noise or vibration they generate).
C. The clinical picture is not attributable to the other varieties of pervasive developmental disorders; specific
development disorder of receptive language (F80.2) with secondary socio-emotional problems’ reactive
attachment disorder (F94.1) or disinhibited attachment disorder (F94.2); mental retardation (F70-F72) with
some associated emotional or behavioral disorders; schizophrenia (F20.-) of unusually early Onset; and Rett’s
syndrome (F84.12).

F84.1 Atypical autism


A. Abnormal or impaired development is evident at or after the age of 3 years (criteria as for autism except for
age of manifestation).
B. There are qualitative abnormalities in reciprocal social interaction or in communication, or restricted,
repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. (Criteria as for autism except that it
is unnecessary to meet the criteria for number of areas of abnormality.)
C. The disorder does not meet the diagnostic criteria for autism (F84.0).
Autism may be atypical in either age of onset (F84.10) or symptomatology (F84.11); the two types are
differentiated with a fifth character for research purposes. Syndromes that are typical in both respects should
be coded F84.12.

F84.10 Atypicality in age of onset


A. The disorder does not meet criterion A for autism (F84.0); that is, abnormal or impaired development is
evident only at or after age 3 years.
B. The disorder meets criteria B and C for autism (F84.0).

22
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 23

TABLE 1.4 (Continued)

F84.11 Atypicality in symptomatology


A. The disorder meets criterion A for autism (F84.0); that is abnormal or impaired development is evident
before age 3 years.
B. There are qualitative abnormalities in reciprocal social interactions or in communication, or restricted,
repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. (Criteria as for autism except that
it is unnecessary to meet the criteria for number of areas of abnormality.)
C. The disorder meets criterion C for autism (F84.0).
D. The disorder does not fully meet criterion B for autism (F84.0).

F84.12 Atypicality in both age of onset and symptomatology


A. The disorder does not meet criterion A for autism (F84.0); that is, abnormal or impaired development is
evident only at or after age 3 years.
B. There are qualitative abnormalities in reciprocal social interactions or in communication, or restricted,
repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. (Criteria as for autism except that
it is unnecessary to meet the criteria for number of areas of abnormality.)
C. The disorder meets criterion C for autism (F84.0).
D. The disorder does not fully meet criterion B for autism (F84.0).

DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder (299.0)


A. A total of at least six items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3):
(1) Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:
(a) marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial
expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction,
( b) failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level,
(c) markedly impaired expression of pleasure in other people’s happiness,
(d) lack of social or emotional reciprocity,
(2) Qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:
(a) delay in or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an attempt to
compensate through alternative modes of communication such as gestures or mime)
( b) in individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a
conversation with others
(c) stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language
(d) lack of varied spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to developmental
level
(3) Restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities, as manifested by at
least one of the following:
(a) encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is
abnormal either in intensity or focus
( b) apparently compulsive adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals
(c) stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g., hand or finger f lapping or twisting, or complex
whole body movements)
(d) persistent preoccupation with parts of objects
B. Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age three: (1) social
interaction, (2) language as used in social communication, or (3) symbolic or imaginative play.
C. Not better accounted for by Rett’s Disorder or Childhood Disintegrative Disorder.
Sources: From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, by American Psychiatric As-
sociation, 1994, Washington, DC: Author; and International Classification of Diseases: Diagnostic Criteria for Re-
search, tenth edition, by the World Health Organization, 1992, Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Reprinted with
permission.

this condition had less to do with its potential course and prognosis (Volkmar & Rutter,
importance for research, for example, relative 1995) although others (e.g., Hendry, 2000)
to the search for a gene or genes that might be have questioned the recognition of the cate-
involved, than its frequency. The limited data gory; these issues are discussed in greater de-
available also suggested some important po- tail in Chapter 3, this Handbook, of this
tential differences from autism in terms of volume.
24 Diagnosis and Classification

not required. In the DSM-IV field trial, the


Asperger’s Disorder
presence of such interest was one of the fea-
In many ways, the inclusion and definition of tures that discriminated individuals with clini-
this condition have been the source of the cal diagnoses of autism from Asperger’s
greatest continuing confusion and controversy disorder. The limited available data (see Chap-
(e.g., Klin, Sparrow, & Volkmar, 1997; see ter 4, this Handbook, this volume, and Klin
also Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume). et al., in press) suggest, not surprisingly, rather
Although Asperger’s original paper (Asperger, poor overall agreement of these different diag-
1944) and his subsequent clinical work (Hip- nostic approaches.
pler & Klicpera, 2003) emphasized the pres- To some extent, these disparities in diag-
ence of circumscribed interests and motor nostic approach parallel broader differences in
delays, they were technically not required in the way the disorder is conceptualized. For ex-
either the ICD-10 or DSM-IV definition that ample, is Asperger best thought of as a milder
was eventually adopted. Indeed, in DSM-IV, it form of autism (Leekam et al., 2000), is it
was emphasized that autism should take diag- characterized by a rather different neuropsy-
nostic precedence; difficulties in the use of chological profile than autism (Klin, Volkmar,
these criteria were quickly noted (Miller & Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Rourke, 1995), or are
Ozonoff, 1997, 2000). As a result, final clo- the social difficulties different from autism
sure on the best definition of this disorder has (Tsai, 1992)? Yet another issue is how and
not yet been achieved. whether motor skills problems are taken into
Given the general dissatisfaction with the account (Ghaziuddin & Butler, 1998) or
definition of Asperger’s disorder (see Chapter whether some other feature, for example,
4, this Handbook, this volume), the unfortunate prosody, might differentiate autism and As-
problem of markedly different approaches to the perger’s disorder (Ghaziuddin & Gerstein,
definition of the disorder has continued compli- 1996). Perhaps the one thing that can be said
cating comparisons of results across studies. with certainty about current diagnostic ap-
There are now a least five rather different proaches is that there is general agreement that
conceptualizations of Asperger’s disorder in the current official approach (as in DSM-IV and
addition to those provided by ICD-10 and ICD-10) has not been easy to operationalize
DSM-IV (Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaziuddin, and has not proven useful for research. Miller
1992; Klin & Volkmar, 1997; Leekam, Libby, and Ozonoff (1997) have raised the cogent
Wing, Gould, & Gillberg, 2000; Szatmari, point that Asperger’s own cases likely would
Bryson, Boyle, Streiner, & Duku, 2003; Tsai, not meet current official criteria for the disor-
1992; Wing, 1981). Unfortunately, these defi- der; a recent report (Hipller & Klicpera, 2003)
nitions are not always easy to operationalize. of cases seen by Asperger may help inform the
Several major sources of disagreement are ap- current debate (see also Eisenmajer et al., 1996;
parent. The first issue has to do with the Howlin, 2003; Szatmari et al., 2003).
precedence rule, which (in DSM-IV and ICD- It must, however, also be noted that even
10) excludes an individual from Asperger’s if given the lack of general agreement on a gen-
the person ever met the criteria for autism. (As eral diagnostic approach, emerging data are
a practical matter, this ends up, largely, revolv- beginning to suggest some important potential
ing around the age at which parents were first differences between Asperger’s and higher
concerned about the child’s development.) The functioning autism, for example, in terms of
second issue concerns the approach to lan- neuropsychological profiles (Klin et al., 1995;
guage delay (usually operationalized by Lincoln, Courchesne, Kilman, Elmasian, &
whether the child spontaneously used mean- Allen, 1998), comorbidity with other psychi-
ingful words by 24 months and phrases by 36 atric disorders (Klin et al., in press), neu-
months; Howlin, 2003; Klin, Schultz, Pauls, & ropsychological profiles and family genetics
Volkmar, in press). A third major issue has to (Volkmar & Klin, 1998) and outcome (Szat-
do with whether the unusual circumscribed in- mari et al., 2003). The critical issue is whether
terests originally described by Asperger Asperger’s can be shown to differ in important
(1944) must be present for diagnosis; in DSM- respects from either autism or PDD-NOS on
IV and ICD-10, these may be present but are measures other than those used in selecting
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 25

cases in the first place; that is, information on has been increasingly recognized in research
the validity of the disorder is needed in areas studies (Bailey et al., 1998). DSM-IV and
such as differences in patterns of comorbidity, ICD-10 take slightly different approaches to
outcome, response to treatment, family history, this category with ICD-10 providing the possi-
or neuropsychological profiles. The relation- bility for more fine-grained distinctions based
ship of Asperger’s disorder to various other on the way in which full criteria for autism or
diagnostic concepts—for example, schizoid another of the explicitly defined PDDs are
disorder, right hemisphere learning disability, not met. An unfortunate editorial change in
and semantic pragmatic processing disorder— DSM-IV produced some difficulties, which
remains an important topic for research (see have now been rectified in DSM-IV-TR.
Klin, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000 for a review). Specifically, prior to DSM-IV, an individual
Replication of findings based on the same di- had to have problems in social interaction and
agnostic criteria used across sites is critical in communication or restricted interests. In
for progress to be made in this area. Until the DSM-IV, this criterion was changed leading
time when a consensus on the definition of the to an unintended further broadening of the
condition emerges, it will be critical for re- concept.
searchers to employ very clear, operational de- Table 1.5 provides a concise summary and
pictions to allow for replication of findings. comparison of the various disorders presently
included within the overarching PDD category.
Atypical Autism/PDD-NOS
Somewhat paradoxically, studies of what is un- CURRENT CONTROVERSIES
doubtedly the more frequent of the PDDs are IN DIAGNOSIS
uncommon (see Chapter 6, this Handbook, this
volume). This subthreshold category receives Although considerable progress has been made
considerable clinical use, and its importance further work is needed in several areas.

TABLE 1.5 Dif ferential Diagnostic Features of Autism and Nonautistic Pervasive
Developmental Disorders
Disorder

Childhood Pervasive
Autistic Disintegrative Developmental
Feature Disorder Asperger’s Rett’s Disorder Disorder-NOS
Age at recognition 0–36 Usually >36 5–30 >24 Variable
(months)
Sex ratio M>F M>F F (?M) M>F M>F
Loss of skills Variable Usually not Marked Marked Usually not
Social skills Very poor Poor Varies with age Very poor Variable
Communication skills Usually poor Fair Very poor Very poor Fair to good
Circumscribed Variable Marked NA NA Variable
interests (mechanical) (facts)
Family history— Sometimes Frequent Not usually No Unknown
similar problems
Seizure disorder Common Uncommon Frequent Common Uncommon
Head growth No No Yes No No
decelerates
IQ range Severe MR Mild MR to Severe MR Severe MR Severe MR to
to normal normal normal
Outcome Poor to good Fair to good Very poor Very poor Fair to good
Adapted from “Nonautistic Pervasive Developmental Disorders,” chap. 27.2, p. 4, by F. R. Volkmar & D. Cohen, in
Psychiatry, R. Michaels et al., eds. Used with permission from Lippincott-Raven Publishers. NA = Not Applicable.
26 Diagnosis and Classification

Comorbid Conditions and Autism diagnosis in DSM-IV and ICD-10. Both sys-
tems are meant to be comprehensive in cover-
The issue of comorbidity with autism has as- age. However, any system that attempts to
sumed increasing importance in recent years; move past the level of symptom description
it is intimately related to the search for sub- must deal with complicated problems of ensur-
groups of autism. It appears likely that having ing clinical utility, reliability, and validity. As
any serious disability—such as autism or intel- a practical matter, this leads to decisions,
lectual disability—increases the risk for other sometimes fairly obvious and sometimes much
problems, and it is likely that, in the past, less so, about relationships between cate-
autism has tended to overshadow the presence gories, including whether one condition takes
of other difficulties (see Dykens, 2000). precedence over another in a diagnostic hierar-
Autism has now been reported to co-occur chy. The ICD-10 system reflects a nosological
with various other developmental, psychiatric, tradition of searching for a single, parsimo-
and medical conditions (Gillberg & Coleman, nious diagnostic label to explain a patient’s
2000). However, much of this literature rests problems. This top-down approach tends to be
on case reports, and this literature fails to ad- concerned with broader, heuristic diagnoses
dress the more central question of whether as- and is less focused on symptoms as such. On
sociations are observed at greater than chance the other hand, DSM-IV and its immediate pre-
levels and, when this is done, results are gener- decessors have tended to be more bottom up in
ally much less striking (Rutter et al., 1994). orientation. They start with symptoms and
An additional problem is that only positive as- move toward broader categories. No single di-
sociations are typically reported; for example, agnosis is expected to convey the entire range
it is somewhat surprising that failure to thrive of a patient’s major problems, and there is
in infancy is so uncommonly reported in in- more comfort with multiple categorical diag-
fants who go on to have autism. noses, each covering a smaller domain of dif-
Evolving diagnostic concepts and research ficulties. In other words, ICD may miss some
findings have sometimes clarified such associ- trees, and DSM may not capture the forest:
ations. For example, Kanner’s original impres- Each approach has inherent advantages and
sion (1943) that persons with autism had limitations (see Volkmar & Schwab-Stone,
normal intellectual potential has been shown 1996). The DSM-IV approach has some advan-
to be incorrect; although the pattern of cogni- tages for clinical utility; that is, important
tive and adaptive abilities in autism is unusual, symptoms are less likely to be overlooked. It
for the majority of children with autism, over- also does not prejudge the issue of comorbid
all scores on cognitive testing are stable relationships. The ICD-10 approach has the
within the mentally retarded range (see Chap- advantage of providing a more robust big pic-
ter 29, this Handbook, Volume 2). On the other ture less focused on single symptoms and
hand, a substantial minority of persons with minimizing what are often spurious or mean-
autism has cognitive abilities in the average or ingless associations.
above-average range. Similarly, it is now well The issue of comorbidity in relation to
recognized that seizure disorders of various autism is further complicated by the nature of
types are associated with autism in about 25% the syndrome. While autism is a lifelong disor-
of cases (see Chapter 18, this Handbook, this der and probably one of the best examples of a
volume). A much smaller proportion of autis- disorder in psychiatry, symptoms change with
tic individuals exhibit fragile X syndrome or age and developmental level. If the approach to
tuberous sclerosis (see Chapter 18, this Hand- diagnosis focuses on symptoms, an individual
book, this volume). Apart from these well- with autism will receive a large number of ad-
recognized associations, the association of ditional diagnoses over the course of the life
autism with other medical and behavioral con- span, including diagnoses that focus on anxi-
ditions is much less convincing (Rutter, Bai- ety, language, social problems, and the like.
ley, et al., 1994). Such a list of additional diagnoses might serve
Issues relating to comorbidity arise from a useful function by cataloging behaviors in
a major difference between approaches to need of clinical attention. But the list does not
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 27

basically change the fundamental conception autistic persons are largely or entirely mute,
that the person has autism. and for some disorders, this presents a pro-
Given the wide range and severity of the found diagnostic problem (Tsai, 1996). For ex-
disabilities experienced by individuals with ample, early investigators incorrectly assumed
autism, it is not surprising that they are vulner- continuity between autism and schizophrenia.
able to many types of behavioral difficulties, While persons with autism may also develop
including hyperactivity, obsessive-compulsive schizophrenia (Petty, Ornitz, Michelman, &
phenomena, self-injury and stereotypy, tics, Zimmerman, 1985), this does not appear to be
and affective symptoms (Brasic, Barnett, Ka- above the level expected in the general popula-
plan, Sheitman, Aisemberg et al., 1994; Ghaz- tion (Volkmar & Cohen, 1991a). Similarly, the
iuddin et al., 1992; Ghaziuddin, Alessi, & issue of comorbid obsessive-compulsive disor-
Greden, 1995; Jaselskis, Cook, & Fletcher, der and autism has been of interest given the
1992; Nelson & Pribor, 1993; Poustka & use of new pharmacological treatments such as
Lisch, 1993; Quintana et al., 1995; Realmuto the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
& Main, 1982). Interpretation of the available (SSRIs; see Chapter 44, this Handbook, Vol-
data is more complex when you move past the ume 2; Gordon, Rapoport, Hamburger, State,
level of behavioral observation and try to con- & Mannheim, 1992; Gordon, State, Nelson,
sider these associations within a causal Hamburger, & Rapoport, 1993; McDougle,
framework. For example, the diagnosis of Price, Volkmar, & Goodman, 1992). While
Tourette’s syndrome requires only the history phenomena suggestive of obsessions or com-
of motor and vocal tics for a year or more. Do pulsions are often observed in adults with
the compulsive behaviors and vocalizations autism (Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, 1985),
emitted by many individuals with autism and levels of such phenomena vary considerably
intellectual disability warrant a second diag- across samples (Brasic et al., 1994; Fom-
nosis of Tourette’s syndrome? When should bonne, 1992; McDougle et al., 1995), and re-
obsessive-compulsive disorder be diagnosed sponse to medication may not be specific to
in a retarded, autistic individual with many diagnosis. In general, it appears that the ritu-
perseverative behaviors? alistic phenomena of autism and typical ob-
Diagnostic systems like DSM-IV and ICD-10 sessions and compulsions cannot simply be
strive for logical consistency in their approach equated (Baron-Cohen, 1989).
to the problem of diagnosis; this usually means Stereotyped motor movements and other
that some degree of hierarchical decision mannerisms are very common in autism but do
must be employed when, for example, fea- not qualify a case for the additional diagnosis
tures that are part of the definition of autism of stereotyped movement disorder. However, a
are observed in other disorders. Thus, since number of case reports and some case series
stereotyped behaviors are common in autism have suggested a potentially more interesting
and are included as a diagnostic feature in association between autism and Tourette’s
both DSM-IV and ICD-10, persons with disorder. In the latter condition, the child ex-
autism cannot also receive a diagnosis of hibits persistent motor and vocal tics (Burd,
stereotyped movement disorder. Similarly, di- Fisher, Kerbeshian, & Arnold, 1987; Leck-
agnostic problems arise with difficulties that man, Peterson, Pauls, & Cohen, 1997; Nelson
are commonly observed to be “associated fea- & Pribor, 1993; Realmuto & Main, 1982). It
tures” of autism, for example, unusual affective remains to be seen whether such an associa-
responses. On the other hand, mental retarda- tion is more frequent than would be expected
tion is not an essential diagnostic feature of by chance alone, particularly since differenti-
autism, and it is thus possible (and important) ation of tics and stereotyped motor manner-
for this diagnosis and one of autism to be made isms can be confusing for less experienced
when both sets of criteria are satisfied. clinicians.
The task of moving from the level of be- Affective symptoms are frequently ob-
havioral problems and symptoms to formal served in persons with autism. These symp-
psychiatric/developmental diagnosis is com- toms include affective lability, inappropriate
plicated by the nature of autism itself. Half of affective responses, anxiety, and depression.
28 Diagnosis and Classification

For higher functioning autistic persons, an which may suggest disorders of attention.
awareness of their difficulties may result in Hellgren, Gillberg, and Gillberg (1994) have
overt clinical depression. There is some sug- described a putative condition characterized
gestion that adolescents with Asperger’s are at by problems in attention, motor control and
particularly high risk for depression (Klin, perception (DAMP) with features of both
Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000). Bipolar disorders PDD and attention deficit disorder.
have also been reported and may respond to Autistic individuals are not immune to any
drug treatment (Gillberg, 1985; Kerbeshian, other known medical conditions (Chapters 16
Burd, & Fisher, 1987; Komoto, Usui, & Hirata, & 18, this Handbook, this volume). Yet, spe-
1984; Lainhart & Folstein, 1994; Steingard & cific associations between autism and general
Biederman, 1987). medical conditions generally have not been
Given the characteristic difficulties in so- sustained by formal research. Although some
cial interaction and communication, as well as investigators (e.g., Gillberg, 1990) suggest that
the frequent association of autism with mental many different associations are common, stud-
retardation, it is not surprising that deploy- ies that employ stringent diagnostic criteria
ment and sustaining of attention would be have not supported this view (e.g., Rutter, Bai-
problematic for individuals with autism (see ley, Bolton, & Le Couter, 1994). In one sense,
Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume). In this issue is simply definitional. If you take a
DSM-III-R, the convention was established very broad view of autism, a large number of
that autism and attention deficit disorder were persons with profound intellectual disability
made mutually exclusive diagnoses. This was will be included in samples of autistic individ-
based on the clinical belief that attentional uals; this population has a marked increase in
problems in autism were better viewed as an the number of medical conditions that may be
aspect of the autistic condition and develop- significantly involved in the person’s develop-
mental level; there was a clinical impression mental difficulties. The difficulties inherent
that stimulant medications used in the treat- in including such cases among those with more
ment of attention deficit disorder often led to strictly defined autism are exemplified in the
deterioration in the behavior of individuals early reports about the association of autism
with autism. The latter notion has now been with congenital rubella. Children with congen-
called into question (see Towbin, 2003, for a ital rubella initially were reported to have
review), and there is little doubt that atten- many autistic-like features and to be very low
tional difficulties are observed in children functioning; over time, however, the diagnoses
with autism (Charman, 1998), but the question of these cases have proven questionable.
of whether such difficulties are sufficient to
justify an additional diagnosis of attention Subtypes of Autism
deficit disorder remains unclear. Attentional
difficulties may be intrinsically associated Investigators have used various approaches to
with developmental problems and may reflect subtype autism and the broader PDD class of
broader difficulties in cognitive organization conditions. Essentially, these attempts have
(Iacoboni, 2000) without necessarily implying fallen into two broad categories. The more
attention deficit disorder. While some have common approach rests on clinical experience
suggested that attention deficit / hyperactivity and the ability of clinician-investigators to no-
disorder should be considered an additional di- tice features that are then used to delineate a
agnosis and target of treatment in persons with specific diagnostic concept. Kanner’s descrip-
autism (Tsai, 1999), firm empirical data on tion of autism and the work of Asperger, Rett,
this issue are lacking. and Heller are all examples of this approach.
Barkely (1990) has noted that the issue of More recent examples include the proposed
attentional problem is of much greater interest typology based on social characteristics pro-
in children with PDD-NOS. Such children do posed by Wing and colleagues (Wing & Gould,
not exhibit classical autism but have persistent 1979). The major alternative is to utilize more
problems in social interaction and the regula- complex statistical procedures to derive sub-
tion of affective responses and behavior, groups or subtypes empirically. It might seem
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 29

more likely that the latter approach would be analyses. It is a testament to the creativity of
more productive, but, somewhat surprisingly, engaged clinicians and to the human capacity
this really has not been the case. to notice regularities that at least so far the di-
agnostic concepts we are presently familiar
Statistical Approaches to Subtyping
with have emerged from clinical work and not
Complex statistical approaches have been from complex statistical analyses. On the other
helpful in developing and validating screening hand, such analyses may be helpful in examin-
and assessment instruments, as well as in de- ing current diagnostic concepts and alternative
veloping criteria to operationalize diagnostic ways to conceptualize syndrome boundaries. It
concepts. Their value in developing new diag- is possible, in the future, that better diagnostic
nostic categories has been limited by several concepts will be derived, for example, within
factors. Approaches such as cluster and factor the broad category of PDD-NOS.
analysis, in the first place, are very dependent Despite these problems, cluster and factor
on the characteristic of the sample being stud- analytic approaches have been used with some
ied and on the information originally pro- frequency. For example, in an early study,
vided; you cannot identify relevant variables or Prior and colleagues (Prior, Boulton, Gajzago,
combinations of variables if they are not mea- & Perry, 1975) observed two clusters of cases.
sured in the sample in the first place. Since One cluster was more similar to Kanner’s orig-
our knowledge regarding the underlying neuro- inal syndrome in terms of early onset and clin-
pathological basis of autism and its relation- ical features and the other with later onset and
ship to development and behavior remains more complex features. Similarly, Siegel, An-
limited, it is not clear exactly what measures ders, Ciaranello, Bienenstock, and Kramer
would best be included in such analyses. An- (1986) identified four possible subgroups in a
other set of issues surrounds a set of interre- larger group of children with PDDs. Two
lated problems: the marked range in syndrome groups appeared to correspond roughly to low
expression associated with age and develop- and higher functioning autism while the other
mental level and issues related to sample selec- two groups were characterized either by
tion and sample size. Nosological research schizotypal features or affective symptoms
using complex statistical models generally re- and behavior problems. Dahl, Cohen, and
quires large and representative samples of pa- Provence (1986) identified two clusters of
tients. Unfortunately, the samples used in most children in the PDD spectrum who had similar
studies are small and not representative. Re- behavior problems but somewhat different pat-
sults may be highly dependent on the original terns of language functioning and onset. De-
sample and may not generalize to other sam- pending on sample and range of variables
ples. This problem is compounded by the fact included in the analyses, various numbers of
that the meaning of behaviors may change with clusters have been derived. The less robust
age and with developmental level. The diagno- clusters—those with fewer cases and very
sis of autism may be particularly difficult to complex clinical features—are less likely to be
make in very young children below the age of observed in subsequent studies. Eaves, Eaves,
3 years. You might assume that the purest form and Ho (1994) used data from over 150 chil-
of autism is exhibited at this young age. How- dren with autism spectrum disorders. In their
ever, as Lord (1995) has shown, the character- sample, four meaningful subtypes emerged
istic symptoms of autism such as repetitive with different behavioral and cognitive pro-
behaviors often do not clearly develop before files. Over half the sample fell into the subtype
age 3 years while significant social deficits, described as typically autistic; approximately
suggestive of autism, may markedly improve 20% were also autistic but were lower func-
after the first two years of life (see also tioning cognitively. The remaining cases
Rogers, 2001). formed two subtypes: One was a higher func-
The strong developmental nature of changes tioning group with similarities to Asperger’s
in syndrome expression means that variables and another with less severe difficulties. Fein,
such as age, developmental level, or IQ them- Waterhouse, Lucci, and Snyder (1985) identi-
selves become important variables in statistical fied eight cognitive profiles that could be
30 Diagnosis and Classification

related to handedness (Soper et al., 1986) but associated medical condition do not simply
not to more usual autistic features. More re- correspond to obvious behavioral subtypes
cently, Waterhouse and colleagues (1996) (Rutter, 1996). As Rutter has noted (2000),
studied a relatively large group of children conditions such as autism are defined on the
with some form of PDD not associated with an basis of their clinical features, and it is likely
overt medical condition; they suggested that at that complex, multifactorial models will be
least two overlapping continua were present, needed to understand underlying pathophysiol-
corresponding roughly to lower and higher ogy. That is, systems such as DSM and ICD are
functioning autism. strongly influenced by pathophysiology when
Methods other than cluster and factor this is known but should not simply be thought
analysis have been employed as well in the of as classifying by cause.
search for subgroups. For example, I. Cohen, As with the more statistically based ap-
Sudhalter, Landon-Jimenez, and Keogh (1993) proaches, clinically inspired approaches also
utilized a novel system of pattern recognition must deal with the major confounding problem
(neural networks) as well as discriminant of intellectual level. For example, the three-
analyses; they argued that the neural network group subtyping (aloof, passive, active-but-
procedure was superior in correctly identify- odd) proposed by Wing and Gould (1979)
ing whether autism was or was not present. In appears to sort children into relatively reliable
a well-controlled study by Cicchetti, Volkmar, groups; the typology has some measure of va-
Klin, and Showalter (1995), however, the lidity as well as potential benefits for planning
neural networks procedure was not as effective interventions (Borden & Ollendick, 1994;
as the simple diagnostic algorithm proposed in Castelloe & Dawson, 1993; Volkmar & Cohen,
ICD-10 and DSM-IV. 1989). However, differences among the sub-
Multivariate methods have also been uti- groups appear to be largely a function of asso-
lized to validate existing diagnostic groupings ciated IQ. When IQ is controlled for,
and new possible subgroups, for example, differences among the groups largely vanish
within the broad PDD-NOS category (see also (Volkmar & Cohen, 1989).
Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). Van Individuals with profound mental retarda-
der Gaag et al. (1995) utilized a multivariate tion exhibit a number of autistic-like features
cluster analysis and demonstrated differences (Wing & Gould, 1979) without, however,
between cases with autistic disorder and a spe- meeting full criteria for autism. Such cases
cific subtype of PDD-NOS (multiplex or mul- have many of the same service needs as those
tiple complex developmental disorder) on the with more strictly defined autism. Various in-
basis of clinical and developmental features. vestigators have, accordingly, proposed a dis-
tinction among primary, higher, and lower
Clinical Approaches to Subtyping
functioning autism given the very different
The issue of subtypes has also been approached patterns of educational need, associated med-
from a clinical standpoint. Wing and Gould ical problems, outcome, family history, and so
(1979) proposed a classification scheme based forth associated with lower and higher IQ
on the nature of observed patterns of social in- (Cohen, Paul, & Volkmar, 1986; Rutter, 1996;
teraction (aloof, passive, active-but-odd; see Tsai, 1992; Waterhouse et al., 1996). This im-
also Chapter 7, this Handbook, this volume). portant issue remains unresolved. Similarly, it
Other classifications have focused on cognitive is clear that, over time, children with severe
profiles (Fein et al., 1985), language problems developmental language disorders go on to ex-
(Rapin, 1991; Rapin & Allen, 1983), presence hibit marked social difficulties (Howlin,
of signs of overt central nervous system dys- Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000) so that the issue of
function (Tsai, Tsai, & August, 1985), and so the connection between language disorders
forth. A decade ago, it appeared that possible and autism remains an important area of
associations of autism with various medical study.
conditions would have major implications for
Developmental Regression
understanding subtypes and etiology. At pres-
ent, however, it appears that distinctions based Various studies have suggested that perhaps
on the presence of a strictly defined etiology or 20% to 25% of children with autism have some
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 31

degree of developmental regression (see Chap- older (and make communicative gains).
ter 3, this Handbook, this volume). Unfortu- Stereotyped mannerisms also become some-
nately, this phenomenon remains poorly what more common when children become
understood and, in part as a result, controver- older while other features (e.g., persistent pre-
sial. Most studies have utilized parent report occupation with parts of objects) are consis-
with all the attendant problems of definition, tently observed.
reliability, and validity. In some cases, parents Examination of some of the items not in-
report a pattern less of regression and more cluded in DSM-IV/ICD-10 also illustrates this
one of developmental stagnation; in other issue. Abnormal pitch/tone is largely a phe-
cases, the report is of a regression but the nomenon observed in older individuals while
history may also be remarkable for prior devel- attachments to unusual objects are less com-
opmental delays. Finally, in some cases, a dra- monly observed in older individuals. Similarly,
matic regression is observed (Siperstein & the phenomenon of hyper- or hyposensitivity to
Volkmar, 2004). The most common pattern is the inanimate environment has a complicated
one in which a few words are apparently ac- developmental course with features exhibited
quired and then lost. The more dramatic cases at some ages and not others (see Chapter 32,
(e.g., where hundreds of words are acquired this Handbook, Volume 2).
and then lost) are often more consistent with a
diagnosis of childhood disintegrative disorder; Autism in Infants and Young Children
however, the latter condition, by definition,
has its onset after age 2. It is possible that Increased awareness (on the part of both the
some of the earlier and more dramatic cases general public and health care providers) and
of regression are expressions of the earliest advances in early diagnosis have led to a
forms of childhood disintegrative disorder. In change in the age at which autism is first diag-
any event, the study of this phenomenon (ide- nosed. A decade ago, diagnosis at age 4 was
ally at the time it happens) using various relatively typical (Siegel, Pliner, Eschler, &
methodologies (genetics, neuroimaging, EEG, Elliott, 1988)—even when parents had been
etc.) is critically needed. concerned much earlier. It is now more com-
mon for specialized diagnostic centers to see
Developmental Change
children at age 2 years (Lord, 1995; Moore &
Important issues of developmental change in Goodson, 2003) or even younger (Klin, Ca-
syndrome expression (over both age and IQ hawarska, Paul, Rubin, Morgan, et al., 2004).
level) have been recognized for many years The increased interest in early diagnosis and
(Rutter, 1970). Diagnostic systems such as the increasing numbers of younger children
DSM-IV and ICD-10 have generally adopted presenting for assessment present special prob-
the stance of providing criteria that are specif- lems for diagnosis. In contrast to older individ-
ically meant to cover this range of syndrome uals, the diagnosis of infants and very young
expression. An alternative, if rather unwieldy, children is more complex (Charman & Baird,
approach is to provide different diagnostic cri- 2002; Cox et al., 1999; Stone et al., 1999) with
teria either for different age groups or for dif- diagnostic stability increasing after about age
ferent levels of impairment (e.g., depending on 2 years (Courchesne, 2002; Dawson et al.,
level of communicative ability). 2002). However, developmental changes in this
Examination of the data from the DSM-IV age group can be marked (Szatmari, Merette,
field trial illustrates some of these issues. For Bryson, Thivierge, Roy, et al., 2002). For ex-
example, if we utilize the phi statistic to evalu- ample, the repetitive behaviors typical of older
ate the ability of criteria to predict autism, the children are much less common in very young
criteria included in DSM-IV and ICD-10 are children (Charman & Baird, 2002; Cox et al.,
generally comparably powerful predictors 1999; Lord, 1995; Moore & Goodson, 2003;
across age and developmental level with some Stone et al., 1999). Social abnormalities may
expectable but not overly dramatic exceptions; become more striking as the child matures
for example, stereotyped language use and (Lord, Storoschuk, Rutter, & Pickles, 1993).
problems in conversation would be expected to A few studies have addressed the appli-
become more common as children become cability of DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria in
32 Diagnosis and Classification

infants and young children. It appears that critically needed. Given the very limited liter-
some young children will meet criteria for ature on the topic of cultural factors, this area
autism, but some may not necessarily fulfill is one ripe for future research. Chapter 48
the required repetitive behavior criteria until (this Handbook, Volume 2) provides an inter-
around their third birthday (Lord, 1996). Less national perspective on this problem.
commonly, a child appears to meet criteria for
Defining the Broader Phenotype
autism but then, over time, makes substantial
gain. Some alternatives to DSM-IV and ICD- Somewhat paradoxically as the definition of
10 have been proposed (e.g., National Center autism has become more elaborated, interest
for Clinical Infant Programs [NCCIP], 1994) has also increased in the broader spectrum of
but have not met with wide acceptance due to difficulties apparently inherited in families
both practical and theoretical concerns. (see Chapter 16, this Handbook, this volume).
Considerable efforts have gone into the de- Most investigators would now agree what is
velopment of methods to facilitate screening transmitted genetically includes not only clas-
and early diagnosis (see Chapter 27, this Hand- sical autism (Kanner, 1943) but a broader
book, Volume 2). Given the apparent associa- range of difficulties variously impacting on
tion of early identification and intervention social development, communication, and/or
with improved outcome (NRC, 2001) the issues behavior. Attempts are now being made to
of early diagnosis have assumed increasing im- stratify families based on various measures
portance. In addition to the various approaches initially designed for use in more stringently
for screening based on history and direct ob- diagnostic autistic samples (Bishop, 1998;
servation, new approaches are needed in which Constantino & Todd, 2003; Lord, 1990; Lord
screening becomes more behavioral and less et al., 2000; Shao et al., 2002; Tadevosyan-
subjective (and thus more readily available in Leyfer et al., 2003; Tanguay, Robertson, &
nonspecialist settings; see Chawarska, Klin, & Derrick, 1998). Such approaches hold promise
Volkmar, 2003). for identifying broader dimensions of func-
tion /dysfunction in families. The development
Cultural Issues and Diagnosis
of new methods for assessing the broader phe-
The issue of cultural factors in the diagnosis of notype (e.g., Bishop, 1998; Constantino &
autism has been the subject of remarkably lit- Todd, 2003) is of great interest in this regard.
tle discussion. As Brown and Rogers (2003) In addition to both the more strictly de-
point out, this is somewhat paradoxical given fined cases of autism, the broader range of
the various governmental and other mandates autism spectrum disorders includes difficul-
for the study of cultural factors. While by no ties that do not fit neatly into our current clas-
means excusing the dearth of studies, several sification scheme. Such cases of atypical
factors likely have operated to reduce interest autism test the boundaries of our classification
in this area. First, the general impression of system but also serve to underscore the impor-
clinicians seeing children from a range of cul- tant point that individuals with these condi-
tures and subcultures around the world is one tions have not always read the textbooks and
of how much more alike than different chil- may exhibit unusual patterns of difficulty sug-
dren are. While variations in treatment and, to gestive of autism in some ways but also with
some extent, theoretical conceptualizations important differences. Children reared in pro-
differ (see Chapter 48, this Handbook, Volume foundly impoverished environments may ex-
2), it is a testament to the robustness of autism hibit marked social difficulties and other
as a diagnostic concept that cultural influences problems suggestive of autism (Rutter, 1999).
are not more striking. One potential exception Similar issues arise with respect to children
(although one tending to prove the rule) relates who are congenitally blind (Hobson & Bishop,
to the high levels of autistic-like behavior in 2003). Yet another set of issues arises with re-
individuals who suffer severe early institu- gard to children who, at least initially, seem to
tional deprivation (Rutter, 1999). More rigor- exhibit problems more suggestive of a lan-
ous and well-controlled studies on the issue of guage disorder but, over time, exhibit a course
social-cultural factors in autism are clearly and outcome in some ways more suggestive of
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 33

autism (Mawhood, Howlin, & Rutter, 2000). disorder (Bishop, 1989, 2002), also remain to
Issues with regard to differentiation of autism be clearly established.
and Asperger’s and language disorders have While DSM-IV and ICD-10 are the most re-
been noted (Bishop, 2000; Bishop & Norbury, cent and most extensively evaluated diagnostic
2002). Cases with unusual features or presen- approaches for autism, they are undoubtedly
tations are of great interest in that they may not the last word on diagnosis. The present
help to clarify syndrome boundaries, under- DSM-IV and ICD-10 systems have the consid-
score areas where knowledge is lacking, and erable advantage of being based on a relatively
may clarify alternative mechanisms or devel- extensive set of data; they have clearly facili-
opmental pathways. For example, while there tated research and service. The dual-use con-
is little disagreement that higher functioning straints on DSM, that is, the use of the same
autism and Asperger’s disorder both are char- criteria for both research and service, meant
acterized by significant problems in social in- that brevity and ease of use were important
teraction in the face of average overall considerations. The ICD-10 system does not,
cognitive ability, the social difficulties appear at least for the research definitions, have this
to arise in the context of rather different devel- constraint. It remains to be seen whether the
opmental pathways and trajectories, for exam- more detailed ICD-10 research definition will,
ple, with preservation of language skills early in the end, predominate. From the point of
on, and possibly later, in Asperger’s but not in view of research, the attempt to link diagnostic
higher functioning autism (see Chapter 5, this instruments specially to diagnostic criteria is a
Handbook, this volume). considerable advantage and may mean that for
research purposes, in effect, the more detailed
CONCLUSION research definition will come to dominate.
Probably the greatest nosological need at
Leo Kanner’s description (1943) of the syn- present is the classification of conditions that
drome of early infantile autism has proven to appear to fall within the broad class of the
be robust and enduring. To a remarkable de- PDDs but do not meet criteria for presently
gree, his observations and intuitions remain recognized disorders. This group of condi-
fresh and inspiring. False leads in the original tions, referred to either as “atypical autism” or
work have been clarified by research. We are “pervasive developmental disorder not other-
also aware of how much work remains 60 wise specified,” includes a larger number of
years later. children than those who are stringently de-
Studies have clarified that the disintegra- fined as autistic. Their nosological status is
tive PDDs (Rett’s disorder and childhood dis- much less well defined (see Chapter 6, this
integrative disorder) differ from strictly Handbook, this volume). Concepts such as
defined autism in various ways (Tsai, 1992; multiplex developmental disorder have been
Volkmar & Rutter, 1995); the study of these proposed for some of these individuals. A large
unusual conditions may be helpful in clarify- subgroup of such cases is associated with se-
ing mechanisms of pathogenesis relevant vere mental handicap. These conditions re-
to autism (see Chapters 3 & 5, this Hand- quire special services similar to those required
book, this volume). The validity of the for autism (Wing & Gould, 1979); their rela-
newest PDD—Asperger’s disorder—apart tionship to strictly defined autism remains an
from higher functioning autism is less clearly area of considerable interest and may have par-
established and results contradictory (al- ticular importance for family-genetic studies
though often based on markedly differing de- (Rutter, 1996). Biological and behavioral re-
finitions of the disorder; Gilchrist et al., 2001; search depends on well-defined groups of pa-
Klin et al., 1995; Manjiviona & Prior, 1999; tients and rigorous application of diagnostic
Miller & Ozonoff, 2000; Ozonoff, Pennington, methodologies. For example, genetic studies
& Rogers, 1991). The boundaries of As- require clear definition of affected individuals
perger’s disorder with autism and other disor- and exclusion of false-positive cases. In turn,
ders, such as schizoid disorder of childhood we can hope that future nosologies will be en-
(Wolff, 1998, 2000) and semantic-pragmatic riched by the inclusion of other types of data,
34 Diagnosis and Classification

including genetic, neuroimaging, neurochemi- Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). A com-
cal, and other behavioral and biological mark- parative study of infantile autism and specific
ers. Thus, there is a critical dialectic between developmental receptive language disorders—
research in nosology and research of other III. Discriminant function analysis. Journal of
types. Advances in both fields are mutually Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7,
383–396.
dependent and have the same goal: enhancing
Bender, L. (1946). Childhood schizophrenia. Amer-
the understanding and care of individuals and ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 17, 40–56.
advancing our understanding of autism and re- Bishop, D. V. (1989). Autism, Asperger’s syndrome
lated conditions (Rutter, 1999). and semantic-pragmatic disorder: Where are
the boundaries? [Special issue: Autism].
Cross-References British Journal of Disorders of Communica-
tion, 24(2), 107–121.
Other syndromes presently included as PDDs Bishop, D. V. (1998). Development of the Chil-
are discussed in Chapters 3 through 6; Chapter dren’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A
21 provides an alternative view of issues of di- method for assessing qualitative aspects of
communicative impairment in children. Jour-
agnosis and classification; changes in syn-
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
drome expression are discussed in Chapters 8
Allied Disciplines, 39(6), 879–891.
through 10. Bishop, D. V. (2000). What’s so special about As-
perger syndrome? The need for further explo-
REFERENCES ration of the borderlands of autism. In A. Klin
& F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Asperger syndrome
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diag- (pp. 254–277). New York: Guilford Press.
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- Bishop, D. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring
ders. Washington, DC: Author. the borderlands of autistic disorder and spe-
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diag- cific language impairment: A study using
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal
ders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag- Disciplines, 43(7), 917–929.
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- Bleuler, E. (1951). Dementia Praecox, or the Group
ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. of Schizophrenia. Translated by J. Zinkin. New
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag- York: International Universities Press. (Origi-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- nal work published 1911)
ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Borden, M. C., & Ollendick, T. H. (1994). An ex-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag- amination of the validity of social subtypes in
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Disorders, 24(1), 23–37.
Amir, R. E., Van den Veyver, I. B., Wan, M., Tran, Brasic, J. R., Barnett, J. Y., Kaplan, D., Sheitman,
C. Q., Francke, U., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (1999). B. B., Aisemberg, P., Lafargue, R. T., et al.
Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X- (1994). Clomipramine ameliorates adventitious
linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding movements and compulsions in prepubertal
protein 2 [See comment]. Nature Genetics, boys with autistic disorder and severe mental
23(2), 185–188. retardation. Neurology, 44(7), 1309–1312.
Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autistichen Psychopa- Brown, J. R., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). Cultural is-
then” im Kindersalter. Archive fur psychiatrie sues in autism. In R. L. Hendren, S. Ozonoff,
und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136. & S. Rogers (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Cou- (pp. 209–226). Washington, DC: American
teur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in rel- Psychiatric Press.
atives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Buitelaar, J. K., Van der Gaag, R., Klin, A., & Volk-
Disorders, 28(5), 369–392. mar, F. (1999). Exploring the boundaries of
Barkely, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactiv- Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Other-
ity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and wise Specified: Analyses of data from the
treatment. New York: Guilford Press. DSM-IV autistic field trial. Journal of Autism
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Do autistic children have and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 33–43.
obsessions and compulsions? British Journal of Burd, L., Fisher, W. W., Kerbeshian, J., & Arnold,
Clinical Psychology, 28(3), 193–200. M. E. (1987). Is development of Tourette dis-
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 35

order a marker for improvement in patients Cohen, I. L., Sudhalter, V., Landon-Jimenez, D., &
with autism and other pervasive developmen- Keogh, M. (1993). A neural network approach
tal disorders? Journal of the American Acad- to the classification of autism. Journal of
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(2), Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(3),
162–165. 443–466.
Candland, D. K. (1993). Feral children and clever Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic
animals: Ref lections on human nature. Oxford traits in the general population: A twin study.
University Press, 411. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5),
Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Classification of Child and 524–530.
Adolescent Psychopathology. Journal of Child Courchesne, E. (2002). Abnormal early brain de-
Psychiatry and Psychology, 37, 3–12. velopment in autism. Molecular Psychiatry,
Cantwell, D. P., Baker, L., & Rutter, M. (1979). 7(Suppl. 2), S21–S23.
Families of autistic children and dysphasic Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-
children: Family life and interaction patterns. Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999).
Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 682–687. Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42
Castelloe, P., & Dawson, G. (1993). Subclassifica- months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R
tion of children with autism and pervasive de- diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and
velopmental disorder: A questionnaire based Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5),
on Wing’s subgrouping scheme. Journal of 719–732.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(2), Dahl, E. K., Cohen, D. J., & Provence, S. (1986).
229–241. Clinical and multivariate approaches to the
Charman, T. (1998). Specifying the nature and nosology of pervasive developmental disor-
course of the joint attention impairment in ders. Journal of the American Academy of
autism in the preschool years: Implications for Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 170–180.
diagnosis and intervention. Autism: Interna- Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J.,
tional Journal of Research and Practice, 2(1), McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neu-
61–79. rocognitive function and joint attention ability
Charman, T., & Baird, G. (2002). Practitioner re- in young children with autism spectrum disor-
view: Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder der versus developmental delay. Child Devel-
in 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Child opment, 73(2), 345–358.
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- DeMyer, M. K., Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson, R. K.
plines, 43(3), 289–305. (1981). Infantile autism reviewed: A decade of
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7(3), 388–451.
Automatic attention cueing through eye move- Dykens, E. M. (2000). Psychopathology in children
ment in 2-year-old children with autism. Child with intellectual disability. Journal of Child
Development, 74(4), 1108–1122. Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
Chess, S., Fernandez, P., & Korn, S. (1978). Behav- plines, 41(4), 407–417.
ioral consequences of congenital rubella. Jour- Eaves, L. C., Eaves, D. M., & Ho, H. H. (1994).
nal of Pediatrics, 93(4), 699–703. Subtypes of autism by cluster analysis. Journal
Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(1),
Showalter, D. (1995). Diagnosing autism using 3–22.
ICD-10 criteria: A comparison of neural net- Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L.,
works and standard multi variate procedures. Ong, B., Gould, J., et al. (1996). Comparison
Journal of Child Neuropsychology, 1(1), 26–37. of clinical symptoms in autism and Asperger’s
Cohen, D. J. (1976). The diagnostic process in disorder. Journal of the American Academy of
child psychiatry. Psychiatric Annals, 6(9), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(11),
29–56. 1523–1531.
Cohen, D. J. (1980). The pathology of the self in pri- Factor, D. C., Freeman, N. L., & Kardash, A. (1989,
mary childhood autism and Gilles de la December). A comparison of DSM-III and
Tourette syndrome. In B. Blinder (Ed.), Psychi- DSM-III-R criteria for autism. Journal of
atric clinics of North America (pp. 383–402). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(4),
Philadelphia: Saunders. 637–640.
Cohen, D. J., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. R. (1986). Is- Fein, D., Waterhouse, L., Lucci, D., & Snyder, D.
sues in the classification of pervasive and other (1985). Cognitive subtypes in developmentally
developmental disorders: Toward DSM-IV. disabled children: A pilot study. Journal of
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psy- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(1),
chiatry, 25(2), 213–220. 77–95.
36 Diagnosis and Classification

Fombonne, E. (1992). Diagnostic assessment in a double-blind comparison of clomipramine, de-


sample of autistic and developmentally im- sipramine, and placebo in the treatment of
paired adolescents [Special issue: Classifica- autistic disorder. Archives of General Psychia-
tion and diagnosis]. Journal of Autism and try, 50(6), 441–447.
Developmental Disorders, 22(4), 563–581. Harper, J., & Williams, S. (1975). Age and type of
Frances, A. J., Widiger, T. A., & Pincus, H. A. onset as critical variables in early infantile
(1989). The development of DSM-IV. Archives autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood
of General Psychiatry, 46, 373–375. Schizophrenia, 5, 25–35.
Ghaziuddin, M., Alessi, N., & Greden, J. F. (1995). Heller, T. (1908). Dementia Infantilis. Zeitschrift
Life events and depression in children with fur die Erforschung und Behandlung des Ju-
pervasive disorders. Journal of Autism and De- genlichen. Schwachsinns, 2, 141–165.
velopmental Disorders, 25(5), 495–502. Hellgren, L., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I. C. (1994).
Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in Children with deficits in attention, motor con-
autism and Asperger syndrome: A further re- trol and perception (DAMP) almost grown up:
port. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re- The contribution of various background to
search, 42(Pt. 1), 43–48. outcome at age 16 years. European Child and
Ghaziuddin, M., & Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic Adolescent Psychiatry, 3(1), 1–15.
speaking style differentiates Asperger syn- Hendry, C. N. (2000). Childhood disintegrative dis-
drome from high-functioning autism. Journal order: Should it be considered a distinct diag-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(6), nosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 77–90.
585–595. Hertzig, M. E., Snow, M. E., New, E., & Shapiro, T.
Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1992). (1990). DSM-III and DSM-III-R diagnosis of
Comorbidity of autistic disorder in children autism and pervasive. Journal of the American
and adolescents. European Child and Adoles- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
cent Psychiatry, 1(4), 209–213. 29(1), 123–126.
Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rut- Hippler, K., & Klicpera, C. (2003). A retrospective
ter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development analysis of the clinical case records of “autis-
and current functioning in adolescents with tic psychopaths” diagnosed by Hans Asperger
Asperger syndrome: A comparative study. and his team at the University Children’s Hos-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry pital, Vienna. In U. Frith & E. Hill (Eds.),
and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 227–240. Autism: Mind and Brain (pp. 21–42). Oxford,
Gillberg, C. (1985). Asperger’s syndrome and re- England: Oxford University Press.
current psychosis: A case study. Journal of Hobbs, N. (1975). Issues in the classification of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(4), children. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
389–397. Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (2003). The pathogen-
Gillberg, C. (1990). Medical work-up in children esis of autism: Insights from congenital blind-
with autism and Asperger syndrome. Brain ness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Dysfunction, 3(5/6), 249–260. Society of London—Series B: Biological Sci-
Gillberg, C. (1991). Debate and argument: Is ences, 358(1430), 335–344.
autism a pervasive developmental disorder? Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry adults with autism with and without early lan-
and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1169–1170. guage delays: Implications for the differentia-
Gillberg, C. (1994). Debate and argument: Having tion between autism and Asperger syndrome.
Rett syndrome in the ICD-10 PDD category Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
does not make sense. Journal of Child Psychol- ders, 33(1), 3–13.
ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000).
35(2), 377–378. Autism and developmental receptive language
Gillberg, C., & M. Coleman. (2000). The biology of disorders—a follow-up comparison in early
the autistic syndromes (3rd ed.). London: Mac adult life: II. Social, behavioural, and psychi-
Keith Press. atric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology
Gordon, C. T., Rapoport, J. L., Hamburger, S. D., and Psychiatry, 41, 561–578.
State, R. C., & Mannheim, G. B. (1992). Dif- Iacoboni, M. (2000). Attention and sensorimotor
ferential response of seven subjects with autis- integration: Mapping the embodied mind. In
tic disorder to clomipramine and desipramine. A. W. Toga & J. C. Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149(3), mapping: The systems (pp. 463–490). San
363–366. Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Gordon, C. T., State, R. C., Nelson, J. E., Ham- Jaselskis, C. A., Cook, E. H., & Fletcher, K. E.
burger, S. D., & Rapoport, J. L. (1993). A (1992). Clonidine treatment of hyperactive
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 37

and impulsive children with autistic disorder. pp. xi–xx). Washington, DC: American Psychi-
Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, atric Association.
12(5), 322–327. Lainhart, J. E., & Folstein, S. E. (1994). Affective
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- disorders in people with autism: A review of
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. published cases. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Kerbeshian, J., Burd, L., & Fisher, W. (1987). opmental Disorders, 24(5), 587–601.
Lithium carbonate in the treatment of two pa- Leckman, J. F., Peterson, B. S., Pauls, D. L., &
tients with infantile autism and atypical bipo- Cohen, D. J. (1997). Tic disorders. Psychiatric
lar symptomatology. Journal of Clinical Clinics of North America, 20(4), 839–861.
Psychopharmacology, 7(6), 401–405. Leekam, S., Libby, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Gill-
Klin, A., Chawarska, K., Paul, R., Rubin, E., Mor- berg, C. (2000). Comparison of ICD-10 and
gan, T., Wiesner, L., & Volkmar, F. R. (2004). Gillberg’s criteria for Asperger syndrome
Autism in a 15 month old child. American [Special issue: Asperger syndrome]. Autism:
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1981–1988. Journal of Research and Practice, 4(1), 11–28.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. Lincoln, A. E., Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., El-
(2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to masian, R., & Allen, M. (1998). Neurobiology
cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical of Asperger syndrome: Seven case studies and
Transactions of the Royal Society of London— quantitative magnetic resonance imaging find-
Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), ings. In E. Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, & L. J.
345–360. Kunc (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high func-
Klin, A., Lang, J., Cicchetti, D. V., & Volkmar, tioning autism? (pp. 145–166). New York:
F. R. (2000). Brief report: Interrater reliability Plenum Press.
of clinical diagnosis and DSM-IV criteria for Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1969). A five to fifteen
autistic disorder: Results of the DSM-IV year follow-up study of infantile psychosis:
autism field trial. Journal of Autism and Devel- III. Psychological Aspects. British Journal of
opmental Disorders, 30(2), 163–167. Psychiatry, 115, 865–882.
Klin, A., Schultz, R., Pauls, D., & Volkmar, F. R. Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1970). A five to fifteen
(in press). Three diagnostic approaches to As- year follow-up study of infantile psychosis.
perger’s Disorder. Journal of Autism and De- British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
velopmental Disorders. ogy, 9, 152–163.
Klin, A., Sparrow, S., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). As- Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two year olds re-
perger’s syndrome. New York: Guilford Press. ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Asperger’s Syn- Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1365–1382.
drome. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Lord, C., Storoschuk, S., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A.
Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Develop- (1993). Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism in
mental Disorders (2nd ed., pp. 94–122). New preschool children. Infant Mental Health Jour-
York: Wiley. nal, 14(3), 234–252.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (Eds.). Magnusson, P., & Saemundsen, E. (2001). Preva-
(2000). Asperger syndrome. New York: Guil- lence of autism in Iceland. Journal of Autism
ford Press. and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 153–163.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, Mahler, M. (1952). On child psychoses and schizo-
D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and phrenia: Autistic and symbiotic infantiles psy-
neuropsychological characterization of As- choses. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 7,
perger syndrome: Convergence with nonverbal 286–305.
learning disabilities syndrome. Journal of Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. (1999). Neuropsycho-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(7), logical profiles of children with Asperger
1127–1140. syndrome and autism. Autism: International
Kolvin, I. (1971). Studies in childhood psychoses: Journal of Research and Practice, 3(4),
Diagnostic criteria and classification. British 327–356.
Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 381–384. Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000).
Komoto, J., Usui, S., & Hirata, J. (1984). Infantile Autism and developmental receptive language
autism and affective disorder. Journal of disorder—a comparative follow-up in early
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(1), adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes.
81–84. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
Kramer, M. (1968). The history of the efforts to and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 547–559.
agree on an international classification of McDougle, C. J., Kresch, L. E., Goodman, W. K.,
mental disorders. In Diagnostic and statistical Naylor, S. T., Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., et al.
manual of mental disorders (2nd ed., (1995). A case-controlled study of repetitive
38 Diagnosis and Classification

thoughts and behavior in adults with autistic Evidence from a family history study of multiple-
disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. incidence autism families. American Journal
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(5), of Psychiatry, 154(2), 185–190.
772–777. Poustka, F., & Lisch, S. (1993). Autistic behaviour
McDougle, C. J., Price, L. H., Volkmar, F. R., & domains and their relation to self-injurious be-
Goodman, W. K. E. (1992). Clomipramine in haviour. Acta Paedopsychiatrica: International
autism: Preliminary evidence of efficacy. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Journal of the American Academy of Child and 56(2), 69–73.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(4), 746–750. Prior, M., Boulton, D., Gajzago, C., & Perry, D.
Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (1997). Did Asperger’s (1975). The classification of childhood psy-
cases have Asperger disorder? A research choses by numerical taxonomy. Journal of
note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16(4),
try, 38(2), 247–251. 321–330.
Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (2000). The external va- Provence, S., & Dahl, K. (1987). Disorders of atyp-
lidity of Asperger disorder: Lack of evidence ical development: Diagnostic issues raised by a
from the domain of neuropsychology. Journal spectrum disorder. In D. Cohen & A. Donnel-
of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 227–238. lan (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive
Moore, V., & Goodson, S. (2003). How well does developmental disorders (pp. 677–689). New
early diagnosis of autism stand the test of York: Wiley.
time? Follow-up study of children assessed for Quintana, H., Birmaher, B., Stedge, D., Lennon, S.,
autism at age 2 and development of an early di- Freed, J., Bridge, J., et al. (1995). Use of
agnostic service. Autism: International Jour- methylphenidate in the treatment of children
nal of Research and Practice, 7(1), 47–63. with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and
Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, Developmental Disorders, 25(3), 283–294.
T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of Rank, B. (1949). Adaptation of the psychoanalytic
autism: The contribution of non-verbal. Jour- technique for the treatment of young children
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and with atypical development. American Journal
Allied Disciplines, 27(5), 657–669. of Orthopsychiatry, 19, 130–139.
National Center for Clinical Infant Programs. Rapin, I. (1991). Autistic children: Diagnosis and
(1994). Diagnostic classification: 0–3. Na- clinical features. Pediatrics, 87(5, Pt. 2),
tional Center for Clinical Infant Programs. 751–760.
Washington, DC: Author. Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental
National Research Council. (2001). Educating language disorders: Nosologic considerations.
young children with Autism. Washington, DC: In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language,
National Academy Press. reading and spelling (pp. 155–184). New York:
Nelson, E. C., & Pribor, E. F. (1993). A calendar Academic Press.
savant with autism and Tourette syndrome: Realmuto, G. M., & Main, B. (1982). Coincidence
Response to treatment and thoughts on the in- of Tourette’s disorder and infantile autism.
terrelationships of these conditions. Annals of Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Clinical Psychiatry, 5(2), 135–140. ders, 12(4), 367–372.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. Rett, A. (1966). Uber ein eigenartiges hirn-
(1991). Executive function deficits in high- tophisces Syndroem bei hyperammonie im
functioning autistic individuals: Relationship Kindersalter. Wein Medizinische Wochen-
to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology schrift, 118, 723–726.
and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081–1105. Ritvo, E. R., & Freeman, B. J. (1977). National So-
Petty, L. K., Ornitz, E. M., Michelman, J. D., & ciety for Autistic Children definition of the
Zimmerman, E. G. (1985). Autistic children syndrome of. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,
who become schizophrenic. Annual Progress in 2(4), 142–145.
Child Psychiatry and Child Development. Rogers, S. (2001). Diagnosis of autism before the
Pickles, A., Starr, E., Kazak, S., Bolton, P., Pa- age of 3. International review of research in
panikolaou, K., Bailey, A., et al. (2000). Vari- mental retardation: Autism (23), 1–31.
able expression of the autism broader Rogers, S., & Ozonoff, S. (in press). What do we
phenotype: Findings from extended pedigrees. know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry critical review of the empirical evidence. Jour-
and Allied Disciplines, 41(4), 491–502. nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
Piven, J., Palmer, P., Jacobi, D., Childress, D., & Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R.
Arndt, S. (1997). Broader autism phenotype: (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychiatric,
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 39

social, and behavioral. Journal of the American Sartorius, N. (1988). International perspectives of
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24(4), 465–473. psychiatric classification. British Journal of
Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to Psychiatry, 152(Suppl.), 9–14.
adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450. Shao, Y., Raiford, K. L., Wolpert, C. M., Cope,
Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnostic validity in child psy- H. A., Ravan, S. A., Ashley-Koch, A. A., et al.
chiatry. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 2, (2002). Phenotypic homogeneity provides in-
2–22. creased support for linkage on chromosome 2
Rutter, M. (1994). Debate and argument: There are in autistic disorder. American Journal of
connections between brain and mind and it is Human Genetics, 70(4), 1058–1061.
important that Rett syndrome be classified Short, A., & Schopler, E. (1988). Factors relat-
somewhere. Journal of Child Psychology and ing to age of onset in autism. Journal of
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35(2), Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18,
379–381. 207–216.
Rutter, M. (1996). Autism research: Prospects and Siegel, B., Anders, T. F., Ciaranello, R. D., Bienen-
priorities. Journal of Autism and Developmen- stock, B., & Kraemer, H. C. (1986). Empiri-
tal Disorders, 26, 257–276. cally derived subclassification of the autistic
Rutter, M. (1999). The Emmanual Miller Memor- syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
ial Lectur 1998: Autism: Two-way interplay tal Disorders, 16(3), 275–293.
between research and clinical world. Journal Siegel, B., Pliner, C., Eschler, J., & Elliott, G. R.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, (1988). How children with autism are diag-
169–188. nosed: Difficulties in identification of chil-
Rutter, M. (2000). Genetic studies of autism: From dren with multiple developmental delays.
the 1970s into the millennium. Journal of Ab- Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pedi-
normal Child Psychology, 28(1), 3–14. atrics, 9(4), 199–204.
Rutter, M. (2002). Classification: Conceptual is- Siegel, B., Vukicevic, J., Elliott, G. R., & Kraemer,
sues and substantive findings. In E. Taylor & H. C. (1989). The use of signal detection the-
M. Rutter (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychi- ory to assess DSM-III-R criteria for autistic
atry (4th ed., pp. 3–17). Oxford, England: disorder. Journal of the American Academy of
Blackwell. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4),
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couter, A. 542–548.
(1994). Autism and known medical conditions: Simon, N. (1978). Kaspar Hauser’s recovery and
Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol- autopsy: A perspective on neurological and
ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, sociological requirements for language devel-
35(2), 311–322. opment. Journal of Autism and Childhood
Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental Schizophrenia, 8(2), 209–217.
psychopathology. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Siperstein, R., & Volkmar, F. R. (2004). Parental
Mussen’s handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. reporting of regression in children with perva-
Socialization, personality and child develop- sive developmental disorders. Journal of
ment (pp. 755–911). New York: Wiley. Autism and Developmental Disorders.
Rutter M., & Gould, M. (1985). Classification. In Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Henry, R. R.,
M. Rutter & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and Zvi, J. C., & Schulman, M. (1986). Handed-
Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches ness patterns in autism suggest subtypes. Jour-
(2nd ed., pp. 304–321). Cambridge, MA: nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Blackwell. 16(2), 155–167.
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J. E., & Robins, E. (1978).
Autism diagnostic interview-Revised. Los An- Research diagnostic criteria. Archives of Gen-
geles: Western Psychological Services. eral Psychiatry, 35, 773–782.
Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1992). Classification of Spitzer, R. L., & Siegel, B. (1990). The DSM-III-R
pervasive developmental disorders: Some con- field trial of pervasive developmental disor-
cepts and practical considerations [Special ders. Journal of the American Academy of
issue: Classification and diagnosis]. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(6),
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(4), 855–862.
459–482. Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1980). Classi-
Rutter, M., Shaffer, D., & Shepherd, M. (1975). A fication in psychiatry. In H. Kaplan, A. Freed-
multiaxial classification of child psychiatric man, & B. Sadock (Eds.), The comprehensive
disorders. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health textbook of psychiatry (3rd ed.). Baltimore:
Organization. Williams & Wilkins.
40 Diagnosis and Classification

Sponheim, E. (1996). Changing criteria of autistic of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
disorders: A comparison of the ICD-10 re- cent Psychiatry, 42(7), 864–872.
search criteria and DSM-IV with DSM-III-R, Tanguay, P. E., Robertson, J., & Derrick, A. (1998).
CARS, and ABC. Journal of Autism and Devel- A dimensional classification of autism spectrum
opmental Disorders, 26(5), 513–525. disorder by social communication domains.
Sponheim, E., & Skjeldal, O. (1998). Autism and Journal of the American Academy of Child and
related disorders: Epidemiological findings in Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(3), 271–277.
a Norwegian study using ICD-10 diagnostic Towbin, K. E. (2003). Strategies for pharmaco-
criteria. Journal of Autism and Developmental logic treatment of high functioning autism
Disorders, 28(3), 217–227. and Asperger syndrome. Child and Adolescent
Steingard, R., & Biederman, J. (1987). Lithium re- Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12(1)
sponsive manic-like symptoms in two individ- 23–45.
uals with autism. Journal of the American Tsai, L. (1992). Is Rett syndrome a subtype of per-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vasive developmental disorder? Journal of
26(6), 932–935. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22,
Stone, W. L., Lee, E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J., 551–561.
Hepburn, S. L., Coonrod, E. E., et al. (1999). Tsai, L. (1996). Brief report: Comorbid psychiatric
Can autism be diagnosed accurately in chil- disorders of autistic disorders. Journal of
dren under 3 years? Journal of Child Psychol- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26,
ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 159–164.
40(2), 219–226. Tsai, L. Y. (1999). Psychopharmacology in autism.
Szatmari, P. (1992a). A review of the DSM-III-R Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(5), 651–665.
criteria for autistic disorder [Special issue: Tsai, L. Y., Tsai, M. C., & August, G. J. (1985).
Classification and diagnosis]. Journal of Brief report: Implication of EEG diagnoses in
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(4), the subclassification of infantile autism. Jour-
507–523. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Szatmari, P. (1992b). The validity of autistic spec- 15(3), 339–344.
trum disorders: A literature review [Special Van der Gaag, R. J., Buitelaar, J., Van den Ban, E.,
issue: Classification and diagnosis]. Journal of Bezemer, M., Njio, L., & Van Engeland, H.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(4), (1995). A controlled multivariate chart review
583–600. of multiple complex developmental disorder.
Szatmari, P. (2000). Perspectives on the classifi- Journal of the American Academy of Child and
cation of Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(8), 1096–1106.
F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Asperger syndrome Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Childhood disintegrative
(pp. 403–417). New York: Guilford Press. disorder: Issues for DSM-IV. Journal of Autism
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Boyle, M. H., Streiner, and Developmental Disorders, 22(4), 625–642.
D. L., & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of out- Volkmar, F. R., Cicchetti, D. V., Bregman, J., &
come among high functioning children with Cohen, D. J. (1992). Three diagnostic systems
autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of for autism: DSM-III, DSM-III-R, and ICD-10
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied [Special issue: Classification and diagnosis].
Disciplines, 44(4), 520–528. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Szatmari, P., Merette, C., Bryson, S. E., Thivierge, ders, 22(4), 483–492.
J., Roy, M. A., Cayer, M., et al. (2002). Quan- Volkmar, F. R., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., &
tifying dimensions in autism: a factor-analytic Bregman, J. (1992). Brief report: Developmen-
study [Comment]. Journal of the American tal aspects of DSM-III-R criteria for autism
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, [Special issue: Classification and diagnosis].
41(4), 467–474. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Szatmari, P., Volkmar, F., & Walter, S. (1995). ders, 22(4), 657–662.
Evaluation of diagnostic criteria for autism Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Disintegra-
using latent class models. Journal of the Amer- tive disorder or “late onset ” autism. Journal of
ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
atry, 34(2), 216–222. Disciplines, 30(5), 717–724.
Tadevosyan-Leyfer, O., Dowd, M., Mankoski, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991a). Comorbid
Winklosky, B., Putnam, S., McGrath, L., et al. association of autism and schizophrenia.
(2003). A principal components analysis of the American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(12),
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Journal 1705–1707.
Issues in the Classification of Autism and Related Conditions 41

Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991b). Debate and (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: A com-
argument: The utility of the term pervasive prehensive textbook (3rd ed., pp. 499–505).
developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psy- Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Volkmar, F. R., Stier, D. M., & Cohen, D. J. (1985).
32(7), 1171–1172. Age of recognition of pervasive developmental
Volkmar, F., Cohen, D., Hoshino, K., Rende, R., & disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Paul, R. (1988). Phenomenology and classifi- 142(12), 1450–1452.
cation of the childhood psychoses. Psychologi- Waterhouse, L., Morris, R., Allen, D., Dunn, M.,
cal Medicine, 18(1), 191–201. Fein, D., Feinstein, C., et al. (1996). Diagnosis
Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (1998). Asperger syn- and classification in autism. Journal of Autism
drome and nonverbal learning disabilities. In and Developmental Disorders, 26(1), 59–86.
E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Asperger Waterhouse, L., Wing, L., Spitzer, R., & Siegel, B.
syndrome or high-functioning autism? Current (1993). Pervasive developmental disorders:
issues in autism (pp. 107–121). New York: From DSM-III to DSM-III-R [Special issue:
Plenum Press. Classification and diagnosis]. Journal of
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & Pauls, D. (1998). Noso- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(4),
logical and genetic aspects of Asperger Syn- 525–549.
drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Weir, K., & Salisbury, D. M. (1980). Acute onset of
Disorders, 28(5), 457–463. autistic features following brain damage in a
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., ten-year-old. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field mental Disorders, 10, 185–191.
trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American Wing, L. (1980). Childhood autism and social
Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1361–1367. class: A question of selection? British Journal
Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., of Psychiatry, 137, 410–417.
& Klin, A. J. (2004). Autism and Pervasive Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Developmental Disorders. Journal of Child account. Psychological Medicine, 11(1),
Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 135–170. 115–129.
Volkmar, F. R., & Nelson, D. S. (1990). Seizure Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
disorders in autism. Journal of the American of social interaction and associated abnormal-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica-
29(1), 127–129. tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Volkmar, F. R., & Rutter, M. (1995). Childhood Disorders, 9(1), 11–29.
disintegrative disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Wolff, S. (1998). Schizoid personality in child-
Autism Field Trial. Journal of the American hood: The links with Asperger syndrome,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, and elec-
34(8), 1092–1095. tive mutism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov
Volkmar, F. R., & Schwab-Stone, M. (1996). Anno- (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high-function-
tation: Childhood disorders in DSM-IV. Jour- ing autism? Current issues in autism
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and (pp. 123–142). New York: Plenum Press.
Allied Disciplines, 37(7), 779–784. World Health Organization. (1992). International
Volkmar F. R., Schwab-Stone, M., & First, M. classification of diseases: Diagnostic criteria
(2002). Classification in child and adolescent for research (10th ed.). Geneva, Switzerland:
Psychiatry: Principles and issues. In M. Lewis Author.
CHAPTER 2

Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive


Developmental Disorders

ERIC FOMBONNE

Epidemiological surveys of autism started of autism-spectrum disorders. This chapter


in the mid-1960s in England (Lotter, 1966, addresses the following five questions:
1967) and have since been conducted in many
countries. Most of these surveys have focused 1. What is the range of prevalence estimates
on a categorical-diagnostic approach to autism for autism and related disorders?
that has relied over time on different sets of 2. What proportion of autism cases is attrib-
criteria. All surveys, however, used a defini- utable to specific associated medical dis-
tion of autism that comprised severe impair- orders?
ments in communication and language, social 3. Is the incidence of autism increasing?
interactions, and play and behavior. This chap- 4. What are the other correlates of autistic-
ter focuses on autism defined as a severe de- spectrum disorders, particularly with re-
velopmental disorder. It does not deal with spect to race and ethnicity?
subtle autistic features or symptoms that 5. What is the role, if any, of cluster reports in
occur as part of other, more specific, develop- causal investigations of autism?
mental disorders, as unusual personality traits,
or as components of the lesser variant of DESIGN OF
autism thought to index genetic liability to EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES
autism in relatives. With the exception of re-
cent studies, other pervasive developmental Epidemiology is concerned with the study of
disorders (PDD) falling short of diagnostic the repartition of diseases in human popula-
criteria for autistic disorder (PDDNOS, As- tions and of the factors that influence it. Epi-
perger syndrome) were generally not included demiologists use several measures of disease
in the case definition used in earlier surveys occurrence. Incidence rate refers to the number
although several epidemiological investiga- of new cases (numerator) of a disease occurring
tions yielded useful information on the rates over a specified period in those at risk of devel-
of these particular PDDs. These data are sum- oping the disease in the population (denomina-
marized separately. This chapter provides an tor, in person × years). Cumulative incidence
up-to-date review of the methodological fea- is the proportion of those who were free of the
tures and substantive results of published epi- disease at the beginning of the observation
demiological surveys. It also updates our period and developed the disease during that
previous review (Fombonne, 2003a) with the period. Measures of incidence are required to
inclusion of eight new studies made available properly estimate morbidity due to a disease,
since then. A key feature of the review was to its possible changes over time, and the risk fac-
rely on summary statistics throughout to derive tors underlying disease status. Prevalence is a
quantitative estimates for rates and correlates measure used in cross-sectional surveys (there

42
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 43

is no passage of time) and reflects the propor- the concept of autism, a shift from autism to
tion of subjects in a given population who, PDD, the recognition of autism in subjects of
at that point in time, suffer from the disease. normal intelligence, and other similar factors
Most epidemiological studies of autism have will all contribute to increase (a) (subjects who
been cross-sectional and are not informative on are now regarded as cases, a, whereas, previ-
incidence (with a few recent exceptions). As a ously, they were included in b, or even d). Thus,
result, prevalence rates have been used to de- even in the absence of a change in the incidence
scribe autism in populations. of the disorder, prevalence estimates (a /N) can
It is useful to summarize how data are col- go up merely for methodological reasons.
lected in a prevalence study (refer to Table 2.1;
and also Fombonne, 2002a). The investigators Selection of Studies
first select a population of a given size (N),
often in a circumscribed geographic area. The studies were identified through systematic
Then, one or more screening stages are orga- searches from the major scientific literature
nized to identify possible cases designated as databases (MEDLINE, PSYCINFO) and from
screen positives (a + b). In a second, diagnostic prior reviews (Fombonne, 1999, 2003a; Wing,
stage, the screen positives (a + b) undergo a 1993). Only studies published in the English
thorough evaluation and are finally classified language were included. Surveys that relied
as cases (a, or true positives) or noncases ( b, on a questionnaire-based approach to define
false positives). The probability that a screen whether a subject was a case or not a case were
positive is a case (a /a + b) is called the positive also excluded because the validity of the diag-
predictive value. The prevalence is then calcu- nosis is unsatisfactory in these studies. Overall,
lated by dividing the number of cases identi- 42 studies published between 1966 and 2003
fied in the diagnostic stage by the size of the were selected that surveyed PDDs in clearly de-
population (a /N). However, the imperfection of marcated, nonoverlapping samples. Of these, 36
the screening process means that this calcula- studies provided information on rates of autis-
tion does not take into account the false nega- tic disorder, 3 studies provided estimates only
tives (c), true cases who were missed in the on all PDDs combined, and 3 studies provided
screening stage. In published autism surveys, data only on high-functioning PDDs. For sev-
there is often no way to estimate (c), although eral studies, the publication listed in the tables
techniques exist that could allow for this. As a is the most detailed account or the earliest one.
result, the prevalence estimate can be seri- When appropriate, however, other published ar-
ously underestimated. When comparing sur- ticles were used to extract relevant information
veys over time, two factors may jeopardize the from the same study.
comparison. Better awareness of the disorder,
improved screening techniques, and detection Survey Descriptions
all contribute to reduce the false negatives (c)
(and as a consequence to increase a). Changes Surveys were conducted in 14 countries, and
in case definition, especially a broadening of half of the results have been published since
1997. Details on the precise sociodemographic
composition and economical activities of the
Table 2.1 Hypothetical Prevalence Study area surveyed in each study were generally
of Autism lacking. Most studies were conducted in pre-
Cases Noncases dominantly urban or mixed areas, however,
(D) (D) with only two surveys (6 and 11) carried out in
Screen positive a b a+b
predominantly rural areas. The proportion of
Screen negative c d c+d children from immigrant families was generally
a+c b+d a+b+c+d=N not available and very low in five surveyed pop-
False negatives (FN) = d; False positives (FP) = b; Pos-
ulations (Studies 11, 12, 19, 23, and 26). Only
itive predictive value (PPV); Prevalence (P) = a + c/N = in Studies 4, 34, and 38 was there a substantial
p(D); Sensitivity (Se, rate of true positives) = a /a + c; minority of children with either an immigrant
Specificity (Sp, rate of true negatives) = d/ b + d. or different ethnic background living in the
44 Diagnosis and Classification

area. The age range of the population included (Study 25) to 35% (Study 40), with a median
in the surveys is spread from birth to early adult value of 14%. Fewer studies could examine
life, with an overall median age of 8.0. Simi- the extent to which uncooperative participa-
larly, in 39 studies, there is huge variation in tion or outright refusal to participate in sur-
the size of the population surveyed (range: veys is associated with the likelihood that the
826–4,590,000), with a median population size corresponding children have autism. Bryson,
of 63,860 subjects (mean = 255,000). About Clark, and Smith (1988; Study 12) provided
half of the studies relied on targeted popula- some evidence that those families who refused
tions ranging in size from 15,870 to 166,860. to cooperate in the intensive assessment phase
had children with ABC scores similar to other
Study Designs false positives in their study, suggesting that
these children were unlikely to have autism.
A few studies have relied on existing admin- Webb, Morey, et al. (2003; Study 40) similarly
istrative databases (i.e., Croen, Grether, produced data showing increasing refusal rate
Hoogstrate, & Selvin, 2002; Gurney et al., in those with fewer ICD-10 PDD symptoms.
2003) or on national registers (Madsen et al., By contrast, in a Japanese study (Sugiyama &
2002) for case identification. Most investiga- Abe, 1989; Study 13) where 17.3% of parents
tions have relied on a two-stage or multistage refused further investigations for their 18-
approach to identify cases in underlying popu- month-old children who had failed a devel-
lations. The first screening stage of these opmental check, follow-up data at age 3 sug-
studies often consisted of sending letters or gested that half of these children still dis-
brief screening scales requesting school and played developmental problems. Whether
health professionals to identify possible cases these problems were connected to autism is
of autism. Each investigation varied in several unknown, but this study points to the possibil-
key aspects of this screening stage. First, the ity that higher rates of developmental disor-
coverage of the population varied enormously ders exist among nonparticipants to surveys.
from one study to another. In some studies Similarly, in Lotter’s study (1966; Study 1),
(3, 17, 20, 24, 33), only cases already known 58 questionnaires covering schools for handi-
from educational or medical authorities could capped children were returned out of the 76
be identified. In other surveys, investigators forms sent out, and an independent review of
achieved extensive coverage of the entire the records showed that 4 of the 18 missing
population, including children attending nor- forms corresponded to autistic children. It
mal schools (Studies 1, 25, 40) or children un- is difficult to draw firm conclusions from
dergoing systematic developmental checks these different accounts. Although there is no
(Studies 13, 19, 22, 32, 36). In addition, the consistent evidence that parental refusal to co-
surveyed areas varied in terms of service de- operate is associated with autism in their off-
velopment as a function of the specific educa- spring, a small proportion of cases may be
tional or health care systems of each country missed in some surveys as a consequence of
and of the year of investigation. Second, the noncooperation at the screening stage. One
type of information sent out to professionals study (40) included a weighting procedure to
invited to identify children varied from simple compensate for nonresponse.
letters including a few clinical descriptors Only two studies (1 and 30) provided an
of autism-related symptoms or diagnostic estimate of the reliability of the screening
checklists rephrased in nontechnical terms, to procedure. The sensitivity of the screening
more systematic screening based on question- methodology (a /(a + c) in Table 2.1) is also dif-
naires or rating scales of known reliability and ficult to gauge in autism surveys. The usual
validity. Third, participation rates in the first epidemiological approach of sampling screened
screening stages provide another source of negative subjects at random to estimate the
variation in the screening efficiency of sur- proportion of false negatives (c/(a + c) in Table
veys. Refusal rates were available for 13 stud- 2.1) has not been used in these surveys because
ies (1, 5, 6, 9, 12, 14, 19, 20, 23, 25, 30, 37, the low frequency of the disorder would make
and 40); the rate of refusal ranged from 0% undertaking such estimations both imprecise
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 45

and costly. The cases that were missed as a re- case determination. It is, furthermore, difficult
sult of noncooperation or imperfect sensitivity to assess the impact of a specific diagnostic
of the screening procedure make it necessary scheme or of a particular diagnostic criterion
to view the prevalence estimates as underesti- on the estimate of prevalence since other pow-
mates of the true rates. The magnitude of this erful method factors confound between-studies
underestimation is unknown in each survey. comparisons of rates. Surprisingly, few studies
Similar considerations about the method- have built in a reliability assessment of the di-
ological variability across studies apply to agnostic procedure; reliability during the in-
the intensive assessment phases. Participation tensive assessment phase was high in seven
rates in these second-stage assessments were surveys (4, 13, 16, 23, 24, 32, 36) and moder-
not always available, either because they had ate in another one (14).
simply not been calculated, or because the de-
sign and/or method of data collection did not CHARACTERISTICS OF
lead easily to their estimation. When available AUTISTIC SAMPLES
(Studies 1, 5, 8, 12, 13, 15, 22, 23, 25, 29, 30,
32, 36), they were generally high, ranging from Data on children with autistic disorders were
76.1% (Study 12) to 98.6% (Study 25). The in- available in 36 surveys (1 to 36; see Table 2.2).
formation used to determine final diagnostic In total, 7,514 subjects were considered to suf-
status usually involved a combination of infor- fer from autism; this number ranged from 6
mants and data sources, with a direct assess- (Studies 18 and 25) to 5,038 (Study 34) across
ment of the person with autism in 21 studies. studies (median: 48; mean: 209). An assessment
The assessments were conducted with vari- of intellectual function was obtained in 21
ous diagnostic instruments, ranging from a studies. These assessments were conducted with
classical clinical examination to the use of bat- various tests and instruments; furthermore, re-
teries of standardized measures. The Autism sults were pooled in broad bands of intellectual
Diagnostic Interview (Le Couteur et al., 1989) level that did not share the same boundaries
and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observational across studies. As a consequence, differences
Schedule (Lord, Risi, et al., 2000) were used in rates of cognitive impairment between stud-
in the most recent surveys. The precise diag- ies should be interpreted with caution. Despite
nostic criteria retained to define caseness vary these caveats, some general conclusions can be
according to the study and, to a large extent, reached (Table 2.2). The median proportion of
reflect historical changes in classification sys- subjects without intellectual impairment is
tems. Thus, Kanner’s criteria and Lotter’s and 29.6% (range: 0% to 60%).1 The corresponding
Rutter’s definitions were used in Studies 1 to figures are 29.3% (range: 6.6% to 100%) for
8 (all conducted before 1982), whereas DSM- mild-to-moderate intellectual impairments,
based definitions took over thereafter as well as and 38.5% (range: 0% to 81.3%) for severe-to-
ICD-10 since 1990. Some studies have relaxed profound mental retardation. Gender reparti-
partially some diagnostic criteria such as an age tion among subjects with autism was reported
of onset before 30 months (Study 6) or the ab- in 32 studies totaling 6,963 subjects with
sence of schizophrenic-like symptoms (Studies autism, and the male/female sex ratio varied
13 and 14). However, most surveys have relied from 1.33 (Study 7) to 16.0 (Study 4), with a
on the clinical judgment of experts to arrive at mean malefemale ratio of 4.31. Thus, no epi-
the final case groupings. It is worth underlining demiological study ever identified more girls
that field trials for recent classifications such than boys with autism, a finding that parallels
as DSM-III-R (Spitzer & Siegel, 1990) or DSM- the gender differences found in clinically re-
IV/ICD-10 (Volkmar, Klin, et al., 1994) have ferred samples (Lord, Schopler, & Revecki,
also relied on the judgment of clinical experts, 1982). Gender differences were more pro-
taken as a gold standard to diagnose autism nounced when autism was not associated with
and calibrate diagnostic algorithms. There-
fore, the heterogeneity of diagnostic criteria
used across surveys is somewhat mitigated by 1
Study 23, which relied on different IQ groupings,
reliance on expert clinical judgment for final has been excluded.
46
TABLE 2.2 Prevalence Surveys of Autistic Disorder
Number of
Size of Subjects Percentage Gender
Target with Diagnostic with Normal Ratio Prevalence
No. Study Country Area Population Age Autism Criteria IQ (MF) Rate/10,000 95% CI

1 Lotter, 1966 United Kingdom Middlesex 78,000 8–10 32 Rating scale 15.6 2.6 (23/9) 4.1 2.7 ; 5.5
2 Brask, 1970 Denmark Aarhus 46,500 2–14 20 Clinical — 1.4 (12/7) 4.3 2.4 ; 6.2
County
3 Treffert, 1970 United States Wisconsin 899,750 3–12 69 Kanner — 3.06 0.7 0.6 ; 0.9
(52/17)
4 Wing, Yeates, United Kingdom Camberwell 25,000 5–14 17a 24 items rating 30 16 (16/1) 4.8 b 2.1 ; 7.5
Brierly, & Gould, scale of Lotter
1976
5 Hoshino et al., 1982 Japan Fukushima- 609,848 0–18 142 Kanner’s — 9.9 2.33 1.9 ; 2.7
Ken criteria (129/13)

6 Bohman, Bohman, Sweden County of 69,000 0–20 39 Rutter criteria 20.5 1.6 5.6 3.9 ; 7.4
Björck, & Sjöholm, Västerbotten (24/15)
1983
7 McCarthy, Ireland East 65,000 8–10 28 Kanner — 1.33 4.3 2.7 ; 5.9
Fitzgerald, & Smith, (16/12)
1984
8 Steinhausen, Göbel, Germany West Berlin 279,616 0–14 52 Rutter 55.8 2.25 1.9 1.4 ; 2.4
Breinlinger, & (36/16)
Wohlloben, 1986
9 Burd, Fisher, & United States North Dakota 180,986 2–18 59 DSM-III — 2.7 3.26 2.4 ; 4.1
Kerbeshan, 1987 (43/16)
10 Matsuishi et al., Japan Kurume City 32,834 4–12 51 DSM-III — 4.7 (42/9) 15.5 11.3 ; 19.8
1987
11 Tanoue, Oda, Asano, Japan Southern 95,394 7 132 DSM-III — 4.07 13.8 11.5 ; 16.2
& Kawashima, 1988 Ibaraki (106/26)
12 Bryson, Clark, & Canada Part of Nova- 20,800 6–14 21 New RDC 23.8 2.5 (15/6) 10.1 5.8 ; 14.4
Smith, 1988 Scotia
13 Sugiyama & Abe, Japan Nagoya 12,263 3 16 DSM-III — — 13.0 6.7 ; 19.4
1989
14 Cialdella & France 1 district 135,180 3–9 61 DSM-III-like — 2.3 4.5 3.4 ; 5.6
Mamelle, 1989 (Rhône)
15 Ritvo et al., 1989 United States Utah 769,620 3–27 241 DSM-III 34 3.73 2.47 2.1 ; 2.8
(190/51)
16 Gillberg, Sweden South-West 78,106 4–13 74 DSM-III-R 18 2.7 9.5 7.3 ; 11.6
Steffenburg, & Gothenburg + (54/20)
Schaumann, 1991d Bohuslän
County
17 Fombonne & du France 4 regions, 14 274,816 9 and 13 154 Clinical- 13.3 2.1 4.9 4.1 ; 5.7
Mazaubrun, 1992 districts ICD-10-like (105/49)
18 Wignyosumarto, Indonesia Yogyakarita 5,120 4–7 6 CARS 0 2.0 (4/2) 11.7 2.3 ; 21.1
Mukhlas, & (SE of
Skirataki, 1992 Jakarta)
19 Honda, Shimizu, Japan Yokohama 8,537 5 18 ICD-10 50.0 2.6 (13.5) 21.08 11.4 ; 30.8
Misumi, Niimi, &
Ohashi, 1996
20 Fombonne, du France 3 districts 325,347 8–16 174 Clinical 12.1 1.81 5.35 4.6 ; 6.1
Mazaubrun, Cans, & ICD-10-like (112/62)
Grandjean, 1997
(continued)

47
48
TABLE 2.2 (Continued)

Number of
Size of Subjects Percentage Gender
Target with Diagnostic with Normal Ratio Prevalence
No. Study Country Area Population Age Autism Criteria IQ (MF) Rate/10,000 95% CI
21 Webb, Lobo, Hervas, United Kingdom South 73,301 3–15 53 DSM-III-R — 6.57 7.2 5.3 ; 9.3
Scourfield, & Fraser, Glamorgan, (46/7)
1997 Wales

22 Arvidsson, Sweden (West Mölnlycke 1,941 3–6 9 ICD-10 22.2 3.5 (7/2) 46.4 16.1 ; 76.6
Danielsson, coast)
Forsberg, Gillberg,
& Johansson, 1997
23 Sponheim & Norway Akershus 65,688 3–14 34 ICD-10 47.1 c 2.09 5.2 3.4 ; 6.9
Skjeldal, 1998 County (23/11)
24 Taylor et al., 1999 United Kingdom North 490,000 0–16 427 ICD-10 — — 8.7 7.9 ; 9.5
Thames
25 Kadesjö, Gillberg, & Sweden Karlstad 826 6.7–7.7 6 DSM-III-R / 50.0 5.0 (5/1) 72.6 14.7 ; 130.6
Hagberg, 1999 (Central) ICD-10
Gillberg’s
criteria
(Asperger
syndrome)
26 Baird, Charman, & United Kingdom South-East 16,235 — 50 ICD-10 60 15.7 30.8 22.9 ; 40.6
Baron-Cohen, 2000 Thames (47/3)
27 Powell et al., 2000 United Kingdom West 25,377 — 62 Clinical / — — 7.8 5.8 ; 10.5
Midlands ICD-10/DSM-IV

28 Kielinen, Linna, & Finland North (Oulu 152,732 — 187 ICD-8/ICD-9/ 49.8 4.12 12.2 10.5 ; 14.0
Moilanen, 2000 et Lapland) ICD-10 (156/50)
29 Bertrand et al., 2001 United States Brick 8,896 — 36 DSM-IV 36.7 2.2 40.5 28.0 ; 56.0
Township, (25/11)
New Jersey
30 Fombonne, Simmons, United Kingdom Angleterre et 10,438 5–15 27 DSM-IV/ 55.5 8.0 (24/3) 26.1 16.2 ; 36.0
Ford, Meltzer, & Pays de ICD-10
Goodman, 2001 Galles
31 Magnússon & Iceland Whole Island 43,153 5–14 57 Mostly ICD-10 15.8 4.2 13.2 9.8 ; 16.6
Saemundsen, 2001 (46/11)
32 Chakrabarti & United Kingdom Staffordshire 15,500 2.5–6.5 26 ICD-10/ 29.2 3.3 (20/6) 16.8 10.3 ; 23.2
Fombonne, 2001 (Midlands) DSM-IV
33 Davidovitch, Israel Haiffa 26,160 7–11 26 DSM-III-R / — 4.2 (21/5) 10.0 6.6 ; 14.4
Holtzman, & Tirosh, DSM-IV
2001
34 Croen, Grether, United States California 4,950,333 5–12 5,038 CDER (Full 62.8 e 4.47 11.0 10.7 ; 11.3
Hoogstrate, & DDS syndrome) (4,116/921)
Selvin, 2002a
35 Madsen et al., 2002 Denmark National 63,859 8 46 ICD-10 — — 7.2 5.0–10.0
register
36 Chakrabarti & United Kingdom Staffordshire 10,903 4–7 24 ICD-10/ 33.3 3.8 (19/5) 22.0 14.4 ; 32.2
Fombonne, 2004 (Midlands) DSM-IV
a
This number corresponds to the sample described in Wing and Gould (1979).
b
This rate corresponds to the first published paper on this survey and is based on 12 subjects among children age 5 to 14 years.
c
In this study, mild mental retardation was combined with normal IQ, whereas moderate and severe mental retardation were grouped together.
d
For the Goteborg surveys by Gillberg et al. (Gillberg, 1984; Gillberg et al., 1991; Steffenburg & Gillberg, 1986), a detailed examination showed that there was overlap
among the samples included in the three surveys; consequently only the last survey has been included in this table.
e
This proportion is likely to be overestimated and to ref lect an underreporting of mental retardation in the CDER evaluations.

49
50 Diagnosis and Classification

mental retardation. In 13 studies (865 subjects) calculations, we arbitrarily adopted the mid-
where the sex ratio was available within the nor- point of this interval as the working rate for
mal band of intellectual functioning, the median autism prevalence, that is, the value of
sex ratio was 5.51. Conversely, in 12 studies 13/10,000.
(813 subjects), the median sex ratio was 1.951
in the group with autism and moderate-to- Associated Medical Conditions
severe mental retardation.
Rates of medical conditions associated with
Prevalence Estimations for autism were reported in 15 surveys and the
Autistic Disorder findings are summarized in Table 2.3. These
medical conditions were investigated by very
Prevalence estimates ranged from 0.7/10,000 different means ranging from questionnaires
to 72.6/10,000 (Table 2.2). Confidence inter- to full medical workups.
vals were computed for each estimate; their Conditions such as congenital rubella
width (difference between the upper and lower and PKU account for almost no cases of
limit of the 95% confidence interval) indicates autism. Prior studies suggesting an association
the variation in sample sizes and in the preci- of congenital rubella (Chess, 1971) and
sion achieved in each study (range: 0.3 −115.9; PKU (Knobloch & Pasamanick, 1975; Lowe,
mean = 11.3). Prevalence rates were nega- Tanaka, Seashore, Young, & Cohen, 1980)
tively correlated with sample size (Spearman with autism were conducted before implemen-
r = −.73; p < .01); small-scale studies tended tation of systematic prevention measures.
to report higher prevalence rates. Likewise, our nil estimate of 0% for autism
When surveys were combined in two groups and neurofibromatosis is consistent with
according to the median year of publication the 0.3% rate found in a large series of 341
(1994), the median prevalence rate for 18 sur- referred cases (Mouridsen, Bachmann-
veys published in the period 1966 to 1993 was Andersen, Sörensen, Rich, & Isager, 1992).
4.7/10,000, and the median rate for the 18 sur- Similarly, the rates found for cerebral palsy
veys published in the period 1994 to 2004 and Down syndrome equally suggest no partic-
was 12.7/10,000. Indeed, the correlation be- ular association. Recent reports (Bregman &
tween prevalence rate and year of publication Volkmar, 1988; Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaziud-
reached statistical significance (Spearman din, 1992; Howlin, Wing, & Gould, 1995) have
r = .65; p < .01); and the results of the 22 sur- focused on the co-occurrence of Down syn-
veys with prevalence rates over 7/10,000 were drome and autism in some individuals. The
all published since 1987. These findings point epidemiological findings give further support
toward an increase in prevalence estimates
in the past 15 to 20 years. To derive a best es-
timate of the current prevalence of autism, TABLE 2.3 Medical Disorders Associated with
it was therefore deemed appropriate to restrict Autism in Recent Epidemiological Surveys
the analysis to 28 surveys published since Number of Median
1987. The prevalence estimates ranged from Studies Rate Range
2.5 to 72.6/10,000 (average 95% CI width: Cerebral palsy 7 1.4 0–4.8
14.1), with an average rate of 16.2/10,000 and Fragile X 9 0.0 0–8.1
a median rate of 11.3/10,000. Similar values Tuberous sclerosis 11 1.1 0–3.8
were obtained when slightly different rules Phenylketonuria 8 0 0–0
and time cutpoints were used, with median Neurofibromatosis 7 0 0–1.4
and mean rates fluctuating between 10 and 13 Congenital rubella 11 0.0 0–5.9
Down syndrome 12 0.7 0–16.7
and 13 and 18/10,000 respectively. From
these results, a conservative estimate for the At least one disorder 16 5.5 0–16.7
current prevalence of autistic disorder is most Epilepsy 12 16.7 0–26.4
consistent with values lying somewhere be- Hearing deficits 8 1.3 0–5.9
Visual deficits 6 0.7 0–11.1
tween 10/10,000 and 16/10,000. For further
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 51

to the validity of these clinical descriptions figure for any medical disorder (excluding
(that the two conditions co-occur in some chil- epilepsy and sensory impairments). Although
dren), although they do not suggest that the this figure does not incorporate other medical
rate of comorbidity is higher than that ex- events of potential etiological significance,
pected by chance once the effects of mental such as encephalitis, congenital anomalies, and
retardation are taken into account. For fragile other rare medical syndromes, it is similar to
X, the low rate available in epidemiological that reported in a recent review of the question
studies is almost certainly an underestimate (Rutter, Bailey, Bolton, & Le Couteur, 1994). It
because fragile X was not recognized until rel- is worth noting that epidemiological surveys of
atively recently, and the most recent surveys autism in very large samples (Studies 15, 17,
did not always include systematic screening and 20) provided estimates in line with our
for fragile X. In line with prior reports (Smal- conservative summary statistics. By contrast,
ley, Tanguay, Smith, & Guitierrez, 1992), claims of average rates of medical conditions
tuberous sclerosis (TS) has a consistently high as high as 24% appear to apply to studies of
frequency among autistic samples. Assuming smaller size and to rely on a broadened defini-
a population prevalence of 1/10,000 for TS tion of autism (Gillberg & Coleman, 1996).
(Ahlsen, Gillberg, Lindblom, & Gillberg, Rates of epilepsy are high among autism sam-
1994; Hunt & Lindenbaum, 1984; Shepherd, ples. The proportion suffering from epilepsy
Beard, Gomez, Kurland, & Whisnant, 1991), it tends also to be higher in studies that have
appears that the rate of TS is about 100 times higher rates of severe mental retardation (as in
higher than that expected under the hypothesis Studies 16, 17, and 20). Age-specific rates for
of no association. Whether epilepsy, localized the prevalence of epilepsy were not available.
brain lesions, or direct genetic effects mediate The samples where high rates of epilepsy were
the association between TS and autism is a reported tended to have a higher median age, al-
matter for ongoing research (Smalley, 1998). though these rates seemed mostly to apply to
The overall proportion of cases of autism school-age children. Thus, in light of the in-
that could be causally attributed to known med- creased incidence of seizures during adoles-
ical disorders therefore remains low. From the cence among subjects with autism (Deykin &
16 surveys where rates of one of seven clear-cut MacMahon, 1979; Rutter, 1970), the epidemio-
medical disorders potentially causally associ- logical rates should be regarded as underesti-
ated with autism (cerebral palsy, fragile X, TS, mates of the lifetime risk of epilepsy in autism.
PKU, neurofibromatosis, congenital rubella, These rates are nonetheless high and support the
and Down syndrome) were available, we com- findings of a bimodal peak of incidence of
puted the proportion of subjects with at least epilepsy in autistic samples, with a first peak of
one of these recognizable disorders. Because incidence in the first years of life (Volkmar &
the overlap between these conditions is ex- Nelson, 1990).
pected to be low and because the information
about multiply-handicapped subjects was not RATES OF OTHER PERVASIVE
available, this overall rate was obtained by sum- DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS
ming directly the rates for each individual
condition within each study. The resulting rate Several studies have provided useful informa-
might, therefore, be slightly overestimated. tion on rates of syndromes that are similar to
The fraction of cases of autism with a known autism, but fall short of strict diagnostic criteria
medical condition that was potentially etiologi- for autistic disorder (Table 2.4). Because the
cally significant ranged from 0% to 16.7%, screening procedures and subsequent diagnos-
with a median and mean values of 5.5% and tic assessments differed from one study to an-
5.9% respectively. Even if some adjustment other, these groups of disorders are not strictly
were made to account for the underestimation comparable across studies. In addition, as they
of the rate of fragile X in epidemiological sur- were not the group on which the attention was
veys of autism, the attributable proportion of focused, details are often lacking on their phe-
cases of autism would not exceed the 10% nomenological features in the available reports.
TABLE 2.4 Relative Rates of Autism and Other Pervasive Developmental Disorders

52
Combined Rate
Prevalence Rate of Autism and Prevalence
No. Study Rates of Autism of Other PDDs Other PDDs Rate Ratio a Case Definition for Other PDDs

1 Lotter, 1966 4.1 3.3 7.8 0.90 Children with some behavior similar to that of
autistic children
2 Brask, 1970 4.3 1.9 6.2 0.44 Children with “other psychoses” or “ borderline
psychotic”
4 Wing, Yates, Brierly, & 4.9 16.3 21.2 3.33 Socially impaired (triad of impairments)
Gould, 1976
5 Hoshino et al., 1982 2.33 2.92 5.25 1.25 Autistic mental retardation

9 Burd, Fisher, & Kerbeshan, 3.26 >7.79 b >11.05 b 2.39 Children referred by professionals with “autistic-
1987 like” symptoms, not meeting DSM-III criteria for IA,
COPDD, or atypical PDD
14 Cialdella & Mamelle, 1989 4.5 4.7 9.2 1.04 Children meeting criteria for other forms of
“infantile psychosis” than autism, or a broadened
definition of DSM-III
17 Fombonne & du Mazaubrun, 4.6 6.6 11.2 1.43 Children with mixed developmental disorders
1992 c
20 Fombonne, du Mazaubrun, 5.3 10.94 16.3 2.05 Children with mixed developmental disorders
Cans, & Grandjean, 1997
26 Baird, Charman, & Baron- 30.8 27.1 57.9 0.9 Children with other PDDs
Cohen, 2000
27 Powell et al., 2000 7.8 13.0 20.8 1.7 Children with other PDDs
29 Bertrand et al., 2001 40.5 27.0 67.4 0.7 Children with PDDNOS and Asperger disorder
32 Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 16.8 36.1 52.9 2.15 Children with PDDNOS
2001
35 Madsen et al., 2002 7.2 22.2 29.4 3.08

36 Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 22.0 24.8 46.8 1.13 Children with PDDNOS
2004
a
Other PDD rate divided by autism rate.
b
Computed by the author.
c
These rates are derived from the complete results of the survey of three birth cohorts of French children (Rumeau-Rouquette et al., 1994).
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 53

Unspecified Pervasive Only a handful (N < 5) of cases were identified


Developmental Disorders in these surveys, with the resulting estimates
of 28 and 48/10,000 being extremely impre-
Different labels (see Table 2.4) have been used cise. By contrast, other recent autism surveys
to characterize these conditions, such as the have consistently identified smaller numbers
triad of impairments involving impairments in of children with AS than those with autism
reciprocal social interaction, communication, within the same survey. In Studies 23 to 27 and
and imagination (Wing & Gould, 1979). These 32 (reviewed in Fombonne & Tidmarsh, 2003)
groups would be overlapping with current di- and Study 36, the ratio of autism to AS rates in
agnostic labels such as atypical autism and each survey was above unity, suggesting that
pervasive developmental disorders not other- the rate of AS was consistently lower than that
wise specified (PDDNOS). Fourteen of the 36 for autism (Table 2.5). How much lower is dif-
surveys yielded separate estimates of the ficult to establish from existing data, but a
prevalence of these developmental disorders, ratio of 51 would appear to be an acceptable,
with 10 studies showing higher rates for the albeit conservative, conclusion based on this
nonautism disorders than the rates for autism. limited available evidence. Taking 13/10,000
The ratio of the rate of nonautistic PDD to the as the rate for autism, this translates into a
rate of autism varied between from 0.44 to rate for AS that would be 2.6/10,000, a figure
3.33 (Table 2.4) with a mean value of 1.6, used for subsequent calculations. A recent
which translates into an average prevalence es- survey of high-functioning PDDs in Welsh
timate of 20.8/10,000 if one takes 13/10,000 as mainstream primary schools has yielded a rel-
the rate for autism. In other words, for two atively high (uncorrected) prevalence estimate
children with autism assessed in epidemiologi- of 14.5/10,000. Of the 17 children contributing
cal surveys, three children were found with se- to this figure, 10 had either Asperger’s disor-
vere impairments that had a similar nature but der or high-functioning autism as a primary di-
that fell short of strict diagnostic criteria for agnosis. Assuming than half of these would
autism. This group has been much less studied have Asperger’s disorder, we could extrapolate
in previous epidemiological studies, but pro- a 4.3/10,000 prevalence, a figure that is in line
gressive recognition of its importance and rel- with other studies. However, much caution
evance to autism has led to changes in the should be applied to this calculation as it is
design of more recent epidemiological surveys based on several assumptions that are impossi-
(see later in this chapter). They now include ble to verify.
these less typical children in the case defini-
tion adopted in surveys. It should be clear from Childhood Disintegrative Disorder
these figures that they represent a substantial
group of children whose treatment needs are Few surveys have provided data on childhood
likely to be as important as those of children disintegrative disorder (CDD), also known
with autism. as Heller syndrome, disintegrative psychosis
(ICD-9), or late-onset autism (see Volkmar,
Asperger Syndrome and Childhood 1992). In addition to the four studies (9, 23, 31,
Disintegrative Disorder 32) of our previous review (Fombonne, 2002b),
another survey has provided new data on CDD
The reader is referred to recent epidemiological (36). Taking the five studies into account
reviews for these two conditions (Fombonne, (Table 2.6), prevalence estimates ranged from
2002b; Fombonne & Tidmarsh, 2003). Epi- 1.1 to 9.2/100,000. The pooled estimate based
demiological studies of Asperger syndrome on seven identified cases and a surveyed popu-
(AS) are sparse, probably because it was ac- lation of 358,633 children, was 1.9/100,000.
knowledged as a separate diagnostic category The upper-bound limit of the associated confi-
only recently in both ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Only dence interval (4.15/100,000) indicates that
two epidemiological surveys have specifically CDD is a rare condition, with 1 case occurring
investigated its prevalence (Ehlers & Gillberg, for every 65 cases of autistic disorder. As cases
1993; Kadesjö, Gillberg, & Hagberg, 1999). of CDD were both rare and already included in
TABLE 2.5 Asperger Syndrome (AS) in Recent Autism Surveys

Assessment Autism Asperger Syndrome

Autism /
Size of Age Rate/ Rate/ AS
Study Population Group Informants Instruments Diagnostic Criteria N 10,000 N 10,000 Ratio

Sponheim & Skjeldal, 1998 65,688 3–14 Parent Parental interview + direct ICD-10 32 4.9 2 0.3 16.0
Child observation, CARS, ABC
Taylor et al., 1999 490,000 0–16 Record Rating of all data available in child ICD-10 427 8.7 71 1.4 6.0
record
Kadesjö, Gillberg, & Hagberg, 826 6.7–7.7 Child ADI-R, Griffiths Scale or WISC, DSM-III-R /ICD-10 6 72.6 4 48.4 1.5
1999 Parent Asperger Syndrome Screening Gillberg’s criteria
Professional Questionnaire (Asperger syndrome)
Powell et al., 2000 25,377 1– 4.9 Records ADI-R DSM-III-R 54 — 16 — 3.4
Available data DSM-IV
ICD-10
Baird, Charman, & Baron- 16,235 7 Parents ADI-R ICD-10 45 27.7 5 3.1 9.0
Cohen, 2000 Child Psychometry DSM-IV
Other data
Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 15,500 2.5–6.5 Child ADI-R, 2 weeks multidisciplinary ICD-10 26 16.8 13 8.4 2.0
2001 Parent assessment, Merrill-Palmer, WPPSI DSM-IV
Professional
Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 10,903 2.5–6.5 Child ADI-R, 2 weeks multidisciplinary ICD-10 24 22.0 12 11.0 2.0
2004 Parent assessment, Merrill-Palmer, WPPSI DSM-IV
Professional
Overall 614 123 5.0
TABLE 2.6 Surveys of Childhood Disintegrative Disorder (CDD)

Size of Prevalence
Target Age Estimate 95% Cl1
Study Country (Region /State) Population Group Assessment N M/ F (/100,000) (/100,000)

Burd, Fisher, & Kerbeshan, United States 180,986 2–18 Structured parental interview and 2 2/— 1.11 0.13 ; 3.4
1987 (North Dakota) review of all data available–DSM-III
criteria
Sponheim & Skjeldal, 1998 Norway 65,688 3–14 Parental interview and direct 1 ? 1.52 0.04 ; 8.5
(Akershus County) observation (CARS, ABC)
Magnusson & Saemundsen, Iceland 85,556 5–14 ADI-R, CARS, and psychological 2 2/— 2.34 0.3 ; 8.4
2001 (whole island) tests—mostly ICD-10
Chakrabarti & Fombonne, United Kingdom 15,500 2.5– 6.5 ADI-R, 2 weeks’ multidisciplinary 1 1/— 6.4 0.16 ; 35.9
2001 (Staffordshire, Midlands) assessment, Merrill-Palmer, WPPSI—
ICD-10/DSM-IV
Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2004 United Kingdom 10,903 2.5– 6.5 ADI-R, 2 weeks’ multidisciplinary 1 1/— 9.2 0–58.6
(Staffordshire, Midlands) assessment, Merrill-Palmer, WPPSI—
ICD-10/DSM-IV
Pooled estimates 358,633 7 6/— 1.9 0.87–4.15

55
56 Diagnosis and Classification

the numerator alongside autism cases in most fifth birthday, thereby optimizing sensitivity of
surveys, we do not provide separate estimates case-finding procedures. Furthermore, the size
of the numbers of subjects suffering from CDD of targeted populations was reasonably small
in subsequent calculations. ( between 9,000 and 16,000), probably allowing
for the most efficient use of research resources.
Prevalence for Combined PDDs Conducted in different regions and countries by
different teams, the convergence of estimates is
Taking the aforementioned conservative esti- striking. Two further results are worth noting.
mates, the prevalence for all PDDs is at least First, in sharp contrast with the prevalence for
36.4/10,000 (the sum of estimates for autism combined PDDs, the separate estimates for
[13/10,000], PDDNOS [20.8/10,000], and AS autistic disorder and PDD-NOS vary widely in
[2.6/10,000]). This global estimate is derived studies where separate figures were available.
from a conservative analysis of existing data. It appears that the reliability of the differentia-
However, six out of eight recent epidemio- tion between autistic disorder and PDD-NOS
logical surveys yielded even higher rates (Table was mediocre at that young age, despite the use
2.7). The two surveys that did not show higher of up-to-date standardized measures. Second,
rates might have underestimated them. In the the rate of mental retardation was, overall,
Danish investigation (Study 35), case finding much lower than in previous surveys of autism.
depended on notification to a National Reg- Although this should not be a surprise for chil-
istry, a method that is usually associated with dren in the PDD-NOS/AS groups, this trend
lower sensitivity for case finding. The Atlanta was also noticeable within samples diagnosed
survey by the Centers for Disease Control and with autistic disorder. To what extent this trend
Prevention (CDC; Study 38) was based on a reflects the previously mentioned differential
very large population (which typically yields classification issues between autism and PDD-
lower prevalence, as described earlier) and NOS or a genuine trend over time toward de-
age-specific rates were, in fact, in the 40-to- creased rate of mental retardation within
45/10,000 range in some birth cohorts (Fom- children with autistic disorder (possibly as a re-
bonne, 2003b). The common design features of sult as earlier diagnosis and intervention) re-
the four other epidemiological inquiries (Stud- mains to be established.
ies 26, 29, 32, 36) that yielded higher rates are In conclusion, the convergence of recent
worthy of mention. First, the case definition surveys around an estimate of 60/10,000 for
chosen for these investigations was that of a all PDD combined is striking, especially when
pervasive developmental disorder as opposed to coming from studies with improved methods.
the narrower approach focusing on autistic dis- This estimate appears now to be the best esti-
order typical of previous surveys. Investigators mate for the prevalence of PDDs currently
were concerned with any combination of severe available.
developmental abnormalities occurring in one
or more of the three symptomatic domains TIME TRENDS
defining PDD and autism. Second, case-finding
techniques employed in these surveys were The debate on the hypothesis of a secular in-
proactive, relying on multiple and repeated crease in rates of autism has been obscured by a
screening phases, involving both different in- lack of clarity in the measures of disease occur-
formants at each phase and surveying the same rence used by investigators, or rather in their
cohorts at different ages, which certainly max- interpretation. In particular, it is crucial to
imized the sensitivity of case identification. differentiate prevalence (the proportion of
Third, assessments were performed with stan- individuals in a population who suffer from a
dardized diagnostic measures (Autism Diag- defined disorder) from incidence (the number
nostic Interview-Revised [ADI-R] and Autism of new cases occurring in a population over
Diagnostic Observation Schedule [ADOS]), time). Prevalence is useful for estimating needs
which match well the dimensional approach re- and planning services; only incidence rates can
tained for case definition. Finally, these sam- be used for causal research. Both prevalence
ples comprised young children around their and incidence estimates will be inflated when
TABLE 2.7 Newer Epidemiological Surveys of PDDs
All
AUTISM PDDNOS and AS PDDs
Rate/ Gender Ratio IQ Normal Rate/ Gender Ratio IQ Normal Rate/
No. Study Age 10,000 (MF) (%) 10,000 (MF) (%) 10,000
26 Baird, Charman, & Baron-Cohen, 2000 7 30.8 15.7 60 27.1 4.5 — 57.9
29 Bertrand et al., 2001 3–10 40.5 2.2 37 27.0 3.7 51 67.5
32 Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2001 4–7 16.8 3.3 29 44.5 4.3 94 61.3
35 Madsen et al., 2002 8 7.7 — — 22.2 — — 30.0
36 Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2004 4–7 22.0 4.0 33.3 35.8 8.7 91.6 58.7
37 Scott, Baron-Cohen, Bolton, & Brayne, 2002 5–11 — — — — — — 58.3 a
38 Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003 3–10 — — — — — — 34.0
39 Gurney et al., 2003 6–11 — — — — — — 52.0
a
Computed by the author.

57
58 Diagnosis and Classification

case definition is broadened and case ascertain- 2003). The population of 0- to 19-year-olds of
ment is improved. Time trends in rates can California was 10,462,273 in July 2002. If one
therefore only be gauged in investigations that applies a somewhat conservative PDD rate of
hold these parameters under strict control over 30/10,000, one would expect to have 31,386
time. These methodological requirements must PDD subjects within this age group living in
be borne in mind while reviewing the evidence California. These calculations do not support
for a secular increase in rates of PDDs. the “epidemic” interpretation, but instead sug-
Five approaches to assess this question have gest that children identified in the DDS data-
been used in the literature: (1) referral statis- base were only a subset of the population
tics, (2) comparison of cross-sectional epidemi- prevalence pool. The increasing numbers re-
ological surveys, (3) repeat surveys in defined flect merely an increasing proportion of chil-
geographic areas, (4) successive birth cohorts, dren accessing services.
and (5) incidence studies. Third, no attempt was ever made to adjust
the trends for changes in diagnostic concepts
Referral Statistics and definitions. However, major nosographic
modifications were introduced during the cor-
Increasing numbers of children referred to spe- responding years, with a general tendency in
cialist services or known to special education most classifications to broaden the concept of
registers have been taken as evidence for an autism (as embodied in the terms autism spec-
increased incidence of autism-spectrum dis- trum or pervasive developmental disorder).
orders. However, trends over time in referred Fourth, age characteristics of the subjects
samples are confounded by many factors such recorded in official statistics were portrayed
as referral patterns, availability of services, in a confusing manner where the preponder-
heightened public awareness, decreasing age at ance of young subjects was presented as evi-
diagnosis, and changes over time in diagnostic dence of increasing rates in successive birth
concepts and practices, to name only a few. cohorts (see Fombonne, 2001). The problems
Failure to control for these confounding factors associated with disentangling age from period
was obvious in some recent reports (Fombonne, and cohort effects in such observational data
2001), such as the widely quoted reports from are well known in the epidemiological litera-
California educational services (Department of ture and deserve better statistical handling.
Developmental Services, 1999, 2003). Fifth, the decreasing age at diagnosis leads
First, these reports applied to numbers, not to increasing numbers of young children being
to rates, and failure to relate these numbers to identified in official statistics or referred to al-
meaningful denominators left the interpreta- ready busy specialist services. Earlier identifi-
tion of an upward trend vulnerable to changes cation of children from the prevalence pool
in the composition of the underlying popula- may result in increased service activity; how-
tion. For example, the population of California ever, it does not mean increased incidence.
was 19,971,000 in 1970 and rose to 35,116,000 Another study of this dataset was subse-
as of July 1, 2002, a change of 75.8%. Thus, quently launched to demonstrate the validity
part of the increase in numbers of subjects of the epidemic hypothesis (MIND Institute,
identified with autism merely reflects the 2002). The investigation was, however, flawed
change in population size, but the DDS reports in its design. The authors relied on DDS data
have ignored or not adequately accounted for and aimed at ruling out changes in diagnostic
this change. practices and immigration into California as
Second, the focus on the year-to-year changes factors explaining the increased numbers.
in absolute numbers of subjects known to Cali- While immigration was reasonably ruled out,
fornia state-funded services detracts from more the study comparing diagnoses of autism and
meaningful comparisons. As of December 2002, mental retardation over time was impossible to
the total number of subjects with a PDD diagno- interpret in light of the extremely low (<20%)
sis was 17,748 in the 0-to-19 age group (includ- response rates. Furthermore, a study only
ing 16,108 autism codes 1 and 2 and 1,640 other based on cases registered for services cannot
PDDs; Department of Developmental Services, rule out that the proportion of cases within the
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 59

general population who registered with ser- increased efficiency over time in case identifi-
vices has changed over time. Assuming a con- cation methods used in surveys as well as
stant incidence and prevalence at two different changes in diagnostic concepts and practices.
time points (meaning there is no epidemic), the Thus, changes in diagnostic practices were re-
number of cases known to a public agency de- ported in Magnusson and Saemundsen’s study
livering services could well increase by 200% (2001) where ICD-9 rates for the oldest co-
if the proportion of cases from the community horts born in the years 1964 to 1983 were
referred to services rises from 25% to 75% in lower than the ICD-10 rates of the most recent
the interval. To rule out this ( likely, as men- 1984 to 1992 birth cohorts. Similarly, lower
tioned) explanation, data over time are needed rates in the oldest birth cohorts were thought
both on referred subjects and on nonreferred to reflect changes in diagnostic practices and
(or referred to other services) subjects. Failure boundaries in Webb, Lobo, Hervas, Scourfield,
to do that precludes drawing any inference to and Fraser’s study (1997). One large survey
the California population from a study of the recently conducted in the United Kingdom
DDS database (Fombonne, 2003b). The con- (Study 24) also documented a steep rise in the
clusions of this report were therefore simply number of cases diagnosed with autism or
unwarranted. atypical autism, and a similar trend for AS.
A recent reanalysis of the California data The interpretation of these trends is, however,
has in fact strongly suggested that switches in unclear because there was no control of drift
diagnostic practices from mental retardation over time in diagnostic practices nor of changes
to autism could also account for increased in service development.
numbers of subjects with an autism diagnosis in The most convincing evidence that method
the California DDS datasets (Croen, Grether, factors could account for most of the variabil-
Hoogstrate, & Selvin, 2002). Jick and Kaye ity in published prevalence estimates comes
(2003) obtained similar data in the United from a direct comparison of eight recent sur-
Kingdom. They showed that the incidence of veys conducted in the United Kingdom and the
specific developmental disorders (including United States (Table 2.8). In each country,
language disorders) decreased by about the four surveys were conducted around the same
same amount that the incidence of diagnoses year and with similar age groups. As there is
of autism increased in boys born from 1990 no reason to expect huge between-area differ-
to 1997. ences in rates, prevalence estimates should
On the whole, evidence from these referral therefore be comparable within each country.
statistics is very weak and certainly does not However, an inspection of estimates obtained
deserve the media attention that it has re- in each set of studies (Table 2.8: right-hand
ceived. Accordingly, proper epidemiological column) shows a 6-fold variation in rates for
studies are needed to assess secular changes in U.K. surveys, and a 14-fold variation in U.S.
the incidence of a disorder. rates. In each set of studies, high rates derive
from surveys that used intensive population-
Comparison of Cross-Sectional based screening techniques, whereas lower
Epidemiological Surveys rates were obtained from studies relying on
administrative methods for case finding. Since
Due to their cross-sectional methodology, no passage of time was involved, the magni-
most epidemiological investigations of autism tude of these gradients in rates can only be at-
have been concerned with prevalence. As tributed to differences in case identification
shown earlier, each epidemiological survey of methods across surveys, and the replication of
autism possesses unique design features that the pattern in two countries provides even
could account almost entirely for between- more confidence in this interpretation. This
studies variations in rates; time trends in rates analysis of recent and contemporaneous stud-
of autism are therefore difficult to gauge from ies shows that no inference on trends in the in-
published prevalence rates. The significant cidence of PDDs can be derived from a simple
correlation mentioned between prevalence rate comparison of prevalence rates over time,
and year of publication could merely reflect since studies conducted at different periods
60 Diagnosis and Classification

TABLE 2.8 Study Design Impact on Prevalence

Age PDD Rate/


Location Size Group Method 10,000

U.K. Studies
Chakrabarti & Staffordshire 15,500 21⁄ 2 –61⁄ 2 Intense screening and 62.6
Fombonne, 2001 assessment
Baird, Charman, & South East Thames 16,235 7 Early screening and 57.9
Baron-Cohen, 2000 follow-up identification
Fombonne, Simmons, England and Wales 10,438 5–15 National household 26.1
Ford, Meltzer, & survey of psychiatric
Goodman, 2001 disorders
Taylor et al., 1999 North Thames 490,000 0–16 Administrative records 10.1

U.S. Studies
Bertrand et al., 2001 Brick Township, New 8,896 3–10 Multiple sources of 67
Jersey ascertainment
Sturmey & James, Texas 3,564,577 6–18 Educational services 16
2001
DDS, 1999 California 3,215,000 4–9 Educational services 15

Hillman, Kanafani, Missouri — 5–9 Educational services 4.8


Takahashi, & Miles,
2000

are likely to differ even more with respect to age groups were included in each survey. The
their methodology. rate in the first survey for the youngest age
The next two approaches are in essence group (which resembles more closely the chil-
equivalent to a comparison of cross-sectional dren included in the other two surveys) was
surveys although specific attempts are made 5.1/10,000. Second, the increased prevalence
to maintain constant some design features in in the second survey was explained by im-
the surveys. proved detection among the mentally retarded,
and that of the third survey by cases born to
Repeat Surveys in Defined immigrant parents. That the majority of the
Geographic Areas latter group was born abroad suggests that mi-
gration into the area could be a key explana-
Repeated surveys, using the same methodology tion. Taken in conjunction with a change in
and conducted in the same geographic area local services and a progressive broadening of
at different points in time, can potentially the definition of autism over time acknowl-
yield useful information on time trends if the edged by the authors (Gillberg, Steffenburg, &
methods are kept relatively constant. The Schaumann, 1991), these findings do not pro-
Göteborg studies (Gillberg, 1984; Gillberg, vide evidence for an increased incidence of
Steffenburg, & Schaumann, 1991; Steffenburg autism.
& Gillberg, 1986) provided three prevalence Two separate surveys conducted on children
estimates that increased over a short period born 1992 to 1995 and 1996 to 1998 in
from 4.0 (1980) to 6.6 (1984) and 9.5/10,000 Staffordshire in the United Kingdom (Table
(1988). The gradient is even steeper if rates for 2.2: Studies 32 and 36) were performed with
the urban area alone are considered (4.0, 7.5, rigorously identical methods for case definition
and 11.6/10,000; Gillberg, Steffenburg, & and case identification. The prevalence for
Schaumann, 1991). However, comparisons of combined PDD was comparable and not statisti-
these rates are not straightforward as different cally different in the two surveys (Chakrabarti
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 61

& Fombonne, in press), suggesting no upward category including ADHD. The large sample
trend in overall rates of PDD during the time size allowed the authors to assess age, period,
interval of the studies. and cohort effects. Prevalence increased regu-
larly in successive birth cohorts; for example,
Successive Birth Cohorts among 7-year-olds, the prevalence rose from
18/10,000 in those born in 1989, to 29/10,000
In large surveys encompassing a wide age in those born in 1991 and to 55/10,000 in those
range, increasing prevalence rates among the born in 1993, suggestive of birth cohort ef-
most recent birth cohorts could be interpreted fects. Within the same birth cohorts, age ef-
as indicating a secular increase in the incidence fects were also apparent since for children born
of the disorder, provided that alternative expla- in 1989 the prevalence rose with age from
nations can confidently be ruled out. This 13/10,000 at age 6, to 21/10,000 at age 9, and
analysis was used in two French surveys (17 33/10,000 at age 11. As argued by the authors,
and 20), which derived from large sample sizes. this pattern is not consistent with what one
In the first study (17), prevalence estimates would expect from a chronic nonfatal condition
were available for the two birth cohorts of chil- diagnosed in the first years of life. Their analy-
dren born in 1972 and 1976 and surveyed in sis also showed a marked period effect that
1985 and 1986. The rates were similar (5.1 and identified the early 1990s as the period when
4.9/10,000) and not statistically different rates started to go up in all ages and birth co-
(Fombonne & du Mazaubrun, 1992). Further- horts. Gurney et al. (2003) further argued that
more, in a subsequent investigation conducted this phenomenon coincided closely with the in-
in 1989 and 1990 in exactly the same areas, the clusion of PDDs in the federal Individual with
age-specific rate of autism for the 1981 birth Disabilities Educational Act (IDEA) funding
cohort was slightly lower (3.1/10,000; Rumeau- and reporting mechanism in the United States.
Rouquette et al., 1994). In any event, the find- A similar interpretation of upward trends
ings were not suggestive of increasing rates in had been put forward by Croen, Grether,
the most recent cohorts. Another survey con- Hoogstrate, and Selvin (2002) in their analysis
ducted with the same methodology but in dif- of the California DDS data.
ferent French regions a few years later (Study
20) led to a similar overall prevalence estimate Incidence Studies
as the first survey (Table 2.2). The latter survey
included consecutive birth cohorts from 1976 Only three studies provided recent incidence
to 1985, and pooling the data of both surveys estimates (Kaye, Melero-Montes, & Jick, 2001;
showed no upward trend in age-specific rates Powell et al., 2000; Smeeth et al., 2004). All
(Fombonne, du Mazaubrun, Cans, & Grand- studies showed an upward trend in incidence
jean, 1997). Some weight should be given to over short periods. In the largest study of 1,410
these results as they derive from a total target subjects, there was a 10-fold increase in the
population of 735,000 children, 389 of whom rate of first recorded diagnoses of PDDs in
had autism. However, the most retarded children United Kingdom general practice medical
with autism were reflected in these studies and, records from 1988 to 1992 and from 2000 to
as a consequence, any upward trend that would 2001 (Smeeth et al., 2004). The increase was
apply specifically to high-functioning subjects more marked for PDDs other than autism
might have gone undetected. but the increase in autism also was obvious.
An analysis of special educational disability However, none of these investigations could de-
from Minnesota showed a 16-fold increase in termine the effect on the upward trend of
the number of children identified with a PDD changes over time in diagnostic criteria, im-
from 2001 to 2002, as compared with 1991 to proved awareness, and service availability.
1992 (Gurney et al., 2003; Study 39). The in-
crease was not specific to autism since during Conclusion on Time Trends
the same period an increase of 50% was ob-
served for all disability categories (except se- The available epidemiological evidence does
vere mental handicap), especially for the not strongly support the hypothesis that the
62 Diagnosis and Classification

incidence of autism has increased. As it stands constant. Finally, good psychometric data on
now, the recent upward trend in rates of preva- cognitive functioning will also be needed to as-
lence cannot be directly attributed to an in- sess trends in various subgroups in light of
crease in the incidence of the disorder. There is the preliminary evidence that patterns of men-
some evidence that diagnostic substitution and tal retardation in autism may be changing. Ob-
changes in the policies for special education as viously, surveillance programs should also
well as the increasing availability of services incorporate measures of risk factors hypothe-
are responsible for the higher prevalence fig- sized to exert causal influences for this group
ures. Most of the existing epidemiological data of disorders.
are inadequate to properly test hypotheses on
changes in the incidence of autism in human IMMIGRANT STATUS, ETHNICITY,
populations. Moreover, the low frequency of SOCIAL CLASS, AND OTHER
autism and PDDs seriously limits power in CORRELATES
most investigations and variations of small
magnitude in the incidence of the disorder are Some investigators have mentioned the possi-
very likely to go undetected. Future investiga- bility that rates of autism might be higher
tions should aim at setting up surveillance pro- among immigrants (Gillberg, 1987; Gillberg,
grams that will allow estimates of the incidence Schaumann, & Gillberg, 1995; Gillberg, Stef-
of PDDs (as opposed to autism only) and the fenburg, & Schaumann, 1991; Wing, 1980).
monitoring of its changes over time. It will be Five of the 17 children with autism identified
crucial to set up parallel investigations in dif- in the Camberwell study were of Caribbean ori-
ferent geographic areas to replicate findings gin (Study 4; Wing, 1980), and the estimated
across areas as a validating tool. Such programs rate of autism was 6.3/10,000 for this group
should focus on age groups where the identifi- compared with 4.4/10,000 for the rest of the
cation and diagnosis of the range of PDDs is population (Wing, 1993). However, the wide
less likely to fluctuate over time. Rapid changes confidence intervals associated with rates from
in the age at first diagnosis and concerns about this study (Table 2.2) indicate no statistically
the validity and stability of diagnostic assess- significant difference. This area of London had
ments among preschool samples require inves- received a large proportion of immigrants from
tigators to focus on older age groups. On the the Caribbean region in the 1960s; and when
other hand, changes in the autistic symptoma- there is migration flux in and out of an area, es-
tology in adolescence and difficulties in service timation of population rates should be viewed
delivery to teenagers (and therefore in case with much caution. Yet, Afro-Caribbean chil-
identification) suggest a focus on younger dren referred from the same area were recently
children. The school-age years (7 to 12 years) found to have higher rates of autism than re-
should therefore be selected for efficient ferred controls (Goodman & Richards, 1995).
monitoring. Mandatory education at that age The sample again was very small (N = 18) and
would facilitate identification and would min- differential referral patterns to a tertiary center
imize potential difficulties in diagnosing high- also providing services for the local area could
functioning subjects at the upper end of this not be ruled out. Only one child was born from
age range. Diagnostic assessments should rely British-born Afro-Caribbean parents in a re-
on standardized measures of known reliability cent U.K. survey (Study 21; Webb, Lobo, et al.,
and validity. Furthermore, developmental and 1997), providing little support to this particular
phenomenological data should be collected at a hypothesis. Similarly, the findings from the
symptomatic level, and uniformly across the Göteborg studies paralleled an increased mi-
whole spectrum of PDDs, remaining free of gration flux in the early 1980s in this area
particular nosological contingencies. Secondary (Gillberg, 1987); they, too, were based on rela-
application of diagnostic algorithms (current tively small numbers (19 children from immi-
and/or future) on datasets containing detailed grant parents). In the same geographic area,
developmental and symptomatic data will then Arvidsson et al. (1997; Study 22) had five chil-
allow for performing meaningful comparisons dren out of nine in their sample with either both
over time, while holding diagnostic groupings parents (N = 2) or one parent (N = 3) having im-
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 63

migrated to Sweden. However, there were no cific methodological context of these investi-
systematic comparisons with rates of immi- gations. Most studies had low numbers of
grants in the population. A positive family his- identified cases, and especially small numbers
tory for developmental disorders was reported of autistic children born from immigrant
in three such cases and a chromosomal abnor- parents, and many authors in these studies re-
mality in one further case. In the Icelandic sur- lied on broadened definitions of autism. Sta-
vey (Study 31), 2.5% of the autism parents were tistical testing was not rigorously conducted
from non-European origin compared to a 0.5% and doubts could be raised in several studies
corresponding rate in the whole population, but about the appropriateness of the comparison
it was unclear if this represented a significant data that were used. Thus, the overall propor-
difference. In Study 23, the proportion of chil- tion of immigrants in the population may be an
dren with autism and a non-European origin inappropriate figure with which to compare
was marginally but not significantly raised observed rates of children from immigrant
compared with the population rate of immi- parents among autistic series. Fertility rates of
grants (8% vs. 2.3%), but this was based on a immigrant families are likely to be different
very small sample (two children of non-Euro- from those in the host populations and call
pean origin). A U.K. survey found comparable for strict age-adjusted comparisons of individ-
rates in areas with contrasting ethnic compo- uals at risk for the disorder. The proportion of
sition (Powell et al., 2000). In the Utah sur- immigrants in the entire population might se-
vey, where a clear breakdown by race was riously underestimate that for younger age
achieved (Ritvo et al., 1989; Study 15, Table groups, and, in turn, this could have given rise
2.2), the autism parents showed no deviation to false positive results. In addition, studies
from the racial distribution of this state. The sampling children through services or clinical
proportion of non-Whites in this study and sources may be biased because ethnicity, race
state was, however, noticeably low, providing and social class are likely to differentially af-
little power to detect departures from the null fect access to these settings. Finally, studies
hypothesis. Other studies have not systemati- were generally poor in their definition of im-
cally reported the proportion of immigrant or migrant status, with unclear amalgamation of
ethnic groups in the areas surveyed. In four information on country of origin, citizenship,
studies where the proportions of immigrant immigrant status, race, and ethnicity. Finally,
groups were low (11, 12, 19, 21), rates of it is unclear what common mechanism could
autism were in the upper range of rates. Con- explain the putative association between im-
versely, in studies of other populations (14, migrant status and autism, since the origins of
17, and 20) where immigrants contributed the immigrant parents (especially in Study 16;
substantially to the denominators, rates were see also Gillberg & Gillberg, 1996) were di-
in the rather low band. The analysis of a large verse and represented in fact all continents.
sample (N = 4,356) of Californian PDD chil- With this heterogeneity in mind, what com-
dren showed a lower risk of autism in children mon biological features might these immigrant
of Mexico-born mothers and a similar risk for families share and what would be a plausible
children of mothers born outside the United mechanism explaining the putative association
States compared with California-born moth- between autism and immigrant status? The
ers (Croen, Grether, & Selvin, 2002). In this possibility of an increased vulnerability to in-
study, the risk of PDD was raised in African trauterine infections in nonimmunized im-
American mothers with an adjusted rate ratio migrant mothers was raised, but not supported,
of 1.6 (95% CI: 1.5 to 1.8); by contrast, the in a detailed analysis of 15 autistic children
prevalence was similar in White, Black, and from immigrant parents (Gillberg & Gillberg,
other races in the population-based survey of 1996). These authors instead posited that
Atlanta (Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003), where parents, and in particular fathers, affected
case ascertainment is likely to be more com- with autistic traits would be inclined to travel
plete than in the previous study. abroad to find female partners more naïve
Taken altogether, the combined results of to their social difficulties. This speculation
these reports should be interpreted in the spe- was based, however, on three observations
64 Diagnosis and Classification

only, and assessment of the autistic traits in increased incidence or prevalence rate ratio in
two parents was not independently obtained. a cluster does not prove anything; this erro-
The hypothesis of an association between neous approach has been referred to in the lit-
immigrant status or race and autism, therefore, erature as the “ Texan sharpshooter ” effect,
remains largely unsupported by the empirical referring to the gunman who shot first and
results. Most of the claims about these possi- then painted a target around the bullet hole. On
ble correlates of autism were derived from the other hand, a negative finding would cer-
post hoc observations of very small samples tainly suggest a random phenomenon.
and were not subjected to rigorous statistical When cluster alarms are associated to a
testing. Large studies have generally failed to possible causal mechanism, it is recommended
detect such associations. to perform focused tests of clustering at other
suspected sources of risk exposure. The clus-
Autism and Social Class ter alarms for childhood leukemia occurring
near a nuclear plant in England were followed
Twelve studies provided information on the so- by investigations of disease incidence at other
cial class of the families of autistic children. nuclear plants, which proved to be negative
Of these, four studies (1, 2, 3, and 5) suggested (Hoffmann & Schlattmann, 1999). However,
an association between autism and social class the potential source of the cluster alarm is
or parental education. The year of data collec- not always identified and, in these instances,
tion for these four investigations was before it is suggested to monitor the incidence of fu-
1980 (Table 2.2), and all studies conducted ture cases in the area of first alarm. Chen,
thereafter provided no evidence for the associ- Connelly, and Mantel (1993) have outlined
ation. Thus, the epidemiological results sug- postalarm monitoring techniques that allow
gest that the earlier findings were probably investigators to confirm or reject alarms,
due to artifacts in the availability of services based on the observation of the time intervals
and in the case-finding methods, as already preceding each of the first five cases diag-
shown in other samples (Schopler, Andrews, & nosed subsequent to the alarm. The approach is
Strupp, 1979; Wing, 1980). a confirmatory technique that ignores the clus-
ter alarm data and thus avoids the aforemen-
Cluster Reports tioned preselection bias. Other techniques,
such as space-time scan statistics (Kulldorff,
Occasional reports of space or time clustering 1999), can confirm or reject a cluster alarm
of cases of autism have raised concerns in the by extending the investigation to a larger area
general public. In fact, only one such report while avoiding selection biases, adjusting for
has been published in the professional litera- population density, confounding variables and
ture (Baron-Cohen, Saunders, & Chakrabarti, multiple testing, and allowing for the precise
1999) that described seven children with location of clusters. They require, however, the
either autism or PDD-NOS living within a few availability of regional or national geocoded
streets from each other in a small town of the data that are usually not available for autism.
Midlands (United Kingdom). The cluster was Other general statistical techniques to assess
first identified by a parent, and the subsequent time and space clustering are reviewed in spe-
analysis was uninformed with proper statisti- cialist journals.
cal procedures and inconclusive as to whether Cluster alarms are likely to represent ran-
this cluster could have occurred by chance dom occurrence in most instances, as illus-
only. The comparison of the incidence or trated by several recent investigations of cluster
prevalence rate within the cluster to that of the alarms for other rare disorders of childhood.
general population (as performed by Baron- Cluster alarms in autism have not been investi-
Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999) is an inappropri- gated with scientific rigor, whereas research
ate technique to assess cluster alarms. The strategies and ad hoc statistical procedures
reason is that, by definition, a preselection exist for that purpose. The approach to such
bias occurs in the delineation of the cluster cluster alarms should be to confirm the alarm
boundaries (Kulldorff, 1999). Thus, finding an in the first place, using the available techniques
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 65

to assess the significance of clusters and to ex- most likely represents changes in the con-
clude random noise in spatial and time distribu- cepts, definitions, service availability, and
tion of the disorder. Only when an alarm has awareness of autistic-spectrum disorders in
been confirmed should researchers set up more both the lay and professional public. To assess
complex epidemiological investigations to in- whether the incidence has increased, method
vestigate risk factors and causal mechanisms. factors that account for an important propor-
tion of the variability in rates must be tightly
CONCLUSION controlled.
Taking 35/10,000 and 60/10,000 as two
Epidemiological surveys of autism and PDDs working rates for the combination of all PDDs,
have now been carried out in several countries. and using U.S. population figures as of July 1,
Methodological differences in case definition 2002, it can be estimated that about 284,000
and case-finding procedures make between- and up to 486,000 subjects under the age of 20
survey comparisons difficult to perform. suffer from a PDD in the United States. These
Despite these differences, some common char- figures carry straightforward implications for
acteristics of autism and PDDs in population current and future needs in services and early
surveys have consistently emerged. Autism is educational intervention programs.
associated with mental retardation in about
70% of the cases and is overrepresented among Cross-References
males (with a male/female ratio of 4.31).
Autism is found in association with some rare Issues of diagnosis of autism spectrum disor-
and genetically determined medical conditions, ders are addressed in Chapter 1 and Chapters
such as tuberous sclerosis. Overall, the median 3 through 6; medical aspects of autism are dis-
value of about 5.5% for the combined rate of cussed in Chapter 20.
medical disorders in autism derived from this
review is consistent with the 5% (Tuchman,
REFERENCES
Rapin, & Shinnar, 1991) to 10% (Rutter, Bai-
ley, Bolton, & Le Couteur, 1994) figures avail- Ahlsen, G., Gillberg, C., Lindblom, R., & Gillberg,
able from other investigations. A majority of C. (1994). Tuberous sclerosis in Western Swe-
surveys has ruled out social class as a risk den: A population study of cases with early
factor for autism, a result once supported by childhood onset. Archives of Neurology, 51,
studies of clinical, that is, less representative, 76–81.
samples. The putative association of autism Arvidsson, T., Danielsson, B., Forsberg, P., Gillberg,
with immigrant status or race is, so far, not C., Johansson, M., & Kjellgren, G. (1997).
borne out by epidemiological studies. The con- Autism in 3–6-year-old children in a suburb of
Goteborg, Sweden. Autism, 2, 163–173.
clusion of a lack of variation in the incidence
Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A.,
of autism according to race or ethnicity is Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., et al. (2000).
reached, however, from a weak empirical base, A screening instrument for autism at 18 months
and future studies might address this issue of age: A 6 year follow-up study. Journal of the
more efficiently. In fact, epidemiological stud- American Academy of Child and Adolescent
ies of autism and PDDs have generally been Psychiatry, 39, 694–702.
lacking sophistication in their investigation of Baron-Cohen, S., Saunders, K., & Chakrabarti, S.
most other risk factors. (1999). Does autism cluster geographically? A
The same considerations apply to the issue research note. Autism, 3, 39–43.
of secular changes in the incidence of autism. Bertrand, J., Mars, A., Boyle, C., Bove, F., Yeargin-
Allsopp, M., & Decouf le, P. (2001). Preva-
The little evidence that exists does not support
lence of autism in a United States population:
this hypothesis, but power to detect time
The Brick Township, New Jersey, investiga-
trends is seriously limited in existing tion. Pediatrics, 108, 1155–1161.
datasets. The debate has been largely con- Bohman, M., Bohman, I. L., Björck, P. O., &
founded by confusion between prevalence and Sjöholm, E. (1983). Childhood psychosis in a
incidence. Although prevalence estimates ap- northern Swedish county: Some preliminary
pear to have gone up over time, this increase findings from an epidemiological survey. In
66 Diagnosis and Classification

M. H. Schmidt & H. Remschmidt (Eds.), Epi- 1987 through 1998. Retrieved July 16, 2004,
demiological approaches in child psychiatry from http://www.dds.ca.gov.
(pp. 164–173). New York: George Thieme Ver- Department of Developmental Services. (2003).
lag Stuttgart. Autism spectrum disorders: Changes in the Cali-
Brask, B. H. (1972). A prevalence investigation of fornia caseload—an update 1999 through 2002.
childhood psychoses. In Nordic Symposium on Retrieved July 16, 2004, from http://www.dds
the Care of Psychotic Children. Oslo: Barne- .ca.gov/Autism /pdf/AutismReport2003.pdf.
psychiatrist Forening. Deykin, E. Y., & MacMahon, B. (1979). The inci-
Bregman, J. D., & Volkmar, F. R. (1988). Autistic dence of seizures among children with autistic
social dysfunction and Down’s syndrome. symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Journal of the American Academy of Child and 136(10), 1310–1312.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 440–441. Ehlers, S., & Gillberg, C. (1993). The epidemiol-
Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., & Smith, I. M. (1988). ogy of Asperger syndrome: A total population
First report of a Canadian epidemiological study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
study of autistic syndromes. Journal of Child atry, 34, 1327–1350.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 4, 433–445. Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism:
Burd, L., Fisher, W., & Kerbeshan, J. (1987). A A review. Psychological Medicine, 29, 769–786.
prevalence study of pervasive developmental Fombonne, E. (2001). Is there an epidemic of
disorders in North Dakota. Journal of the autism? Pediatrics, 107, 411–413.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Fombonne, E. (2002a). Case identification in an
Psychiatry, 26(5), 700–703. epidemiological context. In M. Rutter & E.
Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive Taylor (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry
developmental disorders in preschool children. (pp. 52–69). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Journal of the American Medical Association, Fombonne, E. (2002b). Prevalence of childhood
285(24), 3093–3099. disintegrative disorder (CDD). Autism, 6(2),
Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (in press). Perva- 147–155.
sive developmental disorders in preschool chil- Fombonne, E. (2003a). Epidemiological surveys of
dren: High prevalence confirmed. American autism and other pervasive developmental dis-
Journal of Psychiatry. orders: An update. Journal of Autism and De-
Chen, R., Connelly, R., & Mantel, N. (1993). velopmental Disorders, 33(4), 365–382.
Analysing post-alarm data in a monitoring Fombonne, E. (2003b). The prevalence of autism.
system in order to accept or reject the alarm. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Statistics in Medicine, 12, 1807–1812. 289(1), 1–3.
Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children with congeni- Fombonne, E., & du Mazaubrun, C. (1992). Preva-
tal rubella. Journal of Autism and Childhood lence of infantile autism in 4 French regions.
Schizophrenia, 1, 33–47. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiol-
Cialdella, P., & Mamelle, N. (1989). An epidemio- ogy, 27, 203–210.
logical study of infantile autism in a French Fombonne, E., du Mazaubrun, C., Cans, C., &
department. Journal of Child Psychology and Grandjean, H. (1997). Autism and associated
Psychiatry, 30(1), 165–175. medical disorders in a large French epidemio-
Croen, L. A., Grether, J. K., Hoogstrate, J., & logical sample. Journal of the American Acad-
Selvin, S. (2002). The changing prevalence of emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(11),
autism in California. Journal of Autism and De- 1561–1566.
velopmental Disorders, 32(3), 207–215. Fombonne, E., Simmons, H., Ford, T., Meltzer, H.,
Croen, L. A., Grether, J. K., & Selvin, S. (2002). & Goodman, R. (2001). Prevalence of perva-
Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a Cali- sive developmental disorders in the British na-
fornia population: Who is at risk? Journal of tionwide survey of child mental health.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), Journal of the American Academy of Child and
217–224. Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(7), 820–827.
Davidovitch, M., Holtzman, G., & Tirosh, E. (2001, Fombonne, E., & Tidmarsh, L. (2003). Epidemio-
March). Autism in the Haifa area—an epi- logical data on Asperger disorder. Child and
demiological perspective. Israeli Medical As- Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Amer-
sociation Journal, 3, 188–189. ica, 12, 15–21.
Department of Developmental Services. (1999). Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1992).
Changes in the population of persons with Autism in Downs’ syndrome: Presentation and
autism and pervasive developmental disorders diagnosis. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re-
in California’s Developmental Services System: search, 35, 449–456.
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 67

Gillberg, C. (1984). Infantile autism and other Howlin, P., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1995). The
childhood psychoses in a Swedish region: Epi- recognition of autism in children with Down
demiological aspects. Journal of Child Psy- syndrome: Implications for intervention and
chology and Psychiatry, 25, 35–43. some speculations about pathology. Develop-
Gillberg, C. (1987). Infantile autism in children of mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37,
immigrant parents. A population-based study 406–414.
from Göteborg, Sweden. British Journal of Psy- Hunt, A., & Lindenbaum, R. H. (1984). Tuberous
chiatry, 150, 856–858. sclerosis: A new estimate of prevalence within
Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1996). Autism and the Oxford region. Journal of Medical Genet-
medical disorders: A review of the literature. ics, 21, 272–277.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Jick, H., & Kaye, J. A. (2003). Epidemiology and
38, 191–202. possible causes of autism. Pharmacotherapy,
Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, C. (1996). Autism in im- 23(12), 1524–1530.
migrants: A population-based study from Kadesjö, B., Gillberg, C., & Hagberg, B. (1999).
Swedish rural and urban areas. Journal of In- Autism and Asperger syndrome in seven-year-
tellectual Disability Research, 40, 24–31. old children: A total population study. Journal
Gillberg, C., Schaumann, H., & Gillberg, I. C. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(4),
(1995). Autism in immigrants: Children 327–331.
born in Sweden to mothers born in Uganda. Kaye, J., Melero-Montes, M., & Jick, H. (2001).
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 39, Mumps, measles, and rubella vaccine and the
141–144. incidence of autism recorded by general practi-
Gillberg, C., Steffenburg, S., & Schaumann, H. tioners: A time trend analysis. British Medical
(1991). Is autism more common now than ten Journal, 322, 0–2.
years ago? British Journal of Psychiatry, 158, Kielinen, M., Linna, S. L., & Moilanen, I. (2000).
403–409. Autism in Northern Finland. European Child
Goodman, R., & Richards, H. (1995). Child and and Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 162–167.
adolescent psychiatric presentations of second- Knobloch, H., & Pasamanick, B. (1975). Some eti-
generation Afro-Caribbeans in Britain. British ologic and prognostic factors in early infan-
Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 362–369. tile autism and psychosis. Pediatrics, 55,
Gurney, J. G., Fritz, M. S., Ness, K. K., Sievers, P., 182–191.
Newschaffer, C. J., & Shapiro, E. G. (2003). Kulldorff, M. (1999). Statistical evaluation of
Analysis of prevalence trends of autism spec- disease cluster alarms. In A. B. Lawson et al.
trum disorder in Minnesota [Comment]. (Eds.), Disease mapping and risk assessment
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medi- for public health (pp. 143–149). Chichester,
cine, 157(7), 622–627. West Sussex, England: Wiley.
Hillman, R., Kanafani, N., Takahashi, T., & Miles, Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P.,
J. (2000). Prevalence of autism in Missouri: Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989).
Changing trends and the effect of a compre- Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized
hensive state autism project. Missouri Medi- investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism
cine, 97, 159–163. and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387.
Hoffmann, W., & Schlattmann, P. (1999). An analy- Lord, C., Risi, S., Lembrecht, L., Cook, E., Leven-
sis of the geographical distribution of leukemia thal, B., DiLavore, P., et al. (2000). The Autism
incidence in the vicinity of a suspected point Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A
source: A case study. In A. B. Lawson et al. standard measure of social and communication
(Eds.), Disease mapping and risk assessment deficits associated with the spectrum of
for public health (pp. 396–409). Chichester, autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
West Sussex, England: Wiley. Disorders, 30, 205–223.
Honda, H., Shimizu, Y., Misumi, K., Niimi, M., & Lord, C., Schopler, E., & Revecki, D. (1982). Sex
Ohashi, Y. (1996). Cumulative incidence and differences in autism. Journal of Autism and
prevalence of childhood autism in children Developmental Disorders, 12, 317–330.
in Japan. British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, Lotter, V. (1966). Epidemiology of autistic condi-
228–235. tions in young children: I. Prevalence. Social
Hoshino, Y., Yashima, Y., Ishige, K., Tachibana, R., Psychiatry, 1, 124–137.
Watanabe, M., Kancki, M., et al. (1982). The Lotter, V. (1967). Epidemiology of autistic con-
epidemiological study of autism in Fukushi- ditions in young children: II. Some charac-
maken. Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japon- teristics of the parents and children. Social
ica, 36, 115–124. Psychiatry, 1(4), 163–173.
68 Diagnosis and Classification

Lowe, T. L., Tanaka, K., Seashore, M. R., Young, Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne,
J. G., & Cohen, D. J. (1980). Detection of C. (2002). Brief report: Prevalence of autism
phenylketonuria in autistic and psychiatric spectrum conditions in children aged 5–11
children. Journal of the American Medical As- years in Cambridgeshire, UK. Autism, 6(3),
sociation, 243, 126–128. 231–237.
Madsen, K. M., Hviid, A., Vestergaard, M., Schen- Shepherd, C. W., Beard, C. M., Gomez, M. R., Kur-
del, D., Wohlfahrt, J., Thorsen, P., et al. (2002). land, L. T., & Whisnant, J. P. (1991). Tuberous
A population-based study of measles, mumps, sclerosis complex in Olmsted County, Min-
and rubella vaccination and autism. New En- nesota, 1950–1989. Archives of Neurology, 48,
gland Journal of Medicine, 347(19), 1477–1482. 400–401.
Magnusson, P., & Saemundsen, E. (2001). Preva- Smalley, S. (1998). Autism and tuberous sclerosis.
lence of autism in Iceland. Journal of Autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
and Developmental Disorders, 31, 153–163. ders, 28, 407–414.
Matsuishi, T., Shiotsuki, M., Yoshimura, K., Shoji, Smalley, S. L., Tanguay, P. E., Smith, M., & Gutier-
H., Imuta, F., & Yamashita, F. (1987). High rez, G. (1992). Autism and tuberous sclerosis.
prevalence of infantile autism in Kurume City, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Japan. Journal of Child Neurology, 2, 268–271. 22, 339–355.
McCarthy, P., Fitzgerald, M., & Smith, M. A. Smeeth, L., Cook, C., Fombonne, E., Heavey, L.,
(1984). Prevalence of childhood autism in Ire- Rodrigues, L., Smith, P., et al. (2004). Rate of
land. Irish Medical Journal, 77(5), 129–130. first recorded diagnosis of autism and other
MIND Institute. (2002, October 17). Report to the pervasive developmental disorders in United
Legislature on the Principal Findings from the Kingdom general practice, 1988 to 2001. Man-
epidemiology of autism in California: A com- uscript submitted for publication.
prehensive pilot study. Davis: University of Spitzer, R. L., & Siegel, B. (1990). The DSM-III-R
California. field trial of pervasive developmental disor-
Mouridsen, S. E., Bachmann-Andersen, L., ders. Journal of the American Academy of
Sörensen, S. A., Rich, B., & Isager, T. (1992). Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 855–886.
Neurofibromatosis in infantile autism and Sponheim, E., & Skjeldal, O. (1998). Autism and
other types of childhood psychoses. Acta Pae- related disorders: Epidemiological findings in
dopsychiatrica, 55, 15–18. a Norwegian study using ICD-10 diagnostic
Powell, J., Edwards, A., Edwards, M., Pandit, B. S., criteria. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Sungum-Paliwal, S. R., & Whitehouse, W. Disorders, 28, 217–222.
(2000). Changes in the incidence of childhood Steffenburg, S., & Gillberg, C. (1986). Autism and
autism and other autistic spectrum disorders autistic-like conditions in Swedish rural and
in preschool children from two areas of the urban areas: A population study. British Jour-
West Midlands, UK. Developmental Medicine nal of Psychiatry, 149, 81–87.
and Child Neurology, 42, 624–628. Steinhausen H.-C., Göbel, D., Breinlinger, M., &
Ritvo, E. R., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., Mason- Wohlloben, B. (1986). A community survey of
Brothers, A., Jorde, L., Jensen, W. R., et al. infantile autism. Journal of the American Acad-
(1989). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemi- emy of Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 186–189.
ologic survey of autism: Prevalence. American Sturmey, P., & James, V. (2001). Administrative
Journal of Psychiatry, 146(2), 194–199. prevalence of autism in the Texas school sys-
Rumeau-Rouquette, C., du Mazaubrun, C., Verrier, tem. Journal of the American Academy of Child
A., Mlika, A., Bréart, G., Goujard, J., et al. Psychiatry, 40(6), 621.
(1994). Prévalence des handicaps: Évolution Sugiyama, T., & Abe, T. (1989). The prevalence of
dans trois générations d’enfants 1972, 1976, autism in Nagoya, Japan: A total population
1981. Paris: Editions INSERM. study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to Disorders, 19(1), 87–96.
adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450. Tanoue, Y., Oda, S., Asano, F., & Kawashima, K.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A. (1988). Epidemiology of infantile autism in
(1994). Autism and known medical conditions: Southern Ibaraki, Japan: Differences in preva-
Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol- lence in birth cohorts. Journal of Autism and
ogy and Psychiatry, 35, 311–322. Developmental Disorders, 18, 155–166.
Schopler, E., Andrews, C. E., & Strupp, K. (1979). Taylor, B., Miller, E., Farrington, C. P., Petropou-
Do autistic children come from upper-middle- los, M. C., Favot-Mayaud, I., Li, J., et al.
class parents? Journal of Autism and Develop- (1999, June 12). Autism and measles, mumps,
mental Disorders, 9(2), 139–151. and rubella vaccine: No epidemiological evi-
Epidemiological Studies of Pervasive Developmental Disorders 69

dence for a causal association. Lancet, 353, authority. Developmental Medicine and Child
2026–2029. Neurology, 45(6), 377–384.
Treffert, D. A. (1970). Epidemiology of infantile Wignyosumarto, S., Mukhlas, M., & Shirataki, S.
autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 22, (1992). Epidemiological and clinical study
431–438. of autistic children in Yogyakarta, Indonesia.
Tuchman, R. F., Rapin, I., & Shinnar, S. (1991). Kobe Journal of Medical Sciences, 38(1), 1–19.
Autistic and dysphasic children: II. Epilepsy. Wing, L. (1980). Childhood autism and social
Pediatrics, 88, 1219–1225. class: A question of selection? British Journal
Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Childhood disintegrative of Psychiatry, 137, 410–417.
disorder: Issues for DSM-IV. Journal of Autism Wing, L. (1993). The definition and prevalence of
and Developmental Disorders, 22, 625–642. autism: A review. European Child and Adoles-
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmark, I., cent Psychiatry, 2, 61–74.
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American of social interactions and associated abnor-
Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1361–1367. malities in children: Epidemiology and classi-
Volkmar, F. R., & Nelson, D. S. (1990). Seizure dis- fication. Journal of Autism and Developmental
orders in autism. Journal of the American Disorders, 9(1), 11–29.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1, Wing, L., Yeates, S. R., Brierly, L. M., & Gould, J.
127–129. (1976). The prevalence of early childhood
Webb, E., Lobo, S., Hervas, A., Scourfield, J., & autism: Comparison of administrative and epi-
Fraser, W. I. (1997). The changing prevalence demiological studies. Psychological Medicine,
of autistic disorder in a Welsh health district. 6, 89–100.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Rice, C., Karapurkar, T.,
39, 150–152. Doernberg, N., Boyle, C., & Murphy, C.
Webb, E., Morey, J., Thompsen, W., Butler, C., Bar- (2003). Prevalence of autism in a U.S. metro-
ber, M., & Fraser, W. I. (2003). Prevalence of politan area. Journal of the American Medical
autistic spectrum disorder in children attend- Association, 289(1), 49–55.
ing mainstream schools in a Welsh education
CHAPTER 3

Childhood Disintegrative Disorder

FRED R. VOLKMAR, KATHY KOENIG, AND MATTHEW STATE

Nearly 100 years ago, Theodore Heller, a Vi- recognized. Debate initially centered on issues
ennese educator, reported on six children who of continuity with schizophrenia and, more re-
had exhibited severe developmental regression cently, with autism. The early confusion with
at ages 3 to 4 years following a period of ap- schizophrenia reflected the general presump-
parently normal development. After the re- tion that, more or less, all severe psychiatric
gression, recovery was quite limited. Initially, disturbances reflected psychosis that was
Heller (1908) termed this condition dementia equated with schizophrenia (see Chapter 1,
infantilis; subsequently, other terms have been this Handbook, this volume, for a discussion).
used for the concept, for example, Heller’s Only as various lines of evidence began to sug-
syndrome, disintegrative psychosis, and, more gest the importance of making distinctions
recently, childhood disintegrative disorder was it clear, for example, that autism differed
(CDD). Over the past century, more than 100 from schizophrenia of childhood in a host of
cases have been reported in the world litera- ways. The pioneering studies of Kolvin (1971)
ture; not surprisingly, information on the and Rutter (1972) were particularly important
condition is much more limited than that in this regard since they demonstrated that
available on autism. In this chapter, we review within a large group of “psychotic” children,
the development of the diagnostic concept, there was a bimodal pattern of onset. The
current definitions, information on clinical early-onset group had begun to have troubles
features, epidemiology, course, and validity at birth or within a year or so after birth while
of the condition. We also note areas of contro- the late-onset group developed apparently nor-
versy, and issues that remain to be clarified mally for many years. Clinical features of the
are reviewed. early-onset group included marked impair-
ments in social, cognitive, and language devel-
DEVELOPMENT OF THE DIAGNOSTIC opment similar to those described by Kanner
CONCEPT for autism, whereas the late-onset group ex-
hibited delusions, hallucinations, and other
Although only recently accorded official features more similar to schizophrenia. There
recognition in DSM-IV as a diagnostic con- was no higher than expected frequency of
cept, CDD has a long history. schizophrenia among family members of the
Heller’s work occurred shortly after the early-onset group, but there was such an in-
turn of this century, but many decades were to crease among first-degree relatives in the
pass before his diagnostic concept was widely late-onset group. This observation has been

The authors acknowledge the support of NICHD grant 5P01-HD-03008-28. The case report is reprinted
with permission from Psychoses and Pervasive Developmental Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence, Fred
R. Volkmar, MD, editor, American Psychiatric Association Press, 1996. The authors are grateful to Drs.
Kurita, Malhotra, Gillberg, and Deonna for their kind provision of additional information.

70
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 71

well replicated (e.g., Makita, 1966; Volkmar, meant that historical information, for example,
Cohen, Hoshino, Rende, & Paul, 1988). It is on the pattern and time of onset, was not par-
also of interest that in these studies a handful ticularly relevant to the diagnosis of autism.
of cases did not seem to fall so simply either Thus, children who would have been recog-
into the early- or late-onset group, for exam- nized as having disintegrative psychosis in
ple, 3 of the 83 cases in Kolvin’s series exhib- ICD-9 would usually have been said to have
ited an intermediate age of onset between the autistic disorder in DSM-III-R. This state of
autistic and schizophrenic groups. affairs became even more complex as the
These and other data on the validity of drafts of the ICD-10 revision began to appear.
autism led to its official recognition in DSM-III
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980), Childhood Disintegrative Disorder in ICD-
where it was placed in a new class of disorder, 10 and DSM-IV
pervasive developmental disorder (PDD; see
Chapter 1, this Handbook, this volume). Infan- In contrast to DSM-III-R, ICD-10 included a
tile autism was defined on the basis of marked draft definition of CDD. This definition was
social, language, and other problems arising by largely consistent with earlier work (e.g.,
30 months of age. Partly in recognition of the Heller, 1930; Zappert, 1921), which had
fact that a few children seemed to develop an generally suggested the following diagnostic
autistic-like condition after that time, DSM- features:
III also included a category, childhood onset
pervasive developmental disorder (COPDD). 1. A distinctive pattern of syndrome onset (a
This category was not meant to be analogous to period of several years of normal develop-
Heller’s diagnostic concept; the implicit ment before a marked deterioration)
presumption in DSM-III was that such cases 2. Progressive deterioration (either gradual or
invariably reflected some progressive neu- abrupt) once the syndrome had its onset
ropathological process. However, the ninth re- with loss of skills in multiple areas
vision of the International Classification of 3. Behavioral and affective symptoms
Diseases (ICD-9; World Health Organization 4. An absence of features of gross neurologi-
[WHO], 1978) had included a category for dis- cal dysfunction
integrative psychosis or Heller’s syndrome de-
fined on the basis of “normal or near normal Draft ICD-10 criteria for the condition in-
development in the first years of life, followed cluded apparently normal development for at
by a loss of social skills and of speech together least 2 years with age-appropriate social, com-
with a severe disorder of emotion, behavior, municative, and other skills; a definite loss of
and relationships.” ICD-9 did not, however, skills in more than one area; development of
prove as influential as DSM-III, primarily be- problems in social interaction, communication,
cause the latter system included explicit guide- and restricted patterns of interest or behavior
lines for diagnosis. of the type observed in autism; and a loss of
Although a very detailed definition of interest in the environment. By definition, the
COPDD was included in DSM-III, it quickly disorder could not coexist with autism or any
became clear that this diagnostic concept was other explicitly defined PDD, schizophrenia,
problematic in many respects (Volkmar, 1987), elective mutism, or the syndrome of acquired
and only a handful of reports appeared in the aphasia with epilepsy.
literature (Burd, Fisher, & Kerbeshian, 1988). Using the draft ICD-10 criteria, Volkmar
When DSM-III was revised (DSM-III-R, Amer- and Cohen (1989) identified a series of 10
ican Psychiatric Association, 1987), the cases of apparent CDD from within a larger
COPDD concept was dropped. Diagnostic cri- sample of individuals with the clinical features
teria for autism were expanded in number and of autism. Characteristics of the CDD were
conceptually, and early onset of autism was then contrasted to cases of autism that had
not an essential diagnostic feature; autistic been identified before or after 24 months. This
disorder was the only operationally defined comparison was particularly appropriate to
disorder within the PDD class. These changes the issue of whether CDD simply represented
72 Diagnosis and Classification

late-onset autism since differences between somewhat less detailed and less truly opera-
the groups on some external measure(s) tionalized. In addition, ICD-10 includes loss of
would tend to support the validity of CDD interest in the environment as a diagnostic cri-
apart from late-onset autism. This was indeed terion and is more explicit in indicating that
the case. Cases with late-onset autism tended the actual behavioral criteria for autism must
to be higher functioning while the CDD cases be met. As a practical matter, it would appear
were more likely to be mute, more likely to be that the diagnosis should probably not be made
in residential placement, and so forth. Thus, if the actual behavioral criteria for autism are
it appeared that CDD was not simply late- not met. In both DSM-IV and ICD-10, the age
onset autism; rather, it appeared to have dis- and pattern of onset are particularly important
tinctive features, clinical course, and even for the definition of the condition; that is, there
worse outcome. must be a marked regression after a period of
The inclusion of CDD in ICD-10 repre- prolonged normal development—arbitrarily set
sented a marked divergence from DSM-III-R at 2 years of age. This regression is associated
and clearly had implications for the definition with the acquisition of behaviors commonly
of autism in DSM-IV (American Psychiatric seen in autism. It is hoped that increased
Association, 1994). As part of the DSM-IV re- awareness of the condition will stimulate
vision process, a review (Volkmar, 1992) of greater identification of cases and more re-
CDD identified 77 cases in the world litera- search and that increasingly better guidelines
tures and suggested that while the condition for diagnosis will be developed (Kurita, 1989).
was apparently relatively rare, it seemed to
merit inclusion in DSM-IV because it appeared CLINICAL FEATURES
to differ from autism in important respects
and because it was not (as had previously been The following section provides a discussion of
assumed) always, or even usually, associated the essential clinical features of CDD.
with an identifiable neurological condition
that might account for the deterioration. Onset of Childhood Disintegrative
Inclusion of CDD in DSM-IV was also sug- Disorder
gested by the results of the DSM-IV field trial
for autism and related conditions (Volkmar As noted previously, the onset of CDD is
et al., 1994; Volkmar & Rutter, 1995). Al- highly distinctive and an essential diagnostic
though the field trial was primarily concerned feature.
with the development and validation of the
Age of Onset
DSM-IV diagnosis of autism, 16 cases of CDD
that had been previously evaluated at partici- Development prior to the regression is rela-
pating centers were included. Of even more in- tively prolonged (several years) and should be
terest, an additional 15 cases that met ICD-10 reasonably normal; for example, the child has
criteria for CDD were identified in the field the capacity to speak in sentences by age 2
trial. In these cases, the clinician had not given (WHO, 1990). Heller’s (1930) impression was
CDD as the clinical diagnosis but had noted that onset was often between ages 3 and 5
the presence of various diagnostic features of years, and this range continues to be the case.
the condition. This is not surprising since, par- Volkmar (1992) reported a mean age at onset
ticularly in the United States, clinicians have of 3.4 years in his review of 77 reported cases.
been much less familiar with the diagnostic The issue of the time of onset is particularly
concept. Relative to cases with autism, in the relevant to distinctions of CDD from autism
field trial, cases with CDD were more likely to because it is clear that, in some cases, autism
be mute and had greater degrees of associated is recognized after 24 months of age (although
mental handicap. almost invariably before age 3; Volkmar, Stier,
As noted by Volkmar and Rutter (1995), the & Cohen, 1985). For example, in the case se-
DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for the condition ries collected at Division TEACCH in North
are conceptually very similar. However, for Carolina, over three-fourths of children with
the sake of brevity, the DSM-IV system is autism had been identified by their parents as
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 73

having difficulties by 2 years of age (Short & rized in Figure 3.1. There is a clear and signif-
Schopler, 1988). Diagnosis of autism is also icant difference in the two distributions of
sometimes delayed by the primary clinicians’ onset of the two conditions.
lack of familiarity with the condition (Siegel,
Characteristics of Onset
Pliner, Eschler, & Elliott, 1989).
Cases with late-onset autism (i.e., whose Several different patterns of onset of CDD
difficulties are apparent after age 2 but before have been observed. Occasionally, the condi-
age 3) tend to be higher functioning, and it tion has a relatively abrupt onset (days to
seems likely that case detection may be de- weeks) but sometimes develops more gradually
layed by the relative preservation of cognitive (weeks to months). There may be a premoni-
abilities (Volkmar & Cohen, 1989). Wohlge- tory phase prior to the marked deterioration;
muth, Kiln, Cohen, and Volkmar (1994) com- during this time, the child may be nonspecifi-
pared aspects of deterioration in autism and cally agitated, anxious, or dysphoric.
CDD. In their report, when deterioration in In several case series, the onset of CDD
autism was reported, it typically involved the has been noted to be associated with some
loss of ability to speak in single words or the psychosocial stress or medical event (Evans-
failure of this ability to progress. In contrast, Jones & Rosenbloom, 1978; Kobayashi & Mu-
in CDD the previously acquired level of lan- rata, 1998; Kurita, 1988; Volkmar, 1992).
guage was much higher, and deterioration was However, the significance of stressful events
always observed in multiple areas, that is, not in syndrome pathogenesis is unclear. The
simply limited to speech. group of stressors reported has been diverse,
Occasional ambiguities are sometimes ob- but all share the feature of being relatively
served, for example, a child with recurrent ear common to preschool children, for example,
infections and delayed speech who then goes birth of a sibling or death of a grandparent,
on to develop a more typical CDD presentation hospitalization for elective surgery, or immu-
at age 3. Kurita (1988) has suggested that nizations. It seems unlikely that such associa-
early development, that is, prior to age 2, may tions have etiological significance (Rutter,
not always be perfectly normal, and there 1985). Davidovitch, Glick, Holtzman, Tirosh,
might be a history of mild delay (see also and Safir (2000) noted that this phenomenon,
Kurita, Kita, & Miyake, 1992). Despite cur- that is, of attributing some etiological signifi-
rent diagnostic criteria, some cases of CDD cance to an associated medical or psychoso-
might develop before age 2 although the diag- cial event, is common in parents of children
nosis of such cases is problematic and may be with autism who regress as compared to those
the source of some confusion, that is, both
with autism and with Rett disorder although
the regression in Rett’s is usually relatively 50
CDD
early in life. Similarly, in Landau-Kleffner
Percent of Group

syndrome (of acquired aphasia with epilepsy), 40 Autism


there may be occasional confusion with CDD
or autism, but it appears that the clinical 30
features of this condition and its course are
relatively distinctive (Bishop, 1985, 1994). 20
Further complicating the issue is the problem
of potential early regression in autism, which 10
is variably reported in 20% to 40% of cases
(see Lainhart et al., 2002; Rogers & DiLalla,
1990; Siperstein & Volkmar, 2004; Volkmar 0
<1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
et al., 1985, for a discussion). We return to the
Year of Life
issue of differential diagnosis and current con-
troversies later in this chapter. Data related to Figure 3.1 Age of onset in 160 cases with clinical
age of onset of autism and CDD as abstracted diagnoses of CDD and 316 cases with clinical diag-
from the DSM-IV field trial data are summa- noses of autism.
74 Diagnosis and Classification

who do not. Kobayashi and Murata (1998) re- Given that the child typically has been
ported similar results in their study of chil- speaking in full sentences, often quite well,
dren with setback autism. the development of either total mutism or
marked deterioration in verbal language is also
BEHAVIORAL AND CLINICAL very striking and frequent in CDD. Even for
FEATURES those individuals who subsequently regain
speech, it does not typically return to previous
Table 3.1 provides a summary of clinical fea- levels of communicative ability. Rather, com-
tures in a number of reported cases as well as municative abilities are more similar to those
several cases seen by the authors and not previ- observed in autism with a sparsity of commu-
ously reported. nicative acts, limited expressive vocabulary,
Once CDD is established, it resembles and markedly impaired pragmatic skills.
autism in its phenomenological manifesta- Unusual behaviors including stereotyped
tions. Typically, social skills are markedly behaviors, problems with transitions and
impaired. There is, however, some suggestion change, and nonspecific overactivity are typi-
that the degree of impairment may be slightly cally observed (Malhotra & Singh, 1993). As
less than that observed in autism (Kanner, noted previously, various affective responses
1973; Kurita, 1988; Kurita et al., 1992). Par- that appear inexplicable are often observed at
ents usually report that the loss of social in- the time of syndrome onset. As mentioned pre-
teraction skill is dramatic and of great viously, ICD-10 suggests that a general loss of
concern to them. interest in the environment is also usual. Dete-

TABLE 3.1 Characteristics of Disintegrative Disorder Cases


Cases
1908–1975 1977–1995 1996–2004
Variable N = 48 N = 58 N = 67
Male/ Female
Sex ratio 35/12 49/9 53/14

X SD
Age at onset (years) 3.42 1.12 3.32 1.42 3.21 0.97

X SD
Age at follow-up 8.67 4.14 10.88 5.98 10.25 4.81

Symptoms % of N Cases
Speech deterioration / loss 100 47 100 58 100 54
Social disturbance 100 43 98 57 100 54
Stereotypy/resistance to change 100 38 85 54 68 54
Overactivity 100 42 77 37 59 54
Affective symptoms/anxiety 100 17 78 38 55 54
Deterioration self-help skills 94 33 82 49 66 54
Source: Adapted with permission from “Childhood Disintegrative Disorders: Is-
sues for DSM-IV,” by F. R. Volkmar, 1992, Journal of Autistic Developmental Dis-
orders 22, 625–642; and “Childhood Disintegrative Disorder,” by F. R. Volkmar,
A. Klin, W. D. Marans, & D. J. Cohen, in Autism and Pervasive Developmental Dis-
orders, second edition, 1997, New York: Wiley. Additional cases based on case se-
ries reported by Kurita et al., 1994; “Childhood Disintegrative Disorder:
Re-Examination of the Current Concept,” by S. Malhotra and N. Gupta, 2002, Eu-
ropean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(3), pp. 108–114; Mourdisen et al.,
2000, with additional cases supplied by C. Gillberg and F. R. Volkmar. Note: Re-
sults based on available data.
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 75

rioration in self-help skills, notably in toileting of CDD cases, the child’s behavior and devel-
skills, is striking (Kurita, 1988; Volkmar, opment deteriorate to the much lower level of
1992) and in contrast to autism where such functioning and remain there. On the one
skills are often acquired somewhat late but are hand, no further deterioration occurs, but sub-
not typically dramatically lost. sequent developmental gains appear to be min-
imal (Volkmar & Cohen, 1989). On the other
EPIDEMIOLOGY hand, the marked developmental regression
seems to be followed by a limited recovery;
Epidemiological data on this condition are for example, a child regains the capacity to
limited. This reflects both (1) the true relative speak although usually only in a limited way
and frequency of the condition apart from (Volkmar & Cohen, 1989). Burd, Ivey, Barth,
autism and (2) the likelihood that cases have and Kerbeshian (1998) provided follow-up
been markedly underdiagnosed. In his review data on two children with CDD after 14 years;
(see Chapter 2, this volume, 2002), Fombonne at the time of follow-up, both were severely
notes that estimates range from about 1 to 9 impaired, exhibited seizure disorder, were
cases per 100,000 children. Data from case se- nonverbal, and were in residential treatment.
ries (e.g., the series of consecutive cases re- With greater awareness of the condition, more
ported by Volkmar & Cohen, 1989) indicate cases are being seen at younger ages and even-
that the disorder was one-tenth as common as tual longer term follow-up of these cases will
autism; however, these data were not based on be needed.
a truly epidemiological sample. It is interest- In a small number of cases, the develop-
ing that a rather similar rate was observed of mental deterioration is progressive and does
children with autism who had regressed after not plateau. This appears to be likely if some
age 3 (Rogers & DiLalla, 1990). A different identifiable, neuropathological process can be
study using a somewhat more epidemiologi- identified. If the process is progressive, death
cally based sample suggested a prevalence rate may be the eventual result (Corbett, 1987),
of 1 in 100,000 (Burd, Fisher, & Kerbeshian, and there may be increased mortality if other
1989). It must be emphasized that the relative medical conditions are present. For example,
lack of familiarity of clinicians with this con- two cases reported by Mouridsen, Rich, and
cept makes interpretation of the available data Isager (1998) died in association with suba-
somewhat suspect. cute sclerosing panencephalitis (death at age 9
Initially, it appeared that the condition years) and with tuberous sclerosis (death at
was equally affecting males and females. age 31 years). One case with whom the authors
However, more recent studies have noted a are familiar died in adolescence apparently
preponderance of males similar to that seen in following a seizure. In a handful of cases, the
autism (Lord, Schopler, & Revicki, 1982). It child has been observed to make a noteworthy
also is possible that some cases of Rett syn- recovery.
drome were originally misdiagnosed as having
Heller’s syndrome; that is, Rett’s condition NEUROBIOLOGICAL FINDINGS
was described only in 1966, and the degree of AND ETIOLOGY
the deterioration in Rett’s cases may be sug-
gestive of CDD (Burd et al., 1989; Hill & Although CDD was originally termed demen-
Rosenbloom, 1986; Millichap, 1987; Rett, tia infantilis, Heller’s impression, and that of
1966; Volkmar, 1992). In cases of CDD ob- others, was that CDD was not associated with
served in the past 20 years, there is a high apparent organic disease; this was also origi-
male predominance. nally Kanner’s impression (1943) about
autism. In both cases, this initial impression
COURSE AND PROGNOSIS has had to be modified. It is now clear that
about 25% of individuals with autism have
Information on course and outcome is an im- seizures, often with an onset later than is typ-
portant factor for evaluating the validity of ical in children, and another 25% have vari-
psychiatric conditions. In approximately 75% ous other EEG abnormalities. In the Volkmar
76 Diagnosis and Classification

(1992) review, EEGs had been obtained in In Volkmar’s (1992) review of published
45 cases of apparent CDD. These data are sup- cases, specific neuropathological conditions
plemented by additional, more recent reports of were only occasionally identified. Late onset,
CDD cases (Malhotra & Singh, 1993; Mourid- for example, after age 6, of CDD appears more
sen, Rich, & Isager, 2000; Volkmar & Rutter, likely to be associated with some specific
1995). Seizures have been noted in various case neuropathological process. Given the child’s
reports, for example, Hill and Rosenbloom marked regression, it is now typical for parents
(1986; 2 of 9 cases), Volkmar and Cohen (1989; to consult with many different specialists and
2 of 10 cases), Malhotra and Singh (1993; 1 for various tests, laboratory studies, and diag-
case), and Kurita, Osada, & Miyake (2004; 3 of nostic procedures to be obtained. Other than
10 cases). In the Malhotra and Singh report EEG abnormalities and occasional seizure dis-
(1993), the onset of developmental deteriora- order, such tests usually are not particularly
tion was associated with seizures. Tuchman and productive although they should be under-
Rapin (1997) reported that regression in chil- taken. However, usually even when very exten-
dren with autism spectrum disorder occurred sive medical investigations are undertaken, it
equally in individuals with and without is not possible to identify a specific general
seizures. Similarly, Shinnar and colleagues medical condition that accounts for the child’s
(2001) reported relatively high rates of seizures deterioration (Volkmar, 1992). Even if such an
and autism spectrum disorder in a large cohort etiology can be identified, the diagnosis of
of children with language regression. Although CDD is made and the presence of the associ-
these data are limited, the rates of seizure dis- ated medical condition noted. This is similar
order and EEG abnormality appear to be simi- to the approach in autism that may be associ-
lar to those observed in autism (e.g., Deykin & ated with various general medical conditions
MacMahon, 1979; Rutter, 1985; Volkmar & (Rutter, Bayley, Boulton, & Le Couter, 1994).
Nelson, 1990) and suggest that an EEG is rou- Except for the EEG information and one
tinely indicated as part of clinical assessment. study by Gillberg, Terenius, Hagberg, Witt-
CDD has been associated with various Engerstrom, and Eriksson (1990) on cere-
conditions such as tuberous sclerosis, neu- brospinal fluid (CSF) beta-endorphins, there is
rolipidoses, metachromatic leukodystrophy, a general absence of information regarding the
Addison-Schilder’s disease, and subacute scle- neurochemistry, neuropsychology, neurophysi-
rosing panencephalitis along with literally ology, or neuroanatomy of CDD. Although the
hundreds of other possible causes including small sample sizes and differences in method
metabolic, infectious, genetic, immunopathic, complicate the interpretation of the limited
environmental, and epileptogenic causes (see available data, in general, striking differences
Dyken & Krawiecki, 1983; Mouridsen et al., in brain morphology and structure have not
1998). As noted previously, the impression, for been observed (Mouridsen et al., 2000). Simi-
example, in DSM-III and DSM-III-R was that larly, information on potential genetic factors
such associated conditions are generally found, is extremely limited. Mouridsen et al. (2000)
but this view has not been supported by the reported one case of a boy with an inversion of
data. Selected disorders associated with loss of chromosome 10 (46xy, inv(10) (p11, 21q21.2),
developmental skills are listed in Table 3.2. but the child’s mother also had the same anom-

TABLE 3.2 Selected Disorders Associated with Loss of Developmental Skills


Infections (HIV, measles, CMV) Mitochondrial defecits (e.g., Leigh disease)
Hypothyroidism Subacute sclerosing panencephalitis
Neurolipidosis Metachromatic Leukodystrophy
Addison-Schilder disease Seizures
Angleman syndrome Gangliosidoses
Lipofuscinosis Aminoacidopathies (e.g., PKU)
Source: For an exhaustive list, see “Neurodegenerative Diseases of Infancy and
Childhood,” by P. Dyken and N. Krawiecki, 1983, “Neurodegenerative Diseases of In-
fancy and Childhood, Annals of Neurology, 13, 351–364.
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 77

aly and was a successful professional. In other disorders, Down syndrome, Prader-Willi
reports, no unusual genetic findings have been syndrome, and velocardiofacial syndrome are
noted (e.g., Burd et al., 1998; Russo, Perry, familiar examples of chromosomal disorders
Kolodny, & Gillberg, 1996). This is particu- that are demonstrably genetic in etiology, but
larly unfortunate because there is some reason rarely pass through generations within a family.
to think that possibly the etiology (or etiolo- It is conceivable that CDD, as well, could be the
gies) in CDD may be somewhat more homoge- result of rare sporadic chromosomal rearrange-
neous than those more typically seen in ments. The fact that gross chromosomal abnor-
autism, that is, given the pattern of onset and malities have so far not been found in cases of
the relative infrequency of such cases. CDD does not rule out such a mechanism. The
resolution of routine clinical cytogenetic exams
GENETICS OF CHILDHOOD in instances where a specific gene or locus is
DISINTEGRATIVE DISORDER not known is on the order of 4 million base
pairs of DNA. Any abnormality that involved
Given the relevance of genes for other PDDs, it substantially less genetic material than this
is tempting to speculate about their role in this could be missed, even in the face of the exten-
syndrome as well. Despite the paucity of direct sive clinical work-ups that usually accompany
evidence from population genetic or molecular the diagnosis of developmental regression.
studies, even the limited epidemiological and In addition, chromosomal abnormalities are
case report data do provide some basis for de- certainly not the only form of sporadic muta-
veloping and testing hypotheses about disease tion. The most pertinent recent example can be
etiology. In addition, the recent identification found in Rett’s disorder. Mutations of single
of Methyl CpG Binding Protein 2 (MECP2) as molecules in the DNA chain encoding the
the cause of a majority of cases of classic MECP2 gene account for about 80% of cases
Rett’s disorder (Amir et al., 1999) may turn presenting with the classic phenotype (see
out to provide important clues about promising Amir & Zoghbi, 2000, for review). These spo-
approaches to future genetic studies of CDD. radic cases are most often the result of germ-
The literature reviewed in this chapter sug- line mutations in males (Girard et al., 2001). It
gests that the prevalence of CDD is approxi- is certainly possible that an analogous, rarely
mately 1 per 100,000 live births. In addition, occurring mutation in ova or sperm could ac-
case examples of subjects who have any family count for CDD.
members with a PDD of any kind are unusual The absence of reports of multigeneration
(Zwaigenbaum et al., 2000). Indeed, CDD ap- CDD families suggests that it is unlikely that
pears in the majority of reported instances to the syndrome is inherited in dominant fash-
be “sporadic”; that is, it seems to arise “out of ion. Such disorders are transferred from one
nowhere” within a particular family (Malhotra generation to the next, with only one copy of
& Gupta, 2002). These observations that CDD a defective gene necessary to cause the condi-
is rare and does not often cluster in families tion. While not everyone who inherits such a
must be taken into account in formulating hy- dominant gene develops the full-blown disor-
potheses about the role genes could conceiv- der, nonetheless, we would expect to see suc-
ably play in the syndrome. Furthermore, cessive generations in a family having some
Lainhart and colleagues (2002) reported that evidence of the syndrome. To date, no such
rates of the “ broader autism phenotype” did families have been reported. Alternative
not differ in parents of children with autism forms of Mendelian inheritance might better
with and without regression, suggesting no in- fit the available data but are not entirely satis-
crease in the genetic liability—at least in the factory explanations either. Recessive disor-
autism phenotype characterized by regression. ders, in which one defective gene must be
Though the terms familial and genetic are inherited from each of two parents, can often
often used interchangeably, they are not syn- appear to be sporadic, particularly if the of-
onymous. There are multiple mechanisms that fending versions of a gene are found rarely in
can result in the de novo appearance of genetic a population. In this circumstance, multiple
syndromes. In the realm of neuropsychiatric generations in a family would be unaffected
78 Diagnosis and Classification

until the chance mating of two mutation tively homogeneous clinical entity confined
carriers. If this were the case, however, the to girls appears to have been mistaken. In
risk of having the disorder in a sibling of a reality, the phenotype can vary dramatically
child with CDD would be on the order of 25%. in females from the classic syndrome
The fact that cases of CDD are not commonly described in DSM-IV to mild learning disabil-
reported among first-degree relatives would ities. Moreover, a wide variety of phenotypes
argue against this hypothesis but would not have now also been identified in males (Amir
rule it out. A paucity of familial cases could & Zoghbi, 2000; Couvert et al., 2001; Ham-
also result if parents who have one child with mer, Dorrani, Dragich, Kudo, & Schanen,
CDD tend to stop having additional children. 2002; Kleefstra et al., 2002; Meloni et al.,
A similar explanation would have to be in- 2000). The current understanding of CDD
voked if it was hypothesized that CDD was a could similarly turn out to be incomplete
rare X-linked disorder. The observed male when and if a genetic contribution is identi-
preponderance of 41 (Malhotra & Gupta, fied, either because the manifestations of a
2002; Volkmar, 1992; Volkmar & Rutter, 1995) CDD mutation could be quite variable or be-
could be accounted for if girls were provided a cause there could be gender-related difference
measure of protection by a second X chromo- in how such an abnormality is expressed.
some. Again, however, the recurrence risk for Recent studies of Rett’s disorder have also
male offspring in an affected family would be called into question the notion of homogeneity
on the order of 50%, far higher than what is from a genetic as well as clinical perspective.
observed in the available literature. At present, only about 80% of patients with the
Data that are directly relevant to this issue classic Rett’s disorder phenotype have been
are limited. In their study of the broader autis- confirmed to have a MECP2 mutation. Familial
tic phenotype, Lainhart and colleagues (2002) or atypical cases have proven even more diffi-
reported that parents of probands with regres- cult to characterize. These findings could be
sion and nonregressive autism exhibited simi- the result of an incomplete understanding of the
lar rates of the broader autism phenotype. genomic structure and regulation of MECP2.
Zwaigenbaum and colleagues (2000) reported However, it may be that there are other Rett’s
on two half brothers, one with autism and the disorder loci apart from MECP2. The same
other with CDD. could well apply to CDD. More than one genetic
Finally, in the absence of any other com- abnormality might result in similar clinical pre-
pelling explanation, it could be hypothesized sentations, so any single mechanism might
that CDD is a complex disorder resulting from prove an unsatisfactory solution to the problem
either the chance accumulation of a number of of understanding the transmission of CDD.
rare genetic events, from some unknown envi- Finally, the identification of MECP2 as a
ronmental precipitant that alone or in concert major cause of developmental regression in
with a genetic liability results in the appar- girls came about in large part because of the
ently sporadic emergence of the syndrome, or identification of unusual families among the
from a novel genetic mechanism. much more common sporadic cases of Rett’s
disorder (Amir et al., 1999). The search for
Lessons from Rett Syndrome “outlier ” examples of an already unusual dis-
order could prove to be a similarly profitable
Dramatic advances in the understanding of strategy for studying the genetics of CDD.
the genetic basis of Rett’s disorder may turn
out to have considerable relevance for the Future Genetic Studies of Childhood
study of CDD. While preliminary screenings Disintegrative Disorder
of the MECP2 gene in CDD cases have not
identified causative mutations (M. State and J. The combination of being a rare disorder and
Greally, personal communication, 2004), more the absence of clear familial aggregation of
general lessons may be derived from the study CDD cases certainly presents obstacles to the
of Rett’s disorder. For instance, the strongly study of the genetics of the disorder. Nonethe-
held notion that Rett’s disorder was a rela- less, rapid advances in genetic technologies
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 79

hold promise for the study of CDD. The advent VALIDITY OF CHILDHOOD
of high-resolution chromosomal screening DISINTEGRATIVE DISORDER AS
techniques, the development of extremely sen- A DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORY
sitive proteomic technologies, and the expo-
nential growth in the ability to screen the Diagnostic categories exist for different rea-
genome for mutations will allow investigators sons and purposes (see Rutter, 2003; Rutter
to test hypotheses about the transmission of & Taylor, 2002, for discussion). There are un-
this disorder. As importantly, more concerted derstandable and legitimate tensions between
clinical and epidemiological research can help those who wish to have broader diagnostic
further refine our understanding of the nature categories—for example, to ensure service
of this disorder and provide a more solid foun- provision—and those who advocate for more
dation for future investigations of the potential narrowly defined categories—for example,
role that genes play in the etiology of CDD. for research purposes (see Chapter 1, this
volume). With regard to CDD, these issues
Other Neurobiological Factors and tensions also exist. The validity of the
category has been the object of debate. In this
Despite extensive medical evaluations, the yield section, we summarize some of the informa-
in terms of discovering clearly potentially tion on validity of the condition with respect
causative conditions for the developmental re- to autism and other disorders.
gression is surprisingly low. Russo and col-
leagues (1996) have speculated that a Childhood Disintegrative Disorder
combination of genetic factors and environmen- and Autism
tal stress might be involved and, further, that a
specific immune response might be involved. Although Kanner’s original (1943) impression
Shinnar and colleagues (2001) similarly specu- was that autism was congenital, subsequent
late that the widespread parental report of asso- work has consistently shown that in a minority
ciated stressful events with the onset of the of cases parents report some period of months,
condition might have some etiological signifi- or even a year or more, of normal development
cance. Supporting data for such hypotheses are before symptoms of autism are recognized
limited. In general, the recent presumption has (Kolvin, 1971; Kurita, 1985; Short & Schopler,
been that the stresses reported (e.g., birth of a 1988; Siperstein & Volkmar, 2004; Volkmar
sibling) are not highly unusual for children in et al., 1985, 1988). The cases, sometimes re-
this age group. ferred to as late-onset autism or, particularly
While the cause or causes of CDD remain in the Japanese literature, as setback autism,
unknown, the course of the disorder, the asso- have not, unfortunately, been commonly stud-
ciation with EEG abnormalities and seizure ied, and there is some potential for confusion
disorder, and the occasional association with (or even for overlap) with CDD. That is, al-
known medical conditions suggest that neuro- though, by definition, CDD has its onset after
biological factors are likely central in patho- age 2, with normal previous development, it is
genesis. As with autism, it is possible that possible that this is an artificial distinction,
multiple pathogenic pathways may act to and some cases of onset earlier than age 2 have
produce the condition, and the absence of indeed been reported in the literature (see
clearly identified neuropathological mecha- Figure 3.1). Several other issues further com-
nisms likely reflects more on current research plicate this problem. What is reported as “re-
techniques than the absence of such factors gression” often seems to be more adequately
(Rivinus, Jamison, & Graham, 1975; Wilson, characterized as developmental stagnation; for
1974). Despite the progress in our understand- example, the child says one or two words and
ing of the central nervous system, much re- then speech and subsequent development fail
mains to be discovered. The absence of to progress normally. This state of affairs is
specific neurobiological mechanisms is proba- rather different from that reported in prototyp-
bly more a reflection on the present state of ical development of CDD where a marked loss
science than the absence itself. of functioning in multiple areas occurs. In a
80 Diagnosis and Classification

recent study, Siperstein and Volkmar (2004) any associated neurological or other general
noted that language loss and social skills loss medical condition is clearly indicated in CDD
are often prominent in autism when parents re- although, somewhat surprisingly, even exten-
port loss of skills. Their report also notes that, sive evaluations often fail to reveal an associ-
when regression is strictly defined (e.g., based ated medical condition. In their study of
not only on parental report of regression but dementia using a large cohort of cases in Aus-
also of developmental milestones), demonstra- tralia, Nunn, Williams, and Ouvrier (2002)
ble regression was relatively uncommon. In estimated the prevalence of dementia in child-
their follow-up study of nearly 200 cases of hood to be 5.6 per 100,000 cases. Of the chil-
autism who had lost skills, Kobayashi and Mu- dren identified, in 21% of cases a specific
rata (1998) reported that there were higher etiology could not be determined; they suggest
rates of seizure disorder and lower language the general utility of a broad conceptualiza-
levels associated with reported language set- tion of childhood dementia.
back. Davidovitch and colleagues (2000) ex- In addition to the various metabolic and
amined differences between children with genetic disorders that can cause deteriora-
autism who were reported to have regressed tion, the development of an autistic-like clini-
and those who did not regress; they found little cal picture can follow central nervous system
difference between the groups. Language re- infection or other insult (Weir & Salisbury,
gression is not as commonly reported among 1980). Occasionally, epileptic conditions may
children with overall developmental delay also mimic autism or CDD (Deonna, Ziegler,
(Shinnar et al., 2001). Malin-Ingvar, Ansermet, & Roulet, 1995). This
In a recent study, Kurita et al. (2004) evalu- issue has been most controversial with respect
ated the clinical validity of CDD as defined by to the syndrome of acquired aphasia with
DSM-IV in 10 children with the condition who epilepsy (Landau-Kleffner syndrome) in which
were compared to a larger group of age- and aphasia develops in association with multifo-
gender-matched children with DSM-IV autis- cal spike and spike/wave discharges on EEG
tic disorder with speech loss. Compared to the (Beaumanoir, 1992). This syndrome is rela-
children with autism who had exhibited tively well described but remains poorly un-
speech loss, those with CDD exhibited higher derstood, and, in general, the impression is of
levels of anxiety/dysphoria as well as higher an aphasia rather than a PDD (this condition is
levels of stereotyped behavior. The CDD included in ICD-10 as a developmental lan-
group also had a higher rate of seizure disor- guage disorder but is not recognized in DSM-
der, and there was a suggestion of differences IV). Nonverbal abilities tend to be spared, and
in the profiles on psychological testing. Al- there can be considerable recovery (Bishop,
though differences in levels of retardation 1985). In contrast to CDD where later onset of
were not observed, the authors pointed out that the disorder is usually associated with a worse
the relative youth of the group (mean age of prognosis, later onset in Landau-Kleffner is as-
8.2 years for the CDD cases) limited the value sociated with better outcome (Bishop, 1985).
of such a comparison. Other epileptic conditions may also mimic
autism or CDD (Deonna et al., 1995).
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder and There seems little disagreement that, once
Other Disorders established, CDD resembles autism in terms
of clinical features and course (Militerni,
As noted previously, another area of contro- Bravaccio, & D’Anuono, 1997; Volkmar &
versy has centered on the view of CDD as re- Cohen, 1989). Hendry (2000), however, has
flecting a neuropathological process. There is questioned the utility of CDD as a diagnostic
little disagreement that some brain-based mech- concept, noting the strong overlap in symp-
anism is responsible for the condition; most of toms with autism. Hendry rightly notes the
the debate has centered either on the idea that limitations of the available data, which are
CDD is commonly associated with one or more very small as compared, for example, to that
specific and identifiable other medical condi- on autism. She also raises several relevant
tions. As noted earlier, a thorough search for questions about the difficulties in reliance on
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 81

parental report and so forth. In contrast, Volk- type seen in CDD occurred. The use of mate-
mar and Rutter (1995), in their review of the rials such as baby books, home videotapes, or
results of CDD in the DSM-IV field trial, came movies may be helpful.
to a different conclusion, observing major dif- In CDD, it is essential that the early devel-
ferences both in the dramatic clinical presen- opment be unequivocally normal with the child
tation and course. As they noted, the major able to speak in sentences prior to the onset of
rationale for including CDD as a recognized the condition. If such a history cannot be docu-
diagnostic entity primarily related to its po- mented, a diagnosis of CDD should not be
tential for clarifying basic mechanisms of made; in such cases, a diagnosis of atypical
pathogenesis. autism may be most appropriate. There are
also the exceptions that tend to prove the rule,
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS for example, children who were being reared in
a bilingual context and whose language was
The differential diagnosis of CDD includes the slightly delayed but who otherwise appeared
other PDDs and other conditions. Because of to be developing appropriately prior to the
the very distinctive pattern of onset, historical onset of the regression.
information is critical in making the diagnosis.
Rett’s Disorder
Autism
Rett’s disorder (see Chapter 5, this volume) is
Once established, CDD shares the same essen- occasionally confused with CDD; this reflects
tial features on current clinical examination. the fact that some degree of regression is ob-
Thus, historical information is particularly served in both conditions and that the more
important in distinguishing the two conditions. autistic-like phase of Rett’s disorder may be
In about 75% of cases of autism, parents do not most prominent in the preschool years, that is,
report an unequivocally normal period of de- when diagnostic evaluations are first con-
velopment; usually social development is ducted. For individuals familiar with both con-
markedly delayed and deviant, and language ditions, misdiagnosis is relatively unlikely
fails to develop in expected ways. In a smaller given the very different histories and clinical
group of cases, the parents report either that features of the conditions.
the child seemed to develop normally, usually
up to about 18 to 24 months of age, or that sin- Asperger Syndrome
gle words developed but the child did not go on
to develop more complex speech; such cases In this condition, early language and cognitive
should not be diagnosed with CDD. It is not un- development may seem to have been normal or
common for the presenting complaint in autism near normal. Sometimes parents become aware
to center on language loss and then for a care- of the social and other difficulties as the child
ful history to establish earlier preexisting ab- enters a preschool program. This may, incor-
normalities, for example, in terms of unusual rectly, lead some care providers to suspect that
environmental sensitivities or responses (e.g., there was a marked regression in functioning.
Stein, Dixon, & Cowan, 2000). Complexities However, in Asperger, cognitive functions are
may arise if historical information is absent, relatively preserved, and the truly marked and
the report of a truly prolonged period of nor- severe regression of the kind seen in CDD is
mal development is questionable, the clinician absent.
fails to obtain an adequate history, or the
autistic child is somewhat higher functioning. Childhood Schizophrenia
In the last case, it is possible that early lan-
guage seems to develop in a near normal fash- In rare instances, there may be confusion of
ion, but the parent becomes concerned as more CDD with other psychiatric disorders such as
complex social-communicative difficulties be- schizophrenia. Although very early onset
come apparent. Careful history will usually re- schizophrenia (VERS) is very rare, the degree
veal that no actual period of regression of the of regression and deterioration may suggest
82 Diagnosis and Classification

CDD (see Werry, 1992). However, usually the EVALUATION AND MANAGEMENT
characteristic findings of schizophrenia on
clinical examination will clarify the diagnosis. Assessment of the child with CDD may be most
effectively accomplished by a group of profes-
Landau-Kleffner Syndrome sionals who work together as a team or in close
collaboration with one another. Referrals usu-
The syndrome of acquired aphasia with ally come from primary care providers al-
epilepsy (Landau-Kleffner syndrome) has its though occasionally mental health workers,
onset in children and is characterized by ac- educators, and others may question a diagnosis,
quired aphasia in association with multifocal for example, of autism, because of some fea-
spike and spike/wave discharges on EEG tures of the case that they know to be unusual.
(Beaumanoir, 1992). This syndrome is rela- Given the potential for multiple assessments, it
tively well described but remains poorly is important that the professionals involved in
understood (S. Wilson, Djukic, Shinnar, the child’s care work effectively to avoid frag-
Dharmani, & Rapin, 2003). Children with the mentation and duplication of effort. Various
Landau-Kleffner syndrome usually exhibit professionals may be involved, for example,
marked interest in communication using child psychiatrists, psychologists, speech
nonverbal modalities, and typically the re- pathologists, pediatric neurologists, occupa-
gression is largely confined to the area of lan- tional and physical therapists, and others. It is
guage. Nonverbal abilities tend to be spared, appropriate and desirable to engage parents ac-
and there is often considerable recovery tively in the assessment process.
(Bishop, 1985). In contrast to CDD where Given the unusual history of the onset of
later onset of the disorder is usually associ- CDD, a careful history is particularly critical.
ated with a worse prognosis, later onset in This history should include information re-
Landau-Kleffner is associated with better lated to the pregnancy and neonatal period,
outcome (Bishop, 1985). Other epileptic con- early developmental history, as well as med-
ditions may also mimic autism or CDD (De- ical and family history. As noted previously,
onna et al., 1995), and, as noted previously, a the examination of videotapes may clearly
history of language regression should prompt document the child’s early normal develop-
a careful assessment for possible seizure dis- ment. Information on the pattern and age of
order (Shinnar et al., 2001). onset of the condition is central.
Although extensive medical investigations
Other Associated Medical Conditions usually fail to reveal the presence of another
specific medical condition or specific neu-
As noted earlier, a thorough search for any as- rodegenerative disorder, a careful search for
sociated neurological or other general medical such conditions is indicated. This is particu-
condition is indicated in CDD. Specific find- larly true if aspects of the case are unusual; for
ings on examination or in the history may help example, if the onset is rather later (after age
guide the process of evaluation. If any such 6), or if the deterioration is progressive and
condition is identified, it is then specified does not plateau. Initial consultation with a
on Axis III. Disorders with onset after some pediatric neurologist is always indicated.
period of normal development include gan- Given the severity of these conditions, an EEG
gliosidosis, metachromatic leukodystrophy, and CT or MRI scan are also usually obtained.
Niemman-Pick disease, and so forth. Develop- Tests of communication and cognitive abil-
mental deterioration and the development of ity should be chosen with consideration of the
an autistic-like clinical picture can follow child’s current levels of functioning to obtain
central nervous system infection or other in- estimates of functioning useful in document-
sult (Weir & Salisbury, 1980). Rarely clini- ing subsequent developmental change as well
cally significant regression occur in the as for educational and rehabilitative program-
context of overt seizures (Tuchman & Rapin, ming. The use of developmental and other
1997; S. Wilson et al., 2003). tests typically given to younger children may
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 83

be appropriate, for example (Bayley, 1969; specific pharmacological treatments for CDD
Dunst, 1980; Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975). For although it is likely that many of the same
somewhat higher functioning but nonverbal agents used with some benefit in autism will
children, the Leiter International Perfor- sometimes also be helpful. The use of any
mance Scale (Leiter, 1948) may be useful. As pharmacological agent should include a careful
with children with autism, modifications in assessment of potential risks and benefits.
usual assessment procedures may have to be The families (parents, sibs, and extended
made. Communication scales that may be ap- family members) of patients should be sup-
propriate include the Receptive-Expressive ported. This includes provision of appropriate
Emergent Language Scale (REEL; Bzoch & information about the condition, helping fami-
League, 1971); the Sequenced Inventory lies make use of available local and other re-
of Communicative Development (SICD; sources, and helping family members receive
Hedrick, Prather, & Tobin, 1975); and the mutual support, for example, through support
Reynell Developmental Language Scales groups. Given that the prognosis of CDD ap-
(Reynell & Gruber, 1990). pears, in general, to be somewhat worse than
The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales that of autism, the stresses experienced by
(expanded form; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, parents and siblings may also be greater.
1984) should be administered to document lev-
els of adaptive behaviors. This instrument pro- CASE REPORT
vides useful information both for diagnostic
and programming purposes. Donald was the youngest of three children
As part of the psychiatric or psychological born to college-educated parents. The preg-
assessment, the child should be observed in nancy, labor and delivery, and early develop-
more and less structured activities, for exam- ment were unremarkable. He appeared to be a
ple, during developmental assessment or while normally active and sociable baby. He was
interacting with parents. As part of the history, smiling at 6 weeks, sitting at 7 months, crawl-
the examiner should specifically inquire about ing at 9 months, and walking without support
the child’s current and past social skills (e.g., at 15 months. He had several ear infections in
deferential attachments, interest in parents and the first year of life but said his first words by
peers, use of gaze), communication (receptive 12 months and was speaking in full sentences
and expressive, articulation problems, typical shortly after his second birthday. Videotapes
utterances, level of language organization prior provided by his parents confirmed his appar-
to the regression, unusual features such as ently normal developmental status.
echolalia, and nature of language loss), and re- Shortly after his third birthday, Donald’s
sponses to the environment and motor behaviors parents became concerned about his develop-
(e.g., self-stimulatory behaviors, difficulties ment as, over the course of several weeks, he
with change or transitions). The acquisition and lost both receptive and expressive language
any loss of adaptive skills, for example, toilet- and became progressively less interested
ing, self-care, or related skills, should also be in interaction and the inanimate environ-
reviewed. Observation of the child’s play is ment. He developed various self-stimulatory
helpful, for example, in documenting levels of behaviors and lost the ability to use the toilet
language, cognitive, and social organization independently.
and in observing gross and fine motor skills. No apparent reason for the regression was
Problematic or unusual behaviors relevant to identified. Extensive medical investigations
the diagnosis or likely to present obstacles for were undertaken. Although he was noted to
intervention should be noted. have a borderline abnormal EEG, no specific
Treatment of CDD is essentially the same medical condition that might account for his
as for autism. Methods such as behavior modi- developmental deterioration was identified.
fication and special education should be used Subsequent evaluations at other centers and
to help encourage the acquisition, or reacqui- by other specialists (including pediatric neu-
sition, of basic adaptive skills. There are no rologists and geneticists) similarly failed to
84 Diagnosis and Classification

identify such a condition. There was no family REFERENCES


history of similar problems or developmental
difficulties in members of the immediate or American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag-
extended family. nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
Although he had previously been in a regu- ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
lar nursery school setting, his behavior and de-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
velopmental deterioration warranted his ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
placement in a special educational setting. At American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
4 years old, comprehensive evaluation re- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
vealed that his cognitive skills were at about ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
the 18-month level with language and social Amir, R. E., Van den Veyver, I. B., Wan, M., Tran,
skills at an even lower level. Over the course of C. Q., Francke, U., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (1999).
many months, some, although highly limited, Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X-
expressive language skills returned as he was linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding
able to say an occasional single word. His so- protein 2 [see comment]. Nature Genetics,
23(2), 185–188.
cial unrelatedness, lack of interest in the envi-
Amir, R. E., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (2000). Rett syn-
ronment, and unusual behaviors continued. At
drome: Methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 muta-
the time of follow-up, at age 16, he continues tions and phenotype-genotype correlations.
to be severely impaired. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 97(2),
147–152.
CONCLUSION Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant develop-
ment. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Although described nearly 100 years ago, Beaumanoir, A. (1992). The Landau-Kleffner syn-
CDD remains infrequently reported and very drome. In J. Roger, M. Bureau, C. Dravet, F. E.
Dreifuss, A. Perret, & P. Wolf (Eds.), Epileptic
uncommonly studied. As noted by Rapin (in
seizures in infancy, childhood, and adolescence
press), the classification of children who de-
(2nd ed., pp. 231–243). London: John Libbey.
velop an autistic-like condition after several Bishop, D. V. M. (1985). Age on onset and outcome
years of normal development remains very in “acquired aphasia with convulsive disorder ”
poorly understood. It is clear that, once the (Landau-Kleffner syndrome). Developmental
condition is established, it behaviorally is very Medicine and Child Neurology, 27, 705–712.
similar to autism. The condition differs from Bishop, D. V. M. (1994). Developmental disorders
autism in the nature and pattern of its onset of speech and language. In M. Rutter, E. Tay-
and in course and certain clinical features. lor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent
While it appears very likely that some under- psychiatry: Modern approaches (3rd ed.,
lying neurobiological basis, or bases, of the pp. 546–568). London: Blackwell.
Burd, L., Fisher, W., & Kerbeshian, J. (1988).
condition exists, precise etiological mecha-
Childhood onset pervasive developmental dis-
nisms have not been identified. Past its obvi- order. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
ous importance for affected children and their atry and Allied Disciplines, 29(2), 155–163.
families, studies of such mechanisms may be Burd, L., Fisher, W., & Kerbeshian, J. (1989).
important in providing a better understanding Pervasive disintegrative disorder: Are Rett
of mechanisms of syndrome pathogenesis that syndrome and Heller dementia infantilis sub-
may have relevance to autism (Darby & Clark, types? Developmental Medicine and Child
1992). Neurology, 31(5), 609–616.
Burd, L., Ivey, M., Barth, A., & Kerbeshian, J.
(1998). Two males with childhood disintegra-
Cross-References tive disorder: A prospective 14-year outcome
study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-
Issues of diagnosis and classification are dis- rology, 40(10), 702–707.
cussed in Chapters 1 through 7; medical and Bzoch, K., & League, R. (1971). Receptive Expres-
neurological conditions potentially relevant sive Emergent Language Scale. Gainesville,
to CDD are reviewed in Chapters 16, 18, 19, FL: Language Educational Division, Computer
and 20. Management Corporation.
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 85

Corbett, J. (1987). Development, disintegration Hedrick, D., Prather, F., & Tobin A. (1975). Se-
and dementia. Journal of Mental Deficiency quenced inventory of communicative develop-
Research, 31(4), 349–356. ment. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Couvert, P., Bienvenu, T., Aquaviva, C., Poirier, Heller, T. (1908). Dementia infantilis. Zeitschrift
K., Moraine, C., Gendrot, C., et al. (2001). fur die Erforschung und Behandlung des Ju-
MECP2 is highly mutated in X-linked mental genlichen Schwachsinns, 2, 141–165.
retardation. Human Molecular Genetics, 10, Heller, T. (1930). Uber Dementia infantalis.
941–946. Zeitschrift fur Kinderforschung, 37, 661–667.
Darby, J. K., & Clark, L. (1992). Autism syndrome Hendry, S. N. (2000). Childhood disintegrative dis-
as a final common pathway of behavioral ex- order. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 77–90.
pression for many organic disorders. American Hill, A. E., & Rosenbloom, L. (1986). Disintegra-
Journal of Psychiatry, 149(1), 146. tive psychosis of childhood: Teenage follow-
Davidovitch, M., Glick, L., Holtzman, G., Tirosh, up. Developmental Medicine and Child
E., & Safir, M. P. (2000). Developmental re- Neurology, 28(1), 34–40.
gression in autism: Maternal perception. Jour- Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec-
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
30(2), 113–119. Kleefstra, T., Yntema, H. G., Oudakker, A. R., de
Deonna, T., Ziegler, A., Malin-Ingvar, M., Anser- Vries, B. B., van Bokhoven, H., Hamel, B. C.,
met, F., & Roulet, E. (1995). Reversible behav- et al. (2002). Localization of a gene for non-
ioral autistic-like regression: A manifestation specific X-linked mental retardation (MRX
of a special (new?) epileptic syndrome in a 28 76) to Xp22.3-Xp21.3. American Journal of
month old child: A 2 year longitudinal study. Medical Genetics, 110, 410–411.
Neurocase, 1, 1–9. Kobayashi, R., & Murata, T. (1998). Setback
Deykin, E. Y., & MacMahon, B. (1979). The inci- phenomenon in autism and long term progno-
dence of seizures among children with autistic sis. Archives Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 98,
symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 296–303.
136(10), 1310–1312. Kolvin, I. (1971). Studies in childhood psychoses: I.
Dunst, C. (1980). A clinical and educational man- Diagnostic criteria and classification. British
ual for use with the Uzgiris and Hunt Scales Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 381–384.
Baltimore: University Park Press. Kurita, H. (1985). Infantile autism with speech
Dyken, P., & Krawiecki, N. (1983). Neurodegenera- loss before the age of thirty months. Journal of
tive diseases of infancy and childhood. Annals the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
of Neurology, 13, 351–364. 24(2), 191–196.
Evans-Jones, L. G., & Rosenbloom, L. (1978, Au- Kurita, H. (1988). The concept and nosology of
gust). Disintegrative psychosis in childhood. Heller’s syndrome: Review of articles and re-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, port of two cases. Japanese Journal of Psychia-
20(4), 462–470. try and Neurology, 42, 785–793.
Fombonne, E. (2002). Prevalence of childhood dis- Kurita, H. (1989). Heller’s syndrome as a type of
integrative disorder. Autism: Journal of Re- pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of
search and Practice, 6(2), 149–157. Mental Health, 35, 71–81.
Gillberg, C., Terenius, L., Hagberg, B., Witt- Kurita, H., Kita, M., & Miyake, Y. (1992). A com-
Engerstrom, I., & Eriksson, I. (1990). CSF parative study of development and symptoms
beta-endorphins in childhood neuropsychi- among disintegrative psychosis and infantile
atric disorders. Brain Development, 12(1), autism with and without speech loss. Journal
88–92. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(2),
Girard, M., Couvert, P., Carrie, A., Tardieu, M., 175–188.
Chelly, J., Beldjord, C., et al. (2001). Parental Kurita, H., Osada, H., & Miyake, Y. (2004). Exter-
origin of de novo MECP2 mutations in Rett nal validity of childhood disintegrative disor-
syndrome. European Journal of Human Genet- der in comparison with autistic disorder.
ics, 9(3), 231–236. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Hammer, S., Dorrani, N., Dragich, J., Kudo, S., & ders, 34, 355–362.
Schanen, C. (2002). The phenotypic conse- Lainhart, J. E., Ozonoff, S., Coon, H., Krasny, L.,
quences of MECP2 mutations extend beyond Dinh, E., Nie, J., et al. (2002). Autism, re-
Rett syndrome. Mental Retardation and Devel- gression, and the broader autism phenotype.
opmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8, American Journal of Medical Genetics, 113,
94–98. 231–237.
86 Diagnosis and Classification

Leiter, R. G. (1948). Leiter International Perfor- Rogers, S. J., & DiLalla, D. L. (1990). Age of
mance Scale. Chicago: Stoelting. symptom onset in young children with Perva-
Lord, C., Schopler, E., & Revicki, D. (1982). Sex sive Developmental Disorder. Journal of the
differences in autism. Journal of Autism and American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Developmental Disorders, 12, 317–330. Psychiatry, 29, 863–872.
Makita, K. (1966). The age of onset of childhood Russo, M., Perry, R., Kolodny, E., & Gillberg, C.
schizophrenia. Folia Psychiatric Neurologica (1996). Heller syndrome in a pre-school boy.
Japonica, 20, 111–112. Proposed medical evaluation and hypothe-
Malhotra, S., & Gupta, N. (1999). Childhood disin- sized pathogenesis. European Child and Ado-
tegrative disorder. Journal of Autism and De- lescent Psychiatry, 5(3), 172–177.
velopmental Disorders, 29, 491–498. Rutter, M. (1972, October). Childhood schizophre-
Malhotra, S., & Gupta, N. (2002). Childhood disin- nia reconsidered. Journal of Autism and Child-
tegrative disorder: Re-examination of the cur- hood Schizophrenia, 2(4), 315–337.
rent concept. European Child and Adolescent Rutter, M. (1985). Infantile autism and other per-
Psychiatry, 11(3), 108–114. vasive developmental disorders. In M. Rutter
Malhotra, S., & Singh, S. (1993). Disintegrative & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psy-
psychosis of childhood: An appraisal and case chiatry—Modern approaches (pp. 545–566).
study. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 56, 37–40. London: Blackwell.
Meloni, I., Bruttini, M., Longo, I., Mari, F., Rizzo- Rutter, M. (2003). Categories, Dimensions, and the
lio, F., D’Adamo, P., et al. (2000). A mutation Mental Health of Children and Adolescents.
in the Rett syndrome gene, MECP2, causes Annals of the New York Academy of Science,
X-linked mental retardation and progressive 1008, 1–12.
spasticity in males. American Journal of Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couter, A.
Human Genetics, 67, 982–985. (1994). Autism and known medical conditions:
Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., & D’Anuono, P. Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol-
(1997). Childhood disintegrative disorder: ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
Review of cases and pathological considera- 35(2), 311–322.
tion. Developmental Brain Dysfunction, 10, Rutter, M., & Taylor, E. (2002). Classification:
67–74. Conceptual issues and substantive findings. In
Millichap, J. G. (1987). Rett’s syndrome: A variant E. Taylor & M. Rutter (Eds.), Child and ado-
of Heller’s dementia? [Letter]. Lancet, 1, 440. lescent psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 3–17). Oxford,
Mouridsen, S. E., Rich, B., & Isager, T. (1998). Va- UK: Blackwell Publishing.
lidity of childhood disintegrative psychosis. Shinnar, S., Rapin, I., Arnold, S., Tuchman, R.,
General findings of a long-term follow-up Shulman, L., Ballabasn-Gil, K., et al. (2001).
study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, Language regression in childhood. Pediatric
263–267. Neurology, 24, 183–194.
Mouridsen, S. E., Rich, B., & Isager, T. (2000). A Short, A., & Schopler, E. (1988). Factors relating
comparative study of genetic and neurobiolog- to age of onset in autism. Journal of Autism
ical findings in disintegrative psychosis and and Developmental Disorders, 18, 207–216.
infantile autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neu- Siegel, B., Pliner, C., Eschler, J., & Elliott, G. R.
rosciences, 54(4), 441–446. (1989). How children with autism are diag-
Nunn, K., Williams, K., & Ouvrier, R. (2002). The nosed: Difficulties in identification of chil-
Australian Childhood Dementia Study. Euro- dren with multiple developmental delays.
pean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9,
63–70. 199–204.
Rett, A. (1966). Uber ein eigenartiges hirn- Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).
tophisces Syndroem bei hyperammonie im Vineland adaptive behavior scales (Survey
Kindersalter. Wein Medizinische Wochen- Form). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
schrift, 118, 723–726. Service.
Reynell, J., & Gruber, C. (1990). Reynell Develop- Stein, M. T., Dixon, S. D., & Cowan, C. (2000). A
mental Language Scales—US Edition. Los An- two-year-old boy with language regression and
geles, CA: Western Psychological Services. unusual social interactions. Journal of Devel-
Rivinus, T. M., Jamison, D. L., & Graham, P. J. opmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(4),
(1975). Childhood organic neurological dis- 285–290.
ease presenting as a psychiatric disorder. Tuchman, R. F., & Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in
Archives of Disease in Childhood, 50, 115–119. pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder 87

and epileptiform electroencephalogram corre- disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,


lates. Pediatrics, 99(4), 560–566. 142(12), 1450–1452.
Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. M. (1975). Assessment in Weir, K., & Salisbury, D. M. (1980). Acute onset of
infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological devel- autistic features following brain damage in a
opment. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. ten-year-old. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Volkmar, F. R. (1987). Diagnostic issues in the per- mental Disorders, 10(2), 185–191.
vasive developmental disorders. Journal of Werry, J. S. (1992). Child and adolescent (early
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied onset) schizophrenia: A review in light of
Disciplines, 28(3), 365–369. DSM-III-R. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Childhood disintegrative mental Disorders, 22(4), 601–624.
disorder: Issues for DSM-IV. Journal of Autism Wilson, J. (1974). Investigation of degenerative
Developmental Disorders, 22, 625–642. disease of the central nervous system. Archives
Volkmar, F. R. (1996). The “disintegrative” disor- of Disease in Childhood, 47, 163–170.
ders. In F. Volkmar (Ed.), Childhood psy- Wilson, S., Djukic, A., Shinnar, S., Dharmani, C.,
choses and pervasive developmental disorders & Rapin, I. (2003). Clinical characteristics of
(pp. 223–248). Washington, DC: American language regression in children. Developmen-
Psychiatric Association Press. tal Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(8),
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Disintegra- 508–514.
tive disorder or “late onset ” autism. Journal of Wohlgemuth, D., Kiln, A., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied F. R. (1994, October). Childhood disintegrative
Disciplines, 30(5), 717–724. disorder: Diagnosis and phenomenology. Paper
Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., Hoshino, Y., Rende, presented at the annual meeting of the Ameri-
R. D., & Paul, R. (1988). Phenomenology and can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
classification of the childhood psychoses. Psy- try, New York.
chological Medicine, 18(1), 191–201. World Health Organization. (1978). Mental disor-
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Marans, W. D., & Cohen, ders: Glossary and guide to their classification
D. J. (1997). Childhood disintegrative disor- in accordance with the ninth revision of the in-
der. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), ternational classification of diseases. Geneva,
Autism and pervasive developmental disorders Switzerland: Author.
(2nd ed., pp. 47–59). New York: Wiley. World Health Organization. (1990). International
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., Classification of Diseases (Draft version, 10th
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American Zappert, J. (1921). Dementia infatilies Heller.
Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1361–1367. Monatsschrift fur Kinderheilkunde, 22, 389–391.
Volkmar, F. R., & Rutter M. (1995). Childhood dis- Zwaigenbaum, L., Szatmari, P., Mahoney, W.,
integrative disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Bryson, S., Bartolucci, G., & MacLean, J.
autism field trial. Journal of the American (2000). High functioning autism and child-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, hood disintegrative disorder in half brothers.
34, 1092–1095. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Volkmar, F. R., Stier, D. M., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). ders, 30(2), 121–126.
Age of recognition of pervasive developmental
CHAPTER 4

Asperger Syndrome

AMI KLIN, JAMES MCPARTLAND, AND FRED R. VOLKMAR

Asperger syndrome (AS) is a severe and general population in an attempt to define a


chronic developmental disorder closely related continuum of social function ranging from se-
to autistic disorder and pervasive developmen- vere autism to “normalcy.”
tal disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD- The confusions and controversies surround-
NOS), and, together, these disorders comprise ing the definition and validity of AS mean that
a continuum referred to as the autism spectrum the nosological status of AS as a discrete con-
disorders (ASDs). Having autism as the para- dition separate from autism is still uncertain
digmatic and anchoring disorder in this diag- (Frith, 2004; Volkmar & Klin, 2000). The
nostic category, the ASDs more generally are usage of the term, however, has been justified
characterized by marked and enduring impair- in pragmatic terms to bring more attention to
ments within the domains of social inter- individuals with autism with higher verbal
action, communication, play and imagination, abilities (Wing, 2000) or to less disabled indi-
and a restricted range of behaviors or inter- viduals but with social vulnerabilities accom-
ests. Although the diagnostic criteria for AS panied by a learning style conducive of
are still evolving, the “official” definitions technical knowledge or skills (Baron-Cohen,
provided in the International Classification of Wheelwright, Stone, & Rutherford, 1999) and
Diseases, 10th edition (ICD-10; World Health as a model illustrating the need for research on
Organization [WHO], 1993) and the Diagnos- factors mediating specific manifestations and
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, outcome in the ASDs (Szatmari, 2000). It is
fourth edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric important to keep these various uses of the
Association, 2000) distinguish it from autism term AS as the backdrop to any discussion of
primarily on the basis of a relative preserva- its nosologic and validity status to avoid less
tion of linguistic and cognitive capacities in productive lines of inquiry meant to reconcile
the first 3 years of life. Both in practice and in clinical research data yielded by studies using
research studies, however, the term AS has different definitions, criteria, and approaches
been used to refer to individuals with variedly to this condition. The prevailing attitude
defined manifestations of the ASDs, including among researchers, clinicians, and advocates
autism without mental retardation (or higher alike, however, is that regardless of these vari-
functioning autism [HFA]), “milder ” forms of ous uncertainties, individuals with AS have an
autism marked by higher cognitive and lin- early-onset social disability that impairs their
guistic abilities, and more socially motivated capacity for meeting the demands of everyday
but socially vulnerable adolescents and adults life and that this is an uncontroversial clinical
with unusual and socially interfering circum- fact necessitating, therefore, systematic re-
scribed interests. AS has also been used as a search involving all tools available to clinical
conceptual bridge between autism and the science.

88
Asperger Syndrome 89

In this chapter, we describe the historical in understanding nonverbal cues conveyed


background of AS, its clinical features, and by others.
special considerations for assessment and in- • Idiosyncrasies in verbal communication:
tervention. We review current diagnostic is- Spontaneous communication is character-
sues including validity studies and suggest ized by highly circumstantial utterances
potentially fruitful guidelines for future (e.g., failing to distinguish abstractions
research. from autobiographical narration), long-
winded and incoherent verbal accounts fail-
ASPERGER’S ORIGINAL CLINICAL ing to convey a clear message or thought
CONCEPT (e.g., tangential speech prompted by a se-
ries of associations), and one-sidedness
Hans Asperger (1906–1980) was an Austrian (e.g., failing to demarcate changes of topic
pediatrician with interest in special education or to introduce new material). Both the
who in 1944 described four children ages 6 to style of language and tone of delivery are
11 years who had difficulty integrating so- pedantic, like a “little professor.”
cially into groups despite seemingly adequate • Social adaptation and special interests:
cognitive and verbal skills (1944; Frith pro- There are egocentric preoccupations with
vided an English translation of the original ar- unusual and circumscribed interests that
ticle in 1991, adding an account of Asperger’s absorb most of the person’s attention and
professional background). He originally termed energy, thus precluding the acquisition of
the condition he described Autistischen Psy- practical skills necessary for self-help and
chopathen im Kindesalter, or autistic personal- social integration. Some of these interests,
ity disorders in childhood, echoing Bleuler’s for example, in letters and numbers, are
(1916) use of the term autism in schizophrenia often quite precocious. Later in life, spe-
to signify extreme egocentrism, or the shutting cial interests tend to evolve into specific,
off of relations between the person affected topic-related collections of information
and other people. He contrasted this condition abundant with encyclopedic knowledge,
from schizophrenia, however, by emphasizing such as astronomy or geography.
the stable and enduring nature of the social • Intellectualization of af fect: Emotional pre-
impairments in autistic psychopathy and by sentation is marked by poor empathy, the
voicing the optimistic view that unlike in tendency to intellectualize feelings, and an
schizophrenia, his patients were able to even- accompanying absence of intuitive under-
tually develop some relationships. The term standing of other people’s affective experi-
psychopathy is best translated as personality ences and communication.
disorder (i.e., a group of stable personality • Clumsiness and poor body awareness: Mo-
traits). In Asperger’s time and cultural ethos, toric presentation is characterized by odd
this term did not have the negative connotation posture and gait, poor body awareness, and
of its present usage. The choice of the label clumsiness. Asperger emphasized his pa-
autistic psychopathy also reflected Asperger’s tients’ inability to participate in group
belief that difficulties in socialization repre- sports or other self-help activities involv-
sented the defining feature of the condition. ing motor coordination and integration and
However, he also detailed various clusters of described in detail their poor graphomotor
behavioral symptoms and clinical features that skills.
appeared in conjunction with the social dis- • Conduct problems: The most common
ability. His observations continue to be incor- reason for clinical referral of Asperger’s
porated into current definitions of the disorder patients involved failure at school and
and include: associated behavioral problems including
aggressiveness, noncompliance, and nega-
• Impairment in nonverbal communication: tivism, which were accounted for in terms
There is a reduction in the quantity and di- of their deficits in social understanding
versity of facial expressions and limitations and extreme pursuit of highly circum-
in the use of gesture, as well as difficulties scribed interests. Asperger was particularly
90 Diagnosis and Classification

concerned about his patients’ poor social EVOLVING CONCEPTUALIZATION


adjustment and how they were mercilessly OF ASPERGER SYNDROME
bullied and teased by peers.
• Onset: Asperger thought that the condition Discussions of Asperger’s work were not avail-
could not be recognized in early childhood. able in English until the 1970s. Prior to that,
Speech and language skills as well as cu- however, a handful of related publications in
riosity about the environment in general, English with some relevance to his work had
including people, were thought not to be appeared. For example, Robinson and Vitale
conspicuously deviant. (1954) described three cases of children ages 8
• Familial and gender patterns: Some 30 to 11 who showed a pattern of circumscribed
years prior to the first publication revealing interests reminiscent of Asperger’s patients.
genetic contributions to the etiology of the The children were interested in developmen-
ASDs, Asperger highlighted the familial tally precocious topics such as chemistry,
nature of the condition affecting his pa- nuclear fission, transportation systems, as-
tients, suggesting that similar traits in par- tronomy, electricity, and mortgages, about
ents or relatives were found in almost every which they talked incessantly in one-sided
single case. His patients were almost exclu- conversations with adults and peers. These
sively boys. children were generally socially isolated, play
with other children had to revolve around their
The year before Asperger’s article on autis- interests, and they had overly blunt manners
tic psychopathy appeared in the German child and no concern about their grooming and other
psychiatry literature, Leo Kanner (1943) had social niceties. Kanner (1954) was the invited
published his classic description of 11 children discussant of this paper. The descriptions re-
with “autistic disturbances of affective con- minded him of infantile autism, but he felt that
tact.” Though both authors were unaware of the these children were less socially withdrawn
other’s work, there were many commonalities and more affectively engaged with others, al-
between their patients, including problems with though they lacked friends and did not partici-
social interaction, affect, and communication, pate in group activities. The circumscribed
as well as unusual and idiosyncratic patterns of interests dominated the lives of these children,
interest. The main differences related to As- monopolizing their learning and interfering
perger’s observations that his patients’ speech with their ability to engage others in recipro-
and language acquisition were less commonly cal relationships. Yet, the interests were often
delayed, motor deficits were more likely, onset unusual, consisting of collections of facts
was later in childhood, and all the initial cases rather than conceptual inquiries, making them
occurred in boys. More importantly, however, even more difficult to share with others as a
there were significant differences in terms of hobby or to build on as a possible vocational
aspects and severity of symptomatology in the possibility. It is of interest, therefore, that
areas of social-emotional functioning, speech, Kanner’s incisive clinical observations greatly
language and cognitive skills, motor manner- resembled those of Asperger’s when he was
isms, and circumscribed interests, which to a called to describe children similar to those en-
great extent may have been a function of the countered by Asperger in his original case
specific patients seen by Kanner (who were studies.
primarily preschoolers, less verbal, and more The concept of autistic psychopathy was in-
cognitively disabled) and by Asperger (who troduced to a wider English-speaking reader-
were primarily school-age children, highly ver- ship by Van Krevelen (1963), who made a
bal, and cognitively able). Consequently, Kan- deliberate attempt to distinguish it from Kan-
ner’s description became associated with the ner’s autism. In Van Krevelen’s view (1971),
“classically” cognitively impaired or “lower the conditions are sharply different. Kanner’s
functioning” child with autism, whereas As- autism is manifested from the first months of
perger’s description lends itself more readily life, the child walks before he or she talks,
to an association with the more cognitively speech is delayed or absent, language never at-
able and highly verbal older child with autism. tains the function of communication, there is a
Asperger Syndrome 91

lack of interest in others, and prognosis is poor. outnumbered females, around 20% were girls.
In contrast, Asperger’s condition is manifested These modifications blurred the distinctions
from the third year of life or later, the child originally suggested by Van Krevelen (1971)
talks before he or she walks, language aims at and reiterated by Asperger (1979), bringing
communication but remains one-sided, the the syndrome into an autism spectrum of dis-
child seeks interaction but in an awkward fash- abilities defined by Wing in terms of the triad
ion, and prognosis is rather good. In many of impairments involving social, communica-
ways, van Krevelen’s distinctions remain the tion, and imaginative activities (Wing, 2000).
core argument for those supporting a separa- Wing’s (1981) publication sparked a great
tion between the two conditions. Neither Kan- deal of interest, and the quantity of research
ner (1954) nor Asperger (in at least most of his publications and clinical studies addressing
early writings) ever appeared to make a strong AS has continued to grow steadily (Klin,
argument for a more categorical separation of Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000). Much of this lit-
the two conditions (though Kanner never pub- erature concerns an attempt to define the dis-
lished an explicit account of his views of As- tinction (or lack thereof ) between AS and
perger’s work, and Asperger both limited the HFA. Historically, researchers have used the
use of autistic psychopathy to cognitively able label idiosyncratically, taking liberties to
and highly verbal individuals and appeared to modify or to emphasize elements of the syn-
support a distinction later in life; see Asperger, drome depending on their clinical experiences
1979; and Hippler & Klicpera, 2003). or theoretical stance. This variability renders
An influential review and series of case re- it virtually impossible to compare findings of
ports by Lorna Wing (1981) finally popular- studies adopting different definitions known
ized Asperger’s work among English readers. to yield, not surprisingly, different groups for
She reported 34 cases, ages 5 to 35, of whom comparison (e.g., Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghazi-
19 had a clinical presentation similar to As- uddin, 1992; Klin, Pauls, Schultz, & Volkmar,
perger’s account, whereas 19 had a consistent in press). Some progress has been made to-
current presentation but did not have the char- ward this end through the establishment of
acteristic onset patterns and early history. “official,” though tentative, diagnostic crite-
Concerned that the term psychopathy might ria now incorporated in ICD-10 (WHO, 1993)
connote sociopathic behavior rather than the and DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Associa-
intended personality disorder and hoping to tion, 1994), although this definition has itself
ground the condition in developmental terms, been very controversial.
she renamed the disorder Asperger’s syn-
drome. Wing summarized Asperger’s descrip- Issues in Definition of Asperger Syndrome
tion and proposed some modifications for the
syndrome based on her case studies. Though Although Wing (1981) did not provide a spe-
Asperger thought the condition was unrecog- cific set of categorical diagnostic criteria,
nizable prior to 3 years of age, Wing suggested many case reports and research studies de-
the following difficulties were present in the rived such criteria from her publication and
first 2 years of life: (1) a lack of normal inter- used these to characterize their respective pa-
est and pleasure in other people, (2) babbling tient or subject population. However, accounts
that is limited in quantity and quality, (3) re- often included different sets of criteria, re-
duced sharing of interests and activities, (4) flecting the author’s decision as to which be-
absence of an intense drive to communicate, haviors were necessary, only suggestive, or
both verbally and nonverbally, with others, (5) altogether irrelevant. As noted, such variabil-
speech that is abnormal in terms of delayed ac- ity in diagnostic assignment suggested that
quisition or impoverished content consisting comparisons among studies would be difficult
mainly of stereotyped utterances, and (6) fail- and pointed to the need for a uniform nosology
ure to develop a full repertoire of imaginative of AS (Rutter & Gould, 1985). Despite Wing’s
pretend play. Wing also suggested that AS can (2000) repeated attempts to slow the process
be found in individuals with mild mental retar- that she unwittingly started and echoing Van
dation and that, although in her sample males Krevelen’s (1971) initial approach, the stage
92 Diagnosis and Classification

was set for the field to test the validity status The ICD-10 (WHO, 1993) and DSM-IV def-
of AS relative to HFA. In this context, there initions attempted to bridge over the differ-
was a need for consensual criteria for the defi- ences among previous definitions and to
nition of AS to be universally adopted by clin- contrast AS with autism, without which valida-
ical researchers. Following the framework of tion work would be impossible. To do so, it fo-
current diagnostic systems such as ICD-10 cused primarily on the number of overall
(WHO, 1993) and DSM-IV (1994), this defini- symptoms (greater in autism and lower in AS,
tion had to be categorical (i.e., AS and autism which was intended to capture variable degrees
should be defined in terms of mutually exclu- of severity), excluded the language cluster in-
sive criteria) rather than dimensional (i.e., AS cluded in the diagnosis of autism (indicating
and autism should not differ simply in terms of the absence of severe language impairment in
degree of symptomatology). This task was AS while sidestepping an attempt to define the
complicated by the fact that Wing’s (1981) ac- peculiarities typical of verbal communication
count, from which most subsequent definitions in AS), and specified onset criteria that con-
were derived (Gillberg & Gillberg, 1989; Szat- trasted with those for autism (the absence of
mari, Bartolucci, & Bremner, 1989; Tantam, clinically significant delays in speech, lan-
1988a), implied that the two conditions could guage, and cognitive development in AS). In
not be separated and that AS was a subtype of many respects, this definition was both stricter
autism with only minor differences in clinical than some (particularly relative to the onset
manifestation. Many case reports and research criteria) and less specific than others (as it sub-
studies avoided this issue altogether, particu- sumed the more unique symptoms of AS under
larly by failing to state whether subjects also the autism clusters of social impairment and re-
fulfilled criteria for autism. While the desig- stricted areas of interest). In essence, AS was
nation of Asperger syndrome as a “ variant ” or defined in terms of the criteria for autism
“subtype” of autism (e.g., as highly verbal in- (either presence or absence). The ICD-10 crite-
dividuals with autism with normative or supe- ria for AS are given in Table 4.1.
rior IQs) is justifiable from several practical In practice, the ICD-10 and DSM-IV defin-
perspectives (e.g., to bring attention to a sub- itions make a distinction between autism and
group of people with autism thus facilitating AS solely on the basis of the onset criteria. In
advocacy, to provide a concept around which autism, any concerns prior to the age of 3 years
clients and their families can more easily coa- involving social interaction, social communi-
lesce and share experiences; Wing, 2000; cation, or symbolic/imaginative play are suffi-
Szatmari, 2000), this is not so in formal diag- cient for the criteria to be met. In contrast, any
nostic systems, which are meant, among other concern involving cognitive development (in
things, to provide mutually exclusive cate- essence, typical exploration of and curiosity
gories to facilitate research. Thus, the central about the environment given that the majority
issue in recent years has been whether AS and of children are not developmentally assessed
HFA (and, by necessity, PDD-NOS) are dis- prior to age 3), self-help skills, or more
tinctive in ways that are independent of the broadly defined adaptive behavior (other than
definition used to assign group membership. In social interaction but including social commu-
other words, research has focused on whether nication) would rule out the diagnosis of AS.
these concepts are associated with a different The possibly overinclusive nature of onset cri-
developmental course; different neurocogni- teria for autism and overexclusive nature of
tive, neurobiological, or genetic underpin- onset criteria for AS (and any ambiguities left
nings; or a different outcome (Klin et al., in in the definition, e.g., how to distinguish so-
press; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz, & Klin, cial interaction from social communication)
2004). The end result of this process was the are resolved in terms of the precedence rule—
tentative inclusion of AS in ICD-10 (WHO, if an individual meets criteria for autism, he or
1993). And because ICD-10 and DSM-IV were she cannot be assigned the diagnosis of AS
intended to be equivalent, AS was also in- (Volkmar & Klin, 2000).
cluded in the DSM-IV autism / PDD field trials Although the advent of the DSM-IV defi-
(Volkmar, Klin, Seigel, et al., 1994). nition was intended to create a consensual
Asperger Syndrome 93

TABLE 4.1 ICD-10 Research Diagnostic Heavey, & Le Couteur, 1998; Rutter, 1999;
Guidelines for Asperger Syndrome Volkmar, Klin, & Cohen, 1997). For the pur-
1. There is no clinically significant general delay in pose of this chapter, however, this process has
spoken or receptive language or cognitive highlighted the differences among some influ-
development. Diagnosis requires that single words ential definitions of AS. Table 4.2 presents a
should have developed by 2 years of age or earlier comparison of key diagnostic features among
and that communicative phrases be used by 3 years the most widely used definitions, which in-
of age or earlier. Self-help skills, adaptive
behavior, and curiosity about the environment
clude Asperger’s (1944) original account and
during the first 3 years should be at a level later emphases and changes (Asperger, 1979;
consistent with normal intellectual development. Van Krevelen, 1971), as well as those of Wing
However, motor milestones may be somewhat (1981), Gillberg and Gillberg (1989), Tantam
delayed and motor clumsiness is usual (although (1988a), and Szatmari, Bremner, and Nagy
not a necessary diagnostic feature). Isolated (1989a, 1989b). These definitions are com-
special skills, often related to abnormal
pared with the one formalized in DSM-IV
preoccupations, are common, but are not required
for the diagnosis.
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994),
2. There are qualitative abnormalities in reciprocal which, as noted, is conceptually equivalent to
social interaction (criteria for autism). the one included in ICD-10 (WHO, 1993;
3. The individual exhibits an unusually intense, Volkmar et al., 1994). Items in bold are those
circumscribed interest or restricted, repetitive, features deemed necessary for the diagnosis of
and stereotyped patterns of behavior interests and AS according to the given diagnostic system.
activities (criteria for autism; however, it would
It is evident from Table 4.2 that a direct com-
be less usual for these to include either motor
mannerisms or preoccupations with part-objects parison among the various diagnostic systems is
or nonfunctional elements of play materials). not straightforward. Ghaziuddin and colleagues
4. The disorder is not attributable to other varieties (1992) attempted to compare some of these sys-
of pervasive developmental disorder; simple tems. Having simplified the criteria for compar-
schizophrenia schizotypal disorder, obsessive- ison, the study compared Asperger’s (1944)
compulsive disorder, anakastic personality definition with Wing’s (1981)—which was
disorder; reactive and disinhibited attachment
disorders of childhood.
made to be equivalent to Gillberg and Gillberg’s
(1989) and Tantam’s (1988)—and the defini-
Source: Reprinted with permission from World Health tions of Szatmari and colleagues’ (1989a,
Organization (1993). Disorders of psychological de-
1989b) and ICD-10 (WHO, 1993). Of 15 pa-
velopment (Criteria for Research) (Geneva: WHO),
pp. 154–155. tients identified as having AS according to
Wing’s criteria, only 10 patients met Szatmari
diagnostic starting point for research, it has et al.’s criteria, and only 8 patients met the de-
been consistently criticized as overly narrow scription by Asperger as well as ICD-10 crite-
(Eisenmajer et al., 1996; Szatmari, Archer, ria. The primary reason that seven patients did
Fisman, Streiner, & Wilson, 1995), rendering not fulfill ICD-10 (and Asperger’s) criteria was
the diagnostic assignment of AS improbable or their failure to meet the onset criteria, specifi-
even “ virtually impossible” according to some cally the lack of clinically significant delay
authors (Leekam, Libby, Wing, Gould, & Gill- in speech and language acquisition. Therefore,
berg, 2000; Mayes, Calhoun, & Crites, 2001; despite the lack of required specific social, lan-
Miller & Ozonoff, 2000). While this critique guage and communication, and absorbing inter-
has reinforced the approach of some re- ests criteria (which differ form Asperger’s
searchers to cluster all ASDs together in account), the ICD-10 definition is, in practice,
search of common underlying factors, other re- more restrictive than other systems because of
searchers are pursuing subgrouping efforts be- its emphasis on specific onset patterns (which is
cause of their recognition that autism is a consistent with Asperger’s account).
clinically heterogeneous disorder and that the This state of affairs raises several issues for
characterization of subtypes of PDD might nosology research:
help behavioral and biological research by
allowing the identification of clinically more 1. By simultaneously emphasizing the lack of
homogeneous groups (Bailey, Palferman, communication and cognitive delays in the
94 Diagnosis and Classification

TABLE 4.2 Comparison of Six Sets of Clinical Criteria Defining Asperger Syndrome
Gillberg & Szatmari
Asperger Wing Gillberg Tantam et al. DSM-IV
Clinical Feature* (1944, 1979) (1981) (1989) (1988) (1989) (APA, 1994)

Social Impairment
Poor nonverbal
communication Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Poor empathy Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Failure to develop
friendship Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
(implied)
Language/Communication
Poor prosody and
pragmatics Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Not stated
Idiosyncratic language Yes Yes Not stated Not stated Yes Not stated
Impoverished imagi-
native play Yes Yes Not stated Not stated Not stated Not stated
All Absorbing Interest Yes Yes Yes Yes Not stated Often
Motor Clumsiness Yes Yes Yes Yes Not stated Often
Onset (0–3 years)
Speech delays/deviance No May be present May be present Not stated Not stated No
Cognitive delays No May be present Not stated Not stated Not stated No
Motor delays Yes Sometimes Not stated Not stated Not stated May be present
Exclusion of Autism Yes (1979) No No No Yes Yes
Mental Retardation No May be present Not stated Not stated Not stated Not stated

* Symptoms that are defined as necessary for the presence of the condition are given in bold.

first years of life while failing to specify re- with Asperger’s description. Also, if pursu-
quired social, communication, motor, and ing this strategy, it is unlikely that such
absorbing interest features that are thought poorly detailed, “all or nothing” description
to be typical in AS, the ICD-10 definition of onset characteristics will capture mean-
differentiates autism from AS solely based ingful variability of developmental concepts
on the onset criteria, in fact, irrespective of of interest (e.g., social motivation, joint at-
the nature of the patient’s social impairment tention, nonverbal communication), which
later in life. Whether individuals diagnosed are likely to mediate outcome.
in this manner will have a later presentation 2. By failing to include social, communicative,
consistent with Asperger’s description re- or restricted interest patterns more specific
mains to be documented. Various authors to AS ( by subsuming those under general
(Gillberg & Gillberg, 1989; Leekam, Libby, clusters defining autism), the ICD-10 defi-
Wing, Gould, & Gillberg, 2000; Wing, nition of AS disregards features that could
1981) have reported cases that would not serve as discriminative factors and may be
meet the ICD-10 onset criteria and yet pre- unique among AS patients. For example,
sented with what they thought was AS. Tantam (1988a) suggests that in contrast
However, even higher functioning individu- to the prevailing view of individuals with
als with autism appear to have the onset of autism, individuals with AS may wish to
their condition before age 3 years (Volkmar be sociable and yet fail to establish rela-
& Cohen, 1989). In summary, while it is in- tionships. Although this may also be true
teresting to study eventual clinical presenta- for older adolescents and adults with
tions on the basis of early profiles of autism, it would be unusual for a younger
development, there is no assurance that later child with autism. This was an important
onset will lead to individuals conforming point raised by Van Krevelen (1971), who
Asperger Syndrome 95

thought children with autism disregarded dimensions creating the autism spectrum
others while children with AS approached (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 2002; Constantino &
others in an eccentric fashion. Both were so- Todd, 2003). The second approach—here
cially impaired, but qualitatively different. called the early language approach, makes a
3. Just as autistic social dysfunction can be distinction between AS and HFA by dividing
defined only in the context of the child’s children with ASD and apparently normal cog-
overall developmental level (Rutter, 1978), nitive development into two groups in terms of
there may be a host of developmental fac- language development in the first 3 years of
tors that should be considered in attempts life. A diagnosis of AS is given if the child
to differentiate the social, affective, and achieved single words by age 2 years and
communication presentation of individuals phrase speech (typically defined as non-
with HFA from those with AS. These in- echolalic three-word combinations used mean-
clude chronological age (e.g., less pro- ingfully for communication) by the age of 3
nounced contrast in older adolescents and years. A diagnosis of autism is given if the
adults), IQ and language level (e.g., less child does not meet these criteria (Gilchrist
pronounced contrast in individuals with et al., 2001; Szatmari et al., 1995, 2000). The
higher IQ and language abilities), and the third approach criticizes the first two and of-
presence of any medical condition disrupt- fers an alternative (Klin et al., in press). As to
ing speech and language acquisition early the spectrum approach, it is unlikely, though
in life independently from psychiatric di- not entirely impossible, that single underlying
agnosis (e.g., cleft palate, intermittent factors or dimensions could generate the
hearing loss resulting from chronic ear highly complex and heterogeneous spectrum
infection). of autism-related disorders in a predictable,
quantified manner. As to the early language
In summary, the different diagnostic sys- approach, there is concern that it greatly nar-
tems have provided different sets of criteria rows the potential lines of distinction between
but have not resolved the key issue dominating autism and AS in that other aspects of onset
research on AS since the early 1970s, namely, (e.g., social engagement and social cognitive
its validity status relative to autism. That some patterns), as well as any unique features in
influential authors had not meant to suggest current presentation (e.g., social motivation,
that AS was a distinguishable diagnostic entity verbosity), are disregarded (Volkmar & Klin,
is unlikely to quell this debate. There is a need 2000). More practically, there is concern that
to go beyond the current sterile impasse fuel- because individuals with HFA may not present
ing so much of the current classification de- with speech delays as defined in this diagnos-
bate, which is still based on rather arbitrary tic scheme, there is a potential for the resultant
decisions and vague semantics rather than samples (of individuals with HFA and AS) to
empirical validation of operationalized defini- overlap considerably in terms of other sympto-
tions that reflect more sophisticated develop- matology, thus increasing the potential for
mental constructs. type II errors (i.e., not finding differences).
Three approaches appear to be emerging To address these potential limitations, the
from this discussion. The first, here called the third approach—here called the unique fea-
spectrum approach, disregards the nosologic tures approach (possibly better described as
discussion entirely by equating AS with “ bell-ringers” approach)—emphasizes the fea-
higher functioning ASDs or with some social tures more specifically associated with AS
vulnerability of the kind seen in ASDs (some highlighted by Asperger (1944) and several
individuals with this characterization may not other authors (e.g., Klin & Volkmar, 1997;
meet clinical criteria for a PDD). Proponents Tantam, 1988a). These features are included in
of this approach make the assumptions that all the narratives accompanying the definition of
early-onset and chronic social vulnerabilities AS in ICD-10 (WHO, 1993) and in DSM-IV-TR
share some factors in common and that re- (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) but
search is best invested in better examining and are not included in the respective sets of diag-
possibly quantifying these factors seen as the nostic criteria. In this approach, onset criteria
96 Diagnosis and Classification

are given in more detail. For example, distinc- and 1980. Only an estimated 1.15% was diag-
tions are made between children who isolate nosed with AP. Thus, the condition that he de-
themselves (more typical of autism) and those scribed was likely to be quite specific. The
who seek others, sometimes incessantly, but in estimate of cases falling within the autism
a socially insensitive manner (more typical of spectrum, but without an explicit diagnosis of
AS); and between children whose language is AP, was double that number. Of the AP cases,
delayed, echolalic, or otherwise stereotyped 95% were boys, a manifold increase in the typ-
(more typical of autism) and those whose lan- ical autism gender ratio (4 boys for every girl),
guage is adequate or even precocious and larger but closer to the ratio reported for
whose difficulties in this area are limited to higher functioning ASDs, and closer still to
the communicative use of language (i.e., prag- the gender ratio reported for individuals with
matics; more typical of AS). The added details AS (Volkmar et al., 2004). The clinical con-
are meant to facilitate research into develop- cepts used to describe patients suggested the
mental paths of social disabilities with a notion that these children had social motiva-
greater degree of specificity. Additional mod- tion but were very awkward in approaching
ifications proposed involve the inclusion of others, leading to failure to establish relation-
one-sided verbosity as a necessary communi- ships. For example, “contact disorder ” or “in-
cation criterion in AS and the presence of fac- stinct disorder ” were used to refer to a lack of
tual, circumscribed interests that interfere common sense, impaired “practical intelli-
with both general learning and reciprocal gence” in everyday situations, and a need to
social conversation. These communication learn these skills through their intellect (Hip-
symptoms were introduced with a view to cap- pler & Klicpera, 2003, p. 294). Fifty-four per-
ture the observed greater social motivation cent of cases showed excellent verbal abilities
seen in individuals with AS (relative to HFA). (e.g., fund of information) but impaired non-
Research systematically assessing the util- verbal abilities (e.g., visual-spatial skills),
ity of these approaches is still limited. One re- echoing a learning profile thought to be more
cent study (Klin et al., in press), however, has typical of individuals with AS than of those
shown that three diagnostic schemes for AS with HFA (Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti,
used simultaneously—DSM-IV, the early & Rourke, 1995). On IQ tests, higher verbal IQ
language approach, and the unique features (VIQ) than performance IQ (PIQ) was more
approach—have low agreement in case assign- than twice as common as the reverse (44%
ment and lead to different results in compar- versus 18%), and almost the totality of cases
isons of IQ profiles, patterns of comorbidity, had full-scale IQs within or above the norma-
and familial aggregation of psychiatric symp- tive range. Some resemblance between the
toms across the approach-specific resultant child with AP and one or more family mem-
groups of HFA, AS, and PDD-NOS. bers was observed in 53% of the sample, with
52% of fathers being reported as being simi-
ASPERGER’S WORK REVISITED lar to their child in aspects of social dysfunc-
tion, a much higher estimate of familiality
Hippler and Klicpera (2003) recently con- than observed in autism but similar to some
ducted a retrospective analysis of the clinical recent family genetic data using the more
case records of individuals with autistic psy- specific feature of AS to define the proband
chopathy (AP) diagnosed by Hans Asperger (Klin et al., in press). Eighty-two percent of
and his team in the various clinics that they AP cases were reported to have circum-
practiced. Although there are obvious limita- scribed interests of the kinds subsequently
tions in using current concepts to reanalyze described as particularly specific to AS (e.g.,
data generated through the lens of a different topic-based obsessions). An attempt to apply
era (with its unique system of diagnostic con- the ICD-10 criteria for AS resulted in a diag-
cepts and debates), this paper helps us better nosis of AS in 68% of cases; 25% did not meet
understand Asperger’s work and development the requirement of absence of clinical symp-
of his diagnostic concept. For example, he saw toms before the age of 3; of these, half had
approximately 9,800 children between 1951 some language delays.
Asperger Syndrome 97

Hippler and Klicpera’s article (2003) is of difference (e.g., level and pervasiveness of
very helpful in showing the very specific pro- social disability, more severe in AS), outcome
file of children singled out by Asperger over ( less positive in AS), and relatedness to the
his lifetime, which contrasts markedly with schizophrenia spectrum of disorders (the asso-
some of the current definitions of AS. Still, ciation is stronger in schizoid personality in
there is little to gain from an exegetic analysis childhood).
of this nature, if the validity and utility of the The concept of nonverbal learning disabili-
construct cannot be assessed relative to pre- ties (NLD) was originally proposed by John-
dictions independent of definitions for case son and Myklebust (1971; Myklebust, 1975)
assignment. The development of our under- and was subsequently elaborated and thor-
standing of autism offers an interesting con- oughly researched by Rourke (1989). NLD
trast. Despite the remarkably enduring quality refers to a profile of neuropsychological assets
of Kanner’s (1943) original description, many and deficits that have a significant negative
key issues have been modified as a result of impact on a person’s social and communica-
subsequent research. The fact that Rutter’s tion skills (see Chapter 13, this Handbook, this
(1978) codification of Kanner’s prose proved volume). Deficits in neuropsychological skills
to be an effective and reliable diagnostic tool such as tactile perception, psychomotor coor-
is partially thanks to Kanner’s brilliance, but dination, visual-spatial organization, and non-
it is also a function of the severity of the con- verbal problem solving occur in the presence
dition he described. Given that the syndrome of preserved rote verbal abilities. The particu-
described by Asperger is more equivocal than lar profile of strengths and deficits results in a
Kanner’s autism—overlapping with autism to characteristic learning style such as the ten-
some extent but also maybe shading into ec- dency to overly rely on overlearned behaviors
centric normalcy, it is appropriate that addi- when dealing with novel or complex situations.
tional work is needed to create an effective Poor pragmatics and prosody in speech are
and reliable definition. seen in the presence of relatively preserved
formal language skills such as vocabulary and
ALTERNATIVE DIAGNOSTIC syntax and single-word reading abilities. Dif-
CONCEPTS ficulties in appreciating the subtle, and some-
times obvious, nonverbal aspects of social
Professionals from diverse disciplines such as interaction lead to major deficits in social per-
adult psychiatry, neuropsychology, and neurol- ception and judgment, which often result in
ogy have dealt with individuals with signifi- social isolation and rejection and increased
cant problems in social interaction who did not risk for social withdrawal and serious mood
seem to precisely fit Kanner’s (1943) concept disorders (Rourke, Young, & Leenaars, 1989).
of infantile autism (Klin & Volkmar, 1997). Although potentially associated with a number
Wolff and colleagues (Wolff & Barlow, 1979; of different conditions (Rourke, 1995), it is
Wolff & Chick, 1980) proposed the term often seen in individuals with AS but less so in
schizoid personality in childhood in their de- individuals with autism (Gunter, Ghaziuddin,
scription of children with social isolation and & Ellis, 2002; Klin, Sparrow, Volkmar, Cic-
emotional detachment, unusual communicative chetti, & Rourke, 1995; Lincoln, Courchesne,
style, and rigidity of thought and behavior. Al- Allen, Hanson, & Ene, 1998; Siegel, Minshew,
though the initial emphasis on the condition as a & Goldstein, 1996).
personality disorder resulted in more sketchy Another influential concept was proposed
accounts of developmental course, attempts to by Rapin and Allen (1983), who used the term
reconstruct the developmental history of these semantic-pragmatic disorder to describe cases
children were made (e.g., Wolff, 1991, 1995). in which speech and language skills were ade-
Several attempts have been made to compare quate in form (syntax and phonology) but im-
this concept with AS (e.g., Nagy & Szatmari, poverished in content and use (semantics and
1986; Tantam, 1988b), showing some areas of pragmatics). Bishop (1989, 1998) refined the
similarity (e.g., abnormalities in empathy and descriptions and assessment instrumentation
nonverbal communication) but also some areas used in the characterization of communicative
98 Diagnosis and Classification

difficulties exhibited by these children. More child began to talk before learning to walk.
recent research (Bishop, 2000) has shown that Vocabulary acquisition, however, may be un-
the association between semantic and prag- usual as children may learn complex or adult-
matic deficits is not a necessary one and that a like words, typically associated with a special
term such as pragmatic language impairment interest prior to learning more typical, child-
might be preferable to refer to these children. like vocabulary associated with social play
Other concepts, such as developmental and experiences. Parents may report that once
learning disability of the right hemisphere the child began to talk, there was a pedantic
(Denckla, 1983; Weintraub & Mesulam, quality to their speech, both in terms of their
1983), have been proposed. Although the choice of words and sentence construction
number of labels available and the diversity of (more formal than seen in typical peers) and
disciplines from which they arose are a testa- in terms of the tone of voice and phrasing
ment to the robustness of the underlying clini- (sometimes assuming a “ teaching” quality).
cal phenomena, these various labels have the Attachment patterns to family members are
potential for introducing confusion in noso- often seen as unremarkable, and in contrast to
logic discussion of AS. Their discipline- autism, there are few visible signs of social
specific nature may also inhibit the kinds of disability in the highly familiar environment of
interdisciplinary research that could provide the child’s home. In many ways, these children
an integrated and comprehensive understand- appear to orient to others although they may
ing of individuals with AS. Confusion is par- use them more instrumentally than recipro-
ticularly an issue for parents whose child cally, for example, to speak to them rather than
might receive any of several different diagnos- to truly engage them in a shared pursuit. The
tic labels depending on the training and disci- social disability becomes more apparent when
pline of the clinician with whom they consult. the child is outside the home environment, par-
Given that the various conditions described ticularly in group situations involving same-
reflect primarily differences in discipline- age children. In such situations, they may
specific emphasis ( language and communica- approach other children in inappropriate or
tion versus neuropsychological profiles versus awkward ways, for example, speaking loudly
neurological profiles), thus mapping on differ- when in close proximity, or otherwise becom-
ent levels of discourse, it is clear that these are ing very upset when other children are not
not mutually exclusive concepts. In other willing to play with them following their
words, they describe an overlapping group of agenda, which typically involves the pursuit of
children with social vulnerabilities. Therefore, play or games restricted to a narrow and often
it is likely more helpful if they are not used as developmentally atypical interest. The con-
competing diagnostic concepts but as method- trast in presentation in the home and family
ological approaches to better understand setting relative to social group settings is
aspects of the social, communicative, neuro- likely explained by the nature of the social
psychological, and neurologically based dis- partners. At home, interactions with parents
abilities evidenced in individuals with AS. are highly scaffolded by the adults, who mold
their behavior to adjust to the child’s ap-
CLINICAL FEATURES OF ASPERGER proaches, guiding the interaction and prevent-
SYNDROME ing communication breakdown. In social group
settings involving peers, there is a need for
Onset Patterns more conventional and socially appropriate be-
havior because peers show little tolerance for
Children with AS do not present with clini- deviation from usual expectations. It is typi-
cally significant delays in language acquisi- cal, therefore, for parents to first become con-
tion, cognitive development, or self-help cerned about their children’s behavior at the
skills in the first years of life. In fact, lan- time of transitions to school-like settings (e.g.,
guage acquisition, in terms of vocabulary and informal play groups, nursery, or preschool).
sentence construction, may be precocious in During preschool years, some children may
some cases and parents may report that the develop intense interests about which they
Asperger Syndrome 99

begin to learn a great deal. Some of these may and implied communications (e.g., signs of
be unusual for their age (e.g., geography, ar- boredom, haste to leave, and need for privacy).
rows, storm drains). They may acquire a Chronically frustrated by their repeated experi-
wealth of facts and information related to a ences of failure to engage others and form
given interest, choosing to talk a great deal to friendships, some individuals with AS develop
other children, who fail to show the same in- symptoms of a mood disorder that may require
terest and, consequently, distance themselves treatment, including medication. They also may
from such approaches. Typical experiences an- react inappropriately to, or fail to interpret the
choring a child’s understanding of his or her valence of, the context of the affective inter-
day (e.g., routines at preschool, transition action, often conveying a sense of insensitivity,
times, social rituals) may not convey a sense of formality, or disregard for the other person’s
familiarity, prompting the child to rely (often emotional expressions. They may be able to de-
precociously) on more formal definitions of scribe correctly, in a cognitive and often for-
experiences such as time (e.g., a 3-year-old malistic fashion, other people’s emotions,
may repeatedly ask, “What time is it?”), rigid expected intentions, and social conventions; yet,
rules (an explicitly and verbally defined they are unable to act on this knowledge in an
schedule), or repeated questions of adults. To intuitive and spontaneous fashion, thus losing
some children, these behaviors appear to be re- the tempo of the interaction. Their poor intu-
lated to incapacity to make intuitive sense of ition and lack of spontaneous adaptation are ac-
their own experiences, of nonexplicit nonver- companied by marked reliance on formalistic
bal cues conveyed by others, and of the rules of behavior and rigid social conventions.
changeable behavior of people who make sense This presentation is largely responsible for the
only within the overall context of a social situ- impression of social naiveté and behavioral
ation, yet another construct adding to the expe- rigidity that is so forcefully conveyed by these
rience of confusion in these children. The individuals (Klin & Volkmar, 1997).
pursuit of unchangeable phenomena (that re-
main the same and do not depend on context) Communication Patterns
may have earlier led the child to exhibit a fas-
cination with letters and numbers, which sub- Although significant abnormalities of speech
sequently may evolve into precocious reading and language are not typical of individuals
abilities and fascination with facts, all of with AS, there are at least three aspects of
which may serve to ground the child in what these individuals’ communication patterns that
otherwise would be an overwhelmingly per- are of clinical interest (Klin, 1994). First,
plexing, constantly shifting, and primarily im- speech may be marked by poor prosody, al-
plicit social environment. though inflection and intonation may not be as
rigid and monotonic as in autism (Ghaziuddin
Social Functioning & Gerstein, 1996). They often exhibit a con-
stricted range of intonation patterns that is
In some contrast to the social presentation in used with little regard to the communicative
autism, individuals with AS find themselves so- functioning of the utterance (e.g., assertions
cially isolated but are not usually withdrawn in of fact, humorous remarks). Rate of speech
the presence of other people, typically ap- may be unusual (e.g., too fast) or may lack in
proaching others but in an inappropriate or ec- fluency (e.g., jerky speech), and there is often
centric fashion. For example, they may engage poor volume modulation (e.g., voice is too loud
the interlocutor, usually an adult, in one-sided despite physical proximity to the conversa-
conversation characterized by long-winded, tional partner). The latter feature may be par-
pedantic speech about a favorite and often un- ticularly noticeable in the context of a lack of
usual and narrow topic. They may express inter- adjustment to the given social setting (e.g., in
est in friendships and in meeting people, but a library or a noisy crowd). Second, speech
their wishes are often thwarted by their awk- may often be tangential and circumstantial,
ward approaches and insensitivity to the other conveying a sense of looseness of associations
person’s feelings, intentions, and nonliteral and incoherence. Even though in a very small
100 Diagnosis and Classification

number of cases this symptom may be an indi- Motoric Difficulties


cator of a possible thought disorder, the lack of
contingency in speech is a result of the one- Individuals with AS often present with a his-
sided, egocentric conversational style (e.g., tory of motor difficulties and delayed acquisi-
unrelenting monologues about the names, tion of skills requiring sophisticated motor
codes, and attributes of innumerable TV sta- coordination, such as pedaling a bike, catching
tions in the country), failure to provide the a ball, opening jars, and climbing outdoor play
background for comments and to clearly de- equipment. They are often visibly awkward
marcate changes in topic, and failure to sup- and poorly coordinated and may exhibit un-
press the vocal output accompanying internal usual gait patterns, odd posture, poor manipu-
thoughts. Third, the communication style of lative and handwriting skills, and significant
individuals with AS is often characterized by deficits in visual-motor skills (Gillberg, 1990;
marked verbosity. The child may talk inces- Tantam, 1988a). When tested, children with
santly, usually about a favorite subject, often AS have been shown to experience difficulties
in complete disregard to whether the listener on measures of apraxia, balance, tandem gait,
might be interested, engaged, or attempting to and finger-thumb apposition, suggesting diffi-
interject a comment, or change the subject of culties in proprioception (Weimer, Schatz,
conversation. Despite such long-winded mono- Lincoln, Ballantyne, & Trauner, 2001). It is not
logues, the individual may never come to a clear, however, whether “clumsiness” or poor
point or conclusion. Attempts by the interlocu- motor coordination skills differentiate indi-
tor to elaborate on issues of content or logic or viduals with AS from those with HFA (Green,
to shift the interchange to related topics are Gilchrist, Burton, & Cox, 2002; Smith, 2000),
often unsuccessful. who may show similar difficulties.

Circumscribed Interests Comorbid Features

Individuals with AS typically amass a large From a clinical standpoint, the most common
amount of factual information about a topic in comorbid conditions impacting on individuals
a very intense fashion. The actual topic may with AS are anxiety and depression (Klin &
change from time to time but often dominates Volkmar, 1997). In many cases, anxiety seems
the content of social exchange. Frequently, the to be secondary to a sense of being over-
entire family may be immersed in the subject whelmed by the fast pace and competitive so-
for long periods of time. This behavior is pecu- cial demands of typical peer interactions
liar in the sense that oftentimes extraordinary coupled with a sense of lack of control over the
amounts of factual information are learned outcome of such social events. Similarly, de-
about very circumscribed topics (e.g., snakes, pression appears to emerge as a result of in-
names of stars, TV guides, deep fat fryers, creased awareness of repeated experiences of
weather information, personal information on failure despite an intent, which can be quite in-
members of Congress) without a genuine un- tense, to establish relationships, make friends,
derstanding of the broader phenomena in- or have a romantic experience. Research esti-
volved. This symptom may not always be easily mates of comorbid anxiety and/or depression
recognized in childhood because strong inter- in individuals with AS are as high as 65%
ests in certain topics, such as dinosaurs or (Ellis, Ellis, Fraser, & Deb, 1994; Fujikawa,
fashionable fictional characters, are so ubiqui- Kobayashi, Koga, & Murata, 1987; Ghaziud-
tous. However, in both younger and older chil- din, 2002; Ghaziuddin, Ghaziuddin, & Greden,
dren the special interests typically interfere 2002; Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-Mikhail, & Ghaz-
with learning in general because they absorb iuddin, 1998; Green et al., 2000; Howlin &
so much of the child’s attention and motiva- Goode, 1998). Some data suggest, however,
tion, and they have a very negative impact on that individuals with AS and HFA are equally
their ability to engage others in reciprocal so- at increased risk for problems with anxiety
cial interaction because the interest intrudes and depression, with no differences between
and often dominates conversation with others. the groups (Kim, Szatmari, Bryson, Streiner,
Asperger Syndrome 101

& Wilson, 2000). In some respects, these re- may lead them, for example, to make blunt re-
sults are difficult to reconcile with our experi- quests of a sexual nature; or their intense and
ence, according to which individuals with all-absorbing circumscribed interests may
autism are likely to suffer less anxiety and de- lead them to commit eccentric acts associated
pression because of their lack of involvement with those interests (e.g., accumulate clutter
with others, which in turn, makes them (1) less in the backyard of a multifamily home). More
vulnerable to the challenges inherent in trying typically, however, these individuals are too
to navigate a complex social world and (2) less naïve to become “competent criminals.” In
vulnerable to experiences of inadequacy re- fact, individuals with AS are much more likely
sulting from failure to establish relationships to be victims than victimizers due to their in-
despite repeated (and painful) attempts. ability to read the intents of others, which in a
Several other conditions have been described typical school environment may involve re-
in association with AS, although these descrip- peated mocking and cruel singling out of the
tions are more typical of early published reports child because of his or her eccentricities, poor
that tended to be case studies rather than case- grooming, or social clumsiness. Adults may
control series. These early reports emphasized eventually grow despondent in social situa-
possible associations with Tourette’s syndrome tions, eventually gravitating toward the pe-
(e.g., Gillberg & Rastam, 1992; Kerbeshian & riphery of society.
Burd, 1986; Littlejohns, Clarke, & Corbett, Another association suggested by early re-
1990; Marriage, Miles, Stokes, & Davey, 1993) ports was the presence of schizophrenia in
and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Thomsen, some individuals with AS. In fact, there was
1994). Reports of associations with psychotic some suggestion that AS might, in some sense,
conditions have included reports of psychotic be a “ bridging” condition between autism and
depression and bipolar disorder (manic depres- schizophrenia (Wolff, 1995). Though several
sive psychosis; Gillberg, 1985). research studies have suggested an association
Some reports, though primarily case stud- between the disorders (e.g., Nagy & Szatmari,
ies, have focused on conduct problems, more 1986; Tantam, 1991), the preponderance of
specifically on violent and criminal behavior empirical evidence fails to support this spec-
(e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1988; Everall & Le Cou- ulation. Ghaziuddin, Leininger, and Tsai
teur, 1990; Mawson, Grounds, & Tantam, (1995) found that patients with AS had
1985; Scragg & Shah, 1994; Tantam, 1988c; greater levels of disorganized thinking than an
Wing, 1986). However, the hypothesis that the HFA comparison group, but they were not
combination of high intelligence and verbal more likely to exhibit a thought disorder. Re-
skills with poor empathy and social cognition views of large case series reveal that schizo-
fosters violent or criminal behavior has not phrenia is no more common among individuals
been empirically borne out. Ghaziuddin and with AS than in the general population (Volk-
colleagues (1991) reviewed the literature and mar & Cohen, 1991). And in family genetic
found a lack of support for this speculation. studies, there is no evidence of larger aggrega-
Our own experience suggests that individuals tion of schizophrenia-related symptoms in
with AS do not present with antisocial or so- families of probands with ASD or of ASD-re-
ciopathic characteristics; the absence of em- lated symptoms in families of probands with
pathy connotes poor insight into the social and schizophrenia (see Chapter 16, Handbook,
emotional nature of other people, not an ab- this volume). Though research has not sup-
sence of compassion for their welfare. ported a relationship between AS and schizo-
Individuals with AS often transgress rules at phrenia, several factors have contributed to
school, with people, and in the community at this enduring misperception. The combina-
large, and their behavior may indeed lead to tion of excessive verbalization and poor social
formal encounters with school authorities or judgment and social monitoring in AS or the
law enforcement officers. However, they tendency of persons with AS to speak exces-
typically do not engage in these acts willfully sively about special interests may result in
or maliciously. Their social ineptitude and highly inappropriate and bizarre behavior that
unawareness of social rules and expectations mimics psychotic behavior (e.g., incoherent or
102 Diagnosis and Classification

accelerated speech). Additionally, there has working prevalence rate of 2 per 10,000 pend-
been an overreliance on single-case studies as ing further research. Prevalence estimates
well as some ascertainment biases (Bejerot & will always depend on the definition of AS
Duvner, 1995; Ryan, 1992; Taiminen, 1994). and the adoption of operationalized concepts
that can be applied by epidemiologists with a
PREVALENCE degree of reliability. Hence there is a need for
progress in defining consensual approaches to
Studies of prevalence in general, and of psy- this condition because, from a purely practi-
chiatric disorders in particular, are greatly cal standpoint, prevalence estimates have
affected by the quality and nature of the defi- profound implications for allocation of re-
nition used to single out cases from the gen- sources and service provision.
eral population (e.g., precise versus vague,
discrete versus overlapping with other condi- VALIDITY OF ASPERGER SYNDROME
tions). As discussed earlier, varying criteria
continue to be employed in the diagnosis of As amply documented in the previous sections,
AS, constituting a major confound in any at- the validity of AS as distinct from other condi-
tempt to interpret epidemiological studies of tions, notably HFA, but also PDD-NOS, re-
AS (Ehlers & Gillberg, 1993). Nevertheless, mains controversial (Ozonoff & Griffith, 2000;
consistent with the trend observed in ASDs in Volkmar & Klin, 2000). A lack of uniformity in
general (Fombonne & Tidmarsh, 2003) rates usage of the term also characterizes definitions
of AS appear to be on the rise. Twenty years adopted for the purpose of research, as differ-
ago, an epidemiological study in an area of ent sets of diagnostic criteria have been used by
London (Wing, 1981; Wing & Gould, 1979) different researchers with resulting complica-
reported a prevalence rate of 0.6 to 1.1 per tions for interpretation of research (Ghaziuddin
10,000. However, this study assessed only et al., 1992; Klin et al., in press). There is little
children under the age of 15 with mild mental disagreement about the fact that AS is on a phe-
retardation, clearly a minority among indi- nomenological continuum with autism, particu-
viduals assigned the label AS, thereby very larly in relation to the problems in the areas of
likely underestimating the true prevalence. social and communication functioning (Wing,
Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) reported rates of 1991). For example, within the DSM-III-R
10 to 26 children per 10,000 among children (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) di-
with normal intelligence, with an additional agnostic system, persons with AS would either
0.4 per 10,000 showing the combination of meet criteria for autistic disorder or would be
AS and mental retardation. A subsequent said to exhibit PDD-NOS (Tsai, 1992). What is
study (Ehlers & Gillberg, 1993) calculated a less clear is whether the condition is qualita-
minimum prevalence rate of 3.6 per 1,000 tively different from, rather than just a milder
children (7 to 16 years of age) for a diagnosis form of, autism.
of AS and 7.1 per 1,000 children when includ- Several studies investigating different as-
ing suspected cases. Fombonne and Tidmarsh pects of the disorder have attempted to
(2003) reviewed a number of epidemiological identify discriminating criteria between the
studies that included the diagnostic category two conditions with only mixed results to
of AS and reported a wide range of prevalence date. These studies have involved the neu-
rates, from 0.3 to 48.4 per 10,000, which ropsychological, social-cognitive, neurobio-
clearly reflected methodologic differences logical, genetic, and prognostic aspects of the
across studies. Illustrating a common problem disorders.
in epidemiological research, the highest esti-
mate derived from the study with the smallest Neuropsychological Studies
sample size. Making allowances for the
methodological shortcomings of the available A number of studies have attempted to dis-
studies and for the great ambiguity associ- tinguish AS from other ASDs, and HFA in
ated with the definition of the phenomenon it- particular, by demonstrating a pattern of neu-
self, Fombonne and Tidmarsh suggested a ropsychological performance that uniquely
Asperger Syndrome 103

characterizes the disorders (e.g., Wing, 1998). Coding Subtests). In contrast, individuals with
Though findings are not wholly consistent, autism displayed an isolated skill on a measure
these studies suggest that individuals with of visual parts-to-whole reasoning (Block De-
HFA tend to have greater impairment within sign subtest). Lincoln and colleagues (1998)
the domains of language and verbal compre- carried out a meta-analysis of several studies
hension (Lincoln, Allen, & Kilman, 1995; addressing neuropsychological profiles differ-
Siegel et al., 1996) while exhibiting relative entiating AS from HFA. Results indicated that
strength in nonverbal areas (Klin, Carter, & individuals with AS showed a pattern of
Sparrow, 1997). Several studies have empiri- stronger VIQ relative to PIQ. In contrast, indi-
cally demonstrated significant distinctions be- viduals with HFA exhibited the reverse pat-
tween groups. In one study (Klin, Volkmar, tern, with stronger nonverbal abilities and
Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Rourke, 1995), a group weaker verbal skills. The authors concluded
of individuals with AS and HFA of compara- that autism is characterized by impaired ver-
ble chronological age and full-scale IQ were bal skills, which are intact in AS. Miller and
compared in multiple domains of neuropsy- Ozonoff (2000) also compared groups with
chological function. On average, individuals HFA and AS with similar results but with an
with AS exhibited a significant VIQ-PIQ dif- important caveat. The AS group had higher
ferential with stronger verbal abilities, while VIQ and full-scale IQ and exhibited a greater
individuals in the HFA group tended to have difference between verbal and nonverbal abili-
comparable verbal and performance abilities. ties (with verbal abilities being stronger).
With respect to other domains of neuropsy- However, this study also observed better vi-
chological function, 11 areas were shown to sual-perceptual skills in individuals with AS,
discriminate between the two groups. Some suggesting a more general effect of higher per-
neuropsychological skills represented areas of formance or IQ in the group with AS rather
strength in AS and weakness in HFA, whereas than reverse profiles, suggesting that “AS is
the converse was true for other domains of simply high-IQ autism.” Evidence was sugges-
ability. Six areas of psychological deficit were tive but inconclusive regarding a trend toward
associated with a diagnosis of AS: fine and poorer motor performance in the group with
gross motor skills, visual motor integration, AS. Considering the fact that many studies
visual-spatial perception, nonverbal concept demonstrating this pattern of verbal strengths
formation, and visual memory. Five areas of specific to AS had used varying diagnostic
psychological deficits were negatively corre- criteria, Ghaziuddin and Mountain-Kimchi
lated with a diagnosis of AS: articulation, ver- (2004) compared AS with HFA using unmodi-
bal output, auditory perception, vocabulary, fied, official diagnostic criteria. The study
and verbal memory. Finally, all but three sub- found that, overall, individuals with AS had
jects with AS presented with a neuropsycho- higher VIQ with greater strengths in fund of
logical profile consistent with an NLD knowledge and vocabulary relative to individu-
(Rourke, 1989). In contrast, only one subject als with HFA. A recent review of the literature
in the HFA group did so. These results indi- (Reitzel & Szatmari, 2003) confirmed this pat-
cated an overlap between AS, but not HFA, tern of differential neuropsychological pro-
and NLD, suggesting an empirical distinction files, with individuals with HFA consistently
between AS and HFA based on neurocognitive demonstrating lower scores on measures of
profiles. verbal functioning. Importantly, however, it
Since this study was published, a number of was noted that studies have not consistently
publications have replicated, or partially repli- demonstrated that individuals with AS possess
cated, its findings. Ehlers et al. (1997) com- nonverbal weaknesses or increased spatial or
pared results on an intelligence test across motor problems relative to individuals with
groups with AS and autism. The authors found HFA, echoing Miller and Ozonoff ’s conclu-
that individuals with AS exhibited stronger sions (2000). In other words, differences in
verbal ability with a weakness in subtests mea- neuropsychological profiles were explained in
suring visual-spatial organization and grapho- terms of an overriding effect of higher IQ in in-
motor skills (WISC-R Object Assembly, dividuals with AS relative to those with HFA,
104 Diagnosis and Classification

rather than a double dissociation of strengths creased cognitive demands. Ozonoff and col-
and deficits suggested by the NLD model. leagues (1991a) found that executive function
Several studies have failed to detect signif- did not differ between individuals with AS and
icant differences in neuropsychological pro- HFA; however, it was the most reliable distinc-
files between individuals with AS and HFA. tion between both groups and typical controls,
One of the earliest investigations in this area suggesting the primacy of deficits in this area
was conducted by Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson, underlying a more broadly defined PDD group.
and Bartolucci (1990), who administered a The differences in results and conclusions
comprehensive test battery including intelli- reported in these studies of neurocognitive
gence, achievement, and neuropsychological functioning in AS and HFA most likely reflect
measures to groups with AS and HFA matched different diagnostic approaches and ascertain-
on full-scale IQ. Few differences were de- ment procedures adopted by the various
tected; the AS group tended to perform better groups of investigators. In general, studies
on a test of verbal concept formation (WISC- adopting the most stringent diagnostic proce-
R Similarities subtest), while the HFA group dures, that is, selecting the most phenomeno-
showed higher performance on a test of motor logically prototypical cases, found maximal
speed and coordination (Grooved Pegboard, distinctions between groups (e.g., Klin et al.,
nondominant hand). The authors concluded 1995; Lincoln et al., 1998). Studies that failed
that the lack of differentiation on the neu- to find distinctions between groups (e.g.,
rocognitive battery suggested that the AS and Ozonoff, Rogers, & Pennington, 1991; Szat-
HFA groups could be combined in a more gen- mari et al., 1990) tended to adopt broadened or
eral PDD category. A second study was con- otherwise modified definitions, thereby possi-
ducted by Ozonoff, Rogers, and Pennington bly reducing power to detect results ( however,
(1991). A neuropsychological battery was ad- see Miller & Ozonoff, 2000, for an exception).
ministered to groups with HFA and AS, This assumption, however, cannot be systemat-
matched on full-scale IQ and PIQ, as well as ically tested until investigators from different
chronological age. Although both groups dif- research groups begin to adopt consensual def-
fered on some measures from typical control initions and standardized procedures for case
subjects, they differed from each other only in assignment. For example, one study (Klin
regard to verbal memory, likely an artifact of et al., in press) applying three diagnostic
the significantly higher verbal skills in indi- schemes simultaneously to the same pool of
viduals with AS (Ozonoff, Rogers, & Penning- subjects with AS and HFA found significant
ton, 1991). The authors concluded that the two differences in IQ differential (VIQ relative to
groups could not be reliably differentiated in PIQ) when case assignment followed two of
terms of their neuropsychological profiles. the three diagnostic systems (the DSM-IV ap-
Though much of the research into neuropsy- proach and the unique features approach, as
chological distinctions has focused on verbal discussed previously), but not when case as-
versus nonverbal abilities, concurrent work has signment followed the third system (the early
addressed a particular aspect of neuropsycho- language approach).
logical skills, namely executive functions. One
marginally significant finding in Szatmari and Social-Cognitive Studies
colleagues’ (1990) study was that individuals
with autism performed less well than those A number of research studies have explored
with AS on a single measure of executive func- autistic children’s ability to impute mental
tions (Wisconsin Card Sort Test, perseverative states such as beliefs, desires, and intentions to
errors). Rinehart, Bradshaw, Tonge, Brereton, others and to themselves or to have a theory of
and Bellgrove (2002) assessed executive func- other people’s (and their own) subjectivity—a
tions using visual-spatial tasks subserved by theory of mind (ToM; Baron-Cohen, Tager-
fronto-striatal brain regions. On this measure, Flusberg, & Cohen, 1999). The major hypothe-
individuals with AS were found to have intact sis emerging from this work is that individuals
performance, while those with HFA exhibited with autism lack this capacity and that this
inhibitory deficits that became evident with in- fundamental deficit may explain Wing’s
Asperger Syndrome 105

(Wing & Gould, 1979) triad of symptoms emission tomography to examine brain activ-
defining autism, namely, impairment of social ity in individuals with AS during performance
and communicative functioning and imagina- of a ToM task. Patients with AS, like typical
tive activities (see Chapter 47, this Handbook, controls, showed selective activation in the
Volume 2, for a review). Problems in these medial prefrontal cortex; however, in subjects
abilities, as measured by joint attention and with AS the activity was localized in a
pretend play, are evident in children with slightly different region of the PFC ( lower
autism as early as 18 months (Baron-Cohen, and more anterior). This finding is consistent
Cox, Baird, Sweettenham, & Nightingale, with behavioral data suggesting existing, but
1996; Charman et al., 1997). abnormal, ToM abilities in AS.
Several studies have found evidence sug- Though much research suggests differen-
gesting that differential abilities in ToM may tial ToM abilities in individuals with AS and
help to distinguish AS from autism. Ozonoff, autism, several studies have obtained results
Pennington, and Rogers (1991) and Ozonoff, inconsistent with this conclusion. Dahlgren
Rogers, and Pennington (1991) investigated and Trillingsgaard (1996) found that individ-
“ first- and second-order theory of mind.” uals with AS demonstrated intact ToM skills
First-order attributions refer to a situation in relative to typical individuals, but so did the
which a subject must attribute a mental HFA group; ToM performance was not useful
state—for example, a false belief—to another in differentiating between the two diagnoses.
person, whereas second-order attributions re- A third pattern of results was obtained by
quire recursive thinking about mental states, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, and Jolliffee
in which a subject is required to predict one (1997), who found that individuals with AS
person’s thought about another person’s and HFA were comparably impaired on ad-
thoughts. First-order tasks begin to be mas- vanced tasks of ToM.
tered by age 4 in normally developing children, Although at least some individuals with AS
whereas the ability to make second-order at- demonstrate intact abilities in many of these
tributions develops later, at around the age of 7 laboratory studies, their ability to take the per-
(Perner & Wimmer, 1985). In this study, the spective of another person—to adequately
autistic group exhibited significant impair- evaluate other people’s interests, beliefs, in-
ment in relation to both the AS and an age- and tentions and feelings—is typically impaired in
IQ-matched control group, and the AS group real-life contexts. This observation was origi-
showed intact ToM abilities. Bowler (1992) nally made by Asperger himself (1944) and
also found that, on similar tasks, individuals was reiterated by Van Krevelen (1971), who
with AS showed no impairment in ToM abili- captured this phenomenon in terms of a lack of
ties relative to both IQ-matched typical indi- an intuitive understanding of other people’s
viduals and a group of schizophrenic patients. social behavior or to avail themselves from
Ziatas, Durkin, and Pratt (2003) reported that empathic feelings and from negotiating social
children with AS showed lesser impairments interaction by means of quick-paced, nonver-
in mentalizing abilities than children with bal means. In this context, individuals with AS
autism. Several publications indicate that were said to mediate their social and emo-
these patterns may be evident early in develop- tional exchange through explicit verbal and
ment; assessments at 18 months failed to de- logical means, cognitively, rigidly, and in a
tect impairments in joint attention and pretend rule-governed fashion, rather than affectively,
play in children subsequently diagnosed with intuitively, and in a self-adjusting fashion (see
AS (Baird et al., 2000; Cox et al., 1999). Stud- also Klin, Jones, Schultz, & Volkmar, 2003).
ies of adults have also indicated that patients Studies to date offer provocative intima-
with AS exhibit more subtle impairments in tions of ToM as a useful differentiator between
ToM abilities, as demonstrated by inconsistent AS and autism, but results are not fully consis-
or delayed responses on more sophisticated tent with clinical observations of social experi-
and naturalistic assessments (Royers, Buysse, ences of people with AS. Furthermore, these
Ponnet, & Pichal, 2001; Kaland et al., 2002). findings are vulnerable to several methodologi-
One study (Happé et al., 1996) used positron cal criticisms. Frith (2004) suggested that
106 Diagnosis and Classification

findings pointing to a milder impairment in fast, simultaneous, holistic, and intuitive reac-
ToM among individuals with AS may simply tions (e.g., highly synchronized and constantly
reflect high verbal ability and intelligence; changing facial expressions, posture, and
these cognitive characteristics provide indi- prosodic cues among a host of other dynamic
viduals with AS with an advantage in that they elements). Thus, traditional ToM tasks may
may be able to succeed on ToM tasks by means oversimplify social problem solving, creating
of well-reasoned responses based on logical an illusion of competence not commensurate
inference, rather than true social intuition. In- with demonstrated abilities in real-life social
deed, several studies of ToM abilities have contexts. Aware of this problem, Tager-Flusberg
highlighted the important role played by verbal (2001) contrasted two aspects of ToM compe-
skills on subjects’ performance (e.g., Happé, tence: first, the ability to successfully acquire
1995; Sparrevohn & Howie, 1995). To avoid social information from the environment
the formulation of an explicit task and possible (social perception) and, second, the ability to
verbal mediation, several studies have exam- reason about it (social cognition). She hypothe-
ined the tendency to attribute social meaning sized that autism entails impairment in both
(including ToM) to ambiguous visual displays aspects, but AS involves only problems with
(geometric shapes moving and interacting in a social perception. This conceptualization of-
“social manner ”; e.g., Klin, 2000), showing fers a potential explanation for the problems
significant deficits in ToM skills in individu- experienced by individuals with AS in real life
als with AS, which were similar to those with that are not replicated in lab studies; real-life
HFA. One study (Grossman, Klin, Carter, & situations entail fluid, ambiguous, contextually
Volkmar, 2000) used a paradigm deliberately defined information, while laboratory studies
created to test the impact of verbal mediation tend to provide more explicit information.
in processing social stimuli. In this case, the These criticisms indicate that to truly as-
task involved the processing of facial expres- sess ToM ability as a differentiating factor be-
sions, rather than ToM, although its results are tween AS and HFA, there is a need for more
relevant to the present discussion. Individuals naturalistic and ecologically valid studies. In-
with AS were found to be particularly suscep- vestigators interested in ToM capacities must
tible to language interference in their perfor- devise experimental situations less amenable
mance when facial expressions were presented to logical and verbal solutions. Optimally,
simultaneously with mismatching written ver- these should involve processing of more visual
bal labels. While typical controls’ accuracy rather than verbal stimuli and more naturalis-
and reaction time when identifying facial ex- tic and socially contextualized rather than ab-
pressions were by and large unaffected by this stract and logical situations, and processing
“strooplike” phenomenon, this was not so for time (i.e., latency of response) should be an
the individuals with AS, for whom the faces important parameter to be considered given
appeared less salient than the verbal labels. that, clinically, individuals with AS typically
In this context, Klin, Jones, Schultz, and cannot avail themselves of their formal social
Volkmar (2002) proposed that the success evi- knowledge in quick-paced, simultaneously
denced by individuals with AS in laboratory shifting, social situations.
measures may be a function of the poor eco-
logical validity of the paradigms used. In other Medical and Neurobiological Studies
words, these individuals may be able to “solve”
social problems if they are explicitly formu- A number of studies have attempted to differ-
lated to them, whereas in real life there is a entiate AS from HFA based on neurobiological
need to naturally seek salient social cues and measures. Several reports of neurobiological
generate the problem to be solved, and many abnormalities associated with AS have ap-
such “problems” are not necessarily amenable peared, although no consensus has been
to logical translation or verbal mediation be- reached because many of these reports have
cause this process would imply very slow, se- been case studies or involved very small sam-
quential, piecemeal, and laborious processing, ples and because of the ubiquitous nosologic
whereas real-life social situations demand issues discussed earlier. Wing (1981) noted a
Asperger Syndrome 107

higher than expected frequency of perinatal same area, although it was larger on the right
problems (nearly half of her original sample) side; his images also showed decreased tissue
among this population. However, Gillberg and in the anterior-inferior right temporal lobe,
Gillberg (1989) concluded that reduced obstet- suggesting an atrophic process or a regional
ric optimality was more common in autism neurodevelopmental growth failure. Both fa-
than in AS. Other reports have associated cer- ther and son exhibited a similar neuropsycho-
tain medical conditions with individual cases logical profile, and the similarity of brain
of AS—for example, aminoaciduria (Miles & anomalies and symptoms is suggestive of po-
Capelle, 1987) and ligamentous laxity (Tan- tential familial transmission.
tam, Evered, & Hersov, 1990). Casanova, Bux- A more limited set of studies has employed
hoeveden, Switala, and Roy (2002) found functional imaging and spectroscopy to
abnormalities in the organization of mini- demonstrate differences in brain function and
columns in both AS and autism, suggesting neurotransmitter concentrations between indi-
common underlying neural pathology. A case viduals with AS and typical controls. Murphy
series by Gillberg (1989) reported high fre- and colleagues (2002) used proton magnetic
quencies of medical anomalies, also common resonance spectroscopy to examine the rela-
to both diagnostic groups; however, these re- tionship between abnormalities in frontal and
sults have been challenged (Rutter, Bailey, parietal lobes and clinical symptomatology.
Bolton, & Le Couteur, 1994). Compared to typical controls, patients with
Neuroimaging studies of AS and autism AS had higher levels of N-acetylaspartate
have become one of the most prolific areas of (NAA), creatine and phosphocreatine, and
research in the field. Nevertheless, there is choline, neurotransmitters that serve as indi-
scant data to differentiate neuroanatomical cators of neuronal density, mitochondrial
structures and functions between AS and metabolism, phosphate metabolism, and mem-
autism. Most studies have sampled one group brane turnover. Furthermore, there were posi-
or the other or collapsed them into an ASD tive correlations between neurotransmitter
group. Some studies have, however, focused on levels and clinical presentation: Higher levels
individuals with AS as compared to typical of prefrontal NAA were associated with obses-
controls and found discrepancies in brain sional behavior, and increased prefrontal
structure. Berthier, Starkstein, and Leiguarda choline was associated with social function.
(1990) reported MRI results indicating left The authors concluded that individuals with
frontal macrogyria and bilateral opercular AS exhibit abnormalities in neuronal integrity
polymicrogyria in patients with AS. These of the prefrontal cortex, which relates to their
findings were linked to cortical migration ab- unique pattern of symptoms. McKelvey, Lam-
normalities in a first-degree relative with bert, Mottron, and Shevell (1995) reported on
bipolar disorder. Other studies have reported three patients with abnormal right hemisphere
gray tissue anomalies in subjects with AS as functioning on SPECT imaging, a finding
compared to typical controls (McAlonan et al., consistent with the NLD neurocognitive
2002). Several case studies have revealed left model of AS.
temporal lobe damage (Jones & Kerwin, 1990; A handful of studies have attempted to study
CT scan) and left occipital hypoperfusion differential brain structure in individuals with
(Ozbayrak, Kapucu, Erdem, & Aras, 1991; AS and autism. Studies have suggested that,
SPECT) in patients with AS. In a case study across diagnosis, higher functioning individuals
by Volkmar and colleagues (1996), a father on the autism spectrum tend not to exhibit hy-
and son with AS showed virtually identical ab- poplasia of the neocerebellar vermis, a finding
normalities on their MRIs. The father’s im- more common in lower functioning people on
ages showed a large, bilateral, V-shaped wedge the spectrum that may be associated more with
of missing tissue just superior to the ascending mental retardation than with autism (e.g., Piven
ramus of the Sylvian fissure, at about the level et al., 1992; Piven, Palmer, Jacobi, Childress, &
where the middle frontal gyrus normally inter- Arndt, 1997). In some contrast, Lincoln and
sects with the precentral sulcus. The son’s im- colleagues (1998) reported that individuals
ages showed similar dysmorphology in the with autism (relative to AS) exhibited more
108 Diagnosis and Classification

consistent pathology of the cerebellar vermis, categorical subgrouping, dimensional analyses


thinner posterior corpus callosum, and (e.g., association between quantified brain ac-
smaller anterior corpus callosum. One recent tivation and measurable aspects of the pheno-
study has provided exciting information about type) may prove more powerful in the long
potential neuroanatomical distinctions be- run. Equally important are the initial efforts to
tween subgroups of ASDs. Lotspeich et al. move to measures of interconnectivity of key
(2004) compared individuals with HFA and brain systems subserving cognitive and social
low-functioning autism (LFA) and AS. The processing (see Chapter 19, this Handbook,
study indicated enlarged cerebral gray matter this volume).
volumes in HFA and LFA relative to controls.
The AS group showed enlargement that was in- Genetics
termediate with respect to patients with
autism and typical controls but was not signif- From its original description by Asperger
icantly different from either. The authors in- (1944), there has been evidence for familial
ferred that cerebral gray matter may correlate vulnerabilities in AS. As noted, Asperger ob-
with symptom severity, suggesting that AS served similar characteristics in family mem-
represents the mild end of the continuum. This bers, particularly in fathers. This genetic
study also detected differential relationships predisposition is in keeping with more recent
between brain structure and neurocognitive findings demonstrating that the ASDs are
function; cerebral white matter volume was among the most heritable of psychiatric con-
positively correlated with PIQ only in subjects ditions (Rutter, Bailey, Simonoff, & Pickles,
with AS, and gray matter correlated with PIQ 1997), although knowledge about patterns of
in both HFA and AS, but in different direc- inheritability and genetic etiologies is still
tions (positive correlation in AS, negative cor- limited (see Chapter 16, this Handbook, this
relation in HFA). Another study added a volume). Given the heterogeneity of the
comorbid component, namely Tourette’s syn- ASDs, it is of little surprise, but of great im-
drome (TS) to this discussion. Berthier, Bayes, portance, to consider the fact that the ASDs
and Tolosa (1993) used MRI to study brain are the more severe manifestations of a
structure in patients with comorbid AS and TS broader and possibly more prevalent pheno-
and with patients with TS alone. Results re- type of social-communicative difficulties and
vealed cortical and subcortical abnormalities behavioral rigidities (e.g., Bailey et al., 1995;
in 5 of the 7 patients with AS and TS, but only Le Couteur et al., 1996).
in 1 of the 9 patients with TS only. Consistent with Asperger’s original obser-
Findings to date do not present a clear pic- vation, a number of case reports have reported
ture of the neurobiological underpinnings of similar traits in family members, especially
AS or replicated distinctions among the ASDs. among fathers (Bowman, 1988; DeLong &
In many ways, issues of definition of the ASD Dwyer, 1988; Gillberg, Gillberg, & Steffen-
subgroups make it impossible to derive firm burg, 1992; Volkmar et al., 1996). Others have
conclusions from the available data. Addition- reported data on social vulnerabilities in other
ally, there are methodological issues related to relatives. Multiple births concordant for AS
intersite differences in imaging techniques have been observed (Burgoine & Wing, 1983).
(e.g., Lotspeich et al., 2004). Nevertheless, Volkmar and colleagues (1997) examined fam-
this growing volume of literature holds great ily history data and found that nearly half of
promise, particularly in those studies in which the families surveyed reported information
a more specific brain-behavior relationship is positive to AS, ASDs, and the broader autism
found. In this context, neuroimaging studies, phenotype in first-degree relatives, most com-
and particularly functional MRI research, monly among males. The probands with AS
should be guided by neuropsychological and were more likely (relative to general popula-
social-cognitive models that hold the potential tion rates) to have a sibling diagnosed with
to explain core aspects of the presentation of autism or an ASD; this was twice as likely in
individuals with ASDs. And although analyses male siblings as in female siblings. Rates of
have often been performed on the basis of autism and ASD were also increased in first
Asperger Syndrome 109

cousins. Klin et al. (in press) reported similar and hypothesized neuropsychological, social-
results. When using the unique features diag- cognitive, or neurobiological patterns.
nostic scheme (as described earlier), it was of
interest that the rates of ASDs or the broader Outcome
autism phenotype in parents and grandparents
(males and females) of probands with AS was Asperger’s (1944) initial description pre-
over triple the rate found for the same relatives dicted a positive outcome for many of his pa-
of probands with HFA (17% versus 5%). This tients, largely based on his hope that they
finding suggests even stronger genetic contri- would be able to use their special talents for
butions in families of probands of AS relative the purpose of obtaining employment and lead-
to those of probands with autism (Volkmar & ing self-supporting lives. His observation of
Klin, 2000). similar traits in family members encouraged
Though still limited, there are some reports the notion that patients could marry and raise
of autism and AS co-occurring in different families. Although his account was tempered
members of the same family (e.g., Wing, somewhat by experience (Asperger, 1979),
1981). In our clinical experience and in a pre- Asperger continued to believe that a more pos-
liminary report (Klin et al., in press), we have itive outcome was a central criterion differen-
observed some sibships in which one brother tiating individuals with his syndrome from
has autism and the other has AS. The fact that those with Kanner’s autism. In considering
the triplets reported by Burgoine and Wing autism inclusive of low-functioning cases, this
(1983) had some aspects of their history more would certainly be the case (Gillberg, 1991).
similar to patterns observed in autism also When compared with individuals with HFA,
suggests a common familial link between outcome of individuals with AS is more likely
autism and AS. Collectively, these findings to be more positive on the basis of case re-
point to genetic mechanisms in common not ports, anecdotal evidence, and some prelimi-
only between autism and AS but also among nary studies, but data supporting this
all ASDs (Frith, 2004). hypothesis are still scant. More positive out-
In addition to evidence of familial patterns comes for individuals with AS could simply re-
in AS, direct evidence of a genetic component flect the overall effects of the preserved
has been published. Specific genetic abnor- language and cognitive skills required for the
malities have been reported in case studies of diagnosis (for at least the first years of life).
patients with AS—for example, one case with Nevertheless, some studies support the notion
a balanced de novo translocation (Anneren, of differential outcomes in individuals with
Dahl, Uddenfeldt, & Janols, 1995), one case AS and HFA. Several studies examined out-
with an autosomal fragile site (Saliba & Grif- come in young adults with AS (Newson, Daw-
fiths, 1990), and a possible association with son, & Everaard, 1984/1985; Tantam, 1991)
fragile X syndrome (Bartolucci & Szatmari, and reported that although most remained at
1987). Tentler and colleagues (2003) found home despite high cognitive potential, a mi-
two patients with AS with balanced transloca- nority of them had married and/or held regular
tions on chromosome 17, perhaps suggesting a employment. Though a comparable sample
candidate sequence. Although these findings with autism was not assessed, this outcome ap-
are only preliminary, they provide guidelines pears better than typically expected for indi-
for future research studies and concrete viduals with HFA. Gillberg (1998) suggested
sources for explicating the shared genetic that patients with AS experience more positive
components of AS and other ASDs. outcomes, especially in the domains of acade-
Fast-accruing genetic data have the promise mics and self-help skills. In contrast, Szat-
of moving research on ASD subtypes from the mari, Bartolucci, et al. (1989b) compared
rather imprecise phenotypic comparisons to outcomes in children with AS and HFA and
date to potential endophenotypes mediating noted minimal differences. Others have re-
syndrome expression. For this to happen, how- ported that older individuals and adults with
ever, there is a need for much more refined phe- AS and HFA are so similar that they are diffi-
notypic characterization of both core symptoms cult or impossible to distinguish on the basis of
110 Diagnosis and Classification

outcome (Gilchrist et al., 2001; Howlin, 2003; Summary of Validation Studies


Szatmari et al., 2000). All of these three stud-
ies, however, used the early language approach A common thread throughout the reviewed val-
as a diagnostic scheme for case assignment, idation studies is the sore need for consensual
which, at least in our experience, yields the and reliable diagnostic criteria for AS and
least contrast between the two groups (Klin demonstration of the fidelity of the concept
et al., in press). A recent study (Szatmari, across sites. Future research should attempt to:
Bryson, Boyle, et al., 2003) looked into predic-
tors of outcome by ASD subtype by following 1. Ensure that dif ferences between AS and
children with autism and AS between the ages other conditions are independent of the cri-
of 4 and 6 years through 10 to 13 years using teria defining disorders. Historically, stud-
adaptive behavior (i.e., levels of ability, which ies have employed circular reasoning, with
are norm-based) and measures of autistic results reflecting the diagnostic criteria
symptoms (i.e., levels of disability) and found rather than providing additional informa-
that the power of prediction was stable over tion. Dependent variables should be inde-
time but different for the two groups. The au- pendent of (i.e., not a function of, not
thors found that the association between lan- highly correlated with, not associated with,
guage skills and outcome was stronger in the and not a result of ) diagnostic criteria.
autism group than in the AS group, suggesting 2. Adopt an agreed-on definition, and opera-
that for the individuals with AS, higher lan- tionalize it in much greater detail (including
guage skills did not confer any advantage. Of the process used to obtain data used in diag-
great practical importance, however, was that nostic decision making). One possibility is
the explanatory power of the predictor vari- to modify well-proven diagnostic instru-
ables was greater for communication and so- mentation such as the Autism Diagnostic
cial skills than for autistic symptoms, Interview-Revised (Rutter, Le Couteur, &
suggesting the possibility that adaptive compe- Lord, 2003) and the Autism Diagnostic
tence ( levels of self-sufficiency) and autistic Observation Schedule (Lord, Rutter,
symptomatology ( levels of disability) may DiLavore, & Risi, 1999; see Chapter 28,
constitute rather independent facets of these this Handbook, Volume 2) to include an “AS
children’s development. This finding was cor- module” covering the more unique features
roborated in a recent study (Klin, Saulnier, of AS in every domain of characterization
Sparrow, Cicchetti, Lord, & Volkmar, in (i.e., onset, social, communication, and
press). From a practical perspective, this point narrow interests and behaviors), as well as
suggests that intervention programs should differential diagnostic algorithms keyed to
emphasize both reduction of symptoms and the consensually adopted defining criteria.
improvement of self-sufficiency skills given It is indeed very unlikely that any of the is-
that the focus on the former does not necessar- sues related to the nosologic status of AS
ily imply advancement of the latter. discussed in this chapter will be resolved in
Despite these interesting preliminary data, the absence of an effort of this kind.
it is important to recognize that the status of 3. Include larger samples and multisite com-
research into differential outcomes is still parisons. Much of our understanding of the
quite limited (Howlin, 1997). This is particu- putative differences between AS and HFA
larly regrettable given that of all the possible stems from reports involving case studies
reasons to separate these two conditions, dif- or small case series. Larger studies will not
ferential outcome would probably be the one only improve power to detect differences
with the greatest practical relevance. There is but also increase the likelihood that results
a need to ensure, however, that well-known are meaningful and replicable. And there
mediators of outcome, and particularly IQ and have been striking differences in findings
language levels past the first years of life, are across research groups, which may reflect
not components of the diagnostic definitions, not only different methodologies but also
which would create circularity in the study de- different biases in subject recruitment. To
sign and would likely result in spurious differ- overcome this critical difficulty, there is a
ences between the two groups. need for cross-site studies using tools of the
Asperger Syndrome 111

kinds described earlier within a common individuals with AS. For a more comprehen-
effort to carry out a well-controlled, multi- sive discussion of the issues outlined here, the
center field trial. reader is referred to a more complete text
4. Avoid oversimplifications of key clinical (Klin, Sparrow, Marans, Carter, & Volkmar,
phenomena. Historically, research on AS 2000) and Chapter 29, Volume 2, of this
has failed to acknowledge, describe, opera- Handbook.
tionalize, or quantify the complex set of AS, like the other PDDs, involves delays
clinical phenomena involved in key con- and deviant patterns of behavior in multiple
cepts defining AS such as circumscribed areas of functioning. Thorough evaluation of
interests, social awkwardness in the pres- all relevant domains requires different areas of
ence of social motivation, pedantic style, expertise, including overall developmental
and verbosity as a strategy for social adap- functioning, neuropsychological features, lan-
tation among others. Many studies gloss guage and communication skills, adaptive
over these phenomena, providing limited or functioning, and behavioral status. Therefore,
no description of the sample beyond rather the clinical assessment of individuals with this
vague statements. disorder is most effectively conducted by an
5. Ensure the achievement of, and report data experienced, interdisciplinary team.
on, interrater reliability of procedures used In the majority of cases, a comprehensive
in diagnostic process (e.g., semistructured interdisciplinary assessment will involve the
interviews, best-estimate diagnoses) and, if following components: a thorough develop-
possible, document the entire process to mental and health history, psychological and
make possible the replication of the various communication assessments, and a diagnostic
procedures by members of a dif ferent re- work-up including differential diagnosis.
search team. Further consultation regarding behavioral
management, motor disabilities, possible neu-
Additionally, it may be useful for investiga- rological concerns, comorbidities and associ-
tors to depart from the prevalent model of vali- ated psychopharmacology, and assessment
dation research in which data are collected and related to advanced studies or vocational
compared on the basis of clinical groups training may also be needed. Given the pre-
formed by a priori diagnostic assignments. vailing difficulties in the definition of AS and
Because the validity of ASD subtypes is still the great heterogeneity of the condition, it is
uncertain (see Chapter 1, this Handbook, this crucial that the aim of the clinical assessment
volume), much could be learned by reversing be a comprehensive and detailed profile of the
the process. Researchers could assess meaning- individual’s assets, deficits, and challenges,
ful and interesting patterns (e.g., detailed onset rather than simply a diagnostic label. Effective
and early development patterns, specific neu- educational and treatment programs can be de-
ropsychological, social-cognitive, or neurobio- vised only on the basis of such a profile, given
logical profiles) and then carefully chart the the need to address specific deficits while cap-
range of phenotypic expressions associated with italizing on the person’s various resources and
them. This approach would redefine phenotype strengths.
as a dependent variable and open the door for The psychological assessment aims at es-
the use of statistical techniques to empirically tablishing the overall level of intellectual func-
define the existence of true categories. tioning, profiles of psychomotor functioning,
verbal and nonverbal cognitive strengths and
CLINICAL ASSESSMENT OF weaknesses, style of learning, and adaptive be-
INDIVIDUALS WITH ASPERGER haviors (or independent living skills). At a
SYNDROME minimum, the psychological assessment should
include assessments of intelligence and adap-
The topic of clinical assessment of children tive functioning, although the assessment of
with ASD is reviewed in much greater detail more detailed neuropsychological skills can be
in Chapters 28 through 32, Volume 2, of this of great help to further delineate the child’s
Handbook. In this section, we briefly discuss profiles of strengths and deficits (e.g., in re-
assessment procedures uniquely relevant to gard to organizational skills). A description of
112 Diagnosis and Classification

results should include not only quantified in- skills (e.g., understanding of the language of
formation but also a judgment as to how repre- mental states including intentions, emotions,
sentative the child’s performance was during and beliefs), reciprocity, and rules of conver-
the assessment procedure and a description of sation.
the conditions that are likely to foster optimal The diagnostic assessment should integrate
and diminished performance. For example, the information obtained in all components of the
child’s responses to the amount of structure comprehensive evaluation, with a special em-
imposed by the adult, the optimal pace for phasis on developmental history and current
presentation of tasks, successful strategies to symptomatology. It should include observations
facilitate learning from modeling and demon- of the child during more and less structured pe-
strations, and effective ways of containing off- riods. This effort should take advantage of ob-
task and maladaptive behaviors such as servations in all settings, including the clinic’s
cognitive and behavioral rigidity (e.g., verbal reception area (e.g., contacts with other chil-
perseverations, perfectionism), distractibility dren or with family members), the halls (e.g.,
(e.g., difficulty inhibiting irrelevant re- how the child interacts initially with the exam-
sponses, tangentiality), and anxiety are all im- iners), as well as in the testing room during
portant observations that can be extremely breaks, periods of silence, or otherwise un-
useful for designing and optimizing interven- structured situations. Often, the child’s dis-
tion programs. Within the psychological as- ability is much more apparent during such
sessment, particular emphasis should be periods in which the child is not given any in-
placed on the assessment of adaptive function- struction and has no adult-imposed expecta-
ing, which refers to capacities for personal and tion as to how to behave. Specific areas for
social self-sufficiency in real-life situations. observation and inquiry include the patient’s
The importance of this component of the clini- patterns of special interest and leisure time,
cal assessment cannot be overemphasized. Its social and affective presentation, quality of at-
aim is to obtain a measure of the child’s typi- tachment to family members, development of
cal patterns of functioning in familiar and rep- peer relationships and friendships, capacities
resentative environments such as the home and for self-awareness, perspective-taking and
the school, which may contrast markedly with level of insight into social and behavioral prob-
the demonstrated level of performance and lems, typical reactions in novel situations, and
presentation in the clinic. It provides the clini- ability to intuit another person’s feelings and
cian with an essential indicator of the extent to infer another person’s intentions and beliefs.
which the child is able to use his or her poten- Problem behaviors that are likely to interfere
tial (as measured in the assessment) in the pro- with remedial programming should be noted
cess of adaptation to environmental demands. (e.g., anxiety, temper tantrums). The child’s
A large discrepancy between intellectual level ability to understand ambiguous, nonliteral
and adaptive level signifies that a priority communications (particularly teasing and sar-
should be made of instruction within the con- casm) should be further examined, particu-
text of naturally occurring situations to foster larly in regard to the child’s patterns of
and facilitate the use of skills to enhance qual- response (e.g., misunderstandings of such
ity of life. communications may elicit aggressive behav-
The communication assessment should ex- iors). Other areas of observation involve the
amine nonverbal forms of communication presence of obsessions or compulsions, ritual-
(e.g., gaze, gestures), nonliteral language (e.g., ized behaviors, depression and panic attacks,
metaphor, irony, absurdities, and humor), integrity of thought, and reality testing.
suprasegmental aspects of speech (e.g., patterns One important aspect of the diagnostic
of inflection, stress, and volume modulation), characterization of individuals with AS relates
pragmatics (e.g., turn taking, sensitivity to cues to the need to differentiate diagnostic instru-
provided by the interlocutor), and content, ments from diagnostic screeners. The former
coherence, and contingency of conversation. typically involve comprehensive, semistruc-
Particular attention should be given to persever- tured procedures including both information
ation on circumscribed topics, metalinguistic provided by parents or caregivers and direct
Asperger Syndrome 113

examination and aim at producing a detailed more optimistic about individuals with AS
profile of developmental patterns and current given the common finding of relatively pre-
behavioral presentation needed for diagnostic served social motivation. Unfortunately, there
assignment. The latter typically involve the is currently a paucity of systematic data
completion of a brief checklist by parents, demonstrating the effectiveness of particular
caregivers, teachers, or other professionals, or interventions, although some progress has
by self-report, and aim at identifying those been made in this area (see Klin & Volkmar,
with a higher probability of having the condi- 2003, for a summary of treatment studies and
tion from the general population. It is critical of treatment approaches). Medications may be
to emphasize that screeners are not meant to effective in treating associated features of the
replace the diagnostic process. Rather, they disorder, such as inattention, anxiety, or de-
are meant to provide the basis for further refer- pression, although psychopharmacology can-
ral. Currently, the only diagnostic instrument not yet target core impairments in AS or, for
available that considers the distinction of that matter, other ASDs. In recent years, clini-
autism from AS is the Asperger Syndrome cians and educators experienced with this
(and high-functioning autism) Diagnostic In- population have published a number of vol-
terview (ASDI; Gillberg, Gillberg, Rastam, & umes proffering useful education and inter-
Wentz, 2001), although there are no data sup- vention strategies (e.g., Attwood, 1998; Bashe
porting this instrument’s validity insofar as & Kirby, 2001; Howlin, 1999; Myles & Simp-
the distinction between AS and HFA is con- son, 1997; Ozonoff, Dawson, & McPartland,
cerned. There are no other diagnostic instru- 2002; Powers & Poland, 2002; Wing, 2001). In
ments developed for the specific purpose of general, recommended interventions focus on:
subtyping the ASDs into autism, AS, and (1) devising strategies to take advantage of the
PDD-NOS. As mentioned earlier, there seems unique constellation of strengths characteris-
to be a great need for modification of current tic of AS to compensate for areas of difficulty
instruments to make possible this differentia- and (2) modifying contexts (i.e., environ-
tion, although this recommendation is closely ments) to maximally support the learning and
tied to other developments in validity studies behavioral styles of this population. The rec-
of AS. In contrast, there are a large number of ommended content or treatments should be
diagnostic screeners purported to identify in- tailored, based on a thorough assessment, to
dividuals with AS, some of which appear to be the individual needs of the patient. However,
of help in identifying vulnerabilities within nearly universally applicable foci include ac-
the autism spectrum, but none has been shown quisition of basic social and communication
to differentiate (nor has that been a stated ob- skills, adaptive functioning, organizational
jective) individuals with AS from those with skills, and, depending on what is developmen-
autism or PDD-NOS. tally appropriate, academic or vocational
skills. It is also crucial that any intervention
TREATMENT AND INTERVENTIONS program incorporate techniques to encourage
generalization of acquired skills to ensure the
The topic of treatment and interventions is use of learned abilities in novel contexts.
briefly summarized here, with an emphasis on
issues that are uniquely relevant to individuals Securing Services
with AS. More comprehensive guidelines are
reviewed in Chapters 33 through 44, Volume 2, Despite the great progress in dissemination of
of this Handbook. awareness and understanding of AS, the au-
As in autism, treatment of AS is essen- thorities deciding on eligibility for services
tially supportive and symptomatic, and to a may fail to appreciate the severity of the vari-
great extent, overlaps with the treatment ous disabilities exhibited by individuals with
guidelines applicable to individuals with HFA AS. Proficient verbal skills, overall IQ usually
(Mesibov, 1992). Though a similar set of rec- within the normal range, and a solitary
ommendations applies to individuals with lifestyle often mask outstanding deficiencies
both conditions, interventionists are often observed primarily in novel or otherwise
114 Diagnosis and Classification

socially demanding situations, thus decreasing ical profile of assets and deficits; specific in-
the perception of the very salient needs for tervention techniques should be similar to
supportive intervention. Hence there is a need those usually employed for many subtypes of
for active participation on the part of the clini- learning disabilities, with an effort to circum-
cian, together with parents and possibly an ad- vent the identified difficulties by means of
vocate, to forcefully pursue the student’s compensatory strategies relying on areas of
access to appropriately supportive programs. strength, in this case, usually verbal abilities.
The formalization of the diagnosis in ICD-10 If significant motor and visual-motor deficits
and DSM-IV, together with the directives from exist, the individual should receive physical
the National Research Council report (2001) and occupational therapies and, where avail-
that individuals with all forms of ASDs (in- able, assistive technologies (e.g., using a lap-
cluding AS) should be provided with intensive top to type assignments rather than writing).
and comprehensive educational programs of Occupational therapy should not only focus on
the kinds prescribed for individuals with traditional techniques designed to remediate
autism, has facilitated this process somewhat. motor deficits but also reflect an effort to inte-
In the past, many individuals with AS were di- grate these activities with learning of visual-
agnosed as having learning disabilities with spatial concepts, visual-spatial orientation,
some eccentric features, a nonpsychiatric di- and body awareness.
agnostic label that is much less effective in se-
curing services. Others, who were given the Adaptive Functioning
diagnosis of autism or PDD-NOS, had often to
contend with educational programs designed The acquisition of self-sufficiency skills in all
for much lower functioning children, thus fail- areas of functioning should also be a priority.
ing to have their relative strengths and unique Because individuals with AS tend to rely on
disabilities properly addressed. Yet another rigid rules and routines, these can be used to
group of individuals with AS is sometimes foster positive habits and enhance the person’s
characterized as exhibiting social-emotional quality of life and that of family members. The
maladjustment (SEM), an educational label teaching approach should follow closely the
that is often associated with conduct problems guidelines set earlier (see preceding Learning
and volitional maladaptive behaviors. These subsection) and should be practiced routinely
individuals are often placed in educational set- in naturally occurring situations and across
tings for individuals with conduct disorders, different settings to maximize generalization
thus allowing for the worse mismatch possible, of acquired skills.
namely, of individuals with a very naïve un-
derstanding of social situations with those Behavioral Management
who can and do manipulate social situations to
their advantage without the benefit of self- Challenging behaviors are common among in-
restraint. dividuals with AS. As noted, their motivations
are rarely malicious and are more likely to
Learning stem from difficulties with arousal regulation
and poor emotional insight into self and others.
In educating students with AS, it is crucial to Specific problem-solving strategies, usually
take advantage of their typically strong lan- following a verbal algorithm, may be taught for
guage skills and concrete style of thinking. handling the requirements of frequently occur-
Skills, concepts, appropriate procedures, cog- ring, troublesome situations (e.g., involving
nitive strategies, and behavioral norms may be novelty, intense social demands, or frustra-
most effectively taught in an explicit and rote tion). Training is usually necessary for recog-
fashion using a parts-to-whole verbal instruc- nizing situations as troublesome and for
tion approach, where the verbal steps are in selecting the best available learned strategy to
the correct sequence for the behavior to be ef- use in such situations. Cognitive and behav-
fective. The educational program should be ioral strategies for anxiety management (e.g.,
tailored to the child, not to the diagnosis, and breathing exercises) are often helpful in teach-
derived from the individual’s neuropsycholog- ing students to control negative emotions. In
Asperger Syndrome 115

designing any intervention to control problem- include typical peer models, but it is important
atic behaviors, it is important to collect data to that typical peers receive prior training) work-
both understand the function that they are ing in concert with one or more educators.
serving to the child (i.e., using functional be- They are uniquely beneficial in allowing for
havior analysis) and to ascertain a true esti- didactic training and practice with peers in a
mate of the efficacy of treatment. single setting. Furthermore, they may foster
development of supportive relationships (for
Communication and Social Skills Training both children and their parents) that endure
after group has concluded. Teaching may in-
Because individuals with AS experience com- clude the following:
munication deficits predominantly within the
domain of pragmatics, these skills are best 1. Appropriate nonverbal behavior (e.g., the
taught by or with the support of a communica- use of gaze for social interaction, monitor-
tion specialist with expertise in this area. Con- ing and patterning inflection of voice).
versation skills should be taught directly This may involve imitative drills, working
including self-monitoring level of diction and with a mirror, and so forth.
volume, taking turns in conversation, topic in- 2. Verbal decoding of nonverbal behaviors of
troduction, and maintenance. others (e.g., the meaning of social and af-
The interrelationship between language and fective cues, posture, voice patterns).
social ability makes it helpful to interweave 3. Processing of visual information simulta-
communicative training with interventions to neously with auditory information (to fos-
specifically address social skills. A general ter integration of competing stimuli and
principle for teaching social skills is to explic- facilitate the creation of the appropriate so-
itly define rules for social behavior; individu- cial context of the interaction).
als with AS lack the intuition and social 4. Social and self-awareness.
wherewithal to detect and respond to social in- 5. Social norms and expectations.
formation effectively, and this strategy helps 6. Perspective-taking skills.
them to compensate for these deficits. On an 7. Correct interpretation of ambiguous com-
individual basis, teaching explicit scripts that munications (e.g., nonliteral language).
can be used in social situations (e.g., ap-
proaching a peer) capitalizes on strong rote All of these should be taught, practiced, and
memory abilities; these scripts can then be re- fostered in multiple environments.
hearsed in role play. For children requiring
more basic social interventions, practicing ex- Assistive Technology
pression and recognition of facial expressions
is useful; in students with higher levels of abil- Recent advances in computer technology and
ity, this may involve teaching them to read the expansion of the Internet offer ever-
more subtle social cues. Many aids, including expanding opportunities for individuals with
software programs, have been developed to fa- ASDs. The intellectual capabilities of people
cilitate such goals; in this domain and, more with AS render them viable computer users,
generally, it is helpful to move from static to and, for many individuals, computer-based
dynamic. For example, children may practice work is intrinsically very rewarding. Comput-
recognizing affect in pictures or cartoons, ers are now considered useful in numerous as-
subsequently moving on to videotapes, then fi- pects of intervention. Given the frequency of
nally during in vivo role modeling. A number graphomotor problems, typing academic as-
of excellent guidelines have been published to signments or employment-related tasks allevi-
guide parents and interventionists in teaching ates much of the difficulty associated with
social skills. manual penmanship. Organizational software
Social skills training groups are an effec- and personal data assistants can be immensely
tive and increasingly common intervention useful in helping individuals with schedules,
for working on pragmatics and social abili- to-do lists, and a variety of other organiza-
ties. Such groups involve a small number of tional tasks that are very challenging for those
children with social impairments (they may with executive dysfunction (Ozonoff, 1998).
116 Diagnosis and Classification

The Internet is an excellent medium for par- empathy, social difficulties, and depressive
ents and educators to communicate regarding symptoms, and a more direct, problem-solving
academic goals and behavior management, focus can at times be more beneficial than an
thereby increasing consistency across people insight-oriented approach.
and settings, which is so vital to maximize the
effectiveness of interventions for individuals Pharmacotherapy
with AS. Finally, the computer skills acquired
through these activities may, and often do, be- No medications have been shown to decrease
come a valuable asset in the context of voca- the core social and communicative symptoma-
tional opportunities later in life. tology of individuals with ASDs. However,
pharmacological approaches, typically com-
Vocational Training bined with behavioral interventions and appro-
priate adaptations in the child’s environment,
Adults with AS often fail to meet entry re- can be very helpful in allaying comorbid
quirements for jobs in their area of training or symptoms involving anxiety, depression, inat-
fail to maintain a job because of their poor tention, and others, thus making the child
interview skills, social disabilities, eccentrici- more available to educational and therapeutic
ties, anxiety attacks, and, at times, poor activities. Physicians prescribing medications
grooming. All of these areas may require ap- should be knowledgeable not only of the co-
propriate interventions and preparation prior morbid conditions that they are treating but
to challenging events. To ensure success, it is also, and especially, of the nature of the ASDs.
also important that they are trained for and are Multifaceted symptoms should be treated with
placed in jobs for which they are not neuropsy- a minimal number of agents, and planning of
chologically impaired and in which they will pharmacological intervention should include
enjoy a certain degree of support and shelter. clear definitions of targeted symptoms and
It is also preferable that the job does not in- evaluative procedures to ensure that the med-
volve intensive social demands, time pres- ication is having a positive impact on the
sures, or the need to quickly improvise or child’s functioning and well-being (Towbin,
generate solutions to novel situations. 2003). Continued follow-up is required to ad-
just treatment (e.g., to slowly reach therapeu-
Self-Support tic levels), evaluate effects, and possibly
discontinue or change the intervention if there
Because individuals with AS are usually self- are prohibitive side effects or if there is no
described loners despite an often intense wish demonstrated benefit.
to make friends and have a more active social
life, there is a need to facilitate social contact CONCLUSION
within the context of an activity-oriented
group (e.g., church communities, hobby clubs, Since the previous edition of the Handbook in
and self-support groups). The little experience 1997, the major positive changes involving the
available with the latter suggests that individu- diagnostic construct of AS occurred in areas
als with AS enjoy the opportunity to meet oth- other than progress in clinical research. First,
ers with similar problems and may develop there has been an ever-increasing awareness by
relationships around an activity or subject of the general public and authorities deciding on
shared interest. Internet support groups and supportive services of the needs and strengths
chat rooms have proved to be excellent forums of individuals with this condition. Lorna Wing’s
for individuals with AS and their families to influential introduction of the term in 1981 has,
make contact with others who may share an in- 2 decades later, accomplished its stated goal of
terest in AS or may simply partake in a com- bringing individuals with severe social disabili-
mon enjoyed activity. For many individuals ties accompanied by preserved cognitive and
with AS, a strategy that may help in the man- language skills to the attention of the clinical
agement of an individual’s everyday needs is to community and educational systems. And al-
maintain long-term supportive contact with a though misperceptions remain, there is a gen-
counselor. Therapy can focus on problems of eral consensus that these individuals require
Asperger Syndrome 117

special services without which they cannot Two general lines of research may be adopted
fulfill their potential and live more indepen- to elucidate this issue.
dent and fulfilling lives. Second, with the for- First, if we consider the possibility that the
malization of the disorder and the entitlements two conditions are the same or simply variable
associated with it, there has been a great need manifestations of the same underlying con-
for practical resources addressing these indi- struct—the ASDs—then there is a need to
viduals’ needs for social and communication clarify the dimensions creating this spectrum.
skills training and other areas of intervention With the exception of IQ and language skills,
throughout their life span. In response to this there has not been much progress in elucidat-
need, there has been an exponential growth in ing the potential factors mediating expression
the past few years of publications meant to set of the syndrome and determining eventual
guidelines and strategies for teaching skills outcomes. To this goal, there is a need for re-
necessary for social adaptation, learning, self- search relating, in a quantified manner, spe-
regulation, and vocational success. Most of cific neuropsychological, social-cognitive, or
these strategies have not been systematically neurobiological models to phenotypic expres-
examined, and indeed, this is a great priority sion including measures of outcome such as
for the next decade. levels of symptomatology and of ability or
In contrast to these developments, clinical adaptation. All ASDs have their onset in the
research has been hindered by lack of progress early years of a child’s life. Longitudinal re-
as to the validity status of AS. Much of this search is needed in which key developmental
research has been flawed due in great degree processes hypothesized to mediate phenotypic
to a lack of a consensual, validated, and well- expression are evaluated relative to the range
operationalized definition of the condition. of phenotypic variability later in life. The
While the question of whether AS can be dif- rather vague onset criteria established for the
ferentiated from autism without intellectual ASDs could hardly be expected to elucidate
disabilities may have originated from the the thorny diagnostic issues presented by these
chance referrals to Leo Kanner and Hans conditions later in life. And there are still very
Asperger, it has endured for more substantial few studies that have attempted to examine
reasons. The ASDs refer to a highly heteroge- prospectively the early years of individuals
neous phenotype, which nevertheless is pri- later diagnosed as having AS. As these individ-
marily defined by the pronounced and early uals typically come to the attention of special-
emerging social disability. Given the complex- ized clinics later in childhood (given the
ity of social behavior, it is not unreasonable to absence of the more common reasons for refer-
expect that there may be more than one devel- ral such as language delays and social isola-
opmental pathway generating the variable tion), most of what we know about the onset of
manifestations of these conditions. Therefore, this condition is based on retrospective infor-
the successful identification of more homoge- mation. Yet, the formal definitions of AS de-
neous subtypes can potentially be of great pend entirely on onset patterns as a way of
benefit to behavioral and neurobiological re- differentiating it from autism. In summary,
search seeking to yield more effective treat- there is a need for much greater knowledge
ment strategies and to elucidate the causative about the eventual impact on phenopic expres-
processes underlying the etiologies of these sion of variables ranging from social motiva-
conditions. In the face of this challenge, how- tion to joint attention skills, from sensory and
ever, research studies have failed to produce psychomotor adaptation and self-regulation to
the level of substantive detail in phenotypic executive abilities, and from different learning
characterization and of documentation of lon- styles to different language profiles to gain
gitudinal outcomes required for true progress more knowledge of different pathways to so-
in the understanding of developmental mecha- cial disabilities.
nisms mediating syndrome expression. It is Second, if we consider the possibility that
premature to close the door on this debate be- AS and autism are different conditions, then
cause the data are still limited as to whether there is a need to clarify what is meant by the
the ASDs, and autism and AS in particular, word dif ferent. There are various practical
should be considered the same or different. reasons that a person may justifiably choose
118 Diagnosis and Classification

to see two conditions as different (Szatmari, Cross-References


2000), but the most important ones concern
the fact that they may have different etiologies Other diagnostic concepts are discussed in
or pathogenetic pathways (e.g., genetic mech- Chapters 1, 3, 5, and 6; outcome studies in
anisms), they may respond differently to dif- Chapter 7; and assessment issues in Chapters
ferent kinds of treatment (e.g., because of 27 through 32.
different neurocognitive, social-cognitive, or
neurobiological profiles), or they may imply REFERENCES
differential outcomes (which would signify
more detailed guidelines for intervention to American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
better address the eventual challenges and nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
maximize factors promoting eventual success).
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
None of these important areas of research can, nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
however, be dramatically advanced without a ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
much more detailed, standardized, and quanti- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnos-
fied description of the two conditions. There tic and statistical manual of mental disorders
is a great deal of knowledge about autism in (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
general, but much less about the group of Anneren, G., Dahl, N., Uddenfeldt, U., & Janols,
higher functioning individuals, most of whom L. O. (1995). Asperger syndrome in a boy
are seen in clinics at a later age for the same with a balanced de novo translocation:
reasons that individuals with AS are also not T(17;19) (p13.3;p11) [letter]. American Jour-
seen in early childhood. As suggested earlier, nal of Medical Genetics, 56(3), 330–331.
Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopa-
given the current status of the diagnostic pro-
then” im Kindesalter. Archiv für Psychiatrie
cess feeding into research studies of AS, it is
und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.
unlikely that progress will be made without a Asperger, H. (1979). Problems of infantile autism.
concerted multisite effort adopting rigorous Communication, 13, 45–52.
diagnostic procedures. These should include Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide
new and especially developed diagnostic in- for parents and professionals. Philadelphia:
strumentation operationalized around a con- Jessica Kingsley.
sensual set of defining criteria. Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton,
Finally, interest in AS has opened the door P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995).
to debates that had been until recently very Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evi-
limited. Broader attention to individuals with dence from a British twin study. Psychological
Medicine, 25, 63–77.
AS who are rather eccentric and socially awk-
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Cou-
ward but who may also have special talents in
teur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in rel-
domains of factual knowledge or technological atives. Journal of Autism and Developmental
skills raises questions about the borderlands of Disorders, 28, 369–392.
this condition. For example, to what extent Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A.,
should we stretch the spectrum to encompass a Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., et al. (2000).
much larger group of more successful and not A screening instrument for autism at 18
necessarily disabled people whose social in- months of age: A 6 year follow-up study. Jour-
eptness has had, nevertheless, great impact nal of the American Academy of Child and
over their internal, social, and affective lives Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(6), 694–702.
(Asperger, 1954, as cited in Bosch, 1970; Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). An assessment of violence
Baron-Cohen, 2002)? Whether an approach in a young man with Asperger’s syndrome.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
taking this larger view of the phenotype, larger
29, 351–360.
in fact than the commonly accepted broader
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain
autism phenotype, might contribute to a better theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
understanding of genetic mechanisms needs to 6, 248–254.
be answered. But to do so, there will be a need Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Swettenham,
for increasingly more refined phenotypic mea- J., & Nightingale, N. (1996). Psychological
sures of sociability. markers in the detection of autism in infancy
Asperger Syndrome 119

in a large population. British Journal of Psy- atry. New York: Dover. (Original work pub-
chiatry, 168(2), 158–163. lished 1916)
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, Bosch, G. (1970). Infantile autism. New York:
D. J. (Eds.). (1999). Understanding other Springer-Verlag.
minds: Perspectives from autism (2nd ed.). Ox- Bowler, D. M. (1992). “ Theory of Mind” in As-
ford, England: Oxford University Press. perger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychol-
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffee, T. ogy and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877–893.
(1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evi- Bowman, E. P. (1988). Asperger’s syndrome and
dence from normal adults and adults with autism: The case for a connection. British
autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cogni- Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 377–382.
tion, 4, 311–331. Burgoine, E., & Wing, L. (1983). Identical triplets
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V., & with Asperger’s syndrome. British Journal of
Rutherford, M. (1999). A mathematician, a Psychiatry, 143, 261–265.
physicist and a computer scientist with As- Casanova, M., Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala, A. E., &
perger syndrome: Performance on folk psy- Roy, E. (2002, February). Minocolumnar pathol-
chology and folk physics tests. Neurocase, ogy in autism. Neurology, 58(3), 428–432.
5(6), 475–483. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S.,
Bartolucci, G., & Szatmari, P. (1987). Possible Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants
similarities between the fragile, X., and As- with autism: An investigation of empathy, pre-
perger’s syndrome [letter]. American Journal tend play, joint attention, and imitation. Devel-
of Disorders of Childhood, 141(6), 601–602. opmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789.
Bashe, P. R., & Kirby, B. L. (2001). The OASIS Constantino, J., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic
guide to Asperger syndrome. New York: traits in the general population: A twin study.
Crown. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5),
Bejerot, S., & Duvner, T. (1995). Asperger’s syn- 524–530.
drome or schizophrenia? Nordic Journal of Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-
Psychiatry, 49(2), 145. Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999).
Berthier, M. L., Bayes, A., & Tolosa, E. S. (1993). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42
Magnetic resonance imaging in patients with months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R
concurrent Tourette’s disorder and Asperger’s diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and
syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry, 40(5), 719–732.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), Dahlgren, S. O., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). The-
633–639. ory of mind in non-retarded children with
Berthier, M. L., Starkstein, S. E., & Leiguarda, R. autism and Asperger’s syndrome. A research
(1990). Developmental cortical anomalies in note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Asperger’s syndrome: Neuroradiological try, 37, 759–763.
findings in two patients. Journal of Neuropsy- DeLong, G. R., & Dwyer, J. T. (1988). Correlation
chiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 2(2), of family history with specific autistic sub-
197–201. groups: Asperger’s syndrome and bipolar af-
Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Autism, Asperger’s syn- fective disease. Journal of Autism and
drome and semantic-pragmatic disorder: Developmental Disorders, 18(4), 593–600.
Where are the boundaries? British Journal of Denckla, M. B. (1983). The neuropsychology of
Disorders of Communications, 24, 107–121. social-emotional learning disabilities. Archives
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Chil- of Neurology, 40, 461–462.
dren’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A Ehlers, S., & Gillberg, C. (1993). The epidemiol-
method for assessing qualitative aspects of ogy of Asperger syndrome: A total population
communicative impairment in children. Jour- study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(6), atry, 34(8), 1327–1350.
879–891. Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., & Dahlgren
Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). What’s so special about Sandberg, A. (1997). Asperger syndrome,
Asperger syndrome? The need for further ex- autism and attention disorders: A comparative
ploration of the borderlands of autism. In A. study of the cognitive profiles of 120 children.
Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2,
Asperger syndrome (pp. 278–308). New York: 207–217.
Guilford Press. Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L.,
Bleuler, E. (1951). Lehrbuch der psychiatrie. In Gould, J., Welham, M., et al. (1996). Com-
A. A. Brill (Ed. & Trans.), Textbook of psychi- parison of clinical symptoms in autism and
120 Diagnosis and Classification

Asperger’s disorder. Journal of the American Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., & Ghaziuddin, N.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, (1991). Brief report: Violence in Asperger
35, 1523–1531. Syndrome, a critique. Journal of Autism
Ellis, H. D., Ellis, D. M., Fraser, W., & Deb, S. and Developmental Disorders, 21(3),
(1994). A preliminary study of right hemi- 349–354.
sphere cognitive deficits and impaired social Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., & Ghaziuddin, N.
judgments among young people with Asperger (1992). Brief report: A comparison of the di-
syndrome. European Child and Adolescent Psy- agnostic criteria for Asperger Syndrome. Jour-
chiatry, 3(4), 255–266. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Everall, I. P., & Le Couteur, A. (1990). Firesetting 22(4), 643–649.
in an adolescent with Asperger’s Syndrome. Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmer-Mikhail, E., & Ghaziud-
British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 284–287. din, N. (1998). Comorbidity of Asperger
Fombonne, E., & Tidmarsh, L. (2003). Epidemio- syndrome: A preliminary report. Journal of In-
logic data on Asperger disorder. Child and tellectual Disability Research 42(4), 279–283.
Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Amer- Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rut-
ica, 12, 15–22. ter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development
Frith, U. (Ed.). (1991). Autism and Asperger syn- and current functioning in adolescents with
drome. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- Asperger syndrome: A comparative study.
versity Press. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Frith, U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: Confu- 42, 227–240.
sions and controversies about Asperger syn- Gillberg, C. (1985). Asperger’s syndrome and re-
drome. Journal of Child Psychology and current psychosis: A case study. Journal of
Psychiatry, 45(4), 672–686. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(4),
Fujikawa, H., Kobayashi, R., Koga, Y., & Murata, 389–397.
T. (1987). A case of Asperger’s syndrome in a Gillberg, C. (1989). Asperger syndrome in 23
nineteen-year-old who showed psychotic Swedish children. Developmental Medicine
breakdown with depressive state and at- and Child Neurology, 31, 520–531.
tempted suicide after entering university. Gillberg, C. (1990). Autism and the pervasive de-
Japanese Journal of Child and Adolescent Psy- velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy-
chiatry, 28(4), 217–225. chology and Psychiatry, 31(1), 99–119.
Ghaziuddin, M. (2002). Asperger syndrome: Asso- Gillberg, C. (1991). Outcome in autism and autistic-
ciated psychiatric and medical conditions. like conditions. Journal of the American Acad-
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(3),
Disabilities, 17(3), 138–144. 375–382.
Ghaziuddin, M., Butler, E., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, Gillberg, C. (1998). Asperger syndrome and high-
N. (1994). Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger functioning autism. British Journal of Psychia-
Syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disabilities try, 172, 200–209.
Research, 38(5), 519–527. Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., Rastam, M., & Wentz,
Ghaziuddin, M., & Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic E. (2001). The Asperger Syndrome (and high-
speaking style differentiates Asperger syn- functioning autism) Diagnostic Interview
drome from high-functioning autism. Journal (ASDI ): A preliminary study of a new struc-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(6), tured clinical interview. Autism: Journal of
585–595. Research and Practice, 5(1), 57–66.
Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., & Steffenburg, S.
(2002). Depression in persons with autism: (1992). Siblings and parents of children with
Implications for research and clinical care. autism: A controlled population-based study.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
ders, 32(4), 299–306. 34(5), 389–398.
Ghaziuddin, M., Leininger, L., & Tsai, L. (1995). Gillberg, C., & Rastam, M. (1992). Do some cases
Brief report: Thought disorder in Asperger of anorexia nervosa reflect underlying autistic-
syndrome [Comparison with high-functioning like. Behavioural Neurology, 5(1), 27–32.
autism]. Journal of Autism and Developmental Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Asperger
Disorders, 25(3), 311–317. syndrome: Some epidemiological considera-
Ghaziuddin, M., & Mountain-Kimchi, K. (2004). tions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger atry, 30, 631–638.
syndrome: Comparison with high-functioning Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A.
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental (2000). Social and psychiatric functioning in
Disorders, 34(3), 279–284. adolescents with Asperger syndrome com-
Asperger Syndrome 121

pared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism Kanner, L. (1954). Discussion of Robinson & Vi-
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293. tale’s paper on “Children with circumscribed
Grossman, J. B., Klin, A., Carter, A. S., & Volk- interests.” American Journal of Orthopsychia-
mar, F. R. (2000). Verbal bias in recognition of try, 24, 764–766.
facial emotions in children with Asperger syn- Kerbeshian, J., & Burd, L. (1986). Asperger’s syn-
drome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- drome and Tourette syndrome: The case of the
chiatry, 41(3), 369–379. pinball wizard. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Gunter, H. L., Ghaziuddin, M., & Ellis, H. D. 148, 731–736.
(2002). Asperger syndrome: Tests of right Kim, J., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Streiner, D. L., &
hemisphere functioning and interhemispheric Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence of anxiety
communication. Journal of autism and Devel- and mood problems among children with
opmental Disorders, 32(4), 263–281. autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism: Jour-
Happé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal nal of Research and Practice, 4, 117–132.
ability in the theory of mind task performance Klin, A. (1994). Asperger syndrome. Child and
of subjects with autism. Child Development, Adolescent Psychiatry Clinic of North Amer-
66(3), 843–855. ica, 3, 131–148.
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johans- Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to am-
son, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (1996). “ Theory of biguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning
mind” in the brain: Evidence from a PET scan autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social At-
study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroReport, tribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and
8(1), 197–201. Psychiatry, 41(7), 831–846.
Hippler, K., & Klicpera, C. (2003). A retrospective Klin, A., Carter, A., & Sparrow, S. S. (1997). Psycho-
analysis of the clinical case records of “autis- logical assessment of children with autism. In
tic psychopaths” diagnosed by Hans Asperger D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook
and his team at the University Children’s Hos- of autism and pervasive developmental disorders
pital, Vienna. [Philosophical Transactions of (2nd ed., pp. 418–427). New York: Wiley.
the Royal Society]. Biological Sciences, 358, Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar,
291–301. F. R. (2003). The enactive mind: From actions
Howlin, P. (1997). Prognosis in autism: Do specialist to cognition—lessons from autism. [Philo-
treatments affect long-term outcome. European sophical Transactions of the Royal Society].
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6(2), 55–72. Biological Sciences, 358, 345–360.
Howlin, P. (1999). Children with autism and As- Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., &
perger syndrome: A guide for practitioners and Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying
carers. New York: Wiley. the social phenotype in autism. American
Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895–908.
adults with autism with and without early lan- Klin, A., Pauls, D., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F.
guage delays: Implications for the differentia- (in press). Three diagnostic approaches to
tion between autism and Asperger syndrome. Asperger syndrome: Implications for re-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- search. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ders, 33, 3–13. Disorders.
Howlin, P., & Goode, S. (1998). Outcome in adult life Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Marans, W. D., Carter,
for people with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Assessment is-
In F. R. Volkmar (Ed.), Autism and pervasive de- sues in children and adolescents with As-
velopmental disorders (pp. 209–241). Cambridge, perger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, &
England: Cambridge University Press. S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome
Johnson, D. J., & Myklebust, H. R. (1971). Learn- (pp. 309–339). New York: Guilford Press.
ing disabilities. New York: Grune & Stratton. Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Volkmar, F. R., Cicchetti,
Jones, P. B., & Kerwin, R. W. (1990). Left tempo- D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Asperger syn-
ral lobe damage in Asperger’s syndrome. drome. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Syndrome of
British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 570–572. nonverbal learning disabilities: Neurodevelop-
Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., mental manifestations (pp. 93–118). New
Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. York: Guilford Press.
(2002, May). A new “advanced” test of theory Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1996). The pervasive
of mind: Evidence from children and adoles- developmental disorders: Nosology and pro-
cents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child files of development. In S. Luthar, J. Burack,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 517–528. D. Cicchetti, & J. Wiesz (Eds.), Developmental
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- perspectives on risk and psychopathology. New
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–253. York: Cambridge University Press.
122 Diagnosis and Classification

Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Asperger syn- Mawson, D., Grounds, A., & Tantam, D. (1985).
drome. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Violence and Asperger’s syndrome: A case
Handbook of autism and pervasive develop- study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147,
mental disorders (pp. 94–122). New York: 566–569.
Wiley. Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., & Crites, D. L.
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (Eds.). (2003). Asperger (2001). Does DSM-IV Asperger’s disorder
syndrome. Child and adolescent psychiatric clin- exist? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
ics of North America. Philadelphia: Saunders. 29, 263–271.
Klin, A., Volkmar F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (Eds.). McAlonan, G., Daly, E., Kumari, V., Critchley,
(2000). Asperger syndrome. New York: Guil- H. D., van Amelsvoort, T., Suckling, J., et al.
ford Press. (2002). Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gat-
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, ing in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain, 125(7),
D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and 1594–1606.
neuropsychological characterization of As- McKelvey, J. R., Lambert, R., Mottron, L., & Shev-
perger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology ell, M. I. (1995). Right-hemisphere dysfunc-
and Psychiatry, 36(7), 1127–1140. tion in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child
Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Goode, S., Pickles, A., Neurology, 10(4), 310–314.
Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., et al. (1996). A Mesibov, G. B. (1992). Treatment issues with high-
broader phenotype of autism: The Clinical functioning adolescents and adults with
spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychol- autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.),
ogy and Psychiatry, 37(7), 785–801. High-functioning individuals with autism
Leekam, S., Libby, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Gill- (pp. 143–156). New York: Plenum Press.
berg, C. (2000). Comparison of ICD-10 and Miles, S. W., & Capelle, P. (1987). Asperger’s syn-
Gillberg’s criteria for Asperger syndrome. drome and aminoaciduria: A case example.
Autism: Journal of Research and Practice, 4, British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 397–400.
11–28. Miller, J., & Ozonoff, S. (2000). The external va-
Lincoln, A., Courchesne, E., Allen, M., Hanson, E., lidity of Asperger disorder: Lack of evidence
& Ene, M. (1998). Neurobiology of Asperger from the domain of neuropsychology. Journal
syndrome: Seven case studies and quantitative of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 227–238.
magnetic resonance imaging findings. In Murphy, D. G. M., Critchley, H. D., Schmitz, N.,
E. Schopler, G. Mesibov, & L. J. Kunce (Eds.), McAlonan, G., van Amelsvoort, T., Robert-
Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism? son, D., et al. (2002). Asperger syndrome: A
(pp. 145–166). New York: Plenum Press. proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study
Lincoln, A. J., Allen, M., & Kilman, A. (1995). The of brain. Archives of General Psychiatry,
assessment and interpretation of intellectual 59(10), 885–892.
abilities in people with autism. In E. Schopler Myklebust, H. R. (1975). Nonverbal learning dis-
& G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition abilities: Assessment and intervention. In
in autism (pp. 89–117). New York: Plenum H. R. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning
Press. disabilities (Vol. 3, pp. 85–121). New York:
Littlejohns, C. S., Clarke, D. J., & Corbett, J. A. Grune & Stratton.
(1990). Tourette-like disorder in Asperger’s Myles, B. S., & Simpson, R. (1997). Asperger syn-
syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, drome: A guide for educators and parents.
430–433. Austin, TX: ProEd.
Lord, C., Rutter, M. J., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. Nagy, J., & Szatmari, P. (1986). A chart review of
(1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. schizotypal personality disorders in children.
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Lotspeich, L., Kwon, H., Schumann, C. M., Fryer, ders, 16(3).
S. L., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., Buonocore, M. H., National Research Council. (2001). Educating chil-
et al. (2004). Investigation of Neuroanatomical dren with autism. Washington, DC: National
Differences Between Autism and Asperger Academies Press.
Syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, Newson, E., Dawson, M., & Everaard, T.
61(3), 291–298. (1984–1985). The natural history of able
Marriage, K., Miles, T., Stokes, D., & Davey, M. autistic people: Their management and func-
(1993). Clinical and research implications of tioning in a social context. Communication,
the co-occurrence of Asperger’s and Tourette 19–21.
syndrome. Australian and New Zealander Jour- Ozbayrak, K. R., Kapucu, O., Erdem, E., & Aras,
nal of Psychiatry, 27(4), 666–672. T. (1991). Left occipital hypoperfusion in a
Asperger Syndrome 123

case with Asperger Syndrome. Brain Develop- robehavioral examination of individuals with
ment, 13(6), 454–456. high-functioning and Asperger disorder using
Ozonoff, S. (1998). Assessment and remediation of a fronto-striatal model of dysfunction. Behav-
executive dysfunction in autism and Asperger ioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews,
syndrome. In E. Schopler, G. Mesibov, & L. J. 1(2), 164–177.
Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high func- Robinson, J. F., & Vitale, L. J. (1954). Children
tioning autism? (pp. 263–289). New York: with circumscribed interests. American Jour-
Plenum Press. nal of Orthopsychiatry, 24, 755–764.
Ozonoff, S., Dawson, G., & McPartland, J. (2002). A Rourke, B. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities:
parent’s guide to Asperger syndrome and high- The syndrome and the model. New York, Guil-
functioning autism. New York: Guilford Press. ford Press.
Ozonoff, S., & Griffith, E. M. (2000). Neuropsycho- Rourke, B. (Ed.). (1995). Syndrome of nonverbal
logical function and the external validity of As- learning disabilities: Manifestations in neuro-
perger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & logical disease disorder and dysfunction. New
S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome York: Guilford Press.
(pp. 72–96). New York: Guilford Press. Rourke, B., Young, G. C., & Leenaars, A. A.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1989). A childhood learning disability that
(1991). Executive function deficits in high- predisposes those aff licted to adolescent and
functioning autistic individuals: Relationship adult depression and suicide risk. Journal of
to Theory of Mind. Journal of Child Psychol- Learning Disabilities, 22, 169–185.
ogy and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081–1105. Royers, H., Buysse, A., Ponnet, K., & Pichal, B.
Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-reading
(1991). Asperger’s syndrome: Evidence of an tests: Empathic accuracy in adults with a per-
empirical distinction from high-functioning vasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child
autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 271–278.
chiatry, 32(7), 1107–1122. Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that childhood autism. Journal of Autism and
Mary thinks that . . .”: Attribution of second- Childhood Schizophrenia, 8(2), 139–161.
order beliefs by 5–10 year old children. Jour- Rutter, M. (1999). The Emmanual Miller Memorial
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, Lecture 1998: Autism: Two-way interplay be-
437–471. tween research and clinical world. Journal of
Piven, J., Nehme, E., Simon, J., Barta, P., Pearlson, Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 169–188.
G., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Magnetic reso- Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A.
nance imaging in autism: Measurement of the (1994). Autism and known medical conditions:
cerebellum, pons, and fourth ventricle. Biolog- Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol-
ical Psychiatry, 31, 491–504. ogy and Psychiatry, 35(2), 311–322.
Piven, J., Palmer, P., Jacobi, D., Childress, D., & Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Simonoff, E., & Pickles, A.
Arndt, S. (1997). The broader autism pheno- (1997). Genetic inf luences in autism. In D. J.
type: Evidence from a family study of multiple- Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of
incidence autism families. American Journal autism and pervasive developmental disorders
of Psychiatry, 154, 185–190. (2nd ed., pp. 370–387). New York: Wiley.
Piven, J., Saliba, K., Bailey, J., & Arndt S. (1997). Rutter, M., & Gould, M. (1985). Classification. In
An MRI study of autism: The cerebellum re- M. Rutter & L. Herson (Eds.), Child and
visited. Neurology, 49(2), 546–551. adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches
Powers, M., & Poland, J. (2002). Asperger syn- (2nd ed., pp. 304–321). Oxford, England:
drome and your child: A parent’s guide. New Blackwell.
York: HarperCollins. Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003).
Rapin, I., & Allen, D. (1983). Developmental ADI-R autism diagnostic interview (Revised).
language disorders. In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neu- Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
ropsychology of language, reading and spelling. Ryan, R. M. (1992). Treatment-resistant chronic
New York: Academic Press. mental illness: Is it Asperger’s syndrome?
Reitzel, J.-A., & Szatmari, P. (2003). Cognitive and Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 43(8),
academic problems. In M. Prior (Ed.), Learn- 807–811.
ing and behavior problems in Asperger syn- Saliba, J. R., & Griffiths, M. (1990). Brief report:
drome (pp. 35–54). New York: Guilford Press. Autism of the Asperger type associated with
Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Tonge, B. J., Brere- an autosomal fragile site. Journal of Autism
ton, A. V., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2002). A neu- and Developmental Disorders, 20(4), 569–575.
124 Diagnosis and Classification

Scragg, P., & Shah, A. (1994). Prevalence of As- Taiminen, T. (1994). Asperger’s syndrome or
perger’s syndrome in a secure hospital. British schizophrenia: Is differential diagnosis neces-
Journal of Psychiatry, 165(5), 679–682. sary for adult patients? Nordic Journal of Psy-
Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. chiatry, 48(5), 325–328.
(1996). Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of Tantam, D. (1988a). Annotation: Asperger’s syn-
high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism drome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
and Developmental Disorders, 26(4), 389–406. chiatry, 29(3), 245–255.
Smith, I. M. (2000). Motor functioning in As- Tantam, D. (1988b). Lifelong eccentricity and so-
perger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & cial isolation: II. Asperger’s syndrome or
S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome schizoid personality disorder? British Journal
(pp. 97–124). New York: Guilford Press. of Psychiatry, 153, 783–791.
Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of Tantam, D. (1988c). Lifelong eccentricity and so-
mind in children with autistic disorder: Evi- cial isolation: I. Psychiatric, social, and foren-
dence of developmental progression and the sic aspects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychol- 777–782.
ogy and Psychiatry, 36(2), 249–263. Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger’s syndrome in adult-
Szatmari, P. (2000). Perspectives on the classifica- hood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger
tion of Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. syndrome (pp. 147–183). Cambridge, Eng-
Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger land: Cambridge University Press.
syndrome (pp. 403–417). New York: Guilford Tantam, D., Evered, C., & Hersov, L. (1990). As-
Press. perger’s syndrome and ligamentous laxity.
Szatmari, P., Archer, L., Fisman, S., Streiner, Journal of the American Academy of Child and
D. L., & Wilson, F. (1995). Asperger’s syn- Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(6), 892–896.
drome and autism: Differences in behavior, Tantam, D., Evered, C., & Hersov, L. (1990). As-
cognition, and adaptive functioning. Journal of perger’s syndrome and ligamentous laxity.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Psychiatry, 34, 1662–1671. Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(6), 892–896.
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., & Bremner, R. Tentler, D., Johannesson, T. M., Johansson, M.,
(1989a). Asperger’s syndrome and autism: Rastamm, M., Gillberg, C., Orsmark, C., et al.
Comparison of early history and outcome. (2003). A candidate region for Asperger syn-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, drome defined by two 17p breakpoints. Euro-
31(6), 709–720. pean Journal of Human Genetics, 11(2),
Szatmari, P., Bremner, R., & Nagy, J. N. (1989b). 189–195.
Asperger’s syndrome: A review of clinical fea- Thomsen, P. H. (1994). Obsessive-compulsive dis-
tures. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34(6), order in children and adolescents: A 6–22-year
554–560. follow-up study: Clinical descriptions of the
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Boyle, M. H., Streiner, course and continuity of obsessive-compulsive
D. L., & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of out- symptomatology. European Child and Adoles-
come among high functioning children with cent Psychiatry, 3(2), 82–96.
autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Towbin, K. E. (2003). Strategies for pharmacologic
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), treatment of high-functioning autism and As-
520–528. perger syndrome. Child and Adolescent Psychi-
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L., Wilson, atric Clinics of North America, 12(1), 23–45.
F., Archer, L., & Ryerse, C. (2000). Two-year Tsai, L. Y. (2002). Diagnostic issues in high-
outcome of preschool children with autism or functioning autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesi-
Asperger’s syndrome. American Journal of bov (Ed.), High-functioning individuals with
Psychiatry, 157, 1980–1987. autism (pp. 11–40). New York: Plenum Press.
Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, M. A. J., & Bar- Van Bourgondien, M. E., & Woods, A. V. (1992).
tolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome and Vocational possibilities for high-functioning
autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the adults with autism. In E. Schopler & G. B.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals
Psychiatry, 29, 130–136. with autism (pp. 227–239). New York: Plenum
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A reexamination of the Press.
theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. A. Van Krevelen, D. A. (1963). On the relationship
Burack & T. Charman (Eds.), The development between early infantile autism and autistic
of autism: Perspectives from theory and re- psychopathy. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 30,
search (pp. 173–193). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 303–323.
Asperger Syndrome 125

Van Krevelen, D. A. (1971). Early infantile autism cognitive components. Archives of Neurology,
and autistic psychopathy. Journal of Autism 40, 463–468.
and Child Schizophrenia, 1(1), 82–86. Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Disintegra- account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.
tive disorder or “late onset ” autism. Journal of Wing, L. (1986). Clarification on Asperger’s Syn-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(5), drome (Letters to the Editor). Journal of
717–724. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(4),
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991). Comorbid 513–515.
association of autism and schizophrenia. Wing, L. (1998). The history of Asperger syn-
American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(12), drome. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.),
1705–1707. Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2000). Diagnostic issues autism? (pp. 11–28). New York: Plenum Press.
in Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volk- Wing, L. (2000). Past and future of research on As-
mar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syn- perger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, &
drome (pp. 25–71). New York: Guilford Press. S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Di- (pp. 418–432). New York: Guilford Press.
agnosis and classification of autism and re- Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
lated conditions: Consensus and issues. In of social interaction and associated abnor-
D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook malities in children: Epidemiology and classi-
of autism and pervasive developmental disor- fication. Journal of Autism and Childhood
ders (pp. 5–40). New York: Wiley. Schizophrenia, 9, 11–29.
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & Pauls, D. (1998). Wolff, S. (1991). “Schizoid” personality in child-
Nosological and genetic aspects of Asperger hood and adult life: III. The childhood picture.
Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmen- British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 629–635.
tal Disorders, 28(5), 457–463. Wolff, S. (1995). Loner’s: The life path of unusual
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Schultz, R. B., Bronen, R., children. London: Routledge.
Marans, W. D., Sparrow, S. S., et al. (1996). Wolff, S., & Barlow, A. (1979). Schizoid personal-
Grand rounds in child psychiatry: Asperger ity in childhood: A comparative study of
syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of schizoid, autistic and normal children. Jour-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 118–123. nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20,
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., 19–46.
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). DSM-IV Wolff, S., & Chick, J. (1980). Schizoid personality
Autism / Pervasive Developmental Disorder in childhood: A controlled follow-up study.
Field Trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, Psychological Medicine, 10, 85–100.
151, 1361–1367. World Health Organization. (1993). International
Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., classification of diseases [Chapter V. Mental
& Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive de- and behavioral disorders (including disorders
velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy- of psychological development): Diagnostic cri-
chology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 1–36. teria for research] (10th ed.). Geneva,
Weimer, A. K., Schatz, A. M., Lincoln, A., Ballan- Switzerland: Author.
tyne, A. O., & Trauner, D. A. (2001). “Motor ” Ziatas, K., Durkin, K., & Pratt, C. (2003). Differ-
impairment in Asperger syndrome: Evidence ences in assertive speech acts produced by
for a deficit in proprioception. Journal of De- children with autism, Asperger syndrome, spe-
velopmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22(2), cific language impairment, and normal devel-
92–101. opment. Development and Psychopathology,
Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1983). Devel- 15(1), 73–94.
opmental learning disabilities of the right
hemisphere: Emotional, interpersonal, and
CHAPTER 5

Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder

RICHARD VAN ACKER, JENNIFER A. LONCOLA, AND ERYN Y. VAN ACKER

At least in its classical form, Rett syndrome is with this disorder. This false lead, coupled with
a phenotypically distinct progressive X-linked very limited exposure (Rett, 1969, 1977) of
dominant neurodevelopmental disorder that this information in the English language med-
almost exclusively affects females. The char- ical literature, resulted in a general failure to
acteristic pattern of cognitive and functional recognize Rett syndrome, previously termed
stagnation with subsequent deterioration pro- cerebroatrophic hyperammonemia as a nosolog-
foundly impairs postnatal brain growth and ical entity.
development. Rett syndrome represents one of Unaware of Rett’s work, Bengt Hagberg
the most common causes of mental retardation was working with patients displaying similar
in females, second only to Down syndrome symptoms in Sweden. In 1980, he presented
(Ellaway & Christodoulou, 1999). Stereotypic a paper at the European Federation of Child
hand movements, typically at midline, are one Neurology Societies describing 16 girls he
of the most prominent symptoms. The disorder had observed (Hagberg, 1980). Later, he and
is the first human disease that has been found several colleagues (Hagberg, Aicardi, Dias, &
to result from defects in a protein involved in Ramos, 1983) published in the Annals of Neu-
the regulation of gene expression through its rology their report of 35 girls from France,
interaction with methylated DNA. As such, Portugal, and Sweden with Rett syndrome.
Rett syndrome could hold the key to our under- This landmark account awakened the recogni-
standing of human disorders ranging from tion and interest of clinicians and researchers
some forms of learning disability to autism. and provided credit to Dr. Rett for his pioneer-
Andreas Rett, an Austrian physician, first ing efforts on the disorder that bears his name.
described the disorder following his serendipi- Researchers have learned much about this dis-
tous discovery of two girls seated in his waiting order over the past 2 decades; however, much
room who displayed strikingly similar hand- remains to be discovered.
wringing mannerisms. When he discussed this
with his receptionist, they were able to review CLINICAL PRESENTATION AND
patient records and identify six additional NATURAL HISTORY
patients with similar behavioral characteristics
and developmental histories. Unable to find Individuals with Rett syndrome exhibit a
a known classification for the disorder, Rett unique and characteristic course of develop-
(1966) published a report (in German) de- ment (Naidu, Murphy, Moser, & Rett, 1986).
scribing 22 girls with a syndrome consisting of Prenatal and perinatal histories of these
stereotypic hand movements, dementia, autistic persons are generally unremarkable. Although
behavior, ataxia, cortical atrophy, and hyper- minor pre- and perinatal problems (e.g., mild
ammonemia ( blood ammonia). The reported hypotonia, tremulous neck movements, a low
increased levels of hyperammonemia were sub- intensity of interpersonal contact, and abnor-
sequently found to be only rarely associated mal hand use and language development) can be

126
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 127

identified retrospectively in as many as 80% loss of acquired speech, voluntary grasping,


of the girls (Burford, Keer, & Macleod, 2003; and the purposeful use of the hands (Charman
Charman et al., 2002; Hanefeld, 1985; Leonard et al., 2002). The girls begin to lack sustained
& Bower, 1998; Nomura & Segawa, 1990; interest in persons or objects and demonstrate
Opitz & Lewin, 1987; Sekul & Percy, 1992), it limited interpersonal contact; however, they
is unlikely that these mild symptoms would be maintain eye contact (Holm, 1985; Trevathan &
detected as relevant even with detailed neuro- Naidu, 1988; Witt-Engerstrom, 1987). This
logical or developmental assessment. Excess deterioration occurs very quickly, typically
levels of hand patting, waving, and involuntary within 1 year or less, resulting in severe-to-
movements including alternate opening and profound disabilities and stereotyped behaviors.
closing of the fingers, twisting of the wrists and Deceleration of head growth (acquired micro-
arms, or nonspecific circulating hand-mouth cephaly), coarse, jerky movements of the trunk
movements appear to be the most characteristic and limbs, a stiff-legged, broad-based gait with
early warning signals for the syndrome (Holm, somewhat short steps and swaying movements
1985; Kerr, Montague, & Stephenson, 1987; of the shoulders when walking accompany the
Witt-Engerstrom, 1987). Given the wide range developmental deterioration (Coleman & Gill-
of functioning at this age, such soft signs would berg, 1985; Hanefeld, 1985; Kerr & Stephen-
generally be dismissed. Thus, parents generally son, 1986; Naidu et al., 1986; Percy, Zoghbi, &
report normal physical and mental development Riccardi, 1985). With the loss of purposeful
for the first 6 to 8 months of life as evidenced hand movements, the most prominent symptom
by physical growth and psychomotor and verbal of the syndrome appears, in the form of stereo-
behavior (Gillberg, 1987; Sekul & Percy, 1992). typic hand-to-mouth movements, hand clasp-
This apparently normal period of development ing, and “ hand washing” (see Figure 5.1;
is followed by a slowing or cessation of the ac- Ishikawa et al., 1978; Leiber, 1985).
quisition of developmental milestones with sig- The developmental regression seems to
nificant deviations in the acquisition of skills plateau during the early school years. In some
requiring balance (e.g., walking) in many cases. cases, parents report their daughters attempt
By 15 months, approximately half of the girls to increase their functional use of retained
demonstrate serious developmental delays and skills. The girls become more responsive to
abnormal neurological signs or symptoms. By 3 their environment. As children with Rett syn-
years of age, the children have experienced a drome approach adolescence, they are fre-
rapid deterioration of behavior as evidenced by quently subject to increased spasticity and

Figure 5.1 Stereotypic hand movements in Rett syndrome.


128 Diagnosis and Classification

vasomotor disturbances of the lower limbs, Stage 1 are nonspecific and are not predic-
possible loss of existing ambulation, scoliosis, tive of subsequent deterioration.
and a diminished rate of growth. Facial gri- Stage 2 (onset 1 to 4 years): During this
macing, bruxism (teeth grinding), hyperventi- stage, the syndrome becomes significantly
lation, apnea ( breath holding), aerophagia (air more pronounced as the children lose previ-
swallowing), constipation, and seizure activity ously acquired abilities. A relatively well-
also may accompany the syndrome (Trevathan demarcated period of rapidly declining
& Naidu, 1988). social interaction, stagnation or loss of ac-
Hagberg and Witt-Engerstrom (1986) pro- quired cognitive abilities, and loss of pur-
posed a staging system to facilitate the charac- poseful hand use and speech is evident in
terization of the disorder patterns and profiles most cases. Stereotypic movements, often
from infancy through adolescence. Their sys- virtually continuous during waking hours,
tem suggests four clinical stages and was de- become a prominent symptom. The intellec-
rived from a synthesis of clinical observations tual functioning of the girl with Rett syn-
over the years in 50 Swedish cases of Rett syn- drome during this stage is generally reported
drome. The stage patterns provide average to fall within the severe-to-profound range
guidelines for use when confronted with the di- of mental retardation. Ataxic/apraxic gait
agnostic problems resulting from the complex abnormalities are observed in ambulatory
symptomatology and longitudinal profile of girls. Also when the child is awake, she may
the condition. The four stages are Early Onset display aberrant breathing patterns. Hyper-
Stagnation Stage, Rapid Destructive Stage, ventilation and respiratory pauses (gener-
Pseudostationary Stage, and Late Motor De- ally lasting 30 to 40 seconds) are most
generation Stage. This staging system has common.
gained general acceptance; however, the names This deterioration frequently has
assigned to each stage have been criticized been sufficiently dramatic to simulate a
(Opitz, 1986; Trevathan & Naidu, 1988), and toxic or encephalitic state (Hagberg &
the stages are commonly referred to simply by Witt-Engerstrom, 1986). Parents frequently
number. The symptoms and features of each of report that their children seem irritable. Un-
the four stages are as follows: provoked episodes of screaming and sponta-
neous tantrums are common (Coleman et al.,
Stage 1 (onset 6 to 18 months): The clinical 1988). Seizure activity is present in approxi-
profile at this stage suggests a deterioration, mately one-fourth of the girls during this
or at least a general slowing down (stagna- stage. Sleep abnormalities including delayed
tion) of motor development. Hypotonia is sleep onset and increased night awakenings
typically noted. Deviation from normal de- are manifested by more than three-fourths
velopment is often compensated for or hid- of the girls (Piazza, Fisher, Kiesewetter,
den, in part, by the rapid developmental Bowman, & Moser, 1990).
speed of infancy. For example, most of the Stage 3 (onset 2 to 10 years): Persons with
children can sit independently prior to the Rett syndrome generally demonstrate di-
initiation of this stage but many fail to de- minishing autistic symptomatology and im-
velop the subsequent postural skills needed proved social interaction during this
for balance when crawling, standing, and period. They appear to be more aware of
walking. Yet, Sekul and Percy (1992) report their surroundings and seem to make at-
that as many as 60% of the girls appear to tempts at using residual functional skills.
compensate for this delay with alternative Communication skills are reported to im-
means of locomotion (e.g., rolling, creeping, prove with better interaction. Some girls
or shuffling). Approximately 80% of per- employ eye pointing, babbling, or even
sons with Rett syndrome will attain inde- word pieces to signal communicative intent
pendent ambulation. Thus, additional gross (Sekul & Percy, 1992). Seizures occur in up
motor abilities often are learned during this to 80% of the girls with Rett syndrome
stage, but are delayed in their appearance, (Coleman et al., 1988). Spasticity or rigid-
even as others are lost. The symptoms of ity and scoliosis tend to progress, and jerky,
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 129

truncal ataxia and apraxia become promi- Hagberg, Berg, & Steffenburg, 2001; Iyama,
nent. 1993). These deaths are reported throughout
Stage 4 (onset 10+ years): Progressive mus- the first 3 decades of the life span with their
cle wasting, scoliosis, spasticity, and rigid- peak occurrence falling in the second decade
ity frequently are displayed during this of life. Cardiac disturbances (Percy, 1992;
late stage. Decreasing mobility and late- Sekul et al., 1991), breathing dysfunctions, and
stage second neuron abnormalities (e.g., seizures (Hagberg, 1989) have been suggested
drop-foot abnormalities, remarkably plantar- as possible causes for these premature deaths.
flexed feet) may require the use of a wheel- The four-stage clinical pattern and profile
chair. Spinal cord dysfunction appears to act for Rett syndrome has been reported to be “a
in conjunction with extrapyramidal features sometimes crude and a somewhat simplistic
to lessen mobility (Witt-Engerstrom & Hag- frame” for specifically characterizing and
berg, 1990). Interestingly, this stage is covering the whole profile in all cases (Hag-
marked by increased motor deterioration berg & Witt-Engerstrom, 1986, p. 58). Transi-
only. The person’s cognitive functioning re- tions between stages are often indistinct and
mains stable, while social interaction (eye may be difficult to discern precisely for re-
contact) and attentiveness improve. Seizure search purposes (Philippart, 1986). Even so,
activity often becomes less problematic, al- the staging system has been found to be a use-
lowing for a decrease in the anticonvulsant ful instrument for a more systematic registra-
regimen for some girls. tion, thought, and approach to the complex
clinical manifestations of individuals with the
Although persons over the age of 25 have Rett syndrome as they progress through the
been identified, the oldest being 76 years of disorder.
age (International Rett Syndrome Association,
personal communication, 2002), little system- DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA
atic research has been conducted with this
group to provide information on the course of The diagnostic criteria for Rett syndrome, ini-
the disorder past adolescence. There is some tially developed in the mid-1980s (Hagberg,
evidence to suggest that the life expectancy of Goutieres, Hanefeld, Rett, & Wilson, 1985;
persons with Rett syndrome may be dimin- The Rett Syndrome Diagnostic Criteria Work
ished; however, precise information is not Group, 1988), have recently been revised (Hag-
available at this time. A recent case study of a berg, Hanefeld, Percy, & Skjeldal, 2002). The
60-year-old woman with Rett syndrome was re- fourth edition (revised) of the Diagnostic and
ported by Jacobsen, Viken, and von Tetzchner Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-
(2001). With proper attention and care, this IV-TR) has included Rett syndrome (termed
person was able to regain numerous lost skills Rett’s Disorder) as a subcategory of Pervasive
including ambulation. The report stresses the Developmental Disorder (American Psychiatric
importance of environmental adaptations nec- Association, 1994). The DSM-IV-TR diagnostic
essary for rehabilitation. Failure to keep accu- criteria for 299.80 Rett’s Disorder generally
rate developmental histories, infrequent coincide with those initially proposed by the
diagnostic evaluations of adults, and the proba- Rett Syndrome Diagnostic Criteria Work Group
bility of secondary contractures masking the (1988). The DSM-IV-TR criteria, however, dif-
more classic signs of the disorder impede the fer in some significant ways from the most
identification of older persons with Rett syn- recent revision of the diagnostic criteria devel-
drome. Malnutrition, uncontrolled seizures, oped by members of an international panel of
swallowing difficulties, and health problems experts convened by the International Rett Syn-
secondary to immobility increase the risk for a drome Association (IRSA: Hagberg et al.,
shortened life span in persons with Rett syn- 2002). For example, the DSM-IV-TR indicates
drome. Premature deaths in ambulatory girls “apparently normal psychomotor development
thought to be healthy, except for demonstrating through the first 5 months after birth” whereas
Rett syndrome, and whose seizures were under Hagberg and his associates (2002) indicate that
control have been reported (Hagberg, 1989; psychomotor development is “largely normal
130 Diagnosis and Classification

through the first six months or may be delayed similar to those reported for Rett syndrome. In
from birth” (p. 295). Deceleration of head fact, six males have been registered with the
growth between 5 and 48 months of age is a International Rett Syndrome Association (per-
necessary criterion within the DSM-IV-TR. The sonal communication, 2002). All but one of
revised IRSA diagnostic criteria, on the other these male cases, however, fail to meet the
hand, indicate that postnatal deceleration of strict criteria necessary to be included as con-
head growth will be evident in the majority of firmed classical cases of Rett syndrome. One
cases, but not all. A slight variation between report (Topcu et al., 2002) describes a male
the DSM-IV-TR and the IRSA diagnostic crite- who does, in fact, display classical Rett syn-
ria involves the established age limits for vari- drome and is mosaic (somatic mosaicism) for
ous symptoms. The DSM-IV-TR has adopted a the truncating MECP2 mutation. Nevertheless,
5-month upper age level for normal psychomo- Rett syndrome is almost exclusively seen in
tor development and the lower age limit for loss girls due to the predominant occurrence of mu-
of purposeful hand skills. Thus, these criteria tations on the paternal X chromosome, and also
coincide with the 5-month lower limit for the the presumed early postnatal lethal effect of the
appearance of decelerated head growth. The disease-causing mutations in hemizygous boys
adoption of the 5-month upper age limit for nor- (Topcu et al., 2002).
mal development may prove too restrictive. The
Rett Syndrome Diagnostic Criteria Work Group VARIANTS OF RETT SYNDROME
(1988) provided a footnote to their criteria to
indicate that “apparently normal development The mutation of MECP2, a regulating gene on
may appear for up to 18 months” (p. 426). No the X chromosome, has been linked to the Rett
such notation is provided within the current syndrome (Amir et al., 1999; Hoffbuhr, Moses,
DSM-IV criteria. Jerdonek, & Naidu, 2002). A number of re-
The Hagberg et al. (2002) criteria specify searchers have completed surveys of the muta-
supportive criteria such as disturbances in tions found in various populations of individuals
breathing when awake, bruxism, impaired with Rett syndrome (e.g., Bieber Nielsen et al.,
sleep patterns, impaired muscle tone, muscle 2001; Bienvenu et al., 2000; Cheadle, Gill, &
wasting, and dystonia, to mention a few. The Fleming, 2000; Wan, Lee, & Zhang, 1999;
DSM-IV-TR includes EEG abnormalities, Xiang et al., 2000) and have reported the identi-
seizures, nonspecific brain-imaging abnormal- fication of mutations in MECP2 in 35% to 87%
ities, and severe or profound mental retarda- of those tested.
tion (Axis II ) as associated features and Although mutations of the MECP2 gene ap-
disorders. Thus, the two sources differ in the pear to play a critical role in the development
associated and supportive features of the dis- of Rett syndrome, they cannot yet serve as dis-
order. Moreover, the current DSM-IV-TR crite- tinctive diagnostic markers because other dis-
ria fail to indicate exclusionary criteria that orders have been linked to mutations of the
are critical for differential diagnosis. Table 5.1 MECP2 gene. These mutations have been doc-
provides the revised diagnostic criteria for umented in persons affected with severe en-
Rett syndrome specified by Hagberg and his cephalopathy (Hoffbuhr, Devaney, et al., 2001;
associates (2002) and Rett’s Disorder as pro- Wan et al., 1999), X-linked mental retardation
vided in the DSM-IV-TR. (Meloni, Bruttini, & Longo, 2000), infantile
Classical Rett syndrome, to date, is almost autism (Beyer et al., 2002), mild learning dis-
exclusively described in female patients. Some ability (Hoffbuhr, Devaney, et al., 2001), and
researchers (Clayton-Smith, Watson, Ramsden, an Angelman syndrome-like phenotype (P. Wat-
& Black, 2000; Coleman, 1990; Eeg-Olofsson, son, Black, & Ramsden, 2001). Therefore, as
Al-Zuhair, Teebi, Zaki, & Daoud, 1990; no specific diagnostic marker variable has yet
Jan, Dooley, & Gordon, 1999; Philippart, 1990; been identified for Rett syndrome, clinical ho-
Topcu et al., 2002; Topcu, Topaloglu, Renda, mogeneity is essential for epidemiological and
Berker, & Turanli, 1991), however, have pre- research purposes. The strict diagnostic crite-
sented case studies of males displaying behav- ria previously discussed, however, may result
ioral symptoms and developmental histories in a failure to recognize the spectrum of pheno-
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 131

TABLE 5.1 Diagnostic Criteria for Rett Syndrome and Rett’s Disorder
DSM-IV-TR
Diagnostic Criteria Rett’s Disorder

Necessary Criteria Necessary criteria


1. Apparently normal prenatal and perinatal history A. All of the following:
2. Psychomotor development largely normal through 1. Apparently normal prenatal and perinatal
the first 6 months or may be delayed from birtha development
3. Normal head circumference at birth 2. Apparently normal psychomotor development
4. Postnatal deceleration of head growth in the through the first 5 months after birth
majority B. Onset of all of the following after the period of
5. Loss of achieved purposeful hand skills between normal development:
ages 6 months and 2 1⁄ 2 years 1. Deceleration of head growth between ages 5
6. Stereotypic hand movements such as hand and 48 months
wringing/squeezing, clapping/tapping, mouthing 2. Loss of previously acquired purposeful hand
and washing/rubbing automatisms skills between ages 5 and 30 months with
7. Emerging social withdrawal, communication subsequent development of stereotyped hand
dysfunction, loss of learned words, and cognitive movements (e.g., hand-wringing and hand-
impairment washing)
8. Impaired (dyspraxic) or failing locomotion 3. Loss of social engagement early in the course
(although often social interaction develops later)
Supportive Criteria 4. Appearance of poorly coordinated gait or trunk
1. Disturbances of breathing while awake ( hyper- movements
ventilation, breath holding, forced expulsion of air 5. Severely impaired expressive and receptive
or saliva, air swallowing) language development with severe psychomotor
2. Bruxism (teeth grinding) retardation
3. Impaired sleep pattern from early infancy
4. Abnormal muscle tone successively associated with Associated Features and Disorders
muscle wasting and dystonia 1. Severe or profound mental retardation (Axis II )
5. Peripheral vascomotor disturbances 2. Increased frequency of EEG abnormalities and
6. Scoliosis/ kyphosis progressing through childhood seizure disorders
7. Growth retardation 3. Nonspecific brain imaging abnormalities
8. Hypotrophic small and cold feet: small, thin hands

Exclusionary Criteria
1. Organomegaly or other signs of storage disease
2. Retinopathy, optic atrophy, or cataract
3. Evidence of perinatal or postnatal brain damage
4. Existence of identifiable metabolic or other
progressive neurological disorder
5. Acquired neurological disorder resulting from severe
infections or head trauma
a
Development may appear to be normal for up to 18 months.
Source: From “An Update on Clinically Applicable Diagnostic Criteria in Rett Syndrome,” by B. Hagberg,
F. Hanefeld, A. Percy, and O. Skjeldal, 2002, European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 6, pp. 292–297; the diag-
nostic criteria and information on Rett’s Disorder is from Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
fourth edition, text revision, American Psychiatric Association, 1994, Washington, DC: Author. Reprinted with
permission.

typic manifestations that might be included his associates (2002) have developed revised
under the Rett syndrome classification. criteria for the delineation of variant pheno-
Clinical variants of children with similar types. These criteria are provided in Table 5.2.
clinical courses who do not fulfill all the cur- Atypical forms include individuals with devel-
rent diagnostic criteria have been recognized opmental delays prior to regression or who lack
in the literature (Hagberg, 1995; Hagberg & the initial period of normal development (con-
Skjeldal, 1994; Huppke, Held, Laccone, & genital variant) or who display an early psy-
Hanefeld, 2003; Zappella, 1992). Hagberg and chomotor delay but without regression until
132 Diagnosis and Classification

TABLE 5.2 Diagnostic Criteria for the the variant forms of Rett syndrome may indi-
Delineation of Variant Phenotypes cate the presence of genocopying mutations or
A. Must meet at least three of the following Main (yet unidentified) mutations in regulatory ele-
Criteria: ments of MECP2 (Shahbazian & Zoghbi, 2002).
1. Absence or reduction of hand skills
2. Reduction or loss of babble speech
3. Monotonous pattern of hand stereotypies
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS
4. Reduction or loss of communication skills
5. Deceleration of head growth from first years The presentation of Rett syndrome differs con-
of life siderably depending on the stage and age of
6. Rett syndrome disease profile: a regression observation. For example, a child 4 or 5 years
stage followed by a recovery interaction of age with classical Rett syndrome can be cor-
contrasting with slow neuromotor regression rectly diagnosed with relative ease. Due to
B. Must meet at least 6 of the following Supportive vague symptomology, diagnosis during infancy
Criteria is frequently misinterpreted. Likewise, the late
1. Breathing irregularities stage in adolescence displays a common, com-
2. Bloating/air swallowing
3. Bruxism ( harsh sounding teeth grinding)
plex picture of severe multiple disabilities
4. Abnormal locomotion with secondary contractures resembling any
5. Scoliosis/ kyphosis number of disorders and, therefore, often is
6. Lower limb amyotrophy (weakening and misdiagnosed. To fully understand this condi-
wasting of the muscle) tion, the diagnostician must recognize and
7. Cold, purplish feet, usually growth impaired consider the entire disease process (Trevathan
8. Sleep disturbances including night screaming
& Naidu, 1988).
outbursts
9. Laughing/screaming spells
Only 18% to 23% of the estimated 8,000 to
10. Diminished response to pain 10,000 individuals in the United States af-
11. Intense eye contact /eye pointing flicted with Rett syndrome have been identi-
Note: From “An Update on Clinically Applicable Diag-
fied thus far (International Rett Syndrome
nostic Criteria in Rett Syndrome,” by B. Hagberg, F. Association, personal communication, 2002;
Hanefeld, A. Percy, and O. Skjeldal, 2002, European Moser, 1986). Lack of awareness of this disor-
Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 6, pp. 292–297. der on the part of physicians and clinicians is
undoubtedly a major contributing factor to this
state of affairs. Even when physicians are
school age (delayed onset variant); a group aware of the Rett syndrome, an accurate diag-
whose supportive characteristics do not appear nosis is not always forthcoming. Table 5.3 pre-
until late childhood (atypical or “ form fruste”); sents some of the clinical characteristics by
those with a family recurrence (familial vari- stage and differential diagnoses often assigned
ant); and those who have retained some speech to persons with Rett syndrome.
(“preserved speech variant ”). In the latter The most common nonspecific diagnosis for
group, words or even short sentences may be children with Rett syndrome above age 1 year is
retained, although they are typically not em- reported to be early infantile autism (DSM-IV-
ployed in a functional manner. The head cir- TR—299.00 Autistic Disorder; Olsson, 1987).
cumference of individuals with the form fruste In fact, many children with Rett syndrome
variant often fall within normal limits, yet sig- seem to fulfill the necessary criteria to estab-
nificantly below the norm (Hagberg, Stenbom, lish the diagnosis of infantile autism (Gillberg,
& Witt-Engerstrom, 2000). Individuals with 1987; Olsson, 1987; Olsson & Rett, 1985).
the congenital variant often display a more se- Thus, some researchers (Allen, 1988; Gillberg,
vere phenotype than that found in classical Rett 1989) argued that perhaps Rett syndrome might
syndrome (Hagberg et al., 2000). Mutations in best be thought of as a subtype of autism or
the MECP2 gene have been identified in as overlapping diagnostic entities. The DSM-IV-
many as 50% of the individuals tested with TR includes Autistic Disorder and Rett’s
variant forms of Rett syndrome (Bieber Nielsen Disorder as subcategories of Pervasive De-
et al., 2001; Cheadle et al., 2000). The lower velopmental Disorder. The behavioral pat-
percentage of identified MECP2 mutations in terns, progression, and prognosis of these two
TABLE 5.3 Rett Syndrome: Clinical Characteristics and Dif ferential Diagnosis by Stage

Stage Clinical Characteristics Differential Diagnosis

Early onset stagnation stage Developmental stagnation Benign congenital hypotonia


Onset: 6 to 18 months Deceleration of head/ brain growth Prader-Willi syndrome
Duration: months Disinterest in play activity Cerebral palsy
Hypotonia
Rapid destructive stage Rapid developmental regression Autism
Onset: 1 to 4 years with irritability Psychosis
Duration: weeks to months Loss of hand use Hearing or visual disturbance
Seizures Encephalitis
Hand stereotypies: wringing, clap- Infantile spasms (West syndrome)
ping, tapping, mouthing Tuberous sclerosis
Autistic manifestations Ornithine carbamoyl transferase
Loss of expressive language deficiency
Insomnia Phenylketonuria
Self-abusive behavior (e.g., chew- Infantile neuronal ceroid lipofusci-
ing fingers, slapping face) nosis (INCL)
Plateau stage Severe mental retardation /apparent Spastic ataxic cerebral palsy
Onset: 2 to 10 years dementia Spinocerebral degeneration
Duration: months to years Amelioration of autistic features Leukodystrphies or other storage
Seizures disorders
Typical hand stereotypies: wring- Neuroaxonal dystrophy
ing, tapping, mouthing Lennox-Gastaut syndrome
Prominent ataxia and apraxia Angelman syndrome
Spasticity
Hyperventilation, breath-holding,
aerophagia
Apnea during wakefulness
Weight loss with excellent appetite
Early scoliosis
Bruxism
Late motor deterioration Combined upper and lower motor Unknown degenerative disorder
Onset: 10+ years neuron signs
Duration: years Progressive scoliosis, muscle
wasting, and rigidity
Decreasing mobility: wheelchair-
bond
Growth retardation
Improved eye contact
Staring, unfathomable gaze
Virtual absence of expressive and
receptive language
Trophic disturbance of feet
Reduced seizure frequency

Source: “Diagnostic Criteria for Rett Syndrome,” by The Rett Syndrome Diagnostic Criteria Work Group, 1988,
Annals of Neurology, 23, pp. 425–428. Reprinted with permission.

133
134 Diagnosis and Classification

conditions, nevertheless, differ significantly. TABLE 5.4 Comparison of Rett Syndrome and
Clinically important differences have been Infantile Autism
identified between the Rett syndrome (espe- Rett Syndrome
cially during the latter two stages) and other 1. Normal development to 6 to 18 months
conditions with autism or autistic traits (Naidu 2. Progressive loss of speech and hand function
et al., 1990; Olsson, 1987; Olsson & Rett, 1985, 3. Profound mental retardation in all functional
areas
1990; Percy, Zoghbi, Lewis, & Jankovic, 1988).
4. Acquired microcephaly, growth retardation,
A basic distinction between the two disorders decreased weight gain
can be made on the basis of motor behavioral 5. Stereotypic hand movements always present
analysis (Olsson & Rett, 1985, 1987; Percy, 6. Progressive gait difficulties, with gait and trun-
Zoghbi, et al., 1988). “Whereas autism repre- cal apraxia and ataxia; some may become nonam-
sents a regression of verbal but not motor skills; bulatory
Rett syndrome involves the simultaneous re- 7. Language always absent
8. Eye contact present, and sometimes very intense
gression of both skills” (Percy, Zoghbi, et al., 9. Little interest in manipulating objects
1988, p. S67). Stereotypic behavior associated 10. Seizures in at least 70% in early childhood (vari-
with infantile autism is generally more complex ous seizure types)
and, unlike that in Rett syndrome, often in- 11. Bruxism, hyperventilation with air-swallowing
volves the manipulation of an object with and breath-holding common
preservation of the pincer grasp. Children with 12. Choreoathetoid movements and dystonia may be
present
Rett syndrome reportedly differ from children
with infantile autism with respect to their res- Infantile Autism
piratory pattern (displaying breath-holding, 1. Onset from early infancy
hyperventilation, and air-saliva expulsion); the 2. Loss of previously acquired skills does not occur
3. More scatter of intellectual function. Visual-
presence of ataxia and apraxia, bruxism, hy-
spatial and manipulative skills often better than
poactivity, and a general slowness of move- apparent verbal skills
ments; and an absence of purposeful hand 4. Physical development normal in the majority
movement (Gillberg, 1986; Olsson, 1987; 5. Stereotypic behavior is more varied in manifes-
Percy, Zoghbi, et al., 1988; Sekul & Percy, tation and is always more complex; midline man-
1992). Persons with Rett syndrome demon- ifestations rare
strate a very restricted repertoire of movements 6. Gait and other gross motor functions normal in
first decade of life
that appear monotonous in both form and speed
7. Language sometimes absent; if present, peculiar
(Olsson & Rett, 1985). Van Acker (1987) re- speech patterns always present; markedly
ported that the stereotypic behaviors of persons impaired nonverbal communication
with Rett syndrome were displayed in patterned 8. Eye contact with others typically avoided or
sequences with significant conditional proba- inappropriate
bilities, whereas those of persons with infantile 9. Stereotypic ritualistic behavior usually involves
autism were displayed in a random fashion. skillful but odd manipulation of objects or sen-
sory self-stimulation
Budden (1986) has presented another critical 10. Seizures (usually temporal-limbic complex par-
feature that may help in the differential diagno- tial) in 25% in late adolescence and adulthood
sis of Rett syndrome and infantile autism: Per- 11. Bruxism, hyperventilation, and breath-holding
sons with Rett syndrome frequently develop not typical
appropriate speech before the onset of symp- 12. Dystonia and chorea not present*
toms. On the other hand, children with autism *Extrapyramidal signs may appear in some patients
differ from those with Rett syndrome in that with autism after puberty.
they display overactivity and inappropriate vo- Source: “ The Clinical Recognition and Differential
calizations, and tend to replicate simple motor Diagnosis of Rett Syndrome,” by E. Trevathan and S.
activities or complex movements within a rich Naidu, 1988, Journal of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.),
repertoire of motor behavior (Percy, Zoghbi, pp. S6–S16. Reprinted with permission.
et al., 1988). Table 5.4 presents a comparison of
the clinical manifestations differentiating Rett
syndrome from infantile autism.
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 135

Considering the present concept of infantile Both disorders cause a rapid regression of psy-
autism as a behavioral syndrome, the initial chomotor development and the manifestation of
differential diagnosis of Rett syndrome for hand and finger stereotypies at approximately
some children may prove somewhat problem- the same age. As INCL progresses, however,
atic. Infantile autism, however, is very rare in myoclonus and retinal degeneration becomes
a female, which means that the mere presence apparent and differentiates the two disorders
of severe autistic symptomology in a girl under (Sekul & Percy, 1992). Failure to designate
2 years should prompt the consideration of retinopathy or optic atrophy as exclusionary
Rett syndrome in the differential diagnosis. criteria for Rett’s Disorder within the DSM-IV-
One must also be aware, however, that “a large TR may complicate the differential diagnosis of
percentage of children with Rett syndrome age this disorder.
0 to 6 months or older than 3 to 5 years are not A report by Philippart (1993) links Rett syn-
autistic” (Olsson & Rett, 1987). Thus, physi- drome with tuberous sclerosis, a nerocutaneous
cians and clinicians alike must realize that the disorder. Although this disorder may show ini-
presence of an autistic behavioral syndrome is tial similarities to Rett syndrome, close exami-
not an obligatory condition for a diagnosis of nation of the skin with a Wood’s lamp and
Rett syndrome (Olsson & Rett, 1987). the presence of serial computed tomography
Millichap (1987) has suggested that Rett abnormalities will distinguish this disorder
syndrome might represent a variant of Child- (Sekul & Percy, 1992). Chromosomal disorders,
hood Disintegrative Disorder (Heller’s syn- such as Angelman syndrome (“ happy puppet
drome). Children with Childhood Disintegrative syndrome”) can display similar features to Rett
Disorder develop their symptoms later (e.g., at syndrome. Children with Angelman syndrome,
least age 2 and typically age 3 or 4 years) than however, fail to display a period of normal
individuals with Rett syndrome and normal development and subsequent rapid regression.
neurological findings are reported in persons Acute and chronic encephalitis may be dis-
with the former disease. In a comparison of tinguished by examination of the cerebrospinal
two boys with Childhood Disintegrative Disor- fluid (CSF) and by a characteristic elec-
der and six girls with Rett syndrome, Burd, troencphalography. The loss of language and
Fisher, and Kerbeshian (1989) reported per- the development of seizures in preschool-age
sons afflicted with these disorders differed children are similar in both Rett syndrome and
from children with classic autism. Children with the Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Head circum-
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder (Heller’s ference growth and motor skills are preserved
syndrome) and Rett syndrome displayed nor- in the Landau-Kleffner syndrome.
mal prenatal and perinatal periods, followed In summary, the clinical identification of
by marked developmental regression after Rett syndrome rests on the careful exploration
which they acquired few or no new skills. The of clinical manifestations and the specific
authors have suggested that these children pattern of symptom progression. The differen-
should be distinguished from those with classic tial diagnosis based on clinical observation, al-
autism, and should be classified as “pervasive though frequently difficult at presentation
disintegrative disorder, Heller type” and “per- (especially during the earliest stages), becomes
vasive disintegrative disorder, Rett type.” The much easier after follow-up over several months
DSM-IV-TR has included both Rett’s Disorder to a few years. A diagnosis of Rett syndrome
(Rett syndrome) and Childhood Disintegrative should involve a molecular genetic analysis to
Disorder (Heller’s syndrome) as subcategories identify mutations in the MECP2 gene.
of Pervasive Developmental Disorders.
Stage 2 developmental regressions often sug- EPIDEMIOLOGY
gest neurodegenerative diseases. The earliest
stages of Rett syndrome are difficult to distin- Rett syndrome has been reported to exist on all
guish from infantile neuronal ceroid lipofuscin- the populated continents and in most countries
sis (INCL), an autosomal recessive disease of the world (e.g., Budden, 1986; Goutieres
especially frequent in the Finnish population. & Aicardi, 1985; Hanaoka, Ishikawa, &
136 Diagnosis and Classification

Kamoshita, 1985; Kerr & Stephenson, 1986; studies suggesting the prevalence of Rett syn-
Moodley, 1992). Thus, the literature supports drome has perhaps been overestimated.
the view that Rett syndrome does not seem to No matter which estimate is employed, Rett
be a rare disorder and that it is more or less syndrome seems to be significantly more preva-
universal; additionally less than 2 per 100 lent among girls than phenylketonuria (PKU), a
cases of Rett syndrome display familial rela- condition for which all neonates are screened in
tionships (Zoghbi, 1988). Thus, the pattern of the majority of developed countries (Hagberg,
occurrence of Rett syndrome is dissimilar 1985). As progressive brain disorders and meta-
to that of traditional inborn errors of metabo- bolic diseases together constitute only 5% to
lism (e.g., glactosemia, Hartnup’s disease, ke- 6% (1.5 to 2.0 per 10,000 children) of the eti-
toaciduria, phenylketonuria), which often ologies in persons with severe or profound men-
display strong geographic, ethnic, and familial tal retardation, the Rett syndrome should be
accumulation. considered as an important etiologic factor in
Approximately 2,329 cases of the Rett syn- females, second only to Down syndrome. In
drome worldwide are registered with the Inter- fact, this syndrome might well be responsible
national Rett Syndrome Association, with the for one-fourth to one-third of progressive devel-
following distribution: United States, 1,887; opmental disabilities among females (Hagberg,
Canada, 101; Mexico, 9; and other foreign, 312 1985).
(International Rett Syndrome Association, per-
sonal communication, 2002). The prevalence of ETIOLOGY
the Rett syndrome has been studied, based on
the Swedish registry for mental retardation Since Rett syndrome, at least in its classical
and surveys of neuropediatricians, in a part of form, had only been reported in females cou-
southwestern Sweden comprising five counties pled with a number of familial cases (repre-
and the city of Gothenburg (Hagberg, 1985). In sented in Table 5.5), researchers suspected the
a population of 315,469 children and adoles- existence of an X-linked dominant genetic in-
cents, 6 to 17 years of age, 13 cases were de- heritance as the basis for this disorder. It was
tected, all girls. The corresponding prevalence hypothesized that the genetic mutation would
was about 1 per 15,000 live female births. In be lethal in males, resulting in a spontaneous
their study of 5,400 consecutive referrals to the abortion of the fetus (Comings, 1986; Ric-
pediatric neurology center in western Scotland, cardi, 1986). Genetic mapping in the familial
Kerr and Stephenson (1985) identified 19 cases cases identified a mutation at the Xq28 locus
of Rett syndrome. The resulting prevalence rate (Sirianni, Naidu, Pereira, Pillotto, & Hoffman,
was very similar to that reported by Hagberg 1998). In 1999, researchers (Amir et al., 1999)
(1985); 1 per 12,000 to 13,000 females (Kerr & identified mutations in the MECP2 gene as the
Stephenson, 1986). cause of Rett syndrome.
A prevalence study was conducted within During normal human development, many
several geographic areas of the state of Texas in genes are expressed in a tissue-specific manner.
the United States by researchers at the Baylor That is, the gene must only function in the cre-
College of Medicine (Kozinetz et al., 1993). ation of specific cells. In fact, more than one-
The study employed the Texas Rett Syndrome third of our genes are expressed only within the
Registry and explored females ages 2 through brain. Many of these genes are needed during
18 years. This study was the first with a large critical periods of central nervous system
ethnic mix that would allow an exploration of (CNS) development, and then their expression
racial /ethnic group-specific prevalence of Rett must be turned off. Other genes are required
syndrome. A prevalence estimate of approxi- only after birth and turning these genes on and
mately 1 per 22,800 live female births was off at appropriate times is critical for normal
reported with no significant differences in and proper development (Lombroso, 2000). The
prevalence estimates by race/ethnicity (African MECP2 gene encodes the development of the
American, Caucasian, and Hispanic). This es- transcriptional silencing methyl-CpG binding
timate is lower than that reported in earlier protein-2. This protein plays an important role
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 137

TABLE 5.5 Familial Cases in Rett Syndrome


Number of Pairs
Monozygotic twins
Both females aff licted 8
Only one female aff licted 1
Dizygotic twins (female/female)
Both females aff licted 2
Only one female aff licted 5
Dizygotic twins (female/male)
Female aff licted 8
Full sisters 6
Half-sisters 2
Full cousins 1
Second cousins 2
Second half-cousins 1
Aunt—niece 1
Great-grand aunt—niece 1
Sister and half-brother, both have children with Rett syndrome 1
Mother—daughter 1
Source: “Genetic Aspects of Rett Syndrome,” by H. Y. Zoghbi, 1988, Journal of Child
Neurology, 3(Suppl.), pp. S76–S78. Adapted with permission.

in the “silencing” of various genes during CNS Belichenko, Woodcock, and Woodcock (2001)
development. This protein binds to prescribed suggest the need to explore further those cases
methylated cytosine nucleotides (CpG dinu- in which no mutations of the MECP2 gene have
cleotides) on the DNA. The bound DNA- been identified for individuals who have been
MECP2 complex then interacts with a histone clinically diagnosed with Rett syndrome. They
deacetylase complex and the transcriptional suggest that additional novel MECP2 muta-
co-repressor Sin3A. Together, these repressors tions, other genetic mutations, or external fac-
alter the chromatin making the genes inaccessi- tors may yet be identified to play a role in the
ble to transcriptional activators—in essence, occurrence of Rett syndrome.
silencing the further transcription of that gene. Numerous different mutations of the
The mutations in the MECP2 gene in Rett syn- MECP2 gene have been identified in individu-
drome result in a failure to produce the MECP2 als with Rett syndrome. Van den Veyver and
protein. Thus, the genes that MECP2 would Zoghbi (2001) found 30 missense mutations
normally silence continue to engage in ongoing (single-base changes that result in the substitu-
transcription. The number and nature of the tion of one amino acid for another in the protein
genes for which MECP2 is meant to suppress product), 22 of which are in the methyl-CpG
throughout the genome is as yet unknown binding domain (MBD) of MECP2. Addition-
(Singer & Naidu, 2001); although there is rea- ally they found 35 nonsense mutations (single-
son to believe these genes regulate the develop- base changes that create a termination condon
ment and mature function of the brain and resulting in a shortened, dysfunctional protein
central nervous system (Shahbazian & Zoghbi, product), 12 frameshift mutations (changes in
2002; Webb & Latif, 2001). Genetic analyses the DNA chain that occur when the number of
have reported as many as 87% of the females nucleotides inserted or deleted is not a multiple
displaying the classical Rett syndrome and 50% of three, so that every condon beyond that point
of those displaying a variant form test positive is read incorrectly during translation), and 1
for mutations of the MECP2 gene (Bieber splice-site mutation (failure to remove intron,
Nielsen et al., 2001). These numbers suggest or noncoding DNA, sequences prior to protein
strong support for the current criteria used translation altering the sequence of the protein
for clinical diagnosis of Rett syndrome. Kerr, product).
138 Diagnosis and Classification

Most mutations occur de novo (as new inactivation is skewed. That is, a greater number
spontaneous mutations in the generation of the of the genes on either the paternal or maternal X
sperm or ovum; Dragich, Houwink-Manville, chromosome remain active. Approximately 10%
& Schanen, 2000). Kondo et al. (2000) propose of females in the general population displayed
that Rett syndrome is more frequent in females skewed inactivation of their X chromosomes
since 88% of the sporadic or de novo cases in (Lyon, 1972). As the cell reproduces, the same
their study appeared to have paternally de- gene on each X chromosome remains
rived mutations in MECP2. In contrast, the fa- active (or inactive) in each copy. The severity
milial cases and mutations in a number of of the symptoms in Rett syndrome may be re-
males suspected of displaying a variant of Rett lated to both the number and the location of
syndrome were determined to be maternally the genes with the mutated MECP2 gene. If the
derived. Girard et al. (2001) report that de active gene in most cells holds the mutation
novo MECP2 mutations with paternal origin (skewed XCI ); symptoms will likely be more
were identified in 71% of the families studied. severe. Likewise, if the cells with the active
Two cases of maternal origin MECP2 muta- gene displaying the mutation are more often in-
tion were reported. The high frequency of volved in the encoding for the development of
male germ-line transmission of the mutation is critical tissues (e.g., brain cells) symptoms
consistent with a predominant occurrence of would be more dramatic. Bieber Nielsen et al.
the disease in females (as males do not inherit (2001), however, failed to identify any correla-
their X chromosome from their father). tion between the X chromosome inactivation
If separate mutations affect a single gene, pattern in peripheral blood samples with the
the condition is termed allelic heterogeneity, clinical presentation or severity of symptoms in
and this is found in the majority of disease- a group of Danish women with Rett syndrome.
causing mutations studied to date. Interest- To date, mutations in the MECP2 gene have
ingly, even when different regions of the same been identified in many, but not all individuals
gene are mutated, they often result in the diagnosed with the Rett syndrome (e.g., Bien-
same clinical phenotype among affected venu et al., 2000; Hoffbuhr et al., 2001; Xiang
individuals. Occasionally, however, different et al., 2000). MECP2 mutations have been iden-
mutations within the same gene will result tified in 70% to 90% of sporadic or de novo
in different clinical presentations. This may cases and approximately 50% of familial cases
explain some of the Rett syndrome variants. of Rett syndrome (Shahbazian & Zoghbi, 2001).
One study (Huppke, Laccone, Kramer, Engel, Failure to identify the mutation may result
& Hanefeld, 2000), however, failed to corre- from many factors including the accuracy of the
late the type of mutation and the phenotype. original diagnosis and the methodology used
Thus, factors other than type or position of the to screen for the mutations (e.g., secondary
mutation may influence the severity of the structural content prediction [SSCP], gene se-
symptoms. quencing, denaturing high performance liquid
Another factor hypothesized to play an im- chromatography [DHPLC]). Failure to identify
portant role in the symptoms associated with mutations in the MECP2 gene in individuals
Rett syndrome involves the pattern of X chro- displaying Rett syndrome also might indicate
mosome inactivation (XCI; Amir et al., 2000; that mutations will be found in other genes
Hoffbuhr et al., 2001; Takagi, 2001; Zoghbi, within the same enzymatic pathway as the
Percy, Schultz, & Fill, 1990). At conception, fe- MECP2 gene. Thus, genes encoding for other
males have two X chromosomes, with similar proteins required in the proper methylation of
genes on each (a gene pair). Having the genes the specific DNA sequences, or the deacetyla-
on both X chromosomes remain active results tion of histones might be involved (Lombroso,
in severe effects; thus, one of the genes in each 2000). Only the coding region of the MECP2
pair must be inactivated. Typically, the pattern gene has been carefully analyzed, so mutations
of inactivation is random between the genes on in regulatory elements (similar to other known
the X chromosome provided by the mother and C ➝ T transition mutation disorders) could ac-
those on the X chromosome provided by the count for those cases in which no mutation has
father. In some cases, however, the pattern of been identified (Shahbazian & Zoghbi, 2002).
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 139

As mentioned, mutations of the MECP2 the result of arrested brain development (Arm-
gene have also resulted in disorders that are strong, 2001).
phenotypically distinct from Rett syndrome Brucke et al. (1988) report the most con-
(e.g., nonspecific X-linked mental retardation, spicuous finding from their autopsy studies
congenital encephalopathy with respiratory ar- was the underpigmentation of the substantia
rest; e.g., Clayton-Smith et al., 2000; Hoffbuhr nigra (especially the zona compacta). It con-
et al., 2001). Thus, the mutation in the MECP2 tained many fewer well-pigmented neurons
gene cannot serve as a diagnostic marker vari- for the age of the person (53% to 73%), and
able for Rett syndrome. Genetic research will fewer pigmented granules per neuron,
need to further explore the correlation of geno- whereas the total number of nigral neurons
type and phenotype. More importantly, work to and the triphasic substructure of neurome-
identify the “downstream” genes that are af- lanin were within the normal range. The basal
fected by the mutation of the MECP2 gene and ganglia of some patients showed mild gliosis.
any other gene along the enzymatic pathway These findings were supported in a study of
will help improve our understanding of the 38 patients with Rett syndrome conducted in
pathomechanisms of the disorder. Sweden by Lekman et al. (1989). Brucke
et al. (1988) also reported low melanin con-
NEUROPATHOLOGY tent in the locus coeruleus. As the melanin
pigmentation in the substantia nigra normally
Neuropathological studies have been conducted increases with age, this lack of pigmentation
in Rett syndrome that may assist in a better un- serves as “evidence of a retardation in matu-
derstanding of how ineffective MECP2 activity ration of these neurons (in the substantia
produces Rett syndrome. Although individuals nigra and locus coeruleus) which possibly
with Rett syndrome are typically small for their leads to a decreased synthesis rate of dopa-
age, only the brain weighs less than expected mine and a compensatory enhancement in its
for the height and weight of the individual turnover rate” (Brucke et al., 1988, p. 323). In-
(Armstrong, Dunn, Schultz, et al., 1999). This creased levels of dopamine and serotonin
supports the view that the MECP2 gene appears metabolites in their subject support their hy-
to have its greatest impact on downstream pothesis. The abnormalities of the substantia
genes that code for the development and mature nigra and the related changes in dopamine syn-
function of brain tissue. Accumulated autopsy thesis could account for the prominent move-
studies (e.g., Brucke, Sofic, Killian, Rett, & ment disorder associated with Rett syndrome.
Riederer, 1988; Harding, Tudmay, & Wilson, Receptors for serotonin are hugely in-
1985; Jellinger & Seiteberger, 1986; Missliwetz creased in the brain stem at all ages in persons
& Depastas, 1985) suggest that the brain of the with Rett syndrome (Kerr & Witt-Engerstrom,
individual with Rett syndrome weighs signifi- 2001). Serotonin plays an early role in the de-
cantly less than that of controls matched for termination of later cortical function (Lager-
both age and height. The weight of the average crantz & Srinivasan, 1991) and is also an
brain from an individual with classical Rett important neuromodulator and neurotransmit-
syndrome weighed 950g, which is the brain ter in the mature brain.
weight of a normal 1-year-old child (Arm- Jellinger, Armstrong, Zoghbi, and Percy
strong, 2001). This researcher goes on to note (1988) presented evidence from electron mi-
that many individuals with form fruste and pre- croscopic studies of abnormal neurites in the
served speech variants of Rett syndrome dis- frontal cortex and caudate nucleus with greatly
play head circumferences (and presumably reduced axonal or dendritic connections. In a
brain weights) within normal limits. The de- study of two females with Rett syndrome
crease in brain size and weight appears to begin (Armstrong, 1992), the cortical neurons ap-
after birth (in most cases), starting at 3 to 4 peared to be less mature and demonstrated sig-
months of age. Brain size and weight appear to nificantly decreased dendritic arborization
stabilize in later childhood, and the absence of that did not appear to be age related. This could
markers for significant degenerative disorder be the result of the general growth arrest mani-
support the idea that the decreased brain size is fested in Rett syndrome and may, in part,
140 Diagnosis and Classification

explain the acquired microcephaly witnessed procedure has been developed for the post-
in Stage 1 of the disorder. mortem examination (Percy, Hass, Kolodnyu,
Golgi studies of cerebral cortical dendrites Moser, & Naidu, 1988), and is available from
(Armstrong, Dunn, Antalffy, & Trivedi, 1995) IRSA. Advance arrangements must be made
have identified a reduced dendritic arboriza- with a pathologist so that tissues can be frozen,
tion in the pyramidal neurons of Layers III, IV, optimally within 4 to 6 hours following death.
and V in frontal motor and inferior temporal
regions in the brains of individuals with Rett NEUROANATOMY
syndrome. In the affected regions, apical and
basal dendrites are selectively reduced; basal The development of sophisticated methods for
dendrites of Layers III and V in the frontal visualization of the human central nervous
and motor cortex; basal dendrites of Layer IV system in vivo has provided a means to quan-
of the subiculum, and apical dendrites of Layer tify brain structure and function in persons
V of the motor cortex. The dendrites of the with brain dysfunction. Routine neuroimaging
hippocampal and occipital regions are not sig- studies in persons with Rett syndrome, how-
nificantly reduced. The decreased dendritic ever, have only revealed occasional nonspe-
branching in Rett syndrome suggests that the cific changes. Serial computed tomography
synaptic input is reduced. (CT) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI )
A study by Cornford, Philippart, Jacobs, studies have shown evidence of progressive
Scheibel, and Vinters (1994) reports on the brain atrophy, particularly in the frontal and
neuronal changes in the brain of a girl with Rett temporal regions, in some girls after age 2
syndrome observed in a frontal lobe biopsy per- years (Krageloh-Mann, Schroth, Niemann, &
formed at age 3 years and in the postmortem Michaelis, 1989; Nihei & Naitoh, 1990; No-
brain at age 15 years. Widespread neuronal mi- mura, Segawa, & Hasegawa, 1984; Yano et al.,
tochondrial inclusions and the appearance of 1991). These findings are consistent with the
dendritic retraction in Golgi-stained cortical pathological findings of Jellinger and Seite-
pyramidal and Purkinji neurons were the most berger (1986) reported earlier.
significant neuropathological features. The Decreased cerebral blood flow (to 88% of
Golgi preparations of the frontal cortex and that noted in age-matched controls) was re-
cerebellar folia (autopsy brain) manifested ported in seven persons with Rett syndrome
truncation and thickening of the dendrites and a (Nielsen, Friberg, Lou, Lassen, & Sam, 1990).
degenerate appearance of cortical pyramidal Single photon emission computed tomography
neurons similar to that of an aged brain. Thus, demonstrated significantly decreased cerebral
neuronal and mitochondrial deterioration ap- blood flow to the prefrontal and temporal re-
peared to continue after stabilization of the gions, whereas that to the primary sensorimotor
neurological deterioration (at 3 years) in Rett cortex remained unaffected. A similar pattern
syndrome. This has led these authors to specu- of cerebral blood flow is observed in infants
late, “Rett syndrome could result from inade- (Chugani, Phelps, & Mazziotta, 1987) suggest-
quate maintenance of a full array of neuronal ing that this finding may reflect the growth ar-
contacts, similar to the aging process, in which rest noted in Rett syndrome. Further evidence
such dendritic regression apparently occurs of abnormal cerebral blood flow in Rett syn-
over the span of many years” (p. 430). drome was reported by Yoshikawa et al. (1991).
The pathogenic mechanisms of the morpho- The developmental increase of the frontal-
logical brain lesions and their relations to to-temporal cerebral blood flow ratio demon-
clinical and neurochemical findings in Rett strated in age-matched controls was not ob-
syndrome remain unknown. Neuropathological served in six females with Rett syndrome.
studies at autopsy serve a critical role in at- Employing quantitative methods of analysis
tempts to better understand Rett syndrome. in neuroimaging, Cassanova and associates
The International Rett Syndrome Association (1991) reported smaller cerebral hemispheres
urges parents of persons with this disorder to in 8 persons with Rett syndrome when com-
consider the gift of autopsy should their chil- pared with controls. A decreased area of cau-
dren die prematurely. To this end, a uniform date nucleus, even when the overall smaller
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 141

brain area was taken into account, also was neuroimaging studies could make a significant
noted in the girls with Rett syndrome. Smaller contribution to our understanding of the etiol-
cerebral hemispheres, basal ganglia, corpus ogy, homogeneity, and pathogenesis of this
callosum, cerebellar hemispheres, inferior disorder. Segawa (2001) has presented a rather
olive, and anterior vermis were reported in 13 interesting discussion of the pathophysiology
females with Rett syndrome compared with 10 of Rett syndrome by attempting to relate many
female control subjects (Murakami et al., of the neuropathological findings to clinical
1992). Reiss et al. (1993) completed a quantita- characteristics of the disorder.
tive neuroimaging study that provided in vivo
neuroanatomical correlates of the neurological NEUROCHEMICAL ALTERATIONS
and developmental features of Rett syndrome.
This group reports reduced cerebral volume, a The progressive nature of Rett syndrome after
disproportionate reduction in brain tissue vol- an apparently normal pre- and neonatal period
umes, with a greater decrease of gray matter to is highly suggestive of a metabolic disorder
white, regional variation in the percentage of similar to PKU. The pathogenesis of the dis-
cortical gray matter (with frontal regions show- order, however, remains a mystery. Extensive
ing the greatest decrease), reduced volume of research exploring biogenic amines and endor-
subcortical gray matter (with the caudate nu- phins through the analyses of serum amino
cleus showing significant volume reduction), acids, urine amino and organic acids, lysoso-
and increased cerebrospinal fluid volume when mal enzymes, and routine chemistries has been
controlling for brain volume differences in the undertaken. One hypothesis is that symptoms
females with Rett syndrome. These findings, result from an abnormality in the dopamine
especially those related to the caudate nucleus, system, a neurotransmitter system that regu-
are of interest from a clinical standpoint as lates the control of voluntary movements in the
they may help explain the significant motor extrapyramidal system. This hypothesis, based
and cognitive-developmental symptoms pres- on the decreased pigmentation of the substan-
ent in Rett syndrome. tia nigra and the prominent movement dis-
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI ) studies orders suggestive of extrapyramidal dysfunc-
have compared volumetric brain analyses of in- tion (Zoghbi, Percy, Glaze, Butler, & Riccardi,
dividuals with Rett syndrome to age-matched 1985), led Nomura and her associates (No-
controls (Gotoh et al., 2001; Subramaniam, mura et al., 1984; Nomura, Segawa, & Hig-
Naidu, & Reiss, 1997). Global reductions in urashi, 1985) to speculate that as the disease
grey matter volumes are reported; with exag- progresses the dopamine system becomes hy-
gerated loss within the prefrontal, posterior peractive due to postsynaptic supersensitivity
frontal, and anterior temporal regions. Subra- caused by hypoactive dopamine neurons. Ab-
maniam et al. (1997) reported a preferential re- normality in the dopamine system is supported
duction in the volume of the caudate nucleus. by the finding of a decrease of biogenic amine
Gotoh et al. (2001) identified thinning of the metabolites in cerebrospinal fluid in six chil-
corpus callosum, widening of the prepontin cis- dren with Rett syndrome, the most significant
tern, a narrowing of the brain stem, and cere- reductions being in homovanillic acid (HVA),
bellar atrophy in some individuals with Rett the major dopamine metabolite (Zoghbi et al.,
syndrome. No evidence of active degenerative 1985). These findings were extended to 32
disease, however, had been seen, and the whole girls found to have significant reductions in
brain seemed to be affected by the atrophy. HVA and 4-hydroxy-3-methoxyphenylethylene
The consistency of data obtained through glycol (HMPG), the metabolites of dopamine
the neuroimaging research with results from and norepinephrine (Zoghbi et al., 1989; Zoghbi
neuropathological investigations supports the et al., 1985). An abnormality in this system is
need for continued neuroimaging studies in further supported by demonstration in an au-
Rett syndrome. Longitudinal studies of sub- topsy study of decreasing binding of 3H spiper-
jects from the time the children manifest the one, a ligand with high affinity for dopamine
earliest signs of the syndrome would be espe- D2 receptors, in the putamen (Riederer et al.,
cially enlightening. Information gained from 1985). Hand-mouth stereotypies, hypotonia,
142 Diagnosis and Classification

and ataxia similar to that seen in Rett transferase (ChAT) activity in a series of
syndrome are demonstrated in boys with postmortem brain studies. The decreased
the Lesch Nyhan syndrome. In that disorder, ChAT activity may be related to loss of cholin-
all biochemical aspects of the function of ergic cells, qualitatively similar to the loss in
dopamine neuron terminals in the corpus Alzheimer’s disease. These studies (Wenk
striatum have been found to be decreased up to et al., 1991, 1993) also report decreased ChAT
10% to 30% of control values in autopsy studies activity in the hippocampus and thalamus con-
of three affected cases (Lloyd et al., 1981). sistent with a loss of cholinergic cells in the me-
Studies, however, have failed to replicate the dial septum and vertical limb of the Broca and
findings that abnormal neurotransmitter levels the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus. This
characterize the Rett syndrome. Unlike the six loss of cholinergic cells throughout the basal
cases reported by Zoghbi et al. (1985) where forebrain might well be responsible for the cog-
norepinephrine and dopamine metabolites were nitive stagnation and memory loss characteris-
reduced in comparison to control individuals, tic of Rett syndrome, as has been suggested for
subjects in a more recent investigation (Harris the dementia associated with Alzheimer’s and
et al., 1986) did not demonstrate a reduction in Parkinson’s diseases (Collerton, 1986; White-
metabolites of either of these neurotransmitter house, Price, Clark, Coyle, & DeLong, 1981).
substances. Reduction of dopamine D2 receptor Persons with Rett syndrome have been re-
binding in the putamen, as found in the autopsy ported to display a remarkable tolerance for
study by Riederer et al. (1985) with 3H spiper- pain and a high rate of stereotyped behavior,
one, was not demonstrated in living subjects by seizure activity, and respiratory disturbances.
in vivo positron-emission tomography (PET) These symptoms have been induced in labora-
scanning. Additionally, low normal receptor tory animals exposed to elevated endorphin
binding instead of dopamine receptor supersen- levels. Several research groups have therefore
sitivity was reported (in direct opposition to studied the ß-endorphin system in females
the results reported by Nomura et al., 1985). with Rett syndrome. Budden, Myer, and Butler
Similar findings are reported in a second study (1990) found elevated ß-endorphin immunoreac-
(Riederer et al., 1986), where preliminary bio- tivity in the cerebrospinal fluid of 11 out of the
chemical analyses on plasma, urine, cere- 12 girls studied. These findings were extended
brospinal fluid, and postmortem brain areas as elevated cerebrospinal fluid ß-endorphins
indicated no disturbance of neurotransmitter were reported in 90% of more than 150 persons
function. These researchers suggest various with Rett syndrome (Myer, Tripathi, Brase, &
drug therapies administered to the girls prior to Dewey, 1992). The degree of elevation of the ß-
sample testing and undernutrition (a problem endorphins, however, did not correlate with the
common to girls with the Rett syndrome) might severity of the symptoms (e.g., stereotypy,
influence the synthesis and turnover of these breathing disturbance) or stage of the disorder.
biogenic amines. An alternative hypothesis Contradictory results, however, have been re-
suggests that such a deficit might also be trig- ported (Genazzani, Zappella, Nalin, Hayek, &
gered as a primary consequence of the disease Facchinetti, 1989; Gillberg, Terenius, Hagberg,
process. Efforts to understand the often contra- Witt-Engerstrom, & Eriksson, 1990). These re-
dictory results are complicated further by the searchers report significantly lower levels of ß-
differing laboratory procedures employed by endorphins in girls with Rett syndrome
investigators throughout the world. compared with age-matched controls. As in the
Rett syndrome appears to share neurochemi- biogenic amine studies reported earlier, the
cal features (without the associated neuro- basis for these contradictory findings remains
pathological features) with some age-related unknown.
neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s Perhaps the most promising lead for a neuro-
and Parkinson’s diseases. Wenk and his associ- chemical marker for Rett syndrome has devel-
ates (Wenk, Naidu, Casanova, Kitt, & Moser, oped from a study of five autopsied cases
1991; Wenk, O’Leary, Nemeroff, Bissette, exploring brain and cerebrospinal fluid glycol-
Moser, & Naidu, 1993) have reported decreased ipids (Lekman, Hagberg, & Svennerholm,
cortical and subcortical levels of choline acetyl- 1991). The concentrations of two major brain
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 143

gangliosides, GD1a in frontal gray matter and syndrome (Zappella, 1990; Zappella & Genaz-
GD1b in temporal gray matter, appear to be zani, 1986; Zappella, Genazzani, Facchinetti,
lowered selectively in the cerebral cortex and & Hayek, 1990). Improvements in communica-
cerebellum in girls with Rett syndrome. The tion and a decreased frequency of agitation
ganglioside GD1a is thought to play an impor- episodes were initially reported; however, these
tant role in synaptogenesis since it is prominent improvements failed to recur following the
in synaptic membranes and high concentrations “ washout ” phase of the study. Uncontrolled
are reported during the time when nerve ending studies exploring the effect of other anti-
growth and synapse formation are most intense Parkinsonian drugs aimed at the monoamine
(25th fetal week until age 2 years). Ganglioside system (e.g., L-dopa, pergolide, deprenyl) have
GD1b is rich in axons and accrues more slowly, been undertaken but have failed to provide evi-
reaching maximum concentration at age 20 dence of improvement on a consistent basis.
years. The reduction in GD1a would help ex- L-dopa and Sinemet (DuPont) have been re-
plain the pathogenic findings of decreased den- ported to show benefit for a limited number
dritic arborization reported by Armstrong of patients in the later stages of the disorder
(1992). These findings appear to be specific for when increasing rigidity appeared (Percy &
Rett syndrome; however, replication of a larger Hagberg, 1992). Tetrabenazine, a monoamine
series is required to validate the results. depleter and blocker, resulted in an exacerba-
Synapses within the cerebral cortex, basal tion of symptoms in one patient (Sekul & Percy,
ganglia, and brain stem responsible for move- 1992). Egger, Hofacker, Schiel, and Holthausen
ment and breathing employ the excitatory amino (1992), reported that magnesium orotate or cit-
acid neurotransmitter glutamate. Glutamate- rate (4 to 10 mg/ kg/day) initially given as an
mediated neurotransmission appears to be dis- anticonvulsant (after more traditional anticon-
rupted in persons with Rett syndrome. Two vulsants had failed) resulted in a decrease in
studies (Hamberger, Gillberg, Palm, & Hag- hyperventilation in a girl with Rett syndrome.
berg, 1992; Lappalainien & Riikonen, 1996) They extended their findings to six additional
of the cerebrospinal fluid in girls with Rett patients with Rett syndrome. A decrease in hy-
syndrome report elevated levels of glutamate perventilation was reported in all girls and par-
in young children and significantly reduced ents reported a decrease in their daughters’
levels in older individuals. A study using MR hand stereotypies and episodes of agitation.
spectroscopy (Pan, Lane, Hetherington, & Convulsions were reduced in four of the girls.
Percy, 1999) also replicated these findings. Serum magnesium levels were normal for all
This would suggest that the synaptic levels of patients prior to the start of the treatment, sug-
glutamate may be elevated and could account gesting that the magnesium was acting pharma-
for the cortical hyperexcitability, seizures, cologically rather than correcting a deficit. The
and possibly dendritic pathology and abnor- researchers suggest that the magnesium is coun-
malities in Rett syndrome. teracting intracellular lactic acidosis and serv-
ing as a N-methyl-D-asparate channel blocker,
DRUG THERAPY thus reducing excitotoxic neuronal damage.
A randomized double-blind controlled
To date, there is no cure for the Rett syndrome, crossover trial of L-carnitine (a natural amino
and therapeutic interventions directed at the acid that breaks down long-chain fatty acids in
fundamental mechanisms underlying Rett syn- the blood transporting the particles to the mi-
drome, while limited in number and scope, tochondrial membrane for increased energy
have failed to demonstrate any lasting or sub- production) was performed with 35 girls with
stantive improvements. Rett syndrome (Ellaway et al., 1999). The
study reported improvements in eye contact,
Treatment of Underlying Causes concentration and attention span, reduced day-
time somnolence, increased vocalization, and
Based on the biochemical findings in the re- increased mobility; assessments by both med-
search, the effect of bromocriptine, a dopamine ical personnel and caregivers indicated that
agonist, has been explored in 12 girls with Rett the girls appeared happier.
144 Diagnosis and Classification

Neurotransmitter precursor therapy has (p. 238) during sleep. Robb, Harden, and Boyd
been attempted with the amino acids tyrosine (1989) report, that in their study of 52 girls
(the precursor for dopamine and norepineph- with Rett syndrome, discharges, consisting of
rine) and tryptophan (precursor for serotonin) short waves or spikes, were a common feature.
in nine girls (Nielsen, Lou, & Andresen, 1990). These discharges could be infrequent or almost
No clinical performance or EEG pattern continuous and characteristically were most
changes were observed as a result of treatment. prominent around the middle third of the head.
A double-blind controlled crossover trial of Pronounced EEG abnormalities were most
melatonin (a natural hormone secreted by the often found from 3 to 10 years of age and
pineal gland to help promote drowsiness) was tended to become less severe during the sec-
conducted by McArthur and Budden (1998) to ond decade of life (Niedermeyer et al., 1986;
explore its efficacy on sleep disturbances as- Rett, 1986). Glaze and associates (1987), de-
sociated with Rett syndrome. They report the scribed the progressive changes in the EGG
melatonin decreased sleep onset latency, im- and correlated it with the clinical staging sys-
proved total sleep, and improved sleep effi- tem. Their work is summarized in Table 5.6.
ciency—especially in those subjects with the The EEG changes generally appear at the be-
most disturbed sleep patterns during baseline. ginning of Stage 2 and then follow a stepwise
Naltrexone, an opiate antagonist, was em- progression, with slowing, loss of normal sleep
ployed in a double-blind crossover trial (Percy characteristics, multifocal abnormalities, and,
et al., 1991). The use of an opiate antagonist finally, generalized slow spike and wave activ-
was attempted due to the reports of elevated ity (Verma, Chheda, & Nigro, 1986).
levels of ß-endorphins in girls with Rett syn- Although all girls with Rett syndrome
drome. The motor behavior and other symp- demonstrate abnormal EEG tracings, seizure
toms of the disorder displayed no improvement activity is not universal. Naidu et al. (1986) re-
during the naltrexone treatment phase. In fact, ported that approximately 84% of the girls in
the girls’ performance on the Bayley Scales of their study demonstrated seizures. The most
Infant Development worsened during the phase common types of clinical seizures include
of naltroxone treatment (Percy & Hagberg, generalized tonic-clonic and partial complex
1992). Given the lack of a biochemical marker seizures. Infantile spasms with hypsaarryth-
and a limited understanding of the biological mia, however, may be an early symptom
basis of Rett syndrome, it may not be surpris- (Iyama, 1993). Selection of a specific medica-
ing that drug therapy has not proven particu- tion should be based on clinical seizure type
larly effective. and EEG pattern. Several clinicians (Adkins,
1986; Budden, 1986; Naidu et al., 1986; Philip-
EEG Profile and Seizure Control part, 1986) agree that standard dosages of
Tegretol (carbamazepine) constitute the best
At this time, we must be satisfied to provide seizure management program. Adrenocorticotr-
suitable medication aimed at symptomatic phic hormone or prednisone has been helpful
relief (e.g., seizure activity). The electroen- in treating infantile spasms (Sekul & Percy,
cephalogram (EEG) has been demonstrated to 1992). Hagberg (1985) warns, however, that
be significantly abnormal for persons with Rett many girls with Rett syndrome overreact and
syndrome throughout all but the earliest stage must therefore be taken off the medication.
of the disorder. A study based on the EEG Haas and his associates (1986) have employed
records of 44 persons with Rett syndrome found the ketogenic diet to reduce seizures in girls
abnormal EEG tracings to be almost universal with seizures failing to respond to medications.
(Niedermeyer, Rett, Renner, Murphy, & Naidu, As the girls enter late adolescence, seizure ac-
1986). Abnormal sleep patterns have also been tivity may decrease allowing modification of
noted. Haas, Rice, Trauner, and Merritt (1986) their medication regimen. Staring spells, eye
report the “presence of intermittent episodes of rolling, and other episodic behavior may be ob-
high amplitude bursts of spike wave or slow served in individuals with Rett syndrome and
wave discharges followed by a brief period of do not always indicate seizure activity (Garo-
relative suppression of background activity” falo, Drury, & Goldstein, 1988). Therefore,
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 145

TABLE 5.6 Correlation of EEG Characteristics with Clinical Stages

EEG Characteristics

State Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4

Awake Normal or minimal Marked slowing of Further, gradual Absence of occipital-


slowing of occipital- occipital-dominant slowing of occipital- dominant rhythm;
dominant rhythm and rhythm and back- dominant rhythm, with marked slowing of
background activity ground activity; rare its subsequent disap- background activity
focal spike or sharp- pearance; moderate- (delta frequencies);
wave discharges to-marked slowing of multifocal spike and/or
background activity; sharp-wave discharges
appearance of or generalized slow
multifocal spike and spike-wave pattern
sharp-wave discharges;
during latter part of
this stage, appearance
of generalized slow
spike-wave pattern
Asleep Normal, with well- Less well-defined Absent vertex Almost continuous
defined vertex trans- vertex transients and transients and spindles generalized slow
ients and sleep spindles and subsequent during NREM sleep; spike-wave activity
spindles loss of these sleep multifocal spike
characteristics; ap- and/or sharp-wave
pearance of focal or discharges, with later
multifocal spike and/ development of
or sharp-wave dis- generalized slow
charges spike-wave pattern
during NREM sleep

Source: “Rett Syndrome in the Electroencephalographic Characteristics with Clinical Staging,” by D. G. Glaze,
J. D. Frost, H. Y. Zoghbi, & A. K. Percy, 1987, Archives of Neurology, 44, pp. 1053–1056. Copyright 1987, Amer-
ican Medical Association.

these behaviors should not be treated as benefit from adequate caloric intake must be
seizures without EEG documentation. considered when examining the malnutrition
displayed in some persons with Rett syndrome
GROWTH PATTERNS AND NUTRITION (Missliwetz & Depastas, 1985). Motil, Schultz,
Brown, Glaze, and Percy (1994) report that en-
A pattern of deceleration across all growth ergy expenditure associated with involuntary
measurements, following the first 6 months of motor movement places these girls in a situa-
life, is witnessed in most persons with Rett syn- tion of lower energy balance (energy intake ver-
drome (Schultz et al., 1993). The exact cause of sus expenditure) than controls. The lowered
this remains unknown. A systemic deficiency energy balance in girls with Rett syndrome par-
in mitochondrial energy production as re- alleled their degree of height and weight
ported from muscle biopsy tissue has been deficits, despite similar dietary energy intakes
suggested (Coker & Melnyk, 1991; Schultz between groups. Haas and his associates (Haas
et al., 1993). The best evidence to date, how- & Rice, 1985; Haas et al., 1986) have reported
ever, suggests nutritional, rather than chromo- improved weight gain in conjunction with di-
somal, neurological, or hormonal factors minished stereotyped behavior and better
underlie this failure to grow. Some evidence seizure control with the implementation of a
suggests defects in carbohydrate (Clark et al., high-calorie, high-fat ketogenic diet.
1990; Haas & Rice, 1985; Haas et al., 1986), Reilly and Cass (2001) explored the issue of
ascorbic acid, and glutathione (Sofic, Ried- growth failure in Rett syndrome. They paid spe-
erer, Killian, & Rett, 1987) metabolism. Mal- cial attention to the impact of the feeding prob-
absorption of critical nutrients and failure to lems experienced by many of these individuals.
146 Diagnosis and Classification

Through their own clinical experience and the measure of cognitive ability or of the motor and
results of a questionnaire, these researchers verbal disability. Thus, the assessment of cogni-
identified several common feeding problems tive functioning in persons with Rett syndrome
(see Table 5.7). They have developed a series is particularly problematic.
of management protocols involving dietary sup- Given the effect of Rett syndrome on verbal
plementation, food texture modifications, and and motor behavior, assessment employing tra-
posture modifications. A nutritionist should be ditional standardized measures of cognitive
available to consult with parents and program ability is counterindicated. These measures
staff relative to diet if weight gain is a problem. would be invalid because they would generate
Constipation is a common problem experi- scores that would be indicative of the verbal
enced by persons with Rett syndrome. Many of and motor disability instead of cognitive abil-
the girls fail to consume adequate fluids and ity. For this reason, the cognitive abilities of
fiber which may result in an impacted bowel girls with Rett syndrome traditionally have
(Hunter, 1987). Dietary measures (ingestion been assessed with instruments designed for
of fiber, mineral oil, fruit with high liquid con- evaluating infants. These measures can be ad-
tent, etc.) may prove adequate, although artifi- ministered to persons with very little verbal
cial laxatives, enemas, or suppositories are and motor ability. Generally, however, they re-
often required (Naidu et al., 1990). quire caregivers and others familiar with the
child to make judgments about the child’s abil-
COGNITIVE AND ity and developmental functioning level to
ADAPTIVE FUNCTIONING support data collected through direct observa-
tion. Many of these measures allow flexibility
Rett syndrome is characterized by an especially in the materials employed in testing to maxi-
debilitating combination of an extrapyramidal mize child interest. Thus, these assessments
movement disorder, delayed response latencies, are thought to provide a more accurate picture
and the loss of acquired speech. Such a combi- of developmental functioning than traditional
nation of disabilities significantly limits our IQ tests. Although using infant assessments for
ability to estimate the cognitive functioning of older children with severe impairments is a
persons with this disorder. Traditional methods common practice, these tests were normed and
of cognitive assessment require either unim- standardized on infants. Thus, the score at-
paired motor or verbal responses from the indi- tained might best be interpreted as an estimate.
vidual to generate valid estimates. If the person Along those lines, Demeter (2000) cautions
is required to employ a means of responding that when assessing individuals with severe or
that in itself is impaired, we can never be profound disabilities such as those found in
certain whether the assessed functioning is a Rett syndrome, a standardized instrument is
unlikely to measure the full range of a child’s
TABLE 5.7 Feeding Problems Common in ability. Noting that discrepancies exist be-
Persons with Rett Syndrome tween parental reports of ability and the abil-
Inability to consume adequate calories orally to meet
ity levels indicated by test results, Demeter
energy requirements. suggests that persons with Rett syndrome
Evidence of ongoing aspiration during oral feeding.
often show situation-specific skills that tradi-
tional measures miss. Some children can and
Oral feeding is stressful for the caregiver, the indi-
vidual with Rett syndrome, or both.
do learn to interact within their environment to
get their needs met, yet this capacity may be
Mealtimes are protracted, leaving limited time for
other daily activities.
missed by assessment or assumed to be non-
existent. Demeter suggests that when attempt-
Oral intake is erratic.
ing to assess a child with Rett syndrome, re-
Oral supplementation has failed.
searchers should use criterion-referenced tests
Chronic food/ liquid refusal or aversive behavior dur- to measure a child’s ability to learn new skills
ing mealtime has developed.
as well as multiple observations in natural en-
A safe route for providing regular medication is re- vironments. Further, because children with
quired (e.g., with food or liquid intake).
Rett syndrome are highly social, assessment
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 147

should use social interests or people versus ob- the student is aware that she should activate a
jects to elicit responses. switch but does so randomly)? Further, the
Physical and neurological impairments in- small amount of stimuli provided as distracters
herent in Rett syndrome (e.g., apraxia) may im- in measures such as the PPVT-R allow for a
pede a person’s ability to act on or respond to 25% chance that the student will select the cor-
stimuli in the consistent manner often necessi- rect answer. Administration of assessment in-
tated in standardized testing situations. As a struments to persons with severe disabilities
result, scoring should be completed in a way typically requires that the individual be famil-
that does not penalize these individuals for in- iar with the response capabilities of the person
stances when there is no response. Test admin- being tested. Problems can arise when the ad-
istrators should take into account the child’s ministrator scores an item as correct based on
successful attempts divided by the total number a vague implied response or assumed capabili-
of actual attempts, omitting the times the child ties based on previous knowledge of the child.
was nonresponsive. For example, if a child was Another consideration when assessing the
asked to point to a particular picture and was cognitive abilities with Rett syndrome is visual
only able to do so 6 of the 10 times requested, acuity. In a 1996 study, von Tetzchner et al.,
and got 4 of the 6 attempts correct, then the used the Fagen Test of Infant Intelligence to as-
percentage would be calculated using 4⁄6 as the sess visual function and visual information
raw score instead of 4⁄10. The end result is an ex- processing in a group of girls with Rett syn-
ploration of the ratio of correct to incorrect re- drome and then compared them with a control
sponses free of trials in which the movement group of age-matched typically developing
disorders associated with Rett syndrome pre- children. Results indicated that all the girls in
cluded item selection. the study (n = 41) displayed a visual acuity
One assessment tool suggested by the Inter- below what would be expected in fully devel-
national Rett Syndrome Association (IRSA, oped vision. The authors suggest this may be
2002) for assessing cognitive ability in persons due to syndrome-related arrested cortical and
with Rett syndrome is the Peabody Picture Vo- retinal development or attentional deficits that
cabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R). The PPVT-R cause misleading results. Either way, the au-
requires that students point to the one picture thors suggest that persons with Rett syndrome
that the test administrator verbally labels, out need additional time to process information be
of four displayed. The IRSA suggests a modi- it novel or familiar. Additional response time
fied test protocol (enlarging pictures and must be incorporated into any assessment in-
spreading them farther apart to ease the inter- volving children with Rett syndrome.
pretation of eye-gaze responses, testing over Much of the research on Rett syndrome sug-
multiple days to guard against “down days” and gests that the individuals function within the
possible misinterpretation) to assist in provid- severe-to-profound range of mental retardation.
ing an accurate picture of the child’s skill level. Perry, Sarlo-McGarvey, and Haddad (1991)
One must note, however, that the PPVT-R “. . . employed the Cattell Infant Intelligence Scale
is not a comprehensive test of general intelli- (Cattell, 1940) to assess the cognitive func-
gence, instead it measures only one important tioning of 15 girls with Rett syndrome. Each of
facet of intelligence: vocabulary” (Dunn & the girls tested below the 8-month cognitive
Dunn, 1981, p. 2). Like other measures em- functioning level, with an average of 3 months,
ployed to assess the cognitive ability of per- suggesting profound cognitive deficits. There
sons with Rett syndrome, the validity of the is, however, considerable debate about whether
scores attained must be explored further. children with Rett syndrome experience a true
Assessment measures that employ eye gaze dementia involving cognitive degeneration that
or switch activation may be useful for persons results in severe or profound mental retar-
with Rett syndrome. The question remains dation (e.g., Budden, Meek, & Henighan, 1990;
whether the score attained could be replicated Charnov, Stach, & Didonato, 1989; Rolando,
if the same measure were administered again: 1985) or a cognitive arrest or stagnation at the
Does the test measure cognitive ability or sim- point of the initial motor and language regres-
ply provide an opportunity for responding (e.g., sion (e.g., Fontanesi & Haas, 1988; Kerr et al.,
148 Diagnosis and Classification

1987; Naidu et al., 1986). Charnov and her as- Development (1975), they found the girls re-
sociates (Charnov et al., 1989) evaluated the sembled one another and remained relatively
developmental histories of 16 girls with Rett stable over the 3 years in the sensorimotor do-
syndrome through parent interview (of prere- mains of Object Permanence (Piagetian Stages
gression development) and current functioning I and II ), Vocal Imitation (Piagetian Stages 0
assessment employing the Birth to Three De- and I ), Gestural Imitation (Piagetian Stages 0
velopmental Scale (Bangs & Dodson, 1979). In and I ), and Scheme Actions (Piagetian Stages
all developmental areas, they report that the II and III ). There was considerable individual
current functioning of each subject was sub- variation among the girls in relation to the
stantially below that which the children were sensorimotor domain of Means Ends Abilities,
attributed to have achieved prior to the onset of ranging from Piagetian Stages I to IV. The
the disorder. Interestingly, the developmental girls also demonstrated the greatest level of
skills profile at the time of testing mirrored individual improvement over the 3-year study
that of the preregression skills, although at a (from Piagetian Stage I to Stages II and IV for
lower level of functioning. three of the girls).
There is some support for a hypothesis that Current best estimates suggest that persons
Rett syndrome results in cognitive arrest or with Rett syndrome function within the severe-
stagnation at the developmental level achieved to-profound range of mental retardation follow-
at the age of onset of the condition in combina- ing the regression in Stage 2 of the disorder.
tion with a severe extrapyramidal movement and The areas of gross motor activity and daily liv-
expressive language disorder as opposed to cog- ing skills appear to be more advanced than
nitive dementia (Hagberg & Witt-Engerstrom, other adaptive functions and the girls are ca-
1986; Kerr & Stephenson, 1986; Stephenson & pable of some cognitive improvement over
Kerr, 1987). The extent and timing of the cogni- time. Further research is needed to gain a
tive stagnation may be the result of the differen- fuller understanding of the cognitive abilities
tial timing of the genetic mutation and the of girls with Rett syndrome. Multidisciplinary
number and location of the cells impacted with assessments of children with Rett syndrome
the defective MECP2 gene (Hoffbuhr et al., are required to gather information on factors
2001). Fontanesi and Haas (1988) evaluated the that may influence cognitive development and
cognitive functioning of 18 girls with Rett syn- performance. Inconsistency of response in
drome. They administered the Vineland Adap- the test situation is a common report across
tive Behavior Scales and the Bayley Scales of the studies. This makes standardized testing
Adaptive Abilities in addition to examining difficult, and care should be taken not only
medical and developmental histories and inter- to provide standardized administrations of
viewing parents regarding the age at which their cognitive tests, but also to look across the
daughters attained developmental milestones. daily functioning of the individuals to identify
Their results indicated that “skills not depen- actions that indicate target abilities. For ex-
dent on either language or fine motor function ample, in one of our studies (Van Acker &
are retained at a developmental level equivalent Grant, 1995), one of the girls failed to demon-
to the age of onset ” (p. S23). Gross motor func- strate awareness of a covered object during
tioning, daily living skills, and object perma- testing, suggesting a lack of object perma-
nence were found to be relatively preserved, nence. Later in the testing situation, however,
whereas fine motor control and language func- she dropped a toy and it rolled under the table.
tioning displayed substantial degeneration from She repeatedly looked under the table for the
preregression developmental levels. missing item.
In a longitudinal study of the cognitive
skills in six girls with Rett syndrome, Woody- COMMUNICATION
att and Ozanne (1993) reported patterns of
similarity across the group as well as individ- Communication abilities and subsequent pro-
ual variations across subjects and within gramming for persons with Rett syndrome have
subjects over a period of 3 years. Using the Uz- been the topic of very limited empirical re-
giris and Hunt Scales of Infant Psychological search. Prior to regression, most of the girls
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 149

with this disorder are reported to have devel- facial and gestural communicative behaviors
oped single spoken words and/or word combi- and developed a “dictionary” for those who in-
nations. Comprehension skills appropriate to teract frequently with their child (Interna-
the child’s age also are typically noted (Bud- tional Rett Syndrome Association, 1990).
den, Meek, et al., 1990; Woodyatt & Ozanne, Until recently, persons with multiple dis-
1992). The loss of language skills during re- abilities, such as those displayed in Rett syn-
gression is sufficiently rapid to be mistaken for drome, would have been deemed unlikely
hearing loss. Following the regression phase of candidates to benefit from formal communica-
the disorder, speech and language skills are ob- tion intervention. In the past, the literature has
served to be severely impaired. Verbalizations asserted a powerful relationship between cog-
are typically nonexistent or limited to “non- nition and language development. Intentional
functional” consonant-vowel combinations, ex- communication was thought to require perfor-
cept in the estimated 2% to 4% of girls who mance at Piaget’s (1929) sensorimotor devel-
fall within the preserved speech variant of Rett opment Stage V (Bates, Benigni, Bretherton,
syndrome (Budden, Meek, et al., 1990; Skjel- Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979; Bates, Camaioni,
dal, von Tetzchner, Jacobsen, Smith, & Heil- & Volterra, 1975; Kahn, 1977, 1981, 1984).
berg, 1995; Zappella, 1992). Girls with the Formal testing suggests that girls with Rett
preserved speech variant may retain full sen- syndrome function at a presymbolic language
tence use and may even slowly show an in- level (Woodyatt & Ozanne, 1992, 1993).
crease in vocabulary (von Tetzchner, 1997). Again, one cannot determine whether these re-
However, for the most part, these girls employ sults relate primarily to cognitive deficits or to
their language in a nonfunctional manner. They an expressive language and/or motor disorder.
continue to display significant communication Numerous anecdotal reports however, suggest
impairment with pronounced pragmatic diffi- that these girls may well understand more than
culties (Gillberg, 1997). As the girls approach they can express (Weisz, 1987). Moreover, the
adolescence, improved eye contact, social in- assumption that intentional communication re-
teraction, and communicative intentionality quires Stage V functioning has been called
are reported (Woodyatt & Ozanne, 1993). into question (Reichle & Keogh, 1986; Rice,
Individuals with Rett syndrome become a 1983).
part of the estimated one million children and Persons with Rett syndrome, in fact, have
adults in the United States who are unable to been taught to employ augmentative com-
communicate orally (Diggs, 1981). Most suc- munication systems that involve eye pointing,
cessful communication programs for these communication boards (pictures), facial
girls take advantage of their limited commu- expressions, gestures, and the activation
nicative behaviors (e.g., Donnellan, Mirenda, of switches or computers to indicate some
Mesaros, & Fassbender, 1984). Vocalizations, limited volitional gestures. Van Acker and
facial expressions, gestures, walking toward a Grant (1995) employed a dynamic computer-
desired item or activity, and eye gaze are com- graphic presentation contingent on touch-
mon communicative behaviors displayed by screen activation with three girls to select
persons with Rett syndrome. Parents and edu- desired food items. Results indicated that
cators must attune themselves to their child’s two of the three girls demonstrated a reduc-
communicative behavior and respond contin- tion in stereotypic hand use and significantly
gently to these signals. As with any child, the increased item requesting when provided
critical element in the development of a mean- computer-based requesting instruction. Inter-
ingful communication system depends on the estingly, cognitive assessment of all three
contingent interaction of the person and her en- girls indicated a functioning level below the
vironment (Lewis, Alessandri, & Sullivan, Piagetian Sensorimotor Stage V, yet benefit
1990). When others learn to detect the commu- from instruction was obvious.
nicative behaviors and to respond to them in a Similarly, Hetzroni, Rubin, and Konkol
systematic fashion, a formal and effective sys- (2002) were able to teach three girls with Rett
tem of communication can be developed. Par- syndrome to match picture symbols (PCS or
ents have taken photographs of their daughter’s orthography) or picture symbols plus text to
150 Diagnosis and Classification

the spoken request to “ touch .” Their addressing symptoms by maintaining or im-


results suggested the girls were able to demon- proving functional movement, mobility, pre-
strate a steady learning curve over the symbols venting deformities, and keeping the girls in
employed and to display a partial retention of contingent contact with their environments.
these symbols over time. During the periods of regression associated
Speech and language services appear to be with the disorder, therapeutic intervention is
warranted for persons with Rett syndrome. Pro- especially important and should be more fre-
gramming should emphasize functional recep- quent, because transition skills are at risk
tive and expressive language skills, as well as (Hanks, 1990). Although persons with Rett
cognitive, social pragmatic, and affective com- syndrome display numerous similarities, their
munication skills. Early intervention could specific therapeutic problems and responses to
target eye contact and attending to the environ- treatment vary dramatically (Hanks, 1986,
ment. Cause-and-effect relationships that allow 1990; Lieb-Lundell, 1988; Sponseller, 1989).
the children to gain an understanding of their The therapeutic intervention program, there-
ability to affect their environment are essential fore, must be highly individualized.
for communication training. Though Rett Apraxia and ataxia are frequently the earli-
syndrome affects a person’s ability to attend to est manifestations of motor problems in Rett
objects and stimuli, seemingly reducing the syndrome. Hypotonia interferes with postural
capability to discriminate between items and stability. Fitzgerald, Jankovic, Glaze, Schultz,
therefore making communication intervention and Percy (1990) suggest that the typical
difficult, there is evidence that children with “jerky truncal tremors” may reflect a derange-
Rett syndrome can sustain attention in pre- ment of postural reflexes rather than cerebel-
ferred activities for longer than average periods lar pathology. The girls develop compensatory
of time (J. Watson, Umansky, Marcy, & Repa- increased tone to achieve stability, resulting in
choli, 1996). Implications of this research sup- abnormal movement patterns. A marked fixing
port the idea that interventions should strive or locking of their joints into positions of sta-
to identify and use activities, people, and items bility to counter disruption in balance is typi-
preferred by the child to enhance motivation cal, inhibiting their ability to shift positions.
to learn. Simple switches to activate toys or Thus, the legs are often kept in wide abduction
computer monitors may enhance an awareness while sitting and standing (see Figure 5.2) and
of cause-and-effect relationships. Musical in- weight shift is absent (Hanks, 1986).
struments also have been shown to promote the The girls often demonstrate expressions of
child’s desire to interact with her environ- agitation and fear in response to any movements
ment (Wesecky, 1986; Zappella, 1986). Given
the importance of communication skills, re-
searchers need to identify the range of commu-
nication functioning in, and effective methods
to teach improved communication skills to, per-
sons with Rett syndrome.

ORTHOPEDIC ASPECTS
AND INTERVENTION

Persons with Rett syndrome exhibit multiple


orthopedic and motor movement disorders.
These disorders can vary significantly across
the different phases of the syndrome. Physical
and occupational therapists, therefore, must
play an important role in the care of individu-
als with Rett syndrome (Braddock, Braddock,
& Graham, 1993; Hanks, 1986, 1990). Inten-
sive therapy, while failing to alter the actual Figure 5.2 Stereotypic hand movements in Rett
course of the disease, has been successful in syndrome.
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 151

that are not self-initiated. Similar voluntary respiratory dysrhythmia ( hyperventilation and
movements, however, are not related to these apnea; Kerr et al., 1990) exacerbated stereo-
stress reactions. Lieb-Lundell (1988) reports, typic movements. Increased stereotypic hand
“No amount of practice or exposure alters this movements in Rett syndrome have been re-
response of fear to extrinsically initiated ported to coincide with improved EEG trac-
movement ” (p. 533). ings. Niedermeyer and Naidu (1990) have
Several therapeutic interventions have suggested that this passive finger movement
been found to be successful in the treatment of may block focal and multifocal spike activity.
apraxia-ataxia. Tone reduction techniques simi- This phenomenon has been reported previously
lar to those used with patients afflicted with in Rolandic epilepsy (Sekul & Percy, 1992).
cerebral palsy or impaired through a stroke are Despite their neurological origin, the
appropriate. Interventions might include (1) use stereotypic behaviors of persons with Rett syn-
of the therapy ball, (2) balance-stimulating drome appear to be influenced, at least mini-
floor activities, (3) segmental rolling, and mally, by environmental stimuli. An analogue
(4) rotation and weight shift activities (Hanks, study of the stereotypic behavior of two young
1986, 1990). Vestibular movement activities women with Rett syndrome demonstrated that
(e.g., merry-go-rounds, swings) have also been their moment-to-moment expression appeared
reported as helpful if the child will tolerate this to be influenced by environmental contingen-
intervention (Havlak & Covington, 1989). cies (Wehmeyer, Bourland, & Ingram, 1993).
Efforts to facilitate normal movement They found that both participants increased
while maintaining reduced tone are usually not or decreased their level of stereotypic re-
successful, however (Hanks, 1990). As the sponding as the result of various consequences
girls often resist being moved, close physical within their social setting. For example, one of
contact and a slow, firm approach aimed to the girls would demonstrate a significant in-
reduce the child’s anxiety during physical as- crease in stereotypy when placed in a task de-
sistance is suggested. Individuals with Rett mand situation. The authors speculated that
syndrome often have very long response la- escape and/or avoidance of task engagement
tency. Therapists must provide verbal direc- was negatively reinforcing her stereotypy. Van
tions and encouragement and provide the time Acker (1987) reported a 40% reduction in the
needed for a response. stereotypic hand movements displayed by one
Stereotyped hand movements represent one girl when involved in computer-assisted in-
of the most distinguishing characteristics of struction. These findings were extended in a
the Rett syndrome. Hand wringing, hand wash- later study where the stereotypic behavior of
ing, hand tapping, and hand-to-mouth move- two additional girls displayed a significant de-
ments are common stereotypies resulting in crease during computer-assisted requesting.
the loss of purposeful hand function. These Though stereotypic hand movements are an
movements appear to evolve with age, proceed- intrinsic part of Rett syndrome, there is some
ing from simple, rapid movements to a slower, evidence that girls with the preserved speech
more complex form and ultimately to slow, variant of Rett syndrome retain more functional
less complicated repetitive movements (Clare, hand use than girls with classical Rett syn-
1986). Most researchers consider the stereo- drome. The preserved speech variant is usually
typic hand movements to represent primary associated with a higher ability level in the cog-
circular reactions resulting from an underlying nitive, speech, and motor domains. Umansky
extrapyramidal disorder. Thus, they represent et al. (2001) suggest that differing levels of
nonvolitional movements. Persons with Rett MECP2 protein function may account for dif-
syndrome appear to require great concentra- ferences in object-oriented hand use in girls
tion and effort to break out of the stereotypic with the preserved speech variant. Further-
movements for even brief attempts at purpose- more, as a result of an intervention study con-
ful hand use. ducted with one 6-year-old girl with the
Stereotypic behaviors are more remarkable preserved speech variant of Rett syndrome,
under stressful situations and diminish or dis- Umansky et al. believe that social facilitation
appear momentarily when changing posture of object-oriented hand use is an important fac-
or eating. In some individuals, periods of tor in preservation of this function. Outside
152 Diagnosis and Classification

encouragement of hand use contributes to maintenance of treatment effects has not been
a child’s ability to retain hand movements documented. Treatments with various medica-
and skill. tions such as L-dopa, haloperidol, 5-HTP, and
The suspected neurophysiological etiology various anticonvulsants have proved unreward-
of stereotypy in Rett syndrome has led re- ing thus far (Percy et al., 1985).
searchers to caution practitioners attempting Splinting has been found to be successful in
to modify these behaviors. As these behaviors interrupting hand-to-mouth (Hanks, 1986) and
appear to represent basically involuntary hand wringing (Aron, 1990; Naganuma, 1988)
movements, efforts to change this behavior, es- movements, thus allowing the girls to direct
pecially through the use of aversive conse- their attention to tasks and persons in their en-
quences, seem ill advised (Hanks, 1990). vironment and to reduce the risk of skin break-
Moreover, previous attempts to use operant down related to these high-rate behaviors. In
conditioning procedures to alter symptomatic fact, some persons with Rett syndrome have
behaviors of persons with Rett syndrome have demonstrated improved functional hand use
not proven particularly promising. For exam- while splints were in use (Naganuma, 1988).
ple, differential reinforcement, response inter- Tuten and Miedaner (1989), however, were un-
ruption, and contingent hand restraint have able to replicate the effectiveness of hand
been used to decrease self-injurious behavior splints. The two subjects involved in this study
(Iwata, Pace, Willis, Gamache, & Hyman, displayed no decrease in stereotyped hand
1986). They reported that the hand biting of wringing, nor any subsequent increase in func-
these girls, though moderately reduced tional hand use as a result of hand splint appli-
through intervention, appeared to “ be related cation. Sharpe (1992) employed an alternating
to organic predisposition rather than being treatments design to compare the effectiveness
shaped inadvertently by the environment ” of bilateral hand splints and an elbow orthosis
(p. 164). Facial screening has been attempted, for two girls with Rett syndrome. Both girls
without success, to decrease breathing irregu- demonstrated a decrease in stereotypic hand
larities (Lugaresi, Cirignotta, & Montagna, movements and a corresponding increase in
1985). Recently, however, operant approaches toy play with the use of the elbow orthosis.
employing prompting, backward chaining, The bilateral hand splints had no obvious treat-
shaping, and positive reinforcement have ment effect. No studies have demonstrated any
shown success (Bat-Haee, 1994; Piazza, An- maintenance effects of hand splints over time
derson, & Fisher, 1993; Van Acker & Grant, once the splints have been removed. Addition-
1995). Piazza and her associates (1993) were ally, many persons with Rett syndrome are un-
able to reestablish functional self-feeding in able to tolerate the application of hand or arm
three girls with Rett syndrome through the use splints for even short periods.
of graduated prompting and positive reinforce- Instructional strategies that reinforce be-
ment. These researchers suggest that feeding haviors incompatible with stereotypic move-
may represent a good first step in developing ments are recommended. Many of the girls
improved hand use as the girls generally enjoy accept having the stereotypic behavior inter-
eating and the food serves as a natural positive rupted. In fact, they often seem to relax when
reinforcer. They caution parents and practi- their stereotypic behavior is stopped for short
tioners to be aware that the period needed for periods. This can be accomplished by having
skill acquisition may be longer than that dis- the girls placed into the prone position or by
played in other children with severe or pro- simply holding one hand while assisting func-
found disabilities. Moreover, because these tional hand use (see Figure 5.3). Hanks (1986)
girls display variable progress from day to day, reports, “ Toys that combine bright colors and
data collection to plot change over the course sound and require input from the child are
of the training can help parents avoid becom- helpful in keeping the child involved with the
ing discouraged. Thus, operant procedures, es- environment and making attempts to use her
pecially nonaversive approaches, may prove hands” (p. 250). Encouraging the child to use
effective in the treatment of some behavioral her hands for reaching or switch activation can
characteristics of the Rett syndrome; however, potentially reduce stereotypic hand movements
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 153

skills has been helpful in the improvement of


range of motion and the reduction of discom-
fort (Hanks, 1986; Lieb-Lundell, 1988; Schle-
ichkorn, 1987). Tone reduction activities such
as rotation, weight shift, and vibration have
been reported to result in a temporary reduc-
tion of spasticity (Hanks, 1986, 1990).
Ambulation remains one of the critical
skills to develop and maintain in persons with
Rett syndrome. Many of the girls fail to de-
velop this skill prior to Stage II, while others
Figure 5.3 Interruption of stereotypic behavior to lose this ability as part of the rapid motor de-
promote functional hand use.
generation. As spasticity and apraxia increase,
as reaching for an object and hand-wringing the girls often lose many functional gross
behaviors are incompatible (J. Watson, Uman- motor skills that they had previously achieved.
sky, Marcy, Johnston, & Repacholi, 1996). Additionally, these girls often manifest spatial
Music therapy has also proved useful in the pro- disorientation. Their perception of an upright
motion of functional hand use. The music ap- posture results in a forward, backward, or lat-
pears to increase the level of awareness and the eral leaning. Ambulation may be lost, espe-
instruments motivate efforts to reach out and cially for girls with an orientation toward
interact (Wesecky, 1986). Battery-operated backward leaning, as they are unable to initi-
toys and computers modified to respond to an ate their forward weight shift. They fear
easily activated switch provide an almost limit- falling when attempting weight shift in a for-
less array of possibilities not only to decrease ward direction. The abnormal gait pattern typ-
stereotyped behavior (Van Acker, 1987; Van ically displayed in this syndrome results from
Acker & Grant, 1995) but also to increase a combination of spasticity, ataxia, apraxia,
functional hand use, communication, and cog- compensatory spinal rigidity, and spatial dis-
nitive development (Hanks, 1986; Sponseller, orientation. Asymmetries may develop as one
1989; Zappella, 1986). leg becomes stiffer or weaker. Weight shift is
Bumin, Uyanik, Yilmaz, Kayihan, and accomplished through lateral rocking and
Topcu (2003) report that following 8 weeks of trunk rotation is lost. More simply, because
exposure to the Halliwick method of hy- the center of balance is off, girls often think
drotherapy, an 11-year-old girl with Rett they are falling forward when they are stand-
syndrome displayed increased balance while ing normally. They try to compensate by lean-
walking, decreased stereotypic movements, ing back, fall over, and over time may lose the
and improved purposeful hand use (e.g., im- ability to stand (Kerr et al., 1990).
proved self-feeding). Independent standing and ambulation rep-
Spasticity has been reported as a typical resent a realistic therapy goal, especially in
problem during Stage II of the disorder. This the early years. Many girls while unable to
spasticity may “ vary from a mild increase in walk independently can do so with assistance.
tone in the gastroc-soleus complex, resulting in Such aided ambulation should be encouraged.
toe walking, to severe involvement throughout Weight-bearing exercises, walking, and gait
the body affecting even respiration and swal- training have been successful (Hanks, 1986,
lowing” (Hanks, 1986, p. 248). The resulting 1989, 1990). If appropriate, activities designed
muscle imbalance may lead to severe contrac- to elicit righting and equilibrium responses
tures, especially distally (e.g., a downward might include use of the large therapy ball
pointing of the foot). This spasticity may also (prone positioning and then standing, leaning
be responsible, at least in part, for the high in- forward), weight shifting (seating the child on
cidence of scoliosis in girls with Rett syn- a bench and then tipping the bench slightly
drome (Hanks, 1986; Sponseller, 1989). backward), and ultimately active shift forward
Hydrotherapy emphasizing movement in to come from sitting to standing (Hanks,
the water, range of motion, and basic water 1990).
154 Diagnosis and Classification

Frequently, foot deformities (e.g., ankle Placing the child on her side with the apex of
pronation, plantar flexion inversion and toe the curve down may prove helpful. Exercises
curling) must be corrected with polypropylene designed to maximize use of the muscles the
ankle-foot orthosis, hinged to allow dorsiflex- girl avoids using are in order (e.g., feed and
ion. Attempts to correct pronation foot defor- lead the child by the hand on her hypotonic
mities with below-ankle orthosis has not proven side). Good positioning is vital; and strollers,
effective as they destabilize the child’s gait. wheelchairs, and high chairs should be fitted
Gentle manual stretching, night splints, and properly to produce a symmetrical sitting pos-
weight shifting in controlled standing with the ture and an erect spine (Hanks, 1986; Kjoer-
heels down have all proven useful if started holt & Salthammer, 1989).
prior to signs of actual contractures (Hanks,
1990). EDUCATIONAL IMPLICATIONS
Individuals with the ability to ambulate
must engage in activities that will maintain this Children, even those with severe disabilities,
skill and promote stimulation of the joints and must be provided an appropriate education
muscles (Kjoerholt & Salthammer, 1989). within the least restrictive environment. As a
Walking and stair climbing should be a regular result, school personnel are called on to make
part of the daily routine to maximize these judgments about the appropriateness of cur-
skills. Kjoerholt and Salthammer (1989) cau- riculum and associated instructional strategies
tion that therapists must be patient as the girls for children with exceptional needs. To plan
walk “ very slowly and will often stop without effectively, educators must be cognizant of a
any noticeable reason, probably due to apraxia” child’s current level of functioning, capacity
(p. 84). Many of the common devices for assist- for learning, and potential for meeting reason-
ing ambulation (e.g., push-type walker) are of able instructional objectives.
limited usefulness due to loss of purposeful Given the cognitive, communicative, and
hand function. physical limitations of Rett syndrome, the edu-
Scoliosis (a side-to-side curvature of the cational prognosis is guarded at best. Though
spine) in individuals with Rett syndrome is children may not attain developmental mile-
well documented (Hagberg et al., 1983; Hanks, stones at appropriate times, and indeed may
1986, 1989; Harrison & Webb, 1990; Loder, never attain many skills typical for other chil-
Lee, & Richards, 1989; Sponseller, 1989), and dren their age, children with Rett syndrome can
kyphosis (“ hunchback ”) is common (Rett, and do benefit from educational intervention.
1977; Sponseller, 1989). Together, these defor- Koppenhaver, Erickson, and Skotko (2001)
mities represent the primary musculoskeletal promote the merits of early literacy learning
concern in Rett syndrome. There are no rigid through mother and child storybook reading.
guidelines to predict deformity or to recom- They compared the ability of four girls with
mend treatment. Standard criteria (e.g., sex of Rett syndrome to label common items in
patient, curve pattern, onset of menarche, picture books using picture symbols and aug-
Risser sign) typically useful in determining an mentative communication systems before and
appropriate intervention strategy do not appear after parent training. They report that “access
to be helpful with this syndrome (Hennessy & to communication symbols, assistive tech-
Haas, 1988). Scoliosis results in a muscle im- nologies, and parent training” consistently en-
balance in the area around the curve. The mus- hanced children’s frequency of attending to
cles on one side of the spinal column will be and “responding” through the use of various
spastic and hypertonic, whereas one may no- augmentative communication devices. Kop-
tice atrophic or hypotonic musculature on the penhaver et al., used four intervention strate-
other side. The person often will tend to lean gies with the parents in his study, all of which
toward the hypotonic side. Initially, tone re- would be useful for educators. First, they sug-
duction activities, such as gentle lengthening gest attributing meaning to a child’s attempts
of the concave side and activation of the to communicate, even if the meaning is uncer-
convex side through elicitation of equilibrium tain. Second, prompt the use of communication
reactions are recommended (Hanks, 1990). symbols or devices through natural questions
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 155

and comments, instead of commands. Third, Some intervention techniques often used for
they believe in providing a sufficient wait time children with autism have been adopted for use
after asking questions and then providing a hi- with Rett syndrome. Although there is very lit-
erarchy of prompts, rather than a complete tle empirical evidence that children with Rett
physical guidance after the child is nonrespon- syndrome benefit from these intervention tech-
sive. Finally, Koppenhaver and his associates niques, anecdotal reports from parents are
believe that interventionists ( be they parents often encouraging. These methods include ap-
or educators) should consistently ask questions plied behavior analysis, which uses a discrete
of children that maximize their ability to use trial format to teach new skills, floor time, a
the adaptive equipment they have in front of relationship-based approach focusing on open-
them. Ask questions that have answers on the ing and closing communication circles, and
communication board the child is using (Kop- PECS, a picture exchange communication sys-
penhaver et al., 2001). The key to any success- tem (Hunter, 1999). On the other hand, pub-
ful intervention is to get the child involved in lished empirical studies of some of these
the process, and even though it may take more intervention approaches (e.g., discrete trial for-
effort to engage a child with Rett syndrome, mat instruction, applied behavior analysis tech-
the endeavor is worthwhile. Though these chil- niques to increase functional hand use) have
dren may not be able to read a story on their failed to document their effectiveness with stu-
own, they can participate in and enjoy the ac- dents displaying Rett syndrome (Iwata et al.,
tivity of reading. 1986; Smith, Klevstrand, & Lovaas, 1995).
Individualized Education Plan goals related
to developing a viable communication system CONCLUSION
are worthwhile. Study of seemingly meaning-
less routines and actions of girls with Rett syn- Only a small percentage of the estimated 10,000
drome can help develop communication systems individuals in the United States afflicted with
based on intent and caregivers can make infer- the Rett syndrome have been identified thus far.
ences to help establish meaning and develop Lack of awareness of this disorder on the part of
goal-directed behaviors. Through structured physicians and clinicians is undoubtedly a major
overinterpretation, where adults consistently re- contributing factor to this state of affairs. Even
spond to a certain gaze or gesture as an indica- when physicians are aware of the Rett syn-
tion of some need or want, we can help children drome, an accurate diagnosis is not always
with Rett syndrome gain control over their envi- forthcoming. There is often a hesitation to ac-
ronment and make it a safe, predictable setting cept, as a specific entity, a disorder for which
(von Tetzchner, 1997). there is no definitive diagnostic marker. Today,
Writing Individual Education Plan (IEP) a diagnosis of Rett syndrome continues to re-
goals related to functional skills such as quire a careful analysis of the clinical signs and
drinking, eating, and oral motor skills, hand symptoms (often supported by the identification
use, dressing, cognition, communication, and of a MECP2 mutation).
social skills have all been suggested by the In- Rett syndrome does not, as first suspected,
ternational Rett Syndrome Association (www appear to be a degenerative disease but rather a
.rettsyndrome.org). These goals provide an ex- disorder characterized by arrested neurodevel-
cellent starting point for educators and par- opment caused by a mutation of the MECP2
ents seeking to build an educational program gene and the resulting failure to silence various
for a child with Rett syndrome, but it is im- yet unknown genes during development. The
portant to remember that goals must be indi- discovery of MECP2 as a gene involved in
vidualized and appropriate for the targeted Rett syndrome will potentially lead to the de-
student. Not all goals are appropriate for all velopment of a diagnostic test for early diagno-
children. sis and prenatal testing. Further research into
Though intervention styles and methodol- the cause, pathogenesis, effective treatment,
ogy vary from disability to disability, Rett and ultimately prevention of this disorder is on-
syndrome falls under the same pervasive de- going. Future research could refine our under-
velopmental disorder umbrella as autism. standing of the pathogenesis of this disorder.
156 Diagnosis and Classification

When this happens, there will be increased Armstrong, D., Dunn, J. K., Schultz, R. J., Herbert,
hope for the development of an effective treat- D. A., Glaze, D. G., & Motile, K. J. (1999).
ment (e.g., medication), and, perhaps, a cure Organ growth in Rett syndrome: A postmortem
(e.g., gene therapy). The development of mouse examination analysis. Pediatric Neurology,
models for Rett syndrome will facilitate the 20(2), 125–129.
Aron, M. (1990). The use and effectiveness of
molecular dissection of the pathogenesis of this
elbow splints in the Rett syndrome. Brain and
disorder. Increased research related to treat- Development, 12, 162–163.
ment of symptoms, communication develop- Bangs, T., & Dodson, S. (1979). Birth to Three De-
ment, and educational “ best practices” with velopmental Scale. Hingham, MA: Teaching
individuals displaying Rett syndrome are sorely Resources.
needed. As one of the slogans of the Interna- Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L.,
tional Rett Syndrome Association states, “If & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of sym-
we care today, we can cure tomorrow.” bols: Cognitive communication in infancy. New
York: Academic Press.
Cross-References Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The
acquisition of performatives prior to speech.
Issues in diagnosis of other pervasive develop- Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 407–423.
Bat-Haee, M. A. (1994). Behavioral training of a
mental disorders are discussed in Chapters 1,
young woman with Rett syndrome. Perceptual
3, 4, and 6; assessment issues are discussed in
and Motor Skills, 78, 314.
Chapters 27 through 33; issues in health care Beyer, K. S., Blasi, F., Bacchelli, E., Klauck, S. M.,
are reviewed in Chapter 20. Maestrini, E., & Poustka, A. (2002). Mutation
analysis of the coding sequence of the MECP2
REFERENCES gene in infantile autism. Human Genetics, 111,
305–309.
Adkins, W. N. (1986). Rett syndrome at an institu- Bieber Nielsen, J., Henriksen, K. F., Hansen, C.,
tion for the developmentally disabled. Ameri- Silahtaroglu, A., Schwartz, M., & Tommerup,
can Journal of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), N. (2001). MECP2 mutations in Danish pa-
85–97. tients with Rett syndrome: High frequency of
Allen, D. A. (1988). Autistic spectrum disorders: mutations but no consistent correlations with
Clinical presentation in preschool children. clinical severity or with X chromosome inacti-
Journal of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.), vation pattern. European Journal of Human
S48–S56. Genetics, 9, 178–184.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag- Bienvenu, T., Carrie, A., de Roux, N., Vinet, M. C.,
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- Jonveaux, P., Couvert, P., et al. (2000).
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. MECP2 mutations account for most cases of
Amir, R. E., Van den Veyver, I. B., Schultz, R., Mal- typical Rett syndrome. Human Molecular Ge-
icki, D. M., Tran, C. Q., Dahle, E. J., et al. netics, 9(9), 1377–1384.
(2000). Influence of mutation type and Braddock, S. R., Braddock, B. A., & Graham, J. M.
X chromosome inactivation on Rett syndrome (1993, October). Rett syndrome: An update
phenotypes. Annals of Neurology, 47, 670–679. and review for the primary pediatrician. Clini-
Amir, R. E., Van den Veyver, I. B., Wan, M., Tran, cal Pediatrics, 43, 613–625.
C. Q., Francke, U., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (1999). Brucke, T., Sofic, E., Killian, W., Rett, A., &
Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X- Riederer, P. (1988). Reduced concentrations
linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding and increased metabolism of biogenic amines
protein 2. Nature Genetics, 23, 185–188. in a single case of Rett syndrome: A post-
Armstrong, D. (1992). The neuropathology of mortem brain study. Journal of Neural Trans-
the Rett syndrome. Brain and Development, mission, 68, 315–324.
14(Suppl.), S89–S100. Budden, S. S. (1986). Rett syndrome: Studies of 13
Armstrong, D. (2001). Rett syndrome neuropathol- affected girls. American Journal of Medical
ogy review 2000. Brain and Development, 23, Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 99–109.
S72–S76. Budden, S. S., Meek, M., & Henighan, C. (1990).
Armstrong, D., Dunn, J. K., Antalffy, B., & Communication and oral-motor function in
Trivedi, R. (1995). Selective dendritic alter- Rett syndrome. Developmental Medicine and
ations in the cortex of Rett syndrome. Journal Child Neurology, 32, 51–55.
of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurol- Budden, S. S., Myer, E., & Butler, I. (1990). Cere-
ogy, 54, 195–201. brospinal f luid studies in the Rett syndrome:
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 157

Biogenic amines and ß-endorphins. Brain and Coleman, M. (1990). Is classical Rett syndrome
Development, 12, 81–84. ever present in males? Brain and Development,
Bumin, G., Uyanik, M., Yilmaz, I., Kayihan, H., 12, 31–32.
& Topcu, M. (2003). Hydrotherapy for Rett Coleman, M., Brubaker, J., & Hunter, K. (1988).
syndrome. Rehabilitation Medicine, 35(1), Rett syndrome: A survey of North American
44–45. patients. Journal of Mental Deficiency Re-
Burd, L., Fisher, W., & Kerbeshian, J. (1989). Per- search, 32, 117–124.
vasive disintegrative disorder: Are Rett syn- Coleman, M., & Gillberg, C. (1985). The biology of
drome and Heller dementia infantilis subtypes? autistic syndromes (pp. 45–50). New York:
Developmental Medical Child Neurology, 31(5), Praeger.
609–616. Collerton, D. (1986). Cholinergic function and in-
Burford, B., Keer, A. M., & Macleod, H. A. (2003). tellectual decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Neu-
Nurse recognition of early deviation in devel- roscience, 19, 1–28.
opment in home videos of infants with Rett Comings, D. E. (1986). The genetics of Rett syn-
disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re- drome: The consequences of a disorder where
search, 47, 588–589. every case is a new mutation. American Journal
Cassanova, M. F., Naidu, S., Goldberg, T. E., of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 383–388.
Moser, H. W., Khoromi, S., Kumar, A., et al. Cornford, M. E., Philippart, M., Jacobs, B.,
(1991). Quantitative magnetic resonance imag- Scheibel, A. B., & Vinters, H. V. (1994). Neu-
ing in Rett syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychia- ropathology of Rett syndrome: Case report
try, 3, 66–72. with neuronal and mitochondrial abnormali-
Cattell, P. (1940). Infant intelligence scale. New ties in the brain. Journal of Child Neurology,
York: Psychological Corporation. 9(4), 424–431.
Charman, T., Cass, H., Owen, L., Wigram, T., Demeter, K. (2000). Assessing the developmental
Slonims, V., Weeks, L., et al. (2002). Regres- level in Rett syndrome: An alternative ap-
sion in individuals with Rett syndrome. Brain proach. European Child and Adolescent Psychi-
and Development, 24, 281–283. atry, 9(3), 227–233.
Charnov, E. K., Stach, B. A., & Didonato, R. M. Diggs, C. C. (1981). School services. Language,
(1989, November). Pre and post onset develop- Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 4,
mental levels in Rett syndrome. Paper presented 269–271.
at the American Speech-Language-Hearing As- Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A.,
sociation Convention. St. Louis, MO. & Fassbender, L. L. (1984). Analyzing the
Cheadle, J. P., Gill, H., & Fleming, N. (2000). communicative functions of aberrant behavior.
Long-read sequence analysis of the MECP2 Journal of the Association for Persons with Se-
gene in Rett syndrome patients: Correlation vere Handicaps, 9(3), 201–212.
of disease severity with mutation type and Dragich, J., Houwink-Manville, I., & Schanen, C.
location. Human Molecular Genetics, 9, (2000). Rett syndrome: A surprising result of a
1119–1129. mutation in MECP2. Human Molecular Genet-
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. ics, 9, 2365–2375.
(1987). Positron emission tomography study Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Pic-
of human brain functional development. An- ture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines:
nals of Neurology, 22, 487–497. MN: American Guidance Service.
Clare, A. J. (1986). Rett syndrome: Behind their Eeg-Olofsson, O., Al-Zuhair, A. G. H., Teebi, A. S.,
eyes is more than they can show us. Disserta- Zaki, M., & Daoud, A. S. (1990). A boy with
tion, Thames Polytechnic Incorporating Avery Rett syndrome. Brain Development, 12(5),
Hill College, Kent, United Kingdom. 529–532.
Clark, A., Gardner, Medwin, D., Richardson, J., Egger, J., Hofacker, N., Schiel, W., & Holthausen,
McGann, A., Bonham, J. R., et al. (1990). Ab- H. (1992). Magnesium for hyperventilation in
normalities of carbohydrate metabolism and of Rett’s syndrome. Lancet, 340, 621–622.
OCT gene function in the Rett syndrome. Ellaway, C., & Christodoulou, J. (1999). Rett syn-
Brain and Development, 12, 119–124. drome: Clinical update and review of recent ge-
Clayton-Smith, J., Watson, P., Ramsden, S., & Black, netic advances. Journal of Paediatric and Child
G. C. (2000). Somatic mutation in MECP2 as a Health, 35, 419–426.
non-fatal neurodevelopmental disorder in males. Ellaway, C. J., Williams, K., Leonard, H., Higgins,
Lancet, 356, 830–832. G., Wilcken, B., & Chritodoulou, J. (1999).
Coker, S. B., & Melnyk, A. R. (1991). Rett syn- Rett syndrome: Randomized controlled trial of
drome and mitochondrial enzyme deficiencies. L-carnitine. Journal of Child Neurology, 14,
Journal of Child Neurology, 6, 164–166. 162–167.
158 Diagnosis and Classification

Fitzgerald, P. M., Jankovic, J., Glaze, D. G., genic diet in Rett syndrome. American Journal
Schultz, R., & Percy, A. K. (1990). Extrapyra- of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 225–246.
midal involvement in Rett’s syndrome. Neurol- Hagberg, B. (1980, June). Infantile autistic demen-
ogy, 40, 293–295. tia and loss of hand use: A report of 16
Fontanesi, J., & Haas, R. H. (1988). Cognitive pro- Swedish girl patients. Paper presented at the
file of Rett syndrome. Journal of Child Neurol- Research Session of the European Federation
ogy, 3(Suppl.), S20–S24. of Child Neurology Societies, Manchester,
Garofalo, E., Drury, I., & Goldstein, G. (1988). England.
EEG abnormalities aid diagnosis of Rett syn- Hagberg, B. (1985). Rett syndrome: Swedish ap-
drome. Pediatric Neurology, 4, 350–353. proach to analysis of prevalence and cause.
Genazzani, A., Zappella, M., Nalin, A., Hayek, Y., Brain and Development, 7(3), 277–280.
& Facchinetti, F. (1989). Reduced cere- Hagberg, B. (1989). Rett syndrome: Clinical pecu-
brospinal fluid ß-endorphin levels in Rett syn- liarities, diagnostic approach, and possible
drome. Child Nervous System, 5, 111–113. cause. Pediatric Neurology, 5, 75–83.
Gillberg, C. (1986). Autism and Rett syndrome: Some Hagberg, B. (1995). Clinical delineation of Rett
notes on differential diagnosis. American Jour- syndrome variants. Neuropaediatrics, 26, 62.
nal of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 127–131. Hagberg, B., Aicardi, J., Dias, K., & Ramos, O.
Gillberg, C. (1987). Autistic symptoms in Rett (1983). A progressive syndrome of autism, de-
syndrome: The first two years according to mentia, ataxia, and loss of purposeful hand
mother reports. Brain and Development, 9(5), use in girls: Rett syndrome: Report of 35
499–501. cases. Annals of Neurology, 14(4), 471–479.
Gillberg, C. (1989). The borderland of autism and Hagberg, B., Berg, M., & Steffenburg, U. (2001).
Rett syndrome: Five case histories to highlight Three decades of sociomedical experiences
diagnostic difficulties. Journal of Autism and from West Swedish Rett females 4–60 years of
Developmental Disorders, 19(4), 545–559. age. Brain and Development, 23, S28–S31.
Gillberg, C. (1997). Communication in Rett syn- Hagberg, B., Goutieres, F., Hanefeld, F., Rett, A.,
drome complex. European Child and Adoles- & Wilson, J. (1985). Rett syndrome: Criteria
cent Psychology, 6(Suppl. 1), 21–22. for inclusion and exclusion. Brain and Devel-
Gillberg, C., Terenius, L., Hagberg, B. Witt- opment, 7(3), 372–373.
Engerstrom, I., & Eriksson, I. (1990). CSF ß- Hagberg, B., Hanefeld, F., Percy, A., & Skjeldal, O.
endorphins in childhood neuropsychiatric (2002). An update on clinically applicable di-
disorders. Brain and Development, 12, 88–92. agnostic criteria in Rett syndrome. European
Girard, M., Couvert, P., Carrie, A., Tardieu, M., Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 6, 292–297.
Chelly, J., Beldjord, C., et al. (2001). Paternal Hagberg, B., & Skjeldal, O. H. (1994). Rett variant:
origin of de novo MECP2 mutations in Rett A suggested model for inclusion criteria. Pedi-
syndrome. European Journal of Human Genet- atric Neurology, 11, 5–11.
ics, 9, 231–236. Hagberg, B., Stenbom, Y., & Witt-Engerstrom, I.
Glaze, D. G., Frost, J. D., Zoghbi, H. Y., & Percy, (2000). Head growth in Rett syndrome. Acta
A. K. (1987). Rett syndrome in the electro- Paediatr, 89, 198–202.
encephalographic characteristics with clini- Hagberg, B., & Witt-Engerstrom, I. (1986). Rett
cal staging. Archives of Neurology, 44, syndrome: A suggested staging system for de-
1053–1056. scribing impairment profile with increasing
Gotoh, H., Susuki, I., Maruki, K., Mitomo, M., Hira- age towards adolescence. American Journal of
sawa, K., & Sasaki, N. (2001). Magnetic reso- Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 47–59.
nance imaging and clinical findings examined in Hamberger, A., Gillberg, C., Palm, A., & Hagberg,
adulthood studies on three adults with Rett syn- B. (1992). C.S.F. elevated glutamate in Rett
drome. Brain and Development, 23, S118–S121. syndrome. Neuropediatrics, 23, 212–213.
Goutieres, F., & Aicardi, J. (1985). Rett syndrome: Hanaoka, S., Ishikawa, N., & Kamoshita, S. (1985).
Clinical presentation and laboratory investiga- Three cases of Rett syndrome. In S. Kamoshita
tions in 12 further French patients. Brain and (Ed.), Abstracts for the workshop on Aicardi
Development, 7(3), 305–306. syndrome and Rett syndrome. Tokyo.
Haas, R., & Rice, M. L. A. (1985). Is Rett’s syn- Hanefeld, F. (1985). The clinical pattern of the
drome a disorder of carbohydrate metabolism? Rett syndrome. Brain and Development, 7(3),
Hyperpyruvic acidemia and treatment by keto- 320–325.
genic diet. Annals of Neurology, 18, 418. Hanks, S. B. (1986). The role of therapy in Rett
Haas, R. H., Rice, M. A., Trauner, D. A., & Mer- syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genet-
ritt, A. (1986). Therapeutic effects of a keto- ics, 24(Suppl. 1), 247–252.
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 159

Hanks, S. B. (1989, May). Motor disabilities and Huppke, P., Laccone, F., Kramer, N., Engel, W., &
physical therapy strategies in Rett syndrome. Hanefeld, F. (2000). Rett syndrome: Analysis
Paper presented at the 5th annual conference of MECP2 and clinical characterization of 31
of the International Rett Syndrome Associa- patients. Human Molecular Genetics, 9,
tion, Washington, DC. 1369–1375.
Hanks, S. B. (1990). Motor disabilities in the Rett International Rett Syndrome Association. (1990).
syndrome and physical therapy strategies. Parent idea book: Managing Rett syndrome. Ft.
Brain and Development, 12, 157–161. Washington, MD: Author.
Harding, B. N., Tudmay, A. J., & Wilson, J. (1985). International Rett Syndrome Association. (2002).
Neuropathological studies in a child showing Testing methods. Retrieved October 1, 2002,
some features of the Rett syndrome. Brain and from http://rettsyndrome.org/main /testing.htm.
Development, 7(3), 342–344. Ishikawa, A., Goto, T., Narasaki, M., Yokochi, K.,
Harris, J. C., Wong, D. F., Wagner, H. N., Rett, A., Kitahara, H., & Fukuyama, Y. (1978). A new
Naidu, S., Dannals, R. F., et al. (1986). Positron syndrome (?) of progressive psychomotor dete-
emission tomographic study of D2 dopamine rioration with peculiar stereotyped movement
receptor binding and CSF biogenic amine and autistic tendency: A report of three cases.
metabolites in Rett syndrome. American Jour- Brain and Development, 3, 258.
nal of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 201–210. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Willis, K. D., Gamache,
Harrison, D. J., & Webb, P. J. (1990). Scoliosis in T. B., & Hyman, S. L. (1986). Operant studies
the Rett syndrome: Natural history and treat- of self-injurious hand biting in the Rett syn-
ment. Brain and Development, 12, 154–156. drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
Havlak, C., & Covington, C. (1989). Motor function: 24, 157–166.
Physical and occupational therapy strategies. Iyama, C. M. (1993). Rett syndrome. Advances in
Education and therapeutic intervention in Rett Pediatrics, 40, 217–245.
syndrome. Ft. Washington, MD: International Jacobsen, K., Viken, A., & von Tetzchner, S. (2001).
Rett Syndrome Association. Rett syndrome and ageing: A case study. Dis-
Hennessy, M. J., & Haas, R. H. (1988). The ortho- ability and Rehabilitation, 23, 160–166.
pedic management of Rett syndrome. Journal Jan, M. M. S., Dooley, J. M., & Gordon, K. E.
of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.), S43–S47. (1999). Male Rett syndrome variant: Applica-
Hetzroni, O., Rubin, C., & Konkol, O. (2002). The tion of diagnostic criteria. Pediatric Neurol-
use of assistive technology for symbol identifi- ogy, 20, 238–240.
cation by children with Rett syndrome. Journal Jellinger, K., Armstrong, D., Zoghbi, H. Y., &
of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, Percy, A. K. (1988). Neuropathology of
27, 57–71. Rett syndrome. Acta Neuropathologica, 76,
Hoff buhr, K., Devaney, J. M., LaFluer, B., Sirianni, 142–158.
N., Scacheri, C., Giron, J., et al. (2001). Jellinger, K., & Seiteberger, F. (1986). Neuropathol-
MECP2 mutations in children with and with- ogy of Rett syndrome. American Journal of
out the phenotype of Rett syndrome. Neurol- Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 259–288.
ogy, 56, 1496–1495. Kahn, J. V. (1977). Comparison of manual and oral
Hoffbuhr, K. C., Moses, L. M., Jerdonek, M. A., & language training. Mental Retardation, 12, 3–5.
Naidu, S. (2002). Associations between Kahn, J. V. (1981). A comparison of sign and ver-
MECP2 mutations, X-chromosome inactiva- bal language training with nonverbal retarded
tion and phenotype. Mental Retardation and children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Sci-
Developmental Disabilities, 8, 99–105. ences, 47, 113–119.
Holm, V. A. (1985). Rett syndrome: A case report Kahn, J. V. (1984). Cognitive training and initial
from an audiovisual program. Brain and De- use of referential speech. Topics in Language
velopment, 7(3), 297–299. Disorders, 5, 14–28.
Hunter, K. (1987). Rett syndrome: Parents’ views Kerr, A. M., Belicohenko, P., Woodcock, T., &
about specific symptoms. Brain and Develop- Woodcock, M. (2001). Mind and brain in Rett
ment, 9(5), 535–538. disorder. Brain and Development (Suppl. 1),
Hunter, K. (1999). The Rett syndrome handbook. S44–S49.
Clinton, MD: International Rett Syndrome Kerr, A. M., Montague, J., & Stephenson, J. B. P.
Association. (1987). The hands, and the mind, pre- and
Huppke, P., Held, M., Laccone, F., & Hanefeld, F. post-regression, in Rett syndrome. Brain and
(2003). The spectrum of phenotypes in fe- Development, 9(5), 487–490.
males with Rett syndrome. Brain and Develop- Kerr, A., Southall, D., Amos, P., Cooper, R.,
ment, 25, 346–351. Samuels, M., Mitchell, J., et al. (1990).
160 Diagnosis and Classification

Correlation of electroencephalogram, respira- and metabolites in postmortem brain. Pedi-


tion, and movement in the Rett syndrome. atric Neurology, 5(6), 357–362.
Brain and Development, 12, 61–68. Leonard, H., & Bower, C. (1998). Is the girl with
Kerr, A., & Stephenson, J. B. P. (1985). Rett’s syn- Rett syndrome normal at birth? Developmental
drome in the west of Scotland. British Medical Medicine and Child Neurology, 49, 115–121.
Journal, 291, 579–582. Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W.
Kerr, A., & Stephenson, J. B. P. (1986). A study of (1990). Expectancy, loss of control, and anger
the natural history of Rett syndrome in 23 expression in young infants. Report of the In-
girls. American Journal of Medical Genetics, stitute for the Study of Child Development,
24, 77–83. Robert Wood Johnson Medical School De-
Kerr, A. M., & Witt-Engerstrom, I. (2001). Rett partment of Pediatrics, New Brunswick, NJ.
disorder and the developing brain. Oxford, En- Lieb-Lundell, C. (1988). The therapist’s role in
gland: Oxford University Press. the management of girls with Rett syndrome.
Kjoerholt, K., & Salthammer, E. (1989). Kjoerholt Journal of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.),
Salthammer Fysioterapi: Educational and S31–S34.
therapeutic intervention in Rett syndrome. Ft. Lloyd, K. G., Hornykiewicz, O., Davidson, L.,
Washington, MD: International Rett Syn- Shannak, K., Farley, I., Goldstein, M., et al.
drome Association. (1981). Biochemical evidence of dysfunction
Kondo, I., Morishita, R., & Fukuda, T. (2000). The of brain neurotransmitters in the Lesch-Nyhan
spectrum and paternal origin of de novo mu- syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine,
tants of methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 gene 305, 1106–1111.
(MECP2) in Rett syndrome. American Journal Loder, R. T., Lee, C. L., & Richards, B. S. (1989).
of Human Genetics, 67(Suppl. 2), 386. Orthopedic aspects of Rett syndrome: A multi-
Koppenhaver, D. A., Erickson, K. A., & Skotko, center review. Journal of Pediatric Orthopedics,
B. G. (2001). Supporting the communication 9(5), 557–562.
of girls with Rett syndrome and their mothers Lombroso, P. J. (2000). Genetics of childhood dis-
in storybook reading. International Journal of orders: A gene for Rett syndrome. Journal of
Disability, Development and Education, 48(4), the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
395–410. Psychiatry, 39, 671–674.
Kozinetz, C. A., Skender, M. L., MacNaughton, N., Lugaresi, E., Cirignotta, F., & Montagna, P. (1985).
Almes, M. J., Schultz, R. J., Percy, A. K., et al. Abnormal breathing in the Rett syndrome.
(1993). Epidemiology of Rett syndrome: A Brain and Development, 7, 328–333.
population-based registry. Pediatrics, 91, Lyon, M. F. (1972). X chromosome inactivation
445–450. and developmental patterns in mammals. Bio-
Krageloh-Mann, I., Schroth, G., Niemann, G., & logical Review, 47, 1–35.
Michaelis, R. (1989). The Rett syndrome: McArthur, A. J., & Budden, S. (1998). Sleep dys-
Magnetic resonance imaging and clinical find- function in Rett syndrome: A trial of exoge-
ings in four girls. Brain and Development, 11, nous melatonin. Developmental Medicine and
175–178. Child Neurology, 40, 186–192.
Lagercrantz, H., & Srinivasan, M. (1991). Devel- Meloni, L., Bruttini, M., & Longo, I. (2000). A mu-
opment and function of neurotransmitters/ tation in the Rett syndrome gene, MECP2,
modulator systems in the brain stem. In M. A. causes X-linked mental retardation and pro-
Birke (Ed.), The fetal and neonatal brain stem gressive spasticity in males. American Journal
(pp. 1–20). Cambridge, England: Cambridge of Human Genetics, 67, 982–983.
University Press. Millichap, J. G. (1987). Rett’s syndrome: A variant
Lappalainien, R., & Riikonen, R. S. (1996). High of Heller’s dementia? Lancet, 1, 440.
levels of cerebrospinal f luid glutamate in Rett Missliwetz, J., & Depastas, G. (1985). Forensic
syndrome. Pediatric Neurology, 15, 213–216. problems in Rett syndrome. Brain and Develop-
Leiber, B. (1985). Rett syndrome: A nosological en- ment, 7(3), 326–328.
tity. Brain and Development, 7(3), 275–276. Moodley, M. (1992). Rett syndrome in South Africa.
Lekman, A., Hagberg, B., & Svennerholm, B. A. Annals of Tropical Paediatrics, 12, 409–415.
(1991). Membrane cerebral lipids in Rett syn- Moser, H. W. (1986). Preamble to the workshop on
drome. Pediatric Neurologist, 7, 186–190. Rett syndrome. American Journal of Medical
Lekman, A., Witt-Engerstrom, I., Gottfries, J., Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 1–20.
Hagberg, B. A., Percy, A. K., & Svennerholm, Motil, K. J., Schultz, R., Brown, B., Glaze, D. G., &
L. (1989). Rett syndrome: Biogenic amines Percy, A. K. (1994). Altered energy balance may
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 161

account for growth failure in Rett syndrome. Olsson, B. (1987). Autistic traits in the Rett syn-
Journal of Child Neurology, 9(3), 315–319. drome. Brain and Development, 9(5),
Murakami, J. W., Courchesne, E., Haas, R. H., 491–498.
Press, G. A., & Yeung-Courchesne, R. (1992). Olsson, B., & Rett, A. (1985). Behavioral observa-
Cerebellar and cerebral abnormalities in Rett tions concerning differential diagnosis between
syndrome: A quantitative MR analysis. Amer- the Rett syndrome and autism. Brain and Devel-
ican Journal of Roentgenology, 159, 177–183. opment, 7(3), 281–289.
Myer, E. C., Tripathi, H. L., Brase, D. A., & Dewey, Olsson, B., & Rett, A. (1987). Autism and Rett
W. L. (1992). Elevated CSF beta-endorphin syndrome: Behavioral investigations and dif-
immunoreactivity in Rett’s syndrome: Report ferential diagnosis. Developmental Medicine
of 158 cases and comparison with leukemic and Child Neurology, 29, 429–441.
children. Neurology, 42, 357–360. Olsson, B., & Rett, A. (1990). A review of the Rett
Naganuma, G. (1988). Motor function: Physical syndrome with a theory of autism. Brain and
therapy strategies. Educational and therapeu- Development, 12, 11–15.
tic intervention in Rett syndrome. Ft. Mary- Opitz, J. M. (1986). Rett syndrome: Some com-
land, MD: International Rett Syndrome ments on terminology and diagnosis. American
Association. Journal of Medical Genetics, 24, 27–37.
Naidu, S., Hyman, S., Piazza, K., Savedra, J., Opitz, J. M., & Lewin, S. (1987). Rett syndrome—
Perman, J., Wenk, G., et al. (1990). The Rett A review and discussion of syndrome delin-
syndrome: Progress report on studies at the eation. Brain and Development, 9(5),
Kennedy Institute. Brain and Development, 445–450.
12, 5–7. Pan, J. W., Lane, J. B., Hetherington, H., & Percy,
Naidu, S., Murphy, M., Moser, H. W., & Rett, A. A. K. (1999). Rett syndrome: H spectroscopic
(1986). Rett syndrome: Natural history in 70 imaging at 41 Tesla. Journal of Child Neurol-
cases. American Journal of Medical Genetics, ogy, 14, 524–528.
24(Suppl. 1), 61–72. Percy, A. K. (1992, May). Medical updates and
Niedermeyer, E., & Naidu, S. (1990). Further EEG overviews. Paper presented at the International
observations in children with Rett syndrome. Rett Syndrome Association meeting, San
Brain and Development, 12, 53–54. Diego, CA.
Niedermeyer, E., Rett, A., Renner, H., Murphy, M., Percy, A. K., & Hagberg, B. (1992). Therapy in
& Naidu, S. (1986). Rett syndrome and the Rett syndrome: Drug trials and failures
electroencephalogram. American Journal of (pp. 108–110).
Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 195–200. Percy, A. K., Hass, R., Kolodny, E., Moser, H., &
Nielsen, J. B., Friberg, L., Lou, H., Lassen, N. A., Naidu, S. (1988). Recommendations regard-
& Sam, I. L. (1990). Immature pattern of brain ing handling of the necropsy in Rett syn-
activity in Rett syndrome. Archives of Neurol- drome. Journal of Child Neurology, 3 (Suppl.),
ogy, 47, 982–986. 91–93.
Nielsen, J. B., Lou, H. C., & Andresen, J. (1990). Percy, A. K., Schultz, R., Glaze, D. G., Skender,
Biochemical and clinical effects of tyrosine M., del Junco, D., Waring, S. C., et al. (1991).
and tryptophan in the Rett syndrome. Brain Trail of the opiate antagonist, naltrexone, in
and Development, 12, 143–148. children with Rett syndrome. Annals of Neu-
Nihei, K., & Naitoh, H. (1990). Cranial computed rology, 30, 486.
tomographic and magnetic resonance imaging Percy, A. K., Zoghbi, H. Y., Lewis, K. R., &
studies on the Rett syndrome. Brain and De- Jankovic, J. (1988). Rett syndrome: Qualita-
velopment, 12, 101–105. tive and quantitative differentiation from
Nomura, Y., & Segawa, M. (1990). Clinical fea- autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.),
tures of the early stage of the Rett syndrome. S65–S67.
Brain and Development, 12, 16–19. Percy, A. K., Zoghbi, H., & Riccardi, V. M. (1985).
Nomura, Y., Segawa, M., & Hasegawa, M. (1984). Rett syndrome: Initial experience with an
Rett syndrome—Clinical studies and patho- emerging clinical entity. Brain and Develop-
physiological consideration. Brain and Devel- ment, 7(3), 300–304.
opment, 6, 475–486. Perry, A., Sarlo-McGarvey, N., & Haddad, C.
Nomura, Y., Segawa, M., & Higurashi, M. (1985). (1991). Brief report: Cognitive and adaptive
Rett syndrome—An early catecholamine and functioning in 28 girls with Rett syndrome.
indolamine deficient disorder? Brain and De- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
velopment, 7(3), 334–341. 21(4), 551–556.
162 Diagnosis and Classification

Philippart, M. (1986). Clinical recognition of Rett for speech-language clinicians. Journal of


syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genet- Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 347–359.
ics, 24(Suppl. 1), 111–118. Riederer, P., Brucke, T., Sofic, E., Kienzl, E., Sch-
Philippart, M. (1990). The Rett syndrome in males. necker, K., Eng, D., et al. (1985). Neurochemi-
Brain and Development, 12, 33–36. cal aspects of the Rett syndrome. Brain and
Philippart, M. (1993). Rett syndrome associated Development, 7(3), 351–360.
with tuberous sclerosis in a male and in a fe- Riederer, P., Wieser, M., Wichart, I., Schmidt, B.,
male: Evidence for arrested motor and mental Killian, W., & Rett, A. (1986). Preliminary
development. American Journal of Medical Ge- brain autopsy findings in progredient Rett syn-
netics, 48, 229–230. drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. 24(Suppl. 1), 305–315.
New York: Harcourt, Brace and Jovanovich. Robb, S. A., Harden, A., & Boyd, S. G. (1989). Rett
Piazza, C. C., Anderson, C., & Fisher, W. (1993). syndrome: An EEG study in 52 girls. Neurope-
Teaching self-feeding skills to patients with diatrics, 20(4), 192–195.
Rett syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Rolando, S. (1985). Rett syndrome: Report of eight
Child Neurology, 35, 991–996. cases. Brain and Development, 7(3), 290–296.
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W., Kiesewetter, K., Bowman, Schleichkorn, J. (1987). Rett syndrome: A neurolog-
L., & Moser, H. (1990). Aberrant sleep patterns ical disease largely undiagnosed here. Physical
in children with Rett syndrome. Brain and De- Therapy Bulletin, 2, 16–18.
velopment, 12, 488–493. Schultz, R. J., Glaze, D. G., Motil, K. J., Arm-
Reichle, J., & Keogh, W. (1986). Communication in- strong, D. D., del Junco, D. L., Hubbard, C. R.,
struction for learners with severe handicaps: et al. (1993). The pattern of growth failure in
Some unresolved issues. In R. Horner, L. Meyer, Rett syndrome. American Journal of Disabili-
& B. Fredricks (Eds.), Education of learners ties in Children, 147(6), 633–637.
with severe handicaps: Exemplary services Segawa, M. (2001). Pathophysiology of Rett syn-
(pp. 189–220). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. drome from the standpoint of clinical charac-
Reilly, S., & Cass, H. (2001). Growth and nutrition teristics. Brain and Development, 23, S94–S98.
in Rett syndrome. Disability and Rehabilita- Sekul, E. A., Moak, J. P., Schultz, R., Glaze, D. G.,
tion, 23, 118–128. Dunn, J. K., & Percy, A. K. (1991). Electro-
Reiss, A. L., Faruque, F., Naidu, S., Abrams, M., cardiographic changes in Rett syndrome. An-
Beaty, T., Bryan, R. N., et al. (1993). Neuro- nals of Neurology, 30, 496.
anatomy of Rett syndrome: A volumetric Sekul, E. A., & Percy, A. K. (1992). Rett syndrome:
imaging study. Annals of Neurology, 34(2), Clinical features, genetic considerations, and
227–234. the search for a biological marker. Current
Rett, A. (1966). Uber ein eigenartiges hirnat- Neurology, 12, 173–198.
rophisches syndrom bei hyperammonamie im Shahbazian, M. D., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (2001). Molec-
kindesalter (On an unusual brain atrophy syn- ular genetics of Rett syndrome and clinical
drome in hyperammonaemia in childhood). spectrum of MECP2 mutations. Current Opin-
Wien Med Wochenschr, 116, 723–738. ions in Neurology, 14, 171–176.
Rett, A. (1969). Ein zerebral-atrophisches Syn- Shahbazian, M. D., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (2002). Rett
drom bei Hyperammonamie im Kindesalter syndrome and MECP2: Linking epigenetics
(Hyperammonaemia and cerebral atrophy in and neuronal function. American Journal of
childhood). Forschr Med, 87, 507–509. Human Genetics, 71, 1259–1272.
Rett, A. (1977). A cerebral atrophy associated with Sharpe, P. A. (1992). Comparative effects of bilat-
hyperammonaemie. In P. J. Vinken & G. W. eral hand splints and an elbow orthosis on
Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology. stereotypic hand movements and toy play in
Amsterdam, North Holland: Elsevier Science two children with Rett syndrome. American
Publishing Company. Journal of Occupational Therapy, 46, 134–140.
Rett, A. (1986). History and general overview. Amer- Singer, H. S., & Naidu, S. (2001). Rett syndrome:
ican Journal of Medical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), “ We’ll keep the genes on for you.” Neurology,
21–26. 56(5), 611–617.
Riccardi, V. M. (1986). The Rett syndrome: Genet- Sirianni, N., Naidu, S., Pereira, J., Pillotto, R. F., &
ics and the future. American Journal of Med- Hoffman, E. P. (1998). Rett syndrome: Confir-
ical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 389–402. mation of X-linked dominant inheritance, and
Rice, M. (1983). Contemporary accounts of the localization of the gene to Xq28. American
cognitive/ language relationship: Implications Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 1552–1558.
Rett Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder 163

Skjeldal, O. H., von Tetzchner, S., Jacobsen, K., Van Acker, R. (1987). Stereotypic responding as-
Smith, L., & Heiberg, A. (1995). Rett syn- sociated with Rett Syndrome: A comparison of
drome: Distribution of phenotypes with spe- girls with this disorder and matched subject
cial attention to the preserved speech variant. controls without the Rett Syndrome (Doctoral
Neuropediatrics, 26, 87. dissertation). DeKalb: Northern Illinois
Smith, T., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. (1995). University.
Behavioral treatment of Rett’s disorder: Inef- Van Acker, R., & Grant, S. B. (1995). An effective
fectiveness in three cases. American Journal of computer-based requesting system for persons
Mental Retardation, 100, 317–322. with Rett syndrome. Journal of Childhood
Sofic, E., Riederer, P., Killian, W., & Rett, A. Communication Disorders, 16, 31–38.
(1987). Reduced concentrations of ascorbic Van den Veyver, I. B., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (2001). Mu-
acid and glutathione in a single case of Rett tations in the gene encoding methyl-CpG-
syndrome: A postmortem brain study. Brain binding protein 2 cause Rett syndrome. Brain
and Development, 9(5), 529–531. and Development, 23, S147–S151.
Sponseller, P. D. (1989). Orthopaedic problems Verma, N. P., Chheda, R. L., & Nigro, M. A.
in Rett syndrome. Ft. Washington, MD: Inter- (1986). Electroencephalographic findings in
national Rett Syndrome Association. Rett syndrome. Electroencephalograhy and
Stephenson, J. B., & Kerr, A. M. (1987). Rett syn- Clinical Neurophysiology, 64, 394–401.
drome: Disintegration not dementia. Lancet, 1, von Tetzchner, S. (1997). Communication skills
741. among females with Rett syndrome. European
Subramaniam, B., Naidu, S., & Reiss, A. L. (1997). Child and Adolescent Psychology, 6, 33–37.
Neuroanatomy in Rett syndrome. Neurology, von Tetzchner, S., Jacobsen, K. H., Smith, L.,
48, 399–407. Skjeldal, O. H., Heiberg, A., & Fagen, J. A.
Takagi, N. (2001). The role of X-chromosome inacti- (1996). Vision, cognition and developmental
vation in the manifestation of Rett syndrome. characteristics of girls and women with Rett
Brain and Development (Suppl. 1), S182–S185. syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child
The Rett Syndrome Diagnostic Criteria Work Neurology, 38, 212–225.
Group. (1988). Diagnostic criteria for Rett syn- Wan, M., Lee, S. S. J., & Zhang, X. (1999). Rett syn-
drome. Annals of Neurology, 23, 125–128. drome and beyond: Recurrent spontaneous and
Tpocu, M., Akyerli, C., Sayi, A., Toruner, G. A., familial MECP2 mutations at CpG hotspots.
Kocoglu, S. R., Cimbis, M., et al. (2002). So- American Journal of Human Genetics, 65,
matic mosaicism for a MECP2 mutation asso- 1520–1529.
ciated with classic Rett syndrome in a boy. Watson, J. S., Umansky, R., Marcy, S., Johnston, C.,
European Journal of Human Genetics, 10, & Repacholi, B. (1996). Behavioral competi-
77–81. tion in a case of Rett syndrome. Journal of Ap-
Topcu, M., Topaloglu, H., Renda, Y., Berker, M., & plied Developmental Psychology, 17, 553–575.
Turanli, G. (1991). The Rett syndrome in Watson, J. S., Umansky, R., Marcy, S., & Repa-
males. Brain and Development, 13(1), 62. choli, B. (1996). Intention and preference in a
Trevathan, E., & Naidu, S. (1988). The clinical 3-year-old girl with Rett syndrome. Journal of
recognition and differential diagnosis of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 69–84.
Rett syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, Watson, P., Black, G., & Ramsden, S. (2001). Angel-
3(Suppl.), S6–S16. man syndrome phenotype associated with mu-
Tuten, H., & Miedaner, J. (1989). Effect of hand tations in MECP2, a gene encoding a methyl
splints on stereotypic hand behavior of girls CpG binding protein. Journal of Medical Genet-
with Rett syndrome: A replication study. Phys- ics, 38, 224–228.
ical Therapy, 69(12), 1099–1103. Webb, T., & Latif, R. (2001). Rett syndrome and
Umansky, R., Watson, R., Hoff buhr, K., Painter, the MECP2 gene. Journal of Medical Genetics,
K. M., Devaney, J., & Hoffman, E. (2001). So- 38, 217–223.
cial facilitation of object-oriented hand use in Wehmeyer, M., Bourland, G., & Ingram, D. (1993).
a Rett syndrome variant girl: Implications for An analogue assessment of hand stereotypies
partial preservation of a hypothesized special- in two cases of Rett syndrome. Journal of Intel-
ized cerebral network. Developmental and Be- lectual Disability Research, 37, 95–102.
havioral Pediatrics, 22(2), 119–122. Weisz, C. L. (1987). Range of emotion. Brain and
Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. M. (1975). Assessment in Development, 9(5), 543–545.
infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological devel- Wenk, G. L., Naidu, S., Casanova, M. F., Kitt, C. A.,
opment. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. & Moser, H. (1991). Altered neurochemical
164 Diagnosis and Classification

markers in Rett syndrome. Neurology, 41, Yoshikawa, H., Fueki, N., Suzuki, H., Sakuragawa,
1753–1756. N., & Masaaki, I. (1991). Cerebral blood f low
Wenk, G. L., O’Leary, M., Nemeroff, C. B., Bis- and oxygen metabolism in Rett syndrome.
sette, G., Moser, H., & Naidu, S. (1993). Neu- Journal of Child Neurology, 6, 237–242.
rochemical alterations in Rett syndrome. Zappella, M. (1986). Motivational conf licts in Rett
Developmental Brain Research, 74, 67–72. syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genet-
Wesecky, A. (1986). Music therapy for children ics, 24(Suppl. 1), 143–151.
with Rett syndrome. American Journal of Med- Zappella, M. (1990). A double-blind trial of
ical Genetics, 24(Suppl. 1), 253–257. bromocriptine in the Rett syndrome. Brain and
Whitehouse, P. J., Price, D. L., Clark, A. W., Coyle, Development, 12, 148–150.
J. T., & DeLong, M. R. (1981). Alzheimer dis- Zappella, M. (1992). The Rett girls with preserved
ease: Evidence for selective loss of cholinergic speech. Brain Development, 14, 98–101.
neurons in the nucleus basalis. Annals of Neu- Zappella, M., & Genazzani, A. (1986). Girls with
rology, 10, 122–126. Rett syndrome tested with bromocriptine.
Witt-Engerstrom, I. (1987). Rett syndrome: A retro- Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 98, 780.
spective pilot study on potential early predic- Zappella, M., Genazzani, A., Facchinetti, F., &
tive symptomatology. Brain and Development, Hayek, G. (1990). Bromocriptine in the
9(5), 481–486. Rett syndrome. Brain and Development, 12,
Witt-Engerstrom, I., & Hagberg, B. (1990). The 221–225.
Rett syndrome: Gross motor disability and Zoghbi, H. Y. (1988). Genetic aspects of Rett syn-
neural impairment in adults. Brain and Devel- drome. Journal of Child Neurology, 3(Suppl.),
opment, 12, 23–26. S76–S78.
Woodyatt, G. C., & Ozanne, A. E. (1992). Commu- Zoghbi, H. Y., Milstien, S., Butler, I. J., Smith,
nication abilities and Rett syndrome. Journal E. O., Kaufman, S., Glaze, D. G., et al. (1989).
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, Cerebrospinal f luid biogenic amines and
155–173. biopterin in Rett syndrome. Annals of Neurol-
Woodyatt, G. C., & Ozanne, A. E. (1993). A longi- ogy, 25, 56–60.
tudinal study of cognitive skills and communi- Zoghbi, H. Y., Percy, A. K., Glaze, D. G., Butler,
cation behaviors in children with Rett I. J., & Riccardi, V. M. (1985). Reduction of
syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability biogenic amine levels in the Rett syndrome.
Research, 37, 419–435. New England Journal of Medicine, 313,
Xiang, F., Buervenich, S., Nicolao, P., Bailey, 921–924.
M. E., Zhang, Z., & Anvret, M. (2000). Muta- Zoghbi, H. Y., Percy, A. K., Schultz, R. J., & Fill,
tion screening in Rett syndrome patients. Jour- C. (1990). Patterns of X chromosome inactiva-
nal of Medical Genetics, 37, 250–255. tion in the Rett syndrome. Brain and Develop-
Yano, S., Yamashita, Y., Matsuishi, T., Abe, T., Ya- ment, 12, 131–135.
mada, S., & Shinohara, M. (1991). Four adult
Rett patients at an institution for the handi-
capped. Pediatric Neurology, 7, 289–292.
CHAPTER 6

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not


Otherwise Specified

KENNETH E. TOWBIN

The diagnostic term for conditions that share among them all, and underscored the connec-
the central feature of a severe deficit in social tion between PDD-NOS and autism. Clini-
learning and reciprocity is Pervasive Develop- cally, although individuals with PDD-NOS
mental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified may display milder symptoms than individuals
(PDD-NOS). Individuals with PDD-NOS with autism, the biological features and ge-
have social deficits similar to autism and may netic risks suggest remarkable similarities.
have, in addition, fundamental disturbances in Contemporary questions now focus on the re-
communication, social behavior, emotion reg- lationships between ASDs and a widening
ulation, cognition, and interests. The symp- Broader Autism Phenotype (BAP; Dawson,
toms of these deficits arise during the first Webb, et al., 2002).
years of life, yet their severity or scope do not PDD-NOS is similar, not identical, to
meet the more restrictive criteria for the other autism. There is reliable evidence that individ-
PDDs—Autistic, Asperger’s, Rett’s, or Child- uals diagnosed with PDD-NOS have milder
hood Disintegrative Disorder. Like all these deficits and a better prognosis than those di-
diagnostic terms, PDD-NOS is based on clini- agnosed with autism (Gillberg, 1991). It is
cal presentation and developmental history. It less clear whether these differences hold up
is likely that PDD-NOS is not just one condi- when one compares those with PDD-NOS to
tion. Until recently, the paucity of biological individuals with autism who are matched on
evidence in support of PDD-NOS created the nonverbal IQ and basic grammatical and syn-
impression that the diagnosis was more of a tactical language. It is also unknown whether
concept—a theoretical mild form of autism— the definitions and demarcations of subtypes
than a valid condition. Some argued that the of PDD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
presentation of individuals with PDD-NOS ual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) and
was so diverse that the term was not meaning- World Health Organization International Clas-
ful and did not convey practical information sification of Diseases (ICD-10) are valid or
about impairment and prognosis. Robust find- meaningful. Logically, a comprehensive clas-
ings from recent genetic and family history sification system (Rutter & Gould, 1985) must
studies of autism, as well as mounting psy- provide for conditions that fall short of the
chophysiological and imaging evidence, have severity or range of symptoms of the other
provided a substantial counterargument to PDDs but nevertheless exhibit impairment as a
these propositions. In the past several years, consequence of problems in social reciprocity.
genetic studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders It is unclear whether the current divisions are
(ASDs; Autistic disorder, Asperger’s disor- biologically valid. The ASDs are often viewed
der, and PDD-NOS) have revealed similarities as stratifying along a hypothetical spectrum or

165
166 Diagnosis and Classification

continuum covering the broad clinical range ICD-10 has a category of PDD-unspecified
from those with profound aloofness and mental (F84.9), but this description is not as broad as
retardation to those who appear neurotypical DSM-IV PDD-NOS. Closer scrutiny of the
but nevertheless display a distinctive lifelong clinical guidelines and descriptions in ICD
social or empathic blindness (Wing, 1992). On reveal that a more complete analogue to
this hypothetical spectrum, PDD-NOS is a DSM-IV’s PDD-NOS is the combination of
paradoxical clinical entity. Despite its amor- PDD-unspecified and atypical autism (F84.1;
phous clinical boundaries and the subtlety of World Health Organization [WHO], 1992).
the clinical presentation, PDD-NOS is one of DSM-IV-TR declares that “atypical autism” is
the most important PDDs. Its importance included under their term of PDD-NOS
stems from its relationship to autism, its (p. 84). DSM-IV states that individuals in this
prevalence, and most of all, the impairment category fail to meet age criteria, do not dis-
that it imparts to those who have it. play all the key elements of autism or the other
In this chapter, PDD will be the term for the subtypes of PDD, or that when these other ele-
spectrum that includes all the Pervasive Devel- ments are present, they may not be of suffi-
opmental Disorders including Autistic disor- cient severity to meet the full criteria for
der, PDD-NOS, Asperger’s disorder, and so autism or the other subtypes of PDD. In DSM-
on. The chapter offers an overview of PDD- IV-TR, PDD-NOS was one major exception to
NOS as a conceptual and clinical entity. It be- the policy of making no more than minimal
gins with a conceptual view and review of how changes from DSM-IV. DSM-IV-TR rectifies
DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 represent PDD-NOS. the DSM-IV error that would allow the diagno-
Since PDD-NOS is synonymous with “not any- sis of PDD-NOS in the absence of social im-
thing else in PDD,” it cannot be understood pairments. DSM-IV-TR makes clear that
without competent knowledge of the alterna- PDD-NOS is, foremost, a disorder of “recipro-
tive diagnoses and conditions defined under cal social interaction that is associated with”
PDD. For this reason, a detailed account of the impairments in “either verbal or non-verbal
differential diagnosis is offered. Explication of communication,” or with repetitive behaviors,
descriptive elements such as epidemiology, eti- and/or restricted interests.
ology, and natural history follow; and the re- Despite this clarification, the guidelines for
view ends with a condensed discussion of PDD-NOS in DSM-IV-TR continue to be prob-
treatments and future research directions. lematic because they are vaguely worded and
difficult to translate into clear definitions or
NOSOLOGY explicit operational criteria. By definition, in-
dividuals with PDD-NOS present with fewer
The term PDD first surfaced in DSM-III. or less severe symptoms than those who have
Within the group of PDDs, there were three Autistic or Asperger’s disorder, and do not
conditions: infantile autism, childhood onset meet criteria for Rett’s Disorder or Childhood
PDD, and atypical PDD. In DSM-III-R, this Disintegrative Disorder. Buitelaar and cowork-
was modified once again and PDD was rede- ers (1998) offered an operationalized defini-
fined with only two subtypes: Autistic Dis- tion of PDD-NOS although this has not been
order and PDD-NOS. DSM-IV and ICD-10 generally embraced in clinical or research ac-
created new subgroup diagnoses that previ- tivities. Like individuals with Autistic or As-
ously were under the umbrella term PDD-NOS perger’s disorder, persons with PDD-NOS can
in DSM-III-R or ICD-9. The new subgroups have restricted interests, dedication to non-
with separate designations include Asperger’s functional routines, limited imaginary play,
disorder (ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR), atypical and stereotyped behavior. Unlike those with
autism (ICD-10), Childhood Disintegrative Autistic or Asperger’s disorder, these features
Disorder (ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR), and Rett’s may all be mild or absent. Compared to those
Disorder (ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR). with Autistic disorder, individuals with PDD-
DSM-IV and ICD-10 use diagnostic terms NOS may or may not exhibit deficits in expres-
and guidelines for PDD-NOS in different ways. sive or receptive language.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 167

CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND odd,” insofar as they approach others and de-


sire interaction, but exhibit idiosyncratic and
Although terminology has changed, the exis- egocentric social exchanges. Early in child-
tence of an intermediate or mild PDD condi- hood these active-but-odd individuals might
tion is not a new discovery. Individuals with shrink from social interactions or be shunned
autism-like developmental disorders have been by their young age-mates because of intrusive
recognized for over 100 years (Itard, 1962). behaviors, excessively restrictive demands, or
After Leo Kanner identified the features of overreaction to rebuffs and refusals. In later
autism (1943), a series of reports appeared de- childhood, they display a failure to compre-
scribing individuals who were nearly but not hend and apply ordinary social rules; as adults
quite like those characterized by Kanner (Ben- their deficits become relatively milder and
der, 1946; Despert, 1958). The individuals in subtler. Wing and Gould suggested that it
these reports all exhibited early-onset, pro- takes more time and interaction to recognize
found deficits in relating, but they displayed the deficits in the active-but-odd group than in
other features that were different from those the other groups (Wing & Gould, 1979).
seen in autism, particularly in language and Wing’s vision may be accurate on several
stereotyped movement. counts, but it is not without flaws. First,
The unquestionable existence of these con- Wings’ concept of the spectrum is broader
ditions calls for a comprehensive system of than that of current diagnostic systems (Wing,
classification to provide a corresponding diag- 1997), and she has not offered cut points that
nosis that is clear, logical, practical, and rele- would enable one to differentiate subgroups
vant. Those who wish for such a meaningful and boundaries. Second, to relate the spec-
definition of PDD-NOS have had to revise the trum to diagnostic categories, one must make
definition as the concepts of the PDD change. untested suppositions. The social skills di-
Consequently, the clinical application of PDD- mension may not be sufficient to enable divi-
NOS has been open to criticism on at least two sion in the PDD diagnostic system. The
fronts. On the one side, some claim that PDD- prevailing view is that autism is the most se-
NOS should be removed because the inherent vere end of the continuum, followed by As-
vagueness of criteria (particularly under the perger’s disorder, and then PDD-NOS at the
current criteria) conveys so little information mildest end (Prior et al., 1998). There are oth-
about the person’s deficits, prognosis, and ers who suggest that the order should be Autis-
course that it renders the diagnosis unhelpful tic disorder, PDD-NOS, and Asperger’s
or meaningless. On another side, there is criti- disorder (Kurita, 1997). However, empirical
cism that PDD-NOS should be folded into studies fail to support either generalization.
Autistic disorder because it is so completely Robertson and coworkers (Robertson, Tanguay,
continuous with autism that it does not make L’Ecuyer, Sims, & Waltrip, 1999) found only a
sense to give it a separate diagnostic standing. weak correlation between social communica-
If PDD-NOS is simply a segment of the contin- tion and clinical diagnosis. Similarly, Gilchrist
uum of “Autism Spectrum Disorders,” it is a and coworkers (2001) found prognosis and
redundancy. adaptive function failed to differentiate high-
Wing and coworkers proposed the concept functioning autism (HFA) from Asperger’s
of a continuum of autistic conditions that are disorder.
differentiated by the degree of impairment in A further problem with the continuum
social reciprocity (Wing & Gould, 1979). In model is that it is exceedingly difficult to rec-
that view, individuals who are aloof and resist oncile with a categorical diagnostic system.
interactions with others represent the most se- Systems of nosology require compiling condi-
vere end of the spectrum. Next, in order of de- tions into relatively homogeneous phenotypic
creasing severity, are individuals who are subgroups. Imposing categorical diagnostic
passive but accept interactions when others boundaries on dimensional characteristics
initiate and continue to press on with them. requires the sacrifice of important details
The least severe group is termed “active but and data—the entire procedure becomes a
168 Diagnosis and Classification

procrustean bed. The effort to define bound- be relevant and instructive, nevertheless.)
aries that capture the comparative distinctions PDD-NOS encompasses a unique region in the
between points on a spectrum cannot help but spectrum of PDD and conveys important clini-
generate blurry, confusing definitions with cal information about persons who are so diag-
qualifiers like “relatively,” “significantly,” nosed. PDD-NOS is the diagnosis for a
and “limited amount.” Conversely, the imposi- collection of conditions that share important
tion of specific boundaries (and terms) where features resembling the primary PDDs but to a
none exist creates illusory, false dichotomies milder degree. Other PDD diagnoses do not ac-
that give official sanction to fictional differ- commodate the phenomena exhibited by this
ences. As Cantwell and Rutter suggested group of individuals. Furthermore, PDD-NOS
(1994), psychology divides IQ ranges into sub- denotes a link between this important group of
divisions such as mild, moderate, and severe disorders and the other more narrowly defined
mental retardation. Consider the absence of PDD subtypes. Whether the relationship is
real difference between someone with an IQ of valid is being resolved by longitudinal, patho-
68 who is diagnosed with “mild mental retar- physiological, cognitive, etiologic, and genetic
dation” and another person with an IQ of 72 studies. Some headway is being made on these
whose condition is called “ borderline IQ.” fronts and may lead to a different system of
Even so, as Cantwell and Rutter (1994) have grouping these conditions. It might be said
discussed, categorical systems offer the bene- that PDD-NOS is a work in progress that needs
fits of efficient communication and concep- further investigation. Yet without a place in
tual clarity, which may facilitate treatment. In the diagnostic system, the relationships among
addition, historically, science has condoned these disorders cannot be explored.
imposing categorical distinctions on known di- In clinical use, it appears that PDD-NOS
mensional variables. Another example is the takes on multiple meanings, each of which is
way we divide the electromagnetic wave spec- supported by official diagnostic guidelines.
trum into colors. There are legitimate reasons PDD-NOS has at least four different defini-
to harbor uncertainty about the validity and tions that diagnosticians can employ under dif-
significance of the distinctions among the cur- ferent circumstances:
rent subtypes of PDD.
It follows logically from these conceptual 1. PDD-NOS is not actually a clinical entity
considerations, that the diagnosis of PDD-NOS but a label to use under unfavorable diag-
generates ambiguity and confusion in clinical nostic conditions. For some authorities,
work. The label fails to position a patient’s PDD-NOS is to be used as a default diagno-
current functioning on this extensive contin- sis when information is inadequate or as a
uum. DSM-IV-TR reflects this nebulous quality last resort when the developmental history
by, on the one hand, designating PDD-NOS as is unreliable. In this view, PDD-NOS may
a diagnosis, but on the other, offering only a be a temporary designation or delaying
description without specific operationalized strategy when the absence of reliable infor-
criteria. The text describing PDD-NOS is a mation prevents asserting a more specific
tautology. The DSM-IV-TR description restates PDD diagnosis or until the clinician can ac-
a logically fundamental criterion of any valid quire a trustworthy history.
diagnosis—a collection of disorders that are 2. On the continuum of PDD, PDD-NOS is a
not other disorders (Rutter & Gould, 1985). collection of entities that are relatively
Any diagnostic category that fails this crite- higher functioning, but not qualitatively
rion is redundant. Were it not for its clinical le- distinct. PDD-NOS includes conditions in
gitimacy, the distress of families, and the which impairment in language or restricted
impairment of those with this condition, the interests/repetitive behaviors are mild or
definition of PDD-NOS would be a mere logi- perhaps absent. In this view, PDD-NOS is a
cal absurdity. group of disorders in which, by stringent
Despite its inherent diagnostic circularity criteria, the impairment in one of these do-
and imprecision, the category (and concept) of mains is too mild to permit one to assign
PDD-NOS is indispensable. (Ambiguity may another diagnosis; yet the impairments in
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 169

social relating are far too severe to be con- lated to PDD, but with further study may
sidered a variant of normal (Allen et al., be determined to be clinically distinct from
2001). This is congruent with ICD-10 autism (Volkmar & Cohen, 1988).
Atypical Autism and advances the idea that
PDD-NOS is “mild” autism, that is, “dif- This last view proposes that there are other
ferences in severity rather than type”(Szat- disorders of social relating that are not well
mari, 1997). The boundaries separating characterized currently and might temporar-
“ high-functioning autism” and this kind of ily be placed under PDD-NOS. The evolution
PDD-NOS are exceedingly uncertain. This of Asperger’s disorder (ASP) as a clinical di-
definition of PDD-NOS has evolved from agnosis illustrates this idea. The relationship
an acceptance of an autism spectrum disor- between ASP and autism remains uncertain;
der and relies on such factors as prognosis some believe it is a variant of autism (Frith,
and range of disturbance to define its 2004), or the same as high-functioning autism
points. (Szatmari, Bartolucci, Bremner, Bond, &
3. PDD-NOS is the diagnosis for individuals Rich, 1989; HFA), whereas others, including
who present with a late age of onset of Asperger himself (1979), assert that the con-
autistic symptoms. Rutter and Schopler dition is quite separate from autism. PDD-
(1988) suggest that the positive criteria for NOS may be the best temporary location for
autism include the three core features plus disorders like this that have yet to be fully
early age of onset. Seen this way, PDD- characterized yet display features along the
NOS is the diagnosis when this fourth fea- spectrum of moderate to severe impairment
ture is absent, just as in Definition 2. in social reciprocity. Some candidates for in-
Volkmar and coworkers (Volkmar, Steir, & clusion in this perspective would include
Cohen, 1985) reported that there may be a schizoid disorders in children (Wolff &
wide gap in age of onset and age of clinical Chick, 1980), Multiple Complex Develop-
diagnosis and depend on the reliability of mental Disorder (Buitelaar et al., 1998;
parents’ memories. There are, however, rare Cohen, Paul, & Volkmar, 1986; Towbin,
reports of individuals in whom the age of Dykens, Pearson, & Cohen, 1993), Multiple
onset occurs after age 30 months and who Developmental Impairment (MDI; McKenna
do not meet criteria for Childhood Disinte- et al., 1994), Pragmatic Language Impairment
grative Disorder (Volkmar & Cohen, 1989). (Bishop, 1989), some Infant Regulatory Dis-
4. The heterogeneous clinical entities that orders, and some Reactive Attachment Disor-
comprise PDD-NOS share two critical fea- ders (Richters & Volkmar, 1994).
tures—early onset of symptoms and im-
pairment in social reciprocity. Such a BIOLOGICAL STUDIES OF PDD-NOS
definition implies that the theoretical span
of the autistic spectrum from severe autism There are no biological markers that measure
(and aloofness) to much milder, but never- the “dose” of autism risk or social reciprocity
theless impairing, deficits in social reci- impairment. Attempts to refine the spectrum
procity, may not be continuous. It focuses using phenomenological features have tended
on conditions other than autism that dis- to reinforce evidence favoring a spectrum or
play prominent impairment in relatedness. continuum among the DSM-IV diagnoses of
However, these other conditions also pres- autism to PDD-NOS, rather than any meaning-
ent with additional symptoms that are not ful separation between them (Robertson et al.,
part of the spectrum of autism per se, such 1999; Tanguay, Robertson, & Derrick, 1998).
as impairments in understanding affect, af- Moving away from the DSM-IV view of PDDs,
fective modulation, and patterns of attach- Lord and coworkers (Lord, Leventhal, &
ment. From this perspective, PDD-NOS, in Cook, 2001) have suggested that the phenome-
the current official nosology, includes con- nology better reflects a multidimensional view
ditions with core features of impairment in of the autism spectrum in which there are
both reciprocal social interactions and the three independent dimensions—nonverbal in-
capacity to develop empathy; these are re- tellectual ability, expressive language ability,
170 Diagnosis and Classification

and social reciprocity with repetitive behav- 0%. Concordance for the broader autism spec-
iors/restricted interests. They propose that ge- trum phenotype was 92% versus 0% for MZ
netic studies would benefit from applying such and DZ twins, respectively. Le Couteur and
a model instead of relying on DSM-IV sub- coworkers expanded the original cohort ascer-
types. Further significant promise in redefin- tained by Folstein and Rutter (1977) to include
ing the spectrum of PDD has come from using 20 additional MZ males and 14 additional DZ
information from genetics (Spiker, Lotspeich, male twin pairs. They reported that among MZ
Dimiceli, Myers, & Risch, 2002), neuropsy- co-twins of probands with autism, there was a
chological results (Klin et al., 1999), and func- concordance rate of 60% for autistic disorder
tional behavior (Gillham, Carter, Volkmar, & and 72% for all PDDs. Among dizygotic twins,
Sparrow, 2000). Although exciting neuropsy- rates concordance for autism was 0% and for
chological and neuroimaging findings are the broader phenotype (all PDDs), 10%.
being produced (Piggott et al, 2004; Schultz Family studies offer another method of dis-
et al., 2000; Sparks et al., 2002; Wang et al., covering genetic relationships. Most studies
2004), the effectiveness of these measures to indicate that a PDD-NOS diagnosis is as likely
create meaningful distinctions within PDD as autism in sibs of probands with autism.
(diagnostic specificity) has yet to be demon- Using rigorous family study methodology,
strated. The emerging view is that in the fu- Piven and coworkers (1990) noted higher rates
ture, PDD may be divided in a different way, of “severe social dysfunction and isolation”
perhaps along dimensions of higher versus among adult siblings of children with autism.
lower grammatical /syntactic language acquisi- This was replicated in a subsequent controlled
tion and cognitive ability. This approach sample of parents (Piven, Palmer, et al., 1997).
would replace the systems that are based on Bolton and coworkers’ (1994) study of 99
the amount of stereotypy/restricted interests, probands with autism found an overall rate of
social use of language, and severity of social 5.8% of siblings had a PDD—2.9% Autistic
impairment (Howlin, 2003; Lord et al., 2001; disorder and 2.9% nonautistic PDD. MacLean
Szatmari et al., 2002). Another possibility is and coworkers (1999) suggest that a stronger
that receptive and expressive language impair- case can be made for inheritance of the sever-
ment may be used to differentiate higher and ity of impairment or level of function than for
lower functioning ASDs (Walker et al., 2004). the subtype of PDD. They examined 94 chil-
There is growing biological evidence to dren in 46 “multiplex” (more than one sibling
support a close relationship between PDD- diagnosed with a PDD) families using rigorous
NOS and autism. Robust support comes from diagnostic methods and comparing IQ and
family and genetic studies of PDDs. Twin adaptive functioning. For 31 of 50 sibling
studies that ascertain the presence of autistic pairs, both children received the same diag-
and nonautistic PDD have reliably demon- noses and thus did not reflect significant ag-
strated increased rates of ASP and PDD-NOS gregation of PDD subtypes within families.
among monozygotic co-twins of probands with Their analyses revealed that the only moderate
autism (Bailey et al., 1995; Le Couteur et al., or greater correlations were for nonverbal IQ,
1996; MacLean et al., 1999). In Folstein and adaptive behaviors of socialization, and com-
Rutter’s twin study (1977), the reported con- munication. Thus, they propose that it is more
cordance for narrowly defined autism in important to stratify according to level of
monozygotic (MZ) twins was 36% (versus 0% function than PDD subtype in genetic studies.
in dyzygotic [DZ] twins). When the data were These studies are vulnerable to the criti-
reanalyzed, examining concordance that in- cism that environmental contributions have
cluded higher functioning, socially impaired made significant contributions to the results
relatives, the rate increased to 82% (Folstein (Szatmari et al., 2000). One remedy is to use
& Rutter, 1987). Bailey and coworkers (1995) extended pedigree studies that control for the
evaluated concordance in 45 twin pairs in bias introduced by environmental contribu-
which one twin had autism (25 MZ and 20 tions. Using this method, Szatmari and
DZ). The concordance rate of autism among coworkers found that rates of lesser variant
MZ twins was 60% and among DZ twins was features of autism (i.e., communication
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 171

impairments, social impairments, and repeti- notable that there were no differences between
tive activities) were significantly more likely PDD and control groups in performance on
in biological versus nonbiological relatives of dorsolateral or ventromedial frontal lobe func-
PDD probands. Moreover, rates of these lesser tion tasks at this age. In another series of stud-
variant features were as common in relatives ies, Dawson, Carver, and coworkers (2002)
of the higher IQ, higher functioning probands measured event-related potentials (P400 and
as among those of lower functioning probands. Nc) when processing familiar and unfamiliar
It is particularly valuable that the investigators faces, in the same cohort of 3- to 4-year-olds
examined genetic risk for lesser variant fea- and two comparison groups (children with de-
tures in relatives of nonautistic as well as velopmental disorders and healthy children).
autistic PDD. Similar to findings reported by The autism spectrum group did not demon-
Bolton and coworkers (1994), rates of lesser strate the typical pattern of P400 and Nc re-
variant features were unrelated to the subtype sponse to familiar versus unfamiliar faces
of PDD diagnosis. The genetic risk to relatives although they did display typical differences
was independent of PDD subtype, and lesser to familiar versus unfamiliar objects. There
variant features were as common among rela- were no differences in responses between
tives of nonautistic PDD (“atypical autism” or those with autism and those with PDD-NOS.
ASP) as among relatives of individuals with Similarly, van der Geest and coworkers (van
autism. A noteworthy design limitation of this der Geest, Kemner, Verbaten, & van Engeland,
study is its reliance on reports of relatives 2002) found no differences in gaze fixation to
about other relatives, rather than direct inter- upside-down faces between children with
views of all relatives. This may be partially autism and PDD-NOS children. Both were ab-
offset by use of a control population and good normal compared to age-matched healthy vol-
evidence that family report designs tend to un- unteers. Neither group was abnormal in gaze
derreport, not inflate, the presence of symp- fixation for upright faces.
toms and disorders. In addition, a similar Investigations using theory of mind (ToM)
study of an epidemiological cohort (Micali tasks may distinguish high-functioning from
et al., 2004) revealed similar results. In that low-functioning PDD, but do not differentiate
study there was a sixfold increase in the preva- diagnostic subtypes. Poor performance on
lence of PDDs among family members of ToM tasks is not unique to autism or PDD
probands with a PDD. These rates were across (Pickup & Frith, 2001; Pilowsky, Yirmiya,
all PDD subtypes (Micali et al., 2004). Arbelle, & Mozes, 2000). Differences in per-
formance between individuals with autism and
Neuropsychological Studies those with PDD-NOS appear to be related
more to overall IQ and adaptive function than
Several lines of evidence suggest that individu- to anything that is specific in these diagnoses
als who have PDD-NOS share neuropsycho- (Buitelaar et al., 1999; Serra, Loth, van Geert,
logical features with those who are diagnosed Hurkens, & Minderaa, 2002).
with Autistic disorder (Dawson, Webb, et al.,
2002). Dawson and coworkers (Dawson, Mun- Neuroimaging
son, et al., 2002) found that the performance
of preschool-age children with PDD-NOS was There is one SPECT study that compares indi-
not different from that of children with autism viduals who have “ high-functioning autism”
in measures of joint attention, ventromedial (perhaps ASP and PDD-NOS) with normals
prefrontal cortex function, or dorsolateral (C. Gillberg, 1992). The results suggested that
frontal cortex tasks. The performance on joint the affected individuals displayed temporal
attention tasks was clearly different in the lobe hypoperfusion. Comparing epileptic and
group of children with PDD compared to men- nonepileptic ASD individuals revealed equiva-
tal age-matched control groups. However, the lent temporal lobe perfusion patterns, imply-
primary aim of the study was to investigate the ing that seizures were not likely to cause
relationship between joint attention skills and hypoperfusion. Similarly, a large cohort (N =
executive functions in the frontal lobes. It is 112) of children, adolescents, and adults with
172 Diagnosis and Classification

autism and age-matched controls were exam- TABLE 6.1 Conditions to Be Considered in the
ined with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Dif ferential Diagnosis of PDD-NOS
Hashimoto et al., 1995). Fifteen percent of the A. Conditions within PDD
ASD cohort was considered “ high-functioning” Asperger syndrome
(e.g., IQ > 80). Results suggested that individ- Autism
uals with ASD had diminished volumes in the Childhood disintegrative disorders
cerebellar vermian lobes VI and VII as well as B. Other Developmental Disorders
Developmental language disorder
hypoplasia of the brain stem. These findings
Mental retardation
are in agreement with other studies of individ- Semantic pragmatic disorder
uals with ASDs, which have previously re- C. Disorders That May Onset in Early Childhood
ported vermian hypoplasia, though none Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
heretofore have used controls or a cohort size Avoidant disorder
as convincing as that of the Hashimoto study Generalized anxiety disorder (childhood type)
(Courchesne, 1995). Obsessive compulsive disorder
Sparks and coworkers (2002) compared Overanxious disorder
Reactive attachment disorder
structural MRI among 3- to 4-year-old chil-
Schizophrenia (childhood onset type)
dren with autism ( both high and low function- Schizotypal disorder
ing), developmental delays, and healthy Social phobia
volunteers. Children with PDDs demonstrated
significantly increased total cerebral volume,
and increased ( bilateral) hippocampi and functioning, the broad range of symptoms, and
amygdala volumes compared to total cerebral history related to each domain of social reci-
volume. There were no differences in higher procity, language, and patterns of stereotyped
versus lower functioning children with PDDs, behavior or restricted interests (Filipek et al.,
but those with autism appeared to have even 2000).
larger amygdala volumes bilaterally, compared
to those with PDD-NOS (Sparks et al., 2002). PDD-NOS and Other PDD Entities

Summary of Biological Studies Although experienced clinicians can reliably


recognize a PDD from a disorder that is not a
Considered altogether, studies of patterns of PDD ( kappa of 0.67, 91% agreement), their
inheritance or neuroimaging offer no support ability to differentiate PDD-NOS, ASP, and
for differences among Autistic disorder, As- Autistic disorder is much weaker ( kappa =
perger’s disorder, and PDD-NOS. Future 0.51, 73% agreement; Mahoney et al., 1998).
nosological systems may base the divisions of In Mahoney and coworkers’ study, the largest
the PDD along dimensions related to cognitive disagreements came in discerning PDD-NOS
and language abilities or higher versus lower from the two other PDD diagnoses. This is par-
functioning (Prior et al., 1998; Szatmari et al., ticularly pertinent because PDD-NOS is de-
2002). fined in relationship to these other PDD
diagnoses: Whether one observes PDD-NOS
DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS depends on the definitions and boundaries of
the other PDDs. As definitions (and bound-
Table 6.1 offers diagnoses that may overlap or aries) change, one may legitimately perceive
be confused with PDD-NOS. Ordinarily, con- an individual as having one diagnosis or an-
fusion arises because a patient’s presentation other. This stretches one’s conviction in the
shares one or more features of impairment in meaning of diagnostic terms. Ordinarily,
socialization, language, and restricted pat- PDD-NOS implies that the clinical presenta-
terns of behavior. PDD-NOS may be over- tion is nearly like some other PDD subtype.
looked when excessive or premature emphasis Therefore, diagnosing PDD-NOS demands
is placed on a prominent feature that is associ- that the clinician have a good understanding of
ated with another diagnosis. A more compre- the boundaries and subtleties of each PDD.
hensive assessment should consider current After a discussion of the relationship between
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 173

PDD-NOS and the other specific diagnoses may possess, this differential diagnostic deci-
within PDD, this section takes up other devel- sion will be unreliable. There continues to be
opmental disorders outside PDD, and then disagreement over how much impairment in
other psychiatric disorders that may arise in each of these three domains is needed to
infancy and early childhood. make a reliable conclusion. The implications
As Mahoney and coworkers (1998) and of how much these differences in functioning
Tanguay and coworkers (1998) discovered, the contribute to the prognosis or treatment also
vagueness of PDD-NOS is most evident when have yet to be spelled out. As long as this un-
clinicians must divine the boundaries between certainty persists, cautious consideration of
it, other PDDs, and other syndromes. A source the differential diagnosis will be critical to
of uncertainty is that the three major develop- every evaluation.
mental domains that define impairment in
Asperger’s Disorder
autistic conditions—impairment in social re-
latedness, communication disorder and/or re- Prior to 1980 and the creation of DSM-IV and
striction in imaginative play, and repetitive ICD-10, there was no distinction between
patterns of behavior or restricted interests— PDD-NOS and ASP and thus no need for
are highly qualitative and largely independent practitioners (or researchers) to differentiate
of one another; each might be understood as a between them. Current criteria offer little di-
separate continuous variable (Tanguay et al., rection or guidance for separating PDD-NOS
1998). These complex domains have thwarted from ASP. It is not yet clear whether phenom-
attempts to find meaningful demarcations enological features represent valid differ-
within the spectrum of PDD conditions. Diag- ences between these entities. As yet, there is
nostic guidelines for any of the PDD diagnoses no evidence pointing to difference in etiology
do not offer threshold measures and do not de- or to specific features that influence progno-
clare how much impairment (or competence) is sis, treatment, or genetic recurrence risk. As
necessary in each domain to justify being in- reviewed previously, evidence suggests that
cluded in the diagnosis. Each domain lacks mild degrees of social dysfunction may be
precise definitions and introduces a measure more common in first-degree relatives of in-
of uncertainty into the deliberation. Thus, the dividuals with autism, ASP, or PDD-NOS
experience, training, and procedures used by a than in control populations (Piven et al.,
diagnostician influence whether an individual 1990, 1997; Szatmari et al., 2000; Walker
with an atypical presentation is diagnosed et al., 2004). Kurita (1997) suggests that dif-
with PDD-NOS, autism, or another PDD. ferences in imitation and auditory respon-
In addition, even when the domains can be siveness may be lower in those with ASP than
clearly defined, there is uncertainty over how in individuals with atypical autism akin to
much (or little) impairment is consistent with PDD-NOS. The current diagnostic guidelines
the diagnosis of PDD-NOS. For research pur- state that language development and motor co-
poses, most investigators now turn to the ordination in ASP may be distinctive (Klin &
ADI-R and ADOS-G and define their own al- Shepard, 1994). Individuals with PDD-NOS
gorithms to clarify the boundaries with PDD- may or may not show delays in language devel-
NOS and other PDDs (Tanguay et al., 1998). opment (Walker et al., 2004) and may or may
At the present time, there is no agreed-on cut not be clumsy. The diagnostic guidelines pro-
point for obtaining scores on the ADI-R and posed by Klin and coworkers (1995) are that
ADOS-G for PDD-NOS or ASP. Furthermore, individuals with ASP show normal grammati-
the extensive training necessary to attain reli- cal and syntactic language development, nor-
ability on these instruments has made them mal adaptive functioning aside from
impractical for day-to-day clinical use, de- socialization, all-absorbing special interests/
spite their recent ready availability. Autism skills, and awkwardness in motor tasks. How-
research has not yet produced practical, reli- ever, there is reason to question whether
able, quick instruments to make these distinc- motor clumsiness or all-absorbing interests/
tions. Without these instruments and reliable skills reliably discriminate between PDD-
measures of “ how much autism” an individual NOS and Asperger syndrome (Manjiviona &
174 Diagnosis and Classification

Prior, 1995). Furthermore, it may be that field trial results suggested that clinicians reli-
motor clumsiness is closely linked to early lan- ably could make distinctions between autism,
guage delay (Bishop, 2002). Further studies ASP, and PDD-NOS. However, for PDD-NOS
will be needed to clarify the relevance of more than the other PDD subtypes, there was
motor clumsiness. ICD-10 declares that the va- greater disagreement about how individuals
lidity of the ASP construct (as an entity differ- would be diagnosed under the different diag-
ent from high-functioning autism) remains in nostic systems. Within the group of individu-
doubt (ICD-10), and this is supported by some als with PDD-NOS diagnoses, 71 of 153 cases
investigation (Frith, 2004; Howlin, 2003; Mac- (46%) met criteria for autism in one of the
intosh & Dissanayake, 2004; Miller & Ozonoff, three diagnostic systems (DSM-III, DSM-III-
2000). There are disagreements about whether R, ICD-10) but were beneath criteria for
“language development ” can be termed “nor- autism in one or more of the other systems.
mal” in ASP when pragmatic functions, There was agreement in only 55 of 153 (36%)
prosody, or intonation are so disturbed (Frith, cases that individuals did not meet criteria for
2004; Howlin, 2003; Szatmari, 1997; Szat- autism according to any of the diagnostic
mari, Bryson, Boyle, Streiner, & Duku, 2003; schemes. Mahoney and coworkers’ investiga-
Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson, & Bartolucci, tions (1998) noted a similar pattern of diffi-
1990). A great deal must be learned about ASP culties. In those studies, 143 children with
before its unique features can be specified. autism, Asperger syndrome, atypical autism,
The Task Force on Nomenclature in DSM-IV- or developmental disorders that were not part
TR reflected the ambivalence over whether of PDD were evaluated using ADI-R, ADOS,
ASP and PDD-NOS are separate. On the one Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, and age-
hand, they created a new unique category for appropriate Weschler IQ measures. Comparing
ASP; but they supplied the same numerical consensus best estimate diagnoses, clinician
code (299.80) for it and for PDD-NOS. As a diagnoses, and independent blind expert rater
result, numerical coding searches relying on diagnoses, the investigators reported good
DSM-IV-TR will fail to distinguish individuals agreement for autism and Asperger syndrome,
with PDD-NOS and ASP. but not for PDD-NOS. For autism, the false
positive rate was acceptable at 0.20, 0.03 for
Autistic Disorder
Asperger syndrome, and 0.06 for PDD-NOS.
Determining whether an individual has autism However, the false negative rates told another
or PDD-NOS can baffle even experienced cli- story with autism at 0.05, Asperger syndrome
nicians. This is only partially a result of at- at 0.13, and PDD-NOS at 0.49, revealing “49%
tempting to subdivide a continuum into of the ‘true’ cases of atypical autism were mis-
discrete parts. The absence of measurable diagnosed as autism by the [expert] raters.”
standards and specific cut points defining lev- Uncertainty over the definition and quan-
els of impairment within domains is particu- tification of relatedness compound this diffi-
larly problematic when deciding between culty. Thus, clinicians frequently must face
autism and PDD-NOS. It is not as difficult to the question of how much relatedness is com-
make the diagnosis in mentally retarded indi- patible with autism or PDD-NOS. Experi-
viduals who are relatively skilled nonverbally enced clinicians know that children with
and strikingly aloof, but the confusion over autism may enjoy cuddling, seek comfort
which diagnosis is most appropriate can be when injured, or make eye contact, but may
enormous when evaluating high-functioning also exhibit other features of aberrant and de-
individuals. This was borne out in the DSM-IV layed social interaction. However, less knowl-
field trials (Volkmar et al., 1994). In that edgeable clinicians may hold to the idea that
study, 125 raters who had a range of experi- certain behaviors such as proximity seeking of
ence reviewed histories of 977 patients who caretakers under stress, ability to follow sim-
were thought to have autism or for whom ple commands, or eye contact precludes the di-
autism was a reasonable question in the differ- agnosis of autism. Just how much relatedness
ential diagnosis. Half the raters had consider- and reciprocity are necessary to place one into
able experience in the diagnosis of autism. The PDD-NOS or out of autism remain ambiguous.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 175

A common conundrum for clinicians arises (Volkmar et al., 1994). However, such history
when reassessing persons previously diag- should raise the possibility of CDD, and
nosed with autism, who now show some, among these, Landau-Kleffner syndrome, in-
though much less, impairment in one or two born errors of metabolism, glycogen storage
domains. When a child or adolescent displays disorders, and mitochondrial disorders should
mild, significant impairment in social skills be considered. With the characteristic motor
but previously exhibited all the profound de- impairments, head circumference decelera-
lays and deviance of an autistic disorder, is it tion, and loss of language skills, one must also
more appropriate to diagnose Autistic disorder consider Rett syndrome, particularly in fe-
( high-functioning) or PDD-NOS? There are no males. Landau-Kleffner syndrome (acquired
conventions for how one reconciles disparities aphasia; Beaumanior, 1992) periodically re-
in the presentation when comparing the past ceives particularly wide attention. Concomi-
and present. An outmoded view is that im- tant medical conditions should not preclude
provement itself argues against the diagnosis the diagnosis of autism or PDD-NOS; when
of autism. There is now good evidence that both are displayed, then both the medical and
autistic individuals usually increase their PDD-NOS diagnoses should be used. No phe-
skills over time, including social reciprocity nomenological guidelines specifically distin-
(Lord, 1995). Moreover, if the developmental guish between PDD-NOS and CDD or spell
history is unobtainable or unreliable, then it is out the conditions under which a decline in
likely that an individual would be diagnosed function warrants diagnosing one over the
PDD-NOS. However, diagnostic guidelines are other. In practice, patients with later onset
inconsistent. Generally, the DSM convention routinely receive diagnosis of PDD-NOS in
for other diagnoses (e.g., depression, anxiety preference to CDD, although in the DSM-IV
disorders) is that individuals who display per- field trials, a period of normal development up
sistent impairment, albeit less severe than at to age 2 years, current mutism, and placement
the time of the original diagnosis, are consid- in residential facilities were salient features
ered to have partial improvement rather than a that led clinicians to diagnose CDD (Volkmar
new, less specific, diagnosis (e.g., Anxiety & Rutter, 1995; Volkmar et al., 1994). Re-
Disorder NOS). cently, this group of conditions has received
attention fueled by unsupported theories pos-
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder
tulating vaccine-related increases in PDD
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder (CDD) is a (Fombonne & Chakrabarti, 2001).
rare condition (Malhotra & Gupta, 2002;
Volkmar et al., 1994; Volkmar & Rutter, PDD-NOS and Other Developmental
1995). Typically, as in Heller’s original de- Disorders
scriptions (Heller, 1908), patients with CDD
have a 3-to-4-year history of early unequivo- The descriptions of Developmental Language
cally normal development followed by intellec- Disorders, particularly Semantic-Pragmatic
tual (particularly language skill) decline Disorder (Adams & Bishop, 1989; Bishop &
leading to profound impairment, including so- Adams, 1989) or Pragmatic Language Impair-
cial impairment and unrelatedness. Such a his- ment (PLI; Bishop & Norbury, 2002) accu-
tory is distinctive in comparison with rately portray language impairments seen in
PDD-NOS and is not likely to lead to confu- some individuals with PDD, particularly PDD-
sion. However, a clear-cut history such as this NOS. As defined by Rapin and Allen (1983),
is rare; a decline in function between 18 and Semantic-Pragmatic Disorder is a condition in
24 months is offered for many autistic and which complex language is intact, but use,
PDD-NOS patients who also display a plateau content, and understanding of language are
or mild decline in social development (Volk- impaired. There is active debate over whether
mar et al., 1985). Consequently, a history of Semantic-Pragmatic Disorder should remain
onset after 30 months, language delay, and as a description, separate from PDDs (Bishop,
milder declines in function lead most clini- 1989, 2000), or is synonymous with PDD-NOS
cians to use PDD-NOS rather than CDD (Brook & Bowler, 1992). It is likely that there
176 Diagnosis and Classification

are other conditions described by neuropsy- One source of confusion stems from the
chology or speech and language pathology, separate, parallel, autonomous diagnostic sys-
such as auditory processing disorders, that tems that are used in communication pathol-
also overlap with PDD. ogy and pediatric psychiatry. Communication
Taking a methodical and judicious ap- pathologists do not use psychiatric diagnoses;
proach, Bishop has argued that it is premature they provide accurate descriptions of language
to consider all PLI to be equivalent to PDD- skills. As a result, they may not consider the
NOS although she is unequivocal that individ- broader context of impairments related to so-
uals with PDD-NOS commonly exhibit this cial skills and stereotyped movements that
type of language disorder. One problem is that might shed light on an autistic spectrum con-
PLI is not unique to PDDs and may be seen in dition. Thus, the accurate description by com-
the context of specific language impairment munication pathologists may not correspond to
(SLI ). To explore this, Bishop and Norbury medical nosology. Conversely, psychiatric
(2002) studied children with high-functioning nosology has collapsed the wide, and richly
autism, PLI, typical SLI (i.e., not mixed SLI & varied range of expressive, receptive, and
PLI ), and healthy volunteers, using measures pragmatic language into three mutually exclu-
of basic language, pragmatic language, the sive categories that may not have empirical va-
Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI-R), and lidity. If communication pathologists held
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule themselves to the current system of medical
(ADOS-G). Acknowledging that the metric for nosology, they would be forced to dilute their
PDD-NOS is not codified for the ADI-R, observations and conclusions at the expense of
Bishop found that only 3 of 18 children with considerable breadth and precision. The char-
PLI met criteria for autism on these instru- acterization of communication disorders in
ments. Only 1 child among 18 in the PLI group current psychiatric nosology does not reflect
met criteria for PDD-NOS with a modified the precision of modern speech pathology and,
ADI-R (checklist) and the ADOS-G. An im- understandably, most speech and language ex-
portant question that emerges is whether the perts shun it.
diagnosis based on only current functioning In addition, the diagnostic objectives of the
(as opposed to history) is accurate; if the ADI two fields are dissimilar. The conclusions of-
checklist alone had been used, then 4 of 18 fered by communication pathology focus pre-
would have met criteria for PDD-NOS and 5 of dominantly on elements of language and seek to
18 met criteria for autism (a total of 9/18 provide a clear description of their “piece” of
[50%] having been diagnosed with a PDD). the disorder; whereas the aim for the psychia-
Based on the modified ADI checklist, 6 of 18 trist is to furnish a comprehensive, parsimo-
(33%) of the PLI and 2 of 11 (18%) SLI-T nious diagnostic formulation for the entire
children had PDD features at 4 to 5 years of panoply of symptoms. A communication pathol-
age that no longer were apparent at the time of ogist may recognize syntactic, prosodic, and
the study. Nevertheless, it is also apparent that linguistic impairments but view information re-
50% of PLI children did not exceed criteria for garding circumscribed interests or restricted
PDD currently or in the past based on parent patterns of behavior to be outside his or her
report. Bishop and Norbury underscore that scope. Should this occur, the PDD-NOS patient
the PDD features PLI children exhibited were will be viewed as suffering from a language dis-
almost exclusively related to language criteria order—a description that is accurate as far as it
(such as failing to ask for information, using goes though it may be insufficient as a complete
stereotyped or idiosyncratic words). They did diagnostic formulation.
not display impairment in use of imagination,
and less than 25% had excessive interests or Mental Retardation
ritualized behaviors by history (Bishop & Nor-
bury, 2002). Thus, all PLI may not be a part of Young children who exhibit mental retarda-
the autism spectrum, and until this has been tion, language delay or impairment, and delays
clarified through further study, continued use in social reciprocity occupy a position at an-
of this descriptive, though not diagnostic, term other boundary with PDD-NOS. There is gen-
is warranted. eral agreement that language delays, deficient
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 177

social skills, and stereotyped mannerisms are in misreading affects and impulsive behavior
very common among retarded persons (Breg- can discourage social relationships (Greene
man, 1991; Fraser & Rao, 1991) and abilities et al., 2001; Jensen, Larrieu, & Mack, 1997;
decline as IQ level descends. When social de- Matthys, Cuperus, & van Engeland, 1999;
lays are severe, then autism becomes a serious Pfiffner, Calzada, & McBurnett, 2000). In an
consideration; but milder impairments in so- investigation designed to study this question,
cial reciprocity, language impairments, or the Luteijn and coworkers (2000) reported that
absence of imaginative play create a dilemma. checklist scores for attentional problems in
When are these delays or deficits consonant ADHD children were not different from those
with mental retardation and when is an addi- for children with PDD-NOS and there were
tional diagnosis of PDD-NOS appropriate? large overlaps in scores on social problems.
The impairments must be considered in the Jensen and coworkers also saw this in a study
context of the child’s general level of retarda- of 44 children. Although PDD-NOS children
tion. Even when standardized screening de- exhibited more withdrawal behavior, those
vices such as the Vineland Adaptive Behavior who were diagnosed with “PDD-NOS plus
Scales are employed (Volkmar et al., 1993), ADHD” did not show withdrawal. Moreover, a
they provide little help in determining whether highly distractible child can seem as if he is
delays are commensurate with the level of re- “in his own world” and unrelated (Roeyers,
tardation or represent manifestations and a de- Keymeulen, & Buysse, 1998). Consequently, it
velopmental disorder. Standardized measures is not unusual for children with higher func-
alone cannot answer questions of how much tioning PDD to be diagnosed with only ADHD
lower social performance must be, compared (Perry, 1998; Roeyers et al., 1998). This is par-
with communication or daily living skills, for ticularly the case when a careful developmen-
PDD-NOS to be a legitimate consideration. tal history has not been obtained (Perry, 1998;
There is considerable uncertainty over how Roeyers et al., 1998). Sometimes these children
great a discrepancy must exist in specific do- are seen as “ the worst case ever ” of ADHD or
mains of socialization, language, and stereo- “ treatment refractory ADHD.” Although there
typed interests compared with the general may be similar impairments in disruptive be-
level of retardation among retarded individu- haviors, the children with PDD-NOS appear to
als with PDD-NOS. This uncertainty surfaces have significantly more difficulties in social
in studies of prevalence rates of PDD among understanding and in emotional and behavioral
retarded persons. Some researchers have re- problems related to excessive affective reac-
ported that as many as 30% of mentally re- tions (Jensen et al., 1997).
tarded individuals have a PDD (Gillberg et al., Distinguishing between PDDs and ADHD
1986). This is to be contrasted with the figure can be difficult in individuals who have con-
of 5 to 10 per 10,000 reported among the gen- current moderate-to-severe mental retardation
eral population. It is evident that considerable (MR), developmental language disorder, or
work is necessary to understand the distribu- severe hyperactivity (as in ICD-10 hyperkine-
tion of these features among the general popu- sis). Impairment and delays in social relation-
lation of retarded persons. We still do not ships are common in individuals with ADHD
know how much impairment a retarded or non- plus MR or ADHD plus developmental
retarded patient must exhibit to be appropri- language disorders (Bagwell et al., 2001; No-
ately considered as having PDD-NOS. terdaeme, Amorosa, Mildenberger, Sitter, &
Minow, 2001). Moreover, there is a relatively
PDD-NOS and Conditions with Onset in high frequency of symptoms of impulsiveness
Childhood or inattention in higher functioning individuals
with PDD (Luteijn et al., 2000) and those with
It may not be intuitive that Attention Deficit PDD-NOS (Roeyers et al., 1998). Under DSM-
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) could be con- III-R and DSM-IV, hierarchical rules prohibit
fused with PDD-NOS. Unlike the child with the diagnosis of both conditions, since autism
PDD-NOS, the typical child with ADHD does or PDD-NOS is understood to be primary and
not display extensive problems with relating to symptoms of inattention or distractibility are
others although problems with milder deficits viewed in the context of this larger syndrome.
178 Diagnosis and Classification

In the absence of a detailed developmental his- functioning, or restricted interests that are dis-
tory or when children are very young, ADHD abling for individuals with PDDs. Hierarchical
can be a common preliminary diagnosis or pre- rules in DSM-IV underscore the primacy of
senting complaint. PDD symptoms in persons with anxiety by pro-
hibiting the diagnosis of GAD in persons with
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder PDD conditions. Nevertheless, GAD occasion-
ally surfaces as the presenting diagnosis in
There has always been some ambiguity about higher functioning children with lifelong social
how one separates obsessions and compulsions delays, restricted patterns of behavior, and nar-
from stereotyped movements and restricted row interests. Important features that can dis-
interests. There are suggestions that some con- tinguish GAD are well-developed abilities to
stellations of symptoms are more likely in understand the wishes, desires, and interests
those with PDDs compared with OCD pa- of others and to read nonverbal social interac-
tients, but there are no pathognomonic symp- tions. Often, major problems in persons with
toms in either group (McDougle et al., 1995). GAD stem from an excessive focus on these
When developmentally delayed or retarded elements. Children with GAD have social in-
persons display habits and repetitive behav- teractions that may be excessively personal-
iors, the dividing line can become very vague. ized or unveil self-disparaging worries over
This becomes complicated when trying to un- past or future interactions. However, as a rule,
derstand these behaviors in individuals who they are able to engage in reciprocal conversa-
possess some language but whose developmen- tion, share interests and emotions, and to ac-
tal ages are so low that one would not expect a curately recognize another’s pleasure. They
capacity for self-reflection or an ability to for- tend to see annoyance in others, even when it
mulate concepts of anxiety, senselessness, or is not present, in contrast to those with PDDs,
resistance to performing the acts. Thus, some who are likely to fail to recognize it.
clinicians will diagnose comorbid OCD and
Personality Disorders
mental retardation without eliciting informa-
tion about early developmental features of so- Several personality disorder diagnoses may be
cial relating, language, other unusual interests, ascribed to persons with developmental histo-
and patterns of behavior. It can compound the ries and childhood onset features of PDD-
uncertainty when reverse logic is used, such as NOS. Older children, adolescents, or young
citing a favorable response to serotonin reup- adults with PDD-NOS who exhibit avoidance
take inhibitors (SRIs) as justification of the of social contact but possess more developed
diagnosis of OCD. Certainly, it is possible for language skills may be viewed as having either
persons with developmental disabilities to de- Schizoid or Avoidant Personality Disorder.
velop habit disorders or OCD. However, a Although some argue that the desire for social
wider scope of inquiry that includes a detailed relationships precludes PDD, many higher
behavioral and developmental history (includ- functioning ASP and PDD-NOS individuals
ing language development) is indicated. wish for social contact and are aware that they
are different from others (Wing, 1992). The
Generalized Anxiety Disorder awareness of this difference can be a source of
distress to individuals with PDD-NOS (Wing,
Excessive concern about past events, needs for 1992). Their inability to enter social situa-
constant reassurance, feelings of tension or in- tions successfully can make them appear shy
ability to relax, and self-consciousness may be and in need of considerable reassurance be-
viewed as Generalized Anxiety Disorder fore venturing into novel social situations,
(GAD) in children with PDD-NOS. General- thereby suggesting Avoidant Personality Dis-
ized anxiety can also produce constant worry, order. Somewhat lower functioning individu-
sleep disturbance, and excessive fatigue. als with PDD-NOS may harbor more wariness
Higher functioning individuals with PDD-NOS and prefer to avoid social interaction alto-
may exhibit these symptoms, but individuals gether. This more aloof group resembles the
with GAD do not exhibit impairments in social schizoid children described by Wolff and
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 179

Chick (1980). When a child displays language ress in social reciprocity following profound
disorder, poor pragmatic behaviors (such as delays in early childhood. This person’s devel-
unrelated or circumstantial utterances) he opmental achievements introduce a diagnostic
may be erroneously considered to have problem resembling the one for autistic per-
Schizotypal Personality Disorder, or Schizo- sons described earlier. This is a predicament
phrenia. This is particularly likely when cir- for the clinician who must decide whether this
cumscribed interests and preoccupations are person suffers from one or more of these per-
prominent (Rumsey, Andreasen, & Rapoport, sonality disorders or PDD-NOS. Many clini-
1986). Some consider that ASP should be a cians will favor a personality diagnosis in the
subtype of Schzoid Personality Disorder face of such a history. If a history of language
(Tantam, 1988b). When a reliable develop- development, restricted patterns of behavior,
mental history cannot be obtained, clinicians and narrow interests is not pursued, the assess-
might presume that an adolescent or young ment may yield a personality disorder diagno-
adult has a personality disorder. sis. However, when there is still impairment
Experience in diagnosing autistic spectrum resulting from limited social reciprocity, it is
disorders is important in learning to differenti- inappropriate to reject the diagnosis of PDD-
ate between developmental disorders and per- NOS or autism based on gains in language, or
sonality disorders (Volkmar et al., 1994). social interest.
Features of very early onset with enduring Discarding the diagnosis of PDD-NOS be-
impairment in imaginary play, socialization, cause of improvements and replacing it with a
and restricted patterns of behavior throughout personality diagnosis promotes two illogical
childhood would point toward diagnosis of a concepts. The first proposes that the patient
developmental disorder. There are too few in- recovered from PDD-NOS and went on to de-
vestigations of the developmental history of velop a new, separate disorder affecting per-
adults with unequivocal Avoidant, Schizoid, sonality. This is illogical insofar as the same
or Schizotypal Personality Disorder to com- symptoms, present continuously, are viewed as
pare developmental histories with persons resulting from one disorder in childhood and a
with PDD-NOS. Consequently, it is unknown different disorder later on. The natural history
whether characteristic features of PDD are of PDD-NOS shows that individuals improve.
displayed through childhood and adolescence It is contradictory to suggest, therefore, that
in persons with Avoidant, Schizoid, or Schizo- when a person with PDD-NOS improves, a dif-
typal Personality Disorder. Tantam (1988b) ferent diagnosis should be applied.
suggests that differences do exist, insofar as An alternative formulation is that the indi-
elevated scores on measures of abnormal non- vidual has displayed a personality disorder
verbal expression were correlated with early throughout his or her lifetime with features of
developmental disturbances, whereas schizoid language delay, social impairment, and re-
features were not. Tantam (1988b) report that stricted interests or patterns of behavior. If
features of developmental delays and abnor- this were true, this hypothetical personality
mal nonverbal expression clustered together, disorder would be hierarchically related to
but schizotypal features did not correlate with PDD, continuous with PDD, or a muted form
developmental abnormalities. It is also unclear of PDD. Empirical, longitudinal studies of
whether there are other typical features in such “personality disorders” do not confirm
the childhood course or natural history of this formulation (Kolvin, 1971). Conceptu-
Schizoid, Schizotypal, or Avoidant Personal- ally, this view takes a regressive step by re-
ity Disorder that would permit differentiation verting to the pre-Kolvin era in which the
of these conditions from PDD-NOS. DSM-IV disorders in the autism spectrum, the schizo-
itself points to the complexity of differentiat- phrenia spectrum, and conditions exhibiting
ing high-functioning autism and ASP from social isolation, were all presumed to be re-
Schizoid Personality Disorder (DSM-IV). lated and nosologically warranted being
Consider a hypothetical individual who lumped into a single entity. Wolff and Chick
presents with mild-to-moderate impairment in (1980) have favored a view that autism and
socialization and a history of significant prog- schizophrenia are on a continuum, although
180 Diagnosis and Classification

this dismisses findings suggesting the two are child may be more likely to receive abuse.
distinct (Kolvin, 1971; Volkmar & Cohen, Children with impairments in language and re-
1991; Werry, 1992). latedness who display inflexible patterns and
routines may be more likely to experience mis-
Reactive Attachment Disorder
treatment or neglect by angry parents who do
According to DSM-IV, a child who has a veri- not understand why their child is so exhausting
fied history of psychosocial adversity, termed and unrewarding. An aloof child who does not
“pathogenic care” during the first years of life sleep or eat well, refuses to comply with rules
and displays disinhibited or inhibited re- and limits, and does not become calm when
sponses in social situations, may have Reactive held or reciprocate a smile or greeting may
Attachment Disorder (RAD). Closer scrutiny further inflame abusive or neglectful han-
reveals that many children who undergo pro- dling. An uninformed parent may believe the
found neglect or abuse subsequently show sub- child is oppositional or willfully disobedient.
stantial physical and emotional improvement Thus, a child’s developmental delays could ag-
when they receive compassionate, nurturing gravate and incite as readily as emanate from
care (O’Connor, Rutter, Beckett, Keaveney, & profound mistreatment. Third, RAD may re-
Kreppner, 2000). Others show a protracted or quire reconsideration in light of a broader con-
intractable course, “quasi-autistic features,” cept of PDD. The biological determinants of
and present a clinical conundrum (Rutter, protracted RAD are poorly understood; the
Kreppner, & O’Connor, 2001). Reliable data acute and chronic course of RAD could be ex-
suggest that overactivity, inattention, stereo- plained on the basis of constitutional vulnera-
typed behaviors, and eating problems may per- bility to impairment in social relatedness. It
sist in a small minority of children and are seems possible that the children who display a
highly correlated with the duration of priva- protracted course have a larger dose of depri-
tion (Beckett et al., 2002; Roy, Rutter, & Pick- vation and this may further stress biological
les, 2000). Quasi-autistic features, such as susceptibilities. In this model, abuse and
social aloofness or social impairment symp- neglect become impediments leading to sub-
toms of PDD that arise in a small minority of optimal adjustment but are not the exclusive
children, are not likely to persist (Rutter et al., cause of it.
1999). Thus, at times, the social dysfunction of
Schizophrenia
RAD may resemble PDD-NOS and it may be
difficult to decide which is the more suitable The hierarchical rules for handling co-
diagnosis. As suggested by Richters and Volk- occurring PDD and schizophrenia are contra-
mar (1994), there are several reasons to reflect dictory in DSM-IV and do not stem from em-
carefully on the relationship between PDD- pirical data. When premorbid autism or
NOS and RAD. The first reason is clinical. another PDD exists, the DSM requires the
Children who receive profoundly detrimental presence of prominent hallucinations and delu-
parenting in early life are often just the ones sions for at least a month. If these criteria are
for whom it is most difficult to obtain a reli- met in the context of autism, then both diag-
able developmental history or to determine noses are warranted. However, if another PDD
whether there has been pathogenic care. In the is present (ASP or PDD-NOS), then the diag-
absence of an accurate, reliable developmental nosis of schizophrenia supplants the prior PDD
history, clinicians may prematurely or hastily diagnosis and the only diagnosis is schizophre-
decide that the symptom picture is exclusively nia. The DSM offers no rationale for this deci-
the result of adversity during the child’s early sion. Generally, the guidelines for making
years. The consequence of a hasty decision is diagnoses suggest that a more specific diagno-
that the educational plan for such a child and sis is preferred to one that is “not otherwise
the expectations of care providers are very dif- specified.” The problem in schizophrenia is
ferent when RAD, as opposed to PDD-NOS, is the implication that once hallucinations and
the working diagnoses. Second, although most delusions set in, individuals with early-onset
abused and neglected children are not develop- deficits in social reciprocity are no different
mentally delayed, a developmentally delayed from those without such a history. There is no
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 181

empirical evidence to support eliminating this aggression, and reactivity to disruption in rou-
distinction and some evidence to suggest that tines. It is particularly more likely to be diag-
individuals with insidious onset and early nosed in those with chronic agitation,
premorbid social and language deficits ex- distractibility, and/or sleep difficulties. It
hibit a poorer prognosis and different neu- should be underscored that decreased need for
ropathological features (Cannon et al., 2002; sleep is not the same as the difficulties initiat-
Silverstein, Mavrolefteros, & Close, 2002). It ing sleep that are often seen in persons with
seems more congruous to follow the practice PDDs. As with other disorders, there is a
of applying both diagnoses (e.g., schizophre- growing awareness that the concept of bipolar
nia plus PDD-NOS) instead of discarding disorder may be applied more narrowly by
nonautistic PDD (Volkmar & Cohen, 1991). some investigators than others. Leibenluft and
This is not a trivial consideration. Watkins coworkers (Leibenluft, Charney, Towbin,
and coworkers (Watkins, Asarnow, & Tan- Bhangoo, & Pine, 2003) have suggested ways
guay, 1988) reviewed histories of 18 children of understanding these different views. If
diagnosed with schizophrenia. They reported bipolar disorders are to be narrowly defined,
that 7 subjects (39%) had symptoms that then episodicity must be a cardinal feature
would have been sufficient to meet criteria for (Geller et al., 2002; Leibenluft et al., 2003). In
autism before 30 months. This is somewhat applying the narrow phenotype diagnoses to
greater than the 17% rate (13% autism plus those with PDDs, one must then consider
4% with impairment in two domains) reported whether chronic symptoms such as agitation,
by Alaghband-Rad and coworkers (1995) at irritability, and distractibility are all signifi-
the National Institute of Mental Health cantly increased during a period of elevated
(NIMH) in their cohort of schizophrenic chil- mood and decreased need for sleep. Other-
dren. The NIMH group did not report which wise, there is a high risk that overlapping,
domains within the Autism Diagnostic Inter- chronic symptoms will be used redundantly to
view (ADI ) were impaired. If all subjects with make multiple diagnoses (e.g., ADHD, PDD,
impairment in social reciprocity showed this bipolar disorder; Geller et al., 2002). This
before 36 months and exhibited this premorbid- leaves open the question of how the broader
to-prominent hallucinations/delusions, the rate phenotype condition may be related to narrow
for premorbid PDD could be as high as 63% phenotype bipolar disorder.
(McKenna et al., 1994). A third of the entire
schizophrenic cohort also exhibited develop- Social Phobias
mental language delays (Alaghband-Rad et al.,
1995). In an early paper, the NIMH group Social phobias are the least common anxiety
(Gordon et al., 1994) emphasized the complex- disorders of childhood onset and a very un-
ity of differentiating a PDD from childhood- common early childhood-onset disorder (Bei-
onset schizophrenia and the need for rigorous del, 1991). Patients typically begin having
methodology in discovering features of a PDD symptoms in their teens, although childhood-
in individuals with schizophrenia, although onset cases certainly occur. Of the group with
they did not report on ADI domains. This was generalized social phobias, at least 50% will
even more relevant to their later descriptive experience onset prior to age 12 years (Man-
data on children with “multiple developmental nuzza et al., 1995; Velting & Albano, 2001;
impairment,” a type of Psychosis Not Other- Wittchen & Fehm, 2001). As with several of
wise Specified (McKenna et al., 1994). In that the other conditions previously discussed, a
subgroup, the rate of PDD symptoms at the clinician who narrows the range of possibilities
time of presentation was 39%. This is particu- too quickly risks making a diagnostic error. It
larly important when the diagnosis of schizo- is critical to recognize the difference between
phrenia relies on negative symptoms, and the social aloofness of higher functioning
hallucinations and delusions are only weakly autistic individuals and the extreme shyness or
evident (Minshew, 2001). avoidance accompanied by anxiety that is char-
Bipolar disorder is often diagnosed in acteristic of social phobias or avoidant disor-
persons with PDDs who have irritability, ders. The obstruction of PDD-NOS or social
182 Diagnosis and Classification

phobia may lead a child to display anxiety and capacity to form reciprocal relationships, and
to attempt refraining from social interactions. impoverished affective regulation suggest that
However, the concept of social phobia has tra- MCDD might be appropriately placed in the
ditionally been reserved for individuals who category of PDD. These children display ex-
have preservation of development in other do- treme impairments in the capacity to interact
mains of their lives and have socially appropri- successfully with peers and adults, capacity
ate relationships within a small circle of for empathy, and in their ability to tolerate
family and friends (Beidel, 1991). The typical negative affects such as frustration or anxiety.
individual with social phobia enjoys the com- When distressed, they become highly disorga-
pany of others and will experience loneliness. nized in their behavior and thinking (Dahl,
It is less clear whether the loneliness of high- Cohen, & Provence, 1986). MCDD has spe-
functioning autistic PDD-NOS persons has cific criteria and can be reliably diagnosed and
these same qualities although they certainly differentiated from other childhood-onset dis-
may wish to participate and fit in with their orders of behavior or affect (Buitelaar et al.,
peer group. Individuals with social phobias 1998; Towbin et al., 1993). Impairments in so-
ought to display a strong capacity for under- cial relating are reflected in the diminished
standing complex emotional situations and to amounts and a primitive quality of peer rela-
exhibit spontaneous empathy in social situa- tionships as well as fundamental impairments
tions. A history of normal language, imagina- in the child’s manner of relating to primary
tive play, flexibility in facing novel situations, caretakers. Children with MCDD syndrome ex-
and ordinary social development are crucial. hibit consistent features of avoidance, detach-
Comorbid depressive, anxiety, or substance ment, high degrees of ambivalence, clinging, or
abuse disorders were common in adult subjects intense irritability. Deficits in affective regu-
with social phobia (Mannuzza et al., 1995; lation result in peculiar fears, chronic anxiety,
Velting & Albano, 2001; Wittchen & Fehm, frequent incidents of intense anger, and ex-
2001), but they did not have the pervasive im- treme behavioral reactions. These demonstrate
pairment in relationships and restricted inter- limitations in modulation of internal affective
ests characteristic of high-functioning autistic states and affective expression. In addition, re-
or Aspergerian individuals. Complicating this current episodes of disorganization in thinking
picture is the elevated rate of social phobia or perceptual distortions are characteristic
among relatives of probands with autism (Cohen et al., 1986; Towbin et al., 1993). This
(Piven & Palmer, 1997, 1999; Smalley, Mc- is ordinarily episodic, but thought disorganiza-
Cracken, & Tanguay, 1995) suggesting that so- tion may be more consistent. Problems in
cial phobia could reside within the broader thinking reach neither the proportions nor sus-
phenotype of the autism spectrum. tained intensity that are sufficient for chronic
delusional or psychotic conditions. Changes in
Two Potential Subgroups within PDD- routine or structure often precipitate disorga-
NOS: Multiplex Developmental Disorder nization. This vulnerability is reminiscent of
and Disorders of Attention, Motor the patterned behavior and inflexibility typi-
Control, and Perception cal of PDDs. As the condition is characterized
and its boundaries are explored, it is appropri-
Neither Multiple Complex (or Multiplex) De- ate to place it under the wider umbrella of
velopmental Disorder (MCDD) nor Disorders PDD-NOS (Cohen et al., 1986).
of Attention, Motor Control, and Perception MCDD emerged from observations of a
(DAMP) is a term in the DSM-IV or ICD-10. large number of preschool-age children who
However, they may be closely related to or be presented for developmental evaluations (Dahl
viewed as part of a PDD spectrum. et al., 1986). Rescorla (1986) conducted stan-
MCDD is an early-onset syndrome in which dardized evaluations that suggested these chil-
there are basic deficits in affective modula- dren were an “intermediate group” between
tion, capacity for relating, and thinking. The children with autism and so-called reactive
characteristics of onset in infancy or very children who appeared anxious and depressed.
early childhood, sustained limitations in the The MCDD group showed high scores on a
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 183

factor of “ bizarre” features (confusion, having healthy volunteers on a visual oddball task.
strange ideas, exhibiting strange behaviors, Following in this same effort to discover bio-
and being lost in thoughts). This bizarre factor logical endophenotypes, Jansen and coworkers
was more common among children in an (2000; Jansen, Gispen-de Wied, van der Gaag,
MCDD group and a group of children with & van Engeland, 2003) examined cortisol re-
high-functioning autism (HFA) or Asperger sponses to psychological and physical stress in
syndrome than a comparison reactive group 10 children with MCDD, HFA, and healthy
(Rescorla, 1986). Unlike those with HFA, the volunteers. Results suggested that individuals
MCDD individuals scored highly on a factor with HFA had an increased cortisol response
measuring anxiety and depression that in- to psychological stress, whereas MCDD chil-
cluded items like worrying, being tense and dren had a decreased response compared to
anxious, and demanding attention (Rescorla, healthy volunteers. There were no differences
1986; van der Gaag et al., 1995) similar to between groups in response to physical stress.
comparison “ highly reactive” children. It is particularly interesting that the pattern of
The impairments of children with MCDD reduced cortisol response is frequently ob-
have suggested to some investigators that served in adults with schizophrenia (Jansen
MCDD is an early appearing form of bipolar et al., 1998, 2000).
illness, Borderline Personality Disorder, The neurodevelopmental perspective of
Schizotypal Personality Disorder, or Schizo- schizophrenia has promoted greater interest in
phrenia. Although the affective dysregulation MCDD (Bloom, 1993; Fish, Marcus, Hans,
and impairment in thinking and interpersonal Auerbach, & Perdue, 1992). This concept pro-
relationships are reminiscent of these condi- poses that early-onset neural defects associ-
tions, it is not evident that MCDD is continuous ated with stable cognitive impairments set the
with these Axis I or II conditions or that link- stage for an increased risk of schizophrenia.
ing MCDD to them is appropriate. Until we What is not clear is whether these developmen-
have stronger biological markers or endopheno- tal adversities routinely generate schizophre-
types (Crosbie & Schachar, 2001; Milberger nia, or whether they are nonspecific risk
et al., 1996), it is premature to make these asso- factors that profoundly and adversely affect
ciations. Reading descriptions of these condi- adjustment in adulthood. Sparrow and cowork-
tions, one is struck by how much their clinical ers (1986) reported that measures of social im-
presentations resemble one another (Caplan, maturity and dysfunction had stability and
1994; Tantam, 1988a; Watkins et al., 1988). In- persistence, but that deterioration in function
vestigators have commented on the persistent overall was not seen at 7-year follow-up. Simi-
confusion between so-called childhood-onset larly, Kestenbaum (1983) reported a variety of
schizophrenia and high-functioning autism or outcomes, with psychosis only rarely ensuing
schizophrenia (Szatmari et al., 1990; Tantam, in a similar albeit small cohort. Lofgren and
1988b; Watkins et al., 1988). coworkers (Lofgren, Bemporad, King, Lin-
There is some support from biological stud- dem, & O’Driscoll, 1991) also found no spe-
ies for differentiating MCDD from other cific outcome in children identified as having a
PDDs. Lincoln and coworkers evaluated 30 borderline syndrome. In support of this view,
children with MCDD, ADHD, and healthy vol- Nicholson and coworkers (2001) reported that
unteers on measures of behavior, psychophysi- children ascertained for schizophrenia but
ological tasks, and event-related potentials showing a variation of MCDD called Multiple
(Lincoln, Bloom, Katz, & Boksenbaum, 1998). Developmental Impairment (MDI ), went on to
Results suggested that the 11 MCDD children develop affective disorders or had a continua-
displayed a distinct profile of auditory pro- tion of their presenting symptoms (Nicholson
cessing impairment. In a similar vein, Kemner et al., 2001). Despite being ascertained for
and coworkers (Kemner, van der Gaag, Ver- schizophrenia, none of the cohort went on to
baten, & van Engeland, 1999) reported that vi- develop schizophrenia. Diagnostically, at 2- to
sual event-related potentials to P3 at multiple 8-year follow-up, 12% had developed Schizo-
leads, and Nc in frontal regions differentiated affective Disorder; 15%, Bipolar Disorder; 24%,
MCDD children from those with autism and major depression; and 50% were clinically
184 Diagnosis and Classification

unchanged. Children with MCDD may resem- with a PDD who received the diagnosis of AD
ble those described with schizoid conditions (Hertzig, Snow, New, & Shapiro, 1990; Spitzer
(Wolff & Chick, 1980) or Asperger syndrome & Siegel, 1990). Also, changes in DSM-III-R
(Tantam, 1988b). A syndrome of poor interper- criteria classified individuals as having a PDD
sonal relatedness, odd language, and idiosyn- who would not have met DSM-III criteria
cratic thinking may be a stable, persistent trait. (Spitzer & Siegel, 1990).
DAMP, a disorder first described by Gill- Aside from definitions, prevalence rates
berg, is a comorbid condition of ADHD and are heavily influenced by the investigation’s
Developmental Coordination Disorder (Gill- methodology. Different methods have pro-
berg, 2003). Gillberg has suggested a close duced widely disparate prevalence rates of
link between DAMP and PDD spectrum disor- autism. The discrepancies in results can be ex-
ders, based on overlapping symptoms. Two- plained on the basis of methods of ascertain-
thirds of patients with severe DAMP will ment, diagnostic criteria, and diagnostic
resemble those with ASP or PDD-NOS (Gill- procedures. Investigators who combine infan-
berg, 2003). In a population study of Swedish tile autism and other, more broadly defined
7-year-olds, those with coordination deficits PDD subtypes in their cohort reported esti-
were more likely to display symptoms of social mates of AD 1.9 to 4.9 per 10,000 (Bryson,
impairment; and those with DAMP were even Clark, & Smith, 1988; Ritvo, Freeman, & Pin-
more likely to have these symptoms (Kadesjo gree, 1989; Steinhausen, Gobel, Berinlinger,
& Gillberg, 1999). Children with DAMP were & Wohlleben, 1986; Wing & Gould, 1979).
also more likely to display problems in speech The studies that limited their ascertainment to
production, phonological processing, and read- narrow or strict definitions of infantile (or
ing than children with only ADHD symptoms Kanner’s) autism (“nuclear autism”) gave a
(Kadesjo & Gillberg, 1999). fairly consistent estimates of 2.0 to 2.2 per
10,000 (C. Gillberg & Coleman, 1992; Stef-
EPIDEMIOLOGY fenberg & Gillberg, 1986; Steinhausen et al.,
1986). Gillberg went on to suggest that rising
The prevalence rates reported for PDD-NOS prevalence rates of AD over the past 10 years
have been heavily influenced by changes in di- were a consequence of changes in the diagnosis
agnostic criteria, in the same fashion as the and broadening of definitions. Ritvo and
rates for all the PDDs. Definitions of Autistic coworkers suggested that widely disseminat-
disorder are particularly relevant; a broader or ing information led to better recognition of
narrower definition will influence the size of autism. The Canadian study (Bryson et al.,
the population that is ascertained, and may in- 1988), the first to employ a broad definition
versely influence the size of the nonautistic and research criteria, reported a prevalence
PDD. A methodologically weak study sug- rate of 10 per 10,000 and heralded the current
gested that definitions may also influence period of higher rates.
population classified as mentally retarded Initial studies, like those undertaken by
(Croen et al., 2002). Consequently, the inter- Wing and Gould (1979), reported prevalence
pretation of epidemiological studies must con- rates for nonautistic PDD of 21 per 10,000.
sider the diagnostic system and methods They went on to suggest that approximately 16
employed. per 10,000 children had disorders of “recipro-
In DSM-III, the alternatives to AD were cal social interaction” other than “ typical
Childhood-Onset PDD and Atypical PDD. autism.”
Under DSM-III-R, these latter two categories As a result of concerns about rising rates
were collapsed into PDD-NOS. It can be seen of autism and claims that PDDs are reaching
that a simple comparison of prevalence rates of epidemic proportions, several rigorous stud-
Atypical PDD (DSM-III) and PDD-NOS ies, using operationalized definitions and
(DSM-III-R) will not draw from congruent careful methods, were conducted. The stated
populations. DSM-III-R criteria were broader objective of these studies was to determine
than those in DSM-III (Spitzer & Siegel, 1990; rates for autism, but the growing importance
Volkmar et al., 1994). Compared to DSM-III, of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) led in-
DSM-III-R increased the portion of children vestigators to seek rates for nonautistic PDD
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 185

as well. Chakrabarti and Fombonne (2001) current models suggest that no single contri-
scrupulously examined rates of PDDs in bution is sufficient and that PDDs result from
preschoolers among a population of 320,000 combinations of genetic and life events (Jones
(15,500 children 3 to 5 years old) in the & Szatmari, 2002). No neurochemical find-
United Kingdom. Results showed 97 children ings have been shown to correlate with PDD
with a PDD: 26 (17/10,000) were noted to subtypes.
have AD; 13 (8.4/10,000) had ASP; 1 had Rett The search for biological mechanisms has
syndrome; 1 had CDD; and 56 (36/10,000) cataloged many genetic and medical condi-
were noted to have PDD-NOS. The United tions and anomalies that arise in autistic indi-
States Centers for Disease Control were com- viduals. Specific genetic disorders have been
missioned to examine rates in Brick Town- observed with more than chance frequency in
ship, New Jersey (Bertrand et al., 2001). The cohorts of children with autism (C. Gillberg &
New Jersey study relied primarily on school Coleman, 1992). The most common is fragile
records, and then also on clinicians’ reports, X syndrome, but a long list of other disorders
self-referral of parents, and lists of children of probable genetic etiology including Cor-
provided by parent groups. Children were nelia de Lang syndrome, tuberous sclerosis
then directly evaluated using modern tech- (I. C. Gillberg, Gillberg, & Ahlsen, 1994;
niques and definitions. Results showed 53 Smalley, 1998), Cohen syndrome (Howlin,
children were evaluated directly—22 were di- 2001), and PKU have also been identified in
agnosed through records. The overall rate was association with autism (Gillberg & Coleman,
67/10,000 for all ASDs among children 3 to 1992). However, no condition has been identi-
10 years old. The rate for combined Asperger fied that might be considered a principal or
syndrome and PDD-NOS was 27/10,000. The specific cause of a PDD. Every genetic condi-
authors suggested that the rates for nonautis- tion that has been reported in conjunction with
tic PDD were an underestimate resulting AD has also been reported in individuals who
from the methods and may have overesti- do not have autism. It appears that some ge-
mated AD at the expense of nonautistic PDD netic or metabolic conditions may increase the
(Bertrand et al., 2001). Fiona and coworkers risk for autism, but none have been shown to
(Fiona, Baron-Cohen, Bolton, & Brayne, invariably result in a PDD (Lauritsen, Mors,
2002) examined rates in Cambridge, United Mortensen, & Ewald, 2002). It is likely that
Kingdom, among 5- to 11-year-olds. Rates for the outcome runs a gamut. For example, fragile
ASD were 57/10,000. X syndrome appears to produce a range of so-
Taken together, these studies suggest that cial impairments from severe autism to PDD-
the combined total for all other forms of NOS to developmentally near-appropriate
nonautistic PDD are more common than social reciprocity (Reiss et al., 1986). Most
autism. Fombonne’s (2003) meta-analysis sug- cohorts typically have pooled individuals diag-
gested that a conservative estimate for the nosed as PDD-NOS or Autistic disorder into
prevalence rate of PDD-NOS was 15/10,000— a single group (e.g., “autistic syndromes”;
150% of the rate for autism and 600% of the C. Gillberg, 1992).
rate for Asperger’s disorder. However, until There has been a sustained effort to better
positive, specific criteria are applied to large characterize the cognitive features of children
community-based populations the prevalence with autism (Prior, 1979; Sigman, Ungerer,
of PDD-NOS must remain tentative. In addi- Mundy, & Sherman, 1987). During the past
tion, problems demarcating the boundary be- decade, investigation has resulted in a more
tween severe developmental language disorder precise characterization of the complex cogni-
and PDD continue to plague efforts to obtain tive deficits that arise in autistic individuals
accurate prevalence rates. (Baron-Cohen, Tager-Flusberg, & Cohen, 1993,
Minshew et al., 2002). Individuals with autism
ETIOLOGY display limitations in attention to emotional
expression (Piggott et al., 2004), and in the ca-
The same genetic, neurochemical, and cogni- pacity for affective imitation (Rogers & Pen-
tive abnormalities that have been proposed nington, 1991). They also display relative
for AD are likely to produce PDD-NOS. The strengths on visual-spatial tasks (Caron et al.,
186 Diagnosis and Classification

2004). Until recently, the connection between PDD-NOS were able to predict others’ behav-
these impairments and the profound social im- ior as well as control children, but they had
pediments of autism was obscure. However, greater difficulty than control children in un-
more detailed study has suggested that autistic derstanding wishes and desires in others
persons display consistent, profound limita- (Serra et al., 2002). They also suggest that
tions in the capacity to understand that other children with PDD-NOS may perceive emo-
persons have a mental apparatus with thoughts, tional or psychological characteristics, but do
desires, and intentions that influence their ac- not apply them (Serra, Minderaa, van Geert, &
tions (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). This capacity Jackson, 1999).
for recognition of others’ thoughts and mental
apparatus has been termed “ theory of mind” NATURAL HISTORY
[ToM]). It is the cognitive function that permits
a child to perceive and relate to the mental state There have been few longitudinal studies of
of another. disorders comprising PDD-NOS, particularly
Klin and coworkers (Klin, Volkmar, & when compared to numerous studies of indi-
Sparrow, 1992) have suggested that the study viduals with autism. Narrowly defined autism
of autistic subjects may be unsuitable for in- exhibits a wide range of outcomes (Gillberg &
vestigating ToM problems because most Steffenberg, 1987). A very small number of
autistic individuals have the onset of extreme autistic persons are able to attend college and
social impairment prior to the period when live independently. Although many more make
ToM skills emerge. Thus it is only logical to obvious strides in achieving greater social
expect that primary abilities to recognize awareness, they remain socially odd, require
subtle social signals would be severely im- lifelong supervision, and need educational
paired as well. They propose that it would be support. The largest portion, perhaps 60%,
more appropriate to study individuals with make modest gains or remain severely im-
better language and communication skills paired (Gillberg, 1991). The outcome of
such as those with high-functioning autism, autism has been directly correlated with over-
MCDD, or ASP. Such an approach would per- all IQ, language development, and appearance
mit more detailed examinations of processes of seizures. Seizures occur in as many as 30%
underlying social decisions. Klin and cowork- of cases and are more frequent among individ-
ers have suggested that impairment in theory uals with lower IQ scores.
of mind is more likely to be a consequence Most studies of nonautistic PDD assemble
than the cause of ASDs. cohorts of persons with ASP. Relative to lower
Persons with PDD-NOS or high-function- functioning children with autism, the progno-
ing autism could be particularly suitable sub- sis for those with ASP can be fairly good, al-
jects for researchers who want to learn more though findings of most studies favor
about ToM hypotheses. Bowler (1992) sug- restrained optimism. Szatmari and coworkers
gests that high-functioning autistic individuals (1989) reported a high overall adaptive level
were able to correctly identify the mental state on follow-up, but methodological flaws in se-
of another in an experimental paradigm, but lection and diagnoses may have compromised
could not explain their success by applying an the generalizability of these findings. Most
understanding of mental state. Moreover, suc- others give disappointing results though still
cessful performance under experimental con- reporting better outcomes among those with
ditions did not correlate with adaptive social higher IQ and verbal skills (Venter, Lord, &
function. Possessing a theory of mind did not Schopler, 1991). Asperger believed that the
engender social competence among these sub- disorder bearing his name remained stable
jects (Miller & Ozonoff, 2000; Ozonoff & throughout adolescence and adulthood (1944).
Miller, 1995). Serra and coworkers (2002) Although a majority of the individuals in his
found in a 3-year longitudinal study that chil- cohort made good academic adjustments,
dren with PDD-NOS displayed delays in devel- throughout life they sustained the social
opment of theory of mind compared to healthy deficits first displayed in childhood. It appears
volunteers. They noted that children with that deficits in social impairment improve but
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 187

do not keep pace with gains in IQ among these skills, measured by standardized measures;
higher functioning individuals (Schatz & and language abilities.
Hamdan-Allen, 1995). Only one investigation has been published
There are many opinions on whether ASP on the outcome of individuals diagnosed with
increases the risk of developing other disor- multiplex developmental disorder (van der
ders. Schizophrenia occurred in only one of Gaag, 1993). Van der Gaag reported a variety
200 cases in Asperger’s original cohort of outcome results ascertained with a stan-
(1944). One individual in Wing’s cohort dardized diagnostic assessment of 43 adoles-
(1981) was thought to have schizophrenia. cents and 12 adults previously identified as
Wolff and Chick (1980) conducted a con- MCDD. Seven adolescents (16%) were free of
trolled follow-up study of individuals with any Axis I or II diagnoses. The most common
schizoid personality who were ascertained adolescent Axis I diagnoses were anxiety dis-
using operational criteria. This generally has orders (17%) and mood disorders (10%). Axis
been regarded as a longitudinal study of ASP. II diagnoses in the adolescents identified
In this sample, 2 of 22 persons developed PDD-NOS (37%), schizoid (12%), and schizo-
schizophrenia and nearly half had suicidal typal (2%) personality disorders most com-
ideation. However, “mystical or psychotic monly. Of the 12 adults, 2 (17%) developed
symptoms” were evident in nearly half the co- schizophrenia, 30% had schizoid personality,
hort (Wolff & Chick, 1980). Szatmari and and 17% had Schizotypal Personality Disor-
coworkers (1989) suggested that anxiety, der. Apparently, MCDD may produce varied
obsessive-compulsive symptoms, conduct outcomes; however, it is equally evident that it
symptoms, and schizotypal symptoms were is associated with a great risk of chronic men-
more common in persons with ASP than in tal disorder.
those with autism. In their studies, bizarre
ideation was less common in ASP than in TREATMENT
autism. Wing (1981), too, found anxiety
symptoms to be prominent in her ASP sample. No one treatment, method, or approach has
C. Gillberg (1992) cautioned that clinicians been shown to be effective for individuals with
unfamiliar with the disorder may misinterpret PDD-NOS. The decision to implement a treat-
the bizarre, concrete, and idiosyncratic ment is based on the individual’s strengths,
thoughts and behaviors of adolescents or symptoms, the setting, and the limitations that
adults with ASP as signs of schizophrenia. exist at the time (Towbin, 2003). This approach
This has also been borne out in work that di- reflects the polymorphic symptoms, diverse
rectly ascertains elements of thought disorder deficits, and the wide range of impairments ex-
in higher functioning autistic individuals with hibited by children with PDD-NOS.
language (Dykens, Volkmar, & Glick, 1991; For every patient, treatment begins with a
Ghaziuddin, Leininger, & Tsai, 1995). thoughtful and comprehensive evaluation (Fil-
These studies suggest that individuals with ipek et al., 2000). It cannot be completed in a
PDD-NOS appear to have a better prognosis single session. The objectives of evaluation are
than persons with autism, but they do not fare to detect the predominant symptoms that are
as well as persons with ASP. This appears to impeding the patient’s development; the capaci-
be directly correlated with the cognitive and ties, talents, and resources that can be recruited
language abilities. Differences appear less re- in support of the patient’s care; and the princi-
lated to diagnoses than functional abilities pal limitations under which he or she operates
(Serra et al., 2002; Szatmari, 1997; Szatmari (Towbin, 1994). Such an evaluation must in-
et al., 2000). Obviously, lower functioning clude direct interviews with primary caretakers
autistic persons have the most limited prog- and others who are knowledgeable about the pa-
nosis. Estimating the prognosis of persons tient’s early development. In addition, informa-
with high-functioning autism, compared to tion from others familiar with the patient,
those with PDD-NOS, cannot be reliably ac- including pediatricians, teachers, and extended
complished at this point. The most reliable family can be extremely helpful. Standardized
factors to consider are overall IQ; social measures can provide useful information
188 Diagnosis and Classification

(Filipek et al., 2000) and are discussed else- language and social skills training into a uni-
where in this Handbook. In addition, laboratory tary program of instruction that includes
studies including electroencephalogram, kary- achievement of academic skills. Such an inte-
otype, fragile-X testing, blood studies for grated approach facilitates the child’s ability
quantitative amino acids, and urine for organic to generalize language and social skills across
acids may be important, although these studies settings, persons, and situations. In contrast to
yield negative results in more than 90% of the traditional resource room or “pull-out ”
children with autism (Lauritsen et al., 2002; methods used to assist children with specific
Wassink, Piven, & Patil, 2001). In children learning or speech/ language disorders, the in-
with any focal neurological findings, docu- tegrated classroom model provides children
mented decline in skills, or onset of seizures, with PDD opportunities to practice skills
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI ) scans across a variety of domains in the same setting
allow clinicians to visualize brain structure with the same people. This reduces the number
without radiation risk. For children with PDD- of novel persons, experiences, and settings to
NOS who do not exhibit these features, the which the child must become accustomed.
value of these studies for therapeutic care is There are no generalizable guidelines on
highly questionable (Filipek et al., 2000). classroom assignments and classmate mixtures.
Compiling the history must include partici- Some children benefit most from being placed
pation of family members. Once the history in classrooms with ordinary children. Others
has been collected and a formulation prepared, cannot manage without the consistent special-
the interpretation must permit parent and fam- ized instruction that comes from full-time as-
ily members to raise concerns about the find- signment to special programs in contained
ings and discuss their implications. This, too, classrooms with small classes. Still others
is a process that unfolds over time and requires learn best from a mix of mainstream and spe-
supplemental meetings to reiterate the formu- cialized classroom placements. The tendency to
lation, correct misunderstandings, and refine assign children based on their highest or lowest
the prognosis and plans for the patient. Miscon- functional domain tends to obscure their spe-
ceptions and distortions beset the cause and cific needs. Therefore a child with good aca-
treatment of PDD-NOS. Parents and family demic progress may still require the support
members have the best opportunity to discuss and skills of a specialized classroom setting,
their fears and doubts in an atmosphere of can- while a lower functioning child might do well in
dor and compassion. The opportunity to ex- a mainstream remedial class for some topics, or
plore their fears and hopes permits the family mainstream in nonacademic areas.
to approach treatments that are consistent with Cognitive and behavioral methods are the
the child’s optimal requirements and the fam- most investigated and commonly used inter-
ily’s basic values, beliefs, and capacities. It ventions in the treatment of autistic individu-
also facilitates the family’s collaboration with als and likely are applicable across the
the wider environment that includes the ex- spectrum of PDD conditions. They have been
tended family, community, and school system. directed toward decreasing self-injury, perse-
Appropriate treatment rests on a foundation verative behaviors, and other behaviors that
of educational instruction that relates to the impede social interaction. They have also been
patient’s capacities and limitations. The edu- implemented to promote social competence
cational curriculum should assure that there is and relating (Mesibov, 1984). An example of
adequate emphasis on objectives related to lan- the application of these methods is the Treat-
guage and social skills development. However, ment and Education of Autistic and Related
each child is different; each one possesses par- Communication Handicapped CHildren
ticular skills and requires interventions aimed (TEACCH) program in North Carolina
toward specific maladaptive behaviors. This (Schopler, 1994). As defined by Schopler, a
individuality demands a tailored program con- primary aim of the program is to improve skill
ducted by teachers who are experienced in the levels in socialization and communication.
education of students with developmental de- This is achieved through manipulation of the
lays. Ordinarily, it is appropriate to integrate environment in an effort to accommodate the
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 189

child’s deficit. These can be integrated suc- Sandman, Barron, & Colman, 1990), but at-
cessfully into a school curriculum and rein- tempts to replicate efficacy in controlled trials
forced at home and in other social settings. have been unsuccessful. Some even suggest
Alternative approaches attempt to increase naltrexone may be harmful (Campbell et al.,
social interaction indirectly by increasing 1993; Willemsen-Swinkels, Buitelaar, Nijhof,
language learning (Lovaas, 1987). This is & van Engeland, 1995). There is no simple for-
achieved by modifying the child’s behavior mula for the management of these symptoms.
using parents or hired assistants as therapists There is some evidence that selective seroton-
in intensive one-on-one teaching sessions (Lo- ergic reuptake inhibitor (SSRI ) agents, such as
vaas, 1987). Use of cognitive techniques has clorimipramine, fluvoxamine, and sertraline,
been applied to teaching imaginative play and may be helpful (McDougle, Price, & Volkmar,
toward efforts to have children understand 1994). The SSRIs may also be useful for treat-
“other minds” in false belief paradigms. The ing extreme inflexibility in those with PDDs.
development of prosocial behaviors may have Tantruming in response to changes in routine
enduring effects that will influence adaptation or the presence of novel stimuli would be an-
in adulthood (Mesibov, 1983). However, it other indication for these agents.
remains to be demonstrated that these inter- Symptoms of hyperactivity, inattention,
ventions improve social functioning in sponta- and distractibility may respond to stimulant
neous interaction or increase motivation for agents (Birmaher, Quintana, & Greenhill,
social interaction (Bowler, 1992; Rutter & 1988; Handen, Johnson, & Lubetsky, 2000;
Bailey, 1993). Quintana et al., 1995). However, there may be
Pharmacotherapy can play a useful role in a greater risk of adverse reactions to stimu-
the treatment of PDD-NOS and has been re- lants in PDD patients (Handen et al., 2000)
viewed recently elsewhere (Towbin, 2003) than in children with ADHD. Some studies
and in this Handbook (see Chapter 44). There suggest this may not be the case (Quintana
is no specific agent or class of agents that can et al., 1995), and further study is warranted.
affect the core deficit of PDD-NOS nor one Children with PDD-NOS who are aggres-
that is useful generically for every symptom sive may receive benefit from atypical neu-
that may arise. Nevertheless, specific symp- roleptic agents (Barnard et al., 2002) such as
toms may be responsive to particular agents risperidone (McCracken et al., 2002), ziprasi-
(Towbin, 2003). Assessment of the symptoms done (McDougle, Kem, & Posey, 2002),
rather than the diagnoses presented by a pa- lithium (Steingard & Biederman, 1987), val-
tient becomes much more critical in making proate, carbamazepine, SSRIs (Zubieta &
decisions about pharmacological intervention Alessi, 1993), or propranolol (Ratey et al.,
(Towbin, 2003). Frequent symptoms that lead 1987). There is some suggestion that tra-
patients or their families to consider pharma- zodone may be useful as well (Zubieta &
cological interventions include aggressive or Alessi, 1992). Disorganization, agitation, and
self-injurious behaviors; repetitive stereo- aggression may respond to neuroleptics al-
typic behaviors; hyperkinesis, inattention, and though one wants to minimize the dosage and
distractibility; emotional lability; withdrawal; total duration of treatment with agents like
or extreme tantruming. haloperidol and pimozide (Perry et al., 1989).
Self-injury is one of the most troubling Approaching pharmacological treatment for
symptoms that patients display. These injuries PDD-NOS requires exceptional care and thor-
may be related to compulsive or repetitive oughness (Towbin, 2003). Individuals with
stereotypical patterns of behavior (Zubieta & PDD-NOS are a particularly vulnerable pa-
Alessi, 1993). Atypical neuroleptic agents tient population; they often have difficulty re-
have been employed for the treatment of self- porting side effects and identifying their fears
injurious behaviors (Barnard, Young, Pearson, and worries about medication. As a group,
Geddes, & O’Brien, 2002; McCracken et al., they are more susceptible to misapplication of
2002). Case reports and small studies sug- medications for extended periods. Safeguard-
gested that naltrexone might be useful in some ing their care requires thoughtful considera-
patients (Leboyer, Bouvard, & Dugas, 1988; tion and painstaking technique, and it is
190 Diagnosis and Classification

necessary to move through pharmacological like those worked out for Autistic disorder, but
treatment patiently and steadily. This requires it is possible to make specific statements about
attention to symptom targets, vigilant tracking when the diagnosis is appropriate and when it
of responses, administration of sufficient is not. PDD-NOS should continue to be used
doses over an adequate duration for each when social deficits are a prominent, impair-
agent, and the limiting of polypharmacy to a ing feature of the clinical picture. The correc-
minimum as much as possible (Towbin, 2003). tion to the text in DSM-IV-TR has served to
Reassessment and attending to areas of emphasize that social deficits are necessary
strength are keystones of responsible care. Al- for PDD-NOS. There is general agreement that
though the deficits of PDD-NOS appear to be this means using DSM-IV Group A-1 Criteria
stable over time, persons who have PDD-NOS or their equivalent from ICD-10. Thus, indi-
do grow and develop. As they do, the nature viduals without social deficits but with other
and severity of their symptoms and limitations features of PDD, such as those with only spe-
change. Educational regulations require peri- cific language impairment or with SLI and
odic reevaluation of the child’s academic prog- stereotyped movements should not be diag-
ress. These should also serve as reminders for nosed with PDD-NOS. Also, the impairment
reevaluation of the patient’s overall develop- criterion is crucial, particularly for establish-
ment and symptom profile. Reassessment of ing a boundary between relative weaknesses
developmental and adaptive functioning are that properly fall within the broader autism
important for correcting unwanted side effects phenotype (BAP) rather than PDDs.
of therapeutic interventions or symptomatic There is an emerging consensus that an
misinterpretations that have become clearer adaptation of the Autistic disorder DSM crite-
since initial diagnosis and planning. This is ria could serve as a rough guide to making the
particularly important if plans were laid when diagnosis of PDD-NOS. Implications from the
the child was young or had very limited verbal proposals from Buitelaar and coworkers (1998),
capacities. Repeated psychological and devel- or Walker and coworkers (2004) are that (after
opmental assessments should be conducted excluding all other PDD disorders) a total of at
routinely, perhaps at 2- to 3-year intervals. least three DSM-IV Autistic disorder symp-
toms, at least one of which must be in the social
SUMMARY impairment (Group A-I ) cluster, might form a
template for future PDD-NOS research. In ad-
A review of PDD-NOS reveals that some gains dition, this guideline would omit the criterion
have been made in the past decade in the na- for age of onset.
ture of PDD-NOS and its biological implica- Careful research still must answer myriad
tions. At this point, there are several avenues questions, many of them absolutely basic.
for moving the field ahead. Foremost is that Among the most important are explorations of
the diagnostic entity of PDD-NOS must be the continuity and demarcations within the
sustained. Only by maintaining PDD-NOS wider spectrum of PDD. Longitudinal studies of
will the field allow for further characteriza- the course and prognosis of a well-characterized
tion and research that can move us toward a cohort of children with PDD-NOS would be an
better understanding of the basic neuro- extremely valuable contribution. Similarly,
science, course, and treatments that pertain to studies that compare high- and low-functioning
individuals with these disorders. PDD spectrum children should employ prospec-
The field is also in a better position to make tive longitudinal designs. In addition, we need
more specific recommendations about the di- to understand more about the relationship be-
agnosis of PDD-NOS. For research purposes, tween symptoms and physiology. Further work
it would be helpful to have a consensus about is needed to identify markers that relate to ge-
how a benchmark instrument like the ADI-R netic endowment (“endophenotypes”). These
and ADOS-G could be used to make reliable might stem from neurophysiological, functional
diagnosis of PDD-NOS. magnetic resonance imaging, or neuropsycho-
More work is needed to reach a consensus logical studies, such as those relying on facial
about operational definitions for clinical use, recognition (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill,
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 191

Raste, & Plumb, 2001; Schultz et al., 2000), eye promotes understanding and compassion for
gaze (Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen the complex social, emotional, and cognitive
2002), auditory social recognition (Rutherford, symptoms experienced by those with PDD-
Baron-Cohen, & Wheelwright, 2002), or neu- NOS. For those closest to individuals with
ropsychological performance (Dawson, Webb, PDD-NOS, such knowledge also provides a
et al., 2002). In each case, validation of these coherent framework and creates an opportu-
tasks demands that they be applied across the nity for collaboration in their pursuit to find
continuum of PDDs. help for their loved ones.
Genetic studies hold out the hope of learn-
ing how core autism might be related to high- Cross-References
functioning autism, Asperger syndrome, and
milder PDD conditions. In particular, genetic Specific diagnostic concepts are discussed in
studies that ascertain PDD-NOS probands and Chapters 1 to 5, longitudinal and follow-up
employ state-of-the-art family study methods studies in Chapter 7, assessment issues are re-
are needed. The relationships between multi- viewed in Chapters 27 to 32, behavioral inter-
ple complex developmental disorder, other ventions are discussed in Chapters 32 and 35,
PDD conditions, and schizophrenia are an ad- and pharmacological issues reviewed in Chap-
ditional fruitful research area (McCellan & ter 44.
Werry, 1994). Carefully crafted epidemiologi-
cal investigations will bring us closer to under- REFERENCES
standing the prevalence of these conditions
and highlight the breadth of treatment and ed- Adams, C., & Bishop, D. V. (1989). Conversational
ucational needs. Effective treatments such as characteristics of children with semantic-
cognitive approaches, social skills training, pragmatic disorder: Part I. Exchange struc-
pharmacological agents, and educational mod- ture, turntaking, repairs and cohesion. British
Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24(3),
ifications continue to be indispensable for
211–239.
higher functioning individuals in this group. Alaghband-Rad, J., McKenna, K., Gordon, C. T.,
Overall, clinicians continue to be better in- Albus, K. E., Hamburger, S. D., Rumsey,
formed and able to diagnose and treat higher J. M., et al. (1995). Childhood-onset schizo-
functioning PDD disorders than ever before. phrenia: The severity of premorbid course.
New treatments and clearer definitions have Journal of the American Academy of Child and
provided opportunities for patients previously Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(10), 1273–1283.
viewed as merely idiosyncratic to be under- Allen, D. A., Steinberg, M., Dunn, M., Fein, D., Fe-
stood in a more constructive and positive instein, C., Waterhouse, L., et al. (2001).
way. Kanner’s articulation promoted sensitiv- Autistic disorder versus other pervasive devel-
opmental disorders in young children: Same or
ity to those with autism. Now those with
different? European Child and Adolescent Psy-
higher functioning PDDs may benefit from
chiatry, 10(1), 67–78.
thoughtful interdisciplinary approaches. Asperger, H. (1944). Autistic psychopathy in child-
Modern educational models, cognitive strate- hood (U. Frith, Trans.). In U. Frith (Ed.),
gies, pharmacotherapies, and social skills Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 37–92).
training that have been offered to those with Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
autism can be extended to others who may Press.
have even better prognoses. Instead of being Asperger, H. (1979). Problems of infantile autism.
seen as treatment refractory, psychotic, con- Communication, 13, 45–52.
duct disordered, or oppositional, there are Bagwell, C. L., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W. E., Jr., &
now opportunities to place the symptoms of Hoza, B. (2001). Attention-deficit hyperactiv-
ity disorder and problems in peer relations:
those with higher functioning PDDs into a de-
Predictions from childhood to adolescence.
velopmental and social-emotional context and Journal of the American Academy of Child and
to consider each individual’s potential. This Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1285–1292.
process highlights treatments that are most Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P.,
likely to be helpful and focuses the efforts of Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). Autism
those who teach or treat these individuals. It as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a
192 Diagnosis and Classification

British twin study. Psychological Medicine, Bishop, D. V. (2002). Motor immaturity and spe-
25(1), 63–77. cific speech and language impairment: Evi-
Barnard, L., Young, A. H., Pearson, J., Geddes, J., dence for a common genetic basis. American
& O’Brien, G. (2002). A systematic review of Journal of Medical Genetics, 114(1), 56–63.
the use of atypical antipsychotics in autism. Bishop, D. V., & Adams, C. (1989). Conversational
Journal Psychopharmacology, 16(1), 93–101. characteristics of children with semantic-
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). pragmatic disorder: Part II. What features
Does the autistic child have a “ theory of lead to a judgement of inappropriacy? British
mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24(3),
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, 241–263.
D. J. (1993). Understanding other minds: Per- Bishop, D. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring
spectives from autism. Oxford, England: Ox- the borderlands of autistic disorder and spe-
ford Medical Publications. cific language impairment: A study using
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal
Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7),
in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study 917–929.
with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Bloom, F. E. (1993). Advancing a neurodevelop-
syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal mental etiology for schizophrenia. Archives of
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), General Psychiatry, 50, 224–227.
241–251. Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P.,
Beaumanoir, A. (1992). The Landau-Kleffner syn- Goode, S., Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case-
drome. In J. Roger, M. Bureau, F. E. Dravet, A. control family history study of autism. Journal
Dreyfus, A. Perret, & P. Wolf (Eds.), Epileptic of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(5),
syndromes in infancy, childhood, and adoles- 877–900.
cence (2nd ed., pp. 231–243). London: Libbey. Bowler, D. M. (1992). “ Theory of mind” in As-
Beckett, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, perger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychol-
C., O’Connor, T. G., & Rutter, M. (2002). Be- ogy and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877–893.
havior patterns associated with institutional Bregman, J. D. (1991). Current developments in the
deprivation: A study of children adopted from understanding of mental retardation. Part II:
Romania. Journal of Developmental and Be- Psychopathology. Journal of the American
havioral Pediatrics, 23(5), 297–303. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Beidel, D. C. (1991). Social phobia and overanxious 30(6), 861–872.
disorder in school age children. Journal of the Brook, S. L., & Bowler, D. M. (1992). Autism by
American Academy of Child and Adolescent another name? Semantic and pragmatic im-
Psychiatry, 30, 545–552. pairments in children. Journal of Autism and
Bender, L. (1946). Childhood schizophrenia. Amer- Developmental Disorders, 22(1), 61–81.
ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 17, 40–56. Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., & Smith, I. M. (1988).
Bertrand, J., Mars, A., Boyle, C., Bove, F., Yeargin- First report of a Canadian epidemiological
Allsopp, M., & Decouf le, P. (2001). Preva- study of autistic syndromes. Journal of Child
lence of autism in a United States population: Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 433–445.
The Brick Township, New Jersey, investiga- Buitelaar, J. K., & van der Gaag, R. J. (1998). Diag-
tion. Pediatrics, 108(5), 1155–1161. nostic rules for children with PDD-NOS and
Birmaher, B., Quintana, H., & Greenhill, L. (1988). multiple complex developmental disorder.
Methylphenidate treatment of hyperactive Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
autistic children. Journal of the American 39(6), 911–919.
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 27, 248–251. Buitelaar, J. K., van der Wees., M., Swaab-
Bishop, D. V. (1989). Autism, Asperger’s syn- Barneveld, H., & van der Gaag, R. J. (1999).
drome, and pragmatic semantic disorder: Verbal memory and performance IQ predict
Where are the boundaries? British Journal of theory of mind and emotion recognition abil-
Disorders of Communication, 24, 107–121. ity in children with autistic spectrum disor-
Bishop, D. V. (2000). Pragmatic language impair- ders and in psychiatric control children.
ment: A correlate of SLIA, distinct subgroup Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
or part of the autistic continuum? In D. V. 40(6), 869–881.
Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Small, A. M.,
language impairments in children: Causes Adams, P., Gonzalez, N. M., & Ernst, M.
characteristics, intervention and outcome (1993). Naltrexone in autistic children: Be-
(pp. 99–114). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis. havioral symptoms and attentional learning.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 193

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J.,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1283–1291. McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neu-
Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, rocognitive function and joint attention ability
H., Taylor, A., Murray, R. M., et al. (2002). in young children with autism spectrum disor-
Evidence for early-childhood, pan-develop- der versus developmental delay. Child Devel-
mental impairment specific to schizophreni- opment, 73(2), 345–358.
form disorder: Results from a longitudinal Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G. D., Dager,
birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, S., Friedman, S., Aylward, E., et al. (2002).
59(5), 449–456. Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Ge-
Cantwell, D. P., & Rutter, M. (1994). Classifica- netic, brain, and behavioral perspectives. Devel-
tion: Conceptual issues and substantive find- opment and Psychopathology, 14(3), 581–611.
ings. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov Despert, J. L., & Sherwin, A. C. (1958). Further
(Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: A examination of diagnostic criteria in schizo-
modern approach (3rd ed., pp. 3–21). London: phrenic illness and psychoses of infancy and
Blackwell. early childhood. American Journal of Psychia-
Caplan, R. (1994). Childhood schizophrenia as- try, 114, 784–790.
sessment and treatment: A developmental ap- Dykens, E., Volkmar, F., & Glick, M. (1991).
proach. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Thought disorder in high-functioning autistic
Clinics of North America, 3(1), 15–30. adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Rainville, C., & Disorders, 21(3), 291–301.
Chouinard, S. (2004). Do high-functioning Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek,
persons with autism present superior spatial G. T., Cook, E. H., Jr., Dawson, G., et al.
abilities? Neuropsychologia, 42(4), 467–481. (2000). Practice parameter: Screening and di-
Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive agnosis of autism: Report of the Quality Stan-
developmental disorders in preschool children. dards Subcommittee of the American
Journal of the American Medical Association, Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurol-
285(24), 3093–3099. ogy Society. Neurology, 55(4), 468–479.
Cohen, P. R., & Volkmar, F. R. (1986). Issues in Fiona, J. S., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne,
the classification of pervasive developmental C. (2002). Brief report: Prevalence of autism
disorders: Toward DSM-IV. Journal of the spectrum conditions in children aged 5–11
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, years in Cambridgeshire, U.K. Autism, 6(3),
213–229. 231–237.
Courchesne, E. (1995). New evidence of cerebel- Fish, B., Marcus, J., Hans, S. L., Auerbach, J. G., &
lar and brainstem hyppoplasia in autistic in- Perdue, S. (1992). Infants at risk for schizo-
fants, children and adolescents: The MR phrenia: Sequelae of a genetic neurointegra-
imaging study by Hashimoto and colleagues. tive defect. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- 221–235.
ders, 25(1), 19–22. Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism:
Croen, L. A., Grether, J. K., Hoogstrate, J., & A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of
Selvin, S. (2002). The changing prevalence of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18,
autism in California. Journal of Autism and 297–321.
Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 207–215. Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1987). Autism: Familial
Crosbie, J., & Schachar, R. (2001). Deficient inhibi- aggregation and genetic implications. Journal
tion as a marker for familial ADHD. American of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18,
Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), 1884–1890. 3–29.
Dahl, E. K., Cohen, D. J., & Provence, S. (1986). Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of
Clinical and multivariate approaches to the autism and other pervasive developmental dis-
nosology of pervasive developmental disor- orders: An update. Journal of Autism and De-
ders. Journal of the American Academy of velopmental Disorders, 33(4), 365–382.
Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 170–180. Fombonne, E., & Chakrabarti, S. (2001). No evi-
Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagi- dence for a new variant of measles-mumps-
otides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics, 108(4),
(2002). Neural correlates of face and object E58.
recognition in young children with autism Fraser, W. I., & Rao, J. M. (1991). Recent studies
spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and of mentally handicapped young people’s be-
typical development. Child Development, 73, havior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
700–717. chiatry, 32, 79–108.
194 Diagnosis and Classification

Frith, U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: Confu- operational definition of autism. Journal of
sions and controversies about Asperger syn- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4),
drome. Journal of Child Psychology and 269–278.
Psychiatry, 45(4), 672–686. Gordon, C. T., Frazier, J. A., McKenna, K., Giedd,
Geller, B., Zimerman, B., Williams, M., Delbello, J., Zametkin, A., Zahn, T., et al. (1994). Child-
M. P., Bolhofner, K., Craney, J. L., et al. hood-onset schizophrenia: An NIMH study in
(2002). DSM-IV mania symptoms in a prepu- progress. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(4),
bertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder 697–712.
phenotype compared to attention-deficit hy- Greene, R. W., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V.,
peractive and normal controls. Journal of Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., DuPre, E. P.,
Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, et al. (2001). Social impairment in girls with
12(1), 11–25. ADHD: Patterns, gender comparisons, and
Ghaziuddin, M., Leininger, L., & Tsai, L. (1995). correlates. Journal of the American Academy
Brief report: Thought disorder in Asperger’s of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(6),
syndrome: Comparison with high-functioning 704–710.
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Handen, B. L., Johnson, C. R., & Lubetsky, M.
Disorders, 25(3), 311–318. (2000). Efficacy of methylphenidate among
Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rut- children with autism and symptoms of atten-
ter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development tion-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of
and current functioning in adolescents with Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3),
Asperger syndrome: A comparative study. 245–255.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Murakawa, K., Yoshi-
42(2), 227–240. moto, T., Miyazaki, M., Harada, M., et al.
Gillberg, C. (1991). Outcome in autism and autistic- (1995). Development of the brainstem and
like conditions. Journal of the American Acad- cerebellum in autistic patients. Journal of
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(3), Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(1),
375–382. 1–18.
Gillberg, C. (1992). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Heller, T. (1908). Dementia infantilis. Zeitschrift
Lecture 1991: Autism and autistic-like condi- fur die Erforschung and Behandlung des Ju-
tions: Subclasses among disorders of empathy. genlichen Schwachsinns (Journal of Research
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Treatment of Children with Mental Retar-
33, 813–842. dation), 2 141–165.
Gillberg, C. (2003). Deficits in attention, motor con- Hertzig, M., Snow, M. E., New, E., & Shapiro, T.
trol, and perception: A brief review. Archives of (1990). DSM-III and DSM-III-R diagnosis of
Disease in Childhood, 88(10), 904–910. autism and pervasive developmental disorder
Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1992). The biology of in nursery school children. Journal of the
autistic syndromes: Clinics in developmental American Academy of Child and Adolescent
medicine, Number 126 (2nd ed.). Oxford, En- Psychiatry, 29, 123–126.
gland: Blackwell. Howlin, P. (2001). Autistic features in Cohen syn-
Gillberg, C., Persson, E., Grufman, M., & Them- drome: A preliminary report. Developmental
ner, U. (1986). Psychiatric disorders in mildly Medicine and Child Neurology, 43(10),
and severely mentally retarded urban children 692–696.
and adolescents: Epidemiological aspects. Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning
British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 68–74. adults with autism with and without early lan-
Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, S. (1987). Outcome guage delays: Implications for the differentia-
and prognostic factors in infantile autism and tion between autism and Asperger syndrome.
similar conditions: A population-based study Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
of 46 cases followed through puberty. Journal ders, 33(1), 3–13.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), Itard, J. (1962). The wild boy of Aveyron. New York:
273–287. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Gillberg, I. C., Gillberg, C., & Ahlsen, G. (1994). Jansen, L. M., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., van der
Autistic behavior and attention deficits in Gaag, R. J., ten Hove, F., Willemsen-Swinkels,
tuberous sclerosis: A population based survey. S. W., Harteveld, E., et al. (2000). Unrespon-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, siveness to psychosocial stress in a subgroup
36, 50–56. of autistic-like children, multiple complex de-
Gillham, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & velopmental disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Toward a developmental ogy, 25(8), 753–764.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 195

Jansen, L. M., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., Gademan, Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33,
P. J., De Jonge, R. C., van der Linden, J. A., & 861–876.
Kahn, R. S. (1998). Blunted cortisol response Klin, A., Volkmar, F. V., Sparrow, S. S., Ciccehetti,
to a psychosocial stressor in schizophrenia. D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and
Schizophrenia Research, 33(1/2), 87–94. neuropsycholgical characterization of As-
Jansen, L. M., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., van der perger Syndrome: Convergence with Nonver-
Gaag, R. J., & van Engeland, H. (2003). Dif- bal Learning Disabilities Syndrome. Journal of
ferentiation between autism and multiple com- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36,
plex developmental disorder in response to 1127–1140.
psychosocial stress. Neuropsychopharmacol- Kolvin, I. (1971). Studies in the childhood psy-
ogy, 28(3), 582–590. choses: Part I. Diagnostic criteria and classifi-
Jensen, V. K., Larrieu, J. A., & Mack, K. K. cation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 118,
(1997). Differential diagnosis between atten- 381–384.
tion-deficit / hyperactivity disorder and perva- Kurita, H. (1997). A comparative study of As-
sive developmental disorder: Not otherwise perger syndrome with high-functioning atypi-
specified. Clinical Pediatrics, 36(10), cal autism. Psychiatry and Clinical
555–561. Neurosciences 51(2), 67–70.
Jones, M. B., & Szatmari, P. (2002). A risk-factor Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O.,
model of epistatic interaction, focusing on Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social
autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics, language use in parents of autistic individuals.
114(5), 558–565. Psychological Medicine, 22(1), 245–254.
Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Developmental Lauritsen, M. B., Mors, O., Mortensen, P. B., &
coordination disorder in Swedish 7-year-old Ewald, H. (2002). Medical disorders among
children. Journal of the American Academy of inpatients with autism in Denmark according
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(7), to ICD-8: A nationwide register-based study.
820–828. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- ders, 32(2), 115–119.
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Leboyer, M., Bouvard, M. P., & Dugas, M. (1988).
Kemner, C., van der Gaag, R. J., Verbaten, M. N., & Effects of naltrexone on infantile autism.
van Engeland, H. (1999). ERP differences Lancet, 26, 715.
among subtypes of pervasive developmental Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Goode, S., Pickles, A.,
disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 46(6), Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., et al. (1996). A
781–789. broader phenotype of autism: The clinical
Kestenbaum, C. (1983). The borderline child at spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychol-
risk for major psychiatric disorder in adult ogy and Psychiatry, 37(7), 785–801.
life: Seven case reports with follow-up. In Leibenluft, E., Charney, D. S., Towbin, K. E.,
K. Robson (Ed.), The borderline child: Ap- Bhangoo, R. K., & Pine, D. S. (2003). Defin-
proaches to etiology, diagnosis, and treatment ing clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania.
(pp. 49–81). New York: McGraw-Hill. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(3),
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & 430–437.
Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns Lincoln, A. J., Bloom, D., Katz, M., & Boksen-
during viewing of naturalistic social situations baum, N. (1998). Neuropsychological and neu-
as predictors of social competence in individu- rophysiological indices of auditory processing
als with autism. Archives of General Psychia- impairment in children with multiple complex
try, 59(9), 809–816. developmental disorder. Journal of the Ameri-
Klin, A., & Shepard, B. A. (1994). Psychological can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
assessment of autistic children. Child and try, 37(1), 100–112.
Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North Amer- Lofgren, D. P., Bemporad, J., King, J., Lindem, K.,
ica, 3(1), 53–70. & O’Driscoll, G. (1991). A prospective fol-
Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, low-up study of so-called borderline children.
D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(11),
normed study of face recognition in autism 1541–1547.
and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds re-
Developmental Disorders, 29(6), 499–508. ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. V., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(8), 1365–1382.
Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations Lord, C., Leventhal, B. L., & Cook, E. H., Jr.
of the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of (2001). Quantifying the phenotype in autism
196 Diagnosis and Classification

spectrum disorders. American Journal of Med- McCellan, J., & Werry, J. (1994). Practice parame-
ical Genetics, 105(1), 36–38. ters for the assessment and treatment of chil-
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., dren and adolescents with schizophrenia.
Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). Journal of the American Academy of Child and
The autism diagnostic observation schedule- Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(5), 616–635.
generic: A standard measure of social and McCracken, J. T., McGough, J., Shah, B., Cronin,
communication deficits associated with the P., Hong, D., Aman, M. G., et al. (2002).
spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and De- Risperidone in children with autism and seri-
velopmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. ous behavioral problems. New England Journal
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- of Medicine, 347(5), 314–321.
mal intellectual and educational functioning McDougle, C. J., Kem, D. L., & Posey, D. J. (2002).
in young autistic children. Journal of Consult- Use of ziprasidone for maladaptive symptoms
ing and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9. in youths with autism. Journal of the American
Luteijn, E. F., Serra, M., Jackson, S., Steenhuis, Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
M. P., Althaus, M., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). 41, 921–927.
How unspecified are disorders of children McDougle, C. J., Kresch, L. E., Goodman, W. K.,
with a pervasive developmental disorder not Naylor, S. T., Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J.,
otherwise specified? A study of social prob- et al. (1995). A case-controlled study of repet-
lems in children with PDD-NOS and ADHD. itive thoughts and behavior in adults with
European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, autistic disorder and obsessive-compulsive
9(3), 168–179. disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Macintosh, K. E., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annota- 152(5), 772–777.
tion: The similarities and differences between McDougle, C. J., Price, L. H., & Volkmar, F. R.
autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder: A re- (1994). Recent advances in the pharmacother-
view of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child apy of autism and related conditions. Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 421–434. and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North
MacLean, J. E., Szatmari, P., Jones, M. B., Bryson, America, 3(1), 71–89.
S. E., Mahoney, W. J., Bartolucci, G., et al. McKenna, K., Gordon, C. T., Lenane, M., Kaysen,
(1999). Familial factors inf luence level of D., Fahey, K., & Rapoport, J. L. (1994). Look-
functioning in pervasive developmental disor- ing for childhood onset schizophrenia: The
der. Journal of the American Academy of Child first 71 cases screened. Journal of the Ameri-
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(6), 746–753. can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
Mahoney, W. J., Szatmari, P., Maclean, J. E., try, 33, 636–644.
Bryson, S. E., Bartolucci, G., Walter, S. D., Mesibov, G. B. (1983). Current perspectives and
et al. (1998). Reliability and accuracy of dif- issues in autism and adolescence. In E.
ferentiating pervasive developmental disorder Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Autism in
subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of adolescents and adults (pp. 37–53). New York:
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 278–285. Plenum Press.
Malhotra, S., & Gupta, N. (2002). Childhood disin- Mesibov, G. B. (1984). Social skills training with
tegrative disorder: Re-examination of the cur- verbal autistic adolescents and adults: A pro-
rent concept. European Child and Adolescent gram model. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Psychiatry, 11(3), 108–114. mental Disorders, 14, 395–404.
Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. R. (1995). Comparison Micali, N., Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E.
of Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning (2004). The broad autism phenotype: Findings
autistic children on a test of motor impair- from an epidemiological survey. Autism, 8(1),
ment. Journal of Autism and Developmental 21–37.
Disorders, 25(1), 23–40. Milberger, S., Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Testa,
Mannuzza, S., Schneier, F. R., Chapman, T. F., M., & Tsuang, M. T. (1996). New phenotype
Liebowitz, M. R., Klein, D. F., & Fyer, A. J. definition of attention deficit hyperactivity
(1995). Generalized social phobia. Archives of disorder in relatives for genetic analyses.
General Psychiatry, 52, 230–237. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 67(4),
Matthys, W., Cuperus, J. M., & van Engeland, H. 369–377.
(1999). Deficient social problem solving in Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (2000). The external
boys with ODD/CD, with ADHD, and with validity of Asperger disorder: Lack of evi-
both disorders. Journal of the American Acad- dence from the domain of neuropsychology.
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(3), Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2),
311–321. 227–238.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 197

Minshew, N. J. (2001). Editorial preface. Journal of Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S., & Mozes, T.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children
1–3. with schizophrenia, children with autism, and
Minshew, N. J., Sweeney, J., & Luna, B. (2002). normally developing children. Schizophrenia
Autism as a selective disorder of complex in- Research, 42(2), 145–155.
formation processing and underdevelopment of Piven, J., Chase, G., Landa, R., Wzorek, M., Gayle,
neocortical systems. Molecular Psychiatry, J., Cloud, D., et al. (1991). Psychiatric disor-
7(Suppl 2), S14–S15. ders in parents of autistic individuals. Journal
Nicholson, R., Lenane, M., Brookner, F., Gochman, of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
P., Kumra, S., Spechler, L., et al. (2001). Chil- cent Psychiatry, 30(3), 471–478.
dren and adolescents with psychotic disorder Piven, J., Gayle, J., Chase, G. A., Fink, B., Landa,
not otherwise specified: A 2-to 8-year follow- R., Wzorek, M. M., et al. (1990). A family
up study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42(4), study of neuropsychiatric disorders in the
319–325. adult siblings of autistic individuals. Journal
Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Sitter, S., & Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of cent Psychiatry, 29, 177–189.
attention problems in children with autism and Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in
children with a specific language disorder. Eu- parents from multiple-incidence autism fami-
ropean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(1), lies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
58–66. try, 38(8), 1011–1021.
O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Keav- Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1999). Psychiatric disorder
eney, L., & Kreppner, J. M. (2000). The ef- and the broad autism phenotype: Evidence
fects of global severe privation on cognitive from a family study of multiple-incidence
competence: Extension and longitudinal fol- autism families. American Journal of Psychia-
low-up: English and Romanian adoptees study try, 156(4), 557–563.
team. Child Development, 71(2), 376–390. Piven, J., Palmer, P., Landa, R., Santangelo, S., Ja-
Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching the- cobi, D., & Childress, D. (1997). Personality
ory of mind: A new approach to social skills and language characteristics in parents from
training for individuals with autism. Journal of multiple-incidence autism families. American
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, Journal of Medical Genetics, 25(4), 398–411.
415–434. Prior, M. R. (1979). Cognitive abilities and disabil-
Perry, R. (1998). Misdiagnosed ADD/ADHD: Re- ities in infantile autism: A review. Journal of
diagnosed PDD. Journal of the American Acad- Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 357–380.
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(1), Prior, M. R., Eisenmajer, R., Leekam, S., Wing, L.,
113–114. Gould, J., Ong, B., et al. (1998). Are there sub-
Perry, R., Campbell, M., Adams, P. I., Lynch, N., groups within the autistic spectrum? A cluster
Spencer, E. K., Curen, E. L., et al. (1989). analysis of a group of children with autistic
Long-term efficacy of haloperidol in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychol-
children: Continuous versus discontinuous ogy and Psychiatry, 39(6), 893–902.
drug administration. Journal of the American Quintana, H., Birmaher, B., Stedge, D., Lennon, S.,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Freed, J., Bridge, J., et al. (1995). Use of
28, 87–92. methylphenidate in treatment of children with
Pfiffner, L. J., Calzada, E., & McBurnett, K. autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Devel-
(2000). Interventions to enhance social com- opmental Disorders, 25(3), 283–295.
petence. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental
Clinics of North America, 9(3), 689–709. language disorders: Nosologic considerations.
Pickup, G. J., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Theory of In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language,
mind impairments in schizophrenia: Sympto- reading and spelling (pp. 155–184). New York:
matology, severity and specificity. Psycholog- Academic Press.
ical Medicine, 31(2), 207–220. Ratey, J. J., Bemporad, J., Sorgi, P., Bick, P., Po-
Piggot, J., Kwon, H., Mobbs, D., Blasey, C., Lot- latkoff, S., O’Driscoll, G., et al. (1987). Brief
speich, L., Menon, V., et al. (2004). Emotional report: Open trial effects of beta-blockers on
attribution in high-functioning individuals speech and social behaviors in 8 autistic
with Autistic Spectrum Disorder: A func- adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tional imaging study. Journal of the American Disorders, 17, 439–446.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Reiss, A. L., Feinstein, C., Toomey, K. E., Gold-
43(4), 473–480. smith, B., Rosenbaum, K., & Caruso, M. A.
198 Diagnosis and Classification

(1986). Psychiatric disability associated with Perspectives from autism (pp. 481–504). Ox-
the fragile X chromosome. American Journal ford, England: Oxford Medical Publications.
of Medical Genetics, 23, 393–402. Rutter, M., & Gould, M. (1985). Classification. In
Rescorla, L. A. (1986). Preschool psychiatric dis- M. Rutter, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and ado-
orders: Diagnostic classification and symptom lescent psychiatry: Modern approaches
patterns. Journal of the American Academy of (pp. 304–321). Oxford, England: Blackwell
Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 162–169. Scientific Publications.
Richters, M. M., & Volkmar, F. R. (1994). Case Rutter, M. L., Kreppner, J. M., & O’Connor, T. G.
study: Reactive attachment disorder of infancy (2001). Specificity and heterogeneity in chil-
or early childhood. Journal of the American dren’s responses to profound institutional pri-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179,
33(3), 328–332. 97–103.
Ritvo, E. R., Freeman, B. J., & Pingree, C. (1989). Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1988). Autism and
University of Utah epidemiological survey of pervasive developmental disorders. In M.
autism: Prevalence. American Journal of Psy- Rutter, A. H. Tuma, & I. S. Lann (Eds.), As-
chiatry, 146, 194–199. sessment and diagnosis in child psychopathol-
Robertson, J. M., Tanguay, P. E., L’Ecuyer, S., ogy (pp. 408–434). New York: Guilford Press.
Sims, A., & Waltrip, C. (1999). Domains of Sandman, C. A., Barron, J. L., & Colman, H.
Social Communication Handicap in Autism (1990). An orally administered opiate blocker,
Spectrum Disorder. Journal of the American naltrexone, attenuates self-injurious behavior.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95,
38, 738–745. 93–102.
Roeyers, H., Keymeulen, H., & Buysse, A. (1998, Schatz, J., & Hamdan-Allen, G. (1995). Effects of
November/ December). Differentiating atten- age and IQ on adaptive behavior domains for
tion-deficit / hyperactivity disorder from per- children with autism. Journal of Autism and
vasive developmental disorder not otherwise Developmental Disorders, 25, 51–60.
specified. Journal of Learning Disabilities, Schopler, E. (1994). A statewide program for treat-
31(6), 565–571. ment and education of autistic and related com-
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theo- munication handicapped children (TEACCH).
retical approach to the deficits in infantile Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of
autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, North America, 3(1), 91–103.
137–162. Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright,
Roy, P., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (2000). Institu- R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., et al.
tional care: Risk from family background or (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical
pattern of rearing? Journal of Child Psychol- activity during face discrimination among in-
ogy and Psychiatry, 41(2), 139–149. dividuals with autism and Asperger syndrome.
Rumsey, J. M., Andreasen, N., & Rapoport, J. L. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(4),
(1986). Thought, language, communication, 331–340.
and affective f lattening in autistic adults. Serra, M., Loth, F. L., van Geert, P. L., Hurkens,
Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 771–777. E., & Minderaa, R. B. (2002). Theory of mind
Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheel- in children with “lesser variants” of autism: A
wright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychol-
voice: A study with normal adults and adults ogy and Psychiatry, 43(7), 885–900.
with Asperger syndrome and high-functioning Serra, M., Minderaa, R. B., van Geert, P. L., &
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Jackson, A. E. (1999). Social-cognitive abili-
Disorders, 32(3), 189–194. ties in children with lesser variants of autism:
Rutter, M. L., Andersen-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Skill deficits or failure to apply skills? Euro-
Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., pean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8(4),
et al. (1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following 301–311.
severe early global privation: English and Ro- Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., Mundy, P., & Sherman,
manian Adoptees (ERA) study team. Journal T. (1987). Cognition in autistic children. In
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(4), D. J. Cohen & A. M. Donnellan (Eds.), Hand-
537–549. book of autism and pervasive developmental
Rutter, M. L., & Bailey, A. (1993). Thinking and disorders (pp. 103–120). New York: Wiley.
relationships: Mind and brain (some ref lec- Silverstein, M. L., Mavrolefteros, G., & Close, D.
tions on theory of mind and autism). In S. (2002). Premorbid adjustment and neuropsy-
Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. chological performance in schizophrenia.
Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Schizophrenia Bulletin 28(1), 157–165.
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified 199

Smalley, S. L. (1998). Autism and tuberous sclero- Szatmari, P., MacLean, J. E., Jones, M. B., Bryson,
sis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bartolucci, G., et al.
orders, 28(5), 407–414. (2000). The familial aggregation of the lesser
Smalley, S. L., McCracken, J. T., & Tanguay, P. E. variant in biological and nonbiological rela-
(1995). Autism, affective disorders, and social tives of PDD probands: A family history
phobia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
60(1), 19–26. atry, 41(5), 579–586.
Sparks, B. F., Friedman, S. D., Shaw, D. W., Ayl- Szatmari, P., Merette, C., Bryson, S. E., Thivierge,
ward, E. H., Echelard, D., Artru, A. A., et al. J., Roy, M. A., Cayer, M., et al. (2002). Quan-
(2002). Brain structural abnormalities in tifying dimensions in autism: A factor-ana-
young children with autism spectrum disorder. lytic study. Journal of the American Academy
Neurology, 59(2), 184–192. of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(4),
Sparrow, S. S., Rescorla, L. A., Provence, S., Con- 467–474.
don, S. O., Goudreau, D., & Cicchetti, D. V. Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, A. J., & Bar-
(1986). Follow-up of “atypical” children—A tolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome and
brief report. Journal of the American Academy autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the
of Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 181–185. American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L. J., Dimiceli, S., Myers, Psychiatry, 29(1), 130–136.
R. M., & Risch, N. (2002). Behavioral pheno- Tanguay, P. E., Robertson, J., & Derrick, A.
typic variation in autism multiplex families: (1998). A dimensional classification of autism
Evidence for a continuous severity gradient. spectrum disorder by social communication
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114(2), domains. Journal of the American Academy of
129–136. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(3),
Spitzer, R., & Siegel, B. (1990). The DSM-III-R 271–277.
field trials of pervasive developmental dis- Tantam, D. (1988a). Asperger’s syndrome: Annota-
orders. Journal of the American Academy of tion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, try, 29, 245–255.
855–862. Tantam, D. (1988b). Lifelong eccentricity and so-
Steffenburg, S., & Gillberg, C. (1986). Autism and cial isolation: II Asperger’s syndrome or
autistic-like conditions in Swedish rural and schizoid personality disorder? British Journal
urban areas: A population study. British Jour- of Psychiatry, 153, 783–791.
nal of Psychiatry, 149, 81–87. Towbin, K. E. (1994). Pervasive developmental dis-
Steingard, R., & Biederman, J. (1987). Lithium re- order not otherwise specified: A review and
sponsive manic like symptoms in two individ- guidelines for clinical care. Child and Adoles-
uals with autism and mental retardation: Case cent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 3(1),
report. Journal of the American Academy of 149–161.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 932–935. Towbin, K. E. (2003). Strategies for pharmacologic
Steinhausen, H., Gobel, D., Berinlinger, M., & treatment of high-functioning autism and As-
Wohlleben, B. (1986). A community survey of perger syndrome. Child and Adolescent Psychi-
infantile autism. Journal of the American atric Clinics of North America, 12(1), 23–45.
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 186–189. Towbin, K. E., Dykens, E. D., Pearson, G. S., &
Szatmari, P. (1997). Pervasive developmental dis- Cohen, D. J. (1993). Conceptualizing “Border-
order not otherwise specified. In T. A. Widi- line Syndrome of Childhood” and “Childhood
ger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. Schizophrenia” as a developmental disorder.
First, & W. Davis (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook Journal of the American Academy of Child and
(Vol. 3, pp. 43–54). Washington, DC: Ameri- Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(4), 775–782.
can Psychiatric Press. van der Gaag, R. J. (1993). Multiplex development
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R. S., Bond, disorder: An exploration of the borderlines on
S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of the autistic spectrum. Unpublished master’s
high-functioning autistic children. Journal of thesis, University of Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, van der Gaag, R. J., Buitelaar, J. K., Van den Ban,
213–225. E., Bezemer, M., Njio, L., & van Engeland, H.
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Boyle, M. H., Streiner, (1995). A controlled multivariate chart review
D. L., & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of out- of multiple complex developmental disorder.
come among high-functioning children with Journal of the American Academy of Child and
autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(8), 1096–1106.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N.,
520–528. & van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of
200 Diagnosis and Classification

children with pervasive developmental disor- Wassink, T. H., Piven, J., & Patil, S. R. (2001).
der toward human faces: A fixation time Chromosomal abnormalities in a clinic sample
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- of individuals with autistic disorder. Psychi-
atry, 43(5), 669–678. atric Genetics, 11(2), 57–63.
Velting, O. N., & Albano, A. M. (2001). Current Watkins, J. M., Asarnow, R. F., & Tanguay, P. E.
trends in the understanding and treatment of (1988). Symptom development in childhood-
social phobia in youth. Journal of Child Psy- onset schizophrenia. Journal of Child Psychol-
chology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 127–140. ogy and Psychiatry, 29, 865–878.
Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1991). A fol- Werry, J. (1992). Child and adolescent (early
low-up study of high-functioning autistic chil- onset) schizophrenia: A review in light of
dren. Journal of Child Psychology and DSM-III-R. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Psychiatry, 33(3), 489–507. mental Disorders, 22(4), 601–624.
Volkmar, F. R., Carter, A., Sparrow, S. S., & Cic- Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., Ni-
chetti, D. V. (1993). Quantifying social devel- jhof, G. J., & van Engeland, H. (1995). Failure
opment in autism. Journal of the American of naltrexone hydrochloride to reduce self-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, injurious and autistic behavior in mentally re-
32, 627–632. tarded adults. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1988). Classifica- 52, 766–773.
tion and diagnosis of childhood autism. In E. Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Diagnosis account. Psychological Medicine, 11(1),
and assessment in autism (pp. 71–76). New 115–129.
York: Plenum Press. Wing, L. (1997). The autistic spectrum. Lancet,
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Disintegra- 350(9093), 1761–1766.
tive disorder or “late onset ” autism. Journal of Wing, L. (1992). Manifestations of social problems
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 717–724. in high-functioning autistic people. In E.
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991). Comorbid as- Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), high-func-
sociation of autism and schizophrenia. Ameri- tioning individuals with autism (pp. 129–142).
can Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1705–1707. New York: Plenum Press.
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field in social interaction and associated abnormali-
trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American ties in children: Epidemiology and classifica-
Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367. tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Volkmar, F. R., & Rutter, M. (1995). Childhood Disorders, 9, 11–29.
disintegrative disorder: Results from the field Wittchen, H. U., & Fehm, L. (2001). Epidemiology,
trials for DSM-IV. Journal of the American patterns of comorbidity, and associated dis-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, abilities of social phobia. Psychiatric Clinics
34, 1092–1097. of North America, 24(4), 617–641.
Volkmar, F. R., Shaffer, D., & First, M. (2000). Wolff, S., & Chick, J. (1980). Schizoid personality
PDDNOS in DSM-IV. Journal of Autism and in childhood: A controlled follow-up study.
Developmental Disorders, 30(1), 74–75. Psychological Medicine, 10, 85–100.
Volkmar, F. R., Steir, D., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). World Health Organization. (1992). International
Age of onset of pervasive developmental disor- classification of diseases (10th ed.). Geneva,
der. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, Switzerland: Author.
1450–1452. Zubieta, J. K., & Alessi, N. E. (1992). Acute and
Walker, D. R., Thompson, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., chronic administration of trazodone in the
Goldberg, J., Bryson, S. E., Mahoney, W. J., treatment of disruptive behavior disorders in
et al. (2004). Specifying PDD-NOS: A com- children. Journal of Clinical Psychopharma-
parison of PDD-NOS, Asperger syndrome, and cology, 12, 346–351.
autism. Journal of the American Academy of Zubieta, J. K., & Alessi, N. E. (1993). Is there a
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(2), 72–80. role of serotonin in the disruptive behavior
Wang, A. T., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A., Sigman, M., disorders? A literature review. Journal of
& Bookheimer, S. (2004). Neural correlates of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 3,
facial affect processing in children and adoles- 11–35.
cents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
cent Psychiatry, 43(4), 481–490.
CHAPTER 7

Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders

PATRICIA HOWLIN

In the decades following Kanner’s first ac- is no information on the trajectory that individ-
counts of children with autism, there have been uals follow over the years.
many hundreds of publications dealing with By far the most informative studies are
this disorder. However, only a relatively small those that have traced development from child-
proportion has focused on adults, and fewer hood to adulthood. One of the earliest of these,
still have systematically studied the progress of a follow-up of 96 children reassessed in their
individuals with autism as they move through 20s and 30s, was by Kanner himself (1973).
adolescence and into adulthood. In the follow- The majority of his patients had remained
ing chapter, the prognosis for individuals with highly dependent, living with parents, in shel-
autism in terms of their social outcomes, cogni- tered communities, in state institutions for
tive functioning, employment status, and men- people with learning disabilities, or in psychi-
tal health is explored. The overall pattern of atric hospitals. Outcome was more positive for
change is also considered, and evidence of dete- those with better developed communication
rioration in function during the adolescent or skills, and among this group just over half were
early adult years is assessed. Finally, factors functioning relatively well. Eleven such indi-
that appear to influence outcome in adulthood viduals had jobs and one was at college. Seven
are reviewed. had their own homes and one man (a success-
ful music composer) was married with a child.
SOCIAL OUTCOMES The first systematic outcome studies were
conducted by Rutter and his colleagues (Lock-
Information on life for adults with autism yer & Rutter, 1969, 1970; Rutter, Greenfeld,
comes from a variety of sources. First, there & Lockyer, 1967; Rutter & Lockyer, 1967).
are the fascinating autobiographical accounts Thirty-eight individuals, first diagnosed with
by higher functioning individuals (e.g., Ger- autism as children in the 1950s or 1960s, were
land, 1996; Grandin, 1995; Holliday-Willey, reassessed at age 16 years or older. At follow-
1999; Jolliffe, Landsdown, & Robinson, 1992; up, more than half were in long-term hospital
Lawson, 2002; O’Neill, 1999; Williams, 1992), care, and 3 were living in residential communi-
but these apply to only a minority of people ties. Of the 11 who still lived with their par-
within the autistic spectrum. Second, there is ents, 7 had no outside occupation and only 3
a substantial number of clinically based de- had paid jobs. Overall, 14% were said to have
scriptions of young adults. Although often made a “good” social adjustment; 25% were
very interesting, these tend to be somewhat un- rated as “ fair ” and 61% as “poor.”
systematic (e.g., Creak, 1963; Eisenberg, 1956; Lotter (1974a, 1974b) followed up 29 indi-
Mittler, Gillies, & Jukes, 1966; Newson, Daw- viduals, ages 16 to 18 years, who had been di-
son, & Everard, 1982), and because they do not agnosed as autistic in childhood. Among the
provide data on functioning in childhood, there 22 who had left school, only one had a job, and

201
202 Diagnosis and Classification

almost half were in long-term hospital care. competitively employed, but, again, jobs were
Two individuals were living at home, and five generally at a very low level and the majority
were attending day training centers. Outcome were in sheltered employment or special train-
ratings were very similar to those reported in ing programs; three had no occupation. Only
the study of Rutter. four individuals lived more or less indepen-
More than a decade later, Gillberg and dently. However, Szatmari and his colleagues
Steffenburg (1987) reported on a group of 23 (Szatmari, Bartolucci, Bremner, Bond, & Rich,
individuals age 16 or over living in Sweden. 1989) reported more positive findings for a
Only one person was found to be fully self- group of 12 men and 4 women, all diagnosed as
supporting; of the remainder, around half were high functioning (IQ above 65). At follow-up,
rated as having a “ fair ” outcome and half as they were between 17 and 31 years of age with
“poor ” or “ very poor.” a mean WAIS IQ of 92. Half had attended col-
Kobayashi, Murata, and Yashinaga (1992) lege or university, and over a third were in
used a postal survey to follow up 201 people, regular, full-time employment. Half were de-
ages 18 to 33 years, in Japan. The average scribed as being completely independent, al-
follow-up period was 15 years. Five percent though some of these still lived at home. Over
were still attending school or college; 20% were half had never formed close relationships, but a
employed, mostly in food and service indus- quarter had dated regularly or had long-term
tries. All but three of those with jobs still lived relationships, and one was married.
with their parents; one was in a group home, and Rutter and his colleagues (Howlin, Goode,
two had their own apartments; none were mar- Hutton, & Rutter, 2004; Howlin, Mawhood, &
ried. Around a quarter of the group were rated Rutter, 2000; Mawhood, Howlin, & Rutter,
as having a “good” or “ very good” outcome; the 2000) also conducted two long-term follow-up
same proportion were rated as “ fair,” and just studies of individuals first seen in childhood.
under half were rated as “poor ” or “ very poor.” The first (Howlin et al., 2000; Mawhood et al.,
A telephone survey by Ballaban-Gil, Rapin, 2000) focused on a group of 19 young men
Tuchman, and Shinnar (1996) found that among who had initially been involved in a compari-
45 adults initially diagnosed as children, more son study of children with receptive language
than half (53%) were in residential placements disorder when between 4 and 9 years of age.
and only one was living independently. Eleven At initial assessment, all had a nonverbal IQ
percent were in regular employment (all in of 70 or above, and as adults their mean
menial jobs), and a further 16% were in shel- performance IQ was in the mid 80s. Although
tered placements. Rates of behavioral difficul- the majority had improved over time, all
ties were high, and only three adults were rated showed continuing problems in communica-
as having no social deficits. tion; almost half remained socially isolated,
Even among individuals of higher IQ, out- only three lived independently, and more than
come is often very limited. Rumsey, Rapoport, two-thirds had significant difficulties associ-
and Sceery (1985) followed up 14 men between ated with obsessional or ritualistic tendencies.
18 and 39 years of age, seven of whom had orig- Only five individuals were considered to have
inally been diagnosed by Kanner, and all of a good or moderately good outcome; the re-
whom fulfilled DSM-III criteria for autism. mainder showed substantial impairments.
Despite their mean IQ being around 99, all con- A subsequent study (Howlin et al., 2004)
tinued to have marked social difficulties. Only described outcome in 61 men and 7 women (av-
one individual had friends, and only four were erage age 29 years) who had initially been diag-
in independent employment. Most of the group nosed at a mean age of 7 years. Only individuals
(71%) remained very dependent on their par- with a childhood IQ of at least 50 were included
ents or others for support, and one person was in the study, and as adults their average perfor-
in a state hospital. mance IQ was 75. By follow-up, almost one-
Venter, Lord, and Schopler (1992) described third of the group were in paid employment,
outcomes for 22 individuals age 18 years or over although jobs were mostly poorly paid and low
who had a preschool IQ of 60+ and a mean IQ level. Around a quarter had developed some
of around 90 at follow-up. Around a third were form of friendship with another person in their
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 203

own age group, but close sexual relationships themselves have an autistic spectrum disorder.
were rare and only three individuals were (or Thus, marriage rates must be far higher than
had been) married. Eight individuals had follow-up studies have previously suggested,
achieved relatively high levels of independence and much more needs to be known about pat-
as adults, but most remained very dependent on terns of functioning within “autistic” families
their families or other support services and and what variables are related to success or
few were able to live entirely alone. Overall, difficulties.
around a fifth of the group was rated as having
a “ very good” or “good” outcome, and a similar CHANGES IN OUTCOME OVER
proportion was rated as “ fair ”; almost half was RECENT YEARS
rated as having a “poor ” outcome, and 10%
were in hospital care. Many follow-up reports have attempted to sum-
These and other studies of adult functioning marize outcome among their participants, using
summarized in Table 7.1 show that many adults ratings such as “good,” “ fair,” “poor,” or “ very
continue to remain highly dependent on their poor.” However, comparisons between studies
families or other support services well into need to be treated with caution because of dif-
their late 20s and beyond. Only a minority ferences in sample selection and in the mea-
achieved totally independent living, and even sures used. Most investigations have involved
among the most able groups (e.g., Mawhood relatively small groups of subjects, diagnostic
et al., 2000; Newson et al., 1982; Rumsey et al., criteria are sometimes imprecise, and/or the
1985; Szatmari et al., 1989; Tantam, 1991; quality of data on early intellectual functioning
Venter et al., 1992), most still lived with their is poor. Overall judgments of whether outcome
parents or were in sheltered residential place- is “good,” “ fair,” or “poor ” also tend to be
ments. Outings and other social activities based on variable criteria, and these are often
tended to be organized by parents or care work- poorly defined and rarely backed up by assess-
ers, with very little initiative or choice on the ments of reliability or validity. Generally, how-
part of the people with autism themselves. As a ever, a “good” rating indicates moderate to
consequence, close friendships were rare; and high levels of independence in living and work,
Kanner himself noted that although several of with some friends/acquaintances. “Fair ” indi-
his patients had achieved relatively highly in cates need of support in work and/or daily liv-
work-related areas, their attempts to form per- ing but with some limited autonomy. “Poor ”
sonal friendships “ were much less successful.” usually means living in residential care, hospi-
In most studies, fewer than 5% of participants tal provision, or the parental home with close
had married or had long-term sexual relation- supervision.
ships, although the proportions were higher in As shown in Table 7.1, these ratings vary
the groups described by Szatmari et al. and widely between studies. “Good” ratings, for
Larsen and Mouridsen (1997). In the latter example, range from 0% to 38%, and “poor ”
study, two individuals were also noted as having ratings from 16% to over 80%. There is some
children of their own. Generally, however, few suggestion, however, that the overall outcome
follow-up studies have extended beyond the may have improved somewhat over the past 2
late 20s to early 30s; hence, information about decades, compared with that found in the
marriage and parenting is very limited. A few 1960s and 1970s. Thus, whereas the mean per-
personal accounts by individuals such as Law- centage of those rated as having a “good” out-
son (2002) or Holliday-Willey (1999) describe come in follow-up studies conducted before
the problems experienced by individuals with 1980 was around 10%, over the following 2
autism in coping with marriage and the de- decades the proportion had risen to 20%.
mands of bringing up a family, especially when “Poor ” outcome ratings declined from an aver-
the children, too, share this condition. Never- age of 65% to 46% over the same period.
theless, the fact that autism is largely inherited “Fair ” ratings remained around 25% to 30%.
(International Molecular Genetic Study of One particularly noticeable change has been in
Autism Consortium, 2001; Rutter, 2000) means the frequency of admissions to long-term hos-
that many parents of children with autism will pital care. Around 40% to 50% of individuals
TABLE 7.1 Independence and Social Outcomes in Follow-Up Studies of Adults*

204
Semi/ With Residential Hospital Some Outcome Summary (%)
Independent Parents Provision Care Married Friends
Study (N) Living (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) “Good” “Fair ” “Poor ”

Eisenberg, 1956 (63) 0 46 54 5 21 74


Creak, 1963 (100) 17 40 43
Mitler, Gillies, & Jukes, 1966 (26) 0 74 0 26 74
Lockyer & Rutter, 1970 (38) 0 18 19 53 14 25 61
Kanner, 1973 (96) 8 1 2 11
(1 child)
DeMeyer, Barton, DeMeyer, Norton, Allan, & Steele, 1973 (120) 0 42 10 16 74
Lotter, 1974 (29) 0 28 48 14 24 62
Newson, Dawson, & Everard, 1982 (93) 7 71 16 1 15 7 77 16
Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, 1985 (14) 21 64 7 7 35 35 28
Gillberg & Steffenberg, 1987 (23) 4 61 35 4 48 35
Szatmari, Bartolucci, Bremner, Bond, & Rich, 1989 (16) 33 63 12 0 29 21 38 31 31
Tantam, 1991 (46) 3 41 53 2 1
Kobayashi, Murata, & Yashinaga, 1992 (201) 1 97 2 0 27 27 46
Venter, Lord, & Schopler, 1992 (22) 21 0 0
von Knorring & Häglöf, 1994 (34) 3 9 88
Ballaban-Gil, Rapin, Tuchman, & Shinnar, 1995 (45) 2 45 53 6 ? ?
Larsen & Mouridsen, 1997 (18) 44 17 12 17 22 28 28 44
(2 with
children)
Mawhood, Howlin, & Rutter, 2000 (19) 16 32 47 5 5 21 26 74
Howlin, 2003 (68) 10 38 38 12 4 19 22 19 57

* Summary ratings based on authors’ own classification where provided. Otherwise “Good” = Moderate to high levels of independence in living and/or job; some friends/ac-
quaintances. “Fair ” = Needing support in work and/or daily living but some limited autonomy. Poor = Living in residential care or hospital provision (or parental home but
with close supervision in most activities).
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 205

in the earlier studies moved into such place- tested on nonverbal scales and a wider range of
ments as adults, but there was a marked decline subtests. Other factors, such as gender, initial
from the 1980s onward, with the mean being IQ, or presence of epilepsy were not signifi-
around 6% and, in many cases, far less. How- cantly associated with a decline in scores.
ever, despite the general trend toward the clo- A significant relationship between child-
sure of large residential institutions, for some hood IQ and adult outcome has been reported
individuals it has proved extremely difficult to in several follow-up studies (e.g., Gillberg,
find an alternative to hospital care because 1991; Howlin et al., 2002; Lockyer & Rutter,
their behavioral problems, and especially their 1969, 1970; Lotter, 1978; Rutter, Greenfield,
lack of social understanding, greatly limit their & Lockyer, 1967; Rutter & Lockyer, 1967).
ability to settle into community-based provi- Indeed, Nordin and Gillberg (1998) in their
sion (Howlin et al., 2004). review of outcome research suggest that mea-
sured IQ at the time of diagnosis is one of the
CHANGES IN COGNITIVE ABILITY best single predictors of outcome. However,
although in many cases, IQ scores appear to
Young children with autism are not a particu- show little change over the years, there are
larly easy group to assess using formal psycho- exceptions. For example, in the study by
metric measures. Their problems in verbal Howlin et al. (2004), mean scores tended to
expression and comprehension, together with remain very stable over time, but these could
impaired social understanding and motivation, conceal individual changes. Thus, around
are all potential sources of difficulty. More- one-third of the group showed a drop in per-
over, few cognitive tests are designed for chil- formance IQ, and 16% showed an improve-
dren whose development is markedly delayed ment. On verbal tests, the pattern was in the
or uneven, as occurs in autism. Indeed, the use opposite direction, with over 40% showing an
of standardized tests with children with increase and 7% a decline. The association
autism has been criticized by some practition- between childhood IQ and prognosis in adult
ers as being inappropriate and misleading. life is also a complex one (see later Predictors
Nevertheless, it has been demonstrated that if of Outcome section).
appropriate tests are used, the results can be
both valid and reliable (Clark & Rutter, 1979, EDUCATION AND EMPLOYMENT
1981) and prove remarkably stable over the
long term. Howlin et al. (2004), for example, One major factor affecting outcome in adult-
found little overall change in IQ scores from hood is the adequacy, or otherwise, of educa-
childhood to adulthood over an average period tional provision, and the importance of access
of 22 years. Overall correlations between child to appropriate education for later employment
and adult verbal IQ were also high (r = .68). and social and economic independence is
This relative stability in IQ was also noted by widely recognized. However, research in the
Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) with only 18% of field of disability indicates that although in-
their sample showing a marked change in IQ clusive education can succeed in the infant
from childhood to adolescence or adulthood. years, successful integration becomes progres-
When change did occur, this usually repre- sively more difficult to achieve as children
sented an improvement rather than a decline in grow older. Acceptance, both by mainstream
intellectual ability. Moreover, a study by Hut- peers and teachers, tends to decrease with age
ton (1998) of apparent deterioration in cogni- and the risk of rejection is particularly high
tive ability from childhood to adulthood found for children with more pervasive problems,
that the variable most predictive of decline such as those with autism (Deno, Maruyama,
was the type of test that had been used to as- Espin, & Cohen, 1990; Farrell, 1997). Sponta-
sess IQ in childhood. Individuals who as chil- neous social interactions with typically devel-
dren were assessed only on verbal tests or on oping peers do not tend to occur unless the
small numbers of subtests from nonverbal environment, teaching materials, and chil-
scales were far more likely to have shown a dren’s activities are appropriately structured
significant “deterioration” in IQ than those (Lord, 1995), and, even then, close, reciprocal
206 Diagnosis and Classification

friendships are unlikely to develop (Burack, almost all the children, whatever their intel-
Root, & Zigler, 1997). lectual level, had remained in school of some
There can be no doubt that schooling for sort for at least 10 years. Nevertheless, full in-
children with autism has improved substan- clusion in schools still appears to be the excep-
tially over recent decades. In early studies tion rather than the rule (Howlin et al., 2004;
(DeMeyer et al., 1973; Lockyer & Rutter, 1969, Mawhood et al., 2000; Venter et al., 1992),
1970; Rutter & Lockyer, 1967), the majority and many students with autism leave school
of children, even those who were of higher IQ, without any formal academic or vocational
had received less than 5 years’ schooling in qualifications. Rates of entry to college are
all. One-third of children in the Rutter and even lower, and in the majority of the out-
Lockyer studies, for example, had never at- come studies reviewed in Table 7.2, none of the
tended school. In more recent outcome studies, individuals concerned had attended college or

TABLE 7.2 Educational and Employment Outcomes in Follow-Up Studies of Adults with Autism*

College/
Age University Highest Lowest
Study (n) (Years) IQ (%) Jobs (%) Level Jobs Level Jobs

Lockyer & Rutter, 16+ X = 62 8 Factory work Unpaid shop work


1969, 1970 (38)
Kanner, 1973 (96) 22–29 7 9 Military, banking, Store/ kitchen work
chemist, accountant
Lotter, 1974 (29) 16–18 55–90 4 No information
Newson, Dawson, & X = 23 High 11 22 Few details
Everard, 1982 (93) functioning
Rumsey, Rapoport, 18–39 55–129 14 29 Librarian, cab Janitor, most in
& Sceery, 1985 (14) driver, computing sheltered
workshops
Szatmari, Bartolucci, 17–34 68–110 50 47 Librarian, teacher, Factory workshop
Bremner, Bond, & salesman
Rick,1989 (16)
Tantam, 1991 (46) X = 24 High 4 9 No information
functioning
Kobayashi, Murata, 18–33 23% > 70 2 22 Bus conductor, Industrial work
& Yashinaga, 1992 cook, mechanic
(201)
Venter, Lord, & 18+ X = 90 7 27 Bartender Rest “low level”
Schopler, 1992 (22)
Ballaban-Gil, 18+ 31% > 70 11 All “menial”
Rapin, Tuchman, &
Shinnar, 1996 (45)
Larsen & 32–43 78% > 50 0 22 Driver, office boy, Sheltered factory
Mouridsen, 1997 gardener
(18)
Mawhood, Howlin, 21–26 70–117 22 16 Lab technician Voluntary sheltered
& Rutter, 2000 (19) work
Howlin, Goode, 21+ 51–137 7 34 Scientific officer; Washing up,
Hutton, & Rutter, computing, supermarket, grave
2004 (68) accounts, digger
electronics

* Includes follow-up studies in which majority of participants are 16+ and in which specific data on further educa-
tion /employment are presented.
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 207

university or obtained a degree or similar individuals experiencing lengthy periods with-


qualification. Overall, the average proportion out paid work.
attending college was around 12% (range 0% Although the effectiveness of the supported
to 50%). The numbers obtaining university de- employment model for individuals with intel-
grees ranged from zero to 43%, with the pro- lectual disabilities is well established (Kilsby
portions being greater in groups of higher & Beyer, 1996; McCaughrin, Ellis, Rusch, &
ability (e.g., Mawhood et al., 2000; Rumsey Heal, 1993; Pozner & Hammond, 1993), it
et al., 1985; Szatmari et al., 1989). is only relatively recently that such schemes
Lack of higher education results in individ- have been extended to meet the specific needs
uals falling progressively further behind their of clients with autism. Smith, Belcher, and Juhrs
peers, but even for those who do successfully (1995) describe a wide variety of successful
complete mainstream education and go on to job placements in their Maryland support
obtain college or university qualifications, scheme. These included manufacturing jobs,
follow-up studies indicate that employment such as simple assembly-type work (25 clients);
levels in adulthood are disappointing. Indeed, backroom retail work (44 clients); printing and
for people with any form of disability, the mailing jobs (31); food services (23); ware-
chances of finding or keeping employment in house work (20); recycling and delivery (12);
the open work market are limited. It is esti- and jobs with government organizations,
mated, for example, that even individuals with mainly janitors and office clerks (15). The pro-
mild intellectual disabilities have unemploy- gram is remarkable, not only for the large num-
ment rates as high as 60% to 70%. If employ- ber of clients finding work but also because of
ment is found, job status and stability are its success in placing individuals with very
typically low (Zetlin & Murtaugh, 1990), and limited language, low intellectual ability, and
work experience is frequently very negative challenging behavior, as well as those who
(Szivos, 1990). The situation is much the same were more able. In another U.S.-based pro-
for individuals with autism, including those gram, Keel, Mesibov, and Woods (1997), eval-
who are intellectually very able. Even if they uating job outcomes for 100 clients enrolled in
are successful in getting through the interview the TEACCH program (Treatment and Educa-
process (a major stumbling block for many), tion of Autistic and Related Communication-
jobs tend to be poorly paid and/or to end pre- handicapped Children) found that almost all
maturely—often because of difficulties re- were in work of some kind. Sixty-nine were in
lated to social competence. individual placements, 20 worked in “enclaves”
Table 7.2 summarizes data on rates of em- (i.e., small groups with a job coach in one set-
ployment in adult follow-up studies. Although, ting), and 7 of the least able clients worked in
over the years, there appears to have been some “mobile crews” providing housecleaning ser-
increase in the proportion of individuals with vices. Jobs were mostly in the food service
autism who do find work, the numbers are still field, but around a quarter involved clerical or
relatively low, with the average percent in technical posts.
studies post-1980 around only 24%. Even in the However, although often highly successful,
studies with a focus on high-functioning indi- the focus of such schemes has tended to be on
viduals, the highest proportion reported in work relatively low-level jobs, and few programs
is 47% (Szatmari et al., 1989), and in other have been specifically designed to meet the
studies (e.g., Mawhood et al., 2000) the figure needs of more intellectually able adults with
is well below 20%. Moreover, although some autism, despite their considerable potential.
individuals were reported to have obtained Nevertheless, specialist support for this par-
high-level, well-paid, and responsible jobs, the ticular group can prove highly effective. Maw-
majority had rather menial positions, such as hood and Howlin (1999) evaluated a supported
kitchen hands, unskilled factory workers, or employment program for 30 high-functioning
backroom supermarket staff. In addition, jobs individuals with autistic spectrum disorders
had often been procured through the personal living in London. All had a formal diagnosis
contacts of families rather than through the nor- of autism or Asperger syndrome, a WAIS IQ
mal channels (Howlin & Goode, 1998). Em- score of 70 or above, and had been actively
ployment stability, too, was poor, with many seeking work for some time. Twenty percent
208 Diagnosis and Classification

had a university degree, and two-thirds had small group studies, and there are no system-
other academic or vocational qualifications. atic studies of incidence. However, although
Their work outcomes were compared to those estimates vary (ranging from 4% to 58%;
of a nonsupported, matched comparison group. Lainhart, 1999), by far the most prevalent psy-
During the course of a 2-year pilot program, chiatric disturbances reported are those re-
more than two-thirds of the supported group lated to anxiety and depression. As early as
obtained paid employment, compared with only 1970, Rutter noted the risk of depressive
one-quarter of the control group. Moreover, in episodes occurring in adolescents or older in-
the supported group, the majority of jobs were dividuals with autism, and subsequent reviews
clerical or administrative in nature while only have reported a high frequency of affective
one individual in the comparison group ob- disorders both among individuals with autism
tained a job at this level. Earnings were signif- (Lainhart & Folstein, 1994) and within their
icantly higher in the supported group, and families (Bolton, Pickles, Murphy, & Rutter,
there was a high level of satisfaction with the 1998; Smalley, McCracken, & Tanguay, 1995).
scheme, both among employers and the people Abramson and colleagues (1992) suggest that
with autism themselves. around one-third of people with autism suffer
In the course of the following 5 to 6 years, from affective disorders, and high rates of de-
more than 90 positions have been found, with pression are found among high-functioning in-
more than 80% being in computing, account- dividuals, as well as those of lower ability.
ing, or administration. Other jobs have in- Thus, Tantam (1991), in his study of 85 adults
cluded secretarial, nursery, film processing, with Asperger syndrome, noted that 2% had a
and consultancy work; jobs in science and gov- depressive psychosis and 5% had a bipolar dis-
ernment departments; and positions in house- order. A further 13% suffered from nonpsy-
keeping, sales, warehouses, and telephone and chotic depression and/or anxiety. In the study
postal services. Moreover, at a time when tem- of Rumsey et al. (1985) of 14 relatively high-
porary work contracts are becoming the norm, functioning individuals, generalized anxiety
more than 50% of these placements were per- problems were found in half the sample. Simi-
manent; none have yet been dismissed from lar figures were reported by Wing (1981), who
their job, and several employers have offered found that around a quarter of her group of 18
work to more than one autistic client. individuals with Asperger syndrome showed
Schemes such as this demonstrate conclu- signs of an affective disorder. Bipolar af-
sively how far the employment situation for fective disorders or mania without depression
people with autism can be improved by means tends to be reported less frequently than de-
of specialist help. However, in the absence of pression alone, although Wozniak et al. (1997)
such support, it is all too easy for individuals found that up to 21% of their autism /pervasive
to drift into a life of isolation and loneliness. developmental disorder (PDD) sample had
Without work, opportunities to meet with been diagnosed as having mania.
peers or make friends will be severely re- In reviewing case reports of psychiatric dis-
stricted, and without money most individuals order in individuals with autism and related
are obliged to remain living with their parents disorders (Howlin, 2004), 35 different studies
(or in some form of state-provided residential involving 200 patients age 14 years and older
care). Leisure activities are limited, and fail- were identified. Eighty-six cases were diag-
ure to find suitable work, sometimes despite nosed with autism or PDD; 114 were described
many years of trying, also results in frustra- as having Asperger syndrome or were within
tion, loss of self-esteem, and, for some, entry the high-functioning range of the autistic spec-
into a cycle of anxiety and depression or other trum. As shown in Figure 7.1, by far, the most
psychiatric disturbance (Howlin, 2004). frequent psychiatric diagnoses given (in 56%
of cases) related to depression or anxiety dis-
PSYCHIATRIC DISORDERS orders (including major and minor depression,
mood disorders or bipolar affective disorder,
On the whole, data on mental health problems depression plus anxiety, severe social with-
in autism are based on clinical case reports or drawal, and attempted suicide). Mania alone
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 209

60

50
Percentage of Cases

40

30

20

10

0
A
A

O
Ca

Su
D

M
Bi
To

Sc
Ps
ffe
nx

th
ep

an
po

hi

ic
ta

ta
yc

e
ie
re

ct

id
l

to
zo
ia

r
la

ho
D

ty

iv
ss

e
ni
r

ph
ep

tic
e
io

a
re
re

ni
ss

iso

a
iv

r
eT

de
r,
yp

N
e

O
S
HFA/Asp (N = 114) Aut/PDD (N = 86)

Figure 7.1 Psychiatric diagnoses reported in individuals with autism and those with Asperger syndrome or
high-functioning autism.

occurred much less frequently, in under 3% of differentiated between autism and schizophre-
the total. The relatively high number of cases nia. That is not to say that autism and schizo-
of catatonia reported largely reflects the spe- phrenia never coexist; and, as the present review
cial interest in this disorder of Lorna Wing indicates, there is a number of case reports
and her colleagues (Wing and Shah, 2000). on the comorbidity of the two conditions
This also illustrates how case reports cannot (Clarke, Baxter, Perry, & Prasher, 1999; Petty,
be used to determine the prevalence of psychi- Ornitz, Michelman, & Zimmerman, 1984;
atric illness since the researchers’ particular Sverd, Montero, & Gurevich, 1993). However,
area of expertise or interest will lead to sys- larger scale studies of individuals with autism
tematic bias in the types of cases seen. How- have failed to find any evidence of increased
ever, the figure does provide a rough guide to rates of schizophrenia (Chung, Luk, & Lee,
the relative frequency of different disorders, 1990; Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-Mikhail, & Ghazi-
and data from this and other reviews consis- uddin, 1998). None of the cases followed up
tently suggest that while depressive types of by Kanner, over a period of 40 years, was re-
disorder are relatively common, schizophrenic ported as showing positive psychiatric symp-
illness is much less prevalent. toms (delusions or hallucinations), and Volkmar
and Cohen (1991) found only one individual
Schizophrenia in ASD with an unequivocal diagnosis of schizophrenia
in a sample of 163 cases.
Understanding of the links between autism and Schizophrenia also appears to be un-
schizophrenia has come a long way since Szurek common among more able individuals or those
and Berlin (1956) suggested that clinically with Asperger syndrome. Asperger (1944)
there was no reason to make any sharp distinc- noted that only one of his 200 cases developed
tions between psychosis, autism, atypical devel- schizophrenia, and Wing (1981), in a study of
opment, or schizophrenia. Rutter (1972) was 18 individuals with Asperger syndrome, de-
among the first of many to highlight a number scribes one with an unconfirmed diagnosis of
of crucial variables relating to onset, course, schizophrenia. Rumsey et al. (1985), in their
prognosis, treatment, and family history that detailed psychiatric study, found no evidence
210 Diagnosis and Classification

of schizophrenia. None of the relatively able peared to offer him a means of working
subjects in the studies of Mawhood and col- through difficult situations, and if he became
leagues (2000) or Howlin et al. (2004) had de- particularly agitated, his parents would send
veloped a schizophrenic illness, and only one him off to “ talk to his voices.”
individual in a similar group studied by Szat- A number of other authors have described
mari et al. (1989) had been treated for chronic cases of delusional disorder, various unspeci-
schizophrenia. Tantam (1991) diagnosed three fied psychoses (occasionally associated with
cases of schizophrenia among 83 individuals epilepsy), paranoid ideation, catatonia, and
with Asperger syndrome, but these were all hallucinations (Clarke, Littlejohns, Corbett, &
psychiatric referrals. Joseph, 1989; Ghaziuddin et al., 1992; Rumsey
Volkmar and Cohen (1991) have concluded et al., 1985; Szatmari et al., 1989; Tantam,
that the frequency of schizophrenia in indi- 1991, 2000; Wing & Shah, 2000). Obsessive-
viduals with autism is around 0.6% (roughly compulsive disorders have also been reported
comparable to that in the general population) although it can often prove very difficult to
and, thus, the rate of comorbidity of the two distinguish between these and the ritualistic
conditions is no greater than would be ex- and stereotyped behaviors that are characteris-
pected by chance. Similar findings are tic of autism (Szatmari et al., 1989).
reached in the more recent overview by Lain- As noted in the earliest descriptions of
hart (1999). Thus, although some studies have autism (Kanner, 1971; Lockyer & Rutter,
suggested that there may be an excess of 1970; Lotter, 1966) epilepsy is another com-
schizophrenia among individuals with As- plicating psychiatric factor, and it occurs in
perger syndrome (Wolff & McGuire, 1995), around 25% to 30% of cases (Lord & Bailey,
there is little evidence for such claims (Wing, 2002). The risk of developing fits appears to
1986). be higher among those who are profoundly re-
tarded, but there does not seem to be a marked
Other Psychotic Conditions difference between groups of normal IQ and
those with mild-moderate retardation. Eleven
Although the occurrence of first-rank schizo- (16%) of the adults with an IQ of 50 or above
phrenic symptoms is relatively unusual, there assessed by Howlin et al. (2004) had at least
are reports of individuals who show iso- one fit. In four cases, IQ was between 50 and
lated psychotic symptoms, including delusional 69; in seven, IQ was in the normal range. Oc-
thoughts. Tantam (1991) suggests that the casionally, the onset of epilepsy is associated
delusional content is often linked with autistic- with marked behavioral changes and regres-
type preoccupations. For example, one young sion in adolescence (see later discussion), al-
man described by Wing (1981) could not be de- though this is by no means always the case.
terred from his conviction that some day Bat-
man was going to come and take him away as ARE HIGHER FUNCTIONING
his assistant. Ghaziuddin, Tsai, and Ghaziud- INDIVIDUALS AT GREATER RISK
din (1992) describe another who was unduly OF PSYCHIATRIC DISTURBANCE?
concerned about the ozone layer and believed
the air in Michigan was not pure enough to It is often suggested that the risk of psychiatric
breathe. One of my patients was threatening to disturbance, especially related to depres-
take revenge on the U.S. president and the U.K. sion and anxiety, is particularly great among
prime minister because he believed the Ameri- higher functioning individuals with autism or
can and British Air Control authorities had those with Asperger syndrome. There are sev-
conspired to prevent him from qualifying as an eral reasons for this view. First, because of
airline pilot. Another young man since child- these individuals’ relatively good cognitive
hood had “ voices” to whom he could talk when ability and apparently competent use of lan-
he was particularly angry or upset. He believed guage, they frequently fail to receive the level
firmly that the voices were real, but they did of support they need. Second, despite their
not provoke any distress or make him do things superficially good expressive skills, many
that he did not wish to do. Instead, they ap- have extensive linguistic and comprehension
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 211

difficulties (especially involving abstract or too, into ways of improving the identification
complex concepts), and their understanding of and treatment of psychiatric disorders because
the more subtle aspects of social interaction many clinicians working in adult psychiatric
is often profoundly limited. Such deficits services often know relatively little about peo-
frequently prove an almost insurmountable ple with autism. Thus, the obsessionality, flat-
barrier to social integration. Third, others’ ex- tened affect, poor eye contact, unusual body
pectations of their social and academic poten- movements, and echoed speech that are typical
tial are often unrealistically high, and there of autism may be misinterpreted as symptoms
may be constant pressure for them to “ fit into of psychosis (Volkmar & Cohen, 1991). Im-
normal society.” Finally, their own awareness poverished language (Howlin, 2004), literal in-
of their difficulties and the extent to which terpretation of questions (Wing, 1986), and
they are isolated from others can result in concrete thinking (Dykens, Volkmar, & Glick,
great sadness and very low self-esteem. All 1991) are all additional sources of confusion.
these factors can place enormous pressures on For example, if asked, “Do you ever hear
the individuals concerned and sometimes re- voices when there is no one in the room?” indi-
sult in intolerable levels of anxiety and stress. viduals with autism are almost certain to reply
Nevertheless, there is little evidence of differ- in the affirmative, since they can obviously
ential rates of mental health problems among hear voices coming from many other sources.
subgroups within the autistic spectrum. On It is also important that isolated “symptoms,”
the whole, the findings from the case studies such as the unusual ideas or fixations noted
summarized in Figure 7.1, did not indicate a earlier, be kept in perspective. For example,
higher incidence of such problems in higher when a psychiatric nurse heard of the voices
functioning compared to less able individuals. experienced by the young man described ear-
And, although the former group were some- lier, his parents were warned that he was seri-
what more likely to be diagnosed as having ously mentally ill. Their attempts to persuade
mania or anxiety disorders, this may be be- medical staff that this was not a crisis, but typ-
cause it is much more difficult to diagnose ical behavior, were dismissed as collusion and
these conditions in individuals who have little denial, and it was with great difficulty that
ability to describe their moods and feelings ef- they prevented his being compulsorily detained
fectively (Sturmey, 1998). In their case, the in a psychiatric hospital. This failure to under-
problems may simply be labeled as unspecified stand the characteristic communication and so-
“mood disorders” (see Figure 7.1). cial difficulties associated with autism can
However, many of the clinical case studies give rise to potentially serious misunderstand-
reviewed did not distinguish clearly between ings and misdiagnosis, even in the case of rela-
high-functioning and low-functioning individ- tively able individuals. For those with little or
uals or between those with autism and Asperger no speech, the risks of an incorrect diagnosis
syndrome. Even if separate categories were (or failure to diagnose when problems do exist)
used, diagnostic criteria were rarely specified, are even higher.
and very few reports provided information on Finding the appropriate treatment for peo-
the IQ levels of the individuals concerned. ple with autism who develop additional psy-
Szatmari et al. (1989), in one of the few well- chiatric disorders can also prove difficult.
controlled studies in this area, failed to find Clinical experience suggests that delays in di-
any marked differences in rates of psychiatric agnosis and treatment are particularly undesir-
disturbance between adults with a diagnosis able within this group because behavior
of Asperger syndrome and those with high- patterns that are established during the course
functioning autism although the autism group of the illness (e.g., disturbed waking and
tended to show more bizarre preoccupations. sleeping patterns) can then be very difficult to
In summary, crucial data on the prevalence alter, even when the patient’s condition gener-
and nature of mental health problems across ally has improved (Howlin, 2004). Medication
the autistic spectrum are still lacking, and can be helpful (McDougle, 1997) but rarely
there is a particular need for epidemiological works in isolation. There is little evidence for
studies in this area. Better research is needed, the effectiveness of psychoanalytically based
212 Diagnosis and Classification

interventions (Campbell, Schopler, Cueva, & pneumonia, and complications arising from
Hallin, 1996). Individual psychotherapy or long-term psychotropic medication (Ballaban-
counseling may be beneficial for higher func- Gil et al., 1996).
tioning people, but clinical experience suggests The largest single study of mortality rates
that these approaches must be combined with (Shavelle et al., 2001), based on more than
direct practical advice on how to deal with 13,000 individuals with autism registered on
problems. If appropriately adapted, cognitive the California Department of Developmental
behavioral strategies seem to be of potential Services database, concluded that average
benefit (Hare, Jones, & Paine, 1999; Stoddart, mortality rates were more than double those of
1999) although there is very little systematic the general population. In individuals with
research in this area, and even single case stud- mild mental retardation or those of normal
ies are rare. IQ, deaths from seizures, nervous system dys-
function, drowning, and suffocation were
MORTALITY AND CAUSES OF DEATH three times more common than in nondisabled
controls. Among individuals with more severe
Long-term follow-up studies of children and mental retardation, there was a threefold in-
adolescents with psychiatric disorders have crease in deaths from all causes (other than
demonstrated above-average mortality rates cancer).
compared to age- and sex-matched controls, es- Suicide as a cause of death has been noted
pecially concerning death from “ unnatural in a number of studies. Among the “schizoid”
causes” (suicide, accidents, etc.; Kuperman, individuals (several of whom appeared to
Black, & Burns, 1988; Larsen, Dahl, & Hallum, meet criteria for Asperger syndrome) studied
1990; Östman, 1991; Strauss, 1996). Research by Wolff and McGuire (1995), 10 of 17
also suggests that death rates are higher in indi- women and 17 of 32 men had attempted sui-
viduals with autistic spectrum disorders (Gill- cide. Tantam (1991) described the case of one
berg & Coleman, 2000; Shavelle, Strauss, & man who threw himself into the river Thames
Picket, 2001). Isager, Mouridsen, and Rich because the government refused to abolish
(1999) followed 207 cases with autism or British Summer Time, and he believed that
autism-like conditions over a 24-year period watches were damaged by the necessity of
and found that seven individuals had died, giv- being altered twice a year. In Wing’s group of
ing a crude mortality rate of 3.4%—approxi- 18 individuals with Asperger syndrome, three
mately double the expected rate. Mortality was had attempted suicide although, fortunately,
highest in those with severe-profound learning their attempts had not been successful. One
disabilities or those of higher intelligence. In young man, who had become very distressed
the former group (n = 4), all of whom were by minor changes in his work routine, tried to
in residential institutions, two deaths were at- drown himself but failed because he was a
tributed to choking while unsupervised, one to good swimmer. When he tried to strangle
pneumonia, and one to meningitis. In the more himself, the attempt also failed because, as he
able group (n = 3), who lived either indepen- said, “I am not a very practical person.”
dently or with parents, one death followed an Nordin and Gillberg (1998) have suggested
epileptic attack, and two were due to drug over- that higher death rates of individuals with autis-
doses (one deliberate; the other probably acci- tic spectrum disorders may be due to the associ-
dental). Occasional deaths have been reported, ation of autism with severe mental retardation
too, in general follow-up studies of individuals and epilepsy. However, the preceding examples
with autism (Lotter, 1978). Causes of death indicate that many other causes are also operat-
include car accidents (Kanner, 1973; Larsen & ing. The number of deaths related to the in-
Mouridsen, 1997); encephalopathy, self-injury, adequate medical and physical care of individu-
nephritic syndrome, and asthma (Kobayashi als living in institutions is a particular cause of
et al., 1992); unrecognized volvulus (in a concern, and awareness of the importance of
woman in a long-term psychiatric institution, basic health care could well help to reduce
Larsen & Mouridsen, 1997); status epilepticus deaths within this group. Better understanding
(Howlin et al., 2004); and cases of drowning, of the difficulties that lead some young people
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 213

to attempt suicide could also avoid unnecessary for families to deal with. Lockyer and Rutter
loss of life. (1970), for example, noted that five individuals
(out of 64) in his follow-up studies showed a
ARE THERE DIFFERENCES IN marked deterioration in their communication,
OUTCOME BETWEEN together with progressive inertia, and general
INDIVIDUALS WITH AUTISM AND cognitive decline. Three of these cases had also
ASPERGER SYNDROME? developed epilepsy. Gillberg and Steffenburg
(1987) reported that around a third of their
The issue of whether autism and Asperger syn- sample of 23 autistic individuals presented with
drome are different conditions (albeit part of a temporary (1 to 2 years) aggravation of symp-
the same spectrum of disorders) has been toms, such as hyperactivity, aggressiveness, de-
a source of continuing debate over recent structiveness, and ritualistic behaviors. In
years (cf. Klin, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000; another five cases, the symptoms had persisted,
Schopler, Mesibov, & Kunce, 1998). However, resulting in continuing deterioration, increased
when IQ is adequately controlled for, compar- inertia, loss of language skills, and slow intel-
isons between the two groups have failed to lectual decline. This pattern was more likely to
find any consistent evidence of major group occur in females than males. Von Knorring and
differences in rates of social, emotional, and Häglöf (1993) also noted that of the four indi-
psychiatric problems; current symptomatol- viduals in their sample of 34 who showed a
ogy; motor clumsiness; or neuropsychological “mildly deteriorating course,” three were
profiles (see Howlin, 2003; Macintosh & Dis- women. Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) noted that
sanayake, 2004, for reviews). Moreover, any ratings of problem behaviors had increased in
differences that may be found in early child- almost 50% of their adult sample, although the
hood tend to diminish with age (Gilchrist, nature of these is not defined. In the Japanese
Green, Cox, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 2001; follow-up of 201 young adults, Kobayashi and
Howlin, 2003; Ozonoff, South, & Miller, 2000; his colleagues (1992) found that 31% showed a
Szatmari, Archer, Fisman, Streiner, & Wilson, worsening of symptoms, mainly after the age of
1995). On the basis of current research 10 years, but there was no difference in the pro-
evidence, there is little to support the view portions of males and females who experienced
that Asperger syndrome and high-functioning a loss of skills. Larsen and Mouridsen (1997),
autism are essentially different conditions. in a comparative study of autism and Asperger
Certainly, there is no justification for differ- syndrome, reported that three of the nine cases
entiating between the groups in terms of ac- with Asperger syndrome and two of the nine
cess to support services. Instead, it should be with autism had shown deterioration, mostly
recognized that for all high-functioning indi- occurring in late puberty. In both of these latter
viduals with an autistic disorder, there is a studies, the pattern of deterioration described
need for much improved services throughout was very similar to that outlined by Rutter and
childhood and adulthood if the long-term out- Gillberg and Steffenburg.
come is to be significantly enhanced. In one of the very few systematic investiga-
tions of deterioration over time, Hutton (1998)
HOW COMMON IS DETERIORATION examined data on the emergence of problems
IN ADULTHOOD? in adulthood for 125 individuals. Over a third
were reported to have developed new be-
The transition to adulthood can be a time of havioral or psychiatric difficulties including
upheaval and difficulties for many young psychosis, obsessive-compulsive disorder,
people and their families. It is not surprising, anxiety, depression, tics, social withdrawal,
therefore, that parents of children with autism phobias, and aggression. The average age
approach this life stage with considerable trepi- when these symptoms developed was 26
dation and anxiety. In a number of long-term years, with most people developing symptoms
studies, there have been accounts of an increase prior to the age of 30. “Periodicity,” that is,
in disruptive behaviors in adolescence, and episodes of disturbance occurring at fairly
these can undoubtedly prove very difficult regular and frequent intervals, was noted in
214 Diagnosis and Classification

eight individuals. The increase in problems of to adulthood. Similar improvements have been
this nature was not associated with epilepsy, reported in individuals with severe learning
cognitive decline, or residential placement. disabilities. Thus, Beadle-Brown, Murphy,
However, women were more likely to show an Wing, Shah, and Holmes (2000) reported
increase in problems than men, and individuals changes in scores on the Handicaps, Behaviours
with a lower verbal IQ in childhood were also and Skills schedule (HBS; Wing & Gould,
at greater risk of developing new problems in 1978) for 146 young adults with severe learning
adulthood. Marked deterioration in cognitive disabilities and/or autism over a period of 12
abilities occurred mostly among individuals in years (age at initial assessment 2 to 18 years;
long-term hospital placements. age at follow-up, 13 to 30 years). Although
Although it is clear that some individuals there was no marked change in IQ, self-care
with autism do show an increase in problems as skills (toileting, feeding, grooming, washing,
they grow older, in many studies tracing prog- dressing, etc.) had improved significantly, and
ress from childhood to adulthood, the overrid- there had also been progress in certain areas re-
ing picture is one of improvement over time. lated to educational achievement (e.g., reading,
This was reported in the early follow-up studies writing, numbers, money, and time). There
of Rutter and his group and by Kanner himself, were fewer significant changes in communica-
who noted that for some individuals, particu- tion skills as measured by the HBS although
larly those who become more aware both expressive and receptive scores on the
of their difficulties, mid-adolescence was often Reynell Developmental Language scale had in-
a period of “remarkable improvement and creased significantly. Improvements were re-
change” (Kanner, 1973). Although a third of lated to initial IQ level, with those individuals
the individuals in the Kobayashi et al. (1992) with an IQ below 55 (or who were untestable)
study had shown some increase in problems as children showing less improvement than
during adolescence, over 40% were rated as those with an IQ of 55 or above.
showing marked improvement, generally be- Follow-up studies with a focus on more able
tween 10 and 15 years. Even in the Ballaban- individuals have also documented steady im-
Gil study (1996), where increases in ratings of provements over time. For example, in the stud-
behavioral disturbance were higher than in ies of Mawhood and her colleagues (Howlin
other groups, 16% had improved, and 35% et al., 2000; Mawhood et al., 2000), of 19 young
had shown no deterioration in behavior from men followed up from 7 to 23 years of age, ver-
childhood to adulthood. Many other studies, bal ability on formal IQ tests had increased
both retrospective and prospective, indicate significantly, and in terms of general social
that change over time is more likely to be posi- competence, almost one-third of the group had
tive, rather than negative. Studies using stan- moved from a rating of “poor ” functioning in
dardized assessment instruments such as the childhood to a “good” rating as adults. There
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; was relatively little change, however, in ratings
Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) or the of friendship quality.
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- In summary, while it is evident that skills
Generic (ADOS-G; Lord et al., 2000) have may be lost or problem behaviors increase in
found the severity and frequency of many adolescence or early adulthood, it is also es-
symptoms decrease significantly with time sential to get the picture into perspective.
(Gilchrist et al., 2001; Howlin, 2002; Piven, Conclusions about “improvement ” or “deterio-
Harper, Palmer, & Arndt, 1996). In a study of ration” may depend on the particular measures
more than 400 individuals with autism from 10 used, and whereas individuals may fail to
to 53 years of age, Seltzer et al. (2002) found make progress in certain areas (e.g., in the
clear evidence of improvement on ADI-R scores ability to form close friendships), other skills,
from childhood to adolescence and adulthood. notably those related to communication, may
Verbal and nonverbal communication had im- show positive and significant change. The
proved, as had scores on the Reciprocal Social numbers of adults who show marked deterio-
Interaction domain. Scores on all the items in ration in all aspects of their functioning are,
the Restricted, Repetitive Behaviors and Inter- fortunately, very small and overall regression
ests Domain had also decreased from childhood appears to be the exception, not the rule.
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 215

PREDICTORS OF OUTCOME associated with a poorer outcome but, again,


epilepsy is more likely to occur in individuals
The variability in outcome among individuals with more severe cognitive impairments. So-
with autism has been noted since the very earli- cioeconomic factors and ratings of family ade-
est follow-up studies of Eisenberg and Kanner quacy have also been correlated with prognosis
(Eisenberg, 1956; Kanner & Eisenberg, 1956), in some studies (DeMeyer et al., 1973; Lotter,
and there have been many attempts to try to iso- 1974a, 1974b), but there is little evidence of a
late the variables that best predict later func- direct causal relationship between an impover-
tioning. As noted earlier, educational placement ished or disruptive family background and later
can have a major influence on outcome; and outcome, although, as with any other condition,
Kanner, in his follow-up (1973), noted that disruption at home may well result in an in-
lack of appropriate education was highly dam- crease in problems generally.
aging. Subsequent studies (e.g., Lockyer & Rut- The two factors that have been consistently
ter, 1969, 1970; Lotter, 1974a, 1974b; Rutter, associated with later prognosis are early lan-
Greenfeld, & Lockyer, 1967; Rutter & Lock- guage development and IQ. Very few children
yer, 1967) also noted the association between who have not developed some useful speech by
years of schooling and later outcome. The the age of 5 to 6 years are reported to have a
most positive outcomes are generally reported positive outcome, although occasionally older
for individuals who have attended mainstream children may develop relatively good commu-
schools, but since outcomes are directly af- nication skills. The relationship between long-
fected by pupils’ linguistic and cognitive levels, term outcome and cognitive ability in
the influence of schooling, per se, on long-term childhood has also been noted in many follow-
functioning remains obscure. up studies (Gillberg & Steffenburg, 1987;
The relationship between the severity of Lockyer & Rutter, 1969, 1970; Lotter, 1974a,
autistic symptomatology in early childhood 1974b; Rutter, Greenfeld, & Lockyer, 1967;
and later outcome is also unclear. Rutter and Rutter & Lockyer, 1967). Thus, individuals
colleagues (Lockyer & Rutter, 1969, 1970; who were either untestable as children or who
Rutter & Lockyer, 1967) found no significant had nonverbal IQ scores below 50 were almost
correlation between individual symptoms in invariably reported as remaining highly de-
childhood (other than lack of speech) and pendent. However, more recent studies suggest
adult outcome, although there was a significant that a minimum childhood IQ of 70 is neces-
relationship with the total number of major sary for a positive outcome in adulthood.
symptoms rated. DeMeyer et al. (1973) also Howlin et al. (2004) found that on virtually
reported a relationship between overall sever- every adult measure (academic attainments,
ity of autistic symptoms and later progress. In communication skills, reading and spelling,
contrast, Lord and Venter (1992) found no employment status, social independence), in-
association between prognosis and total num- dividuals with a childhood IQ below 70 were
ber of early symptoms as rated on the ADI. Of significantly more impaired than those with an
greater predictive value were the degree of initial IQ of 70+. Only one individual with an
language abnormality and the level of disrup- IQ between 50 and 69 obtained a “good” out-
tion caused by stereotyped and repetitive come rating in adulthood. Nevertheless, even
behaviors. among the 45 individuals in this study with an
The possible impact of many other variables initial IQ above 70, outcome was very mixed.
remains uncertain. In almost every follow-up Thus, although almost one-third of this sub-
study in which women have been involved group were rated as having a “good” or “ very
(many studies are exclusively male), outcome good” outcome, 22% were rated as only “ fair ”
has been poorer for females than males. How- and 44% obtained ratings of “poor ” or “ very
ever, the number of women participants has poor.” Moreover, those individuals with an IQ
generally been very small and the differences above 100 did no better as a group than those
found rarely reach significance; the tendency with an IQ in the 70 to 99 range. Indeed, sev-
for females to be of lower IQ also complicates eral individuals in this lower range achieved
the issue (Lord & Schopler, 1985). In some considerably more highly as adults than many
studies, the presence of epilepsy has been with a childhood IQ of above 100.
216 Diagnosis and Classification

Childhood performance on nonverbal tests have little information on what is associated


of intelligence, while being a relatively good with such improvement.
predictor of outcome, is by no means a perfect To some extent, it may prove easier to iden-
one, and Lord and Bailey (2002) have proposed tify correlates of “poor ” outcome than the
that childhood verbal IQ is a far more reliable variables predictive of good prognosis. In the
indicator of later functioning. However, in the Howlin et al. study, as already noted, most in-
Howlin et al. study, although correlations be- dividuals with an initial performance IQ
tween child and adult verbal IQ were highly below 70 remained highly dependent as
significant, there was a sizable subgroup of adults. Moreover, no one with a childhood
individuals who, despite being unable to score performance IQ below 70 and a verbal IQ
at all on verbal tests when younger, subse- below 30 achieved even a “ fair ” rating in
quently made considerable improvement in this adulthood, and only one individual with a per-
area. Over a third of individuals who were formance IQ below 70 coupled with a verbal
“ untestable” on verbal measures initially ob- IQ below 50 did so.
tained a verbal IQ equivalent of at least 70 at Identifying the reasons that some individu-
follow-up, and several of these children were als make significant improvements in their
subsequently rated as having a “good” or “ very general levels of functioning over time while
good” outcome as adults. In the case of other others show little or no change has major im-
children, who were able to obtain a verbal IQ plications for our understanding of autism and
score when first assessed, the relationship with of the factors influencing the trajectory from
adult outcome was very variable. While a third childhood to adulthood. It may be, as Kanner
of those who scored above 50 on verbal IQ postulated, that the presence of additional
tests as children obtained outcome ratings of skills or interests (e.g., specialized knowledge
“good” or “ very good” in adulthood; one-third in particular areas or competence in mathe-
were rated as “ fair ” and a further third as matics, music, or computing), which allow in-
having a “poor ” or “ very poor ” outcome. Even dividuals to find their own “niche” in life and
among the few children who scored above 70 thus enable them to be more easily integrated
on a verbal IQ test initially, less than half were into society, is of crucial importance. Alterna-
rated as having a “good”/“ very good” outcome tively, the ability to function adequately in
as adults. Thus, again, although statistically adult life may depend as much on the
there is a positive correlation between early degree of support offered ( by families, educa-
verbal IQ and later prognosis, from an individ- tional, employment, and social services) as
ual, clinical perspective, this variable has only much as basic intelligence (Lord & Venter,
limited predictive value. 1992; Mawhood & Howlin, 1999).
Lord and Bailey (2002) have also suggested
that the presence of useful speech by age 5 is CONCLUSION
highly predictive of later outcome. Certainly,
for many young children it is much easier to Although admissions to hospital care have
obtain information of this kind than to obtain fallen and expectations about the future for
a verbal IQ score, although there may be some people with disabilities generally have risen
problems of recall if interviewing parents over the years, dedicated services for adults
of older individuals. However, in the Howlin with autism would not seem to have kept pace
et al. (2004) study, even this variable was only with the growth in specialist provision for
weakly associated with adult outcome. Over children with this disorder. Overall, it is evi-
40% of children who had little or no language dent that the majority of individuals with
when first diagnosed had subsequently devel- autism, regardless of their intellectual level,
oped useful language, and, the higher their continue to experience many problems in adult
linguistic levels as adults, the more likely life. However, it is also clear that outcome can
were they to do well on a range of other out- depend crucially on the degree and appropri-
come measures. Other research has pointed to ateness of support that is provided beyond the
the impact that improvements in language school years and into adulthood. Thus, al-
may have on the developmental trajectory of though the focus of much recent research has
children with autism (Szatmari, 2000), but we been on the importance of early intervention
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 217

programs (National Research Council, 2001), Clarke, D. J., Baxter, M., Perry, D., & Prasher, V.
true social inclusion will only be possible if (1999). The diagnosis of affective and psy-
the long-term needs of adults, as well as chil- chotic disorders in adults with autism. Autism:
dren with autism, are fully recognized and ad- International Journal of Research and Practice,
equately supported. 3, 149–164.
Clarke, D. J., Littlejohns, C. S., Corbett, J. A., &
Joseph, S. (1989). Pervasive developmental
REFERENCES disorders and psychoses in adult life. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 692–699.
Abramson, R. K., Wright, H. H., Cuccara, M. L., Creak, M. (1963). Childhood psychosis: A review
Lawrence, L. G., Babb, S., Pencarinha, D., of a 100 cases. British Journal of Psychiatry,
et al. (1992). Biological liability in families 109, 84–89.
with autism. Journal of the American Acad- DeMeyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMeyer, W. E., Norton,
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, J. A., Allan, J., & Steele, R. (1973). Prognosis in
370–371. autism: A follow-up study. Journal of Autism
Asperger, H. (1944). Autistic psychopathy in child- and Childhood Schizophrenia, 3, 199–246.
hood (U. Frith, Trans.). In U. Frith (Ed.), Deno, S., Maruyama, G., Espin, G., & Cohen, C.
Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 37–92). (1990). Educating students with mild disabili-
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University ties in general education classrooms: Min-
Press. nesota alternatives. Exceptional Children, 572,
Ballaban-Gil, K., Rapin, I., Tuchman, R., & Shin- 150–161.
nar, S. (1996). Longitudinal examination of Dykens, E., Volkmar, F. R., & Glick, M. (1991).
the behavioral, language, and social changes in Thought disorder in high functioning autistic
a population of adolescents and young adults adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental
with autistic disorder. Pediatric Neurology, 15, Disorders, 21, 303–314.
217–223. Eisenberg, L. (1956). The autistic child in adoles-
Beadle-Brown, J., Murphy, G., Wing, L., Shah, A., cence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 112,
& Holmes, N. (2000). Changes in skills for 607–612.
people with intellectual disability: A follow- Farrell, P. (1997). The integration of children with
up of the Camberwell Cohort. Journal of Intel- severe learning difficulties: A review of the
lectual Disability Research, 44, 12–24. recent literature. Journal of Applied Research
Bolton, P., Pickles, A., Murphy, M., & Rutter, M. in Intellectual Disabilities, 10, 1–14.
(1998). Autism, affective and other psychiatric Gerland, G. (1996). A real person: Life on the out-
disorders: Patterns of familial aggregation. Psy- side. London: Souvenir Press.
chological Medicine, 28, 385–395. Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., & Ghaziuddin, N.
Burack, J. A., Root, R., & Zigler, E. (1997). Inclu- (1992). Co-morbidity of autistic disorder in
sive education for children with autism: Re- children and adolescents. European Journal of
viewing ideological, empirical and community Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1, 209–213.
considerations. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmer-Mikhail, E., & Ghazi-
(Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive devel- uddin, N. (1998). Comorbidity in Asperger
opmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 796–807). New syndrome: A preliminary report. Journal of
York: Wiley. Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 279–283.
Campbell, M., Schopler, E., Cueva, J. E., & Hallin, Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Rutter, M., & Le
A. (1996). Treatment of autistic disorder. Couteur, A. (2001). Development and current
Journal of the American Academy of Child and functioning in adolescents with Asperger syn-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 134–143. drome: A comparative study. Journal of Child
Chung, S. Y., Luk, F. L., & Lee, E. W. H. (1990). A Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 227–240.
follow-up study of infantile autism in Hong Gillberg, C. (1991). Outcome in autism and autistic-
Kong. Journal of Autism and Developmental like conditions. Journal of the American Acad-
Disorders, 20, 221–232. emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30,
Clark, P., & Rutter, M. (1979). Task difficulty and 375–382.
task performance in autistic children. Journal Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (2000). The biology of
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, the autistic syndromes (3rd ed.). Oxford, En-
271–285. gland: MacKeith Press.
Clark, P., & Rutter, M. (1981). Autistic children’s Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, S. (1987). Outcome
responses to structure and to interpersonal de- and prognostic factors in infantile autism and
mands. Journal of Autism and Developmental similar conditions: A population-based study
Disorders, 11, 201–217. of 46 cases followed through puberty. Journal
218 Diagnosis and Classification

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre-
272–288. nia, 1, 119–145.
Grandin, T. (1995). The learning style of people Kanner, L. (1973). Childhood psychosis: Initial
with autism; An autobiography. In K. A. Quill studies and new insights. New York: Winston /
(Ed.), Teaching children with autism: Strategies Wiley.
to enhance communication and socialization Kanner, L., & Eisenberg, L. (1956). Early infantile
(pp. 33–52). New York: Delmar. autism 1943–1955. American Journal of Or-
Hare, D., Jones, J. P. R., & Paine, C. (1999). Ap- thopsychiatry Psychiatry, 26, 55–65.
proaching reality: The use of personal con- Keel, J. H., Mesibov, G., & Woods, A. V. (1997).
struct assessment in working with people with TEACCH-Supported employment programme.
Asperger syndrome. Autism; International Jour- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
nal of Research and Practice, 3, 165–176. ders, 27, 3–10.
Holliday-Willey, L. (1999). Pretending to be nor- Kilsby, M., & Beyer, S. (1996). Engagement and in-
mal. London: Jessica Kingsley. teractions: A comparison between supported
Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high functioning employment and day service provision. Journal
adults with autism with and without early lan- of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 348–358.
guage delays: Implications for the differentia- Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (Eds.).
tion between autism and Asperger syndrome. (2000). Asperger syndrome. New York: Guil-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ford Press.
ders, 33, 3–13. Kobayashi, R., Murata, T., & Yashinaga, K. (1992).
Howlin, P. (2004). Autism and Asperger syndrome: A follow-up study of 201 children with autism
Preparing for adulthood (2nd ed.). London: in Kyushu and Yamaguchi, Japan. Journal of
Routledge. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22,
Howlin, P., & Goode, S. (1998). Outcome in adult 395–411.
life for individuals with autism. In F. Volkmar Kuperman, S., Black, D. W., & Burns, T. L. (1988).
(Ed.), Autism and developmental disorders Excess mortality among formerly hospitalized
(pp. 209–241). New York: Cambridge Univer- child psychiatric patients. Archives of General
sity Press. Psychiatry, 45, 277–282.
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. Lainhart, J. E. (1999). Psychiatric problems in in-
(2004). Adult outcomes for children with dividuals with autism, their parents and sib-
autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- lings. International Review of Psychiatry, 11,
chiatry 45, 212–229. 278–298.
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L. M., & Rutter, M. (2000). Lainhart, J. E., & Folstein, S. E. (1994). Affective
Autism and developmental receptive language disorders in people with autism: A review of
disorder: A follow-up comparison in early published cases. Journal of Autism and Devel-
adult life: II. Social, behavioural and psychi- opmental Disorders, 24, 587–601.
atric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology Larsen, F. W., Dahl, V., & Hallum, E. (1990). A 30-
and Psychiatry, 41, 561–578. year follow-up study of a child psychiatric
Hutton, J. (1998). Cognitive decline and new prob- clientele: I. Demographic description. Acta
lems arising in association with autism. Un- Paediatrica Scandinavica, 81, 39–45.
published doctoral dissertation, University of Larsen, F. W., & Mouridsen, S. E. (1997). The out-
London. come in children with childhood autism and
International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Asperger syndrome originally diagnosed as
Consortium. (2001). A genomewide screen for psychotic: A 30-year follow-up study of sub-
autism: Strong evidence for linkage to chro- jects hospitalized as children. European Child
mosome 2q, 7q, and 16p. American Journal of and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 181–190.
Human Genetics, 69, 570–581. Lawson, W. (2002). Understanding and working
Isager, T., Mouridsen, S. E., & Rich, B. (1999). with the spectrum of autism: An insider’s view.
Mortality and causes of death in pervasive London: Jessica Kingsley.
developmental disorders. Autism: Interna- Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1969). A five to fifteen
tional Journal of Research and Practice, 3, year follow-up study of infantile psychosis:
7–16. III. Psychological aspects. British Journal of
Jolliffe, T., Landsdown, R., & Robinson, T. (1992). Psychiatry, 115, 865–882.
Autism: A personal account. London: National Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1970). A five to fifteen
Autistic Society. year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: IV.
Kanner, L. (1971). Follow-up study of eleven Patterns of cognitive abilities. British Journal
autistic children originally reported in 1943. of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 152–163.
Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders 219

Lord, C. (1995). Facilitating social inclusion: Ex- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
amples from peer intervention programs. In 41, 547–559.
E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning McCaughrin, W., Ellis, W., Rusch, F., & Heal, L.
and cognition in autism (pp. 221–239). New (1993). Cost-effectiveness of supported em-
York: Plenum Press. ployment. Mental Retardation, 31, 41–48.
Lord, C., & Bailey, A. (2002). Autism spectrum McDougle, C. J. (1997). Psychopharmacology. In
disorders. In M. Rutter & E. Taylor (Eds.), D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Hand-
Child and adolescent psychiatry (4th ed., book of autism and pervasive developmental
pp. 636–663). Oxford, England: Blackwell. disorders (2nd ed., pp. 707–729). New York:
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Wiley.
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A Mittler, P., Gillies, S., & Jukes, E. (1966). Progno-
revised version of a diagnostic interview sis in psychotic children: Report of a follow-up
for carers of individuals with possible perva- study. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research,
sive developmental disorders. Journal of 10, 73–83.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, National Research Council. (2001). Educating chil-
659–685. dren with autism (Committee on Educational
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Di Lavore, P. C., & Risi, S. Interventions for Children with Autism). Wash-
(2000). Autism Diagnostic Observation Sched- ington, DC: National Academy Press.
ule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psycholog- Newson, E., Dawson, M., & Everard, T. (1982). The
ical Services. natural history of able autistic people: Their
Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1985). Differences in sex management and functioning in a social con-
ratios in autism as a function of measured in- text. Unpublished report to the Department of
telligence. Journal of Autism and Development Health and Social Security, London.
Disorders, 15, 185–193. Nordin, V., & Gillberg, C. (1998). The long-term
Lord, C., & Venter, A. (1992). Outcome and follow- course of autistic disorders: Update on follow-
up studies of high functioning autistic individ- up studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
uals. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), 97, 99–108.
High functioning individuals with autism O’Neill, J. L. (1999). Through the eyes of aliens: A
(pp. 187–200). New York: Plenum Press. book about autistic people. London: Jessica
Lotter, V. (1966). Epidemiology of autistic condi- Kingsley.
tions in young children. I: Prevalence. Social Östman, O. (1991). Child and adolescent psychi-
Psychiatry, 1, 163–173. atric patients in adulthood. Acta Paediatrica
Lotter, V. (1974a). Factors related to outcome in Scandinavica, 84, 40–45.
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Child- Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Miller, J. N. (2000).
hood Schizophrenia, 4, 263–277. DSM-IV defined Asperger syndrome: Cogni-
Lotter, V. (1974b). Social adjustment and placement tive, behavioural and early history differentia-
of autistic children in Middlesex: A follow-up tion from high-functioning autism. Autism:
study. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizo- International Journal of Research and Practice,
phrenia, 4, 11–32. 4, 29–46.
Lotter, V. (1978). Follow-up studies. In M. Rutter Petty, L. K., Ornitz, E. M., Michelman, J. D., &
& E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of Zimmerman, E. G. (1984). Autistic children
concepts and treatment (pp. 475–496). New who become schizophrenic. Archives of Gen-
York: Plenum Press. eral Psychiatry, 41, 129–135.
Macintosh, K. E., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annota- Piven, J., Harper, J., Palmer, P., & Arndt, S. (1996).
tion: The similarities and differences between Course of behavioural change in autism: A ret-
autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder. Jour- rospective study of high-IQ adolescents and
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, adults. Journal of the American Academy of
419–420. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 523–529.
Mawhood, L. M., & Howlin, P. (1999). The out- Pozner, A., & Hammond, J. (1993). An evaluation
come of a supported employment scheme for of supported employment initiatives for dis-
high functioning adults with autism or As- abled people. Research Series No. 17. London:
perger syndrome. Autism; International Jour- Department of Employment.
nal of Research and Practice, 3, 229–253. Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R.
Mawhood, L. M., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychiatric
Autism and developmental receptive language social and behavioural outcomes. Journal of
disorder—a follow-up comparison in early the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
adult life: I. Cognitive and language outcomes. 24, 465–473.
220 Diagnosis and Classification

Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia re- functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism
considered. Journal of Autism and Childhood and Developmental Disorders, 19, 213–226.
Schizophrenia, 2, 315–337. Szivos, S. E. (1990). Attitudes to work and their re-
Rutter, M. (2000). Genetic studies of autism: From lationship to self-esteem and aspirations among
the 1970s into the Millennium. Journal of Ab- young adults with a mild mental handicap.
normal Child Psychology, 28, 3–14. British Journal of Mental Subnormality, 36,
Rutter, M., Greenfeld, D., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A 108–117.
five to fifteen year follow-up of infantile psy- Szurek, S., & Berlin, I. (1956). Elements of psy-
chosis: II. Social and behavioural outcome. chotherapeutics with the schizophrenic child
British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 1183–1189. and his parents. Psychiatry, 19, 1–19.
Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A five to fifteen Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger’s syndrome in adult-
year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: I. hood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger
Description of the sample. British Journal of syndrome (pp. 147–183). Cambridge, England:
Psychiatry, 113, 1169–1182. Cambridge University Press.
Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Kunce, L. J. (1998). Tantam, D. (2000). Psychological disorder in ado-
Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism? lescents and adults with Asperger’s syndrome.
New York: Plenum Press. Autism: International Journal of Research and
Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Shattuck, P. T., Practice, 4, 47–62.
Orsmond, G., Swe, A., & Lord, C. (2002). The Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1992). A
symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in ado- follow-up study of high functioning autistic
lescence and adulthood. Manuscript submitted children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
for publication. chiatry, 33, 489–507.
Shavelle, R. M., Strauss, D. J., & Pickett, J. (2001). Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991). Comorbid as-
Causes of death in autism Journal of Autism sociation of autism and schizophrenia. Ameri-
and Developmental Disorders, 6, 569–576. can Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1705–1707.
Smalley, S., McCracken, J., & Tanguay, P. (1995). von Knorring, A.-L., & Häglöf, B. (1993). Autism in
Autism, affective disorders, and social phobia. northern Sweden. A population based follow-up
American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neu- study: Psychopathology. European Child and
ropsychiatric Genetics), 60, 19–26. Adolescent Psychiatry, 2, 91–97.
Smith, M., Belcher, R., & Juhrs, P. (1995). A guide Williams, D. (1992). Nobody nowhere. London:
to successful employment for individuals with Corgi Books.
autism. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Stoddart, K. (1999). Adolescents with Asperger account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.
syndrome: 3 case studies of individual and Wing, L. (1986). Clarification on Asperger’s syn-
family therapy. Autism: International Journal drome [Letter to the Editor]. Journal of Autism
of Research and Practice, 3, 255–272. and Developmental Disorders, 16, 513–515.
Sturmey, P. (1998). Classification and diagnosis of Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1978). Systematic recording
psychiatric disorders in persons with develop- of behaviors and skills of retarded and psy-
mental disabilities. Journal of Developmental chotic children. Journal of Autism and Child-
and Physical Disabilities, 10, 317–330. hood Schizophrenia, 8, 79–97.
Sverd, J., Montero, G., & Gurevich, N. (1993). Wing, L., & Shah, A. (2000). Catatonia in autistic
Brief report: Case for an association between spectrum disorders. British Journal of Psychi-
Tourette’s syndrome, autistic disorder and atry, 176, 357–362.
schizophrenia-like disorder. Journal of Autism Wolff, S., & McGuire, R. J. (1995). Schizoid per-
and Developmental Disorders, 23, 407–414. sonality in girls: A follow-up study. What are
Szatmari, P. (2000). Perspectives on the classifica- the links with Asperger’s syndrome? Journal of
tion of Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 793–818.
Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syn- Wozniak, J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Frazier,
drome (pp. 403–417). New York: Guilford Press. J., Kim, J., Millstein, R., et al. (1997). Mania
Szatmari, P., Archer, L., Fisman, S., Streiner, D., & in children with pervasive developmental dis-
Wilson, F. (1995). Asperger’s syndrome and order revisited. Journal of the American Acad-
autism: Differences in behaviour, cognition emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36,
and adaptive functioning. Journal of the Amer- 1552–1559.
ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- Zetlin, A., & Murtaugh, M. (1990). Whatever hap-
atry, 34, 1662–1670. pened to those with borderline IQs? American
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R. S., Bond, Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 463–469.
S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high
SECTION II

DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR

The concept of pervasive developmental dis- ing of the disparity between verbal and perfor-
orders (PDDs) implies that individuals with mance skills in Asperger’s syndrome, and how
autism and related conditions display diffi- does this pattern relate to social difficulties?
culties across a range of developmental do- To understand the behavioral and develop-
mains, rather than simply in one or another mental findings among individuals with perva-
aspect of development. The unfolding and sive developmental disorders, it is necessary to
maturation of basic competencies are af- study individuals with different levels of cogni-
fected to a greater or lesser degree, and there tive ability (from profoundly retarded through
are varied downstream behavioral conse- normal intelligence); at different chronological
quences of earlier difficulties. The patterns ages (from early childhood through adulthood);
of dysfunction, the extent of impairment, and and with various observational, laboratory,
the areas of relatively better or even normal interview, and other approaches that have
functioning differ among individuals within demonstrated reliability and validity. The in-
one category of disorder and also among the terpretation of findings requires thoughtful
types of disorders. While the pathways of de- consideration of possible methodological prob-
velopment—socialization, communication, lems, including how representative the sample
perception and attention, and cognition—are is of the full population of individuals with the
separated in theoretical discussion and re- disorder, the adequacy of control and contrast
search, the minds of children are not so neatly groups, how well the behavioral measure cap-
divisible by chapter headings or disciplinary tures the function that is to be studied, and the
designations. Thus, the complex interactions validity or the measures as well as other issues
between the domains and changing relations concerning design of instruments and studies.
among them at different phases of develop- Research in developmental psychopathology
ment also need to be considered. must be as rigorous and replicable as in any
The scientific study of development and be- other area of psychological study.
havior of individuals with autism and other The developmental psychopathological per-
disorders aims at defining the nature of the un- spective on autism and similar conditions ex-
derlying dysfunctions. What are the specific plains the empirical findings concerning
types of social dysfunctions exhibited by indi- atypical behavior and development within the
viduals with autism and how do these differ context of normal principles of development.
from those seen in Asperger syndrome, Rett’s, From this perspective, the concepts of normal
or other forms of mental retardation? In development highlight the specific types of de-
autism, what accounts for the relatively better viations, abnormalities, rates, and patterns of
performance on some cognitive tasks (e.g., development of individuals and groups with
those that call upon rote memory) in contrast pervasive disorders. In turn, the study of indi-
with others (e.g., those that require particular viduals with autism and other conditions is
types of social judgment)? What is the mean- used to test and expand hypotheses about

221
222 Development and Behavior

preconditions of normal development and the Adolescence may be quite difficult for these
unfolding of basic competencies, for example, children as they experience an upsurge of sex-
the relations among cognitive, social, and af- ual and aggressive behavior; for higher func-
fective development. tioning individuals, young adulthood may be a
Autism and the other pervasive disorders time of heightened loneliness and depression
almost always are chronic conditions; how- as they recognize the profound nature of their
ever, the functioning of individuals is not sta- difficulties, their differences from others, and
tic. While intellectual abilities tend to remain their limited opportunities. This can be a par-
relatively stable, individuals with autism and ticular problem for more able individuals with
other pervasive disorders mature and change higher functioning autism, Asperger’s, or
during their lives, just as other children and PDD-NOS. The study of development during
adolescents do. For example, individuals with the lifespan is important for practical as well
strictly defined autism usually tend to become as theoretical reasons. The chapters in this
increasingly social during their later childhood Section describe the major domains in which
and adolescent years; occasionally, children individuals with pervasive disorders manifest
with pervasive disorders show dramatic im- their cardinal problems. A fuller understand-
provements in social and adaptive functioning ing of behavioral changes throughout develop-
and may seem only odd or eccentric in adult- ment is critical for understanding not only the
hood. For some individuals, progress is slow natural history of these disorders, but also for
or, sadly, in some cases minimal. Furthermore, designing interventions appropriate for each
new difficulties can also emerge over time. developmental level.
CHAPTER 8

Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood

KATARZYNA CHAWARSKA AND FRED R. VOLKMAR

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder char- SYMPTOMS OF AUTISM IN EARLY


acterized by symptom onset prior to the third CHILDHOOD
birthday. Until relatively recently, its early
symptoms have usually been ascertained retro- Studies on parental recognition of develop-
spectively through parent reports because the mental abnormalities in autism suggest that
majority of children did not receive the diag- approximately 30% (De Giacomo & Fom-
nosis until preschool or early school age. In the bonne, 1998) to 54% (Volkmar, Stier, &
past decade, however, advances in early diag- Cohen, 1985) of parents of children diagnosed
nosis research and the reports stressing the with autism register their first concerns before
efficacy of early intervention made the transi- their child’s first birthday and at least 80% to
tion to studying autism in the first 3 years of 90% recognize their child’s abnormalities by
life both possible and imperative (National 24 months (De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998).
Research Council, 2001). In addition to the These estimates are based primarily on par-
importance of early identification of autism ents’ retrospective reports and, thus, may be
for treatment, particularly in families where confounded by passage of time, limited exper-
there is a known genetic risk, early identifica- tise regarding typical development, and possi-
tion provides the opportunity for studying the ble underestimation of the significance of
disorder before confounding effects of treat- perceived difficulties in early development.
ment, development of compensatory strate- For that reason, they are likely to represent the
gies, and comorbid disorders have begun to upper-bound limit of the actual age of symp-
impact its manifestation. This chapter con- tom onset in autism (De Giacomo & Fom-
tains a review of research regarding the symp- bonne, 1998; Volkmar et al., 1985).
toms of autism in infancy and early childhood,
specific developmental profiles observed in First Year of Life
this population, stability of the diagnosis, and
a brief discussion of the challenges and oppor- Defining a set of developmentally sensitive di-
tunities that earlier diagnosis of the condition agnostic criteria for autism in infants and tod-
will present. dlers is an inherently difficult task (Lord &

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
velopment (grants 1-PO1-HD35482-01, 5 P01-HD042127-02), the National Institute of Mental Health
(STAART grant U54-MH066594), and the National Alliance of Autism Research grants. The authors thank
Amy Sanchez for her help in the preparation of this chapter.

223
224 Development and Behavior

Risi, 2000). Infancy is a period of the most dy- Kurita, 1985; Lord, Shulman, & DiLavore,
namic growth and change; thus, the same be- 2004; Rogers & DiLalla, 1990; Tuchman &
havior (or absence thereof ) in one narrowly Rapin, 1997). It is not clear whether regressive
defined period of time gains clinical signifi- autism constitutes a neurobiological subtype
cance and becomes indicative of abnormal of autism, represents variable expression of the
development only a few months later. For in- same genetic factors, or is an even earlier man-
stance, predominance of exploratory play is ifestation of essentially the same phenomenon
typical and adaptive for children under the age seen in childhood disintegrative disorder (see
of 12 months but may signify developmental Volkmar, Koenig, & State, Chapter 3, this
difficulties when it extends into the second Handbook, this volume).
year of life and is not followed by symbolic Despite the fact that a significant propor-
and generative forms of play (Losche, 1990; tion of parents report concerning behaviors
Piaget, 1954). The preintentional use of physi- prior to the first birthday, the direct evidence
cal gestures such as reach and grasp to pursue regarding clinical presentation of infants with
desirable objects is typical of children under autism in this age range is still very limited.
the age of 9 months. It is expected, however, Apart from a series of clinical case studies and
that this gesture becomes synchronized with parent retrospective report, the most direct ev-
eye contact soon thereafter as an index of idence comes from studies analyzing video-
emerging intentional communication (Bates, tapes of children who were subsequently
1979). A persistent lack of such synchroniza- diagnosed with autism.
tion becomes symptomatic of disruption in so-
Case Studies
cial communication in the second year of life.
Moreover, some early symptoms of autism be- In his description of 11 cases of autism, Kan-
come less pronounced over time as children ac- ner (1943/1968) noted that while abnormalities
quire language and begin to benefit from of speech and cognitive functions, as well as
intervention programs targeting, among oth- repetitive behaviors and insistence on same-
ers, play or specific joint attention skills. Situ- ness, emerge over time as the child acquires
ational factors such as variability of clinical motor and cognitive skills necessary for the ab-
presentation depending on the extent of atten- normalities to manifest themselves, the autistic
tional, cognitive, and behavioral difficulties, aloneness, or the “inability to relate them-
amount of structure and support provided dur- selves in the ordinary way to people and situa-
ing testing, nature of the task (e.g., verbal ver- tions” (p. 243), is present since birth. This
sus nonverbal), and novelty and complexity of social isolation is evident from very early on in
the environment may also have impact on early their self-sufficiency and ability to occupy
diagnosis (National Research Council, 2001). themselves for long periods of time. Moreover,
Among other factors that might hinder the Kanner suggested that these children have dif-
early detection of autism is the regression or, ficulties in adjusting body posture while being
as it is sometimes termed, the setback phenom- held by another person and in assuming an an-
enon. While the initial observations of cases of ticipatory posture in preparation of being
autism suggested the presence of social devel- picked up. Others reported limited eye contact
opment abnormalities from birth (Kanner, and decreased social responsivity (Dawson, Os-
1943/1968), further clinical observations re- terling, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000; Klin et al.,
vealed a subgroup of children who reportedly 2004; Sparling, 1991), as well as lack of motor
developed normally though the first 18 to 20 imitation and imitative babbling (Dawson et al.,
months of life, but then experienced loss of 2000). Among other symptoms described in
language skills and decreased interest in usual case studies are those relating to arousal
activities along with withdrawal from social regulation and motor development. More
interactions (Eisenberg & Kanner, 1956; Volk- specifically, infants that are later diagnosed
mar & Cohen, 1989). Recent estimates suggest with autism may demonstrate excessive tremu-
that between 20% and 40% of children with lousness and excessive startle response in the
autism experience regression prior to the sec- perinatal period (Sparling, 1991), arousal reg-
ond birthday (Fombonne & Chakrabarti, 2001; ulation difficulties, sleep difficulties, unusual
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 225

sensitivity to stimuli (particularly hypersensi- regulation are specific to autism in the first
tivity to touch), oral-motor problems, as well year of life.
as motor difficulty related to both hypo- and
Videotape Analysis Studies
hypertonia present by 6 months of age (Daw-
son et al., 2000). Although intriguing, case Several studies of home videos suggest that in-
studies have a number of profound limitations, fants with autism are distinguishable both
including lack of data regarding universality of from their typical and developmentally de-
the described symptoms, as well as their speci- layed peers in the first year of life.
ficity to autism (see also Stone, 1997, for re-
view). Autism versus Typical Development Mae-
stro and colleagues (2002) studied videotapes
Parent Report
of infants 6 months and younger who were
Retrospective parent report studies suggest later diagnosed with autism and compared the
that the early symptoms cluster around results with video recordings of typical con-
deficits in early emerging social interaction trols matched for CA. Among the characteris-
skills and may include arousal regulation dif- tics considered were visual attention and
ficulties. In a retrospective interview study by affective responsiveness to social and nonso-
Klin, Volkmar, and Sparrow (1992), parents of cial stimuli. Infants with autism showed di-
preschoolers with autism were interviewed minished visual attention to people, sought
with the Vineland Adaptive Behaviors Scale others less frequently, and were less likely to
(Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) to deter- smile at others and vocalize as compared to
mine whether children with autism exhibit im- typically developing infants. They were also
pairments in social behaviors that typically less likely to anticipate others’ aim and to ex-
emerge prior to the first birthday. Five behav- plore objects orally or manually. At the same
iors were the most frequently endorsed as time, there were no differences between the
never performed by children with autism as groups in terms of visual attention and affec-
compared with a developmentally delayed con- tive responses to objects. Moreover, behaviors
trol group matched for mental age (MA), related to communication or repetitive behav-
chronological age (CA), and intelligence quo- iors occurred in the same frequency in both
tient (IQ): showing anticipation of being groups.
picked up, showing affection toward familiar In slightly older infants (8 to 10 months),
people, showing interest in children or peers the only behavior that distinguished children
other than siblings, reaching for a familiar per- with early-onset autism from typical peers
son, and playing simple interaction games with was diminished response to their name, while
others. Rogers and DiLalla (1990) addressed other social behaviors ( looking at others,
the question of the earliest manifestations of looking at the face while smiling, and orient-
autism in a group of 39 children (mean age 45 ing to name), communication behaviors
months) referred to a specialized clinic and (vocalizations consisting of vowel and vowel-
diagnosed with infantile autism or pervasive consonant combinations), and functional and
developmental disorder (PDD). Parents of nonfunctional repetitive behaviors did not
children with symptom onset before 12 (Werner et al., 2000). The diminished re-
months expressed concerns primarily about sponse to name persisted and retained its
their abnormal temperamental characteristics, power to differentiate between the two groups
as the children were described as either ex- at the age of 12 months (Osterling & Dawson,
tremely difficult or very passive. Reported 1994). Thus, it appears that already in the
symptoms included irritability, inability to be first months of life, there is a lower sensitivity
soothed, and erratic physiological patterns, or to and salience of social stimuli as compared
being too good, undemanding, and happy to with typical children. However, approximately
play alone in a crib. Other symptoms included 70% of children with autism also experience
lack of stranger anxiety that typically emerges delays in various areas of development; thus,
around 8 months. It is not clear, however, to to identify what is uniquely due to autism,
what extent these symptoms related to self- the behavior of infants with autism must be
226 Development and Behavior

compared to that of infants without autism tified three behaviors that were particularly
who have developmental delays. useful for identifying infants with autism: ori-
enting to name, looking at people, and looking
Autism versus Mental Retardation Baranek at objects held by people.
(1999) rated the videotaped material of 39
Summary
children ages 9 to 12 months on early-emerg-
ing social-cognitive behaviors ( looking, gaze Based on the analysis of samples of videotaped
aversion, response to name, social touch re- diaries, it appears that in the first year of life
sponses, and affective expressions), as well as infants with early-onset autism can be distin-
various repetitive and sensory-seeking and guished from typical and developmentally de-
avoiding behaviors. The only behavior that re- layed children matched for CA. During the
liably distinguished infants with autism from first 6 to 8 months, the affected infants show
the two other groups was poor response to diminished visual attention to people, which
name. However, when a profile of predictor may signify limited salience of and interest in
variables was considered, infants with autism the social environment. They tend to seek oth-
tended to exhibit excessive mouthing, aversion ers less frequently and are less likely to engage
to social touch, lower frequency of orienting to in early social communicative exchanges in-
visual stimuli, and poor response to name as volving smiling at others and vocalizing. At
compared to MR and typical controls. The the same time, they are no different from typi-
study suggested that in the last quarter of the cal children where interest in and exploration
first year of life, autism might manifest not of objects are concerned. This pattern is con-
only in social communication difficulties (re- sistent with the hypothesis of the earliest dis-
sponse to name) but also in diminished interest ruption of social development in autism, but
in nonsocial visual stimuli and unusual sen- the present evidence is not sufficient to deter-
sory behaviors. mine whether diminished social orientation in
Behavior of 12-month-old infants with the first months of life is unique to autism or
autism as well as infants later diagnosed with whether it is shared with other developmental
MR and 20 typically developing infants was an- disorders. In the second half of the first year
alyzed for a wide range of social-communicative when typically developing infants begin to re-
as well as motor and sensory behaviors (Oster- spond differentially to verbal stimuli in gen-
ling, Dawson, & Munson, 2002). The behav- eral and to the sound of their own name in
iors of interest included gaze ( looking at faces, particular, infants with autism begin to show a
attention to people, attention to objects not startling lack of such sensitivity. This particu-
held by others), joint attention ( looking at an lar deficit sets them apart consistently from
object held by another, alternating gaze be- both typical and developmentally delayed
tween a person and an object, and pointing), peers and persists throughout the early pre-
communication and language (seeking contact school years (Lord, 1995). In the visual domain,
with an adult, participation in reciprocal so- affected infants continue to be less responsive
cial games, immediate imitation, orienting to and pay less attention to people in their envi-
name), as well as motor behaviors (repetitive ronment. At about the same time, typically de-
motor actions, sitting unassisted, crawling, veloping infants become capable of integrating
pulling to a stand, standing unassisted, and their interactions with people with the explo-
walking). Infants with autism and MR differed ration of objects and begin to engage in visual
from those with only MR in terms of the fre- joint attention behaviors (Bruner, 1981). Al-
quency and duration of two behaviors: orient- though deficits in visual joint attention are one
ing to name and looking at people. Infants with of the most reliable symptoms of autism in the
autism ( both with and without MR) differed second year of life and beyond, at 12 months
from typical controls on the same items and they are only beginning to emerge and might
use of gestures, looking at objects held by oth- manifest in a lower frequency of looking at
ers, and repetitive actions, but not on rate of objects held by others. Despite reports of dif-
vocalizations or looking at objects not held by ficulties in sensory sensitivities, arousal regu-
others. A discriminant function analysis iden- lation, motor difficulties, and impaired vocal
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 227

and motor imitation based on parent report present in children under the age of 24 months
and single-case studies, none of these factors including social behaviors ( limited imitation,
have been reported as a result of analysis of preference for being alone, not looking at oth-
the videotaped materials. This discrepancy ers, lack of interest in interactive games), af-
may be due to selective taping, but it may also fective behaviors (no social smile, limited
suggest that these symptoms are not unique to facial expressions, and empty smile), and
infants with autism. sensory behaviors (no response to name, be-
having as if deaf, insensitivity to pain, hyper-
Second and Third Years sensitivity to the taste of food; Hoshino et al.,
1982).
A majority of parents of children with autism
Parent Prospective Report
begin to recognize and seek medical or psy-
chological advice about their children’s devel- Parental reports regarding current develop-
opmental disturbances in the second and third mental concerns are less likely to be affected
years of life (De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998; by selective recollection and can be compared
Rogers & DiLalla, 1990; Short & Schopler, with direct clinical observations (Cox et al.,
1988). Concerns are usually triggered by a lack 1999; Dahlgren & Gillberg, 1989; Gillberg
of skill progress (e.g., speech does not develop et al., 1990; Klin et al., 1992; Lord, 1995;
as expected), loss of skills (e.g., loss of words, Stone, Lee, et al., 1999).
eye contact, or interest in others), and emer-
gence of abnormal behaviors (e.g., proclivity Autism versus Language Disorder A longi-
for spinning things or motor mannerisms). tudinal study by Cox and his colleagues (Cox
et al., 1999) followed a group of forty-six 20-
Parent Retrospective Report
month-old infants identified through a screen-
Studies comparing clinical presentation of ing with the Checklist for Autism in Toddlers
children with autism with typical children (CHAT; Baird et al., 2000; Baron-Cohen,
(Hoshino et al., 1982; Ornitz, Guthrie, & Allen, & Gillberg, 1992; Baron-Cohen, Cox,
Farley, 1977) and children with other devel- Baird, Sweettenham, & Nighingale, 1996) as
opmental disabilities (Wimpory, Hobson, being at risk for autism. The ICD-10 diagnosis
Williams, & Nash, 2000) suggest that the dif- at 42 months indicated that the sample con-
ferences between groups begin to cluster tained 8 children with autism, 13 with
around the core areas of autistic psychopath- nonautistic PDD, 9 with language disorder, and
ology. While the abnormalities in the area of 15 typical children. Parents were interviewed
social interaction and communication con- with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised
tinue to unfold, unusual sensory interests and (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994), a
repetitive behaviors begin to emerge (e.g., structured investigator-based interview used in
Ornitz et al., 1977). A study on parents of differential diagnosis of autism. Two items of
preschool children with autism and develop- the ADI-R consistently differentiated the
mental delays employing the Detection of autism group at 20 and 42 months: point for in-
Autism by Infant Sociability Interview terest and use of conventional gestures. At 20
(DAISI ) suggests that behaviors that differ- months, the range of facial expressions item
entiated the two diagnostic groups fell into was also consistently endorsed. At 42 months,
two categories: dyadic or social interaction several new items—seeking to share enjoy-
(i.e., raising arms up to be picked up, fre- ment, offering comfort, nodding, and imagina-
quency and intensity of eye contact, preverbal tive play—were endorsed as more pathological
turn taking, and using noises communica- by parents of children with autism. No items
tively), as well as triadic or person-object- from the repetitive behaviors and stereotypical
person interactions ( joint attention, i.e., patterns of behaviors scale differentiated be-
referential use of eye contact, offering and tween the groups at any time point.
giving objects, pointing to objects, and fol-
lowing others’ pointing) (Wimpory et al., Autism versus Developmental Delay Lord
2000). Others reported symptom clusters (1995) reported a follow-up study of thirty
228 Development and Behavior

2-year-old children who were referred for a Autism Only Stone, Hoffman, Lewis, and
clinical evaluation to a multidisciplinary de- Ousley (1994) examined behavioral character-
velopmental disabilities clinic for a differen- istics of a group of 26 children between the
tial diagnosis of autism. Parents were ages of 24 and 44 months diagnosed with
interviewed with the Autism Diagnostic Inter- autism. The majority of children in this age
view-Revised ADI-R (Le Couteur et al., 1989) range exhibited lack of awareness of others,
supplemented with the items appropriate for impaired imitation, abnormal social play, ab-
children under age 2. All children were reeval- normal nonverbal communication, and absence
uated a year later. The item-by-item analysis of imaginative play. Deficits in social inter-
of 30 of the ADI questions (29 items from the action and nonverbal communication were more
original Le Couteur et al. diagnostic algo- prominent than presence of repetitive behaviors
rithm and one additional item, attention to and restricted interests and activities. Parents
voice) identified a set of items that discrimi- and clinicians rarely endorsed abnormal speech
nated 2-year-old children with autism from production, abnormal speech content, and im-
other children with developmental delays. paired conversational skills, as most of the chil-
These items in the social domains were: seek- dren in this age group were nonverbal. At the
ing to share own enjoyment, directing atten- same time, children with autism showed no ab-
tion, use of other’s body as a tool, interest in normality in comfort seeking, suggesting they
other children, greeting, and social reciproc- were able to use their parents as a source of re-
ity. In the area of communication, attention to assurance, and they enjoyed cuddling and af-
voice, pointing, and understanding of gestures fection. In addition, no increase in distress
discriminated between the groups. Among the over change and insistence on routines was
items comprising the repetitive and restricted noted, suggesting that the need for sameness
behaviors domain, hand and finger manner- may emerge and become more apparent later in
isms as well as unusual sensory behaviors dis- development.
criminated between the groups. Two of these
behaviors, directing attention (showing) and Autism versus Developmental Delay One
attention to voice, were found particularly of the most well-known and widely used in-
useful in differentiating between the diagnos- struments for diagnostic observation is the
tic groups, as they identified correctly 82.8% Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale-Generic
of children. (ADOS-G). The ADOS-G is composed of four
At age 3, four items correctly classified all modules designed for children with different
the subjects: use of other’s body, attention to levels of language skills, with Module 1 de-
voice, pointing, and finger mannerisms. At signed for preverbal and nonverbal children
this age, more children with autism showed (Lord et al., 2000). The ADOS-G provides a
abnormalities in gaze, limited range of facial DSM-IV-based algorithm for the diagnosis of
expressions, and limited comfort-seeking be- autism, nonautistic PDD, and non-PDD. Mod-
haviors. In addition, the number of children ule 1 of the ADOS-G was modeled directly on
with nonautistic disorders showing abnormal its prelinguistic (PL) precursor, the PL-ADOS
behaviors decreased from the age of 2 to 3, al- (DiLavore, Lord, & Rutter, 1995). Using the
lowing for clearer differentiation between the PL-ADOS, DiLavore and colleagues examined
two groups. Behaviors related to directing at- social and communicative behaviors as well as
tention, abnormal gaze, and facial expressions the presence of unusual sensory interests and
have been found equally disturbed across the stereotypical behaviors in a group of 38- to 61-
range of language skills and nonverbal func- month-old preschool children diagnosed clini-
tioning (Lord & Pickles, 1996). cally with autism or developmental delay.
Children with autism consistently showed im-
Observational Studies
paired use of nonverbal behaviors to regulate
Direct observational and experimental studies social interactions, including eye contact, fa-
provide the most reliable source of informa- cial expressions directed to others, and social
tion regarding syndrome expression in young smile, combined with impaired ability to share
children. pleasure with others. Lack of social reciproc-
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 229

ity and spontaneous joint attention was no- cated that although these behaviors are highly
table, along with increased use of other’s body specific to autism, 80% of parents of children
to communicate, and decreased ability to dif- who were later diagnosed with autism did not
ferentiate between parent and examiner. They report these behaviors as being abnormal at 18
were also less likely to direct their vocaliza- months (Baird et al., 2000). It is plausible that
tions at others, use gestures, and respond to early on, parents have difficulties in detecting
name. Increased frequency of restricted, and reporting abnormalities in these specific
repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behav- behaviors, or the difficulties are not universal
iors was noted as well. Adrien and colleagues in children with autism at this age. Moreover,
(Adrien et al., 1992) studied the validity and the population screened in the original Baron-
reliability of the Infant Behavioral Summa- Cohen et al. (1996) study did not include chil-
rized Scale (IBSS) for assessment of 6- to 48- dren with more severe developmental delays
month-old children with autism, children and disabilities further limiting generalizabil-
with developmental delays, and typical con- ity of the finding to a broader population of in-
trols. They identified 19 out of 33 items that fants with autism. A report based on the use of
reliably differentiated children with autism the CHAT with parents of 3-year-olds with
from their developmentally delayed peers. The autism and developmental delays suggests that
items were related to socialization (e.g., ig- further modification of the critical set of
nores people, prefers aloneness), communica- items may lead to increased sensitivity with-
tion ( lack of vocal communication, lack of out significant loss in specificity (Scambler
appropriate facial expressions, poor imitation et al., 2001), but the utility of the new criteria
of gestures or voices), attention and percep- for primary screening remains to be tested in
tion (e.g., easily distracted, none or bizarre the general population.
reaction to auditory stimuli, abnormal eye To address some of the shortcomings of the
contact), stereotyped behaviors and unusual CHAT, Robins et al. developed a 23-item ex-
postures, as well as inappropriate use of ob- tension of the original screening instrument
jects. These findings suggest besides abnor- designed for screening 24-month-old children
malities in the key diagnostic areas, young (Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers
children with autism show significant atten- [M-CHAT]; Robins et al., 2001). It was tested
tional deficits, and hyposensitivity to auditory on a large group of children screened at well-
stimuli. baby visits as well as children enrolled in early
intervention programs. A discriminant function
Prospective Screeners
analysis identified a set of predictors that in-
Reports regarding early manifestations of cluded joint attention items (protodeclarative
autism in infancy and early childhood have pointing, following a point, and bringing
been put to the test in a series of prospective objects to show), social relatedness items (in-
studies attempting to identify children at risk terest in other children, imitation), and com-
in general (Baird et al., 2000; Baron-Cohen munication (responding to name) consistent
et al., 1992, 1996; Charman, Baron-Cohen, with other reports regarding symptoms of
Baird, et al., 2001; Robins, Fein, Barton, & autism at the age of 2 (Baron-Cohen et al.,
Green, 2001) and referred populations (Robins 1996; Lord, 1995). The actual rates of false
et al., 2001; Scambler, Rogers, & Wehner, negative and false positive cases remain to be
2001; Stone, Coonrod, & Ousley, 2000). The determined on completion of a follow-up as-
early reports on the use of the Checklist for sessment of the cohort (see also Stone, Chapter
Autism in Toddlers (CHAT), a measure com- 27, this Handbook, Volume 2).
bining parent report and physician observa-
Summary
tion, suggested that at 18 months, symptoms of
autism are likely to include abnormalities in Retrospective studies on early manifestations
the development of protodeclarative pointing, of autism relying on parent report and video-
abnormalities in gaze monitoring, as well as tape analysis suggest that in the first year of
pretend play (Baron-Cohen et al., 1992, 1996). life, a set of developmentally sensitive diag-
However, a subsequent follow-up study indi- nostic signs of autism is likely to include
230 Development and Behavior

decreased visual attention to people and dimin- interest in peers, social games and turn-taking
ished response to name (see Table 8.1). The ab- exchanges; low frequency of looking referen-
normal sensory responses, including excessive tially at parents; and preference for being
mouthing and aversion to touch, have been re- alone. Vocal and motor imitation appear de-
ported less consistently in the first year of life. layed compared to the children’s overall devel-
In the second and third years of life, symptoms opmental level. A limited range of facial
of autism in most children intensify and expressions and infrequent instances of shar-
spread to multiple areas of functioning. In the ing affect have been reported as well. In the
small minority of children experiencing a de- area of communication, the most striking dif-
velopmental regression, the onset of difficul- ferences relate to early emerging social com-
ties is often marked by loss of language skills municative exchanges through nonverbal and
and a decrease in social interest by 18 to 24 vocal or verbal means. Affected children have
months. In the second year, typical infants difficulties using conventional gestures. They
undergo a tremendous growth spurt regard- do not point spontaneously to show things and
ing social interactions, imaginative play, and have difficulties in understanding or respond-
nonverbal and verbal communication, while ing to such gestures by others. They continue
infants with autism begin to show syndrome- to have difficulties in responding to speech in
specific difficulties in these areas. In the so- general and to their name in particular by, for
cial domain, the most frequently reported instance, reorienting and looking at an adult.
symptoms are diminished eye contact; limited Moreover, the reports stress that children have

TABLE 8.1 Symptoms Dif ferentiating Infants and Toddlers with Autism from Typical and
Developmentally Delayed Peers

Stereotypical Behaviors and


Social Interaction Communication Repetitive Patters

First year Limited ability to anticipate Poor response to name Excessive mouthing
being picked up Infrequent looking at objects Aversion to social touch
Low frequency of looking at held by others
people
Little interest in interactive
games
Little affection toward famil-
iar people
Content to be alone
Second and Abnormal eye contact Low frequency of verbal or Hand and finger mannerisms
third year Limited social referencing nonverbal communication Inappropriate use of objects
Limited interest in other chil- Failure to share interest (e.g., Repetitive interests/play
dren through pointing, giving, and
Unusual sensory behaviors
showing)
Limited social smile ( hyper or hyposensitivity to
Poor response to name sounds, textures, taste, visual
Low frequency of looking at
people Failure to respond to commu- stimuli)
nicative gestures (e.g., point-
Limited range of facial ing, giving, and showing)
expressions
Use of others’ body as tool
Limited sharing of affect /
enjoyment Unusual vocalizations

Little interest in interactive


games
Limited functional play
No pretend play
Limited motor imitation
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 231

difficulties using words and vocalizations ing parental expectations, or more accurate
communicatively. In verbal children, unusual observation and reporting (Lord, 1995).
features such as echolalia may be present. Al- The videotaped material collected by par-
though still infrequent, some stereotypic and ents provides the most direct window into the
repetitive behaviors begin to emerge, espe- behavior of the youngest infants subsequently
cially as children reach 3 years of age. Among diagnosed with autism. However, these data
them are hand and finger mannerisms, as well can be biased by selective taping. For in-
as unusual sensory-seeking or avoidance be- stance, case studies as well as parent reports
haviors. The symptoms in children with autism suggest the presence of abnormalities in
intensify with time and become more pro- arousal regulation, extreme fussiness or
nounced. At the same time, in children with placidity, and a lack of stranger anxiety in the
developmental delays, some of the autistic-like first year of life. None of these qualities have
behaviors observed early in development ap- been reported based on the analysis of video-
pear to diminish in frequency and intensity, al- taped materials, and it is not clear whether
lowing for clearer discrimination between the they are not specific to autism or whether
groups. such episodes are selectively excluded from
the family video diaries.
Methodological Limitations Although the specific sets of behavioral
criteria differentiating children with autism
The results of the studies targeting symptoms from other disabled populations in the first 3
of autism in the first 3 years of life are very years of life are slowly beginning to emerge,
encouraging and, in many respects, conver- making direct comparisons and seeking com-
gent. Nonetheless, several methodological monalities across studies are still difficult for
limitations inherent in both the type of avail- several reasons. First, there is a limited con-
able sources of information and selection of sistency among studies in terms of specific be-
comparison groups are worth highlighting. haviors selected for observation and their
The primary sources of information regarding operational definitions, which makes compar-
the earliest syndrome expression continue to isons between the studies problematic at times.
be parent retrospective reports and video di- Second, the comparison groups vary, which,
aries. Parent interview data, although invalu- combined with a very limited number of stud-
able, may be confounded by selective recall, ies, limits the generalizability of the results.
limited knowledge about normal develop- While in some studies autism is compared to
ment, or denial. Moreover, studies comparing language disorders, others select groups with
parental reports of concurrent symptoms and developmental delays of mixed origin, includ-
expert clinical observations suggest that while ing known genetic syndromes. Still other stud-
parents tend to be accurate in reporting nega- ies focus on consecutive referrals to clinics
tive symptoms, they do much worse as far as specializing in PDD and subsequently com-
the positive symptoms are concerned (Stone pare those diagnosed with PDD to non-PDD
et al., 1994). Specifically, parents often report disorders. Naturally, the sets of symptoms dif-
reliably on a failure to participate in early so- ferentiating the diagnostic groups in these
cial games, songs and routines, and preference studies are likely to differ. Nonetheless, the
for solitary activities. However, they have progress in studies on early symptoms and di-
more difficulties judging typicality and re- agnosis of autism in recent years has been im-
porting deficits in joint attention behaviors pressive and marks the beginning of intensive
and pretend play, the most prototypical symp- and interdisciplinary research programs tar-
toms of autism in the second and third years of geting infants and toddlers with PDD.
life (Charman, Baron-Cohen, Baird, et al., Longitudinal studies of very young children
2001). Further, changes in the syndrome ex- with autism (e.g., 12 to 24 months of age) as
pression over time based on parent report can well as studies on high-risk populations of
be misleading because it is not clear to what younger siblings of children with autism will
extent the changes reflect true increase in fre- help elucidate these diagnostic conundrums in
quency and intensity of the symptoms, grow- the near future.
232 Development and Behavior

EARLY DIAGNOSIS et al., 1999; Lord, 1995; Rutter, Le Couteur, &


Lord, 2003). In 2-year-olds, ADI-R tends to
Lowering the age of autism diagnosis raises a overdiagnose children with severe develop-
question of both sensitivity and specificity of mental delays and underdiagnose higher func-
the state-of-the-art diagnostic instruments and tioning children with some emerging gestures
procedures, which have been created for and and words (Lord, 1995). Both sensitivity (pro-
used successfully in older children. portion of children with autism identified cor-
rectly by ADI ) and specificity (proportion of
Early Diagnosis and DSM-IV children without autism classified correctly by
ADI ) in children under the age of 3 years were
Although the diagnostic criteria for autism in around 50% (Chawarska, Klin, Paul, & Volk-
the DSM-IV have very good sensitivity and mar, submitted; Cox et al., 1999; Lord, 1995).
specificity and cover a range of syndrome ex- At the age of 3 years, the ADI-R appears to
pression with regard to age and degree of MR, yield results more consistent with the clinical
especially when applied to children 4 years diagnosis (Cox et al., 1999; Lord, 1995). The
and older (Volkmar et al., 1994), they may be higher functioning children who at the age of 2
of limited use in the diagnosis of the youngest did not meet diagnostic criteria were reported
population of children with autism (Lord, to have more autistic features in their lan-
1995; Stone, Lee, et al., 1999). Diagnostic cri- guage, and they met all diagnostic criteria;
teria, such as failure to develop peer relation- however, the overdiagnosis of children with se-
ships, impaired conversational skills, and vere developmental delays in 3-year-olds re-
stereotyped language, are usually not applica- mained an issue (Lord, 1995). It is not clear if
ble to children under 3 years of age (Stone, the improvement in the sensitivity and speci-
Lee, et al., 1999). Among the criteria most fre- ficity of the ADI-R was due to the emergence
quently and consistently endorsed by clini- of more consistent patterns of symptoms (e.g.,
cians in this study were impaired use of deficits in understanding of gestures, limited
nonverbal behaviors and lack of social reci- range of facial expressions, and shared enjoy-
procity in the social domain and delayed de- ment, as well as unusual features of language),
velopment of spoken language in the higher parental expectations, or higher accu-
communication domain. Children in this age racy of parental reporting in older, previously
range displayed fewer symptoms from the diagnosed children (Lord & Magill-Evans,
stereotyped and repetitive behaviors domain; 1995).
the most commonly ( but not necessarily con-
sistently) endorsed symptom was preoccupa- Early Diagnosis and Clinical Experience
tion with stereotyped and repetitive patterns
of interests. These results suggest that further Diagnosis of autism in children under the age
research on the utility of the DSM-IV criteria of 3 based on clinical observation appears to
in children under the age of 3 is needed; if only be the most stable and reliable method
some of the diagnostic criteria are applicable, (Adrien et al., 1992; Chawarska et al., sub-
a different algorithm for the youngest group mitted; Cox et al., 1999; Gillberg et al., 1990;
may be considered necessary (Stone, Lee, Klin et al., in press; Lord, 1995; Stone, Lee,
et al., 1999). et al., 1999). Results of the only two studies
reporting on diagnosis of autism in children
Early Diagnosis and Autism Diagnostic under 24 months suggests that in this age
Interview-Revised group, clinical diagnosis was relatively stable,
with 75% to 90% children diagnosed with
Despite the fact that the ADI-R (Lord et al., autism retaining the diagnosis at follow-up,
1994) has been found highly effective in diag- and the remaining cases receiving another
nosing autism in individuals over the age of 4 PDD diagnosis (Chawarska et al., in press-a;
years, its utility for diagnosis of very young Cox et al., 1999). Thus, when a broader con-
children (specifically, individuals with MA cept of “autistic spectrum disorder ” (ASD)
below 2 years) is rather problematic (Cox was applied to the diagnosis in the second
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 233

year of life all children positively identified also in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers
as on the spectrum continued to exhibit symp- (Chawarska et al., submitted; Cox et al., 1999;
toms of ASD at 3 years. However, the rate of Lord, 1995; Stone, Ousley, et al., 1999). Not
cases identified falsely as not on the spec- surprisingly, the reliability of clinical diagno-
trum at 20 months was very high in this study sis is highly correlated with the extent of clini-
(Cox et al., 1999). It is not clear whether the cians’ experience evaluating young children
high rates of false negative cases at 20 months (Stone, Ousley, et al., 1999). Expert clinicians
is due to problems with diagnostic criteria are likely to simultaneously consider a number
used at this age or whether they might be at- of complementary factors, including the
tributed to a later onset or increase in sever- child’s history, developmental level, adaptive
ity of symptoms. functioning, verbal and nonverbal communica-
In 2- and 3-year-old children, the clinical tion, and level of social engagement and imagi-
diagnosis continues to be stable, and the rate of nation, leading to a more accurate estimate of
false negatives diminishes (Lord, 1995; Stone, the probability of the child’s having autism
Lee, et al., 1999). Lord (1995) examined sta- (Lord & Risi, 2000). In general, the short-term
bility of autism diagnosis in a sample of 2- stability of clinical diagnosis is very high.
year-olds referred for a differential diagnosis Consistent with the findings reported on older
and reassessed at the age of 4. At follow-up, children (Volkmar et al., 1994), the PDD-NOS
88% of 4-year-olds retained the diagnosis. The diagnosis is less stable (Stone, Lee, et al.,
remaining 12% children were rediagnosed 1999). Although most of these children remain
with a nonautism disorder (developmental on the spectrum at the age of 3, in some chil-
delay and specific language disorder). Only dren the symptoms worsen and become consis-
14% of children who were initially diagnosed tent with the diagnosis of autism. The rate of
with a nonautistic disorder at the age of 2 re- false negative cases; that is, the proportion of
ceived a diagnosis of autism at age 3. Stone children who were classified either as typical
and her colleagues (1999) also examined the or as having various non-PDD disabilities,
reliability and stability of the clinical diagno- varies highly depending on the study and is
sis over a 1-year period in a group of children likely to be negatively correlated with the age
under the age of 3 years diagnosed with autism at first diagnosis and the child’s cognitive
or PDD-NOS. A follow-up assessment indi- level. Late onset of autism in children and lim-
cated that the clinical diagnosis was the most ited sensitivity of the diagnostic procedures
reliable and stable for children exhibiting are among the factors responsible for cases
symptoms of autism, as 72% of children re- that go undetected in the first 3 years of life.
tained the diagnosis, 24% improved and re-
ceived a PDD-NOS diagnosis, and only 4% SPECIFIC AREAS OF FUNCTIONING
were diagnosed with a disorder outside the
autism spectrum. Children diagnosed at age 2 Early observations (Kanner, 1943/1968) and
with PDD-NOS had a more variable outcome. more recent experimental studies (Charman
Although 92% of them remained on the spec- et al., 1997; Chawarska, Klin, Paul, & Volk-
trum, in about half of the children, symptoms mar, in press-b; Cox et al., 1999; Dawson &
worsened and they were diagnosed with Adams, 1984; Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, &
autism. The remaining half showed no or only Rinaldi, 1998; Dawson, Munson, et al., 2002;
slight improvement. Because only children Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Mundy, Sig-
with an ASD diagnosis were included in this man, Ungerer, & Sherman, 1986; Sigman &
study, the rate of false negative cases was not Ungerer, 1981, 1984) suggest that in the first
examined. years of life, development of children with
autism progresses at varying rates and, in
Summary some cases, is characterized by significant de-
lays and abnormalities in certain areas of de-
Clinical diagnosis continues to constitute the velopment but not others. This developmental
gold standard in diagnosis of ASDs not only in decalage persists over time, and in school-age
school-age children (Volkmar et al., 1994) but children manifests in syndrome-specific
234 Development and Behavior

patterns of cognitive, communication, and apparent at the age of 5 years and are ex-
adaptive skills (Ehlers et al., 1997; Freeman, pressed in impaired performance on tasks con-
Ritvo, Yokota, Childs, & Pollard, 1988; Joseph cerning rule learning, visual recognition
et al., 2002; Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cic- memory, working spatial memory, and prepo-
chetti, et al., 1995; Volkmar et al., 1994). tent response inhibition (Dawson, Meltzoff,
Although young children with autism pre- Osterling, & Rinaldi, 1998; McEvoy, Rogers,
sent with many syndrome-specific deficits, & Pennington, 1993). Such developmental
delays in sensorimotor development and exec- trends suggest that the elementary EF skills
utive function in infants and preschool chil- emerging early in development may not be af-
dren with autism do not appear either specific fected differentially in autism (Dawson, Mun-
or universal. The sensorimotor stage of cogni- son, et al., 2002).
tive development extends from birth through
the age of language acquisition and symbolic Attentional Functioning
thinking (Piaget, 1954) and in typical devel-
opment concludes by the end of the second Although a number of attentional abnormalities
year of life. In the classic Piagetian sense, in autism have been documented, their extent,
sensorimotor intelligence encompasses con- as well as their centrality to autism, remains to
cepts of object permanence, as well as space be clarified (see Burack, Enns, Stauder, Mot-
and causality. A number of studies indicate tron, & Randolph, 1997; Tsatsanis, Chapter 13,
that although young children with autism ex- this Handbook, this volume, for a review). One
perience delays in these areas, these deficits of the important aspects of attention is the abil-
are not syndrome specific (Chawarska et al., ity to select salient elements or features of the
in press; Cox et al., 1999; Dawson, Munson, environment for further processing (James,
et al., 2002; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984), and 1890/1950). Studies on older individuals with
eventually, most children with an MA above autism suggest that their spontaneous visual
24 months master the central sensorimotor attention to people in general and to faces in
concepts (Morgan, Cutrer, Coplin, & Rod- particular is diminished as compared with de-
rigue, 1989). velopmentally delayed groups (Klin, Jones,
Executive function (EF) skills encompass a Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen, 2002; Volkmar &
broad range of cognitive abilities including Mayes, 1990). A similar trend has been ob-
planning, flexibility of thought and action, in- served in infants and toddlers with autism. In
hibition of irrelevant responses and stimuli, as the first year of life, affected infants visually
well as working memory (i.e., holding online orient less frequently to people as compared
mental representations). A number of studies with typical and developmentally delayed
on adults, school-age children, and adolescents controls (Baranek, 1999; Maestro et al., 2002;
with autism suggest impairments in EF (Ben- Osterling et al., 2002). This selective bias per-
netto, Pennington, & Rogers, 1996; Ozonoff, sists in the second year and beyond (Dawson,
Pennington, & Rogers, 1991; Pennington & Meltzoff, Osterling, & Rinaldi, 1998; Dawson,
Ozonoff, 1996), but the relationship between Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998;
autism and EFs is unclear (Bennetto et al., Swettenham et al., 1998). For instance, Swet-
1996; Russell, Jarrold, & Henry, 1996; Russell, tenham and colleagues examined frequency
Jarrold, & Hood, 1999). Several studies em- and distribution of spontaneous visual atten-
ploying a wide range of tasks tapping EF tion directed at people and objects during a
skills—such as prepotent response inhibition, free play session in a group of 20-month-old
spatial or object working memory, set shifting children with autism, developmental delays,
and action monitoring—indicate that deficits and typical peers. Infants with autism spent a
in preschool children with autism are not syn- greater proportion of their play focused on ob-
drome specific and are comparable to those ob- jects and a significantly smaller proportion of
served in developmentally delayed controls time fixating visually on people and monitor-
matched for MA (Dawson, Munson, et al., ing their behavior as compared with the two
2002; Griffith, Pennington, Wehner, & Rogers, control groups. Similarly, preschool children
1999). Specific deficits in EF become more with autism have been found to monitor com-
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 235

municative behaviors of adults interacting nation in a group of preschool children with


with them much less frequently than matched MA of approximately 22 months and expres-
children with developmental language disorder sive language age of 14 to 15 months. Al-
and typical children (McArthur & Adamson, though children with autism had a similar rate
1996). At the age of 5, children with autism of well-formed canonical vocalizations ( bab-
were less likely to orient to both social and bling), their vocalizations had more abnormal
nonsocial stimuli than the two comparison vocal quality than the developmentally de-
groups, but this impairment was more severe layed comparison group and included squeals,
when social stimuli were concerned (Dawson, growls, and yells.
Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998).
Nonverbal Communication
Several hypotheses have been proposed to ac-
count for such patterns of visual attention. One Disturbances in the emergence of nonverbal
such hypothesis suggests that children with communication are one of the most prototypi-
autism may avoid complex visual stimuli such cal and most extensively studied features of
as faces (Swettenham et al., 1998). Another autism in infants and preschool children. Non-
suggests that children with autism may have a verbal communication skills reflect motiva-
preference for perfect contingencies and, thus, tion to communicate, understanding of how to
avoid inherently unpredictable and variable so- communicate, and basic representational skills
cial stimuli (Dawson & Lewy, 1989). Yet an- (Bates, 1979; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). In the
other hypothesis suggests that the deficit in first 9 months of typical development, infants
social attention might be related to motivation are able to effectively communicate their
and salience of social rewards (Dawson, needs by a variety of means, including reach-
Carver, et al., 2002). ing for a desirable object or fussing and crying.
These communicative attempts are usually di-
Preverbal Communication rected at the goal itself and not at the person
that might be instrumental in attaining the
Delays in development of speech and commu- goal. At about 9 months, infants begin to direct
nication are usually the first to be noticed by their communicative attempts at adults by, for
parents (53.7%) and the most frequently re- instance, making eye contact with an adult
ported (74.4%; De Giacomo & Fombonne, while reaching for a distant toy. Along with
1998). Compared to typically developing chil- this change, infants begin to substitute the
dren, most children with autism develop lan- early emerging physical gestures (e.g., an
guage later, and their language development is open-hand reach) with conventional gestures
marked by the presence of unusual features such as pointing or showing. Their commu-
(see Tager-Flusberg, Lord, & Paul, Chapter nicative behavior becomes protoimperative, as
12, this Handbook, this volume). it involves using human agents to attain nonso-
cial goals, as well as protodeclarative, involv-
Vocalizations
ing the use of nonsocial means for social
The available data suggest that preverbal chil- purposes of, for instance, sharing attention
dren with autism show abnormal patterns of (Bates, 1979). Emergence of these behaviors
sound production. Based on the analysis of vo- at the end of the first year of life marks, ac-
calizations of three prelinguistic children with cording to Bates, the beginnings of intentional
autism ages 2.5 to 4 years, Wetherby, Yonclas, communication, communication in which a
and Bryan (1989) noted that although their child is aware a priori that his or her behavior
rate of communication was within the normal will have an effect on a listener.
range for their stage of language development, This developmental transition is of particu-
they had defects in well-formed syllable pro- lar importance for the discussion of deficits in
duction and displayed overproduction of atypi- nonverbal communication in autism. As com-
cal vocalizations such as growling, tongue pared with children with MR or Down syn-
clicking, and trills. More recently, Sheinkopf, drome matched for MA, preverbal children
Mundy, Oller, and Steffens (2000) studied with autism communicate less frequently
vowel-like and consonant-like sounds and into- (Stone, Ousley, Yoder, Hogan, & Hepburn,
236 Development and Behavior

1997; Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, Milders, & Brown, 1997; Volkmar & Mayes,
1988) and use less complex combinations of 1990; Whalen & Schreibman, 2003). More-
nonverbal behaviors to communicate (Stone over, the sequence of skill acquisition appears
et al., 1997). Specifically, 2-year-old children to differ from that observed in typical develop-
with autism are less likely to use eye contact, ment, which may be suggestive of the develop-
conventional gestures such as distal and proxi- ment of alternative compensatory processing
mal pointing and showing gestures; are more strategies (Carpenter, Pennington, & Rogers,
likely to manipulate the examiner’s hand using 2002).
hand-over-hand gestures; and are less likely to
pair their communicative gestures with eye Exploration and Play
contact and vocalizations compared with their
developmentally delayed peers (Stone et al., Piaget (1962) was the first to stress the pro-
1997). At the same time, the autism and MR gressive nature of mental representation in
groups do not differ in terms of the proportion play and its role in developing and understand-
of communicative acts that involve reaching, ing symbols. In the first year of life, play
giving objects, touching objects, or vocalizing. consists of nonsymbolic, undifferentiated ex-
A disproportionately high number of the ploration of sensorimotor characteristics of
communicative behaviors that are observed in objects, their texture, color, details, and the
young children with autism serve a purpose of sounds they produce. In the second year of life,
requesting objects or actions (i.e., protoimpera- play evolves into functional-relational play
tive communication) with very few communica- closely tied to the conventional functions of
tive behaviors aimed at directing another’s objects, which then gives way to pretend play.
attention to an object or event (i.e., protodeclar- This form of play becomes increasingly gener-
ative communication) (Baron-Cohen, 1989; ative as children enact activities first in sim-
Curcio, 1978; Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, Ri- ple and then in multistep pretend scenarios
naldi, & Brown, 1998; Loveland & Landry, using, first, objects, then placeholders (Tamis-
1986; Mundy & Crowson, 1997; Mundy, Sig- LeMonda & Bornstein, 1991). The onset of
man, & Kasari, 1994; Mundy et al., 1986, symbolic play usually coincides with the be-
1990; Roeyers, Van Oost, & Bothuyne, 1998; ginning of language; in both cases, the child
Sigman, Mundy, Sherman, & Ungerer, 1986; manifests the ability to represent an arbitrary
Sigman & Ruskin, 1999; Stone et al., 1997). stimulus (e.g., a block or a word) as something
This act of recruiting or following the atten- else (e.g., an airplane), ignoring, in a sense, the
tion of another person for the purpose of shar- primary, or first-order representation of a
ing interest or enjoyment is often referred to block as a block and focusing on its secondary,
as joint attention (Bruner, 1975; Mundy & symbolic aspect (Piaget, 1962).
Sigman, 1989; Tomasello, 1995). Joint atten- The videotape analysis studies do not pro-
tion behaviors have strong predictive relation- vide strong evidence regarding disrupted ob-
ships with receptive and expressive language ject exploration in the first year of life. Infants
development and nonverbal communication, as with autism appear to attend visually to ob-
well as social-cognitive development. (see jects with similar frequency as their typical
Mundy & Burnette, Chapter 25, this Hand- and developmentally delayed peers (Baranek,
book, this volume, for a review). 1999; Maestro et al., 2002; Osterling et al.,
Although the ability to respond to and initi- 2002). However, 9- to 12-month-old infants
ate joint attention bids in autism increases over with autism have been reported to mouth ob-
time, especially in highly structured contexts jects more frequently (Baranek, 1999), which
(Leekam, Hunnisett, & Moore, 1998; Leekam, might suggest emerging sensory-seeking be-
Lopez, & Moore, 2000), individuals with haviors or an extended phase of low-level sen-
autism continue to have difficulties using these sory exploration. Although at 12 months the
skills adaptively and spontaneously in more behaviors of children with autism tend to be
naturalistic situations (Baron-Cohen, Baldwin, more repetitive than those of typical peers,
& Crowson, 1997; Baron-Cohen, Campbell, this attribute does not differentiate them from
Karmiloff-Smith, Grant, & Walker, 1995; Klin developmentally delayed peers (Baranek,
et al., 2002; Leekam, Baron-Cohen, Perrett, 1999; Osterling et al., 2002).
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 237

The second year of life is dominated by in- Ratner, 1993) and thus are central for studies
creasingly complex functional play with some of autistic psychopathology (Rogers & Pen-
elements of pretense emerging toward the sec- nington, 1991). Motor imitation, or copying a
ond birthday. While syndrome-specific im- feature of the body movements of a model, re-
pairments in play have been widely described quires detection and analysis of movements,
in preschool and school-age children, it is not that is, the ability to translate visual informa-
clear whether they are already present in the tion regarding body movements of others into
second year of life. A study of a small group of matching motor output as well as appreciation
20-month-old infants with autism (MA = 17 for an intentional aspect of such movement
months) suggests that at this age, the impair- (Tomasello et al., 1993). In contrast, in emula-
ments in play may not be fully differentiated, tion the action copied has to do with reproduc-
as infants with autism engage in functional tion of the object movement rather than the
play with a similar frequency as the CA- and model’s body movement.
MA-matched developmentally delayed con- Studies on imitation in school-age children
trols (Charman et al., 1997). Likewise, their with autism consistently report deficits in this
rate of pretend play does not differ from MA- area as compared to other developmentally
matched developmentally delayed controls but challenged populations (Bartak, Rutter, &
is lower than CA-matched typical peers. These Cox, 1975; Rogers, 1999; Rogers & Penning-
findings are congruent with a longitudinal ton, 1991). Specifically, difficulties with imi-
analysis of videotapes focused on the develop- tation but not emulation of actions of others
ment of presymbolic play in a small group of (Hobson & Lee, 1999), including imitation of
children with autism and typical controls fol- both meaningful and meaningless hand and
lowed between 4 and 42 months of age face movements (Rogers, Bennetto, McEvoy,
(Losche, 1990). The study suggested that up to & Pennington, 1996; Smith & Bryson, 1998),
21 months of age, children with autism en- have been reported.
gaged in various explorations of objects with While it is not clear whether, in children
frequency and quality similar to their typical with autism, deficits in imitation and emula-
peers, but starting at 22 months, the frequency tion are already present in the first year of life,
of goal-oriented exploration reached a plateau the existing evidence suggests that such
in infants with autism, while it continued to deficits are detectable at the end of the second
diversify and increase in frequency in typical year of life (Charman, Baron-Cohen, Swetten-
controls. ham, et al., 2001; Charman et al., 1997).
Deficits in functional and symbolic play in Twenty-month-old infants with autism were
relation to other cognitive skills have been able to emulate fewer actions with objects than
well documented in preschool children with their matched developmentally delayed controls.
autism (McDonough, Stahmer, Schreibman, & The frequency of emulation was associated with
Thompson, 1997; Mundy et al., 1986; Sigman the level of nonverbal functioning, but impair-
& Ruskin, 1999; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984; ments observed in the autism group appeared
Stone & Caro-Martinez, 1990). While during syndrome specific. Syndrome-specific impair-
the preschool period, play skills continue to ments in gesture imitation in toddlers and
develop and can be enhanced through prompts, preschoolers with autism (Dawson & Adams,
scaffolding, and modeling, children with 1984; Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, & Ri-
autism continue to engage in little spontaneous naldi, 1998; Roeyers et al., 1998; Rogers,
functional and pretend play and their play Hepburn, Stackhouse, & Wehner, 2003; Sig-
lacks in generativity (Lewis & Boucher, 1988; man & Ungerer, 1984) and emulation of ac-
McDonough et al., 1997; see Chapter 14, this tions with objects (Dawson et al., 1998;
Handbook, this volume). Roeyers et al., 1998; Rogers et al., 2003) have
been reported as well. Finally, imitation skills
Motor Imitation tend to be below the level expected based on
other aspects of sensorimotor development,
Motor imitation and emulation play an impor- such as object permanence, and they are posi-
tant role in the emergence of both symbolic and tively correlated with frequency of sponta-
social-cognitive skills (Tomasello, Kruger, & neous social and communicative behaviors
238 Development and Behavior

(Dawson & Adams, 1984) and language skills Thus, similar to older children, young children
(Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). with autism exhibit syndrome-specific patterns
of developmental and adaptive skills.
Adaptive Functioning
Attachment
Studies of adaptive functioning, or the devel-
opment and application of abilities in order to Attachment, or affective bond between a child
achieve personal independence and social suf- and a mothering figure (Ainsworth, Blehar,
ficiency (Cicchetti & Sparrow, 1990) in Waters, & Wall, 1978), has been extensively
school-age children with autism, suggest that studied in children with autism. The results of
compared with MA- or IQ-matched controls, numerous studies using the Strange Situation
these children have overall lower scores (Lord paradigm provide limited evidence of syndrome-
& Schopler, 1989) and present with greater specific deficits in this area (Capps, Sigman,
variability in the profile of adaptive skills & Mundy, 1994; Rogers, Ozonoff, & Maslin-
(Burack & Volkmar, 1992; Klin et al., 1992; Cole, 1993; Waterhouse & Fein, 1998). These
Volkmar et al., 1987). In particular, they show findings appear counterintuitive considering
specific deficits in the area of socialization numerous reports regarding parental percep-
relative to other areas of adaptive behaviors as tion of their children’s impoverished affective
well as cognitive skills (Volkmar, Carter, bond with their mother, as well as presence of
Sparrow, & Cicchetti, 1993). This pattern of attachments to unusual transitional objects
relative social impairment as compared to (e.g., hard objects or objects of specific
other adaptive skills and cognitive levels ap- classes; Volkmar, Cohen, & Paul, 1986). It has
pears highly characteristic of autism but not been hypothesized that children with autism,
other nonautistic PDD disorders (Gillham, although capable of developing a strategic
Carter, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000). form of attachment, may lack the affiliative
Parent retrospective interview data suggest form of social behaviors and, thus, may differ-
that young children with autism show deficits entiate parents from strangers and seek their
in the development of social adaptive skills proximity for comfort but are less likely to dis-
that include skills usually mastered in the first play affiliative behaviors for purely social pur-
year of life (Klin et al., 1992). Klin and col- poses (Waterhouse & Fein, 1998).
leagues examined the adaptive behaviors of a
group of 4-year-old children with autism, and EARLY INTERVENTION
children with developmental delays matched
for MA, CA, and IQ, using the Vineland Adap- A growing body of work has highlighted the
tive Behaviors Scale (Vineland; Sparrow et al., importance of early intervention in autism
1984). They reported difficulties in skills usu- and related disorders. As summarized in the
ally mastered by 6 to 8 months including, the National Research Council (2001) review of
ability to anticipate being picked up, interest early intervention, a number of programs
in other children, playing simple interactive have been able to provide reasonably strong
games with others, and showing affection to- evidence on intervention efficacy. Differ-
ward others. A specific pattern of delays in the ences among the programs include aspects of
development of adaptive behaviors has been theoretical orientation, the degree to which
reported in toddlers and preschoolers with the child or teacher sets the learning agenda,
autism (Stone, Ousley et al., 1999). Stone and and the degree to which the approach is based
her colleagues documented that compared to on developmental principles. Areas of simi-
CA- and MA-matched controls, 2-year-old larity have included intensity of treatment
children with autism had significantly lower (averaging about 27 hours a week), the impor-
age equivalent scores in the Communication tance of treatment structure, and intensive
and Socialization domains. Their scores in teaching approaches as well as a focus on
these areas were also significantly lower as helping the child become more able to learn
compared to their overall mental level, suggest- independently.
ing profound difficulties in translating their The most widely studied intervention ap-
cognitive potential into real-life functioning. proach has been applied behavioral analysis
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 239

(ABA), although a growing body of work has diaries, and direct observations have ad-
focused on more eclectic intervention models vanced the field toward identification of de-
(National Research Council, 2001). It is grat- velopmentally sensitive diagnostic criteria
ifying that controlled studies are beginning to for infants and toddlers with autism. These
appear (Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, symptoms cluster around deficits in early
2002; Smith, Groen, & Wynn, 2000). Much of emerging social reciprocity and nonverbal
the available research is based on older (i.e., communication skills. Although unusual re-
preschool) children, and it remains unclear sponses to sensory stimuli and motor manner-
how easily current approaches can be adapted isms are often present in the second and third
to work with infants and very young children. years of life, their frequency and intensity are
The problem of early intervention in in- highly variable. Clinical diagnosis, particu-
fancy will likely increase substantially over larly when determined by an experienced
the next decade as the age of first diagnosis clinician, continues to constitute the gold
continues to fall (Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, standard as compared to traditional diagnos-
Schultz, & Klin, 2004). It is even possible that tic instruments. Its short-term stability as
more physiologically or genetically based well as specificity and sensitivity is high,
methods may be able to detect potential risk particularly in children over the age of 24
for autism in the first months of life. The ob- months. Similar to older children, infants and
servation that the age at which treatment is preschoolers with autism present with highly
begun may be an important factor in predict- variable profiles of strengths and weaknesses.
ing outcome (Harris & Handleman, 2000) Although young children with autism fre-
highlights the urgency of this problem. It will quently experience developmental delays,
be important for treatment studies in this some areas of development appear specifi-
population to focus on specific processes; for cally affected in autism. These include abnor-
example, attempts have been made to focus malities in selective visual attention to social
on developmental skills, which are presumed stimuli, nonverbal communication, delays and
to underlie other areas of difficulty (Kasari, abnormalities in play development, deficits
2002). Data on outcome from older children in imitation and emulation skills, and delays
enrolled in intervention also make it clear that in acquisition of social and communicative
even with good interventions, some children adaptive skills. In contrast, although delays in
do not improve as well as others; understand- sensorimotor and EF skills have been re-
ing this phenomenon remains an important ported, they do not appear to be syndrome
focus of theory and research (Mundy, 2003). specific.
Although there has been a growing interest in Early detection of autism and other PDDs
teaching social skills to older children with is likely to remain a focus of autism research.
autism, the applicability of such approaches National Institutes of Health as well as nu-
to younger children remains to be explored. It merous private foundations set early detection
is likely that more effective methods will of autism and its underlying mechanisms as
focus on the social-communicative underpin- one of their priorities. The future directions
nings of subsequent development. Such work of research are likely to include efforts toward
will force both clinicians and researchers to refinement of the diagnostic criteria including
attempt to disentangle the complex interrela- other PDD disorders as well as identification
tionships of early developmental process and of predictors of outcome. It will be critical
to address issues of mechanisms and treat- to turn attention to high-risk populations of
ment moderators as well an individual re- siblings of children with autism and address
sponse differences. essential questions regarding their develop-
mental trajectories and the mechanisms un-
CONCLUSION derlying the emergence of autism.

In the past decade, interest in early diagnosis Cross-References


and developmental profiles of children with
PDDs has increased substantially. Studies Issues of diagnosis and classification are
based on parent report, analysis of videotaped discussed in Section I (Chapters 1 through 7);
240 Development and Behavior

development in school-age children and in ado- significance of the eyes? British Journal of De-
lescents and adults with autism spectrum dis- velopmental Psychology, 13(4), 379–398.
orders are addressed in Chapters 9 and 10, Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Sweettenham,
respectively; screening and diagnostic instru- J., & Nighingale, N. (1996). Psychological
ments are reviewed in Chapters 27 and 28. markers in the detection of autism in infancy
in a large population. British Journal of Psy-
Also, see Chapter 14 for a review of play and
chiatry, 168(2), 158–163.
imitation development and Chapter 25 for re- Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A com-
view of issues related to joint attention in parative study of infantile autism and specific
autism. developmental receptive language disorder: I.
The children. British Journal of Psychiatry,
REFERENCES 126, 127–145.
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cogni-
Adrien, J. L., Barthelemy, C., Perrot, A., Roux, S., tion and communication in infancy. New York:
Lenoir, P., Hameury, L. et al. (1992). Validity Academic Press.
and reliability of the Infant Behavioral Sum- Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
marized Evaluation (IBSE): A rating scale for (1996). Intact and impaired memory func-
the assessment of young children with autism tions in autism. Child Development, 67(4),
and developmental disorders. Journal of 1816–1835.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(3), Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech
375–394. acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19.
Adrien, J. L., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., Perrot, A., Bruner, J. S. (1981). The social context of language
Hameury, L., Larmande, C., et al. (1993). Blind acquisition. Language and Communication,
ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon 1(Suppl. 2/3), 155–178.
family home movies. Journal of the American Burack, J. A., Enns, J., Stauder, J., Mottron, L., &
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Randolph, B. (1997). Attention in autism: Be-
32(3), 617–626. havioral and electrophysiological evidence. In
Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, D. J. Cohen, & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Hand-
S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psycholog- book of autism and pervasive developmental
ical study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, disorders. New York: Wiley.
NJ: Erlbaum. Burack, J. A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Develop-
Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., ment of low- and high-functioning autistic
Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., et al. (2000). children. Journal of Child Psychology and
A screening instrument for autism at 18 Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33(3),
months of age: A 6-year follow-up study. Jour- 607–616.
nal of the American Academy of Child and Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). At-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(6), 694–702. tachment security in children with autism.
Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A Development and Psychopathology, 6(2),
retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor 249–261.
and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. E., & Rogers, S. J.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive
ders, 29(3), 213–224. skills in young children with autism. Journal
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2),
autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Develop- 91–106.
ment and Psychopathology, 1(3), 185–189. Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Baird, G., Cox, A.,
Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Wheelwright, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (2001).
Can autism be detected at 18 months? The Commentary: The Modified Checklist for
needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Autism in Toddlers. Journal of Autism and De-
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839–843. velopmental Disorders, 31(2), 145–148.
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J.,
(1997). Do children with autism use the Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing
speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy
the code of language? Child Development, precursors to language and theory of mind.
68(1), 48–57. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498.
Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S.,
A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are chil- Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants
dren with autism blind to the mentalistic with autism: An investigation of empathy, pre-
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 241

tend play, joint attention, and imitation. Devel- Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., & Ri-
opmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789. naldi, J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. R. of early symptoms of autism. Child Develop-
(in press-a). Autism spectrum disorders under ment, 69(5), 1276–1285.
the age of 2 years: Stability and change over Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi,
time in syndrome expression. Journal of Child J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism
and Psychology and Psychiatry. fail to orient to naturally occurring social
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. R. stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental
(in press-b). Diagnostic and developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.
profiles of 2-year-olds with Autism Spectrum Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J.,
Disorders. Journal of the American Academy McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neu-
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. rocognitive function and joint attention ability
Christopher, J. A., Sears, L. L., Williams, P. G., in young children with autism spectrum disor-
Oliver, J., & Hersh, J. (2004). Familial, med- der versus developmental delay. Child Devel-
ical and developmental patterns of children opment, 73(2), 345–358.
with autism and a history of language regres- Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl,
sion. Journal of Developmental and Physical P. (2000). Case study of the development of an
Disabilities, 16(2), 163–170. infant with autism from birth to two years of
Cicchetti, D. V., & Sparrow, S. S. (1990). Assess- age. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
ment of adaptive behavior in young children. In chology, 21(3), 299–313.
J. H. Johnson & J. Goldman (Eds.), Develop- De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental
mental assessment in clinical child psychology: recognition of developmental abnormalities in
A handbook. Pergamon general psychology se- autism. European Child and Adolescent Psy-
ries (Vol. 163, pp. 173–196). New York: Perga- chiatry, 7(3), 131–136.
mon Press. DiLavore, P. C., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (1995). Pre-
Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron- linguistic autism diagnostic observation
Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999). schedule. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 Disorders, 25(4), 355–379.
months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Sandberg, A.,
diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Dahlgren, S., Hjelmquist, E., et al. (1997). As-
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), perger syndrome, autism and attention disor-
719–732. ders: A comparative study of the cognitive
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and profiles of 120 children. Journal of Child Psy-
communication in mute autistic children. Jour- chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 38(2), 207–217.
8(3), 281–292. Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S.
Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms (2002). Intensive behavioral treatment at
in the first two years of life: A preliminary school for 4- to 7-year-old children with
population study of infantile autism. European autism: A 1-year comparison controlled study.
Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sci- Behavior Modification, 26(1), 49–68.
ences, 238(3), 169–174. Eisenberg, L., & Kanner, L. (1956). Early infantile
Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and so- autism, 1943–55. American Journal of Or-
cial responsiveness in autistic children. Jour- thopsychiatry, 26, 556–566.
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12(2), Fombonne, E., & Chakrabarti, S. (2001). No evi-
209–225. dence for a new variant of measles-mumps-
Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagi- rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics, 108(4),
otides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. 1–8.
(2002). Neural correlates of face and object Freeman, B., Ritvo, E. R., Yokota, A., Childs, J., &
recognition in young children with autism Pollard, J. (1988). WISC-R and Vineland
spectrum disorder, developmental delay and Adaptive Behavior Scale scores in autistic
typical development. Child Development, children. Journal of the American Academy of
73(3), 700–717. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(4),
Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, attention, 428–429.
and the socioemotional impairments of indi- Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., Jakobs-
viduals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), son, G., Dahlgren, S. O., Lindblom, R., et al.
Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (1990). Autism under age 3 years: A clini-
(pp. 49–74). New York: Guilford Press. cal study of 28 cases referred for autistic
242 Development and Behavior

symptoms in infancy. Journal of Child Psy- of the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
plines, 31(6), 921–934. Disciplines, 33(5), 861–876.
Gillham, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A., Volkmar, F., Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., &
Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Toward a developmental Rourke, B. (1995). Validity and neuropsycho-
operational definition of autism. Journal of logical characterization of Asperger syn-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), drome: Convergence with nonverbal learning
269–278. disabilities syndrome. Journal of Child Psy-
Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
& Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in 36(7), 1127–1140.
young children with autism. Child Develop- Kurita, H. (1985). Infantile autism with speech
ment, 70(4), 817–832. loss before the age of thirty months. Journal of
Harris, S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (2000). Age and the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
IQ at intake as predictors of placement for 24(2), 191–196.
young children with autism: A four- to six- Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P.,
year follow-up. Journal of Autism and Develop- Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989).
mental Disorders, 30(2), 137–142. Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized
Hobson, R., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identi- investigator-based instrument. Journal of
fication in autism. Journal of Child Psychology Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3),
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 363–387.
649–659. Leekam, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Perrett, D., Milders,
Hoshino, Y., Kumashiro, H., Yashima, Y., Tachi- M., & Brown, S. (1997). Eye-direction detec-
bana, R., Watanabe, M., & Furukawa, H. (1982). tion: A dissociation between geometric and
Early symptoms of autistic children and its di- joint attention skills in autism. British Journal
agnostic significance. Folia Psychiatrica et of Developmental Psychology, 15(Pt. 1),
Neurologica, 36, 367–374. 77–95.
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. Leekam, S., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998).
New York: Dover. (Original work published Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children
1890) with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Joseph, R. M., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lord, C. (2002). Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(7),
Cognitive profiles and social-communicative 951–962.
functioning in children with autism spectrum Leekam, S., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Atten-
disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- tion and joint attention in preschool children
chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(6), 807–821. with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2),
Kanner, L. (1968). Autistic disturbances of affec- 261–273.
tive contact. Acta Paedopsychiatrica: Interna- Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, in-
tional Journal of Child and Adolescent structed and elicited play in relatively able
Psychiatry, 35(4–8), 98–136. (Original work autistic children. British Journal of Develop-
published 1943) mental Psychology, 6(4), 325–339.
Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early inter- Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds re-
vention programs for children with autism. ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
ders, 32(5), 447–461. plines, 36(8), 1365–1382.
Klin, A., Chawarska, K., Paul, R., Rubin, E., Mor- Lord, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (1995). Peer inter-
gan, T., Wiesner, L., et al. (2004). Clinical actions of autistic children and adolescents.
case conference: Autism at the age of 15 Development and Psychopathology, 7(4),
months. American Journal of Psychiatry, 611–626.
161(11): 1981–1988. Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (1996). Language level and
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & nonverbal social-communicative behaviors in
Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns autistic and language-delayed children. Jour-
during viewing of naturalistic social situations nal of the American Academy of Child & Ado-
as predictors of social competence in individu- lescent Psychiatry, 35(11), 1542–1550.
als with autism. Archives of General Psychia- Lord, C., & Risi, S. (2000). Diagnosis of autism
try, 59(9), 809–816. spectrum disorders in young children. In
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). A. M. Wetherby & B. M. Prizant (Eds.),
Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations Autism spectrum disorders: A transactional de-
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 243

velopmental perspective. Communication and McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F.
language intervention series (Vol. 9, (1993). Executive function and social commu-
pp. 11–30). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. nication deficits in young autistic children.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. and Allied Disciplines, 34(4), 563–578.
(2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Morgan, S. B., Cutrer, P. S., Coplin, J. W., & Rod-
Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of rigue, J. R. (1989). Do autistic children differ
social and communication deficits associated from retarded and normal children in Piaget-
with the spectrum of autism. Journal of ian sensorimotor functioning? Journal of Child
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
205–223. plines, 30(6), 857–864.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Mundy, P. (2003). The neural basis of social im-
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A re- pairments in autism: The role of the dorsal
vised version of a diagnostic interview for medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate
caregivers of individuals with possible perva- system. Journal of Child Psychology and
sive developmental disorders. Journal of Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(6),
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 793–809.
659–685. Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention
Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1989). The role of age at and early social communication: Implications
assessment, developmental level, and test in for research on intervention with autism. Jour-
the stability of intelligence scores in young nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel- 27(6), 653–676.
opmental Disorders, 19(4), 483–499. Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical
Lord, C., Shulman, C., & DiLavore, P. (2004). Re- implications of joint-attention deficits in
gression and word loss in autistic spectrum autism. Development and Psychopathology,
disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and 1(3), 173–183.
Psychiatry, 45(5), 936–955. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A lon-
Losche, G. (1990). Sensorimotor and action devel- gitudinal study of joint attention and language
opment in autistic children from infancy to development in autistic children. Journal of
early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1),
and Psychiatry, 31, 749–761. 115–128.
Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint at- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint
tention and language in autism and develop- attention, developmental level, and symptom
mental language delay. Journal of Autism and presentation in autism. Development and
Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 335–349. Psychopathology, 6(3), 389–401.
Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Barbieri, F., Casella, C., Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T.
Cattaneo, V., Cavallaro, M., et al. (2001). (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism:
Early behavioral development in autistic chil- The contribution of non-verbal communica-
dren: The first 2 years of life through home tion measures. Journal of Child Psychology
movies. Psychopathology, 34(3), 147–152. and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27(5),
Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., 657–669.
Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002). Attentional National Research Council (Ed.). (2001). Educat-
skills during the first 6 months of age in ing children with autism. Washington, DC: Na-
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the tional Academy Press.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Ornitz, E. M., Guthrie, D., & Farley, A. H. (1977).
Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239–1245. The early development of autistic children.
McArthur, D., & Adamson, L. B. (1996). Joint at- Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre-
tention in preverbal children: Autism and de- nia, 7(3), 207–229.
velopmental language disorder. Journal of Osterling, J. A., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(5), recognition of children with autism: A study
481–496. of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of
McDonough, L., Stahmer, A., Schreibman, L., & Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3),
Thompson, S. J. (1997). Deficits, delays, and 247–257.
distractions: An evaluation of symbolic play Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A.
and memory in children with autism. Develop- (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants
ment and Psychopathology, 9(1), 17–41. with autism spectrum disorder versus mental
244 Development and Behavior

retardation. Development and Psychopathol- autism. Development and Psychopathology,


ogy, 14(2), 239–251. 3(2), 137–162.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Henry, L. (1996). Work-
(1991). Executive function deficits in high- ing memory in children with autism and with
functioning autistic individuals: Relationship moderate learning difficulties. Journal of
to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), Disciplines, 37(6), 673–686.
1081–1105. Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two in-
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive tact executive capacities in children with
functions and developmental psychopathology. autism: Implications for the core executive
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of
and Allied Disciplines, 37(1), 51–87. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2),
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the 103–112.
child. New York: Basic Books. Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003).
Piaget, J. (1962). The stages of the intellectual de- Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: WPS
velopment of the child. Bulletin of the Men- Edition [Manual]. Los Angeles: Western Psy-
ninger Clinic, 26(3), 120–128. chological Services.
Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, Scambler, D., Rogers, S. J., & Wehner, E. A.
J. A. (2001). The modified checklist for (2001). Can the Checklist for Autism in Tod-
autism in toddlers: An initial study investigat- dlers differentiate young children with autism
ing the early detection of autism and perva- from those with developmental delays? Journal
sive developmental disorders. Journal of of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), cent Psychiatry, 40(12), 1457–1463.
131–144. Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Oller, D., & Steffens,
Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Bothuyne, S. (1998). M. (2000). Vocal atypicalities of preverbal
Immediate imitation and joint attention in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel-
young children with autism. Development and opmental Disorders, 30(4), 345–354.
Psychopathology, 10(3), 441–450. Short, A. B., & Schopler, E. (1988). Factors relat-
Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imita- ing to age of onset in autism. Journal of Autism
tion deficit in autism. In J. Nadel & G. But- and Developmental Disorders, 18(2), 207–216.
terworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer,
(pp. 254–283). New York: Cambridge Univer- J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, men-
sity Press. tally retarded and normal children and their
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Penning- caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology
ton, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27(5),
high-functioning adolescents with autism 647–656.
spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67(5), Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and
2060–2073. change in the social competence of children
Rogers, S. J., & DiLalla, D. L. (1990). Age of with autism, Down syndrome, and develop-
symptom onset in young children with perva- mental delays. Monographs of the Society for
sive developmental disorders. Journal of the Research in Child Development, 64(1), 1–114.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1981). Sensorimotor
Psychiatry, 29(6), 863–872. skills and language comprehension in autistic
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
Wehner, B. (2003). Imitation performance in ogy, 9(2), 149–165.
toddlers with autism and those with other de- Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and
velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy- language skills in autistic, mentally retarded,
chology and Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. and normal children. Developmental Psychol-
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. ogy, 20(2), 293–302.
(1993). Developmental aspects of attachment Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1998). Gesture
behavior in young children with pervasive de- imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures
velopmental disorders. Journal of the Ameri- and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology,
can Academy of Child and Adolescent 15(6–8), 747–770.
Psychiatry, 32(6), 1274–1282. Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000).
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theo- Randomized trial of intensive early interven-
retical approach to the deficits in infantile tion for children with pervasive developmental
Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood 245

disorder. American Journal on Mental Retarda- Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social
tion, 105(4), 269–285. cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham
Sparling, J. W. (1991). A prospective case report of (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in
infantile autism from pregnancy to four years. development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Erlbaum.
ders, 21(2), 229–236. Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H.
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and
(1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: In- Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495–552.
terview edition, survey form manual. Circle Tuchman, R. F., & Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in
Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures
Stone, W. L. (1997). Autism in infancy and early and epileptiform electroencephalogram corre-
childhood. In D. J. Cohen, & F. R. Volkmar lates. Pediatrics, 99(4), 560–566.
(Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive de- Volkmar, F. R., Carter, A., Sparrow, S. S., & Cic-
velopmental disorders. New York: Wiley. chetti, D. V. (1993). Quantifying social devel-
Stone, W. L., & Caro-Martinez, L. M. (1990). Nat- opment in autism. Journal of the American
uralistic observations of spontaneous commu- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
nication in autistic children. Journal of Autism 32(3), 627–632.
and Developmental Disorders, 20(4), 437–453. Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Disintegra-
Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Ousley, O. Y. tive disorder or “late onset ” autism. Journal of
(2000). Screening Tool for Autism Two-Year- Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
Olds (STAT): Development and preliminary Disciplines, 30(5), 717–724.
data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., & Paul, R. (1986). An
Disorders, 30(6), 607–612. evaluation of DSM-III criteria for infantile
Stone, W. L., Hoffman, E. L., Lewis, S. E., & Ous- autism. Journal of the American Academy of
ley, O. Y. (1994). Early recognition of autism. Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 190–197.
Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P.,
148, 174–179. Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field
Stone, W. L., Lee, E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J., trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American
Hepburn, S. L., Coonrod, E. E., et al. (1999). Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1361–1367.
Can autism be diagnosed accurately in chil- Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T.,
dren under 3 years? Journal of Child Psychol- & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive de-
ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy-
40(2), 219–226. chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Hepburn, S. L., Hogan, 45(1), 135–170.
K. L., & Brown, C. S. (1999). Patterns of Volkmar, F. R., & Mayes, L. C. (1990). Gaze be-
adaptive behavior in very young children with havior in autism. Development and Psycho-
autism. American Journal on Mental Retarda- pathology, 2(1), 61–69.
tion, 104(2), 187–199. Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Goudreau, D., Cic-
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, chetti, D. V., Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987).
K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal Social deficits in autism: An operational ap-
communication in two- and three-year-old proach using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Scales. Journal of the American Academy of
Developmental Disorders, 27(6), 677–696. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(2),
Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., 156–161.
Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The Volkmar, F. R., Stier, D. M., & Cohen, D. J. (1985).
frequency and distribution of spontaneous at- Age of recognition of pervasive developmental
tention shifts between social and nonsocial disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,
stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and 142(12), 1450–1452.
nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Waterhouse, L., & Fein, D. (1998). Autism and the
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry evolution of human social skills. In F. R. Volk-
and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 747–753. mar (Ed.), Autism and pervasive developmen-
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1991). tal disorders Cambridge monographs in child
Individual variation, correspondence, stabil- and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 242–267).
ity, and change in mother and toddler play. In- Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
fant Behavior and Development, 14(2), Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N.
143–162. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism
246 Development and Behavior

spectrum disorder before one year of age—A early identification. Journal of Speech and
retrospective study based on home videotapes. Hearing Disorders, 54(2), 148–158.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint atten-
ders, 30(2), 157–162. tion training for children with autism using
Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & behavior modification procedures. Journal of
Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
communication of normal children from the Disciplines, 44(3), 456–468.
prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Wimpory, D. C., Hobson, R., Williams, J., & Nash,
Speech and Hearing Research, 31(2), 240–252. S. (2000). Are infants with autism socially en-
Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G., & Bryan, A. A. gaged? A study of recent retrospective
(1989). Communicative profiles of preschool parental reports. Journal of Autism and Devel-
children with handicaps: Implications for opmental Disorders, 30(6), 525–536.
CHAPTER 9

The School-Age Child with an


Autistic Spectrum Disorder

KATHERINE A. LOVELAND AND BELGIN TUNALI-KOTOSKI

For the child with an autistic spectrum dis- By the age of 6, children with ASD have al-
order (ASD), the elementary school years ready begun to diverge from one another ac-
bring challenges associated with the changing cording to characteristics such as degree of
expectations that accompany increasing phys- language delay and intellectual deficit. These
ical and behavioral maturity. During the pe- divergent developmental paths have much to do
riod from ages 6 to 12, the child with ASD with later outcome; perhaps the best-known
faces transitions to new learning environ- example of this divergence is the better out-
ments, contact with new peers and adults, and come observed for those children who have ac-
departures from familiar places and routines. quired some oral language by age 5 (Rutter,
These changes affect many domains of func- Greenfield, & Lockyer, 1967). Thus, we can
tioning, as the child is required to adapt to expect that children with ASD but without
more complex and demanding social environ- mental retardation will respond differently to
ments, to learn more sophisticated skills, to the challenges of the school years than those
communicate at a higher level, and to process with both ASD and mental retardation.
more information. Such experiences, which Often the behavior of the child with ASD in
are common to children of school age, are par- the school years is more obviously discrepant
ticularly challenging for those with ASD, who from that of nondisabled age-mates than it was
have not only developmental delays in multi- earlier in life. That is, domains of development
ple domains but also difficulty adjusting to such as social and communicative functioning
changes in their environments. may become more rather than less divergent
At the same time, however, most children from their expected trajectories during this
with ASD make progress during the school period, particularly in the child with more se-
years, acquiring new skills and learning to cope vere autistic behavior. A lack of normal peer
with new people, places, and events. The devel- relationships, the absence or paucity of pre-
opmental path followed by an individual child tend play, the presence of repetitive behaviors
during this period will be difficult to predict: It or focused interests, and a marked impairment
is the complex product of a dynamic process of social relatedness become clearly delin-
linking neural maturation with environmental eated in contrast to normative expectations for
influences, learning, and the child’s own self- children in this age group. This apparently in-
regulatory activity (Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994). creasing discrepancy is due in part to changes
However, some trends in development can be in expectations for the child’s behavior: For
outlined. example, the inability to follow directions, to

The authors thank Emese Nagy, MD, PhD, for her invaluable help in the preparation of this chapter and
Stacy Reddoch for her editorial assistance.

247
248 Development and Behavior

initiate interactions, or to inhibit motor stereo- practice it is difficult to distinguish these cate-
typies is more obvious in an 8-year-old than in gories reliably and without overlap (Volkmar
a 3-year-old. It is also partly due to the cumu- & Klin, 2000). In addition, there remain indi-
lative effect of environmental experiences on viduals who, although manifesting some symp-
the child with ASD, whose social and emo- toms of ASD, do not clearly meet criteria for
tional experiences of the world and opportuni- the diagnosis of AD or AS, either because they
ties to learn have differed sharply from those lack some required symptoms or because their
of typically developing children, because of severity of autistic symptoms is not great
the child’s own tendency to interact differ- enough. Although advances are being made in
ently with the world (Loveland, 2001). the study of the broader autism phenotype, at
Although it is possible to identify such present it is difficult to interpret the research
broad trends as those discussed earlier, the pic- literature as to the development of children in
ture of “ typical” development in the school-age these possible subtypes, because of inconsis-
child with ASD is complicated by the fact that tent methods of categorization across studies
classification of some children on the autistic and insufficient information about the taxo-
spectrum of disorder remains controversial nomic validity of various categories. This state
(Tsai, 1992). Although there are now well- of affairs contributes to difficulty in identify-
accepted methods for reliable and valid diagno- ing developmental expectations for individual
sis of autism (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, children with ASD in the school years.
1999; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994), there In this review, we provide a picture of de-
is today a surprising amount of disagreement velopment in school-age children with ASD
about the possibility of subcategories of the with and without mental retardation, keeping
autistic spectrum and about the relationship of in mind that the relevant literature includes
autistic disorder to other diagnostic categories, studies of children diagnosed not only with
such as Asperger syndrome, or proposed cate- AD (DSM-IV) but also with other pervasive
gories such as nonverbal learning disability developmental disorders, such as pervasive de-
or semantic-pragmatic language disorder. This velopmental disorder-not otherwise specified
lack of agreement reflects in part a tension be- (PDD-NOS) or Asperger syndrome. We should
tween categorical and dimensional approaches expect, in considering development during this
to classification, with research tending to sup- age period, that the organization and trajec-
port a picture of ASD as multidimensional and tory of development in any number of domains
multiply determined, but with clinical and ed- may differ among children with ASD of vary-
ucational practice based on assignment to dis- ing intellectual ability. At the same time, how-
crete diagnostic categories within the larger ever, it is possible to look for continuities as
category of pervasive developmental disorders well as discontinuities between development of
(DSM-IV). Despite the persistence of discrete autistic spectrum behaviors in the school years
diagnostic categories of ASD in clinical prac- and earlier and later development (Michelotti,
tice, there is considerable heterogeneity among Charman, Slonims, & Baird, 2002). Although
persons diagnosed with ASD (Gillberg & Cole- there is a wide-ranging literature on the behav-
man, 1992; Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & ioral development of children with ASD, we
Cohen, 2002a) even within the same category. focus in this chapter on several areas of partic-
The autistic spectrum of disorder (Wing & ular interest to the development of the child
Gould, 1979) may include subtypes differing with ASD from ages 6 to 12.
in etiology, clinical presentation, or develop-
mental course, as well as in the level of cogni- DEVELOPMENTAL ISSUES FOR
tive, social, or language disability (Volkmar THE SCHOOL-AGE CHILD WITH
et al., 1994). (These issues are explored in AUTISTIC SPECTRUM DISORDER
greater depth in the following section.) For ex-
ample, although both DSM-IV and ICD-10 dis- In the following section several areas are
tinguish between autistic disorder (AD) and discussed that are of special importance for
Asperger syndrome (AS; Asperger, 1944), in the development of children with ASD. These
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 249

include social and adaptive behavior, play, lan- pied with stereotypies or other repetitive in-
guage and communication, emotion, school ad- terests. These children with ASD are noted for
justment, and academic achievement. unresponsiveness and failure to initiate inter-
actions with both peers and adults (Freitag,
Social and Adaptive Behavior 1970; Loveland & Landry, 1986; Trad, Bern-
stein, Shapiro, & Hertzig, 1993). They often
Deficits in social behavior and social under- do not play with other children or demonstrate
standing are particularly characteristic of per- interest in friendships (Rutter, 1974). Deficits
sons with ASD. Since autism was first in their ability to use gaze and gesture appro-
described by Kanner (1943), these deficits priately in social situations lead to frequent
have been recognized as an essential compo- failures to communicate (Buitelaar, van Enge-
nent of the syndrome. They first become obvi- land, de Kogel, de Vries, & van Hooff, 1991).
ous in the preschool years, when a failure to Aloof children with ASD may be sufficiently
establish peer relationships, a lack of normal unresponsive that it is very difficult to direct
relatedness with familiar people, a preference and maintain their attention; thus, it may
for aloneness, poor eye contact and gesture, be easier to get their attention using proximal
tactile defensiveness, and lack of initiative in rather than distal stimulation (e.g., touching
communication become evident in most chil- the child’s hand rather than pointing to some-
dren with ASD (Rutter, 1978; Wing & Gould, thing). They may seem at times to be deaf,
1979). In some respects, social deficits may even though they are not. Lacking a complete
gradually decrease in severity during the participation in the usual set of social signals
school years, as the child begins to benefit and routines that govern human interactions,
from intervention and from learning to cope in aloof children with ASD are distinctly handi-
familiar situations and with familiar people capped in social situations. Although they
(Gillberg, 1984; Gillberg & Coleman, 1992). may exhibit emotion, their emotions are not
However, in general, the social deficits seen in necessarily tied to contexts easily interpreted
the preschool child with ASD tend to persist by others and thus can be puzzling and frus-
through the school years and beyond, changing trating to caregivers. Tantruming is common,
the form in which they are manifested and particularly when the child is frustrated by
showing the effects of maturation and develop- disruption of a routine or by other circum-
ment of the individual (Rutter & Garmezy, stances the child cannot control. Individuals
1983). Even children who have made excellent with these characteristics are most often seen
progress in multiple domains as a result of in the preschool age group, but some continue
early interventions are likely to retain some so- in this manner into later childhood, adoles-
cioemotional differences (Bailey, 2001). cence, and adulthood; these are most likely to
Although there is considerable heterogene- be persons with significant mental retardation.
ity among children with ASD in the presen- Quite often these children have a difficult and
tation of their social behavior (Klin et al., lengthy adjustment to a new school placement,
2002a), some generalizations can be made. For and social problems are likely to arise in the
example, Wing and colleagues have described classroom.
three subtypes of social behavior that capture The passive group includes children who do
many of the manifestations of ASD seen in the not actively avoid social contact with others
school-age child (Wing & Attwood, 1987; but who nevertheless lack the spontaneous and
Wing & Gould, 1979). intuitive grasp of social interaction that is
The aloof children are those most likely shared by normally developing children. They
to be described as “classically autistic.” They will accept the social approaches of others, but
do not seek, and may actively avoid, contact often do not have the skills to respond appro-
with others, and they may become very dis- priately. Their communication and play be-
tressed if it is thrust on them. They do not ini- haviors are rigid and sometimes stereotyped.
tiate communication (even though some can These individuals tend to function at a some-
speak), and much of their time may be occu- what higher developmental level than those in
250 Development and Behavior

the aloof group, with more language and fewer to exercise age-appropriate social judgment
motor stereotypies, and they are in general and social behavior. These situations can lead
“easier to manage.” Although passive children to difficulties for the school-age child who
with ASD can be easier to manage than those cannot meet the social expectations of peers.
who are aloof, they require considerable help Such children are at high risk for peer victim-
to relate to peers in the classroom or other sit- ization or shunning (Little, 2002), although
uations. Some children who start out display- there is a great deal of variation in how well
ing the aloof pattern of behavior later have a accepted children with ASD are when placed
better fit with the passive group. Thus, presen- with peers in regular classes (inclusion). Some
tation as aloof versus passive may depend to research suggests that despite the fact that
some extent on the child’s developmental level they are frequently less liked, able children
or IQ, and a transition from one category to with ASD are no more likely than other stu-
the other may reflect maturation as well as ac- dents to report feeling lonely and that their
cumulation of social experiences. limited awareness of social factors may in
The active-but-odd children are those who are some cases insulate them from feeling ex-
usually described as having high-functioning cluded (Chamberlain, 2002). A recent study
autism or Asperger syndrome. They actively found that persons with ASD do not make the
seek out contact with others, but the form and same judgments about other people’s trustwor-
quality of their social approaches are unusual thiness as controls (Adolphs, Sears, & Piven,
and often inappropriate. These more able chil- 2001). The authors concluded that the amyg-
dren with ASD experience difficulty in relat- dala, a structure in the temporal lobe of the
ing socially to peers and others, even though brain, is dysfunctional in autism and that its
they may have considerable language skills and dysfunction impairs the ability to perceive so-
may be interested in communicating with oth- cially relevant information and link it to social
ers. Characteristic of this group are behaviors knowledge and self-regulation of social behav-
such as repetitive questioning, inappropriate ior. If so, it is possible to see how difficulty
touching, conversation focused exclusively on judging the meaning of peers’ facial expres-
the child’s own narrow interests, and odd pos- sions and social behaviors could lead to incor-
tures, gestures, and facial expressions. Their rect judgments about social situations, as well
social behavior and communication seem to re- as inappropriate behavior.
flect a view of the social world that is literal Some of the social difficulties experienced
and concrete, and they show a limited aware- by children with ASD may be related to cogni-
ness of the feelings, thought, and motives of tive limitations, such as differences in patterns
others. However, these individuals are more of attention. Pierce, Glad, and Schreibman
intellectually able, and as a consequence, their (1997) found that the ability of verbal children
autistic spectrum characteristics are sometimes with ASD to interpret social situations is re-
identified later than those of other children duced when they must attend to multiple as-
with ASD. In contrast to less intellectually able pects of the situation. Also, research on visual
children, active-but-odd children may be fixations has shown that individuals with
aware to some extent that they are different autism do not necessarily attend to the same
and not always accepted by others, which can aspects of situations, as do other people. In
be a source of distress. They also tend to pre- viewing scenes of social behavior, adolescents
fer rigid and predictable routines in their daily with autism showed reduced time attending to
life, such that unexpected events, new people, eyes and more time attending to mouths than
and unfamiliar surroundings can be very stress- did controls (Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, &
ful. When they are highly stressed, they may Cohen, 2002b).
regress to behaviors displayed at earlier ages Despite their many social deficits, there are
(e.g., tantruming, self-stimulatory behaviors) a number of areas of social behavior in which
or even exhibit signs of psychosis (Wing & children with ASD do appear to have strengths
Attwood, 1987). Moreover, because they are and to make progress during the school years.
relatively able, they are often placed in classes For example, children with ASD usually dis-
or other situations in which they are expected play signs of attachment to parents and other
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 251

caregivers, including distress upon separation (Volkmar & Klin, 1993). Such studies have the
(Volkmar, Cohen, & Paul, 1986). Studies have potential to reveal patterns of development
suggested that a basic capacity for attachment across age groups and with reference to norma-
exists in children with ASD and that their tive data. Studies using adaptive behavior
attachment behavior is not different in kind scales to study social behavior in ASD have
from that of other developmentally delayed clearly shown that social and interpersonal
children or that of younger typically develop- skills of persons with ASD are poorer than
ing children (Ozonoff & South, 2001; Shapiro, would be expected based on their IQ and over-
Sherman, Calamari, & Koch, 1987; Sigman & all developmental level and that persons with
Mundy, 1989; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). Most ASD are usually weaker in this area than are
children with ASD do form attachments, al- comparable persons with other developmental
though some aspects of attachment, particu- disabilities (Ando & Yoshimura, 1979; Ando,
larly more cognitive aspects, may be impaired Yoshimura, & Wakabayashi, 1980; Klin, Volk-
in these children (Dissanayake & Sigman, mar, & Sparrow, 1992; Loveland & Kelley,
2001). Moreover, children with ASD respond 1988, 1991; Rodrigue, Morgan, & Geffken,
differently to different persons and in differ- 1991; Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, 1985;
ent situations (Landry & Loveland, 1988; Schatz & Hamden-Allen, 1995; Sparrow &
Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). These findings sug- Cicchetti, 1987; Volkmar et al., 1987). Such
gest that children with ASD are not actually differences have even been found as early
indifferent to other persons, but are aware to as ages 2 to 3 years (Stone, Ousley, Hepburn,
varying extents that different persons hold dif- Hogan, & Brown, 1999) and are thought to
ferent significance for them. In addition, continue into adulthood (Njardvik, Matson, &
studies have found that children with ASD in Cherry, 1999). Deficits in adaptive and social
the preschool and school years demonstrate skills may extend to those children and adults
mirror self-recognition, provided they have who have developmental disorders that are
reached a mental age level comparable to that closely related to or may co-occur with ASD,
at which young nondisabled children achieve such as fragile X syndrome (Cohen, 1995;
self-recognition (G. Dawson & McKissick, Fisch, Simensen, & Schroer, 2002).
1984; Ferrari & Matthews, 1983). These stud- The adaptive deficits of ASD do not appear
ies suggest that whatever the basis of the social to be a result of developmental delay alone
deficits in persons with ASD, it is probably not (Rodrigue et al., 1991), but rather a robust pat-
a failure to distinguish self from other. Thus, tern associated with the syndrome of ASD and
at least some of the foundations for normal so- one that persists over development in both
cial behavior appear to be present in children higher and lower functioning persons of both
with ASD. genders (Freeman et al., 1991; Kraijer, 2000;
Efforts to support and increase the use of McLennan, Lord, & Schopler, 1993). Some dif-
appropriate social behavior in children with ferences have been found between children
ASD have used a variety of methods. Verbal with ASD of lower and higher IQ, however, as
children with ASD can benefit from being to the relationships among domains of cognitive
taught strategies such as self-monitoring dur- and adaptive behaviors. There is some evidence
ing social interactions (Morrison, Kamps, that social delays are more severe, relative to
Garcia, & Parker, 2001) and may benefit from other domains of functioning, for those chil-
social training that involves peers without dren with ASD who also have mental retarda-
ASD (Kamps et al., 2002). tion (Burack & Volkmar, 1992) and that for
Although there is considerable clinical and them, adaptive levels are more closely related
research evidence that children with ASD are to IQ than for children with mental retardation
deficient in social skills, there is a need to doc- who do not have ASD (Carpentieri & Morgan,
ument the consequences of these deficits as 1996). Liss et al. (2001) compared higher and
they affect the everyday life of children with lower functioning children with autism to IQ
ASD. One approach to studying the impact and age-matched controls without autism and
of social deficits on the lives of children with found that for lower functioning children with
ASD is by use of standardized test instruments autism, adaptive levels were highly related to
252 Development and Behavior

IQ. However, for the more able children, adap- the school years, but they will do so at a slower
tive skills were more closely related to verbal pace than nondisabled age-mates.
ability and were strongly related to autistic
symptomatology. Other studies have found Play
that individuals with a diagnosis of Asperger
syndrome have better developed adaptive Play is an important complex motivated behav-
skills than those with a diagnosis of autism ior through which children learn to practice a
(McLaughlin-Cheng, 1998; Szatmari, Archer, wide variety of new skills in their social envi-
Fisman, Streiner, & Wilson, 1995). ronment. Sensory-motor play provides knowl-
In general, the social /adaptive skills of edge about the child’s own body and helps in
children with ASD do not necessarily continue developing a frame of reference for the world.
to increase with advancing age, as would ordi- Manipulative games lead infants to learn about
narily be expected. Some studies have found objects, to control their environments, and to
little or no relationship between level of so- develop fine-motor skills. From infancy on-
cial /adaptive skills and chronological age in ward, children learn about basic social behav-
children or adolescents with ASD (Jacobson & ioral patterns and social relationships from
Ackerman, 1990; Loveland & Kelley, 1988, social play, as well as practicing and increas-
1991). This pattern may reflect a tendency ing language and gestural communication and
to regress or to reach plateaus in development, other skills. Therefore, if children lack devel-
or it may reflect the wide variability in perfor- opmentally appropriate play skills, as is true of
mance often seen among children with ASD. many children with ASD, they will be placed
However, other studies have found that certain at a serious disadvantage in their development
adaptive skills, such as communication and (Boucher, 1999).
self-care, have a more predictable relationship Typically developing children ordinarily
with age, with older children having more begin to engage in reciprocal social play (such
skills than younger ones (Ando & Yoshimura, as tickle games) in the first year of life and in
1979; Ando et al., 1980; Loveland & Kelley, functional play with objects shortly thereafter.
1991). Thus, although the literature clearly Pretend or symbolic play, in which one thing or
shows that the development of social skills is person is used to represent another in an imagi-
delayed in persons with ASD, it is less clear nary action sequence, is usually present by 2 to
what to expect for the development of these 3 years. Though the youngest children play to-
skills in individual children. For this reason, gether in parallel, cooperative play gradually
population-specific norms for adaptive skills develops, so that by school age most typically
have begun to be developed for persons with developing children engage in elaborated pre-
ASD so that progress of individual children tend play and games with others. As children
with ASD can be compared with that of age mature, these cooperative types of play require
peers with a similar diagnosis (A. S. Carter increasing levels of sophistication in social
et al., 1998). cognition and social interaction skills, as well
as in language, memory, motor skills, and self-
Conclusions: Social and Adaptive Skills
regulation.
Although deficits in social skills relative By contrast, young children with ASD are
to other areas of functioning are characteristic often observed not to engage in reciprocal play,
of children with ASD, their manifestations pretend play, or cooperative play well past the
vary widely in the school-age child. These age and developmental levels at which such play
manifestations are linked to the severity of would ordinarily be expected to emerge. In-
ASD and the child’s level of cognitive func- stead, they may play repetitively with objects
tioning. Some children with ASD, particularly (e.g., lining up, spinning, or mouthing them) or
those with mental retardation, may exhibit re- engage primarily in passive activities such as
gressions or plateaus in social development, watching television or videos. Young children
where little progress is made over a period of with ASD have been found to have different
time. In general, children with ASD can be ex- patterns of interest and attention to toys than do
pected to make progress in social skills during other children and to behave differently during
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 253

toy-related communicative interactions (D. K. play with other children if the toys were more
O’Neill & Happé, 2000). More developed chil- attractive in that play area or more play time
dren may pursue with great absorption a topic was allotted in that play area (Hoch, McComas,
of special interest to them that is not shared Johnson, Faranda, & Guenther, 2002). Children
with others (e.g., see the case of James, dis- in a study by Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz,
cussed later). Engagement in pretend and in co- Rivera, and Greer (2002) received reinforce-
operative activities with peers tends to be ment for playing with toys; the children’s time
lacking unless specifically encouraged and sup- spent in passive behavior decreased, and they
ported. It has been hypothesized that early so- spent significantly more time with toys.
cial, cognitive, and affective deficits in ASD Another approach to improving play has
such as in affective attunement (Hobson, 1990), been to emphasize interventions designed to
metarepresentational ability (Craig & Baron- improve early social-communication skills, the
Cohen, 1999), and social orienting (Mundy, development of which is thought to be impor-
Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, 1987) adversely tant to development of play and other skills.
affect development of the social and cognitive Because socioemotional skills are interrelated
skills necessary for the development of play. in development, training some basic skills,
Early nonverbal communication skills have such as joint attention, can indirectly affect
been found to be a predictor of the extent to others, such as play, empathy, imitation, and
which children with ASD initiate play inter- language skills in children with ASD (Whalen,
actions when they are school age (Sigman & 2001). Development of other social skills such
Ruskin, 1999). It is difficult to imagine how co- as imitation may also enhance play behavior.
operative play can occur in a child who lacks In an intervention in which adults imitated
the ability to engage in joint attention interac- them, children with ASD increased their
tions. Similarly, a child who lacks adequate em- engagement in reciprocal social play (Field,
pathy and social self-regulation skills will have Field, Sanders, & Nadel, 2001). In a review of
difficulty taking part in both dyadic play and 16 empirical studies, Hwang and Hughes
organized games with larger groups. In addi- (2000) found that early training in social com-
tion, children with ASD are not necessarily mo- munication skills positively affected a wide
tivated to play with other children, to interact, range of socioemotional and cognitive skills,
and share experiences with others; hence, they including imitative play, in children with ASD
often do not do so, even when they possess cog- although there was limited generalization of
nitive and motor skills sufficient to engage in skills to settings other than that in which
shared play activities. training was received.
To varying extents, play does develop over Modeling play and communication can be a
time in most children with an ASD. Many highly effective intervention to teach children
children with ASD do acquire functional play to make more conversation and social initia-
and some symbolic play by the time they reach tions during play interactions. Verbal children
school age, but the level of their play is with ASD have been found to be able to learn
frequently behind that of age peers (McDo- appropriate play comments and to apply these
nough, Stahmer, Schreibman, & Thompson, in play activities with their siblings (Taylor,
1997; Riguet, Taylor, Benaroya, & Klein, Levin, & Jasper, 1999). Typically developing
1981; E. Williams, Reddy, & Costall, 2001). peers are also important resources for children
A number of studies have examined methods with ASD to learn about play and to practice
to increase appropriate play behavior in pre- socioemotional skills. Children with ASD can
school and school-age children with an ASD. be motivated by various methods to choose to
Behavioral reinforcement techniques have play with other children (Smith, Lovaas, &
been found to be effective in increasing cooper- Lovaas, 2002). When playing with typically
ative and interactive play behavior in children developing children, high-functioning children
with ASD. When children with ASD were given with ASD engaged in interactive play and
a choice whether to play alone with toys or produced more interactive speech with their
move to a play area common with other children peers. After a 16-week playgroup intervention,
and share toys with them, they increased their children with ASD displayed more symbolic
254 Development and Behavior

play and improved language skills, and they often make more initiations in play with their
maintained these behavioral changes well be- children than do parents of control children
yond the therapy (Zercher, Hunt, Schuler, & (El-Ghoroury & Romanczyk, 1999). As a result,
Webster, 2001). children with ASD are less likely to initiate
Some studies suggest that children with when playing with their parents than when play-
ASD benefit more from interventions to in- ing with their siblings. This finding suggests
crease play when they are given an active role. that to encourage cooperative play in children
When children with ASD were allowed to make with ASD, it is more effective to allow the child
choices about the toys or games used during a time to initiate play activities than to attempt to
language intervention session, they showed sig- direct the course of play. With intervention,
nificantly greater engagement in social play play interaction can be synchronized to an opti-
than did those for whom clinicians chose the mal level that allows sufficient space for chil-
games and toys, even if the clinician’s choices dren’s initiations. Mother-child play sessions
were based on the child’s preferences (C. M. can, therefore, be an excellent way to facilitate
Carter, 2001). Jahr, Eldevik, and Eikeseth interaction and can lead to increased coopera-
(2000) found that modeling of cooperative play tive play (Sasagawa, Oda, & Fujita, 2000).
interactions was not effective in increasing ap-
Conclusions: Play
propriate play behavior in verbal children with
ASD unless the children were required to de- Children with ASD are usually delayed in the
scribe the modeled interaction verbally before development of play skills, particularly pre-
attempting it themselves. tend and cooperative play, although in most
Numerous studies have found that the capac- children play continues to develop and improve
ity to pretend is impaired in children with ASD during the school years. Delays in play devel-
(e.g., Scott, Baron-Cohen, & Leslie, 1999). opment appear to be related in part to delays in
However, some studies have found that children social communication skills. Interventions to
with ASD are able to engage in pretend play, if improve social communication and imitation
play is structured for them and if they are skills may benefit development of play, and
prompted to do so (Jarrold, Boucher, & Smith, likewise play-based interventions have been
1996). There is some evidence that themes from shown to benefit development of social com-
the child’s own focused interests can be used in munication skills. The play of children with
interventions to enhance the child’s motivation ASD may be enhanced by allowing them an ac-
to play (e.g., a child who is preoccupied with tive role in choosing games, toys, and play
airplanes may be more engaged in games about themes that interest them.
flying and planes; Baker, 2000).
Although socioemotional problems in autism Language and Communication
do not originate from insensitive parenting,
parental sensitivity can positively affect the de- Deficits in language and communication are
velopment of communication, play, and imagina- characteristic of the school-age child with
tion in children with ASD. Siller and Sigman ASD. Many children with ASD still have lit-
(2002) measured synchronization of caregivers’ tle language by age 5 or 6, and in those chil-
behavior when they played with control chil- dren, deficits in nonverbal communication are
dren, with children with developmental delays, usually also evident (Loveland & Landry,
and with children with ASD. Caregivers of chil- 1986). For example, lower IQ and younger
dren with ASD demonstrated similar synchro- school-age children with ASD may have con-
nization with the children’s behavior as did tinued difficulty in joint attention interac-
caregivers of control children or children with tions, where gestures such as pointing,
developmental delays. However, children whose showing, and gaze-following are used to di-
caregivers showed good behavioral synchro- rect attention and establish a shared focus of
nization during play had better developed so- interest (Landry & Loveland, 1988; Loveland
cioemotional skills in later years. & Landry, 1986; Mundy et al., 1986). These
Perhaps in response to the child’s perceived children may fail to use or respond to such
social difficulties, parents of children with ASD gestural behaviors or may do so inconsis-
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 255

tently, leading to marked difficulty in main- various psychiatric conditions (schizophrenia,


taining social-communicative interactions. Tourette’s syndrome). It can be a symptom
These deficits in social communication are a of brain damage, dementia, Alzheimer’s, or
significant barrier to learning, as much effort Pick’s disease, and can appear after a circum-
must be expended just to direct and maintain scribed lesion in the left medial frontal
the child’s attention. Some children without lobe and the supplemental motor area (Hadano,
oral language do successfully acquire a vocab- Nakamura, & Hamanaka, 1998). Echolalia
ulary of signs or learn to use communicative resulting from the isolated impairment of
aids such as pictures representing common re- the speech area in the brain can occur despite
quests (e.g., a picture of a toilet to represent a the presence of an inability to produce spon-
request to go to the bathroom). taneous speech or to comprehend language
By contrast, the presence of speech before (Mendez, 2002). Some echolalic speech may
age 5 is an indicator for a better prognosis be a result of the disinhibition of an acoustic-
in children with ASD (Rutter, 1983) and is char- verbal motor reflex following isolation of
acteristic of those who become higher function- the language network from the surrounding
ing. In those school-age children with ASD networks, resulting in a “closed loop” for
who do develop language, speech is likely to be speech that is heard (Linetsky, Planer, &
pragmatically inappropriate as well as develop- Ben-Hur, 2000). Unintentional, or nonfunc-
mentally delayed. Among the characteristic fea- tional, echolalia of this kind could be a phe-
tures of language in children with ASD are nomenon similar to unintentional imitation
immediate and delayed echolalia; pronoun rever- occurring after the disinhibition of frontal
sals; unusual intonation; bizarre, idiosyncratic, network, most likely involving the mirror neu-
or “metaphorical” speech including neologisms; ron system (Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzo-
and stereotyped or repetitive speech. However, latti, 1996; Lhermitte, 1983). However, some
those children who are high functioning and echolalic speech in children with ASD appears
develop considerable language skills may pri- to have a functional role in communication and
marily exhibit more subtle manifestations of thus may be differently mediated in the brain.
language disorder, such as oddities of conversa- A number of studies have suggested that
tional interaction. These latter children, despite echolalia serves a purpose in the development
their pragmatic difficulties, may exhibit un- of language in children with ASD, possibly
usual strengths in some aspects of language de- by allowing the child to take a conversational
velopment, such as word decoding skills leading turn and thereby remain involved in a social-
to unusually early reading or hyperlexia (Frith communicative exchange (Fay, 1973; Prizant
& Snowling, 1986; O’Connor & Hermelin, & Duchan, 1981). Prizant and Duchan and
1994). Some of the characteristics of language McEvoy et al. (1988) found that immediate
in children with ASD are discussed in more de- and delayed echolalia can serve a variety
tail next. of functions in conversational exchanges, in-
cluding turn taking, declarative statements,
Echolalia
rehearsal, self-regulatory utterances, yes an-
Echolalia, the repetition of others’ or one’s swers, and requests. However, one should be
own speech, is a feature of normal language ac- cautious in assigning specific meanings to the
quisition in infants. Up to 75% of verbal per- echolalic utterances of children with ASD;
sons with ASD exhibit echolalia at some point such meanings depend to a large extent on con-
(Prizant, 1983). Although echolalic speech is textual cues and the responses of the listener;
considered characteristic of children with ASD thus, they can be open to a variety of interpre-
(Rutter, 1968), it is by no means present in tations (Loveland, Landry, Hughes, Hall, &
all of them, nor is it independent of develop- McEvoy, 1988; McEvoy et al., 1988; Rydell &
mental level (Fay & Butler, 1968; Howlin, Mirenda, 1994). The findings of these studies
1982; McEvoy, Loveland, & Landry, 1988). suggest that echolalia may be best viewed as a
Echolalia also is not present only in persons communicative strategy used by children with
with ASD. It persists or reappears in some ASD who cannot consistently produce sponta-
forms of pathological development and in neous speech. It is also possible that echolalia
256 Development and Behavior

itself aids the process of language acquisition, with atypical stress in two intonation patterns,
perhaps by sustaining the social-interactional whereas children with Asperger syndrome dif-
context in which conversation (and learning) fered from control children in only a few into-
takes place (Rydell & Mirenda, 1994). How- nation patterns. These findings suggest that in
ever, a study by Tager-Flusberg and Calkins general, the speech and auditory comprehen-
(1990) in which the utterances of children sion of children with ASD are compromised by
with ASD were transcribed over the period of deficits in the ability to use and understand
a year found that their imitative utterances are the prosody of language in a meaningful way.
not necessarily of greater length or complexity
Personal Pronouns
than their spontaneous speech. Thus, although
echolalia may facilitate conversational skills, Errors in use of personal pronouns (particu-
it does not necessarily facilitate grammatical larly, I /you pronoun reversals) have long been
development. Nevertheless, echolalic speech described as characteristic of verbal children
may be related in predictable ways to the de- with ASD (Bartak & Rutter, 1974; Fay, 1979;
velopment of language in children with ASD. Kanner, 1944). Many typically developing chil-
McEvoy et al. found that the proportion of dren do make some pronoun reversals at around
echolalic language by children with ASD was age 2, but only for a limited time (Charney,
greatest at lower language levels, suggesting 1980; Chiat, 1982; Loveland, 1984). Similarly,
that as children acquire more language, less most persons with ASD do not make pronoun
and less of it is echolalic. We should, there- reversals consistently or frequently (Lee, Hob-
fore, expect to see a gradual replacement of son, & Chiat, 1994; Loveland & Landry, 1986;
echolalic communication with spontaneous Tager-Flusberg, 1989), and some may instead
speech over time in school-age children with substitute proper names for I and you pronouns
ASD who are continuing to acquire language. (Jordan, 1989). Some evidence suggests pro-
noun errors are more common in children with
Characteristics of Speech
higher functioning autism than with Asperger
The speech of children with ASD often sounds syndrome (Szatmari, Bartolucci, & Bremner,
different from that of other children. Verbal 1989). These errors are particularly striking
children with autistic disorder or Asperger when they occur in school-age children with
syndrome have been found to produce frequent ASD, because they usually appear inconsistent
articulation errors or unintelligible utterances with the child’s overall level of language
or utterances that are inappropriate in phrasing development. Although in the past, pronoun er-
(Shriberg, Paul, McSweeny, Klin, & Cohen, rors in autism were interpreted as indicating a
2001). Ordinarily, prosody helps to clarify the confusion between the concepts of “I” and
meaning of utterances within a conversation “ you” (i.e., self and other; Bettleheim, 1967),
and thus adds an important channel of commu- more recently they have been interpreted
nication. Compared with the speech of age- to show that there is a confusion of social roles,
mates without ASD, the speech of children cognitive perspectives, or linguistic means
with ASD may sound more erratic and lacking of representing them (Charney, 1981; Fay,
in the prosodic characteristics of normal 1979; Loveland & Landry, 1986). Loveland
speech; at times, prosodic patterns may seem and Landry showed that appropriate use of I
to conflict with meaning as, for example, when and you by preschool and school-age children
a question intonation appears with a statement with autism was positively related to their abil-
or a greeting. Persons with ASD also have dif- ity to initiate joint attention interactions by
ficulties using information from prosody to means of gesture, suggesting that use of these
understand what others say (Baltaxe, 1984). terms is in fact closely tied to the achievement
Children with ASD are able to use stress for of a basic social reciprocity. Lee et al. (1994)
emphasis in an utterance—but it may appear in found that although school-age children and
an atypical place in the sentence (Baltaxe & adolescents with autism made few pronoun
Simmons, 1985; Fine, Bartolucci, Ginsberg, & reversal errors in tests of pronoun use, they
Szatmari, 1991). Verbal children with autism were reported to make them sporadically in
were found to use significantly more words their everyday life. This suggested that they
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 257

know how to use the pronouns but that they may increase greeting and requesting skills of chil-
have difficulty identifying their own or others’ dren. Other techniques, such as contingent
social roles in some situations, resulting in imitation and modeling, have been successful
errors. Research suggests that although chil- in increasing positive affect and gaze behavior
dren with autism do often have difficulty (Harris, Handleman, & Fong, 1987). Func-
learning to use first- and second-person pro- tional communication training can be helpful to
nouns correctly, they acquire personal pro- less verbal children with ASD in reducing the
nouns in the same order as do children with expression of difficult, disruptive behaviors.
language delays or no disability: first-person, For example, if disruptive behaviors are fol-
third-person, and then second-person pronouns lowed by a break session and children can ask
(Baltaxe & D’Angiola, 1996). for and receive a break, gradually they will pro-
duce fewer disruptive behaviors and will in-
Conversational Skills
crease the number of requests for breaks (R. E.
Many children with ASD do not reach a level of O’Neill & Sweetland-Baker, 2001). Attaining
language development at which true conversa- the ability to communicate needs in socially ac-
tional exchanges are possible. However, as de- ceptable ways is an important developmental
scribed earlier, some forms of echolalic or step for children with ASD, which greatly facil-
stereotyped speech can function communica- itates their adjustment to the classroom and
tively, and children with ASD who have little other settings.
spontaneous language may nevertheless use In more verbally able children with ASD, a
these forms of speech to engage in reciprocal delay in social skills related to language use
communicative interactions (Hurtig, Ensrud, & (i.e., in language pragmatics) may be present
Tomblin, 1982; Prizant & Duchan, 1981). This together with relatively preserved grammar,
skill is a highly important one for the school- a large vocabulary, and a high degree of flu-
age child with ASD, who, with its advent, has ency (Tager-Flusberg et al., 1990). This pattern
acquired one of the keys for accessing the social of development in high-functioning persons
world. Thus, it is important that less verbal with ASD may reflect an adequate develop-
children with ASD be encouraged to engage in ment of basic language skills such as phonetics
whatever level of conversational interaction is but a specific impairment of more complex
possible for them, using echolalia, stereotyped and interpretive language skills, including
questions, delayed echoes, and other kinds of comprehension (Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegel,
speech to scaffold their entry into this essential 1995). As a result, the high-functioning school-
social experience. age child with ASD is commonly described
Numerous strategies have been used to facil- as “ very verbal” but at the same time “a poor
itate verbal and nonverbal communication in communicator.”
children with ASD who are learning language. Conversational speech of more verbally
In younger and less verbal children, behavioral able children with ASD is usually described as
approaches have been helpful in teaching chil- deficient in a variety of ways. For example, the
dren to use language in functional ways. For child’s conversation may focus on limited top-
example, a time-delay strategy can be used in ics of interest to no one but the child (e.g., read-
which a trainer presents a stimulus (e.g., food) ing maps), speech may be pedantic and formal
and waits a few seconds before prompting the in situations where this style is out of place,
child to respond. This technique, along with re- socially inappropriate statements or questions
inforcement and social interactive training may be produced (e.g., “You’ve sure gotten
techniques, has been successful in increasing fat!”), references may be difficult to follow be-
the verbal communication of children who al- cause of a failure to consider the speaker’s
ready have some language, including the fre- point of view, intonation and prosody may be
quency of verbal requests, greetings, expression odd or uninformative (Fine et al., 1991), and
of affection, and naming pictures (Ingenmey & neologisms or other idiosyncratic speech may
Van Houten, 1991; Taylor & Harris, 1995). be used (Baltaxe, 1977; Fine et al., 1991; Love-
Charlop and Trasowech (1991) successfully land, Tunali, Kelley, & McEvoy, 1989). In ad-
taught a time-delay technique to parents to help dition, children with ASD may be somewhat
258 Development and Behavior

unresponsive to the speech of conversational were comparison subjects with DS, but that
partners, or they may respond in unexpected some did so after seeing such responses mod-
ways that suggest they have difficulty identify- eled (Loveland & Tunali, 1991). This study
ing and maintaining a topic of discourse. Even suggested that at least some persons with ASD
when children with ASD are gaining structural may be to some extent aware of a listener’s
language skills over development, they may not point of view, but they may not know how to
be gaining discourse skills at a comparable rate respond appropriately.
(Tager-Flusberg & Anderson, 1991). One of the most important skills related
The conversational deficiencies of children to social communication is the ability to
with ASD have been well described. Research make appropriate inferences about the other’s
by Fine, Bartolucci, Szatmari, and Ginsberg communicative intentions (Sabbagh, 1999). In
(1994) on cohesive links in the conversational conversation, verbal children with ASD fre-
discourse of children and adolescents with quently misinterpret the intentions of others
high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome (Hough, 1990) and are often literal and con-
found that the high-functioning autism group crete as well as socially inappropriate, even
did not tend to link their utterances to earlier though they may speak fluently (Joanette,
statements in the conversation, suggesting they Goulet, & Hannequinn, 1990). Even high-
may not be as attuned to the conversational functioning children and adolescents with ASD
context as a speaker without ASD would be. have considerable difficulty adjusting to the
The Asperger group, by contrast, made errors needs of conversational partners, for example,
such as referring to things for which there was by providing appropriate amounts of informa-
no prior referent. These individuals seemed tion, judging when information is relevant or
to be insufficiently aware of the listener’s interesting, and avoiding ambiguity. Ordinar-
need for information. During conversation, ily, conversational partners can read and inter-
children with ASD were found to be less likely pret each other’s communicative intentions.
to offer new or relevant information, to pro- Children with ASD have been found to have
duce fewer narratives, and sometimes not to difficulty identifying conversational violations
respond to questions, although they were not (Loveland, Pearson, Tunali-Kotoski, Ortegon,
different from control children in gesture & Gibbs, 2001; Surian, Baron-Cohen, & Van
use (Capps, Kehres, & Sigman, 1999). Similar der Lely, 1996) and to have special difficulty
findings have resulted from studies of referen- in recognizing another person’s communicative
tial communication in verbal persons with intentions. Not surprisingly, children with ASD
ASD. Loveland et al. (1989) asked children have been found to have difficulty on tasks
and adolescents with ASD or Down syndrome measuring humor or indirect speech. For exam-
(DS) to learn a game and teach it (verbally) ple, they have been found to make significantly
to another person, with the learner giving more errors in understanding jokes, even if
three levels of increasingly specific prompts they understand the difference between jokes
as needed. Even though both groups had and simple stories (Ozonoff & Miller, 1996).
learned the game equally well, those with ASD St. James and Tager-Flusberg (1994) found that
required much more specific prompting to when interacting with their mothers, children
convey the necessary target information to an- with autism were unlikely to use humor. Happé
other person, suggesting that they had diffi- (1993) found that only subjects with autism
culty selecting and organizing information to who passed a second-order theory of mind task
convey to a listener. This study also suggested were able to recognize sarcasm. Understanding
that verbal persons with ASD are insuffi- irony and sarcasm requires some ability to rea-
ciently aware of the listener’s need for infor- son about other persons’ thoughts and feelings,
mation. Further, a study on the ability to make since the intention of the speaker is at variance
conversational responses within an accepted with the surface meaning of what is said. Thus,
social framework (social scripts) found that the well-documented deficiency of persons
children and adolescents with ASD were less with ASD in understanding mental states may
likely to give helpful or empathetic responses well contribute to deficiencies in conversa-
to a conversational partner’s distress than tional skills (Baron-Cohen, 1995).
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 259

However, the relationship between conversa- Several studies have focused on this issue.
tional skills and understanding mental states is Tager-Flusberg and Quill (1987) and Bruner
not completely straightforward. Using various and Feldman (1993) found that persons with
methods, children can be taught to improve ASD told stories that were less complex,
their conversational abilities, to initiate conver- shorter, and contained more grammatical errors
sation, to take turns, to listen more attentively, than those of nondisabled persons of similar
and to maintain or change a topic. However, developmental level. A later study by Tager-
these changes do not necessarily improve un- Flusberg (1995) also found that stories of
derstanding of mental states or other complex children with autism and mental retardation
sociocognitive skills. Even after successful were shorter and less complex and contained
communication training, children with ASD did fewer causal statements and that they were less
not improve their performance on false belief likely to include a resolution or introduce new
tasks (Chin & Bernard-Opitz, 2000), and, simi- characters. Other studies have examined the
larly, following mental state teaching, children content of stories, concluding that children
with autism showed little improvement on com- with ASD are likely to talk less about charac-
munication measures (Hadwin, Baron-Cohen, ters’ mental states (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, &
Howlin, & Hill, 1997). Frith, 1986) and that their narratives are prag-
Research indicates that school-age children matically deficient, including neologisms and
with ASD are likely to have difficulty making idiosyncratic expressions not usually found in
the social judgments that ordinarily guide con- the narratives of other children (Loveland,
versation. Although this difficulty is likely to McEvoy, Tunali, & Kelley, 1990). Children
have something to do with a failure to under- with autism have been found to be less creative
stand other persons, their mental states, and in- and to provide fewer imaginative elements
tentions, research also suggests conversational during storytelling than control children do.
difficulties may be related to children’s confu- Children with Asperger syndrome, however,
sion about how to act on what they know about have been found to be better able to produce
others. When children with ASD are given imaginative characters (Craig & Baron-Cohen,
added structure or prompts, they can frequently 2000). Children with autism and children with
speak more informatively (Loveland & Tunali, developmental delays were found to be less able
1991; Loveland et al., 1989). It is thus some- than typically developing children to identify
what encouraging to conclude that in many the causes of their characters’ internal states
cases, children with ASD know more than they when narrating stories; instead, they tended
say, and with appropriate external structuring, simply to label emotions or actions (Capps,
they can communicate more effectively. Losh, & Thurber, 2000). When children with
autism and children with mental retardation but
Narrative Storytelling
without autism were matched on language abil-
There is a small but growing literature docu- ity, Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan (1995) found
menting that verbal children with ASD can tell no group differences in narrative length, lexical
stories of various kinds. Story narratives are of elements, and mental state terms in sponta-
special interest for the study of ASD because neous narratives. However, compared with con-
they are an example of discourse for which trols, children with autism and children with
fairly well-defined cultural expectations exist mental retardation or learning disability gave
(e.g., stories are expected to have a distinct be- fewer emotion-related responses to questions
ginning, middle, and end) and because they about their stories, and children with autism
presuppose considerable interpersonal aware- had difficulty explaining emotional states.
ness between speakers, if the story is to be un- When retelling stories, children with autism
derstood. Children with ASD, however, might were found to perform similarly to children
be expected to have an imperfect grasp of cul- with Williams syndrome, but worse than
tural expectations as well as impaired interper- control children, when talking about informa-
sonal awareness. Thus, we should expect to see tional elements of the story. However, when
differences between the story narratives of talking about emotional elements, children with
children with and without ASD. autism performed worse than both children
260 Development and Behavior

with Williams syndrome and control children Emotion


(Pearlman-Avnion & Eviatar, 2002). Thus, chil-
dren with ASD may have special difficulty Emotional behavior is an essential part of the
with imaginative and emotional aspects of sto- child’s social development, providing a basis
ries, including mental states, even though struc- for communication and for an understanding
tural aspects of storytelling may be intact. It is of self and others. Children without ASD en-
not surprising that success on theory of mind gage in affective interactions from early in in-
tasks has been found to be closely related to fancy (e.g., Stern, 1985), and before they reach
narrative abilities of children with ASD but not preschool, they can not only produce readily
in those with other developmental delay (Capps recognizable facial expressions but also iden-
et al., 2000; Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 1995). tify simple emotions in others. However, some
Some evidence suggests verbal children and more advanced skills, such as matching audi-
adolescents with ASD, particularly those of tory and facial expressions of emotion or
lower IQ, may not have a grasp of the conven- elicited imitation of emotional expressions,
tional, culturally determined story “schema.” may not be fully mastered until after the age
Loveland, McEvoy, et al. (1990) reported that of 6 (Brun, 2001; Brun, Nadel, & Mattlinger,
some of their subjects with ASD, when asked 1998). During the school years, emotional
to tell the story of a puppet show, responded skills continue to develop and may reach a
by describing the shape, color, or movements ceiling in later childhood when they become
of the puppets (“Puppets. They are red and similar to those of adults.
green. They go up and down . . .”) but without In individuals with ASD, however, the pic-
conveying any kind of story. This type of re- ture of emotional development may be quite
sponse may indicate that these individuals lack different. Because it has been hypothesized
a grasp of what a story is, perhaps reflecting a that children with ASD are centrally deficient
failure of acculturation (Bruner & Feldman, in relating emotionally to others (Hobson,
1993; Loveland & Tunali, 1993). 1993), much of the research on emotion in peo-
Studying narratives in children with autism ple with ASD has been devoted to determining
can be a powerful tool to explore aspects whether they have a special deficit in under-
of discourse and pragmatics that are not usu- standing or expressing emotion. This point
ally accessible with standard language tests. remains controversial, and the evidence sug-
The study of story narratives in children with gests that emotional differences in development
ASD, though now only beginning, may eventu- are not unique to those with an ASD (W. Jones,
ally provide a window into the child’s growing Bellugi, et al., 2001). For example, persons
social and cultural awareness. with developmental disabilities other than ASD
(e.g., Down syndrome, learning disabilities)
Conclusions: Language
have also been found to have affective deficien-
and Communication
cies in some studies (Hobson, Ouston, & Lee,
Like their social skills, the communication 1989; Loveland, Fletcher, & Bailey, 1990).
skills of school-age children with ASD vary Nevertheless, it is clear that children with
widely according to degree of autistic impair- ASD display emotional responses that seem
ment and level of development. Although many unusual, inappropriate, excessive, or inade-
children with ASD make considerable progress quate compared with the responses of other
in communication during the school years, children in similar situations (Capps, Kasari,
impairments of social aspects of communica- Yirmiya, & Sigman, 1993; Joseph & Tager-
tion remain a significant problem for most. Re- Flusberg, 1997; Yirmiya, Kasari, Sigman, &
cent research suggests that verbal children with Mundy, 1989). Also, they often behave in ways
ASD are capable of more sophisticated use of that suggest they are not aware of or concerned
language (e.g., storytelling) than was previ- with the feelings of others or that they do not
ously thought; that they may communicate more understand the consequences of feelings in
effectively when given prompting or modeling other people. Self-reports of high-functioning
of appropriate conversational language; and persons with autism indicate that their experi-
that even echolalic speech may contribute to the ences of emotional life are often confusing
development of conversational skills. and aversive, including fear, anxiety, sadness,
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 261

and frustration (Grandin, 1995; R. S. Jones, emotional impairment of children with ASD is
Zahl, & Huws, 2001; Wahlberg & Rotatori, more apparent. When asked to match emotional
2001). Some research suggests that autism can expressions from faces and voices, children
be categorized along the spectrum of empathy with autism have been found to be less able to
disorders (Gillberg & Coleman, 1992). Dis- identify the pictures that match the voices
orders such as autism, Asperger syndrome, at- (Hobson et al., 1988; Loveland et al., 1995) and
tention deficit disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, to spend less time looking at pictures that
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and match the voices (Haviland, Walker-Andrews,
anorexia nervosa share a profile of impair- Huffman, & Toci, 1996). Although children
ment in understanding and interpreting other with ASD seem to find emotional expression
people’s thoughts, feelings, and intentions. easier to perceive in moving faces than in static
Although empathy requires well-developed ones (Gepner, Deruelle, & Grynfeltt, 2001), in-
theory of mind skills and well-functioning creased complexity in a situation, such as infor-
emotion perception and recognition, it is much mation from more than modality or more rapid
more than these subskills separately. presentation, may make emotion perception
Many studies have found persons with ASD (and social perception in general) more diffi-
to have difficulty recognizing the affective cult for them (Loveland et al., 2001; Pierce
expressions of others and in sharing affect et al., 1997).
in communicative situations (Hobson et al., Efforts to teach children with ASD to
1988; Loveland et al., 1995; Snow, Hertzig, & understand or recognize emotions have met with
Shapiro, 1987; Weeks & Hobson, 1987). Per- varying success. Verbally able children with
sons with ASD have also been found to have ASD can be taught to improve their perfor-
differences in their production of spontaneous mance on tasks of emotional understanding, al-
and elicited affective expressions, with fewer though this improvement was not followed by
positive expressions and more unusual or anom- improvement in conversational skills, pretend
alous expressions than comparison subjects play, or other domains (Hadwin, Baron-Cohen,
(Loveland et al., 1994; Yirmiya et al., 1989). Howlin, & Hill, 1996; Hadwin et al., 1997).
Although some studies have suggested there Behavioral techniques (Gena, Krantz, McClan-
may be an underlying deficit in perception of nahan, & Poulson, 1996; Okuda, Inoue, & Ya-
affect in children with ASD (e.g., Loveland mamoto, 1999; Shaw, 2001; Stafford, 2000) and
et al., 1995), other studies have not always computer-based training programs (Silver &
found specific affective deficits in ASD or have Oakes, 2001) have been used to train specific
found no differences between persons with emotional recognition and responding skills.
ASD and comparison subjects matched for ver- Such methods can result in increased appropri-
bal mental age (MA; Ozonoff, Pennington, & ate responding, but they vary in their effective-
Rogers, 1990; Prior, Dahlstrom, & Squires, ness among children and the results have not
1990). Thus, the affective deficiencies of chil- always generalized to performance in real-life
dren and adults with ASD may reflect, in part, settings.
developmental delay, and their affective devel-
Conclusions: Emotion
opment may be closely related to their level of
language development. Although there is abundant clinical evidence
Another possible reason for inconsistent that the emotional development of children
findings on emotion tasks in ASD is differences with ASD differs from that of other children,
in strategies used to perform tasks. Some evi- laboratory research studies on this topic have
dence from functional brain imaging suggests not consistently found evidence of deficiencies
that individuals with ASD may process facial in recognizing, understanding, or expressing
and affective information differently than emotion. In the school-age child with ASD,
typically developing peers (Pierce, Muller, Am- affective deficiencies likely contribute to dif-
brose, Allen, & Courchesne, 2001). Moreover, ficulties forming peer relationships. For exam-
when emotional perception skills are chal- ple, a child who fails to recognize when he or
lenged with information coming through multi- she has offended others will have difficulty
ple sensory channels, as with vocal and facial making friends. More research is needed to ex-
expressions of emotion together, the socio- plore the affective behavior and perceptions of
262 Development and Behavior

children with ASD in natural settings to iden- many children with ASD have symptoms of
tify the consequences of affective deficiencies hyperactivity (Eaves & Ho, 1997), which can
for the child’s social development. adversely affect academic achievement. How-
ever, teaching self-monitoring of attention
School Adjustment and and performance can help to improve the aca-
Academic Achievement demic performance of children with ASD.
Sixth-grade students with autism significantly
Beginning with the initial transition to school, improved the accuracy and productivity of
children with ASD face numerous challenges their classroom work as well as their achieve-
in the complex school environment, both social- ment test scores after an intervention in-
emotional and academic. The social-emotional voking self-monitoring of attention (Takeuchi
and academic challenges faced by children & Yamamoto, 2001). Students with ASD can
with ASD appear to share many of the same also improve their academic performance
roots. when they are given the opportunity to make
As they develop, children must learn to choices in the process of learning, for exam-
negotiate the changing social expectations of ple, deciding the order in which they will com-
both peers and adults. Because of their dif- plete tasks (Moes, 1998).
ficulty in interpreting social subtleties and Academically, while some students with
regulating their own social behavior, however, ASD experience no special difficulties, others
children with ASD have difficulty meeting display learning problems, to varying degrees.
social expectations at an age-appropriate level. Many children with ASD who have significant
This limitation can adversely affect peer developmental delays are nonverbal and may
relationships in school, participation in group not be developmentally ready for instruction in
activities, and even ability to learn in the academic subjects. For these children, a pri-
classroom setting. As a consequence, many mary goal may be the establishment of basic
children with ASD feel puzzled, frustrated, communication skills, along with the ability to
anxious, and inadequate in the social context attend to instruction and participate in a learn-
of the school. ing situation. Behavioral techniques and spe-
A rigid work style and cognitive in- cialized communication therapy have proven
flexibilities can also contribute to difficulty effective in promoting these goals. When lan-
adjusting to the school environment at an guage is attained, most still require structure
age-appropriate level, such that for some chil- and individualized assistance in many, if not
dren, even small departures from expected all, academic areas.
routines (such as a change in classroom seat- Many verbal children with ASD have
ing) may result in major adjustment difficul- better-developed nonverbal than verbal skills,
ties as well as feelings of insecurity and performing relatively well on visual-spatial
anxiety. The well-documented impairment in tasks but having difficulty with those that
executive functioning common to more intel- depend more on language, particularly oral
lectually able children with ASD (Pennington and written expression. Unfortunately, much
& Ozonoff, 1996) can make it difficult for of what happens in the classroom depends on
them to keep track of assignments, complete reading, listening, and speaking, and even
homework and test papers, and allocate their arithmetic involves reading numerals. As a re-
time. It can also adversely affect social and sult, children whose nonverbal skills are much
emotional functioning, as children experience better developed than their verbal skills (par-
limitations in coping and problem solving. ticularly oral language) may be viewed as less
Other common vulnerabilities such as atten- intellectually able than they really are and thus
tion /concentration difficulties, impulsivity, may be placed in classes that do not challenge
and abnormalities in various sensory modali- them sufficiently in their areas of strength.
ties (e.g., tendency to become overstimulated Others, particularly those who meet criteria
or specific auditory, tactile, or visual sensitivi- for Asperger syndrome, may excel at language-
ties) only contribute to the self-management based tasks and be less strong in the nonverbal
difficulties of children with ASD. For example, area. These children are also difficult to serve
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 263

appropriately in the school, since their intel- Instead, for such children, these skills are in-
lectual abilities and need for academic chal- tact or even precocious (Rumsey, 1992). For in-
lenge may greatly exceed their social maturity stance, a number of investigators have studied
and self-management abilities. As a result, hyperlexia, in which the child displays excep-
they, too, may be placed in less than ideal tionally well-developed reading skills relative
classroom settings. For example, a very bright to IQ or mental age (Goldberg, 1987; O’Connor
child may be ready for the intellectual de- & Hermelin, 1994; Whitehouse & Harris,
mands of a gifted-track class but may find the 1984). Hyperlexia is commonly found in a sub-
social and attentional demands of the class too group of high-functioning children with ASD.
stressful and frustrating. In both instances, it O’Connor and Hermelin studied two children
is important that multiple aspects of the with high-functioning ASD and hyperlexia,
child’s functioning—not only academic test ages 5 and 8. They were paired for comparison
scores or speech—be considered in arriving at with two normally developing children of aver-
an optimal placement. It is also important that age reading level and tested at 6-month inter-
the appropriate level of supports be available vals over 2 years and later at ages 9 and 12. The
to the child in the classroom and at home and reading ability of the children with ASD was
necessary modifications be considered. For very advanced for their chronological and men-
example, some children are helped by the pres- tal ages, especially in phonological decoding
ence of an aide in the classroom who provides skills. Their comprehension was also good
help and redirection as needed. Others benefit but at a level more commensurate with mental
from longer time to complete work, assistive age. Their reading was much faster than com-
writing devices such as keyboards, lists and parison subjects’ reading, especially with dif-
schedules, special seating arrangements, and ficult material. These findings suggest some
the presentation of work in small units. degree of dissociation between phonological
In general, children with ASD do not neces- decoding skills and semantic comprehension in
sarily share a characteristic set of academic these children. In another study, Grigorenko
difficulties, but instead, exhibit deficits that et al. (2002) studied a sample of 80 children
appear related to their individual patterns of with developmental delays, ages 2 to 12, for in-
strengths and weaknesses over development. cidence of hyperlexia. While the frequency of
As a result, individual educational needs vary hyperlexia was not significantly different for
considerably. Even intellectually able students boys and girls, the children with PDD had sig-
with ASD can have different academic profiles nificantly more incidents of hyperlexia as com-
than typically developing individuals (Siegal, pared to children with non-PDD diagnoses.
Goldstein, & Minshew, 1996). Minshew, Gold- Additional research is needed to clarify the role
stein, Taylor, and Siegel (1994) compared the of hyperlexia in the cognitive and language de-
academic achievement levels of high-function- velopment of higher functioning children with
ing males with autism and a comparison group ASD and its implications for education.
without autism, matched on variables such as Like hyperlexia, dyslexia has also been
age, gender, IQ, race, and SES. As compared studied to better understand the specific
to the comparison group, those with autism academic differences seen in some school-age
had significantly more difficulty on reading children with ASD. In their comparative study
comprehension tasks. However, there were no of children with ASD and children with
significant differences on spelling, computa- dyslexia matched for reading age, Frith and
tional tasks, and mechanical reading. Great Snowling (1983) found that the children with
care is needed in the evaluation of profiles of dyslexia had superior skills in comprehension
ability for children with ASD, whose individ- and use of semantic context but had difficul-
ual educational needs may not fit readily with ties with phonological processing. The chil-
the prepared programs of their home school dren with ASD, by contrast, had problems in
districts. comprehension and the use of semantic con-
Some higher functioning school-age children text. This finding is consistent with the find-
with ASD do not have significant delays in ings from Rumsey and Hamburger’s (1990)
basic reading, spelling, and arithmetic skills. study in which high-functioning men with
264 Development and Behavior

ASD had better phonological and rote audi- cally been a source of frustration and stress for
tory memory skills than comparison subjects parents of school-age children with ASD (Tu-
with severe dyslexia. These findings, taken to- nali & Power, 1993; Unger & Powell, 1980).
gether, suggest that many verbal children with More recently, however, research on educat-
ASD may have an advantage in some aspects ing children with ASD has provided a number
of reading (i.e., phonological decoding) and a of new and important educational directions
disadvantage in others (e.g., comprehension), (Schreibman, 1988). Among the major develop-
relative to children without ASD of compara- ments in education is emphasis on comprehen-
ble mental age. sive and functional curricula, teacher training,
During the school years, children with ASD and education (Dunlap, Koegel, & Egel, 1979;
experience significant changes in cognitive, Halle, 1982); home-based intervention pro-
emotional, social, and adaptive development, grams that involve parent training (Ozonoff &
and, consequently, it is important that their ed- Cathcart, 1998); focus on the optimal educa-
ucational programs be adapted to their chang- tional environment and classroom instructions
ing needs over time. To study the effects of (e.g., inclusion; McDonnell, Thorson, & Mc-
age on academic functioning, Goldstein, Min- Quivey, 1998); mainstreaming and emphasis
shew, and Siegel (1994) examined a group of on learning in the natural setting (Kamps,
high-functioning individuals with autism and Walker, Maher, & Rotholtz, 1992); transition-
control subjects with no autism (mean ages, 16 ing to small group formats with a modified
and 15, respectively). Younger subjects with or curriculum (Kamps et al., 1992), integrating
without autism performed equally well on choice-making opportunities (Moes, 1998);
psychoeducational measures of procedural peer tutoring (Kamps, Locke, Delquadri, &
skills, even on complex tasks that required in- Hall, 1989); transition of the child and the ser-
terpretation, while children with autism per- vices to less restrictive and more productive
formed more poorly on tasks administered community-based settings (Schopler & Mesi-
with complex linguistic instructions. Older bov, 1983); and a more comprehensive treat-
children with autism, however, performed ment /intervention that is longitudinal and age
poorly on interpretive tasks. These develop- appropriate (Schreibman, 1988). Despite these
mental changes and their implications for aca- exciting changes, appropriate programs, much-
demic performance underscore the need for needed services, and the research to im-
individualized educational plans and the need prove our knowledge in teaching the school-age
to monitor children’s educational plans closely child with ASD remain limited. As the number
over time. of children needing ASD-related services in-
During recent years, there has been a signif- creases, schools are increasingly called on to
icant increase in the body of literature that stretch their already limited resources to pro-
focuses on specific academic needs and ser- vide these services. In many areas of the United
vice delivery to individuals with ASD and their States, children with ASD cannot easily obtain
families. As we have come to understand more access to appropriate classroom placements,
about ASD and the special needs of these teachers, and aides trained to work with chil-
children and their families, educational ap- dren with ASD, needed assistive devices and
proaches to this group have changed signifi- modifications, or supportive programs such
cantly; however, it was not until the mid-1970s as home-based interventions and social skills
that educational systems began to respond to training.
their specific needs. Before this period, many Despite the limited availability of services,
public school programs were not accessible to individuals with ASD are now more widely
children with ASD (Schreibman, 1988). Fami- served in both public and private schools than
lies had to create their own resources through in the past. As a result, more of them are com-
private organizations and were often left with- pleting academic high school programs and
out guidance and support. Given this history, even college. The challenges for these individ-
difficulties in dealing with the school systems, uals and their families continue as they move
finding the appropriate programs, and gaining into greater independence and vocational
access to the available services have histori- choices.
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 265

Conclusions: School Adjustment and limited ability to give self-report. As the issue
Academic Achievement of comorbidity in ASD begins to receive more
interest and attention, studies have begun to
Although it presents opportunities for learning
focus on the relationship between ASD and
and development, school also presents many
disorders such as attention deficit / hyperactiv-
challenges for the child with ASD, both aca-
ity disorder (ADHD), anxiety and mood dis-
demic and social in nature. Children with ASD
orders, and OCD. For instance, it has been
often benefit from school placements in which
reported that the risk of psychosis is higher
educators are sensitive to their social and
than expected in Asperger syndrome (Clarke,
emotional as well as their academic needs. Be-
Littlejohns, Corbett, & Joseph, 1989; Gillberg,
cause children with ASD vary widely in skills
1985). Anxiety disorders, sometimes associated
and profiles of ability, it is often difficult to
with depression, are also common in ASD
meet their educational needs in the classroom.
(Frith, 1991). These findings and others serve
Both lower functioning and higher functioning
to emphasize that school-age children with
children with ASD are difficult to serve, in
ASD are at risk for psychiatric disorders and
that they do not necessarily learn or develop in
that it is important to identify and treat these
the same ways as other children (e.g., children
disorders whenever possible. In this section, we
with hyperlexia). There is a need for contin-
review several of the most common disorders
ued research, both on the neurodevelopmental
that affect children with ASD.
basis of learning in children with ASD and on
techniques to facilitate learning of academic
skills. At the same time, children and adoles- Stereotyped, Repetitive, and
cents with ASD have greater opportunities Ritualistic Behaviors
today than in the past, and many have attained
Stereotyped, repetitive, or ritualistic be-
educational success at an unprecedented level.
haviors are an essential diagnostic feature in
ASD (Gray & Tonge, 2001; Lord et al., 1994;
PSYCHIATRIC AND Militerni, Bravaccio, Falco, Fico, & Palermo,
BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS 2002; Rutter, 1985; Turner, 1999; Wing &
Gould, 1979). Although repetitive movements
Although children with ASD exhibit behavioral such as handclapping and rocking are often ob-
and developmental characteristics specific to served in younger or less able children with
the autistic spectrum, they may have additional autism, stereotyped or ritualistic behaviors
behavioral and psychiatric disorders such as of various kinds are also present in older or
obsessive-compulsive or ritualistic behaviors, more developed individuals. Bartak and Rutter
hyperactivity/inattention, psychosis, mood dis- (1976), in their study of 19 children with
orders, or anxiety. Such disorders can be of autism of average intelligence, found that al-
equal or even greater concern to families than most half had stereotypical movements and
are autistic behaviors, because they can lead resisted changes in the environment. Unusual
to increased difficulty with behavior manage- but intense interests such as weather systems,
ment, learning, and social relationships. By maps, and geography, as well as unusual and
the later school years, many children with repetitive play activity (e.g., reading the tele-
ASD are receiving treatment for comorbid dis- phone book or train / bus schedules for fun,
orders, whether through psychotropic medica- playing with the same toy[s] repetitively),
tion, behavior modification techniques, or other were all common. Despite their diagnostic sig-
modalities. However, given the complexity and nificance, abnormal repetitive behaviors have
varying degrees of severity of autistic symp- received much less interest from researchers
toms, it can be a challenge to identify and sepa- than the social and communication deficits as-
rate these symptoms from those of a potentially sociated with this group of disorders, and the
coexisting psychiatric disorder. In the develop- exploration of these behaviors in ASD has not
ing child with ASD, this task is particularly dif- been approached as systematically (Bodfish,
ficult because of the changing manifestations Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000). A recurring
of the disorder over time as well as the child’s question is the relationship of such behaviors
266 Development and Behavior

in ASD to the symptoms of OCD, that is, OCD, are less organized and less complex
whether they may be viewed as belonging on (Swedo & Rapoport, 1989). Whereas OCD
the same continuum of disorder, whether they behaviors are usually described as egodys-
share common origins, and whether they have tonic (i.e., recognized by the individual as un-
a similar role in the psychological life of the desirable), the similar behaviors present in
individual. individuals with ASD are thought to be
There is some evidence that the presentation egosyntonic (i.e., recognized by the individual
of repetitive, stereotyped, and ritualistic be- as acceptable and desirable; Baron-Cohen,
haviors in persons with ASD changes with 1989; Swedo & Rapoport, 1989). However, re-
development (Militerni et al., 2002), progress- search has challenged this view of OCD in
ing from repetitive sensory motor activities to persons without ASD. For instance, children
more complex and elaborated activities that with OCD do not always present with anxiety
may take many forms and may resemble the (Berg, Zahn, & Behar, 1986); also, many per-
symptoms of OCD. A number of studies in- sons with OCD have egosyntonic obsessions
dicate that even though some characteristically and compulsions and may lack insight into the
autistic behaviors, such as impaired social senselessness of their behaviors (Insel &
interaction and communication and motor Akiskal, 1986). Moreover, due to their social
stereotypies, are prominent in young children, and communication difficulties, even high-
complex stereotyped behaviors and routines re- functioning individuals with ASD may not ap-
sembling OCD behaviors are less frequent pear to be resisting their compulsions or to be
(Cox et al., 1999; Kroeker, 2001; Vostanis affected by associated emotional distress,
et al., 1998). In other words, these manifesta- making it difficult to identify a coexisting
tions of repetitive behavior and thought may be OCD (Tsai, 1992). Thus, the relationship be-
evidenced only in children of a more advanced tween ASD and OCD is not clear at this time
developmental level (Kroeker, 2001). This con- and is deserving of further study.
sistent finding suggests that a developmental A number of investigators have focused
process may be involved in the emergence of on the neurobiology of stereotypic (and self-
obsessional symptoms, and a greater level of injurious) behaviors in ASD as well as other
maturity may be needed for the development diagnostic categories such as mental retarda-
of these obsessional features (Gray & Tonge, tion (Stein & Niehaus, 2001) and have at-
2001). Although some studies have found that tempted to identify their basis in the child’s
ritualistic and repetitive behaviors are more developing brain. For instance, Pierce and
common and more intense during middle child- Courchesne (2001) found that in children with
hood and tend to diminish during adolescence autism, but not in controls, the rate of stereo-
and adulthood (Mesibov & Shea, 1980), other typed behavior was negatively related to the
investigators report that these symptoms are size of the cerebellar vermis lobules VI-VII
often retained during adulthood. For instance, and positively related to frontal lobe volume.
Rumsey et al. (1985) found that many adult J. H. Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, and Per-
autistic men, regardless of their level of intel- rett (2001) argued that early developmental
lectual functioning, exhibited a number of ritu- failure in a recently discovered class of neu-
alistic behaviors and compulsions, such as rons in frontal cortex (mirror neurons) is
putting objects in certain places, hand wash- likely to create a number of developmental im-
ing, and stereotyped touching. These behaviors pairments observed in ASD, including stereo-
can closely resemble those of persons with typed mimicking, such as echolalia.
OCD, raising the question of a possible rela- Stereotypical and repetitive behaviors, in-
tionship between ASD and OCD, as well as the cluding motor stereotypies, are not unique to
issue of differential diagnosis and treatment. ASD. They are also commonly found in persons
In studies that compared the ritualistic with a variety of other developmental disorders
obsessive-compulsive behaviors observed in such as mental retardation (Rojahn & Sisson,
ASD to those observed in OCD, it has been ar- 1990), psychiatric disorders (e.g., OCD, schizo-
gued that the stereotypies seen in ASD, while phrenia), and neurological conditions such as
superficially resembling the stereotypies of Parkinson’s disease and Tourette’s syndrome
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 267

(Bodfish et al., 2000). J. E. Dawson, Matson, and the function of a specific behavior may be
and Cherry (1998) examined the most common less important than previously believed.
maladaptive behaviors (i.e., aggression, self-
Conclusions: Stereotyped, Repetitive, and
injurious behaviors, and stereotypies) in three
Ritualistic Behaviors
diagnostic groups: autism, PDD-NOS, and
mental retardation. They found that these be- Ritualistic, stereotyped, and repetitive be-
haviors had similar functions in the life of haviors can vary widely in presentation in
the individual, regardless of diagnostic group. children with ASD, in part because of the de-
However, Bodfish et al. found that adults with velopmental level of the individual child.
autism had significantly more and severe com- There is a need to investigate whether develop-
pulsions, stereotypy, and self-injury than those mental continuities exist between the stereo-
without autism. They also found that the repeti- typed movements commonly seen in younger
tive behavior severity and severity of autism and lower functioning individuals with ASD
were closely related. The authors concluded and the OCD-like behaviors observed in more
that although abnormal repetition is observed developed individuals with ASD. The resem-
in disorders other than autism, there is a pat- blance of the ritualistic, stereotyped, and
tern of higher frequency of occurrence and repetitive behaviors of ASD to those of per-
greater severity associated with autism. Thus, sons with mental retardation and those with
these behaviors remain a source of significant other psychiatric disorders such as OCD sug-
concern for individuals with ASD through the gests possible similarities in underlying patho-
school years and beyond. physiology among these disorders that require
Although repetitive and ritualistic behaviors further investigation. Both pharmacological
are not unique to ASD, they are among the and behavioral approaches have been used
most troubling features of the syndrome, from with some success to reduce the severity of rit-
the standpoint of parents, teachers, and peers. ualistic, stereotyped, and repetitive behaviors
The need to reduce or manage these behav- in children with ASD.
iors has generated a significant number of
studies on intervention and treatment using Attention Deficit / Hyperactivity Disorder
both psychopharmacologic and behavioral in-
terventions. Some investigators have conducted School-age children with ASD frequently dis-
controlled trials of the efficacy of selective play characteristics that are associated with
serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) such ADHD (Goldstein, Johnson, & Minshew,
as fluvoxamine (Kauffmann, Vance, Pumar- 2001). Symptoms such as inattention, hyperac-
iega, & Miller, 2001), clomipramine (Gordon, tivity, or impulsivity as well as some associ-
Rapoport, Hamburger, State, & Mannheim, ated features (e.g., low frustration tolerance,
1992), naltrexone (Anderson et al., 1997; P. G. temper outbursts, mood lability, poor concen-
Williams, Allard, Sears, Dalrymple, & Bloom, tration, excessive insistence that requests be
2001), mirtazapine (Posey, Guenin, Kohn, met) are observed in many settings, including
Swiezy, & McDougle, 2001), and risperidone school. For example, compared with boys with
(McDougle et al., 1998), among others, with Down syndrome, boys with autism were found
varying but promising results for the reduction to move more rapidly between different activi-
of stereotypical, repetitive behaviors. A group ties when in their usual environments and to be
of behavioral intervention studies that focused more likely to engage in one activity at a time
on changing children’s environments (e.g., in a sequential manner (Ruble, 1998). This
teaching orienting responses to environmental style of activity may reflect attentional limita-
stimuli) also reported positive outcomes (Frea tions. These behaviors are among those most
& Hughes, 1997; Hall, 1997; Shabani, Wilder, frequently reported by parents of children with
& Flood, 2001). In their studies on the function ASD, and they can negatively affect the child’s
and treatment of stereotypical behaviors, emotional well-being as well as social and aca-
Kennedy, Meyer, Knowles, and Shukla (2000) demic performance. They can be observed
concluded that the causes of stereotypy in chil- early in life and tend to continue through the
dren and adolescents with ASD are complex, school years, adolescence, and adulthood.
268 Development and Behavior

As in children without ASD, attention and those with ASD tend to share some charac-
deficits and hyperactivity in children with teristics, they can be clearly differentiated by
ASD may present differently at different ages the type and severity of other characteristics.
or levels of development. Whereas preschool- Studies of cognitive processes in children
ers may display a great deal of motor activity, with ASD have suggested that information pro-
older children are likely to become gradually cessing differences may be involved in the ex-
less active, but to remain inattentive or dis- pression of ADHD symptoms in these children.
tractible. Symptoms of attention deficit and There is also some evidence that impairments
hyperactivity often result in the placement of of attention and arousal may be involved in the
children with ASD in self-contained or other underlying neurodevelopmental mechanisms
highly structured classroom settings where of ASD (G. Dawson & Lewy, 1989; Hutt,
distractions are minimized and tasks are pre- Hutt, Lee, & Ounsted, 1964; Rimland, 1964;
sented in small steps. Wainwright-Sharp & Bryson, 1993). Some in-
Despite these clinical observations, there is vestigators have studied the autonomic corre-
limited literature on comorbidity of ASD and lates of attention and arousal, while others
ADHD. One of the reasons for this is the way have investigated attention at the behavioral
the DSM system defines these disorders. Like level in children with ASD. Although it is not
the DSM-III-R, DSM-IV specifies that if the yet clear whether abnormalities in arousal play
symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity a role in ASD (James & Barry, 1980), research
occur during the course of a PDD, an addi- suggests that persons with ASD are impaired
tional diagnosis of ADHD is not given. This in basic information processing and attentional
discourages the clinician from thinking of the operations (Wainwright-Sharp & Bryson, 1993),
child with ASD as having an attention deficit such that the response to sensory stimuli may
disorder, even when symptoms are severe. be delayed or attenuated (Courchesne, 1987;
Nevertheless, many children with ASD are Zahn, Rumsey, & Van Kannen, 1987). Given
identified and treated for such symptoms. One that a number of studies have found attenuated
study found 30% of their sample of higher responding on tasks requiring selective atten-
functioning children with ASD were being tion (Ciesielski, Courchesne, & Elmasian, 1990),
treated with psychotropic medication for it has been suggested that although children
symptoms of inattention, distractibility, or hy- with ASD may not have difficulty registering
peractivity, and 20.2% were taking stimulants information, they may instead have difficulties
(Martin, Scahill, Klin, & Volkmar, 1999). In in processing it (Courchesne, Lincoln, Yeung-
an attempt to explore the comorbidity issue, Courchesne, Elmasian, & Grillon, 1989; Wain-
Clark, Feehan, Tinline, and Vostanis (1999) wright-Sharp & Bryson, 1993). Goldstein et al.
examined a group of children with ADHD, (2001) found that higher functioning children
asking their parents to rate them on a measure and adults with autism had deficits on mea-
of autism. In this study, 65% to 80% of the sures of psychomotor speed and cognitive flex-
parents reported significant difficulties in so- ibility, but not on measures representative of
cialization and peer interactions as well as sustaining and encoding factors of attention.
nonverbal and pragmatic communication in Another reported difficulty in this population
their children with ADHD. Luteijn et al. is overselectivity and a resulting limited use of
(2000) reported that according to parent re- incoming information (Lovaas, Schreibman,
port, children with PDD-NOS and those with Koegel, & Rehm, 1971; Rincover & Ducharme,
ADHD both have problems in behaving appro- 1987). Executive function deficits have been
priately in social situations. However, the chil- found in both ADHD and autism, but not in
dren with PDD-NOS have more significant conduct disorder or Tourette’s syndrome (Pen-
difficulties than do children with ADHD alone nington & Ozonoff, 1996). Impairments in
in social interaction and communication. Chil- motor inhibition were found to be specific to
dren with ASD and children with ADHD have ADHD, while impairment in verbal working
also been found to differ in their pattern of re- memory was found to be specific to autism.
sponding to sensory experiences (Ermer & These studies serve to emphasize that problems
Dunn, 1998). Thus, while children with ADHD with attention and information processing
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 269

more generally may be present in all persons ing may contribute to anxiety problems in
with ASD to some degree and that they are ASD; anxiety can arise when a child has dif-
likely to affect numerous areas of functioning. ficulty generating new solutions to problems or
changing strategies or when a child becomes
Conclusions: Attention
overwhelmed by too many alternatives or de-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
mands. Although some situations that produce
Attention deficits and hyperactivity are anxiety in children with ASD could be ex-
among the most frequently reported and per- pected to do so in other children as well (e.g.,
vasive problems for children with ASD. These meeting peers at a new school, answering diffi-
symptoms are also widely treated with med- cult questions), children with ASD typically
ication, although there are few studies on have more difficulty than other children in reg-
ADHD in ASD and its treatment. In the school ulating their emotions and behavior in response
years, hyperactivity usually diminishes, but to such situations. As a result, they may be
problems in attention are likely to remain. Al- more likely to react to the escalating anxiety of
though current diagnostic practice discourages such a situation by “acting out ” in some way.
dual diagnosis of ASD and ADHD, recent re- Biological vulnerabilities may also play a
search on the brain and attention in ASD sug- role in increased anxiety in children with
gests impairment of attention may play an ASD. Studies of the families of persons with
important role in the development of the syn- ASD have suggested that they have a higher
drome. The relationship of ASD to attention than expected level of psychiatric disorders,
deficits on a clinical level should receive fur- including anxiety disorders (Abramson et al.,
ther investigation. 1992; Piven & Palmer, 1999). Personality
traits present in families of ASD, including
Anxiety anxiety, shyness, irritability, and oversensitiv-
ity, may be associated with a genetic liability
Anxiety is an important but little-studied prob- for ASD (Murphy et al., 2000). Even among
lem in children and adults with ASD (Lainhart, children with related disorders, autistic behav-
1999). The effect of anxiety on children with ior can be associated with anxiety; in school-
ASD can be severe, and it may be manifested age girls with fragile X, social communication
in tantruming, aggression, agitation, irritability, deficits similar to those seen in ASD have
noncompliance, fearfulness, and other undesir- been found to be associated with the presence
able behaviors. Families and teachers commonly of anxiety (Mazzocco, Kates, Baumgardner,
report that challenging behaviors increase when Freund, & Reiss, 1997). Although these and
children with ASD experience situations that other studies suggest that symptoms of anxiety
produce anxiety, such as unexpected changes in may be closely related to the neurobiological
routine or new social situations. In fact, as Gro- differences that lead to ASD, it remains for
den, Cautela, Prince, and Berryman (1994) ar- further research to determine the mechanisms
gued, anxiety and stress may contribute to many of such a relationship.
of the typical behavioral manifestations of The treatment of anxiety in children with
autism, including unusual fears, stereotypies, ASD must be a high priority for research. In
and symptoms resembling those of OCD. a study of psychotropic medications used by
Children with ASD, particularly those with individuals with ASD, Martin et al. (1999)
higher functioning autism or Asperger syn- found that 65% of those taking medication
drome, are reported to have higher rates of anx- did so for treatment of anxiety-related dis-
iety problems than children without an ASD orders. Among the commonly used medica-
(Gillott, Furniss, & Walter, 2001; Kim, Szat- tions for anxiety in ASD are antidepressants
mari, Bryson, Streiner, & Wilson, 2000). Al- such as fluoxetine (Koshes, 1997) and bus-
though the reasons for increased anxiety in pirone (Buitelaar, van der Gaag, & van der
children with ASD have not been established, Hoeven, 1998). Some children are also re-
difficulties in cognition and self-regulation ported to respond well to cognitive or behav-
may be involved. For example, cognitive limita- ioral approaches to reducing anxiety and
tions such as impairment in executive function- associated behavior problems (Cullain, 2002).
270 Development and Behavior

Conclusions: Anxiety of affective disorder or suicide. After their re-


view, the investigators noted that the three crit-
Anxiety is very common among children with
ical features of an affective disorder (i.e., a
ASD, and it may lead to various maladaptive
change in mood, a change in the individual’s
behaviors, including acting out. Because anxi-
view of himself and the world, and the appear-
ety is so pervasive among children with ASD, it
ance of vegetative symptoms) were rarely
is important that families, clinicians, and edu-
reported by these individuals, making the diag-
cators recognize its effects and its contribution
nostic assessment particularly challenging.
to the expression of other behavioral problems.
These rare but important cases help emphasize
Although cognitive and self-regulatory deficits
the fact that both high- and low-functioning in-
may contribute to anxiety, research suggests
dividuals with ASD are vulnerable to affective
that a vulnerability to anxiety may be linked to
disorders.
the biological basis of ASD.
Conclusions: Affective Disorders
Affective Disorders Though apparently common in persons with
ASD, affective disorders are not easy to diag-
Depression is one of the most common psychi- nose, particularly in children. There is a great
atric disorders in persons with ASD, particu- need for research on methods of diagnosis and
larly in those who are higher functioning. treatment of affective disorders in both higher
Despite their average to above-average abili- and lower functioning individuals with ASD.
ties in intellectual, language, adaptive, and
academic areas, higher functioning individuals EXAMPLES OF DEVELOPMENT IN
with ASD experience chronic difficulties in THE SCHOOL-AGE CHILD WITH
social interactions and relatedness and are AUTISTIC SPECTRUM DISORDER
often painfully aware of their impairment. In
the school years, when peer relationships and The trends in development of the child with
social skills become a crucial developmental ASD during the school years are best observed
task, developmental delays in this area gener- through longitudinal follow-up. The following
ate a great deal of frustration, anxiety, and cases, each of whom was seen from preschool
distress, which in turn increase the likelihood through adolescence, illustrate some of the is-
of psychiatric difficulties. However, when sues that arise in development of children with
symptoms that are suggestive of an affective ASD with and without mental retardation, re-
disorder develop, it is often difficult to make a spectively. (Names and some details of these
formal diagnosis because of the individual’s individuals have been changed to protect con-
difficulty in communicating feelings and ex- fidentiality.)
periences (Lainhart & Folstein, 1994).
Comorbidity of affective disorders with As- Joan, a Girl with Autistic
perger syndrome has been reported (DeLong & Spectrum Disorder and Moderate
Dwyer, 1988; Gillberg, 1985), as has the case Mental Retardation
of an individual with autism who also had de-
pression and trichotillomania (Hamdan-Allen, Joan was first seen at a medical center psychi-
1991). Kurita and Nakayasu (1994) reported a atric clinic at the age of 4 years, 8 months (4;8).
rare case of a 20-year-old male with autism At the time of her first assessment, Joan lived
presenting with seasonal affective disorder and with her mother and stepfather. Her parents
trichotillomania. Lainhart and Folstein (1994) were divorced the year before. As an infant,
reviewed previously published cases of individ- Joan was reported to have had recurrent ear in-
uals with ASD and an additional diagnosis of fections and delayed motor milestones. Be-
affective disorder. Half of these individuals tween the ages of 12 and 18 months, she was
were female, and almost all subjects had some reported to have displayed a sudden change in
degree of mental retardation. The onset of af- behavior, with loss of previously acquired lan-
fective disorder was during childhood for 35% guage and onset of screaming episodes, run-
of the subjects, and 50% had a family history ning, twirling, spinning, and social withdrawal.
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 271

Joan was enrolled in an early childhood inter- was characterized primarily by echolalia, but
vention program through her school district at she was sometimes able to respond to direct
age 3;6 on the basis of her documented lan- questions or commands and could point to
guage delay. She was initially referred to the named colors and parts of the body. Responses
clinic for developmental evaluation and diagno- to gestures such as pointing were inconsistent,
sis, and she continued to receive follow-up eval- and she displayed little expressive gesture.
uations at intervals over the succeeding 12 Her language was found to be at a 30- (expres-
years. The records of this series of assessments sive) to 36-month (receptive) level, although
depict the trends in her cognitive, social, and nonverbal intelligence was at a mental age of
language development. 4;9. At this point, Joan’s behavior was charac-
When first examined, Joan was restless, hy- terized by repetitive behaviors such as spin-
peractive, and uncooperative, with little eye ning objects, hyperactivity and distractibility,
contact and few signs of social relatedness. some inappropriate affect, and poor social re-
She explored her environment in an aimless latedness.
manner, touching objects and spinning them. It At Joan’s next evaluation at the clinic,
was easier to get her attention using proximal she was 10;7 years old. In the interval, she con-
rather than distal stimulation (e.g., touching tinued to be served in self-contained special
her hand rather than pointing). She did not ini- education classrooms and to receive speech/
tiate social or communicative interaction by language intervention. She was reported to
speech or gesture but sometimes responded to have no friendships with peers at school at this
initiations by others. Developmental testing time, although her behavior problems there had
required frequent breaks and the presentation decreased. Her cooperativeness was distinctly
of items in small units. She frequently did not improved since the previous evaluation, and
attend and had to be reminded to look at what she required much less external structuring to
her hands were doing. She often perseverated, complete the assessment. She required more
and self-stimulatory and challenging behav- structure on tasks that were more difficult for
iors tended to interfere with testing. Joan her (verbal tasks) and less on those that were
often responded impulsively but did not like easier (nonverbal tasks). She exhibited little
to be asked to redo her work. However, with affect, but seemed to know when she was
considerable structuring her score fell above performing well; she said, “Good!” to herself
the 30-month level on the nonverbal items whenever she responded correctly to an item.
of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Despite continued problems in attention, Joan
Mental Scale; she demonstrated receptive and was able to attain a nonverbal mental age of 5;9
expressive language at approximately a 24- to on the Leiter International Performance Scale
30-month level, including both spontaneous (nonverbal, NV, IQ 55), demonstrating skills
language and echolalic speech. It was recom- in matching by color, shape, and number, and
mended that she receive speech/ language in- in reproducing simple block designs. However,
tervention and continue in a structured early more abstract items, such as matching by use,
childhood intervention program. were too difficult for her. Language skills had
Joan was next seen at age 7;1, after having improved to a 3- to 4-year level, with greater
been enrolled in school-based intervention vocabulary, increased ability to respond to
for several years. She was then in a self- more complex questions and requests, skilled
contained special education first-grade class- repetition of word and number strings, and de-
room for children with severe disabilities, creased echolalia. More of Joan’s language was
where she was reported to display hyper- now spontaneous, in two- and three-word utter-
activity, short attention span, and behavior ances, although delayed echoes also appeared.
problems. Motor, cognitive, and social delays Joan still rarely initiated communication, but
prevented her participation in age-appropriate her responsiveness to others had increased.
physical play, such as riding bicycles and tak- Receptive language was still better developed
ing part in team sports. Joan also was not yet than expressive. Assessment of adaptive behav-
independent in toileting, dressing, or eating. ior (Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales) indi-
Upon assessment, her expressive language cated that Joan had few skills for self-care
272 Development and Behavior

(although she was now toilet trained) and that vocabulary had increased to about a 6-year
her social skills were at a 21⁄ 2 -year level. level. Immediate and delayed echolalia was still
Joan was seen again a year later at age 11;6. present. Her academic achievement was found
Her cooperation at this evaluation was excel- to be in the range of kindergarten to second
lent, with considerably reduced distractibility grade, with strengths in spelling, word attack,
and hyperactivity. Social relatedness with and letter-word identification (Woodcock-
examiners was also improved, as shown by Johnson). Joan’s adaptive skills had increased
Joan’s responsiveness to attempts to redirect to a 4- to 7-year level, with particular weakness
her attention. Socially inappropriate behaviors in interpersonal relationships.
were fewer, but still present (e.g., pulling her Joan was evaluated again at age 16;4. She
dress up over her head). Nonverbal intelligence had made some progress in all areas, but her
showed developmental progress (NVMA 6;3, expressive language and social adaptive skills
NVIQ 59), but language remained at a 3- to were still in the 4-year range. Joan continued
4-year level, with echolalia and perseveration to have significant difficulty with attention
present in much of her speech. At the same and concentration, expressive and receptive
time, Joan began to show signs of growing language, social skills, and adaptive behavior.
insight into her own behavior. For example, Her overall IQ was in the moderate range of
when frustrated by being unable to answer a mental retardation. She was involved in ex-
question, she once said, “Joan is sad.” Joan’s tracurricular activities at her church and in
adaptive skills also showed progress, with im- Special Olympics, and she had begun to have
provement in self-care and social skills, and friends in her class at school.
notable strength in written communication
Interpretation
skills, relative to oral.
At age 15;2, Joan was seen again. At this The case of Joan illustrates progression from a
time, she was enrolled in a self-contained life classical autism presentation in the preschool
skills class at her local high school, with indi- years to a significantly improved present-
vidual speech therapy, occupational therapy, ation in adolescence, though still on the autis-
and adaptive physical education, but was main- tic spectrum of disorder. Joan exhibits many
streamed for lunch, music, drama, and typing. features typical of the child with ASD who is
During the school day, an educational aide ac- moderately to severely impaired: She has a
companied Joan and assisted her individually in history of repetitive motor activities such as
most of her activities. Joan was reported to spinning objects; her language and communi-
have made significant advances in social be- cation are significantly more impaired than
havior. For example, although she still rarely her nonverbal skills; it is difficult to direct and
initiated conversation, she interacted with maintain her attention; she has been hyper-
other students if placed in a group situation. active, and her behavior has been difficult to
Joan also seemed more interested in pleasing control; she has had few social relationships
the examiners during her assessment, and she with peers; her play and exploration are very
responded well to praise. Although Joan had immature; and she has inappropriate affect
greatly improved in her ability to attend and and poor social judgment. However, her devel-
to remain on task, she still required structuring opment from the preschool years through
to complete more difficult tasks, both at school the school years and into adolescence reveals
and during her assessment. In contrast to her developmental trends in a number of areas.
earlier assessments, Joan’s problems with at- First, Joan gained nonverbal cognitive skills at
tention and persistence now tended to take the a fairly constant rate throughout the period
form of distractibility and impulsive respond- of study, with a nonverbal IQ remaining stable
ing rather than hyperactivity. Her behavior also between 55 and 60. Thus, she continued
was improved, although some inappropriate to gain skill in nonverbal reasoning, visual
affect and speech were still present. Joan’s as- motor, and constructional skills from pre-
sessment revealed continued progress in school through adolescence. By contrast, her
nonverbal skills (NVMA 7;5; NVIQ 57) but language has apparently reached a plateau at
little progress in oral language skills, although about a 4-year developmental level that was
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 273

attained by about age 12, with greater weak- skills have both increased. Her assessments
ness in expressive than receptive language. As document steady increases in responsiveness,
a result, on reaching adolescence, Joan is fur- cooperativeness, social awareness, and relat-
ther behind age-mates in academic progress edness, as well as the beginnings of social in-
than might be expected based on her nonverbal sight and peer friendships. Some of these
IQ. In addition, her significant language delay changes may have been facilitated by Joan’s
and autistic social deficits have combined to gradual increase in attentional and behavioral
make her seem somewhat less intellectually self-control, which may have helped her to
able than she may actually be; this has meant benefit both from instruction and from social
that over the years she has received fewer op- experience.
portunities to mix socially with nondisabled
age-mates and has had somewhat lower expec- James, a Boy with an Autistic Spectrum
tations set for her in school than would be de- Disorder and Above-Average Intelligence
sirable. During the years from ages 6 to 12, the
primary priorities of Joan’s educational pro- James, the only child of his parents, was first
gram were to develop language and control brought to a medical center psychiatric out-
behavior. patient clinic for an evaluation at the age of 5.
Joan’s adaptive skills have also increased The presenting problems were severe and fre-
over the years she has been assessed, but like quent temper tantrums, extreme inattention and
her language skills, they have progressed more hyperactivity, restlessness, sleep difficulties,
slowly than her nonverbal skills and have sensory abnormalities (e.g., did not like to
reached an apparent plateau in adolescence. By be touched, was overly sensitive to loud noises),
age 12, she had mastered most basic self-care extreme discomfort in response to changes in
skills and had increased her social and com- routines, and some self-stimulatory behaviors.
munication skills; however, her recent slow Up to the age of 1 year, James reportedly had
progress in adaptive behavior may indicate recurrent ear infections and was diagnosed with
that she is having difficulty making a transi- asthma at the age of 13 months. However, he re-
tion to the greater independence, social judg- portedly never had a severe attack and did not
ment, and peer-oriented activities expected of have any asthma symptoms after age 2. Devel-
adolescents. opmental milestones including language were
Dramatic changes have taken place in Joan’s met within the expected time frames, with some
attention and behavior. Starting from a state delays in socialization and toilet training. His
of hyperactivity, uncooperativeness, and fre- parents reported that James was not very inter-
quent motor self-stimulation behaviors as a ested in interacting with his peers. They also
preschooler, she became in the school years sig- reported that James taught himself how to read
nificantly better controlled, better able to focus and write, could tell the day of the week that
attention and persist on tasks, and less disrup- various dates fell on, and had an outstanding
tive in class. By adolescence, she was no longer memory, recalling past events with every minor
hyperactive, although she still needed external detail. At the age of 5, his level of nonverbal in-
structuring to persist on difficult tasks. Thus, telligence was measured by the Leiter Inter-
over time Joan developed improved self-control national Performance Scale at an IQ of 145. Al-
in a variety of situations, partly as a result of though exact scores were not available, both his
structured intervention and partly as a result of receptive and expressive language scores had
maturation. been previously assessed to be “above age
Less dramatic but still significant changes level.” His preacademic skills assessed by the
have taken place in Joan’s social behavior from Wide Range Achievement Test were also signif-
preschool to adolescence. As a preschooler, icantly above average. During this time, James
Joan resembled the passive type of individual was described as a “somewhat anxious” child
described by Wing, in that she rarely initiated who was concerned about nuclear war and had
but did respond to others. She has continued to some other fears (e.g., fear of heights). These
be a relatively passive communicator, although anxieties were not severe enough to create
her interest in others and her communication concern for his parents. Because he had some
274 Development and Behavior

significant autistic-like behaviors but did not fit James was evaluated again at age 12. At this
the picture of the typical child with autism, time, despite significant improvement in many
James was given a formal diagnosis of PDD- difficult behaviors (e.g., tantrums), he had be-
NOS at the end of this evaluation. come more and more socially isolated, spending
At age 9, James came for a follow-up evalu- hours every day involved in a fantasy world of
ation with a set of more specific concerns. His imaginary cities and countries. He began to
parents reported that James had started to ex- draw complicated maps of these places, dis-
hibit many unusual behaviors and did not re- cussing in great detail their populations, cli-
spond to medication (i.e., Ritalin) that was mates, imports, exports, and so on. Although he
prescribed for his attentional difficulties and showed an obvious desire to be with his peers,
hyperactivity. He was often extremely anxious he simply did not know how to approach them.
and would suddenly dwell on a given thought During this time, James also started to display
and become restless and (through a chain of appetite and sleep disturbances, decreased en-
associations) would reach a catastrophic con- ergy, and difficulty concentrating and was pre-
clusion that would create a state described as scribed antidepressant medication with a good
“panicky.” For example, while at school, he response. His assessment scores continued to
would look out the window on a sunny day and indicate above-average intellectual and lan-
think of possible rain later. For James, rain guage skills and extremely well-developed aca-
meant destructive weather, which made him demic skills. James was in honors classes in
think of tornados. Consequently, he would be- almost every subject. However, his adaptive be-
come so anxious that he would not be able to havior scores were in the impaired range in so-
remain in the classroom. James also displayed cialization and self-help skills. As a teenager,
a significant amount of oppositional behavior he worked closely with one of the authors (BT),
and severe temper tantrums that lasted for receiving supportive therapy, medications, and
hours. During the time since his previous eval- social skills training. Difficulties in socializa-
uation, James also developed a number of ritu- tion continued to be the major issue. James
alistic and obsessive-compulsive behaviors graduated from high school and later enrolled
that kept him preoccupied for long periods of in a community college, where he pursued stud-
time (e.g., touching the trash can a certain ies in information technology.
number of times before leaving the house,
Interpretation
watching the Weather Channel for hours to
avoid unexpected tornados). James’s case highlights the many puzzling but
During this time, James also became more fascinating developmental and diagnostic chal-
interested in his age-mates and developed an lenges of ASDs. At the age of 5, James pre-
intense but unrequited attachment to a female sented as an extremely bright youngster who
classmate that triggered the development of did not display many obvious developmental
paranoid beliefs and experiences. These diffi- delays. Moreover, some skills such as visuo-
culties, which interfered significantly with spatial memory and academic skills were pre-
his and his family’s daily lives, necessitated cocious. However, he exhibited a number of
pharmacological and psychotherapeutic inter- unusual behaviors consistent with an ASD that
ventions. His second assessment at this time were relatively easy to identify. Although
yielded a much more uneven profile, with intel- there was a gap between James and his peers in
lectual skills in the high average range (Wech- social skills, it was not the major concern
sler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, when compared with his other difficulties
Full Scale IQ=111) but with social skills and such as lack of control in behavior (e.g., severe
social comprehension in the impaired range. temper tantrums, no delay of gratification,
His self-help and coping skills (Vineland Adap- inattention, hyperactivity).
tive Behavior Scales) were also assessed to be Over the years between preschool and ado-
much lower than expected levels. His academic lescence, James continued to acquire skills in
scores, however, were significantly above aver- the cognitive and academic domains that kept
age in all major areas. James was in regular him at or above the level of his nondisabled age-
classes ( honors classes in some subjects) with mates. Like Joan, he learned to control his be-
no remedial academic assistance. havior better in a variety of contexts. However,
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 275

while his nondisabled age-mates made prog- This review of developmental issues for the
ress in the cognitive domain and overtook school-age child with ASD suggests important
some of his early achievements, James’s initial directions for future research.
mild difficulties in the adaptive domain, espe- Despite the recent research emphasis on
cially socialization, became major handicaps lack of social understanding in persons with
over the years, making the gap between him ASD, we still do not know enough about how
and his age peers significantly wider. children with ASD actually view their world.
Perhaps the most interesting aspect of Little is known about how they attend to and
this case is the different presentation of perceive social and emotion information, in-
“autistic-like” behaviors/characteristics over terpret situations, make social judgments, un-
development. As James faced challenging de- derstand what is said to them, and interpret the
velopmental tasks (e.g., peer interaction and impact of their behaviors on others, or about
socialization experiences), his well-developed the person with ASD as a member of a society
intellectual capacity made him painfully or culture. The complexities and subtleties in-
aware of his deficiencies. This in turn made volved in real-time social interactions may not
him more vulnerable to additional emotional be adequately captured in the laboratory with
discomfort with feelings of isolation, with- simple, easily controlled tasks (Klin et al.,
drawal, sadness, and overall emotional dis- 2002a; Loveland, 2001). Moreover, the infre-
tress. After his third evaluation, James met the quent occurrence of certain behaviors (e.g.,
diagnostic criteria for major depression in ad- neologisms in speech, pronoun errors) may be
dition to his ASD diagnosis. Similarly, the in- highly important, even though these behaviors
tensity and the complexity of his “obsessive” are not readily observed in the laboratory.
rituals raised the question of comorbidity of Studies that are based on more natural situa-
OCD and an ASD. A further consideration is tions, while still informed by current theoreti-
the changing nature of diagnostic standards cal issues, will provide us with richer data,
during James’s lifetime. His early diagnosis of allowing a more realistic understanding of so-
PDD-NOS reflected the fact that his presenta- cial functioning in ASD and can make it possi-
tion was not classically autistic and that at the ble to gain a better understanding of the more
time it was difficult to be certain whether he subtle social and communicative deficits ob-
entirely met criteria for autism. It is possible, served in high-functioning persons with ASD.
but not certain, that were he assessed as a At present, much remains to be learned
young child today, he would be classified with about brain development in children with ASD
Asperger syndrome. Taken altogether, James’s with respect to specific, known changes in
case forces us to examine the relationships the behavioral and clinical manifestations of
among different manifestations of his im- ASD over time. Although approaches to study-
paired functioning across the stages of devel- ing brain and behavior in ASD have become in-
opment. While we can explain some of the creasingly more sophisticated over the years,
changes in terms of James’s developmental we have not yet succeeded in integrating these
maturation, we are left with many puzzling approaches to form a more comprehensive neu-
questions about the development of his ASD rodevelopmental model of ASD. In construct-
and the eventual outcome for this intelligent ing such a model, it will be important also to
young man. consider not only the changing and reciprocal
nature of the relationship between brain and
CONCLUSION behavior over development but also the self-
organizing activities of the developing child
ASD, like other developmental disorders, is (Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994). The advent of be-
not a static condition that once visited upon havioral genetics approaches to ASD, as well as
the child, remains the same with increasing animal models, opens new avenues through
age. Instead, ASD is manifested differently as which the neurodevelopmental basis of ASD can
the child develops, reflecting the maturation be addressed. A greater understanding of these
of neural and behavioral systems, the effects factors in ASD may help to explain the comor-
of learning and experience, the activity of the bidity or overlap in symptoms with other condi-
individual, and their reciprocal interactions. tions such as ADHD and anxiety disorders, as
276 Development and Behavior

well as the reasons that mental retardation is tally retarded children. Journal of Autism and
present in most individuals with ASD. Developmental Disorders, 9, 83–93.
Finally, as identification of children with Ando, H., Yoshimura, I., & Wakabayashi, S.
ASD has taken place at earlier and earlier (1980). Effects of age on adaptive behavior
ages, both the need and the opportunity for levels and academic skill levels in autistic and
mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism
early intervention have increased. Although
and Developmental Disorders, 10, 173–184.
treatments using methods such as applied be- Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autistischen psychopa-
havior analysis, psychotropic medication, and then” im Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie
social skills training have all benefited chil- und Nervenkrakheiten [Autistic psychopathol-
dren with ASD, additional research to develop ogy in childhood], 117, 76–136.
more effective programs and services is criti- Bailey, K. J. (2001). Social competence of children
cally needed. The challenges faced by the with autism classified as best-outcome follow-
school-age child with ASD are many and mul- ing behavior analytic treatment. Dissertation
tidimensional, and they vary with the needs Abstracts International, 61(12-B), 6696.
and developmental levels of the individual. To Baker, M. J. (2000). Incorporating children with
autism’s thematic ritualistic behaviors into
help the child with ASD deal successfully with
games to increase social play interactions with
school, peers, physical maturation, changing
siblings. Dissertation Abstracts International,
family relationships, and other challenges of 60(12-B), 6353.
the school years, programs must reflect an Baltaxe, C. (1977). Pragmatic deficits in the lan-
awareness not only of the deficits characteris- guage of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pedi-
tic of ASD but also of the growing capabilities atric Psychology, 2, 176–180.
of the developing child. Baltaxe, C. (1984). Use of contrastive stress in nor-
mal, aphasic and autistic children. Journal of
Cross-References Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 97–105.
Baltaxe, C., & D’Angiola, N. (1996). Referencing
Autism in infancy and in adolescence and skills in children with autism and specific lan-
guage impairment. European Journal of Disor-
adulthood is discussed in Chapters 8 and 10,
ders of Communication, 31, 245–258.
respectively; model programs are discussed in Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1985). Prosodic de-
Chapter 41, and health care issues are re- velopment in normal and autistic children.
viewed in Chapter 20. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Issues in
autism: Communication problems in autism
REFERENCES (pp. 95–125). New York: Plenum Press.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Do autistic children have
Abramson, R. K., Wright, H. H., Cuccaro, M. L., obsessions and compulsions? British Journal of
Lawrence, L. G., Babb, S. P., Pencrinha, D., Child Psychology, 28, 193–200.
et al. (1992). Biological liability in families Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay
with autism. Journal of the American Acad- on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge,
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, MA: MIT Press.
370–371. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1986).
Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnor- Mechanical, behavioral, and intentional under-
mal processing of social information from standing of picture stories in autistic children.
faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
science, 13, 232–240. 4, 113–125.
Anderson, S., Hanson, R., Malecha, M., Oftelie, Bartak, L., & Rutter, M. L. (1974). The use of
A., Erickson, C., & Clark, J. M. (1997). The personal pronouns by autistic children. Jour-
effectiveness of naltrexone in treating task at- nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4,
tending, aggression, self-injury and stereo- 217–222.
typic mannerisms of six young males with Bartak, L., & Rutter, M. L. (1976). Differences be-
autism or pervasive developmental disorders. tween mentally retarded and normally intelli-
Journal of Developmental and Physical Dis- gent autistic children. Journal of Autism and
abilities, 9(3), 211–242. Childhood Schizophrenia, 6, 109–120.
Ando, H., & Yoshimura, I. (1979). Effects of age Berg, C. J., Zahn, T. P., & Behar, D. (1986). Child-
on communication skill levels and prevalence hood obsessive-compulsive disorder: An anxi-
of maladaptive behaviors in autistic and men- ety disorder. In R. Gittelman (Ed.), Anxiety
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 277

disorders of childhood (pp. 126–135). New Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28,
York: Guilford Press. 193–204.
Bettleheim, B. (1967). The empty fortress. New Carpentieri, S., & Morgan, S. B. (1996). Adaptive
York: Free Press. and intellectual functioning in autistic and
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & nonautistic retarded children. Journal of Autism
Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive and Developmental Disorders, 26, 611–620.
behavior in autism: Comparisons to mental re- Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Wang,
tardation. Journal of Autism and Developmen- J. J., Lord, C., Dawson, G., et al. (1998). The
tal Disorders, 30, 237–243. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supple-
Boucher, J. (1999). Editorial: Interventions with mentary norms for individuals with autism.
children with autism—methods based on play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15, ders, 28, 287–302.
1–5. Carter, C. M. (2001). Using choice with interactive
Brun, P. (2001). The psychopathology of emotions in play to increase language skills in children
children: The importance of typical develop- with autism. Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
ment [Psychopathologie de l’emotion chez tional, 61(12-A), 4730.
l’enfant: L’importance des donnees developpe- Chamberlain, B. O. (2002). Isolation or involve-
mentales typiques]. Enfance, 53, 281–291. ment? The social networks of children with
Brun, P., Nadel, J., & Mattlinger, M. J. (1998). The autism included in regular classes. Disserta-
emotional hypothesis in autism [L’hypothese tion Abstracts International, 62(8-A), 2680.
emotionnelle dans l’autisme.] Psychologie Charlop, M. H., & Trasowech, J. E. (1991). In-
Francaise, 43, 147–156. creasing autistic children’s daily spontaneous
Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, 24, 747–761.
H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under- Charney, R. (1980). Speech roles and the develop-
standing other minds: Perspectives from autism ment of personal pronouns. Journal of Child
(pp. 267–291). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- Language, 7, 509–528.
versity Press. Charney, R. (1981). Pronoun errors in autistic chil-
Buitelaar, J. K., van der Gaag, J., & van der Ho- dren: Support for a social explanation. British
even, J. (1998). Buspirone in the management Journal of Disorders of Communication, 15,
of anxiety and irritability in children with per- 39–43.
vasive developmental disorders: Results of an Chiat, S. (1982). If I were you and you were me: The
open-label study. Journal of Clinical Psychia- analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-reversing
try, 59, 56–59. child. Journal of Child Language, 9, 359–379.
Buitelaar, J. K., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, K., de Chin, H. H., & Bernard-Opitz, V. (2000). Teaching
Vries, H., & van Hooff, J. A. (1991). Differ- conversational skills to children with autism:
ences in the structure of social behaviour of Effect on the development of a theory of mind.
autistic children and non-autistic retarded Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- ders, 30, 569–583.
chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 995–1015. Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development
Burack, J., & Volkmar, F. (1992). Development of and self-regulatory structures of the mind. De-
low- and high-functioning autistic children. velopment and Psychopathology, 6, 533–549.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Ciesielski, K. T., Courchesne, E., & Elmasian, R.
Allied Disciplines, 33, 607–616. (1990). Effects of focused selective attention
Capps, L., Kasari, C., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. tasks on event-related potentials in autistic
(1993). Parental perception of emotional ex- and normal individuals. Electroencephalog-
pressiveness in children with autism. Journal raphy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75,
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 207–220.
475–484. Clark, T., Feehan, C., Tinline, C., & Vostanis, P.
Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Con- (1999). Autistic symptoms in children with at-
versational abilities among children with tention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Euro-
autism and children with developmental de- pean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8,
lays. Autism: International Journal of Research 50–55.
and Practice, 2, 325–344. Clarke, D. J., Littlejohns, C. S., Corbett, J. A., &
Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). The Joseph, S. (1989). Pervasive developmental
frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad: Nar- disorders and psychoses in adult life. British
rative competence in children with autism. Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 692–699.
278 Development and Behavior

Cohen, I. L. (1995). Behavioral profiles of autistic Autism (Vol. 23, pp. 239–266). San Diego, CA:
and nonautistic fragile X males. Developmen- Academic Press.
tal Brain Dysfunction, 8, 252–269. Dunlap, G., Koegel, R., & Egel, A. (1979). Autistic
Courchesne, E. (1987). A neurophysiological view of children in school. Exceptional Children, 45,
autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), 552–558.
Neurobiological issues in autism (pp. 285–324). Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (1997). School place-
New York: Plenum Press. ment and academic achievement in children
Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A. J., Yeung-Courchesne, with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of
R., Elmasian, R., & Grillon, C. (1989). Patho- Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 9,
physiologic findings in nonretarded autism and 277–291.
receptive developmental language disorder. El-Ghoroury, N. H., & Romanczyk, R. G. (1999).
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Play interactions of family members towards
ders, 19, 1–17. children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron- Developmental Disorders, 29, 249–258.
Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999). Ermer, J., & Dunn, W. (1998). The sensory profile:
Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months A discriminant analysis of children with and
of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagno- without disabilities. American Journal of Occu-
sis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- pational Therapy, 52, 283–290.
try, 40, 719–732. Fay, W. (1973). On the echolalia of the blind and of
Craig, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity the autistic child. Journal of Speech and Hear-
and imagination in autism and Asperger syn- ing Disorders, 38, 478–489.
drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Fay, W. (1979). Personal pronouns and the autistic
Disorders, 29, 319–326. child. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Craig, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Story-telling Disorders, 9, 247–260.
ability in children with autism or Asperger Fay, W., & Butler, B. (1968). Echolalia, IQ, and the
syndrome: A window into the imagination. Is- developmental dichotomy of speech and lan-
raeli Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sci- guage systems. Journal of Speech and Hearing
ences, 37, 64–70. Research, 11, 365–371.
Cullain, R. E. (2002). The effects of social stories Ferrari, M., & Matthews, W. S. (1983). Self-
on anxiety levels and excessive behavioral ex- recognition deficits in autism: Syndrome spe-
pressions of elementary school-aged children cific or general developmental delay. Journal
with autism. Dissertation Abstracts Interna- of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 13,
tional, 62(7-A), 2383. 317–324.
Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, atten- Field, T., Field, T., Sanders, C., & Nadel, J. (2001).
tion, and the socio-emotional impairments of Children with autism display more social be-
individuals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), haviors after repeated imitation sessions.
Autism: Nature, diagnosis and treatment Autism: International Journal of Research and
(pp. 49–79). New York: Guilford Press. Practice, 5(3), 317–323.
Dawson, G., & McKissick, F. C. (1984). Self- Fine, J., Bartolucci, G., Ginsberg, G., & Szatmari,
recognition in autistic children. Journal of P. (1991). The use of intonation to communi-
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 14, cate in pervasive developmental disorders.
383–395. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Dawson, J. E., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. 32, 771–782.
(1998). An analysis of maladaptive behaviors Fine, J., Bartolucci, G., Szatmari, P., & Ginsberg,
in persons with autism, PDD-NOS, and mental G. (1994). Cohesive discourse in pervasive de-
retardation. Research in Developmental Dis- velopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
abilities, 19(5), 439–448. Developmental Disorders, 24, 315–329.
DeLong, G. R., & Dwyer, J. J. (1988). Correlation of Fisch, G. S., Simensen, R. J., & Schroer, R. J.
family history with specific autistic subgroups: (2002). Longitudinal changes in cognitive and
Asperger’s syndrome and bipolar affective dis- adaptive behavior scores in children and adoles-
ease. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- cents with the Fragile X mutation or autism.
abilities, 18, 593–600. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Dissanayake, C., & Sigman, M. (2001). Attachment 32(2), 107–114.
and emotional responsiveness in children with Frea, W. D., & Hughes, C. (1997). Functional anal-
autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International ysis and treatment of social-communicative
review of research in mental retardation: behavior of adolescents with developmental
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 279

disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavioral Goldstein, G., Minshew, N. J., & Siegel, D. J.
Analysis, 30(4), 701–704. (1994). Age differences in academic achieve-
Freeman, B. J., Rahbar, B., Ritvo, E. R., Bice, ment in high-functioning autistic individuals.
T. L., Yokota, A., & Ritvo, R. (1991). The sta- Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu-
bility of cognitive and behavioral parameters ropsychology, 16(5), 671–680.
in autism: A twelve-year prospective study. Gordon, O. T., Rapoport, J. T., Hamburger, J. D.,
Journal of the American Academy of Child and State, R. C., & Mannheim, G. B. (1992). Dif-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 479–482. ferential response of seven subjects with autis-
Freitag, G. (1970). An experimental study of the tic disorder to clomipramine and desipramine.
social responsiveness of children with autistic American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 363–366.
behaviors. Journal of Experimental Child Psy- Grandin, T. (1995). How people with autism think.
chology, 9, 436–453. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learn-
Frith, U. (1991). Autism and Asperger syndrome. ing and cognition in autism: Current issues in
London: Cambridge University Press. autism (pp. 137–156). New York: Plenum
Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for Press.
meaning and reading for sound in autistic and Gray, K. M., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). Are there early
dyslexic children. British Journal of Develop- features of autism in infants and preschool chil-
mental Psychology, 1, 329–342. dren? Journal of Pediatrics and Child Health,
Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1986). Comprehension 37(3), 221–226.
in hyperlexic readers. Journal of Experimental Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., Pauls, D. L., Senft, R.,
Child Psychology, 42, 392–415. Hooper, C., & Volkmar, F. R. (2002). A De-
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, scriptive Study of Hyperlexia in a Clinically
G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor Referred Sample of Children with Develop-
cortex. Brain, 119(Pt 2):593–609. mental Delays. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Gena, A., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., & mental Disorders, 32, 3–12.
Poulson, C. L. (1996). Training and general- Groden, J., Cautela, J., Prince, S., & Berryman, J.
ization of affective behavior displayed by (1994). The impact of stress and anxiety on in-
youth with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior dividuals with autism and developmental dis-
Analysis, 29(3), 291–304. abilities. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.),
Gepner, B., Deruelle, C., & Grynfeltt, S. (2001). Behavioral issues in autism: Current issues in
Motion and emotion: A novel approach to the autism (pp. 177–194). New York: Plenum Press.
study of face processing by young autistic Hadano, K., Nakamura, H., & Hamanaka, T.
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental (1998). Effortful echolalia. Cortex, 34, 67–82.
Disorders, 31(1), 37–45. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K.
Gillberg, C. (1984). Autistic children growing up: (1996). Can we teach children with autism to
Problems during puberty and adolescence. De- understand emotions, belief, or pretence? De-
velopmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 26, velopment and Psychopathology, 8, 345–365.
122–129. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K.
Gillberg, C. (1985). Asperger’s syndrome and re- (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have an
current psychosis—A neuropsychiatric case effect on the ability to develop conversation in
study. Journal of Autism and Developmental children with autism? Journal of Autism and
Disorders, 15, 389–397. Developmental Disorders, 27, 519–537.
Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1992). The biology of Hall, L. J. (1997). Effective behavioural strategies
the autistic syndromes (2nd ed.). New York: for the defining characteristics of autism. Be-
Cambridge University Press. haviour Change, 14(3), 139–154.
Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxi- Halle, J. W. (1982). Teaching functional language
ety in high-functioning children with autism. to the handicapped: An integrative model of
Autism: International Journal of Research and the natural environment teaching techniques.
Practice, 5, 277–286. Journal of the Association for the Severely
Goldberg, T. E. (1987). On hermetic reading abili- Handicapped, 7, 29–36.
ties. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- Hamden-Allen, G. (1991). Brief report: Trichotil-
orders, 17, 29–44. lomania in an autistic male. Journal of Autism
Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. and Developmental Disorders, 21, 79–82.
(2001). Attentional processes in autism. Jour- Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative compe-
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, tence and theory of mind in autism: A test of
31(4), 433–440. relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119.
280 Development and Behavior

Harris, S. L., Handleman, J. S., & Fong, P. L. or mental retardation. Journal of Autism and
(1987). Imitation of self-stimulation: Impact Developmental Disorders, 20, 205–219.
on the autistic child’s behavior and affect. Jahr, E., Eldevik, S., & Eikeseth, S. (2000). Teach-
Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 9, 1–21. ing children with autism to initiate and sustain
Haviland, J. M., Walker-Andrews, A. S., Huffman, cooperative play. Research in Developmental
L. R., & Toci, L. (1996). Intermodal percep- Disabilities, 21(2), 151–169.
tion of emotional expressions by children with James, A. L., & Barry, R. J. (1980). A review of
autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical psychopathology in early onset psychosis.
Disabilities, 8(1), 77–88. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 6, 506–525.
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On acquiring knowledge Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1996). Gener-
about people and the capacity to pretend: Re- ativity deficits in pretend play in autism.
sponse to Leslie. Psychological Review, 97(1), British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
114–121. 14, 275–300.
Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., & Hannequinn, D. (1990).
of mind. Hove, England: Erlbaum. Right hemisphere and verbal communication.
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). Emotion New York: Springer-Verlag.
recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and Jones, R. S. P., Zahl, A., & Huws, J. C. (2001).
voices. Psychological Medicine, 18, 911–923. First-hand accounts of emotional experiences
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1989). Recog- in autism: A qualitative analysis. Disability
nition of emotion by mentally retarded adoles- and Society, 16(3), 393–401.
cents and young adults. American Journal on Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly,
Mental Retardation, 93, 434–443. J., Lincoln, A., et al. (2001). Hypersociabil-
Hoch, H., McComas, J. J., Johnson, L., Faranda, N., ity: The social and affective phenotype of
& Guenther, S. (2002). The effects of magni- Williams syndrome. In U. Bellugi & M. St.
tude and quality of reinforcement on choice George (Eds.), Journey from cognition to
responding during play activities. Journal of brain to gene: Perspectives from Williams’
Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 171–181. syndrome (pp. 43–71). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Hough, M. S. (1990). Narrative comprehension in Press.
adults with right and left hemisphere brain Jordan, R. R. (1989). An experimental comparison
damage: Theme organization. Brain and Lan- of the understanding and use of speaker-
guage, 38, 253–277. addressee personal pronouns in autistic chil-
Howlin, P. (1982). Echolalic and spontaneous phrase dren. British Journal of Disorders of Communi-
speech in autistic children. Journal of Child cation, 24, 169–179.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 281–293. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1997). An in-
Hurtig, R., Ensrud, S., & Tomblin, J. B. (1982). vestigation of attention and affect in children
The communicative function of questions pro- with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of
duction in autistic children. Journal of Autism Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(4),
and Developmental Disorders, 12, 57–69. 385–396.
Hutt, C., Hutt, S. J., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. (1964). Kamps, D., Locke, P., Delquadri, J., & Hall, R. V.
Arousal and childhood autism. Nature, 204, (1989). Increasing academic skills of students
908–909. with autism using fifth grade peers as tutors.
Hwang, B., & Hughes, C. (2000). The effects of so- Education and Treatment of Children, 12,
cial interactive training on early social com- 38–51.
municative skills of children with autism. Kamps, D., Royer, J., Dugan, E., Kravits, T., Gon-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- zalez-Lopez, A., Garcia, J., et al. (2002). Peer
ders, 30(4), 331–343. training to facilitate social interaction for ele-
Ingenmey, R., & Van Houten, R. (1991). Using time mentary students with autism and their peers.
delay to promote spontaneous speech in an Exceptional Children, 68, 173–187.
autistic child. Journal of Applied Behavioral Kamps, D., Walker, D., Maher, J., & Rotholtz, D.
Analysis, 24, 591–596. (1992). Academic and environmental effects
Insel, T. R., & Akiskal, H. S. (1986). Obsessive- of small group arrangements in classrooms for
compulsive disorder with psychotic features: students with autism and other developmental
A phenomenologic analyses. American Journal disabilities. Journal of Autism and Develop-
of Psychiatry, 143, 1527–1533. mental Disorders, 22, 277–293.
Jacobson, J., & Ackerman, L. (1990). Differences in Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec-
adaptive functioning among people with autism tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 281

Kanner, L. (1944). Early infantile autism. Journal Lainhart, J. E., & Folstein, S. E. (1994). Affective
of Pediatrics, 25, 211–217. disorders in people with autism: A review of
Kauffmann, C., Vance, H. B., Pumariega, A. J., & published cases. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Miller, B. (2001). Fluvoxamine treatment of a opmental Disabilities, 24, 587–601.
child with severe PDD: A single case study. Psy- Landry, S. H., & Loveland, K. A. (1988). Commu-
chiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, nication behaviors in autism and developmen-
64(3), 268–277. tal language delay. Journal of Child Psychology
Kennedy, C. H., Meyer, K. A., Knowles, T., & and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 29,
Shukla, S. (2000). Analyzing the multiple 621–634.
functions of stereotypical behavior for stu- Lee, A., Hobson, R. P., & Chiat, S. (1994). I, you,
dents with autism: Implications for assessment me, and autism: An experimental study. Jour-
and treatment. Journal of Applied Behavioral nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Analysis, 33(4), 559–771. 24, 155–176.
Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, Lhermitte, F. (1983). “Utilisation behaviour ” and
D. L., & Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes.
of anxiety and mood problems among children Brain, 106, 237–255.
with autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism: Linetsky, E., Planer, D., & Ben-Hur, T. (2000).
International Journal of Research and Practice, Echolalia-palilalia as the sole manifestation of
4, 117–132. nonconvulsive status epilepticus. Neurology,
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & 55, 733–734.
Cohen, D. (2002a). Defining and quantifying Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D. S., Dunn,
the social phenotype in autism. American M., Feinstein, C., et al. (2001). Predictors and
Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 909–916. correlates of adaptive functioning in children
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & with developmental disorders. Journal of
Cohen, D. (2002b). Visual fixation patterns Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2),
during viewing of naturalistic social situations 219–230.
as predictors of social competence in individu- Little, L. (2002). Middle-class mothers’ percep-
als with autism. Archives of General Psychia- tions of peer and sibling victimization among
try, 59, 809–816. children with Asperger’s syndrome and non-
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). verbal learning disorders. Issues in Compre-
Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations of hensive Pediatric Nursing, 25(1), 43–57.
the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of Child Lord, C., Rutter, M. L., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 861–876. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Sched-
Koshes, R. J. (1997). Use of f luoxetine for ule—WPS (WPS ed.). Los Angeles: Western
obsessive-compulsive behavior in adults with Psychological Services.
autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, Lord, C., Rutter, M. L., & Le Couteur, A. (1994).
578. The Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A
Kraijer, D. (2000). Review of adaptive behavior revised version of a diagnostic interview for
studies in mentally retarded persons with caregivers of individuals with possible perva-
autism /pervasive developmental disorder. Jour- sive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
30(1), 39–47. Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., &
Kroeker, R. (2001). Rhythmic behaviors in typi- Rehm, R. (1971). Selective responding by
cally developing infants, and infants with later autistic children to multiple sensory inputs.
diagnosed autism or developmental delay. Dis- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 211–222.
sertation Abstracts International, 62(5-B), Loveland, K. A. (1984). Learning about points of
2517. view: Spatial perspective and the acquisition
Kurita, H., & Nakayasu, N. (1994). Brief report: of ‘I /you.’ Journal of Child Language, 2,
An autistic male presenting seasonal affective 535–556.
disorder (SAD) and trichotillomania. Journal Loveland, K. A. (2001). Toward an ecological the-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, ory of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N.
687–692. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The develop-
Lainhart, J. E. (1999). Psychiatric problems in in- ment of autism: Perspectives from theory and
dividuals with autism, their parents and sib- research (pp. 17–37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
lings. International Review of Psychiatry, 11, Loveland, K. A., Fletcher, J., & Bailey, V. (1990).
278–298. Verbal and nonverbal communication of events
282 Development and Behavior

in learning-disability subtypes. Journal of Clin- Luteijn, E. F., Serra, M., Jackson, S., Steenhuis,
ical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, M. P., Althaus, M., Volkmar, F. R., et al.
433–447. (2000). How unspecified are disorders of chil-
Loveland, K. A., & Kelley, M. (1988). Develop- dren with a pervasive developmental disorder
ment of adaptive behavior in adolescents and not otherwise specified? A study of social
young adults with autism and Down syndrome. problems in children with PDD-NOS and
American Journal of Mental Retardation, 93, ADHD. European Child and Adolescent Psy-
84–92. chiatry, 9, 168–179.
Loveland, K. A., & Kelley, M. (1991). Develop- Martin, A., Scahill, L., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R.
ment of adaptive behavior in preschoolers with (1999). Higher-functioning pervasive develop-
autism and Down syndrome. American Journal mental disorders: Rates and patterns of psy-
of Mental Retardation, 96, 13–20. chotropic drug use. Journal of the American
Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint at- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
tention and communication in autism and 38, 923–931.
language delay. Journal of Autism and Devel- Mazzocco, M. M., Kates, W. R., Baumgardner,
opmental Disorders, 16, 335–349. T. L., Freund, L. S., & Reiss, A. L. (1997).
Loveland, K. A., Landry, S. H., Hughes, S. O., Autistic behaviors among girls with fragile X
Hall, S. K., & McEvoy, R. E. (1988). Speech syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
acts and the pragmatic deficits of autism. tal Disorders, 27, 415–435.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, McDonnell, J., Thorson, N., & McQuivey, C.
593–604. (1998). The instructional characteristics of in-
Loveland, K. A., McEvoy, R. E., Tunali, B., & Kel- clusive classes for elementary students with
ley, M. (1990). Narrative story-telling in severe disabilities: An exploratory study. Jour-
autism and Down syndrome. British Journal of nal of Behavioral Education, 8, 415–437.
Developmental Psychology, 8, 9–23. McDonough, L., Stahmer, A., Schreibman, L., &
Loveland, K. A., Pearson, D. A., Tunali-Kotoski, Thompson, S. J. (1997). Deficits, delays, and
B., Ortegon, J., & Gibbs, M. C. (2001). Judg- distractions: An evaluation of symbolic play
ments of social appropriateness by children and memory in children with autism. Develop-
and adolescents with autism. Journal of Autism ment and Psychopathology, 9(1), 17–41.
and Developmental Disorders, 31, 367–376. McDougle, C. J., Holmes, J. P., Carlson, D. C., Pel-
Loveland, K. A., & Tunali, B. (1991). Social scripts ton, G. H., Cohen, D. J., & Price, L. H. (1998).
for conversational interactions in autism and A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of
Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Devel- risperidone in adults with autistic disorder
opmental Disorders, 21, 177–186. and other pervasive developmental disorders.
Loveland, K. A., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(7), 633–641.
language in autism and the theory of mind hy- McEvoy, R. E., Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H.
pothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron- (1988). Functions of immediate echolalia in
Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Understanding other minds: Perspectives from opmental Disorders, 18, 657–688.
autism (pp. 247–266). Oxford, England: Ox- McLaughlin-Cheng, E. (1998). Asperger syndrome
ford University Press. and autism: A literature review and meta-
Loveland, K. A., Tunali, B., Kelley, M., & McEvoy, analysis. Focus on Autism and Other Develop-
R. E. (1989). Referential communication and mental Disabilities, 13(4), 234–245.
response adequacy in autism and Down syn- McLennan, J. D., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1993).
drome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, Sex differences in higher functioning people
401–413. with autism. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., mental Disorders, 23, 217–227.
Brelsford, K., Ortegon, J., & Pearson, D. Mendez, M. F. (2002). Prominent echolalia from iso-
(1995). Intermodal perception of affect in per- lation of the speech area. Journal of Neuropsy-
sons with autism or Down syndrome. Develop- chiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14(3), 356.
ment and Psychopathology, 7, 409–418. Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (1980. March). Social
Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D., and interpersonal problems of autistic adoles-
Brelsford, K., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R. (1994). cents and adults. Meeting of the Southeastern
Imitation and expression of facial affect in Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, Michelotti, J., Charman, T., Slonims, V., & Baird,
433–444. G. (2002). Follow-up of children with language
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 283

delay and features of autism from preschool of the inference of causal events and emotion
years to middle childhood. Developmental Med- in the sentence. Japanese Journal of Behavior
icine and Child Neurology, 44, 812–819. Therapy, 25(1), 7–22.
Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., Falco, C., Fico, C., & O’Neill, D. K., & Happé, F. G. E. (2000). Noticing
Palermo, M. T. (2002). Repetitive behaviors in and commenting on what’s new: Differences
autistic disorder. European Journal of Child and similarities among 22-month-old typically
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(5), 210–218. developing children, children with Down syn-
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1995). drome and children with autism. Developmen-
Speech and language in high-functioning autis- tal Science, 3(4), 457–478.
tic individuals. Neuropsychology, 9, 255–261. O’Neill, R. E., & Sweetland-Baker, M. (2001).
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Taylor, G., & Siegel, Brief report: An assessment of stimulus gener-
D. J. (1994). Academic achievement in high alization and contingency effects in functional
functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clin- communication training with two students with
ical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
261–270. Disorders, 31(2), 235–240.
Moes, D. (1998). Integrating choice-making op- Ozonoff, S., & Cathcart, K. (1998). Effectiveness
portunities within teacher-assigned academic of a home program intervention for young chil-
tasks to facilitate the performance of chil- dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Devel-
dren with autism. Journal of the Association opmental Disorders, 28, 25–32.
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 23, Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration
319–328. of right hemisphere contributions to the prag-
Morrison, L., Kamps, D., Garcia, J., & Parker, D. matic impairments of autism. Brain and Lan-
(2001). Peer mediation and monitoring strate- guage, 52, 411–434.
gies to improve initiations and social skills for Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. (1990).
students with autism. Journal of Positive Be- Are there specific emotion perception deficits
havior Interventions, 3, 237–250. in young autistic children? Journal of Child
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 343–361.
(1986). Defining the social deficits of autism. Ozonoff, S., & South, M. (2001). Early social
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, development in young children with autism:
27, 657–669. Theoretical and clinical implications. In
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. G. Bremner & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell hand-
(1987). Nonverbal communication and play book of infant development: Handbooks of de-
correlates of language development in autistic velopmental psychology (pp. 565–588). Malden,
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental MA: Blackwell.
Disorders, 17(3), 349–364. Pearlman-Avnion, S., & Eviatar, Z. (2002). Narra-
Murphy, M., Bolton, P. F., Pickles, A., Fombonne, tive analysis in developmental social and
E., Piven, J., & Rutter, M. L. (2000). Personal- linguistic pathologies: Dissociation between
ity traits of the relatives of autistic probands. emotional and informational language use.
Psychological Medicine, 30, 1411–1424. Brain and Cognition, 48, 494–499.
Njardvik, U., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive
(1999). A comparison of social skills in adults functions and developmental psychopathology.
with autistic disorder, pervasive develop- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
mental disorder not otherwise specified, and 37(1), 51–87.
mental retardation. Journal of Autism and De- Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Evidence for
velopmental Disorders, 29(4), 287–295. a cerebellar role in reduced exploration and
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M. A., Ortiz, E., stereotyped behavior in autism. Biological
Rivera, C. M., & Greer, R. (2002). Teaching Psychiatry, 49, 655–664.
children with autism to prefer books or toys Pierce, K., Glad, K. S., & Schreibman, L. (1997).
over stereotypy or passivity. Journal of Positive Social perception in children with autism: An
Behavior Interventions, 4, 80–87. attentional deficit? Journal of Autism and De-
O’Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1994). Two autistic velopmental Disorders, 27, 265–282.
savant readers. Journal of Autism and Develop- Pierce, K., Muller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., &
mental Disorders, 24, 501–515. Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs
Okuda, K., Inoue, M., & Yamamoto, J. (1999). outside the fusiform “ face area” in autism: Ev-
Teaching reading comprehension for students idence from functional MRI. Brain, 124(10),
with developmental disabilities: Acquisition 2059–2073.
284 Development and Behavior

Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1999). Psychiatric disorder Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuro-
and the broad autism phenotype: Evidence psychological divergence of high-level autism
from a family study of multiple-incidence and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and De-
autism families. American Journal of Psychia- velopmental Disorders, 20, 155–168.
try, 156, 557–563. Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R.
Posey, D. J., Guenin, K. D., Kohn, A. E., Swiezy, (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychi-
N. B., & McDougle, C. J. (2001). A naturalis- atric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal
tic open-label study of mirtazapine in autistic of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
and other pervasive developmental disorders. 24, 465–473.
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychophar- Rutter, M. L. (1968). Concepts of autism: A review
macology, 11(3), 267–277. of research. Journal of Child Psychology and
Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. (1990). Psychiatry, 9, 1–25.
Autistic children’s knowledge of thinking and Rutter, M. L. (1974). The development of infantile
feeling states in other people. Journal of Child autism. Psychological Medicine, 4, 147–163.
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- Rutter, M. L. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In
plines, 31, 587–601. M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reap-
Prizant, B. M. (1983). Language acquisition and praisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 1–25).
communicative behavior in autism: Toward an New York: Plenum Press.
understanding of the whole of it. Journal of Rutter, M. L. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 296–307. pathogenesis of autism. Journal of Child Psy-
Prizant, B. M., & Duchan, J. F. (1981). The func- chology and Psychiatry, 24, 513–531.
tions of immediate echolalia in autistic chil- Rutter, M. L. (1985). Infantile autism and other per-
dren. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, vasive developmental disorders. In M. Rutter &
46, 241–249. L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychia-
Riguet, C. B., Taylor, N. D., Benaroya, S., & Klein, try: Modern approaches (pp. 545–566). Oxford,
L. S. (1981). Symbolic play in autistic, Down’s, England: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
and normal children of equivalent mental age. Rutter, M. L., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmen-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, tal psychopathology. In E. M. Hetherington
11(4), 439–448. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4,
Rimland, B. (1964). Infantile autism. New York: pp. 775–911). New York: Wiley.
Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rutter, M. L., Greenfield, D., & Lockyer, L.
Rincover, A., & Ducharme, J. M. (1987). Variables (1967). A five to fifteen-year follow-up study
influencing stimulus overselectivity and “ tun- of infantile psychosis: II. Social and behav-
nel vision” in developmentally delayed chil- ioral outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry,
dren. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 113, 1187–1199.
91, 422–430. Rydell, P. J., & Mirenda, P. (1994). Effects of high
Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B., & Geffken, G. R. and low constraint utterances on the produc-
(1991). A comparative evaluation of adaptive tion of immediate and delayed echolalia in
behavior in children and adolescents with young children with autism. Journal of Autism
autism, Down Syndrome, and normal develop- and Developmental Disorders, 24, 719–735.
ment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Sabbagh, M. (1999). Communicative intentions and
Disabilities, 21, 187–196. language: Evidence from right-hemisphere
Rojahn, J., & Sisson, L. A. (1990). Stereotyped be- damage and autism. Brain and Language, 70,
havior. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Handbook of be- 29–69.
havior modification with the mentally retarded Sasagawa, E., Oda, H., & Fujita, T. (2000). Effec-
(2nd ed., pp. 181–223). New York: Plenum tiveness of the Dohsa-Hou on mother-child in-
Press. teractions: Children with Down syndrome and
Ruble, L. A. (1998). Comparative study of the nat- autism. Japanese Journal of Special Education,
ural habitat behaviors of children with autism 38(1), 13–22.
and children with down syndrome: An ecolog- Schatz, J., & Hamdan-Allen, G. (1995). Effects of
ical approach. Dissertation Abstracts Interna- age and IQ on adaptive behavior domains for
tional, 59(5-B), 2459. children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Rumsey, J. M. (1992). Neuropsychological studies Developmental Disorders, 25(1), 51–60.
of high-level autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesi- Schopler, E., & Mesibov, G. B. (1983). Autism in
bov (Eds.), High functioning individuals with adolescents and adults. New York: Plenum
autism (pp. 41–64). New York: Plenum Press. Press.
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 285

Schreibman, L. (1988). Autism. Newbury Park, CA: Smith, T., Lovaas, N. W., & Lovaas, O. I. (2002).
Sage. Behaviors of children with high-functioning
Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Leslie, A. (1999). autism when paired with typically developing
If pigs could f ly: A test of counterfactual rea- versus delayed peers: A preliminary study. Be-
soning and pretence in children with autism. havioral Interventions, 17(3), 129–143.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., & Shapiro, T. (1987).
17(3), 349–362. Expression of emotion in young autistic chil-
Shabani, D. B., Wilder, D. A., & Flood, W. A. dren. Journal of the American Academy of Child
(2001). Reducing stereotypic behavior through and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836–838.
discrimination training, differential reinforce- Sparrow, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1987). Adaptive
ment of other behavior, and self-monitoring. behavior and the psychologically disturbed
Behavioral Interventions, 16, 279–286. child. Journal of Special Education, 21, 89–100.
Shapiro, T., Sherman, M., Calamari, G., & Stafford, N. (2000). Can emotions be taught to a
Koch, D. (1987). Attachment in autism and low functioning autistic child? Early Child
other developmental disorders. Journal of the Development and Care, 164, 105–126.
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 26, Stein, D. J., & Niehaus, D. (2001). Stereotypic self-
480–484. injurious behaviors: Neurobiology and psy-
Shaw, S. (2001). Behavioral treatment for children chopharmacology. In D. Simeon & E. Hollander
with autism: A comparison between discrete (Eds.), Self-injurious behaviors: Assessment and
trial training and pivotal response training in treatment (pp. 29–48). Washington, DC: Ameri-
teaching emotional perspective-taking skills. can Psychiatric Press.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 61(11-B), Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the in-
6121. fant. New York: Basic Books.
Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., St. James, P. J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). An
& Cohen, D. J. (2001). Speech and prosody observational study of humor in autism and
characteristics of adolescents and adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Devel-
high-functioning autism and Asperger syn- opmental Disorders, 24(5), 603–617.
drome. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Hepburn, S. L., Hogan,
44, 1097–1115. K. L., & Brown, C. S. (1999). Patterns of
Siegal, D. J., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. (1996). adaptive behavior in very young children with
Designing instruction for the high-functioning autism. American Journal on Mental Retarda-
autistic individual. Journal of Developmental tion, 104(2), 187–199.
and Physical Disabilities, 8, 1–19. Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H.
Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attach- (1996). Are children with autism deaf to gricean
ments in autistic children. Journal of the maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 55–72.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Swedo, S. E., & Rapoport, J. L. (1989). Phenom-
Psychiatry, 28, 74–81. enology and differential diagnosis of obsessive-
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and compulsive disorder in children and adolescents.
change in the social competence of children In J. L. Rapoport (Ed.), Obsessive-compulsive
with autism, Down syndrome, and develop- disorder in children and adolescents (pp. 13–32).
mental delays. Monographs of the Society for Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Research in Child Development, 64, 1–114. Szatmari, P., Archer, L., Fisman, S., Streiner,
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Attachment D. L., & Wilson, F. (1995). Asperger’s syn-
behaviors in autistic children. Journal of Autism drome and autism: Differences in behavior,
and Developmental Disorders, 14, 231–244. cognition, and adaptive functioning. Journal of
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
parents of children with autism predict the Psychiatry, 34(12), 1662–1671.
subsequent development of their children’s Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., & Bremner, R. (1989).
communication. Journal of Autism and Develop- Asperger’s syndrome and autism: Comparison
mental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89. of early history and outcome. Developmental
Silver, M., & Oakes, P. (2001). Author Evaluation Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 709–720.
of a new computer intervention to teach people Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989. April). An analysis of
with autism or Asperger syndrome to recog- discourse ability and internal state lexicons in
nize and predict emotions in others. Autism: a longitudinal study of autistic children. Soci-
International Journal of Research and Practice, ety for Research in Child Development,
5(3), 299–316. Kansas City, Missouri.
286 Development and Behavior

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). Once upon a ribbit: Sto- Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behavior
ries narrated by autistic children. British Jour- in autism: a review of psychological research.
nal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45–49. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The 40, 839–849.
development of contingent discourse ability in Unger, D. G., & Powell, D. R. (1980). Supporting
autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology families under stress: The role of social net-
and Psychiatry, 32, 1123–1134. works. Family Relations, 29, 566–574.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Calkins, S. (1990). Does im- Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., & Paul, R. (1986). An
itation facilitate the acquisition of grammar? evaluation of DSM-III criteria for infantile
Evidence from a study of autistic, Down’s autism. Journal of the American Academy of
syndrome, and normal children. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 25, 190–197.
Child Language, 17, 591–606. Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (1993). Social develop-
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baum- ment in autism: Historical and clinical perspec-
berger, T., Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Dias, tives. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, &
A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds:
acquisition in autistic and Down’s syndrome Perspectives from autism (pp. 40–55). Oxford,
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental England: Oxford University Press.
Disorders, 20, 1–21. Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2000). Diagnostic
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Quill, K. (1987). Story- issues in Asperger syndrome. In Asperger
telling and narrative skills in verbal autistic syndrome (pp. 25–71). New York: Guilford
children. Society for Research in Child Devel- Press.
opment, Baltimore, Maryland. Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P.,
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). At- Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field
tributing mental states to story characters: A trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American
comparison of narratives produced by autistic Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367.
and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Goudreau, D., Cic-
Psycholinguistics, 16(3), 241–256. chetti, D. V., Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987).
Takeuchi, K., & Yamamoto, J. I. (2001). A case Social deficits in autism: An operational ap-
study of examining the effects of self- proach using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
monitoring on improving academic perfor- Scales. Journal of the American Academy of
mance by a student with autism. Japanese Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26,
Journal of Special Education, 38, 105–116. 156–161.
Taylor, B. A., & Harris, S. L. (1995). Teaching Vostanis, P., Smith, B., Corbett, J., Sungum-Paliwal,
children with autism to seek information: R., Edwards, A., Gingell, K., et al. (1998).
Acquisition of novel information and general- Parental concerns of early development in chil-
ization of responding. Journal of Applied Be- dren with autism and related disorders. Autism:
havior Analysis, 28, 3–4. International Journal of Research and Practice,
Taylor, B. A., Levin, J., & Jasper, S. (1999). In- 2(3), 229–242.
creasing play-related statements in children Wahlberg, T., & Rotatori, A. F. (2001). Interview
with autism toward their siblings: Effects of with a high functioning adult with autism. In
video modeling. Journal of Developmental and T. Wahlberg et al. (Eds.), Autistic spectrum
Physical Disabilities, 11, 253–264. disorders: Educational and clinical interven-
Trad, P. V., Bernstein, D., Shapiro, T., & Hertzig, tions. Advances in special education (Vol. 14,
M. (1993). Assessing the relationship between pp. 269–300). Oxford, England: Elsevier.
affective responsivity and social interaction in Wainwright-Sharp, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1993).
children with pervasive developmental disor- Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning
der. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- people with autism. Journal of Autism and De-
orders, 23, 361–377. velopmental Disorders, 23, 1–13.
Tsai, L. Y. (1992). Diagnostic issues in high- Weeks, S., & Hobson, R. P. (1987). The salience of
functioning autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesi- facial expression for autistic children. Journal
bov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
autism (pp. 11–40). New York: Plenum Press. Disciplines, 28, 137–151.
Tunali, B., & Power, T. (1993). Creating satisfac- Whalen, C. M. (2001). Joint attention training for
tion: A psychological perspective on stress children with autism and the collateral ef-
and coping in families of handicapped chil- fects on language, play, imitation, and social
dren. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- behaviors. Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
atry, 34, 945–957. tional, 61(11-B), 6122.
The School-Age Child with an Autistic Spectrum Disorder 287

Whitehouse, D., & Harris, J. C. (1984). Hyperlexia Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
in infantile autism. Journal of Autism and De- of social interaction and associated abnormal-
velopmental Disabilities, 14, 281–289. ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica-
Williams, E., Reddy, V., & Costall, A. (2001). Tak- tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ing a closer look at functional play in children Disorders, 9, 11–29.
with autism. Journal of Autism and Develop- Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P.
mental Disorders, 31(1), 67–77. (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autistic,
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & mentally retarded, and normal children. Jour-
Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,
and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral 725–736.
Reviews, 25(4), 287–295. Zahn, T. P., Rumsey, J. M., & Van Kannen, D. P.
Williams, P. G., Allard, A., Sears, L., Dalrymple, (1987). Autonomic nervous system activity in
N., & Bloom, A. S. (2001). Brief report: Case autistic, schizophrenic, and normal men: Ef-
reports on naltrexone use in children with fects of stimulus significance. Journal of Ab-
autism: controlled observations regarding ben- normal Psychology, 96, 135–144.
efits and practical issues of medication man- Zercher, C., Hunt, P., Schuler, A., & Webster, J.
agement. Journal of Autism and Developmental (2001). Increasing joint attention, play and
Disorders, 31(1), 103–108. language through peer supported play. Autism:
Wing, L., & Attwood, A. (1987). Syndromes of International Journal of Research and Practice,
autism and atypical development. In D. Cohen, 5(4), 374–398.
A. Donellan, & R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of
autism and pervasive developmental disorders
(pp. 3–19). New York: Wiley.
CHAPTER 10

Adolescents and Adults with Autism

VICTORIA SHEA AND GARY B. MESIBOV

The clinical presentation and psychoeduca- 1989; Von Knorring & Haeggloef, 1993), al-
tional needs of adolescents and adults with though in many cases there are some residual
autism have not yet been studied as extensively characteristics of social, communication, and/or
as those aspects of children with autism. How- behavioral idiosyncrasies.
ever, there are some empirical follow-up stud-
ies of adolescents and adults who were initially Developmental Course
diagnosed with autism as children, and there is
also a growing professional literature of educa- The research literature indicates that at adoles-
tional and other therapeutic interventions for cence some individuals with autism improve
adolescents and adults. This chapter reviews markedly, others experience deterioration in
what is known about adolescents and adults functioning (e.g., increased aggression, in-
with autism (the term autism is used in this creasingly rigid or repetitive behavior, loss of
chapter for the range of autism spectrum disor- skills), and many continue a stable, matura-
ders or ASD) and then discusses significant tional course.
clinical topics related to these age groups.
Improvement
OUTCOME STUDIES A number of follow-up studies have reported
general symptomatic improvement with in-
It is clear that almost all children with autism creasing age. Kanner, Rodriguez, and Ashenden
grow up to be adolescents and then adults with (1972) indicated that “a remarkable change
autism. From the Maudsley Hospital long-term took place” (p. 29) in early-mid teens for a sub-
study (Rutter, 1970) and Leo Kanner’s (1973) group of patients who, according to the authors,
collection of papers including follow-up reports “ became uneasily aware of their peculiarities
of his patients, through Lotter’s (1978) careful and began to make a conscious effort to do
review of outcome studies, to current literature something about them” (p. 29) and later went
reviews (Howlin & Goode, 1998; Nordin & Gill- on, in most cases, to independent living and
berg, 1998), the consensus of the research and higher education or gainful employment.
clinical literature is that autism is almost always Of the 201 Japanese families surveyed
a lifelong disabling condition. However, several by Kobayashi, Murata, and Yoshinaga (1992),
authors have indicated that a small number of 43% reported “marked improvement ” in their
individuals diagnosed with autism as children youngsters, generally between the ages of 10 to
would not meet diagnostic criteria for autism in 15 years. Ballaban-Gil, Rapin, Tuchman, and
later years (e.g., DeMyer et al., 1973; Lovaas, Shinnar (1996) found that behavioral improve-
2000; Nordin & Gillberg, 1998; Piven, Harper, ment was reported for 9% to 18% of their het-
Palmer, & Arndt, 1996; Rumsey, Rapoport, & erogeneous sample, depending on the behavior.
Sceery, 1985; Rutter, 1970; Seltzer et al., 2003; Eaves and Ho (1996) reported cognitive or be-
Szatmari, Bartolucci, Bremner, Bond, & Rich, havioral improvement in 37% of their sample of

288
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 289

76 children followed for 4 years into early ado- skills, language comprehension, conversational
lescence. More optimistically, Piven et al. abilities, participation in group activities, and
(1996) found that parents of their sample of 38 appropriate classroom behavior.
“ high IQ” adolescents and adults reported that Most recently, Seltzer et al. (2003) reported
compared to their functioning at age 5 years, comparisons of Autism Diagnostic Interview-
82% had improved in communication, 82% Revised (ADI-R) “current ” versus “lifetime”
had improved in social interactions, and 55% scores for a sample of 251 adolescents (mean
had improved in restricted and repetitive be- age 15.71 years) and 154 adults (mean age
haviors. Similarly, the MIND Institute (Byrd 31.57 years) who had been diagnosed with
et al., 2002) study of 100 families with late autism in childhood. Significant symptom re-
adolescents with autism found that 88% of par- duction was reported for both groups in all
ents reported improvements in communication three domains of the ADI-R (Communication,
or language, 83% reported improvements in Reciprocal Social Interaction, and Restricted,
socialization, and 75% reported improvements Repetitive Behaviors and Interests). Based on
in behavior, interests, or activities (although current scores, only 54.8% of the total sample
it should be noted that parents of school-age met ADI-R cutoff scores in all three domains,
children were even more likely to indicate whereas 96.5% met these cutoffs based on
that their child’s autism had improved). Piven lifetime scores. In spite of the significant re-
et al. (1996) suggested that one developmental duction in symptoms and increase in social
trajectory for early childhood autism involves and communication skills from childhood to
significant improvement in all areas of symp- adulthood, according to the authors:
tomatology, to the point that “consideration
should be given to diagnosing autism in adults That the disorder changes in its manifestation over
who met criteria for autism as children and the life course does not . . . indicate that affected in-
continue to have impairments in related do- dividuals have any less of a need for services and
supports as they move through adolescence into
mains of behavior ” (p. 528) even if they don’t
adulthood and midlife than they did in childhood.
currently meet diagnostic criteria. Rather, developmentally appropriate services are
In a study of 59 clients receiving treat- needed for adolescents and adults with ASD diag-
ment in North Carolina’s Treatment and Educa- noses. (p. 579)
tion of Autistic and Related Communication-
handicapped CHildren (TEACCH) program,
Deterioration
scores on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale
(Schopler, Reichler, DeVellis, & Daly, 1980) Nordin and Gillberg’s (1998) review suggested
decreased (i.e., reflected improvement) by that 12% to 22% of adolescents show cognitive
an average of three points (on a scale from 15 or behavioral deterioration, although these fig-
to 60) between mean age 8.7 years and mean ures were derived from retrospective reports,
age 15.9 years. Significant improvements were not prospective studies. In the follow-up study
found for the group on ratings of imitation, of Kobayashi et al. (1992), families indicated
body movement, use of objects, adapting to that 32% of their youngsters had shown behav-
changes, responding to sounds, appropriate use ioral deterioration during their teenage years,
of near sensors (touch, taste, and smell), verbal with a reported peak at ages 12 to 13 years. Ven-
and nonverbal communication, and activity ter, Lord, and Schopler (1992) reported that in a
level (Mesibov, Schopler, Schaffer, & Michal, group of 58 high-functioning children, “ Two
1989). adolescents (one male, one female) experienced
Cross-sectional studies also suggest im- gradual cognitive and behavioral deteriorations
provement with age. Ando and colleagues in their mid-teens that plateaued in late adoles-
(Ando & Yoshimura, 1979; Ando, Yoshimura, cence; neither had seizures or any evidence of
& Wakabayashi, 1980), in a cross-sectional hard neurological signs after extensive investi-
study of 24 younger (ages 6 to 9 years) and 14 gations” (p. 494). Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996)
older (ages 11 to 14 years) individuals with found that families reported that problem be-
autism, reported higher skills levels in the haviors had worsened (or were being treated
older group in terms of toilet training, eating with medication) in 24 of 54 adolescents
290 Development and Behavior

(44%) and 22 of 45 adults (49%); further, in- drome to report feeling that they were “ walking
creased rates of behavioral difficulties since on eggshells” (p. 285).
childhood were reported at all cognitive levels Various empirical studies have documented
except for “normal /near normal” adolescents the prevalence of difficult behaviors in adoles-
(p. 218, Table 2). Eaves and Ho (1996), in a cents and adults. Rumsey et al. (1985) de-
sample of 76 individuals first evaluated as scribed a sample of 14 men with autism, 9 of
children (ages 3 to 12 years), reported behav- whom had intelligence in the average range.
ioral deterioration over a 4-year period in five Six individuals (five of them with average
individuals. Wing and Shah (2000) described, in intelligence) had significant, although infre-
a subgroup of adolescents, the development of quent, temper outbursts. Ballaban-Gil et al.
catatonia, defined as “increased slowness af- (1996) found that behavioral difficulties
fecting movement and verbal responses; diffi- and/or behavioral medication were reported in
culty in initiating and completing actions; 69% of their sample of 99 adolescents and
increased reliance on physical or verbal prompt- adults. The number of individuals with prob-
ing by others; and increased passivity and ap- lem behaviors or psychotropic medication was
parent lack of motivation” (p. 357) often inversely related to estimated intelligence
associated with unusual gait, postures, or range, but even in the highest functioning
“ freezing” at thresholds or in sitting positions, group (adults with normal /near normal intelli-
and in some cases preceded by periods of very gence), 7 of 13 individuals were reported by
agitated, at times aggressive, behavior. Gilchrist their families to have behavioral difficulties.
et al. (2001) reported that many of the individu- Howlin, Mawhood, and Rutter (2000) re-
als in their sample of 20 adolescents with As- ported assessment results for a group of 19 non-
perger syndrome were reported by their parents retarded adults with autism (mean age 23 years,
on the ADI to have more problems and symp- 9 months; mean performance IQ 94). Family
toms in adolescence than in early childhood. members of 18 of these 19 adults responded to
Seltzer et al. (2003) reported that 47 of 405 the ADI and reported that 3 individuals (17%)
individuals developed symptoms in adolescence showed “severe” levels of “autistic-type behav-
or adulthood that were not present in childhood, ior,” 10 individuals (56%) had “moderate” dis-
based on ADI-R interviews with parents. turbances in this area, and 5 (28%) individuals
had “no/minimal” problems.
Ongoing Behavioral Difficulties
Similarly, Howlin, Goode, Hutton, and Rut-
Even if their skills have not deteriorated, ter (2004) assessed the adult functioning of a
many adolescents and adults with autism are re- group of 68 individuals who had childhood non-
ported by their parents to exhibit significant be- verbal IQs of 50 or above and found that 23
havior problems, including resistance to change, (35%) were rated by their parents on the ADI as
compulsions, unacceptable sexual behavior, having moderate autism-related problem behav-
tantrums, aggression, and/or self-injurious be- iors, and 7 (11%) were rated as having severe
havior (DeMyer, 1979; Fong, Wilgosh, & Sob- problems. The authors found that “autistic-type
sey, 1993). Several authors have pointed out behaviors (routines, rituals, stereotypies, etc.)”
that even if the frequency of difficult behaviors were not strongly associated with IQ, and that:
decreases, the result of such behaviors on the
part of individuals who are taller, heavier, and Although the more able group was less likely to
stronger can be more distressing or even dan- show very severe difficulties in these areas, the
gerous than the same behaviors in childhood distribution of such symptoms was generally fairly
(Gillberg, 1991; Harris, Glasberg, & Del- evenly spread and within each IQ band, the major-
molino, 1998; Mesibov, 1983; Nordin & Gill- ity of individuals continued to have at least mild to
berg, 1998; Rutter, 1970). Marcus (1984) moderate problems associated with repetitive and
described the feelings of “ burnout ” experi- stereotyped behaviors. (p. 226)
enced by many parents of older youngsters with
autism. Seltzer, Krauss, Orsmond, and Vestal Intelligence
(2001) reported that mothers of 13 adults with
autism and mental retardation were much more An important determinant of developmental
likely than mothers of adults with Down syn- course in autism is the individual’s level of in-
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 291

telligence or mental retardation. The preva- Rapin, MD). The distribution of estimated in-
lence of mental retardation and the stability of telligence among adults was as follows: se-
IQ from childhood to adolescence or adulthood vere to profound mental retardation, 47%;
have been the focus of several investigations. mild to moderate mental retardation, 22%;
normal to near normal intelligence, 29% (in-
Autism and Mental Retardation
determinate, 2%). Among adolescents, the
Historically, estimates of the prevalence of curve was shifted markedly higher: severe to
mental retardation in the population of individ- profound mental retardation, 19%; mild to
uals with autism have been in the range moderate mental retardation, 37%; normal to
of 70% to 80% (Fombonne, 1999; National near normal intelligence, 42% (indetermi-
Research Council, 2001b; Tager-Flusberg, nate, 2%).
Joseph, & Folstein, 2001). However, because The recent study for the California Legis-
the broad autism spectrum includes so many lature by the University of California’s MIND
individuals with intelligence in the average Institute (Byrd, 2002) indicated that a record
range or above (e.g., people with Asperger syn- review revealed that mental retardation was di-
drome and some individuals with Pervasive agnosed in 50% of a sample of individuals with
Developmental Disorder-not otherwise speci- autism born between 1983 and 1985, but in only
fied [PDD-NOS]), the prevalence of mental re- 22% of a sample of individuals born between
tardation within the broad autism spectrum is 1993 and 1995. In a different study of popula-
now thought to be considerably lower than the tion trends in California, Croen, Grether,
figure of 70% to 80% (Bryson & Smith, 1998; Hoogstrate, and Selvin (2002a, 2002b) found
Lord & Bailey, 2002). Chakrabarti and Fom- that only 37% of children with autism born be-
bonne (2001) reported that the rate of mental tween 1987 and 1994 and served by the Califor-
retardation in the preschool children diagnosed nia Department of Developmental Services also
with PDD (i.e., the broad autism spectrum) in had diagnoses of mental retardation (although
a region of England was 26%. Gillberg (1998) there were factors that suggested that this figure
has suggested that the rate of mental retarda- could be either an underestimate [because some
tion within the broad autism spectrum might etiologies of mental retardation were excluded]
be as low as 15%. or an overestimate [because some findings of
Further, there are indications that the cur- mental retardation were not included in the
rent prevalence of mental retardation may be records]).
lower than the traditional figure of 70% to 80% A different perspective on the relationship
for younger cohorts of individuals with autism of autism and mental retardation is found in
because of the increased availability of early the work of Bryson and Smith (1998), who re-
intervention and special education. Eaves and ported that approximately 25% of a population
Ho (1996) described their sample of youngsters sample of adolescents and young adults with
with ASD born between 1974 and 1984 as hav- mental retardation also had autism. Similarly,
ing “autism in the third generation” (p. 558), Morgan et al. (2002) reported an autism preva-
using the generational metaphor of Wak- lence rate of 30% in a large sample of adults
abayashi and Sugiyama (cited in Kobayashi with mental retardation. Steffenburg, Gill-
et al., 1992) of the “ first generation” ( born be- berg, and Steffenburg (1996) reported a rate
fore 1960 and without access to special educa- of ASD of 38% in a sample of adolescents with
tion) and the “second generation” ( born 1960 both mental retardation and epilepsy.
to 1972 when special education was only incon- Even without autism, having an IQ < 50 in
sistently available). In the Eaves and Ho sample childhood is almost always associated with sig-
of 76 “ third-generation” youngsters, at adoles- nificant limitations and dependence in adult-
cence only 52% had performance IQs less than hood (i.e., inability to earn a living, need for
70, and 62% had verbal IQs below 70. supervised residential situation and special-
Similarly, Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) pub- ized vocational or day programming). Outcome
lished follow-up results of an unselected is somewhat more variable for individuals with
group of 54 adolescents and 45 adults with IQs between 50 and 70, although adult status
autism who had initially been diagnosed dur- of only marginal social and economic self-
ing childhood by one clinician (Isabelle sufficiency is common (Baroff, 1999).
292 Development and Behavior

The combination of mental retardation and of a larger group of children with normal non-
autism is particularly powerful in affecting the verbal intelligence who had originally been
individual’s developmental status as an adult. studied at ages 4 to 9 years by Bartak, Rutter,
People with both autism and mental retarda- and Cox (1975). These nine students had com-
tion have significantly poorer functioning in pleted the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
terms of education, work, living situation, and dren (WISC) as children and the Wechsler
general independence than those with autism Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) as
and average intelligence (Howlin & Goode, adults. Their mean verbal IQ increased from 66
1998; Howlin et al., 2004; Nordin & Gillberg, to 82 between early childhood and adulthood,
1998; Rutter, 1970). Coexisting autism at all while their performance IQ remained in the
levels of intelligence adds difficulties in terms broadly normal range (mean performance IQ
of comprehension and social use of language, decreased from 94 to 83). There was marked
understanding social cues and expectations, individual variability in verbal IQ scores, and
organizing behavior, and rigid attachment to comparisons of test scores were somewhat dif-
routines (Mesibov, Shea, & Schopler, in press; ficult to interpret because of different test in-
Van Bourgondien, Mesibov, & Castelloe, struments used at different ages (i.e., WISC
1989). Howlin et al. (2004) make the point that versus WAIS-R). Nevertheless, the results indi-
even for individuals without mental retarda- cated that the verbal intelligence of the group
tion, “Outcome tends to be very variable and it of autistic youngsters with normal-near normal
seems that the fundamental deficits associated nonverbal intelligence generally improved sub-
with autism, in particular the degree of ritual- stantially over the course of their adolescence.
istic and stereotyped behaviors may, at times, Similarly, Howlin et al. (2004), in an adult
‘swamp’ the effects of a relatively high IQ” outcome study of 68 children (ages 3 to 15
(p. 226). years) with autism and initial nonverbal IQ >
50, found that verbal IQ was quite stable for
Stability of IQ
youngsters who initially had verbal IQs > 70
In general, IQ scores from childhood to adoles- and that there were considerable increases
cence and adulthood of groups of people with among most (69%) of those with initial verbal
autism are stable (Lockyer & Rutter, 1969; IQs between 30 and 69. Further, even among the
Nordin & Gillberg, 1998). Most of the individ- 31 individuals who were untestable or had ver-
ual changes that do occur are in the direction bal IQs < 30 at initial assessment, as adults four
of improvement (e.g., Freeman et al., 1991) had verbal IQs of 50 to 69, and nine had verbal
with several exceptions: the incidents of signif- IQs above 70. Performance IQ in this study was
icant deterioration in adolescence discussed more stable, with most individuals staying in the
previously and cases in which individuals with same band (i.e., > 100, 70 to 99, or 50 to 69) or
good nonverbal intelligence fail to develop ver- changing by one band.
bal language (e.g., Howlin et al., 2004; Lord & Overall, recent research indicates that
Venter, 1992). while some individuals with autism make dra-
Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) reported that in matic gains or experience significant losses in
their heterogeneous sample of 99 children, two cognitive skills, typically, IQs are stable be-
individuals went from the range of severe men- tween childhood and adulthood.
tal retardation in childhood to the normal /near
normal range at follow-up as adolescents or Language
adults (all ranges were estimates based on
family report). Ten other individuals moved up Various research reports have described the
to the next higher range (i.e., from severe to types of language impairments associated with
mild/moderate retardation or from mild/mod- ASD in adolescence and adulthood.
erate to normal /near normal) while six individ-
Patterns of Speech and
uals moved down one range.
Language Characteristics
Mawhood, Howlin, and Rutter (2000) re-
ported the adult (ages 21 to 26 years) IQ scores Most adolescents and adults with autism
of nine individuals with autism who were part continue to demonstrate abnormalities in
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 293

speech and language (Baltaxe & Simmons, However, the Mawhood et al. (2000) study
1992; Howlin, 1997, 2003; Rumsey et al., of 19 adults who had average nonverbal IQs as
1985; Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Twachtman- children found significant variability in adult
Cullen, 1998), although the literature suggests language skills: Eight spoke in good sentences
overall improvements in language skills from (although half had difficulty sustaining con-
childhood to adulthood. versations), five had immature speech, and six
Kobayashi et al. (1992) reported that in were either mute, echolalic, or at the level of
their sample of 197 young adults with autism, single-word utterances.
16% communicated fluently with good vocab- Shriberg et al. (2001) analyzed the speech
ulary, 31% communicated with language that patterns of a group of 30 adolescents and
was unusual or inappropriate in some way, adults with either high-functioning autism or
32% understood some language but did not Asperger syndrome. Compared to a normal
communicate verbally, 9% used echolalia, and control group, these clinical groups had higher
12% did not vocalize meaningfully. rates of misplaced stress, excessive loudness,
Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) reported im- nasality, articulation errors, and repetitions of
provement from childhood to adulthood in sounds, syllables, or words, with additional in-
expressive and receptive language in most indi- dications of more high-pitched/falsetto voices
viduals with normal /near normal intelligence and slower speech. The authors concluded:
and similar improvement in some of those with
mild/moderate mental retardation. In their Although the obtained differences did not suggest
sample of adolescents and adults, 23% of those gross involvement, even infrequent voice and reso-
with normal /near normal intelligence were de- nance differences can affect listeners’ perceptions
of a speaker’s emotional status and attractiveness.
scribed by their families as having essentially
These findings are consistent with the percept of a
normal expressive language. “pedantic” style of speech often attributed to indi-
Seltzer et al. (2003) described the results of viduals with HFA [high-functioning autism] and
ADI-R interviews with 405 families of adoles- AS [Asperger syndrome]. (p. 1110)
cents and adults as indicating: “ There was a
general pattern of abatement of symptoms, re- Lord (1996) reported that a group of 20
flecting improved overall use of language, im- high-functioning adolescents was developmen-
proved ability to communicate nonverbally, and tally delayed in their use of “mental” verbs
reduced stereotyped, repetitive, or idiosyn- (e.g., think, wonder) and that both higher and
cratic speech” (p. 571). In particular, they re- lower functioning adolescents used an in-
ported that “nearly two-thirds (60.2%) of those creased number of unusual words or phrases
who were not able to speak in three-word compared to typical children.
phrases on a daily basis at age 4–5 years [were]
currently able to do so” (p. 575); in the overall Outcome Measures
sample, 70% currently demonstrated this level The lack of meaningful, spontaneous speech by
of language skill. age 5 years has historically been associated
Similarly, according to Venter et al. (1992) with poor adult outcome (Eisenberg, 1956;
in their study of 58 individuals who had nonver- Gillberg & Steffenberg, 1987; Lotter, 1974;
bal IQs of at least 60 as children, by the age of 5 Rutter, 1970). There are numerous reports of
years, only 39 of 58 had useful speech (defined individuals who began speaking after this age
as an age equivalent of 13 months on the (e.g., Ballaban-Gil et al., 1996; DeMyer et al.,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [PPVT] and 1973; Howlin, 2003; Howlin et al., 2004;
expressive use of at least five words used daily Nordin & Gillberg, 1998; Rumsey et al., 1985;
for communication); but at follow-up an aver- Rutter, Greenfeld, & Lockyer, 1967; Venter
age of 8 years later (mean age 14.69 years), all et al., 1992; Windsor, Doyle, & Siegel, 1994).
of the subjects had useful speech, 54 of 58 However, according to Lord and Bailey (2002):
(93.1%) obtained a basal (approximately 2-year
level) on the PPVT, and 52 of 58 (89.6%) could A child who does not have fluent speech by age 5
complete the verbal portion of a Wechsler test years may still make significant gains, but the later
(Lord & Venter, 1992). these gains come, the less likely the child’s language
294 Development and Behavior

will be f lexible and complex, and the more likely and 38.09 (Socialization). Similarly, Howlin
language delays of some sort may reduce his or her et al. (2000) reported that their group of adults
level of independence. (p. 639) (ages 21 to 26 years) had a mean verbal IQ of 82
but mean Vineland standard scores of 51.11
In the Mawhood et al. (2000) study, it is in- (Communication), 65.1 (Daily Living Skills),
teresting that the best predictor of the adults’ and 46.4 (Socialization).
composite language score (which included ex- Similarly, the group of 20 adolescents (ages
pressive grammar, receptive understanding of 11 to 19 years) studied by Green, Gilchrist,
complex instructions, and conversational abili- Burton, and Cox (2000) had a mean IQ of 92,
ties) was their childhood score on the PPVT (a but only 50% were independent in the most
test of receptive vocabulary only). Scores on basic of self-care skills (i.e., bathing and brush-
this test were also a significant predictor of ing teeth). These authors further reported:
social relationships in adulthood in this sample
(Howlin et al., 2000). Similarly, Venter et al. None of these normally intelligent young adults
(1992) found that adding PPVT scores to a re- were considered by their parents capable of pur-
gression analysis significantly increased the chasing major items or engaging in leisure activi-
power of early childhood measures to predict ties independently outside the home. Only a
adolescent adaptive behavior scores. However, handful were able to travel at all independently,
make any decisions about self-care, or even use the
Gilchrist et al. (2001) found that early lan-
telephone. (p. 290)
guage milestones and abnormalities were not
related to adolescent social functioning in a
This phenomenon of the discrepancy between
sample of high-functioning youngsters. Simi-
cognitive skills and daily functioning was also
larly, Mayes and Calhoun (2001), in a study of
described by Siegel:
a slightly younger sample with average cogni-
tive skills, reported that early delays in speech Typically these young people do know how to make
and language versus timely attainment of lan- their bed, do laundry, microwave a pizza, and brush
guage milestones did not differentiate groups their teeth adequately. Left on their own, however,
of children in terms of later clinical status in many never make their bed, wear only disheveled
the areas of autistic symptoms, expressive lan- clothes, eat mainly lots of junk food, and have
guage skills, or cognitive skills. chronically bad breath. (Siegel, 1996, p. 296)
In summary, the research literature indi-
cates that language skills generally improve Several authors have stressed the clinical
markedly from childhood to adulthood in importance of assessing the adaptive behavior
groups of people with autism (although not skills of individuals with autism and normal
necessarily in all individuals), but impair- IQs, because these individuals may be denied
ments remain, even in individuals with aver- various public services on the grounds that they
age intelligence. are not mentally retarded. Assessment of adap-
tive functioning is important for all individuals
with autism, but documenting significantly im-
Adaptive Behavior
paired adaptive functioning is particularly cru-
A robust finding in the research literature on cial for supporting more cognitively capable
individuals with autism is that adaptive behav- adolescents and adults with autism in obtaining
ior is usually markedly lower than intelligence, needed services, such as special educational ac-
particularly among those with higher intelli- commodations, residential supports, vocational
gence (Bölte & Poustka, 2002; Bryson & training, and supplemental income (Bryson &
Smith, 1998; Carter et al., 1998; Freeman et al., Smith, 1998; Klin & Volkmar, 2000).
1991; Lockyer & Rutter, 1969; Rumsey et al.,
1985). For example, Venter et al. (1992) re- Academic Achievement and
ported that at follow-up (ages 10 to 37 years), Higher Education
their sample had a mean Full Scale IQ of 79.21,
but mean Vineland standard scores of 47.57 According to Venter et al. (1992), the academic
(Communication), 49.05 (Daily Living Skills), achievement of groups of youngsters with
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 295

autism has increased over the years, probably Psychiatric and Emotional Problems
because of greater access to educational oppor-
tunities and services. However, their study Bryson and Smith (1998) reported preliminary
and several others have also found that scores results from a population-based study of ado-
on academic achievement tests often reflect lescents and young adults (ages 14 to 20 years)
functional deficits relative to intelligence. For indicating that “at least 40% of individuals with
example, in their sample many of the adoles- ADI-R defined autism experienced psychiatric
cents and adults with IQs of 90 or above ob- ‘episodes’ ” (p. 100) with mood disorders being
tained academic achievement scores below particularly common. Consistent with this find-
age level on all of the achievement subtests ing, a number of researchers have reported
except reading/decoding (i.e., they showed that depression is the most common psychiatric
deficits in reading/comprehension, spelling, condition in clinical samples of adults with
and basic computation skills). autism spectrum disorders (Ghaziuddin, Ghazi-
Similarly, Mawhood et al. (2000) reported uddin, & Greden, 2002; Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-
mean academic achievement age equivalents Mikhail, & Ghaziuddin, 1998; Howlin, 2000).
for their sample of adults with a mean IQ of Although depression among people with autism
82 as follows: reading accuracy, 12.17 years; is often thought to be a reaction to stresses and
reading comprehension, 10.64 years; mean social isolation, there have also been several re-
spelling, 10.82 years. Further, in the Howlin ports of increased incidence of depression
et al. (2004) sample of adults, the subgroup of among mothers of children with autism prior to
individuals with average intelligence and the births of these children, suggesting a genetic
“ very good” outcome obtained the following loading for depressive illness in people with
age equivalents: reading accuracy, 12.2 years; autism (Bolton, Pickles, Murphy, & Rutter,
reading comprehension, 10.5 years; spelling, 1998; Piven & Palmer, 1999; Piven et al., 1991;
13.1 years. Smalley, McCracken, & Tanguay, 1995). Anxi-
Minshew, Goldstein, Taylor, and Siegel ety disorders (including obsessive-compulsive
(1994) reported that a sample of 54 adult men disorder) are also frequently reported in sam-
with autism did not differ from a control group ples of adolescents and adults with autism
on tests of “mechanical” academic skills such (Green et al., 2000; Rumsey et al., 1985;
as word attack, spelling, and computation, Seltzer et al., 2001; Szatmari et al., 1989).
but did show significant deficits in reading In terms of more severe mental illness, most
comprehension. Similarly, Minshew, Gold- researchers have concluded that there is no
stein, and Siegel (1997) found significant dif- strong evidence for increased risk for schizo-
ferences from carefully matched controls in a phrenia among adolescents and adults with
group of 33 adolescents and adults with autism autism (Ghaziuddin et al., 2002; Howlin &
(mean age 21 years) on several measures of Goode, 1998), but there have been some re-
reading comprehension. ports of individuals with both conditions, as
Although historically many adults with well as reports of isolated paranoid or delu-
autism have not developed functional reading sional thinking or auditory hallucinations
and mathematics skills above the elementary (Clarke, Baxter, Perry, & Prasher, 1999; Howlin,
school level, there are also numerous reports 2000, 2003; Rumsey et al., 1985; Szatmari et al.,
that a small percentage of individuals with 1989; Wing, 1981). Bipolar disorder (manic-
autism have attended and graduated from depression) has also been reported (Clarke
college and graduate schools (Green et al., et al., 1999; Howlin, 2000; Tantam, 2000).
2000; Howlin, 2003; Howlin et al., 2004;
Kanner, 1973; Rumsey et al., 1985; Szatmari Epilepsy
et al., 1989; Tantam, 1991; Venter et al., 1992).
Individuals with autism spectrum disorders and Reports of the prevalence of epilepsy vary
advanced degrees include Temple Grandin, (Shavelle, Strauss, & Pickett, 2001; Tuchman,
PhD (Grandin, 1995); Terese Jolliffe, PhD (Jol- 2000), but review articles generally indicate
liffe, Lansdown, & Robinson, 1992); and Liane a rate of 20% to 33% (Bryson & Smith, 1998;
Holliday Willey, PhD (Willey, 1999). Nordin & Gillberg, 1998; Rapin, 1997).
296 Development and Behavior

Epilepsy occurs at all levels of intelligence but emigrated, 1 had disappeared, and 12 had died.
is found most frequently in association with This number of deaths, although small, repre-
mental retardation (Rutter, 1970; Volkmar & sented a significantly higher death rate within
Nelson, 1990; Wolf & Goldberg, 1986) or the group with PDDs than the expected mortal-
marked developmental regression (Gillberg & ity rate in the general population matched for
Steffenburg, 1987; Kobayashi & Murata, 1998; age, gender, and length of follow-up. Of the 12
Rutter et al., 1967). deaths, five were due to physical disease, one
There seems to be a bimodal distribution of was suspected to be related to a seizure, four
age of onset of seizures: before 5 years or were accidents (three suspected of being re-
during early adolescence (Howlin, 2000; Rutter, lated to a seizure), and two were suicides. Only
1970; Tuchman, 2000; Volkmar & Nelson, one of the deaths was in the group with mild
1990). Kobayashi et al. (1992) reported that 36 mental retardation; five of the individuals had
of 188 (19.1%) of their sample of 99 children moderate to severe mental retardation, and six
with autism (of varying IQ levels) developed had IQs at or above 84.
epilepsy, with onset for 17 of these 36 (47%) be- Shavelle et al. (2001) analyzed the causes of
tween 11 and 14 years. Giovanardi Rossi, Posar, death over a 14-year period (1983 to 1997) of
and Parmeggiani (2000) reported that for a all ambulatory individuals with autism in
group of 27 adolescents and young adults with the California state database (202 deaths
epilepsy or EEG paroxysmal abnormalities among 13,111 individuals). Results indicated
without seizures, onset for 66.7% was after age that the mortality rate for individuals with
12 years. The MIND Institute study (Byrd, autism, particularly for females and for people
2002) indicated a prevalence rate of epilepsy of with moderate, severe, or profound mental re-
9.8% among individuals with autism born be- tardation, was elevated compared to that of the
tween 1993 and 1995 (ages approximately 7 to 9 general population. Seizures, suffocation, and
years at the time of the study) but 14.8% among drowning were the causes of death that most
individuals born between 1983 and 1985 (ap- accounted for this difference at all levels of in-
proximately 17 to 19 years of age); although this telligence. Similarly, Patja, Iivanainen, Vesala,
was a cross-sectional study, this finding could Oksanen, and Ruoppila (2000) reported that in
be interpreted to reflect additional diagnoses of Finland epileptic seizures were a significant
epilepsy during the late childhood to adolescent risk factor for decreased life expectancy, and
period. Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) commented on the
high number of reported incidents of drowning
Mortality among children with autism.
Causes of death reported in other follow-up
Autism itself is not a degenerative disorder studies of children with autism into adoles-
and, in fact, is typically characterized by de- cence or adulthood include 1 of a sample of 23
velopmental progression, but associated med- of a heart condition (Gillberg & Steffenberg,
ical conditions (particularly epilepsy) and/or 1987); 1 of a sample of 32 from pneumonia
accidents related to significant mental retarda- subsequent to a diagnosis of “diffuse degenera-
tion have been known to contribute to some tive process” (Lotter, 1974); 1 of a sample
early deaths. (Note that since autism was first of 79 from status epilepticus (Howlin et al.,
described in the mid-1940s, most individuals 2004); 2 of a sample of 64 during seizures
diagnosed with autism as children are only (Rutter, 1970); 2 of a sample of 96, 1 unex-
now moving past middle age.) pectedly (with a history of seizures) and 1 by
Several studies have looked at the issues of being hit by a truck (Kanner, 1973); 3 of a
rates and causes of death through middle age sample of 106 from aspiration pneumonia,
among people with autism. Isager, Mouridsen, drowning, and “complications of chronic ad-
and Rich (1999) reviewed the death records as ministration of psychotropic medications”
of late 1993 of essentially all children with (Ballaban-Gil et al., 1996, p. 219); and 4 of a
PDDs in Denmark who were born between sample of 201 from suspected encephalopathy,
1945 and 1980. From a total group of 341 indi- self-inflicted head injury, nephrotic syndrome,
viduals, 324 were still living in Denmark, 4 had and asthma (Kobayashi et al., 1992).
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 297

The reports of Patja et al. (2000) and This suggested that many of these adults with
Shavelle et al. (2001) suggest that individuals autism were in residential placements because
with autism and mental retardation generally they were difficult for their parents to manage
have fewer of the risk factors associated with or live with.
typical adult lifestyles, such as smoking, For adults with both autism and mental re-
drinking alcohol, traffic and occupational ac- tardation, the challenges of living completely
cidents, and suicide. Hardan and Sahl (1999) independently are unlikely to be met (Nordin &
reported that suicidal ideation and behaviors Gillberg, 1998; Howlin et al., 2004). Household
were found in a handful of children and adoles- maintenance, money management, time man-
cents with mild-moderate mental retardation agement, and social self-protection are com-
and autism or PDD-NOS, and suicidal thoughts plex tasks that less capable adults with autism
or behavior in high-functioning adults has also simply don’t have the skills to manage success-
been reported by Wing (1981) and Tantam fully. If parents or other family members are
(1991). not available to provide the needed support,
In spite of the statistically increased death guidance, and supervision, these must be pro-
rate in groups of individuals with autism spec- vided from another, typically public, source.
trum disorders (with or without mental retarda- The specific forms these services take vary
tion), it is clear that the vast majority of people based on a number of factors, including fi-
with autism live at least through middle age and nances, philosophical trends in public service
almost certainly beyond. Therefore, many peo- delivery (Harris et al., 1998), and personal
ple with autism will outlive their parents, choice. The current trend toward “self-deter-
which has tremendous implications for living mination” (i.e., public financial support for
arrangements and other service needs for adults services chosen or designed by individual con-
with autism. sumers; www.self-determination.com) may
sometimes result in situations of mutually
Living Arrangements chosen roommates, if all the variables of com-
patible individuals, finances, real estate, avail-
In the general American culture, at the end of ability of other services, and transportation
adolescence most individuals leave their fam- fall into place (and stay in place). However, for
ily’s home and either live independently or the most part, adults with both autism and
live with other young people, sometimes in mental retardation either remain at home with
personal relationships and sometimes as mutu- their families or live in group care settings
ally chosen or temporarily assigned room- arranged by others. Ideally, service providers
mates. The living situations of adults with in these settings have specialized knowledge
autism, however, are typically quite different: of autism, in addition to knowledge of mental
Independent living or living with peers is rare. retardation (Persson, 2000; Van Bourgondien,
Seltzer et al. (2001) reported that direct in- Reichle, & Schopler, 2003).
quiries of the state Mental Retardation / Devel- Even adults with autism and average intelli-
opmental Disabilities agencies in New York gence have difficulty with independent living.
and Massachusetts yielded the following infor- Howlin and Goode (1998) reviewed the living
mation: In New York in 1998, 54% of agency arrangements reported in several earlier stud-
clients with autism ages 20 to 29 still lived ies (Mawhood, 1995, as cited in Howlin &
with their parents, and 34% of those ages 30 to Goode, 1998; Rumsey et al., 1985; Szatmari
39 still did so. In Massachusetts in 1997, 42% et al., 1989; Venter et al., 1992). According to
of agency clients with autism ages 18 to 30 this review, among 66 adults (ages 18 to 39
lived with their parents, and 23% of those years) without mental retardation, only 11 in-
older than 30 still did so. Although these fig- dividuals were described as living indepen-
ures reflected significant dependence on par- dently (one of these with daily contact and
ents by adults with autism, the figures were advice from his mother and one in a “sheltered
low compared to adults of the same age with accommodation” [p. 220]); 17 were in some
only mental retardation (Seltzer et al., 2001), kind of supervised or supported residential
many of whom remain at home even longer. placement, and 38 individuals were still living
298 Development and Behavior

with their parents, although some of these in- office worker, page in the foreign language
dividuals were reportedly quite independent. section of a library, bus boy in a restaurant,
Similarly, Ballaban-Gil et al. (1996) re- truck loading supervisor, helper in a drug
ported that of 13 adults (ages 18 to 29 years) store” (Kanner et al., 1972, p. 28); music com-
without mental retardation, 1 was living inde- poser/former Navy meteorologist (Kanner,
pendently, 3 were in residential programs, and 1973); janitor, cab driver, library assistant,
9 were living with parents. In the Howlin et al. keypunch operator (Rumsey et al., 1985); li-
(2000) follow-up study of 19 nonretarded indi- brary worker, physics tutor, salesman (semi-
viduals with autism at ages 21 to 26 years, 3 managerial), factory worker (Szatmari et al.,
were living independently, 9 were in residen- 1989); astronomy professor, mathematician,
tial programs, 1 was in a hospital, and 6 were chemist, technologist, civil servant, musician,
living with their parents. expert in heraldry (Asperger, 1991); physio-
In the Howlin et al. (2004) study of 68 indi- therapist, bus conductor, automobile mechanic,
viduals (ages 21 to 48 years) who had childhood cook, office worker (Kobayashi et al., 1992);
performance IQs above 50, approximately 10% laboratory technician (Howlin et al., 2000); and
were living independently or with minimal “scientific officer [for an] oil company; electri-
supervision as adults. However, the authors in- cal work, cartographer, postal assistant, factory
dicated that the sheltered residential arrange- work, computing, accounts, fabric designer ”
ments for a number of individuals may have (p. 216, Howlin et al., 2004).
been a reflection of the limited availability of Several researchers have noted that factors
housing options or the limited wage-earning po- such as the local economy and the availability
tential of these individuals rather than a reflec- of specialized vocational training programs and
tion of their self-care and independent living supports significantly improve the vocational
skills. success of individuals with autism (Kobayashi
Venter et al. (1992) summarized the issue et al., 1992; Lord & Venter, 1992; Mawhood &
of living arrangements in their sample: Howlin, 1999). Specific supported employment
strategies and programs are described by
Because of increasingly graded options for residen- Mawhood and Howlin (1999); McClannahan,
tial services, these placements are changing almost MacDuff, and Krantz (2002); Keel, Mesibov,
every day, but . . . placements have been the result and Woods (1997); Smith, Belcher, and Juhrs
of a great deal of work by parents and professionals (1995); and Van Bourgondien and Chapman
to help the young adults with autism find the least (Mesibov, Shea, & Schopler, in press).
restricted environment in which they can cope. Smith et al. (1995) indicated that the
(p. 540)
stereotype that all people with autism require
jobs with rigid routines, in quiet environments,
Employment is not true: “Workers with autism have man-
aged to avoid jobs that emphasize their weak-
The ability to maintain full-time, independent nesses in language and social skills and find
employment is one of the defining criteria of employment under numerous job titles in a va-
“normal” adult functioning in our culture. Most riety of industries” (p. 285). Similarly, Keel
reports of the adult employment status of indi- et al. (1997) indicated: “Most people with
viduals with autism spectrum disorders find autism are able to handle a variety of tasks
that only a minority of these individuals meet within their jobs as long as there is a pre-
this criterion (Howlin & Goode, 1998). Never- dictable routine or [emphasis added] a schedule
theless, beginning with Kanner’s own patients, to follow and what is expected is clear to them
some adults with ASD have independently held at all times” (p. 6). Several authors stress that
full-time jobs. This is rare enough that many individualized support that facilitates commu-
studies list the specific jobs, which have in- nication and problem solving involving related
cluded computer operator, clerk (Rutter, 1970); aspects of life (e.g., transportation, social in-
“ bank teller, laboratory technician, duplicating terests, stress management, money manage-
machine operator, accountant, ‘blue collar job’ ment) is important for successful employment
at an agricultural research station, general of individuals with autism (Keel et al., 1997;
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 299

McClannahan et al., 2002; Mesibov, Shea, & www.autism-society.org); and (3) the book by
Schopler, in press). Debbaudt (2001).
Higher functioning individuals are particu-
Marriage larly vulnerable to being victims of crime or
exploitation if they spend time in the commu-
A small number of individuals with autism are nity with inadequate support or supervision.
reported to have married. These reports have Conversely, several reports have suggested an
come from several sources: follow-up studies of association between Asperger syndrome/ high-
children diagnosed with autism (Howlin, 2003; functioning autism and criminal behavior
Howlin et al., 2004; Kanner, 1973; Szatmari (Baron-Cohen, 1988; Kohn, Fahum, Ratzoni,
et al., 1989; Tantam, 1991); reports of parents & Apter, 1998/2002; Mawson, Grounds, &
who have been diagnosed as a result of their Tantam, 1985; Murrie et al., 2002; O’Brien,
children’s diagnostic evaluation (Ritvo, Mason- 2002; Scragg & Shah, 1994; Siponmaa, Kris-
Brothers, Freeman, & Pingree, 1988; Ritvo, tiansson, Jonson, Nydén, & Gillberg, 2001;
Ritvo, Freeman, & Mason-Brothers, 1994); au- Tantam, 1991), although this suggestion has
tobiographies (e.g., An Asperger Marriage, by also been disputed (Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaz-
Chris and Gisela Slater-Walker; Pretending to iuddin, 1991; Hall & Bernal, 1995; Howlin &
be Normal: Living with Asperger’s Syndrome, by Goode, 1998), and Murrie et al. (2002) have
Liane Willey); and other professional literature made the point that “ the majority of persons
(e.g., Aspergers in Love: Couple Relationships with Asperger’s syndrome are scrupulously
and Family Af fairs, by Maxine Aston). The law-abiding” (p. 66).
majority of adults with autism, however, are Nevertheless, Frith (1991) reported:
generally reported to have either limited
social contacts, relatively superficial relation- Autistic people, and particularly those of the As-
ships, or social interactions mainly in the con- perger type, have been involved in some difficult
text of community groups and organizations forensic cases. Sometimes their offenses are part of
their single-minded pursuit of a special interest,
(Ballaban-Gil et al., 1996; Howlin et al., 2000;
sometimes the result of a defensive panic-induced
Howlin et al., 2004; Rumsey et al., 1985). action and sometimes the consequence of a com-
plete lack of common sense. . . . Typically the As-
Autism and the Criminal Justice System perger individual, when apprehended, does not
seem to feel guilt, does not try to conceal or excuse
Unfortunately, people with many types of de- what he or she did, and may even describe details
velopmental disabilities, including autism, are with shocking openness. (p. 25)
more likely than typically developing individ-
uals to be victims of crime (Debbaudt, 2001; Similarly, in reviewing the literature, Howlin
National Research Council, 2001a) and/or po- (1997, 2000) suggested that run-ins with the
tentially criminal sexual or financial exploita- legal system were likely to be related to perse-
tion (e.g., Howlin et al., 2004; Murrie, Warren, verative interests or unusual thinking patterns
Kristiansson, & Dietz, 2002). Further, people on the part of individuals with Asperger syn-
with autism may have more difficulty than drome/ high-functioning autism, not malicious
others in interacting with the police, whether or unscrupulous motives. For example, several
as victims, witnesses, or suspects, because of “crimes” reportedly committed by individuals
their limited communication skills, unusual with high-functioning autism were related to
behaviors, or social misperception of situa- a fascination with washing machines, trains,
tions. Suggestions for increasing safety of both chemical reactions, fire, poisons, and other dan-
children and adults with autism are available gerous topics. Other crimes were apparently re-
from (1) materials from the South Carolina lated to heightened sensory sensitivity (e.g.,
Autism Society (www.scautism.org/protect aggression toward a crying baby), extreme dis-
.html); (2) a special section of the 2003, sec- tress when routines were interrupted ( leading to
ond edition, Advocate publication of the aggression toward the source of interruption),
Autism Society of America (scheduled to be limited understanding of social norms (particu-
available online in late 2004 for members at larly related to sexual behavior), and social
300 Development and Behavior

naiveté (such that the individuals with autism Green et al., 2000), independent self-help
were “set up” to commit illegal acts that they skills are often problematic. Sometimes these
didn’t fully understand). skills have not been mastered in childhood and
O’Brien (2002) reported on a comparison continue to cause difficulties in adolescence
of offenders with mental retardation (“intel- and beyond.
lectual disability”) either with or without In addition to the global delays associated
autism. The offenses committed by people with mental retardation, specific aspects of
with both autism and mental retardation, com- autism (e.g., sensory issues, difficulties with
pared to those of people with only mental re- sequencing, and limited awareness of social ex-
tardation, were less likely to involve drugs or pectations) can interfere with learning skills in
alcohol, were less likely to involve concrete the areas of dressing, eating, and bathing. Fur-
gain, and were likely to be committed during ther, some individuals with autism and mental
daylight hours, consistent with the relative so- retardation, even if they do not have toileting
cial naiveté associated with autism. accidents, have never completely mastered wip-
Overall, it appears that because of the social ing themselves after using the toilet. This issue
and communication difficulties of people with is particularly problematic to address in adoles-
autism, they are unfortunately at increased risk cence or adulthood, when privacy concerns and
for being victims of crime or being caught up caregivers’ modesty may interfere with direct
in law enforcement or legal situations beyond instruction in the bathroom, even though this is
their comprehension. Incidents of illegal be- likely to be necessary.
havior on the part of more cognitively ad- Further, there are increased needs in the
vanced individuals with autism have also been area of personal hygiene beginning in adoles-
reported. cence. With females, issues related to menstru-
ation can be difficult for families, teachers,
CLINICAL ISSUES and other caregivers to handle. For example,
sensory sensitivities and dislike of changes in
The clinical issues that confront adolescents routine may lead to resistance or refusal to use
and adults with autism vary based on both age sanitary napkins. Some girls and women with
and developmental level, particularly intelli- autism might not recognize that they have their
gence level. Individuals with both autism and periods or may not have the communication
mental retardation have many issues and chal- skills to inform their caregivers. Occasionally,
lenges in common irrespective of age, while some females with autism become fascinated
age and legal independence play a greater role with the sensory qualities of the menstrual
among those with autism and average intelli- blood. Further, learning to change sanitary
gence. napkins and dispose of the used ones requires
individualized and private instruction for many
Adolescents and Adults with Autism and adolescents. Finally, some girls and women
Mental Retardation may experience premenstrual distress that neg-
atively affects their mood and behavior, yet
Common challenges and clinical needs within they may not be able to communicate the nature
the population of individuals with both autism of their discomfort. Another aspect of skill de-
and mental retardation include the continued velopment that is important for adolescent fe-
development or refinement of self-help skills; males with autism is learning to put on and
issues related to sexuality; the need for ongo- wear a bra.
ing supervision and legal protection; the skills Adolescent boys must generally learn to
and supports needed to engage in meaningful, shave (or at least to tolerate being shaved),
productive work; and typical, but painful, life either with an electric razor (which has
events such as grief and loss. the advantage of being safe, but the disadvan-
tage of being noisy and vibrating) or with a
Self-Help Skills
different kind of razor. Both boys and girls
For more impaired individuals with autism at this age need to learn to put on and wear
(and even some high-functioning individuals; deodorant.
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 301

Teaching hygiene and self-help skills to ado- adults with autism by Konstantareas and Lun-
lescents or adults with autism and mental retar- sky (1997).
dation is typically best accomplished through Many individuals can learn, after frequent
the principles of: redirection, to go to a specific private place to
masturbate. Sometimes changing the individ-
• Individualizing goals and methods rather ual’s wardrobe (e.g., from sweatpants to jeans)
than relying on a standard curriculum is helpful. Also, keeping people engaged with
• Eliminating unpleasant sensory stimuli as other interesting, meaningful activities can re-
much as possible duce the frequency of masturbation in public.
• Breaking skills down into small steps Sex education for individuals with adequate re-
• Using objects or pictures in sequence to ceptive language typically includes informa-
communicate the sequence of steps tion about body parts, hygiene for genital
• Providing frequent practice areas, the concepts of privacy and degrees of
• Following the desired behavior with a clear relationships (i.e., stranger, staff or supervisor,
finish and enjoyable activity or object family member, friend, casual date, boyfriend
/girlfriend/going steady, fiancé/fiancée, hus-
Sexuality
band/wife), acceptable public and private
Van Bourgondien, Reichle, and Palmer (1997) behaviors for various types of relationships,
reviewed the limited literature on sexuality masturbation, sexual intercourse, and special
and autism and indicated that while some pro- topics based on the individual’s interests, expe-
fessionals have questioned the sexual interests riences, and questions (Koller, 2000; Melone &
and sex education needs of individuals with Lettick, 1983; Shea & Gordon, 1984).
autism, empirical research has revealed that
Guardianship
sexuality is, in fact, an important issue for
many of them. In the United States, parents automatically
The most typical sexual behavior among in- cease to be a child’s guardian when that
dividuals with autism, both with and without child reaches the legal age of adulthood, which
mental retardation, is masturbation (DeMyer, varies from state to state but is generally
1979; Haracopos & Pedersen, 1992, as cited in 18 years. At that point, children are legally
Van Bourgondien et al., 1997; Ousley & Mesi- independent unless guardianship has been
bov, 1991). Ruble and Dalrymple (1993) docu- arranged through the legal system. Parents of
mented parents’ concerns that many adolescents youngsters with significant mental retardation
and adults with significant mental retardation (with or without autism) may assume that con-
and autism touched their genitals or mastur- tinued guardianship is automatic, so the need
bated in public without knowing that this be- for legal proceedings is an important issue for
havior might disturb or offend others. Van professionals to help families anticipate. Many
Bourgondien et al. (1997) reported results of a states have various forms of guardianship,
survey of group home staff about the sexual be- including full guardianship of the individual
haviors of a group of 89 adults with mental re- and his or her assets, as well as more limited
tardation and autism who lived in the homes. guardianship involving only specified life de-
Caregivers reported that overt sexual behavior cisions (e.g., medical decisions but not where
was common, including masturbation (75% of the individual lives). It is important for fami-
men, 24% of women); masturbation with vari- lies to obtain competent legal advice in their
ous objects (26% of the men who masturbated, state before the child’s legal age of adulthood
but 0% of the women who masturbated); sexual in order to consider the most appropriate
arousal from visual stimulation, including arrangements to safeguard both the individ-
other people (17% of men, 18% of women); and ual’s well-being and his or her rights.
sexual behaviors involving or directed toward
Work
other people (35% of men, 29% of women).
Additional sexually related interests ( holding Typical Americans work at paying jobs for a
hands, hugging, kissing, petting, etc.) were re- variety of reasons: They need money to pay for
ported in a study of 15 late adolescents and shelter, food, clothes, and so on; they enjoy the
302 Development and Behavior

work itself; they like the social aspects of their • A visual cue for what to do when the work
jobs; and they like to keep themselves occu- is finished (e.g., a concrete reward, a sym-
pied with productive activity. For many adults bol of a favored activity, or a cue to go to
with both autism and mental retardation, some the break area)
of these sources of motivation are not relevant.
Because of their mental retardation, they These same principles are used in designing
may not understand the instrumental value of supported employment plans for more cogni-
money or the costs of daily life, and, in addi- tively capable workers, for example:
tion, they may realistically not be able to earn
enough money to pay for most of the things • Teaching employees to organize their work
they need. Also, because of their cognitive dis- materials (which can include file folders
abilities, the work they can do may not be in- for office-type work)
herently interesting. Further, because of their • Teaching employees to use written lists
autism, they may not enjoy the social aspects both for organizing their work and for
of jobs; in fact, social interactions at work checking off items as they are completed
might be experienced as stressful, albeit nec- • Teaching employees to use a written daily
essary, parts of their daytime life. However, or weekly schedule
with specialized supports, work can be made
meaningful, productive, and satisfying for Not all adults with autism and mental
many adults with autism (Mawhood & Howlin, retardation find typical sheltered or supported
1999; Smith, Belcher, & Juhrs, 1995). work environments enjoyable, however. The
The TEACCH supported employment pro- TEACCH program has developed the Carolina
gram (Keel et al., 1997; Mesibov, Shea, & Living Learning Center, set in a semirural loca-
Schopler, in press) uses the principles of Struc- tion near the university town of Chapel Hill
tured Teaching (Schopler, Mesibov, & Hearsey, (Mesibov, Shea, & Schopler, in press; Van
1995) in work settings. Specifically, individu- Bourgondien & Reichle, 1997). The setting pro-
alized visual strategies are used to answer four vides a wide range of work activities (including
essential questions: (1) What work am I sup- various aspects of gardening, facility mainte-
posed to do? (2) How much work am I supposed nance, and other outside work in addition to
to do? (3) How will I know that I am progress- structured work activities indoors) and commu-
ing toward being finished? (4) What happens nity recreation. The Autism Society of North
next? For employees with autism and signifi- Carolina has established the Creative Living
cant mental retardation, answering these ques- program, which combines music and art activi-
tions generally includes some of the following ties, community recreation, prevocational ac-
strategies: tivities, and volunteer jobs at local businesses
and agencies (e.g., animal shelters and public
• Organizing work supplies into some kind of libraries) with more traditional supported em-
containers ployment opportunities.
• Organizing these materials in a way that
Difficult Life Events
encourages the employee to work from left
to right or top to bottom Unfortunately, difficult life events such as ill-
• Providing visual information about the ness and death of loved ones, undergoing
work that is to be performed (e.g., a model medical procedures, being hurt or abused, and
of the finished product or a demonstration so on occur in the lives of individuals with
of how to manipulate the materials) autism. A series of picture books for adoles-
• Providing visual information that work is cents and adults about such life events can be
moving toward completion (e.g., the sight an important resource for families and coun-
of the container of materials becoming selors to help these individuals understand
emptier, a check-off picture list of the var- what is happening and perhaps ask questions or
ious steps of the task, or the sight of an express feelings and concerns. This series,
hourglass emptying or a timer moving to- Books Beyond Words, by Sheila Hollins and col-
ward 0) leagues, is available from www.rcpsych.ac.uk
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 303

/publications/ bbw/index.htm (Royal College genuinely satisfied with having only brief or
of Psychiatrists, 17 Belgrave Square, London focused contacts with other people.
SW1X 8PG). Therapeutic interventions at this age in-
clude:
Adolescents with Autism and
Average Intelligence • Explicitly teaching adolescents about social
and emotional topics and techniques for fit-
Adolescents with autism and average intelli- ting in better, if not completely, with the
gence face challenges similar to those of their customs of their social world
typical peers, namely learning to deal with in- • Teaching organizational strategies (if these
creased expectations at school and more com- have not already been mastered), such as
plex social issues. clarifying assignments, using checklists of
needed books and materials, structuring
School
homework time, following a checklist of
Adolescence presents more able adolescents chores, making a calendar of weekly plans,
with multiple challenges at school, since many and so on
capable adolescents spend at least part, or per- • Arranging, if needed, for modified assign-
haps all, of their school day in regular educa- ments, additional time on tests and projects,
tion classes in typical middle schools and high reduced handwriting demands, alternative
schools. However, although high-functioning methods of testing, and so on
adolescents often perform academically at or • Arranging time and encouragement for ac-
above grade level in certain subjects, the orga- tivities that are enjoyable and relaxing
nizational and social expectations in middle (even if they are solitary or somewhat atyp-
school and high school (e.g., keeping track of ical) to counteract the stresses of daily life
multiple assignments and long-term projects; at school
moving quickly between classes; avoiding vio- • Providing emotional support
lation of subtle social taboos) can be over- • Providing supervision by adults to prevent
whelming (Klin & Volkmar, 2000). Further, bullying and reduce teasing
experiences of being teased, bullied, or ostra-
cized, which are major sources of distress even Sometimes medication for anxiety and/or
for typical youngsters at this age, can be in- depression is also indicated.
tense and excruciatingly painful for some ado-
lescents with autism (Green et al., 2000). Adults with Autism and
Relationships with Peers Average Intelligence

Many intellectually capable adolescents with Clinical areas in which adults with autism and
autism are interested in having friends, but average intelligence may have special needs
are not skilled at developing or maintaining include going to college, keeping a job, and
friendships. The nuances of socially accept- managing issues associated with adult rela-
able dress, speech, mannerisms, topics of con- tionships and sexuality.
versation, and typical adolescent viewpoints
College
on the world (e.g., “Rules are meant to be bro-
ken”) are typically difficult for adolescents As noted previously, it is clear from the re-
with autism to understand. Even those who search and clinical literature that some adults
want to have friends are often so unsuccessful with autism can be successful at college and in
at their daily social interactions that many some advanced graduate programs (typically in
eventually retreat to their rooms, their comput- fields such as science and research, engineer-
ers, their televisions, and their special inter- ing, computer technology, library/information
ests. Sometimes, however, it appears that science, and accounting). Again, the organiza-
distress over limited social relationships is tional and social aspects of college and gradu-
more of an issue for families than for the ado- ate school are almost always more problematic
lescents with autism themselves, who might be than the academic content. Williams and
304 Development and Behavior

Palmer (“Preparing for College: Tips for Stu- • Providing information about where to find
dents with HFA /Asperger’s Syndrome,” avail- help or guidance on a daily basis (and a
able at http://www.teacch.com /teacch_e.htm), fallback plan in case the regular supervisor
described the following strategies: is absent)
• Supplying employers with information
• Preparing in advance for daily life on cam- about autism, the specific client, and a con-
pus (e.g., living in a dorm, food service op- tact person from the supported employment
tions, doing laundry, sharing a bathroom, program
using the library, time management)
Sexuality
• Having a single room
• Learning to use “ to-do” lists and a small Individuals with autism and average intelli-
appointment book for time management gence are likely to be involved in community-
• Identifying helpful advisors about social based social situations with the potential to
and logistical issues lead to sexual opportunities and experiences.
• Learning to ask for help (including support However, given the subtle and frequently un-
from the Disability Services office) spoken communications and expectations in
• Learning to identify stressors and coping social /sexual situations, difficulties with un-
mechanisms derstanding these situations and knowing how
• Choosing classes that provide a balanced to behave are not surprising. According to
and manageable workload Howlin (1997), problems related to sexuality
• Creating a social life around special inter- in individuals with autism and average intelli-
ests gence generally involve the social, rather than
• Arranging time for exercise as a stress the physical, aspects of sexuality. Social /sex-
management tool ual problems outlined by Howlin can include
an intense determination to date and marry in
Work
order to “ be like everyone else” (p. 245), yet
Even adults with autism who have average in- limited social understanding and skills neces-
telligence have historically had limited voca- sary for establishing and maintaining intimate
tional success without specialized supports partnerships; “obsessions and infatuations”
(Howlin & Goode, 1998; Lord & Bailey, 2002; (p. 247) with desired partners which may not
Nordin & Gillberg, 1998). Adults with autism be reciprocated; simple, naïve understanding
who can master the intellectual aspects of of sexual feelings; and vulnerability to sexual
jobs in competitive employment may have sig- exploitation. Occasionally, there are also prob-
nificant difficulties with the organizational lems involving behaviors that are interpreted
aspects of their jobs (e.g., time management, by others as sexual, even though they were not
accepting changes in routine, and keeping intended in this way (e.g., being inadequately
work materials organized) and with the social dressed in public or touching other people with
aspects (e.g., knowing how and when to chat only friendly or helpful intentions, such as
or joke with coworkers, what to do during picking lint off their shirts). Further, the
breaks, and how to ask for help as needed). In combination of social-communication differ-
addition to the Structured Teaching strategies ences and sensory sensitivities may make sex-
discussed earlier, some of the techniques used ual expression within marriage less frequent
with higher functioning adults with autism in and/or less “romantic” than spouses might
the TEACCH Supported Employment pro- wish (Aston, 2002). Very clear, explicit com-
gram include: munication about socially desirable, effective
behaviors and the perspectives of others may
• Explaining social expectations in written be needed to help more capable adults with
form autism find ways to meet their needs for
• Providing supportive counseling sessions social, physical, and sexual contact with oth-
reinforced with written summaries of plans ers, while remaining safe from exploitation
and suggestions and abuse.
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 305

CONCLUSION Clinical Web Sites

The developmental course and adult outcome of www.aspergersyndrome.org


autism spectrum disorders can essentially be www.TheGrayCenter.org
viewed in two ways. From the perspective of www.tonyattwood.com
normal development and the independent adult-
www.teacch.com
hood that parents wish for their children,
autism is a serious disability that usually does Clinical Books
not permit those results. On the other hand,
compared to their severely atypical early devel- Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger syndrome: A guide
opment, over time, many children with autism for parents and professionals. Bristol, PA:
show improvements in skills and socially ac- Jessica Kingsley.
ceptable behavior, and most families adapt to Bashe, P., & Kirby, B. (2001). The OASIS guide to
the special needs of their offspring (Sanders & Asperger syndrome. New York: Crown.
Morgan, 1997). Bolick, T. (2001). Asperger syndrome and adoles-
cence: Helping preteens and teens get ready for
Adolescence can be a particularly challeng-
the real world. Gloucester, MA: Fair Winds
ing time for some individuals with autism Press.
spectrum disorders and their families, while Cumine, V., Leach, J., & Stevenson, G. (1998). As-
for others it is a time of increased skill devel- perger syndrome: A practical guide for teach-
opment and social awareness. Since the very ers. London: David Fulton.
first patients were identified by Kanner (1943) Debbaudt, D. (2001). Autism, advocates, and law
and Asperger (1991), a few individuals have enforcement professionals. Bristol, PA: Jessica
developed extremely well in terms of living in- Kingsley.
dependently, working, supporting themselves, Faherty, C. (2000). What does it mean to me?
and even marrying. Many more who have aver- Arlington, TX: Future Horizons.
Fullerton, A., Stratton, J., Coyne, P., & Gray, C.
age intelligence or mild mental retardation
(1996). Higher functioning adolescents and
have been able to master academic skills in
young adults with autism. Austin, TX: ProEd.
public schools, obtain and keep meaningful Hodgdon, L. (1999). Solving behavior problems in
jobs (through specialized supports), and enjoy autism. Troy, MI: QuirkRoberts.
social and family activities and relationships, Howlin, P. (1997). Autism: Preparing for adult-
even if these are somewhat idiosyncratic. It hood. New York: Rutledge.
is unfortunately true that some individuals Janzen, J. (2003). Understanding the nature of
with autism, particularly those with very sig- autism: A guide to the autism spectrum disor-
nificant mental retardation, limited functional ders. San Antonio, TX: Communication Skill
language, and/or intense interests or rigid be- Builders.
haviors remain very disabled. Adults with co- McAfee, J. (2001). Navigating the social world. Ar-
lington, TX: Future Horizons.
existing autism and mental retardation need
Mesibov, G. B., Adams, L. W., & Klinger, L. G.
both specialized residential services and spe- (1998). Autism: Understanding the disorder.
cialized supports for working or engaging New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
in other meaningful and satisfying activities Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Adams, L. W. (2001).
during the day. Both residential and voca- Understanding Asperger syndrome and high
tional /day program services require money, functioning autism. New York: Kluwer Aca-
trained staff, transportation, and a welcoming demic/ Plenum Press.
community. Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Schopler, E. (in press).
The TEACCH approach to autism spectrum
disorders. New York: Kluwer Academic/
Cross-References Plenum Press.
Ozonoff, S., Dawson, G., & McPartland, J. (2002).
Outcome in autism is discussed in Chapter 7; A parent’s guide to Asperger syndrome and
autism in young and school-age children is ad- high-functioning autism: How to meet the chal-
dressed in Chapters 8 and 9, respectively. Vo- lenges and help your child thrive. New York:
cational supports are discussed in Chapter 43. Guilford Press.
306 Development and Behavior

Powers, M., & Poland, J. (2002). Asperger syn- and without co-morbid mental retardation.
drome and your child. New York: Harper Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 33,
Academic. 165–172.
Smith, M. D., Belcher, R. G., & Juhrs, P. D. (1995). Bolton, P. F., Pickles, A., Murphy, M., & Rutter, M.
A guide to successful employment for individu- (1998). Autism, affective and other psychi-
als with autism. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. atric disorders: Patterns of familial aggrega-
tion. Psychological Medicine, 28, 385–395.
REFERENCES Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (1998). Epidemiology
of autism: Prevalence, associated characteris-
Ando, H., & Yoshimura, I. (1979). Effects of age tics, and implications for research and service
on communication skill levels and prevalence delivery. Mental Retardation and Developmen-
of maladaptive behaviors in autistic and men- tal Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 97–103.
tally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Byrd, R. (2002). Report to the Legislature on
Developmental Disorders, 9, 83–93. the principle findings from the epidemiology
Ando, H., Yoshimura, I., & Wakabayashi, S. of autism in California: A comprehensive
(1980). Effects of age on adaptive behavior pilot study. University of California Davis,
levels and academic skill levels in autistic and MIND Institute. Available from http://www
mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism .ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/ html /news
and Developmental Disorders, 10, 173–184. /autismreport.htm.
Asperger, H. (1991). “Autistic psychopathy” in Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Wang,
childhood. In U. Frith (Ed. & Trans.), Autism J.-J., Lord, C., Dawson, G., et al. (1998). The
and Asperger syndrome. New York: Cambridge Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supple-
University Press. (Original work published mentary norms for individuals with autism.
1944) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Aston, M. (2002). The other half of Asperger syn- ders, 28, 287–302.
drome: A guide to an intimate relationship with Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive
a partner who has Asperger syndrome. Shawnee developmental disorders in preschool children.
Mission, KS: Autism Asperger Publishing. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Ballaban-Gil, K., Rapin, I., Tuchman, R., & 285, 3093–3099.
Shinnar, S. (1996). Longitudinal examination Clarke, D., Baxter, M., Perry, D., & Prasher, V.
of the behavioral, language, and social changes (1999). The diagnosis of affective and psy-
in a population of adolescents and young chotic disorders in adults with autism: Seven
adults with autistic disorder. Pediatric Neurol- case reports. Autism, 3, 149–164.
ogy, 15, 217–223. Croen, L. A., Grether, J. K., Hoogstrate, J., &
Baltaxe, C. A. M., & Simmons, J. Q., III. (1992). Selvin, S. (2002a). The changing prevalence of
A comparison of language issues in high- autism in California. Journal of Autism and
functioning autism and related disorders with Developmental Disorders, 32, 207–215.
onset in childhood and adolescence. In E. Croen, L. A., Grether, J. K., Hoogstrate, J., &
Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), High func- Selvin, S. (2002b). Descriptive epidemiology
tioning individuals with autism (pp. 201–225). of autism in a California population: Who is at
New York: Plenum Press. risk? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Baroff, G. (1999). Mental retardation: Nature, Disorders, 32, 217–224.
cause, and management (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Debbaudt, D. (2001). Autism, advocates, and the
Brunner/ Mazel. law enforcement professionals: Recognizing
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). An assessment of violence and reducing risk situations for people with
in a young man with Asperger’s syndrome. autism spectrum disorders. London: Jessica
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Kingsley.
29, 351–360. DeMyer, M. K. (1979). Parents and children in
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A com- autism. Washington, DC: Winston and Sons.
parative study of infantile autism and specific DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Norton,
developmental receptive language disorder: I. J. A., Allen, J., & Steele, R. (1973). Prognosis
The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, in autism: A follow-up study. Journal of Autism
126, 127–145. and Childhood Schizophrenia, 3, 199–246.
Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2002). The relation be- Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (1996). Brief report: Sta-
tween general cognitive level and adaptive be- bility and change in cognitive and behavioral
havior domains in individuals with autism with characteristics of autism through childhood.
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 307

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures: And other
ders, 26, 557–569. reports from my life with autism. New York:
Eisenberg, L. (1956). The autistic child in adoles- Random House.
cence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1112, Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A.
607–612. (2000). Social and psychiatric functioning in
Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: adolescents with Asperger syndrome com-
A review. Psychological Medicine, 29, 769–786. pared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism
Fong, L., Wilgosh, L., & Sobsey, D. (1993). The ex- and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293.
perience of parenting an adolescent with Hall, I., & Bernal, J. (1995). Asperger’s syndrome
autism. International Journal of Disability, De- and violence. British Journal of Psychiatry,
velopment and Education, 40, 105–113. 166, 262.
Freeman, B. J., Rahbar, B., Ritvo, E. R., Bice, Hardan, A., & Sahl, R. (1999). Suicidal behavior in
T. L., Yokota, A., & Ritvo, R. (1991). The sta- children and adolescents with developmental
bility of cognitive and behavioral parameters disorders. Research in Developmental Disabili-
in autism: A twelve-year prospective study. ties, 20, 287–296.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Harris, S. L., Glasberg, B., & Delmolino, L.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 479–482. (1998). Families and the developmentally de-
Frith, U. (1991). Asperger and his syndrome. In U. layed adolescent. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M.
Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of psychological
(pp. 1–36). New York: Cambridge Press. treatment protocols for children and adoles-
Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. cents (pp. 519–548). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2002). Depression in persons with autism: Howlin, P. (1997). Autism: Preparing for adult-
Implications for research and clinical care. hood. London: Rutledge.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Howlin, P. (2000). Outcome in adult life for more
ders, 32, 299–306. able individuals with autism or Asperger syn-
Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., & Ghaziuddin, N. drome. Autism, 4, 63–83.
(1991). Brief report: Violence in Asperger Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning
syndrome: A critique. Journal of Autism and adults with autism with and without early lan-
Developmental Disorders, 21, 349–354. guage delays: Implications for the differentia-
Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmer-Mikhail, E., & Ghazi- tion between autism and Asperger syndrome.
uddin, N. (1998). Comorbidity of Asperger Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
syndrome: A preliminary report. Journal of ders, 33, 3–13.
Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 279–283. Howlin, P., & Goode, S. (1998). Outcome in adult
Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rut- life for people with autism and Asperger’s
ter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development syndrome. In F. R. Volkmar (Ed.), Autism and
and current functioning in adolescents with pervasive developmental disorders: Cambridge
Asperger syndrome: A comparative study. monographs in child and adolescent psychiatry
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (pp. 209–241). New York: Cambridge Univer-
and Allied Disciplines, 42, 227–240. sity Press.
Gillberg, C. (1991). Outcome in autism and autistic- Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M.
like conditions. Journal of the American Acad- (2004). Adult outcome for children with
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
375–382. chiatry, 45, 212–229.
Gillberg, C. (1998). Asperger syndrome and high- Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000).
functioning autism. British Journal of Psychia- Autism and developmental receptive language
try, 172, 200–209. disorder: A follow-up comparison in early
Gillberg, C., & Steffenberg, S. (1987). Outcome adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychi-
and prognostic factors in infantile autism and atric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology
similar conditions: A population based study and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41,
of 46 cases followed through puberty. Journal 561–578.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, Isager, T., Mouridsen, S. E., & Rich, B. (1999).
273–287. Mortality and causes of death in pervasive de-
Giovanardi Rossi, P., Posar, A., & Parmeggiani, A. velopmental disorders. Autism, 3, 7–16.
(2000). Epilepsy in adolescents and young Jolliffe, T., Lansdown, R., & Robinson, T. (1992).
adults with autistic disorder. Brain Develop- Autism: A personal account. Communication,
ment, 22, 102–106. 26, 12–19. (Available from the National
308 Development and Behavior

Autistic Society, 393 City Road, London Lord, C., & Venter, A. (1992). Outcome and fol-
EC1V 1NG, United Kingdom) low-up studies of high-functioning autistic in-
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- dividuals. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with
Kanner, L. (1973). Childhood psychosis: Initial autism (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum
studies and new insights. Washington, DC: Press.
Winston and Sons. Lotter, V. (1974). Factors related to outcome in
Kanner, L., Rodriguez, A., & Ashenden, B. (1972). autistic children. Journal of Autism and Child-
How far can autistic children go in matters of hood Schizophrenia, 4, 263–277.
social adaptation? Journal of Autism and Lotter, V. (1978). Follow-up studies. In M. Rutter
Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 9–33. & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of
Keel, J. H., Mesibov, G. B., & Woods, A. (1997). concepts and treatment (pp. 475–495). New
TEACCH-supported employment program. York: Plenum Press.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Lovaas, O. I. (2000). Experimental design and
ders, 27, 3–9. cumulative research in early behavioral inter-
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Treatment and vention. In P. J. Accardo, C. Magnusen, & A. J.
intervention guidelines for individuals with Capute (Eds.), Autism: Clinical and research
Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, issues (pp. 133–161). Baltimore: York Press.
& S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome Marcus, L. M. (1984). Coping with burnout. In E.
(pp. 340–366). New York: Guilford Press. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), The ef fects
Kobayashi, R., & Murata, T. (1998). Setback phe- of autism on the family (pp. 311–326). New
nomenon in autism and long-term prognosis. York: Plenum Press.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 296–303. Mawhood, L., & Howlin, P. (1999). The outcome
Kobayashi, R., Murata, T., & Yoshinaga, K. (1992). of a supported employment scheme for high-
A follow-up study of 201 children with autism functioning adults with autism or Asperger
in Kyushu and Yamaguchi areas, Japan. Jour- syndrome. Autism, 3, 229–254.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000).
22, 395–411. Autism and developmental receptive language
Kohn, Y., Fahum, T., Ratzoni, G., & Apter, A. disorder: A comparative follow-up in early
(1998). Aggression and sexual offense in As- adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes.
perger’s syndrome. Israel Journal of Psychia- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
try and Related Sciences, 35, 293–299. and Allied Disciplines, 41, 547–559.
Abstract obtained from PsychINFO database, Mawson, D., Grounds, A., & Tantam, D. (1985). Vi-
1999-10154-006, 2002. olence and Asperger Syndrome: A case study.
Koller, R. (2000). Sexuality and adolescents with British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 566–569.
autism. Sexuality and Disability, 18, 125–135. Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2001). Non-
Konstantareas, M. M., & Lunsky, Y. J. (1997). So- significance of early speech delay in children
ciosexual knowledge, experience, attitudes, with autism and normal intelligence and impli-
and interests of individuals with autistic disor- cations for DSM-IV Asperger’s disorder.
der and developmental delay. Journal of Autism Autism, 5, 81–94.
and Developmental Disorders, 27, 397–413. McClannahan, L. E., MacDuff, G. S., & Krantz,
Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1969). A five- to fifteen- P. J. (2002). Behavior analysis and interven-
year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: III. tion for adults with autism. Behavior Modifi-
Psychological aspects. British Journal of Psy- cation, 26, 9–26.
chiatry, 115, 865–882. Melone, M. B., & Lettick, A. L. (1983). Sex educa-
Lord, C. (1996). Language in high-functioning tion at Benhaven. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesi-
adolescents with autism: Questions about de- bov (Eds.), Autism in adolescents and adults
viance and delay. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (pp. 169–186). New York: Plenum Press.
(Eds.), Adolescence: Opportunities and chal- Mesibov, G. B. (1983). Current issues in autism
lenges: Rochester symposium on developmen- and adolescence. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesi-
tal psychopathology (Vol. 7, pp. 149–165). bov (Eds.), Autism in adolescents and adults
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester (pp. 37–53). New York: Plenum Press.
Press. Mesibov, G. B., Schopler, E., Schaffer, B., &
Lord, C., & Bailey, A. (2002). Autism spectrum Michal, N. (1989). Use of the Childhood
disorders. In M. Rutter & E. Taylor (Eds.), Autism Rating Scale with autistic adolescents
Child and adolescent psychiatry (4th ed., and adults. Journal of the American Academy of
pp. 636–663). Malden, MA: Blackwell Science. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 538–541.
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 309

Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Schopler, E. (in press). Persson, B. (2000). Brief report: A longitudinal
The TEACCH approach to autism spectrum study of quality of life and independence
disorders. New York: Kluwer Academic/ among adult men with autism. Journal of
Plenum Press. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30,
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. 61–66.
(1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in Piven, J., Chase, G. A., Landa, R., Wzorek, M.,
autism: Profile of a complex information pro- Gayle, J., Cloud, D., et al. (1991). Psychiatric
cessing disorder. Journal of the International disorders in the parents of autistic individuals.
Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Taylor, H. G., & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 471–478.
Siegel, D. J. (1994). Academic achievement in Piven, J., Harper, J., Palmer, P., & Arndt, S.
high functioning autistic individuals. Journal (1996). Course of behavioral change in
of Clinical and Experimental Neurology, 16, autism: A retrospective study of high-IQ ado-
261–270. lescents and adults. Journal of the American
Morgan, C. N., Roy, M., Nasr, A., Chance, P., Hand, Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
M., Mlele, T., et al. (2002). A community sur- 35, 523–529.
vey establishing the prevalence rate of autistic Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1999). Psychiatric disorder
disorder in adults with learning disability. and the broad autism phenotype: Evidence
Psychiatric Bulletin, 26, 127–130. from a family study of multiple-incidence
Murrie, D. C., Warren, J. I., Kristiansson, M., & autism families. American Journal of Psychia-
Dietz, P. E. (2002). Asperger’s syndrome in try, 156, 557–563.
forensic settings. International Journal of Rapin, I. (1997). Autism. The New England Journal
Forensic Mental Health, 1, 59–70. of Medicine, 337, 97–104.
National Research Council. (2001a). Crime victims Ritvo, E. R., Mason-Brothers, A., Freeman,
with developmental disabilities: Report of a B. J., & Pingree, C. (1988). Eleven possibly
workshop. Committee on Law and Justice; autistic parents. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Joan Persilia, Joseph Foote, and Nancy A. opmental Disorders, 18, 139–143.
Crowell (Eds.), Commission on Behavioral and Ritvo, E. R., Ritvo, R., Freeman, B. J., & Mason-
Social Sciences and Education. Washington, Brothers, A. (1994). Clinical characteristics
DC: National Academy Press. of mild autism in adults. Comprehensive Psy-
National Research Council. (2001b). Educating chiatry, 35, 149–156.
children with autism. Committee on Educa- Ruble, L. A., & Dalrymple, N. J. (1993).
tional interventions for children with autism, Social /sexual awareness of persons with
Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences autism: A parental perspective. Archives of
and Education. Washington, DC: National Sexual Behavior, 22, 229–240.
Academy Press. Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R.
Nordin, V., & Gillberg, C. (1998). The long-term (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychi-
course of autistic disorders: Update on follow- atric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal
up studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
97, 99–108. 24, 465–473.
O’Brien, G. (2002). Dual diagnosis in offenders Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to
with intellectual disability: Setting research adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450.
priorities: A review of research findings con- Rutter, M., Greenfeld, D., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A
cerning psychiatric disorder (excluding per- five- to fifteen-year follow-up study of infan-
sonality disorder) among offenders with tile psychosis: II. Social and behavioural out-
intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual come. British Journal of Psychiatry, 113,
Disability Research, 46, 21–30. 1183–1199.
Ousley, O. Y., & Mesibov, G. B. (1991). Sexual at- Sanders, J. L., & Morgan, S. B. (1997). Family
titudes and knowledge of high-functioning stress and adjustment as perceived by parents
adolescents and adults with autism. Journal of of children with autism or Down syndrome:
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, Implications for intervention. Child and Fam-
471–481. ily Behavior Therapy, 19, 15–32.
Patja, K., Iivanainen, M., Vesala, H., Oksanen, H., Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Hearsey, K. (1995).
& Ruoppila, I. (2000). Life expectancy of Structured Teaching in the TEACCH system.
people with intellectual disability: A 35-year In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learn-
follow-up study. Journal of Intellectual Dis- ing and cognition in autism (pp. 243–268).
ability Research, 44, 591–599. New York: Plenum Press.
310 Development and Behavior

Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., DeVellis, R. F., & Abstract obtained from PsychINFO database,
Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classifica- 2003, Abstract No. 2000-00345-002.
tion of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R., Bond, S.,
Rating Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high-
Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103. functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism
Scragg, P., & Shah, A. (1994). Prevalence of As- and Developmental Disorders, 19, 213–225.
perger’s syndrome in a secure hospital. British Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). Understanding the lan-
Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 679–682. guage and communicative impairments in
Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Orsmond, G. I., & autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International
Vestal, C. (2001). Families of adolescents and review of research in mental retardation: Vol.
adults with autism: Uncharted territory. In 23. Autism (pp. 185–205). San Diego, CA:
L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of re- Academic Press.
search in mental retardation: Vol. 23. Autism Tager-Flusberg, H., Joseph, R., & Folstein, S.
(pp. 267–294). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. (2001). Current directions in research on
Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Shattuck, P. T., autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental
Orsmond, G., Swe, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The Disabilities Research Reviews, 7, 21–29.
symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger syndrome in adult-
adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Autism hood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger
and Developmental Disorders, 33, 565–581. syndrome (pp. 147–183). New York: Cam-
Shavelle, R. M., Strauss, D. J., & Pickett, J. (2001). bridge University Press.
Causes of death in autism. Journal of Autism Tantam, D. (2000). Psychological disorder in ado-
and Developmental Disorders, 31, 569–576. lescents and adults with Asperger syndrome.
Shea, V., & Gordon, B. N. (1984). Growing up: A Autism, 4, 47–62.
social and sexual education picture book Tuchman, R. (2000). Treatment of seizure disorders
for young people with mental retardation. and EEG abnormalities in children with autism
Chapel Hill, NC: University of North spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and De-
Carolina–Chapel Hill. (Available from the velopmental Disorders, 30, 485–489.
Autism Society of North Carolina, 505 Ober- Twachtman-Cullen, D. (1998). Language and com-
lin Road, Suite 230, Raleigh, NC 27605 or munication in high functioning autism and As-
www.autism-society.nc.org) perger syndrome. In E. Schopler, G. B. Mesibov,
Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., & L. J. Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high
Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (2001). Speech functioning autism? (pp. 199–225). New York:
and prosody characteristics of adolescents and Plenum Press.
adults with high-functioning autism and As- Van Bourgondien, M. E., Mesibov, G. B., & Castel-
perger syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, loe, P. (1989). Adaptation of clients with autism
and Hearing Research, 44, 1097–1115. to group home settings. Paper presented at the
Siegel, B. (1996). The world of the autistic child. National Conference of the Autism Society of
New York: Oxford University Press. America, Seattle, WA.
Siponmaa, L., Kristiansson, M., Jonson, C., Nydén, Van Bourgondien, M. E., & Reichle, N. C. (1997).
A., & Gillberg, C. (2001). Juvenile and young Residential treatment for individuals with
adults mentally disordered offenders: The role autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.),
of child neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of Handbook of autism and pervasive develop-
the American Academy of Psychiatry and the mental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 691–706). New
Law, 29, 420–426. Abstract obtained from York: Wiley.
PsychINFO database, 2003, Abstract No. Van Bourgondien, M. E., Reichle, N. C., & Palmer,
2001-09782-008. A. (1997). Sexual behavior in adults with
Smalley, S., McCracken, J., & Tanguay, P. (1995). autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Autism, affective disorders, and social phobia. Disorders, 27, 113–125.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 60, Van Bourgondien, M. E., Reichle, N. C., &
19–26. Schopler, E. (2003). Effects of a model treat-
Smith, M. D., Belcher, R. G., & Juhrs, P. D. (1995). ment approach on adult with autism. Journal of
A guide to successful employment for individu- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33,
als with autism. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. 131–140.
Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, U. Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1992). A
(1996). Psychiatric disorders in children and follow-up study of high-functioning autistic
adolescents with mental retardation and active children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 53, 904–912. chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, 489–507.
Adolescents and Adults with Autism 311

Volkmar, F. R., & Nelson, D. S. (1990). Seizure dinal case study of autism. Journal of Speech
disorders in autism. Journal of the American and Hearing Research, 37, 96–105. Abstract
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, obtained from PsychINFO database, 2003, Ab-
29, 127–129. stract No. 1994-31255-001.
Von Knorring, A. L., & Haeggloef, B. (1993). Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Autism in northern Sweden: A population- account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.
based follow up study: Psychopathology. Eu- Wing, L., & Shah, A. (2000). Catatonia in autistic
ropean Journal of Child and Adolescent spectrum disorders. British Journal of Psychi-
Psychiatry, 2, 91–97. Abstract obtained from atry, 176, 357–362.
PsychINFO database, 2003, Abstract No. Wolf, L., & Goldberg, B. (1986). Autistic children
1994-37828-001. grow up: An eight to twenty-four year follow-
Willey, L. H. (1999). Pretending to be normal. up study. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 31,
Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley. 550–556.
Windsor, J., Doyle, S. S., & Siegel, G. M. (1994).
Language acquisition after autism: A longitu-
CHAPTER 11

Social Development in Autism

ALICE S. CARTER, NAOMI ORNSTEIN DAVIS, AMI KLIN, AND FRED R. VOLKMAR

The syndrome of early infantile autism was agnostic and assessment instruments devel-
first described in 1943 by Leo Kanner. In his oped for autism typically emphasize social
remarkably enduring paper, he reported on 11 factors (Parks, 1984) as do current diagnostic
children who exhibited what Kanner thought criteria for the disorder (American Psychiatric
to be a congenital lack of interest in other peo- Association, 1994).
ple, or autism, from the Greek autos, meaning Daily social encounters with persons with
“self.” In contrast to the very limited interest autism illustrate the severity of the social
these children had in the social environment, deficit seen in autism. In addition, social in-
they often were highly interested in aspects of teraction with persons with autism who vary
the inanimate environment. For example, a in chronological age and developmental level
child might appear not to recognize his or her highlight the complex issues posed by develop-
parents but would become panicked if the fur- mental changes and syndrome heterogeneity. A
niture were rearranged. Kanner regarded the young autistic child may prefer to spend most
social dysfunction and the unusual responses of his or her time engaged in solitary activi-
to the environment to be the two essential fea- ties. He or she may fail to respond differen-
tures of the syndrome. Throughout the broad tially to a strange person and, particularly in
range of syndrome expression, it is remarkable very young and the most impaired individuals,
that now, 60 years later, the social disability of may have relatively little interest in social
persons with autism remains probably the interaction—even with his or her parents. In
most striking, and poorly understood, aspect contrast to normally developing infants, for
of the condition (Lord, 1993). whom the social environment is of greatest
Social deficits have been repeatedly de- interest, the younger autistic child may be ex-
scribed in persons with autism (e.g., Rimland, quisitely sensitive to the nonsocial environ-
1964; Rutter, 1978; Wing, 1976). Although ment and may become profoundly distressed in
some social skills emerge over time, even response to a change in the manner in which
adults with autism who are “ higher function- food is organized on his or her plate. The older
ing” have major problems in social relation- child with autism often exhibits evidence of
ships (Volkmar & Cohen, 1985). Subsequent specific attachments to parents and may pas-
work has modified Kanner’s original descrip- sively accept bids for social interaction. How-
tion in important ways, but social deviance has ever, rarely is social interaction initiated when
continued to be recognized as a significant there is no other nonsocial goal motivating the
phenomenological aspect of the syndrome. Di- social initiation. The highest functioning indi-

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
velopment (Grants 1-PO1-HD35482-01, 5 P01-HD042127-02) and the National Institute of Mental Health
(STAART grant U54-MH066594). The support of the National Alliance of Autism Research is gratefully
acknowledged.

312
Social Development in Autism 313

viduals with autism may be very interested in dysfunction as a diagnostic feature of autism,
social interaction, but their odd and eccentric specific aspects of social development in
social styles and limited capacities to under- which individuals with autism evidence im-
stand or anticipate others’ internal emotional pairments, and theoretical models for under-
states, intentions, and motivations make it standing autistic social dysfunction. To the
very difficult to negotiate the nuances of so- extent possible, research findings are pre-
cial interaction (Klin, Jones, Schultz, & Volk- sented within a developmental context. This
mar, 2003). approach helps to emphasize the distinctive-
Higher functioning persons with autism ness and complexity of the course of social de-
may evidence the following social deficits: (1) velopment in autism.
failure to establish a joint frame of reference
for the transaction (e.g., may begin a discussion SOCIAL DYSFUNCTION AS A
without providing the listener with adequate DIAGNOSTIC FEATURE OF AUTISM
background information), (2) failure to take so-
cial norms or the listener’s feelings into ac- While certain aspects of Kanner’s original de-
count (e.g., approaching an unfamiliar adult and scription proved to be false leads for research,
remarking, “You’re very fat ”), and (3) exclu- his phenomenological report of autism has
sive reliance on limited, conventional conversa- proven remarkably enduring. Several aspects
tional stratagems or stereotyped expressions as of his initial report deserve particular men-
the child elaborates some idiosyncratic interest tion. First, Kanner emphasized that social de-
or echoes a previous statement (e.g., “Do you viance and delay was a hallmark, if not the
know about prime numbers?”). Failures in the hallmark, of autism. He was careful to contrast
use of nonverbal cues for modulating social in- the social interest of children with autism with
teraction are common even in those individu- that of normally developing infants and em-
als who never speak; mute persons with autism phasized that it was the autistic social dys-
may fail to make appropriate use of eye con- function that was distinctive. This emphasis
tact, fail to respond to extralexical social sig- has been continuously reflected in the various
nals, and seem to avoid interaction. As social official and unofficial guidelines for the diag-
relationships develop, they typically lack the nosis of autism that have appeared since Kan-
richness and intimacy seen even in young nor- ner’s original report. The need for diagnostic
mally developing children. guidelines became more critical during the
Significant progress has been made in the late 1970s as the validity of autism as a diag-
past 20 years in understanding developmental nostic category became more clearly estab-
aspects of syndrome expression, understand- lished. Various attempts, both categorical and
ing the nature of the social dysfunction as re- dimensional, have been made to specify the
flected in specific developmental processes, nature of the social deficit.
and formulating broader theoretical views of Rutter (1978) emphasized that the unusual
autistic social dysfunction. With the advent of social development observed in autism was
improved instrumentation and shared diagnos- one of the essential features for definition,
tic tools, the field has moved beyond disagree- was distinctive, and was not just a function of
ments over fundamental aspects of definition associated mental retardation. Early epidemi-
and diagnosis, resolving some of the core ological studies, for example, Wing and Gould
methodological problems that impeded re- (1979), also highlighted some of the difficul-
search. Moreover, with enthusiasm for social ties in assessing social development relative to
neuroscience growing (Insel & Fernald, overall cognitive ability, particularly among
2004), autism research is no longer character- the more severely handicapped. By 1980,
ized by what was previously a widely held, al- there was general agreement on the need to in-
beit implicit, “cognitive primacy hypothesis” clude autism in official diagnostic systems
(Cairns, 1979). such as the American Psychiatric Associa-
This chapter selectively reviews the topic of tion’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
social development in autism. It is organized Mental Disorders, third edition (DSM-III;
around several broad areas of interest: social American Psychiatric Association, 1980). The
314 Development and Behavior

DSM-III definition of infantile autism labeled change of name of the disorder from infantile
the social deficit as “pervasive”; the use of autism to autistic disorder.
this term was really most appropriate for the Given the greater number and better speci-
youngest and most impaired children, that is, fication of criteria for social dysfunction, the
consistent with the name of the category. The DSM-III-R system had the major advantage
term residual autism was available for persons over previous DSM versions of suitability for
who had once exhibited the pervasive social statistical evaluation. For example, Siegel, Vu-
deficit but no longer did so. As a practical mat- kicevic, Elliott, and Kraemer (1989) reana-
ter, it was clear that some social skills did lyzed clinician ratings of DSM-III-R criteria
emerge over time, and imprecision regarding for autism by employing signal detection
the nature of the social deficit was clearly analysis, an approach designed to identify the
problematic (Volkmar, 1987a). most robust criterion or combination of crite-
In the subsequent revision of the DSM-III ria that can reliably be used for diagnosis.
(DSM-III-R, American Psychiatric Associa- Consistent with Kanner’s original impression
tion, 1987), the nature of the social deficit in and subsequent research, this analysis indi-
autism was defined with greater attention to cated that the social criteria were the most po-
developmental variation. Qualitative impair- tent predictors of diagnosis. Unfortunately,
ment in social interaction was retained as one however, the broader orientation of DSM-III-R
of three essential diagnostic features for autis- also led to a change in the threshold of diagno-
tic disorder (in addition to impairments in sis, with many individuals who previously
communication and a restricted range of inter- would not have met criteria for infantile autism
ests/activities). Within the social domain, an classified with a diagnosis of autistic disorder.
individual had to exhibit at least two items Major revisions were made in DSM-IV (Ameri-
from a list of five criteria to demonstrate a so- can Psychiatric Association, 1994), which, in
cial deficit (see Table 11.1). The DSM-III-R the end, paralleled the major changes in the In-
criteria also included many examples to clarify ternational Classification of Diseases (ICD-
the nature of the social deficits that were de- 10; World Health Organization [WHO], 1993,
scribed. The DSM-III-R approach to the defini- see Chapter 1). In both DSM-IV and ICD-10
tion of the social dysfunction in autism qualitative impairment in social interaction
reflected an awareness of the developmental has been maintained as one of the essential
changes in syndrome expression and the recog- diagnostic features (see Table 11.1). The
nition that the social skills that did emerge over DSM-IV revision included a reduction in the
time were unusual in quality and/or quantity. number of criteria and the detail of these cri-
This change was also reflected in the official teria. Problems in at least two of the four

TABLE 11.1 Evolution of the Definition of Social Dysfunction in Autism

Rutter (1978) Social delays/deviance but not just secondary to mental retardation
DSM-III (APA, 1980) Pervasive social problems
DSM-III-R (APA, 1987) Qualitative impairment in social interaction:
at least two of the following:
1. Lack of awareness of others
2. Absent /abnormal comfort seeking
3. Absent /impaired imitation
4. Absent /abnormal social play
5. Gross deficits in ability to make peer friendships
DSM-IV (APA, 1994) Qualitative impairment in social interaction:
ICD-10 (WHO, 1993) at least two of the following:
1. Marked deficits in nonverbal behaviors used in social interaction
2. Absent peer relations relative to developmental level
3. Lack of shared enjoyment /pleasure
4. Problems in social-emotional reciprocity
Social Development in Autism 315

areas listed in Table 11.1 are required for the via parent report. Although developed to ad-
autistic social dysfunction to be considered dress diagnostic caseness, attempts have been
present. These same guidelines remain in place made to determine whether ADI scores might
today and, with the exception of work to ex- also be used as continuous phenotypes (e.g.,
tend understanding of the social dysfunction Spiker, Lotspeich, Dimiceli, Myers, & Risch,
and other aspects of the diagnosis to younger 2002). Observational instruments to assess di-
ages (Charman & Baird, 2002), have not been mensional aspects of social-communication,
changed. The growing body of work on autism such as the Early Social-Communication Scales
as it appears in infancy may have important (ESCS; Mundy, Hogan, & Doehring, 1994),
implications for development of age-specific have been extremely valuable in developing a
criteria in the future (see Chapter 8, this more thorough understanding of the early emer-
Handbook, this volume). gence of social-communication deficits in
In addition to categorical approaches to the autism (see Mundy & Burnette, Chapter 25,
definition of social dysfunction, various rating this Handbook, this volume). A widely avail-
scales, interviews, and checklists have been able, norm-referenced test, the Vineland
used to provide dimensional definitions (e.g., Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla, &
Constantino et al., 2003). Dimensional defini- Cicchetti, 1984), has also been used to provide
tions are particularly important in the area of a metric for social dysfunction in autism. The
social dysfunction where, in contrast to cogni- Vineland is a semistructured parent interview
tive or language ability, well-developed, norm- that assesses day-to-day adaptive functioning
referenced tests of ability have not generally in the areas of communication, daily living,
been available. Some instruments, such as the and socialization. Volkmar and colleagues
Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI; Lord, (1987) reported that, relative to overall cogni-
Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994), assess the individ- tive abilities, children with autism exhibited
ual’s typical pattern of social engagement as much lower than expected social skills in com-
well as highly unusual social features to deter- parison to a mental age-matched group (see
mine whether a child meets criteria for disorder Figure 11.1). Information collected from the

120
Ratio Age Equivalent/MA x 100

100

80

60

40

20

0
Rec* Exp** Wrt Per Dom Cmt Int*** Ply* Cop**
Autistic Developmental Disorders

Figure 11.1 Ratios of Vineland age-equivalent scores to mental age in children with autism versus other
developmental disorders. Cop = Coping; Cmt = Community; Dom = Domestic; Exp = Expressive communication;
Int = Interpersonal relationships; Per = Personal skills; Ply = Play and leisure time; Rec = Receptive communica-
tion; Wrt = Written communication.
* p < .05, ** p = < .01. *** p < .001. Adapted from “Social Deficits in Autism: An Operational Approach Using
the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,” by F. R. Volkmar et al., 1987, Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, pp. 156–161.
316 Development and Behavior

Vineland and signal detection methodology Vineland is currently undergoing revision and
has also been used to demonstrate that delays has been expanded to include a larger number
in social skills are robust predictors of the di- of items that are relevant to capture the social
agnosis of autism, even when compared to de- functioning of individuals with autism.
lays in communication (Gillham, Carter,
Volkmar, & Sparrow, 2000; Volkmar, Carter, STUDYING SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Sparrow, & Cicchetti, 1993). A series of stud- IN AUTISM
ies using the Vineland generally supports the
notion that individuals with autism demon- Early studies of the social development of chil-
strate deficits in social skills that are greater dren with autism (e.g., Ornitz, Guthrie, & Far-
than expected relative to overall developmen- ley, 1977; Volkmar, Cohen, & Paul, 1986)
tal level (e.g., Freeman et al., 1991; Loveland were often based on retrospective parent re-
& Kelley, 1991; Rodrigue, Morgan, & Geff- ports rather than direct observations. More re-
ken, 1991; Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, cently, researchers have employed pediatric
1985; Stone, Ousley, Hepburn, Hogan, & record review (e.g., Fombonne et al., 2004),
Brown, 1999). Table 11.2 presents Vineland used family videotaped records (e.g., Oster-
items that distinguish between children with ling, Dawson, & Munson, 2002), and studied
autism and both age and mental age-matched infant siblings of children diagnosed with
controls. autism to examine early social functioning.
In contrast to many of the instruments fo- Consistent with Kanner’s impression that the
cused specifically on autism, the Vineland is social deviance associated with autism is pres-
an important tool because it assesses more fa- ent from birth, research suggests that, at least
miliar and normative, developmentally appro- in retrospect, parents’ reports often concern
priate skills. Supplementary norms for the the child’s development in the first year of life
Vineland have been developed for use with in- (cf. Rogers, 2004). In a minority of cases, the
dividuals with autism and may have potential child’s development is reported to be normal
utility in screening for autism (Carter, Volk- or near normal before the parents become con-
mar, Sparrow, Wang, Lord, et al., 1998). The cerned, usually between the ages of 18 months

TABLE 11.2 Vineland Socialization Items Dif ferentiating Autistic Children from Age
and MA-Matched Controls

Expected Age
Item (Years–Months) p<
Shows interest in new objects/people < 0-2 .05
Anticipates being picked up by caregiver < 0-2 .01
Shows affection to familiar persons 0-4 .001
Shows interest in children /peers other than siblings 0-4 .001
Reaches for familiar person 0-5 .001
Plays simple interaction games 0-6 .001
Uses household objects for play 0-7 .05
Shows interest in activities of others 0-8 .01
Imitates simple adult movements 0-7 .01
Laughs/smiles in response to positive statements 0-11 .01
Calls at least two familiar people by name 0-11 .01
Participates in at least one activity/game with others 1-7 .05
Imitates adult phrases heard previously 1-11 .05
Sources: Items drawn from the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, by
S. Sparrow, D. Balla, and D. Cicchetti, 1984, Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service. Data ab-
stracted from “Autistic Social Dysfunctions: Some Limitations of the Theory of Mind Hypothesis,” by A. Klin,
F. R. Volkmar, and S. S. Sparrow, 1992, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, pp. 861–876. Expected
age is the median age at which the behavior is present in the general population, cases matched on age and mental
age and included in comparison only if mental age of the pair was equal to that typically associated with the be-
havior in the general population.
Social Development in Autism 317

and 3 years (Fombonne et al., 2004; Rogers, along with methodological advances and re-
2004). Given that autism appears to be an search approaches that enhance our under-
early-onset disorder, it is somewhat paradoxi- standing of trajectories of social behaviors.
cal that observational data on very young chil- Given the explosion of interest in this area over
dren with autism are highly limited. The the past decade, it is not possible to review
paucity of research on young children reflects every study that is relevant. Rather, we include
the fact that diagnosis is most difficult in this model exemplars that highlight core features
age group (Lord, 1996) and that even when and recent progress. Finally, we do not address
parents are concerned about the child’s devel- interventions that focus on enhancing social
opment, a considerable period of time often functioning. However, it is notable that signifi-
elapses before the diagnosis is given (Siegel, cant progress is being made with respect to
Pliner, Eschler, & Elliott, 1988; Stone & translating into clinical practice findings
Rosenbaum, 1988). An exciting new develop- about specific deficits in social functioning
ment is the initiation of multiple studies of in- and their relation to enhanced adaptation (cf.
fant siblings of affected children, after an Rogers, 2000).
international collaborative demonstrated the
feasibility of this work. As these studies prog- SPECIFIC SOCIAL PROCESSES
ress, our understanding of the earliest course IN AUTISM
of social dysfunction in autism will undergo
dramatic expansion (see Chawarska, Klin, and Children, adolescents, and adults with autism
Volkmar, 2003). exhibit deficits in multiple aspects of social
While it is not true that all persons with processing. In the next section we highlight gaze
autism are uninterested in all other people and joint attention, play, attachment behaviors,
(Lord, 1993) and it is true that some social peer relations, and affective development.
functioning may improve over time, autistic
social behavior is rarely “normal,” and even Gaze
the highest functioning individuals show obvi-
ous signs of social deviance (Volkmar, 1987b; Normal infants come into the world “pre-
Wing & Gould, 1979). However, despite the wired” by evolution with the motivation and
centrality of social deficits in the diagnosis of capacity to begin establishing an immediate
autism, trajectories of social behaviors in social relationship with their caregivers
autism are a new focus of study, and the nature (Povinelli, 1993). Newborn infants show pref-
of the processes underlying social gains during erence for facelike stimuli and have a basic ca-
childhood remains relatively unexamined. There pacity to direct eye gaze toward faces (e.g.,
is some controversy about whether gains made Farroni, Csibra, Simion, & Johnson, 2002;
are steady and progressive or whether, for the Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991).
individual child, gains (and sometimes losses) In the first months of life, typically developing
in skill levels follow a more unpredictable infants demonstrate selective attention to so-
course. In addition, discrepancies between cial stimuli and preferential attention to
skills levels in various areas (Burack & Volk- human faces. For example, very young infants
mar, 1992) and rates of advance in different show a preference for the human face over
areas may not directly correlate with age other patterns (Olson & Sherman, 1983; Spitz,
(Loveland & Kelley, 1991) or cognitive level 1965), and normal newborns orient themselves
(Szatmari et al., 2000). This issue remains perceptually and motorically toward their par-
controversial (Ando, Yoshimura, & Wak- ents (Mayes, Cohen, & Klin, 1993). Moreover,
abayashi, 1980; Loveland & Kelley, 1991). by 2 months of age, infants begin to scan the
In the next sections, specific social deficits eye region within faces preferentially (Hain-
evident among individuals with autism are line, 1978; Haith, Bergman, & Moore, 1977),
described in the context of normative social and by 4 months of age, infants can discrimi-
development. Deficits and delays in the emer- nate the direction of an interactive partner’s
gence of these aspects of social behavior gaze (Bloom, 1974; Caron, Caron, Roberts, &
among individuals with autism are described, Brooks, 1997; Hains & Muir, 1996). Thus, by
318 Development and Behavior

4 months of age, infants perceive the move- underlying strategies or mechanisms for gaze
ment associated with a shift in gaze as a direc- processing may be present by 2 years of age
tional cue (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & (Chawarska et al., 2003). In another recent
Simion, 2000; Hood, Willen, & Driver, 1998). study, Senju, Tojo, Dairoku, and Hasegawa
Specifically, 4-month-old infants show shorter (2004), observed that children with autism did
saccadic reaction time to targets that appear in not evidence the expected preferential gaze
locations that are consistent with gaze location shifting in response to a social cue. In addi-
when compared to those that are not consistent tion, a more general pattern of difficulty shift-
with gaze location. However, due to limited vi- ing controlled attention was observed. More
sual acuity, it may be more appropriate to con- work is needed to fully understand the mecha-
sider 4-month-olds as being sensitive to nisms that underlie developmental processes
directed motion, or head turning, rather than involved in gaze shifting among individuals
gaze, per se (Farroni et al., 2000). with autism.
Facial expressions and eye contact are the Even before the introduction of eye-tracking
most frequent modes of communication be- technologies, studies with preschool-age and
tween the preverbal infant and his or her older children revealed that the human face
mother (Ling & Ling, 1974) and involve the holds little interest for children with autism
sharing of affective states. This nonverbal fa- (Volkmar, 1987b). Eye gaze is frequently re-
cial “dialogue” between infant and caregiver ported to be abnormal by parents of children
provides the context for very early socializa- with autism. For example, in one retrospective
tion, providing critical opportunities for learn- study, 90% of parents of children with autism
ing (Adamson, 1995; Tomasello, Kruger, & reported that their child often, very often, or
Ratner, 1993). However, whereas typically de- almost always avoided eye contact (Volkmar
veloping infants spend a significant propor- et al., 1986). Data based on global parental
tion of time engaging in eye contact with their impression and systematic studies suggest that
caregivers, individuals with autism fail to es- there may be important contextual influences
tablish this pattern of mutual gaze (Volkmar on eye contact. For example, eye contact has
& Mayes, 1990). These gaze deviations appear been shown to increase with adult structure
specific to autism and are not observed among (Dawson, Hill, Spencer, Galpert, & Watson,
children with developmental delays or who are 1990; Sigman, Mundy, Sherman, & Ungerer,
later diagnosed with mental retardation. Ori- 1986) and change depending on the situation
enting toward social cues in the environment (Kasari, Sigman, & Yirmiya, 1993). Similarly,
provides relevant information about interests the nature of task demands may influence
and dangers as well as about the attentional amounts of eye contact (Dawson & Adams,
and emotional states of others (Chawarska, 1984; Dawson & Galpert, 1990). Gaze behav-
Klin, & Volkmar, 2003; Leekam, Lopez, & ior also varies as a function of developmental
Moore, 2000). level. More developmentally advanced chil-
Recent advances in eye-tracking and com- dren exhibit an increased frequency of eye
puter modeling technologies have increased in- contact (Dawson & Galpert, 1990; Kasari
terest in the study of gaze, and studies of gaze et al., 1993; Leekam et al., 2000; Mundy, Sig-
are now an integral component of social neuro- man, & Kasari, 1994).
science in autism. For example, in a recent Moreover, deficits in gaze may be ampli-
study of 2-year-olds with autism, Chawarska fied by naturalistic demands to coordinate eye
and colleagues (2003) demonstrated that al- gaze and tracking of others’ gaze with gestural
though in naturalistic settings toddlers with and verbal systems of expression and under-
autism do not follow the gaze of others, they standing. For example, younger children with
are sensitive to directional cues inherent in eye autism are less likely to use gaze to augment
movement. However, counter to expectations, other sources of information about ambiguous
toddlers with autism had shorter reaction interactions (Phillips, Baron-Cohen, & Rutter,
times to biological eye movements and similar 1992). Similar problems in the integration of
reaction times to nonbiological movement. gaze and other nonverbal behaviors have been
This pattern of findings suggests that different noted by other investigators (e.g., Buitelaar,
Social Development in Autism 319

van Engeland, de Kogel, de Vries, & van emergence of intersubjectivity, the coconstruc-
Hooff, 1991). tion of shared emotional meaning between
parent and caregiver (Stern, 1987; Trevarthen
Interest in Social Speech & Aitken, 2001). This failure to achieve inter-
subjectivity in persons with autism also re-
Some of the earliest evidence of social drive is sults in a lack of a series of behaviors known
the typical newborn’s preference for the as joint attention, which typically emerges in
human voice, especially its mother’s voice, the 8- to 12-month age period (Bakeman &
over other sounds (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). Adamson, 1984; Hannan, 1987). Joint atten-
Vocal communications between infants and tion is a preverbal social communicative skill
caregivers constitute an important aspect of that involves sharing with another person the
social interchange even before speech is ac- experience of a third object or event (Bruner,
quired. For example, long before the infant can 1983; Schaffer, 1984). Typically developing
respond differentially to the verbal content of infants will, for instance, smile and point at a
speech, he or she can respond with great accu- toy they find interesting, alternately looking at
racy to tone and pitch of voice (Lewis, 1963). the toy and to their mother. Similarly, typi-
While the precise mechanisms by which early cally developing infants will follow the par-
reciprocal social interactions facilitate the ent’s eye gaze and/or point as they turn to
emergence of lexical-communicative speech show an object of interest in the distance.
remain unclear, Bruner (1983) and other theo- However, such triadic exchanges are consis-
rists have emphasized the role of such transac- tently impoverished in children with autism of
tions for subsequent linguistic development. similar mental age (Mundy, Sigman, Kasari,
However, in the case of autism, even very 1990; see also Mundy, Chapter 25, this Hand-
young children appear to lack a preference for book, this volume).
speech sounds over other kinds of sounds Given that this behavior typically emerges
(Klin, 1991, 1992; Osterling & Dawson, prior to 1 year of age, but diagnosis of autism
1994). In addition to the typical delays in the is not typically made until sometime in the
onset of speech (Stone, Hoffman, Lewis, & second year, a handful of studies have retro-
Ousley, 1994), young children with autism spectively reviewed home movies/videos of
exhibit atypical preverbal vocalizations children with autism as infants. Osterling and
(Sheinkopf, Mundy, Oller, & Steffens, 2000), Dawson (1994), for example, reviewed video-
depressed rates of preverbal communication tapes of first birthday parties of 22 children
(Wetherby, Prizant, & Hutchinson, 1998), as (11 with autism and 11 who were developing
well as a restricted range of communicative normally). Data were collected on social, af-
behaviors—particularly those concerned with fective, communicative, and joint attention be-
regulation (Mundy & Stella, 2000). Recent ex- haviors as well as for symptoms suggestive of
perimental work in toddlers (Paul, Chawarska, autism. The children with autism exhibited
Klin, & Volkmar, 2004) suggests a general de- fewer social and joint attention behaviors and
crease in interest in listening to speech and a more autistic symptoms. Behaviors such as
lack of development of preference for typical pointing, showing objects, looking at others,
language patterns. If this lack of interest in so- and orienting in response to name could be
cial stimuli is in fact present from birth, it is used to differentiate the groups. More re-
likely that children with autism would fail to cently, Osterling et al. (2002) studied the
initiate and integrate the basic interpersonal same phenomena among 20 children later di-
patterns that are believed to be the foundation agnosed with autism spectrum disorder
for all later communication. (ASD), 14 later diagnosed with mental retar-
dation, and 20 typically developing children.
Joint Attention This study replicated and extended the 1994
paper, demonstrating that the children with
The absence or deviance of gaze behaviors and autism exhibited fewer social and joint atten-
other forms of early nonverbal interchange in tion behaviors and more atypical autism-
children with autism also interferes with the specific behaviors than both the typically
320 Development and Behavior

developing and developmentally delayed developmentally matched children with mental


groups. Thus, the limited social and joint at- retardation (see Tager-Flusberg, Paul, & Lord,
tention behaviors were not a function of devel- Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume).
opmental delays but appear to be central to the As with other aspects of gaze behavior, de-
diagnosis of autism. velopmental relationships and correlates of
In autism, relative failures may be apparent joint attention have been observed. Children
in showing or pointing to objects. When chil- with autism who are functioning at lower devel-
dren do show or point, they are much less opmental levels across other domains also usu-
likely to alternate gaze at the interactive part- ally show lower levels of joint attention (Mundy
ner and a desired or interesting object /activity et al., 1994). Joint attention has been related to
than a typically developing child would. In- language abilities (Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, &
deed, deficits in joint attention are among the Sherman, 1987), gains in language abilities
most striking and persistent problems in over time (Charman et al., 2003; Siller & Sig-
younger children with autism (Lewy & Daw- man, 2002), and aspects of executive function-
son, 1992; Loveland & Landry, 1986; Mundy ing (McEvoy, Rogers, & Pennington, 1993).
et al., 1994; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sher- Finally, as with other social behaviors,
man, 1986, Chapter 25, this Handbook, this deficits in joint attention skills have implica-
volume). Even when joint attention is ob- tions beginning very early in life, affecting the
served, its quality is unusual, with minimal ability of children with autism to engage with
coordination of gaze, vocalizations, and ges- others and to forge social relationships. In
tures. Further complicating the social interac- contrast, typically developing children are
tions of children with autism is that they may able to use joint attention to share their affec-
also show less positive affect directed toward tive experiences with their caregivers vis-à-vis
others in social exchanges and may even avoid objects and events in the world long before
positive praise (Kasari, Sigman, Mundy, & they develop language.
Yirmiya, 1990).
Rather than a complete deficit in joint at- Imitation
tention skills, a specific pattern of joint atten-
tion skills and deficits is usually apparent in Deficits in the areas of imitation have impor-
children and adults with autism. Individuals tant consequences for other aspects of devel-
with autism may, for example, display proto- opment. The ability of an infant to share
imperative gesturing, while protodeclarative experiences with its caregiver with regard to
gesturing is usually completely absent (Baron- an object of reference is an important context
Cohen, 1989; Curcio, 1978; Gomez, Sarria, & for symbolic development (Werner & Kaplan,
Tamarit, 1993; Kasari et al., 1990). Protoim- 1963). The capacity to imitate also appears to
peratives involve the use of gaze and/or ges- be a prerequisite for the acquisition of subse-
tures to gain another person’s aid in obtaining a quent symbolic activities. Children with
particular object or outcome (e.g., pointing to a autism display serious deficits across different
box of cookies on a high shelf ). Protodeclara- types of imitation tasks (Prior, 1979). Various
tives involve similar combinations of eye con- studies have documented deficits in this area
tact and gesturing, but solely with the aim of (see Smith & Bryson, 1994, for a review).
calling another person’s attention to an object Infants and children with autism produce
or experience, that is, without any instrumental less spontaneous imitation of the actions of
purpose (e.g., showing a parent that he or she their parents (Dawson & Adams, 1984; Melt-
has found an interesting toy; Bates, 1976). zoff & Gopnik, 1993), and they are less adept
Among children with autism who are preverbal, at elicited imitation (Charman & Baron-
communication appears to be almost entirely Cohen, 1994; Stone & Caro-Martinez, 1990).
requestive. Thus, even those children with Studies have consistently revealed that younger
autism who display coordination of eye contact children with autism consistently have prob-
with gestures and actions tend not to use it lems in the imitation of simple body move-
merely to share an awareness or an experience ments and those that involve objects (e.g.,
of an object or event, as do normal children and DeMyer, Barton, & Norton, 1972; Stone,
Social Development in Autism 321

Lemanek, Fishel, Fernandez, & Altemeier, as repetitive and stereotyped object manipula-
1990; Stone, Ousley, & Littleford, 1995). For tion and nonfunctional use of objects (Black,
example, in a recent study, Rogers, Hepburn, Freeman, & Montgomery, 1975; Mundy et al.,
Stackhouse, and Wehner (2003) demonstrated 1986; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984b; Stone et al.,
that toddlers with autism evidenced delays rel- 1990). For example, a toy truck may interest
ative to developmentally delayed and typically the child only to the extent that parts of it may
developing children in specific types of imita- be spun or whirled. In comparison to children
tion skills including oral-facial imitation (e.g., with mental retardation, comparatively less
extending and wiggling tongue) and imitation symbolic play is observed among children with
of actions on objects (e.g., patting a squeaky autism (DeMyer, Mann, Tilton, & Loew,
toy with elbow). Deficits in reciprocal social 1967; Stone & Lemanek, 1990; Wing, Gould,
play, characterized by infant games such as Yeates, & Brierley, 1977).
peekaboo and patty-cake, which integrate imi- Consistent with a less developmentally ma-
tation and social dialogue, are also noted by ture capacity for play, materials may be of
parents of children with autism (Klin, 1992). greater interest to children with autism be-
As with other social behaviors, important ef- cause of the way they taste or feel, rather than
fects of developmental level and context are their potential for symbolic or constructive
observed (Dawson & Adams, 1984; Sigman & play. In addition, play in younger children with
Ungerer, 1984b). In Rogers’ and colleagues re- autism is highly repetitive in nature (Sherman,
cent study of toddlers (2003), oral-facial and Shapiro, & Glassman, 1983; Stone et al.,
object imitation skills were related to overall 1990). Thus, deficits are observed in both
developmental level and to an estimate of functional and symbolic play (e.g., Sigman &
autism severity, but results did not confirm Ungerer, 1984b; Stone et al., 1990). Consistent
Stone, Ousley, and Littleford’s (1997) finding with the emergence of other social skills, sym-
of relations between imitation and expressive bolic play is sometimes present. However,
language or play skills. when present, the qualitative nature of the
play of children with autism differs from that
Play observed among typically developing children
and children with Down syndrome. Symbolic
Play skills normally develop within the first 2 play acts of the majority of children with
years of life. At first, objects are simply ma- autism were characterized as object substitu-
nipulated, mouthed, or visually regarded. tions, and they evidenced fewer play acts in-
Later the child moves from simple manipula- volving attributions of false properties and no
tions and inspection to combining objects in symbolic play acts involving a reference to an
play (e.g., stacking blocks) as spatial relation- absent object.
ships are explored. Functional use of play ob- The development of functional and symbolic
jects, such as using a cup to feed a doll, play skills is intimately related to differentia-
typically develops toward the end of the first tion of objects and actions and the progressive
year of life. True symbolic play typically de- independence of thought processes from con-
velops during the latter half of the second year crete reality (Piaget, 1951) as children develop
of life as play objects become completely inde- functional or symbolic play skills. Limited
pendent of action and play is no longer con- symbolic play in autism may emerge from so-
strained by an object’s physical properties (see cial difficulties or as part of a more general
Singer, 1996). The development of play skills problem of symbolic thought and language.
parallels other aspects of cognitive develop- Symbolic play skills may also be related to
ment as the child acquires the capacity for broader symbolic development, including the
symbolic thought (Piaget, 1951). emergence of differential attachment (Sigman
The autistic child’s play stands in stark & Ungerer, 1984b) and the emergence of sym-
contrast to the richness of play in the typically bolic language (see Tager-Flusberg, Paul, &
developing child. Parents’ reports of the play Lord, Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume).
of children with autism suggest that it is char- In a recent paper, Rutherford and Rogers
acterized by lack of social engagement as well (2003) compared the role of two cognitive
322 Development and Behavior

theories—theory of mind and executive func- processes is suggested by the observation that
tion—in the development of pretend play skills. even neglected or abused infants typically
Although young children with autism did show form attachments as do infants with mental
expected deficits in pretend play, the authors handicap (Moser-Richters & Volkmar, 1996;
were not able to establish a clear link with Thompson, 1996), and such processes may be
either of the two cognitive theories (see also observed in various ways in infancy, for exam-
Libby, Powell, Messer, & Jordan, 1998). Con- ple, through specific attention to the mother’s
tinued research in this area is needed to clarify voice (Mills & Melhuish, 1974).
the cognitive factors that affect play skills. A recent meta-analytic analysis of studies
Many aspects of the play of children with employing the Strange Situation (Ainsworth,
autism remain to be studied, such as the devel- Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) demonstrated
opmental unfolding of patterns of social play, that autism is compatible with a secure
the extent to which children with autism can attachment organization (Rutgers, Bakermans-
differentiate fantasy from reality, and devel- Kranenburg van Ijzendoorn, & van Berckelaer-
opmental features of the relations of observed Onnes, 2004). Moreover, Rutgers and
individual differences in play to cognitive and colleagues revealed that, although children
social factors. What is becoming clearer is that with autism were more likely to have insecure
it is possible to teach discrete components of attachment organization than their typically
social play to children with autism (see Terp- developing peers, higher functioning individu-
stra, Higgins, & Pierce, 2002, for a review). als with autism were no more likely to be inse-
Studies that manipulate such skill acquisition curely attached than their typically developing
have the potential to greatly inform under- peers. Thus, the risk for insecurity appears
standing of associations among specific social limited to those individuals with autism and
play skills and other behaviors. cognitive deficits.
It is important to emphasize that the behav-
Attachment ior observed in the typical laboratory setting
may not reflect behavior in more naturalistic
By the end of the first year of life, typically settings; for example, children with autism
developing infants have established a coher- may evidence diminished or odd attachment
ent pattern of social behaviors, referred to as behaviors (Lord, 1993). Although younger
attachment, that serve to maintain proximity children with autism often exhibit proximity
to the caregiver and facilitate exploration seeking more frequently toward their mother
(Bowlby, 1969). During their first year, typi- relative to a stranger and increase their prox-
cally developing infants learn to respond dif- imity to the mother following reunion with her
ferentially to their caregivers and other (Sigman & Mundy, 1989; Sigman & Ungerer,
individuals. The complex processes underly- 1984a), the quality of these behaviors may be
ing attachment are one of the evolved charac- unusual (Rogers, Ozonoff, & Maslin-Cole,
teristics on which infant survival is based 1993). Various developmental correlates of
(Freedman, 1974; see also Chapter 22, this these behaviors have also been noted (Capps,
Handbook, this volume). Attachment behaviors Sigman, & Mundy, 1994; Rogers, Ozonoff, &
are characterized by the child’s concern for Maslin-Cole, 1991, 1993; Sigman & Ungerer,
maintaining proximity with its caregiver and 1984a). More recently, Dissanayake and
extreme distress in the face of that caregiver’s Crossley (1997), examining separations and
absence (Bowlby, 1969; Rutter, 1981). When reunions in naturalistic settings, found that
the infant achieves a secure attachment organi- children with autism (and children with Down
zation, the caregiver acts as a “ home base” syndrome) showed fundamentally similar at-
from which the child may engage in exploration tachment behaviors but greater variability in
of the world. Early patterns of infant-caregiver behaviors across three observation sessions.
interaction can be related to the quality of later The greater variability may impact parents’
attachment, which are, in turn, related to sub- experience, such that they do not share in a
sequent cognitive and social skills (see Seifer working model of dyadic attachment security.
& Schiller, 1995). The strength of these Even though behavioral research has clearly
Social Development in Autism 323

documented the presence of attachment behav- interaction over time (Wing & Gould, 1979).
iors and a high rate of attachment security, it In older children, the social failures in commu-
also is clear that the perception of parents with nication are most evident as children fail to
respect to attachment behaviors is very differ- initiate social interchange and have difficul-
ent (Volkmar et al., 1986). ties taking another person’s point of view into
In addition, although social attachments do account (Volkmar & Cohen, 1985).
not always develop when expected, idiosyn- Indeed, there is increasing evidence that
cratic attachments to objects are sometimes children and adolescents with autism rarely
seen (Volkmar, 1987b). Young children who develop typical peer relationships (Koning &
are developing normally often form attach- Magill-Evans, 2001; Le Couteur et al., 1989;
ments to transitional objects, typically soft Orsmond, Krauss, & Seltzer, 2004). Observa-
and cuddly materials that aid them with tran- tional studies highlight deficits in social initi-
sitions of various sorts. When younger chil- ations to peers relative to both typically
dren with autism have attachments to objects, developing and cognitively impaired peers
these attachments are almost always odd in (Jackson et al., 2003), and direct interviews of
quality. For example, younger children with higher functioning children and adolescents
autism may be attached to objects that are reveal greater difficulty defining central ele-
hard (e.g., cereal boxes, metal cars), or they ments of what determines friendship relation-
may be attached to a class of objects rather ships as well as greater feelings of loneliness
than a specific object (e.g., a magazine of a (Bauminger & Kasari, 2000).
certain type but not a specific magazine). In Among individuals on the autism spectrum,
the DSM-IV field trial (Volkmar et al., 1994), those with less well-developed cognitive and
attachments to unusual objects were noted to verbal skills make fewer initiations to peers
be of low frequency but, when present, very (Hauck, Fein, Waterhouse, & Feinstein, 1995;
specific to the diagnosis of autism. The sig- Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). In addition to mak-
nificance of such objects and their relation to ing fewer initiations, some children with
the more typical transitional objects seen in autism respond less often to the approaches of
normally developing children remains to be others and often appear more content when
understood. left alone (Attwood, Frith, & Hermelin, 1988;
Volkmar, 1987b). In contrast to their interac-
Peer Relations tions with peers, children with autism are
more likely to approach adults than children
Over the course of typical development, social (Hauck et al., 1995; Jackson et al., 2003). Per-
skills become increasingly differentiated as haps due to this tendency to approach adults,
children develop peer relations, prosocial when social relationships do develop, these
skills, and an increasing capacity for self- tend to be with adults. It is also quite likely
regulation (Schaffer, 1984; Singer, 1996). that adults will be more accepting of deviant
Among individuals with autism, however, lim- social behaviors. It is notable that in a recent
ited interest in social interaction and reduced study of 235 adolescents and adults with
initiation of social contact with peers remain autism living at home, only 8% of the sample
apparent over time. Mutual or cooperative play were rated by mothers on the ADI (Lord et al.,
of the type usually expected among school-age 1994) as having a friendship that involved var-
children is typically absent, and many children ied, mutually responsive, and reciprocal activ-
with autism prefer to be left alone to engage in ities, and almost half of the sample (46.4%)
self-stimulatory and other unusual activities. were reported to have no peer relationships
In older children, there is typically a failure to (Orsmund et al., 2004). Using more relaxed
engage in social interchange with peers, and criteria for defining friendships, an additional
cooperative play is usually absent; they make 20% of respondents had at least one “ friend”
far fewer approaches to peers than other chil- with whom they engaged in a joint activity out-
dren (Koning & Magill-Evans, 2001; Le Cou- side an organized setting. Approximately one-
teur et al., 1989). Some individuals may in fact fourth of the sample had at least one peer
become more passive or odd in their style of relationship within an organized setting.
324 Development and Behavior

For some individuals with autism, social appear to have difficulty recognizing emotions
interest expands significantly during adoles- in others, although it is not clear whether there
cence accompanied by continued gains in so- is truly a perceptual difficulty with recogniz-
cial skills (Rutter, 1970; Schopler & Mesibov, ing facial affect or a more cognitive-affective
1983). Unfortunately, however, even when so- inability to infer others’ mental states
cial interest increases, marked problems usu- (DeGelder, 1991; Hobson, 1986, 1990, 1993;
ally remain as the individual has difficulty in Hobson, Chapter 15, this Handbook, this vol-
dealing with social rules and conventions and ume; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1990).
with the reciprocal give-and-take inherent to Children with autism have been observed to
social situations (Church, Alinsanski, & have difficulties in both the spontaneous ex-
Amanullah, 2000; Rutter, 1983; Seltzer et al., pression and purposeful reproduction of affec-
2003). There are often particular difficulties tive responses. With respect to emotional
with learning, and then in generalizing, the expression, during early childhood the overall
rules of social interchange (Schopler & Mesi- impression of young children with autism may
bov, 1983). Even though gradual improvement be one of social aloofness or disengagement
is common, unfortunately, some individuals (Wing & Gould, 1979). Throughout the life
lose skills during adolescence. span, their range of expression, frequency of
Higher functioning adults may desire to different expressions, and integration of affec-
make social contact. These individuals often tive displays are unusual relative to typically
have marked problems in developing friend- developing and cognitively impaired individu-
ships and relating to others because the practi- als (Ricks & Wing, 1975; Snow, Hertzig, &
cal ability to carry on the complex tasks Shapiro, 1987; Yirmiya, Kasari, Sigman, &
related to intense social interaction is a source Mundy, 1989). Moreover, the integration of
of much difficulty. Feelings of inadequacy and appropriate affective displays into the ongoing
isolation are common (Bemporad, 1979; Kan- social interaction is an area of particular diffi-
ner, Rodriquez, & Ashden, 1972; Volkmar & culty (Dawson et al., 1990; Kasari, Sigman,
Cohen, 1985). Kanner et al. (1972) suggested Baumgartner, & Stipek, 1993; McGee, Feld-
that those individuals with good outcome on man, & Chernin, 1991). Even when smiling is
follow-up had learned, by adolescence, to per- observed, it is much less likely to be coordi-
ceive themselves as unusual and were able, in a nated with gaze toward others or to be elicited
rudimentary way, to develop strategies for in social interactions (e.g., Dawson et al.,
coping with their disability. 1990). Persons with autism have also been
A variety of intervention programs target- noted to have difficulties with the imitation of
ing peer and social skills have been developed. facial displays of emotion; relative to Down
Despite some methodological flaws (e.g., syndrome subjects, those with autism were
small numbers of participants, lack of appro- more likely to produce unusual facial displays
priate measures of outcome), a small body of (Loveland et al., 1994).
research now documents the success of many Specific difficulties in facial recognition
of these programs (see McConnell, 2002, and and processing information conveyed by
Rogers, 2000, for reviews). However, many of human faces, and particularly human emotion,
the programs that have been studied empiri- have been noted. In an early study, Langdell
cally remain inaccessible to the general public, (1978) observed age-related changes in as-
and many programs that are used more com- pects of facial recognition in autism. Various
monly (e.g., social stories and social skills studies have similarly suggested problems in
groups in schools) need to be examined more the recognition of faces. These problems in-
rigorously for effectiveness (Rogers, 2000). clude the recognition of unfamiliar faces rela-
tive to nonsocial objects (Boucher & Lewis,
Affective Development 1992) and difficulties in utilizing contextual
cues (Teunisse & de Gelder, 1994). At least
In the second or third year of life, typically some aspects of the face, at least as depicted in
developing children begin to recognize and label photographs, can be utilized as sources of in-
their own and others’ emotional states (Brether- formation by children with autism (Volkmar,
ton & Beeghly, 1982). Children with autism Sparrow, Rende, & Cohen, 1989). A more
Social Development in Autism 325

complete discussion of the controversies in catalyst for the wide range of deficits observed
this area is presented by Hobson (Chapter 15, in multiple developmental domains in autism
this Handbook, this volume). Significant ad- or as a result of, or part of, a cascade of devel-
vances in understanding neural processes un- opmental consequences caused by a primary
derlying facial recognition have also occurred deficit in another developmental domain.
(see Volkmar, Klin, Schultz, Chawarska, & Challenges for theoretical models that attempt
Jones, 2003). to explain social behavior in autism include:
Differences in emotion expression, imita- (1) the very broad range of syndrome expres-
tion, and recognition have implications for sion in autism and related conditions, (2) the
the development of empathy. Experiments need to encompass observed developmental
designed to elicit empathic response on the changes, and (3) the likelihood that different
part of autistic individuals have found an ab- etiologies may underlie somewhat different
sence of expressed concern in response to a phenotypes (Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz,
display of distress by an adult (Bacon, Fein, & Klin, 2004). A brief review of several recent
Morris, Waterhouse, & Allen, 1998; Sigman, theoretical approaches serves to illustrate
Kasari, Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992). This may their advantages and disadvantages for under-
be due in part to differences in attention to standing autistic social dysfunction.
others’ negative affective states. For example, Undoubtedly the most productive theoreti-
children with autism may less frequently look cal model has been the theory of mind (ToM)
at an adult who is demonstrating distress hypothesis (Baron-Cohen, 1995). This theory
(Bacon et al., 1998; Sigman et al., 1992). suggests that the characteristic deficits in so-
Children with autism also usually fail to re- cial interaction arise due to a basic problem in
spond with prosocial behaviors such as giv- intersubjectivity (Trevarthen & Aitken, 2001),
ing, sharing, helping, or offering comfort or that is, as a fundamental inability to conceptu-
affection (Lord, 1993; Ohta, Nagai, Hara, & alize mental phenomena in self and other.
Sasaki, 1987), which suggests a diminished Within this theory, individuals with ASDs are
degree of awareness of a range of others’ af- presumed to be unable to understand the be-
fective states. liefs, intentions, feelings, and desires of others
Diminished affective social responsiveness, and hence are unable to negotiate the social
with respect to both disruption in emotion world successfully (Baron-Cohen, 1988). This
identification and expression, likely con- theory view has a number of clear advantages;
tributes to broader problems in social relations for example, it accounts for the marked social
as parents, peers, and other interactive part- difficulties over the entire range of syndrome
ners may have more difficulty reading the cues expression. Unfortunately, careful analysis of
of the individual with autism as well as more the hypothesis also reveals a number of diffi-
difficulty anticipating the limits of the indi- culties. Theory of mind skills are strongly re-
vidual with autism with respect to empathy lated to language so that many higher
and ability to enter into intersubjective emo- functioning individuals with autism or As-
tional states. perger’s can do the usual theory of mind tasks
despite being very disabled socially (Bowler,
THEORETICAL MODELS OF AUTISTIC 1992; Dahlgren & Trillingsgaard, 1996). De-
SOCIAL DYSFUNCTION velopmental issues pose another problem be-
cause the social difficulties of autism are ones
Over the past decade, several different theo- that appear well before theory of mind skills,
retical approaches have been put forth to con- at least as the latter are usually conceptualized
ceptualize the fundamental basis of social (Klin, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 1992; see also
dysfunction in autism. A comprehensive, de- Frye, Zelazo, & Falfai, 1995).
velopmentally based theory that can address An alternative theoretical approach has fo-
the changing presentation across the develop- cused on executive functioning (EF) skills,
mental course is critical for understanding the which include a group of abilities that allow
neurobiology of social deficits in autism. maintenance of the set for problem solving to
These theories vary with respect to viewing solve an overarching goal. These abilities in-
dysfunction in the social domain as either the clude the ability for forward planning and set
326 Development and Behavior

shifting, which are presumed to be the skills rules of social interaction. This model, embed-
most impacted in autism (Ozonoff, 1997). ded in emerging findings from cognitive neu-
This theory encompasses a number of the dif- roscience, has a number of potential
ficulties that children with autism have in advantages, particularly for understanding the
learning. These include the tendency to perse- apparent paradox of individuals with autism
verate or engage in inappropriate, off-task re- and Asperger’s, who function at a high level
sponses and the tremendous trouble that cognitively, but remain profoundly impaired
individuals with autism often have in applying socially. Further empirical work is clearly
knowledge in real-world contexts (Volkmar needed on this hypothesis.
et al., 2003). Furthermore, some aspects of As evident from these descriptions, still
the brain circuitry involved in EF skills have needed is a comprehensive theoretical per-
been identified, for example, the dorsolateral spective that accounts for the multiple devel-
prefrontal cortex (Pennington & Ozonoff, opmental trajectories and divergent patterns of
1996). There is also the suggestion that EF social functioning observed among children,
difficulties may aggregate within families adolescents, and adults with ASDs. The dra-
(Hughes, Leboyer, & Bouvard, 1997; Hughes, matic increase in empirical research on social
Plumet, & Leboyer, 1999). However, difficul- dysfunction among individuals with autism is
ties with the EF hypothesis arise in several re- increasing understanding of phenotypic as-
spects since EF deficits are not unique to pects of social dysfunction in autism, which
autism but are observed in a number of disor- will inform advances in genetics and basic
ders (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996) and do not neurobiology. Reciprocally, the discovery of
correlate straightforwardly with the degree of genetic and neurobiological markers will in-
social impairment (Dawson & Meltzoff, 1998; form the identification of subtypes of pheno-
see also Ozonoff, South, & Provencal, Chap- types along the autism spectrum. It is quite
ter 22, this Handbook, this volume). likely that multiple neurobiological processes
A third major theoretical approach has fo- may result in similar phenotypic outcomes and
cused on difficulties in the ability of individu- that regulatory processes may cause differen-
als with autism to integrate information into tiation in outcomes among individuals who
coherent or meaningful wholes, that is, in cen- share common neurobiological markers; in
tral coherence (Frith, 2003; Happe, 1996). other words, multi- and equifinality (Cicchetti
Problems with attention, appreciation of con- & Rogosch, 1996) likely characterize the so-
text, and overall meaning are presumed to cial developmental processes that underlie
arise from weak central coherence. While at- ASDs (see also Brothers, 1989; Brothers &
tractive as a hypothesis, supportive empirical Ring, 1992, for a discussion of related issues).
data have been sparse (Mottron, Peretz, &
Menard, 2000). It could also easily be argued CONCLUSION
that difficulties in central coherence are the
result of severe social disability rather than Considerable research has been conducted that
the reverse (Volkmar et al., 2003). contributes to our understanding of social
A new approach, the enactive mind (EM) deficits in autism since the last publication of
model, takes a somewhat different theoretical this Handbook. Major methodological ad-
view (Klin et al., 2003; Klin, Chapter 26, this vances have benefited from technological in-
Handbook, this volume), viewing the develop- novations that promote improved assessment
ment of cognitive-symbolic capacities as re- of social behavior (e.g., see Insel, 1992; Insel
sulting from action on the environment with & Fernald, 2004). For example, new technolo-
social cognition emerging from within a social- gies available to track eye gaze and to develop
interactive context. In this view, the disruption complex emotion stimuli have greatly en-
of these processes in autism produces a gen- hanced knowledge about gaze and emotion
eral orientation toward the world of things recognition. In addition, the widespread use of
rather than specifically to people. As a result, integrated diagnostic tools (i.e., ADI and
individuals with autism and related disorders Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
take a very different approach to learning the [ADOS]; Lord et al., 2000) enables more accu-
Social Development in Autism 327

rate comparisons across studies and samples. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag-
Further, earlier detection and improvements in nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
diagnostic methods for very young children ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
have encouraged studies of the early emer- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
gence of social dysfunction. Finally, dramatic nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
advances in brain imaging techniques have led
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
to the emergence of the new field of social nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
neuroscience (Insel & Fernald, 2004). ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Consistent with Kanner’s (1943) earliest Ando, H., Yoshimura, I., & Wakabayashi, S.
descriptions, social deficits remain a hallmark (1980). Effects of age on adaptive behavior
feature of the disorder. At the same time, in levels and academic skill levels in autistic and
contrast to early depiction of the absence of mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism
social behaviors, it is now clear that there is and Developmental Disorders, 10, 173–184.
great variability across individuals with Attwood, A., Frith, J., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The
autism with respect to both overall level of so- understanding and use of interpersonal ges-
tures by autistic and down syndrome children.
cial functioning in day-to-day settings and per-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
formance on laboratory-based tasks, which
ders, 18, 241–258.
may or may not be congruent with behavior in Bacon, A. L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L.,
naturalistic settings. Because the social relat- & Allen, D. (1998). The responses of children
edness dysfunction remains a central, if not with autism to the distress of others. Journal
driving, component of autism, a current chal- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28,
lenge facing researchers is to explain the relat- 129–142.
edness that is present. Moreover, whereas Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating
more work is needed to further describe multi- attention to people and objects in mother-
ple aspects of social functioning in individuals infant and peer-infant interaction. Child De-
with autism, it is critical to design studies that velopment, 55, 1278–1289.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic
begin to focus on underlying mechanisms. Fi-
deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective?
nally, longitudinal studies and intervention Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
studies may begin to address the complex in- ders, 18(3), 379–403.
terplay observed among cognitive, commu- Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role-taking
nicative, and social deficits as well as within and protodeclarative pointing in autism.
components of social behaviors. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
7, 113–127.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay
Cross-References
on autism and theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Aspects of communication, attention, and cog-
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acqui-
nition are discussed in Chapters 12 through
sition of pragmatics. New York: Academic
15; Chapters 8 through 10 provide a summary Press.
of aspects of natural history. Diagnostic fea- Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness
tures of autism are discussed in Chapters 1 and friendship in high-functioning children
through 7. Theoretical perspectives on social with autism. Child Development, 71, 447–456.
development are addressed in Chapters 21 Bemporad, J. R. (1979). Adult recollections of a
through 26. formerly autistic child. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 9, 179–197.
Black, M., Freeman, B. J., & Montgomery, J.
REFERENCES (1975). Systematic observation of play behav-
ior in autistic children. Journal of Autism and
Adamson, L. B. (1995). Communication develop- Childhood Schizophrenia, 5(4), 363–371.
ment in infancy. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Bloom, K. (1974). Eye contact as a setting event for
Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, infant learning. Journal of Experimental Child
S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psycho- Psychology, 17, 250–263.
logical study of the strange situation. Oxford, Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face
England: Erlbaum. recognition in relatively able autistic children.
328 Development and Behavior

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry International Journal of Language and Com-
and Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 843–859. munication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. At- Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003).
tachment. New York: Basic Books. Automatic cueing through eye movement in
Bowler, D. M. (1992). “ Theory of mind” in As- 2-year-old children with autism. Child Devel-
perger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychol- opment, 74(4), 1108–1122.
ogy and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877–893. Church, C., Alinsanski, S., & Amanullah, S.
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking (2000). The social, behavioral, and academic
about internal states: The acquisition of an ex- experiences of children with Asperger syn-
plicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychol- drome. Focus on Autism and other Develop-
ogy, 19, 906–921. mental Disabilities, 15, 12–20.
Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinal-
empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, ity and multifinality in developmental psycho-
10–19. pathology. Development and Psychopathology,
Brothers, L., & Ring, B. (1992). A neuroethologi- 8(4), 597–600.
cal framework for the representation of minds. Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D.,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 107–118. Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy,
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use lan- S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quan-
guage. New York: Norton. titative measure of autistic traits: Compari-
Buitelaar, J. K., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, son of the social responsiveness scale with
K. H., de Vries, H., & van Hooff, J. (1991). the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Jour-
Differences in the structure of social behav- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
iour of autistic children and non-autistic re- 33(4), 427–433.
tarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32(6), communication in mute autistic children. Jour-
995–1015. nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8,
Burack, J. A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Develop- 282–292.
ment of low- and high-functioning autistic Dahlgren, S. O., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). The-
children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- ory of mind in non-retarded children with
chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33(3), 607–616. autism and Asperger’s syndrome. A research
Cairns, R. B. (1979). Social development: The ori- note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
gins and plasticity of interchanges. San Fran- try, 37, 759–763.
cisco: Freeman. Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and so-
Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). At- cial responsiveness in autistic children. Jour-
tachment security in children with autism. De- nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12,
velopment and Psychopathology, 6, 249–261. 209–226.
Caron, R. F., Caron, A. J., Roberts, J., & Brooks, R. Dawson, G., & Galpert, L. (1990). Mothers’ use of
(1997). Infant sensitivity to deviation in dy- imitative play for facilitating social respon-
namic facial-vocal displays: The role of eye re- siveness and toy play in young autistic chil-
gard. Developmental Psychology, 33, 802–813. dren. Development and Psychopathology, 2(2),
Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Wang, 151–162.
J. J., Lord, C., Dawson, G., et al. (1998). The Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., &
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supple- Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges be-
mentary norms for individuals with autism. tween young autistic children and their moth-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18,
ders, 28(4), 287–302. 335–345.
Charman, T., & Baird, G. (2002). Practitioner re- Dawson, G., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1998). Neuropsy-
view: Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder chological correlates of early symptoms of
in 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Child autism. Child Development, 69(5), 1276–1285.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(3), 289–305. DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bond-
Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Another ing: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices.
look at imitation in autism. Development and Science, 171, 1174–1176.
Psychopathology, 6, 403–413. DeGelder, B. (1991). Face recognition and lip-
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., reading in autism. European Journal of Cogni-
Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Pre- tive Psychology, 3, 69–86.
dicting language outcome in infants with DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., & Norton, J. A. (1972).
autism and pervasive developmental disorder. A comparison of adaptive, verbal, and motor
Social Development in Autism 329

profiles of psychotic and non-psychotic sub- Hainline, L. (1978). Developmental changes in vi-
normal children. Journal of Autism and Child- sual scanning of face and nonface patterns by
hood Schizophrenia, 2(4), 359–377. infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
DeMyer, M. K., Mann, N. A., Tilton, J. R., & Loew, chology, 25, 90–115.
L. H. (1967). Toy-play behavior and use of Hains, S. M. J., & Muir, D. W. (1996). Infant sensi-
body by autistic and normal children as re- tivity to adult eye direction. Child Develop-
ported by mothers. Psychological Reports, ment, 67, 1940–1951.
21(3), 973–981. Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. (1977).
Dissanayake C., & Crossley, S. A. (1997). Autistic Eye contact and face scanning in early in-
children’s responses to separation and reunion fancy. Science, 218, 179–181.
with their mothers. Journal of Autism and De- Hannan, T. (1987). A cross-sequential assessment
velopmental Disorders, 27(3), 295–312. of the occurrences of pointing in 3- to 12-
Farroni, R., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. month-old human infants. Infant Behavior and
(2002). Eye contact detection in humans from Development, 10, 11–22.
birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Happe, F. G. (1996). Studying weak central coher-
Sciences, 99, 9602–9695. ence at low levels: Children with autism do not
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Brockbank, M., & succumb to visual illusions. A research note.
Simion, F. (2000). Infants use of gaze direc- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
tion to cue attention: The importance of per- 37(7), 873–877.
ceived motion. Visual Cognition, 7, 705–718. Hauck, M., Fein, D., Waterhouse, L., & Feinstein, C.
Freedman, D. G. (1974). Human infancy: An evolu- (1995). Social initiations by autistic children to
tionary perspective New York: Erlbaum. adults and other children. Journal of Autism
Freeman, B. J., Rahbar, B., Ritvo, E. R., Bice, and Developmental Disorders, 25(6), 579–595.
T. L., Yokota, A., & Ritvo, R. (1991). The sta- Hobson, P. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of
bility of cognitive and behavioral parameters expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psy-
in autism: A twelve-year prospective study. chology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Hobson, P. (1990). On acquiring knowledge about
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 479–482. people and the capacity to pretend: Response
Fombonne, E., Heavey, L., Smeeth, L., Rodrigues, to Leslie (1987). Psychological Review, 97,
L. C., Cook, C., Smith, P. G., et al. (2004). 114–121.
Validation of the diagnosis of autism in gen- Hobson, P. (1993). Understanding persons: The
eral practitioner records. BMC Public Health, role of affect. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-
4, 5. Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma other minds: Perspectives from autism
(2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell. (pp. 204–227). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
Frye, D., Zelazo, P., & Falfai, T. (1995). Theory of versity Press.
mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive De- Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998).
velopment, 10, 483–527. Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention
Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., Jakobsson, in human infants. Psychological Science, 9,
G., Dahlgren, S. O., Lindbolm, R., et al. 131–134.
(1990). Autism under age 3 years: A clinical Hughes, C., Leboyer, M., & Bouvard,M. (1997).
study of 28 cases referred for autistic symp- Executive function in parents of children
toms in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology with autism. Psychological Medicine, 27(1),
and Psychiatry, 31, 921–934. 209–220.
Gillham, J. G., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Hughes, C., Plumet, M. H., & Leboyer, M. (1999).
Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Toward a developmental Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: In-
operational definition of autism. Journal of creased prevalence of executive dysfunction
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 30(4), and superior spatial span amongst siblings of
269–278. children with autism. Journal of Child Psy-
Gomez, J., Sarria, E., & Tamarit, J. (1993). The chology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
comparative study of early communication 40(5), 705–718.
and theories of mind: Ontogeny, phylogeny, Insel, T. (1992). Oxytocin: A neuropeptide for af-
and pathology. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager- filiation: Evidence from behavioral, receptor
Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding autoradiographic, and comparative studies.
other minds: Perspectives from autism Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17, 3–35.
(pp. 397–426). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- Insel, T. R., & Fernald, R. D. (2004). How the brain
versity Press. processes social information: Searching for
330 Development and Behavior

the Social Brain. Annual Review of Neuro- Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An ap-
science, 27, 697–722. proach to the study of autism. Journal of Child
Jackson, C. T., Fein, D., Wolf, J., Jones, G., Hauck, Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268.
M., Waterhouse, L., et al. (2003). Responses Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P.,
and sustained interactions in children with Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989).
mental retardation and autism. Journal of Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(2), investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism
115–121. and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387.
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Mor- Leekam, S. R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). At-
ton, J. (1991). Newborn’s preferential tracking tention and joint attention in preschool chil-
of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. dren with autism. Developmental Psychology,
Cognition, 40(1/2), 1–19. 36, 261–273.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- Lewis, M. (1963). Language, thought and personality
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 227–250. in infancy and childhood. London: G. Harapth.
Kanner, L., Rodriguez, A., & Ashden, B. (1972). Lewy, A. L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimula-
How far can autistic children go in matters of tion and joint attention in young autistic chil-
social adaptation. Journal of Autism and dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 9–33. 20(6), 555–566.
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Baumgartner, P., & Stipek, Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R.
D. J. (1993). Pride and mastery in children (1998). Spontaneous play in children with
with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and autism: A reappraisal. Journal of Autism and
Psychiatry, 34(3), 353–362. Developmental Disorders, 28, 487–498.
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. Ling, D., & Ling, A. H. (1974). Communication de-
(1990). Affective sharing in the context of velopment in the first three years of life. Jour-
joint attention interactions of normal, autistic nal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17,
and mentally retarded children. Journal of 146–159.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, Lord, C. (1993). The complexity of social behav-
87–100. ior in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Yirmiya, N. (1993). Fo- Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding
cused and social attention of autistic children other minds: Perspectives from autism
in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar (pp. 292–316). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally re- versity Press.
tarded, and normal children. Development and Lord, C. (1996). Follow-up of two-year-olds re-
Psychopathology, 5(3), 403–414. ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children’s listening Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(8), 1065–1076.
preferences in regard to speech: A possible Lord, C., Risi, S., Lembrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr.,
characterization of the symptom of social Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000).
withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Develop- The Autism Diagnostic Observation Sched-
mental Disorders, 21, 217–250. ule—generic: A standard measure of social
Klin, A. (1992). Listening preferences in regard to and communication deficits associated with
speech in four children with developmental the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and
disabilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
Psychiatry, 33, 763–769. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur A. (1994).
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. Autism diagnostic interview: A revised ver-
(2003). The enactive mind—from actions to sion of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of
cognition: Lessons from autism. In U. Frith & individuals with possible pervasive develop-
E. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain. Ox- mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel-
ford, England: Oxford University Press. opmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). Loveland, K. A., & Kelley, M. L. (1991). Develop-
Autistic social dysfunctions: Some limita- ment of adaptive behavior in preschoolers with
tions of the theory of mind hypothesis. Jour- autism or Down syndrome. American Journal
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, on Mental Retardation, 96(1), 13–20.
861–876. Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention
Koning, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and and communication in autism and language
language skills in adolescent boys with As- delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental
perger syndrome. Autism, 5, 23–36. Disorders, 16, 335–349.
Social Development in Autism 331

Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint
D. A., Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Chen, attention, developmental level, and symptom
R. (1994). Imitation and expression of facial presentation in autism. Development and
affect in autism. Development and Psycho- Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.
pathology, 6(3), 433–444. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T.
Mayes, L., Cohen, D., & Klin, A. (1993). Desire (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism:
and fantasy: A psychoanalytic perspective on The contribution of non-verbal communication
theory of mind and autism. In S. Baron- measures. Journal of Child Psychology and
Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen Psychiatry, 27(5), 657–669.
(Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspec- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T.
tives from autism (pp. 450–464). Oxford, En- (1987). Nonverbal communication and play
gland: Oxford University Press. correlates of language development in autistic
McConnell, S. R. (2002). Interventions to facilitate children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
social interaction for young children with Disorders, 17, 349–364.
autism: Review of available research and rec- Mundy, P., & Stella, J. (2000). Joint attention, so-
ommendations for educational intervention cial orienting, and nonverbal communication
and future research. Journal of Autism and De- in autism. In A. M. Wetherby & B. M. Prizant
velopmental Disorders, 32, 351–372. (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders: A transac-
McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. tional developmental perspective (pp. 55–77).
(1993). Executive function and social commu- Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
nication deficits in young autistic children. Ohta, M., Nagai, Y., Hara, H., & Sasaki, M. (1987).
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Parental perception of behavioral symptoms in
34, 563–578. Japanese autistic children. Journal of Autism
McGee, G. G., Feldman, R. S., & Chernin, L. and Developmental Disorders, 17, 549–564.
(1991). A comparison of emotional facial dis- Olson, G., & Sherman, T. (1983). Attention, learn-
play by children with autism and typical ing, and memory in infants. In P. H. Mussen
preschoolers. Journal of Early Intervention, (Series Ed.), & M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos
15(3), 237–245. (Volume Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Meltzoff, A., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of im- Infancy and developmental psychobiology (Vol.
itation in understanding persons and develop- 2, pp. 1001–1080). New York: Wiley.
ing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Ornitz, E. M., Guthrie, D., & Farley, A. H. (1977).
Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under- Early development of autistic children. Jour-
standing other minds: Perspectives from autism nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7,
(pp. 333–366). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- 207–229.
versity Press. Orsmond, G. I., Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M.
Mills, M., & Melhuish, E. (1974). Recognition of (2004). Peer relationships and social and
mother’s voice in early infancy. Nature, 252, recreational activities among adolescents and
123–124. adults with autism. Journal of Autism and De-
Moser-Richters, M., & Volkmar, F. R. (1996). Re- velopmental Disorders, 34(3), 245–256.
active attachment disorder. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni-
Child and adolescent psychiatry: A comprehen- tion of children with autism: A study of lst
sive textbook (2nd ed., pp. 498–501). Balti- birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism
more: Williams & Wilkins. and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.
Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A.
Local and global processing of music in high- (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants
functioning persons with autism: Beyond with autism spectrum disorder versus mental
central coherence? Journal of Child Psychol- retardation. Developmental Psychopathology,
ogy, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(8), 14(2), 239–251.
1057–1065. Ozonoff, S. (1997). Causal mechanisms of autism:
Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1994). A pre- Unifying perspectives from an information-
liminary manual for the abridged Early Social processing framework. In D. J. Cohen & F. R.
Communication Scales. Miami, FL: Author. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of Autism and Per-
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A lon- vasive Developmental Disorders (2nd ed.,
gitudinal study of joint attention and language pp. 868–879). New York: Wiley.
disorders in autistic children. Journal of Autism Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990).
and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–123. Are there specific emotion perception deficits
332 Development and Behavior

in young autistic children? Journal of Child atric disorders. Journal of Child and Adoles-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 343–361. cent Psychiatry, 30, 483–488.
Parks, S. L. (1983). The assessment of autistic chil- Rogers, S., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1993).
dren: A selective review of available instru- Developmental aspects of attachment behavior
ments. Journal of Autism and Developmental in young children with pervasive developmen-
Disorders, 13, 255–267. tal disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Paul, R., Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. Psychiatry, 32, 1274–1282.
(2004). The development of preverbal commu- Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R.
nication in toddlers with autism spectrum dis- (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychi-
orders. Paper presented at the annual meeting atric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal
of the American Psychological Association, of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
Honolulu, Hawaii, August, 2004. 24, 465–473.
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van
functions and developmental psychopathology. Ijzendoorn, M. H., & van Berckelaer-Onnes,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, I. A. (2004). Journal of Child Psychology and
37(1), 51–87. Psychiatry, 45(6), 1123–1134.
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). Cogni-
The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evi- tive underpinnings of pretend play in autism.
dence from normal infants and children with Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
autism or mental handicap. Development and ders, 33(3), 289–302.
Psychopathology, 4(3), 375–383. Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to
Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams and imitation in adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2,
childhood. New York: Norton. 435–450.
Povinelli, D. (1993). Reconstructing the evolution Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In
of mind. American Psychologist, 48, 493–509. M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A
Prior, M. R. (1979). Cognitive abilities and disabil- reappraisal of concepts and treatment. New
ities in infantile autism: A review. Journal of York: Plenum Press.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 357–380. Rutter, M. (1981). Maternal deprivation reassessed
Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, com- (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin.
munication, and the use of symbols in normal Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the patho-
and autistic children. Journal of Autism and genesis of autism. Journal of Child Psychology
Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221. and Psychiatry, 24, 513–531.
Rimland, B. (1964). Infantile autism. New York: Schaffer, R. (1984). The child’s entry into a social
Appleton, Century Crofts. world. London: Academic Press.
Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B., & Geffken, G. R. Schopler, E., & Mesibov, G. (Eds.). (1983). Autism
(1991). A comparative evaluation of adaptive in Adolescents and Adults. New York: Plenum
behavior in children and adolescents with Press.
autism, Down Syndrome, and normal develop- Seifer, R., & Schiller, M. (1995). The role of par-
ment. Journal of Autism and Developmental enting sensitivity, infant temperament, and
Disabilities, 21, 187–196. dyadic interaction in attachment theory and
Rogers, S. (2000). Interventions that facilitate so- assessment. Monographs of the Society for Re-
cialization in children with autism. Journal of search in Child Development, 60(Serial no.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 244), 146–174.
399–409. Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Shattuck, P.,
Rogers, S. (2004). Developmental regression in Orsmond, G. I., Swe, A., & Lord, C. (2003).
autism spectrum disorders. Mental Retarda- Changes in the symptoms of autism in adoles-
tion and Developmental Disabilities Research cence and adulthood. Journal of Autism and
Reviews, 10, 139–143. Developmental Disorders, 33, 565–581.
Rogers, S., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T.
Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in (2004). Ref lexive orienting in response to eye
toddlers with autism and those with other de- gaze and an arrow in children with and with-
velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy- out autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
chology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Psychiatry, 45(3), 445–458.
44(5), 763–781. Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Oller, D., & Steffens,
Rogers, S., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1991). M. (2000). Vocal atypicalities of preverbal
A comparative study of attachment behavior in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel-
young children with autism or other psychi- opmental Disorders, 30(4), 345–354.
Social Development in Autism 333

Sherman, M., Shapiro, T., & Glassman, M. (1983). dren. Journal of the American Academy of
Play and language in developmentally disor- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(6),
dered preschoolers: A new approach to classi- 836–838.
fication. Journal of the American Academy of Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984).
Child Psychiatry, 22, 511–524. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines,
Siegel, B., Pliner, C., Eschler, J., & Elliott, G. R. Minnesota: American Guidance Service.
(1988). How children with autism are diag- Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L. J., Dimiceli, S., Myers,
nosed: Difficulties in identification of chil- R. M., & Risch, N. (2002). Behavioral pheno-
dren with multiple developmental delays. typic variation in autism multiplex families:
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, Evidence for a continuous severity gradient.
199–204. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114(2),
Siegel, B., Vukicevic, J., Elliott, G. R., & Kraemer, 129–136.
H. C. (1989). The use of signal detection the- Spitz, R. (1965). The first year of life. New York:
ory to assess DSM-III-R criteria. Journal of International Universities Press.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Stern, D. (1987). The interpersonal world of the
Psychiatry, 28, 542–548. human infant. New York: Basic Books.
Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yir- Stone, W. L., & Caro-Martinez, L. (1990). Natural-
miya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative istic observations of spontaneous communica-
emotions of others by autistic, mentally re- tion in autistic children. Journal of Autism and
tarded, and normal children. Child Develop- Developmental Disorders, 20, 437–454.
ment, 63, 796–807. Stone, W. L., Hoffman, E. L., Lewis, S. E., & Ous-
Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attach- ley, O. Y. (1994). Early recognition of autism:
ments in autistic children. Journal of Child Parental reports vs. clinical observation.
Psychiatry, 28, 74–81. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medi-
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. cine, 148(2), 174–179.
(1986). Social interactions of autistic, men- Stone, W. L., & Lemanek, K. L. (1990). Parental
tally retarded, and normal children and their report of social behaviors in autistic
caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and preschoolers. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Psychiatry, 27, 647–656. mental Disorders, 20(4), 513–522.
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and Stone, W. L., Lemanek, K. L., Fishel, P. T., Fer-
change in the social competence of children mandez, M. C., & Altemeier, W. A. (1990).
with autism, Down syndrome, and develop- Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of
mental delays. Monographs of the Society for autism in young children. Pediatrics, 86,
Research in Child Development, 64(1, Serial 267–272.
No. 256). Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Hepburn, S. L., Hogan,
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984a). Attachment be- K. L., & Brown, C. S. (1999). Patterns of
haviors in autistic children. Journal of Autism adaptive behavior in very young children with
and Developmental Disorders, 14, 231–244. autism. American Journal of Mental Retarda-
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984b). Cognitive tion, 104(2), 187–199.
and language skills in autistic, mentally re- Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. (1995,
tarded, and normal children. Developmental March). A comparison of elicited imitation in
Psychology, 20, 293–302. young children with autism and developmental
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of delay. Poster session presented at the annual
parents of children with autism predict the Gatlinburg Conference on Research and The-
subsequent development of their children’s ory in Mental Retardation and Developmental
communication. Journal of Autism and Devel- Disabilities.
opmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89. Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D.
Singer, J. (1996). Cognitive and affective implica- (1997). Motor imitation in young children
tions of imaginative play in childhood. In M. with autism: What’s the object? Journal of Ab-
Lewis (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: A normal Child Psychology, 25, 475–485.
comprehensive textbook (2nd ed., pp. 202–210). Stone, W. L., & Rosenbaum, J. L. (1988). A com-
Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. parison of teacher and parent views of autism.
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
action in autism: A critical review. Psycholog- ders, 18, 403–414.
ical Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273. Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L., Wilson,
Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., & Shapiro, T. (1987). F., Archer, L., & Ryerse C. (2000). Two-year
Expression of emotion in young autistic chil- outcome of preschool children with autism or
334 Development and Behavior

Asperger’s syndrome. American Journal of trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. Ameri-
Psychiatry, 157(12), 1980–1987. can Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1361–1367.
Terpstra, J. E., Higgins, K., & Pierce, T. (2002). Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T.,
Can I play? Classroom-based interventions for & Klin, A. J. (2004). Autism and Pervasive
teaching play skills to children with autism. Developmental Disorders. Journal of Child
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 135–170.
Disabilities, 17. Volkmar, F. R., & Mayes, L. C. (1990). Gaze be-
Teunisse, J.-P., & de Gelder, B. (1994). Do autistics havior in autism. Developmental Psychopathol-
have a generalized face processing deficit? In- ogy, 2, 61–70.
ternational Journal of Neuroscience, 77(1–2), Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. A., Goudreau, D., Cic-
1–10. chetti, D. V., Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987).
Thompson, R. A. (1996). Attachment theory and Social deficits in autism: An operational ap-
research. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and adoles- proach using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
cent psychiatry: A comprehensive textbook Scales. Journal of the American Academy of
(2nd ed., pp. 126–133). Baltimore: Williams Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 156–161.
& Wilkins. Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Rende, R. D., &
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cohen, D. J. (1989). Facial perception in
Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sci- autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
ences, 16, 495–552. chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 30(4), 591–598.
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant inter- Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol forma-
subjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical ap- tion. New York: Wiley.
plications. Journal of Child Psychology, Wetherby, A. M., Prizant, B. M., & Hutchinson, T.
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(1), 3–48. (1998). Communicative, social-affective, and
Volkmar, F. R. (1987a). Diagnostic issues in the symbolic profiles of young children with
pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of autism and pervasive developmental disorder.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(3), American Journal of Speech-Language Pathol-
365–369. ogy, 7, 79–91.
Volkmar, F. R. (1987b). Social development. In Wing, L. (1976) Early childhood autism. New York:
D. Cohen & A. Donnellan (Eds.), Handbook of Pergamon Press.
autism and pervasive developmental disorders Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
(p. 4160). New York: Wiley. of social interaction and associated abnormal-
Volkmar, F. R., Carter, A., Sparrow, S. S., & Cic- ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica-
chetti, D. V. (1993). Quantifying social devel- tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
opment in autism. Journal of Child and Disorders, 9, 11–29.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 627–632. Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S. R., & Brierley, L. M.
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). A first per- (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally re-
son account of the experience of infantile tarded and in autistic children. Journal of
autism by Tony W. Journal of Autism and De- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(2),
velopmental Disorders, 15, 47–54. 167–178.
Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., & Paul, R. (1986). An World Health Organization. (1993). International
evaluation of DSM-III criteria for infantile Classification of Diseases (ICD-10, 10th edi-
autism. Journal of the American Academy of tion). Diagnostic Criteria for Research.
Child Psychiatry, 25, 190–197. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Schultz, R., Chawarska, Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P.
K., & Jones, W. (2003). The social brain in (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autistic,
autism. In M. Brune, H. Ribbert, & Wulf mentally retarded, and normal children. Jour-
Schienfenhovel (Eds.), The social brain: Evo- nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,
lution and pathology (pp. 167–196). New 725–735.
York: Wiley.
Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P.,
Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field
CHAPTER 12

Language and Communication in Autism

HELEN TAGER-FLUSBERG, RHEA PAUL, AND CATHERINE LORD

Knowledge about human communication is cen- reference to language delay at all (Cohen, Sud-
tral to theory and clinical practice in the field halter, Landon-Jimenez, & Keogh, 1993; Lord,
of autism. Milestones in language and commu- Storoschuk, Rutter, & Pickles, 1993; Siegel, Vu-
nication play major roles at almost every point kicevic, Elliott, & Kraemer, 1989). Although
in development in understanding autism. Most expressive language level at age 5 was an impor-
parents of autistic children first begin to be tant discriminator of higher versus lower func-
concerned that something is not quite right in tioning older children and adults with autism
their child’s development because of early de- (Rutter, 1970), simple characterization of lan-
lays or regressions in the development of speech guage history did not add predictive power
(Short & Schopler, 1988). Functional language for outcome within a high-functioning group
use by school age has been shown to be related of adults (Howlin, Goode, Hutton, & Rutter,
to better long-term outcomes in autism 2004). Asperger syndrome (AS) is an autism
(DeMyer et al., 1973; Paul & Cohen, 1984a). spectrum disorder (ASD) characterized by lack
Fluency and flexibility of expressive language of general delays in language and cognition but
are dimensions underlying the distinction be- by marked social deficits. Its existence suggests
tween “ high-functioning” and “low function- that, even though abnormalities in communica-
ing” autism in school age or adolescence. A tion are a core feature of pervasive develop-
history of language delay can be particularly mental disorders, slower language acquisition is
crucial in differentiating autism from other not necessary or sufficient for a diagnosis
psychiatric disorders in high-functioning adults within the spectrum of disorders associated
(Lord & Venter, 1992). with autism.
Even though autism is often first recognized In addition, evidence from numerous sources
because of slow or unusual patterns of speech suggests that the social and linguistic environ-
development, many early aspects of the lan- ments of autistic children, most of whom have
guage deficit associated with it overlap with active, loving, and determined parents and
other disorders (Beitchman & Inglis, 1991; teachers, can be quite different from those of
Bishop & Adams, 1989). Thus, though skill in other children. Thus, initial deficits in language
language is important to the functioning of peo- acquisition and in social or cognitive factors af-
ple with autism, delays in expressive language fecting language may be compounded by expe-
in the early preschool years are not specific riential differences (Konstantareas, Zajdemann,
to autism (Cantwell, Baker, & Mattison, 1980). Homatidis, & McCabe, 1988; Siller & Sigman,
When there is a good description of a child’s 2002). The root of this difference is thought to
early social history and use of objects, the be the limited nature of the social and linguis-
diagnosis of autism can often be made without tic opportunities that these youngsters provide

335
336 Development and Behavior

to others (Doussard-Roosevelt, Joe, Bazhen- exist within both the speaking and nonspeaking
ova, & Porges, 2003; Lord, Merrin, Vest, & autistic populations.
Kelly, 1983).
The history of autism has included wax- THE STUDY OF
ing and waning of interest in language and LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
communication, from interpreting language IN TYPICAL POPULATIONS
abnormalities as secondary to deficits in so-
cial-emotional functioning (Kanner, 1943), to In order to provide a context in which we can
the view that autism impairments are the re- evaluate the impairments in language and com-
sult of primary linguistic disorder (Rutter, munication that characterize autism spectrum
1970), to an exclusive focus on pragmatic im- disorders, we begin with a brief overview of
pairments (Baltaxe, 1977), to interest in using language acquisition in typically developing
language to study other behaviors, particularly children.
higher order cognitive abilities, such as theory
of mind (Baron-Cohen, 1993). It is now recog- Early Communicative Intent
nized that language in autism is extremely
variable and that there are likely to be sub- Parents often recognize the absence of early
groups of individuals within the autism spec- communication in their young children with
trum that have distinct language profiles, some autism sometime during the second year, when
of which are similar to those found in other de- the majority of children the same age begin to
velopmental language disorders. have established vocabularies of numerous
Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (2003) identified words (Short & Schopler, 1988). However, non-
two language phenotypes among verbal children handicapped infants show communicative be-
with autism: children with normal linguistic haviors even from the first weeks and months of
abilities (phonological skills, vocabulary, syn- life, including recognizing their mothers’
tax, and morphology) and children with autism voice, synchronizing their patterns of eye gaze,
and impaired language that is similar to the movements, facial expressions of affect, as well
phenotype found in specific language impair- as vocal turn taking (Fernald, 1992).
ment. There may also be other subgroups on the Infants typically exhibit a variety of com-
autism spectrum that reflect different kinds of municative behaviors by the end of their first
language disorder. For example, a significant year that, to a knowing observer, are not usu-
number of children with autism never acquire ally seen in autism. These nonverbal communi-
speech. It is unlikely that all these children re- cation patterns have been found to express the
main mute for the same reason, especially since same intentions for which words will be used
recent reports suggest that the proportion of in the coming months, such as requesting ob-
nonspeakers within the autistic population is jects, rejecting offered actions, calling atten-
decreasing as early intervention becomes more tion to objects or events, and commenting on
prevalent (Goldstein, 2002). One potential sub- their appearance (Bates, 1976; Carpenter,
group within nonspeakers, for instance, may ex- Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). These intents are
perience verbal apraxia or apraxia of speech, a expressed first with simple gestures, such as
neuromotor deficit that affects the ability to reaching to indicate a request or pushing away
produce speech sounds, sound sequences, and to indicate rejection, then by more complex
prosodic features (Darley, Aronson, & Brown, gestures, such as pointing to request or shak-
1975). If this subgroup exists, however, it is ing the head to mean “no,” and then gradually
likely to account for a small minority of non- accompanied by and, in some cases, replaced
speakers with ASD (Rogers, 2004). Since little by vocalization and speech (Acredolo &
is known about language capacities in nonspeak- Goodwyn, 1988; Adamson & Bakeman, 1991;
ing children with autism, due to a dearth of Bloom, 1993).
communication research on these children with- Another achievement that normally occurs
out functional language, the causes of failure to toward the end of the first year is the begin-
acquire speech are primarily speculation at this ning of the understanding of words. At first,
time. Nonetheless, it is likely that subgroups a few words associated with games such as
Language and Communication in Autism 337

pat-a-cake or so big will be recognized. In- that happens to be present (Baldwin, 1991),
fants gradually become more active respon- suggesting that they can now understand the in-
ders to these routines (Bruner, 1975). By 12 tentions of others within language contexts.
months, merely saying the words (“Let’s play Similar findings for learning words to describe
pat-a-cake!” or “Show me your nose”) in a fa- actions have been reported for 2-year-olds
miliar context will often elicit a spontaneous (Tomasello & Kruger, 1992).
action, such as clapping or touching the nose, Prior to age 2, most children begin combin-
from the child. ing words to form two-word “ telegraphic” sen-
tences (Brown, 1973), encoding a small set of
First Words meanings. Children talk about objects by nam-
ing them and by discussing their locations or at-
Conventional use of language begins around 12 tributes, who owns them, and who is doing
months (see Table 12.1), when toddlers usually things to them. They also talk about other peo-
say their first recognizable words. At this age, ple, their actions, their locations, their own ac-
children also show clear evidence of under- tions on objects, and so forth. Objects, people,
standing some words or even simple phrases, actions, and their interrelationships preoccupy
responding appropriately to specific words the young typically developing child. Thus,
outside the context of routine games (Hutten- early language development, from gestures to
locher, 1974; Tomasello & Kruger, 1992). Dur- single words to beginning sentences, is in many
ing the 12- to 18-month period, there is a ways a remarkably organized process that re-
gradual increase in both receptive and expres- flects both how young children think about the
sive vocabulary. The words children learn in world (e.g., recognition of the coming and going
this period name objects and people, usually of things and people) and what is important
those on which the child acts (e.g., daddy, to them (e.g., things that they can act on, inter-
mommy, cookie, ball) and describe relation- esting events such as going outside or wiping
ships among objects (e.g., “all gone,” “more”; up a spill). Individual differences exist among
Fenson et al., 1994). Children also learn social typically developing children, but language ac-
words to be used in rituals such as greetings. quisition is not a random process. There are
Much like early gestures, first words are often generally clear links between forms (i.e., ges-
used to express ideas, such as appearance ture, words, syntax) and functions (e.g., why the
(“Uh-oh”), disappearance (“All gone”), and re- child is trying to communicate) over time.
currence (“More”), related to the child’s devel- Toddlers often appear to understand every-
oping notions of object permanence (Bloom & thing they hear; however, studies of early lan-
Lahey, 1978; Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1987). guage comprehension in highly structured
By the age of 18 months, expressive vocabu- settings have suggested that young children do
lary size reaches an average of about 50 to 100 not understand many more words than they are
words (Fenson et al., 1994; Nelson, 1973), and able to say (Bloom, 1974). When parents are
the “ word explosion” begins. This period may asked to report the kinds of words and instruc-
be punctuated by many requests from children tions that their young children are able to
for adults to label things in the world around understand, they typically give much higher
them, and words are now learned very quickly, estimates than is observed during formal test-
often after only a single exposure without any ing. Using a standard questionnaire (Fenson
explicit instruction. This stage marks an impor- et al., 1994), parents estimated that their 8-
tant turning point as children are no longer month-olds understood an average of 6 phrases
learning via association; instead, they under- and about 20 words, increasing to an average
stand the referential nature of words (Nazzi & of 23 phrases and 169 words by 16 months.
Bertoninci, 2003) and are able now to use words Comprehension in ordinary situations may be
to get new information about the world (Halli- achieved by a variety of nonlinguistic strate-
day, 1975). By 16 to 19 months, infants are able gies that allow children to respond to what
to use nonverbal cues, such as an adult’s eye their parents say, when in fact they are re-
gaze, to make fine distinctions between an ob- sponding to what their parents do or what they
ject that an adult is naming and another object know about the way things usually happen
338
TABLE 12.1 A Summary of Milestones in Typical Language Development

12 to 15 months 18 months 24 to 36 months 3 to 4 years 4 to 7 years

Semantics Average expressive vocabu- Average expressive vocabu- Average expressive vocabu- Average expressive vocabu- Average expressive vocabu-
lary size at 15 months: lary size at 18 months: lary size at 24 months: lary size at 3 years: lary size at 6 years:
10 words 100 words ( ±105) 300 words ( ±75) 900 words 2,500 words
Average receptive vocabu- Average receptive vocabu- Average receptive vocabu- Comprehension strategies Average receptive vocabu-
lary size at 15 months: lary size at 18 months: lary size at 24 months: include supplying most lary size at 6 years:
50 words 300 words 900 words probable missing informa- 8,000 words
Comprehension strategies Comprehension strategies Comprehension strategies tion in answer to difficult Comprehension strategies
include attending to objects include acting on objects in include interpreting sen- questions include overreliance on
named, and doing what is the way mentioned, inter- tences according to knowl- word order to process sen-
usually done preting sentences as re- edge of probable events tences that use unusual word
quests for child action order, such as passives

Syntax First productions are single- Average age of first word Average MLU at 24 months: Average MLU at 4 years: Average MLU at 5 years:
word holophrases; one word combinations: 18 months 1.92 ( ±0.5) 4.4 ( ±0.9) 5.6 ( ±1.2)
carries the force of a whole (normal range: 14 to 24 Average MLU at 30 months: Grammatical morphemes Use of complex sentences
sentence months) 2.54 ( ±0.6) become more consistent increases from less than
First word combinations ex- Average MLU at 36 months: Mature forms of negatives 10% to more than 20% of
press basic semantic rela- 3.16 ( ±0.7) and questions develop all utterances
tions with consistent word
order

Phonology Most productions have CV Back stops, fricatives, and 9 to 10 different consonants Most sounds are produced Almost all sounds are pro-
or CVCV (consonant glides are added to the con- are used in initial position; correctly duced correctly
vowel /consonant vowel con- sonant inventory 5 to 6 in final; stops at all Consonant blends are used Phonological processes are
sonant vowel combinations, CVC syllable shapes begin places of articulation are no longer used; a few distor-
Some phonological simplifi-
e.g., “ ba” or “mama”) form to be used used; liquids appear tions on difficult sounds
cation processes may persist
Front stops and nasals are 50% of consonants are pro- Two-syllable words and ini- (/s/, / l /, /r/) may persist
Speech is nearly 100%
most frequent consonants duced correctly tial consonant clusters are Phonological analysis skills
intelligible
used by a majority of are learned for reading and
children spelling
70% of consonants are
correct; speech is 50%
intelligible
Pragmatics Average rate of communica- Average rate of communica- Average rate of communica- Talk about past and future Language is used to predict,
tions: 1 per minute tions: 2 per minute tions: 5 per minute events increases reason, negotiate
Requests and comments are Requests and comments are Requests and comments are More options for politeness
used; communication is ac- used; words predominate; used; children begin to ask are acquired
complished by combining gestural /vocal communica- questions and convey new New communicative func-
gestures with speechlike tion decreases information; word combina- tions (projecting, narrating,
vocalizations tions predominate imagining, etc.) are
expressed

Play Conventional, functional Symbolic play using self as Pretend play involving Sequences of events are Fantasy themes are played
play actor others and using multiple played out (preparing food, out
schemes setting table, eating) Child or doll can take
Child engages in dialogues, multiple roles
talking for all characters Elaboration of planning and
narrative story lines in-
cluded in play

MLU = mean length of utterance


CV = consonant vowel
CVCV = consonant vowel consonant vowel

339
340 Development and Behavior

(Chapman, 1978). Such strategies include a ideas within one utterance (“I’ll go get it if
child looking at whatever his or her parent is you give me a bite of your candy”) free the
looking at (“See the balloon!”), doing what- child’s language from dependence on nonlin-
ever is usually done in this situation (“Brush guistic contexts for interpretation. Whereas
your hair ”), and interpreting sentences as a an adult had to use knowledge of the child
request for the child to do something. Few and the situation to interpret “Daddy shoe”
parents truly test their children’s language (The shoe that belongs to Daddy? Daddy put
comprehension by asking them to do things on the shoe?), the morphologically marked
completely out of context (e.g., asking a child “Daddy’s shoe” is unambiguous and inter-
to go get Mommy’s keys from the bedroom pretable by anyone.
during a family meal). In addition to changing their use of gram-
The period of 18 to 24 months is also a time matical form, children between 3 and 5 years
of important developments in conversational of age also change the ideas that they express
ability. Children now begin to understand the in their sentences. Earlier utterances generally
“conversational obligation” to answer speech described actions and objects that were imme-
with speech (Chapman, 1981). They reliably diately present. During later preschool years,
ask and answer routine questions (“Where’s the sentence content expands to allow for refer-
doggy?” “What’s this?” “What does the cow ence to events that are remote in time and
say?”) and can now genuinely take their own space. Children begin to use their language in
part in a back-and-forth linguistic exchange. more diverse ways (Dore, 1978) to include
imaginative, nonliteral, interpretive, and logi-
The Acquisition of Linguistic Structures cal functions.
At this time, a variety of more advanced
The preschool period (from 2 to 5 years) is the conversational and other discourse skills also
time during which the child’s language evolves emerge and become refined. Children increase
from simple telegraphic utterances to fully their ability to maintain and add new informa-
grammatical forms. In addition to rapidly ac- tion to the conversational topic, to clarify and
quiring new vocabulary, the child goes through request clarification of misunderstood utter-
a process of approximating more and more ances, to make their requests or comments
closely the grammar of the language spoken using polite or indirect forms, and to choose
in the home. There is evidence of the child’s the appropriate speech style on the basis of the
active role as a hypothesis-generator in the speaker’s role and the listener’s status (Bates,
frequent occurrence of overgeneralized forms, 1976). Children also begin to engage in differ-
such as “goed,” “comed,” and “mouses” (Caz- ent types of discourse including storytelling,
den, 1968; Pinker, 1999). These errors are recounting events, and personal narratives, all
taken as evidence that the child is indeed ac- of which follow cultural conventions for these
quiring a rule-governed system, rather than diverse genres of linguistic reporting.
learning these inflections by imitation or on a
word-by-word basis. The Elaboration of Language
As the child’s grammar becomes more
complex, sentence length increases (Brown, Although children have acquired most of the
1973; Loban, 1976; Miller & Chapman, 1981), sentence structure of their language by age 5,
and children begin to use a variety of sentence syntactic development continues into the school
forms including statements, negation, and years as children learn devices for elaborating
questions. As structures in simple sentences their utterances, expressing coreference rela-
approach the adult model, complex sentences tions using pronouns (e.g., “When Mom wakes
using embedded clauses (“Whoever wins can up, she’ll help me dress”), and condensing more
go first ”) and conjoined clauses (“ Then it information into each sentence by increasing
broke and we didn’t have it any more”) emerge the proportion of dependent clauses (Loban,
(Paul, Chapman, & Wanska, 1980). The abili- 1976). Children also gradually learn to use and
ties to encode ideas grammatically (“Daddy’s to comprehend the more complex, optional sen-
shoe” versus “Daddy shoe”) and to relate tence types in their language, such as passives
Language and Communication in Autism 341

(“ The boy was hit by the car ”; Lempert, 1978). who also has only five different words but uses
They learn to use syntactic cues not only to de- them to express a range of different meanings
code semantic relations within sentences but (as described earlier) marked in a number of
also to identify the connections between sen- different ways (gesture, words, simple syntax,
tence elements and those given previously in intonation) throughout each day. There is vari-
the discourse (Paul, 1985). Semantic and con- ability within the normal range in the develop-
versational abilities continue to develop during ment of expressive language (Rutter & Lord,
the school years. Vocabulary size is still in- 1987) though, on close inspection, individual
creasing, and new words are now being learned differences within the normal range do not re-
from reading as well as from conversation. semble the kinds of patterns of communication
School-age children gradually acquire the abil- delay seen in autism. It is important that
ity to communicate with precision, to take the recognition of individual differences does not
listener’s viewpoint into account in formulating lead to underestimating communication delays
an utterance (Asher, 1978), and to tell more usually seen in autism.
complex, well-structured narratives.
COMMUNICATION AND
Issues from the Study of Language DEVELOPMENT IN AUTISM
Development in Typical Children
In this section, we explore the unique charac-
Several issues arise in determining how to fit teristics of the development of language in
the different patterns of language development children with autism, in comparison to typical
seen in autism into models of normal language development, and the corresponding implica-
acquisition. One source of confusion to par- tions for language research.
ents and professionals is the question of con-
sistency. Both children with autism and, on Course and Developmental Change
occasion, typically developing children may
use a new word for a few days but then fail to As noted earlier, there is enormous variation in
continue to use this word in appropriate con- the timing and patterns of acquisition of lan-
texts. Are these “real” words? Does the child guage among children with autism. A minority
have them stored somewhere in the brain to be of children, usually diagnosed with AS, do not
used if sufficiently motivated? Two questions show any significant delays in the onset of lan-
arise: (1) How do we set standards for what guage milestones. In contrast, most individuals
is a reasonable level of consistency? (2) How with autism begin to speak late and develop
broad do the contexts have to be in which speech at a significantly slower rate than typi-
we can reliably expect a behavior? For exam- cally developing children (Le Couteur, Bailey,
ple, we might expect a 10-month-old to under- Rutter, & Gottesman, 1989). Because autism is
stand “ bubbles” only in the bathtub, but by 18 not usually diagnosed until age 3 or 4, there is
months, the child should be able to say and un- relatively little information about language in
derstand the same word in a variety of differ- very young children with autism. Various ret-
ent situations. The development of these sorts rospective studies using parent report and
of standards may be particularly helpful for videotapes collected during infancy and the
parents and primary care professionals trying toddler years suggest that by the second year of
to evaluate the seriousness of a possible com- life, the communication of most children with
munication delay in a very young child. autism is different from other children
Another source of confusion is that if (Dahlgren & Gillberg, 1989). Several studies
a person does not have a reasonable level of have found that, as early as 1 year of age, very
knowledge about the breadth and depth of young children with autism are less responsive
typical language development, it is fairly easy to their names or to someone speaking com-
to fail to notice its absence in autism. For ex- pared to other children (Lord, 1995; Osterling
ample, a child who occasionally says five & Dawson, 1994), and they are less responsive
words but does so without clear communica- to the sound of their mother’s voice (Klin,
tive intent is very different from another child 1991). In one study (Lord, Pickles, DiLavore,
342 Development and Behavior

& Shulman, 1996), 2-year-old children judged To gauge the developmental timing of lan-
very likely to have autism had mean expressive guage milestones for children with autism, we
and receptive language ages of less than 9 are generally dependent on parental report.
months, in contrast to other skills falling be- Most diagnostic interviews, such as the Autism
tween 16 and 21 months. Not only was their Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Rutter,
language severely delayed at 2, but also their Le Couteur, & Lord, 2003), include questions
expressive skills continued to develop at a about the age of first words and phrases. (See
slower rate through age 5 compared to Lord et al., 2004, for examples of regression
nonautistic children with developmental delays questions from a modified ADI for toddlers.)
at similar nonverbal levels. Lord and her colleagues have found that re-
About 25% of children with autism are peated administrations of the ADI-R revealed
described by their parents as having some that the ages that parents reported these lan-
words at 12 or 18 months and then losing them guage milestones increased with the age of the
(Kurita, 1985). A recent large-scale system- child at the time of the interview (Lord, Risi,
atic longitudinal study of toddlers by Lord, & Pickles, 2004; Taylor, 2004). This system-
Shulman, and DiLavore (2004) found that this atic “ telescoping” means that parents of older
kind of “language regression” after a pattern children with autism are more likely to recall
of normal language onset was unique to autism their children’s language as being even more
and not found among children with other de- delayed than they did when their children were
velopmental delays. Generally, the regression younger.
is a gradual process in which the children do Both within and across categories of chil-
not learn new words and fail to engage in com- dren with ASDs, there is significant variability
municative routines in which they may have in the rate at which language progresses among
participated before. Language loss occurred those children who do acquire some functional
in these children when they still had relatively language (Lord et al., 2004). In the preschool
small expressive vocabularies and before period and beyond, certain nonverbal skills, es-
the word explosion. Lord and her colleagues pecially the frequency of initiating joint atten-
found that children who experienced loss of tion, and imitation, are strong predictors of
words also lost some social skills, supporting language acquisition for children with autism
the findings from Goldberg and her colleagues (Charman et al., 2003; Rogers, Hepburn, Stack-
(Goldberg et al., 2003), and that similar losses house, & Wehner, 2003; Sigman & Ruskin,
of social skills occurred in a smaller group 1999). There is also a significant correlation
of children with autism who had not yet used between IQ and language outcomes, although
words at the time of loss (Luyster et al., in higher levels of nonverbal IQ are not always as-
press). This phenomenon is quite different sociated with higher level language skills
from the regression that is associated with dis- (Howlin, Goode, Hutton, & Rutter, 2004; Kjel-
integrative disorder (see Chapter 4, this vol- gaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001). Although few
ume), which typically occurs at a later time longitudinal studies of language acquisition
and involves loss of advanced linguistic skills among verbal children with autism have been
and communication to no speech. Though the conducted, the research suggests that during
skills children with autism may have had be- the preschool years, progress within each do-
fore the regression are often minimal, it is still main of language (e.g., vocabulary, syntax)
confusing and heartbreaking for parents to follows similar pathways as has been found
watch their children lose any component of for typically developing children (e.g., Tager-
communicative skill, fleeting though it may Flusberg & Calkins, 1990). Individuals with
have been. Studies have demonstrated only a autism continue to make progress in language
minimal relationship between language regres- and related developmental domains well beyond
sion in autism and later prognosis or outcome, the preschool years. Paul, Cohen, and Caparulo
with children who had regressions having, on (1983), in a longitudinal study of children
average, slightly lower verbal IQ scores at with aphasic and autistic disorders, showed that
school age than children with no history of loss comprehension ability at early ages was related
(Richler et al., in press). to degree of improvement in social relations in
Language and Communication in Autism 343

late adolescence and early adulthood. Paul and rates for ASDs increase, it is not easy to disen-
Cohen (1984a) suggested that both comprehen- tangle improvements in language skills across
sion and expressive abilities continue to im- the autism spectrum from an increase in the
prove in these populations through adolescence diagnosis among higher functioning, more ver-
and adulthood, although expressive skills show bal individuals.
greater rates of improvement. This pattern may
occur because speech is more accessible than Articulation
comprehension and is more often a direct target
of remedial efforts. In another series of follow- Among children with autism who speak, articu-
up studies in Britain, almost all of the partici- lation is often normal or even precocious
pants with autism or developmental language (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Pierce &
disorders showed substantial improvements in Bartolucci, 1977). However, Bartak and col-
formal aspects of language into adulthood leagues (Bartak, Rutter, & Cox, 1975) found
(Cantwell & Baker, 1989). However, the group articulation development to be somewhat slower
with autism, who had serious receptive lan- than normal. These delays were more transient
guage deficits in early childhood, remained in a group of high-functioning boys with autism
more severely language delayed as a whole than in language-level-matched nonautistic
(Rutter, Mawhood, & Howlin, 1992). They had boys with severe receptive-expressive delays in
more severe behavioral limitations compared to middle childhood (Rutter et al., 1992) and may
the nonautistic language-disordered group, who be the result of later onset of language mile-
had a much broader range of outcome, from stones. Still, Shriberg et al. (2001) reported
total independence and good language skill to that one-third of speakers with high-functioning
severe psychiatric disorder and continued ex- autism (HFA) and with AS retained residual
pressive language problems. speech distortion errors on sounds such as /r/,
Some children with autism never acquire / l /, and /s/ into adulthood, whereas the rate of
functional language; many of these children these errors in the general population is 1%.
have very low nonverbal IQ scores. Epidemio- Bartolucci, Pierce, Streiner, and Tolkin-
logical studies indicate that about half the popu- Eppel (1976) showed that phoneme frequency
lation remains nonverbal by middle childhood distribution and the distribution of phonologi-
(Bryson, Clark, & Smith, 1988); however, re- cal error types in a small group of children
cent longitudinal studies of children referred for with autism was similar to that of mentally
possible autism at early ages have suggested that handicapped and typical children matched for
the proportion of children with ASD who do not nonverbal mental age. The less frequent the
use words to speak is less than 20% (Lord et al., phoneme’s use in the language, the greater was
2004). Such a statistic is clearly affected by the number of errors. Phonological perception
variation in who is studied: What age are the among the groups also was similar. These
participants with autism? Are they recruited findings indicate that the developmental tra-
from special education services or clinics or jectory for phonological development in autism
from broader populations? What about the ef- follows the same path as in other groups of
fect of education and treatment? The statistic is children, although a higher rate of distortion
also affected by how useful speech is defined: errors is seen in adult speakers.
Are single words enough? Simple sentences? Two caveats should be noted. First, diffi-
How spontaneous do they have to be? How often culties in articulation are relatively common
do they have to be used? How intelligible must in nonautistic children with intellectual handi-
they be? caps. Thus, the fact that there is no difference
There is some optimism that with more between autistic and IQ-matched children
children receiving earlier diagnoses and thus with mental handicaps does not mean that no
better access to early intensive interventions, children with autism have articulation difficul-
especially for language and communication ties. Second, there are a relatively small num-
skills, the proportion of children with autism ber of autistic children who are identified as
who fail to acquire functional language is high functioning on the basis of nonverbal tests
diminishing. Nevertheless, as the prevalence during preschool but who have extraordinary
344 Development and Behavior

difficulties in producing intelligible speech. Needleman, 1991). Thus, while overall lexical
These children are not likely to be included in knowledge may be a relative strength in autism,
many studies of language because they are rel- the acquisition of words that map onto mental
atively rare. By the time they are 10 or 12 years state concepts may be specifically impaired in
old, fluent language often becomes an implicit this disorder.
criteria for the category of “ high functioning.” Abnormal use of words and phrases has been
Little is known about either the existence or described in autism for many years (Rutter,
phenomenology of this pattern of development. 1970). In samples of high-functioning adoles-
cents and adults, a significant minority has
Word Use been shown to use words with special meanings
(Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, 1985; Volden &
Word use in autism can be observed by asking Lord, 1991) or “metaphorical language” use,
two rather different questions: (1) Do children as Kanner (1946) described this unusual phe-
with autism use and understand words as nomenon. In most cases, these words or phrases
belonging to the same categories as other peo- were modifications of ordinary word roots or
ple? and (2) Is there anything unusual about phrases that produced slightly odd sounding,
how individuals with autism use words? The but comprehensible, terms such as “commend-
answer to both questions is yes. In the first ment ” for praise or “cuts and bluesers” for cuts
case, studies have shown that verbal children and bruises. These terms were not radically
with autism use semantic groupings (e.g., different from those used occasionally by
bird, boat, food) in very similar ways to cate- mentally handicapped or younger, nonhandi-
gorize and to retrieve words (Boucher, 1988; capped children matched on expressive lan-
Minshew & Goldstein, 1993; Tager-Flusberg, guage level, except that they were more
1985). High-functioning children and adoles- frequent in the autistic population. Only sub-
cents with autism can score well on standard- jects with autism produced neologisms or odd
ized vocabulary tests, indicating an unusually phrases for which the root was not fairly obvi-
rich knowledge of words (Fein & Waterhouse, ous, though these, too, were relatively rare
1979; Jarrold, Boucher, & Russell, 1997; (Volden & Lord, 1991). Increased language
Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001) and an ability was associated with increased (propor-
area of relative strength for some individuals tions as well as absolute numbers) peculiarities
with autism. At the same time, Tager-Flusberg and perseveration in individuals with autism.
(1991) found that children with autism often Conversely, in a nonautistic mentally handi-
fail to use their knowledge of words in a nor- capped group, oddities decreased steadily as
mal way to facilitate performance on retrieval expressive language ability improved (Volden
or organizational tasks. & Lord, 1991). Rutter (1987) suggests that
At the same time, it appears that certain these abnormal uses of words may be func-
classes of words may be underrepresented in the tionally similar to the kinds of early word
vocabularies of children with autism. For exam- meaning errors made by young typically de-
ple, Tager-Flusberg (1992) found that the chil- veloping children. It is their persistence in
dren participating in a longitudinal language autism that defines them as abnormal, and
study used hardly any mental state terms, par- they may reflect the fact that children with
ticularly terms for cognitive states (e.g., know, autism are not sensitive, because of their so-
think, remember, pretend). These findings were cial impairments, to the corrective feedback
replicated in research including older children provided by their parents.
with autism (Storoschuk, Lord, & Jaedicke, Pedantic speech and being overly precise in
1995; Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 1994). Other a rather concrete way are also descriptors fre-
studies suggest that children with autism have quently used with individuals with HFA or
particular difficulties understanding social- AS (Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaziuddin, 1992),
emotional terms as measured on vocabulary though these qualities can be very difficult to
tests such as the Peabody Picture Vocabulary quantify. Wing (1981) commented on the lan-
Test (Eskes, Bryson, & McCormick, 1990; Hob- guage of people with AS as having a “ bookish”
son & Lee, 1989; Van Lancker, Cornelius, & quality exemplified by the use of obscure
Language and Communication in Autism 345

words. She considered pedantic speech to be dren or children with mental retardation in the
one of the main clinical features of this disor- mastery of certain grammatical morphemes
der (Burgoine & Wing, 1983). Mayes, Volk- (Bartolucci, Pierce, & Streiner, 1980; Howlin,
mar, Hooks, and Cicchetti (1993) found that 1984). Bartolucci et al. (1980) found that chil-
the presence of peculiar language patterns was dren with autism were more likely to omit cer-
one of the best discriminators of pervasive de- tain morphemes, particularly articles (a, the),
velopmental disorder from language disability. auxiliary and copula verbs, past tense, third-
person present tense, and present progressive.
Syntax and Morphology Tager-Flusberg (1989) also found that children
with autism were significantly less likely to
Relatively few studies have systematically mark past tense than were matched controls
investigated grammatical aspects of language with Down syndrome. Bartolucci and Albers
acquisition in autism. The longitudinal study (1974) compared children with autism to con-
of six autistic boys conducted by Tager- trols in performance on a task designed to
Flusberg and her colleagues found that these elicit production of present progressive -ing
children followed the same developmental and past tense -ed for different verbs. The chil-
path as an age-matched comparison group dren with autism performed well on the present
of children with Down syndrome who were progressive form, as did the controls. The chil-
part of the study and to normally developing dren with autism were, however, significantly
children drawn from the extant literature impaired on the past tense elicitation trials.
(Tager-Flusberg et al., 1990). The children This finding was replicated in a recent study of
with autism and Down syndrome showed sim- over 60 children with autism who were given
ilar growth curves in their Mean Length of Ut- tasks to elicit both the past tense and the third-
terance (MLU), which is usually taken as a person present tense (Roberts, Rice, & Tager-
hallmark measure of grammatical develop- Flusberg, 2004). The sample was divided into
ment. At the same time, in a follow-up study those who had scores within the normal range
using the same language samples, Scarbor- on standardized language tests and those who
ough, Rescorla, Tager-Flusberg, Fowler, and were significantly below the mean. Only those
Sudhalter (1991) compared the relationship with impaired language scores performed
between MLU and scores on a different index poorly on the tense tasks. Across these studies,
of grammatical development, which charts then, marking tense was impaired among chil-
the emergence of a wide range of grammati- dren with autism. Roberts et al. (2004) inter-
cal constructions: the Index of Productive pret their findings as evidence that a subgroup
Syntax (IPSyn). The main findings were that of children with autism have grammatical
at higher MLU levels, MLU tended to signifi- deficits that are similar to those reported
cantly overestimate IPSyn scores for the among children with specific language impair-
subjects with autism, suggesting that for the ment (cf. Rice, 2004).
children with autism, the relatively limited Studies of other sentence forms in sponta-
growth in IPSyn reflects the tendency to make neous language have generally indicated that
use of a narrower range of constructions and children with autism are similar to mental-
to ask fewer questions, which accounts for a age-matched youngsters in terms of the acqui-
significant portion of the IPSyn score. sition of rule-governed syntactic systems
Several studies of English-speaking chil- (Bartak et al., 1975; Pierce & Bartolucci,
dren with autism investigated the acquisition 1977). Children with autism, mental handicap,
of grammatical morphology, based on data or developmental language disorders lag in
from spontaneous speech samples. Some of language development relative to nonverbal
these studies must be interpreted with caution mental age. It seems very likely that syntactic
as they included very small numbers of chil- development in children with autism is more
dren who varied widely in age, mental age, and similar than dissimilar to normal development.
language ability. Two cross-sectional studies It often proceeds at a slower pace and is re-
found differences between children with lated to developmental level more than to
autism and a comparison group of typical chil- chronological age, although it may not keep
346 Development and Behavior

pace with other areas of development (Tager- nicative functions that they found were served
Flusberg, 1981a). by immediate echolalia: turn taking, asser-
Studies of adults with autism (Paul & tions, affirmative answers, requests, rehearsal
Cohen, 1984a) suggest that this development to aid processing, and self-regulation. Delayed
eventually reaches a plateau in at least some echoes can be used communicatively to request
individuals with autism. Adults with autism re-creations of the scenes with which the re-
did significantly more poorly on measures of marks were originally associated, such as a
syntactic production in free speech than adults child saying, “You’re okay” in a sympathetic
with mental handicap matched for nonverbal tone of voice if he falls down. They can serve
IQ. The lags shown by children and adoles- other functions as well. Baltaxe and Simmons
cents with autism are often more severe than (1977) showed that the bedtime soliloquies of
those of other children with comparable delays an 8-year-old autistic child contained frequent
earlier in childhood. In research, these delays examples of delayed echolalia, which they
are often less obvious because children with suggested the child used as a base for
autism who are not delayed on nonverbal analyzing linguistic forms that she was in the
tests are generally grouped with children with process of acquiring, as found among some
autism who are more severely delayed. More- nonautistic children (Weir, 1962).
over, it is now clear that among children with Although echolalia is one of the most classic
autism, there are different subgroups, some of symptoms of autism (Kanner, 1946), not all
whom have impaired language while others children with autism echo, nor is echoing seen
have normal language, as measured on stan- only in autism. Echoing—particularly immedi-
dardized tests (Tager-Flusberg, 2003). The en- ate repetition—occurs in blind children, in chil-
tire autism group is erroneously compared to a dren with other language impairments, in older
more homogeneous control group of nonautis- people with dementia, and, perhaps most impor-
tic children with mental handicap (Lord & tantly, in some normally developing children as
Pickles, 1996). These concerns highlight the well (Yule & Rutter, 1987). McEvoy, Loveland,
need for more studies that are longitudinal in and Landry (1988) found that immediate
design, providing follow-up into late adoles- echolalia was most frequent in children with
cence or adulthood, with a focus on individual autism who had minimal expressive language
variation among participants with ASDs. but was not closely associated with chronologi-
cal or nonverbal mental age. Shapiro (1977) and
Echolalia Carr, Schreibman, and Lovaas (1975) found that
children with autism were most likely to echo
One of the most salient aspects of deviant immediately questions and commands that they
speech in autism is the occurrence of echolalia. did not understand or for which they did not
Echolalia is the repetition, with similar intona- know the appropriate response.
tion, of words or phrases that someone else A substantial minority, but not all, of verbal
has said. It can be immediate; for example, a autistic adolescents and adults are described
child repeats back her teacher’s greeting, “Hi, by their parents as having engaged in delayed
Susie,” exactly as it was said to her. It can be echolalia at some point in their development
delayed, as in the case of a child who ap- (Le Couteur et al., 1989). Echolalia has been
proaches his father and says, “It’s time to tickle offered as evidence of “gestalt ” processing in
you!” as a signal that he wants to be tickled, re- autism (Frith, 1989). Prizant (1983) proposed
peating a phrase he has heard his parents say in that children with autism are especially depen-
the past. dent on the gestalt approach to acquiring lan-
Echolalia was once viewed as an undesir- guage (cf. Peters, 1983) and that this is
able, nonfunctional behavior (Lovaas, 1977). evident in their reliance on echolalia. Tager-
However, other clinicians, beginning with Fay Flusberg and Calkins (1990) investigated
(1969) and elaborated by Prizant and col- whether variations in echolalia were tied to
leagues (see Chapter 36, this Handbook, Vol- differences in the process by which grammar
ume 2), have emphasized that often echolalia was acquired in autism, when compared to
serves specific functions for the child. Prizant language-matched groups of typically develop-
and Duchan (1981) highlighted six commu- ing children and young children with Down
Language and Communication in Autism 347

syndrome. As predicted, the children with autism autism, but they are more common in individu-
at the early stages of language development als with autism than in any other population
produced the most echolalic and formulaic (Le Couteur et al., 1989; Lee, Hobson, &
speech. For all children, echolalia declined Chiat, 1994). It is interesting that Tager-
rapidly over the course of development. To Flusberg (1994) found that among small groups
investigate whether children with autism of young children with autism, all of them went
used echolalia as a means for acquiring new through a stage of reversing pronouns, though
grammatical knowledge, Tager-Flusberg and as they got older, the more linguistically ad-
Calkins compared echolalic and nonecholalic vanced children stopped making these errors.
spontaneous speech drawn from the same The majority of the time, children used pro-
language sample for length of utterances using nouns correctly; reversal errors averaged only
MLU and for the complexity of grammatical 13% of all pronouns produced.
constructions using IPSyn. If imitation is Kanner (1943) originally attributed pronoun
important in the acquisition of grammatical reversals to echolalia. Some examples, such
knowledge, then length and grammatical com- as the child who says, “Carry you!” seem
plexity should be more advanced in echolalic to reflect this relationship. Other accounts
than in spontaneous speech produced at the have considered the linguistic or information-
same developmental point. This hypothesis processing demands in having to shift and mark
was not confirmed for any of the children reference (Rice et al., 1994). Within autism,
in this study. On the contrary, across all lan- difficulty using pronouns is generally viewed as
guage samples, spontaneous utterances were part of a more general difficulty with deixis, the
significantly longer and included more ad- aspect of language that codes shifting reference
vanced grammatical constructions. These find- between the speaker and the listener. For ex-
ings suggest that echolalia is not an important ample, in labeling a person by name (e.g.,
process in facilitating grammatical develop- “James”), the label remains the same without
ment in autism, though it clearly reflects a regard to who is speaking whereas, for pro-
different conversational style and plays an im- nouns, whether James is referred to as “I”
portant role in children’s communication with or “ you” depends on whether he is the speaker
others, especially when they have very limited or the listener during a particular conversation.
linguistic knowledge. Deixis is marked not only by pronouns but
In summary, although immediate and de- also in various ways in different languages. In
layed echolalia are salient features of autistic English, these include various determiners (e.g.,
speech, they are neither synonymous with whether a speaker uses “ this” or “ that ” depend-
nor unique to this syndrome. Although some ing on previous reference or location of an ob-
echolalia in autism may appear to be nonfunc- ject) or the selection of verbs (e.g., “come” and
tional or self-stimulatory, both immediate and “go”) and verb tense.
delayed echolalia can serve communicative Most current interpretations of pronoun er-
purposes for the speaker. rors in autism view them as a reflection of the
difficulties that children with autism have in
Use of Deictic Terms conceptualizing notions of self and other as they
are embedded in shifting discourse role between
Confusion of personal pronouns (e.g., when a speaker and listener (Lee et al., 1994; Tager-
child asks for a drink by saying, “Do you want Flusberg, 1993). Their difficulty understanding
a drink of water?”) is another frequently men- discourse roles is related to impaired social
tioned atypical language behavior associated communicative functioning, specifically con-
with autism. As with other aspects of deviant ceptual perspective-taking (Loveland, 1984),
language, pronoun reversal sometimes occurs and may be related to their broader social
in children with language disorders other than deficits (see Chapter 12, this volume).
autism or in blind children (Fraiberg, 1977),
and it may even be present briefly in the lan- Suprasegmental Aspects of Language
guage of some normally developing children
(Chiat, 1982). As with echolalia, pronoun re- Paralinguistic features such as vocal quality,
versal errors may not occur in all children with intonation, and stress patterns are another
348 Development and Behavior

frequently noted speech characteristic of indi- also been reported (Pronovost et al., 1966).
viduals with ASDs (Rutter et al., 1992). Odd Fay (1969) reported frequent whispering among
intonation patterns associated with autism children who echo.
seem to be one of the most immediately recog- Research on AS suggests that these abnor-
nizable clinical signs of the disorder. However, malities in intonation and prosody are even
defining what constitutes autism-related par- more prevalent for children and adults with AS
alinguistic abnormalities so that clinicians can than for individuals with autism who had ac-
make reliable judgments about them has been quired language. Eisenmajer and his colleagues
quite challenging (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, (Eisenmajer et al., 1996) compared the clinical
1994; Lord et al., 2000; Volkmar et al., 1994), behaviors of children with autism and children
in part, perhaps because of the number of with AS using in-depth interviews conducted
different ways in which language can sound with their parents. The children with AS were
unusual. more likely to be described by their parents as
There are several levels of prosodic func- having an unusual tone of voice such as flat or
tion: grammatical, pragmatic, and affective monotonous quality. The most systematic di-
(Merewether & Alpert, 1990). Grammatical rect investigation of prosodic features in AS
prosody includes cues to the type of utterance was conducted by Shriberg et al. (2001). They
(e.g., questions end with rising pitch) and dif- analyzed speech samples collected during a di-
ferent stress patterns used to distinguish dif- agnostic interview, the Autism Diagnostic Ob-
ferent parts of speech (e.g., marking the word servation Schedule (ADOS), which was
present with stress on the first syllable if used conducted with the adolescent and adult partic-
as a noun). Pragmatic stress may highlight new ipants with autism or AS. The main findings
information or draw the listener’s attention were that about one-third of the participants
to the significance of the message expressed with AS had distorted speech and articulation
(e.g., “Are you the writer of this note?” versus problems, and two-thirds expressed prosodic
“Are you the writer of this note?”). Affective abnormalities at grammatical, pragmatic, or af-
prosody conveys the speaker’s feelings or atti- fective levels. Like Asperger’s case studies
tudes and may include variations in vocal tone (Asperger, 1991), quite a number of the study
and speech rate. Failure to use and appreciate participants had loud, high voices with a nasal
intonational cues, then, will likely not only af- tone. Koning and Magill-Evans (2001) investi-
fect the emotional tone of a verbal exchange gated whether adolescents with AS were able
but also hamper its comprehensibility. to use nonverbal cues, including facial expres-
Intonational peculiarities frequently are as- sion, body gestures, and prosody, to interpret
sociated with ASDs. The most frequently cited the feelings of people acting in videotaped
is monotony (see Fay & Schuler, 1980). These scenes. They found that the adolescents with
patterns were formerly attributed to emotional AS were significantly worse than controls in
states thought to be present (or absent) in autis- interpreting the emotions and relied least on
tic individuals and were originally thought prosodic information. These findings suggest
to reflect flat affect, the failure to express per- that people with AS not only are impaired in
sonality, or repressed anger (see Lord & Rutter, expressive prosody but also have difficulty
1994). Fay and Schuler (1980) also describe comprehending prosodic information ex-
a subset of autistic individuals who used a pressed by others. The reasons behind these
singsong rather than flat pattern. Goldfarb, deficits in suprasegmental features remain ob-
Braunstein, and Lorge (1956) and Pronovost, scure. Frith (1969) showed that like typically
Wakstein, and Wakstein (1966) found unusu- developing children, children with autism re-
ally high fundamental frequency levels in autis- called stressed words better than unstressed
tic speakers. Other voice disorders, such as ones, especially when the stress was placed on
hoarseness, harshness, and hypernasality, have content words. On the other hand, children
been identified (Pronovost et al., 1966). Our with autism seemed less able than typical chil-
own clinical observations detected hyponasal- dren to take advantage of stress cues for mean-
ity in some children with autism. Poor control ing (see also Baltaxe, 1984). Thurber and
of volume, with unexplained fluctuations, has Tager-Flusberg (1993) found autistic children
Language and Communication in Autism 349

produced fewer nongrammatical pauses than behavior of young preverbal children with
controls matched on verbal mental age when autism and a group of comparison children
telling a story from a wordless picture book. with developmental delays (Sheinkopf, Mundy,
There was no difference in grammatical pauses Oller, & Steffens, 2000). Although the children
(i.e., those between phrases). Deviance in into- with autism did not have difficulty with the
nation seems unlikely to be due solely to simple expression of well-formed syllables (i.e., canon-
perceptual or motor deficits. More fundamental ical babbling), they did display significant
aspects of the autistic syndrome reflected in impairments in vocal quality (i.e., atypical
higher level language and communicative be- phonation). Specifically, autistic children pro-
haviors, such as understanding of other persons, duced a greater proportion of syllables with
related social cognitive deficits, and/or ability atypical phonation than did comparison chil-
to plan and execute a complex action, may con- dren. The atypicalities in the vocal behavior of
tribute to how autistic children learn to use in- children with autism were, however, unrelated
tonation and other paralinguistic features. to individual differences in joint attention skill,
There seems no doubt that there is some- suggesting that a multiple process model may
thing different about the way in which the be needed to describe early social-communica-
stream of sound associated with speech is tion impairments in children with autism.
produced in many persons with autism. Ricks Taken together, these findings suggest that
and Wing (1976) carried out one of the first the source of the difference between the vo-
studies in this area. They studied parents’ calizations of the young children with autism
identification of the meaning of the prelinguis- and those of other young, nonverbal children
tic vocalizations of autistic children and found was not just in social intent, but also in a more
that parents of children with autism were un- basic aspect of the form of the vocalization be-
able to understand the preverbal vocalizations ginning very early in development.
of other children with autism, even though
they could understand their own child’s mes- Language Comprehension in Autism
sages. In contrast, parents of typically devel-
oping children could understand vocalizations Most research on the language of individuals
of typical children who were not their own, as with autism centers on their productive capaci-
well as those of their own child. These find- ties. In contrast, less attention has been focused
ings were not replicated in a later study by on their comprehension skills. This is unfortu-
Elliott (1993). nate because early response to language, a
Historically, autistic children have been de- likely precursor to comprehension, is one of the
scribed as babbling less frequently than other strongest indicators of autism in very young
children during early childhood. However, El- children (Dahlgren & Gillberg, 1989; Lord,
liott (1993) found no difference in the fre- 1995). Charman and his colleagues collected
quency with which preverbal, developmentally data on early language development from a large
delayed 2-year-olds, preverbal typically devel- group of preschool-age children with autism
oping 10- to 12-month-olds, and 2-year-olds using a parent report measure: the MacArthur
with autism produced vocalizations in situa- Communicative Development Inventory (Char-
tions that were intended to engage the children man, Drew, Baird, & Baird, 2003). They found
socially (e.g., watching a balloon fly around the that comprehension of words was delayed rela-
room); however, a smaller proportion of the tive to production, though, like typically devel-
children with autism vocalized than in the com- oping children, in absolute terms the children
parison groups. Moreover, the vocalizations the with autism understood more words than they
children with autism did produce were less produced. The continuation of significant delays
likely to be paired with other nonverbal com- in comprehension is also one of the strongest
munication, such as shifts in gaze or gesture or differentiators of HFA from specific language
changes in facial expression than they were for disorders (Rutter et al., 1992).
the other children (Hellreigel, Tao, DiLavore, Bartak et al. (1975) and Paul and Cohen
& Lord, 1995). Sheinkopf and his colleagues (1984a) showed that individuals with autism
conducted a detailed examination of the vocal performed more poorly on standardized
350 Development and Behavior

measures of language comprehension than par- their likelihood of occurring in the real world
ticipants with aphasic or mental handicap at (e.g., knowing that a mother is more likely to
similar nonverbal mental age levels. Studies of pick up a baby than a baby pick up a mother).
very young children (Paul, Chawarska, Klin, & Tager-Flusberg (1981b) concluded that children
Volkmar, 2004) suggest that comprehension with autism have difficulty applying their
skills are depressed relative to production in knowledge about the probabilities of occur-
the second year of life, while the gap tends to rence of events in the world to the task of un-
narrow, with receptive skills moving closer to derstanding sentences.
expressive levels, in the third to fourth year. In a partial replication of this study, Gad-
Studies that have compared receptive and ex- des (1984) showed that children with autism
pressive language skills among somewhat older were much less consistent in identifying proba-
children with autism using standardized tests ble events that involved relationships between
have found that receptive skills as measured by people (e.g., the mother feeds the baby) than
standard scores tend to be comparable to ex- were very young normally developing children
pressive on vocabulary tests as well as tests of with lower or equivalent expressive abilities.
higher order language processing (Jarrold This occurred when the relationships were
et al., 1997; Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, acted out by dolls, even when no comprehen-
2001). Yet, there is a clear clinical impression sion of language was required. Thus, the diffi-
that among verbal children with autism, com- culties may lie in comprehending the situation
prehension is more significantly impaired and what is probable in it, as well as compre-
(Tager-Flusberg, 1981a). hending the word order that depicts the situa-
More insight into the mechanisms that un- tion. Paul, Fischer, and Cohen (1988) found
derlie the impression of impaired language that although children with autism used simi-
comprehension in autism comes from experi- lar strategies in sentence comprehension as
mental studies. Sigman and Ungerer (1981) other children, they always performed less
looked at language comprehension and sensori- competently. Together, these findings suggest
motor performance in children with autism that children not only may have limited ability
with mental ages of about 2 years. They found to integrate linguistic input with real-world
sophisticated performance on object perma- knowledge but also, in some cases, may lack
nence tasks but poor performance on receptive knowledge about social events used by nor-
language measures. They suggested that senso- mally developing children to buttress emerg-
rimotor skills play a small role in the acquisi- ing language skills and to acquire increasingly
tion of language. Play skills, on the other hand, advanced linguistic structures (Lord, 1985).
were highly related to receptive language level, Another source of difficulty in compre-
particularly those forms of play that were di- hending language in everyday situations rather
rected outward toward dolls. Thus, the more so- than in standardized testing situations may be
cial aspects of cognition, those involving the the ability to integrate nonverbal cues to help
imaginary creation of a scene with dolls and in- interpret linguistic input. Examples include
teractions between people, appear to be more noticing the smile on another’s face, the way
related to the understanding of language than in which another person touches you, the tone
are those involving knowledge about objects, of his or her voice, as well as the words, to de-
which can be learned with very little social in- termine whether someone is being affection-
teraction. Tager-Flusberg (1981b) investigated ate, teasing, or being aggressive (Loveland,
sentence comprehension using experimental 1991; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1990;
tasks that assessed the use of different strate- St. James & Tager-Flusberg, 1994).
gies. Children with autism performed at the Paul and Cohen (1985) looked at the ability
same level as typical controls in their use of a of matched groups of participants with autism
word order strategy for processing sentences and mental handicap to understand indirect re-
(interpreting noun-verb-noun sequences as quests for action (e.g., “Can you color this cir-
agent-action-object); however, they were less cle blue?”) of varying syntactic complexity.
likely to use a semantically based probable The two groups, with IQs in the mildly to mod-
event strategy, interpreting sentences based on erately retarded range, performed similarly in
Language and Communication in Autism 351

a context in which the request intent of the ut- in which individuals find themselves. Thus, in
terance was made explicit (e.g., “I’m going to considering deficits in language use, factors
tell you to do some things. Can you . . .”). How- such as what individuals are expected to do,
ever, the autism group performed significantly what they are given the opportunity to do,
worse when the same requests were presented and what they usually do all must be consid-
in an unstructured context with no prefacing ered. Stone and Caro-Martinez (1990), in an
cue as to the intention of the utterance. The au- observational study of the spontaneous commu-
thors concluded that the individuals with nication of children with autism of varying
autism are impaired in the ability to determine abilities placed in special classrooms, found
the speaker’s intention without explicit cuing differences in the functions about which the
over and above any syntactic comprehension children communicated. These differences
deficit that might be present. This pattern may were related to chronological age, nonverbal IQ,
be an example of why, in their follow-up of and whether the children’s primary mode of
HFA individuals, Rutter et al. (1992) found a communication was through speech or motor
strong relationship between language compre- acts. Children who did not talk engaged in more
hension and social functioning in adulthood, social routines than verbal children. Children
with no similar finding for adults identified as with speech were more likely to use language to
having specific receptive and expressive lan- offer new information. They communicated to a
guage impairment as children. greater number of different people (rather than
For individuals with autism, understanding just the teacher) and were more likely to ad-
language in conversational and other dis- dress communications to peers as well as adults
course contexts remains a significant chal- than children without speech. McHale, Simeon-
lenge because semantic and pragmatic aspects sson, Marcus, and Olley (1980) showed that
of language are so closely linked to nonverbal autistic students communicated more in the
social communication and other aspects of so- presence of their teachers than in their absence
cial adaptation. and directed their communication only to
adults, not to peers.
Language Use Across different language levels, children
with autism also share important similarities.
Language use or the pragmatic aspects of lan- Despite deficits in spontaneous speech, most
guage in autism has been studied from a variety children on the autism spectrum do attempt to
of perspectives. This domain has been the focus use their language to communicate even if only
of research for the past several decades because in limited ways. Bernard-Opitz (1982) showed
problems have been found across all individuals that communicative performance of one child
with ASDs. Studies on language use including with autism varied with different interlocutors
specific, unusual aspects of language use such and in different settings, indicating some social
as delayed echolalia and neologisms, as well as awareness in his use of language. However, rate
language used to describe particular phenomena of initiation of spontaneous communications in
such as mental states or emotions, are discussed autism is often described as very low. In the
elsewhere in the chapter; this section focuses study by Stone and Caro-Martinez (1990), the
on research on speech acts, referential commu- modal frequency in school was two or three
nication, discourse, and narration. spontaneous communicative acts per child per
One of the most interesting characteristics hour. Only half of the children ever directed a
of language use in autism is that it has aspects communication to a peer across multiple obser-
that are constant across development and as- vations. Several other investigators have shown
pects that change. As with the development autistic children to have less frequent and less
of social behavior (Lord, 1995), some of the varied speech acts in free play or more open-
changes occur because children improve in ended situations, even when their responses to
their communicative abilities; other changes highly structured situations were similar to
occur because situational demands for commu- those of control groups (Landry & Loveland,
nication are different for children of different 1989; Mermelstein, 1983; Wetherby & Prut-
ages and for adults and vary with the contexts ting, 1984). General studies of younger children
352 Development and Behavior

with autism find that they rarely use language the right amount to include in an utterance
for comments, showing off, acknowledging the (Lord et al., 1989). For example, when asked
listener, initiating social interaction, or re- the question, “Did you and your sister do any-
questing information. Even among older thing besides rake leaves over the weekend?” a
higher functioning children, language is rarely participant responded, “Yes.” This answer, al-
used to explain or describe events in a conver- though correct in a strictly syntactic sense,
sational context (Ziatas, Durkin, & Pratt, fails to appreciate the listener’s real purpose in
2003). The speech acts that are missing or asking the question. It fails to provide the so-
rarely used in the conversations of children cially appropriate amount of information in re-
with autism all have in common an emphasis sponse. On the other hand, another adolescent
on social rather than regulatory uses of lan- with autism, when asked how his day had gone,
guage (Wetherby, 1986). There are also simi- began the account with a description of the
larities in abnormalities in language use exact time when he awakened, the bathroom
across verbal individuals with autism who where he washed his face, and the color of his
show a range of expressive language abilities. toothbrush. Similar findings were reported by
Difficulties in listening, talking to self, prob- Surian and his colleagues using a structured ex-
lems in following rules of politeness, and perimental task (Surian, Baron-Cohen, & Van
making irrelevant remarks occur in many chil- der Lely, 1996).
dren and adults with autism (Baltaxe, 1977; Few differences have been reported between
Rumsey, Rapoport, & Sceery, 1985). subjects with HFA and AS, although Shriberg
Difficulties in social uses of language, espe- et al. (2001) found that young adults with AS
cially in conversations and other discourse con- were significantly more garrulous than those
texts, have also been widely noted for people with HFA. Ghaziuddin and Gerstein (1996) in-
with both HFA and AS by clinicians and re- cluded monologue speech as part of their defi-
searchers (e.g., Klin & Volkmar, 1997; see nition of pedantic speech style, which suggests
Landa, 2000, for review). Chuba, Paul, Miles, that people with AS do not engage much in turn
Klin, and Volkmar (2003) reported on conver- taking during reciprocal conversations with
sational behaviors in 30 adolescents with either other people and may also talk too much. Ram-
HFA or AS who were engaged in semistruc- berg, Ehlers, Nyden, Johansson, and Gillberg
tured conversational interviews with clinicians. (1996) found that children with AS were im-
Findings revealed that for both diagnoses, con- paired in taking turns during dyadic conversa-
versational errors were inconsistent, rather than tions, providing some support for this view.
constant. Nonetheless, it was possible to distin- Adams, Green, Gilchrist, and Cox (2002)
guish teenagers with ASD from those with typ- compared conversational samples collected
ical development (TD) in terms of the quantity from adolescents with AS and a group of age-
of conversational errors. No TD subject made and IQ-matched children with severe conduct
more than five errors within a 30-minute disorder. Although there were no overall signif-
sample, whereas all subjects with HFA and AS icant group differences in verbosity, the adoles-
made more than eight errors. The most robust cents with AS tended to talk more during
differences observed were in the areas of gaze conversational contexts that focused on emo-
and intonation, while remaining differences tional topics. A few participants with AS were
centered on ability to share topics and infer extremely verbose. The groups were similar in
others’ informational state. Similarly, Paul and their ability to respond to questions and com-
Feldman (1984) reported in a case series pre- ments offered by their conversational partner,
sentation that highly verbal adolescents and but a qualitative analysis of responses revealed
adults with autism showed difficulties in iden- that the participants with AS had more prag-
tifying the topic initiated by the conversational matic problems such as providing an inadequate
partner and in providing a relevant comment. or tangential response especially when dis-
They had difficulty judging, on the basis of cussing an unusual event or personal narrative.
cues in the conversation and on the basis of Children and adolescents with autism per-
general knowledge about what listeners could form less well on tasks of referential commu-
reasonably be expected already to have in their nication (Loveland, Tunali, McEvoy, & Kelley,
knowledge store, how much information was 1989), although many can identify another
Language and Communication in Autism 353

person’s visual perspective (Baron-Cohen, interlocutor’s remark. They also had difficulty
1989; Hobson, 1984). More social and/or more gracefully terminating topics. In role-playing
complex aspects of referential communication, situations that required the subject to lead the
such as those that affect narration and dis- conversation, teenagers with ASD generally
course, are particularly affected (Hemphill, were unable to take assertive conversational
Picardi, & Tager-Flusberg, 1991). Children roles. Paradoxically, then, adolescents with
with autism often have difficulty dealing ASD showed difficulty in responding contin-
with new information (Tager-Flusberg & An- gently to others’ conversational input and in
derson, 1991). They produce more noncontin- appropriately guiding conversations to elicit
gent utterances, with patterns similar to those remarks from an interlocutor. Taken together,
of language-impaired children but with pro- these results suggest a basic difficulty in es-
portionately more errors (Baltaxe & D’Angi- tablishing and maintaining reciprocity in con-
ola, 1992). Hurtig, Ensrud, and Tomblin versation—in the ability to engage in mutual,
(1980) reported that persistent and persevera- cooperative social dialogue.
tive questioning generally did not serve the Studies of the ability of individuals with
purpose of requesting information in autistic autism to produce narrative discourse have
children but was communicative, often func- also provided information about the ways in
tioning as a means of initiating interaction or which persons with autism organize and con-
getting attention. vey their thoughts to others. In general, stud-
Bishop, Hartley, and Weir (1994) studied a ies have found that, commensurate with their
group of children with semantic-pragmatic language ability, children and adults with
disorder who had some social and communica- autism are able to narrate stories and follow
tive behaviors that overlap with autism and simple scripts for common social events, such
pervasive developmental disorder. They found as a birthday party. Particular difficulties in
that, in these more verbally fluent children, making causal statements were found in one
there was a higher proportion of utterances that study (Tager-Flusberg, 1995), but these find-
were initiations rather than responses. This ings were not replicated in a later study
finding seemed to account for how language- (Capps, Losh, & Thurber, 2000). Loveland and
impaired children, including some with her colleagues asked individuals with autism
autism, could be considered talkative, even or Down syndrome, matched on chronological
though the total amount of language they pro- and verbal mental age, to retell the story they
duce is not higher than that of other children. were shown in the form of a puppet show or
Paul and Cohen (1984b) studied responses video sketch (Loveland, McEvoy, Tunali, &
to requests for clarification in adults with Kelley, 1990). Compared to the controls, the
autism or mental retardation matched for children with autism were more likely to ex-
nonverbal IQ. They found that although the hibit pragmatic violations including bizarre
participants with autism were just as likely to or inappropriate utterances and were less
respond to requests for clarification, their an- able to take into consideration the listener’s
swers were less specific than those of the needs. Some of the participants with autism in
nonautistic participants. They were also less this study even failed to understand the story
likely to add information that might be of help as a representation of meaningful events, sug-
to the listener, suggesting that they had diffi- gesting that they lacked a cultural perspective
culty judging what piece of information was underlying narrative (Bruner & Feldman,
relevant. 1993; Loveland & Tunali, 1993). Norbury and
Chuba et al. (2003) used conversational Bishop (2003), however, found few differences
probes and role playing to examine the prag- between narrative skills of children with ASD
matic abilities of adolescents with HFA and and those with specific language impairment,
AS. In these more structured conversational suggesting that difficulties with stories may
situations, as in more naturalistic interviews be common to children with communication
discussed earlier, subjects with ASD had sig- impairments.
nificantly more difficulty than controls with Taken together, these studies of pragmatic
TD in responding to topics introduced by oth- skills in verbal autistic individuals echo the sug-
ers and in making comments contingent on the gestions of studies of nonverbal communication
354 Development and Behavior

in young autistic children. Although basic in- Written scripts, social stories, graphic organiz-
tention to communicate often exists, the autis- ers, reminder cards, and lists are useful in in-
tic person has impaired skill in participating in creasing social and communicative behavior for
communicative activities involving joint refer- individuals with autism who read (e.g., Gray,
ence or shared topics. This is particularly true 2000; Krantz & McClannahan, 1998). Nonethe-
in supplying new information relevant to a lis- less, these individuals can have relative deficits
tener’s purposes. The strategies used by an in- in comprehension, particularly when longer,
dividual with autism to maintain conversation more complex texts, such as narratives, are in-
are less advanced than syntactic ability would volved (Walhberg & Magliano, 2004).
predict, as is the ability to infer the interlocu- While developmental level-appropriate lit-
tor’s implicit intentions. eracy skills are the norm in autism, there is a
One difference between individuals with subset of children with ASD who show re-
autism and other populations with language im- markable decoding ability (Grigorenko, Klin,
pairments has been that, in most groups with & Volkmar, 2003). These children are often
language impairment, the more a child talks, referred to as hyperlexic. They usually begin
the less likely it is that the language will have reading words before they get to school and are
unusual characteristics. In contrast, two studies obsessive in their interest in letters, writing,
with autistic children and adolescents showed and reading (Nation, 1999). However, Grig-
that subjects’ unusual aspects of language orenko et al. (2003) point out that hyperlexia is
and lack of cohesiveness increased with the not synonymous with autism. Their review re-
amount of speech (Caplan, Guthrie, Shields, & veals that only 5% to 10% of children with
Yudovin, 1994; Volden & Lord, 1991). In autism show hyperlexia, although this rate is
autism, difficulties in explaining and predicting much higher than that which occurs in other-
behavior seem to be related both to general lan- wise normal development. Moreover, hyper-
guage deficits and to deficits in specific cogni- lexia is not specific to autism; it is also seen
tive functions, such as metarepresentation and in a variety of other disabilities including
using the information at hand (Tager-Flusberg Turner syndrome, Tourette’s syndrome, and
& Sullivan, 1994). Because most, though not mental retardation. Although hyperlexia is
all, individuals with autism have significant de- more prevalent in autism than in these dis-
lays as well as deviance in language, they are orders, it can occur in conjunction with
doubly handicapped in communication. nonautistic disabilities. The hallmarks of hy-
perlexia are advanced word recognition in
Reading children who otherwise have significant cogni-
tive, linguistic, or social handicaps; a compul-
Many children with autism have an early sive preoccupation with reading, letters, or
interest in letters and numbers, and some learn writing; and a significant discrepancy between
to read words without any direct instruction strong word recognition and weak comprehen-
(Loveland & Tunali-Kotoski, 1997). Decoding, sion of what has been read. Children with
or pronouncing written words, and spelling tend autism who show hyperlexia are often baffling
to be relative strengths for many individuals to families, because their independent, early
with ASD. These strengths are especially note- acquisition of word recognition contrasts so
worthy when children with autism are com- sharply with their severe handicaps in social
pared to other individuals with histories of communication and learning in other areas.
language delay, who tend to do especially Hyperlexia is, to some extent, a “savant ” skill,
poorly in reading and writing. Children with like other special abilities occasionally seen in
autism typically show literacy skills that are children with autism (e.g., drawing, calcula-
on par with their overall developmental level tion, music, calendar calculation), which fails
(Loveland & Tunali-Kotoski, 1997; Myles to connect to general intellectual and func-
et al., 2002) and can understand simple reading tional abilities. Like other savant skills,
passages at grade level (Ventner, Lord, & hyperlexia can be used as a starting point for
Schopler, 1992). Written material has been teaching other, more functional behaviors, but
shown in a variety of studies to provide a help- direct instruction and intensive practice will
ful medium of intervention for these children. be necessary to move from the unprocessed
Language and Communication in Autism 355

“ word calling” that is characteristic of this Functional magnetic resonance imaging


syndrome to more purposeful and communica- (fMRI ) is beginning to be used, as well, to in-
tive uses of reading. vestigate online language processing in autism.
Just, Cherkassky, Keller, and Minshew (2004)
Theories of Origin investigated brain activation during sentence
comprehension. Reliable differences were
Recently, increasingly sophisticated neuroimag- found between subjects with HFA and TD in
ing and neurophysiological measures have activation in the basic language areas of the
offered a promise for autism of eventual docu- cortex. Subjects with HFA showed higher acti-
mentation of anatomical and functional dif- vation in Wernicke’s ( left laterosuperior tem-
ferences in the brain. Although a replicable, poral) region, which is traditionally associated
consistent, meaningful neuroanatomic or neuro- with language comprehension (particularly,
physiological basis for autism has not yet been understanding words), and lower activity in
identified (Bailey, Phillips, & Rutter, 1996), Broca’s area ( left inferior frontal gyrus), usu-
there have been some small-scale studies that ally associated with production and grammar.
have investigated structural brain abnormalities Functional connectivity between cortical re-
related to language using magnetic resonance gions also appeared lower in the subjects
imaging (MRI ). with HFA.
In the normal population, left cortical re- New discoveries about brain structure and
gions, especially in key language areas (peri- function are also being incorporated into
sylvian region, planum temporale, and thinking about origins of autistic language
Heschel’s gyrus), are enlarged relative to the difficulties. For example, Rogers (2004) has
size of those regions in the right hemisphere. noted research showing the presence of “mir-
Herbert and her colleagues compared 16 boys ror neurons” (Bekkering, 2002), which are ac-
with autism (all with normal nonverbal IQ tivated both when a movement is seen and
scores) to 15 age-, sex-, and handedness- when it is made. This “resonance” may facili-
matched typically developing controls (Her- tate imitation of motor activities. Rogers spec-
bert et al., 2002). Their main findings were ulates that specific mirror neurons for sights
that the boys with autism had significant re- and sounds associated with speech may exist
versal of asymmetry in the inferior lateral that could impact the ability to imitate and
frontal cortex, which was 27% larger in the learn from language input. Children with
right hemisphere compared to 17% larger in ASDs, who are known to have special difficul-
the left hemisphere for the normal controls. ties with vocal imitation (Stone, Lemanek,
There were also significant differences be- Fishel, Fernandez, & Altemeier, 1990), may be
tween the autism and control groups in the impacted by deficits in these mirror neuron
asymmetry patterns in the planum temporale. systems, which might provide one element of
While both groups showed a left hemisphere etiology of speech delays and deviance in these
asymmetry, this was more extreme in the autis- syndromes. Further studies are clearly needed
tic boys (25% leftward asymmetry for autism to explore the structural abnormalities in brain
compared to only 5% in the controls). These regions subserving language in both children
findings for the planum temporale were not and adults with autism. Although research ex-
replicated in a study comparing adults with ploring the neurobiology of language impair-
autism and age-matched normal controls ment in autism is still in the early stages,
(Rojas, Bawn, Benkers, Reite, & Rogers, 2002). together with advances in molecular genetics,
Rojas and his colleagues found that their adults the likelihood is that, in the long term, neuro-
with autism had significantly reduced left biological approaches will contribute signifi-
hemisphere planum temporale volumes and no cantly to our understanding and treatment of
hemispheric asymmetry in this important lan- language and communication in autism.
guage region. Perhaps methodological differ- Any robust theoretical model for communi-
ences can explain these conflicting findings. cation abnormalities in autism must have
Rojas et al. studied adults rather than children, several characteristics. It needs to describe a
included women in their sample, and their course that goes awry very early in develop-
groups were not matched for IQ. ment and that has a range of consequences,
356 Development and Behavior

from severe language disability involving no munication and social cognition? How do lin-
representational-communication system, to guistic comprehension deficits relate to the
more circumscribed abnormalities primarily various aspects of deviant language seen in the
affecting the pragmatics of connected dis- syndrome? What triggers the initial failure of
course. It needs to be related to other social social cognition and joint attention that seems
and cognitive functions, but not completely to be associated with such pervasive commu-
accounted for by other factors. That is, there nicative difficulties? Like so many other ques-
are children and adults without apparent syn- tions about autism, the answers to these are
tactic and semantic difficulties who share the likely to be neither simple nor universally
social difficulties seen in autism (as in AS), true. A wide and heterogeneous range of com-
and there are children and adults with severe municative behaviors and function is seen in
to profound mental handicap or with specific the syndrome. Whatever the biological explana-
language disorders who make substantial im- tion, communication disorders in autism are
provements in social areas and/or nonverbal most likely affected by deficits in the ability to
cognitive functioning but who remain signifi- process information about social situations and
cantly impaired in spoken language. Thus, it how people behave when interacting with
appears that, although outcome and severity each other at every point in development. This
of social and cognitive deficits in autism are deficit must be addressed in any attempt to
related to language level, these factors are remediate autistic communicative disorders. In
also independent to some degree. addition, although they are integrally tied to
A complete theoretical account of language broader cognitive and social deficits, delays in
impairment ultimately needs to delineate the the ability to understand and produce words
underlying mechanisms that explain these very and sentences may have an even greater effect
different patterns of language acquisition and on the lives of individuals with autism than
impairment in autism. It is likely that across they do on persons with other handicaps. The
different children, different mechanisms may double handicap of delay and deviance in
be impaired. For some, communicative deficits autism means that we cannot assume that either
are related most closely to social impairments individuals with autism or those who provide
in decoding nonverbal cues and understanding their linguistic environments can naturally
other minds. For other children, these social- compensate for these deficits without carefully
cognitive impairments may be more severe, considered intervention. This intervention must
leading to the inability to understand language include understanding of how these deficits are
as an intentional symbolic system, which may manifested in particular children or adults and
impede them from even entering the linguistic the communicative contexts in which each indi-
system as marked by the absence of rudimen- vidual needs to function.
tary comprehension skills and severe joint at-
tention deficits. Additional mechanisms that
Cross-References
may be directly or indirectly linked to lan-
guage acquisition in children with autism in- Aspects of classification and diagnosis are
clude oral motor skills, imitation, and auditory discussed in Chapters 1 to 7; developmental
processing and attentional systems. We are changes in autism, in Chapters 8 through 15;
still at the early stages of developing theoreti- and language intervention, in Chapters 36
cal accounts to explain the individual variation through 38.
in language outcomes in ASDs that encompass
all levels of analysis from genetics to neu-
ropathology to cognition and behavior. REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic


CONCLUSION gesturing in normal infants. Child Develop-
ment, 59(2), 450–466.
Many questions remain to be answered about Adams, C., Green, J., Gilchrist, A., & Cox, A.
communication in autism. For example, how is (2002). Conversational behaviour of children
odd intonation related to deficits in com- with Asperger syndrome and conduct disorder.
Language and Communication in Autism 357

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S. J., & Streiner, D. (1980).
43, 679–690. Cross-sectional studies of grammatical mor-
Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1991). The devel- phemes in autistic and mentally retarded chil-
opment of shared attention during infancy. An- dren. Journal of Autism and Developmental
nals of Child Development, 8, 1–41. Disorders, 10, 39–50.
Asher, S. R. (1978). Referential communication. In Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S., Streiner, D., & Tolkin-
G. J. Whitehurst & B. J. Simmerman (Eds.), Eppel, P. (1976). Phonological investigation of
The functions of language and cognition. New verbal autistic and mentally retarded subjects.
York: Academic Press. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre-
Asperger, H. (1991). “Autistic psychopathy” in nia, 6, 303–315.
childhood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and As- Bates, E. (1976). Language in context. New York:
perger syndrome (pp. 37–91). Cambridge: Academic Press.
Cambridge University Press. (Original work Beitchman, J. H., & Inglis, A. (1991). The contin-
published 1944) uum of linguistic dysfunction from pervasive
Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). developmental disorders to dyslexia. Psychi-
Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, ge- atric Clinics of North America, 14, 95–111.
netic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological Bekkering, H. (2002). Common mechanisms in the
perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and observation and execution of finger and mouth
Psychiatry, 37, 89–126. movements. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.),
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the The imitative mind: Development, evolution,
achievement of joint reference. Child Develop- and brain basis (pp. 163–182). Cambridge:
ment, 62, 875–890. Cambridge University Press.
Baltaxe, C. (1977). Pragmatic deficits in the lan- Bernard-Opitz, V. (1982). Pragmatic analysis of
guage of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pedi- the communicative behavior of an autistic
atric Psychology, 2, 176–180. child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
Baltaxe, C. (1984). Use of contrastive stress in nor- ders, 47, 99–109.
mal, aphasic and autistic children. Journal of Bishop, D. V., & Adams, C. (1989). Conversational
Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 97–105. characteristics of children with semantic-
Baltaxe, C., & D’Angiola, N. (1992). Cohesion in pragmatic disorder: II. What features lead to a
the discourse interaction of autistic, specific judgment of inappropriacy? British Journal of
language-impaired, and normal children. Jour- Disorders of Communication, 24(3), 241–263.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Bishop, D., Hartley, J., & Weir, F. (1994). Why and
22, 1–22. when do some language-impaired children
Baltaxe, C. A. M., & Simmons, J. Q. (1977). Bed- seem talkative? A study of initiation in conver-
time soliloquies and linguistic competence in sations of children with semantic-pragmatic
autism. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor- disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
der, 42, 376–393. Disorders, 24(2), 177–197.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role-taking and Bloom, L. (1974). Talking, understanding and
protodeclarative pointing in autism. British thinking. In R. C. Scheifelbusch & L. L. Lloyd
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, re-
113–127. tardation and intervention (pp. 285–311). Bal-
Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal timore: University Park Press.
psychology to belief-desire psychology: The de- Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to
velopment of a theory of mind, and its dysfunc- language: Acquiring the power of expression.
tion. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & Cambridge, England Cambridge University
D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Press.
Perspectives from autism (pp. 59–82). Oxford, Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language develop-
England: Oxford University Press. ment and language disorders. New York: Wiley.
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A com- Boucher, J. (1988). Word fluency in high-functioning
parative study of infantile autism and specific autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel-
developmental receptive language disorder: I. opmental Disorders, 18, 637–645.
The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge:
126, 127–145. Harvard University Press.
Bartolucci, G., & Albers, R. J. (1974). Deictic cat- Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts.
egories in the language of autistic children. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19.
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre- Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind
nia, 4, 131–141. and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen,
358 Development and Behavior

H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Un- in preschool children with autism spectrum
derstanding other minds: Perspectives from disorder using the MacArthur Communicative
autism. Oxford, England: Oxford University Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal
Press. of Child Language, 30, 213–236.
Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., & Smith, T. M. (1988). Chiat, S. (1982). If I were you and you were
First report of a Canadian epidemiological me: The analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-
study of autistic syndromes. Journal of Child reversing child. Journal of Child Language, 9,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 433–445. 359–379.
Burgoine, E., & Wing, L. (1983). Identical triplets Chuba, H., Paul, R., Miles, S., Klin, A., & Volkmar,
with Asperger’s syndrome. British Journal of F. (2003, November). Assessing pragmatic skills
Psychiatry, 143, 261. in individuals with autism spectrum disorders.
Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1989). Infantile Presentation at the National Convention of the
autism and developmental receptive dysphasia: American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-
A comparative follow-up into middle child- tion, Chicago.
hood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Cohen, I. L., Sudhalter, V., Landon-Jimenez, D., &
Disorders, 19, 19–30. Keogh, M. (1993). A neural network approach
Cantwell, D. P., Baker, L., & Mattison, R. E. to the classification of autism. Journal of Autism
(1980). Psychiatric disorders in children with and Developmental Disorders, 23, 443–466.
speech and language retardation. Archives of Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms
General Psychiatry, 37, 423–426. in the first two years of life: A preliminary
Caplan, R., Guthrie, D., Shields, W. D., & Yudovin, population study of infantile autism. European
S. (1994). Communication deficits in pedi- Archives of Psychiatric and Neurological Sci-
atric complex partial seizure disorders and ence, 283, 169–174.
schizophrenia. Development and Psychopath- Darley, F., Aronson, A., & Brown, J. (1975). Motor
ology, 6, 499–517. speech disorders. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “ The DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Norton,
frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: J. A., Allen, J., & Steel, R. (1973). Prognosis in
Narrative competence in children with autism. autism: A follow-up study. Journal of Autism
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, and Childhood Schizophrenia, 3, 199–245.
193–204. Dore, J. (1978). Requestive systems in nursery
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). school conversations: Analysis of talk in its
Social cognition, joint attention, and commu- social context. In R. Campbell & P. Smith
nicative competence from 9 to 15 months of (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of
age. Monographs of the Society for Research in language. New York: Plenum Press.
Child Development, 63(4), 176. Doussard-Roosevelt, J., Joe, C., Bazhenova, O., &
Carr, E., Schreibman, L., & Lovaas, O. L. (1975). Porges, S. (2003). Mother-child interaction in
Control of echolalic speech in psychotic chil- autistic and nonautistic children: Characteris-
dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, tics of maternal approach behaviors and child
3, 331–351. social responses. Development and Psycho-
Cazden, C. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb pathology, 15, 277–295.
inflections. Child Development, 39, 443–448. Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L.,
Chapman, R. (1978). Comprehension strategies in Gould, J., Welham, M., et al. (1996). Compar-
children. In J. F. Kavanagh & W. Strange ison of clinical symptoms in autism and As-
(Eds.), Speech and language in the laboratory, perger’s disorder. Journal of the American
school and clinic (pp. 308–327). Cambridge, Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry,
MA: MIT Press. 35(11), 1523–1531.
Chapman, R. (1981). Exploring children’s commu- Elliott, M. J. (1993). Prelinguistic vocalizations in
nicative intents. In J. Miller (Ed.), Assessing autistic, developmentally delayed, and nor-
language production in children: Experimental mally developing children. Unpublished mas-
procedures. Baltimore: University Park Press. ter’s thesis, University of North Carolina,
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Greensboro.
Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Pre- Eskes, G. A., Bryson, S. E., & McCormick, T. A.
dicting language outcome in infants with (1990). Comprehension of concrete and ab-
autism and pervasive developmental disorder. stract words in autistic children. Journal of
International Journal of Language and Com- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20,
munication Disorders, 38, 265–285. 61–73.
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. Fay, W. (1969). On the basis of autistic echolalia.
(2003). Measuring early language development Journal of Communication Disorders, 2, 38–47.
Language and Communication in Autism 359

Fay, W., & Schuler, A. L. (1980). Emerging lan- Gray, C. (2000). The new social story book. Arling-
guage in autistic children. Baltimore: Univer- ton, TX: Future Horizons.
sity Park Press. Grigorenko, E., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003).
Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1979). Autism is not a Hyperlexia: Disability or superability? Jour-
disorder of language. Paper presented at the nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44,
meeting of the New England Child Language 1079–1091.
Association, Boston. Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean:
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, Exploration in the development of language.
D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early London: Edward Arnold.
communicative development. Monographs of Hellreigel, C., Tao, L., DiLavore, P., & Lord, C.
the Society for Research in Child Development, (1995). The ef fect of context on nonverbal so-
59(5, Serial No. 242). cial behaviors of very young autistic children.
Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations Paper presented at the biannual meetings of
to infants as biologically relevant signals: An the Society for Research in Child Develop-
evolutionary perspective. In J. H. Barkow & ment, Indianapolis, IN.
L. Cosmides (Eds.), Adapted mind: Evolution- Hemphill, L., Picardi, N., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
ary psychology and the generation of culture (1991). Narrative as an index of communicative
(pp. 391–428). London: Oxford University competence in mildly mentally retarded chil-
Press. dren. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 263–279.
Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the Blind. New Herbert, M. R., Harris, G. J., Adrien, K. T.,
York: Basic Books. Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Lange, N. T., et al.
Frith, U. (1969). Emphasis and meaning in recall in (2002). Abnormal asymmetry in language as-
normal and autistic children. Language and sociation cortex in autism. Annals of Neurol-
Speech, 2, 29–38. ogy, 52, 588–596.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Hobson, R. P. (1984). Early childhood autism and
New York: Blackwell. the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism
Gaddes, J. (1984). Probable events and sentence com- and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.
prehension in autistic children. Unpublished se- Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related
nior honors thesis, University of Alberta, and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evi-
Edmonton, Alberta. dence from the British Picture Vocabulary
Ghaziuddin, M., & Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic Scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental
speaking style differentiates Asperger syn- Disorders, 19, 601–623.
drome from high-functioning autism. Journal of Howlin, P. (1984). The acquisition of grammatical
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, morphemes in autistic children: A critique and
585–595. replication of the findings of Bartolucci,
Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism
(1992). Brief report: A comparison of the di- and Developmental Disorders, 14, 127–136.
agnostic criteria for Asperger syndrome. Jour- Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, (2004). Adult outcome for children with
22, 643–649. autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Goldberg, W., Osann, K., Filipek, P., Laulhere, T., chiatry, 45, 212–229.
Jarvis, K., Modahl, C., et al. (2003). Language Hurtig, R., Ensrud, S., & Tomblin, J. B. (1980).
and other regression: Assessment and timing. Question production in autistic children: A lin-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- guistic pragmatic perspective. Paper presented
ders, 33, 607–616. at the University of Wisconsin Symposium on
Goldfarb, W., Braunstein, P., & Lorge, I. (1956). A Research in Child Language Disorders, Madi-
study of speech patterns in a group of schizo- son, WI.
phrenic children. American Journal of Or- Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language
thopsychiatry, 26, 544–555. comprehension. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories
Goldstein, H. (2002). Communication Intervention in cognitive psychology (pp. 331–368). Hills-
for children with autism: A review of treat- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ment efficacy. Journal of Autism and Develop- Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Lan-
mental Disorders, 32, 373–396. guage profiles in children with autism: Theoret-
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The develop- ical and methodological implications. Autism:
ment of categorization in the second Journal of Research and Practice, 1, 57–76.
year and its relation to other cognitive and Just, M., Cherkassky, V., Keller, T., & Minshew, N.
linguistic developments. Child Development, (2004). Cortical activation and synchroni-
58, 1523–1531. zation during sentence comprehension in
360 Development and Behavior

high-functioning autism: Evidence of under- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-


connectivity. Brain, 10, 1093–2003. ders, 24, 155–176.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- Lempert, H. (1978). Extrasyntactic factors affect-
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. ing passive sentence comprehension in young
Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical lan- children. Child Development, 49, 694–699.
guage. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kinder-
242–246. garten through grade 12. Urbana, IL: National
Kjelgaard, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An in- Council of Teachers of English.
vestigation of language impairment in autism: Lord, C. (1985). Autism and the comprehension of
Implications for genetic subgroups. Language language. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov
and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308. (Eds.), Communication problems in autism
Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children’s listening (pp. 257–281). New York: Plenum Press.
preferences in regard to speech: A possible Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two year-olds re-
characterization of the symptom of social with- ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
drawal. Journal of Autism and Developmental Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(8), 1365–1382.
Disorders, 21, 29–42. Lord, C., Merrin, D. J., Vest, L., & Kelly, K. M.
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (1997). Asperger’s syn- (1983). Communicative behavior of adults
drome. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), with an autistic four-year-old boy and his non-
Handbook of autism and pervasive develop- handicapped twin brother. Journal of Autism
mental disorders (pp. 94–122). New York: and Developmental Disorders, 13, 1–17.
Wiley. Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (1996). The relationship
Koning, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and between expressive language level and nonver-
language skills in adolescent boys with As- bal social communication in autism. Journal of
perger syndrome. Autism: Journal of Research the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
and Practice, 5(1), 23–36. Psychiatry, 35(11), 1542–1550.
Konstantareas, M., Zajdemann, H., Homatidis, S., Lord, C., Pickles, A., DiLavore, P. C., & Shulman,
& McCabe, A. (1988). Maternal speech to C. (1996). Longitudinal studies of young chil-
verbal and higher functioning versus nonver- dren referred for possible autism. Paper pre-
bal and lower functioning autistic children. sented at the biannual meetings of the
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- International Society for Research in Child and
ders, 18, 647–656. Adolescent Psychopathology, Barcelona, Spain.
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leven-
interaction skills for children with autism: A thal, B., DiLavore, P., et al. (2000). The
script-fading procedure for beginning readers. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, Generic: A standard measure of social and
191–202. communication deficits associated with the
Kurita, H. (1985). Infantile autism with speech loss spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and De-
before the age of 30 months. Journal of the velopmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory
191–196. of language development in autism spectrum
Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger disorders. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), De-
syndrome and high-functioning autism. In velopmental language disorders: From pheno-
A. Klin, F. Volkmar, & S. Sparrow (Eds.), As- types to etiologies (pp. 7–29). Mahwah, NJ:
perger syndrome (pp. 125–155). New York: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Guilford Press. Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (1994). Autism and perva-
Landry, S. H., & Loveland, K. A. (1989). The ef- sive developmental disorders. In M. Rutter,
fect of social context on the functional com- L. Hersov, & E. Taylor (Eds.), Child and ado-
munication skills of autistic children. Journal lescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (3rd
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), ed., pp. 569–593). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
283–299. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J.,
Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Rutter, M., & Gottes- Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism
man, I. (1989, August 3–5). Epidemiologically Diagnostic Observation Schedule: A standard-
based twin study of autism. Paper presented at ized observation of communicative and social
the First World Congress on Psychiatric Genet- behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ics, Churchill College, Cambridge, England. Disorders, 19, 185–212.
Lee, A., Hobson, R. P., & Chiat, S. (1994). I, Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994).
you, me and autism: An experimental study. Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A re-
Language and Communication in Autism 361

vised version of a diagnostic interview for care- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23,
givers of individuals with possible pervasive 79–90.
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and McEvoy, R. E., Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H.
Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. (1988). The functions of immediate echolalia
Lord, C., Shulman, C., & DiLavore, P. (2004). Re- in autistic children: A developmental perspec-
gression and word loss in autistic spectrum tive. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and orders, 18, 657–668.
Psychiatry, 45, 936–955. McHale, S., Simeonsson, R. J., Marcus, L. M., &
Lord, C., Storoschuk, S., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. Olley, J. G. (1980). The social and symbolic
(1993). Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism in quality of autistic children’s communication.
preschool children. Infant Mental Health Jour- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
nal, 14, 234–252. ders, 10, 299–310.
Lord, C., & Venter, A. (1992). Outcome and follow- Merewether, F. C., & Alpert, M. (1990). The com-
up studies of high-functioning autistic individ- ponents and neuroanatomic bases of prosody.
uals. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Journal of Communication Disorders, 23,
High-functioning individuals with autism 325–336.
(pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum Press. Mermelstein, R. (1983). The relationship between
Lovaas, O. I. (1977). The autistic child. New York: syntactic and pragmatic development in autis-
Irvington. tic, retarded, and normal children. Paper
Loveland, K. (1984). Learning about points of presented to the Eighth Annual Boston Uni-
view: Spatial perspective and the acquisition versity Conference on Language Develop-
of “I /you.” Journal of Child Language, 11, ment, Boston.
535–556. Miller, J., & Chapman, R. S. (1981). The relation
Loveland, K. (1991). Social affordances and inter- between age and mean length of utterance in
action: II. Autism and the affordances of the morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-
human environment. Ecological Psychology, 3, search, 24, 154–162.
99–119. Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism
Loveland, K., McEvoy, R. E., Tunali, B., & Kelley, an amnesic disorder? Evidence from the Cali-
M. L. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism fornia Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology,
and Down syndrome. British Journal of Devel- 7, 209–216.
opmental Psychology, 8, 9–23. Myles, B., Hilgenfeld, T., Barnhill, G., Griswold,
Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative D., Hagiwara, T., Simpson, R. (2002). Analy-
language in autism and the theory of mind hy- sis of reading skills in individuals with As-
pothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron- perger syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other
Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen Developmental Disabilities, 17, 1088–3576.
(Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspec- Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A
tives from autism. Oxford, England: Oxford developmental perspective. Psychological Bul-
University Press. letin, 125(3), 338–355.
Loveland, K., Tunali, B., McEvoy, R. E., & Kelley, Nazzi, T., & Bertoninci, J. (2003). Before and after
M. L. (1989). Referential communication the vocabulary spurt: Two modes of word ac-
and response adequacy in autism and Down’s quisition. Developmental Science, 6, 136–142.
syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learn-
301–313. ing to talk. Monographs of the Society for Re-
Loveland, K., & Tunali-Kotoski, B. (1997). The search in Children Development, 38(1–2, Serial
school-age child with autism. In D. Cohen & F. no. 149).
Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and per- Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative
vasive developmental disorders (pp. 283–308). skills of children with communication impair-
New York: Wiley. ments. International Journal of Language and
Luyster, R., Richler, J., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Communication Disorders, 38, 287–314.
Bernier, R., Dunn, M., et al. (in press). Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni-
Early regression in social communication in tion of children with autism: A study of first
autistic spectrum disorders. Developmental birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism
Neuropsychology. and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–258.
Mayes, L., Volkmar, F., Hooks, M., & Cicchetti, Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
D. (1993). Differentiating Pervasive Develop- (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits
mental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified in young autistic children? Journal of Child
from autism and language disorders. Journal Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 343–362.
362 Development and Behavior

Paul, R. (1985). The emergence of pragmatic com- ior and language comprehension in fourteen
prehension: A study of children’s understand- children diagnosed as atypical or autistic. Ex-
ing of sentence-structure cues to given /new ceptional Children, 33, 19–26.
information. Journal of Child Language, 12, Ramberg, C., Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Johansson, M.,
161–179. & Gillberg, C. (1996). Language and prag-
Paul, R., Chapman, R. S., & Wanska, S. (1980). The matic functions in school-age children on the
development of complex sentence use. Paper autism spectrum. European Journal of Disor-
presented at the meeting of the American ders of Communication, 31(4), 387–413.
Speech and Hearing Association, Detroit, MI. Rice, M. L. (2004). Growth models of developmental
Paul, R., Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. language disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. War-
(2004, July). Profiles of communication in tod- ren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders:
dlers with autism spectrum disorders. Invited From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 207–240).
paper presented at the American Psychological Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Association, Honolulu, HI. Rice, M. L., Oetting, J. B., & Marquis, J. (1994).
Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1984a). Outcomes of se- Frequency of input effects on word compre-
vere disorders of language acquisition. Journal hension of children with specific language im-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, pairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing
405–422. Research, 37(1), 106–121.
Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1984b). Responses to con- Richler, J., Luyster, R., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Daw-
tingent queries in adults with mental retarda- son, G., Bernier, R., et al. (in press). Is there a
tion and pervasive developmental disorders. regressive “phenotype” of autism spectrum
Applied Psycholinguistics, 349–357. disorder associated with the measles-mumps-
Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Comprehension of rubella vaccine? A CPEA study. Journal of
indirect requests in adults with mental retar- Autism & Developmental Disorders.
dation and pervasive developmental disorders. Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1976). Language, com-
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, munication and use of symbols. In L. Wing
475–479. (Ed.), Early childhood autism (pp. 93–134).
Paul, R., Cohen, D. J., & Caparulo, B. K. (1983). A Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
longitudinal study of patients with severe de- Roberts, J., Rice, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004).
velopmental disorders of language learning. Tense marking in children with autism. Ap-
Journal of the American Academy of Child plied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448.
Psychiatry, 22, 525–534. Rogers, S. (2004). Evidence-based intervention for
Paul, R., & Feldman, C. (1984). Communication language development in young children with
deficits in autism. Paper presented at the Insti- autism. In T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Ef-
tute for Communication Deficits in Autistic ficacy of intervention for autism. New York:
Youth, Columbia University, New York. Guilford.
Paul, R., Fischer, M. L., & Cohen, D. (1988). Sen- Rogers, S., Hepburn, S., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner,
tence comprehension strategies in children E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers
with autism and specific language disorders. with autism and those with other developmen-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- tal disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and
ders, 18, 669–680. Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.
Peters, A. (1983). The units of language acquisi- Rojas, D., Bawn, S., Benkers, T., Reite, M., &
tion. New York: Cambridge University Press. Rogers, S. (2002). Smaller left hemisphere
Pierce, S., & Bartolucci, G. (1977). A syntactic in- planum temporale in adults with autistic disor-
vestigation of verbal autistic, mentally re- der. Neuroscience Letters, 328, 237–240.
tarded, and normal children. Journal of Autism Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, M. D., & Sceery, W. R.
and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 121–134. (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychi-
Pinker, S. (1999). How the mind works. Annals of the atric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal
New York Academy of Sciences, 882, 119–127. of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
Prizant, B. (1983). Echolalia in autism: Assessment 24, 465–473.
and intervention. Seminars in Speech and Lan- Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to
guage, 4, 63–77. adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2, 435–450.
Prizant, B., & Duchan, J. (1981). The functions of Rutter, M. (1987). The “ what ” and “ how” of lan-
immediate echolalia in autistic children. Journal guage development: A note on some out-
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 241–249. standing issues and questions. In W. Yule &
Pronovost, W., Wakstein, M., & Wakstein, D. M. Rutter (Eds.), Language development and dis-
(1966). A longitudinal study of speech behav- orders (pp. 159–170). London: MacKeith Press.
Language and Communication in Autism 363

Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). St. James, P. J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). An
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). observational study of humor in autism and
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Rutter, M., & Lord, C. (1987). Language impair- opmental Disorders, 24, 603–617.
ment associated with psychiatric disorder. In Stone, W. L., & Caro-Martinez, L. M. (1990). Nat-
W. Yule, M. Rutter, & M. Bax (Eds.), Lan- uralistic observations of spontaneous com-
guage development and disorders: Clinic in de- munication in autistic children. Journal of
velopmental medicine, 101/102 (pp. 206–233). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20,
London: Simp/Seineman. 437–453.
Rutter, M., Mawhood, L., & Howlin, P. (1992). Lan- Stone, W., Lemanek, K., Fishel, P., Fernandez, M.,
guage delay and social development. In P. & Altemeier, W. (1990). Play and imitation
Fletcher & D. Hall (Eds.), Specific speech and skills in the diagnosis of autism in young chil-
language disorders in children: Correlates, dren. Pediatrics, 86, 267–272.
characteristics and outcomes (pp. 63–78). Lon- Storoschuk, S., Lord, C., & Jaedicke, S. (1995).
don: Whurr. Autism and the use of mental verbs. Paper pre-
Scarborough, H. S., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, sented at the Society for Research in child De-
H., Fowler, A. E., & Sudhalter, V. (1991). The velopment, Indianapolis, IN.
relation of utterance length to grammatical Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H.
complexity in normal and language-disordered (1996). Are children with autism deaf to
groups. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 23–45. Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1,
Shapiro, T. (1977). The quest for a linguistic model 55–72.
to study the speech of autistic children: Stud- Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981a). On the nature of lin-
ies of echoing. Journal of the American Acad- guistic functioning in early infantile autism.
emy of Child Psychiatry, 16, 608–619. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Sheinkopf, S., Mundy, P., Oller, K., & Steffens, M. ders, 11, 45–56.
(2000). Vocal atypicalities of preverbal autis- Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981b). Sentence comprehen-
tic children. Journal of Autism and Develop- sion in autistic children. Applied Psycholin-
mental Disorders, 30, 345–354. guistics, 2, 5–24.
Short, C. B., & Schopler, E. (1988). Factors relat- Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). The conceptual basis
ing to age of onset in autism. Journal of Autism for referential word meaning in children with
and Developmental Disorders, 18, 207–216. autism. Child Development, 56, 1167–1178.
Shriberg, L., Paul, R., McSweeney, J., Klin, A., Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). A psycholinguistic per-
Cohen, D., & Volkmar, F. (2001). Speech and spective on language development in the autis-
prosody characteristics of adolescents and tic child. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: New
adults with high-functioning autism and As- directions in diagnosis, nature and treatment
perger syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, (pp. 92–115). New York: Guilford Press.
and Hearing Research, 44, 1097–1115. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in
Siegel, B., Vukicevic, J., Elliott, G., & Kraemer, H. the free recall of autistic children: Further ev-
(1989). The use of signal detection theory to idence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal
assess DSM-III-R criteria for autistic disorder. of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk
Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 542–548. about psychological states: Deficits in the
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child
change in the social competence of children Development, 63, 161–172.
with autism, Down syndrome and develop- Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals
mental delays. Monographs of the Society about the understanding of minds in children
for Research in Child Development, 64, Serial with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-
No. 256. Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understand-
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1981). Sensorimotor ing other minds: Perspectives from autism
skills and language comprehension in autistic (pp. 138–157). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- versity Press.
ogy, 9, 149–166. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Dissociations in form
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of and function in the acquisition of language by
parents of children with autism predict the autistic children. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.),
subsequent development of their children’s Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of
communication. Journal of Autism and Devel- atypical children (pp. 175–194). Hillsdale, NJ:
opmental Disorders, 32, 77–89. Erlbaum.
364 Development and Behavior

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). “Once upon a ribbit ”: and non-ostensive contests. Journal of Child
Stories narrated by autistic children. British Language, 19, 311–333.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, Van Lancker, D., Cornelius, C., & Needleman, R.
45–59. (1991). Comprehension of verbal terms for
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Language impairments emotions in normal, autistic, and schizophrenic
in children with complex neurodevelopmental children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7,
disorders: The case of autism. In Y. Levy & J. 1–18.
Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across Ventner, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, D. (1992). A
populations: Toward a definition of specific follow-up study of high-functioning autistic
language impairment (pp. 297–321). Mahwah, children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
NJ: Erlbaum. chiatry, 33, 489–507.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The Volden, J., & Lord, C. (1991). Neologisms and idio-
development of contingent discourse ability in syncratic language in autistic speakers. Jour-
autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
and Psychiatry, 32, 1123–1134. 21, 109–130.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Calkins, S. (1990). Does im- Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegal, B., Szatmari, P.,
itation facilitate the acquisition of grammar? Lord, C., Campbell, M., et al. (1994). Field
Evidence from a study of autistic, Down syn- trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV. American
drome and normal children. Journal of Child Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1361–1367.
Language, 17, 591–606. Wahlberg, T., & Magliano, J. P. (2004). The ability
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Noin, I., Baum- of high function individuals with autism
berger, T., Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Denis, to comprehend written discourse. Discourse
A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language Processes, 38(1), 119–144.
acquisition in autistic and Down Syndrome Weir, R. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague,
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental The Netherlands: Mouton.
Disorders, 20, 1–22. Wetherby, A. (1986). Ontogeny of communication
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identi- functions in autism. Journal of Autism and De-
fying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. velopmental Disorders, 16, 295–316.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci- Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles
ety, Series, B, 358, 303–314. of communicative and cognitive-social abili-
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). A sec- ties in autistic children. Journal of Speech and
ond look at second-order belief attribution in Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
Disorders, 24, 577–586. account. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Taylor, A. (2004). Telescoping in parent reports on Disorders, 9, 11–29.
milestones in autism. Unpublished master’s Yule, W., & Rutter, M. (1987). Language develop-
thesis, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL. ment and disorders. London: MacKeith.
Thurber, C., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). Pauses Ziatas, K., Durkin, K., & Pratt, C. (2003). Differ-
in the narratives produced by autistic, men- ences in assertive speech acts produced by
tally retarded, and normal children as an index children with autism, Asperger syndrome, spe-
of cognitive demand. Journal of Autism and cific language impairment, and normal devel-
Developmental Disorders. opment. Development and Psychopathology,
Tomasello, M., & Kruger, A. C. (1992). Joint atten- 15, 73–94.
tion on actions: Acquiring verbs in ostensive
CHAPTER 13

Neuropsychological Characteristics in
Autism and Related Conditions

KATHERINE D. TSATSANIS

Autism represents the paradigmatic pervasive world. Core deficits in regulation, integration,
developmental disorder (PDD) but is itself and flexibility are represented in these models.
characterized by considerable clinical variabil-
ity. Symptoms and signs typify this psychiatric OVERVIEW
syndrome (as well as others) rather than etiol-
ogies; in the face of substantial phenotypic Autism and related spectrum conditions are
heterogeneity, the identification of core psycho- neurodevelopmental disorders that primarily
logical markers can be helpful toward studies involve disruptions of social development, im-
of pathogenesis. There are numerous competing paired verbal and nonverbal communication,
theories to account for pathways of development and behavioral disturbances. Reviewing neu-
in autism. In the literature on the psychologi- ropsychological factors in autism and PDD
cal and developmental aspects of the disorder, presents several challenges as the literature is
specific social-cognitive as well as general expansive and diverse. Psychological models
perceptual cognitive and learning mechanisms that currently dominate the field include the-
have been examined (Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, ory of mind, central coherence, and executive
Schultz, & Klin, 2004). Within each of these functions, with related research findings inter-
divisions, there are specific skills and pro- preted and integrated into these specific con-
cesses proposed to have causal explanatory structs. Because these models are reviewed
value, including joint attention and face pro- elsewhere in this Handbook, they will not be
cessing skills on the one hand, and sensory critically evaluated here. Social-cognitive
perception, attention, memory, and executive mechanisms and language development also
functions on the other. fall within the domain of neuropsychological
An approach to understanding autism that function, but each is addressed in a separate
puts faith in the explanatory power of a single chapter. In this chapter, focus is placed on spe-
construct is highly suspect. At the very least, cific cognitive learning mechanisms later po-
the complexity and variability of expression of sitioned in the context of a broader discussion
the syndrome argues against a unitary cause. of relevant issues in the field.
Moreover, the proposed constructs are them- Despite the noted quantity of research,
selves neither unitary nor fully explicated. Al- methodological issues present a special chal-
though individual researchers may emphasize lenge in this review because they render find-
different components of functioning and use a ings among studies equivocal and limit general
distinct language, they are alike in fundamen- conclusions. A major source of variability lies
tal ways. Each of their theories represents an in subject selection, which includes age, level
attempt to characterize how individuals with of functioning, choice of control groups, and
autism acquire and process information and in number of subjects. The possibility of clinical
turn form an internal representation of the heterogeneity between groups of individuals

365
366 Development and Behavior

with autism is a crucial concern that highlights often appear to involve a sensory stimulatory
the need for a well-characterized sample and component (e.g., visual fascination with spin-
appropriate controls. In addition, a more ning wheels, fans, string), and it is argued that
clearly systematic approach to the investiga- disturbances of movement, such as hand flap-
tion of these domains of functioning will ad- ping and whirling, may in fact provide wanted
vance understanding. sensory input through visual and proprioceptive
One unequivocal characteristic of PDD is channels (Ornitz, 1983). Variability of response
clinical variability; children with various to sensory input may also be linked to the indi-
forms of PDD may share many core features, vidual’s state of arousal. Grandin (1995) makes
but their individual pathways to learning or a connection between her own problems in sen-
cognitive profiles have unique characteristics. sory reactivity and her unresponsive affect (or
Treatment approaches are enhanced when the shutting down) and fearful states.
core areas of strength and vulnerability for in- In current research, autistic children’s un-
dividuals with autism are identified and evalu- usual responsivity to sensory stimuli and un-
ated across stages of development. In the usual patterns of behavior are understudied
current discussion, the specific assets and relative to other symptom areas, such as social,
deficits identified are in most cases neither language, and cognitive deficits. Yet, an abnor-
specific to nor characteristic of all individuals mal response to sensory stimulation is consis-
with autism. They do, however, represent tently found to differentiate between children
meaningful areas for clinical consideration and with autism and developmentally matched con-
recognize significant programs of research. trols in studies of early behavioral characteris-
tics (Adrien et al., 1992; Dahlgren & Gillberg,
SENSORY PERCEPTION 1989; Osterling & Dawson, 1994; Stone, 1997).
This cluster of behaviors includes: empty gaze;
In early emerging cognitive theories of autism, visual fascination with patterns and movements;
sensory disturbances are well documented and failure to react to sounds/appearing deaf; hy-
viewed as a primary area of deficit. Ornitz and posensitivity to pain, cold, or heat; hypersen-
Ritvo (1968b) detailed the range of hypo- and sitivity to taste; and inappropriate use of
hypersensitivities affecting each of the senses objects (e.g., interest in the sensory aspects
in over 150 cases of autism. Based on their ob- of objects, such as licking/mouthing, peering,
servations, they postulated that an inability to or interest in texture). Abnormal sensory re-
adequately modulate sensory input in children activity appears to differentiate children with
with autism manifests itself in alternating autism from typical and mixed developmen-
states of excitement (e.g., spinning, hand flap- tally delayed groups by 21⁄ 2 years of age, al-
ping, hypersensitivity to stimuli) and inhibi- though children with fragile X syndrome also
tion (e.g., nonresponsiveness) and that this show increased sensory symptoms (Rogers,
state of dysregulation, or homeostatic imbal- Hepburn, & Wehner, 2003).
ance, in turn leads to perceptual inconstancy. Studies using parent questionnaires also re-
From this account, inadequate modulation of port more severe or more frequent sensory
sensory input in autism produces inconsistent symptoms in young (3 through 6 years of age;
and disordered perceptions of external events. Watling, Deitz, & White, 2001) and school-
The ( lack of ) coherence of these children’s age children with autism (Kientz & Dunn,
perceptions is considered to impact on early 1997) as well as children with Asperger syn-
developmental achievements including social drome (AS; Dunn, Myles, & Orr, 2002), with
relating and communication (Ornitz, 1974, the need for external validation using clinical
1983; Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968a, 1968b, 1976). assessment methods. A significant association
Descriptions of unusual responses to sen- was not found when the relationship between
sory stimuli are readily found in first-person sensory symptom severity and severity of
accounts of autism. For example, Temple autism symptomotology was examined (Kientz
Grandin (1992) makes note of her profound hy- & Dunn, 1997; Rogers et al., 2003), but degree
persensitivity to touch and sound. The repeti- of abnormal sensory responsivity and impair-
tive behaviors that are manifest in autism also ments in adaptive behavior were related
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 367

(Rogers et al., 2003). The sensory qualities et al., 1994) proposed that attention was a cen-
associated with autism do not appear to be tral deficit in autism and that the neocerebellum
specific to the disorder, nor is there evidence was an important structure in the coordination
of a direct relationship between these dist- of attention and arousal systems, presenting evi-
urbances and the central social and commu- dence for its abnormal development in autism
nicative impairments, but there is sufficient (Courchesne, Saitoh, et al., 1994; Courchesne,
evidence to indicate that unusual responses to Yeung-Courchesne, Press, Hesselink, & Jerni-
sensory stimuli are a significant feature. gan, 1988; Murakami, Courchesne, Press, Yeung-
The sensory features in autism merit fur- Courchesne, Hesselink, 1989).
ther clinical and research attention. One ap-
proach to the presence of these sensory ATTENTION
disturbances has been to appreciate more
fully their connection with levels of arousal, Specific behavioral observations as well as
attention, emotional regulation, and action or programs of research give emphasis to impair-
adaptive goal-directed behavior (Anzalone & ments in attention in individuals with autism.
Williamson, 2000; Laurent & Rubin, 2004). Attention is a core capacity that is central to
Ornitz and Ritvo (1968a; Ornitz, 1983, 1988) the processes of information reduction, re-
also proposed neurological substrates for a sponse selection, and preparation for eventual
disturbance in the modulation of sensory action. New information arrives in the form of
input in autism. Their earlier writings pointed a continuous flow of both internal and external
to the reticular activating system and vestibu- stimuli, and individuals develop an increasing
lar system. More recently, Ornitz (1988) has capacity to override the impulse to attend to
also emphasized a role for the thalamus. In what is most striking or novel or desired in
their seminal paper, Damasio and Maurer order to anticipate, direct, or guide attention
(1978) concluded that multiple brain regions based on prior knowledge and internal goals.
are involved in autism, including the mesial Behavioral qualities of individuals with autism
frontal lobes, mesial temporal lobes, basal often include an intense focus on unusual
ganglia, and thalamus (specifically, dorsome- features of objects and repetitive activities,
dial and anterior nuclear groups). The thala- attention to nonsalient aspects of the environ-
mus has traditionally been referred to as the ment, and difficulty shifting focus or transi-
sensory gateway of the cortex, but a current tioning from one activity to the next. The
perspective of this structure suggests that it is preliminary research on attention in autism
involved in multiple processes that permit the points to potential areas of relative function
transmission, tuning, and integrated process- and dysfunction, with the early emphasis on
ing of information in the brain. A magnetic the role of sensory modulation systems in
resonance imaging (MRI ) study of the thala- autism supplanted in this literature by an em-
mus in individuals with autism indicated that phasis on attentional systems.
despite increases in cortical size, the thalamus Sustained attention for simple repetitive
does not appear to develop to the expected de- visual information is generally intact in indi-
gree in individuals with autism (Tsatsanis viduals with autism compared to developmen-
et al., 2003). tally matched controls, as measured by
The early emphasis on the role of sensory continuous performance tasks (Buchsbaum
modulation systems was later supplanted by an et al., 1992; Casey, Gordon, Mannheim, &
emphasis on attentional systems. The focus Rumsey, 1993; Garretson, Fein, & Water-
moved away from response to sensory input to house, 1990; Goldstein, Johnson, & Minshew,
the processes involved in the identification and 2001; Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegel, 1997;
selection of relevant information in general. Pascualvaca, Fantie, Papageorgiou, & Mirsky,
This was reflected in deficit terminology such as 1998), whereas a preliminary study of atten-
stimulus overselectivity, directed attention, and tion in Asperger syndrome (AS) was sugges-
attentional shifts. In addition, at least one group tive of an attention deficit, specifically
of researchers (Courchesne, 1995; Courchesne, manifest in an inconsistent or variable re-
Saitoh, et al., 1994; Courchesne, Townsend, sponse pattern to stimuli in a sustained visual
368 Development and Behavior

attention task (Schatz, Weimer, & Trauner, mechanisms in future research. Individuals
2002). Deficits in attention in autism are typi- with autism and AS show deficiencies with re-
cally reported for more complex tasks requiring gard to processing the most essential or salient
filtering, selective attention, and shifts in atten- information from stimulus-rich environments
tional focus. On a target discrimination task, (Klin, Jones, Schultz, & Volkmar, 2003) and
low-functioning individuals with autism were attending to meaningful or shared aspects of a
found to benefit from the presentation of a tar- learning situation, namely, those not explicitly
get location cue, but were more susceptible to stated (Klin, 2000), both of which are of great
nontarget distracters than their developmentally relevance to the daily lives of individuals with
matched counterparts (Burack, 1994). Ciesiel- autism. Developmentally, a related concept is
ski, Courchesne, and Elmasian (1990) compared that of joint attention. Joint attention skills,
high-functioning adults with autism to age- acts of coordinating attention between interac-
matched controls on a divided-attention task in- tive social partners and environmental stimuli,
volving two modalities—vision and audition. are a distinguishing deficit area for children
The adults with autism showed significantly with autism (considered in more depth in
higher false alarm rates on this task, indicating Chapter 25). Orienting to the social overtures
greater difficulty ignoring the competing stim- of others requires that specific information is
uli. This result was also interpreted as persever- registered, attended to, and has meaning. The
ative behavior or a failure to shift from one findings of attentional limitations in both the
response modality to the other. The findings social and cognitive arenas reflect two theo-
from other studies support the notion that indi- retical perspectives that have historically
viduals with autism may have difficulty shifting dominated the literature. The first asserts that
attention both within and between modalities social impairments in individuals with autism
(Casey et al., 1993; Courchesne, Townsend, are a secondary effect of a primary deficit in
et al., 1994; Townsend, Harris, & Courchesne, cognitive functioning (e.g., Courchesne, Town-
1996; Wainwright-Sharp & Bryson, 1996). The send, et al., 1994; DeLong, 1992; Hughes,
two important variables identified in these stud- Russell, & Robbins, 1994; Ozonoff, 1995;
ies were speed and expectancy, suggesting that Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991;
individuals with autism have specific difficulty Ozonoff, Strayer, McMahon, & Filloux, 1994).
making rapid changes in their expectations. Re- In the second, social impairment, and specifi-
searchers who place this deficit at a higher cally social cognition, is considered to be the
order level suggest that it is part of a more gen- primary deficit in autism (e.g., Baron-Cohen,
eral difficulty with executive control originat- Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Happe & Frith, 1995).
ing from frontal lobe dysfunction, as evidenced The pattern of relative strengths in atten-
by specific deficits on measures of attention tion, namely, the ability to sustain attention to
that require cognitive flexibility or shifting be- simple repetitive visual information (i.e., sta-
tween categories, sets, or rules (Goldstein et al., tic visual cues) versus relative vulnerabilities
2001; Ozonoff et al., 2004; Pascualvaca et al., in attention such as determining salient aspects
1998). Others find evidence for a more basic de- of the environment and shifting attention, has
ficiency in broadening the spread of visual at- significant implications for educational pro-
tention (e.g., the attentional spotlight) and gramming. Therapists and educators can, for
difficulty moving from local to global process- example, introduce consistent and static visual
ing in support of the theory of weak central co- supports in the environment and within spe-
herence (Mann & Walker, 2003). Burack, Enns, cific activities to enhance attention to salient
Stauder, Mottron, and Randolf (1997) in their pieces of information and to the need to shift
review originally emphasized a deficit in volun- to a new piece of information when relevant.
tary control over appropriate sizing of atten- For young children with autism who are at a
tional gaze to allow for efficient task prelinguistic or emerging language level, so-
performance or focus on what is relevant in the cial orienting and joint attention are relevant
context of ignoring what is extraneous. programmatic goals. As infants begin to coor-
The evidence from behavioral research dinate their attention to objects and people,
points to the importance of clarifying these they also begin to direct the attention of others
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 369

to their objects of desire or interest (initiate (e.g., stimulus-response learning), and proce-
joint attention). They further monitor others’ dural mechanisms, but show a more limited
focus of attention (respond to joint attention capacity for flexibility, abstraction, and gen-
bids), deriving vast amounts of information eralization. It is consistently found that indi-
about interests and dangers in the environ- viduals with autism show intact digit span and
ment, and begin their participation in social intact immediate recall for semantically unre-
learning opportunities (Adamson, 1995; lated lists of words relative to ability-matched
Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Mundy, Sigman, & and, in some cases, normal controls (Bennetto,
Kasari, 1994; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Pennington, & Rogers, 1996; Goldstein et al.,
Sherman, 1986; Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 2001; Lincoln, Allen, & Kilman, 1995). There
1993; Ulvund & Smith, 1996). There is evi- is also evidence to suggest well-developed as-
dence further to support the relationship be- sociative learning mechanisms (Boucher &
tween these early developmental processes and Warrington, 1976; Minshew & Goldstein,
language development in autism (Charman 2001; Minshew, Goldstein, Muenz, & Payton,
et al., 2001, 2003; Lord & Schopler, 1989; 1992). However, overall performance on the
Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Mundy et al., immediate recall of semantically related lists
1994). Establishing early intentional behaviors of words was impaired for autistic subjects
and a consistent means for expressing intent relative to ability-matched and normal controls
appear to have a pivotal role in other aspects of (Boucher & Warrington, 1976; Tager-Flusberg,
development in individuals with autism. 1991). When a cued recall paradigm was used,
the autistic subjects benefited from the provi-
MEMORY sion of semantic cues, suggesting a deficit in
retrieval versus encoding processes. Individu-
It is well to understand the role of memory in als with autism may equally encode the mean-
autism because very few aspects of higher ings of the words presented but be deficient in
cognitive function and learning could operate their ability to employ a strategic search to as-
successfully without some memory contribu- sist their retrieval of the unrecalled semanti-
tion. Memory is often treated as a unitary con- cally related words. The results of similar list
struct but should be recognized as comprising learning tasks also offer some support for inef-
multiple interrelated systems. Both the behav- ficiency in the ability to actively organize in-
ioral and neuropsychological research evi- formation during learning and retrieval for
dence suggests the usefulness of considering individuals with autism, reflected, for exam-
different aspects of memory in autism. Some ple, in an impaired serial position effect or
such individuals show extraordinary memory failure to group words into conceptual cate-
for discrete domains of knowledge. At the gories (Bennetto et al., 1996; Minshew &
same time, they are observed to have tremen- Goldstein, 1993; Minshew et al., 1992; Ren-
dous difficulty navigating their daily environ- ner, Klinger, & Klinger, 2000). This last effect
ment, such as remembering where objects and was also found in adults with AS (Bowler,
belongings are located and remembering their Matthews, & Gardiner, 1997). Tasks requiring
schedule of classes and activities or morning a greater level of semantic organization also
routine. Further, the capacity of higher func- appear to impact the memory performance of
tioning individuals to provide a reliable ac- autistic subjects, which may reflect a more
count of the day’s activities or to recollect general deficiency related to complexity of the
personal experiences is more limited than material to be remembered (Fein et al., 1996;
might be predicted based on level of language Minshew & Goldstein, 2001).
or cognitive functioning alone. In the context Organization has a role in memory as do
of these observations, provisional conclusions other executive control processes. Working
that can be drawn from studies of memory memory tasks require the ability to simulta-
function in autism and AS are considered next. neously attend to, recall, and act on informa-
Clinical observations suggest that individu- tion held in an online state. This aspect of
als with autism typically achieve learning memory function is often considered in the do-
through rote memory, classical conditioning main of executive function and is needed for
370 Development and Behavior

completion of many such problem-solving children with autism showed significantly


tasks. Although deficits in working memory poorer recall of the first three words of the
may be found in autism, they do not appear to lists (primacy effect) but comparable recall of
be specific to this disorder. In a recent study the last three words (recency effect). It is pro-
(Russell, Jarrold, & Henry, 1996), when pre- posed that the last three words in the list are
sented with working memory capacity tasks, maintained in the articulatory loop of the
both children with autism and children with working memory system. This account is con-
moderate learning difficulties performed sig- sistent with the results of Russell et al.’s
nificantly more poorly than the normal con- (1996) study indicating that the articulatory
trols on all three tasks. There was no loop is intact in individuals with autism. In
difference, however, in the performance be- contrast, recall of the first three words of the
tween the two comparison groups. The authors study episode requires the individual to con-
concluded that working memory deficits are sciously recollect and reexamine the study list,
not specific to autism but are likely related to thereby drawing on episodic memory.
a general deficit in information processing Boucher (1981b) also examined memory for
(marked by level of intellectual functioning). recent events in three groups of children who
Ozonoff and Strayer (2001) also reported no ranged in age from 10 to 16 years. They in-
autism-specific impairments in working mem- cluded children with autism, mentally retarded
ory in the context of a significant association children, and normal controls. The children
between IQ, age, and performance on working were present for a 1- to 2-hour session, during
memory tasks. Bennetto et al. (1996) ostensi- which they participated in a variety of activi-
bly found a different set of results, but a care- ties. At the end of the session, the materials
ful consideration of the tasks used in their were cleared away and the children were asked
study is more suggestive of an evaluation of to recall the session’s events. Single-word re-
episodic versus working memory processes sponses and gestures were acceptable. Boucher
(see later discussion). reported a significant difference among the re-
Most memory tasks comprise both implicit call scores of all three groups. The children
and explicit learning components. Explicit with autism recalled significantly fewer events
memory, also referred to as declarative mem- relative to both comparison groups. In addi-
ory, is considered with reference to two sub- tion, the performance of the mentally retarded
systems defined by Tulving (1972) as episodic children was poorer than that of the normal
and semantic memory. Episodic memory refers controls. It is argued that this type of task also
to the system involved in recollecting particu- assesses episodic memory because it requires
lar experiences, whereas semantic memory the children to think back to and reexperience
refers to factual knowledge or knowledge of a prior subjective event. Notably, Boucher
the world. Bowler and colleagues examined (1981b) observed that the autistic children’s
episodic memory function in adults with AS in recall improved when they were provided with
a series of studies and reported impairments in cueing strategies. Again, the deficit appears
source memory as well as greater reliance on to lie in retrieval (e.g., making use of context-
knowing and less reliance on remembering rel- dependent cues) versus encoding processes.
ative to controls (e.g., Bowler, Gardiner, & The results of a second related study appear
Grice, 2000; Gardiner, Bowler, & Grice, to support this interpretation (Boucher & Lewis,
2003). Episodic memory function has also 1989). In this case, children were asked to recall
been examined in autistic subjects, although activities that they had taken part in several
often not the stated focus of study. Boucher months earlier. The performance of children
(1981a) compared the performance of autistic with autism was compared to learning-disabled
children and ability-matched controls on a test controls under free recall (e.g., open questions)
of immediate recall of a series of word lists. and cued recall (e.g., leading questions) condi-
There was no significant difference between tions. Whereas the free recall scores differed
the total recall scores of the two groups; how- significantly, the cued recall scores were not
ever, there was a significant difference be- significantly different between the two groups.
tween their primacy and recency scores. The Although the children with autism engaged in
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 371

little self-cueing, they were able to benefit from been found (Bowler et al., 1997; Gardiner
cues provided by the examiner. et al., 2003; Renner et al., 2000). Whereas age
Bennetto et al. (1996) examined whether and level of intellectual ability are more
individuals with autism display a pattern of strongly associated with performance on ex-
deficits that is similar to patients with frontal plicit memory tasks, task complexity (e.g.,
lesions. Measures of temporal order memory, level of conceptual analysis or semantic orga-
source memory, and working memory were nization) is likely to be a strong factor in both.
administered. The subjects consisted of high- Atypical patterns of results on list learning
functioning autistic (HFA) children and chil- tasks (e.g., with manipulations to examine the
dren with learning disabilities. A comparison effect of variables such as levels of processing,
of the performance of the two groups showed associative value, and redundancy) also sug-
that the children with autism were signifi- gest that it is the relation between semantic
cantly impaired on all tasks. Notably, the memory and episodic memory that explains
autistic group also displayed significantly impairment on explicit memory tasks (Bevers-
more intrusion errors on a list-learning task, dorf et al., 2000; Toichi & Kamio, 2002, 2003).
which was interpreted as a deficit in source There may not be a problem in semantic mem-
memory. Bennetto et al. observed that the chil- ory per se ( but rather effective retrieval from
dren with autism in their study displayed a semantic memory), but the question remains
pattern of memory function that is similar to whether the organization of semantic memory
that of patients with frontal lesions and inter- in individuals with autism is similar to that of
preted their findings in terms of a general typically developing individuals.
deficit in working memory. However, the tasks There are a small number of researchers
that they employed in their study appeared who have speculated that at least some of the
more consistent with a usual approach to the characteristics of autism could be explained in
assessment of episodic memory. On the Sen- terms of a memory deficit. Boucher (1981a;
tence and Counting Span tasks, subjects were Boucher & Warrington, 1976) suggested that
required to process information, but they were the pattern of memory performance of autistic
not asked to simultaneously store that informa- individuals is similar to that of patients with
tion. Rather, the subjects were asked to recall medial temporal lobe amnesic disorder; how-
their responses at the end of the task. Wheeler, ever, the cumulative research evidence does
Stuss, and Tulving (1997) identified three not support this proposal. DeLong (1992) pos-
types of tests that assess episodic memory. tulated that autism is the developmental syn-
One approach is to assess aspects of the learn- drome of hippocampal dysfunction in the
ing episode that are not central to the target in- young child and proposed that individuals with
formation. The Sentence and Counting span autism are not able to benefit from experiential
tasks appear to fulfill this requirement. The learning (i.e., integrating past and present ex-
items requested for recall on these tasks periences to create a structure of meaning),
would be recoverable only through a conscious which may account for a typically rote or rigid
recollection of the study episode. In addition, manner when engaged in events and interac-
Wheeler et al. identified tests of memory for tions and the need for explicit preparation
source and memory for temporal order, specif- when entering a novel context or social situa-
ically Corsi’s task, as measures of episodic tion. Powell and Jordan (1993) discuss a role
memory. Again, these appear to be precisely for episodic memory and its implications for
the kinds of instruments employed in the Ben- teaching autistic individuals how to learn.
netto et al. study. There is also some support for abnormalities in
In contrast to episodic memory, implicit brain regions identified as central to memory
memory does not require conscious or inten- functions. Notably, the pathophysiology of
tional recollection of experiences although they autism implicates regions including the hip-
are shown to have an effect on current perfor- pocampal formation, amygdala, and cerebel-
mance. When perceptual processing tasks have lum (Abell et al., 1999; Aylward et al., 1999;
been used to examine implicit memory in Bachevalier, 1994; Bachevalier & Merjanian,
autism and AS, no evidence of impairment has 1994; Bauman & Kemper, 1994; Courchesne
372 Development and Behavior

et al., 1988; Kemper & Bauman, 1993; Schultz, Strayer, 1997; Ozonoff et al., 1994; Rumsey &
Romanski, & Tsatsanis, 2000). Clinical (e.g., Hamburger, 1990; Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson,
executive function) studies support a role for & Bartolucci, 1990). Conceptual flexibility
frontal lobe dysfunction in autism. Bennetto versus perceptual or attentional flexibility (or
et al. (1996) observed that the autistic children simple inhibitory control) appears to be the
in their study displayed a pattern of memory predominant deficit in higher functioning indi-
function that is similar to that of patients with viduals (Goldstein et al., 2001; Ozonoff et al.,
frontal lesions. Evidence from PET studies in- 1994, 2004; Ozonoff & Strayer, 1997). Addi-
dicates that episodic retrieval is associated tionally, rule learning and shifting within a
with increased blood flow in the right pre- rule or category are within the range of normal
frontal cortex. In addition, episodic encoding function (Berger, Aerts, van Spaendonck,
has been consistently associated with activa- Cools, & Teunisse, 2003; Minshew et al.,
tion in the region of the left prefrontal cortex 2002; Ozonoff et al., 2004). Higher function-
(reviewed by Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000). ing individuals with autism show some capac-
ity to learn rules and procedures as well as
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION identify concepts but are challenged to abstract
information to attain concepts or develop flex-
Although there is some overlap with the do- ible strategies for problem solving, which may
mains of attention and memory, distinct mea- be evidenced most directly by perseverative
sures, concepts, and neuroanatomical regions errors or persisting in a strategy even when it
characterize this literature, especially per- is not successful.
taining to function in the prefrontal cortex and Strong group differences in performance
measures developed to assess such function. have also been found on the Tower of Hanoi
Several behavioral characteristics of individu- and modified versions of this task (Bennetto
als with autism are reminiscent of the kinds of et al., 1996; Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff
impairments seen in patients with prefrontal et al., 1991, 1994, 2004; Szatmari et al.,
cortical damage. These characteristics include 1990). Ozonoff et al. (1991) reported that the
response perseveration, disinhibition, narrow Tower of Hanoi provided the highest dis-
range of interests, failure to plan, difficulty criminatory power between groups of high-
taking the perspective of others, and lack of functioning children with autism and matched
self-monitoring. A failure to generalize newly controls, relative to other measures (e.g., the-
learned concepts is also ascribed to deficits in ory of mind, memory, emotion perception, and
higher cognitive functioning. This led at least visual spatial tasks). This finding was particu-
one group of researchers (e.g., Ozonoff and larly interesting because the control group
colleagues) to have proposed a primary role for consisted of children who might also be ex-
executive function deficits in autism and im- pected to show executive function deficits
plicate frontal regions in its expression. (e.g., 25% of the control sample met criteria
Executive function corresponds to the abil- for attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder
ity to maintain an appropriate problem-solving [ADHD]). Deficits were found at both lower
set to guide future (goal-directed) behavior. and higher levels of IQ in individuals with
It is composed of a set of abilities including autism on a related task (Stockings of Cam-
inhibition, set shifting, planning, memory, bridge), and the expected age-related im-
and self-monitoring. From studies of tradi- provement in planning efficiency was not
tional executive function tasks, such as the observed in the autistic group, indicating that
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, it is observed these impairments may be most severe in ado-
that for individuals with autism, the capacity lescence (Ozonoff et al., 2004). This may ex-
to deal with complex information or new situa- plain failures to find group differences in
tions is limited by deficits in cognitive flexi- younger children with autism on executive
bility and/or an incomplete understanding of function tasks (e.g., Griffith, Pennington,
novel /abstract concepts (Bennetto et al., 1996; Wehner, & Rogers, 1999).
Goldstein et al., 2001; Minshew, Meyer, & These tests are commonly used measures of
Goldstein, 2002; Ozonoff & McEvoy, 1994; executive function that require the individual
Ozonoff, Pennington, et al., 1991; Ozonoff & to solve a problem by planning before acting
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 373

and identifying the subgoals needed to reach a regions in the prefrontal cortex are distin-
target goal. The task also typically taps rule fol- guished: Damage to dorsolateral regions is as-
lowing and procedural learning. It is proposed sociated with impairment in high-level
that the challenge for higher functioning indi- cognitive ability, whereas damage to orbital
viduals with autism is related to planning effi- regions is associated with disturbances in so-
ciency (Ozonoff et al., 2004) and resolving cial and affective behavior (e.g., Damasio,
goal-subgoal conflicts (Goldstein et al., 2001). 1985). Ozonoff notes that because the frontal
When children with HFA and ADHD were com- cortex is central to the regulation of both
pared on a range of tasks involving executive higher cognitive and social emotional behavior
functions, the HFA group showed broad-based and individuals with autism display deficits in
deficits but were distinguished by more signifi- both of these domains, then frontal lobe dys-
cant difficulties with planning and cognitive function may be able to account for the im-
flexibility, whereas the ADHD group showed pairments seen in autism. Recent results of
deficits in inhibiting a prepotent response and neuroimaging studies do offer preliminary
verbal fluency (Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, support for frontal lobe involvement in autism
Roeyers, & Sergeant, 2004). Parent ratings of (Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Luna et al., 2002;
the behavior in these groups of children also Minshew, Luna, & Sweeney, 1999; Zilbovicius
yield a range of elevations on executive function et al., 1995). Notably, however, tasks designed
scales although, again, the autism group is dis- to tap cognitive function associated with
tinguishable by deficits in flexibility whereas frontal and medial temporal regions of the
the ADHD (combined) group exhibits more se- brain were more strongly related to communi-
vere inhibitory deficits when compared (Gioia, cation symptoms (Joseph & Tager-Flusberg,
Isquith, Kenworthy, & Barton, 2002). 2004) and adaptive behavior (Ozonoff et al.,
Individuals with AS also show deficits in 2004) than to autism severity or specific autism
executive functioning and may perform symptoms related to social interaction and
equally poorly as individuals with HFA (Man- repetitive behavior. In their sample of verbal,
jiviona & Prior, 1999; Miller & Ozonoff, 2000; school-age children, executive control abilities
Ozonoff, Rogers, & Pennington, 1991; Szat- did not explain theory of mind performance
mari et al., 1990). From Goldman-Rakic when nonverbal ability and language level
(1987), the function of the prefrontal cortex is were controlled, with the exception of a task
to guide behavior by internal representations that required the combined capacity of in-
of the outside world, without direct stimula- hibitory control and working memory (Joseph
tion from the environment. These deficits can & Tager-Flusberg, 2004). The latter task did
be related more generally to an inability to dis- not have similar explanatory power for autism
engage from immediate environmental cues symptoms; rather, the higher order cognitive
and be guided by internal rules or mental rep- task (Tower test) did contribute to communica-
resentations. Indeed, a preliminary study of tion symptoms. This may be related to a more
individuals with AS revealed a significant general deficit in planning and organization
deficit in spatial working memory but no im- in the context of online discourse (Joseph &
pairment in strategy formation on a spatial Tager-Flusberg, 2004).
task (Morris et al., 1999). The authors sug- Concrete thinking and limited abstraction
gested that individuals with AS may have more abilities have long been observed in individu-
general difficulty in accessing different types als with autism (Adams & Sheslow, 1983; Rut-
of visual and spatial representations to guide ter, 1978; Tsai, 1992) and can be related to
behavior, which may also account for difficul- overall developmental or intellectual level.
ties on problem-solving tasks. However, another relevant dichotomy when
Ozonoff (1995) and others (Hughes, 2001; thinking about concrete versus abstract /con-
Russell, 1997) have argued for the contribu- ceptual ability is that of internally controlled
tions of executive functions in understanding processes or generative demands and exter-
the range of impairments in autism. Frontal nally generated problem-solving strategies and
lobe abnormalities in autism are also hypothe- external structures (e.g., the solution is inher-
sized, by reason that executive function is me- ent in or constrained by the problem). This
diated by regions of the frontal lobes. Two latter question is reminiscent of Klin et al.’s
374 Development and Behavior

(2003) discussion of “open domain” and on the Wechsler scales are typically signifi-
“closed domain” tasks in the context of social cantly impaired relative to strengths on sub-
environment as well as research suggesting in- tests involving visual perceptual or spatial
tact rule categorization in the face of impaired analysis and integration, such as Block Design
formation of prototypic mental representations and Object Assembly. The discrepancy be-
when a concrete rule is not available on cogni- tween verbal and nonverbal abilities in autism
tive tasks (e.g., Klinger & Dawson, 2001). In also needs to be examined in the context of
both instances, individuals with autism appear factors such as age and overall level of ability.
to be more capable of knowing through a set of Whereas the Wechsler scales are recom-
rules versus knowing through learning formed mended for more able and verbally proficient
from repeated experience. It is not surprising, children with autism, the Leiter International
then, that intervention approaches emphasize Performance Scale Revised (Leiter-R) is valu-
consistency, routine, and predictability, through able for the larger group of children with
the use of visual and verbal supports for plan- autism who have more profound communica-
ning events across the day, preparing for chal- tion, attentional, and behavioral difficulties.
lenging situations, making the implicit rules of In a group of children who presented with
engagement explicit, as well as teaching gener- significant language limitations and obtained
alization (e.g., applying the rules in naturalis- a Vineland Expressive Communication age-
tic situations). equivalent score at or below 3 years of age,
Leiter-R scores indicated higher nonverbal IQ
COGNITIVE PROFILES scores with strengths on subtests drawing pri-
marily on visualization skills and particularly
Level of cognitive functioning for individuals spatial reasoning (Tsatsanis et al., 2003). Using
with a PDD spans the entire range, from pro- the WISC-III, Mayes and Calhoun (2003) found
found mental retardation to superior intellect. strengths in lexical knowledge relative to ver-
The terms low-functioning (IQ < 70) and high- bal reasoning in both high- and low-IQ groups
functioning (IQ > 70) autism are often used in of older children, but a selective strength in vi-
the literature to denote subgroups based on sual spatial ability in their low-IQ group.
differences in level of intellectual ability. This Within the younger age group, assessed using
distinction has also been applied to the chil- the Stanford-Binet, IV, relative strengths in vi-
dren described by Kanner (1943) and Asperger sual processing were found for both IQ groups,
(1944). Whereas Kanner’s description is con- as well as a strength in rote memory. The dis-
sistent with the classically autistic or lower parity between verbal and nonverbal abilities
functioning child with autism, Asperger’s de- observed in the younger group was not ob-
scription has been associated with the less im- tained in the group of older children, repre-
paired, more verbal, and older child with senting an increase in verbal IQ (VIQ) versus
autism (Klin & Volkmar, 1997). change in nonverbal ability. Ghaziuddin and
The intellectual profiles of individuals Mountain-Kimchi (2004) also found no differ-
with autism have been reviewed extensively, ence in WISC-III VIQ and performance IQ
and it is typically found that visual and visual (PIQ) scores overall in their sample of subjects
spatial processing are well preserved and fre- with HFA (mean age 12.42 years). Current
quently a strength relative to verbal abilities knowledge regarding cognitive abilities in in-
(e.g., Ghaziuddin & Mountain-Kimchi, 2004; dividuals with autism is at minimum consis-
and Barnhill, Hagiwara, Myles, & Simpson, tent for a scattered profile; cognitive function
2000; Lincoln et al., 1995; Mayes & Calhoun, may not be well integrated, yielding isolated
2003, for a review). This finding is consistent strengths and a broad range of deficits. Longi-
with the observations of Temple Grandin tudinal studies are needed to test evidence of
(1992), a high-functioning individual with continuity and discontinuity pertaining to cog-
autism, who emphasizes her own visually me- nitive profiles over time and whether specific
diated approach to learning and making sense performance profiles (versus overall level of
of the world. Selected verbal subtests, such as ability) on measures of cognitive functioning
Comprehension (assessing understanding or predict outcome on measures of autistic symp-
common sense reasoning and social judgment), tomotology or social ability and disability.
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 375

The intellectual profiles of individuals with the context of variability in the cognitive pro-
AS point to a pattern of better verbal relative files within each group. Additionally, the re-
to poorer perceptual organizational skills strictive onset criteria for AS relative to HFA
overall (Ehlers et al., 1997; Ghaziuddin & ( leading to differences in early language abili-
Mountain-Kimchi, 2004; Lincoln, Courchesne, ties) is considered by some to represent an-
Allen, Hanson, & Ene, 1998; Ozonoff, South, other confound.
& Miller, 2000), with notable exceptions (e.g.,
Szatmari et al., 1990). Intragroup analyses in- CONCLUSION
dicate significantly higher global IQ scores
and a significant split between VIQ and PIQ Although autism is a syndrome that is defined
with VIQ > PIQ on average for AS as a group primarily in behavioral terms, there has been
relative to HFA. In consideration of these find- considerable research devoted to the various
ings, a particular neuropsychological model, cognitive impairments that characterize indi-
nonverbal learning disability (NLD; Rourke, viduals with this disorder. In association with
1989), has been proposed as a source of exter- these findings, competing theories have arisen
nal validity for AS (Klin & Volkmar, 1997; concerning the primacy of a specific deficit in
Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Rourke, explaining the disorder. The complexity and
1995). In brief, the NLD profile involves a pat- clinical heterogeneity that is typical of the
tern of functioning of better developed verbal PDDs is reflected not only in these accounts
relative to visual, tactile, and complex motor but also in differences in subject characteris-
skills as well as better reading and spelling tics. With fundamental disparities in subject
skills relative to arithmetic. Klin et al. (1995) selection and diagnostic assignment, questions
reported that deficits that were predictive of remain regarding the neuropsychological phe-
AS were fine motor skills, visual motor integra- notype in autism, including aspects of the phe-
tion, visual spatial perception, nonverbal con- notype that are specific to the disorder, how
cept formation, gross motor skills, and visual the phenotype changes with development, and
memory. Deficits that were identified as not how these constructs might explain the funda-
predictive of AS included articulation, verbal mental social and adaptive impairments as
output, auditory perception, vocabulary, and well as any response to treatment in this popu-
verbal memory. This finding was also reflected lation. It is reasonable to expect that if a psy-
more generally in the pattern of IQ scores in the chological process is causatively linked to the
two groups as the AS group showed a signifi- pathogenesis of autism, then levels of disrup-
cant and unusually large verbal-performance tion in the given area should hold a quantified
discrepancy ( higher VIQ compared to PIQ and proportional relationship to levels of, for
score), whereas no such discrepancy was exhib- example, social competence in daily life (Volk-
ited by the HFA group. Preserved verbal mem- mar et al., 2004). To date, few studies have at-
ory skills, relative to individuals with HFA and tempted to measure this predictive relationship
relative to their own abilities, have been re- (Dawson et al., 2002; Klin, Jones, Schultz,
ported by others (e.g., Gunter, Ghaziuddin, & Volkmar, & Cohen, 2002).
Ellis, 2002; Ozonoff et al., 1991). In addition, The questions of phenotypic boundaries
better reading/decoding relative to mechanical and diagnostic categorization are far from re-
arithmetic skills is found (Griswold, Barnhill, solved but are well to be considered from a
Myles, Hagiwara, & Simpson, 2002). These developmental perspective. Cross-sectional
results have important implications for inter- studies predominately compare clinical char-
vention, suggesting that one treatment modal- acteristics between groups and, in some
ity may be preferred for children and cases, examine the validity of categorical di-
adolescents with AS (e.g., verbally mediated agnostic distinctions using independent exter-
strategies), relative to children with HFA (e.g., nal markers. As noted, however, these studies
visual supports; Volkmar, Klin, Schultz, Rubin, tend to yield equivocal findings, limited by
& Bronen, 2000). This is an area of ongoing the fact that results on the dependent mea-
debate complicated by differences in diagnos- sures are impacted by the ways in which sub-
tic approaches that make it difficult to compare jects were assigned to groups in the first
studies (Klin & Volkmar, 2003), especially in place. A related and powerful approach to the
376 Development and Behavior

nosologic question is to consider mediators of Global features of brain function are far more
outcome through longitudinal associations and likely to be bound up in the coordination and
the mapping of developmental trajectories from relation among things (cooperating to form co-
the earliest stages of development using the si- herent patterns) than they are to be revealed in
multaneous examination of social and neu- an approach where one level of analysis has
ropsychological processes. This serves not only priority over any other.
to identify diagnostic pathways but also to
study fundamental mechanisms of social devel- Cross-References
opment and their relationship to cognitive and
neurobiological factors. Such studies are lim- Aspects of executive functioning are addressed
ited in number and have tended to rely on re- in Chapter 22; theory of mind and central co-
stricted measures of early and later processes. herence theories of autism are addressed in
Another more recent line of research has Chapters 23 and 24, respectively. Joint attention
been to identify specific aspects of the broader is discussed in Chapter 25; the enactive mind
autism phenotype, such as measurable compo- model of autism is addressed in Chapter 26.
nents not detected by the unaided eye that
might fall along the pathway between disease
REFERENCES
and distal genotype (e.g., endophenotypes;
Gottesman & Gould, 2003). Reciprocally, Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham,
identification of the genes involved in the dis- R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., et al. (1999).
order may also help to define the phenotype. The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based
This latter approach places a stronger empha- whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuro-
sis on dimensionality and the measurement of Report, 10, 1647–1651.
continuously distributed traits. The challenge Adams, A. V., & Sheslow, B. V. (1983). A devel-
has been to identify and develop such mea- opmental perspective of adolescence. In
E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Autism in
sures. Innovative behavioral and neurofunc-
adolescents and adults (pp. 11–36). New York:
tional methodologies have been used to assess
Plenum Press.
salient social constructs and suggest that di- Adamson, L. B. (1995). Communication develop-
mensionality can be achieved if we focus on ment in infancy. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
processes that are very early emerging (Klin Adrien, J. L., Barthelemy, C., Perrot, A., Roux, S.,
et al., 2003; Volkmar et al., 2004). Lenoir, P., Hameury, L., et al. (1992). Validity
The development of more effective methods and reliability of the Infant Behavioral Sum-
of measurement to expand our understanding marized Evaluation (IBSE): A rating scale for
of the neuropsychological characteristics of the assessment of young children with autism
autism should encompass at least two ap- and developmental disorders. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 22, 375–394.
proaches. Because of the amount of inherent
Anzalone, M. E., & Williamson, G. G. (2000). Sen-
structure available in the clinical or research
sory processing and motor performance in
framework, subtle impairments in problem solv- autism spectrum disorders. In A. M. Wetherby
ing, organization, and behavioral activation & B. M. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spectrum disor-
may not be easily revealed. Yet, the fundamen- ders: A transactional developmental perspec-
tal deficit for individuals with autism is that of tive. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
initiating complex behavior in unstructured Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopa-
settings. This investigative focus requires a then” im kindersalter. Archive fur Psychiatrie
movement away from externally controlled or und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.
constrained tasks toward gaining access to in- Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Honey-
ternally controlled, experientially driven mech- cutt, N. A., Augustine, A. M., Yates, K. O., et al.
(1999). MRI volumes of amygdala and hip-
anisms. Acknowledgment of autism as a
pocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic ado-
complex heterogeneous disorder signifies that lescents and adults. Neurology, 53, 2145–2150.
understanding will be increased through mul- Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe struc-
tiple levels of analysis as well as the integra- tures and autism: A review of clinical and ex-
tion of tools and information from other perimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32,
disciplines (e.g., genetic and brain research). 627–648.
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 377

Bachevalier, J., & Merjanian, P. (1994). The contri- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4),
bution of medial temporal lobe structures in 295–304.
infantile autism: A neurobehavioral study in pri- Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M.
mates. In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), (1997). Asperger’s syndrome and memory:
The neurobiology of autism (pp. 146–169). Bal- Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neu-
timore: Johns Hopkins University Press. ropsychologia, 35, 65–70.
Barnhill, G., Hagiwara, T., Myles, B. S., & Simp- Buchsbaum, M. S., Siegel, B. V., Jr., Wu, J. C., Ha-
son, R. L. (2000). Asperger syndrome: A study zlett, E., Sicotte, N., & Haier, R. (1992). Brief
of the cognitive profiles of 37 children and report: Attention performance in autism and
adolescents. Focus on Autism and Other Devel- regional brain metabolic rate assessed by
opmental Disabilities, 15, 146–153. positron emission tomography. Journal of
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22,
Does the autistic child have a “ theory of 115–125.
mind?” Cognition, 21, 37–46. Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bull- persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of
more, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., et al. an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Ab-
(1999). Social intelligence in the normal and normal Psychology, 103, 535–543.
autistic brain: A fMRI study. European Jour- Burack, J. A., Enns, J. T., Stauder, J. E. A., Mottron,
nal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898. L., & Randolf, B. (1997). Attention and autism:
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1994). Neu- Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.
roanatomic observations of the brain in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Hand-
In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The book of autism and pervasive developmental dis-
neurobiology of autism (pp. 119–145). Balti- orders (pp. 226–247). New York: Wiley.
more: Johns Hopkins University Press. Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cogni-
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. tion II: An empirical review of 275 PET and
(1996). Intact and impaired memory functions fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835. science, 12, 1–47.
Berger, H. J. C., Aerts, F. H. T. M., van Spaendonck, Casey, B. J., Gordon, C. T., Mannheim, G. B., &
K. P. M., Cools, A. R., & Teunisse, J. P. (2003). Rumsey, J. M. (1993). Dysfunctional attention
Central coherence and cognitive shifting in rela- in autistic savants. Journal of Clinical and Ex-
tion to social improvement in high-functioning perimental Neuropsychology, 15, 933–946.
young adults with autism. Journal of Clinical and Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J.,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 502–511. Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing
Beversdorf, D. Q., Smith, B. W., Crucian, G. P., An- joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy
derson, J. M., Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., precursors to language and theory of mind.
et al. (2000). Increased discrimination of “ false Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498.
memories” in autism spectrum disorder. Pro- Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J.,
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Pre-
the United States of America, 97, 8734–8737. dicting language outcome in infants with
Boucher, J. (1981a). Immediate free recall in early autism and pervasive developmental disorder.
childhood autism: Another point of behavioral International Journal of Language and Com-
similarity with the amnesic syndrome. British munication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.
Journal of Psychology, 72, 211–215. Ciesielski, K. T., Courchesne, E., & Elmasian, R.
Boucher, J. (1981b). Memory for recent events in (1990). Effects of focused selective attention
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel- tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and
opmental Disorders, 11, 293–301. normal individuals. Electroencephalography
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1989). Memory impair- and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 207–220.
ments and communication in relatively able Courchesne, E. (1995). New evidence of cerebellar
autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and brainstem hypoplasia in autistic infants,
and Psychiatry, 30, 99–122. children, and adolescents: The MR imaging
Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory study by Hashimoto and colleagues. Journal of
deficits in early infantile autism: Some simi- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25,
larities to the amnesic syndrome. British Jour- 19–22.
nal of Psychology, 67, 73–87. Courchesne, E., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R.,
Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Grice, S. J. Press, G. A., Lincoln, A. J., Haas, R. H., et al.
(2000). Episodic memory and remembering in (1994). Abnormality of cerebellar vermian
adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of lobules, VI & VII in patients with infantile
378 Development and Behavior

autism: Identification of hypoplastic and hy- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
perplastic subgroups with MR imaging. Amer- Disorders, 20, 101–114.
ican Journal of Roentgenology, 162, 123–130. Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H.,
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are ex-
Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Murakami, ecutive functioning deficits in attention deficit
A., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting atten- hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of
tion in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behav- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 836–854.
ioral Neuroscience, 108, 848–865. Ghaziuddin, M., & Mountain-Kimchi, K. (2004).
Courchesne, E., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A., Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger
Hesselink, J. R., & Jernigan, T. L. (1988). Hy- syndrome: Comparison with high-functioning
poplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI & VII autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
in autism. New England Journal of Medicine, Disorders, 34, 279–284.
318, 1349–1354. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Kenworthy, L., & Bar-
Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms ton, R. M. (2002). Profiles of everyday execu-
in the first two years of life: A preliminary tive function in acquired and developmental
population study of infantile autism. European disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 121–137.
Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sci- Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of primate
ences, 238, 169–174. prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by
Damasio, A. R. (1985). The frontal lobes. In K. M. representational memory. In V. B. Mountcastle,
Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neu- F. Plum, & S. R. Geiger (Eds.), Handbook of
ropsychology (2nd ed., pp. 339–375). New physiology: The nervous system (pp. 373–417).
York: Oxford University Press. Bethesda: American Physiological Society.
Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neuro- Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J.
logical model for childhood autism. Archives (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Jour-
of Neurology, 35, 777–786. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., 31, 433–440.
McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neu- Gottesman, I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The en-
rocognitive function and joint attention abili- dophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymol-
ties in young children with autism spectrum ogy and strategic intentions. American Journal
disorder versus developmental delay. Child of Psychiatry, 160, 636–645.
Development, 73, 345–358. Grandin, T. (1992). An inside view of autism. In E.
DeLong, G. R. (1992). Autism, amnesia, hip- Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), High func-
pocampus, and learning. Neuroscience and tioning individuals with autism (pp. 105–126).
Biobehavioral Reviews, 16, 63–70. New York: Plenum Press.
Dunn, W., Myles, B. S., & Orr, S. (2002). Sensory Grandin, T. (1995). How people with autism think.
processing issues associated with Asperger syn- In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learn-
drome: A preliminary investigation. American ing and cognition in autism (pp. 137–156).
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56(1), 97–102. New York: Plenum Press.
Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Dahlgren- Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A.,
Sandberg, A., Dahlgren, S.-O., Hjelmquist, E., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in
et al. (1997). Asperger syndrome, autism, and young children with autism. Child Develop-
attention disorders: A comparative study of the ment, 70, 817–832.
cognitive profiles of 120 children. Journal of Griswold, D. E., Barnhill, G. P., Myles, B. S., Hagi-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 207–217. wara, T., & Simpson, R. L. (2002). Asperger
Fein, D., Dunn, M. A., Allen, D. M., Aram, R., syndrome and academic achievement. Focus on
Hall, N., Morris, R., et al. (1996). Neuropsy- Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities,
chological and language data. In I. Rapin 17, 94–102.
(Ed.), Preschool children with inadequate com- Gunter, H. L., Ghaziuddin, M., & Ellis, H. D.
munication: Developmental language disorder, (2002). Asperger syndrome: Tests of right
autism, low IQ (pp. 123–154). London: Mac hemisphere functioning and interhemispheric
Keith Press. communication. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Gardiner, J. M., Bowler, D. M., & Grice, S. J. opmental Disorders, 32, 263–281.
(2003). Further evidence for preserved prim- Happé, F. G. E., & Frith, U. (1995). Theory of
ing and impaired recall in adults with As- mind in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesi-
perger syndrome. Journal of Autism and bov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism
Developmental Disorders, 33, 259–269. (pp. 177–198). New York: Plenum Press.
Garretson, H. B., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. Hughes, C. (2001). Executive dysfunction in
(1990). Sustained attention in children with autism: Its nature and implications for every-
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 379

day problems experienced by individuals with Laurent, A. C., & Rubin, E. (2004). Challenges in
autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, emotional regulation in Asperger syndrome
& P. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of and high-functioning autism. Topics in Lan-
autism: Perspectives from theory and research guage Disorders, 24(4), 286–297.
(pp. 255–275). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lincoln, A. J., Allen, M. H., & Kilman, A. (1995).
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). The assessment and interpretation of intellec-
Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. tual abilities in people with autism. In E.
Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning
Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The and cognition in autism (pp. 89–117). New
relationship between theory of mind and exec- York: Plenum Press.
utive functions to symptom type and severity Lincoln, A., Courchesne, E., Allen, M., Hanson, E., &
in children with autism. Development and Ene, M. (1998). Neurobiology of Asperger syn-
Psychopathology, 16, 137–155. drome: Seven case studies and quantitative mag-
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- netic resonance imaging findings. In E. Schopler
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or
Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. L. (1993). The con- high-functioning autism? Current issues in
tribution of neuropathologic studies of the un- autism (pp. 145–163). New York: Plenum Press.
derstanding of autism. Neurologic Clinics, 11, Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1989). The role of age at
175–186. assessment, developmental level, and test in
Kientz, M. A., & Dunn, W. (1997). A comparison of the stability of intelligence scores in young
the performance of children with and without autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel-
autism on the Sensory Profile. American Jour- opmental Disorders, 19, 483–499.
nal of Occupational Therapy, 51(7), 530–537. Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, K. E., Lazar,
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to am- N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., et al.
biguous visual stimuli in higher functioning (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in
autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social At- autism: An fMRI study of spatial working
tribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology, memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840.
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. (1999). Neuropsycho-
763–769. logical profiles of children with Asperger syn-
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar, drome and autism. Autism, 3, 327–356.
F. R. (2003). The enactive mind: From actions Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003). Analysis of
to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophi- WISC-III, Stanford-Binet, IV, & academic
cal Transactions of the Royal Society, Biologi- achievement test scores in children with
cal Sciences, 358, 345–360. autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Disorders, 33, 329–341.
Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (2000). The external
during viewing of naturalistic social situations validity of Asperger disorder: Lack of evi-
as predictors of social competence in individu- dence from the domain of neuropsychology.
als with autism. Archives of General Psychia- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109,
try, 59(9), 809–816. 227–238.
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Asperger’s syn- Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism
drome. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), an amnesic disorder? Evidence from the Cali-
Handbook of autism and pervasive develop- fornia Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology,
mental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 94–122). New 7, 209–216.
York: Wiley. Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2001). The pattern
Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2003). Asperger syn- of intact and impaired memory functions in
drome: Diagnosis and external validity. Child autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North chiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 1095–1101.
America, 12, 1–13. Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., &
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological func-
D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and tioning in nonmentally retarded autistic indi-
neuropsychological characterization of As- viduals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
perger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology, Neuropsychology, 14, 749–761.
Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 36, Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J.
1127–1140. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in
Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype autism: Profile of a complex information pro-
formation in autism. Development and Psycho- cessing disorder. Journal of the International
pathology, 13(1), 111–124. Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.
380 Development and Behavior

Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni-
Oculomotor evidence neocortical systems but tion of children with autism: A study of first-
no cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurol- birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism
ogy, 52, 917–922. and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.
Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Ozonoff, S. (1995). Executive functions in autism.
Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learn-
between concept formation and concept iden- ing and cognition in autism (pp. 199–219).
tification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334. New York: Plenum Press.
Morris, R. G., Rowe, A., Fox, N., Feigenbaum, Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G.,
J. D., Miotto, E. C., & Howlin, P. (1999). Spa- Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., et al. (2004). Perfor-
tial working memory in Asperger’s syndrome mance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test
and in patients with focal frontal and temporal Automated Battery subtests sensitive to
lobe lesions. Brain and Cognition, 41, 9–26. frontal lobe function in people with autistic
Mundy, P., & Gomes, A. (1998). Individual differ- disorder: Evidence from the Collaborative
ences in joint attention skill development in Programs of Excellence in Autism Network.
the second year of life. Infant Behavior and Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Development, 21, 469–482. ders, 34, 139–150.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longi- Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudi-
tudinal study of joint attention and language de- nal study of executive function and theory of
velopment in autistic children. Journal of Autism mind development in autism. Development and
and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128. Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
attention, developmental level, and symptom (1991). Executive function deficits in high-
presentation in autism. Development and functioning autistic individuals: Relationship
Psychopathology, 6, 389–401. to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
(1986). Defining social deficits of autism: The Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991).
contribution of nonverbal communication mea- Asperger’s syndrome: Evidence of an empiri-
sures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- cal distinction. Journal of Child Psychology
atry, 27, 657–659. and Psychiatry, 32, 1107–1122.
Murakami, J. W., Courchesne, E., Press, G. A., Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Miller, J. N. (2000).
Yeung-Courchesne, R., & Hesselink, J. R. DSM-IV-defined Asperger syndrome: Cogni-
(1989). Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size tive, behavioral and early history differentia-
and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in tion from high-functioning autism. Autism, 4,
autism. Archives of Neurology, 46, 689–694. 29–46.
Ornitz, E. M. (1974). The modulation of sensory Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory
input and motor output in autistic children. function in nonretarded children with autism.
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
nia, 4, 197–215. ders, 27, 59–77.
Ornitz, E. M. (1983). The functional neuroanatomy Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evi-
of infantile autism. International Journal of dence of intact working memory in autism.
Neuroscience, 19, 85–124. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Ornitz, E. M. (1988). Autism: A disorder of directed ders, 31, 257–263.
attention. Brain Dysfunction, 1, 309–322. Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., &
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968a). Neurophysi- Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities
ological mechanisms underlying perceptual in autism and Tourette syndrome: An informa-
inconstancy in autistic and schizophrenic chil- tion processing approach. Journal of Child
dren. Archives of General Psychiatry, 19, Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1032.
22–27. Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou,
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968b). Perceptual M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional capac-
inconstancy in early infantile autism: The syn- ities in children with autism: Is there a general
drome of early infant autism and its variants deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism
including certain cases of childhood schizo- and Developmental Disorders, 28, 467–478.
phrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 18, Powell, S. D., & Jordan, R. R. (1993). Being subjec-
76–98. tive about autistic thinking and learning to
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1976). The syn- learn. Educational Psychology, 13, 359–370.
drome of autism: A critical review. American Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2000).
Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 609–622. Implicit and explicit memory in autism: Is
Neuropsychological Characteristics in Autism and Related Conditions 381

autism an amnesic disorder? Journal of Autism on episodic memory in autism reconsidered.


and Developmental Disorders, 30, 3–14. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Rogers, S., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Par- ders, 33, 151–161.
ent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. H. (1993).
with autism and those with other developmen- Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sci-
tal disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop- ences, 16, 495–552.
mental Disorders, 33(6), 631–642. Townsend, J., Harris, N. S., & Courchesne, E.
Rourke, B. P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabili- (1996). Visual attention abnormalities in
ties: The syndrome and the model. New York: autism: Delayed orienting to location. Journal
Guilford Press. of the International Neuropsychological Soci-
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neu- ety, 2, 541–550.
ropsychological divergence of high-level Tsai, L. Y. (1992). Diagnostic issues in high-
autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism functioning autism. In E. Schopler & G. B.
and Developmental Disorders, 20, 155–168. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals
Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can with autism (pp. 11–40). New York: Plenum
bring about an inadequate “ theory of mind.” Press.
In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive dis- Tsatsanis, K. D., Dartnall, N., Cicchetti, D., Spar-
order (pp. 256–304). Oxford, England: Oxford row, S., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2003). A
University Press. comparison of performance on the Leiter and
Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Henry, L. (1996). Work- Leiter-R in low-functioning children with
ing memory in children with autism and with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Disorders, 33(1), 23–30.
Child Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Dis- Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory.
ciplines, 37, 673–686. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organi-
Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definitions of zation of memory. New York: Academic Press.
childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Ulvund, S., & Smith, L. (1996). The predictive va-
Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 139–161. lidity of nonverbal communication skills in in-
Schatz, A. M., Weimer, A. K., & Trauner, D. A. fants with perinatal hazards. Infant Behavior
(2002). Brief report: Attention differences in and Development, 19, 441–449.
Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and De- Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Schultz, R., Rubin, E., &
velopmental Disorders, 32, 333–336. Bronen, R. (2000). Clinical case conference:
Schultz, R. T., Romanski, L. M., & Tsatsanis, K. D. Asperger’s disorder. American Journal of Psy-
(2000). Neurofunctional models of Autistic chiatry, 2(157), 262–267.
Disorder and Asperger syndrome: Clues from Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T.,
neuroimaging. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive de-
S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome. New velopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy-
York: Guilford Press. chology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.
Stone, W. L. (1997). Autism in infancy and early Wainwright-Sharp, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1996).
childhood. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar Visual-spatial orienting in autism. Journal of
(Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive de- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26,
velopmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 266–282). 423–438.
New York: Wiley. Watling, R. L., Deitz, J., & White, O. (2001). Com-
Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, M. A. J., & Bar- parison of Sensory Profile score of young chil-
tolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome and dren with and without autism spectrum
autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the disorders. American Journal of Occupational
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Therapy, 55(4), 416–423.
Psychiatry, 29, 130–136. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997).
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in Toward a theory of episodic memory: The
the free recall of autistic children: Further ev- frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness.
idence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354.
of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430. Zilbovicius, M., Garreau, B., Samson, Y., Remy, P.,
Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2002). Long-term mem- Barthelemy, C., Syrota, A., et al. (1995). De-
ory and levels-of-processing in autism. Neu- layed maturation of the frontal cortex in child-
ropsychologia, 40, 964–969. hood autism. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2003). Long-term mem- 152, 248–252.
ory in high-functioning autism: Controversy
CHAPTER 14

Imitation and Play in Autism

SALLY J. ROGERS, IAN COOK, AND ADRIENNE MERYL

Over the past 20 years, the developmental as- Piaget delineated, children with autism showed
pects of autism have been a central focus of syndrome-specific impairments in only two:
research activities. This developmental lens imitation and play. Furthermore, Piaget (1962)
for viewing autism focuses attention on the suggested that symbolic play developed from
evolving symptoms of autism, influenced by imitation, particularly deferred imitation, as
the interplay of biology and experience. This children developed the ability to represent
interactive, developmental framework has sev- mentally events they had experienced and re-
eral implications that strongly affect current produce them at a later time. This theoretical
research ideas: (1) There is some plasticity in linkage between imitation and pretend play
the evolution of the symptoms, (2) individual gains support from the symptom pattern seen in
differences in course and outcomes will be af- early autism, and this chapter focuses on re-
fected by an individual’s experiences as well search findings in these two areas.
as the individual biology of the disorder, and Thus, this chapter reviews what is currently
(3) early developmental course will have a known about imitative abilities and play char-
substantially greater impact on outcomes than acteristics that distinguish autism from other
later events. In contrast to research in the pe- disorders. We also examine the nature of indi-
riod between 1960 and 1980 and in response vidual differences in play and imitation skills
to this developmental orientation, the past 20 among children with autism. Finally, we con-
years have seen much more emphasis on un- sider the proposed mechanisms thought to un-
derstanding autism as early in life as possible derlie the autism impairments in imitation and
and searching for autism-specific deviations play. For the purposes of this chapter, play is
in the early developmental processes that lead defined as play with objects, rather than social
to language, social, and cognitive develop- play with people.
ment, both to understand the developmental
processes involved in the disorder and to con- IMITATION
ceptualize treatment strategies for maximiz-
ing outcomes. Roles of Imitation in Development
Strongly influenced by Piaget’s model of
cognitive development, the developmental stud- In normal infant and early childhood develop-
ies of autism of the past 20 years have carefully ment, imitative ability is considered to be a key
dissected early development. In the first major mechanism for cultural transmission of skills
papers reflecting this approach, Marian Sigman and knowledge, serving an apprenticeship, or
and her colleagues (Sigman & Ungerer, 1984; learning function, helping young children learn
Ungerer & Sigman, 1981) found that of the mul- complex, goal-directed behavior patterns from
tiple areas of sensorimotor development that others (Baldwin, 1906; Bruner, 1972; Piaget,

The authors were partially supported by NICHD U19 HD35468. Dr. Rogers also received support from
NIDCD R21 05574. The help of Debbie Schilling and Huanh Meyer is gratefully acknowledged.

382
Imitation and Play in Autism 383

1962; Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu, & Mosier, 1993; ioral means, or whether “mimicry” of body
Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 1993; Uzgiris, movements by themselves should be considered
1999). A less emphasized function of imitation imitation (see Tomasello et al., 1993). Given
involves social interpersonal communication. the definition of imitation that pervades the
Imitation of body movements and postures, fa- autism studies, we define imitation as the pur-
cial expressions, and vocal behavior permeate poseful reproduction of another’s body move-
social and emotional exchanges, providing a ments, whether novel or familiar.
key mechanism for emotional synchrony and While the view of imitation as a powerful
communication between social partners, from tool for learning instrumental actions from
early infancy throughout the lifespan (Gopnik others has been present in developmental psy-
& Meltzoff, 1994; Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rap- chology for many years, Meltzoff and Moore’s
son, 1994; Uzgiris, 1981). (1977) discovery of oral imitation in infants in
How imitation is defined is crucial when the first days and weeks of life required con-
reviewing imitative studies, since there are siderable revision of the view of the role of
many nonimitative ways in which behavior ac- imitation in development. While the evolution-
quisition can be socially influenced. The re- ary utility of imitation in older infants and
search on social behavior acquisition in children as a powerful learning tool is clear
animals has delineated these processes in the (Rogoff et al., 1993), what might the evolution-
following way (see Byrne & Russon, 1998; ary role of neonatal oral imitations serve? Uz-
Heyes & Galef, 1996; Tomasello et al., 1993; giris (1981) was the first to suggest that in
Want & Harris, 2002): Stimulus enhancement early infancy, imitation may primarily serve
is the tendency to pay attention to or aim social communication and interpersonal devel-
responses toward a particular object or place opment. Trevarthen, Kokkinaki, and Fiamenghi
after observing a conspecific’s actions. In the (1999) have extended this view, suggesting that
case of stimulus enhancement, the observer’s the core function of human imitation is the
actions on the object are generated through sharing of motives or intentions, which is at the
trial and error learning as opposed to reenact- heart of its other functions, including but not
ing the model’s behavior, but the chance that limited to sharing emotional states, instrumen-
the trial and error learning will take place with tal learning, and continuing interactions.
the object is elevated as a result of the model’s Rogers and Pennington (1991), following
behavior. Emulation is a process in which the Stern’s (1985) model of interpersonal develop-
goal of the other is made overt as a result of ment, suggested that early deficits in imitation
the other’s actions and that goal becomes a could lead to impaired metarepresentation abil-
goal for the observer also. The observer then ities characteristic of children with autism.
attempts to reproduce the completed goal by Meltzoff and Gopnik (1993) took this idea fur-
whatever means he or she comes up with from ther, suggesting that imitation serves social
his or her own behavioral repertoire. While the development by providing a mechanism for ac-
preceding processes do not reflect direct acqui- quiring mental state understanding. Gopnik and
sition of another’s behavior through observa- Meltzoff (1994) proposed that early imitation
tion and thus are not truly imitative, response initially provides the infant with shared experi-
facilitation is an increase in the frequency of a ences of interpersonal connectedness via bod-
behavior already in an individual’s repertoire ies and movements. In the next few months of
as a result of seeing it performed by another. life, imitation of facial expressions leads to a
This kind of performance is considered to re- shared experience of emotional expressions and
flect imitation by most infant researchers. Ac- inner sensations, and then to a shared sense of
tion level imitation occurs when the observer motives and intentions underlying communica-
fully demonstrates the behavior of another, in- tion in the 9- to 12-month period, thus laying
cluding novel acts, and acts that match the the groundwork for intersubjectivity and devel-
minor details and the style of the model’s ac- oping theory of minds.
tion. There is disagreement in the field about Is there supportive evidence for the role of
whether this should involve a reproduction of early imitation in social relations? In line with
the goals of the model, as well as the behav- Gopnik and Meltzoff (1994), Kugiumutzakis
384 Development and Behavior

(1999) suggested that the crucial social element find significant group differences in perfor-
in early imitation is sharing affect via facial, mance on tasks of both immediate and deferred
vocal, and physical matchings. Heimann and imitation of familiar actions with realistic ob-
colleagues have provided the only longitudinal jects in a sample with a mean age just under 5
data that address this hypothesis. Their find- years old. In a sample of older children (mean
ings demonstrate: (1) relationships between age 8.1 years), Hammes and Langdell (1981)
neonatal imitative ability and social responses found that although imitation of actions with
to the mother in 3-month-olds and (2) positive imaginary objects (pantomime) and imitation
relationships between 3-month-old imitation of actions with a counterconventional object
and 12-month-old imitation (Heimann, 1998; (e.g., using a cup as a hat) distinguished the
Heimann & Ullstadius, 1999). Nor is this lim- children with autism from children with mental
ited to infancy. The research on emotional con- retardation matched on language abilities, per-
tagion has provided a body of evidence on the formance on the imitation of actions with real
role of facial and postural imitation in rapid objects did not differentiate the two groups. In
sharing of emotional states between people a sample of adolescents, Hobson and Lee (1999)
across the lifespan (as reviewed in Hatfield did not find an autism-specific deficit in imita-
et al., 1994). tion when movements were analyzed in terms
of functional actions on objects, This differ-
Imitation Performance in Autism ence in the performance of older and younger
age groups may be due to maturing imitative
Difficulty imitating other people’s movements abilities in autism or to methodological issues
has been reported in autism in a variety of involving coding systems or choices of tasks
studies across the past 30 years. The studies that are too simple, resulting in ceiling effects
reviewed all involve autism versus matched (as seen in a study by Charman and Baron-
clinical comparison groups in order to examine Cohen in 1994, which used a task designed for
the question of specificity of the imitation 7-month-old infants with subjects with a mean
problem in autism. These studies used a vari- chronological age [CA] close to 12 years).
ety of imitative tasks: actions on objects, imi-
Imitation of Body Movements
tation of body movements, and imitation of
(Intransitive Acts)
facial movements.
An autism-specific deficit in imitating body
Actions on Objects
movements has been consistently, but not uni-
Studies in this area provide the most mixed versally supported. Of all the tasks analyzed
findings and the strongest developmental rela- in the first study of imitation in autism (De-
tions of the three areas. An investigation of Myer et al., 1972), imitation of body move-
the youngest sample of children with autism ments generated the most robust effects of
documented an autism-specific deficit in imita- all the imitation tasks. Ohta (1987) found sig-
tion of simple actions on objects (Charman nificant differences between high functioning
et al., 1997). The 20-month-old subjects with children with autism and nonverbal IQ-matched
autism performed significantly worse than a typical preschoolers on imitation of simple
matched clinical comparison group on tasks in- hand movements. Rogers, Bennetto, McEvoy,
volving imitation of simple actions on familiar and Pennington (1996) found an autism-
objects. Several other comparative studies of specific deficit on single and sequential non-
preschoolers with autism have demonstrated meaningful hand movements in high-functioning
object imitation deficits, using both conven- adolescents. Dawson et al. (1998) found an
tional and nonconventional acts (Dawson, Melt- autism deficit relative to developmentally de-
zoff, Osterling, & Rinaldi, 1998; DeMyer et al., layed and typical control children on familiar
1972; Rogers, Stackhouse, Hepburn, & Wehner, and novel hand movements. Aldridge, Stone,
2003; Stone, Ousley, & Littleford, 1997). In Sweeney, and Bower (2000) found an autism
contrast, several other groups have not identi- deficit in gestural imitation in a sample of 2- to
fied such difficulties. McDonough, Stahmer, 4-year-olds relative to cognitively matched nor-
Schreibman, and Thompson (1997) failed to mally developing infants. Smith and Bryson
Imitation and Play in Autism 385

(1998) found an autism deficit on single hand did not differ significantly in the number of
postures for high-functioning children with identifiable imitations of emotional facial ex-
autism. Bennetto (1999) also found body imita- pressions, the autism group made significantly
tion deficits among a group of high-functioning more unusual and mechanical expressions than
older children compared to clinical controls and the control group. Given the consistency of the
isolated the difficulty to the kinesthetic repro- findings in the literature, oral-facial imitation
ductions of limb postures. Two comparative appears to be specifically impaired in autism.
studies that did not find any autism-specific While it is well established that a signifi-
deficits used very infantile tasks and had cant percentage of people with autism do
ceiling effects that may have accounted for not acquire speech, we have few explanations
their null results (see Charman & Baron- for this phenomenon (see Rodier, 2000, for a
Cohen, 1994; Morgan, Cutrer, Coplin, & Ro- model based on brain differences affecting
drigue, 1989). In summary, in contrast to the cranial nerve function). Lord and colleagues
findings on imitation of actions on objects, have demonstrated that level of retardation
studies of imitation of body movements have does not fully explain the lack of speech in
repeatedly yielded autism-specific deficits autism (Lord & Pickles, 1996). The consis-
across a wide range of IQ and language levels tently replicated finding of autism-specific
and across all ages studied. (While it may seem difficulties with oral-facial imitation (see
counterintuitive, the presence of echolalia does also Rogers et al., 2003; Sigman & Ungerer,
not indicate preserved imitative abilities. Cur- 1984) and the strong relationship of oral-
cio [1978] found that children with echolalia facial imitation to speech ( both in autism and
could produce almost no abstract forms of pan- in typical development) have led to the sug-
tomime. Rogers and Pennington [1991] sug- gestion that a specific oral-motor or speech
gested that echolalia was part of the auditory dyspraxia might underlie lack of speech devel-
rehearsal loop, a distinct system from the motor opment for a subgroup of children with autism
processes involved in action imitations. Thus, (DeMyer, Hingtgen, & Jackson, 1981; Page &
echolalia should not be considered an example Boucher, 1998; Rogers, 1999; Rogers et al.,
of motor imitation.) 1996).
Oral-Facial Imitations Relations among the Three
Kinds of Tasks
Like body movements, oral-motor movements
are consistently reported to be severely af- Is imitation across the three kinds of tasks a
fected in autism, though this area has been unitary phenomenon? So far the evidence is con-
much less well studied. Rogers et al. (2003) tradictory. While Stone and colleagues (1997)
found that oral-motor imitation was more im- reported a dissociation between imitation of ac-
paired than imitation of body movements in tions on objects and imitation of body/facial
toddlers with autism compared to clinical and actions in young children with autism, Rogers
typical controls. Rapin (1996) reported greater et al. (1996, 2003) found all three different
oral-motor impairment in both high- and lower types of imitations to be significantly related in
functioning children with autism than with toddlers with autism, and they found hand and
clinical comparison groups. In a small compar- face imitations to be significantly related in
ative study, Adams (1998) reported a greater adults (no object imitations were tested in that
level of oral-motor difficulty in children with study). However, the correlations are in the .40
autism than in the CA-matched typical com- to .70 range, demonstrating that these are not to-
parison group. Rogers et al. (1996) found an tally overlapping phenomena.
overall deficit in facial imitation for their high-
functioning adolescent subjects with autism Developmental Correlates of Imitation
relative to a CA- and verbal IQ VIQ-matched
clinical control group, as did Dawson et al. Several studies of normal development in
(1998) with a much younger sample. Loveland, infancy have specifically linked early infant
Tunali-Kotoski, Pearson, and Brelsford (1994) motor imitation skills to later social responsiv-
found that although their subjects with autism ity to a parent. As reported earlier, Heimann
386 Development and Behavior

and Ullstadius (1999) reported relationships imitation and speech therapy hours predicted
between frequency of imitation in newborns language ability at age 4 whereas SES, age 2
and frequency of gaze aversion to the other play skills, and joint attention did not.
three months later. Furthermore, the same au- Only one study has directly tested the ef-
thors reported consistencies between facial im- fects of imitation on social responsivity in
itation in 3-month-olds and both manual and autism. Dawson and Adams (1984) demon-
vocal imitation at 12 months. Finally, Forman strated that imitation differentially facilitated
and Kochanska (2001) reported a longitudinal other kinds of social engagement for more se-
study of toddlers seen at 14 and 22 months that verely impaired young children with autism,
demonstrated stability between imitation of but this did not hold true for young, higher func-
the mother and cooperation with her requests tioning children. Two longitudinal studies also
both concurrently and predictively. There is support the hypothesis that imitation affects
also evidence of concurrent relationships be- social functioning in autism. Stone et al. (1997)
tween imitation skills and social responsivity. reported that early imitation predicted later
Uzgiris (1999) reported a study demonstrating language development and play abilities. Rogers
relationships between 12-month-olds’ amount and colleagues (2003) found that early imita-
of affective imitation with their mothers and tion was a better predictor of outcomes in lan-
the emotionally congruent expressions shared guage, IQ, and social skills than dyadic social
with other people. The research of Asendorpf behavior.
and Baudonniere (1993) and Nadel and Peze
(1993) have demonstrated toddlers’ use of syn- Delay versus Deviance
chronous imitations as a main vehicle for re-
ciprocal peer interactions, demonstrated in Though some have proposed that the imitation
complex rounds of nonverbal imitations. Thus, deficit in autism marks a delayed as opposed to
several different findings attest to the social deviant course of development (Stone et al.,
impact of infant imitation. 1997), Carpenter, Pennington, and Rogers
(2002) found that children with autism differ
In Autism
from typically developing children in terms of
While the directionality of the effects from the sequence in which imitation and other so-
imitation to social engagement are clearly laid cial cognitive skills develop across the infancy
out in theories of typical development, it is period. Whereas joint engagement and attention
less clear in developmental theories of autism. following skills emerged prior to imitative
Rogers (1999) has hypothesized the same di- learning in the developmentally delayed group
rectionality in autism: That early imitation (a pattern also seen in normal development),
problems contribute to impaired social devel- imitative learning preceded the development
opment in autism. However, Hobson (1989), of the other social-cognitive skills for the
among others, has suggested that a more gen- subjects with autism. Their results suggest
eral early social impairment leads to lessened that the role of imitation in the development
imitation of other people. of social-communicative abilities differs in
While directionality is not yet established, autism and that the course of development
relationships between imitation and delayed or diverges from the normal path around the in-
disordered development of several social and teraction of joint attention and imitation. Car-
communicative abilities have been described. penter et al. suggested that the use of imitation
In one of the earliest studies of imitation, Cur- without joint attention (or with diminished joint
cio (1978) reported concurrent relationships attention skills) may explain the atypical lin-
between imitation and social communication in guistic features observed in autism such as
a group of nonverbal children with autism. This echolalia, “metaphorical speech,” pronoun
relationship has been reported by others as well reversal, and the abnormal use of questioning
(Dawson & Galpert, 1990; Rogers et al., 2003; intonation for statements. In addition to these
Sigman & Ungerer, 1984; Stone et al., 1997). findings, the previously reviewed reports of
Stone and Yoder (2001) found that, after con- continuing imitation impairments in high-
trolling for language skills, at age 2, only motor functioning adolescents and adults with
Imitation and Play in Autism 387

autism support deviance, rather than delay, in (1998). Thus, while problems in praxis have
imitative development in autism. been supported in autism studies, they appear
confounded with general motor problems.

Possible Mechanisms Underlying the Motor Problems in Autism


Imitation Problem Motor problems have frequently been reported
in autism. Damasio and Maurer (1978) care-
Praxis and Body Mapping
fully described the many motor symptoms
The idea that the imitation problem in autism seen in autism in an early report. Kohen-Raz,
might be due to dyspraxia was first suggested by Volkmar, and Cohen (1992) reported striking
DeMyer et al. (1981). They suggested that differences in children with autism on tasks in-
dyspraxia in autism was of sufficient severity volving standing balance on unstable surfaces.
to prevent the child with autism from participat- Lack of typical hand dominance has been
ing in everyday nonverbal communication, con- demonstrated (Hauck & Dewey, 2001). Manjiv-
tributing to the inability to learn the meaning iona and Prior (1995) reported clinically signif-
and use of nonverbal communicative acts. Dys- icant levels of general motor impairments in a
praxia and its adult counterpart, apraxia, refer majority of children with diagnoses of autism
to impairments in the ability to plan and execute or Asperger syndrome compared to test norms.
movements in the absence of other motor symp- Rapin (1996) reported that hypotonia, limb
toms (Ayres, 2000). The dyspraxia hypothesis in dyspraxia, and stereotypies were all more fre-
autism has been suggested by others as well, quent in a group of children with autism than
both to explain autism-specific difficulties with those with other communication problems. In
imitation and pantomime tasks (Bennetto, 1999; some of the most intriguing reports, home video
Jones & Prior, 1985; Rogers et al., 1996) and studies of infants later diagnosed with autism
to explain nonimitative problems with motor suggest that some motor differences may be
planning and sequencing (Hill & Leary, 1993; present in autism before the first birthday
Hughes, 1996; Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegel, (Baranek, 1999; Osterling, Dawson, & Munson,
1997). Clinicians have long suggested that chil- 2002; Teitelbaum, Teitelbaum, Ney, Fryman, &
dren with autism have poor body awareness, Maurer, 1998).
which might contribute to their difficulties with However, in a direct test of the dyspraxia
praxis in terms of planning and executing an hypothesis, no autism-specific motor difficul-
imitative movement (Hill & Leary, 1993). The ties were found by Rogers et al. (2003) on fine
findings of the Rogers et al. (1996) study in- motor, gross motor, and nonimitative praxis
volved widespread deficits in imitation and pan- performance in a comparative study of a group
tomime, classic tests of praxis. of toddlers with autism compared to both de-
Bennetto (1999) examined several aspects velopmentally matched clinical controls and
of praxis in autism, including body mapping, typically developing children. Yet, the children
visual representation of the movement, and with autism demonstrated an imitation deficit.
motor execution. Examining high-functioning Thus, even though motor functioning accounted
older children with autism and well-matched for a significant amount of the variance in imi-
clinical controls, she found no group differ- tation scores in this study (a finding also re-
ences in the ability to map locations onto the ported by Bennetto, 1999, and Smith & Bryson,
body and no differences in visual recognition 1998), the evidence did not support a general-
memory for the movements, even after delay. ized dyspraxia as the main mechanism underly-
The group differences in this study involved ing overall imitation deficits in autism.
one specific aspect of motor execution: limb The age and functioning level of the subjects
postures. She also demonstrated that perfor- appear to influence findings of motor deficits
mance on a standard motor test revealed sig- in groups with autism. Comparative studies
nificant group differences and accounted for that report autism-specific motor differences
much, but not all, of the variability in imita- have involved high-functioning children with
tion performance in the subjects with autism, autism compared to clinical controls (Bennetto,
findings also reported by Smith and Bryson 1999; Smith & Bryson, 1998). However, when
388 Development and Behavior

younger or lower functioning subjects are ex- ability of subjects with autism to remember the
amined, different findings emerge. Several tasks correctly over time.
studies have compared children with autism to Dawson et al. (1998) demonstrated signifi-
a group with mental retardation matched on cant correlations between executive function
CA and mental age (MA) (Hauck & Dewey, tasks and infant imitation tasks in preschoolers
2001; Kohen-Raz et al., 1992; Rapin, 1996; with autism. However, the size of the correla-
Rogers et al., 2003), and all have reported es- tions revealed that much of the variability in
sentially equivalent levels of motor perfor- imitation scores was not accounted for by ex-
mance (though in Hauck & Dewey, 2001, the ecutive function performance. The executive
groups differed on established handedness function hypothesis lacks some face validity in
preference). Thus, the general motor problem explaining difficulty with imitation in young
in autism may not differ in kind or severity children with autism as several recent pub-
from that seen in other groups with motor dif- lished studies have demonstrated unimpaired
ficulties, such as children with retardation. If executive function performance in young chil-
a nonspecific praxis deficit reflects a general- dren with autism compared to controls (Daw-
ized central nervous system impairment rather son et al., 2002; Griffith, Pennington, Wehner,
than an autism-specific motor problem, then it & Rogers, 1999). Thus, while executive func-
cannot explain the imitation impairment in tions may play some role in imitative skill, the
autism. evidence does not support this as a primary
mechanism for explaining the imitation diffi-
Executive Functions: Sequencing and
culties in autism.
Working Memory
Symbolic Content
Rogers and Pennington (1991) proposed that an
executive function deficit might lead to prob- In a study by Rogers and colleagues (1996)
lems with imitation, given that imitation may in which actions with symbolic content were
have a working memory component. The neu- compared with nonmeaningful actions, subjects
rological literature has demonstrated that with autism never performed differentially
patients with frontal lesions have motor se- worse on the meaningful conditions. Of four
quencing deficits (Kimberg & Farah, 1993). significant group differences found on the hand
An executive function deficit has been consis- and face tasks, only one was on a meaningful
tently reported in studies of older children task whereas three were found on nonmeaning-
and adults with autism (Bennetto, Pennington, ful tasks. Furthermore, autism-specific differ-
& Rogers, 1996; Ozonoff & McEvoy, 1994; ences on nonsymbolic tasks have been reported
Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991; Prior & from several studies (Bennetto, 1999; Smith &
Hoffman, 1990; Rumsey & Hamburger, 1988; Bryson, 1998). Thus, difficulties with symbolic
Russell, 1997). However, the evidence for an content do not appear to explain the imitation
executive function component in imitation deficit in autism.
problems is mixed. Rogers et al. (1996) demon-
Kinesthesia
strated that imitating manual sequences was
more impaired for the group with autism than a Perceptual-motor studies by Hermelin and
clinical comparison group. However, autism- O’Connor (1970) led to the suggestion that chil-
specific deficits in single movements appear to dren with autism may express abnormalities in
be as marked as deficits in sequential move- the integration of visual and kinesthetic input,
ments. Smith and Bryson (1998) and Bennetto which could certainly impair imitation of body
(1999) also reported that adding the sequenc- movements. One method for highlighting the
ing element to the nonmeaningful hand imita- role of kinesthesia in imitation is to prevent the
tion task did not lead to a decline in the autism subject from viewing his or her movements.
group’s performance. Finally, several studies Studies that have manipulated the imitator’s
have explicitly examined working memory for view of his or her movement copying attempts
the stimuli (Bennetto, 1999; Rogers et al., have found that the nonvisible gesture imita-
1996; Smith & Bryson, 1998). No study has tion items tend to differentiate autism and con-
reported any group difference involving the trol groups more than any other kind of task
Imitation and Play in Autism 389

(Roeyers, Van Oost, & Bothuyne, 1998). How- tions of another’s acts, imitation, and commu-
ever, nonvisible presentations increase the error nication of meaning, with critical links to lan-
rate in typical and developmentally delayed guage (Rizzolatti & Arbib, 1998).
participants as well as individuals with autism Recent functional imaging studies in hu-
(Smith & Bryson, 1998). Bennetto (1999) mans have identified parallel networks of cells
identified reproduction of limb postures— in frontal regions of humans that fire during
kinesthesia—as the most affected component observation of finger movements and fire
of imitation in her subjects with autism. Con- more rapidly when the observation is accom-
vergence of these findings makes kinesthesia panied by performance of the same action by
an area that should be investigated further. the observer (Rizzolatti et al., 2003). Observa-
tion of hand actions has been shown to result
Cross-Modal Matching and Body Mapping
in activity in the premotor cortex and Broca’s
Difficulties with cross-modal matching and area in humans (Iacoboni et al., 1999). This
body mapping might impair imitative ability area of premotor cortex has shown some evi-
but have been examined in only one imitation dence of mirror neuron activity and has been
study. Bennetto (1999) tested this explicitly implicated in reading facial emotion in a
using a task that examined body awareness. The normal population (Nakamura et al., 1999).
group with autism showed no differences from J. H. Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, and Per-
comparison subjects in accurately identifying rett (2001) propose that mirror neurons may
specific locations on their own bodies in re- facilitate understanding of others’ actions and
sponse to a line drawing of a body profile with intentions and that they may be involved in the
certain locations highlighted (also see Hobson, development of language, executive function,
Ouston, & Lee, 1989). This is an understudied and theory of mind abilities. Failure to de-
area that should be investigated further, but velop an intact mirror neuron system (or alter-
thus far, we have no evidence that cross-modal natively, failure to develop the mechanisms
transfer is the impaired function responsible necessary for proper regulation of such a sys-
for imitation impairments in autism. tem) could impair the development of these ca-
pabilities in humans. Williams and colleagues
Neural Mechanisms suggest this as a model for autism.
Two studies have examined mirror neuron
The information processing models of imitation functioning in autism. Individuals with autism
underlying several of the preceding studies showed less involvement in this mirror neuron
have been severely challenged by the discovery region during emotional interpretation (Naka-
of specialized neurons in the superior temporal mura et al., 1999). A very small study of imita-
sulcus (STS) of monkeys that appear to be ded- tion in Asperger syndrome did not reveal group
icated to the processing of visual information differences, but power problems may have been
about the actions of others (Rizzolatti, Fadiga, present (Avikainen, Kulomaki, & Hari, 1999).
Fogassi, & Gallese, 2003). Some of these neu- Mirror neurons have also been suggested as the
rons appear to code basic postures of the face, neural mechanism by which we understand the
limbs, or whole body, whereas others appear to intent of others’ actions. Several studies of in-
be involved in coding the movement of body tentionality in autism have demonstrated that
parts in relation to objects or goals. A subset of children with autism do not show difficulties
these neurons, identified in the prefrontal cor- on a simple intentionality task (Aldridge et al.,
tex in monkeys, fires when a specific action 2000; Carpenter, Pennington, & Rogers, 2001)
(such as reach and grasp) has been performed although mechanisms other than reading inten-
by the monkey as well as when the monkey tionality may underlie this task (see Huang,
observes another monkey performing the same Heyes, & Charman, 2002). Thus, the existence
specific action. These neurons have been la- of this mirror neuron system and its role in fa-
beled “mirror neurons” and are located in cilitating imitation (and other synchronous be-
Brodmann area 44, which corresponds to haviors between people) may provide us with
Broca’s area in the human brain. This finding new understanding of brain-behavior relations
suggests potential connections among observa- involved in imitation, but this line of research
390 Development and Behavior

is in its infancy and needs to be fully explored on imitating the other person as a person. The
in both typical and atypical groups. observer may reproduce the model’s actions
because the actions invoke a representation of
Methodological Issues an outcome in the environment, creating an in-
tention in the imitator to carry out the intended
Methodological issues may underlie the dis- act, rather than to imitate the model’s motor
crepancies in findings across imitation studies, movements. Tomasello’s distinction between
which have occurred most often on tasks in- emulation learning and cultural learning may
volving actions on objects and in studies assess- be relevant here. In emulation learning, the in-
ing older and higher functioning subjects. Task dividual’s goal is to create a specific result in
characteristics that appear to influence results the environment. In cultural learning, the ob-
include the novelty, difficulty level, and con- server not only directs attention to the other’s
ventionality of the movement. One difficulty in activity and to the objects involved but also at-
many of the methods used in these studies has tempts to be like the other person, to perceive
to do with task affordances and conventional the situation the way the other sees it.
actions. When people are asked to imitate con- How can these be teased apart? Hobson,
ventional actions on common objects that tap Lee, and Brown (1999) provide a helpful ex-
specific affordances of the object (rolling a car, ample. The tasks given to the subjects had
marking with a pen, hitting a drum with a stick, both an instrumental function and an affec-
etc.), you must question to what extent imita- tive quality to them, and the accuracy of the
tion of the precise movements of the model is instrumental function and the stylistic quality
required. A simpler and nonimitative process, were rated separately. Both the participants
such as stimulus enhancement or repetition of with autism and those with other developmen-
previously learned and automatic actions, could tal disorders could perform the instrumental
suffice. To control for other interpretations, it aspects of the tasks accurately. However,
is important that object imitation tasks involve those with autism were much poorer than the
novel acts that are not directly elicited by the controls at imitating the affective quality of
object’s unique features. the movement. The authors proposed that
The effect of the type of task chosen for im- what distinguished their subjects with autism
itation of actions on objects was illustrated by from the clinical comparison group was not so
Roeyers et al. (1998). The group of 18 young much their inability to imitate the actions
children with autism in their study performed modeled but rather their deficiency in their at-
significantly worse than a well-matched group tempts to imitate the person who modeled
of children with retardation on imitation of them. They suggested further that in typically
gestures and on imitation of actions on objects, developing infants, it may be these goal-irrele-
with the imitation of gestures more impaired vant aspects of imitation such as the imitation
than imitation of actions on objects. However, of affective tone and body language that con-
the causal effect of the action on the object tribute to establishing the intersubjective con-
seemed to have an impact on the magnitude of tact, or the “like me” experience with others
the difference in group performance. The ac- (Meltzoff & Gopnik, 1993). To understand
tion task that best served to discriminate the the imitation problems in autism, we must
two groups involved an object that did not tend to these distinctions.
produce a sensorimotor effect. This brings up a A final methodological issue concerns scor-
related methodological issue: How do we begin ing practices. Those studies that have reported
to examine the different functions of imita- differences on body imitations in older and
tion? While on the surface, all imitation tasks higher functioning persons have tended to use
seem inherently social, Tomasello (1998) more detailed scoring systems that involve
has argued that, in an instrumental, goal- analysis of the movements on videotape. How-
directed, and object-oriented act, the focus of ever, the typical scoring system used in many
the imitative behavior may be on the means- studies involves live ratings of “correct, partial,
ends relations inherent in the act, rather than or fail.” While the differences among younger
Imitation and Play in Autism 391

subjects may be extensive enough to be cap- uniformly affected in autism. Functions in-
tured in live ratings, such scoring systems may volving instrumental learning of meaningful
not be sensitive to imitation differences in actions on objects may be less affected than
older and higher functioning persons, which the function of imitation in facilitating social
may be more subtle and require a more fine- interactions. Oral-facial and body imitation
grained analysis. may particularly subserve the social aspect,
while imitations of acts on objects may par-
Summary of Imitation ticularly serve the instrumental aspect. There
has been little examination of relationships
The imitation studies in autism appear to have between imitative ability and social behavior
established that imitation is specifically im- in autism. In addition to continued investiga-
paired in autism, from the earliest time at tion into possible mechanisms underlying the
which autism can be diagnosed, persisting into imitation deficit and brain-behavior relations
adulthood, in both higher functioning and lower related to imitative behavior, future research
functioning groups. More than most other neu- should consider the possibility that different
ropsychological areas of impairment in autism, functions of imitation may be differentially
imitation thus appears to meet the four criteria affected in autism.
for a primary psychological deficit in autism:
universality, specificity, precedence, and per- PLAY IN AUTISM
sistence (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1991). Several
mechanisms hypothesized to account for the For the purposes of this chapter, play is defined
imitation deficit in autism, such as symbolic as the purposeful manipulation of objects in
content, visual representation, cross-modal which exploration and practice of effects appear
transfer, and working memory, have been ex- to be the child’s goals. Play is considered a pow-
amined and rejected. The mechanism with the erful means by which the young of many species
greatest support is motor planning/execution, master skills that will eventually be important
which accounts for some, but not all, of the for their development and survival (Bruner,
variance in imitation performance in autism. 1972). Piaget (1962) considered play to be an
However, general motor problems do not appear intrinsically motivated activity, in which carry-
to be specific to autism, as similar levels of ing out the activity is pleasurable. He distin-
motor impairment are also found in children guished between sensorimotor play, involving
with mental retardation who do not have object manipulation as a means for practice and
autism. Thus, motor difficulties (or dyspraxia) mastery of action schemas, and symbolic, or
certainly contribute to the imitation problems pretend play, which grows out of the child’s de-
in autism, but are not necessarily a primary veloping ability for mental representation and
mechanism underlying imitation problems in provides a means of practicing and understand-
autism. An exception to this may involve oral ing the events of the social world.
imitation. For the few studies that have specifi- Symbolic play is generally defined as play
cally examined oral-facial movements, there in which absent elements are represented
is consistent evidence of dysfunction and devia- through objects, gestures, and language in the
tion from typical patterns. Furthermore, the play. This may take the form of animating the
relationships between oral-facial imitations play characters or by representing absent ob-
and speech development have been repeatedly jects through object substitution or pantomime
found to be large and significant. Thus, a spe- (pantomime would seem to play a very impor-
cific oral-motor dysfunction may be involved in tant role in bridging between imitation and
autism, leading directly in its severe form to symbolic play, since pantomime fuses the con-
impairments in speech development specific to cepts of deferred imitation and pretending).
autism and perhaps to imitation of facial ex- Pretend play generally appears in a toddler’s
pressions as well. repertoire by 18 months and becomes increas-
Neither the various types of imitation ingly elaborated over the preschool period
nor the various functions of imitation may be (McCune-Nicholich, 1977).
392 Development and Behavior

Autism-Specific Findings in trols. In these early papers arises another


Pretend Play theme that reoccurs throughout the symbolic
play literature in autism—the improved ability
In 1975, Ricks and Wing reviewed what was of children with autism to carry out pretend
known about communication, conceptual de- play in scaffolded conditions. These two pa-
velopment, and play in autism. “ The central pers, examined together, highlight issues that
problem, present in even the most mildly hand- are not yet resolved: (1) methodological issues
icapped autistic people, appears to be a spe- concerning appropriate methods for eliciting
cific difficulty in handling symbols, which symbolic play in children with autism and
affects language, nonverbal communication, (2) conceptual concerns involving the cognitive
and many other aspects of cognitive and social processes involved in imitating and sponta-
activity” (p. 214). Lack of symbolic play neously producing symbolic play schemas.
was considered to be one of the main symp- A number of papers in the 1980s replicated
toms of this inner lack of symbolic capacity. In findings from these two papers, with increas-
1977, Wing, Gould, Yeates, and Brierley pub- ing attention to methodological issues involved
lished the first major research paper on sym- in administration and rating of play schemas,
bolic play in autism. The group documented as well as matching of clinical populations.
two original findings that would stand the Sigman and Ungerer (1984) and Mundy, Sig-
test of time: (1) There is a paucity of sponta- man, Ungerer, and Sherman (1986) replicated
neous symbolic play in children with autism Wing’s earlier findings of autism-specific
whose developmental functioning level ap- deficits in three related areas: frequency of
pears mature enough to support symbolic play; spontaneous pretend play acts, frequency and
and (2) for those who demonstrated symbolic complexity of symbolic sequences, and fre-
play acts, their play appeared repetitive and quency of different symbolic acts. These find-
stereotypic, lacking the typical variety of dif- ings were obtained for both spontaneous play
fering play acts seen in comparison groups of acts and for play that occurred in response to
similar mental ages. an adult model. However, in a finding that has
This paper was followed in 1981 by two come to have large repercussions, these authors
important comparative papers that used adult also reported that the children’s nonsymbolic
scaffolded conditions to stimulate symbolic play was similarly affected. Under spontaneous
play. Hammes and Langdell (1981) compared play conditions, children with autism demon-
pretend play acts using increasingly abstract strated fewer functional and sensorimotor play
props imitated from video models of eight chil- acts and fewer dif ferent play acts. However, un-
dren with autism who had little or no language like their symbolic play, adult modeling and
and eight children with mental retardation, prompting resulted in normalizing the fre-
matched for mental and chronological age. They quency of functional play. These studies con-
reported that children with autism imitated the tinued to emphasize symbolic deficits as a core
play acts with real objects similarly to compar- part of the autism picture.
ison children, but differed in their lack of use
of pantomimed or symbolically transformed Symbolic Play as Metarepresentation
acts. In contrast to Wing et al.’s (1977) view,
they suggested that the children’s difficulty There were conceptual problems with this
was not due to a problem with symbol forma- early view of autism as a problem of symbolic
tion, but rather with flexible manipulation of abilities. From the Piagetian standpoint, while
symbols. Riguet, Taylor, Benaroya, and Klein symbolic play and symbolic language involve
(1981) compared three groups of children in mental representation, so do stage 6 object per-
free play and modeling conditions, with the manence, means end relations, and spatial rela-
findings of better performance of all children tions, which also require the child to operate
in scaffolded conditions and poorer imitation from internalized, mental models of the world
and lower levels of symbolic play in scaf- (Piaget, 1962), and which do not present spe-
folded conditions for children with autism cial problems for children with autism (Morgan
than language-matched clinical or typical con- et al., 1989; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984).
Imitation and Play in Autism 393

Alan Leslie’s (1987) landmark paper pro- Challenges to the


vided a new interpretation of pretend play. He Metarepresentational Account
suggested that, unlike other acts that require
representational thought, pretend play in- While the metarepresentational account of
volves a more complex representational symbolic play problems in autism was theoret-
stance. The child needs to simultaneously ically satisfying, several challenges to this in-
hold two representations in mind: the primary, terpretation arose out of findings over the next
or veridical representation, and the newly as- decade, involving: (1) new evidence of sym-
signed pretend identity (a state of double bolic play abilities in autism, (2) evidence of
knowledge; McCune-Nicholich, 1981), both problems with nonsymbolic play in autism, and
during his or her own play and when faced (3) symbolic immaturities in typical children.
with pretend play of others. The child decou-
Evidence of Intact Symbolic Abilities
ples the representations from his or her real-
world roles and assigns his or her new pretend The uniformity of findings in symbolic play
identities, representing the pretend world deficits in autism was severely challenged
alongside the real world. Leslie suggested that when Lewis and Boucher (1988) published a
aspects of pretend representations were simi- paper demonstrating equivalent performance of
lar to those seen in mental state representa- children with autism and controls under condi-
tions in that reference, truth, and existence tions that involved no modeling. In an effort to
relations among primary representations are isolate the cognitive deficit underlying children
suspended. Defining pretend play as a meta- with autism’s performance problems in sym-
representational act drew parallels between bolic play, these authors developed a method
the cognitive processes involved in theory of for separating symbolic play competence from
mind tasks and those involved in pretend play. performance. The task involved dolls and cars,
Leslie suggested that the poor performance of miniature objects, and junk objects in two con-
children with autism on both tasks was due to ditions: spontaneous and elicited. Unlike other
a difficulty in cognitive decoupling necessary studies, in the elicited condition, the symbolic
for metarepresentation. idea was verbally suggested, but not modeled,
Building on Leslie’s theory, Baron-Cohen by the adult, who asked the children, “Show me
(1987) reported a very carefully constructed how you would make a . . .”
study of spontaneous play, in which he set out to Using the strict definitions of symbolic play
correct earlier methodological inconsistencies, suggested by Leslie and measures of quantity,
laid out a clear rationale for matching groups, quality, and duration, the authors demonstrated
and suggested tight definitions of symbolic and that there was the expected autism-specific
functional play. He also pointed out the prob- problem in the spontaneous condition. However,
lems of using adult modeling and the resulting there was no autism difference in the use of
confusion between imitation and symbolic acts substitute or imaginary objects in the elicited
in previous studies. In a study of completely condition, which the authors interpreted as sug-
spontaneous play using junk props, miniatures, gesting that children with autism did not have a
and dolls, he compared 10 verbal children with problem with the representational aspects of
autism, 10 with mental retardation, and 10 typi- play, but rather with generation of play ideas—
cal children, all matched for verbal ability of an executive problem involving generativity
4 years. This study thus involved higher func- rather than a representational problem.
tioning children with autism than had been pre- This paper created great controversy, but the
viously reported on. The children with autism several replications that followed (Charman &
produced much less symbolic play than com- Baron-Cohen, 1997; Jarrold, Boucher, & Smith,
parison groups, but no differences in functional 1996; Lewis & Boucher, 1995; McDonough
play. Supporting Leslie’s hypothesis, Baron- et al., 1997) all supported the initial findings.
Cohen suggested the symbolic play deficit in Additionally, Lewis and Boucher’s replication
autism reflected an impairment in metarepre- specifically put in a direct test of generativity:
sentation, which he believed to be the primary a condition that examined the number of differ-
psychological impairment in autism. ent ideas a child could generate in a specific
394 Development and Behavior

time period. They found autism-specific dif- another. They could represent absent objects
ferences in the generativity task, but not in in play, assign new identities to existing ob-
the elicited play condition—findings they inter- jects, and ignore the salience of the object’s
preted as supporting the idea that the symbolic true identity while representing a different
play problem in autism is a production problem identity. These findings of preserved function
stemming from (1) a lack of generativity of and the competence/performance contrast thus
ideas and (2) difficulty shifting attention from challenged Leslie’s metarepresentational ac-
a current behavior to a new behavior. count of the symbolic play problem in autism.
Other papers published in the 1990s also
Evidence of Impaired Sensorimotor and
demonstrated intact skills in children with
Functional Play
autism in certain kinds of symbolic play tasks.
Kavanaugh and Harris (1994) tested Leslie’s A second crucial challenge to the metarepre-
(1987) suggestion that understanding another’s sentational explanation of play deficits in
pretend play required the same metarepresenta- autism concerns autism-specific differences in
tional decoupling that production required play that had no symbolic aspects to it. Non-
another way of separating competence from symbolic play can be categorized as sensorimo-
performance. Children were shown pretend tor or functional. Sensorimotor play involves
acts with six different small animals (e.g., the manipulation of the objects for their sensori-
experimenter held a teapot over the animal and motor properties, and functional play involves
said he or she was pretending to pour tea on it). combining objects and forming play acts in
Children were then asked, “How does the . . . ways that reflect social conventions—using ob-
look now?” and were asked to select one of jects in the way they are typically used (eating
three pictures showing the animal in various and drinking from plates, cups, and utensils) or
physical states, including the real state, the pre- combining objects in socially conventional
tend state, and another transformed state. The ways (placing cups on saucers).
children with autism performed better than The findings about nonsymbolic play dif-
the comparison group on these tasks, which ferences in autism are somewhat mixed.
were also completed by typically developing Baron-Cohen (1987) reported that 8-year-old
children age 30 months. The authors inter- children with autism did not differ from care-
preted the findings as supporting the idea that fully matched comparison groups in their
the symbolic play problem is one of production, functional play. Sigman and Ruskin (1999)
not one of understanding, as supported also by similarly reported no autism-specific differ-
treatment studies in autism that have demon- ences in functional play skills in their sample
strated improved symbolic play after modeling of 70 preschoolers with autism.
(Goldstein, Wickstrom, Hoyson, Jamieson, & However, the majority of studies examining
Odom, 1988; Rogers & Lewis, 1989; Thorp, functional and sensorimotor play have reported
Stahmer, & Schreibman, 1995). While a repli- group differences. Tilton and Ottinger (1964)
cation of this study also demonstrated no group found that children with autism differed from
differences (Jarrold, Smith, Boucher, & Harris, mentally retarded and normal children in their
1994), floor effects for both groups made organization of play behaviors. Using very
further interpretation of results difficult. Fi- careful definitions of play, Sigman and Ungerer
nally, Jarrold et al. demonstrated that children (1984) reported that in spontaneous play condi-
with autism were as able to use ambiguous and tions, young children with autism with low
counterconventional props to enact pretend se- verbal mental ages produced fewer functional
quences as well as comparison groups. Further- play acts, especially with dolls, fewer different
more, the children with autism did not have functional play acts, and fewer sequences than
trouble switching sets from the real to the pre- a comparison group of children with mental
tend identity of these objects. retardation. However, in an adult modeling and
These various studies of symbolic play prompting condition, the group differences
demonstrated that children with autism could were no longer present. Similar findings have
form and manipulate symbols associated with been reported by Mundy et al. (1986), Lewis
play acts when the symbol was suggested by and Boucher (1988), Blanc et al. (2000), and
Imitation and Play in Autism 395

McDonough et al. (1997). E. Williams, Reddy, the toddler period demonstrates the infant’s
and Costall (2001) found striking qualitative new ability to generate multiple representa-
differences in both functional and sensorimotor tions of reality. The infant can use multiple
play of children with autism, In addition to models to substitute for each other and, in
abnormalities in rates and levels of sensorimo- doing so, represent past events, coming events,
tor and functional play, the proportion of imma- and as-if events. To Perner, acting as-if does
ture to more mature play appears affected in not require metarepresentation. Substitution of
autism. Sigman and Ungerer and Libby et al. representations does not require metarepresen-
documented that children with autism spent tation; it instead requires the use of multiple
equivalent amounts of time playing in immature models, which young children mark in their
types of play as well as more mature types of play. The capacity for metarepresentation de-
play, while comparison children spent the ma- velops slowly across the preschool period and
jority of their play time in more mature play. is later reflected by much more advanced pre-
Thus, there is considerable evidence that tend play, especially role play, with others.
children with autism exhibit qualitative dif- Perner’s view is nicely supported by work
ferences compared to typically developing, of Tomasello, Striano, and Rochat (1999),
developmentally delayed, and other clinical who also question the symbolic interpretation
populations in their nonsymbolic play skills of early pretend play, both from their own and
and that the differences somewhat mirror the DeLoache’s (1995) findings concerning young
differences seen in their symbolic play: more preschoolers’ difficulties in understanding
repetition, less novelty, and less diversity of what miniatures represent. In two studies of 18-
play schemas, with immature patterns predom- to 35-month-old children’s understanding and
inating. These differences are not well ac- production of symbols as representations of an
counted for by the metarepresentational model. absent object, Tomasello and his colleagues
found that this ability gradually developed over
Symbolic Difficulties in Typical Children
this age period, virtually absent at 18 months,
The final challenge to the metarepresentational well developed and integrated at 35 months,
theory of pretend play covered here comes and spotty in 26-month-olds. The authors sug-
from other cognitive theorists. There is a basic gest that early pretend play of the sort seen in
developmental problem in the metarepresenta- 18- to 24-month-olds and older is heavily scaf-
tional account. How can pretend play, which folded by either adult language or through de-
is developing in 18-month-olds, involve the same ferred imitation of previously seen events. Only
metarepresentational abilities as theory of gradually can children go on to use symbols
mind, which in typical development, does not that are socially acquired, as well as newly in-
develop until age 4? Several theorists suggest vented, in individually creative ways.
that early pretend play can be accounted for by
simpler processes than metarepresentation. Pi- Development of the
aget (1962), distinguishing between mental rep- Generativity Hypothesis
resentations and the mental manipulation of
symbols, suggested that neither early words nor The preceding findings represent powerful
early pretend play was synonymous with symbol challenges to the metarepresentational hypoth-
use. He believed that early words, early pretend esis and led Christopher Jarrold and colleagues
play, and deferred imitation all demonstrated to focus on the competence/performance ques-
the formation and use of mental representa- tion. In a 1996 paper, they reported three
tions of previous experiences. He suggested that experiments, each exploring one or more di-
manipulation of symbols in thought was a later mensions of symbolic understanding of chil-
accomplishment of the 2- to 4-year-old preoper- dren with autism. Two of these, replications of
ational period. Kavanaugh and Harris (1994) and of Lewis and
Other theorists share somewhat similar Boucher (1988), have already been described
views. Perner (1991) suggests that the onset of (Jarrold et al., 1996). The third experiment ex-
pretend play, object permanence, language, amined generativity. The children were first
and the other representational milestones in asked to generate as many pretend acts as they
396 Development and Behavior

could, first without props, then with props (a ated with pretend play but not with sensorimo-
ruler, a scarf, candle, etc.). In both conditions, tor play. Generativity accounted for 27% of the
children with autism generated fewer pretend variance in pretend play scores in the total
acts than controls in both conditions. group and correlated significantly with sensori-
In an integration of the findings of compe- motor play scores, even when verbal ability was
tences and difficulties of children with autism partialled out. (See Blanc et al., 2000 for a sim-
in pretend play studies, Jarrold considered ilar report on a small n study.)
problems both with inhibition and with genera- However, in a somewhat contradictory find-
tivity as possible obstacles to pretend play for ing, Dawson and colleagues (Dawson et al.,
children with autism (Jarrold, 1997; Jarrold 1998) used both a spontaneous and an elicited
et al., 1996). A problem with inhibition could paradigm to stimulate pretend play in a group
impair symbolic play by making it difficult to of 20 preschoolers with autism and with clini-
inhibit the true nature of an object and shift to cal and typical comparisons. While the ex-
a hypothetical identity (Harris, 1993). From pected symbolic play deficit was present in the
the empirical evidence, Jarrold concluded that children with autism, symbolic play scores did
there was little support for the idea that chil- not correlate with performance on an executive
dren with autism had difficulty inhibiting the function measure tapping working memory and
real use of a prop in order to assign a substitute inhibition—the delayed response task. Instead,
use. In contrast, difficulties with generativity it correlated at .72 with scores from a measure
of play ideas had considerable support across of orbital prefrontal cortex—the delayed non-
studies. He suggested that this generativity match to sample (DNMS) task, which taps pri-
deficit occurred in many areas of functioning, mary medial temporal lobe functions.
citing research by others in word fluency, free While the arguments supporting the execu-
recall, and drawing studies that demonstrated tive function /generativity hypothesis are well
parallel results. In a detailed analysis of the reasoned and databased, the field has just
kinds of executive problems seen in autism, Jar- begun to examine relationships between gener-
rold suggests that difficulties generating new ativity (or, more broadly, executive functions)
behavior and difficulties in maintaining goals and performance on symbolic play tasks. The
in working memory could account for both the usefulness of this theory will be determined
patterns of reduced generativity and impulsive by the findings of additional studies that di-
behavior seen in children with autism in their rectly explore these relationships.
play and in many other situations (Jarrold,
1997; see also Harris & Leevers, 2000). Specificity of Symbolic Play
Tests of the Executive Deficits to Autism
Function/Generativity Hypothesis
Wing et al.’s (1977) study suggested that sym-
Very few studies have examined relations be- bolic play problems were unique to autism. As
tween any executive function measures and seen from the review thus far, this finding has
symbolic play abilities. Rutherford and Rogers been universally supported among studies of
(in press) examined the relationship among play spontaneous symbolic play. While it has been
maturity, joint attention, and executive func- suggested that blind children have similar
tion in a study of 28 very young children with kinds of difficulties developing symbolic play
autism and both delayed and typical compari- (Fraiberg, 1977), there is evidence that sym-
son groups. Two executive function tasks were bolic play may develop better than was initially
used: spatial reversal, which tests set shifting, expected in blind children. Rogers and Puchal-
and a generativity task, which examined num- ski (1984) documented the presence of pretend
ber of different play acts generated to single play acts in a group of blind children at age 25
toys without any particular function. Children months and found expected associations with
with autism were equivalent to both compari- language development and sensorimotor abili-
son groups on both executive function tasks. ties. Hobson et al. (1999) compared the sym-
However, for the entire group of children, both bolic play of a group of blind children and a
executive function tasks were strongly associ- group of children with autism, matched on age
Imitation and Play in Autism 397

and language level. The blind children demon- nonverbal mental ages and elicited pretend play
strated many more symbolic play acts than skills in a sample of 2-year-olds with autism,
the children with autism, though the play was 90% of which had not yet developed speech. The
not complex. Thus, the current data suggest absence of relationships between language and
that the degree of difficulty that children with play in these two studies may be due to the very
autism have producing symbolic play is unique. young ages and essentially nonverbal status of
these toddlers with autism (the children with
Brain Behavior Correlates other diagnoses the same age and language level
shared the expected relations).
We have no neuroimaging studies of pretend The relationship of pretend play to language
play, or even pantomime, at this time and very development is interesting in light of the vary-
few neuropsychological models of pretend ing theories concerning the nature of the pre-
play. While the preceding generativity theory tend play problem. It was the presence of both
would emphasize frontal lobe contributions, play and language problems in autism that led
Dawson et al. (1998) demonstrated a correla- to the early symbolic deficit hypothesis. Mind
tion between symbolic play and medial tempo- theory would also expect language and play to
ral lobe tasks rather than executive function be related, given that a main purpose of lan-
tasks. This area is in great need of attention. guage is sharing of mental states and learning
language requires awareness of the contents of
Individual Differences and Developmental the speaker’s mind. However, the generativity
Correlates of Symbolic Play in Autism hypothesis would not necessarily predict that
language and symbolic play should be related.
There are clearly large differences in perfor- This is further complicated by the studies that
mances of individual children with autism on raise questions about how “symbolic” early
both functional and pretend play tasks. IQ and pretend play really is (or, for that matter, how
language level (which are themselves closely symbolic early language is). It is important to
related in autism) demonstrate associations remember the modeling or imitation procedure
with symbolic play skills. Baron-Cohen (1987) used in most of the symbolic play procedures.
reported that children with autism who could Imitation is correlated with various develop-
demonstrate pretend play acts had significantly mental skills, including language development
higher verbal and nonverbal IQs than non- in autism and in normal development. Thus, it
pretenders. Sigman and Ungerer (1984) also is possible that the relationships between sym-
reported correlations between language devel- bolic play and language may be mediated by
opment and pretend play in autism, though not the imitation skills in play studies that provide
with sensorimotor play. However, Sigman and play models for the children to imitate. The na-
Ruskin (1999) reported that both functional ture of the relationship between pretend play
play and symbolic play were related to concur- and language development in autism needs fur-
rent language abilities. They also reported sig- ther study.
nificant correlations between play and joint
attention behavior in preschoolers with autism, Delay versus Deficit
but these were mediated by general develop-
mental age, a precursor of language develop- As Libby and colleagues pointed out (1998),
ment, or with attention switching, which has the relationships between symbolic play devel-
implications for executive function. opment and language development in autism
However, two groups studying very young would suggest that symbolic play is delayed but
children with autism report an absence of corre- eventually develops in those children with the
lations between pretend play and verbal lan- cognitive abilities to acquire it. However, the
guage. Charman (2003) reported no relationship unusual pattern of differences in symbolic and
between play and language either concurrently functional play—the lack of fluency, the repet-
or predictively from age 20 to age 42 months. itiveness of the play, the continuing use of very
Similarly, Rutherford and Rogers (2003) re- immature sensorimotor patterns as well as
ported no relationship between either verbal or higher level patterns, and other aberrations in
398 Development and Behavior

the expression of play—indicates that play is in autism—the impairment of social learning


qualitatively different in autism, a deficit as mechanisms in autism.
well as a delay. Earlier, this chapter reviewed what is cur-
rently known about problems of imitation in
Experiential Effects on Pretend Play and autism. Given the early reliance of pretend
the Ecological Model of Autism play on deferred imitation, you would expect
that these two skills are related, both in typi-
In addition to developmental maturity, child- cal development and in autism. To our knowl-
hood experiences affect symbolic play de- edge, there are currently no published data on
velopment in typically developing children. the relationships between deferred imitation
While the tendency of developmentalists is to and symbolic play.
view symbolic play as a universal developmen- Second, reenacting life events requires that
tal accomplishment, cross-cultural studies do children attend to social events and be ori-
not support this view. Symbolic play as defined ented to others, their actions on objects, and
here seems to be to some extent a phenomenon their interactions. As Sigman and Ungerer sug-
of middle-class Western cultures. In other cul- gested in 1984, lack of typical pretend play
tures, there may be much rough and tumble early in life in autism may reflect the lack of
play or practice play, and there may be role social learning on the part of the young child
play in which the children act out adult roles with autism or lack of pleasure in the social
but without symbolic transformation (Feitel- routines of life. Note that lack of social learn-
son, 1977; Feitelson & Ross, 1973). In addi- ing would affect functional play, which also
tion, the play of Western children who are involves the expression of socially conven-
severely socioeconomically deprived is marked tional ways of acting on objects, as well as
by continued sensorimotor practice play quali- symbolic play. Relationships between play
ties (Murphy, 1972). Western preschoolers skills and social engagement have been de-
from rural settings also demonstrate a paucity scribed by Sigman and Ruskin (1999) in a
of symbolic play transformations compared to large longitudinal study in which preschool
their middle class suburban peers (Feitelson & functional and symbolic play skills predicted
Ross, 1973). Thus, symbolic play is to some ex- adolescent peer engagement, but not language
tent a cultural phenomenon, supported by adult development, in a large group of children with
provision of play materials and play space, as autism. The sociocultural aspects of pretend
well as by adult psychological support through play have been eloquently summarized by
active participation and modeling of pretend Tomasello et al. (1999): “. . . the process of
play and encouragement and respect for chil- symbolic play development, as other cultural
dren’s pretend play activities. skills such as language development, may be
While adults often think of children’s pre- seen as a delicate interplay between children’s
tend play as involving fantastic images and emerging skills to interact with the world in
deeply imaginative and creative activities, culturally conventional ways, and their emerg-
the reality of early pretend play is that it in- ing skills to use these cultural conventions in
volves a replaying of daily life events (and individually creative ways” (p. 583).
again reflects the fuzzy boundaries between Finally, this kind of “acculturation” theory
defferred imitation and early pretend play). is congruent with the already described rela-
Young children play out their lives: bedtime tionships between language and symbolic play,
routines; mealtime routines; family dramas; since both rely heavily on acculturation, or so-
and trips to the doctor, zoo, vet, and MacDon- cial learning and imitation, in the toddler pe-
ald’s; using real objects, miniatures, and neu- riod. Given the autism-specific effects on early
tral objects, as well as verbal scripts associated social orienting documented by Osterling and
with these activities. The learning mechanism Dawson (1994); Baranek (1999); Werner, Daw-
appears to involve deferred imitation or social son, Osterling, and Dinno (2000); and others in
learning. And this leads to the final hypothesis infant video studies, there is every reason to be-
concerning the nature of pretend play deficits lieve that this would result in a diminished be-
Imitation and Play in Autism 399

havioral repertoire early enough to affect both than a typical group matched for developmen-
functional and symbolic play (see Loveland, tal level (although the typical group is a very
2001, for a compelling description of this eco- useful third group in these studies, since it
logical model of autism). Whether the lack of provides a point of comparison for the data
social modeling, social learning, and experien- from the clinical comparison group). Because
tial differences can fully account for the diffi- symbolic play has been found to have relation-
culties in spontaneous production of functional ships with language abilities in some studies, it
and symbolic play is unclear (Harris, 2000). is crucial that groups are matched for expres-
While we currently have no studies that sive language skill rather than nonverbal or
describe relationships among symbolic play overall IQ or MA. Some researchers have ar-
and social interest, social orientation, and imi- gued that this needs to include a measure of
tation in early autism, the intervention evi- expressive language complexity, rather than a
dence is useful here. A number of studies have measure of picture naming vocabulary only,
documented improvements in children with since picture naming may overestimate the
autism’s pretend play through a variety of fo- language skills of children with autism.
cused treatment interventions, both behavioral In terms of tasks, a pervasive problem has
and relational. These studies have demon- been in the inconsistent definition of pretend
strated increases in frequency and complexity play. Acts to self, acts to dolls, and use of
of symbolic play after treatment and corollary miniatures have been classified as symbolic
increases in social and communicative interac- acts in some studies and as functional play acts
tions with others. This treatment literature in others. Baron-Cohen (1987) has suggested a
provides some indirect support for the hypoth- clear definition of symbolic play, and Libby
esis that there is an experiential aspect to the et al. (1998) have provided an excellent classi-
symbolic play difficulties seen in autism and fication system for discriminating sensorimo-
that interventions focused on increasing chil- tor play from functional play, as well as clear
dren’s experience and motivation for such play definitions of symbolic play in line with
has effects on play complexity, play frequency, Baron-Cohen’s. While both of these systems
and increased social engagement. classify doll play as functional (even when the
The social hypothesis may present us with doll is exhibiting agency), the unique and spe-
an independent explanation for the symbolic cific problems that children with autism have
play difficulties in autism, or it may be inter- with doll play may illustrate that doll play in-
twined with the generativity hypothesis, in that volves symbolic rather than functional play
when social engagement and social learning are and should be empirically examined.
not providing new play content and ideas, chil- Moving to the issue of task administration,
dren are left to the mercy of their own imma- the state of the science in this area requires that
ture and meager repertoire of sensory motor experimenters examine children’s competence
acts to create play schemas. separate from their performance of pretend
play. Having adults model pretend play acts and
Methodological Issues then scoring the child’s productions as symbolic
acts confounds imitation with symbolic produc-
A variety of methodological problems pervade tion and impedes interpretation of findings, as
the studies of pretend play in autism and have nicely discussed by Libby, Powell, Messer, and
been well discussed by Baron-Cohen (1987) Jordan (1997). Data have been most typically
and Jarrold et al. (1993). While the more re- coded as frequency counts from videotape.
cent studies demonstrate improved methods, There are some tools in the literature that pro-
both researchers and readers of research must vide developmental play age equivalents (e.g.,
be sensitive to the design challenges involved. Fewell, 1992), which can be useful in examining
Selection of comparison groups is an ongoing developmental correlates. However, if such tools
challenge in autism studies. The question are used, it is critical that the definitions of
of autism-specific differences requires an age- pretend play in those tests be examined to
matched clinical comparison group, rather make sure that symbolic play is being classified
400 Development and Behavior

according to the principles described earlier. Fi- with neuropsychological correlates, and with
nally, what frequency variables are gathered social and environmental variables. The ques-
and analyzed has significant effect on the inter- tion of brain-behavior relations has only begun
pretation of group differences. As the studies to be mentioned. Finally, we need to explore the
from Sigman’s lab have illustrated, examining theorized relations among immediate imitation,
total number of play acts in a category can give deferred imitation, pantomime, and symbolic
quite a different picture than examining number play. Such efforts will most likely have a great
of novel play acts or maturity of play acts. Qual- payoff, for studying the primary symptoms of
itative differences may be missed when more autism comparatively and at multiple levels of
fine-grained aspects of the play are not consid- analysis have taken us far in understanding
ered. Finally, both ceiling and floor effects have autism and have broadened considerably our un-
arisen in some of the symbolic play studies. derstanding of normal development.
Making sure that all the groups in a study are
appropriately challenged by the task is crucial CONCLUSION
for identifying autism-specific differences or
lack of differences. The research literature in imitation and sym-
bolic play clearly demonstrates the severity
Summary of Play with which autism affects these skills. The
early appearance of these two skills in normal
The work in symbolic play in autism seems development and their seeming importance in
poised at the edge of new developments. The human social, communicative, and cognitive
increasing evidence that typically developing development indicate that their impairment in
children are not actually using symbols as such autism may have powerful roles in determining
until ages 3 to 4 requires us to view these early outcomes in autism. From the work that has
pretend play schemas as something other than been done in imitation, it appears that there is
symbolism per se. Metarepresentational theory a fundamental difficulty with imitation of
is not a persuasive explanation for these other people’s actions in autism that permeates
deficits due to the timing of the deficits, the many different kinds of tasks and performance
lack of symbolic ability of typically developing across both highly scaffolded and natural set-
children in the early stages of pretend play, and tings. In contrast, performance of functional
the parallel impairments between pretend play and symbolic play varies according to setting,
and simpler types of play. Executive dysfunc- with normalized levels of play demonstrated
tion theories capture several aspects of the in certain types of scaffolded situations and
play problems in autism: the predominance of the most impaired performance demonstrated
repetitive, simple play behavior, both in senso- in spontaneous or free play situations. This
rimotor schemes and in symbolic schemes; the competence-performance distinction in sym-
ability to demonstrate understanding and pro- bolic play appears to indicate that mediating
duction of symbolic transformations under var- variables are at work, and the area of executive
ious conditions; and the lack of fluency in dysfunctions, particularly generativity deficits,
spontaneously generating schemas. Yet, we do is a prime candidate. Thus, the current litera-
not currently have convincing data of early ex- ture leads us to consider that imitation may be
ecutive function differences in autism, nor do the more primary of the autism deficits, with
we have data that clearly links play difficulties play abnormalities reflecting effects of intel-
with other executive function variables. The lectual impairment, executive dysfunction,
ecological, or social learning theory, is attrac- possible experiential deficits, and imitation
tive, but it needs to be developed and tested. decrements. Developmental theory links imi-
Symbolic play research would benefit from tation and symbolic play, a hypothesis that has
refinement of our methods. We need straightfor- not yet been tested. If this is indeed the case,
ward procedures that are consistently used these two areas of difficulty may in fact repre-
across studies so that data can be compared sent one core impairment in autism. We await
more easily. In each new study, we need to ex- studies that examine performance in these two
amine relations with other developmental skills, skill areas to other levels of analysis, in autism
Imitation and Play in Autism 401

and in other developmental groups, while at the Blanc, R., Tourrette, C., Delatang, N., Roux, S.,
same time examining the role of experience and Barthelemy, C., & Adrien, J. L. (2000). Regu-
environment, to understand the meaning of lation of symbolic activity and development of
these skill deficits in the development of the communication in children with autistic disor-
behavioral phenotype in autism. der. European Review of Applied Psychology,
50, 369–381.
Bruner, J. S. (1972). Nature and uses of immatu-
Cross-References rity. American Psychologist, 27, 687–708.
Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by
Developmental aspects of autism are addressed imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral
in Chapters 8 to 10, social development in and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–721.
autism is reviewed in Chapter 11 and com- Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
municative development in Chapter 12, affec- (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in
tive aspects of autism are reviewed in Chapter children with autism. Journal of Autism and De-
velopmental Disorders, 31, 589–599.
15, and theoretical perspectives are reviewed
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
in Chapters 21 through 26.
(2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive
skills in young children with autism. Journal
REFERENCES of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32,
91–106.
Adams, L. (1998). Oral-motor and motor-speech Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal
characteristics of children with autism. Focus skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabili- the Royal Society, 358, 315–324.
ties, 13, 108–112. Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Another
Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & look at imitation in autism. Development and
Bower, T. G. (2000). Preverbal children with Psychopathology, 6, 403–413.
autism understand intentions of others. Devel- Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Brief re-
opmental Science, 3, 294–301. port: Prompted pretend play in autism. Journal
Asendorpf, J. B., & Baudonniere, P. (1993). Self- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
awareness and other-awareness: Mirror self- 321–328.
recognition and synchronic imitation among Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S.,
unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants
29, 88–95. with autism: An investigation of empathy, pre-
Avikainen, S., Kulomaki, T., & Hari, R. (1999). tend play, joint attention, and imitation. Devel-
Normal movement reading in Asperger sub- opmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.
jects. Neuroreport, 10, 3467–3470. Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and
Ayres, A. J. (2000). Developmental dyspraxia and communication in mute autistic children. Jour-
adult onset apraxia. Torrance, CA: Sensory In- nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8,
tegration International. 281–292.
Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Social and ethical interpre- Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neuro-
tations in mental development. New York: logical model for childhood autism. Archives
Macmillan. of Neurology, 35, 777–786.
Baranek, G. (1999). Autism during infancy: A ret- Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and so-
rospective video analysis of sensory-motor cial responsiveness in autistic children. Journal
and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209–226.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Dawson, G., & Galpert, L. (1990). Mothers’ use of
ders, 29, 213–224. imitative play for facilitating social responsive-
Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic ness and toy play in young autistic children. De-
play. British Journal of Developmental Psy- velopment and Psychopathology, 2, 151–162.
chology, 5, 139–148. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., & Rinaldi,
Bennetto, L. (1999). A componential approach to J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of
imitation and movement deficits in autism. early symptoms of autism. Child Development,
Dissertation Abstracts International, 60(2-B), 69, 1276–1285.
819. Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., Mc-
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. Partland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neurocog-
(1996). Intact and impaired memory functions nitive function and joint attention ability in
in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835. young children with autism spectrum disorder
402 Development and Behavior

versus developmental delay. Child Develop- (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives
ment, 73, 345–358. from autism (pp. 182–202). Oxford, England:
DeLoache, J. S. (1995). Early symbol understand- Oxford University Press.
ing and use. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology Harris, P., & Leevers, H. J. (2000). Pretending,
of learning and motivation (pp. 65–114). New imagery, and self-awareness in autism. In
York: Academic Press. S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J.
DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Per-
W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., et al. spectives from developmental cognitive neuro-
(1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizo- science (2nd ed., pp. 182–202). Oxford,
phrenic, and nonpsychotic subnormal children. England: Oxford University Press.
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L.
nia, 2, 264–287. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York: Cam-
DeMyer, M. K., Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson, R. K. bridge University Press.
(1981). Infantile autism reviewed: A decade of Hauck, J. A., & Dewey, D. (2001). Hand preference
research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 388–451. and motor functioning in children with autism.
Feitelson, D. (1977). Cross-cultural studies of rep- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
resentational play. In B. Tizard & D. Harvey ders, 31, 265–278.
(Eds.), Biology at play (pp. 6–14). Philadel- Heimann, M. (1998). Imitation in neonates, in older
phia: Lippincott. infants and in children with autism: Feedback
Feitelson, D., & Ross, G. S. (1973). The ne- to theory. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective
glected factor—play. Human Development, 11, communication and emotion in early ontogeny
291–296. (de la Maison des Science de l’homme ed.,
Fewell, R. R. (1992). Play Assessment Scale (5th pp. 89–104). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge Uni-
rev.). (Available from University of Miami, versity Press.
Mailman Center for Child Development, P.O. Heimann, M., & Ullstadius, E. (1999). Neonatal
Box 014621, Miami FL 33101) imitation and imitation among children with
Forman, D. R., & Kochanska, G. (2001). Viewing autism and Down’s syndrome. In J. Nadel &
imitation as child responsiveness: A link be- G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy
tween teaching and discipline domains of so- (pp. 235–253). Cambridge, England: Cam-
cialization. Developmental Psychology, 37, bridge University Press.
198–206. Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1970). Psychologi-
Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the blind: Com- cal experiments with autistic children. New
parative studies of blind and sighted infants. York: Pergamon Press.
New York: New American Library. Heyes, C. M., & Galef, B. G. (Eds.). (1996). Social
Goldstein, H., Wickstrom, S., Hoyson, M., learning in animals: The roots of culture. San
Jamieson, B., & Odom, S. L. (1988). Effects of Diego, CA: Academic Press.
sociodramatic play training on social and com- Hill, D. A., & Leary, M. R. (1993). Movement distur-
municative interaction. Education and Treat- bance: A clue to hidden competencies in persons
ment of Children, 11, 97–117. diagnosed with autism and other developmental
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1994). Minds, bod- disabilities. Madison, WI: DRI Press.
ies, and persons: Young children’s understand- Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of
ing of the self and others as reflected in autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature, di-
imitation and theory of mind research. In S. T. agnosis and treatment (pp. 22–48). New York:
Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Bocchia Guilford Press.
(Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and human Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and
(pp. 166–186). New York: Cambridge Univer- identification in autism. Journal of Child Psy-
sity Press. chology and Psychiatry, 40, 649–660.
Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism
& Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism
young children with autism. Child Develop- and Developmental Disorders, 29, 45–56.
ment, 70, 817–832. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1989). Nam-
Hammes, J. G. W., & Langdell, T. (1981). Precur- ing emotion in faces and voices: Abilities and
sors of symbol formation and childhood autism. disabilities in autism and mental retardation.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
11, 331–346. 7, 237–250.
Harris, P. (1993). Pretending and planning. In S. Huang, C. T., Heyes, C., & Charman, T. (2002).
Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen Infants’ behavioral reenactment of “Failed
Imitation and Play in Autism 403

Attempts”: Exploring the roles of emulation Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1995). Generativity in the
learning, stimulus enhancement, and under- play of young people with autism. Journal
standing of intentions. Developmental Psychol- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25,
ogy, 38, 840–855. 105–122.
Hughes, C. (1996). Planning problems in autism at Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R.
the level of motor control. Journal of Autism (1997). Imitation of pretend play acts by chil-
and Developmental Disorders, 26, 99–109. dren with autism and Down syndrome. Journal
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). 365–383.
Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Sci- Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R.
ence, 286, 2526–2528. (1998). Spontaneous play in children with
Jarrold, C. (1997). Pretend play in autism: Execu- autism: A reappraisal. Journal of Autism and
tive explanations. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism Developmental Disorders, 28, 487–497.
as an executive disorder (pp. 101–133). Ox- Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (1996). Language level and
ford, England: Oxford University Press. nonverbal social-communicative behaviors in
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1993). Sym- autistic and language-delayed children. Jour-
bolic play in autism: A review. Journal of Autism nal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent
and Developmental Disorders, 23, 281–307. Psychiatry, 35, 1542–1550.
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. K. (1996). Loveland, K. A. (2001). Toward an ecological the-
Generativity deficits in pretend play in ory of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N.
autism. British Journal of Developmental Psy- Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The develop-
chology, 14, 275–300. ment of autism: Perspective from theory and
Jarrold, C., Smith, P. K., Boucher, J., & Harris, P. research (pp. 17–37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(1994). Comprehension of pretense in children Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D. A.,
with autism. Journal of Autism and Develop- & Brelsford, K. A. (1994). Imitation and ex-
mental Disorders, 24, 433–455. pression of facial affect in autism. Development
Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abil- and Psychopathology, 6, 433–444.
ities and neurological signs in autistic chil- Manjiviona, J., & Prior, M. (1995). Comparison of
dren. Journal of Autism and Developmental Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autis-
Disorders, 15, 37–46. tic children on a test of motor impairment.
Kavanaugh, R. D., & Harris, P. L. (1994). Imagin- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
ing the outcome of pretend transformations: ders, 25, 23–40.
Assessing the competence of normal children McCune-Nicholich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor
and children with autism. Developmental Psy- intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity
chology, 30, 847–854. through analysis of pretend play. Merrill-
Kimberg, D. Y., & Farah, M. J. (1993). A unified ac- Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89–99.
count of cognitive impairments following McCune-Nicholich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic
frontal lobe damage: The role of working mem- functioning: Structure of early pretend games
ory in complex, organized behavior. Journal of and potential parallels with language. Child
Experimental Psychology, 122, 411–428. Development, 52, 785–797.
Kohen-Raz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. McDonough, L., Stahmer, A., Schreibman, L., &
(1992). Postural control in children with Thompson, S. J. (1997). Deficits, delays, and
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental distractions: An evaluation of symbolic play
Disorders, 22, 419–432. and memory in children with autism. Develop-
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1999). Genesis and develop- ment and Psychopathology, 9, 17–41.
ment of early infant mimesis to facial and Meltzoff, A., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of im-
vocal models. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth itation in understanding persons and develop-
(Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 36–59). ing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen,
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Un-
Press. derstanding other minds: Perspectives from
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The autism (pp. 335–366). Oxford, England: Ox-
origins of “ Theory of Mind.” Psychological ford University Press.
Review, 94, 412–426. Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, in- facial and manual gestures by human neonates.
structed and elicited play in relatively able Science, 198, 75–78.
autistic children. British Journal of Develop- Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J.
mental Psychology, 6, 325–339. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in
404 Development and Behavior

autism: Profile of a complex information pro- Vol. 3. Models and integrations (pp. 117–159).
cessing disorder. Journal of the International Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representa-
Morgan, S. B., Cutrer, P. S., Coplin, J. W., & Ro- tional mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
drigue, J. R. (1989). Do autistic children differ Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in
from retarded and normal children in Piagetian childhood. New York: Norton.
sensorimotor functioning? Journal of Child Prior, M., & Hoffman, W. (1990). Brief report:
Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 857–864. Neuropsychological testing of autistic children
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. through an exploration with frontal lobe tests.
(1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
The contribution of non-verbal communication ders, 20, 581–590.
measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool children with inadequate
Psychiatry, 27, 657–669. communication: Developmental language disor-
Murphy, L. B. (1972). Infants’ play and cognitive der. London: Mac Keith Press.
development. In M. W. Piers (Ed.), Play and Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, com-
development (pp. 119–126). New York: Norton. munication, and the use of symbols in normal
Nadel, J., & Peze, A. (1993). What makes immediate and autistic children. Journal of Autism and
imitation communicative in toddlers and autis- Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.
tic children. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), Riguet, C. B., Taylor, N. D., Benaroya, S., & Klein,
New perspectives in early communication devel- L. S. (1981). Symbolic play in autistic, Down’s,
opment (pp. 139–156). London: Routledge. and normal children of equivalent mental age.
Nakamura, K., Kawashima, R., Ito, K., Sugiura, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
M., Kato, T., Nakamura, A., et al. (1999). Ac- 11, 439–448.
tivation of the right inferior frontal cortex Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language
during assessment of facial emotion. Journal within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21,
of Neurophysiology, 82, 1610–1614. 188–194.
Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V.
autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial (2003). From mirror neurons to imitation:
relationships, conceptualization, and gestural Facts and speculations. In A. Meltzoff & W.
imitation. Journal of Autism and Developmen- Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development,
tal Disorders, 17, 45–62. evolution, and brain bases (pp. 247–266). Cam-
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
tion of children with autism: A study of first Rodier, P. M. (2000). The early origins of autism.
birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism Scientific American, 282, 56–63.
and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257. Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Bothuyne, S. (1998).
Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. (2002). Immediate imitation and joint attention in
Early recognition of one-year old infants with young children with autism. Development and
autism spectrum disorder versus mental retar- Psychopathology, 10, 441–450.
dation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation
239–251. deficit in autism. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth
Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudi- (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 254–283).
nal study of executive function and theory of Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
mind development in autism. Development and Press.
Psychopathology, 6, 415–431. Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R. E., & Pen-
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. nington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime
(1991). Executive function deficits in high- in high functioning adolescents with autism
functioning autistic children: Relationship to spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67,
theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology 2060–2073.
and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105. Rogers, S. J., & Lewis, H. (1989). An effective day
Page, J., & Boucher, J. (1998). Motor impairments treatment model for young children with per-
in children with autistic disorder. Child Lan- vasive developmental disorders. Journal of the
guage Teaching and Therapy, 14, 233–259. American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1991). A neurosci- Psychiatry, 28, 207–214.
entific perspective on continuity and disconti- Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theo-
nuity in developmental psychopathology. In retical approach to the deficits in infantile
D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3,
symposium on developmental psychopathology: 137–162.
Imitation and Play in Autism 405

Rogers, S. J., & Puchalski, C. (1984). Development Tilton, J. R., & Ottinger, D. R. (1964). Comparison
of symbolic play in visually impaired infants. of the toy play behavior of autistic, retarded,
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 3, and normal children. Psychological Reports,
57–64. 967–975.
Rogers, S. J., Stackhouse, T., Hepburn, S. L., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Intending that others jointly
Wehner, E. A. (2003). Imitation performance attend. Pragmatics and Cognition, 6, 1998,
in toddlers with autism and those with other 229–243.
developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy- Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H.
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. Sciences, 16, 495–552.
(1993). Guided participation in cultural activ- Tomasello, M., Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (1999). Do
ity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of young children use objects as symbols? British
the Society for Research in Child Development, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17,
58, 1–161. 563–584.
Rumsey, J., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsy- Trevarthen, C., Kokkinaki, T., & Fiamenghi, G. A.
chological findings in high-functioning men (1999). What infant’s imitations communicate.
with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, in infancy (pp. 127–185). Cambridge, England:
10, 201–221. Cambridge University Press.
Russell, J. (1997). Executive functioning in autism. Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. and language comprehension in autistic chil-
Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (in press). The dren. Journal of the American Academy of Child
cognitive underpinnings of pretend play. Jour- Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and during infancy. International Journal of Be-
change in the social competence of children havioral Development, 4, 1–12.
with autism, Down syndrome, and develop- Uzgiris, I. C. (1999). Imitation as activity: Its devel-
mental delays. Monographs of the Society for opmental aspects. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth
Research inn Child Development, 64, 1–114. (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 186–206).
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984). Cognitive and Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, Press.
and normal children. Developmental Psychol- Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). Social learning:
ogy, 20, 293–302. Compounding some problems and dissolving
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1998). Gesture imi- others. Developmental Science, 5, 39–41.
tation in autism: I. Nonsymbolic postures and Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N.
sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism
747–770. spectrum disorder before one year of age: A
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the retrospective study based on home videotapes.
human infant. New York: Basic Books. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. 30, 157–162.
(1997). Motor imitation in young children Williams, E., Reddy, V., & Costall, A. (2001). Tak-
with autism: What’s the object? Journal of Ab- ing a closer look at functional play in children
normal Child Psychology, 25, 475–485. with autism. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spo- mental Disorders, 31, 67–77.
ken language level in children with autism Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Per-
spectrum disorders. Autism, 5, 341–361. rett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons
Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Ney, J., Fryman, J., and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
& Maurer, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in Review, 25, 287–295.
infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S., & Brierley, L.
autism. Proceedings of the National Academy (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally re-
of Sciences, USA, 95, 13982–13987. tarded and in autistic children. Journal of Child
Thorp, D. M., Stahmer, A. C., & Schreibman, L. Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 167–178.
(1995). Effects of sociodramatic play training
on children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 25, 265–282.
CHAPTER 15

Autism and Emotion

PETER HOBSON

The characteristic of autism that most im- pathogenesis of the syndrome? Is any one of
pressed Kanner (1943) about his 11 cases of these theoretical options likely to lead to a
children with “autistic disturbances of affec- satisfactory explanation of autism? Or are we
tive contact ” was their “inability to relate framing our questions in the wrong way?
themselves in the ordinary way to people and
situations from the beginning of life” (p. 242). CONCEPTUAL ISSUES
In the course of his case histories, Kanner
recorded a variety of clinical features that re- Kanner (1943) did not restrict himself to com-
flected the children’s seeming unawareness of menting on the children’s limited affective en-
the people around them and their impervious- gagement with people, pivotal though this
ness to the human significance of the sur- seemed. He also referred to their ways of relat-
rounding world. He concluded: “. . . further ing to things, for example, by showing repeti-
study of our children may help to furnish con- tive and often highly restricted interests. One
crete criteria regarding the still diffuse no- aspect of this disorder that has attracted much
tions about the constitutional components of attention in the subsequent literature is the way
emotional reactivity” (p. 250). in which children with autism have a paucity of
It has taken several decades to disentangle symbolic play; another is their relative inflexi-
Kanner’s original take on autism from another bility in adjusting language to the context in
and quite separate thesis about the disorder: which it is used and, especially, according to
that it might be caused by cold or other- meanings that depend on the psychological ori-
wise pathological mothering. For a long time, entations of speakers and listeners. Such im-
it seemed that only by positing a primary lin- pairments are not considered to be emotional
guistic or cognitive disorder could you reject for obvious reasons. If children with autism are
the suggestion that this is a psychogenic distur- unable to engage in creative, symbolic play,
bance or avoid the implication that the chil- even when they seem to be trying their best to
dren’s characteristic and severely restricting use play materials, you hardly want to say that
limitations in creative and context-sensitive they are prevented by emotional or motiva-
thinking are somehow incidental or of sec- tional factors; if they are simply at a loss when
ondary importance. Now freed from this trying to communicate, it would be wrong (not
constraint, we can acknowledge that social- to say perverse) to attribute this to their feel-
affective engagement may be disrupted by con- ings about the situation.
stitutional abnormalities that have potentially And yet these apparently clear examples
far-reaching developmental repercussions. may lead us to become overconfident about the
This change brings fresh grounds for un- conceptual boundaries that separate cognitive,
ease, however. Is there really going to be a test conative, and affective domains of psychologi-
of whether perceptual, cognitive, motiva- cal functioning. There is not only the deep
tional, or affective deficits are primary in the philosophical issue about what (if not some-

406
Autism and Emotion 407

thing like feelings or experiences) connects us relation to a shared world, are not only heavily
humans and our thoughts with what those imbued with affect and motivational force
thoughts are “about ”—the representations of a (especially through the “pull” of other peo-
computer are not like thoughts, because they ple’s attitudes) but also transformational for
do not have this natural aboutness and have to the growing child’s ability to achieve a kind of
be interpreted by humans—but also the devel- mental space required for symbolic thinking.
opmental issue of what thoughts, feelings, and Given that there are a number of things we
motivations develop out of. It is at least plausi- want to know about the role of emotion in the
ble that certain qualities of our thinking (for pathogenesis and expression of autism, it is not
example, some of the ways we discriminate surprising that methods have evolved to inves-
this from that, or generalize from one object or tigate the domain from various standpoints.
situation or event to another) are dependent on Each method has its own strengths and limi-
the ways that things affect us or lend them- tations, and each its own standards for
selves to actions that have meaning because of methodological adequacy. There are important
accompanying feelings. In this sense, at least, insights to be gained from individual case
there are emotional bases for thinking. Or descriptions, clinical accounts of groups of
again, there are forms of “ feeling perception”: children, systematic observational studies, in-
To see a smile as a smile is to have a propen- terviews with informants such as parents,
sity to react with feelings, and it may be only quasi-experimental investigations, more strictly
because we have the capacity to perceive and controlled experiments on specific aspects of
relate to others’ expressions in this way that the disorder, and family and related studies elu-
we come to understand smiles as expressive of cidating the role of genetic and environmental
inner states and ultimately come to compre- factors in pathogenesis. As discussed later,
hend the nature of people with minds (Hobson, there is also value in studying atypical forms
1993a, 1993b). More radically still, if one per- of autism, and neurofunctional studies will un-
son can share experiences with someone else doubtedly bring new insights. Some of these
only because of affective coordination be- studies yield results that increase our knowl-
tween the two and if sharing of this kind is re- edge of what is or is not characteristic of
quired for a range of transactions that occur autism, some are more concerned with the de-
between people—for example, an adult point- gree to which given deficits are specific to the
ing out things to a child or negotiating those emotional domain, and others again point to
forms of symbolic meaning embodied in lan- alternative pathways of abnormal develop-
guage and creative play—then the cognitive ment. In the following discussion, a less than
implications of supposedly “emotion-specific” comprehensive sample of studies from the vo-
impairments are wide-ranging indeed (Hob- luminous and increasingly convergent litera-
son, 2002). ture is presented.
Therefore, once you adopt a developmental Case descriptions comprise the most diffi-
perspective, you can no longer assume that cult part of that literature to summarize. The
psychological categories that seem to function following excerpt from Kanner (1943) illus-
fairly well when applied to adults are also trates just how much we stand to gain by paying
applicable to earlier phases of development. close attention to clinical detail. It concerns 6-
What have become paradigmatic cases of think- year-old Frederick attending Kanner’s clinic
ing or willing or perceiving in adults, and seem- for the first time:
ingly separable from feelings, may have
originated in infantile states that implicated He was led into the psychiatrist’s office by a nurse,
who left the room immediately afterward. His fa-
each of these functions as inseparable aspects
cial expression was tense, somewhat apprehensive,
of the infant’s relations with the world. If
and gave the impression of intelligence. He wan-
so, the challenge may become one of distin- dered aimlessly about for a few moments, showing
guishing among different modes of infant relat- no sign of awareness of the three adults present. He
edness. In this case, we need to respect how then sat down on the couch, ejaculating unintell-
intersubjective modes of relatedness that occur igible sounds, and then abruptly lay down, wear-
between infants and other people, sometimes in ing throughout a dreamy-like smile. . . . Objects
408 Development and Behavior

absorbed him easily and he showed good attention nosed with autism were compared with 10
and perseverance in playing with them. He seemed children, matched for age and developmental
to regard people as unwelcome intruders to whom level, who did not have autism. This meant that
he paid as little attention as they would permit. when parents were asked about the children’s
When forced to respond, he did so brief ly and re- behavior in the first two years of life, they
turned to his absorption in things. When a hand
were recalling events from only 6 to 24 months
was held out before him so that he could not possi-
bly ignore it, he played with it brief ly as if it were a
previously, and their memories were not dis-
detached object. (p. 224) torted by knowledge of autism.
The parents’ reports indicated that as in-
Allow this vivid portrayal to linger in your fants, those with autism had a number of
mind, as we turn to systematic controlled stud- abnormalities in the area of person-to-person
ies of socioemotional impairments in children nonverbal communication and interpersonal
and adolescents with autism. contact. Not one of the infants with autism had
shown frequent and intense eye contact, en-
SYSTEMATIC STUDIES OF THE gaged in turn taking with adults, or used noises
EARLY YEARS communicatively, whereas half of the control
children were reported to show each of these
In considering what is basic to autism, special kinds of behavior. There were also fewer infants
importance is attached to determining those with autism who greeted or waved to their par-
features that characterize the earliest phases ents, raised their arms to be picked up, directed
of development. feelings of anger and distress toward people,
were sociable in play, or enjoyed and partici-
Parental Reports pated in lap games. In each of these respects,
there were clear limitations in their affective
Parental reports afford an important perspec- engagement with others. Lord, Storoschuk, Rut-
tive on the early clinical features of autism. For ter, and Pickles (1993) also reported that par-
example, Dahlgren and Gillberg (1989) asked ents of young children with autism gave accounts
mothers of a population sample of matched in- of abnormalities across a range of socioemo-
dividuals with and without autism, then be- tional and communicative behavior, including
tween 7 and 22 years of age, to complete a socially directed babble.
130-item questionnaire on the child’s behavior In addition, the interviews of Wimpory
in the first two years of life. Among the fea- et al. (2000) revealed group differences in the
tures that discriminated the children with infants’ ways of relating to other people with
autism were their isolation, their lack of play, reference to objects and events in the environ-
their failure to attract attention to their own ac- ment. For example, not one of the infants with
tivity, their lack of smiling at times when you autism, but at least half the infants in the con-
might expect it, and their empty gaze (cf. Wing, trol group, was reported to offer or give ob-
1969, whose study included several control jects to others in the first two years of life.
groups; and Stone & Lemanek, 1990; Vostanis The same was true of pointing at objects or
et al., 1998). Even in such a condensed sum- following others’ points. Few children with
mary of results from a less than sensitive mea- autism were said to show objects to others, and
sure, we sense the emotional implications of not one was said to have looked between an ob-
what is being reported. ject of interest and an adult, for example, when
A more in-depth approach adopted by the infant wanted something out of reach. Here
Wimpory and colleagues (Wimpory, Hobson, we seem to be moving beyond what is paradig-
Williams, & Nash, 2000) was to interview par- matically emotional—and yet, do we suppose
ents of very young children who were referred that the behavior just described lacks emo-
to a child development center with difficulties tional underpinnings? The evidence clearly
in relating to and communicating with others. suggests that the children’s lack of interper-
At the time of interview, the undiagnosed chil- sonal engagement extends to circumstances in
dren were between 32 and 48 months old, and which they might share experiences of the
it was only subsequently that 10 children diag- world with other people. They appear to be not
Autism and Emotion 409

only less connected with other people for their to assess how matched autistic and nonautistic
own sake but also less connected with or able developmentally delayed 3- to 6-year-olds and
to share others’ affective attitudes to a shared mental age-matched typically developing chil-
world (Kasari, Sigman, Mundy, & Yirmiya, dren with a mean age of 2 years expressed af-
1990). fect toward the experimenter in the contexts of
joint attention and requesting. Subjects’ facial
Observational and Experimental Studies expressions were coded second by second for
a total of 8 minutes, using a standardized cod-
Some studies have employed diagnostic instru- ing instrument (the Maximally Discriminative
ments alongside direct observations of young Movement Coding System designed by Izard
children with autism. For example, Stone, Hoff- [1979]). Although the autistic children showed
man, Lewis, and Ousley (1994) supplemented uniformly low levels of positive affect toward
parental interviews to gather information on the the adult, they diverged most markedly in their
current behavior of 2- to 4-year-old children decreased level of positive feeling during situa-
with autism, with systematic clinical observa- tions of joint attention. These were the situa-
tions. They reported that there were deficits in tions in which the typically developing children
imitation, nonverbal communication (including smiled most of all, sharing their feelings with
expressions of interest in things through eye the other person.
contact or pointing), responsiveness to others, This evidence of autism-specific abnormal-
and social and imaginative play. Lord (1995) ity in face-to-face affective coordination is
assessed 30 2-year-old children referred for supported by two further studies. In the first,
possible autism using a modified version of Snow, Hertzig, and Shapiro (1987) videotaped
the Autism Diagnostic Interview along with a 10 autistic children between 21⁄ 2 and 4 years
rating scale for direct observations and re- and 10 age- and nonverbal mental age-matched
assessed them one year later to ascertain which developmentally delayed children as they in-
children received a diagnosis of autism at this teracted with the mother, a child psychiatrist,
later stage. On reexamining the data from the and a nursery school teacher who were told
earlier assessment, she concluded that the 2- to behave “just as they normally would” in a
year-olds with autism differed from the other comfortable room stocked with toys. Twenty
children with developmental disorders in spe- 15-second intervals of child interaction with
cific aspects of (1) communicative behavior: each partner were coded using a checklist of
their lack of response to another person’s voice, emotionally expressive actions such as smiles
absence of pointing, and failure to understand and laughter. Whereas almost all the positive
gesture; (2) social reciprocity: lack of seeking affect of the nonautistic children was ex-
to share their enjoyment, failure to greet, un- pressed toward the other person, the autistic
usual use of others’ bodies, lack of initiative in children’s less frequent displays of affect were
directing visual attention, and lack of interest as likely to occur at seemingly random, self-
in children; and (3) restricted, repetitive behav- absorbed moments as in the context of social
ior: hand and finger mannerisms and unusual interaction.
sensory behavior. In the second study, Dawson, Hill, Spencer,
Therefore, there are firm indications that, Galpert, and Watson (1990), videotaped 16
from an early age, socioemotional engagement autistic children ages 2 to 6 years and 16 typi-
is a distinctive domain of abnormality in chil- cally developing children matched for recep-
dren with autism. The following controlled tive language interacting with their mothers in
studies begin to tease out some of the charac- three different contexts: free play, a more
teristics of this disability. structured situation in which the mother asked
the child to help her put away some toys, and a
INTERPERSONAL COORDINATION face-to-face situation over snack time. (This
OF AFFECT kind of comparison with nonretarded children
leaves some uncertainty whether differences in
Kasari et al. (1990) employed videotapes of the autistic children’s behavior might reflect
semistructured child-experimenter interactions mental retardation rather than autism per se.)
410 Development and Behavior

The findings were interesting not only for the seemed that they were less influenced by the
group differences that emerged but also for fearful attitudes of those around them. Again,
the fact that there were no significant differ- we find evidence that autistic children are rela-
ences in the autistic children’s frequency or tively unengaged, not only in one-to-one inter-
duration of gaze at the mother’s face, nor dif- personal-affective transactions but also with
ferences in the frequency or duration of smiles another person’s emotional attitudes toward ob-
in the face-to-face interaction over a snack. jects and events in the world.
However, children with autism were much less These studies have inspired more recent
likely than typically developing children to investigations of 20-month-olds by Charman
combine their smiles with eye contact in a sin- et al. (1997). Children’s videotaped reactions
gle act that seemed to convey an intent to com- to an investigator’s feigned hurt revealed that
municate feelings. In addition, whereas 10 of only 4 of 10 children with autism but every
14 typically developing children with codable one of the nonautistic children looked at the in-
data smiled in response to their mother’s vestigator’s pained face. When a potentially
smile, only 3 of 15 autistic subjects ever did so. anxiety-provoking toy (e.g., a robot) was placed
It was also observed that the mothers of the on the floor a short distance from the child, the
children with autism were less likely to smile children with autism very rarely switched their
in response to their children’s smiles, which gaze between toy and adult to check out the toy
were rarely combined with sustained eye con- (see also Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse, &
tact. You might question how much sharing or Allen, 1998, for related results with somewhat
coordination of affective states was taking older children). In each respect, these very
place between the mothers and children. young children seemed unconnected with the
Further studies from the UCLA group feelings of others.
(Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992) have The preceding research illustrates how
examined other forms of interpersonal coordi- children with autism have characteristic ab-
nation of affect. Participants were 30 young normalities in reciprocal and mutual social en-
autistic children with a mean age of under 4 gagement and shared affective relatedness
years and closely matched nonautistic retarded with the surrounding world. Although there is
and typically developing children. The tech- much to be said for studying such interper-
nique was to code these children’s behavior sonal relations in context, it has also proved
when an adult pretended to hurt herself by hit- instructive to analyze the expression and per-
ting her finger with a hammer, simulated fear ception of emotion in experimental settings
toward a remote-controlled robot, and pretended that allow for more precise definition of spe-
to be ill by lying down on a couch for a minute, cific profiles of impairment.
feigning discomfort. In each of these situations,
children with autism were unusual in rarely EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIVENESS
looking at or relating to the adult. When the
adult pretended to be hurt, for example, chil- Ricks (1975, 1979) tape-recorded six 3- and
dren with autism often appeared unconcerned 4-year-old nonverbal autistic children, six
and continued to play with toys. When a small nonverbal nonautistic retarded children of
remote-controlled robot moved toward the child the same age, and six typically developing in-
and stopped about four feet away, the parent and fants between 8 and 11 months old in four situ-
the experimenter, who were both seated nearby, ations. The first was a “request ” situation,
made fearful facial expressions, gestures, and when the child was hungry and his favorite
vocalizations for 30 seconds. Almost all of the meal was prepared and shown to him; the sec-
nonautistic children looked at an adult at some ond was an occasion of frustration, when the
point during this procedure, but fewer than half meal was withheld for a few moments; the third
of the children with autism did so and then only was one of greeting, when the child saw his
briefly. The children with autism not only were mother on waking in the morning or when she
less hesitant than the mentally retarded children returned to the room after an absence; and in
in playing with the robot but also played with the fourth, involving pleased surprise, the child
it for substantially longer periods of time. It was presented with a novel and interesting
Autism and Emotion 411

stimulus, the blowing up of a balloon, or the displayed a variety of unique and ambiguous
lighting of a sparkler firework. The recordings expressions that were not displayed by any of
of the children’s vocalizations in each of these the other children. Although the authors de-
situations were edited and played back to the scribed these in terms of negative and incon-
mothers of the children. The mother’s task gruous “ blends” of expression, for example, of
was to identify (1) in which context each vo- fear with anger or anger with joy, it is uncertain
calization had been recorded, (2) her own whether what might normally serve as reliable
child, and (3) the nonautistic child. The sec- indices of fear, anger, and so on had the same
ond set of recordings comprised the request meanings here. The evidence suggests that for
vocalizations of all six autistic children, and autistic children, the intrapersonal coordina-
the task was for the mother to identify her tion of expressions might be abnormal, with ob-
own child. vious implications for the patterning of the
When this kind of procedure was conducted children’s personal and interpersonal affective
with recordings of typically developing in- experiences.
fants’ vocalizations, the mothers could easily
identify the “message” of each signal of every LATER CHILDHOOD AND
infant but found difficulty in identifying which ADOLESCENCE
signals came from their own child. When the
tapes of the autistic and nonautistic retarded Lee and Hobson (1998) videotaped children
subjects were presented to the autistic chil- and adolescents greeting an unfamiliar person
dren’s mothers, these mothers, too, could rec- and later taking their leave. Compared with
ognize the contexts from which their own matched, mentally retarded control children,
autistic child’s vocalizations had been derived, only half as many of the children and adoles-
and they could also identify the signals of the cents with autism gave spontaneous expres-
one nonautistic child on tape (often explaining sions of greeting, and many failed to respond
that the child “sounded normal”). What they even after prompting. All the young people
were unable to do was to recognize the contexts without autism made eye contact, but a third
associated with the vocalizations of autistic of those with autism failed to do so; no fewer
children other than their own. Each of these than 17 of the former group smiled, but only
children’s signals seemed to be idiosyncratic. six of those with autism smiled. In the farewell
Correspondingly, and in contrast with the par- episode, half the individuals without autism,
ents of normal children, they could readily and but only three of those with autism, made eye
unerringly identify their own child from the contact and said a goodbye. Not only were
various vocalizations. Ricks concluded that there fewer than half as many autistic as
whereas typically developing infants seem to nonautistic individuals who waved in response
have an unlearned set of emotionally commu- to a final prompt, but their waves were
nicative vocalizations, autistic children either strangely uncoordinated and limp.
do not develop these signals or, having reached Another study of emotionally expressive ges-
the age of 3 to 5, they no longer use them. How- tures takes a rather different form. Attwood,
ever, their idiosyncratic signals do have emo- Frith, and Hermelin (1988) observed adoles-
tional meanings. cents with both autism and Down syndrome in-
Yirmiya, Kasari, Sigman, and Mundy (1989) teracting with their peers for a total of twenty
studied videotapes of semistructured child- 30-second periods in the playground and at the
experimenter interactions involving activated dinner table. All 15 Down subjects interacted
toys, a song-and-tickle social game, a turn- socially during the period of observation, but
taking activity, and a balloon-blowing episode. only 11 of the 18 autistic children did so. Al-
The children’s facial expressions were coded though the mean number of gestures per inter-
second by second, using the anatomically based action did not distinguish the groups, there were
scheme of Izard (1979). The principal findings differences in the kinds of gestures employed.
were that children with autism were more flat Both groups used simple pointing gestures and
or neutral in affective expressions than were gestures to prompt behavior, such as those to in-
control children, but more important, they dicate “Come here” or “Be quiet ”; but whereas
412 Development and Behavior

10 of 15 individuals with Down syndrome used 1988b, for cross-modal matching of facial and
at least one expressive gesture such as giving a vocal expressions vis-à-vis cross-modal match-
hug of consolation, making a thumbs-up sign, or ing of appearances and sounds of things and ac-
covering the face in embarrassment, not one tions; Hobson, Ouston, & Lee, 1989 for naming
such gesture was seen in the autistic group. emotions in faces and voices vis-à-vis naming
Such observations of spontaneous social en- photographs and sounds of nonemotional stim-
gagement are complemented by the work of uli; and Hobson & Lee, 1989, for naming
investigators who have asked autistic individu- emotional vis-à-vis nonemotional pictures that
als to pose emotionally expressive faces and appear in a standard IQ test). Novel research
voices. Thus, in an experiment conducted by approaches are delineating abnormalities in
Langdell (1981), judges rated children’s at- specific aspects of social and emotion percep-
tempts to make happy and sad faces as more tion in autism, such as in the visual scanning of
inappropriate than those of nonautistic retarded faces (Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen,
children. In a more elaborate study by Macdon- 2002; Pelphrey et al., 2002) and in compen-
ald et al. (1989), raters judged that high- satory strategies used to process emotional
functioning autistic subjects’ posed facial and information (e.g., Grossman, Klin, Carter, &
vocal expressions were more “odd” than those Volkmar, 2000).
of control subjects, and the (photographed) Yet, controversy in this domain is far
faces were also less easily classified with from over (e.g., Buitelaar, Van der Wees,
respect to the emotions expressed. When Love- Swaab-Barneveld, & Van der Gaag, 1999; Love-
land et al. (1994) tested children’s ability to land et al., 1997). Two of the most serious
imitate as well as produce expressions of facial sources of scientific contention about this body
affect on instruction, the children with autism of research are that (1) often the group differ-
not only found difficulty but also produced ences in the profiles of performance on index
more bizarre and mechanical expressions. In and control tasks have been quantitatively
deliberate as well as spontaneous expressions of small, and (2) when emotion recognition abili-
emotion, therefore, the evidence points to qual- ties are adjudged solely with reference to sub-
itative as well as quantitative abnormalities in jects’ levels of language ability, rather than with
individuals with autism. reference to performance on control tasks of ap-
I have already remarked how in the realm of propriate difficulty, the differences sometimes
interpersonal relations, perception of people disappear. Despite claims to the contrary, how-
and their expressiveness is only partly dissocia- ever, even when individuals are matched accord-
ble from the emotional experience that such per- ing to verbal ability, group differences are often
ception engenders in the heart (and the “I”) of substantial (see later examples). There remain
the beholder. Correspondingly, investigations of outstanding issues to do with the specificity of
emotion perception are not divorced from the emotion recognition in facial, vocal, and other
other studies we have been considering. expressions (e.g., Boucher, Lewis, & Collis,
There is now a substantial body of experi- 1998; Klin, Sparrow, et al., 1999). Here, even if
mental research to suggest that not only are certain of the perceptual processing deficits ex-
children with autism abnormal in the ways they tend beyond the emotional domain, their most
express emotion, but also there are autism- significant developmental implications might
specific deficits in emotion perception and un- arise through an impact on specifically socioe-
derstanding. Some of the most persuasive evi- motional perception and the interpersonal rela-
dence for autism-specific deficits has come tions for which such perception is needed.
from studies that have compared sizeable Let us reexamine a little of the evidence. It
groups of closely matched autistic and non- is important to be clear on what these studies
autistic retarded subjects on tests that com- are aiming to achieve and the methods that are
pare performance in judging emotion-related needed to address those aims (Hobson, 1991).
and emotion-unrelated materials, such as It is not expected that experiments will provide
photographs, drawings, or videotape and audio- a quantitative estimate of the size of real-life
tape recordings of people vis-à-vis nonpersonal group differences in emotion perception for a
objects (e.g., see Hobson, Ouston, & Lee, number of reasons that include the effects of
Autism and Emotion 413

matching procedures (to match by verbal abil- 1999, for a related approach) to test whether in-
ity is to match by a partial index of social co- dividuals with autism who were matched for
ordination, which may be emotion-dependent, verbal ability with nonautistic people might
thereby controlling out some of the group dif- be impaired in matching people’s faces accord-
ference you are trying to examine), the artifi- ing to (1) happy, unhappy, angry, and afraid
ciality of the test materials and test situation emotions and (2) identities. The subjects’ task
(which may be more manageable for children was to match emotions expressed by different
with autism than real-life emotional ex- individuals and to match people’s identities
changes), and the possibility that successful although they were expressing different emo-
judgments may be made with superficial un- tions. To explore the possibility that autistic
derstanding of emotion itself. For example, children might perform well by applying some
suppose some children with autism were to form of nonemotional perceptual analysis, we
apply abnormal perceptual strategies and iden- repeated the two forms of task with modifica-
tify a smile as an upturned mouth (which is not tions: First, the faces on the cards to be sorted
the usual way to apprehend a smile). To be had blanked-out mouths, and second, they had
confident that you are testing the ability to blanked-out mouths and foreheads. Our inten-
register the subjective emotional meanings of tion was to retain the “ feel” of the emotions
expressions, you need to devise task materials even in these latter materials (to establish an
that are difficult to interpret unless you per- advantage for emotion-sensitive subjects) while
ceive emotional meaning. In this respect, con- at the same time to reduce the availability of
sider the study of Ozonoff, Pennington, and non-emotion-related cues (to thwart alternative
Rogers (1991), who tested a group of high- strategies of sorting). In a final condition, the
functioning autistic individuals and a hetero- standard photographs and the full faces for
geneous control sample, matched for age and sorting were each presented upside down to tilt
verbal IQ, on a battery of tests including one the balance in the opposite direction in favor of
of “emotion perception.” A photograph of a participants with abnormal strategies of face
face displaying an emotional expression served perception.
as the target, and the subject was asked to The first result was that whereas on the
choose one of four photographs that “ felt the identities task, the performance of the two
same way.” Correct choices varied from the groups showed a similar steady decline as the
target in the identity of the model and the in- photographs became increasingly blanked out,
tensity of the expressed affect. There were 34 on the emotions task, the performance of autis-
items, half of which contained distractor pho- tic subjects worsened more abruptly than that
tographs that shared similar perceptual fea- of control subjects as cues to emotion were pro-
tures with correct choices. For example, a face gressively reduced. It seemed that the children
expressing fear was used as a distractor for the with autism were relatively unable to use the
target emotion of surprise, since both emo- “ feel” in the faces to guide performance. In ad-
tions share the feature of an open mouth. Nine dition, correlations between individual sub-
emotions were depicted: four “simple emo- jects’ scores on the identity and emotion tasks
tions” ( happiness, sadness, anger, and fear) were higher for autistic than for nonautistic
and five “complex emotions” (surprise, shame, subjects, again suggesting that the participants
disgust, interest, and contempt). The autistic with autism might have been sorting the ex-
group performed significantly less well than pressive faces by nonemotional perceptual
the control group in matching both simple and strategies. Second, whereas the performance of
complex emotions and made a higher number control participants slumped in sorting upside-
of errors on items with an obvious perceptual down faces, the children with autism became
foil. The authors considered that perhaps they significantly superior to the control group on
were using a different, more perceptually dri- matching both “identities” and “emotions” (see
ven matching strategy. also Langdell, 1978).
Such considerations determined the design A second approach to defeating “abnor-
adopted by Hobson, Ouston, and Lee (1988a; mal” perceptual strategies concerns judg-
see also Celani, Battacchi, & Arcidiacono, ments of gestural expressions of emotion
414 Development and Behavior

(Moore, Hobson, & Lee, 1997; see also Jen- asked to do so. We added five new emotionally
nings, 1973; Weeks & Hobson, 1987, for expressive sequences to the five already de-
equally strong evidence for group differences scribed: These showed the point-light person
between language-matched groups in judging in states of itchiness, boredom, tiredness, cold,
emotion in facial expressions). Here, children and hurt. When these sequences were shown
and adolescents with and without autism— one by one, we said: “I want you to tell me
again, matched for verbal ability as well as age what the person is feeling.” Alongside this test
and comparable in the productivity of what involving emotions and other attitudes, there
they said during the task—were shown video- was a test for the recognition of nonemotional
tape sequences of people’s moving bodies de- actions: lifting, chopping, hopping, kicking,
picted merely by dots of light attached to the jumping, pushing, digging, sitting, climbing,
trunk and limbs. As a stringent control task, and running. Here the instructions were: “I
we tested the children’s abilities to judge ac- want you to tell me what the person is doing.”
tions such as digging or pushing. First, we pre- The tasks were adjusted to exclude items on
sented separate 5-second sequences of the which there were ceiling or floor effects and
point-light person enacting in turn the ges- to equate tasks for level of difficulty. The par-
tures of surprise, sadness, fear, anger, and hap- ticipants with autism were not significantly
piness. In the surprise sequence, for example, different in their scores on the actions task
the person walked forward and suddenly (mean score 5 of 8 correct, compared with 6
checked his stride and jerked backward with of 8 for the control group); but on the emotions
his arms thrown out to the side and gave a sigh task, where once again the control group
of relief; in the sad sequence, the person achieved a mean score of 6 of 8 correct, the
walked forward with a stooped posture, children with autism had a mean score of only
paused, and sighed, then raised his arms out 2 of 8 correct. Here is striking evidence of a
slowly and allowed them to drop to his sides, specific limitation in recognizing emotions in
and finally seated himself in a slumped man- children carefully matched for verbal ability
ner and put his head in his hands. In each case, and linguistic productivity.
adults who saw the videotapes were 100% ac-
curate in judging the expressions. THE CASE OF
The children were told: “You’re going to SELF-CONSCIOUS EMOTIONS
see some bits of film of a person moving. I
want you to tell me about this person. Tell me Thus far, we have concentrated on so-called
what’s happening.” In response, all but one of “simple” emotions that are often presumed to
the nonautistic children made a spontaneous be less cognitively elaborated than feelings
comment about the person’s emotional state such as coyness, guilt, pride, or shame. One as-
for at least one presentation, and most referred pect of the latter emotions is that they appear
to emotions on two or more of the five se- to implicate self-consciousness, although it re-
quences. In contrast, 10 of the 13 children mains open to question how far such self-
with autism never referred to emotional states, consciousness also assumes “primitive” forms
whether correctly or incorrectly. In the case of in humans. Bosch (1970) remarked how the
the children and adolescents with autism, it child with autism often seems to lack a sense
was the person’s movements and actions rather of self-consciousness and shame and to be
than feelings that were reported. For example, missing something of the “ ‘self-involvement,’
the sad figure was described as “ walking and the acting with, and the identification with the
sitting down on a chair,” “ walking and flap- acting person” (p. 81). In my view, this pro-
ping arms and bent down,” and “ walking and cess of identifying with others is critical in es-
waving his arms and kneeling down . . . hands tablishing the kinds of inward-facing attitudes
to face.” Almost none of the responses were that come to be experienced as guilt and other
wrong, but very few referred to feelings. self-conscious emotions; also in my view, this
A final task was designed to explore how is an area of basic impairment in autism. The
accurately the children and adolescents could crux is not that you need a certain cognitive
name actions and emotions when explicitly architecture to feel guilt—rather, you need to
Autism and Emotion 415

have internalized attitudes to yourself through Kasari, Chamberlain, & Bauminger, 2001), the
identifying with the attitudes of others. Then children could cite situations eliciting pride,
there is the additional matter of how individu- but provided instances that were less personal
als with autism reflect (self-consciously) on and in some ways more stereotyped (e.g., fin-
all of their feelings and other aspects of them- ishing their homework or winning games) than
selves, because self-reflection brings in emo- was the case with control children.
tional considerations concerning the route by Results were similar concerning guilt.
which and the attitudes with which children Kasari et al. (2001) describe how high-IQ chil-
come to reflect in this way. We may learn a lot dren with autism can report feeling guilt, but
from listening to what people with autism say compared with control children they provide
about themselves and their emotional life and fewer self-evaluative statements and are more
noting how they say it. likely to describe situations in terms of rule
What, then, have controlled studies re- breaking, disruptiveness, and damage to prop-
vealed about self-conscious feelings in indi- erty, rather than those of causing physical
viduals with autism? Young children with or emotional harm to others. The researchers
autism who are not severely cognitively im- conclude that for children with autism, guilt
paired do remove rouge from their faces when appears to be defined in terms of memorizable
they perceive themselves in a mirror (Dawson rules and actions, such as taking toys from
& McKissick, 1984; Neuman & Hill, 1978; school, stealing cookies, running away, and
Spiker & Ricks, 1984). What most do not show so on, rather than in interpersonal, empathic
are the signs of coyness typical of young, typi- terms.
cally developing and nonautistic, retarded chil- Similar results emerge when the focus turns
dren. Thus, autistic children can make use of to embarrassment. According to Capps et al.
their own reflection to register what it means (1992) and Kasari et al. (2001), children with
to have their body marked, and they are likely autism are likely to give examples of embar-
to act accordingly in trying to remove the mark rassing situations that are external and uncon-
from their face. What is far less certain is trollable, whereas matched typically developing
whether such behavior is motivated by a con- children often give more specific and personal
cern with the way they “look ” to other people examples that relate to controllable events.
and with the evaluative attitudes that others Especially frequent are reports of feeling em-
may entertain in seeing them marked in an un- barrassment because of teasing by others, a rel-
usual manner (Hobson, 1990). atively rare response from nonautistic children;
This suggestion receives some support from but references to the presence of an audience
a study conducted by Kasari, Sigman, Baum- are relatively infrequent.
gartner, and Stipek (1993) with young autistic These results are also supported and ex-
and nonautistic retarded subjects (mean age 42 tended by recent studies by Chidambi (2003;
months) and mental age-matched typically de- Chidambi, Hobson, & Lee, 2003). He inter-
veloping children (mean age 23 months). Each viewed parents of children with autism ages 7
subject completed a puzzle, and the investiga- to 12 years, as well as parents of age- and
tor and parent reacted neutrally; then the child ability-matched nonautistic children, to dis-
completed a second puzzle, and after three sec- cover whether and in what ways the children
onds, both adults gave praise. Although chil- manifested signs of a range of self-conscious
dren with autism were like mentally retarded emotions such as pride, guilt, shame, embar-
and typically developing children in being in- rassment, jealousy, envy, coyness, flirting, and
clined to smile when they succeeded with the pity, as well as “simple” emotions. The results
puzzles, autistic children were less likely were complex in that certain group differences
to draw attention to what they had done or to emerged only when attention was given to the
look up to an adult and less likely to show plea- subtlety of the responses, but there were clear
sure in being praised. Their pride assumed a and significant group differences for feelings
strangely asocial form. In assessments of pride such as embarrassment, flirting, guilt, and
in high-functioning children and adolescents shame. For example, flirtatious behavior was re-
with autism (Capps, Yirmiya, & Sigman, 1992; ported to be absent in all but one of 10 children
416 Development and Behavior

with autism, but present in 9 children with the nature of jealousy are reminiscent of three
learning disabilities; guilt was reported in only other reports. In a study by Yirmiya, Sigman,
2 of 10 children with autism (and here it Kasari, and Mundy (1992), high-functioning
seemed circumscribed), but 9 of 10 control young adolescents with autism scored lower
children. Although pride was frequently re- than control participants in reporting empathic
ported in children with autism, it was generally feelings in response to videotapes of emotional
restricted to scholastic achievements and scenarios, an ability correlated with full-scale
seemed less personal in quality than in the IQ only for the group with autism. This sug-
nonautistic children. gested to the authors that the children might
There is accumulating evidence that chil- have been employing cognitive strategies in in-
dren with autism neither manifest nor refer to terpreting social situations. Lee and Hobson
these “complex” emotions with the quality of (1998) conducted “self-understanding inter-
interpersonal engagement that characterizes views” and reported that children with autism
children who do not have autism. What we not only were restricted in the feelings they ex-
need is a theoretical perspective that intro- pressed about themselves but also failed to men-
duces order and understanding into these ob- tion friends or being members of a social group.
servations. We have already seen one example Bauminger and Kasari (2000) described how
where evidence from autism promises to clar- children with autism spoke of loneliness but
ify and extend our thinking: Manifestations failed to refer to the more affective dimension
of pride appear to be of two distinct kinds, of being left out of close intimate relationships.
only one of which—the most interpersonal— As Bauminger (2004) suggests, children with
is remarkable for its relative absence in autism appear to have difficulty in considering
autism. But there are other issues concerning interpersonal relationships when reflecting on
which we have much to discover. What of jeal- their emotional experiences.
ousy, for example? In Chidambi’s interview The upshot of all these studies is that we
study previously mentioned, approximately cannot presume which aspects of emotion, even
half the parents of children with autism re- “complex” emotion, are or are not absent in
ported signs of jealousy, but there appeared to autism; and the patterning of the children’s in-
be limited contexts within which jealousy was terpersonal behavior and reports of their expe-
shown and very restricted individuals (usu- riences are very likely to elucidate in which
ally only the mother) toward whom jealousy respects there are dissociable aspects to emo-
was expressed. Bauminger (2004) reported tional development in typical as well as atypi-
that in two jealousy-eliciting conditions—one cal cases.
in which the child’s parent praised another There is another setting that provides evi-
child’s picture while ignoring his or her own dence of previously unforeseen (and relative)
child’s and another in which the parent en- “normality” in the interpersonal relations of
gaged in affectionate play exclusively with the children with autism: the Strange Situation
other child—the majority of children with (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) in
autism displayed clear indications of jealousy. which reactions to separations from and re-
There was not a group difference from control unions with a parent can be studied. There are
participants in this respect. The children with several published studies that indicate how
autism tended to express themselves by acting young autistic children do respond to separa-
toward the parent, rather than looking at him tion from and reunion with their caregivers, at
or her. In separate tests, the children with least in the short term (Rogers, Ozonoff, &
autism were less proficient in recognizing jeal- Maslin-Cole, 1991; Shapiro, Sherman, Cala-
ousy in a picture, and only half could produce mari, & Koch, 1987; Sigman & Mundy, 1989;
personal and affective (as opposed to social- Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). Many (not all) 2- to
cognitive) examples of jealousy, whereas all 5-year-old autistic children are like matched
the control children could do so. nonautistic retarded children in showing some-
These latter findings of a difficulty encoun- what variable reactions to the departure of
tered by children with autism in describing their the caregiver, sometimes showing behavioral
own jealousy and (apparently) understanding and/or mood, changes such as fretting, and in
Autism and Emotion 417

responding to reunion by spending more time orientation of others. By means of this process
alongside the caregiver than the stranger. of identification in the context of triadic per-
When allowance is made for their sometimes- son-person-world relations, they are lifted out
idiosyncratic behavior, a substantial number of of their one-track, inflexible perspective to
autistic children are rated as “securely at- apprehend things and events “according to the
tached” (Rogers et al., 1991; Shapiro et al., other.” This process is not only critically dis-
1987). The children’s relationship with their rupted in children with autism, but also criti-
caregivers is clearly different from that with a cally important for the development of
stranger. It is still unclear how the quality and context-sensitive symbolic thinking (Hobson,
longer term implications of such attachments 1993a, 1993b, 2002).
conform with those of nonautistic children This theoretical perspective locates the
(see also Rogers et al., 1991), and as we have “ final common pathway” to autism in what
seen, there are important respects in which fails to happen between people and in their
children with autism do not relate to their par- mutual relations with the surroundings. Inso-
ents normally. far as this perspective is valid, you might be
Just as in the case of jealousy, there may turn led to investigate whether there are a variety
out to be biologically based determinants of in- of routes by which the critical experiences of
terpersonal relationships (and remember that identifying with (and being moved by) some-
goslings become attached, even though they do one else’s attitudes to a shared world could be
not have a highly sophisticated emotional life, impaired. In this context, studies of atypical
as far as we know) that are dissociable from as- autism may be especially revealing. Here I
pects of interpersonal relatedness; and we may consider two such cases: congenital blindness
yet have to revise some of our conventional ideas and Romanian orphans.
about which aspects of emotional relatedness do When colleagues and I became intrigued by
or do not require sophisticated cognitive under- the social impairments of congenitally blind
pinnings. Finally, but importantly, we must not children, there were already clinical reports
neglect the fact that in many of the studies suggesting that both autistic features and the
cited, there are a small but notable number of syndrome of autism might be more prevalent in
children with autism who do seem to register children who suffered congenital blindness
others’ feelings, both toward themselves and to- (e.g., Fraiberg, 1977; Keeler, 1958; Rogers &
ward a shared world, and who manifest a range Newhart-Larson, 1989). It seemed to us that
of emotions, sometimes in interpersonal con- congenital blindness might deprive an infant of
texts—and this, too, needs investigation. something essential in the domain of emo-
tional relatedness: the ability to perceive, be
CONGENITALLY BLIND CHILDREN moved by, and identify with the attitudes of
AND ROMANIAN ORPHANS someone else as these are directed toward ob-
jects and events in a visually shared world.
I have been presenting the case, as much by il- Whereas sighted children with autism are not
lustration as by logical argument, that there is moved by others’ feelings because they do not
developmental continuity between aspects of experience typical forms of “ feeling percep-
typically developing infants’ emotional life tion” toward the expressions of others, so con-
and qualities of infants’ subsequent social and genitally blind children are not only restricted
cognitive relations that appear less obviously in the expressions they perceive but also de-
“emotional” in nature. I have also implied that prived of the insight that different attitudes may
some of these lines of development may be dis- be directed to the same visually specified object
cerned through the specific profile of social or event—and, therefore, that objects and events
and cognitive (and perceptual and motiva- may have multiple (context-sensitive, person-
tional) impairments and limitations in individ- dependent, even symbolic) meanings. In other
uals with autism. Especially important here is words, congenital blindness might predispose
the idea that through emotional engagement ( but not predetermine) a child to develop
with other people, typically developing infants autism, for the reason that blindness unhinges
are “moved” to assume the psychological the very fulcrum of early development that is
418 Development and Behavior

said to be lacking in sighted children with jects, communication of all kinds, motor coor-
autism. dination, and interactive play. Only the blind
To establish whether the evidence was in children had a tendency to echo back what
keeping with this hypothesis, Brown, Hobson, other people said—arguably, a revealing index
Lee, and Stevenson (1997) tested children who of their difficulty in identifying with a person-
had been totally or near-totally blind from anchored linguistic perspective that shifts from
birth, between 3 and 9 years of age, and with person to person. In a separate study of such
no identifiable disorder of the nervous system, children, Minter, Hobson, and Bishop (1998)
available in six schools for visually impaired reported evidence of the children’s difficulties
children. The children’s behavior was rated on specially modified “ theory of mind” tests of
over at least three periods of 20 minutes at false belief and representational change.
free play, in the classroom during a lesson, and The message from these studies of blind
in a session of language testing, and the results children is that there may be more than one
were complemented by teacher reports. It way to develop autism because there may be
turned out that no fewer than 10 of the 24 chil- more than one kind of barrier to experiencing
dren satisfied the clinical criteria for autism— personal relations toward other people—peo-
a proportion that is about 1,000 times as great ple with whose attitudes to a shared world an
as you would expect in sighted children. In individual can identify.
a subsequent study (Hobson, Lee, & Brown, Evidence that bears on this thesis comes
1999), nine of the blind children who met the from another, initially unexpected, quarter:
criteria for autism were compared with nine studies by Michael Rutter and colleagues
sighted children with autism matched for age (1999) of 4- to 6-year-olds who had been placed
and IQ. The two groups were similar in many in Romanian orphanages early in the first year
respects, but there were indications that the of life and moved to the United Kingdom in
blind children were not so impaired in their their first or second year. In summary, about
emotional expressions and that their relations one in 16 of the children showed a picture that
with people were better. The majority gave the closely resembled that of autism, and a further
impression of being less severely autistic. In one in 16 presented with milder autistic fea-
the clinical judgment of the child psychiatrist tures. Severely affected children displayed
investigator, only two of the nine blind chil- problems with social relationships and impov-
dren displayed the quality of social impair- erished reciprocal communication with others,
ment that was characteristic of the sighted but lack of empathy toward others, poverty of eye-
autistic children, a quality that involves the to-eye gaze and gestures in social exchanges,
special feel you have of a lack of emotional and limited language and to-and-fro conversa-
contact. When children are blind from birth, tion. A majority of the children had preoccupa-
they are predisposed to autism even if their so- tions with sensations and intense interests of
cial impairment is somewhat less profound unusual kinds. Yet, there was something atypi-
than in sighted children with autism. Their cal about the autism, for example, when the
lack of vision plays a role in causing the pic- children made spontaneous efforts to communi-
ture of autism, even when their intrinsic social cate with sign language or other kinds of social
disability is not so severe. approach.
We also predicted that even in those whose It is too early to be confident what these
social relations were less affected by blindness, studies mean. A picture emerges to suggest
there would be some indication of autistic-like that particular forms of very severe depriva-
problems. To test this prediction, we compared tion of emotionally patterned interpersonal
the children who were not autistic with sighted experience—whether this occurs through con-
children from mainstream schools who were stitutional abnormalities in the child that re-
similar in age and IQ. Not one of the main- strict the effective social environment and
stream children showed any autistic-like behav- weaken or deflect emotional pushes and pulls
ior, whereas every one of the blind children did that occur between people, or through specific
so. The two groups differed in several respects, forms of perceptual handicap, or even through
including relating to people, responses to ob- appalling privation of social input—may have
Autism and Emotion 419

autism-related social and cognitive implica- of typical early development because they
tions. We may be drawing closer to an account alert us to the profound developmental signif-
that accords due weight to social-emotional icance of the coordination of subjective states
factors in the pathogenesis of autism. between individuals; and we should not over-
estimate the decisive contribution of any
THE ESSENCE OF AUTISM particular methodological approach to deter-
mining the specific bases of autism, when the
There are additional perspectives that are cer- pathogenesis of autism appears to be complex
tain to shed further light on the nature and role and, to some extent, different in different
of what we might broadly call emotional children.
deficits in children with autism. One such per-
spective comes from effectiveness of interven- CONCLUSION
tions that facilitate emotional engagement
between autistic children and others (e.g., Any explanation of autism will need to trace
Bauminger, 2002; Rogers, 2000). Another in- the origins and repercussions of what Kanner
volves the study of related impairments in rel- (1943) called the children’s abnormality in af-
atives of affected individuals (as reviewed in fective contact with others. We should pay
Bailey, Palferman, Heavey, & Le Couteur, special attention to the quality of emotional
1998). Here we may discern attributes that, impairment in the interpersonal domain, for it
while they do not constitute grounds for the would seem to be in the interpersonal do-
development of autism itself, nevertheless be- main—and specifically, in respect to sharing
tray something of whatever genetic predisposi- subjective states and coordinating attitudes
tion to social-affective impairment exists in a with other people vis-à-vis the world, as in
substantial proportion of individuals with episodes of joint attention—that a critical ab-
autism. A third perspective comes from the normality is to be found. To be sure, nearly all
burgeoning research on the neurofunctional children with autism have constitutional ab-
correlates of emotional experience and even normalities, but these may be of diverse kinds
empathy, both in typically developing individ- and arise on the bases of diverse etiologies.
uals (e.g., Decety & Chaminade, 2003) and in Not all of these may be characterized as pri-
those with autism. Although it may be too marily emotional in nature, as the example of
soon to derive firm conclusions from func- congenital blindness testifies. Yet, perhaps
tional brain scans about the status and speci- each cause operates through a final common
ficity of reported abnormalities in neuronal pathway that implicates abnormality in the
activity during emotion-related tasks such as patterned coordination of affectively config-
those involving the perception of emotion in ured subjective states between the affected
faces (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Critch- child and others. Affected children appear to
ley et al., 2000; Schultz et al., 2000), it already have a restricted propensity to identify with
seems likely that such investigations will other people, to move toward, and (in part) as-
demonstrate how neurological dysfunction un- similate the other person’s attitude and psy-
derpins and reflects the behavioral and experi- chological orientation to the world.
ential abnormalities of people with autism It seems doubtful whether this essentially
described in this chapter—and in so doing, interpersonal abnormality will prove to be the
will highlight afresh the need to derive an ade- result of some single perceptual or cognitive
quate developmental story of mind-brain de- deficit, although as in the case of congenital
velopment in autism. blindness, lower level domain-general deficits
It remains to step back and reflect on play a pathogenic role in at least some cases.
autism and emotion—and to consider how re- From a complementary perspective, it seems
shaping our concepts of emotion may help us doubtful that we will arrive at an adequate
to understand autism and how investigations theory of the syndrome of autism unless we
of autism may help us rethink emotion. We accord this special form of interpersonal
should not underestimate the value of clinical disorder—one that prevents or derails the
observations of autism for our understanding coordination of subjective states involving
420 Development and Behavior

feelings—a pivotal place within our multi- Buitelaar, J. K., Van der Wees, M., Swaab-
level explanatory scheme. Barneveld, H., & Van Der Gaag, R. (1999).
Theory of mind and emotion-recognition func-
tioning in autistic spectrum disorders and psy-
Cross-References
chiatric control and normal children.
Development and Psychopathology, 11, 39–58.
Developmental stages of autism are discussed in
Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1992).
Chapters 8, 9, and 10. Social and communication Understanding of simple and complex emo-
development are discussed in Chapters 11 and tions in non-retarded children with autism.
12. Joint attention is discussed in Chapter 25. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
33, 1169–1182.
REFERENCES Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L.
(1999). The understanding of the emotional
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & meaning of facial expressions in people with
Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hills- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Disorders, 29, 57–66.
Attwood, A., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J.,
understanding and use of interpersonal ges- Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants
tures by autistic and Down’s syndrome chil- with autism: An investigation of empathy, pre-
dren. Journal of Autism and Developmental tend play, joint attention, and imitation. Devel-
Disorders, 18, 241–257. opmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.
Bacon, A. L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., Chidambi, G. (2003). Autism and self-conscious
& Allen, D. (1998). The responses of autistic emotions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
children to the distress of others. Journal of University College, London.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, Chidambi, G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (2003, No-
129–142. vember). A multifaceted investigation of pride,
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Cou- guilt, and shame in children with autism. Poster
teur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in rel- presentation to the 7th International Autism
atives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Europe Congress.
Disorders, 28, 369–392. Critchley, H., Daly, E., Bullmore, E., Williams, S.,
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bul- Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., et al.
more, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., et al. (2000). The functional anatomy of social be-
(1999). Social intelligence in the normal and havior: Changes in cerebral blood f low when
autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Jour- people with autistic disorder process facial ex-
nal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898. pression. Brain, 123, 2203–2212.
Bauminger, N. (2002). The facilitation of social- Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms
emotional understanding and social interaction in the first two years of life: A preliminary
in high-functioning children with autism: Inter- population study of infantile autism. European
vention outcomes. Journal of Autism and Devel- Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sci-
opmental Disorders, 32, 283–298. ences, 238, 169–174.
Bauminger, N. (2004). The expression and un- Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., &
derstanding of jealousy in children with Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges be-
autism. Development and Psychopathology, tween young autistic children and their moth-
16, 157–177. ers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18,
Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness 335–345.
and friendship in high-functioning children Dawson, G., & McKissick, F. C. (1984). Self-
with autism. Child Development, 71, 447–456. recognition in autistic children. Journal of
Bosch, G. (1970). Infantile autism (D. Jordan & I. Autism and Development Disorders, 14,
Jordan, Trans.). New York: Springer-Verlag. 383–394.
Boucher, J., Lewis, V., & Collis, G. (1998). Famil- Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural corre-
iar face and voice matching and recognition in lates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia,
children with autism. Journal of Child Psy- 41, 127–138.
chology and Psychiatry, 39, 171–181. Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the blind. Lon-
Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. don: Souvenir Press.
(1997). Are there “autistic-like” features in Grossman, J. B., Klin, A., Carter, A. S., & Volk-
congenitally blind children? Journal of Child mar, F. R. (2000). Verbal bias in recognition of
Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 693–703. facial emotions in children with Asperger syn-
Autism and Emotion 421

drome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Kasari, C., Sigman, M. D., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N.
chiatry, 41, 369–379. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On the origins of self and the attention interactions of normal, autistic and
case of autism. Development and Psychopath- mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism
ology, 2, 163–181. and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87–100.
Hobson, R. P. (1991). Methodological issues for Keeler, W. R. (1958). Autistic patterns and defec-
experiments on autistic individuals’ percep- tive communication in blind children with
tion and understanding of emotion. Journal retrolental fibroplasias. In P. H. Hoch &
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, J. Zubin (Eds.), Psychopathology of commu-
1135–1158. nication (pp. 64–83). New York: Grune &
Hobson, R. P. (1993a). Autism and the development Stratton.
of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., &
Hobson, R. P. (1993b). The emotional origins of so- Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns of
cial understanding. Philosophical Psychology, naturalistic social situations as predictors of
6, 227–249. social competence in individuals with autism.
Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. Lon- Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.
don: Palgrave Macmillan. Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti,
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A
and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evi- normed study of face recognition in autism
dence from the British Picture Vocabulary and related disorders. Journal of Autism and
Scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.
Disorders, 19, 601–623. Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An ap-
Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism proach to the study of autism. Journal of Child
and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268.
and Developmental Disorders, 29, 45–56. Langdell, T. (1981). Face perception: An approach
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988a). to the study of autism. Unpublished doctoral
Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating dissertation, University College, London.
faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18, Lee, A., & Hobson, R. P. (1998). On developing
911–923. self-concepts: A controlled study of children
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988b). and adolescents with autism. Journal of Child
What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1131–1141.
Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453. Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds referred
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1989). Nam- for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology
ing emotion in faces and voices: Abilities and and Psychiatry, 36, 1365–1382.
disabilities in autism and mental retardation. Lord, C., Storoschuk, S., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, (1993). Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism in
7, 237–250. preschool children. Infant Mental Health Jour-
Izard, C. E. (1979). The maximally discriminative nal, 14, 234–252.
facial movement coding system (MAX). Newark: Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Orte-
University of Delaware, Instructional Re- gon, J., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K. A., et al.
sources Center. (1997). Emotion recognition in autism: Verbal
Jennings, W. B. (1973). A study of the preference and nonverbal information. Development and
for af fective cues in autistic children. Unpub- Psychopathology, 9, 579–593.
lished doctoral dissertation, Memphis State Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D. A.,
University. Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- (1994). Imitation and expression of facial af-
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. fect in autism. Development and Psychopathol-
Kasari, C., Chamberlain, B., & Bauminger, N. ogy, 6, 433–444.
(2001). Social emotions and social relation- Macdonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., Le
ships: Can children with autism compensate. Couteur, A., Evered, C., et al. (1989). Recog-
In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & nition and expression of emotional cues by
P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child
(pp. 309–323). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 865–877.
Kasari, C., Sigman, M. D., Baumgartner, P., & Minter, M., Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (1998).
Stipek, D. J. (1993). Pride and mastery in chil- Congenital visual impairment and “ theory of
dren with autism. Journal of Child Psychology mind.” British Journal of Developmental Psy-
and Psychiatry, 34, 352–362. chology, 16, 183–196.
422 Development and Behavior

Moore, D., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Com- Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N.
ponents of person perception: An investigation (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of
with autistic, nonautistic retarded and normal others by autistic, mentally retarded, and nor-
children and adolescents. British Journal of mal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.
Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423. Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attachments
Neuman, C. J., & Hill, S. D. (1978). Self- in autistic children. Journal of the American
recognition and stimulus preference in autistic Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
children. Developmental Psychobiology, 11, 28, 74–81.
571–578. Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Attachment
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. behaviors in autistic children. Journal of Autism
(1991). Executive function deficits in high- and Developmental Disorders, 14, 231–243.
functioning autistic children: Relationship to Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., & Shapiro, T. (1987).
theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology Expression of emotion in autistic children.
and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836–838.
G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual Spiker, D., & Ricks, M. (1984). Visual self-
scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism recognition in autistic children: Developmental
and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261. relationships. Child Development, 55, 214–225.
Ricks, D. M. (1975). Vocal communication in pre- Stone, W. L., Hoffman, E. L., Lewis, S. E., &
verbal normal and autistic children. In N. O’- Ousley, O. Y. (1994). Early recognition of
Connor (Ed.), Language, cognitive deficits, and autism: Parental reports vs clinical observa-
retardation (pp. 75–80). London: Butterworth. tion. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent
Ricks, D. M. (1979). Making sense of experience to Medicine, 148, 174–179.
make sensible sounds. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Stone, W. L., & Lemanek, K. L. (1990). Parental
Before speech (pp. 245–268). Cambridge, En- report of social behaviors in autistic
gland: Cambridge University Press. preschoolers. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Rogers, S. J. (2000). Interventions that facilitate mental Disorders, 20, 513–522.
socialization in children with autism. Journal Vostanis, P., Smith, B., Corbett, J., Sungum-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, Paliwal, R., Edwards, A., Gingell, K., et al.
399–409. (1998). Parental concerns of early develop-
Rogers, S. J., & Newhart-Larson, S. (1989). Charac- ment in children with autism and related disor-
teristics of infantile autism in five children with ders. Autism, 2, 229–242.
Leber’s congenital amaurosis. Developmental Weeks, S. J., & Hobson, R. P. (1987). The salience
Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 598–608. of facial expression for autistic children. Jour-
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28,
(1991). A comparative study of attachment be- 137–152.
haviour in young children with autism or other Wimpory, D. C., Hobson, R. P., Williams, J. M., &
psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Nash, S. (2000). Are infants with autism so-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, cially engaged? A study of recent retrospec-
30, 483–488. tive parental reports. Journal of Autism and
Rutter, M., Andersen-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bre- Developmental Disorders, 30, 525–536.
denkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. Wing, L. (1969). The handicaps of autistic chil-
(1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following se- dren: A comparative study. Journal of Child
vere early global privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 1–40.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 537–549. Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P.
Schultz, R., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, A., (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autis-
Anderson, A., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Ab- tic, mentally retarded and normal children.
normal ventral temporal cortical activity dur- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
ing face discrimination among individuals with 30, 725–735.
autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P.
General Psychiatry, 57, 331–340. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-
Shapiro, T., Sherman, M., Calamari, G., & Koch, D. functioning children with autism. Child Devel-
(1987). Attachment in autism and other devel- opment, 63, 150–160.
opmental disorders. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
26, 485–490.
SECTION III

NEUROLOGICAL AND
MEDICAL ISSUES

Autism and other pervasive developmental dis- guide the formation of brain structures, the
orders are brought to clinical attention and are mechanisms underlying the laying down and
diagnosed on the basis of distinctive distur- pruning of pathways, the sensitive timing of
bances in behavior and development. There is brain connections and the formation of
broad consensus among clinicians and re- synapses, the ways in which the templates for
searchers, however, that autism and associated higher cognitive processes are programmed,
syndromes represent the surface or phenotypic and the many interactions between parallel
manifestations of underlying neurobiological systems that relate to behavioral regulation
diatheses or biological genotypes. For this rea- (such as the cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical
son, these conditions are sometimes described pathways). The biology of uniquely human
as neuro-behavioral or neuropsychiatric disor- functioning—abstract thinking, mature social
ders, to emphasize their neurobiological un- relations—is at the cutting edge of research.
derpinnings. As more is learned, we will be Availability of newer methods for studying the
able to trace the expression of the underlying functioning brain already are illuminating how
neurobiological dysfunctions in autism and the brain processes visual and auditory stimuli
these other conditions through each level of and the steps between physical sensation and
brain and behavioral organization—-from the conscious awareness, and about the parts of
level of the gene(s) involved in brain formation the brain that are activated during different
and functioning, through the emergence and mental tasks. The availability of postmortem
functioning of specific brain systems, to the brains will give new access to studying brain
appearance of symptoms and signs of the dis- structure and function and integrating these
orders during early childhood and the full pic- findings with the functioning of the brain dur-
ture of the clinical conditions. ing life, using methods such as functional mag-
Advances in molecular and behavioral ge- netic resonance imaging. We can look forward
netics, neurochemistry, neuro-imaging, and to being able to integrate these various neuro-
other related fields in the neurosciences are biological approaches with careful studies of
providing a deeper understanding of the nor- behavior and development.
mal development of the central nervous sys- Far less is known about the functioning of
tem (CNS) and the integrated functioning of the brain during the first years of life and dur-
the CNS during complex tasks (such as think- ing the course of early development. The un-
ing, imagining, reading, listening, inhibiting, derstanding of normal development and the
or planning behavior). There are fascinating ways in which dysfunctions may arise during
leads about what particular brain systems are embryogenesis and postnatally will provide a
involved in normal processes and may be im- framework for understanding the developmen-
plicated in neurological, psychiatric, and de- tal disorders such as autism. Already, more
velopmental disorders. We are only at the first than 1,000 forms of mental retardation are
stages of understanding how regulatory genes known and for many there are specific genetic

423
424 Neurological and Medical Issues

and biological findings that help clarify their neurology, neurochemistry, and neuro-
etiology. The hope is that similar, rigorous imaging—relating to autism and pervasive
studies will help clarify the causal pathways disorders. One chapter also describes the
that lead to the clinical syndrome of autism and medical conditions that are sometimes associ-
the other pervasive developmental disorders. ated with autism and similar disorders. Only
We are still a long way from reaching this goal, through basic, neurobiological research will it
but the advances in the neurosciences have be possible to provide firmly based genetic
been explosive and we can anticipate that these counseling, diagnose pervasive disorders in
findings will have direct relevance to sub- utero, offer highly ameliorative and hopefully
groups of individuals with pervasive disorders. curative therapies, and fundamentally alter
The following chapters highlight major the natural history of these conditions.
domains of neuroscience research—genetics,
CHAPTER 16

Genetic Influences and Autism

MICHAEL RUTTER

When Kanner (1943) first described autism, he autism in the general population (about 2 to 4
suggested that it resulted from an inborn defect cases per 10,000 as defined at that time), the
of presumably constitutional origin. Neverthe- rate of autism in siblings (then estimated to be
less, over the next three decades, the possible about 2%) was very high; furthermore, the cy-
role of genetic factors tended to be dismissed. togenetic techniques available in the 1960s
In part, this was because the zeitgeist at that were quite primitive so that the failure to show
time was one of expecting environmental causes anomalies was essentially noncontributory.
for all forms of psychopathology. This was the There were two crucial findings from this
era of supposed “refrigerator ” parents of autis- first twin study. First, despite the small num-
tic children and of “schizophrenogenic” moth- bers, there was a significant monozygotic-
ers (see Rutter, 1999b). However, reviews by dizygotic (MZ-DZ) difference in concordance.
geneticists were equally dismissive (Hanson & The fact that the population base rate of autism
Gottesman, 1976). The emphasis tended to be was so low implied a strong underlying genetic
placed on the lack of vertical transmission (i.e., liability. Second, concordance within MZ pairs
the rarity with which children with autism included a range of cognitive and social deficits
had parents with autism), the very low rate of and not just the seriously handicapping condi-
autism in siblings, and the lack of identified tion of autism itself. This implied that the ge-
chromosome anomalies associated with autism netic liability extended beyond autism proper.
(Rutter, 1967). It also raised questions about the diagnostic
boundaries of autism and led to an appreciation
QUANTITATIVE GENETICS of a need to consider the likelihood of a broader
phenotype of autism or of lesser variants of the
Twin Studies same condition.
During the late 1980s and early 1990s,
An awareness that the logic of these arguments genetic research into autism advanced through
was faulty (Rutter, 1968) led Folstein and Rut- further twin studies and genetic-family studies.
ter (1977a, 1977b) to undertake the first small Both the Scandinavian Twin Study (Steffen-
scale (N = 21) twin study of autism. The ear- burg et al., 1989) and a further British twin
lier reasoning on genetic influences was false study (Bailey et al., 1995) confirmed the great
because follow-up studies had shown that few strength of genetic influences on the underlying
autistic people developed love relationships, liability for autism. The British study included
and that it was very rare for them to have chil- four key design features. First, there was total
dren, and hence vertical transmission would population screening of the cases, with all clin-
not be expected; also relative to the rate of ics and special schools in the country contacted

I am greatly indebted to members of the International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium, and
especially to Anthony Monaco, for much of my thinking on genetic issues. I am also most grateful to Anthony
Bailey for helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this chapter.

425
426 Neurological and Medical Issues

and all twin registers examined. Second, syste- gested that the number of susceptibility
matic standardized methods of diagnosis using genes might be much higher than that. Un-
both parental interviews (Le Couteur et al., certainties also remain on the likelihood
1989) and observation of the child (Lord that the susceptibility genes will involve rel-
et al., 1989) were employed. Third, there was atively common allelic variations or rather
thorough screening for medical conditions and rare variants reflecting a disease mutation
chromosomal abnormalities in order to focus on (Pritchard, 2001).
the study of genetic factors in idiopathic 3. The finding that the genetic liability for
autism. Fourth, blood groups were used to autism extended to include a broader phe-
test for zygosity. That was important because notype was confirmed. Some 90% of MZ
the marked behavioral differences associated pairs were concordant for mixtures of
with autism sometimes led parents and profes- social and cognitive deficits that were qual-
sionals to infer that the twins were nonidentical, itatively similar to those found in tradi-
whereas in fact they were identical. tional autism, but milder in degree (i.e., the
Four main findings were particularly crucial: broader phenotype). This applied, however,
to only about 1 in 10 DZ pairs. Focusing on
1. The huge disparity in concordance rates for the 10 MZ and 20 DZ pairs’ discordant for
autism between monozygotic (MZ; N = 25) autism or autism-spectrum disorders, it was
and dizygotic (DZ; N = 20) pairs (60% ver- shown that there was a similar contrasting
sus 5%—if the rate in siblings as well as concordance for this broader phenotype,
DZ twins is used to estimate the figure, the difference being statistically signifi-
twins and DZ twins being genetically com- cant (Bailey et al., 1995; Le Couteur et al.,
parable) confirmed the earlier findings on 1996). A follow-up of the original Folstein
the strength of the genetic influence. Quan- and Rutter sample also showed that this
titative analyses indicated a heritability in broader phenotype is associated with im-
excess of 90%. portant deficits in social functioning that
2. The exceedingly low rate of concordance in continued into adult life.
DZ pairs compared with that in MZ pairs 4. An examination of 16 MZ pairs concordant
pointed to the likelihood of epistatic effects for autism or autism spectrum disorders
involving synergistic interaction among sev- showed that there was enormous clinical
eral genes. The pattern was not compatible heterogeneity even when pairs shared ex-
with a single gene Mendelian disorder. The actly the same segregating genetic alleles.
fall-off rate from MZ to DZ twins, together Surprisingly, individuals within MZ pairs
with that from first-degree to second-degree were no more alike in IQ or symptomatol-
relatives, was used by Pickles and colleagues ogy than were pairs selected at random
(1995) to estimate the number of genes that from different twin pairs (Le Couteur
were likely to be involved. The logic of this et al., 1996).
analysis is based on the fact that whereas
MZ twins share 100% of their genes, and
100% of all combinations of their genes, the Family Studies
situation is quite different in DZ twins and
siblings. On average, they share 50% of their Family-genetic studies were important in
segregating genes but this means that they order to determine the rate of autism in sib-
will share only a quarter of two gene combi- lings and in parents, to check family patterns
nations, and an eighth of three gene com- of transmission in case there were single-gene
binations. The findings from the analysis by Mendelian variants (this cannot be assessed
Pickles and colleagues (2000) suggested that from twin studies), and to better delineate the
three or four genes were most probable, but breadth and pattern of the possible broader
that any number between 2 and 10 genes was phenotype. The Maudsley Hospital Study and
a possibility (depending on the relative the Johns Hopkins Study, initially planned to-
strength of the effect of any one of these gether, provided the first systematic findings
genes). However, Risch et al. (1999) sug- on sizable samples using standardized mea-
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 427

sures. Subsequent studies have added to the between severity of autism and the level of
findings in important ways. family loading seemed to apply only to cases
The Maudsley Hospital Family Study used of autism in which there was some useful
measurement methods directly comparable speech (Pickles et al., 2000). The implication
with those in the British Twin Study and simi- is that when autism is associated with a very
larly excluded families in which the autism was severe lack of language skills, it might be ge-
associated with some known medical condition netically different in some way. However, this
that was likely to be causal. The families of 99 remains only as a possibility to be further
individuals with autism were compared sys- tested, rather than as an established fact.
tematically with 36 families of individuals The Johns Hopkins study was particularly
with Down syndrome using exactly the same important because of its evidence on the prob-
methods of measurement (Bolton et al., 1994). able importance of pragmatic language prob-
There was direct assessment of all first-degree lems (Landa, Wzorek, Piven, Folstein, &
relatives, and systematic standardized reports Isaacs, 1991; Landa et al., 1992), of social ab-
on more distant relatives. The rate of autism in normalities (Piven et al., 1990, 1991), and of
the siblings of autistic individuals was found to unusual personality features (Piven et al.,
be 3%, with an additional 3% showing some 1994). The early findings had particularly em-
form of, more broadly conceptualized, autism phasized the familial loading of language
spectrum disorder. No cases of autism or autism delay but, although this seemed to be part of
spectrum disorder were found in the siblings the overall picture in some cases, the later
of individuals with Down syndrome. As in the findings from all studies have suggested the
twin study, a broader phenotype of autism, probably greater importance of social deficits
comprising mixed patterns of cognitive and so- (Bailey, Palferman, Heavey, & Le Couteur,
cial deficits and repetitive stereotyped interest 1998; Folstein & Piven, 1991; Piven, Palmer,
patterns, were even more frequent. Depending Jacobi, Childress, & Arndt, 1997; Rutter, Bai-
on how stringent a definition was used, the ley, Simonoff, & Pickles, 1997).
comparative rates of the broader phenotype as The family study by Szatmari and col-
compared with Down syndrome families were leagues (2000) differed from the others in its
about 12% versus 2%, or 20% versus 3%. The strategy of comparing biological and nonbio-
findings provided striking evidence that the logical relatives (the latter being represented
broader phenotype might be much more com- by stepparents and adoptive families). The
mon in the general population than previously finding that social, communicative, and repeti-
considered. tive behaviors were all more common in bio-
Because some data on families (August, logical relatives supported the inference that
Stewart, & Tsai, 1981) had suggested that the familial loading reflected a genetically
autism that was accompanied by profound mediated liability. They also found that the
mental retardation might be somewhat differ- rate of broader phenotype in relatives was
ent from the rest of autism in which nonverbal higher in multiplex than simplex families and
IQs were above 50, the Maudsley Hospital was greater when the proband with autism had
group undertook a further family study to de- an IQ above 60.
termine whether this might be the case (Pick- As well as language deficits in the relatives
les et al., 2000; Starr et al., 2001). However, in of individuals with autism, the early findings
contrast with the earlier suggestion, the rate of had suggested that there might also be a famil-
autism and of the broader phenotype in the rel- ial loading for reading and spelling difficul-
atives was not significantly different from that ties. However, that now seems not to be the
found in the first family study. The only possi- case. Fombonne, Bolton, Prior, Jordan, and
ble lead was the uncertain indication that cog- Rutter (1997) undertook a detailed analysis of
nitive problems in the relatives might be cognitive patterns in relatives in the Maudsley
somewhat more common when autism was as- Family Study. The findings showed that nei-
sociated with profound retardation. What did ther low IQ nor specific problems in reading or
emerge, from the combination of the two stud- spelling showed an increased loading in the
ies, was the finding that the linear association families of individuals with autism if these
428 Neurological and Medical Issues

problems were not accompanied by other man- members but it was not associated, at either the
ifestations of the broader phenotype. In other individual or the family level, with cognitive or
words, although reading and spelling difficul- social deficits. The cause of the increased rate
ties could constitute an important part of the of depression in the families of individuals with
broader phenotype, these specific cognitive autism remains unclear but it does not seem to
deficits, when they occurred in isolation, did reflect a genetic liability to autism.
not seem to be indicators of a genetic liability Retrospective studies of individuals with
to autism. Also, family data suggest that schizophrenia at one time led to the claim that,
phonological processing deficits (in contrast to although not diagnosed at the time in childhood,
pragmatic deficits) are not part of the broad schizophrenia had been preceded by an autism
autism phenotype (Bishop et al., 2004). Simi- spectrum disorder. However, prospective stud-
larly, language impairments do constitute an ies of individuals with autism have not found
important part of the broader phenotype but that autistic individuals have an increased
ordinarily they do not appear to be genetically rate of schizophrenia in adult life (Volkmar &
connected to autism if they occur without Cohen, 1991). Family studies, similarly, have
either social deficits or circumscribed interest shown no increase in the familial loading for
patterns. Interestingly, both Fombonne et al. schizophrenia in the relatives of individuals
and Piven and Palmer (1997) found that the with an autism spectrum disorder (Bolton et al.,
relatives of individuals with autism tended 1998). It is possible, however, that obsessive-
to show a cognitive pattern with verbal skills compulsive disorder, which is somewhat more
that were superior to visuospatial skills. This frequent in the relatives of individuals with
is the opposite of what is ordinarily found autism, may index an underlying genetic liabil-
in autism. This unexpected finding requires ity to autism (Bolton et al., 1998).
confirmation from other studies but, if con- The evidence on the reality, and relative
firmed, other research will be required to de- frequency, of the broader phenotype of autism
termine what it means. has been well demonstrated in numerous stud-
Over the years, more and more clinical fea- ies and the concept is no longer controversial.
tures, particularly including affective disorder Nevertheless, the precise boundaries of the
and social anxiety (DeLong & Nohria, 1994; broader phenotype have yet to be established
Smalley, McCracken, & Tanguay, 1995), came and clear-cut criteria for differentiating the
to be added to possible variations of the broader broader phenotype of autism from the many
phenotype. The key question was whether these other varieties of social deficit have yet to be
affective features reflected the same genetic li- determined. Similarly, although it is apparent
ability that underlies autism, or whether it re- that the broader phenotype occurs in at least
flected some other genetic or environmental 10% to 20% of the first-degree relatives of in-
mechanism. Bolton, Pickles, Murphy, and Rut- dividuals with autism, it is not yet known
ter (1998) confirmed that the rates of clinically whether it constitutes a common, but still a
significant affective disorder were increased in qualitatively distinct, category, or whether, in-
the relatives of individuals with autism and stead, it constitutes a continuously distributed
Murphy et al. (2000), as in the Johns Hopkins dimension.
study (Piven et al., 1994), found that the rela- There are two crucial differences, too,
tives of individuals with autism showed an in- between the broader phenotype and autism as
crease in the traits of shyness and aloofness and traditionally diagnosed. Unlike autism, the
also the traits of anxiety and oversensitivity. broader phenotype is not associated with men-
However, the meanings of these two sets of tal retardation, and it is not associated with
traits were somewhat different. The evidence epilepsy. As yet, we do not know why that is so.
suggested that shyness and aloofness were man- Questions arise as to whether the broader phe-
ifestations of the broader phenotype whereas notype represents a lesser “dose” of genetic li-
anxiety and oversensitivity were related to anx- ability, a different pattern of susceptibility
iety or depressive disorders, rather than to the genes, or some kind of “ two-hit ” mechanism in
broader phenotype of autism. Depression was which an additional risk factor is required to
associated with depression in other family take individuals over the threshold from the
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 429

broader phenotype into a more seriously handi- tial findings were often presented as providing
capping disorder. a “minimum figure,” failing to appreciate the
dangers of relying on findings based on small
CHROMOSOMAL ABNORMALITIES samples. The ratio of false positives to true
AND GENETICALLY DETERMINED positives in a small sample is necessarily much
MEDICAL CONDITIONS greater than that in a large sample and the size
of a difference between groups is no guide to
During the 1970s and 1980s, there was a range the true strength of the association. To achieve
of medical studies of autism demonstrating statistical significance, the difference in a
positive findings (Rutter, 1999a). Gillberg small sample is bound to be a large one, and the
(1992) claimed that 37% of cases of autism true difference will almost certainly be very
were associated with a diagnosable medical much smaller (see Cohen, Cohen, & Brook,
condition and, on this basis, argued that a wide 1995; Pocock, 1983). With respect to the frag-
range of intrusive medical investigations (in- ile X anomaly, there was the additional con-
cluding lumbar puncture, EEG, brain imaging, cern that, during the 1980s, this had to be
and metabolic studies) should be undertaken as diagnosed using problematic cell culture meth-
a routine (Gillberg, 1990; Gillberg & Coleman, ods. The fragile X anomaly was initially often
1996). However, Rutter and colleagues (Rutter, diagnosed on the basis of only 1% to 3% of X
Bailey, Bolton, & Le Couteur, 1994), putting chromosomes showing a fragile site. Once
together the evidence from several studies, DNA techniques were available, it became
concluded that the rate was probably more like clear that this was an unwarranted inference
10%. Nevertheless, this figure is certainly suf- (Gurling, Bolton, Vincent, Melmer, & Rutter,
ficiently high to mean that all individuals sus- 1997). Available estimates indicate that fragile
pected of having an autism spectrum disorder X anomalies are present in less than 5% of in-
should have a careful medical assessment. The dividuals with autism (Bailey et al., 1993;
question of whether they should have invasive Dykens & Volkmar, 1997). This is still a mean-
investigations is a rather separate matter. The ingful association, the rate of which is well
key issue is how often the investigations lead above that expected on the basis of the rates
to a diagnosis that cannot be obtained more of both in the general population. In other
straightforwardly through clinical history and words, it seems reasonable to assume that the
examination. The answer seems to be that it is fragile X anomaly can play a causal role in the
rare for the tests to reveal undiagnosed medical etiology of autism, even though it does so only
conditions; many of the supposedly abnormal in a small proportion of cases. Nevertheless, it
laboratory findings have no unambiguous clini- should be noted that although the fragile X
cal implications; and the clinical value seems anomaly constitutes an uncommon cause of
so slight as not to justify the distress inevitably autism, social abnormalities (of a kind that in-
caused to young children if such investigations cludes, but is not confined to autism) are fre-
are routinely undertaken. Rather, the recom- quently present in individuals with the fragile
mendation is that a thorough clinical history X anomaly (Reiss & Dant, 2003).
and examination should be routine and that the There are individual case reports of associ-
decision as to whether to undertake systematic ations between autism and anomalies of one
laboratory investigations should be decided on sort or another on almost all chromosomes
the basis of the clinical findings. Further inves- (Gillberg, 1998; Yu et al., 2002); most of these
tigations will be mandatory in some cases but are based on single cases, but a few have been
they should not be undertaken routinely in a replicated, and the meaning of many of the
mindless fashion that ignores the need for clini- anomalies is quite unknown. Nevertheless,
cal decision making. there is consistent evidence of chromosome 15
Similar issues have arisen with respect to anomalies associated with autism (Buxbaum
chromosomal abnormalities. Thus, the early re- et al., 2002; Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley, 2001;
ports on the fragile X anomaly led to claims Kim et al., 2002; Nurmi et al., 2003; Shao
that this was a very common cause of autism et al., 2003). Most involve interstitial duplica-
(Gillberg & Wahlstrom, 1985). Moreover, ini- tions of maternal origin; the parental effect
430 Neurological and Medical Issues

suggests the involvement of genomic imprint- for psychiatric disorders, as well as for other
ing. The mechanisms underlying the associa- multifactorial conditions (Maestrini, Mar-
tion between chromosome 15 anomalies and low, Weeks, & Monaco, 1998; Rutter, 2000;
autism remain unclear but the reality of the as- Rutter, Silberg, O’Connor, & Simonoff,
sociation appears well established. 1999). Most particularly, the combination of
With respect to medical conditions, the the development of robotic techniques and the
association with autism is most firmly estab- availability of a very large number of poly-
lished in the case of tuberous sclerosis (Smal- morphic micro-satellite and single nucleotide
ley, 1998). One percent to 4% of individuals genetic markers has made a total scan of the
with autism have tuberous sclerosis and the rate genome a practical possibility. In essence,
may be as high as 8% to 14% among the sub- there are two main approaches that may be
group of those with a seizure disorder. Tuber- followed—namely, linkage and association
ous sclerosis is a wholly genetic Mendelian strategies. They work on a somewhat differ-
disorder associated with a gene on either chro- ent principle and they involve a different mix
mosome 9 or chromosome 16. In about two- of advantages and disadvantages.
thirds of cases the mutations arise de novo, Linkage studies examine co-inheritance—
rather than being inherited. Opinions differ on meaning inheritance within families in which
whether an abnormal tuberous sclerosis gene there is a linkage between the gene locus being
directly influences the development of autism, studied and the condition being investigated.
or whether the association with autism comes Traditionally, this approach was mainly ap-
about because a susceptibility gene for autism plied to very large families involving many
lies in close proximity to a tuberous sclerosis individuals who were affected with the condi-
gene, or because the brain abnormalities result- tion being studied. However, the approach
ing from tuberous sclerosis predispose toward required specification of the mode of inheri-
autism. The last possibility is suggested by the tance involved and that is not known in the
evidence that the occurrence of autism is, in case of autism, any more than it is with most
part, a function of epilepsy and low IQ, and in psychiatric conditions. Also, as a consequence
part a function of the size and location of the of the relatively low frequency of autism, the
tubers occurring in the brain (Bolton & Grif- collection of sizable samples of extended fam-
fiths, 1997; Bolton, Park, Higgins, Griffiths, & ilies with many affected members is not a
Pickles, 2002; Lauritsen & Ewald, 2001; practical proposition. A further disadvantage
Weber, Egelhoff, McKellop, & Franz, 2000). of the traditional family approach was that it
It has also been claimed that autism is asso- was unclear how to deal with cases in which
ciated with neurofibromatosis (Gillberg & the diagnostic status was uncertain. Because
Forsell, 1984) but the reality and strength of of these features, most research groups have
the association remains somewhat uncertain switched to the study of affected sib pairs, or
(Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley, 2001). Early re- affected relative pairs (Rutter, Silberg, et al.,
ports had suggested that autism might be asso- 1999). The strategy requires the collection of
ciated with phenylketonuria, but it is not clear samples in which there are two or more af-
whether this was an important association be- fected individuals in the same family and it is
cause the findings on autism were not based on necessary only to concentrate on those for
standardized measurement (Folstein & Rutter, whom the diagnosis is unambiguous. The
1988). In any case, it is no longer a relevant analysis then determines whether the co-oc-
issue in most developed countries because currence of particular gene loci coincides with
newborn screening has meant that untreated the diagnosis to an extent that exceeds chance
phenylketonuria is extremely rare. expectations. With this approach, it is not nec-
essary to specify in advance where the locus is
MOLECULAR GENETICS expected to be. Rather, there can be a scanning
of the whole genome, using markers that are
Over the past dozen years or so, there have sufficiently close to one another to ensure that
been important technical advances that have if there is a relevant susceptibility gene locus
facilitated the search for susceptibility genes it will be picked up.
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 431

The strategy (which can be applied to large relation to a susceptibility gene that is very
pedigrees as well as affected relative pairs; close to the markers used, or is the trait
Davis, Schroeder, Goldin, & Weeks, 1996) has marker itself. Second, there are statistical
the huge advantage of involving a minimum of problems in determining the significance of
untestable assumptions but it has two consider- case control differences when a very large
able disadvantages. First, it will detect gene number of gene markers have to be tested.
loci only when there are relatively strong ef- Third, the pooling of all cases means that the
fects. Second, even when a likely gene locus groups are made up on the basis of the assump-
has been identified, the area of the chromo- tion that the susceptibility gene concerns the
some within which the gene should be found in disorder, rather than the components of the
a complex genetic disorder is very large. This disorder (see the discussion that follows).
means that the area will include a very large
number of possible genes. Accordingly, it will Single Gene Major Mutations:
usually be necessary to combine linkage stud- The Case of Rett Syndrome
ies with association strategies.
Association studies are based on the quite Rett syndrome is a progressive neurodevelop-
different strategy of using linkage disequilib- mental disorder with an incidence of about 1
rium to search for differences between cases in 10,000 in girls (Hagberg, 1985; Hagberg,
and controls in allelic patterns. They have the Aircardi, Dias, & Ramos, 1983). Classically,
advantage of being better able than linkage the girls develop normally until 6 to 18 months
strategies to detect very small genetic effects of age, then gradually lose speech and pur-
(Risch & Merikangas, 1996). The concern poseful hand use, developing microcephaly,
with association strategies derives from the seizures, social impairment, ataxia, intermit-
fact that stratification bias may arise because tent hyperventilation, and stereotypic hand
cases and controls differ in their allelic pat- movements. Witt-Engerström and Gillberg
terns as a result of their ethnic origins, rather (1987) noted that the majority of cases of Rett
than for any reason to do with the disorder syndrome were initially suspected of having
being studied. Opinions differ as to how big a autism because of the social impairment. Gill-
problem this is if major ethnic differences berg (1989) went on to argue that the apparent
have been taken into account (Ardlie, Lunetta, symptomatic similarities might mirror com-
& Seielstad, 2002; Cardon & Palmer, 2003). mon pathophysiological abnormalities at the
Nevertheless, it is desirable to control for strat- brainstem level. However, although Rett syn-
ification biases and this is possible through drome may, in the past, have been misdiag-
the transmission disequilibrium test (TDT), nosed as autism, the social features of Rett
which also tests for both association and link- syndrome and autism are somewhat different
age (Malhotra & Goldman, 1999; Spielman & (Olsson & Rett, 1985, 1987). In 1999, Amir
Ewens, 1996). Ordinarily, it requires DNA and colleagues showed, using a systematic
samples on trios—meaning an affected child gene screening method, that a mutation in the
and both parents. A further limitation of asso- MECP2 gene was the cause of many cases of
ciation strategies, at least up to now, has been Rett syndrome. This has now been confirmed
the fact that they rely on the availability of by numerous other investigators. Most cru-
candidate genes, which are singularly lacking in cially, it has been shown that mouse models
the case of autism. It has been claimed that the of the MECP2 mutation causes a neurobe-
technique of DNA pooling (in other words, havioral syndrome that is closely similar to
combining DNA samples across cases and simi- that found in humans (Guy, Hendrich, Holmes,
larly across controls) provides a possible way Martin, & Bird, 2001; Shahbazian et al.,
forward (Barcellos et al., 1997; Daniels et al., 2002). The evidence is clear-cut that the ge-
1998). There are three possible difficulties with netic mutation constitutes a single gene disor-
the use of DNA pooling. First, the number of der with the genetic abnormality providing a
markers required would be very much greater sufficient explanation for the clinical syn-
than in linkage studies because the association drome, without the need to invoke any other
strategies can produce positive findings only in genetic or environmental factors.
432 Neurological and Medical Issues

However, as is usual with single gene disor- different groups (Auranen et al., 2002, 2003;
ders, it has been found that there are several Barrett et al., 1999; Buxbaum et al., 2001; Col-
different MECP2 mutations that have gener- laborative Linkage Study of Autism [CLSA],
ally similar phenotypic consequences (Amir 1999; International Molecular Genetic Study
et al., 2000). Also, as is commonly the case of Autism Consortium [IMGSAC], 1998,
with single gene mutations, it has been found 2001b; Liu et al., 2001; Philippe et al., 1999;
that the clinical picture is more varied than Risch et al., 1999; Shao, Raiford, et al., 2002;
was at first considered to be the case (Shah- Shao, Wolpert, et al., 2002; Yonan et al.,
bazian et al., 2002). Because of this, several 2003), of which the first was the International
research groups have studied samples of Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consor-
patients with autism in order to determine tium (1998, 2001b). The findings have been
whether mutations in the MECP2 gene might reviewed by Folstein and Rosen-Sheidley
be present. Two of the studies (Beyer et al., (2001), Gutknecht (2001), and Lamb, Moore,
2002; Vourc’h et al., 2001) concluded that mu- Bailey, and Monaco (2000; Lamb, Parr, Bai-
tations in the coding region of MECP2 did not ley, & Monaco, 2002). A locus on chromosome
play a major role in autism susceptibility. 7q has been found in four of the studies
However, a third study (Carney et al., 2003) (IMGSAC, 1998, 2001a), the collaborative
reported that 2 out of 69 cases of autism did linkage study of autism—CLSA (1999; Ashley-
show the MECP2 mutation. Apparently, the Koch et al., 1999; Philippe et al., 1999; Shao,
affected individuals did not show an overall Wolpert, et al., 2002), and confirmed in a
syndrome that was similar to Rett syndrome meta-analysis (Badner & Gershon, 2002). It is
(although they did have some Rett syndrome undoubtedly encouraging that several differ-
features). The balance of evidence suggests ent groups have come up with comparable
that the MECP2 mutation invariably leads to a findings regarding a locus on chromosome 7.
serious neurodevelopmental disorder, that in However, the peak of linkage is quite wide and
the great majority of cases this approximates the precise location has not been identical in
to the clinical picture of Rett syndrome, but all the studies. This is not necessarily a con-
occasionally ( but not often) the clinical pic- cern because simulation studies have shown
ture may be somewhat different and, perhaps, that there is a large variation in peak location
uncommonly it may take the form of autism. It relative to actual disease-gene location
seems unlikely, however, that the MECP2 mu- (Roberts, MacLean, Neale, Eaves, & Kendler,
tation has any broader significance in relation 1999), the strength of the finding varies
to autism. However, Zoghbi (2003) has argued across studies. The postulated locus on chro-
that both autism and Rett syndrome could turn mosome 7q includes several key candidate
out to be similar disorders of synaptic modula- genes. Thus, it is close to the location of the
tion or maintenance. FOXP2 gene, which has been shown to be re-
sponsible for an unusual familial speech and
Genome-Wide Screens of Sib language disorder (Lai, Fisher, Hurst,
Pair Samples Vargha-Khadem, & Monaco, 2001). It was
suggested that autism and severe language
The discovery of the MECP2 mutation was impairment might derive from the same gene
important, but the evidence that autism is a in this area (Folstein & Mankoski, 2000) but
multifactorial disorder and that multiple sus- the available evidence suggests that this is not
ceptibility genes are likely to be operative im- likely to be the case (Newbury et al., 2002).
plies that it should be expected that, in most The human reelin gene also maps to an area
instances, these genes would be normal, com- on 7q that is close to the peak of linkage
mon, allelic variations rather than rare patho- found in relation to autism. The reelin protein
logical mutations, although the latter might plays an important role in neuronal migration
also contribute to the liability (Pritchard, during brain development and, hence, consti-
2001; Rutter, 2004). Genome-wide scans of tutes a plausible candidate gene in respect of
sib pair samples in order to detect susceptibil- autism. However, the evidence to date sug-
ity gene loci have been published by eight gests that it probably does not play a major
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 433

role in autism etiology (Bonora et al., 2003), family-based association method with the
although it is too early to conclude that it plays Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE),
no role. found a significant association with two in-
The strongest evidence for linkage in the In- tronic markers of a cerebella patterning gene
ternational Molecular Genetic Study of Autism located on chromosome 7 ( but no association
Consortium findings (IMGSAC, 2001b) was a with the flanking exons). Ramoz et al. (2004),
susceptibility region on chromosome 2q. This using a partially overlapping data set, found
was also the case in the findings of the association with common intronic polymor-
Buxbaum and colleagues (2001) group, and of phisms in a gene located on chromosome 2 that
the Shao and colleagues group (Shao, Raiford, is involved with mitochondrial aspartate/glu-
et al., 2002; Shao, Wolpert, et al., 2002). Again, tamate function. Both findings require replica-
the region of chromosome 2q includes a number tion before conclusions can be drawn, but it is
of interesting genes that are potential candi- of interest that the postulated genetic variants
dates for a role in the etiology of autism. are common and that they are concerned with
Other positive linkage findings have been re- regulating functions rather than the production
ported for chromosome 16p (IMGSAC, 2001b; of polypeptides as such.
Liu et al., 2001), chromosome 1p (Risch et al., One of the striking findings with respect to
1999), chromosome 3 (Auranen et al., 2002, autism concerns the marked male preponder-
2003; Shao, Raiford, et al., 2002; Shao, Wolpert, ance. Not surprisingly, therefore, attention
et al., 2002), chromosome 13q (CLSA, 1999), 5q has focused on the X chromosome in order
(Liu et al., 2001), 5p (Yonan et al., 2003), 17q to determine whether X linkage might be re-
(IMGSAC, 2001b; Yonan et al., 2003), 19p and sponsible. However, with the exception of
19q (Liu et al., 2001), and Xq (Shao, Raiford, Shao and colleagues (Shao, Raiford, et al.,
et al., 2002; Shao, Wolpert, et al., 2002). 2002; Shao, Wolpert, et al., 2002), the find-
ings have been generally negative (Hallmayer
Candidate Gene Strategies et al., 1996; Yirmiya et al., 2002) and the evi-
dence of father to son transmission of autism
The literature contains several positive findings (which could not involve the X chromosome)
with respect to candidate genes (Folstein & indicates that it is unlikely that X linkage is
Rosen-Sheidley, 2001). They had been promis- responsible for the overall male preponder-
ing, for a variety of good reasons—including ance (Hallmayer et al., 1996). Findings for the
their role in neurotransmitters (such as sero- Y chromosome have also been negative (Ja-
tonin, dopamine, and glutamate), because of main, Quach, et al., 2002). Skuse and his col-
connections with chromosome anomalies asso- leagues (Skuse, 2000; Skuse et al., 1997) put
ciated with autism, or because of their proxim- forward a somewhat different hypothesis.
ity to the locations derived from the genome They found that in subjects with Turner syn-
screen linkage findings. However, the findings drome (who have an XO karyotype), social in-
so far are contradictory and inconclusive with teraction difficulties were much more
many failures to replicate. Although it is too prevalent in those who inherited the X chro-
early to rule out any of the candidate gene pos- mosome from the mother as compared with
sibilities, the evidence that they do have an those who inherited it from the father. It was
actual role in the liability to autism is so far argued that there might be an imprinted locus
unconvincing. A possible exception to the gen- on the X chromosome that might serve to
erally negative story may be provided by the re- make males more susceptible because their X
port that mutations in two X-linked genes chromosome would have to have been inher-
coding the neuroligands NLGN3 and LNNLGN4 ited from the mother. By contrast, females
are associated with autism spectrum disorders would have an X chromosome from each par-
(Jamain, Betancur, et al., 2002). However, this ent, that from the father possibly providing
finding has yet to be replicated. some protection from a genetic liability to
Recently, two further claims have been autism that derived from some other (presum-
made with regard to possible susceptibility ably autosomal) locus. The hypothesis is in-
genes for autism. Gharani et al. (2004), using a triguing and it has a certain amount of support
434 Neurological and Medical Issues

within Skuse’s Turner syndrome data set ( but see Hodge, Greenberg, Betancur, & Gill-
(Skuse, 2003). However, despite the fact that berg, 2002; Visscher, 2002). If this finding
the hypothesis was first put forward over half were valid, it would suggest that being a twin
a dozen years ago, no replications have been constitutes a risk factor for autism. If that were
reported, apart from a single case report the case, it might either reflect obstetric com-
(Donnelly et al., 2000), which inevitably plications or the effect of developmental per-
raises questions about the original findings. turbations. Congenital anomalies have been
found to be more common in individuals with
EPIDEMIOLOGICAL FINDINGS autism and these probably index the ways in
which development, which is probabilistically
One of the most striking features of the find- rather that deterministically programmed, may
ings on the rate of autism spectrum disorders go awry (Vogel & Motulsky, 1997). Thus, con-
in the general population is the enormous in- genital anomalies are more common in twins
crease that has taken place over the past half than in singletons and are more common in
century (Fombonne, 1999, 2003; Rutter, in children born to older mothers (Myrianthopou-
press). The first study by Lotter (1966) sug- los & Melnick, 1977; Rutter et al., 1990). Thus,
gested a rate of about four cases per 10,000. it could be that developmental perturbations
This contrasts starkly with the estimates from enhance the adverse effects of a genetic liabil-
the best recent studies of between 30 and ity to autism. However, it should be noted that
60 cases per 10,000 (Baird et al., 2000; congenital anomalies show an increased rate in
Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2001). It is clear a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Also,
that this increase is largely a function of better some skepticism is necessary with respect to
ascertainment combined with a considerable the supposed finding that the rate of twinning
broadening of the diagnostic criteria. Thus, is increased in autism. Ascertainment biases
for example, in recent studies, a high are likely to have played a major role and it is
proportion of the children with autism spec- noteworthy that the most systematically ascer-
trum disorders had a normal level of measured tained twin sample of Bailey and colleagues
intelligence, whereas that applied to only a (1995) did not include a significant excess of
small minority in the early studies. However, monozygotic twins; nor did Hallmayer and col-
whether or not this constitutes the whole story leagues’ (2002) Australian twin sample. It may
remains quite uncertain (Bock & Goode, be concluded that more evidence is needed on
2003). It is possible that there has been a real the postulated increased risk for autism associ-
rise in the incidence of autism and the avail- ated with being a twin but, on the evidence
able data neither confirm nor disconfirm the available to date, it is not likely that being a
suggestion. If there has been a true rise, it monozygotic twin is a major risk factor.
would certainly have to involve the operation
of environmental risk factors of some kind. Obstetric Complications

NONGENETIC RISK FACTORS Numerous studies have shown a significant as-


sociation between autism and obstetric com-
Monozygotic Twinning as a Risk Factor plications (Tsai, 1987). Three very different
hypotheses may be put forward to account for
There tends to be an assumption that the non- the finding: (1) It could be a secondary conse-
genetic factors involved in the etiology of quence of birth order effects; (2) it could re-
autism must involve specific environmental flect brain damage brought about by obstetric
risks. However, that is not necessarily the case complications, however caused; or (3) it could
(Jensen, 1997; Molenaar, Boomsma, & Dolan, reflect an epiphenomenon in which the obstet-
1993). Greenberg and colleagues (Greenberg, ric complications derive from the presence of
Hodge, Sowinski, & Nicoll, 2001), and also Be- a genetically abnormal fetus. These possibili-
tancur and colleagues (Betancur, Leboyer, & ties were systematically examined by Bolton
Gillberg, 2002) reported an apparent excess of and colleagues (1997), who concluded that it
twins among affected sibling pairs with autism was unlikely that the association reflected en-
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 435

vironmentally caused brain damage ( because, these suggestions (Fombonne & Chakrabarti,
among other things, the main excess of compli- 2001; Taylor et al., 2002; Uchiyama, Kuro-
cations were mild, rather than severe), and sawa, & Inaba, submitted). It may be con-
that, rather, either the obstetric adversities cluded that it is quite implausible that MMR is
represented an epiphenomenon or derived from generally associated with a substantially in-
some shared risk factors. creased risk for autism. It is not possible to
rule out the possibility that there may be occa-
Measles-Mumps-Rubella Vaccination sional idiosyncratic responses to MMR that
involve autism, but there is no good evidence
In 1998, on the basis of totally inadequate ev- that this happens.
idence, Wakefield and colleagues suggested Somewhat similar concerns have been ex-
that autism might be caused by adverse ef- pressed with respect to the possibility that
fects stemming from measles-mumps-rubella Thimerosal, a vaccine preservative that con-
(MMR) vaccination. The suggestion arose be- tains ethyl mercury, might cause autism
cause of an observed temporal association be- (Bernard, Enayati, Redwood, Roger, & Bin-
tween the timing of the MMR vaccination and stock, 2001). The question has biological plau-
the timing of the first manifestations of sibility in that it is known that mercury, in high
autism. Such an association would, of course, dosage, can cause neurodevelopmental seque-
be expected by chance alone because the first lae (Clarkson, 1997; Stratton, Gable, & Mc-
manifestations are usually evident at about the Cormick, 2001). The opportunity to test this
age when it is recommended that MMR be hypothesis arose in Denmark, where, from
given. Subsequently, it was suggested that the 1970 onward, the only Thimerosal-containing
MMR vaccination was responsible for the vaccine was the whole cell pertussis vaccine.
major rise in the incidence of diagnosed Between April 1992, and January 1997, the
autism that has occurred over time. The hy- same vaccine was used but without Thimerosal,
pothesis has been examined in a number of and the vaccine was then replaced by an acel-
different ways, all of which have produced lular pertussis vaccine. Data from the Danish
findings that run counter to the hypothesis Psychiatric Central Register was used to com-
(Rutter, in press). First, the close temporal as- pare the rate of autism and autism-spectrum
sociation has not been confirmed (Farrington, disorders in individuals who received only
Miller, & Taylor, 2001; Taylor et al., 1999) Thimerosal-free vaccinations and those con-
and the rise in the rate of autism does not fol- taining Thimerosal. The Danish civil regis-
low the pattern that would be expected on the tration system allowed identification of the
basis of the MMR effect. Thus, the beginning vaccinations used in each child and the num-
of the rise began before the introduction of ber of doses given (thereby allowing calcula-
MMR; there was no stepwise increase in the tion of the total Thimerosal dosage received).
rate of autism following the introduction of No difference in the rate of autism spectrum
MMR; and the rate did not plateau during the disorders was found between the groups that
period when MMR vaccination rates were differed with respect to receipt of Thimerosal
both high and stable (Dales, Hammer, & (Hviid, Stellfeld, Wohlfahrt, & Melbye, 2003).
Smith, 2001; Hillman, Kanafani, Takahashi, The causal hypothesis could also be tested by
& Miles, 2000; Kaye, Melero-Montes, & Jick, looking at time trends in the incidence of autism
2001). Most crucially, too, the rate of autism among children between 2 and 10 years of age,
in Japan continued to rise after MMR ceased both before and after removal of Thimerosal
to be used (Honda, Shimizu, & Rutter, in from vaccines (K. E. Madsen et al., 2003). The
press). Case-control comparisons have been findings showed that the discontinuation of
similarly negative (K. M. Madsen et al., Thimerosal-containing vaccines in 1992 was
2002). It had been further suggested that the followed by an increase in the incidence of
autism associated with MMR usually involved autism and not the predicted decrease (see Rut-
developmental regression and was accompa- ter, in press). The natural experiment provided
nied by bowel abnormalities (Wakefield et al., by the removal of the postulated risk factor
1998). However, the evidence runs counter to (Thimerosal) provided a good opportunity to
436 Neurological and Medical Issues

test the causal hypothesis, with findings that children remained markedly handicapped, the
were completely negative. The two-phase ret- autistic features diminished as they grew older.
rospective cohort study by Verstraeten et al. In any case, the findings are of very limited
(2003), undertaken using a very large health contemporary relevance in view of the rarity
records database in the United States similarly of congenital rubella following vaccination
found no significant increased risk for autism programs.
associated with Thimerosal usage. There is no
reason to suppose that Thimerosal is likely to Phenocopies
be a general risk factor for autism spectrum
disorders and certainly it cannot account for Over the past decade or so, evidence has accu-
the rise in the rate of diagnosed autism in Den- mulated on the existence of what appear to be
mark, as found also in other countries. As with phenocopies—meaning clinical features that
MMR, the data do not allow testing of the dif- look somewhat like autism but are not due
ferent hypothesis of a rare, unusual, idiosyn- to the same genetic liability. Thus, atypical
cratic response to Thimerosal in individual syndromes of autism have been found to be as-
children, although there is no available evi- sociated with congenital blindness (Brown,
dence to indicate that such a response actually Hobson, & Lee, 1997), with profound institu-
occurs. Moreover, the available evidence sug- tional privation (Rutter, Anderson-Wood,
gests that vaccines containing Thiomersal do et al., 1999), and with a mixed bag of medical
not seem to raise blood concentration of mer- conditions all with profound mental retarda-
cury above safety levels (Pichichero, Cerni- tion (Rutter et al., 1994). In each case, the
chiari, Lopreiato, & Treanor, 2002), although clinical pictures are somewhat atypical and the
this conclusion must be tentative in view of the implication is that the syndromes do not in-
paucity of evidence on what is a safe level. volve the same genetic liability that applies to
ordinary idiopathic autism. However, although
Other Environmental Factors it seems highly likely that these atypical syn-
dromes are distinct from the more usual vari-
Case reports and small scale studies have eties of idiopathic autism, that remains an
suggested a range of possible other environmen- inference rather than an established fact.
tal risk factors for autism (see Folstein & A further possible phenocopy concerns the
Rosen-Sheidley, 2001; Medical Research Coun- marked social impairments that are associated
cil [MRC], 2001; Nelson, 1991; Rodier & with many cases of the severe developmental
Hyman, 1998). These include maternal hy- disorders of receptive language (Clegg, Hollis,
pothyroidisim, congenital hypothyroidism, ma- Mawhood, & Rutter, in press; Howlin, Maw-
ternal thalidomide use, maternal valproic acid hood, & Rutter, 2000). Although the social im-
use, maternal cocaine or alcohol use, and con- pairments found in association with these
genital cytomegalovirus infection. It is quite severe cases of language delay differ from the
possible that these factors play at least a con- syndrome of autism in many respects, it is nev-
tributory causal role in individual cases but it ertheless striking that the marked difficulties
seems unlikely that they constitute commonly in social functioning persist into mid-adult life
operating risk factors for autism. and are accompanied by considerable impair-
Probably the best evidence is that concerned ment. It is also noteworthy that there was a sig-
with a possible causal link between congenital nificant impairment on theory of mind tasks,
rubella and autism (Chess, 1977; Chess, Kern, albeit, not as severe as usually associated with
& Fernandez, 1971). The findings derive from autism.
a systematically studied large sample of
children with congenital rubella and the obser- GENETIC PARTITIONING OF AUTISM
vations have been supported by other investi-
gators. However, it is noteworthy that the Genetic findings throughout internal medicine
follow-up showed that the course of apparent have made it clear that, whether dealing with
autism in these children tended, on the whole, single gene conditions or multifactorial disor-
to be rather different from that associated with ders, genetic heterogeneity must be expected.
idiopathic autism. In particular, although the Accordingly, there have been various attempts
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 437

to determine whether such heterogeneity can also seemed a possibility in the Le Couteur and
be indexed by clinical features. colleagues (1996) monozygotic twins study.
However, there needs to be caution in the inter-
Familial Clustering pretation of this finding because of the peak age
of onset of epilepsy in individuals with autism
Familial clustering could provide important being late adolescence. This means, inevitably,
clues in this connection. Le Couteur and col- that many younger children will be misclassi-
leagues (1996) used the strategy of comparing fied as not having epilepsy when, in reality, they
phenotypic variations within and between are due to develop epilepsy when older. The
monozygotic pairs to examine the question. It similarity in age within pairs will influence
was argued that variation within each monozy- clustering for epilepsy much more than it will
gotic pair could not index genetic heterogene- for features that are manifest from the preschool
ity because both twins must share all of the years onward.
same genes. By contrast, there is every reason Epilepsy aside, the evidence on familial
to suppose that different pairs of monozygotic clustering has not been particularly infor-
twins will vary genetically as much as any mative on possible indices of genetic hetero-
other population of individuals with autism. geneity. The IMGSAC study (Bailey, personal
Variation within real monozygotic pairs was communication, 2004) found no clustering for
compared with the variation within pairs cre- the degree of language delay ( but this finding
ated statistically by having the pair made up of is inevitably influenced by inclusion /exclusion
one twin from one pair and one twin from an- criteria used in the study). Nevertheless, the
other, different, pair. The findings were strik- lack of clustering does cast doubt on any hy-
ing, and surprising, in showing that, within the pothesis that autism and Asperger syndrome
monozygotic pairs that were concordant for are genetically distinct ( because the diagnostic
autism, there was as much variation in symp- criteria usually employed specify a lack of
tom severity and pattern and in cognitive level significant language delay for the diagnosis of
as that found within these pseudo-pairs that Asperger syndrome). However, although lan-
had been created statistically. For example, in guage delay as such does not seem to show
one true monozygotic pair there was an IQ dif- any marked familial clustering, the sib pair
ference of over 50 points. It was evident that, studies have shown a significant (although not
even when the susceptibility genes were ex- marked) tendency for sibs in each pair to be
actly the same, very wide phenotypic variation more similar on their degree of abnormalities
was still possible. The findings provided few on social reciprocity, communication, and
clues on possible clinical indices of genetic repetitive behavior than are unrelated individu-
heterogeneity. als with autism. Both verbal and performance
Inevitably, the sample size was small and the IQ measures similarly showed some familial
same issues can be examined on much larger aggregation in the IMGSAC study (Bailey, per-
numbers by using affected sib pairs. Although sonal communication, 2004), although not in
it cannot necessarily be assumed that the sus- the Spiker et al. (1994) study. The findings on
ceptibility genes for autism will be the same in repetitive stereotyped behavior were, however,
two pairs of siblings (whereas that could be somewhat different in that familial aggregation
assumed with monozygotic twin pairs), it is was not influenced by IQ or language-related
certainly likely that the genetic heterogeneity measures whereas the other features were in-
within affected sib pairs would be substantially fluenced by language levels.
less than that in the population as a whole. Rele- In all the studies, the cases included have
vant studies have been undertaken by Spiker and had to meet specified diagnostic criteria for
colleagues (1994), Silverman and colleagues autism or for a broader concept of autism
(2002), and IMGSAC (Bailey, personal commu- spectrum disorder. Frequently, too, there has
nication, 2004). The familial question was also been exclusion of individuals with profound
studied by Szatmari and colleagues (1996, mental retardation. There are very good prac-
2000). In the IMGSAC study, the strongest indi- tical reasons for molecular genetic studies to
cation of familial clustering was for epilepsy; adopt rigorous standardized diagnostic criteria
although the sample size was very small, this for inclusion and exclusion but it is important
438 Neurological and Medical Issues

to appreciate that this will have implications velopmental regression are almost certainly
for findings on familial clustering. Thus, for heterogeneous. At one extreme, there is the fre-
example, if the genetic liability to autism rep- quent phenomenon of children who gain just a
resents a continuously distributed risk dimen- few words of vocabulary and then subsequently
sion, familial clustering would be more lose this minimal amount of speech. In such
appropriately examined without exclusions. cases, there are inevitable doubts about the re-
Equally, if autism associated with profound ality of the developmental regression. At the
mental retardation is genetically different, fa- other extreme, there are cases of children who
milial clustering is likely to show this only if gained substantial language and who then
cases of autism across the whole IQ distribu- lost well-established language skills. The origi-
tion are included. For the moment, it is diffi- nal version of the ADI-R (Lord, Rutter, & Le
cult to go much beyond the rather general Couteur, 1994) provided a somewhat uncertain
conclusion that there is some tendency for af- differentiation of patterns of developmental
fected relatives in the same family to be more regression. The current version (Le Couteur,
alike in their autistic features, and in their Lord, & Rutter, 2003; Rutter, Le Couteur, &
cognitive functioning, than unrelated individu- Lord, 2003) provides much better assessment
als with autism, but neither the pattern nor the but it has been in use for too short a time
extent of clustering gives clear guidance on to produce data on regression in large samples.
how autism spectrum disorders might better Second, although comparisons of multiplex
be subdivided. cases and singletons have often been used as
a way of subdividing groups according to
Multiplex-Singleton Comparisons: strength of genetic influence, it constitutes a
Developmental Regression methodologically weak approach (Rutter et al.,
1990) particularly with a relatively uncommon
Recently, strong claims have been made that disorder such as autism. When most nuclear
use of the MMR vaccine leads to an unusual families are quite small, there are bound to be
variety of autism that is especially character- many singleton cases that would have shown a
ized by developmental regression (Torrente familial loading if the families had been larger
et al., 2002; Wakefield et al., 2000, 2002). As (Eaves, Kendler, & Schulz, 1986). Accordingly,
already noted, the epidemiological evidence although the conclusion clearly must be that
provides no support for this claim. However, it there is no evidence that cases of autism with
could still be the case that autism involving de- developmental regression are etiologically dis-
velopmental regression might be etiologically tinct, the inference is necessarily a relatively
distinct. As already noted in the findings from weak one.
the IMGSAC study (Parr et al., poster, 2002),
there was no familial loading for regression. Linkage Evidence
Another approach to the question is provided
by comparison between multiplex cases and For obvious reasons, the starting point for
singletons. The rationale is that in families in most molecular genetic studies of psychiatric
which there are two affected siblings the disorders has been the traditional diagnostic
autism may have a stronger genetic component concept. However, it is entirely possible that
than cases in which there is only one affected individual genes will provide a susceptibility,
family member. Parr and colleagues found that not for the syndrome as a whole, but rather for
the rate of regression in the large IMGSAC some components of it. Thus, some findings
sample of multiplex families was closely com- suggested that this might be the case in rela-
parable with that reported in previous studies tion to different components of dyslexia (Grig-
of singletons. The finding indicates that, in orenko et al., 1997) although subsequent
cases where strong genetic influences may be research findings have raised queries (Fisher
inferred, there is no reduced rate of regression. et al., 1999, 2002). In relation to autism, some
Thus, the hypothesis that cases of autism with studies have found stronger evidence for link-
regression represent an environmentally caused age in autism relative pairs with evidence of
subgroup has no empirical support. However, language delay or a family history of language
two caveats are necessary. First, cases of de- delay (Alarcón et al., 2002; Bradford et al.,
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 439

2001; Buxbaum et al., 2001; Shao, Raiford, Fremolle, 1999; Lainhart, Piven, Wzorek, &
et al., 2002; Shao, Wolpert, et al., 2002), al- Landa, 1997; Woodhouse et al., 1996). The po-
though this was not found in the IMGSAC tential importance for subdividing cases of
study. Alarcón and colleagues (2002) applied autism is provided by the fact that in both cases,
nonparametric multipoint linkage analyses to although the correlate is a common one, it is far
the three main traits derived from the Autism from universal. Accordingly, it would appear
Diagnostic Interview, and concluded that there worthwhile to examine linkage findings sepa-
may be separate quantitative trait loci for lan- rately according to the presence or absence of
guage and for stereotyped behavior—both on either of these features (Veenstra-VanderWeele
chromosome 7. At present, the evidence is far et al., 2002). The situation is complicated, how-
too fragmentary for any firm conclusions. ever, by the fact that in both cases there is evi-
Nevertheless, the general strategy of either de- dence that the same biological features may
termining whether the linkage evidence is also be elevated in other family members (Cook
stronger in relation to particular phenotypi- & Leventhal, 1996).
cally different subgroups of individuals with Minor congenital anomalies or dysmorphic
autism, or looking for evidence of linkage in features of one kind or another may well con-
relation to particular dimensions or subcompo- stitute another useful differentiating feature
nents of autism, remain worthwhile strategies (Miles & Hillman, 2000). It appears that cases
(Folstein, Down, Mankoski, & Tadevosyan, of autism without major or minor congenital
2003). It would be similarly worthwhile to use anomalies have a much higher frequency of af-
cognitive measures as a way of subdividing fected relatives and also show a much stronger
autism—for example, according to the pres- male-female sex ratio.
ence or absence of severe mental retardation,
or the presence or absence of unusual special Epigenetic Mechanisms
talents or splinter skills.
Baron-Cohen (2003) has suggested that high
Neurocognitive Endophenotypes levels of prenatal testosterone may lead to an
exaggeration of masculine features and that
There has been increasing interest in the possi- autism might constitute, in effect, an extreme
bility of using cognitive findings to define of maleness (Baron-Cohen, 2002; Baron-Cohen,
endophenotypes that are not synonymous with Richler, Bisarya, Gurunathan, & Wheelwright,
the diagnostic symptoms pattern but which 2003). The starting point for this suggestion
may constitute the relevant genetically influ- was the marked male preponderance associ-
enced traits (Rutter, 2004). On this basis, ated with autism. The suggestion that autism
Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (2003) presented constitutes an extreme of maleness remains
evidence that there may be two different sub- highly speculative. Autism is far from the only
types in autism—one based on language abili- neurodevelopmental condition involving a male
ties and one based on IQ discrepancy scores. preponderance (Rutter, Caspi, & Moffitt,
The language deficit group was found, using 2003). Attention deficit / hyperactivity disor-
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI ), to have a der, dyslexia, and developmental language dis-
reversal of the usual brain asymmetry. The orders all similarly show a male excess.
group with a discrepantly high nonverbal IQ Nevertheless, it is possible that sex hormone
was found to have a large head size and large levels in the prenatal period affect gene ex-
brain volume, and more severe autism symp- pression in some manner, through epigenetic
toms. The subgroupings would seem likely to mechanisms. Although it is not at all likely
be useful in genetic analyses. that high levels of prenatal testosterone cause
autism, it is possible that they might have a
Biological Findings contributory role in conjunction with genetic
risk determined in other ways.
Two well-established biological correlates of There is a need to consider possible ge-
autism are elevated platelet serotonin (Cook & nomic imprinting; hence, possible loci for sus-
Leventhal, 1996) and increased head circum- ceptibility genes need to be considered with
ference (Fombonne, Rogé, Claverie, Courty, & respect to differential maternal and paternal
440 Neurological and Medical Issues

transmission (Reik & Walter, 2001). Also, 15q11–13. However, the genome screens
other epigenetic effects involving DNA methy- undertaken to date have failed to find any
lation may turn out to be important means by linkage in this region (Folstein & Rosen-
which as yet to be identified environmental in- Sheidley, 2001). Chromosome transloca-
fluences affect gene expression (Jaenisch & tions of various kinds have also been
Bird, 2003; Robertson & Wolffe, 2000). reported on chromosome 7 (see references
in Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley, 2001). How-
FUTURE DIRECTIONS ever, most have not concerned the area in
which linkage has been found, and, as with
Quantitative Trait Loci chromosome 15, they have not proved par-
(QTL) Approaches ticularly useful as guides to candidate
genes. The notion of using chromosome ab-
As already noted, the finding that the broader normalities as a lead is a reasonable one but
phenotype of autism probably occurs in as it has not had much success to date.
many as one in five first-degree relatives of in- 2. The second approach is provided by genes
dividuals with autism has raised the possibility that are concerned with one of the neuro-
that the susceptibility to autism may be based transmitters that might plausibly be involved
on a continuously distributed trait in the popu- in autism (see Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley,
lation that extends far beyond the syndrome of 2001). Although there have been some posi-
autism. Constantino and Todd (2003) reported tive findings, they have not been replicated
that autistic features, as measured by their and, again, leads have not proved as useful
social responsiveness scale, were continuously as was hoped.
distributed and were moderately to highly heri- 3. The third approach has been to select can-
table as judged by twin sample findings. Spiker didate genes on the basis of the combina-
and colleagues (Spiker, Lotspeich, Dimiceli, tion of their position near linkage signals
Myers, & Risch, 2002), showed much the same that have been found on genome scans and
(using the ADI-R) within a group of multiplex functions that might plausibly be related to
families with autism spectrum disorders. Sev- autism. Thus, this has applied to the reelin
eral of the major autism research groups in the gene and the FOXP2 gene, among a variety
world are currently involved in developing mea- of others (see Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley,
sures of different facets of autism that can pro- 2001). The two genes known to be associ-
vide a differentiation of individuals who do not ated with tuberous sclerosis have also been
have the syndrome of autism as such. It is likely thought to provide possible pointers.
that QTL analyses may provide a useful addi-
tional strategy in molecular genetic studies. In Unfortunately, so far, none of these approaches
that connection, as well as studying the distri- has paid off in the case of autism, but the
bution of autistic traits in all family members, strategies remain potentially worthwhile.
there is something to be said for studying ex-
tremely discordant sib pairs in which one has Subdivisions within Autism
the full syndrome of autism and the other com-
pletely lacks any autistic features (Risch & The potential value of considering molecular
Zhang, 1995, 1996). genetic strategies in relation to possibly mean-
ingful subgroups within autism has already
Leads for Candidate Genes been noted in the section dealing with genetic
dissection.
Three main approaches have been followed in
selecting possible candidate genes for autism: Animal Models and Studies of
Postmortem Brain Tissues
1. Attention has been paid to the location of
chromosomal abnormalities. As noted ear- Just as genetic findings can provide invaluable
lier, the best evidence concerns the mater- leads for other forms of biological investiga-
nal duplications found on chromosome tion, so biological findings can provide useful
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 441

leads for genetic research. This is most obvi- conflicting evidence, this remains more uncer-
ously the case with respect to evidence from tain. Chromosome anomalies of one kind or an-
animal models and from the study of post- other are found in some 5% to 10% of cases of
mortem brain tissues (see Bock & Goode, autism. In most cases, the causal significance
2003). Both areas of research, in relation to of these chromosome anomalies remains quite
autism, are in their infancy and no strong leads uncertain. Nevertheless, their potential impor-
for candidate genes have yet emerged. How- tance is sufficient for karyotyping of chromo-
ever, as there is further progress in these areas, somes to be a routine investigation in all cases
good leads may become available. of suspected autism. Possibly, too, this should
include a more detailed study of the imprinted
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS region of chromosome 15 using Fluorescence
In Situ Hybridization—(FISH).
Initially, Kanner (1943) had postulated that
autism was likely to be of constitutional origin. Genetic Counseling
In the years that followed, however, many clini-
cians and researchers came to believe that Given the strength of genetic influences on
autism was largely caused by “refrigerator par- autism, it is clearly essential that genetic coun-
enting” and other types of maladaptive up- seling be available to all families that want it
bringing (see Rutter, 1999a). This concept of (Simonoff, 1998; Simonoff & Rutter, 2002).
autism was later dropped—partly because of a The clinical issues involved are quite complex.
lack of evidence in support of environmental As with any other form of genetic counseling
causation and in part because of the growing (Simonoff, 2002), it is essential to make a
evidence in favor of the view of autism as a neu- careful diagnosis of the suspected autism
rodevelopmental disorder. The finding of strong spectrum disorder in the key individual whose
genetic influences on autism also played a part problem led to the need for genetic counseling.
in the demise of the refrigerator parent notions. In addition, however, it is essential to obtain a
But, more particularly, the genetic evidence led detailed and thorough family history followed,
to an appreciation that the unusual personality as necessary, by a detailed individual clinical
features seen in some parents might represent assessment of the possibly affected family
genetically influenced traits rather than envi- members. The main difficulty in this connec-
ronmental causation of autism. tion arises from the uncertainties as to exactly
what should, and should not, be included in the
Clinical Assessment and broader phenotype. This means that, unlike the
Medical Investigations situation with the genetic counseling needed
for Mendelian medical disorders, the counsel-
The genetic findings have also been crucial in ing needs (at least in the first instance) to be
making a systematic medical assessment of in- provided by clinicians who are expert in the
dividuals with autism standard practice. Thus, assessment of both autism spectrum disorders
it would now be mandatory to examine all and the broader phenotype pictures with which
children suspected of autism for possible indi- they are associated.
cations of tuberous sclerosis, followed by the The second issue is the need to help families
appropriate medical investigations when there understand the difference between the absolute
were positive findings from clinical examina- risk and the relative risk of autism spectrum
tion. Although the fragile X anomaly accounts disorders and associated conditions. Thus,
for only a very small proportion of cases of sticking with autism spectrum disorders, the
autism, it is important that the anomaly is available evidence suggests that the absolute
identified when present because of its implica- rate in siblings is about 6%. In other words, the
tions for family counseling. Accordingly, DNA absolute likelihood of autism being present in a
methods need to be used in all cases to deter- second child in the same family is quite low.
mine whether the fragile X anomaly is present. This is so despite the fact that the risk relative
Possibly, the same may apply to the MECP2 to the general population is very high—some
gene for Rett syndrome, although in view of the 20 to 50 times increased, the specific figure
442 Neurological and Medical Issues

depending a bit on what assumptions are made sumed that if there is a particularly heavy fa-
about the general population base rate. Family milial loading for either autism as such or for
members need to be helped to understand how varieties of the broader phenotype, the recur-
this could happen as a result of autism develop- rence risk is likely to be greater. However, the
ing as a consequence of the inheritance of difficulties of being at all precise over the in-
several susceptibility genes, rather than the crease in risks stems from the inevitable unreli-
possession of one gene that leads fairly directly ability of familial loading figures that are
to autism. Thus, the counselor will need to ex- based on a relatively small number of relatives
plain how, although siblings share about half (Rutter et al., 1990) and uncertainties that de-
their segregating genes on average, the propor- rive from the difficulties in specifying just
tion of multiple gene combinations that they which social, communicative, and behavioral
share is very much lower than that. In other abnormalities in relatives are part of the
words, other family members are quite likely broader phenotype of autism, rather than due to
to have one or another of the various suscepti- something else (Bailey et al., 1998). Con-
bility genes associated with autism but they versely, if the autism is associated with some
may not have either enough of the susceptibil- reasonably clear-cut environmental risk factor,
ity genes or the necessary pattern of suscepti- it might be thought that the recurrence risk of
bility genes that leads to the syndrome as a autism should be lower than average in the gen-
whole. Two considerations complicate the ad- eral population. The difficulty here is in know-
vice that may be given. First, although the rate ing what is a true environmental cause and
of autism in siblings is about 6% overall, the effect. Thus, for example, if the autism is asso-
recurrence rate of autism (meaning the rate of ciated with particularly severe obstetric abnor-
autism in a second sibling following occur- malities that have been associated with
rence of autism in a previous sibling) may be neonatal problems, it would seem reasonable to
somewhat higher—possibly about 8%. Second, infer the probability of some environmentally
the risk that another child in the same family mediated causal influence. By contrast, how-
will have some variety of the broader pheno- ever, the mere presence of obstetric complica-
type is very much higher than the expectation tions or low birthweight or premature gestation
for the syndrome of autism. It is difficult to would not be sufficient on its own.
quantify in the absence of firm knowledge on The guidelines with respect to genetic coun-
the boundaries of the broader phenotype but it seling are, first, that the counselor should
is likely to be in the region of 20% rather than provide the family with as honest and well in-
6%. The available evidence suggests that these formed an account as is possible in the present
broader phenotype abnormalities are function- state of knowledge. This should include as clear
ally important (that is to say, they lead to diffi- a statement as possible about what is reasonably
culties in the children with them) but they are definite with respect to what is said and what is
much less handicapping than autism as such. much more uncertain (indicating why and how
These figures all refer to the average expec- the uncertainty arises). Second, the counselor’s
tations in relation to idiopathic autism. Clearly, job is to provide the family with the informa-
it is highly desirable to be able to individualize tion that is necessary for them to come to their
the expectations to provide much greater preci- own decision on whatever issue is being consid-
sion. This is straightforward enough in terms of ered. It is not acceptable for the counselor to at-
the obvious prior need to rule out specific con- tempt to push the family in one direction or
ditions such as the fragile X anomaly or tuber- another. Third, counseling must pay due atten-
ous sclerosis. However, it is much more tion to the ethical issues involved. For example,
difficult to individualize the expectations in it is not at all uncommon for the parents of an
cases of idiopathic autism. It is likely that if autistic child to want advice on the risks that
there are already two siblings in the family with an unaffected sibling will have a child with
autism, the recurrence risk in relation to a third autism. Unless the unaffected sibling (i.e., the
child is likely to be well above 8% but the evi- parent or potential parent of the grandchild) is
dence is lacking to be more precise than that. part of the consultation, it would be improper
On commonsense grounds, too, it must be pre- for advice to be given. Of course, it is entirely
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 443

proper for grandparents to be concerned about succeed eventually in identifying the neural
the risks for one of their grandchildren but ad- basis of autism. Identification of the suscepti-
vice should not be given without the agreement bility genes will not, of course, do that on its
and full involvement of the actual parents. own. Genes code for proteins and not for psy-
It is very common for families to ask chiatric disorders or behaviors (Rutter, 2004).
whether or not there is some genetic test that Many areas of science will be needed in delin-
could be done that would help particularize eating the indirect pathways leading from sus-
the recurrence risks that are involved. With ceptibility genes through effects on proteins
the exception of the testing for the fragile X and protein products, through physiological and
anomaly and the MECP2 gene (and testing neurochemical processes, and ultimately to the
for chromosome abnormalities), there are no proximal pathway that leads to the syndrome of
applicable genetic tests. However, even when autism (Rutter, 2000). This wide-ranging pro-
some susceptibility genes for idiopathic autism gram of research will need to consider how and
are found, there will still be marked limita- why there is a transition in some individuals
tions in what can be achieved by genetic from the broader phenotype to the more seri-
screening. The point is that with a multifactor- ously handicapping disorder of autism and why
ial disorder, the finding that someone has a autism and autism spectrum disorders are so
susceptibility gene does not translate easily much more frequent in males than in females
into a person-specific risk. That is because, (Rutter, Caspi, et al., 2003). If the susceptibil-
unlike the situation with a Mendelian single ity genes are concerned, not with autism as
gene disorder, the risks are probabilistic and such, but with continuously distributed risk
may well vary according to other background characteristics or subcomponents of autism,
genetic factors, as well as being possibly con- there will be the further need to sort out why
tingent on the co-occurrence of some impor- and how they come together to constitute the
tant environmental risk factors. The findings syndrome as a whole (Bock & Goode, 2003).
on the APOE4 gene and Alzheimer’s disease
well illustrate the problem. The risk for Alz- Prevention and Treatment
heimer’s disease if someone is homozygous for
the APOE4 allele is quite strong but it consti- The ultimate goal, of course, is that this knowl-
tutes neither a necessary nor a sufficient cause edge on the neural basis of autism, together
(Liddell, Williams, & Owen, 2002). There are with a parallel understanding of the mode of
individuals with the APOE4 who will not de- operation of identified environmental risk fac-
velop Alzheimer’s disease no matter how long tors, will enable the development of new meth-
they live and there are many individuals with- ods of prevention and intervention that will be
out the APOE4 gene who will nevertheless de- much more effective than anything that we have
velop Alzheimer’s disease. Also, for reasons available today. To what extent knowledge on
that remain ill understood, the risk varies the pathophysiology of autism will in fact lead
across ethnic groups (Kalaria, 2003). Whether to effective methods of prevention or interven-
or not the same will apply with susceptibility tion will, inevitably, depend on just what that
genes for autism remains quite unknown but it pathophysiology comprises. Nevertheless, at the
is important to be realistic that, even when moment, it remains a puzzle that there is every
susceptibility genes have been found, there reason to suppose that autism constitutes a sys-
will be considerable difficulties in translating temwide disorder rather than being the result of
the findings into a person-specific risk. That is some localized brain lesion, but yet neurochem-
particularly so when, at present, we know so ical investigations have been so inconclusive
little about environmental risk factors. and the results of pharmacological treatments
so extremely disappointing.
Neural Basis for Autism
CONCLUSION
The real potential value of genetic research in
autism lies in the probability that it will provide At present, the clinical payoff from genetic re-
invaluable leads for biological studies that will search has been quite modest and it remains to
444 Neurological and Medical Issues

be seen just what it will deliver, but there is for allelic association on chromosome 3q25–27
every reason to suppose that susceptibility in families with autism spectrum disorders
genes for autism will be identified during the originating from a subisolate of Finland. Molec-
next decade (probably much earlier than that) ular Psychiatry, 8, 879–884.
and that ultimately the biological understand- Badner, J. A., & Gershon, E. S. (2002). Regional
meta-analyses of published data supports link-
ing, which should follow from the studies to
age of autism with markers on chromosome 7.
which this identification will give rise, will Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 56–66.
transform clinical practice in the field of Bailey, A., Bolton, P., Butler, L., Le Couteur, A.,
autism spectrum disorders in ways that should Murphy, M., Scott, S., et al. (1993). Prevalence
be beneficial for children and their families. of the fragile X anomaly amongst autistic
twins and singletons. Journal of Child Psychol-
Cross-References ogy and Psychiatry, 34, 673–688.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P.,
Issues of diagnosis are discussed in Chapters 1 Simonoff, E., Yuzda, F. Y., et al. (1995).
and 3 through 6, epidemiology is discussed in Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evi-
dence from a British twin study. Psychological
Chapter 2, other neurobiological issues are dis-
Medicine, 25, 63–77.
cussed in Chapters 17 through 20.
Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Cou-
teur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in rel-
REFERENCES atives. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 28, 369–392.
Alarcón, M., Cantor, R. M., Liu, J., Gilliam, T. C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A.,
The Autism Genetic Resource Exchange Con- Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., et al. (2000).
sortium, & Geschwind, D. (2002). Evidence for A screening instrument for autism at 18 months
a language quantitative trait locus on chromo- of age: A 6-year follow-up study. Journal of the
some 7q in multiplex autism families. American American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 60–71. Psychiatry, 39, 694–702.
Amir, R. E., van den Veyver, I. B., Schultz, R., Mal- Barcellos, L. F., Klitz, W., Field, L. L., Tobias, R.,
icki, D. M., Tran, C. Q., Dahle, E. J., et al. Bowcock, A. M., Wilson, R., et al. (1997). As-
(2000). Influence of mutation type and X chro- sociation mapping of disease loci, by use of a
mosome inactivation on Rett syndrome pheno- pooled DNA genomic screen. American Jour-
types. Annals of Neurology, 47, 670–679. nal of Human Genetics, 61, 734–747.
Amir, R. E., van den Veyver, I. B., Wan, M., Tran, Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain
C. Q., Francke, U., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (1999). theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X- 6, 248–254.
linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential dif ference:
protein 2. Nature Genetics, 23, 185–188. Men, women and the extreme male brain. Lon-
Ardlie, K. G., Lunetta, K. L., & Seielstad, M. don: Allen Lane.
(2002). Testing for population subdivision and Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gu-
association in four case control studies. Amer- runathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The
ican Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 304–311. systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults
Ashley-Koch, A., Wolpert, C. M., Menold, M. M., with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
Zaeem, L., Basu, S., Donnelly, S. L., et al. autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophi-
(1999). Genetic studies of autistic disorder cal Transactions of the Royal Society, London,
and chromosome 7. Genomics, 61, 227–236. B., 358, 361–374.
August, G. J., Stewart, M. A., & Tsai, L. (1981). Barrett, S., Beck, J. C., Bernier, R., Bisson, E.,
The incidence of cognitive disabilities in the Braun, T. A., Casavant, T. L., et al. (1999). An
siblings of autistic children. British Journal of autosomal genomic screen for autism: Collab-
Psychiatry, 138, 416–422. orative linkage study of autism. American
Auranen, M., Vanhala, R., Varilo, T., Ayers, K., Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric
Kempas, E., Ylisaukko-Oja, T., et al. (2002). Genetics, 88, 609–615.
A genomewide screen for autism-spectrum Bernard, S., Enayati, A., Redwood, L., Roger, H.,
disorders: Evidence for a major susceptibility & Binstock, T. (2001). Autism: A novel form
locus on chromosome 3q25–27. American of mercury poisoning. Medical Hypotheses, 56,
Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 777–790. 462–471.
Auranen, M., Varilo, T., Alen, R., Vanhala, R., Betancur, C., Leboyer, M., & Gillberg, C. (2002).
Ayers, K., Kempas, E., et al. (2003). Evidence Increased rate of twins among affected sibling
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 445

pairs with autism. American Journal of Human polymorphism and autism. Molecular Psychia-
Genetics, 70, 1381–1383. try, 7, 311–316.
Beyer, K. S., Blasi, F., Bacchelli, E., Klauck, S. M., Buxbaum, J. D., Silverman, J. M., Smith, C. J., Kil-
Maestrini, E., Poustka, A., et al. (2002). Mu- ifarski, M., Reichert, J., Hollander, E., et al.
tation analysis of the coding sequence of the (2001). Evidence for a susceptibility gene for
MECP2 gene in infantile autism. Human Ge- autism on chromosome 2 and for genetic het-
netics, 111, 305–309. erogeneity. American Journal of Human Ge-
Bishop, D. V. M., Maybery, M., Wong, D., Maley, netics, 68, 1514–1520.
A., Hill, W., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are Cardon, L. R., & Palmer, L. J. (2003). Population
phonological processing deficits part of the stratification and spurious allelic association.
broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Lancet, 361, 598–604.
Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, Carney, R. M., Wolpert, C. M., Ravan, S. A., Shah-
1283, 54–60. bazian, M., Ashley-Kioch, A., Cuccaro, M. L.,
Bock, G., & Goode, J. (Eds.). (2003). Autism: et al. (2003). Identification of MECP2 muta-
Neural basis and treatment possibilities (No- tions in a series of females with autistic disor-
vartis Foundation Symposium 251). Chich- der. Pediatric Neurology, 28, 205–211.
ester, England: Wiley. Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive
Bolton, P. F., & Griffiths, P. D. (1997). Associa- developmental disorders in preschool children.
tion of tuberous sclerosis of temporal lobes Journal of the American Medical Association,
with autism and atypical autism. Lancet, 349, 285, 3093–3099.
392–395. Chess, S. (1977). Follow-up report on autism in con-
Bolton, P. F., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., genital rubella. Journal of Autism and Child-
Goode, S., Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case- hood Schizophrenia, 7, 69–81.
control family history study of autism. Jour- Chess, S., Kern, S. J., & Fernandez, P. B. (1971).
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, Psychiatric disorders of children with congeni-
877–900. tal rubella. New York: Brunner/ Mazel.
Bolton, P. F., Murphy, M., Macdonald, H., Whit- Clarkson, T. W. (1997). The toxicology of mercury.
lock, B., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (1997). Ob- Critical Review of Clinical Laboratory Science,
stetric complications in autism: Consequences 34, 369–403.
or causes of the condition? Journal of the Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (in
American Academy of Child and Adolescent press). Developmental language disorders: A
Psychiatry, 36, 272–281. follow-up in later adult life: Cognitive, lan-
Bolton, P. F., Park, R. J., Higgins, J. N., Griffiths, guage and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of
P. D., & Pickles, A. (2002). Neuro-epileptic Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
determinants of autism spectrum disorders Collaborative Linkage Study of Autism. (1999). An
in tuberous sclerosis complex. Brain, 125, autosomal genomic screen for autism. Ameri-
1247–1255. can Journal of Medical Genetics, 88, 609–615.
Bolton, P., Pickles, A., Murphy, M., & Rutter, M. Cohen, P., Cohen, J., & Brook, J. S. (1995). Bring-
(1998). Autistic, affective and other psychi- ing in the sheaves, or just gleaning? A
atric disorders: Patterns of familial aggrega- methodological warning. International Jour-
tion. Psychological Medicine, 28, 385–395. nal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5,
Bonora, E., Beyer, K. S., Lamb, J. A., Parr, J. R., 263–266.
Klauck, S. M., Benner, A., et al. (2003). Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic
Analysis of reelin as a candidate gene for traits in the general population: A twin study.
autism. Molecular Psychiatry, 8, 885–892. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 524–530.
Bradford, Y., Haines, J., Hutcheson, H., Gardiner, Cook, E. H., & Leventhal, B. L. (1996). The sero-
M., Braun, T., Sheffield, V., et al. (2001). In- tonin system in autism. Current Opinion in Pe-
corporating language phenotypes strengthens diatrics, 8, 348–354.
evidence of linkage to autism. American Jour- Dales, L., Hammer, S. J., & Smith, N. J. (2001).
nal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Ge- Time trends in autism and in MMR
netics, 105, 539–547. immunization coverage in California. Journal
Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Are of the American Medical Association, 285,
there autistic-like features in congenitally 1183–1185.
blind children? Journal of Child Psychology Daniels, J., Holmans, J., Williams, N., Turic, D.,
and Psychiatry, 38, 693–703. McGuffin, P., Plomin, R., et al. (1998). A sim-
Buxbaum, J. D., Silverman, J. M., Smith, C. J., ple method for analyzing microsatellite allele
Greenberg, D. A., Kilifarski, M., Reichert, J., image patterns generated from DNA pools and
et al. (2002). Association between a GABRB3 its application to allelic association studies.
446 Neurological and Medical Issues

American Journal of Human Genetics, 62, geneous disorder. Nature Reviews: Genetics, 2,
1189–1197. 943–955.
Davis, S., Schroeder, M., Goldin, L. R., & Weeks, Folstein, S. E., & Rutter, M. (1977a). Genetic in-
D. E. (1996). Nonparametric simulation-based f luences and infantile autism. Nature, 265,
statistics for detecting linkage in general pedi- 726–728.
grees. American Journal of Human Genetics, Folstein, S. E., & Rutter, M. (1977b). Infantile
58, 867–880. autism: A genetic study of 21 pairs. Journal of
DeLong, R., & Nohria, C. (1994). Psychiatric fam- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 297–321.
ily history and neurological disease in autistic Folstein, S. E., & Rutter, M. (1988). Autism: Fa-
spectrum disorders. Developmental Medicine milial aggregation and genetic implications.
and Child Neurology, 36, 441–448. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Donnelly, S. L., Wolpert, C. M., Menold, M. M., ders, 18, 3–30.
Bass, M. P., Gilbert, J. R., Cuccaro, M. L., Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism:
et al. (2000). Female with autistic disorder and A review. Psychological Medicine, 29, 769–786.
monosomy X (Turner syndrome): Parent-of- Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of
origin effect of the X chromosome. American autism and other pervasive developmental dis-
Journal of Medical Genetics, 96, 312–316. orders: An update. Journal of Autism and De-
Dykens, M., & Volkmar, F. (1997). Medical condi- velopmental Disorders, 33, 365–382.
tions associated with autism. In D. J. Cohen & Fombonne, E., Bolton, P., Prior, J., Jordan, H., &
F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and Rutter, M. (1997). A family study of autism:
pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., Cognitive patterns and levels in parents and
pp. 388–410). New York: Wiley. siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Eaves, L. J., Kendler, K. S., & Schulz, S. C. (1986). chiatry, 38, 667–683.
The familial-sporadic classification: Its power Fombonne, E., & Chakrabarti, S. (2001). No evi-
for the resolution of genetic and environmental dence for a new variant of measles-mumps-
etiologic factors. Journal of Psychiatric Re- rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics, 108, E58.
search, 20, 115–130. Fombonne, E., Rogé, B., Claverie, J., Courty, S., &
Farrington, C. P., Miller, E., & Taylor, B. (2001). Fremolle, J. (1999). Microcephaly and macro-
MMR and autism: Further evidence against a cephaly in autism. Journal of Autism and De-
causal association. Vaccine, 19, 3632–3635. velopmental Disorders, 29, 113–119.
Fisher, S. E., Francks, C., Marlow, A. J., MacPhie, Gharani, N., Benayed, R., Mancuso, V., Brzustow-
I. L., Newbury, D. F., Cardon, L. R., et al. icz, L. M., & Millonig, J. H. (2004). Associa-
(2002). Independent genome-wide scans identify tion of the homeobox transcription factor,
a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influ- ENGRAILED 2, with autism spectrum disorder.
encing dyslexia. Nature Genetics, 30, 86–91. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 474–484.
Fisher, S. E., Marlow, A. J., Lamb, J., Maestrini, E., Gillberg, C. (1989). The borderland of autism and
Williams, D. F., Richardson, A. J., et al. (1999). Rett syndrome: Five case histories to highlight
A quantitative-trait locus on chromosome 6p diagnostic difficulties. Journal of Autism and
influences different aspects of developmental Developmental Disorders, 19, 545–559.
dyslexia. American Journal of Human Genetics, Gillberg, C. (1990). Medical work-up in children
64, 146–156. with autism and Asperger syndrome. Brain
Folstein, S. E., Down, M., Mankoski, R., & Tade- Dysfunction, 3, 249–260.
vosyan, O. (2003). How might genetic mecha- Gillberg, C. (1992). Autism and autism-like condi-
nisms operate in autism? In G. Bock & J. tions: Sub-classes among disorders of empathy.
Goode (Eds.), Autism: Neural basis and treat- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33,
ment possibilities (Novartis Foundation Sym- 813–842.
posium 251, pp. 70–83). Chichester, England: Gillberg, C. (1998). Chromosomal disorders and
Wiley. autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Folstein, S. E., & Mankoski, R. E. (2000). Chromo- Disorders, 28, 415–425.
some 7q: Where autism meets language disor- Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1996). Autism and
der? American Journal of Human Genetics, 67, medical disorders: A review of the literature.
278–281. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
Folstein, S. E., & Piven, J. (1991). Etiology of 38, 191–202.
autism: Genetic inf luences. Pediatrics, 87, Gillberg, C., & Forsell, C. (1984). Childhood psy-
767–773. chosis and neurofibromatosis—more than a
Folstein, S. E., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Genet- coincidence? Journal of Autism and Develop-
ics of autism: Complex aetiology for a hetero- mental Disorders, 14, 1–8.
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 447

Gillberg, C., & Wahlstrom, J. (1985). Chromosome Hodge, S. E., Greenberg, D. A., Betancur, C., &
abnormalities in infantile autism and other Gillberg, C. (2002). Response to Visscher.
childhood psychoses: A population study of 66 American Journal of Human Genetics, 71,
cases. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu- 996–999.
rology, 27, 293–304. Honda, H., Shimizu, Y., & Rutter, M. (submitted)
Greenberg, D. A., Hodge, S. E., Sowinski, J., & No ef fect of MMR withdrawal on the incidence
Nicoll, D. (2001). Excess of twins among af- of autism: A total population study.
fected sibling pairs with autism: Implications Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000).
for the etiology of autism. American Journal of Autism and developmental receptive language
Human Genetics, 69, 1062–1067. disorder: A comparative follow-up in early
Grigorenko, E. L., Wood, F. B., Meyer, M. S., Hart, adult life: II. Social behavioural, and psychi-
L. A., Speed, W. C., Shuster, A., et al. (1997). atric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology
Susceptibility loci for distinct components of and Psychiatry, 41, 561–578.
developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and Hviid, A., Stellfeld, M., Wohlfahrt, J., & Melbye, M.
15. American Journal of Human Genetics, 60, (2003). Association between Thimerosal-
27–39. containing vaccine and autism. Journal of the
Gurling, H. M. D., Bolton, P. F., Vincent, J., American Medical Association, 290, 1763–1766.
Melmer, G., & Rutter, M. (1997). Molecular International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism
and cytogenetic investigations of the fragile X Consortium. (1998). A full genome screen for
region including the FRAX and FRAX E CGG autism with evidence for linkage to a region on
trinucleotide repeat sequences in families chromosome 7q. Human Molecular Genetics,
multiplex for autism and related phenotypes. 7, 571–578.
Human Heredity, 47, 254–262. International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism
Gutknecht, L. (2001). Full-genome scans with Consortium. (2001a). Further characteriza-
autistic disorder: A review. Behavior Genetics, tion of the autism susceptibility locus AUTS1
31, 113–123. on chromosome 7. Human Molecular Genetics,
Guy, J., Hendrich, B., Holmes, M., Martin, J. E., & 10, 973–982.
Bird, A. (2001). A mouse MECP2-null muta- International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism
tion causes neurological symptoms that mimic Consortium. (2001b). A genomewide screen for
Rett syndrome. Nature Genetics, 27, 322–326. autism: Strong evidence for linkage to chromo-
Hagberg, B. (1985). Rett’s syndrome: Prevalence somes 2q, 7q and 16p. American Journal of
and impact on progressive severe mental retar- Human Genetics, 69, 570–581.
dation in girls. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, Jaenisch, R., & Bird, A. (2003). Epigenetic regula-
74, 405–408. tion of gene expression: How the genome inte-
Hagberg, B., Aircardi, J., Dias, K., & Ramos, O. grates intrinsic and environmental signals.
(1983). A progressive syndrome of autism, de- Nature Genetics Supplement, 33, 245–254.
mentia, ataxia and loss of purposeful hand use Jamain, S., Betancur, C., Quach, H., Philippe, A.,
in girls: Rett syndrome; report of 35 cases. An- Fellous, M., Giros, B., et al. (2002). Linkage
nals of Neurology, 14, 471–479. and association of the glutamate receptor 6
Hallmayer, J., Glasson, E. J., Bower, C., Petterson, gene with autism. Molecular Psychiatry, 7,
B., Croen, L., Grether, J., et al. (2002). On 302–310.
the twin risk in autism. American Journal of Jamain, S., Quach, H., Quintana-Murci, L., Betan-
Human Genetics, 71, 941–946. cur, C., Philippe, A., Gillberg, C., et al. (2002).
Hallmayer, J., Hebert, J. M., Spiker, D., Lotspeich, Y chromosome haplogroups in autistic subjects.
L., McMahon, W. M., Petersen, P. B., et al. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 217–219.
(1996). Autism and the X chromosome: Multi- Jensen, A. R. (1997). The puzzle of nongenetic
point sib-pair analysis. Archives of General variance. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grig-
Psychiatry, 53, 980–983. orenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and envi-
Hanson, D. R., & Gottesman, I. (1976). The genet- ronment (pp. 42–88). Cambridge, England:
ics, if any, of infantile autism and childhood Cambridge University Press.
schizophrenia. Journal of Autism and Schizo- Kalaria, R. N. (2003). Dementia comes of age in
phrenia, 6, 209–233. the developing world. Lancet, 361, 888–889.
Hillman, R. E., Kanafani, N., Takahashi, T. N., & Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec-
Miles, J. H. (2000). Prevalence of autism in tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Missouri: Changing trends and the effect of a Kaye, J. A., Melero-Montes, M., & Jick, H. (2001).
comprehensive state autism project. Missouri Mumps, measles, and rubella vaccine and
Medicine, 97, 159–163. the incidence of autism recorded by general
448 Neurological and Medical Issues

practitioners: A time trend analysis. British loci. American Journal of Human Genetics, 69,
Medical Journal, 322, 460–463. 327–340.
Kim, S.-J., Herzing, L. B. K., Veenstra-Vander- Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J.,
Weele, J., Lord, C., Couchesne, R., Leventhal, Jordan, H., & Mawhood, L. (1989). Autism Di-
B. L., et al. (2002). Mutation screening and agnostic Observation Schedule: A standard-
transmission disequilibrium study of ATP10C ized observation of communicative and social
in autism. American Journal of Medical Genet- behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 114, 137–143. Disorders, 19, 185–212.
Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994).
F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: A re-
gene is mutated in a severe speech and lan- vised version of a diagnostic interview for care-
guage disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523. givers of individuals with possible pervasive
Lainhart, J., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., & Landa, R. developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
(1997). Macrocephaly in children and adults Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
with autism. Journal of the American Academy Lotter, V. (1966). Epidemiology of autistic condi-
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, tions in young children: 1. Prevalence. Social
282–290. Psychiatry, 1, 124–137.
Lamb, J. A., Moore, J., Bailey, A., & Monaco, A. P. Madsen, K. M., Hviid, A., Vestergaard, M.,
(2000). Autism: Recent molecular genetic ad- Schendel, D., Wohlfahrt, J., Thorsen, P., et al.
vances. Human Molecular Genetics, 9, 861–868. (2002). A population-based study of measles,
Lamb, J. A., Parr, J. R., Bailey, A. J., & Monaco, mumps, and rubella vaccination and autism.
A. P. (2002). Autism: In search of susceptibil- New England Journal of Medicine, 347,
ity genes. Neuromolecular Medicine, 2, 11–28. 1477–1482.
Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Gayle, J. O., Madsen, K. E., Lauritsen, M. B., Pedersent, C. B.,
Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social Thorsen, P., Plesner, A.-M., Andersen, P. H.,
language use in parents in autistic individuals. et al. (2003). Thimerosal and the occurrence
Psychological Medicine, 22, 245–254. of autism: Negative ecological evidence from
Landa, R., Wzorek, M., Piven, J., Folstein, S. E., & Danish population-based data. Pediatrics, 112,
Isaacs, C. (1991). Spontaneous narrative dis- 604–606.
course characteristics of parents of autistic Maestrini, E., Marlow, A. J., Weeks, D. E., &
individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re- Monaco, A. P. (1998). Molecular genetic stud-
search, 34, 1339–1345. ies of autism. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Lauritsen, M., & Ewald, H. (2001). The genetics of mental Disorders, 28, 427–437.
autism. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 103, Malhotra, A. K., & Goldman, D. (1999). Benefits
411–427. and pitfalls encountered in psychiatric genetic
Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A. J., Goode, S., Pickles, association studies. Biological Psychiatry, 45,
A., Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., et al. (1996). 544–550.
A broader phenotype of autism: The clinical Medical Research Council. (2001). MRC review of
spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychol- autism research: Epidemiology and causes.
ogy and Psychiatry, 37, 785–801. London: Author.
Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). Miles, J. H., & Hillman, R. E. (2000). Value of a
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los An- clinical morphology examination in autism.
geles: Western Psychological Services. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 91,
Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., 245–253.
Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989). Molenaar, P. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Dolan, C. V.
Autism Diagnostic Interview: A standardized (1993). A third source of developmental dif-
investigator-based interview. Journal of Autism ferences. Behavior Genetics, 23, 519–524.
and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387. Murphy, M., Bolton, P., Pickles, A., Fombonne, E.,
Liddell, M. B., Williams, J., & Owen, M. J. (2002). Piven, J., & Rutter, M. (2000). Personality
The dementias. In P. McGuffin, M. J. Owen, & traits in the relatives of autistic probands. Psy-
I. I. Gottesman (Eds.), Psychiatric genetics chological Medicine, 30, 1411–1424.
and genomics (pp. 341–393). Oxford, England: Myrianthopoulos, N. C., & Melnick, M. (1977).
Oxford University Press. Malformations in monozygotic twins: A pos-
Liu, J., Nyholt, D. R., Magnussen, P., Parano, E., sible source of environmental inf luence on
Pavone, P., Geschwind, D., et al. (2001). A the developmental genetic clock. In E. Inouye
genomewide screen for autism susceptibility & H. Nishimusa (Eds.), Gene-environment
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 449

interactions in common diseases (pp. 206–220). Piven, J., Gayle, J., Chase, J., Fink, B., Landa, R.,
Baltimore: University Park Press. Wrozek, M., et al. (1990). A family history
Nelson, K. B. (1991). Prenatal and perinatal factors study of neuropsychiatric disorders in the
in the etiology of autism. Pediatrics, 87, adult siblings of autistic individuals. Journal
761–766. of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Newbury, D. F., Bonora, E., Lamb, J. A., Fisher, cent Psychiatry, 29, 177–183.
S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Baird, G., et al. (2002). Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in
FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for parents from multiple-incidence autism fami-
autism or specific language impairment. Ameri- lies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
can Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 1318–1327. try, 38, 1011–1022.
Nurmi, E. L., Amin, T., Olson, L. M., Jacobs, Piven, J., Palmer, P., Jacobi, D., Childress, D., &
M. M., McCauley, J. L., Lam, A. Y., et al. Arndt, S. (1997). The broader autism pheno-
(2003). Dense linkage disequilibrium mapping type: Evidence from a family study of multiple-
in the 15q11-q13 maternal expression domain incidence autism families. American Journal of
yields evidence for association in autism. Mo- Psychiatry, 154, 185–190.
lecular Psychiatry, 8, 624–634. Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Landa, R., Lainhart, J.,
Olsson, B., & Rett, A. (1985). Behavioural obser- Bolton, P., Chase, G. A., et al. (1994). Personal-
vations concerning differential diagnosis be- ity characteristics of the parents of autistic in-
tween the Rett’s syndrome and autism. Brain dividuals. Psychological Medicine, 24, 783–795.
and Development, 7, 281–289. Pocock, S. J. (1983). Clinical trials: A practical ap-
Olsson, B., & Rett, A. (1987). Autism and Rett proach. Chichester, England: Wiley.
syndrome: Behavioural investigations and dif- Pritchard, J. K. (2001). Are rare variants responsi-
ferential diagnosis. Developmental Medicine ble for susceptibility to complex diseases?
and Child Neurology, 29, 429–441. American Journal of Human Genetics, 69,
Parr, J. R., Baird, G., Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., 124–137.
Fombonne, E., Bailey, A. J., et al. (poster) Ramoz, N., Reichert, J. G., Smith, C. J., Silverman,
Autism regression in a genetically related J. M., Bespalova, I. N., Davis, K. L., et al.
sample. (2004). Linkage and association of the
Philippe, A., Martinez, M., Guilloud-Bataille, M., mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier
Gillberg, C., Rastam, M., Sponheim, E., et al. SLC25A12 gene with autism. American Jour-
(1999). Genome-wide scan for autism suscep- nal of Psychiatry, 161, 662–669.
tibility genes. Human Molecular Genetics, 8, Reik, W., & Walter, J. (2001). Genomic imprinting:
805–812. Parental inf luence on the genome. Nature Re-
Pichichero, M. E., Cernichiari, E., Lopreiato, J., & views: Genetics, 2, 21–32.
Treanor, J. (2002). Mercury concentrations Reiss, A. L., & Dant, C. C. (2003). The behavioral
and metabolism in infants receiving vaccines neurogenetics of fragile X syndrome: Analyz-
containing Thiomersal: A descriptive study. ing gene-brain-behavior relationships in child
Lancet, 360, 1737–1741. developmental psychopathologies. Develop-
Pickles, A., Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Bailey, A., ment and Psychopathology, 15, 927–968.
Le Couteur, A., Sim, L., et al. (1995). Latent Risch, N., & Merikangas, K. (1996). The future of
class analysis of recurrence risk for complex genetic studies of complex human diseases.
phenotypes with selection and measurement Science, 273, 1516–1517.
error: A twin and family history study of Risch, N., Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L., Nouri, N.,
autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, Hinds, D., Hallmayer, J., et al. (1999). A ge-
57, 717–726. nomic screen of autism: Evidence for a multi-
Pickles, A., Starr, E., Kazak, S., Bolton, P., Pa- locus etiology. American Journal of Human
panikolau, K., Bailey, A. J., et al. (2000). Genetics, 65, 493–507.
Variable expression of the autism broader phe- Risch, N., & Zhang, H. (1995). Extreme discordant
notype: Findings from extended pedigrees. sib pairs for mapping quantitative trait loci in
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, humans. Science, 268, 1584–1589.
41, 491–502. Risch, N., & Zhang, H. (1996). Mapping quantita-
Piven, J., Chase, G. A., Landa, R., Wzorek, M., tive trait loci with extreme discordant sib pairs:
Gayle, J., Cloud, D., et al. (1991). Psychiatric Sampling considerations. American Journal of
disorders in the parents of autistic individuals. Human Genetics, 58, 836–843.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Roberts, S. B., MacLean, C. J., Neale, M. C., Eaves,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 471–478. L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1999). Replication of
450 Neurological and Medical Issues

linkage studies of complex traits: An examina- Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003).
tion of variation in location estimates. Ameri- ADI-R Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised.
can Journal of Human Genetics, 65, 876–884. Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological
Robertson, K. D., & Wolffe, A. P. (2000). DNA Services.
methylation in health and disease. Nature Re- Rutter, M., Silberg, J., O’Connor, T., & Simonoff,
views: Genetics, 1, 11–19. E. (1999). Genetics and Child psychiatry: I.
Rodier, P., & Hyman, S. (1998). Early environmen- Advances in quantitative and molecular genet-
tal factors in autism. Mental Retardation and ics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Developmental Disorders Research Review, 4, try, 40, 3–18.
121–128. Shahbazian, M. D., Young, J. I., Yuva-Paylor, L. A.,
Rutter, M. (1967). Psychotic disorders in early child- Spencer, C., Antalffy, B., Noebels, J., et al.
hood. In A. J. Coppen & A. Walk (Eds.), Recent (2002). Mice with truncated MeCP2 recapitu-
developments in schizophrenia (pp. 133–158). late many Rett syndrome features and display
Ashford, Kent, England: Headley Bros/RMPA. hyperacetylation of histone H3. Neuron, 35,
Rutter, M. (1968). Concepts of autism: A review of 243–254.
research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Shao, Y. J., Cuccaro, M. L., Hauser, E. R., Raiford,
chiatry, 9, 1–25. K. L., Menold, M. M., Wolpert, C. M., et al.
Rutter, M. (1999a). The Emanuel Miller Memor- (2003). Fine mapping of autistic disorder to
ial Lecture 1998. Autism: Two-way interplay chromosome 15q11-q13 by use of phenotypic
between research and clinical work. Journal subtypes. American Journal of Human Genet-
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, ics, 72, 539–548.
169–188. Shao, Y. J., Raiford, K. L., Wolpert, C. M., Cope,
Rutter, M. (1999b). Psychosocial adversity and C. A., Ravan, S. A., Ashley-Koch, A. A., et al.
child psychopathology. British Journal of Psy- (2002). Phenotypic homogeneity provides in-
chiatry, 174, 480–493. creased support for linkage on chromosome 2 in
Rutter, M. (2000). Genetic studies of autism: From autistic disorder. American Journal of Human
the 1970s into the millennium. Journal of Ab- Genetics, 70, 1058–1061.
normal Child Psychology, 28, 3–14. Shao, Y. J., Wolpert, C. M., Raiford, K. L., Menold,
Rutter, M. (2004). Pathways of genetic influences on M. M., Donnelly, S. L., Ravan, S. A., et al.
psychopathology. European Review, 12, 19–33. (2002). Genomic screen and follow-up analysis
Rutter, M. (in press). Autism research: Lessons for autistic disorder. American Journal of Med-
from the past and prospects for the future. Acta ical Genetics, 114, 99–105.
Paediatrica. Silverman, J. M., Smith, C. J., Schmeidler, J., Hol-
Rutter, M., Anderson-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bre- lander, E., Lawlor, B. A., Fitzgerald, M., et al.
denkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (2002). Symptom domains in autism and re-
(1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following severe lated conditions: Evidence for familiality.
early global privation. Journal of Child Psy- American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neu-
chology and Psychiatry, 40, 537–549. ropsychiatric Genetics, 114, 64–73.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A. Simonoff, E. (1998). Genetic counseling in autism
(1994). Autism and known medical conditions: and pervasive developmental disorders. Jour-
Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
ogy and Psychiatry, 35, 311–322. 28, 447–456.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Simonoff, E., & Pickles, A. Simonoff, E. (2002). Genetic counseling. In
(1997). Genetic inf luences and autism. In D. J. M. Rutter & E. Taylor (Eds.), Child and ado-
Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of lescent psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 1101–1113).
autism and pervasive developmental disorders Oxford, England: Blackwell.
(2nd ed., pp. 370–387). New York: Wiley. Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (2002). Autism and
Rutter, M., Bolton, P., Harrington, R., Le Couteur, other behavioral disorders. In D. L. Rimoin,
A., Macdonald, H., & Simonoff, E. (1990). J. M. Connor, R. E. Pyeritz, & B. R. Korf
Genetic factors in child psychiatric disorders: (Eds.), Emery and Rimoin’s principles
I. A review of research strategies. Journal of and practice of medical genetics (4th ed.,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 3–37. Vol. 3, pp. 2873–2893). London: Churchill
Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Mak- Livingstone.
ing sense of sex differences in psychopathol- Skuse, D. H. (2000). Imprinting, the X chromo-
ogy: Unifying issues and research strategies. some, and the male brain: Explaining sex dif-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, ferences in the liability to autism. Pediatric
1092–1115. Research, 47, 9–16.
Genetic Inf luences and Autism 451

Skuse, D. H. (2003). X-linked genes and the neural Szatmari, P., MacLean, J. E., Jones, M. B., Bryson,
basis of social cognition. In G. Bock & S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bartolucci, G., et al.
J. Goode (Eds.) Autism: Neural basis and treat- (2000). The familial aggregation of the lesser
ment possibilities (Novartis Foundation Sympo- variant in biological and nonbiological rela-
sium 251, pp. 84–108). Chichester, England: tives of PDD probands: A family history study.
Wiley. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Skuse, D. H., James, R., Bishop, D., Coppihn, B., 41, 579–586.
Dalton, P., Aamodt-Leeper, G., et al. (1997). Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identi-
Evidence from Turner’s syndrome of an im- fying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism.
printed X-linked locus affective cognitive Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci-
functioning. Nature, 387, 705–708. ety, London, B., 358, 303–314.
Smalley, S. L. (1998). Autism and tuberous sclero- Taylor, B., Miller, E., Farrington, C. P., Petropoulos,
sis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- M. C., Favot-Mayaud, I., Li, J., et al. (1999).
orders, 28, 407–414. Autism and measles, mumps, and rubella vac-
Smalley, S. L., McCracken, J. T., & Tanguay, P. E. cine: No epidemiological evidence for a causal
(1995). Autism, affective disorders and social association. Lancet, 353, 2026–2029.
phobia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Taylor, B., Miller, E., Lingam, R., Andrews, N., Sim-
60, 19–26. mons, A., & Stowe, J. (2002). Measles, mumps,
Spielman, R. S., & Ewens, W. J. (1996). Invited and rubella vaccination and bowel problems
editorial: The TDT and other family-based or developmental regression in children with
tests for linkage disequilibrium and associa- autism: Population-based study. British Medical
tion. American Journal of Human Genetics, Journal, 324, 393–396.
59, 983–989. Torrente, F., Ashwood, P., Day, R., Machado, N.,
Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L. J., Dimiceli, S., Myers, Furlano, R. I., Anthony, A., et al. (2002).
R. M., & Risch, N. (2002). Behavioral pheno- Small intestinal enteropathy with epithelial
typic variation in autism multiplex families: IgG and complement deposition in children
Evidence for a continuous severity gradient. with regressive autism. Molecular Psychiatry,
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114, 7, 375–382.
129–136. Tsai, L. (1987). Pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors in
Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L. J., Kraemer, H. C., Hall- autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.),
mayer, J., McMahon, W., Petersen, P. B., Neurobiological issues in autism (pp. 179–189).
et al. (1994). Genetics of autism: Characteris- New York: Plenum Press.
tics of affected and unaffected children from Uchiyama, T., Kurosawa, M., & Inaba, Y. (submit-
37 multiplex families. American Journal of ted) Does MMR vaccine cause so-called “re-
Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, gressive autism”?
54, 27–35. Veenstra-VanderWeele, J., Kim, S.-J., Lord, C.,
Starr, E., Berument, S. K., Pickles, A., Tomlins, M., Courchesne, R., Akshoomoff, N., Leventhal,
Bailey, A., Papanikolau, E., et al. (2001). A B. L., et al. (2002). Transmission disequilib-
family genetic study of autism associated with rium studies of the serotonin 5-HT2A receptor
profound mental retardation. Journal of Autism gene (HTR2A) in autism. American Journal of
and Developmental Disorders, 32, 89–96. Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics),
Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C., Hellgren, L., Anders- 114, 277–283.
son, L., Gillberg, I. C., Jakobsson, G., et al. Verstraeten, T., Davis, R. L., DeStefano, F., Lieu,
(1989). A twin study of autism in Denmark, Fin- T. A., Rhodes, P. H., Black, S. B., et al. (2003).
land, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. Journal of Safety of Thimerosal-containing vaccines: A
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 405–416. two-phased study of computerized health
Stratton, K., Gable, A., & McCormick, M. C. maintenance organization databases. Pedi-
(Eds.). (2001). Immunization safety review: atrics, 112, 1039–1048.
Thimerosal-containing vaccines and neurode- Visscher, P. M. (2002). Increased rate of twins
velopmental disorders. Washington, DC: Na- among affected sib pairs. American Journal of
tional Academy Press. Human Genetics, 71, 995–996.
Szatmari, P., Jones, M. B., Holden, J., Bryson, Vogel, F., & Motulsky, A. G. (1997). Human genet-
S. E., Mahoney, W. J., Tuff, L., et al. (1996). ics: Problems and approaches (3rd ed.). Berlin,
High phenotypic correlations among siblings Germany: Springer-Verlag.
with autism and pervasive development disor- Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991). Comorbid as-
ders. American Journal of Medical Genetics: sociation of autism and schizophrenia. Ameri-
Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 67, 354–360. can Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1704–1707.
452 Neurological and Medical Issues

Vourc’h, P., Bienvenu, T., Beldjord, C., Chelly, J., of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17,
Barthélémy, C., Müh, J. P., et al. (2001). No 149–150.
mutations in the coding region of the Rett syn- Woodhouse, W., Bailey, A., Rutter, M., Bolton, P.,
drome gene MECP2 in 59 autistic patients. Eu- Baird, G., & Le Couteur, A. (1996). Head
ropean Journal of Human Genetics, 9, 556–558. circumference in autism and other pervasive
Wakefield, A. J., Anthony, A., Murch, S. H., Thom- developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psy-
son, M., Montgomery, S. M., Davies, S., et al. chology and Psychiatry, 37, 785–801.
(2000). Enterocolitis in children with develop- Yirmiya, N., Pilowsky, T., Tidhar, S., Nemanov, L.,
mental disorders. American Journal of Gas- Altmark, L., & Ebstein, R. P. (2002). Family-
troenterology, 95, 2285–2295. based and population study of a functional
Wakefield, A. J., Murch, S. H., Anthony, A., Linnell, promoter-region monoamine oxidase A poly-
J., Casson, D. M., Malik, M., et al. (1998). Ileal- morphism in autism: Possible association
lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific with, I. Q. American Journal of Medical Genet-
colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder ics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 114, 284–287.
in children. Lancet, 351, 637–641. Yonan, A. L., Alarcón, M., Cheng, R., Magnusson,
Wakefield, A. J., Puleston, J. M., Montgomery, S. M., P. K. E., Spence, S. J., Palmer, A. A., et al.
Anthony, A., O’Leary, J. J., & Murch, S. H. (2003). A genomewide screen of 345 families
(2002). Review article: The concept of entero- for autism-susceptibility loci. American Jour-
colonic encephalopathy, autism and opioid nal of Human Genetics, 73, 886–897.
recepter ligands. Alimentary Pharmacology Yu, C.-E., Dawson, G., Munson, J., D’Souza, I., Os-
Therapeutics, 16, 663–674. terling, J., Estes, A., et al. (2002). Presence of
Weber, A. M., Egelhoff, J. C., McKellop, J. M., & large deletions in kindreds with autism. Amer-
Franz, D. N. (2000). Autism and the cerebel- ican Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 100–115.
lum: Evidence from tuberous sclerosis. Journal Zoghbi, H. Y. (2003). Postnatal neurodevelopmen-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, tal disorders: Meeting at the synapse? Science,
511–517. 302, 826–830.
Witt-Engerström, I., & Gillberg, C. (1987). Letter
to the editor: Rett syndrome in Sweden. Journal
CHAPTER 17

Neurochemical Studies of Autism

GEORGE M. ANDERSON AND YOSHIHIKO HOSHINO

The behavioral, emotional, and cognitive prognostic utility in the future. In this chapter,
symptoms presented by autistic individuals we deal primarily with neurochemical studies
clearly indicate that central nervous system measuring levels of neurotransmitters; their
(CNS) functioning is altered in autism. The metabolities; and associated enzymes in blood,
early onset, pervasive nature, and chronicity of urine, or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Separate
autism also point directly to brain abnormality. sections on neurochemistry cover, in order,
Furthermore, twin and family studies strongly each of the three major central monoamine
suggest that autism has a genetic basis (Cook, neurotransmitters: serotonin, dopamine, and
2001; Folstein & Piven, 1991; Lauritsen & norepinephrine. Following sections concern re-
Ewald, 2001; Lotspeich & Ciaranello, 1993; search on the hypothalamic-pituitary axis, the
Rutter & Schopler, 1987; J. Young, Newcorn, & neuropeptides, amino acids, and acetylcholine,
Leven, 1989; see Chapter 16). and the purines and related compounds.
Neurochemical studies of autistic individu- Our review of these neurochemical and
als have been undertaken to examine processes related neuroendocrine studies concentrates
related to neural transmission in the central and on recent findings and attempts to point out the
peripheral nervous systems. The search for neu- more promising areas for research. A number
rochemical alterations and causes in autism is of prior reviews have covered the biochemical
given impetus by the rapid advance of basic research of autism either exclusively (Ander-
neuroscience and the success of neuropharma- son, 1987, 2002; Anderson, Horne, Chatterjee,
cology in the relatively specific treatment of a & Cohen, 1990; Cohen & Young, 1977; Cook,
range of neurological and psychiatric disorders 1990; DeMyer et al., 1981; McBride, Ander-
and symptoms. There is an increasing recogni- son, & Mann, 1990; Ritvo, 1977; J. G. Young,
tion that autism is polygenetic and heteroge- Kavanagh, Anderson, Shaywitz, & Cohen,
netic, and that its neurobiology may be best 1982; Yuwiler, Geller, & Ritvo, 1985) or as
approached by examining the component and part of more general reviews (Anderson &
continuous traits that combine in a particular Cohen, 2002).
individual to produce autism (Anderson &
Cohen, 2002; Folstein & Rosen-Sheidley, 2001; SEROTONIN
McBride, Anderson, & Shapiro, 1996). Neuro-
chemical and neuroendocrine measures offer Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine; 5-HT) is
promising endophenotypes for investigation. an important neurotransmitter in the central
A wide range of neurotransmitter and neu- nervous system, where it is involved in con-
roendocrine systems have been examined. This trolling a number of important functions
examination, which we review in detail, has and behaviors, including sleep, mood, body
made its main goal the elucidation of etiology; temperature, appetite, and hormone release
but it also has been carried on in the hope that (Iverson & Iverson, 1981; Lucki, 1998). Cell
particular measures might have diagnostic and bodies of most central neurons utilizing 5-HT

453
454 Neurological and Medical Issues

as a neurotransmitter are located in the mid- attempted to examine 5-HT synthesis rates
brain; however, the neurons make connections in autism (Chugani et al., 1999); however
throughout the brain and spinal cord. Sero- methodological issues (Shoaf et al., 2000)
tonin is synthesized from its amino acid pre- make interpretation of reported differences
cursor, tryptophan (TRP), by hydroxylation difficult. Ongoing imaging studies of 5-HT re-
and decarboxylation, it is predominately me- ceptor density in brain regions, as well as post-
tabolized to 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5- mortem brain studies examining 5-HT-related
HIAA), by the enzyme monoamine oxidase neurochemistry, will provide valuable new
(MAO). perspectives on 5-HT neurobiology in autism.
Serotonin is the neurotransmitter that has The neurochemical studies, along with those
stimulated the most neurochemical research in examining the metabolism as well as the be-
autism. Initial interest in the possible role of havioral and neuroendocrine effects of the
5-HT in autism arose from a consideration of its 5-HT precursors, TRP and 5-hydroxytrypto-
role in perception. The powerful effects of sero- phan (5-HTP), are reviewed.
tonergic hallucinogens, such as lysergic acid
diethylamide (LSD), stimulated speculation Blood 5-HT
around 5-HT and led to early studies of platelet
5-HT in autism (Schain & Freedman, 1961). Al- The greatest number of 5-HT studies in autism
though much of the work has focused on the concern the measurement of blood levels of 5-
platelet hyperserotonemia of autism, a number HT. A general consensus has been reached,
of other observations have contributed to dating from Schain and Freedman’s original
the increasing interest in 5-HT. Reports of a observation in 1961, that group mean levels of
critical role for 5-HT during embryogenesis blood (platelet) 5-HT are increased in autism.
(Buznikov, 1984) and in the development of Much of the 5-HT-related research in autism
the central nervous system (Janusonis, Gluncic, has been directed toward further characteriz-
& Rakic, 2004; Waage-Baudet et al., 2003; ing the elevation and attempting to elucidate
Whitaker-Azmitia, 2001; Whitaker-Azmitia, the causes. In a more recent study of a large
Druse, Walker, & Lauder, 1996; Zhou, Auer- and relatively homogenuous group, it was re-
bach, & Azmitia, 1987) have made 5-HT of spe- ported that platelet 5-HT is bimodally distrib-
cial interest in neurodevelopmental disorders. uted in autism and Pervasive Developmental
Early studies of serotonergic drugs as pos- Disorder (PDD; Mulder et al., 2004). It now
sible therapeutic agents were not particularly appears that hyperserotonemia can be ratio-
promising. The 5-HT-releasing agent fenflu- nally defined and that approximately half of
ramine, despite initial enthusiasm, also has not individuals with PDD can be placed in this cat-
been found to be of much use in treating autis- egory. Careful examination of those hyper-
tic symptoms. However, a number of small serotonmic individuals in the upper mode
treatment studies of 5-HT selective reuptake should facilitate research in this area.
inhibitors—including clomipramine, fluvox- A major line of research has focused on try-
amine, and fluoxetine—have suggested that ing to identify the physiological mechanism of
manipulation of the serotonergic system may the elevation. Serotonin in blood derives from
be of some benefit (Posey & McDougle, 2000). that synthesized in the wall of the gut; it is
Candidate gene studies and linkage analyses, stored in platelets while circulating and is ca-
while not definitive, have suggested that 5-HT- tabolized to 5-HIAA by monoamine oxidase
related genes, and especially the 5-HT trans- (MAO) after uptake into lung, liver, and capil-
porter gene (HTT) may have some association lary endothelium (Anderson, Stevenson, &
with disorder risk or symptom expression Cohen, 1987). These aspects of blood 5-HT,
(Cook, 2001). and the factor(s) that might cause the increase
To assess central and peripheral 5-HT func- in autism, have been discussed in detail (An-
tion in autism, researchers have measured CSF derson, 2002; Anderson et al., 1990; Hanley,
and urine levels of the major metabolite of 5- Stahl, & Freedman, 1977).
HT, 5-HIAA, and blood and urine levels of 5- Research on the platelet storage of 5-HT
HT itself. Initial brain imaging studies have has been extensive. At first it appeared that
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 455

there might be differences between normal (Minderaa, Anderson, Volkmar, Akkerhuis, &
and autistic subjects in terms of the number of Cohen, 1987). Furthermore, no correlation be-
platelets (Ritvo et al., 1970) and in platelet ef- tween urine 5-HIAA and whole blood 5-HT lev-
flux of 5-HT (Boullin et al., 1971). However, els was observed in autistic or normal subjects,
it appears that these platelet indices, as well although hyperserotonemic autistic individuals
as the number of platelet 5-HT uptake sites, may have had slightly higher urine levels of
are normal in autism (Anderson, Minderaa, 5-HIAA compared to other autistic subjects or
van Bentem, Volkmar, & Cohen, 1984; Boullin to normals. These data regarding 5-HIAA sug-
et al., 1982; Yuwiler et al., 1975). A study gest that normal amounts of 5-HT are produced
of hyperserotonemic relatives of children with in autistic individuals. In a subsequent study, no
autism found some suggestive differences in differences in urinary excretion of 5-HT itself
platelet 5-HT uptake and the numbers of were seen between autistic and control subjects
platelet 5-HT-type 2 receptors in subgroups of (Anderson, Minderaa, Cho, Volkmar, & Cohen,
the relatives (Cook et al., 1993). 1989), and in other related studies no group dif-
It should also be noted that no differences ferences were seen for free plasma levels of 5-
in platelet levels of the catabolic enzyme HT (Cook, Leventhal, & Freedman, 1988).
MAO have been found in autism (Giller et al., Taken together, these observations indicate that
1980; J. G. Young et al., 1982). Unfortunately, the platelet of autistic individuals is exposed to
because 5-HT is principally metabolized by normal levels of 5-HT. This in turn suggests
MAO-A rather than the form found in platelets, that there is an alteration in the platelet’s han-
MAO-B, these studies of MAO are not defini- dling of 5-HT, at least in the hyperserotonemic
tive. Studies of 5-HT synthesis include those subgroup.
examining urine levels of 5-HIAA and 5-HT,
and those in which TRP was administered. Tryptophan Metabolism

Urine 5-HIAA and 5-HT Tryptophan, an essential amino acid, is the di-
etary precursor of 5-HT and of the vitamin
Because most 5-HT produced in the body is nicotinic acid. It has been shown that the level
eventually metabolized to and excreted as of TRP in the brain is determined to some ex-
5-HIAA (Udenfriend, Titus, Weissbach, & Pe- tent by plasma levels of free (nonprotein-
terson, 1959), urine levels of 5-HIAA are a bound) TRP. Hoshino, Yamamoto, et al. (1984)
good indicator of the rate of 5-HT synthesis, at determined plasma free and total TRP levels
least as long as routes of metabolism and elimi- and blood serotonin levels simultaneously and
nation are not altered significantly. There have reported that both plasma free TRP and blood
been relatively few studies of urine 5-HIAA 5-HT levels were significantly higher in autis-
excretion in autistic subjects. One major study tic children than in normal control subjects. In
reported elevated levels (6.08 versus 3.23 addition, there tended to be a significant cor-
mg/day) of 5-HIAA in autistic subjects com- relation between the plasma free TRP level
pared to mentally retarded individuals (Hanley and several clinical rating scales in autistic
et al., 1977). In addition, a greater increase in children, although there was no correlation be-
5-HIAA was seen for autistic subjects after a tween blood 5-HT and free TRP levels in these
TRP load (12.95 versus 6.52 mg/day). Two pre- children. In contrast, Anderson, Volkmar,
vious studies (Partington, Tu, & Wong, 1973; et al. (1987) reported that whole blood TRP
Schain & Freedman, 1961) had not detected concentrations tended to be slightly ( but not
differences in urine 5-HIAA excretion between significantly) lower in unmedicated autistics
autistic and normal individuals, although in one compared to normal controls, while Takatsu,
of the studies hyperserotonemic autistic sub- Onizawa, and Nakahato (1965) had previously
jects did have elevated urine 5-HIAA levels. reported that total plasma TRP was reduced in
Urinary excretion of 5-HIAA in a group of autism.
individuals with autism who were not receiving Several investigators have attempted to
medication was observed to be very similar demonstrate metabolic alterations in the
to that seen in an age-matched control group serotonin metabolism of autistic children by
456 Neurological and Medical Issues

administering large oral doses of L-trypto- normal baseline levels of plasma prolactin and
phan (L-TRP). Schain and Freedman (1961) have observed an apparently normal increase in
performed TRP (one gram) loading tests in prolactin after chronic treatment with
autistic and mildly retarded children but found dopamine blockers (Minderaa et al., 1989). In
no differences in blood 5-HT and urinary 5- a detailed study of the neuroendocrine re-
HIAA concentration between the two groups. sponse to the serotonergic agent fenfluramine,
On the other hand, Hanley et al. (1977) re- McBride and colleagues (1989) found that
ported that TRP (1g) loading raised urinary autistic subjects had a blunted prolactin re-
5-HT levels in hyperserotonemic autistic chil- sponse (with normal baseline prolactin levels).
dren but lowered urinary 5-HT levels in mildly This was interpreted to suggest that central 5-
retarded children having normal levels of HT type-2 receptor functioning might be re-
blood serotonin. In both groups the TRP load duced on autism. Simultaneous studies of the
caused a slight decrease of blood 5-HT and a responsivity of the platelet 5-HT type-2 recep-
marked increase of urinary 5-HIAA excretion. tor also showed a blunted response in the autis-
In a more recent study (Cook et al., 1992) an tic subjects. Results from neuroimaging and
oral TRP load was not observed to increase postmortem brain studies of 5-HT type-2 re-
blood 5-HT levels in relatives of autistic indi- ceptors will be of special interest, given these
viduals. However, depletion of plasma TRP by reports.
the use of an amino acid drink has been re-
ported to exacerbate symptoms in autism (Mc- CSF 5-HIAA
Dougle et al., 1996).
Levels of 5-HIAA and other monoamine
Neuroendocrine Studies of metabolites have been widely measured in CSF
Serotonergic Functioning in order to estimate brain turnover of the par-
ent neurotransmitters (Garelis, Young, Lal, &
Several groups (Hoshino, Tachibana, et al., Sourkes, 1974). Nearly all 5-HT is metabolized
1984; Hoshino et al., 1983; Sverd, Kupretz, to 5-HIAA before elimination from the brain,
Winsberg, Hurwic, & Becker, 1978) have ex- and a substantial route for egress of brain
amined the effect of L-5-HTP on serotonin me- 5-HIAA is through the CSF (Aizenstein &
tabolism and hypothalamo-pituitary function Korf, 1979; Meek & Neff, 1973). It is clear that
in autistic children. Hoshino and colleagues ad- certain drugs and treatments known to affect
ministered L-5-HTP to autistic children and brain 5-HT turnover have corresponding ef-
normal controls and measured chronological fects on levels of CSF 5-HIAA (Kirwin et al.,
changes of blood serotonin, plasma human 1997; S. N. Young, Anderson, & Purdy, 1980),
growth hormone (HGH), and prolactin (PRL). and it has been shown that CSF 5-HIAA is not
After loading, blood serotonin showed a contaminated with 5-HT or 5-HIAA arising
smaller increase compared with normal con- elsewhere in the body. The close approach to
trols, although the baseline levels of blood the brain afforded by CSF measurements is at-
serotonin were significantly higher in autistic tractive; however, the invasiveness of the lum-
children. The levels of plasma HGH observed bar puncture required has limited the number
after 5-HTP-stimulated release were similar in of studies carried out in autistic individuals.
the groups studied, as were baseline HGH Three studies have been performed using
concentrations. However, lower baseline levels probenecid to block the transport of 5-HIAA
of prolactin and a blunted prolactin response and other acidic compounds out of CSF. In two
to 5-HTP were present in the autistic group of the studies, levels of 5-HIAA were observed
(Hoshino, Tachibana, et al., 1984; Hoshino to be similar (Cohen, Shaywitz, Johnson, &
et al., 1983). These results might be explained Bowers, 1974) or slightly lower (Cohen, Ca-
on the basis of diminished central serotonergic parulo, Shaywitz, & Bowers, 1977) in autistic
functioning or enhanced activity of tuberoin- subjects compared to nonautistic psychotic
fundibular dopamine neurons known to exert a children. In a third probenecid study, no con-
powerful inhibitory control on prolactin re- trol groups were used; however, a few of the
lease. In contrast, other researchers have found autistic subjects did not show the expected
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 457

increase in 5-HIAA after probenecid adminis- et al., 1974). In previously discussed studies of
tration (Winsberg, Sverd, Castells, Hurwic, & CSF 5-HIAA in autistic individuals, measure-
Perel, 1980). In studies of baseline levels of ments of HVA also were made. In two of the
CSF 5-HIAA, no significant differences have three studies using probenecid to block trans-
been observed between autistic and control port of the acid metabolites out of CSF (Cohen
subjects (Gillberg & Svennerholm, 1987; et al., 1974, 1977), no significant group differ-
Gillberg, Svennerholm, & Hamilton-Hellberg, ences were observed between autistic children
1983; Narayan, Srinath, Anderson, & Meundi, and various comparison groups. Comparison
1993). In summary, the CSF studies suggest groups included nonautistic psychotic (atypi-
that if central 5-HT metabolism is altered in cal), aphasic, motor disordered, and neurologi-
autism, the alteration does not involve a wide- cally disordered (contrast) children. In both
spread or marked change in 5-HT turnover. studies, CSF HVA did tend to be lower in autis-
tic children compared to nonautistic psychotic
DOPAMINE children, and in one of the studies (Cohen et al.,
1974), HVA values were reported to be lower in
Most dopamine (DA) containing neurons lie the more disturbed autistic individuals. A third
in the midbrain. Dopaminergic neurons appear study employing the probenecid technique did
to be especially important in the control of not include measurements made in comparison
motor function, in cognition, and in regulating groups; however, the increases in CSF HVA
hormone release. Dopamine is synthesized from seen after probenecid appeared normal (Wins-
the dietary amino acids, phenylalanine or tyro- berg et al., 1980).
sine, by hydroxylation and decarboxylation. In later CSF studies, probenecid was not ad-
Dopamine can be subsequently converted to ministered. In a study carried out in Sweden, the
norepinephrine and epinephrine by the action baseline, unperturbed, concentrations of CSF
of the enzymes dopamine-ß-hydroxylase and HVA were observed to be elevated approxi-
phenylethanolamine-N-methyltransferase. Once mately 50% in the autistic group compared to an
released from the neuron, DA is enzymatically age- and sex-matched control group of neurolog-
degraded by MAO and catechol-O-methyltrans- ically disordered children (Gillberg & Svenner-
ferase (COMT) to homovanillic acid (HVA) and holm, 1987; Gillberg et al., 1983). However, two
3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC). other studies of baseline CSF HVA in autism
The DA blockers (the neuroleptics or major have not seen significant elevations in autistic
tranquilizers) have been observed to be ef- individuals compared to controls (Narayan, Sri-
fective in treating some aspects of autism. nath, et al., 1993; Ross, Klykylo, & Anderson,
This, and the fact that certain symptoms of 1985). This question of whether CSF levels are
autism—such as stereotypies and hyperactiv- increased in autism has been the subject of
ity—can be induced in animals by increasing debate (Gillberg, 1993; Narayan, Srinath, et al.,
DA function, has suggested that central DA 1993; Narayan, Srinath, & Anderson, 1993).
neurons may be overactive in autism. Central Taken together, the CSF studies do not appear to
dopamine function has been assessed in hu- provide strong support for the idea that central
mans by several methods, including post- DA turnover is increased in autism. However, the
mortem measurements of DA, its metabolites, discrepancies between the studies suggest that
and receptors in brain tissue; positron emis- further research in this area is warranted.
sion tomography (PET scanning); CSF mea-
surements of HVA and DOPAC; and blood or Plasma and Urine Measures of
urine measures of DA, HVA, and DOPAC. Dopamine Function

CSF Homovanillic Acid Unfortunately, the relationship of peripheral


measures of DA, HVA, and DOPAC to central
Studies in humans and in animals have indi- DA function is unclear. Although at least some
cated that changes in central dopamine turnover of the HVA found in blood and urine arises
are reflected to some extent in CSF levels of the from the brain, the exact proportion has not
principal dopamine metabolite, HVA (Garelis been well established. It has been estimated
458 Neurological and Medical Issues

that approximately 25% of blood or urine HVA of endocrine function (plasma LH and FSH lev-
is of central origin (Elchisak, Polinsky, Ebert, els). In a study of the effects of L-Dopa on the
Powers, & Kopin, 1978; Maas, Hattox, Greene, secretion of growth hormone (HGH), Realmuto
& Landis, 1980). On the other hand, peripheral and colleagues (Realmuto, Jensen, Reeve, &
DA itself is known to arise almost completely Garfinkel, 1990) found that, while autistic sub-
from the adrenal, kidney, and the sympathetic jects had normal peak responses in plasma
nervous system, rather than from the brain. HGH, they had a delayed response compared to
In the one study of plasma HVA levels in controls.
autism, no differences were observed between
unmedicated autistic subjects and normal con- NOREPINEPHRINE
trols (Minderaa et al., 1989). Slight, nonsignifi-
cant increases in plasma HVA were seen in Norepinephrine (NE) is an important neuro-
autistics medicated with neuroleptics. In two transmitter in both the central nervous system
studies examining baseline plasma levels of (CNS) and in the peripheral sympathetic ner-
prolactin, a hormone under powerful tonic in- vous system. Central and peripheral NE is pro-
hibitory control by dopaminergic tuberinfidibu- duced from DA through the action of the
lar neurons, no group differences have been enzyme dopamine-ß-hydroxylase. Upon re-
seen (McBride et al., 1989; Minderaa et al., lease, most central NE is metabolized by MAO
1989). and COMT to 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol
Although several groups have reported that (MHPG), whereas peripheral NE is predomi-
the urinary excretion of HVA is increased in nantly converted to vanillylmandelic acid
autism, in a large study of urinary DA and HVA (VMA).
differences were not observed between autistic Most central NE-containing neurons have
and control groups in the rate of urinary ex- their cell bodies localized in one section of the
cretion of these compounds (Minderaa et al., hindbrain, the locus coeruleus. These neurons
1989). Studies of the catabolic enzyme COMT, project in a diffuse manner to many areas of
which along with MAO converts DA to HVA, the brain and spinal cord and are crucial in
have found similar activities in red blood cells processes related to arousal, anxiety, stress re-
of autistic and control subjects (Giller et al., sponses, and memory. Drugs that lessen central
1980; O’Brien, Semenuk, & Spector, 1976). A NE function, such as clonidine, have been used
study of CSF levels of one form of tetrahydro- to treat withdrawal symptoms (Redmond &
biopterin, a cofactor in the synthesis of DA, has Huang, 1979). Other agents that increase cen-
found lower levels in autistic subjects (Tani, tral noradrenergic functioning, such as yohim-
Fernell, Watanbe, Kanai, & Langstrom, 1994). bine and desipramine, increase arousal or serve
as antidepressants. Norepinephrine also serves
Neuroendocrine Studies of as the major neurotransmitter in postganglionic
Dopamine Functioning sympathetic nervous neurons. These neurons
serve to control autonomic functions and are
Ritvo et al. (1971) designed a study to assess balanced against cholinergic neurons that ener-
neurochemical, behavioral, and neuroendocrine vate the same organs. When sympathetic sys-
effects of L-Dopa administration. Following a tem activity predominates, the characteristic
17-day placebo period, four hospitalized autis- flight-or-fight response is elicited.
tic boys received the DA precursor, L-Dopa, Activity of central NE neurons has been as-
for 6 months. Results indicated a significant de- sessed by determining CSF levels of NE and
crease in blood 5-HT concentrations and a sig- MHPG. The CSF measures probably reflect
nificant increase in platelet counts. Urinary NE activity in the spinal cord, as well as in the
excretion of 5-HIAA decreased significantly in brain, and some small contribution of blood
one patient, and a similar trend was noted in MHPG to CSF MHPG has been demonstrated
others. However, no changes were observed in (Kopin, Gordon, Jimerson, & Polinsky, 1983).
the clinical course of the disorder, the amount Blood and urine levels of MHPG also have
of motility disturbances (stereotypic behavior), been measured in order to gauge central NE
percentages of REM sleep time, or in measures function; however, the proportion of MHPG in
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 459

these fluids that originates in the CNS relative according to whether they reflect basal func-
to that arising from NE released by sympa- tioning of the stress response systems or their
thetic neurons is not clear (Maas & Leckman, reactivity to acute stress, the results are actu-
1983). Other NE metabolites, VMA and ally quite consistent.
normetanephrine (NMN), along with MHPG, Overall, the CSF, plasma, and urine findings
have been widely measured in urine and indicate that baseline sympathetic and adrenal
plasma to assess activity of the sympathetic function is probably not greatly altered in
nervous system. autism; however, there is some indication that
The studies examining NE metabolism, in some autistic individuals the sympathetic
other measures of noradrenergic and adrener- nervous system may be hyper-responsive to
gic functioning, and indices of hypothalamic- stress. This notion is consistent with reports
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity have that clonidine may be of some benefit in treat-
produced a variety of results. However, as seen ing patients with autism (Frankhauser, Karu-
in Table 17.1, if the measures are grouped manchi, German, Yales, & Karumanchi, 1992).

TABLE 17.1 Stress Response Systems in Autism: Sympathetic/Adrenomedullary and Hypothalamic-


Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Function
Measure Finding* Reference
Measures of Basal Functioning
Urine
Norepinephrine NC Launay et al., 1987; Martineau et al., 1992; Minderaa et al., 1994.
↓ J. G. Young et al., 1978
↑ Barthelemy et al., 1988
Epinephrine NC Minderaa et al., 1994
MHPG NC Launay et al., 1987; Minderaa et al., 1994
↓ Barthelemy et al., 1988; J. G. Young, Cohen, Hattox, et al., 1981
VMA NC Minderaa et al., 1994
Cortisol NC Richdale & Prior, 1992

Plasma /Serum
MHPG NC Minderaa et al., 1994; J. G. Young, Cohen, Hattox, et al., 1981
DBH NC Lake et al., 1977; S. N. Young et al., 1980
Cortisol NC Tordjman et al., 1997; Nir et al., 1995; Sandman et al., 1990
↓ Curin et al., 2003; Hoshino, Ohno, et al., 1984; Jensen et al., 1985
Cerebrospinal Fluid
MHPG NC Gillberg & Svennerholm, 1987; J. G. Young, Cohen, Kavanaugh, et al., 1981

Measures of Acute Response


Plasma
Norepinephrine ↑ Lake et al.,1977; Leboyer et al., 1992; Leventhal et al., 1990
Adrenocorticotropin ↑ Tordjman et al.,1997
ß-Endorphin ↑ Tordjman et al.,1997
NC Herman, 1991; Weizman et al., 1988

Cardiovascular
BP/ heart rate ↑ Hirstein et al., 2001; Kootz & Cohen, 1981; S. Tordjman, personal comm., 2003
*Key: ↑ = Increased in autism; ↓ = Decreased in autism; DBH = Dopamine-ß-hyroxylase; MHPG = Methoxyhy-
droxyphenylethyleneglycol; NC = No change or difference in autism; VMA = Vanillylmandelic acid.
460 Neurological and Medical Issues

STUDIES OF THE HYPOTHALAMIC- variations in 11-OHCS levels in the autistic


PITUITARY-ADRENAL AXIS individuals.
Several groups have performed the dexam-
Research examining hypothalamic-pituitary ethasone suppression test (DST) in autistic sub-
(HPA) function in autism can be divided into jects. Hoshino, Ohno, et al. (1984) reported that
two main categories: a large number of studies after DST low-functioning (IQ < 60) autistics
have looked at HPA function in order to assess examined were nonsuppressors, whereas high-
the stress response, in another avenue of re- functioning autistic subjects had nearly normal
search HPA function has been studied in hopes suppression of cortisol secretion. Similarly, in a
of determining the functioning of neurochemi- group of 12 low-functioning autistic individu-
cal systems that are involved in the regulation als (IQ < 30), Jensen, Realmuto, and Garfinkel
of neuroendocrine secretion. The hypothalamic- (1985) found 10 were nonsuppressors. In both
pituitary-adrenal axis plays a critical role in the studies, baseline cortisol levels observed in the
stress response and is closely interrelated with autistic group were similar to those seen in
the sympathetic nervous system. the control groups (Hoshino, Ohno, et al., 1984;
Jensen et al., 1985). Hoshino and colleagues
Cortisol Secretion (1987) have also reported similar abnormalities
with the DST when measuring cortisol levels
The glucocorticoid cortisol is released from in saliva. In other studies, normal levels of
the adrenal cortex in response to stress; in- plasma cortisol (Sandman, Barron, Chicz-
creased amounts also are normally released in Demet, & Demet, 1990; Tordjman, Anderson,
the early morning. The secretion is under con- et al., 1997) or slightly elevated levels of uri-
trol of adrenocorticotropin hormone (ACTH) nary cortisol (Richdale & Prior, 1992) were
released from the pituitary; ACTH release is seen in the autistic group.
in turn under control of corticotropin-releasing On the whole, it would appear that baseline
factor (CRF) produced in the hypothalamus. secretion of cortisol and ACTH are not greatly
Levels of cortisol or its metabolites in plasma, altered in autism (see Table 17.1). However,
saliva and urine, and plasma levels of ACTH, questions regarding possible abnormalities in
have been measured to assess HPA function. the diurnal rhythm remain to be addressed. It
Normally, cortisol inhibits its own release by does seem clear that lower IQ autistic subjects
suppressing CRF and ACTH release. The sta- do not suppress cortisol secretion after dexam-
tus of this feedback system has been studied ethasone to the same extent as normal or con-
extensively in depression using the dexameth- trol subjects. Possible treatment effects of an
asone suppression test (DST; Gwirtsman, ACTH analogue (Buitelaar, van Engeland, van
Gerner, & Sternbach, 1982). Studies in autism Ree, & de Wied, 1990) added to the interest in
have examined basal levels of ACTH and corti- this area, but have not led to definitive results.
sol, diurnal variations, and the response to
dexamethasone (a synthetic glucocorticoid) Thyroid Hormone and TRH Test
and other provocative agents.
The balance of the studies report either Aspects of thyroid function in infantile autism
normal or elevated baseline secretion of cor- and the efficacy of triiodothyronine (T3) treat-
tisol, and a failure to suppress cortisol release ment of autistic children have been studied by
after dexamethasone, in autistic subjects. several investigators (Campbell, Small, et al.,
Maher, Harper, Macleay, and King (1975) 1978; Sherwin, Flach, & Stokes, 1958). Kahn
reported an increased release of cortisol in re- (1970) reported diminished values of T3 uptake
sponse to insulin in autistic children com- in 45 of 62 autistic children. On the other hand,
pared to retarded control subjects, whereas Abbassi, Linscheid, and Coleman (1978) and
Yamazaki, Saito, Okada, Fujiede, and Yama- Cohen, Young, Lowe, and Harcherik (1980)
shita (1975) found a normal increase in 11- have investigated T3, T4, and TSH (thyroid
hydroxycorticosteriods (11-OHCS) after stimulating hormone, thyrotropin) concentra-
pyrogen stress in autistic subjects. The latter tions in 13 autistic children and found no clini-
investigators also reported abnormal diurnal cal evidence for hypothyroidism, reporting that
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 461

all had levels within the normal range. Camp- droepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS), Tordj-
bell, Small, et al. (1978) have conducted a man and colleagues (1995) found no differences
placebo-controlled crossover study of behav- between the autistic and control groups. The
ioral effects of T3 in 30 young, clinically euthy- group similarities in the androgens were seen in
roid autistic children and reported that T3 did both the pre- and postpubertal subjects. More
not differ from placebo, although, as a group, recently, it has been suggested that differences
the lower IQ autistic children responded to T3. in early androgen exposure may influence the
Campbell, Hollander, Ferris, and Greene expression of autism (Manning, Baron-Cohen,
(1978) performed the thyrotropin-releasing Wheelwright, & Sanders, 2001; Tordjman, Fer-
hormone (TRH) test in psychotic children. rari, Sulmont, Duyme, & Roubertoux, 1997)
After administering synthetic TRH intra- and the use of digit length ratios in assessing
venously to 10 young psychotic children, this has been suggested (Manning et al., 2001).
plasma T3, TSH, and prolactin (PRL) were
measured over time. In general, there was an PEPTIDE RESEARCH
elevated response to TSH and a delayed or
blunted response of T3 in psychotic children. The important role of peptides in central neu-
Suwa et al. (1984) examined hypothalamo- rotransmission and neuromodulation is well
pituitary function by means of the TRH test in established. Neuropeptides have been shown
4 children with autism. Hyperresponse of PRL to be crucial to processes related to emotion,
to TRH was observed in one of the children appetite, pain perception, and sexual behavior.
with autism. Moreover, 3 of the 4 autistic chil- Measurement of CSF, plasma, and urine levels
dren showed a hyperresponse of TSH to TRH. of specific or uncharacterized peptides in
Similarly, Hoshino et al. (1983) reported that schizophrenia and depression has not clearly
6 autistic children showed an elevated re- indicated whether peptides have etiological
sponse of TSH to TRH. Unlike Suwa et al., significance in these disorders. In autism, the
they found a blunted response of PRL to TRH. work can be divided into studies of specific
In contrast, Hashimoto and colleagues (1991) opioid peptides and more general studies of
found a blunted TSH response to TRH in a peptide excretion patterns.
large group (N = 41) of children with autism Opioid peptides, such as the enkephalins and
and others have observed normal hormone re- the endorphins, appear to be endogenous lig-
sponses to TRH (P. A. McBride, personal ands for receptors activated by morphine and
communication). Congenital hypothyroidism related compounds. Several investigators have
has been described in a number of patients theorized that the opioid peptides are involved
with autism (Gillberg & Coleman, 1992; Gill- in producing at least some of the symptoms of
berg, Gillberg, & Koop, 1992; Ritvo et al., autism (Colette, 1978; Panksepp, 1979; Sand-
1990). If the finding is not coincidental, this man, 1991, 1992). In particular, similarities
may indicate that hypothyroidism increases between behaviors seen in opiate-injected ani-
the risk for autism in vulnerable individuals. mals and those displayed in autistic subjects
(decreased pain perception, behavioral persis-
Sex Hormones tence, self-injurious behavior, poor social rela-
tions) have suggested that the opioid peptides
So far, there have been no reports on the thera- are hyperfunctional in autism. The hypothesis
peutic effect in autism of sex-related hormones has been tested by measuring levels of opioids
such as lutenizing hormone (LH) and follicle- in plasma and CSF, and by administering the
stimulating hormone (FSH). However, Hoshino opiate antagonist, naloxone, to self-injurious
et al. (1983) have performed a LH-RH test in and autistic subjects.
six autistic boys. They administered synthetic Previous research on the plasma opioids
LH-RH intravenously and measured plasma re- yielded somewhat inconsistent results, with
lease of LH and FSH; both LH and FSH exhib- some investigators finding elevations in
ited a blunted response to LH-RH stimulation. autism, while others have found little differ-
In a study of plasma levels of the gonadal and ence between groups (Barrett, Feinstein, &
the adrenal androgens, testosterone, and dehy- Hold, 1989; Bernstein, Hughes, Mitchell, &
462 Neurological and Medical Issues

Thompson, 1987; Coid, Allolio, & Rees, 1983; volved in autism (Reichelt & Knivsberg, 2003),
Leboyer, Bouvard, & Dupes, 1988; Sandman any further research should be directed toward
et al., 1990; Tordjman, Anderson, et al., 1997; establishing which specific peptide species are
Weizman et al., 1984). A study examining ß- increased or decreased in autistic subjects.
endorphin fragments in plasma has reported an Subsequent identification of the specific pep-
extreme elevation in C-terminal fragments in tides which might be abnormal in autism would
autistic individuals (Leboyer et al., 1994); fur- be desirable in order to determine the etiologi-
ther work on this aspect is warranted. Studies cal significance of the possible abnormalities.
of CSF opioids have reported increased levels Recent research along these lines that has found
of met-enkephalin (Gillberg, Terenius, & Lon- differences in peptides in neonatal blood spots
nerholm, 1985; Ross, Klykylo, & Hitzeman, of autistic children is questionable given the
1987) and increased or unaltered (Nagamitsu, lack of disorder specificity and the failure to
1993) ß-endorphin in autistic subjects. While replicate (Nelson et al., 2001). Similarly, re-
CSF opioids are presumably derived from cen- ported differences in plasma oxytocin process-
tral sources, plasma ß-endorphin has a periph- ing (Green et al., 2001), although interesting
eral origin. In fact, ß-endorphin appears to from a theoretical perspective (Insel, O’Brien,
be released along with ACTH and probably & Leckman, 1999), need careful replication.
should be considered a human stress hormone.
Initial tests of the effect of naloxone on self- AMINO ACIDS AND ACETYLCHOLINE
injurious behavior in mentally retarded individ-
uals were promising (Sandman et al., 1983). A number of inborn errors of amino acid me-
This result supported the idea of a hyperfunc- tabolism have been identified, and several of
tional opioid system, at least with respect to these disorders, such as phenylketonuria, his-
this one dimension of behavior. However, fur- tidinemia, and homocystinuria affect the cen-
ther studies of naloxone’s effects did not tend tral nervous system and have severe behavioral
to demonstrate clear clinical effects of the opi- consequences (Scriver & Rosenberg, 1973).
oid antagonists in treating autism (Campbell Sylvester, Jorgensen, Mellerup, and Rafael-
et al., 1990; Herman, 1991; Leboyer et al., sen (1970) surveyed amino acid excretion in 178
1992). children suffering from different psychiatric
The urinary excretion of unidentified disorders, including psychosis, neurosis, charac-
peptides and peptide complexes in autism has ter disorder, mental deficiency, and other func-
been described in a qualitative manner in tional disturbances. In no case was a specific
several reports (Gillberg, Trygstad, & Foss, hyperaminoaciduria found. Johnson, Wiersema,
1982; Isarangkun, Newman, Patel, Duruibe, & and Kraft (1974) analyzed amino acid composi-
Abou-Issa, 1986; Reichelt, Saelid, Lindback, & tion of hair protein and found no significant dif-
Boler, 1986; Reichelt et al., 1981). Distinctive ferences between autistic and control children.
patterns of urinary peptides have been reported In 1978, T. L. Perry, Hansen, and Christie mea-
to occur in several childhood neuropsychiatric sured amino compounds and organic acids in
illnesses, including autism. Although there CSF, plasma, and urine of autistic and control
have been a number of reports of differences children. Similar levels of most compounds were
between autistic and control subjects in terms observed in the two groups; however, the mean
of their patterns of peptide excretion the stud- concentration of ethanolamine in CSF was sig-
ies are far from definitive. The relatively non- nificantly higher in autistic children than in
specific nature of the analytical separations control subjects. Based on this finding, they
and detection processes employed and the non- suggested that a subgroup of autistic children
quantitative aspect of the studies hinder inter- possibly may have a brain disorder involving
pretation. In a collaborative study (Le Couteur, ethanolamine metabolism.
Trygstad, Evered, Gillberg, & Rutter, 1988), Kotsopoulos and Kutty (1979) and Rutter
researchers did not find reproducible differ- and Bartak (1971) have reported cases showing
ences between autistic and control subjects’ ex- features of infantile autism who exhibited his-
cretion of peptides. Although differences in tidinemia, with histidine blood levels several
peptide handling are still hypothesized to be in- times higher than normal. It is not clear whether
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 463

coexistence of autism and histidinemia is coin- Dunn, Edwards-Brown, & Feinberg, 2002),
cidental, if not histidinemia may have consti- have stimulated considerable interest in this
tuted a necessary but not sufficient factor area. Further neurobiological research is
leading to the clinical condition of autism. clearly called for and some consideration has
An association between phenylketonceria been given to the possible utility of choliner-
and autism has been noted (Friedman, 1969). gic agents in the treatment of autism.
In a subsequent study, Lowe, Tanaka, Seashore,
Young, and Cohen (1980) surveyed 65 children PURINES AND
with pervasive developmental disturbance RELATED COMPOUNDS
(autism and atypical childhood psychosis) using
standard urinary amino acid screening methods A good deal of attention has been paid to the
and found three children exhibiting PKU. role of cyclic AMP (adenosine-3′, 5′-cyclic
The children were treated with low phenylala- monophosphate) as a second messenger in the
nine diets and showed improvement in function- mechanism of neural transmission. The en-
ing and developmental level after treatment. zymes involved in brain synthesis (adenylate cy-
The study underlined the relevance of urinary clase) and decomposition (phosphodiesterase)
amino acid screening for children being evalu- of cyclic AMP are more active in the brain than
ated for serious developmental disturbances of in other body organs. Norepinephrine, among
childhood. Other work on aromatic amino acid other neurotransmitters, elevates intracellular
precursors of the catecholamines (phenylala- cyclic AMP after interacting with membrane re-
nine) and the indoleamines (TRP) found that ceptors; the elevation of cyclic AMP appears
autistic subjects had reduced intestinal absorp- crucial to the subsequent neuronal firing. Cyclic
tion of the compounds (Naruse, Hayashi, Take- GMP (guanosine-3′, 5′-cyclic monophosphate)
sada, Nakane, & Yamazaki, 1989). Although is a nucleotide related to cyclic AMP and also
an attempt was made to relate these peripheral has second messenger properties.
findings to some central alteration in Winsberg et al. (1980) measured cyclic
monoamine metabolism, this relationship is not AMP in CSF of autistic children and reported
at all clear. that levels were increased in all by probenecid
Abnormalities in plasma glutamate and administration; however, no comparison to con-
gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) have been trol groups was made. Hoshino et al. (1980)
reported in autism (Aldred, Moore, Fitzgerald, determined plasma cyclic AMP levels in psy-
& Waring, 2003; Dhossche et al., 2002), and re- chiatric diseases of children, such as early in-
cent postmortem brain studies have observed fantile autism, hyperkinetic mental retardation,
differences in hippocampal GABA receptor attention deficit disorder, and Down’s syn-
density and alterations in the synthetic enzyme drome. The plasma cyclic AMP levels were
(glutamic acid decarboxylase) that converts higher in autistic and hyperkinetic mentally re-
glutamate to GABA (Blatt et al., 2001; Fatemi tarded children compared to normal children
et al., 2002). The neurochemical findings, as and were positively correlated with the hyper-
well as apparent association of GABA receptor activity score. In children with attention deficit
genes with autism risk, indicate that further re- disorder, the plasma cyclic AMP level was sig-
search in this area is warranted. nificantly lower than in normal children and
The general area of cholinergic mecha- was not correlated with the hyperactivity score.
nisms in autism has been relatively neglected Goldberg, Hattab, Meir, Ebstein, and Bel-
due to difficulties in assessing central and maker (1984) reported that an examination of
peripheral cholinergic metabolism and func- plasma cyclic AMP and cyclic GMP in 18 pa-
tioning. However, recent striking findings tients with childhood autism, 7 patients with
of altered cholinergic receptors in cortical re- pervasive developmental disorder, and 12 age-
gions of postmortem brain specimens ob- and sex-matched healthy controls revealed that
tained from patients with autism (Lee et al., plasma cyclic AMP was significantly elevated
2002; E. K. Perry et al., 2001), along with re- by over 100% in both groups of patients with
ported alterations in vivo brain choline levels childhood-onset psychoses compared with
using nuclear resonance spectroscopy (Sokol, controls, although plasma cyclic GMP was not
464 Neurological and Medical Issues

elevated. They did not examine the correlation decreased adenylosuccinase activity in the liver
between plasma cyclic AMP, GMP, and clini- and absence of activity in the kidney. They sug-
cal symptoms, including hyperkinesis. The gested that the accumulation of both suc-
origin of plasma cyclic AMP remains unclear; cinylpurines in the CSF implies that there is
the compound has been assumed to be derived also a deficiency of this enzyme in the brain
from peripheral organs, such as the liver, kid- and that this may be the basic defect in a sub-
neys, lungs, and adrenals, as well as the brain. group of children with autism. This work was
Sankar (1971) determined red blood cell followed up with a study of autistic siblings
(erythrocyte) ATPase activity before and after having a markedly lowered Vmax of adenylo-
lysing of the cells and reported that the level of succinase (Barshop, Alberts, & Gruber, 1989).
ATPase in lyzate was significantly higher in The molecular basis of the three cases of se-
autistic-like schizophrenic children compared vere retardation with autistic features has been
to normal controls. The addition of magnesium identified; the affected children are homozy-
ions to the lyzate further increased the ATPase gous for a point mutation while their family
activity especially in the case of schizophrenic members are heterozygous (Stone et al., 1992).
children. Based on this result, he suggested The point mutation in the purine nucleotide
that red cell membranes of schizophrenic chil- biosynthetic enzme, adenylosuccinate lyase,
dren display either decreased permeability to thus segregates with the disorder.
ATP or to some other factor(s) necessary for
ATPase activity in the cell. CONCLUSION
Uric acid is the final end-product of all
purine pathways and hyperuricosuria (increased On surveying the field of neurochemical re-
urinary excretion of uric acid) has been re- search in autism, it is notable how few repli-
ported to occur in up to a quarter of the autistic cated differences have been found between
children studied in the United States and France autistic and normal subjects. The studies re-
(Page & Coleman, 2000; Rosenberger-Diesse & porting similarities between autistic and nor-
Coleman, 1986). These observations warrant mal subjects should not be considered negative
further investigation, given the high proportion studies because they have served to narrow the
of patients suggested to be so affected (though field of investigation. The relatively few dif-
elevated rates of gout have not been reported in ferences that have been reported tend to stand
autism). A more specific form of uric acid al- out. Most robust and well replicated is the in-
teration has been reported to be present in a 3- crease in whole blood 5-HT seen in autism.
year-old boy with unusual autistic behavior, However, abnormalities also have been re-
who was shown to have an excessive rate of uric ported in peptide excretion and in neuroen-
acid synthesis due to an increase in the purine docrine and HPA functioning, in amino acid
enzyme phosphoribosylpyrophosphate sythetase levels, uric acid excretion, and central cholin-
in his fibroblasts (Becker, Raivio, Bakay, ergic and gabaergic receptors.
Adams, & Nyhan, 1980). Other children with Certainly an elucidation of the factor(s)
this enzyme abnormality have now been re- causing the elevation of blood 5-HT would be
ported (Christen, Hanfeld, Duley, & Simmonds, of interest. Additional studies of peptide ex-
1992; Simmonds, Webster, Lingham, & Wilson, cretion, hormone release, amino acid, and
1985). Jaeken and Van den Berghe (1984) re- purine metabolism seem warranted, given the
ported that succinyladenosine and succiny- reported abnormalities, their possible rele-
laminoimidazole carboxamide riboside were vance to central neurotransmitter function,
found in body fluids (CSF, plasma, and urine) in and the compounds’ physiological importance.
three children with severe infantile autism. Finally, the diurnal rhythms of, and the effects
Their presence indicates a deficiency of the en- of stress on, NE, epinephrine (adrenaline), and
zyme adenylosuccinase, which is involved in cortisol appear to be potentially fruitful areas
both de novo synthesis of purines and the forma- of research.
tion of adenosine monophosphate from inosine The direction of future research on the bio-
monophosphate. Moreover, according to their chemical basis of autism no doubt will be influ-
report, assays in one patient revealed markedly enced by advances in the basic neurosciences
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 465

and by parallel studies in the biological psychia- Anderson, G. M., Minderaa, R. B., van Bentem, P.-
try of other mental disorders. In general, the P. G., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1984).
availability of postmortem tissue should greatly Platelet imipramine binding in autistic sub-
enhance assessment of central neurochemistry jects. Psychiatry Research, 11, 133–141.
in autism. In the future, a greater consensus Anderson, G. M., Stevenson, J. M., & Cohen, D. J.
(1987). Steady-state model for plasma free
should be reached as to just which aspects of
and platelet serotonin in man. Life Sciences,
neurochemical functioning are abnormal in 41, 1777–1785.
autism. The application of improved techniques Anderson, G. M., Volkmar, F. R., Hoder, E. L.,
of neurochemical assessment also should allow McPhedran, P., Minderaa, R. B., Young, J. G.,
a more complete picture to be drawn. et al. (1987). Whole blood serotonin in autistic
and normal subjects. Journal of Child Psychia-
Cross-References try and Psychology, 28, 885–900.
Barrett, P. R., Feinstein, C., & Hole, W. T. (1989).
Neurologic and genetic aspects of autism are Effects of naloxone and naltrexone on self-
injury: A double-blind placebo-controlled analy-
discussed in Chapters 16, 18, and 19; psy-
sis. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 93,
chopharmacology is reviewed in Chapter 44.
644–651.
Barshop, B. A., Alberts, A. S., & Gruber, H. E.
REFERENCES (1989). Kinetic studies of mutant human
adenylosuccinase. Biochemica et Biophysica
Abbassi, V., Linscheid, T., & Coleman, M. (1978). Acta, 999, 19–23.
Triiodothyronine (T3) concentration and ther- Barthelemy, C., Bruneau, N., Cottet-Eymard, J. M.,
apy in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Domenech-Jouve, J., Garreau, B., Lelord, G.,
Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 383–387. et al. (1988). Urinary free and conjugated cat-
Aizenstein, M. L., & Korf, J. (1979). On the elimina- echolamines and metabolites in autistic chil-
tion of centrally formed 5-hydroxy-indoleacetic dren. Journal of Autism and Developmental
acid by cerebrospinal fluid and urine. Journal of Disorders, 18, 583–591.
Neurochemistry, 32, 1227–1233. Becker, M. A., Raivio, K. O., Bakay, B., Adams,
Aldred, S., Moore, K. M., Fitzgerald, M., & War- W. B., & Nyhan, W. L. (1980). Variant human
ing, R. H. (2003). Plasma amino acid levels in phosphoribosylpyrophosphate synthetase al-
children with autism and their families. Jour- tered in regulatory and catalytic functions.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Journal of Clinical Investigations, 65, 109–120.
33, 93–97. Bernstein, G. A., Hughes, J. R., Mitchell, J. E., &
Anderson, G. M. (1987). Monoamines in autism: Thompson, T. (1987). Effects of narcotic an-
An update of neurochemical research of a per- tagonist on self-injurious behavior: A single
vasive developmental disorder. Medical Biol- case study. Journal of the American Academy of
ogy, 65, 67–74. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 886–889.
Anderson, G. M. (2002). Genetics of child- Blatt, G. J., Fitzgerald, C. M., Guptill, J. T., Booker,
hood disorders: XLV. Autism: Part 4. Sero- A. B., Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. L. (2001).
tonin in autism. Journal of the American Density and distribution of hippocampal
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, neurotransmitter receptors in autism: An au-
41, 1513–1516. toradiographic study. Journal of Autism and De-
Anderson, G. M., & Cohen, D. J. (2002). Neuro- velopmental Disorders, 31, 537–543.
chemistry of childhood psychiatric disorders. Boullin, D. J., Coleman, M., O’Brien, R. A., & Rim-
In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and adolescent psy- land, B. (1971). Laboratory predictions of in-
chiatry: A comprehensive textbook (3rd ed., fantile autism based on 5-hydroxytryptamine
pp. 47–60). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. efflux from blood platelets and their correla-
Anderson, G. M., Horne, W. C., Chatterjee, D., & tion with the Rimland E-2 score. Journal of
Cohen, D. J. (1990). The hyperserotonemia of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 63–71.
autism. Annals of the New York Academy of Boullin, D. J., Freeman, B. J., Geller, E., Ritvo,
Sciences, 600, 333. E. R., Rutter, M., & Yuwiler, A. (1982). To-
Anderson, G. M., Minderaa, R. B., Cho, S. C., Volk- ward the resolution of conf licting findings.
mar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). The issue of Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
hyperserotonemia and platelet serotonin expo- ders, 12, 97–98.
sure: A preliminary study. Journal of Autism Buitelaar, J. K., van Engeland, H., van Ree, J. M.,
and Developmental Disorders, 19, 349–351. & de Wied, D. (1990). Behavioral effects of
466 Neurological and Medical Issues

Org 2766, a synthetic analog of the adrenocor- of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8,
ticotrophic hormone (4–9), in 14 outpatient 477–479.
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel- Cook, E. H. (1990). Autism: Review of neuro-
opmental Disorders, 20, 467–478. chemical investigation [Review]. Synapse, 6,
Buznikov, G. A. (1984). The action of neurotrans- 292–308.
mitters and related substances on early em- Cook, E. H. (2001). Genetics of autism. Child and
bryogenesis. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Amer-
25, 23–59. ica, 10, 333–350.
Campbell, M., Anderson, L. T., Small, A. M., Lo- Cook, E. H., Anderson, G. M., Heninger, G. R.,
cascio, J. J., Lynch, N. S., & Choroco, M. C. Fletcher, K. E., Freedman, D. X., & Leventhal,
(1990). Naltrexone in autistic children: A dou- B. L. (1992). Tryptophan loading in hyper-
ble-blind and placebo controlled study. Psy- serotonemic and normoserotonemic adults. Bi-
chopharmacology Bulletin, 26, 130–135. ological Psychiatry, 31, 525–528.
Campbell, M., Hollander, C. S., Ferris, S., & Greene, Cook, E. H., Arora, R. C., Anderson, G. M., Berry-
L. W. (1978). Response to thyrotropin-releasing Kravis, E. M., Yan, S., Yeoh, H. C., et al.
hormone stimulation in young psychotic chil- (1993). Platelet serotonin in hyperserotonemic
dren: A pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, relatives of children with autistic disorder.
3, 195–201. Life Sciences, 52, 2005–2015.
Campbell, M., Small, A. M., Hollander, C. S., Kor- Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., & Freedman, D. X.
ein, J., Cohen, I. L., Kalmijn, M., et al. (1978). (1988). Free serotonin in plasma: Autistic
A controlled crossover study of triiodothyro- children and their first degree relatives. Bio-
nine in autistic children. Journal of Autism and logical Psychiatry, 24, 488–491.
Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 371–381. Curin, J. M., Terzic, J., Petkovic, Z. B., Zekan, L.,
Christen, H. J., Hanfeld, F., Duley, J. A., & Sim- Terzic, I. M., & Susnjara, I. M. (2003). Lower
monds, H. A. (1992). Distinct neurological cortisol and higher ACTH levels in individuals
syndrome in two brothers with hyperuri- with autism. Journal of Autism and Develop-
caemia. Lancet, 340, 1167–1168. mental Disorders, 33, 443–448.
Chugani, D. C., Muzik, O., Behen, M. E., Rothermal, DeMyer, M. K., Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson, R. K.
R., Lee, J., Chugani, H. T. (1999). Developmen- (1981). Infantile autism reviewed: A decade of
tal changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 388–451.
in autistic and non-autistic children. Annals of Dhossche, D., Applegate, H., Abraham, A.,
Neurology, 45, 287–295. Maertens, P., Bland, L., Bencsath, A., et al.
Cohen, D. J., Caparulo, B. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & (2002). Elevated plasma gamma-aminobutyric
Bowers, M. B., Jr. (1977). Dopamine and sero- acid (GABA) levels in autistic youngsters:
tonin metabolism in neuropsychiatrically dis- Stimulus for a GABA hypothesis of autism.
turbed children: CSF homovanillic acid and Medical Science Monitor, 8, PR1–6.
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid. Archives of Gen- Elchisak, M. A., Polinsky, R. J., Ebert, M. H., Pow-
eral Psychiatry, 34, 545–550. ers, J., & Kopin, I. J. (1978). Contribution of
Cohen, D. J., Shaywitz, B. A., Johnson, W. T., & plasma homovanillic acid (HVA) to urine and
Bowers, M. B., Jr. (1974). Biogenic amines in CSF HVA in the monkey and its pharmacoki-
autistic and atypical children: Cerebrospinal netic disposition. Life Sciences, 23, 2339–2348.
f luid measures of homovanillic acid and 5- Fatemi, S. H., Halt, A. R., Stary, J. M., Kanodia,
hydroxyindoleacetic acid. Archives of General R., Schulz, S. C., & Realmuto, G. R. (2002).
Psychiatry, 31, 845–853. Glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 and 67 kDa
Cohen, D. J., & Young, J. G. (1977). Neurochem- proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and
istry and child psychiatry. Journal of the cerebellar cortices. Biological Psychiatry, 52,
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 16, 805–810.
353–411. Folstein, S. E., & Piven, J. (1991). Etiology
Cohen, D. J., Young, J. G., Lowe, T. L., & of autism: Genetic inf luences. Pediatrics,
Harcherik, D. (1980). Thyroid hormone in 87(Suppl.), 767–773.
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel- Folstein, S. E., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Ge-
opmental Disorders, 10, 445–450. netics of autism: Complex aetiology for a
Coid, J., Allolio, B., & Rees, L. H. (1983). Raised heterogeneous disorder. Nature Reviews: Ge-
plasma metenkephlin in patients who habitu- netics, 2, 943–955.
ally mutilate themselves. Lancet, 2, 545. Frankhauser, M. P., Karumanchi, V. C., German,
Colette, J. W. (1978). Speculation on similarities M. L., Yales, A., & Karumanchi, S. D. (1992).
between autism and opiate addiction. Journal A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 467

efficacy of transdermal clonidine in autism. sion test: Clinical and theoretical review. Jour-
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 53, 77–82. nal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 321–327.
Friedman, E. (1969). The “autistic syndrome” and Hanley, H. G., Stahl, S. M., & Freedman, D. X.
phenylketonuria. Schizophrenia, 1, 249–261. (1977). Hyperserotonemia and amine metabo-
Garelis, E., Young, S. N., Lal, S., & Sourkes, T. L. lites in autistic and retarded children. Archives
(1974). Monoamine metabolites in lumbar of General Psychiatry, 34, 521–531.
CSF: The question of their origin in relation to Hashimoto, T., Aihara, R., Tayama, M., Miyazaki,
clinical studies. Brain Research, 79, 1–8. M., Shirakawa, Y., & Kuroda, Y. (1991). Re-
Gillberg, C. (1993). Comment on CSF HVA. Bio- duced thyroid-stimulating hormone response to
logical Psychiatry, 34, 746. thyrotropin-releasing hormone in autistic boys.
Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1992). The biology of Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
the autistic syndromes (2nd ed., p. 131). Lon- 33, 313–319.
don: Mac Keith Press. Herman, B. H. (1991). Effects of opioid receptor an-
Gillberg, C., & Svennerholm, L. (1987). CSF tagonists on the treatment of autism and self-
monoamines in autistic syndromes and other injurious behavior. In J. J. Ratey (Ed.), Mental
pervasive developmental disorders of early retardation: Developing pharmacotherapies
childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151, progress in psychiatry (Vol. 32, pp. 107–137).
89–94. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Gillberg, C., Svennerholm, L., & Hamilton-Hellberg, Hill, S. D., Wagner, E. A., Shedlorski, J. G., & Sears,
C. (1983). Childhood psychosis and mono- S. P. (1977). Diurnal cortisol and temperature
amine metabolites in spinal f luid. Journal of variation of normal and autistic children. Devel-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, opmental Psychobiology, 10, 579–583.
38–96. Hirstein, W., Iversen, P., & Ramachandran, V. S.
Gillberg, C., Terenius, L., & Lonnerholm, G. (1985). (2001). Autonomic responses of autistic chil-
Endorphin activity in childhood psychosis. dren to people and objects. Proceedings of the
Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 780–783. Royal Society of London. Series B Biological
Gillberg, C., Trygstad, O., & Foss, J. (1982). Child- Sciences, 268, 1883–1888.
hood psychosis and urinary excretion of pep- Hoshino, Y., Kumashiro, H., Yashima, Y., Kaneko,
tides and protein-associated peptide M., Numato, Y., Oshima, N., et al. (1980).
complexes. Journal of Autism and Developmen- Plasma cyclic AMP level in psychiatric dis-
tal Disorders, 12, 229–241. eases of childhood. Folia Psychiatrica et Neu-
Gillberg, I. C., Gillberg, C., & Koop, S. (1992). Hy- rologica Japonica, 34, 9–16.
pothyroidsim and autism spectrum disorders. Hoshino, Y., Ohno, Y., Yamamoto, T., Tachibana,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry R., Murata, S., Yokoyama, F., et al. (1984).
and Allied Disciplines, 33, 531–542. Dexamethasone suppression test in autistic
Giller, E. L., Young, J. G., Breakefield, X. O., Car- children. Japanese Journal of Clinical Psychia-
bonari, C., Braverman, M., & Cohen, D. J. try, 26, 100–102.
(1980). Monoamine oxidase and catechol-O- Hoshino, Y., Tachibana, R., Watanabe, M., Murata,
methyltransferase activities in cultured fi- S., Yokoyama, F., Kaneko, M., et al. (1984).
broblasts and blood cells from children with Serotonin metabolism and hypothalamic-
autism and the Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. pituitary function in children with infantile
Psychiatry Research, 2, 187–197. autism and minimal brain dysfunction. Japanese
Goldberg, M., Hattab, J., Meir, D., Ebstein, R. P., & Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 26, 937–945.
Belmaker, R. H. (1984). Plasma cyclic AMP Hoshino, Y., Watanabe, M., Tachibana, R., Murata,
and cyclic GMP in childhood-onset psychoses. S., Kaneko, M., Yashima, Y., et al. (1983). A
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- study of the hypothalamus-pituitary function
ders, 14, 159–164. in autistic children by the loading test of
Green, L., Fein, D., Modahl, C., Feinstein, C., Wa- 5HTP, TRH and LH-RH. Japanese Journal of
terhouse, L., & Morris, M. (2001). Oxytocin Brain Research, 9, 94–95.
and autistic disorder: Alterations in peptide Hoshino, Y., Yamamoto, T., Kaneko, M.,
forms. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 609–613. Tachibana, R., Watanabe, M., Ohno, Y., et al.
Guthrie, R. D., & Wyatt, R. J. (1975). Biochem- (1984). Blood serotonin and free tryptophan
istry and schizophrenia: III. A review of child- concentration in autistic children. Neuropsy-
hood psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 12, chobiology, 11, 22–27.
18–32. Hoshino, Y., Yokoyama, F., Watanabe, M., Murata, S.,
Gwirtsman, H., Gerner, R. H., & Sternbach, H. Kaneko, M., & Kumashiro, H. (1987). The diur-
(1982). The overnight dexamethasone suppres- nal variation and response to dexamethasone
468 Neurological and Medical Issues

suppression test of saliva cortisol level in autistic Lauritsen, M. B., & Ewald, H. (2001). The genetics
children. Japanese Journal of Psychiatry and of autism. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
Neurology, 41, 227–235. 103, 411–427.
Insel, T. R., O’Brien, D. J., & Leckman, J. F. Leboyer, M., Bouvard, M. P., & Dupes, M. (1988).
(1999). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: Is Effects of naltrexone on infantile autism.
there a connection? Biological Psychiatry, Lancet, 1, 715.
45(2), 145–157. Leboyer, M., Bouvard, M. P., Launay, J. M., Tableau,
Isarangkun, P. P., Newman, H. A., Patel, S. T., Du- F., Waller, D., Kerdelhue, B., et al. (1992). Brief
ruibe, V. A., & Abou-Issa, H. (1986). Potential report: A double-blind study of naltrexone on
biochemical markers for infantile autism. Neu- infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Develop-
rochemical Pathology, 5, 51–70. mental Disorders, 22, 309–319.
Iverson, S. D., & Iverson, L. L. (1981). Behavioral Leboyer, M., Bouvard, M. P., Recasens, C.,
pharmacology (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford Philippe, A., Guilloud-Bataille, M., Bondoux,
University Press. D., et al. (1994). Differences between plasma
Jaeken, J., & Van den Berghe, G. (1984). An infan- N- and C-terminally directed ß-endorphin im-
tile autistic syndrome characterized by the munoreactivity in infantile autism. American
presence of succinylpurines in body f luids. Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1797–1801.
Lancet, 2, 1058–1061. Le Couteur, A., Trygstad, O., Evered, C., Gillberg,
Janusonis, S., Gluncic, V., & Rakic, P. (2004). C., & Rutter, M. (1988). Infantile autism and
Early serotonergic projections to Cajal-Retzius urinary excretion of peptides and protein-
cells: Relevance for cortical development. associated peptide complexes. Journal of Autism
Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 1652–1659. and Developmental Disorders, 18, 181–190.
Jensen, J. B., Realmuto, G. M., & Garfinkel, B. D. Lee, M., Martin-Ruiz, C., Graham, A., Court, J.,
(1985). The dexamethasone suppression test Jaros, E., Perry, R., et al. (2002). Nicotinic re-
in infantile autism. Journal of the American ceptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 263–265. in autism. Brain, 125, 1483–1495.
Johnson, R. J., Wiersema, V., & Kraft, I. A. (1974). Leventhal, B. L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Morford, M.,
Hair amino acids in childhood autism. Journal Ravitz, A., & Freedman, D. X. (1990). Relation-
of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, ships of whole blood serotonin and plasma nor-
187–188. epinephrine within families. Journal of Autism
Kahn, A. A. (1970). Thyroid dysfunction. British & Developmental Disorders, 20, 499–511.
Medical Journal, 4, 495. Lotspeich, L. J., & Ciaranello, R. D. (1993). The
Kirwin, P. D., Anderson, G. M., Chappell, P. D., neurobiology and genetics of infantile autism.
Saberski, L., Leckman, J. F., Geracioti, T. D., International Review of Neurobiology, 35,
et al. (1997). Assessment of diurnal variation 87–129.
of cerebrospinal f luid tryptophan and 5- Lowe, T. L., Tanaka, K., Seashore, M. R., Young,
hydrxyindoleacetic acid in healthy human fe- J. G., & Cohen, D. J. (1980). Detection of
males. Life Sciences, 60, 899–907. phenylketonuria in autistic and psychotic chil-
Kopin, I. J., Gordon, E. K., Jimerson, D. C., & dren. Journal of the American Medical Associ-
Polinsky, R. J. (1983). Relationship between ation, 243, 126–128.
plasma and cerebrospinal f luid levels of 3- Lucki, I. (1998). The spectrum of behaviors inf lu-
methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol. Science, 219, enced by serotonin. Biological Psychiatry, 44,
73–75. 151–162.
Kotsopoulos, S., & Kutty, K. M. (1979). Histidine- Maas, J. W., Hattox, S. E., Greene, N. M., & Lan-
mia and infantile autism. Journal of Autism dis, B. H. (1980). Estimates of dopamine and
and Developmental Disorders, 9, 55–60. serotonin synthesis by the awake human brain.
Lake, R., Ziegler, M. G., & Murphy, D. L. (1977). Journal of Neurochemistry, 34, 1547–1549.
Increased norepinephrine levels and decreased Maas, J. W., & Leckman, J. F. (1983). Relation-
DBH activity in primary autism. Archives of ships between CNS noradrenergic function
General Psychiatry, 35, 553–556. and plasma and urine MHPG and other norepi-
Launay, J. M., Bursztejn, C., Ferrari, P., Dreux, C., nephrine metabolites. In J. W. Maas (Ed.),
Braconnier, A., Zarifian E., et al. (1987). Cat- MHPG: Basal mechanisms and psychopathol-
echolamines metabolism in infantile autism: A ogy (pp. 33–43). New York: Academic Press.
controlled study of 22 autistic children. Jour- Maher, K. R., Harper, J. F., Macleay, A., & King,
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, M. G. (1975). Peculiarities in the endocrine
17, 333–347. response to insulin stress in early infantile
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 469

autism. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
eases, 161, 180–184. 43, 491–499.
Manning, J. T., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Nagamitsu, S. (1993). CSF ß-endorphin levels in
& Sanders, G. (2001). The 2nd to 4th digit pediatric neurologic disorders. Kurume Med-
ratio and autism. Developmental Medicine and ical Journal, 40, 223–241.
Child Neurology, 43, 160–164. Narayan, M., Srinath, S., & Anderson, G. M.
Martineau, J., Barthelemy, C., Jouve, J., Muh, J. P., (1993). CSF HVA in autism (in reply). Biolog-
& Lelord, G. (1992). Monoamines (serotonin ical Psychiatry, 34, 746–747.
and catecholamines) and their derivatives in Narayan, M., Srinath, S., Anderson, G. M., &
infantile autism: Age-related changes and drug Meundi, D. B. (1993). Cerebrospinal fluid levels
effects. Developmental Medicine & Child Neu- of homovanillic acid and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic
rology, 34, 593–603. acid in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 33,
McBride, P. A., Anderson, G. M., Hertzig, M. E., 630–635.
Sweeney, J. A., Kream, J., Cohen, D. J., et al. Naruse, H., Hayashi, T., Takesada, M., Nakane, A.,
(1989). Serotonergic responsivity in male & Yamazaki, K. (1989). Metabolic changes in
young adults with autistic disorder. Archives of aromatic amino acids and monoamines in in-
General Psychiatry, 46, 213–221. fantile autism and development of new treat-
McBride, P. A., Anderson, G. M., & Mann, J. J. ment related to the finding. No to Hattatsu
(1990). Serotonin in autism. In E. F. Coccaro [Brain and Development], 21, 181–189.
& D. L. Murphy (Eds.), Serotonin in major Nelson, K. B., Grether, J. K., Croen, L. A.,
psychiatric disorders (pp. 47–68). Washington, Dambrosia, J. M., Dickens, B. F., Jelliffe, L. L.,
DC: American Psychiatric Press. et al. (2001). Neuropeptides and neurotrophins
McBride, P. A., Anderson, G. M., & Shapiro, T. in neonatal blood of children with autism or
(1996). Autism research: Bringing together mental retardation. Annals of Neurology, 49,
approaches to pull apart the disorder. Archives 597–606.
of General Psychiatry, 53, 980–983. Nir, I., Meir, D., Zilber, N., Knobler, H., Hadjez, J.,
McDougle, C. J., Naylor, S. T., Cohen, D. J., Agha- & Lerner, Y. (1995). Brief report: Circadian
janian, G. K., Heninger, G. R., & Price, L. H. melatonin, thyroid-stimulating hormone, pro-
(1996). Effects of tryptophan depletion in lactin, and cortisol levels in serum of young
drug-free adults with autistic disorder. Archives adults with autism. Journal of Autism and De-
of General Psychiatry, 53, 993–1000. velopmental Disorders, 25(6), 641–654.
Meek, J. L., & Neff, N. H. (1973). Is cerebrospinal O’Brien, R. A., Semenuk, G., & Spector, S. (1976).
f luid the major avenue for the removal of 5- Catechol-O-methyltransferase activity in ery-
hydroxyindoleacetic acid from the brain? Neu- throcytes of children with autism. In E. Cole-
ropharmacology, 12, 497–499. man (Ed.), The autistic syndromes (pp. 43–49).
Minderaa, R. B., Anderson, G. M., Volkmar, F. R., Amsterdam: North Holland.
Akkerhuis, G. W., & Cohen, D. J. (1987). Uri- Page, T., & Coleman, M. (2000). Purine metabo-
nary 5-HIAA and whole blood 5-HT and tryp- lism abnormalities in a hyperuricosuric sub-
tophan in autism and normal subjects. class of autism. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta,
Biological Psychiatry, 22, 933–940. 1500, 291–296.
Minderaa, R. B., Anderson, G. M., Volkmar, F. R., Panksepp, J. (1979). A neurochemical theory of
Akkerhuis, G. W., & Cohen, D. J. (1994). No- autism. Trends in Neuroscience, 2, 174–177.
radrenergic and adrenergic functioning in Partington, M. W., Tu, J. B., & Wong, C. Y. (1973).
autism. Biological Psychiatry, 36, 237–241. Blood serotonin levels in severe mental retar-
Minderaa, R. B., Anderson, G. M., Volkmar, F. R., dation. Developmental Medicine and Child
Harcherik, D., Akkerhuis, G. W., & Cohen, Neurology, 15, 616–627.
D. J. (1989). Neurochemical study of dopamine Perry, E. K., Lee, M. L., Martin-Ruiz, C. M.,
functioning in autistic and normal subjects. Court, J. A., Volsen, S. G., Merrit, J., et al.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and (2001). Cholinergic activity in autism: Abnor-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 200–206. malities in the cerebral cortex and basal fore-
Mulder, E. J., Anderson, G. M., Kema, I. P., de Bildt, brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158,
A., van Lange, N. D. J., et al. (2004). Platelet 1058–1066.
serotonin levels in pervasive developmental dis- Perry, T. L., Hansen, S., & Christie, R. G. (1978).
orders and mental retardation: Diagnostic group Amino compounds and organic acids in CSF,
differences, within group distribution and be- plasma, and urine of autistic children. Biologi-
havioral correlates. Journal of the American cal Psychiatry, 13, 575–586.
470 Neurological and Medical Issues

Posey, D. J., McDougle, C. J. (2000). The pharma- monoamine effects in fenf luramine treatment
cotherapy of target symptoms associated with of autism. Annals of Neurology, 18, 394.
autistic disorder and other pervasive develop- Ross, D. L., Klykylo, W. M., & Hitzeman, R.
mental disorders. Harvard Review of Psychia- (1987). Reduction of elevated CSF ß-endorphin
try, 8, 45–63. by fenf luramine in infantile autism. Pediatric
Realmuto, G. M., Jensen, J. B., Reeve, E., & Neurology, 3, 83–86.
Garfinkel, B. D. (1990). Growth hormone re- Rutter, M., & Bartak, L. (1971). Causes of infantile
sponse to L-dopa and clonidine in autistic autism: Some considerations from recent re-
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental search. Journal of Autism and Childhood
Disorders, 20, 455–465. Schizophrenia, 1, 20–32.
Redmond, D. E., Jr., & Huang, Y. W. (1979). New Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1987). Autism and per-
evidence for a locus coeruleus-norepinephrine vasive developmental disorders: Concepts and
connection with anxiety. Life Sciences, 25, diagnostic issues. Journal of Autism and Devel-
2149–2162. opmental Disorders, 17, 159–186.
Reichelt, K. L., Hole, K., Hamberger, A., Saelid, Sandman, C. A. (1991). The opiate hypothesis in
G., Edminson, P. D., Braestrup, C. B., et al. autism and self-injury. Journal of Child and
(1981). Biologically active peptide-containing Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 1, 237–248.
fractions in schizophrenia and childhood Sandman, C. A. (1992). Various endogenous opi-
autism. Advances in Biochemistry and Psy- oids and autistic behavior: A response to Gill-
chopharmacology, 28, 627–643. berg. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Reichelt, K. L., & Knivsberg, A. M. (2003). Can Disorders, 22, 132–133.
the pathophysiology of autism be explained by Sandman, C. A., Barron, J. L., Chicz-Demet, A., &
the nature of the discovered urine peptides? Demet, E. M. (1990). Plasma ß-endorphin and
Nutritional Neuroscience, 6, 19–28. cortisol levels in autism. American Journal of
Reichelt, K. L., Saelid, G., Lindback, T., & Boler, Mental Retardation, 95, 84–92.
J. B. (1986). Childhood autism: A complex dis- Sandman, C. A., Patta, P. C., Banon, J., Hoehler,
order. Biological Psychiatry, 21, 1279–1290. F. K., Williams, C., Williams, C., et al. (1983).
Richdale, A. L., & Prior, M. R. (1992). Urinary cor- Naloxone attenuates self-abusive behavior in
tisol circadian rhythm in a group of high-func- developmentally disabled clients. Applied Re-
tioning children with autism. Journal of Autism search in Mental Retardation, 4, 5–11.
and Developmental Disorders, 22, 433–446. Sankar, D. V. S. (1971). Studies on blood platelets,
Ritvo, E. R. (1977). Biochemical studies of chil- blood enzymes, and leukocyte chromosome
dren with the syndromes of autism, childhood breakage in childhood schizophrenia. Behav-
schizophrenia and related developmental dis- ioral Neuropsychiatry, 2, 2–10.
abilities: A review. Journal of Child Psychol- Schain, R. J., & Freedman, D. X. (1961). Studies on
ogy and Psychiatry, 13, 373–379. 5-hydroxyindole metabolism in autistic and
Ritvo, E. R., Mason-Brothers, A., Freeman, B. J., other mentally retarded children. Journal of
Pingree, C., Jenson, W. R., McMahon, W. M., Pediatrics, 58, 315–320.
et al. (1990). The UCLA–University of Utah Scriver, C. R., & Rosenberg, L. E. (1973). Amino
epidemiologic survey of autism: The etiologic acid metabolism and its disorders. Philadel-
role of rare diseases. American Journal of Psy- phia: Saunders.
chiatry, 147, 1614–1621. Sherwin, A. C., Flach, F. F., & Stokes, P. E. (1958).
Ritvo, E. R., Yuwiler, A., Geller, E., Kales, A. Treatment of psychoses in early childhood
Rashkis, S., Schicor, A., et al. (1971). Effects with triiodothyronine. American Journal of
of L-dopa in autism. Journal of Autism and Psychiatry, 115, 166–167.
Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 190–205. Shoaf, S. E., Carson, R. E., Hommer, D., Williams,
Ritvo, E. R., Yuwiler, A., Geller, E., Ornitz, E. M., W. A., Higley, J. D., Schmall, B., et al. (2000).
Saeger, K., & Plotkin, S. (1970). Increased The suitability of 11C-alpha-methyl-L-tryptophan
blood serotonin and platelets in early infantile as a tracer for serotonin synthesis. Journal of
autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23, Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 20,
566–572. 244–252.
Rosenberger-Diesse, J., & Coleman, M. (1986). Simmonds, H. A., Webster, D. R., Lingham, S., &
Brief report: Preliminary evidence for multi- Wilson, J. (1985). An inborn error of purine
ple etiologies in autism. Journal of Autism and metabolism, deafness and neurodevelopmental
Developmental Disorders, 16, 385–392. abnormality. Neuropediatrics, 16, 106–108.
Ross, D. L., Klykylo, W. M., & Anderson, G. M. Sokol, D. K., Dunn, D. W., Edwards-Brown, M., &
(1985). Cerebrospinal f luid indoleamine and Feinberg, J. (2002). Hydrogen proton magnetic
Neurochemical Studies of Autism 471

resonance spectroscopy in autism: Preliminary Weizman, R., Weizman, A., Tyrano, S., Szekely,
evidence of elevated choline/creatine ratio. B., Weissman, B. A., & Sarne, Y. (1984).
Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 245–249. Humoral-endorphin blood levels in autistic,
Stone, R. L., Aimi, J., Barshop, B. A., Jaeken, J., schizophrenic and healthy subjects. Psy-
Van den Berghe, G., Zalkin, H., et al. (1992). chopharmacology, 82, 368–370.
A mutation in adenylosuccinate lyase associ- Whitaker-Azmitia, P. M. (2001). Serotonin and
ated with mental retardation and autistic fea- brain development: Role in human develop-
tures. Nature Genetics, 1, 59–63. mental diseases. Brain Research Bulletin, 56,
Suwa, S., Naruse, H., Ohura, T., Tsuruhara, T., 479–485.
Takesada, M., Yamazaki, K., et al. (1984). In- Whitaker-Azmitia, P. M., Druse, M., Walker, P., &
f luence of pimozide on hypothalamo-pituitary Lauder, J. M. (1996). Serotonin as a develop-
function in children with behavioral disorders. mental signal. Behavioural Brain Research, 73,
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 9, 37–44. 19–29.
Sverd, J., Kupretz, S. S., Winsberg, B. G., Hurwic, Winsberg, B. G., Sverd, J., Castells, S., Hurwic, M.,
M. J., & Becker, L. (1978). Effect of L-5- & Perel, J. M. (1980). Estimation of monoamine
hydroxytryptophan in autistic children. Jour- and cyclic-AMP turnover and amino acid con-
nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, centrations of spinal fluid in autistic children.
171–180. Neuropediatrics, 11, 250–255.
Sylvester, O., Jorgensen, E., Mellerup, T., & Yamazaki, K., Saito, Y., Okada, F., Fujiede, T., &
Rafaelsen, O. J. (1970). Amino acid excretion in Yamashita, I. (1975). An application of neu-
urine of children with various psychiatric dis- roendocrinological studies in autistic children
eases. Danish Medical Bulletin, 17, 166–170. and Heller’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and
Takatsu, T., Onizawa, J., & Nakahato, M. (1965). Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 323–332.
Tryptophan metabolism disorder and thera- Young, J. G., Cohen, D. J., Brown, S. L., & Ca-
peutic diet in children with infantile autism. parulo, B. K. (1978). Decreased urinary free
Amino Acids, 5, 13–14. catecholamines in childhood autism. Journal
Tani, Y., Fernell, E., Watanbe, Y., Kanai, T., & of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
Langstrom, B. (1994). Decrease in 6R-5, 6, 7, 17, 671–678.
8-tetrahydrobiopterin content in cerebrospinal Young, J. G., Cohen, D. J., Hattox, S. E., Kavanagh,
f luid of autistic patients. Neuroscience Letters, M. E., Anderson, G. M., Shaywitz, B. A., et al.
181, 169–172. (1981). Plasma free MHPG and neuroendocrine
Tordjman, S., Anderson, G. M., McBride, P. A. responses to challenge doses of clonidine in
Hall, L. M., Ferrari, P., & Cohen, D. J. Tourette’s syndrome: Preliminary report. Life
(1995). Plasma androgens in autism. Journal Sciences, 29, 1467–1475.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, Young, J. G., Cohen, D. J., Kavanagh, M. E., Lan-
295–304. dis, H. D., Shaywitz, B. A., & Maas, J. W.
Tordjman, S., Anderson, G. M., McBride, P. A., (1981). Cerebrospinal f luid, plasma, and uri-
Hertzig, M. E., Snow, M. E., Hall, L. M., nary MHPG in children. Life Sciences, 28,
et al. (1997). Plasma ß-endorphin, adrenocor- 2837–2845.
ticotropin hormone, and cortisol in autism. Young, J. G., Kavanagh, M. E., Anderson, G. M.,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Shaywitz, B. A., & Cohen, D. J. (1982). Clini-
38, 705–715. cal neurochemistry of autism and associated
Tordjman, S., Ferrari, P., Sulmont, V., Duyme, M., disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
& Roubertoux, P. (1997). Androgenic activity tal Disorders, 12, 147–165.
in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, Young, J. G., Newcorn, J. H., & Leven, L. I. (1989).
154, 1626–1627. Pervasive developmental disorders. In H. I.
Udenfriend, S., Titus, E., Weissbach, H., & Peter- Kaplan & B. J. Saddock (Eds.), Comprehensive
son, R. E. (1959). Biogenesis and metabolism textbook of psychiatry (pp. 1772–1787). Balti-
of 5-hydroxyindole compounds. Journal of Bi- more: Williams & Wilkins.
ological Chemistry, 219, 335–344. Young, S. N., Anderson, G. M., & Purdy, W. C.
Waage-Baudet, H., Lauder, J. M., Dehart, D. B., (1980). Indoleamine metabolism in rat brain
Kluckman, K., Hiller, S., Tint, G. S., et al. studied through measurements of tryptophan,
(2003). Abnormal serotonergic development in 5-hydroxyindoleactic acid and indoleactetic
a mouse model for the Smith-Lemli-Opitz acid in cerebrospinal f luid. Journal of Neuro-
syndrome: Implications for autism. Interna- chemistry, 34, 309–315.
tional Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, Yuwiler, A., Geller, A., & Ritvo, E. R. (1985). Bio-
21(8), 451–459. chemical studies of autism. In E. Lajtha (Ed.),
472 Neurological and Medical Issues

Handbook of Neurochemistry (pp. 671–691). Zhou, E. C., Auerbach, S., & Azmitia, E. C.
New York: Plenum Press. (1987). Denervation of serotonergic fibers in
Yuwiler, A., Ritvo, E. R., Geller, E., Glousman, R., the hippocampus induced a trophic factor
Schneiderman, G., & Matsuno, D. (1975). Up- which enhance the maturation of transplanted
take and efflux of serotonin from platelets of serotonergic neurons but not norepinephrine
autistic and nonautistic children. Journal of neurons. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 17,
Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 83–98. 235–246.
CHAPTER 18

Neurologic Aspects of Autism

NANCY J. MINSHEW, JOHN A. SWEENEY, MARGARET L. BAUMAN, and SARA JANE WEBB

Autism is now widely accepted as being a dis- ology that originates at the level of DNA or its
order of brain development and, hence, of neu- transcription for brain development.
rologic origin. Histopathologic abnormalities Geneticists have calculated a heritability
of multiple brain structures and alterations in index of .90 or above for autism. The rapid
brain weight were described 2 decades ago, drop-off in cases from first- to second- and
providing definitive evidence of a brain basis third-degree relatives supports a polygenetic
for this disorder. Recently, structural imaging mode of inheritance. Each case is suspected to
studies have provided evidence of early abnor- reflect the influence of several autism suscep-
malities in brain growth that coincide with, if tibility genes, but not the same genes in each
not predate, the onset of clinically recognized case. Geneticists hypothesize that there are 10
symptoms. Functional imaging studies have to 20, if not more, autism susceptibility genes.
provided evidence of underconnectivity of neo- Several phenotypic findings are thought to be
cortical neural systems for social, communica- linked to autism susceptibility genes, in par-
tion, and reasoning abilities related to the core ticular the prominent incidence of affective
symptoms of autism. From a neuropathologic disorder in first-degree relatives, the expres-
perspective, the onset of the neurodevelopmen- sion of characteristics of the clinical syndrome
tal events appears to be no later than 28 to 30 in first-degree relatives, and macrocephaly.
weeks, based on the limited available autopsy The frequent co-occurrence of autistic
material. Neuropathologic and neuroimaging symptomatology with tuberous sclerosis, a dis-
studies suggest abnormalities in the elabora- order also characterized by overgrowth of
tion of dendritic and axonal ramifications, the cerebral white matter, may also provide a link
establishment of synaptic connections, the se- to the genetics and neurobiology of autism.
lective elimination of neuronal processes, and Identification of the genes involved in affec-
the development of white matter tracts. How- tive disorder and tuberous sclerosis may iden-
ever, most of the developmental neurobiology tify regions of DNA with increased likelihood
of autism remains speculative and uncon- for containing genes related to neurodevelop-
firmed. Autism is a sporadic disorder, the core mental errors in autism and the impairments
features of which are faithfully reproduced exhibited by less affected relatives. Genetic
both neuropathologically and clinically from studies are vigorously attempting to identify
case to case, suggesting a common pathophysi- the susceptibility genes for autism. Interesting

This work was supported by a NICHD Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism grant U19 HD35469,
Nancy Minshew, MD, director. We gratefully acknowledge the participation of individuals with autism and
control subjects in all research studies. We also appreciate the work of Sara Eddy, who helped with the re-
search and writing of sections of this chapter and the final formatting assistance of Kelsey Woods, Rosie
Christ, and Bernadette MacDonald, who provided secretarial assistance.

473
474 Neurological and Medical Issues

findings have included the serotonin trans- minicolumns in the cerebral cortex and the
porter gene and linkage to chromosome 17 emerging perspective of autism as a distrib-
(Cook et al., 1997); the linkage between the uted neural systems disorder rather than a
absence of language in autism and chromo- focal brain disorder.
some 7 and, more recently, 2q (mitochondrial Neurologic conceptualizations or models
aspartate/glutamate carrier gene; Ramoz et al., proposed for autism have altered in accordance
2004); and the association with the homeobox with the changing structural and functional
transcription factor Engrailed 2 (Gharani, Be- characterization of autism. Current neurobio-
nayed, Mancuso, Brzustowicz, & Millonig, logic theories postulate single or multiple
2004). While none of these have been con- deficits in complex or higher order cognitive
firmed yet, the results attest to the progress abilities, dysfunction at the neural systems level
being made and the likelihood of success in of brain organization, and a central role for
the future in finding the genes for autism. cerebral cortex in the final common pathophys-
Substantial progress has also been made in iology of the clinical manifestations. Such the-
the past 2 decades in defining the behavioral ories represent a closer approximation of the
neurology of autism, that is, the cognitive and pathophysiology of autism, but certainly not
brain basis for the behavior. Over the past 10 the final step. The goal of research underway
years, the capability for characterizing autism is to define with precision the cognitive and
has improved substantially as a result of ad- neural basis of all of the features of the clini-
vances in research diagnostic methods and cri- cal syndrome, the developmental neurobiology
teria used to define autism and the evolution in of the structural and functional abnormalities
technology for the in vivo study of central ner- of the brain, the genes involved in the develop-
vous system (CNS) function and structure. mental neurobiology, and other etiologies that
The introduction of magnetic resonance imag- might trigger the disruption in brain develop-
ing (MRI ) made studies of children and re- ment. This knowledge will result in a substan-
peated studies feasible, and the development tial improvement in diagnosis and in all phases
of functional MRI (fMRI ) is leading to further of intervention and will pave the way for the
understanding of the brain basis of cognitive development of definitive neurobiologic inter-
impairments and intact or enhanced abilities ventions designed to ameliorate the brain ab-
in autism. Structural studies have provided ev- normalities underlying autism.
idence of increased supratentorial total brain
and white matter volumes, increased brain CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM
weight, and above-average head circumference FUNCTION IN AUTISM
on the one hand and, on the other, truncation
of the dendritic tree development of neurons in The neurophysiologic integrity of neural path-
limbic structures and decreased neuronal num- ways in the brain can be investigated with sev-
ber in the cerebellum. Functional studies are eral methods, most commonly through studies
revealing evidence of underconnectivity in the of evoked potentials and oculomotor physiology.
distributed networks of cortical centers that These methods provide information about
subserve the core symptoms of autism, includ- neural pathways at multiple levels of the neu-
ing social, language, and reasoning. Areas of raxis and about selected aspects of sensory,
intact or superior abilities appear to have en- motor, and cognitive function. Both evoked po-
hanced local connectivity. This constellation tentials and oculomotor physiology were origi-
of structural and functional findings has de- nally applied to autism shortly after their
fined two intriguing “paradoxes”: (1) the dis- introduction to medical science. Subsequently,
sociation between the CNS localizations of two types of scientific pursuits were under-
some structural and functional findings and taken: neurophysiological studies and neuropsy-
(2) the contrast between “ too much brain” in chological studies. Neurophysiologic studies
some regions and “ too little brain” in other re- typically assess functional integrity of the ner-
gions. These paradoxes are starting to resolve vous system. Neuropsychological studies in-
with the recent histologic findings of Casanova, vestigate brain function at an integrative level
Buxhoeveden, Switala, and Roy (2002) in beyond the more basic functions assessed with
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 475

neurophysiologic methods and thus potentially potentials, each with a characteristic latency
provide an intermediate step between basic range and spatial distribution. Amplitude of
laboratory findings and clinical symptom ex- EPs varies with the characteristics of the stim-
pression. The first era of this research in ulus or cognitive task and, in the case of the
autism, ending in the early to mid-1980s, cognitive potentials, with subject characteris-
largely documented results of clinical studies tics. Some potentials also have a developmental
that had been conducted with problematic ex- trajectory, which adds yet another dimension to
perimental designs. Research studies since experimental design and data interpretation.
1980 have defined a neurophysiologic profile EPs are typically classified as either exoge-
in autism that is characterized by abnormali- nous (evoked by perception of an external stim-
ties in cognitive processing and neocortical uli) or endogenous (evoked by mental processes
circuitry, with intact early information pro- or behavior). Exogenous potentials occur within
cessing, simple cognitive abilities, and very the first 40 milliseconds of the stimulus and
subtle disturbances in posterior fossa circuitry are typically an obligatory response of brain
(e.g., brainstem and cerebellum). neurons to a sensory stimulus. Their amplitude
Findings using both neurophysiological and and latency are influenced by the physical
neuropsychological methods are dependent on properties of the stimulus, specifically its in-
the cognitive abilities and cooperation of high- tensity, duration, and frequency. These poten-
functioning autistic individuals. These indi- tials are affected by the auditory or visual
viduals provide the opportunity to define the acuity of the subject but are minimally af-
qualitative features of the deficits and pre- fected by attention, motivation, and level of
served abilities and to study the disorder in the consciousness. Exogenous potentials are often
absence of mental retardation in order to de- used to test perception of the stimulus in the
termine abnormalities solely related to autism. visual or auditory modality.
These findings have major implications for the Endogenous potentials are elicited by stim-
neurobiology of autism as a whole since high- ulus paradigms that require the subject to per-
functioning individuals have the same disorder form a perceptual analysis of a stimulus such
as low-functioning individuals. as distinguishing one tone from another. These
potentials occur between 30 milliseconds and
Evoked Potentials: Sensory and 1 second following the stimulus and depend
Cognitive Processing primarily on the setting in which the stimulus
occurs. They are relatively independent of the
Evoked potentials (EPs) are recordings from physical properties of the stimulus but are in-
an array of scalp electrodes of the neural re- fluenced by subject attention, comprehension
sponse to a sensory stimulus or cognitive chal- of the task, past experience with the task, im-
lenge task. The EPs or event-related potentials portance of task performance to the subject,
(ERPs) originate from a population of syn- and the subject’s ability level and knowledge.
chronous firing neurons in the brain, are time- Thus, all the various sources of influence on
locked to stimulus presentation, and can be human perception may have an influence on en-
averaged to distinguish them from background dogenous potentials.
electroencephalographic activity. Stimuli, such EPs can be recorded during both passive
as a visual image or a tone, trigger electrical and active tasks. In passive tasks, subjects are
activity within the brain within a millisecond not required to make a verbal or behavioral re-
time course, and simple sensory stimuli can sponse, and these tasks are appropriate for
evoke a prototypical response at many loca- younger children and low-functioning individ-
tions in the brain. For “ higher ” perceptual and uals. With few exceptions, EP research in
cognitive processes, scalp EPs likely reflect autism to date has been confined to the study
the activity and contributions of many differ- of auditory (exogenous) brainstem potentials,
ent neural systems that overlap in time and auditory (endogenous) potentials during atten-
spatial distribution. Given that EP recordings tional control and modulation, and, to a very
reflect the activation of neural circuitry, the limited extent, the visual (endogenous) poten-
resulting waveform is composed of multiple tials during passive viewing or low demand
476 Neurological and Medical Issues

tasks (e.g., target detection). Notable for their of age and the second by 3 years, which may
relative absence from the neurophysiologic lit- reflect different aspects of brainstem matura-
erature in autism are studies of the social, lan- tion (Aminoff, 1992; Zimmerman, Morgan, &
guage, and reasoning deficits inherent to the Dubno, 1987). Gender also impacts the norms
definition of this disorder. This bias is likely for these potentials, with females generally
due to the fact that these tasks require under- having shorter latencies with higher ampli-
standing of verbal directions or selective be- tudes and shorter I-V and III-V intervals com-
havioral responses and are often difficult to pared to males. Middle ear, cochlear, eighth
modify to fit the event-related nature of EP nerve, and brainstem pathology involving or
methodology. A second major research limita- immediately adjacent to the auditory pathways
tion of EPs is the degree to which scalp- may cause abnormalities in these potentials.
recorded EPs can be localized to generators The first study of brainstem auditory
within the brain. This limitation reflects evoked potentials (BAEPs) in autism reported
methodological constraints, as very few stud- normal results (Ornitz, Mo, & Olson, 1980;
ies have used high-density recording arrays, Ornitz & Walter, 1975) but was followed by a
making it difficult to employ source localiza- number of studies emphasizing abnormalities
tion algorithms; thus, little is known of the (Gillberg, Rosenthal, & Johansson, 1983;
precise localization of EP abnormalities. Rosenblum et al., 1980; Skoff, Mirsky, &
Turner, 1980; Student & Sohmer, 1978, 1979;
Auditory Brainstem Evoked
Tanguay & Edwards, 1982; Tanguay, Edwards,
Potentials (Exogenous)
Buchwald, Schwafel, & Allen, 1982; Taylor,
These potentials are generated within the first Rosenblatt, & Linschoten, 1982; Thivierge,
10 milliseconds following a click stimulus and Bedard, Cote, & Maziade, 1990; Wong &
reflect early neural activity in the auditory Wong, 1991). The latter studies reported ab-
pathway. Waves I and II represent activity in normalities consisting of moderate to severe
the eighth cranial nerve and are commonly abnormalities or absence of wave I (sen-
used to assess hearing in infants and noncoop- sorineural deafness) and delays in brainstem
erative children. Wave III is thought to result transmission time in 20% to 60% of the autis-
from activity in the lower pons and in the area tic subjects. Over time, the abnormalities de-
of the superior olive, and wave V from activity scribed in these studies were traced to various
in the upper pons and area of the inferior col- methodological limitations. The abnormalities
liculus. Waves I, III, and V are the most reli- reported by Student and Sohmer (1978) were
able waveforms. At lower stimulus intensities found to be secondary to equipment error and
or higher stimulation rates, only wave V re- were retracted (Student & Sohmer, 1979). Sev-
mains clinically reliable (Courchesne, Courch- eral abnormal findings from other studies
esne, Hicks, & Lincoln, 1985; Galambos & were traced to limitations in subject selection
Hecox, 1978; Starr, Sohmer, & Celesia, 1978; or ERP methodology. Subject selection proce-
Stockard, Stockard, & Sharbrough, 1978). dures often failed to exclude autistic subjects
Waves II and IV are often not measured be- with causes of brain damage other than
cause of lack of established clinical utility autism, to assess audiologic function, to ex-
(Chiappa & Gladstone, 1978; Rumsey, Grimes, clude individuals with hearing loss, and to
Pikus, Duara, & Ismond, 1984). match autistic and healthy subjects on age and
The most reliable measurements are the la- gender. Technical limitations of these early
tencies of the waves, particularly wave V, and studies included the failure to consider the im-
the interpeak intervals between waves I, III, pact of age and gender on measurements, to
and V, which are used to assess the integrity of differentiate between brainstem transmission
the brainstem auditory pathway, thalamocorti- times that were too short and too long when
cal pathway, and auditory nerve. In healthy in- determining the incidence of abnormalities, to
dividuals, the latencies of waves I and V assess measurement reliability, and to use reli-
achieve adult values by 2 years of age. The able EP measurements or procedures. The
maturation of the I-V interpeak latency (IPL) largest well controlled of these early studies
exhibits two phases, one completed by 1 year (Tanguay & Edwards, 1982; Tanguay et al.,
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 477

1982) emphasized abnormalities but actually abnormalities in the brainstem auditory ERPs
found very few abnormal brainstem transmis- were neither necessary nor sufficient to pro-
sion values except at the lowest stimulus inten- duce autism. These authors further emphasized
sity, and these abnormal values were in both that the previously demonstrated abnormalities
directions. When the autistic and control in longer latency sensory and cognitive EPs in
groups in this study were matched on both age these autistic subjects (Courchesne, Kilman,
and gender, no statistically significant abnor- Galambos, & Lincoln, 1984) were not the down-
malities were found. stream consequence of abnormalities in brain-
These various design and methodological stem auditory pathways but rather the result of
issues were rigorously addressed in the studies an abnormality in higher auditory processing.
of Rumsey et al. (1984) and Courchesne, In the early 1990s, reports of abnormal
Courchesne, et al. (1985). In a study of 25 BAEP reemerged (McClelland, Eyre, Watson,
children and adults with a wide range of Calvert, & Sherrard, 1992; Thivierge et al.,
autism severity, Rumsey et al. (1984) found 1990; Wong & Wong, 1991). However, it was
prolonged brainstem transmission (>3 SD of again difficult to interpret the significance of
laboratory norms) in one autistic subject and those findings for autism because of the inclu-
one normal control. Three autistic subjects sion of individuals with tuberous sclerosis,
were found to have shortened brainstem trans- fetal cytomegalovirus, Rett syndrome, and
mission times, which accounted for the few fragile X syndrome (Wong & Wong, 1991) and
group differences observed. Based on the re- the use of idiosyncratic methodology with un-
view of prior work and the results of their clear physiologic significance for brainstem
study, Rumsey and colleagues (1984) con- potentials (Thivierge et al., 1990). McClelland
cluded: “Reports of prolonged transmission et al. (1992) reported normal BAEPs in all
times in a substantial percentage of autistic autistic subjects under age 14 years and in all
children may be attributable to concomitant high-functioning autistic subjects. However,
identifiable neurological disease and peripheral the autism group failed to show brainstem au-
hearing impairments, inadequately matched ditory potential latency decreases after 14
control groups, high artifact levels, and poor years of age; this is in contrast to the decreases
reliability of measurements.” found in the normal control group (n = 54).
In 1985, Courchesne, Courchesne, et al. Since age effects on these potentials are
evaluated the BAEP of 14 high-functioning largely confined to the first 3 years of life, the
(PIQ > 70) and 14 normal control adolescents difference between autistic and control sub-
and young adults at slow, medium, and fast jects after 14 years of age may be related more
rates of stimulation; soft, medium, and loud to subject factors, such as how representative
stimulus intensities; right and left ear stimula- the control group was of normative values,
tion; and rarefaction and compression clicks. rather than to diagnosis. Although the investi-
The authors also controlled for body tempera- gators interpreted their data as evidence of a
ture. In a follow-up study, they further empha- delay in brainstem myelination, myelination of
sized the limitations of using small numbers of the brainstem is a very early developmental
normal subjects to represent population norms event that is complete within the first year of
(Grillon, Courchesne, & Akshoomoff, 1989). life. Abnormal brainstem myelination would
This study found no group differences in the result in abnormalities in BAEPs prior to age
auditory brainstem EPs among autistic sub- 14 in the autism group; these results seem in-
jects, normal controls, and clinical norms consistent with delayed myelination as an ex-
under any of the stimulus conditions. Exami- planation. Alternatively, this pattern of
nation of the individual cases further revealed findings, if demonstrated to be valid, might re-
that every autistic subject in the study had nor- flect the observations of Bauman and Kemper
mal auditory EPs. of the persistence of a fetal pattern in inferior
Courchesne, Courchesne, et al. (1985) con- olivary circuits, which they suggested may be
cluded that there was unlikely to be an abnor- vulnerable to deterioration during the teens.
mality in autism in the brainstem auditory In a more recent study, Maziade et al. (2000)
pathways that generate early ERPs and that investigated the BAEP in autistic individuals
478 Neurological and Medical Issues

and their unaffected relatives. Compared to the numbers of subjects (Buchwald et al., 1992;
control group, the autistic participants had pro- Grillon et al., 1989). Grillon et al. found no ab-
longed I-III IPLs. Some of the unaffected first- normalities in Na, Pa, and Nb latencies and
degree relatives also showed this same Wave Na-Pa and Pa-Nb amplitudes in their
prolongation of the early brain-auditory evoked study of eight autistic young adults and age-,
response. However, in about 50% of the fami- gender-, and performance IQ-matched normal
lies, neither the autistic participants nor the controls. Buchwald et al. studied Pa and P1 in
parents showed a significant IPL prolongation. 11 high-functioning autistic subjects and re-
Therefore, the authors concluded that prolon- ported normal Pa amplitude and latency. In
gation of the IPL is not a necessary or suffi- contrast, P1 in the autistic subjects was re-
cient cause in the development of autism ported to be smaller in amplitude and did not
(Maziade et al., 2000). Maziade et al. also habituate with increasing stimulus rate. The
found cases where the parents had prolonged failure of habituation was interpreted as evi-
IPL, but their autistic child did not, and the dence of a disturbance in the input of the
opposite pattern where the parents did not brainstem ascending reticular activating sys-
have prolonged IPL, but the autistic child did. tem cholinergic neurons to the thalamus. How-
Nagy and Loveland (2002) proposed that it ever, it is equally plausible, given the data in
might be beneficial to subdivide the impaired this study, that the failure of P1 to habituate
and nonimpaired subjects into separate represents diminished thalamic and auditory
groups. This finding and its familial pattern cortical feedback inhibition on P1 generators.
require further investigation. Additional data are required to differentiate
Two additional studies have recently been between these two possibilities.
conducted on BAEP with inconsistent results. To date, there is one study investigating the
Rosenhall, Nordin, Brantberg, and Gillberg P50 gating response. The P50 gating response
(2003) found the III-V IPL was significantly reflects the decline of the amplitude of the P50
prolonged in male and female autistic individ- component of the AEP to the second of a pair of
uals with normal hearing. There were, how- clicks and is thought to measure stimulus filter-
ever, no significant differences in the latencies ing or inhibitory mechanisms. Children with
for wave III or the I-V IPL. These findings are autism spectrum disorder have been described
inconsistent with the results of a study by as both hypersensitive and hyposensitive to au-
Chen et al. (2003), which found that a group of ditory sound and it has been suggested that peo-
children with autism had longer latencies than ple with autism may have difficulty filtering
a control group in waves III and V as well as sensory input (Kootz, Marinelli, & Cohen,
longer IPLs in wave I-II and I-V. 1982). Kemner, Oranje, Verbaten, and van En-
Given the inconsistent results of the various geland (2002) found that children between 7
BAEP studies, it is apparent that further re- and 13 years of age with autism and controls
search is needed to determine whether a solid both demonstrated P50 suppression. The au-
relationship exists between prolonged auditory thors suggest that children with autism demon-
brainstem transmission times and autism. strate normal excitability of the neuronal
substrate related to P50 gating. While these re-
Auditory Middle Latency Potentials
sults are consistent with Ornitz and colleagues
The middle latency auditory potentials occur (1993) who found normal stimulus filtering
10 to 50 milliseconds following a click stimu- using a measure of prepulse inhibition of the
lus and consist of several positive and negative acoustic startle response (Ornitz, Lane, Sug-
components: (1) Na (10 to 25 milliseconds), yama, & de Traversay, 1993), further work is
(2) Pa (25 to 40 milliseconds), (3) Nb (40 to 50 needed on larger as well as younger samples.
milliseconds), and (4) P1 (50 to 65 millisec-
Long Latency Auditory Potentials
onds). These potentials are thought to repre-
sent activity in the thalamus, thalamocortical The long latency potentials to a frequent stim-
radiations, and primary auditory cortex. Only ulus occur after 50 milliseconds and consist of
two studies have investigated middle latency a large negative (N1)-positive (P2) complex,
potentials in autism and then only in small which is maximal in amplitude at the vertex
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 479

(Goodin, 1992). The generators of these po- within the typical range in terms of latency in
tentials are unknown. At least part of this ac- children with autism (Bruneau et al., 1999;
tivity is from neural areas that can be Lincoln, Courchesne, Harms, & Allen, 1995;
activated by more than one sensory modality. Novick, Vaughan, Kurtzberg, & Simson, 1980,
Like the early and middle latency potentials, but see Oades, Walker, Geffen, & Stern, 1988).
this complex is a stimulus-related response; The N1c component is thought to be generated
thus the amplitude and latency of these poten- within the auditory association cortex ( lateral
tials are related to stimulus intensity and fre- surface of the superior temporal gyrus). In
quency and are relatively independent of contrast to the N1b, the N1c wave showed the
subject attention. The long latency potentials most significant differences between the sub-
to the frequent stimulus reach adult levels by 5 jects with autism and the two control groups
to 6 years if not before (Courchesne, 1978; (normal control group and mentally retarded
Finley, Faux, Hutcheson, & Amstutz, 1985; subjects without autistic symptoms). At bilat-
Goodin, Squires, & Starr, 1978; Kurtzberg eral temporal sites, the autism group’s N1c
et al., 1984; Polich & Starr, 1984). In the mid- wave had a smaller amplitude and longer la-
teens to early 20s, the P2 latency begins to in- tency than either of the other groups (Bruneau
crease linearly with age. Gender also has et al., 1999). Additionally, the N1c wave of the
a significant impact on these potentials autism group showed a hemispheric differ-
(Stockard et al., 1978). ence. While the left and right hemispheres of
The auditory N1 and P2 potentials to fre- the control groups showed an increase in the
quent tones have been examined in autism in N1c peak amplitude with increasing stimulus
only a few studies. Courchesne and colleagues intensity, the autism group failed to show this
(Courchesne, Courchesne, et al., 1985; Cour- effect on the left side.
chesne, Lincoln, Kilman, & Galambos, 1985; Similar results were found (Bruneau, Bonnet-
Courchesne, Lincoln, Yeung-Courchesne, El- Brilhault, Gomot, Adrien, & Barthélémy, 2003)
masian, & Grillon, 1989) did not find any dif- when these authors investigated N1c wave and
ferences in N1 or P2 amplitude or latency the verbal and nonverbal abilities in children
between autistic subjects and normal controls with autism. The N1c wave was found to have
to a listening task and a two-tone task. Ferri both smaller amplitude and a longer latency in
et al. (2003) found that the N1 latency was sig- the autistic group when 750-Hz tones of vary-
nificantly shorter and that amplitude was not ing intensity were presented. The amplitude of
significantly different in the autistic group the N1c wave also increased with increasing
using 1000 Hz tones. Contrary to these results, stimulus intensity in both the healthy control
Seri, Cerquiglini, Pisani, and Curatolo (1999) group and the group with autism, but again the
found the N1 response in tuberous sclerosis effect was not seen on the left side for the
patients with autistic behaviors to have pro- autism participants, suggesting a lateralized
longed latencies with lower amplitudes. How- disturbance in early cortical sensory process-
ever, while this indicates a deficit in auditory ing in the left hemisphere. In a contrasting re-
sensory processing, due to the tuberous sclero- port, the N1c was found to not be delayed in 7-
sis, these results cannot be viewed as charac- to 11-year-old children with autism with nor-
teristic of autism alone. mal mental functioning during passive listen-
Bruneau, Roux, Adrien, and Barthélémy ing but was delayed in the same children when
(1999) investigated auditory processing at the the task involved selective attention or word
cortical level using late auditory EPs (N1 classification (Dunn, Vaughan, Kreuzer, &
wave-T complex) to pure tones in 4- to 8-year- Kurtzer, 1999).
old autistic children with mental retardation.
Auditory Cognitive Potentials
Bruneau et al. (1999) specifically looked at
two negative peaks, the N1b and the N1c, The long latency response to a rare auditory
which peak around 140 ms and 170 ms, re- stimulus is considerably different from the re-
spectively. The N1b component is thought to sponse to the frequent stimulus, consisting of a
have cortical generators on the superior tem- negative (N1)-positive (apparent P2)-negative
poral plane; this component appears to be (N2)-positive (P3) complex (Goodin, 1992).
480 Neurological and Medical Issues

The apparent P2 is so named because it repre- “less” processing. Lincoln, Courchesne, Harms,
sents the sum of the stimulus-related P2 and the and Allen (1993) also reported decreased am-
event-related P165. The P3 component has a la- plitudes of P3b responses to auditory stimuli in
tency of 300 to 400 milliseconds after a rare young nonretarded autistic children, indicating
“odd-ball” stimulus, and its amplitude is maxi- difficulty in modifying expectancies to contex-
mal in the midline over the central and parietal tually relevant sequences of auditory informa-
scalp regions. The P3 peak can be further tion and perhaps a general disturbance in
resolved into two components referred to as habituation processes that interfere with the
P3a and P3b. The P3a subcomponent appears discrimination of novel information. The reac-
to be constant regardless of subject attention, tion times of responses to oddball or rare stim-
whereas the P3b component appears to be sen- uli by the autistic children in this study were
sitive to task requirements (Squires, Squires, & normal, indicating intact sensory processing
Hillyard, 1975). These long-latency ERPs can and task compliance. Similar findings of de-
be recorded to stimuli in any of the sensory creased P3b amplitude have been reported by
modalities and can be recorded in response to other investigators during visual /audio divided
the unexpected omission of an anticipated attention tasks (Ciesielski, Courchesne, & El-
stimulus. These potentials are relatively insen- masian, 1990; Courchesne et al., 1989) and
sitive to stimulus intensity but are very sensi- have been linked to deficits in language func-
tive to change in the ease with which targets tion (Dawson, Finley, Phillips, Galpert, &
can be distinguished from nontargets, by alter- Lewy, 1988).
ations in the ratio of target to nontarget stim- Of note, a subsequent study of children with
uli, or by fluctuations in subject attention to pervasive developmental disorder, using a simi-
the stimulus. The N2 and P3 components are lar auditory detection task, has partially failed
markedly prolonged in young children and de- to replicate these results. Kemner et al. (1995)
crease in latency with increasing age until replicated Courchesne et al. (1984, 1985) with
reaching adult values in adolescence and respect to decreased A / Pcz/300 potentials in
young adulthood. Thereafter, there is a grad- children with autism ages 8 to 13 years who
ual increase in latency with increasing age. were matched to children with typical develop-
Gender does not impact latency, but P3 ampli- ment, ADHD, and dyslexia. However, no differ-
tude tends to be larger in females. ences were found to the mismatch negativity,
Numerous papers have reported abnormali- N1, and P3 components, and the lateralization
ties in auditory cognitive potentials in autism. of components. Unexpectedly in an auditory
In selective attention paradigms, unexpected, task, the authors also found P3 differences at
rare, or novel auditory (or visual) probes are the occipital midline lead, which was sug-
inserted into a sequence of attended, standard, gested as reflecting abnormal cortical organi-
or expected sounds. This can be conducted zation to auditory input (Kemner, Verbaten,
during a passive task, in which the subject Cuperus, Camfferman, & Van Engeland, 1995).
simply listens to the sequence of sounds or Further, in an easy and hard auditory detec-
during an active task in which the subject tion task with a secondary passive visual task,
presses a button when a target sound occurs. all subjects demonstrated increased amplitude
Abnormal auditory P300 potentials to rare to the auditory stimuli with increased task
tones and stimulus omissions in autism were difficulty. However, children (and to a lesser
first reported by Novick et al. (Novick, extent adolescents) with pervasive develop-
Kurtzberg, & Vaughan, 1979; Novick et al., mental disorder failed to demonstrate a de-
1980) and then replicated by Courchesne and crease in P3 amplitude to the irrelevant visual
colleagues (1984; Courchesne, Lincoln, et al., probes with increased auditory task difficulty.
1985). In general, the autism group did per- The authors suggested that this might reflect
ceive the novel information as being novel; a failure to allocate processing resources
that is, the novel unexpected sounds evoked a (Hoeksma, Kemner, Verbaten, & van Enge-
different EP response, but the autism group land, 2004).
compared to controls exhibited smaller P3 The P3 has also been used to assess re-
amplitudes to the novel probes suggestive of sponses to affective and nonaffective prosodic
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 481

language discrimination. Erwin et al. (1991) detection of a deviant stimulus, the P3a is
reported normal P3 responses in adult autistic thought to deal with the events that will ulti-
individuals of normal intelligence during mately result in a behavioral response due to
prosodic discriminations (Erwin et al., 1991). that unexpected stimulus.
From a language standpoint, the tasks used in Only a few studies have looked at the MMN
this study were of low difficulty, and the and/or the P3a wave in relation to autism, and
prosodic discriminations were well within the the results remain inconclusive. Seri et al.
abilities of adults with autism of normal IQ, (1999) investigated autism in individuals with
whose most prominent impairments are in tuberous sclerosis and found that MMN was
higher order prosodic discriminations related present in all of the subjects but that the MMN
to satire and innuendo. One study (Strandburg latency was significantly longer in the subjects
et al., 1993) reported increased P3 responses with autistic behavior. In a study done by Ferri
to oddball stimuli in a visual continuous per- et al. (2003), the amplitude of the MMN in
formance task and a visually presented idiom low-functioning autistic individuals to the de-
recognition task. In contrast to the simple viant stimuli (1300 Hz sinusoidal tone rather
prosodic discriminations of the Erwin study, than the standard 1000 Hz) was significantly
idiomatic language is a well-documented ele- larger than in the normal control group. Ferri
ment of the deficits in higher order language et al. (2003) suggested that their results may
abilities in high-functioning individuals with indicate problems with auditory sensory pro-
autism. The Strandburg finding could indicate cessing at early stages in the temporal cortex.
a modality effect with different P3 effects to Additionally, the MMN latencies for the autis-
some auditory and visual stimuli or an indica- tic group tended to be shorter (Ferri et al.,
tion that there may be some situations where 2003; Gomot, Giard, Adrien, Barthelemy, &
autistic individuals may have a greater alloca- Bruneau, 2002). Gomot et al. suggested that
tion of attention to some tasks to compensate the shortened MMN latencies may be the re-
for dysfunction in cognitive processes that sult of heightened cerebral reactivity to audi-
would otherwise interfere with performance. tory change, which allows autistic individuals
Two additional automatic components of to detect variations in auditory inputs more
auditory ERPs, the mismatch negativity (MMN) quickly than controls. However, Kemner et al.
and the P3a wave, have been investigated in (1995), with children with autism compared to
relation to autism. The MMN is an index of 3 control groups matched on age found no dif-
auditory processing that occurs due to a devia- ferences in the MMN (also see Ceponiene
tion in repetitive auditory stimulation (Uther, et al., 2003) and P3.
Jansen, Huotilainen, Ilmoniemi, & Näätänen, There has been one study that has measured
2003). The “mismatch” occurs when the ex- MMN in relation to speech processing in
pected standardized auditory stimulus is re- young children. In children aged 3 to 4 with
placed with an unexpected stimulus. It is autism, Kuhl and colleagues (2005) found that
considered to be automatic because even when the children with autism did not show MMN
the stimulus is not being attended to, the responses to changes in speech syllables while
MMN still appears when an atypical stimulus typical children matched on mental age or
is presented (Uther et al., 2003). In cases chronological age did. In addition, as a group,
where the deviant or novel stimulus is largely the children with autism also failed to show a
different from the frequent and expected stan- listening preference for human speech over
dard stimulus, a P3a wave is exhibited just non-speech. When the autism group was di-
after the MMN (Friedman, Cycowicz, & vided based on their preference for human
Gaeta, 2001). The P3a wave (or novelty P3) is speech versus nonhuman speech, the group that
a component of the P3 that is said to engage the preferred human speech did show a MMN that
frontal lobe in response to novel stimuli. It is was similar to the typical children; the group
thought that the P3a is a late stage of novelty that preferred nonspeech did not. The authors
processing most likely related to the evaluative suggest that the lack of MMN in some of the
aspects of the orienting response (Friedman children with autism suggests central auditory
et al., 2001). So while the MMN represents the deficits although additional work would be
482 Neurological and Medical Issues

needed to determine if the lack of MMN was Nc to be small and often absent to auditory and
reflective of a failure to differentiate complex visual stimuli and to the omission of auditory
auditory signals or specific to speech (Kuhl, and visual stimuli. P3b was also documented to
Coffey-Corina, Padden, & Dawson, 2005). be small, demonstrating the coexistence of ab-
In Ferri et al.’s (2003) study, the P3a wave normalities in these two cognitive potentials.
also revealed significant differences between When Kemner, van der Gaag, Verbaten, and
the autistic group and the control group. The van Engeland (1999) had participants perform
P3a amplitude increased with age in the con- a visual task with an oddball stimulus compo-
trol group, but in the autistic group, the oppo- nent, they were not able to replicate Courchesne
site pattern was found: Larger amplitudes et al.’s (1987, 1989) findings that the Nc is
were seen in childhood with decreasing ampli- smaller in autistic children, suggesting that the
tudes in early adulthood (Ferri et al., 2003). effect may be modality specific.
Since the P3a is thought to be an automatic Last, the N4 component is a negative com-
and involuntary response that represents a ponent that peaks at approximately 400 mil-
later stage of novelty processing (Friedman liseconds after stimulus presentation and is
et al., 2001), Ferri et al. (2003) proposed that thought to be related to semantic processing
autistic individuals may be better at shifting and varies based on the expectation of the as-
their attention to novel stimuli in childhood sociation between words. For example, the am-
than in adulthood. Ceponiene et al. (2003) plitude of the N4 is related to the degree to
looked at the P3a and how it was affected which a word is related to the semantic context,
when changes in simple tones, complex tones, with incongruent words resulting in greater am-
and vowels were presented using standard and plitude and longer latencies (Kutas, Linda-
deviant stimuli. High-functioning autistic par- mood, & Hillyard, 1984). In children 7 to 11
ticipants showed normal P3a responses for years of age with autism and nonverbal IQ-
changes in both simple and complex tones, but matched typical children, Dunn and colleagues
no P3a was present for changes in the vowel. (1999) found that the autism group failed to
Given that the complex tones were similar to show differential N4 amplitude for targets (ex-
the speech sounds in complexity, it seems that pected animal word) and nontargets (incon-
the “speechness” of the vowel sounds acted as gruent nonanimal word). The typical group did
a limiting factor in the autistic group’s orient- show a differential response between the two
ing abilities, which may indicate a deficit in categories. The authors interpreted this find-
vowel exclusive attention that could hinder ing as a failure of the children with autism to
verbal communication in autistic individuals develop an expectancy for the within category
(Ceponiene et al., 2003). words, possibly suggestive of abnormal lexical
A fifth potential, the Nc, has been the sub- organization. In addition, as reported earlier,
ject of more recent investigation in autism children with autism also showed increased la-
(Courchesne, Elmasian, & Yeung-Courchesne, tencies for the N1 and P2 components.
1987; Courchesne et al., 1989). Nc is the earli-
Visual and Somatosensory
est endogenous component to appear develop-
Cognitive Potentials
mentally and is elicitable in infants. In general,
the Nc is thought to reflect an obligatory atten- Fewer EP studies have been conducted with vi-
tional response and is elicited by both auditory sual than auditory stimuli. Some data suggest
and visual stimuli; it is known to vary based on that late visual evoked responses (P3b) to novel
frequency of stimulus presentation, familiarity stimuli are abnormal (Novick et al., 1979; Ver-
with the stimulus, and habituation (Nelson, baten, Roelofs, van Engeland, Kenemans, &
1994). Nc has an onset at around 100 to 200 Slangen, 1991), but they may be less impaired
milliseconds with a peak amplitude at about than auditory responses (Courchesne et al.,
350 to 450 milliseconds and is maximal in am- 1989; Courchesne, Lincoln, et al., 1985). Stud-
plitude over the front of the scalp. ies of somatosensory ERPs in autism also have
Nc abnormalities also appear to be a promi- demonstrated abnormal P3 responses (Kemner,
nent cognitive potential finding in autism. Verbaten, Cuperus, Camfferman, & van Enge-
Courchesne et al. (1987, 1989) have reported land, 1994).
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 483

Townsend et al. (2001) studied visuospatial individuals show faster N170 latencies to faces
processing in a group of high-functioning compared to nonface stimuli and show faster
autistic individuals, specifically looking at the activation to upright compared to inverted
late positive complex (LPC), which is said to faces. Three- to 4-year-old and 6-year-old
consist of three main components: an early children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
fronto-centrally maximal response, a parietally also fail to show a temporal benefit to process-
maximal P3b, and a posterior positive slow ing faces in comparison to typical developing
wave. In the autistic group, a delay was found in and developmental-delayed control groups
the early frontal LPC responses as well as (Webb, Bernier, Panagiotides, Paul, & Daw-
smaller amplitude for the parietal maximal LPC son, 2003; Webb, Bernier, Shook, Paul, &
responses. Autistic participants also had accu- Dawson, 2004; Webb, Dawson, Bernier, &
racy difficulties when targets were displayed in Panagiotides, 2004). These results were inter-
the visual periphery. These findings may indi- preted as reflecting aberrant neural circuitry
cate impairments in spatial orienting and/or resulting in less efficient processing strategies
encoding of spatial information (Townsend (Dawson et al., 2005).
et al., 2001). As to later stage visual processing EPs,
Both early and late EP components of vi- Dawson et al. (2002) evaluated children with
sual processing have been studied during face ASD to evaluate face and object recognition
and object perception and memory in children ability in comparison to children with delayed
and adults with autism. In typical adults, faces development (DD) and children with typical
evoke a specific ERP component that is nega- development (TD). Participants (ages 3 to 4
tive going and peaks at approximately 170 mil- years) were shown a picture of their mother (fa-
liseconds. This N170 is more negative in the miliar face), a picture of an unfamiliar female
right than left hemisphere to faces; more nega- face, a picture of a familiar object (favorite
tive to eyes, inverted faces, and upright faces toy), and a picture of an unfamiliar toy. They
than other stimuli; and faster to upright faces found that unlike the TD and DD children,
than other face parts and other stimuli (e.g., children with ASD did not show amplitude
Bentin, Allison, Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, differences in P400 or Nc when viewing the
1996). This component is thought to reflect familiar face versus the unfamiliar face. How-
early stage processing of faces and has been ever, similar to the TD and DD children, ASD
hypothesized to be indicative of configural children did show amplitude differences in
processing. The N170 undergoes a prolonged both P400 and Nc when they were looking at a
developmental course with decreases in ampli- picture of a familiar object (a favorite toy)
tude and latency from 4 to 14 years of age (Tay- versus an unfamiliar one. This is of interest
lor, McCarthy, Saliba, & Degiovanni, 1999). In because while the ASD children showed dif-
addition to early stage processing of faces, face ferential brain activity for objects, they did not
and object memory differentially evoke later show this pattern for faces, indicating a social
stage components such as the Nc, P400, and processing deficit (Dawson et al., 2002). This
slow wave in children. These components are finding agrees with those found in related stud-
influenced by stimulus familiarity, repetition, ies, such as one by Pelphrey et al. (2002), which
and task directions. investigated how individuals with autism scan
Individuals with autism use atypical behav- human faces. The autistic participants spent
ioral strategies for processing faces and have less time fixating on feature areas of the face,
impaired face and emotion memory (Dawson, such as the eyes, mouth, and nose, than did the
Webb, & McPartland, 2005). McPartland, Daw- control group (Pelphrey et al., 2002).
son, Webb, Carver, and Panagiotides (2004)
found that high-functioning adolescents and Oculomotor Physiology: Motor and
adults with autism show delayed N170 laten- Cognitive Physiology
cies to faces compared to nonface stimuli
(e.g., furniture) and fail to show a temporal As with the EP literature in autism, eye move-
benefit for the processing of upright compared ment studies in the 1960s and 1970s reported
to inverted faces; age- and IQ-matched typical abnormalities involving postrotatory nystagmus
484 Neurological and Medical Issues

and nystagmus during REM sleep (Ornitz, cal regions involved in basic oculomotor con-
Brown, Mason, & Putnam, 1974; Ornitz, trol. By varying saccadic eye movement tasks
Forsythe, & de la Pena, 1973; Ornitz & Ritvo, in different ways, such as by instructing sub-
1968a; Ornitz et al., 1968; Ornitz et al., 1969; jects to look away from lights, requiring de-
Ritvo et al., 1969). However, these investiga- layed responses to locations that need to be
tors subsequently concluded that their early remembered for brief periods of time, and cue-
findings were the result of idiosyncratic meth- ing locations where targets will be presented,
ods, and a repetition of these studies using various cortical visual attention systems can
current methodology revealed only one statis- be evaluated.
tically significant result (Ornitz, Atwell, Ka- Eye movement studies of autism have in-
plan, & Westlake, 1985). Only a few other cluded analysis of the oculomotor effects of
studies have been added to this literature. vestibular challenge that bear on the func-
To assess vestibular function, eye move- tional integrity of the cerebellum (Minshew,
ment responses to caloric challenge, or rota- Furman, Goldstein, & Payton, 1990; Ornitz
tion of the head or body, can be assessed by et al., 1985); saccadic and pursuit responses to
measuring the duration and amplitude of the visual stimuli that assess the functional in-
procedure-induced nystagmus as well as the tegrity of the brainstem, cerebellum, and neo-
velocity of the slow phase component. Eye cortex (Minshew et al., 1990; Rosenhall,
movement studies also can provide important Johansson, & Gillberg, 1988); and volitional
information about the well-characterized cor- saccadic eye movement subserved by frontal
tical and subcortical regions that control eye and parietal cortex (Minshew, Luna, &
movement activity. Such eye movement studies Sweeney, 1999).
can be viewed as complementary to sensory- Ornitz et al. (1985) tested 22 autistic
evoked responses, in that they provide infor- patients and reported slightly prolonged postro-
mation primarily about the motor system and tary nystagmus, though there was no distur-
sensorimotor integration rather than about bance in gain of the slow component. These
sensory information processing. For quantita- equivocal findings are difficult to interpret be-
tive eye movement studies, patients are typi- cause the study was performed in the dark,
cally taken to a dark room, their heads are and there can be considerable subject variabil-
comfortably restrained so that head and eye ity in performance in this situation based on
movements are not confounded, and various idiosyncratic factors such as whether subjects
eye movement tasks are performed. Eye move- imagine a visual stimulus moving with them or
ments can be recorded noninvasively by electro- a stimulus that is stationary (Leigh & Zee,
oculography (EOG) procedures (similar to 1991). In addition, the statistically significant
those used in EEG studies to identify eye difference was confined to one parameter, the
movement “artifacts”), camera-based record- vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) time constant,
ing systems, or direct monitoring of the reflec- and appeared to be the result of two outliers in
tion of an infrared light source from the the data rather than a trend for the group as a
corneal-scleral margin. whole. Goldberg, Landa, Lasker, Cooper, and
Responses to lights moving abruptly from Zee (2000) performed a VOR experiment mea-
one point to another can be measured ( latency, suring the duration of vestibular responses and
accuracy, peak velocity) to assess the integrity tilt-suppression of postrotatory nystagmus and
of saccadic eye movements, and the tracking found that tilt-suppression of the VOR was not
of slowly but steadily moving targets can be impaired in children with autism, nor were
evaluated to assess smooth pursuit eye move- there any differences in the vestibular re-
ments (Leigh & Zee, 1991). These procedures sponses during the rotation, which suggests
approximate those used in neurological exami- that the cerebellar nodulus, the uvula, and the
nations but are conducted under controlled vestibular system are not dysfunctional. Rosen-
conditions and in ways that are amenable to hall et al. (1988) reported hypometric horizon-
quantitative analysis. Examining reflexive tal visually guided saccades to unpredictable
saccadic responses to unpredictable lights can targets in 6 of 11 low-IQ autistic children.
be informative about the integrity of subcorti- Pursuit eye movements were described as nor-
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 485

mal. However, some patients were taking CNS- cades may be due to attentional difficulties in
active medications that affect eye movements, autism and was further explored by van der
cooperation was frequently a problem, and the Geest, Kemner, Cammfferman, Verbaten, and
healthy subjects had normal IQ in comparison van Engeland (2001) using a gap-overlap para-
to the high rates of mental retardation in the digm. In both conditions, subjects fixated on a
autistic subjects. cross in the center of the screen, while waiting
In a study of voluntary saccadic eye move- to look at a square that was to appear on the
ments known to be subserved by discrete re- right or left side of the cross. In the overlap
gions in frontal and parietal cortex, Minshew condition, the cross remained on the screen
et al. (1999) found significant abnormalities in when the square appeared, but in the gap con-
cortically controlled eye movements in 26 dition, the cross disappeared 200 milliseconds
autistic subjects compared to 26 age, IQ and before the appearance of the square.
gender-matched controls. The autistic subjects In comparison to the control group, the
demonstrated significant impairments both in autistic group in this study showed no differ-
the ability to willfully suppress saccades to ences in performance on the gap or overlap
unpredictable targets and to shift gaze to re- conditions (van der Geest et al., 2001). This
membered target locations. These findings, to- suggests a deficiency in attentional engage-
gether with findings of intact visually guided ment, which may be related to dysfunction in
saccades in the same cases, indicate a distur- several areas of the brain, including the frontal
bance in the cortical connectivity required for eye fields, the superior colliculus, and/or pari-
volitional control of saccadic eye movements. etal cortex.
Recently, Goldberg et al. (2002) recorded eye
movements of high-functioning autistic individ- Postural Physiology
uals on antisaccade, memory-guided saccade
(MGS), predictive saccade, and gap/overlap Studies of postural function are another
tasks. In comparison to normal subjects, high- method for providing direct and specific evi-
functioning individuals with autism had dence of the physiologic integrity of the
greater difficulty voluntarily suppressing eye vestibular system including the cerebellum.
movements to visual targets, replicating the Although posterior fossa circuitry contributes
Minshew et al. (1999) findings. Koczat, Rogers, significantly to postural function, contribu-
Pennington, and Ross (2002) performed a study tions from more widespread regions are also
to determine whether this deficit in memory- important. Kohen-Raz, Volkmar, and Cohen
guided saccades is also found in the parents of (1992) conducted a study of 91 autistic chil-
individuals with autism. Using a similar oculo- dren and adolescents ages 6 to 20 years
motor delayed response task, they found simi- chronologically and 8 months to 7 years in
lar deficits in unaffected family members. mental age using a computerized posturo-
To further define the circuitry underlying graphic procedure that evaluated the effects of
spatial working memory in autism, Luna et al. various stresses to this system. These children
(2002) completed an fMRI study using this were compared to 166 normal 4- to 11-year-
same task. This study demonstrated signifi- old children, 18 mentally retarded children (7
cantly less activation in dorsolateral prefrontal to 16 years chronologically), and 20 normal
cortex and posterior cingulate in the 11 high- adults with vestibular disease. Postural sway
functioning autistic subjects compared to 6 was recorded as changes in weight distribution
healthy controls during the performance of as subjects stood on plates, one for the heel
this task. and toe of each foot. The autistic subjects and
Kemner, Verbaten, Cuperus, Camfferman, the mentally retarded nonautistic subjects had
and van Engeland (1998) found that autistic significantly lower postural stability than the
children make more saccades during and be- control subjects, performing at the level of
tween stimulus presentations than the healthy preschool children even as adolescents. The
individuals and children with attention deficit autism group showed paradoxically better sta-
disorder, regardless of the type of stimulus bility when vision was occluded or somatosen-
shown. The abnormally high number of sac- sory input restricted by standing on pads. They
486 Neurological and Medical Issues

also exhibited unusual monopodal or tripodal matched in age, IQ, and gender using dynamic
stances. The latter two findings were not seen posturography. Autistic individuals with asso-
in the mentally retarded nonautistic subjects. ciated neurologic, genetic, infectious, meta-
In this study, the postural impairment of the bolic, or seizure disorders, as well as those
autistic subjects was comparable to that of the taking any medication known to affect the
small group of mentally retarded nonautistic measurements under investigation, were ex-
subjects; it was only the paradoxical improve- cluded from the study. Compared to previous
ment in stability with visual occlusion and so- studies, this is the only one that coupled floor
matosensory restriction that distinguished and/or visual surround motion to postural
them from the mentally retarded nonautistic sway. Autistic subjects were found to have re-
subjects. There were also some limitations to duced postural stability and delayed develop-
this study, as roughly a third of the sample was ment of postural stability. Postural stability
taking neuroleptic medications, and many were was reduced under all conditions but was only
unable to maintain cooperation through all clinically significant when somatosensory
procedures. input was disrupted alone or in combination
In Gepner, Mestre, Masson, and de Schonen with other sensory challenges. Postural control
(1995) studied five mentally retarded autistic did not begin to improve in the autistic sub-
children ages 4 to 7 years and 12 normal con- jects until 12 years of age and never achieved
trols. The subjects stood on a force platform adult levels, whereas in normal controls it im-
with three strain gauges measuring shifts in proved steadily from age 5 years to 15 to 20
their center of gravity. The mentally retarded years when it reached adult levels. There were
children with autism had impaired postural no abnormalities in the adaptation ratios, mea-
stability compared to the normal children surements primarily dependent on motor con-
under eyes closed and static visual conditions trol. Rather, the abnormalities in postural
but were more stable to visually perceived en- control were indicative of deficits in sensori-
vironmental motion than controls. It was not motor integration, which is dependent on a
clear whether this was due to general lack of widely distributed multineuronal system that
attention to surrounding motion or to a deficit typically involves the basal ganglia, supplemen-
in motion processing. tary motor cortex, anterior cingulate cortex,
More recently, Molloy, Dietrich, and Bhat- and subcortical connections more generally.
tacharya (2003) investigated the postural sta- The decreased postural stability found in
bility of 8 children with ASD ages 5 to 12 this study is part of a more pervasive impair-
years of age with a receptive language level of ment in movement that ranges from fine and
at least 4 years. The participants stood on a gross motor apraxia to the planning and execu-
firm or foam platform with their eyes opened tion of skilled motor sequences. The evidence
or closed. This study measured both sway area that deficits in motor sequences and now sen-
and sway length over a 30-second time period sory integration are integral parts of the
for each of the four conditions. ASD children autism syndrome suggests that the neural ab-
were, in general, found to have an increased normalities responsible for autism are not
sway area. Contrary to Kohen-Raz et al.’s restricted to the neural systems involved in so-
(1992) findings, this study found that when the cial, language, and reasoning abilities. Rather,
visual cues, somatosensory cues, or both were the motor and sensory deficits suggest more
modified, the sway area increased; that is, they general involvement of neural circuitry related
were less stable, not more stable. Based on to a cytoarchitectural feature of brain organi-
these findings, children with ASD exhibited a zation required for higher levels of integration
deficit in the capacity of afferent systems to of information.
coordinate postural stability. In others words,
their postural instability was the result of a Neuropsychologic Profile
deficit in sensory integration.
Minshew, Sung, Jones, and Furman (2004) Comprehensive studies of neuropsychologic
tested the postural stability of 79 nonretarded functioning in autism are relatively recent, hav-
high-functioning autistic individuals ages 5 to ing awaited a consensus on the diagnostic cri-
52 years compared to 61 healthy controls teria and research methods for defining autism
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 487

in verbal individuals with IQs in the normal was not that of a general deficit syndrome or
and near-normal range. Studies of multiple do- mental retardation, since autistic subjects did
mains of function within the same individuals as well or better than age- and IQ-matched
with autism remain few, as the tendency of controls in preserved areas and substantially
most studies has been to focus on a single area below expectations based on age and IQ in
of function. The rationale for studies of multi- deficit areas.
ple domains, from a neurologic perspective, is The profile of neuropsychologic function-
to determine the common feature of impair- ing in autism is one of deficits in complex or
ments and common feature of intact abilities, higher order cognitive and neurologic abilities
as common features may provide clues to the as defined relative to age and IQ expectations
essential aspects of the neural basis of autism. and intact or enhanced simpler abilities rela-
The first such study in 1988 reported a pat- tive to age and IQ expectations. This profile
tern of neuropsychologic functioning charac- has been replicated in a second sample of
terized by the predominance of deficits in adults with high-functioning autism (Min-
abstraction and conceptual reasoning, relative shew, unpublished data) and in children 8 to
sparing of memory and language, and intact 15 years of age with high-functioning autism
visuospatial, sensoriperceptual, and motor (Minshew, unpublished data).
abilities in 10 autistic men with average full- A subsequent fMRI study demonstrated an
scale and verbal IQ scores (Rumsey & Ham- analogous pattern of brain activation with en-
burger, 1988, 1990). Several other laboratories hanced activation in Wernicke’s area for word
confirmed this general profile of relative processing and diminished activation in Broca’s
deficits and strengths (Minshew, Goldstein, area for sentence processing. In addition, there
Muenz, & Payton, 1992; Ozonoff, Pennington, was reduced synchronization among cortical
& Rogers, 1991; Prior & Hoffman, 1990). regions indicating reduced functional connec-
In a more detailed investigation of each tivity (Just, Cherkassky, Keller, & Minshew,
neuropsychologic domain and both the audi- 2004). Other unpublished studies from this
tory and visual modalities, Minshew, Gold- laboratory have demonstrated a similar profile
stein, and Siegel (1997) reported evidence in with an executive function task and a social
33 autistic individuals of deficits in complex cognitive task as well as a task requiring the
tasks across all domains involving both the vi- transfer of information from the language area
sual and auditory modalities and preserved to the visual imagery area. In other words,
function on tests of simpler abilities in these there is generalized underdevelopment of the
same domains (see Table 18.1). functional connectivity of higher order neural
Thus, deficits were found in concept forma- systems underlying the core symptoms of
tion and problem solving, higher order inter- autism. Thus, there is a reduction in informa-
pretative language abilities, memory for tion processing capacity that particulary af-
complex material, skilled motor abilities or fects integrative circuitry and integrative
praxis, and higher cortical sensory perception. functions. Documenting this profile moves us
Auditory and visual modalities appeared one large step closer to understanding the cog-
equally involved. This pattern of widespread nitive and brain basis of the behaviors that de-
deficits in complex or higher order abilities fine autism.
This profile is furthermore the key to be-
havioral intervention. Normal or typical indi-
TABLE 18.1 Intact or Enhanced Abilities viduals function, interact, and communicate
and Deficits on the basis of skills on the right half of Table
Intact or Enhanced Cognitive Weaknesses 18.1, which are areas of deficits for individu-
Attention
als with high-functioning autism and Asperger
Sensory perception disorder. However, intact abilities as indicated
Elementary motor Complex motor on the left-hand side of Table 18.1 mislead
Simple memory Complex memory many into thinking that individuals with
Formal language Complex lanuage autism have intact skills on the right hand side
Rule learning Concept formation of the table. Individuals with high-functioning
Visuospatial processing
autism and Asperger disorder frequently do
488 Neurological and Medical Issues

not respond to or understand fully reasoning, in the cerebellum and related inferior olive.
concepts, insight, and complex language (right When compared with controls, the autistic
hand side of the table skills). In contrast, they subjects showed reduced neuronal size and
do respond to facts and details ( left hand side increased cell packing density in the hippo-
of the table skills). This has lead to a mismatch campus, amygdala, mammillary body, anterior
between the intervention (right hand side of cingulate gyrus, and medial nucleus of the sep-
the table skills) and the affected individual tum. All of these regions are known to be re-
( left hand side of the table skills). lated to one another by interconnecting circuits
and comprise a major portion of the limbic
CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM system of the brain.
STRUCTURE IN AUTISM Studies of the CA1 and CA4 pyramidal neu-
rons of the hippocampus, using the rapid Golgi
The structure of the brain in autism has been technique, have demonstrated reduced com-
investigated with a small number of anatomic plexity and extent of dendritic arbors in these
studies and a growing number of neuroimaging cells (Raymond, Bauman, & Kemper, 1996).
studies. Most of the neuroanatomic studies Although small cell size and increased cell
have involved examinations of structures and packing density was found in the medial septal
cells and only recently has work begun on neu- nucleus (MSN) in the autistic brains, a differ-
ronal architecture and synaptic number. Neu- ent pattern of abnormality was observed in
roimaging studies have a long history but the nucleus of the diagonal band of Broca
actual contributions began with magnetic res- (NDB). In this nucleus, enlarged but otherwise
onance imaging. normal-appearing neurons were found in the
NDB of all autistic subjects under the age of 13
Neuropathology years. In contrast, these same neurons were
noted to be small in size and markedly reduced
Our understanding of the neuroanatomical ab- in number in all of the autistic subjects over the
normalities in the autistic brain has been ham- age of 22 years (Bauman & Kemper, 1994).
pered by the limited availability of suitable A systematic survey of the remainder of the
autopsy material and the lack of an animal forebrain in these nine autistic cases showed
model. The few autopsy studies reported by no abnormalities in the striatum, pallidum,
the mid-1970s showed a paucity of findings. In thalamus, hypothalamus, basal forebrain, and
the absence of hard data, a number of brain re- bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Kemper,
gions were hypothesized as possible sites of unpublished data). The only other abnormality
abnormality based on clinical features of the noted in these brains was a minor malforma-
disorder and evidence obtained from neuro- tion of the orbitofrontal cortex in one hemi-
physiologic studies (Boucher & Warrington, sphere. Bailey et al. (1998), however, noted
1976; Coleman, 1979; Damasio & Maurer, neocortical malformations in four of the six
1978; Delong, 1978; Maurer & Damasio, brains he examined. These included irregular
1982; Ornitz & Ritvo, 1968b; Vilensky, De- laminar patterns, areas of increased neuronal
masio, & Maurer, 1981). density, abnormally oriented pyramidal cells,
In 1984, the results of a systematic analysis areas of cortical thickening, and increased
of the brain of a 29-year-old man with well- numbers of neurons in layer I. More recently,
documented autism studied by means of whole Casanova, Buxhoeveden, Switala, and Roy
brain serial section, in comparison with an (2002) observed that, in comparison to con-
identically processed age- and sex-matched trols, neocortical minicolumns were smaller,
control, was reported (Bauman & Kemper, less compact, and more numerous in the three
1984). A more detailed description was pub- areas studied. The authors suggested that,
lished the following year (Bauman & Kemper, since inhibitory GABAergic double bouquet
1985). Since that initial report, eight additional cells define the microcolumnar organization,
cases have been similarly studied (Bauman & they might be a site of abnormality.
Kemper, 1994). All nine brains demonstrated Outside the forebrain, additional abnormal-
abnormalities in the limbic system as well as ities have been reported in the cerebellum and
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 489

related inferior olive. A significant reduction ties have also been reported in the brainstem
in the number of Purkinje cells has been demon- by Bailey et al. (1998), which have included a
strated throughout the cerebellar hemispheres, dysplastic configuration of the lamella of the
most prominently in the posteriolateral neocere- inferior olive and the presence of ectopic neu-
bellar cortex and adjacent archicerebellar cor- rons lateral to the inferior olive.
tex (Arin, Bauman, & Kemper, 1991; Ritvo To date, postmortem studies of the brain in
et al., 1986). In contrast to the findings in the autism have failed to show any abnormalities
hemispheres, detailed quantitative analysis of of gross brain structure. Myelination has been
Purkinje cell number in the vermis has shown found to be comparable to controls in all cases,
no statistically significant differences when both microscopically and by MRI (Bauman &
compared with age- and sex-matched controls Kemper, 1994). However, in 1993, Bailey,
(Bauman & Kemper, 1996). In addition to these Luthert, Bolton, Le Couteur, and Rutter re-
cerebellar cortical findings, abnormalities have ported that three of four autopsied brains from
been noted in the fastigial, globose, and embo- autistic subjects were heavier than expected
liform nuclei in the roof of the cerebellum, for age and sex. Since that time, it has become
which, like the findings in the NDB of the sep- apparent that although most autistic children
tum, appear to differ with the age of the pa- are born with normal head circumferences, the
tient. As in the NDB, small pale neurons, trajectory of head growth in these children
which are reduced in number, characterize tends to significantly accelerate during the
these cerebellar nuclei in all of the adult sub- preschool years (Lainhart et al., 1997). This
jects. In all of the younger brains, however, observation has been supported by MRI stud-
these same neurons as well as those of the den- ies in which the brain volume was observed to
tate nucleus are enlarged and present in ade- increase most markedly between 2 and 4.5
quate numbers (Bauman & Kemper, 1994). years of age, followed by deceleration of brain
No evidence of retrograde cell loss or atro- growth in older autistic children (Courchesne
phy has been found in the principal olivary nu- et al., 2001). Although both gray and white
cleus of the brainstem in any of the autistic matter volumes were found to be increased, the
brains, areas which are known to be related to major changes involved the cerebral and cere-
the abnormal regions of the cerebellar cortex bellar white matter. Subsequently, Carper,
(Holmes & Stewart, 1988). In human pathol- Moses, Tigue, and Courchesne (2002) observed
ogy, neuronal cell loss and atrophy of the infe- that, although several brain regions showed in-
rior olive have been invariably observed creased gray and white matter enlargement in
following perinatal and postnatal Purkinje cell 2- to 3-year-old autistic children, the greatest
loss (Greenfield, 1954; Norman, 1940). This volumetric increase was found in the frontal
cell loss is presumably due to the close rela- lobe, with the occipital lobe being virtually
tionship of the olivary climbing fiber axons to unaffected. More recently, Herbert et al. (2004)
the Purkinje cell dendrites (Eccles, Iro, & observed that the white matter increase in
Szentagothai, 1967). In the adult autistic brain, both autism and children with developmental
despite the markedly reduced numbers of cere- language disorders (DLD) appeared to pri-
bellar Purkinje cells, the olivary neurons have marily involve the radiate white matter,
been found to be present in adequate numbers which myelinates later than the deep white
but small in size. In contrast, the olivary neu- matter, a concept that appears to be consis-
rons in the younger brains were significantly tent with the unusual postnatal head growth
enlarged but otherwise normal in appearance reported in autism.
and number (Kemper & Bauman, 1998). Of in- Coincident with the reports of increased
terest is a single case report of an autistic indi- brain volume in childhood autism has been
vidual with Mobius syndrome describing the observation of increased brain weight in
decreased numbers of neurons in the facial nu- this same age group. In a review of 19 post-
cleus and superior olive and shortening of the mortem cases obtained from autistic subjects
distance between the trapezoid body and the less than 13 years of age, brain weight was
inferior olive (Rodier, Ingram, Tisdale, Nel- found to be heavier than expected for age and
son, & Roman, 1996). Additional abnormali- sex by 100 to 200 grams, and this difference
490 Neurological and Medical Issues

was statistically significant when compared in nicotinic receptors and an increase in brain-
with controls (Bauman & Kemper, 1997). In derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the
contrast, brains from adult autistic subjects, basal forebrain but no M1 abnormalities (Perry
over the age of 21 years, were noted to be et al., 2001). In a more recent study, Lee et al.
lighter in weight than expected for age and sex. (2002) noted that three of four nicotinic recep-
The pathogenesis of this brain enlargement is as tors were found to be decreased in the cerebel-
yet unknown. It has been hypothesized, how- lar cortex. No significant changes were noted
ever, that this unusual early brain growth may in M1 and M2 receptors or in choline acetyl-
reflect the presence of increased numbers of transferase activity. In 2004, Martin-Ruiz
neurons and/or glia, premature and accelerated et al., using complementary measures of recep-
proliferation of synapses, axonal and dendritic tor expression, found that reduced gene expres-
arbors, and/or increased myelination. Given the sion of the a4b2 nicotinic receptor in the
presence of atypical information processing, cerebral cortex was a major feature of the neu-
which has been characteristically observed rochemical pathology of autism, while post-
clinically and with fMRI in autistic individuals, transcriptional abnormalities of both a4b2 and
it seems reasonable to consider the possibility a7 subtype were evident in the cerebellum. The
that abnormalities in the composition and authors concluded that dendritic and/or synap-
structure of components of cortical neurons tic nicotinic receptor abnormalities may be in-
and/or of the myelin sheath might significantly volved in the disruption of the development of
contribute to both the increased brain size and cerebral and cerebellar circuitry in autism.
cognitive impairment observed in this disorder.
Whatever the underlying cause or causes, the Theories of Pathogenesis
differences in brain weight and brain volume,
combined with microscopic changes with age, What can be learned from the neuroanatomic
suggest that, although the disorder appears to evidence acquired to date? The pattern of
begin before birth, autism is also associated small neuronal cell size and increased call
with a postnatal ongoing process. packing density, which characterizes much of
While the defining of neuroanatomic abnor- the limbic system of the autistic brain, suggests
malities of the autistic brain will continue to a curtailment of development in this circuitry.
be an important line of investigation, it will be Studies in human and nonhuman primates sup-
equally important to expand our understanding port the role of the limbic system structures in
of the underlying neurochemical profile in learning, memory, behavior, and emotion. In
these same regions. To date, Chugani et al. the cerebellum, because of the known tight re-
(1997) have reported decreased serotonin syn- lationship between the Purkinje cells and the
thesis in the dentatothalamocortical pathway cells of the inferior olivary nucleus, reduced
in seven autistic boys using positron emission numbers of Purkinje cells in the face of pre-
tomography (PET). Fatemi, Stary, Halt, and served olivary neurons strongly suggest that
Realmuto (2001) have described decreased the process that resulted in these abnormali-
amounts of reelin and Bcl-2 proteins in 44% of ties had its onset before birth. In addition,
the autistic cerebella studied. Reelin is impor- there is growing evidence that the cerebellum
tant for brain development and appears to play may be important for the mediation and modu-
a role in the process of synapse elimination, lation of some aspects of language, learning,
and Bcl-2 is important for programmed cell attention, and affective behavior.
death. Blatt at el. (2001), studying neurotrans- Although much progress has been made in
mitter receptors in the autistic hippocampus, autism research over the past 20 years, many
have documented reduced binding of GABAa questions remain before we can truly under-
receptors but no change in serotonin, choliner- stand the neural mechanisms that result in the
gic, or kainate receptors. Several studies have clinical features of autism. Given the contin-
also investigated cholinergic activity in the ued short supply of suitable autopsy material,
autistic brain. A decrease in nicotinic and M1 research would be aided significantly by the
receptors has been reported in the parietal cor- availability of an animal model for anatomic,
tex. The frontal cortex also showed a decrease neurochemical, and neurophysiologic study.
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 491

Neurochemical analysis of blood, urine, and (PEG) and then computerized axial tomogra-
spinal fluid have shown inconsistent findings, phy (CT). The second era began with MRI and
and related neuropharmacological research an appreciation of the importance of excluding
has been discouraging in many cases. Neuro- autistic subjects who had underlying causes
chemical analysis of brain tissue is beginning for their autism so that abnormalities found on
to emerge but is hampered by small samples imaging could be linked solely to autism.
and quality of tissue available for study. How-
Clinical Imaging
ever, with a more concerted effort to obtain
autopsy material, combined with rapidly im- After several decades of research reviewed in
proving technology, it is likely that significant the previous version of this chapter, it was de-
advances in these areas can be expected within termined that in the absence of an associated
the next 5 to 10 years. disorder such as tuberous sclerosis or fetal
In the future, it will be important for neu- rubella, the most common neuroradiologic
roanatomic research in autism to focus more finding in autism was one of normal neu-
intensely on a systematic correlation between roanatomy. As mentioned in the next section,
location and degree of histoanatomic abnor- Research Imaging, a minority of autistic indi-
mality and the clinical characteristics of the viduals have ventricular enlargement not re-
patient, thereby expanding our knowledge of lated to increased intracranial pressure or to
site-specific developmental brain dysfunction autism severity, and thus it has no diagnostic
and behavior. Further, what is now known or treatment implications. Very rarely, other
about the brain in autism should not only serve findings such as an arachnoid cyst may be
as a guide for research questions in genetics, present. In the absence of clinical manifesta-
immunology, neurophysiology, and neurochem- tions referable to this focal abnormality, man-
istry, but also provide the defining yardstick agement and treatment are unchanged. Such
against which possible etiologic hypotheses focal abnormalities are not etiologic of autism,
and related biological investigations in both which from all available data is the result of a
animals and humans can be measured. bilateral, largely symmetric neurologic abnor-
mality involving multiple levels of the neuraxis.
Structural Brain Imaging Thus, in the absence of an unusual clinical
course, such as focal findings, the late appear-
The neuroanatomy of autism can also be inves- ance of symptoms or a progressive or fluctuat-
tigated with imaging methods. Because imag- ing course (Volkmar, 1992, 1994), or evidence
ing can be performed in living subjects, it on history or examination of an associated dis-
provides a much more accessible window into order, such as tuberous sclerosis or focal neuro-
the brain in autism. The types of information logic findings, clinical imaging is unlikely to be
it can provide are different. In some ways, it is useful and should not be a routine part of the
more limited with regard to structural infor- neurologic evaluation of autistic children (Fil-
mation but in other ways it provides a broader ipek et al., 1999; Rapin, 1991).
picture because of the larger number of sub-
Research Imaging
jects that can be surveyed and the capacity to
measure the relative size of structures and their As with EPs and eye movements, the first era
developmental dynamics. Functional imaging of imaging research in autism from the 1960s
also provides information about the status of through the early 1980s largely demonstrated
the brain that neuropathologic information the relationship of gross anatomic abnormali-
cannot. (Functional Imaging in Autism is the ties to causes of brain damage other than
subject of a separate chapter in this Hand- autism (see Minshew & Dombrowski, 1994,
book.) Likewise diffusion tensor imaging and for detailed review of early literature). Sev-
tracking methods are opening entirely new and eral reports have provided excellent reviews
valuable views of white matter connectivity in of recent literature (Brambilla et al., 2003;
autism that promise exciting insights into the Cody, Pelphrey, & Piven, 2002; Nicolson &
pathophysiology. The first era of neuroimaging Szatmari, 2003; Palmen & van Engeland,
in autism began with pneumoencephalograms 2004).
492 Neurological and Medical Issues

The first imaging studies to employ rigor- measurements, which considers all relevant in-
ous screening procedures were those of M. S. fluences on brain structure size, specifically
Campbell et al. (1982) and Rosenbloom, age, gender, IQ, height, and possibly socioeco-
Campbell, and George (1984). With the excep- nomic status.
tion of a low incidence of ventricular enlarge- Thus far, no studies are available that pro-
ment, the CT scans of these 58 autistic vide a comprehensive analysis of the whole
children were normal. Mild to moderate ven- brain and its component structures in even
tricular enlargement was present in 25% of the medium-size samples. Such an integrated per-
45 subjects in the study of M. S. Campbell spective is essential to understanding the neu-
et al. (1982) and 15% of the 13 autistic sub- roanatomy of autism, as this is a neural systems
jects in the Rosenbloom et al. (1984) study. In disorder and the structures are interconnected
both studies, ventricular size was unrelated to and a developmental disorder in which the de-
all clinical indices examined, and thus its rela- velopmental dynamics of brain growth are dif-
tionship to the pathophysiology was unknown. ferentially disturbed.
These two studies were followed by the nega-
Total Brain Volume and Growth
tive report of Prior, Tress, Hoffman, and Boldt
(1984); Harcherik et al. (1985); and Creasey In 1992, Piven and colleagues reported an in-
et al. (1986), concluding that autistic individu- crease in the midsagittal, supratentorial brain
als without other neurologic conditions were area in 15 high-functioning autistic men com-
“ very unlikely to have detectable CT abnor- pared to well-matched control groups (Piven
malities” (Harcherik et al., 1985) and that the et al., 1992). Subsequently, Piven and col-
“cerebral defect in autism was likely to be mi- leagues reported an increase in the total brain
croscopic without major gross anatomic corre- volume of the cerebral hemispheres down to
late” (Creasey et al., 1986). These conclusions the lower boundary of the brainstem in 22
were consistent with the neuropathologic find- autistic male adolescents and young adults
ings of Bauman and Kemper (1985), consisting (Piven et al., 1995). The increase in cerebral
of abnormalities at the microscopic level but no volume involved both brain parenchyma and
gross structural abnormalities other than a 100 the lateral ventricles, but the increase was not
to 200 gm increase in brain weight. These accounted for by the increase in ventricular
findings and those that followed became the volume. This sample was subsequently ex-
basis for current clinical recommendations panded to 35 autistic adolescents and adults
that imaging not be a routine part of the evalu- with the same increase in total brain volume.
ation of children with ASD (see Practice Pa- The latter study also found that the increase in
rameter, Filipek et al., 1999). brain volume involved the occipital, parietal,
The current era of imaging research began and temporal lobes but not the frontal lobes.
with the introduction of MRI technology and Similar findings were reported by Filipek and
morphometric methodology for deriving volu- colleagues (1992) in 9 high-functioning and 13
metric measurements of brain structures. The low-functioning autistic children (n = 22) be-
advantage of this method, especially for chil- tween 6 and 10 years of age. They found an in-
dren and adolescents, is the absence of radia- crease in total brain volume localized to the
tion, making studies of them and repeated same occipital, parietal, and temporal regions
studies feasible and safe. The other great but found that it was largely the result of in-
strengths of MRI are its other applications, creased white matter volume.
including fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging, dif- Recent studies have expanded on this early
fusion tensor fiber tracking, and magnetic res- finding. Courchesne and colleagues (2001)
onance spectroscopy. studied 60 autistic and 50 normal boys be-
The limitations of MRI research studies in tween 2 and 16 years. Thirty of these autistic
autism over the past 15 years have been inade- children were scanned between 2 and 4 years
quate sample sizes and the inability to add of age, and 90% were found to have larger than
samples across research sites to define the sta- normal brain volumes, indicating increased
tus of structures across the age and severity early brain growth. One-third of these or 10
range in autism. An additional need is for a children met criteria for macrencephaly. Cere-
representative normative database of brain bral gray matter was increased by 12% and
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 493

cerebral white matter by 18% compared to and cerebellum, which, with the exception of
controls. The other 30 children were scanned caudate, showed significantly larger volumes.
between 5 and 16 years of age and demon- The investigators proposed: “ These differ-
strated that brain growth decelerated with ences in volumes and proportions suggest that
brain volume normalizing in autism by later autistic brains have nonuniform differences in
childhood. In a second study, Sparks et al. scaling as compared to controls: it appears
(2002) examined brain volume in forty-five 3- that the first group of regions is scaled compa-
to 4-year-old children with ASD and found an rably to controls, the second is scaled some-
increase in total brain volume. At 6 to 7 years what smaller and the third is scaled larger.”
of age, brain volume was no longer increased in These findings are intriguing and represent a
this group of children. In a third study of 67 different way of viewing the brain in autism,
nonretarded children, adolescents and adults more akin to a systems approach.
with autism and 83 healthy controls ages 8 to One other interesting study of the cerebral
46 years, Aylward, Minshew, Field, Sparks, cortex in autism pertains to the gyral pattern.
and Singh (2002) found an increase in total Hardan, Jou, Keshavan, Varma, and Minshew
brain volume in those ages 8 to 12 years but not (2004) examined the gyrification index, the
in those older than 12 years of age. It is inter- ratio of total to outer cortical contour, to mea-
esting that both the children, adolescents, and sure the cerebral folding pattern on a frontal
adults had enlarged head circumferences, sug- coronal slice in 30 non-mentally retarded
gesting that even the adults had had enlarged autistic individuals and 32 matched normal
brain volumes as children. A very recent study controls. They found that cortical folding was
of 21 7- to 15-year-old high-functioning ASD increased on the left in children and adoles-
children and adolescents revealed proportion- cents but not adults with autism. They also
ate increases (6%) in total brain volume and found that cortical folding decreased bilater-
proportionate increases in cerebral gray mat- ally with age in autism but not in controls.
ter but not white matter compared to controls Since the amount of cortical folding may be a
(Palmen et al., in press). Unlike some other reflection of cortical volume, this is an in-
studies, the cortical gray matter was evenly af- triguing finding and an important measure-
fected in all lobes. A fourth important study ment that needs to be pursued in multiple brain
was that of Herbert et al. (2004) of 13 autistic regions and a larger sample.
children, which reported that the source of the In summary, the data thus far appear con-
brain enlargement was the outer zone of radi- sistent in indicating an early acceleration in
ate white matter. This white matter consists of brain growth leading to increased brain vol-
connections between immediately adjacent ume in early childhood, which several studies
cortical regions and longer distant intrahemi- found normalizes in mid-childhood (Aylward
spheric connections. Herbert et al. (2003) also et al., 2002; Courchesne et al., 2001). Other
did an interesting factor analysis of intercorre- studies have supported this developmental pro-
lations between the sizes of brain structures. file indirectly by failing to find differences in
The volumes of cerebral cortex, cerebral white brain volume in adolescents or adults (McAlo-
matter, and amygdala-hippocampus were sig- nan et al., 2002; Murphy et al., 2002; Rojas,
nificantly different from controls after adjust- Bawn, Benkers, Reite, & Rogers, 2002; Town-
ment for total brain size but not basal ganglia, send et al., 2001). However, Piven, Arndt, Bai-
brainstem, or cerebellum. Based on the factor ley, and Andreasen (1996) and Hardan,
analysis, the structures fell into three groups Minshew, Mallikarjuhn, and Keshavan (2001)
based on intercorrelations in their sizes. One reported increased brain volume in adolescents
factor was cerebral white matter, which and adults with autism and a recent study of 21
showed a trend toward being disproportion- high-functioning adolescents and adults with
ately larger. A second factor was cerebral ASD also found increased total brain volume
cortex-hippocampus-amygdala, which showed (Palmen et al., 2004). This study found a pro-
trends toward being disproportionately smaller. portionate diffuse increase in cerebral gray
The third factor was central white matter-gray matter but not white matter. Thus, it is not clear
matter consisting of globus pallidus-putamen, what the status is of brain volume in adoles-
caudate, putamen, diencephalon, brainstem, cence and adulthood in autism. The statistical
494 Neurological and Medical Issues

significance or lack of in older individuals may sight into the understanding of the develop-
depend on sample size and on the presence of a mental neurobiology of autism.
few autistic individuals in the sample with
Corpus Callosum
megalencephaly, e.g., outliers. There are clearly
autistic adults with megalencephaly. Thus, Because neural connectivity is a central issue
there are likely to be two perhaps interrelated in autism, the corpus callosum as the major
phenomena. One is the early acceleration in white matter structure connecting the two
brain growth that appears to coincide with the hemispheres has become of particular interest.
presentation if not the onset of symptoms. The It is interesting that only cortical layer III con-
second and third are the increase in the mean tributes axons that cross in the corpus callosum.
head circumference for the autism group as a The corpus callosum is typically segmented
whole and the 20% that have megalencephaly, into regions for measurement that correspond
which may both represent the consequences of to the cortical origin of their fibers. The Wi-
overgrowth and the right shift of the curve for telson method involves seven regions, but some
the autism group as a whole, a continuous phe- studies in autism have used three or five seg-
nomenon, or it could represent two separate ments. The most recent study has used voxel-
phenomenon with those having persistent based morphometry (Chung, Dalton, Alexander,
megalencephaly being a separate subgroup and & Davidson, 2004). Ten studies of the corpus
separate biologic phenomenon from the early callosum have been carried out in autism.
overgrowth. Since parents can have large With the exception of two that employed 0.5
heads without having autism, this physical fea- Tesla scanners (Elia et al., 2000; Gaffney, Ku-
ture may represent a susceptibility gene that perman, Tsai, Minchin, & Hassanein, 1987)
acts in concert with other genes to produce a and one that found no differences (Herbert
case of autism. Consistent with this is the re- et al., 2004), seven have reported a reduction
cent observed co-occurrence of the G allele of in the size of the corpus callosum (Chung
the HoxA1 gene with macrocephaly in autism et al., 2004; Egaas, Courchesne, & Saitoh,
(Conciatori et al., 2004). It is clear that white 1995; Filipek et al., 1992; Hardan, Minshew,
matter is an important contributor to the in- & Keshavan, 2000; Manes et al., 1999; Piven,
crease in brain volume and that it is probably Bailey, Ranson, & Arndt, 1997; Saitoh, Cour-
the outer radiate zone related to intrahemi- chesne, Egaas, Lincoln, & Schreibman, 1995).
spheric and corticocortical connectivity that In some cases, the size of the corpus callosum
matures postnatally that is involved in autism. was only disproportionately small relative to
The increase in white matter as the major total brain volume and was not reduced in size
source of the increase in brain volume is consis- independent of brain volume. Of the ones re-
tent with the increase in brain weight in autism porting a reduction in the size of the corpus
and the density of myelin. Gray matter volume callosum, all reported a decrease in the body
may also be affected but data are sparse on and posterior regions except one (Hardan
this. Also, several studies have suggested that et al., 2000), which reported a reduction in the
it is the early maturing posterior regions of anterior region corresponding to frontal cor-
cerebral cortex that are increased in volume, tices in the sample of nonretarded autistic sub-
not the late maturing frontal cortex, but again jects. The most recent study (Chung et al.,
there are little data on this and some conflict- 2004) of 16 high-functioning autistic adoles-
ing evidence. The difference between the gray- cents and young adults reported a reduction in
white matter volume results of the Herbert the white matter concentration in the genu,
et al. studies and those of the Palmen et al. rostrum, and splenium and demonstrated that
studies may relate to the methods of image this was the result of hypoplasia and not atro-
analysis which segmented tissue into gray ver- phy, something that prior studies had not been
sus white matter. Finally, the recent factor able to do. A preliminary study using tensor-
analysis of brain structure volumes of Herbert based methods reported a reduction in the
et al. (2003) suggests that there are systems genu, spenium, and midbody in 15 autistic
differences in the dysregulation of brain children (Vidal et al., 2003). However, in some
growth in autism that are likely to provide in- studies it appears that the reduction was the
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 495

result of a few subjects rather than a reduction the right in 78% of normal people. Rojas et al.
of the size of the corpus callosum in all sub- (2002) measured the volume of the gray matter
jects. It is therefore important that future stud- in the left and right planum temporale and in
ies begin to examine individual subject data Heschl’s gyrus in 15 autistic adults and 15 nor-
and not just group differences. mal controls. In the control subjects, the left
In summary, numerous structural imaging planum temporale was larger than the right,
studies have provided evidence of reduced size but in the autistic subjects the structures were
of the corpus callosum and, hence, reduced in- essentially the same size in the two hemi-
terhemispheric connectivity. In some cases, spheres, reflecting a lack of the normal spe-
the reduction has been relative to total brain cialization and hypertrophy of the left planum
volume, which was increased in autism, and in temporale. Heschl’s gyrus was larger on the
other cases the reduction was absolute, for ex- left than the right in both the control and autis-
ample, with and without correction for total tic subjects, demonstrating that the lack of
brain volume. The reductions have primarily specialization in the planum temporale was not
affected the body and/or posterior regions. It a generalized left hemisphere phenomenon. Of
is notable that the reductions in the corpus cal- note, the normal controls in this study had sub-
losum do not appear to be entirely consistent stantially higher IQ scores than the autistic sub-
with the regions of the cortex that are dysfunc- jects, though total brain volume and left and
tional, particularly in the case of frontal dys- right hemisphere volumes were not different
function. Executive dysfunction is a pervasive across groups. In contrast, in a study of 7- to 11-
problem in autism, and you would expect a year-old children, Herbert et al. (2002) found
consistent reduction across studies in the cor- more extreme left asymmetry in the planum
responding callosal segments; yet, only one temporale in the autism group (n = 16) com-
study found this to be the case. This raises the pared to the control group (n = 15) all with IQ
issue of whether an increase or decrease in scores over 80. In a third study, De Fossé and
brain tissue corresponds to improved or im- colleagues (2004) reported exaggerated leftward
paired function. It is therefore critical that asymmetry of the planum temporale in language
future structural studies pair structural mea- impaired autistic children (6 to 13 years with
surements with functional measurements. Al- mean VIQ 75) and children with specific lan-
though total brain volume is increased early in guage impairment, but not in non language im-
brain development in autism, this coincides paired children with autism (mean VIQ 97) or
with presentation of symptoms and brain dys- typically developing control children.
function. Thus, too much brain is not good in The left inferior prefrontal gyrus or Broca’s
this case. In the case of the corpus callosum, area has also been reported to be abnormal in
there has been only one paper purported to in- autism in a few studies. Abell and colleagues
vestigate interhemispheric cognitive abilities (1999) reported a decrease in the gray matter
and reported impairments (Nydén, Carlsson, density in this cortical region using voxel
Carlsson, & Gillberg, 2004). However, the mea- based morphometry. Herbert and colleagues
sures used were not true measures of interhemi- (2002) also found a right ward asymmetry in 7-
spheric communication so it is not clear if there to 11-year old children with autism. The study
is impaired interhemispheric function in of Fosse and colleagues (2004) similarly found
autism. In addition, if only a small subset of a right ward asymmetry in the frontal language
autistic subjects have reduced corpus callosum cortex (reversal of the normal asymmetry) but
area then such measures would need to be only in the autism group with language impair-
compared to those who clearly have and do not ment and in the specific language impairment
have reduced area to shed light on structural- group but not in the autism group without cur-
functional correlations. rent language impairment.
These reports of abnormalities in the sym-
Cortical Language Areas
metry patterns of the language cortex are sup-
The left planum temporale is involved in re- ported by reports of increased left-handedness
ceptive speech and is commonly referred to as and ambidextrousness in autism, another reflec-
Wernicke’s area. It is larger on the left than tion of the lack of the normal left hemisphere
496 Neurological and Medical Issues

specialization (Escalante-Mead, Minshew, & smaller, and one reported increased hippo-
Sweeney, 2003). In a similar vein, there was a campal volume.
small series of CT studies in the 1980s in Two of the five studies of autistic children
autism that proposed abnormalities in hemi- found no differences in hippocampal volume.
spheric specialization. The studies were incon- In 1995, Saitoh et al. measured the posterior
sistent and the issue was dropped (see Minshew hippocampal formation (CA1-CA4, dentate
& Dombrowski, 1994, for review), but there is gyrus, subiculum) in one single oblique coro-
a recent reemergence of studies suggesting a nal slice. They found no difference between 33
failure of the normal developmental special- autistic children (12 with mental retardation
ization of function and structure in the brain. and 30 males) and 23 control children (19
Thus, this study is probably the first of many males), whether the autistic subjects were re-
to demonstrate the developmental failure of tarded or whether they had a history of
regional specialization in the brain in autism. seizures. Piven, Bailey, Ranson, and Arndt
replicated these findings in 1998, comparing
Cingulate Cortex
the hippocampal volumes of 35 autistic sub-
The anterior cingulate gyrus was investigated jects (26 males) and 36 controls (20 males),
in one study (Haznedar et al., 1997) in seven both before and after adjusting for gender,
autistic subjects and seven controls, which performance IQ, and total brain volume.
found the right anterior cingulate to be signifi- Herbert et al. (2003) and Saitoh, Karns, and
cantly smaller when corrected for total brain Courchesne (2001) both reported smaller hip-
volume in the autism group. There was also pocampal volumes in children with autism.
less activation of this structure during a verbal Herbert et al. reported smaller hippocampal
learning task. This sample was expanded to 17 volume only after correction for total brain
subjects with ASD and 17 controls with repli- volume in 15 boys with high-functioning
cation of this finding (Haznedar et al., 2000). autism compared to 17 control boys. In this
In addition, the expanded study reported hy- study, however, the combined volumes of the
pometabolism in both the anterior and poste- amygdala and hippocampus were measured,
rior cinguli. In an fMRI study of spatial and no separate measurement of the hippocam-
working memory, Luna et al. (2002) found re- pus was made. In 2001, Saitoh et al. found
duced activity in posterior cingulate cortex smaller area dentata (dentate gyrus and CA4)
and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in 11 high- both before and after correction for total brain
functioning autistic subjects compared to 6 volume in 59 autistic children (52 males) com-
healthy controls. These few studies reporting pared to 51 controls (40 males). Only cross-
evidence of structural and functional abnor- sectional areas, not volumes, were measured,
malities of the cingulate cortex are consistent however.
with the increased cell packing density re- The only study that has reported increased
ported by Bauman and Kemper (1994) in neu- hippocampal volume, Sparks et al. (2002),
ropathological studies. Further studies are used 45 subjects (38 males) between the ages
obviously needed, though, before any conclu- of 3 and 4 compared with 26 age-matched con-
sions can be made about the volume of this trols (18 males). Increased hippocampal vol-
structure in autism. ume, proportional to the increase in total brain
volume, was found. The hippocampus was no
Hippocampus
longer enlarged in these children at age 6 to 7
The hippocampus is involved in associative years, nor was their total brain volume. It has
memory and linking multiple inputs, allowing been suggested that increased hippocampal
for representations of the relations between el- volume might be seen only in younger subjects,
ements of scenes or events (Cohen et al., 1999). and individuals with autism might exhibit ar-
Like many other brain structures, structural rested growth or perhaps increased apoptosis
imaging abnormalities have been inconsistent over time.
in the hippocampus in autism. Four of eight In the adult studies, Haznedar et al. (2000)
studies reported no differences in hippocam- investigated the hippocampal volume of 17
pal volume in autism compared to control adults with ASD and 17 age- and sex-matched
groups. Three found hippocampal volume to be controls and did not find any significant differ-
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 497

ence. Howard et al.’s (2000) study of 10 male functioning autism and 14 age-, sex-, IQ-,
adults with high-functioning autism and 10 height-, weight-, and socioeconomic status-
age-, sex-, and verbal IQ-matched controls re- matched controls. Amygdala volumes were
ported similar results; however, a trend toward found to be significantly smaller in the autism
a smaller hippocampus was found in the group both before and after correction for
autism group. Aylward et al. (1999) studied total brain volume. Pierce, Muller, Ambrose,
hippocampal volume in 14 male adolescents Allen, and Courchesne (2001) reported bilat-
and adults with high-functioning autism and eral decrease in amygdala volume in seven
14 male controls matched on age, IQ, height, male adults and eight male controls matched
weight, and socioeconomic status. Hippocam- on age, sex, and handedness. In 2000, Hazne-
pal volume was found to be smaller in autistic dar et al. studied 17 adults with ASD (15
subjects only after correction for total brain males) and 17 adult age- and sex-matched con-
volume, which was larger in autistic subjects. trols (15 males). When the subject group was
In summary, studies of the hippocampus divided into autism and Asperger syndrome
have reported diverse results. The growth of categories, the subjects with Asperger syn-
this structure is dynamic in autism and so are drome had a significantly larger left amygdala
abnormalities. To define the structural pathol- than the subjects with autism.
ogy, it will be essential to control for age, gen- The two studies investigating child subjects
der, and IQ differences between groups and to had contradictory results. Sparks et al. (2002)
define changes over the age span and in rela- found bilateral increased amygdala volume in
tion to total brain volume and the cerebral cor- 45 children (38 males) with ASD between the
tex and other structures to which it is heavily ages of 3 and 4, as compared to 26 age-matched
connected. controls (18 males). They found the increase in
amygdala volume to be proportional to the in-
Amygdala
crease in total brain volume. However, in the
The amygdala is associated with social behav- most severely affected children (the core
ior and cognition (Adolphs, 2001, 2002; autism group), amygdala enlargement was pres-
Adolphs, Baron-Cohen, & Tranel, 2002). Since ent even without correction for total brain vol-
social impairment is one of the main symp- ume, suggesting a positive correlation between
toms of autism, the amygdala has often been amygdala volume and the severity of symp-
thought to be abnormal in autism and the sub- toms. The increase in amygdala volume con-
ject of considerable structural and functional tinued to be present at 6 to 7 years of age in
investigation. Three of seven studies investi- this group when total brain volume was not dif-
gating amygdala volume in autism have re- ferent from controls. Contrary to these find-
ported increased volume, three have reported ings, Herbert et al. (2003) found decreased
decreased volume, and one study found no dif- amygdala volume in 15 male children with
ference between the amygdala volumes in the high-functioning autism, compared to 17 con-
autism and control group. trol boys, but only after correction for total
Among the five studies investigating amyg- brain volume. This study also used the com-
dala volumes of adults, two reported increased bined volumes of the hippocampus and amyg-
volume, two reported decreased volume, and dala, with no separate measurements.
one reported no difference between the autis- In summary, the studies of the amygdala are
tic and control groups. Abell et al. (1999) contradictory at all ages. A limitation is the in-
investigated the amygdala volumes of 15 high- ability to add data across sites to achieve a
functioning autistic adults (12 males) com- large sample size. This is a critical structure to
pared with 15 IQ-matched controls (12 males). understand in light of the social and emotional
Increased gray matter was found in the amyg- impairments in autism. It is hoped that studies
dala and periamygdaloid cortex. Howard et al. will overcome this obstacle over the next 5
(2000) found bilateral increased amygdala years.
volumes in 10 high-functioning autistic males
Basal Ganglia
compared to 10 age-, sex-, and verbal IQ-
matched controls. Aylward et al. (1999) studied The caudate nucleus, putamen, and globus pal-
14 male adolescents and adults with high- lidus, which together make up the basal ganglia,
498 Neurological and Medical Issues

are believed to be involved in stereotypic and was underdevelopment of connections between


repetitive behavior in autism and perhaps cortical and subcortical regions in the autism
other aspects of the motor problems. Sears group. This is an interesting finding but a ten-
et al. (1999) reported increased caudate nu- tative one in need of much further investiga-
cleus volume, proportional to the increase in tion before any conclusions can be drawn about
total brain volume, in 35 high-functioning this structure in autism.
autistic adults compared to 36 adult controls
Cerebellum
of comparable age, sex, and IQ. The authors
replicated their findings in another 13 autistic There has been considerable imaging attention
subjects and 25 controls. The size of this struc- focused on the cerebellum in autism, initially
ture correlated with repetitive behavior in because the vermis was accessible to measure-
these subjects. In 2003, Herbert et al. reported ment on mid-sagittal sections. Rather quickly
an increase in the globus pallidus-putamen pro- there ensued a controversy in the literature
portional to the increase in brain volume, but about the presence or absence of vermal hy-
no differences in the caudate nucleus in 15 poplasia. For a while, it was even argued that
high-functioning autistic boys compared to 17 the vermis was the seat of autism in the brain
control boys. In a study of 40 high-functioning largely on the basis of unproven hypotheses
autistic individuals between 8 and 45 years of that it was the site of a cognitive deficit in
age and 41 healthy controls, Hardan, Kilpatrick, shifting attention. Subsequently, neuropsycho-
Keshavan, and Minshew (2003) found no dif- logic and neurophysiologic tests using multiple
ferences in the volumes of the caudate and different methods demonstrated that the
putamen when controlling for total brain vol- deficit in shifting attention was related to the
ume. They were not able to achieve satisfactory frontal lobe and executive processes, not the
intraclass correlation coefficients for the cerebellum and elementary attentional pro-
globus pallidus and thus did not report mea- cesses (Minshew et al., 1999; Ozonoff, Strayer,
surements for this structure. They also reported McMahon, & Filloux, 1994; Pascualvaca,
impaired motor performance primarily involv- Fantie, Papageorgiou, & Mirsky, 1998). The
ing praxis and reduced grip strength in the initial imaging reports of vermal hypoplasia
autistic subjects. The reduction in grip strength were never independently replicated. Ulti-
is consistent with functional abnormalities in mately, the finding to be replicated was in-
the basal ganglia whereas the abnormalities in creased volume of the cerebellar hemispheres
praxis are more consistent with cerebral corti- proportionate to total brain volume.
cal dysfunction. Thus, studies of the basal gan-
Cerebellar Hemispheres
glia have been few and the data inconclusive
about the status of this structure in autism. Several studies have reported an increase in
overall cerebellar volume in children (Herbert
Thalamus
et al., 2003; Sparks et al., 2002) and adults
The thalamus has reciprocal connections with (Hardan et al., 2001; Piven et al., 1997) with
nearly all major brain structures and is partic- autism. These studies involved a total of 60
ularly interesting with regard to synchroniza- children with autism or ASD and 43 matched
tion of information processing and the sensory controls and 51 adults with autism and 55 con-
symptoms in autism. There has been only one trols. The increase in cerebellar volume was
reported study of thalamic volume in 12 high- generally proportionate to the increase in total
functioning adolescents and adults compared brain volume. Other studies evaluated cerebel-
to 12 matched healthy controls (Tsatsanis et al., lar volume (Allen & Courchesne, 2003) and
2003). There were no differences in thalamic did not find differences in subject populations
volume before adjustment for total brain vol- that also did not have increases in total brain
ume. In the control group, there was a positive volume. Thus, it appears that cerebellar vol-
correlative between thalamic volume and total ume is increased proportionately to total brain
brain volume but only when the groups were volume in autism in most studies. This finding
divided using a split median procedure to yield is in contrast to neuropathologic reports of re-
high and low brain volume groups. The investi- duced Purkinje cells. However, the increase in
gators interpreted these findings to mean there volume, both total cerebellar and total brain
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 499

volume, measured in imaging studies appears ies often compared mentally retarded autistic
to be the result of increased white matter and subjects to non-mentally retarded controls or
not the result of changes in neuronal popula- failed to control for total brain size (Gaffney,
tions. A few studies have reported a dispro- Kuperman, Tsai, & Minchin, 1988; Hashimoto
portionate increase in the volume of the et al., 1989, 1992, 1993, 1995). Other studies
cerebellum on the order of a 40% increase rel- found no abnormalities of the brainstem (Elia
ative to the increase in the volume of the cere- et al., 2000; Garber & Ritvo, 1992; Hardan
brum (Courchesne et al., 2001; Palmen et al., et al., 2001; Hsu et al., 1991; Piven et al., 1992).
in press; Palmen, 2004). Histologic studies of the brainstem have not
revealed cell loss and thus do not provide an
Cerebellar Vermis
anatomic correlate for imaging studies report-
In a series of publications since 1988, Courch- ing a reduction in size. In summary, the stud-
esne and colleagues reported a decrease in the ies of the brainstem are inadequate to date
midsagittal area of vermal lobules VI and VII because they have primarily measured mid-
(Ciesielski et al., 1990; Courchesne, Yeung- sagittal areas and most have failed to control
Courchesne, Press, Hesselink, & Jernigan, for the effects of IQ and total brain size. Those
1988; Murakami, Courchesne, Press, Yeung- that have adequately controlled for key vari-
Courchesne, & Hesselink, 1989; Saitoh et al., ables have failed to document structural abnor-
1995) in the absence of an alteration in the malities of the brainstem in autism.
midsagittal area of the pons (Hsu, Yeung-
Ventricles
Courchesne, Courchesne, & Press, 1991) and
suggested an intrinsic abnormality of neocere- Although early CT studies reported clinically
bellar vermis. Subsequently, Courchesne and apparent lateral ventricular enlargement,
colleagues reported both a selective hypopla- sometimes asymmetric, in up to 15% of autis-
sia and hyperplasia of the neocerebellar ver- tic subjects, this has not been investigated to
mis (Courchesne et al., 1994). However, these any degree by MRI research studies since. Lat-
studies failed to control for IQ. Numerous eral ventricular size has been reported to be
studies that have collectively examined vermal increased in one study (Howard et al., 2000)
size in a wide age and IQ range of autistic sub- but not in two other studies (Hardan et al.,
jects have failed to replicate these findings 2001; Piven et al., 1995). This needs further
when autistic and control subjects were well study in a large sample. In early studies, lateral
matched on age, gender, and IQ (Filipek, ventricular enlargement was not related to any
1999; Filipek et al., 1992; Garber & Ritvo, of the clinical parameters investigated. How-
1992; Garber et al., 1989; Hardan et al., 2001; ever, regression as a mode of presentation was
Hashimoto, Murakawa, Miyazaki, Tayama, & not considered as a possible factor and should
Kuroda, 1992; Hashimoto, Tayama, Miyazaki, be. Palmen et al. have reported a 40% increase
Murakawa, & Kuroda, 1993; Holttum, Min- in the lateral and third ventricular volumes in
shew, Sanders, & Phillips, 1992; Kleiman, 21 medication naïve high-functioning autistic
Neff, & Rosman, 1992; Levitt et al., 1999; children aged 7 to 15 years compared to con-
Manes et al., 1999; Nowell, Hackney, Muraki, trols, in the context of a 6% increase in total
& Coleman, 1990; Piven et al., 1992, 1997; brain volume. In a sample of 21 adolescents
Ritvo & Garber, 1988). In summary, although with PDD and 21 controls, a similar increase in
much was made about early reports of vermal ventricular volumes was also increased (Pal-
hypo- and hyperplasia of lobules VI and VII, men et al., 2004).
these MRI findings appear to be related to In a study of 16 high-functioning autistic
lack of matching between the autism and con- (HFA) adults and 19 healthy controls, Hardan
trol groups on IQ (Filipek, 1999; Filipek et al., et al. (2001) reported an increase in the vol-
1992; Palmen & van Engeland, 2004; Piven ume of the third ventricle after adjustment for
et al., 1992, 1997) and were never confirmed. intracranial volume. The significance of this
finding, as with other findings related to the
Brainstem
ventricles in autism, is that it might reflect re-
The brainstem has been reported to be de- duction in the size of the brain structure that
creased in area in a few studies, but these stud- surrounds it, for example, the thalamus.
500 Neurological and Medical Issues

Enlargement of the fourth ventricle has tendon reflexes are variable. In some children,
been reported in a single study but not in seven they are markedly diminished; in others, they
other studies (see Brambilla et al., 2003, for a are normal or increased. Babinski signs are
review). down going. Toe walking is not uncommon.
Gait lacks the normal fluidity reflecting di-
CLINICAL NEUROLOGY minished postural balance, incoordination,
and motor planning problems. Motor apraxia is
This section encompasses certain aspects of nearly universal. Stereotypies are present in
the neurologic examination not considered in about one-third of children. Complaints of sen-
the standard assessment of autism. Thus, al- sory sensitivity to sound, light, and texture are
though behavioral child neurologists would frequent, but objective sensory signs are not,
also assess social function, language, and repet- with the exception of errors on fingertip num-
itive behavior these are not discussed here. In- ber writing (see Chapter 32, this Handbook,
stead, this section covers long tract signs, Volume 2, for further information about the
motor signs, head circumference, minor physi- motor and sensory abnormalities in autism).
cal anomalies, the electroencephalogram and
epilepsy from a clinical perspective. Head Circumference

The Neurologic Examination In Kanner’s original description of autism, 5


of the 11 children had large heads (Kanner,
The neurologic examination in autism is re- 1943). Studies since then have determined that
markable for the absence of findings, other 20% of individuals with idiopathic autism
than the manifestations of autism, and for have macrocephaly, for example, a head cir-
above-average head circumference in some but cumference > 97 percentile or >2 SD above
not all individuals (Bailey et al., 1993, 1995). the mean (Fombonne, Roge, Claverie, Coury,
Microcephaly is not part of this syndrome if & Fremolle, 1999; Lainhart, 2003). The in-
subjects are screened for associated disorders. crease in head circumference is typically dis-
The presence of microcephaly should suggest proportionate to height. A large but normal
the presence of fetal infection with rubella or size head (>50 percentile but <97%) is even
cytomegalovirus, Rett syndrome, or a chromo- more common. The mean head circumference
somal anomaly. Cranial nerve examination is for the autism population is at the 60 to 70 per-
normal except for the facial nerve. Most indi- centile for the standardization sample. Macro-
viduals with autism have a “sober ” or masked cephaly in autism is usually not present at
facial expression during interactions, although birth, though it is on occasion. It appears to
they exhibit expression or animation in re- develop in the first or second year of life,
sponse to internally experienced emotion. though head circumference data documenting
Their attempts at imitation of facial emotion this are very limited. It has also been docu-
typically result in stilted expressions as a re- mented to develop as late as 4 years of age
sult of problems with praxis and perhaps (Lainhart et al., 1997; Mason-Brothers et al.,
weakness. Czapinski and Bryson (2003) have 1987). One retrospective study of head cir-
reported evidence of reduced and weak muscle cumference measurements suggested that the
movements in the eye and mouth regions but onset of macrocephaly in most autistic chil-
not the brow region and suggested this was ev- dren was between birth and 6 to 14 months of
idence of abnormalities of the vii cranial nerve age when head circumference was increased
nucleus. There are many interesting studies of by 1.67 SDs to the 84th percentile (Courch-
facial movement ongoing, both expression and esne, Carper, & Akshoomoff, 2003), although
the capacity of autistic individuals to detect such early changes in head circumference were
expression, and it will be interesting to see not found by Lainhart and collaborators in the
their results. Oral-motor apraxia or dyspraxia multi-site data from the Collaborative Pro-
may also become apparent on examination of grams of Excellence in Autism. The increase
cranial nerves. Muscle tone is subtly de- in head circumference in autism corresponds
creased. Joints tend to be hypermobile. Deep to an increase in brain volume, which plateaus
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 501

in mid-childhood and then normalizes as a re- trimester either by environmental insults or


sult of brain growth in the normal population genetic abnormalities. Such features must be
(Aylward et al., 2002; Courchesne et al., 2001; differentiated from normal variations in physi-
Sparks et al., 2002). From adolescence on, cal features. It is important to consider family
brain volume appears to be normal in the autis- resemblance and ethnic variations in assessing
tic population, even though head circumfer- for MPAs. An accurate assessment of MPAs
ence remains enlarged (Aylward et al., 2002). depends on the skills of a geneticist or an ap-
Additional data are needed to determine propriately trained technician. A number of
whether the increased head circumference and studies have reported an increase in MPAs in
brain volume reflect a subpopulation of the autism (M. B. Campbell, Geller, Small, Petti,
autism population or a shift upward in size for & Ferris, 1978; Gualtieri & Adams, 1982;
the population as a whole. At present, there is Links, Stokwell, Abichandani, & Simeon,
no apparent relationship with severity of 1980; Miles & Hillman, 2000; Rodier, Bryson,
autism or gender. However, macrocephaly is & Welch, 1997; Steg & Rapoport, 1975;
familial, in that in one study, 62% of autistic Walker, 1977). Most of these studies used
individuals with macrocephaly had a parent Waldrop’s Minor Physical Anomaly Scale,
with macrocephaly (Stevenson, Schroer, Skin- which was originally developed on the basis of
ner, Fender, & Simensen, 1997). Miles, Had- the features of Down syndrome. It, therefore,
den, Takahashi, and Hillman (2000) found that has its limitations. Of the studies using this
45% of autistic children with macrocephaly scale, nearly half of autistic children were
had a parent with macrocephaly, but 37% of found to have five to six MPAs. These studies
the normocephalic autistic children also had a also demonstrated that MPAs are not specific
parent with macrocephaly, suggesting that to autism in that they were also found in chil-
macrocephaly in the parents might reflect the dren with schizophrenia and other psychiatric
presence of an autism susceptibility gene. disorders, learning disabilities, speech disor-
Head circumference is not, however, a screen- ders, and hyperactivity as well as in typically
ing test for autism, nor is macrocephaly a reli- developing children. Some studies found a re-
able early sign of autism. As per Lainhart’s lationship in autism between MPA and mental
well-articulated clarification, “For every retardation, but MPAs are also seen in autism
10,000 infants born, 600 would be expected to in the absence of mental retardation.
have increased rates of head growth and typi- The two most recent studies are those of
cal development, and 10 would be expected to Rodier et al. (1997) and Miles and Hillman
have increased rates of head growth and (2000). About 50% of autistic children have
autism” (Lainhart, 2003, p. 394). one MPA (Rodier et al., 1997). Only 20% ap-
There is no increase in microcephaly in id- pear to have multiple MPAs (Miles & Hillman,
iopathic autism over the occurrence in refer- 2000). There are no specific physical anom-
ence samples. When present in autism, it is alies that are consistently associated with
consistent with small stature. In the one study autism. Rodier et al. reported that posterior ro-
that found an increase in microcephaly in tation of the external ears was specific to
autism, it occurred in cases with associated autism rather than to developmental disabili-
medical conditions (Fombonne et al., 1999; ties in general. They also found that foot size
Lainhart et al., 1997). Hence, the presence of was disproportionately small relative to stature.
microcephaly or even small head size in an Interpupillary distance was reduced, but these
autistic child, particularly if it is dispropor- two MPAs are not different from other children
tionate to height, should suggest an associated with developmental disabilities. Miles and
medical or genetic disorder. Hillman (2000) classified autistic children
using the Waldrop’s Minor Physical Anomaly
Dysmorphic Features Scale and found that 22% were clearly abnor-
mal in that they had six or more MPAs and
Minor physical anomalies (MPAs) are consid- were easily recognizable as abnormal and dif-
ered significant because they indicate disrup- ferent from their parents. In this study, the
tion of embryonic development during the first greater the number of MPAs, the greater the
502 Neurological and Medical Issues

chance of having a genetic syndrome and of cent study emphasized a high frequency of
having an abnormal MRI. partial seizures (Rossi, Okun, Yachnis, Quis-
In summary, children with autism should be ling, & Triggs, 2002), which is consistent with
examined for MPAs, and those whose appear- the high frequency of frontal discharges re-
ance is different from their parents should be ported in another study (Kawasaki et al.,
referred for genetic evaluation as they are at 1997). The clinical recognition of epilepsy in
greater risk for having a chromosomal abnor- the autism population is complicated by the
mality or genetic syndrome. potential confusion of partial and partial com-
plex seizures with the social detachment and
Epilepsy in Autism stereotyped movements of autistic individuals.
Two decades ago, Gillberg and Schaumann
The occurrence of epilepsy and abnormal (1983) emphasized the failure to recognize
electroencephalograms (EEGs) in individuals partial complex seizures in two autistic chil-
with autism was among the earliest evidence dren hospitalized for behavior problems until
of a neurologic rather than a psychogenic telemetry was used. Control of the seizures re-
basis for autism. Since then, it has become sulted in major improvement in the function of
clear that epilepsy occurs in association with the children and reduction in the severity of
autism far above chance co-occurrence. the autistic symptoms in both cases. Another
Epilepsy occurs in 2% to 3% of the general impediment to the clinical recognition or even
population but in one-third or so of the autism suspicion of seizures is that clinicians familiar
population, indicating a shared genetic basis with autism have a tendency to attribute all
for autism and epilepsy (Kawasaki, Yokota, such behavior to autism, and neurologists fa-
Shinomiya, Simizu, & Niwa, 1997; Rossi, miliar with seizures are usually unfamiliar
Parmeggiani, Bach, Santucci, & Visconti, with autistic behavior and thus have great dif-
1995; Tuchman & Rapin, 2002). ficulty identifying clinical seizures in autistic
Epilepsy is defined as two unprovoked individuals. It is, therefore, important to con-
seizures. The prevalence of epilepsy reported in sider partial complex seizures in any autistic
autism has varied across studies depending on child or adolescent whose behavior or level of
the age distribution of the sample, the degree of function undergoes a sustained deterioration
mental retardation, and the type of language for which there is no identifiable explanation
disorder. The older the age of the subjects, the such as change in the environment or alteration
greater the proportion with mental retardation, of previously effective medications. Clinically,
and the more with severe language disorder, the seizures are commonly manifested in autism
higher the prevalence of epilepsy. by episodes from 30 seconds to hours of irri-
The onset of epilepsy in autism has two tability, aggression and rage, or staring, which,
peaks: one before 5 years of age and one after when prolonged, is associated with loss of
10 to 12 years of age, with the most cases pre- memory for events. The onset of depression is
senting after 10 years of age. Although these another potential explanation for deterioration
two age periods are when most seizure disor- that is often not considered. Since these indi-
ders present, a new onset seizure disorder can viduals lack the capacity for concept formation
occur at any age in autism. Epilepsy in autism and this insight and judgment, they are unable
is also highly associated with mental retarda- to self-assess their symptoms to inform others
tion. Individuals with normal or near-normal about the symptoms of depression. Instead they
intelligence and autism and without any asso- make the same literal conclusions about what is
ciated risk factor for epilepsy such as birth in- causing their behavior when they are depressed
jury or family history of epilepsy have a low as they do about other aspects of life. The treat-
risk of developing epilepsy (7%; Tuchman, ment of epilepsy in autism is not different from
Rapin, & Shinnar, 1991a, 1991b). The third its treatment in other populations.
risk factor for epilepsy in autism is severe re- A related issue is the occurrence of transi-
ceptive expressive language disorder, which is tory cognitive impairment in association with
also called verbal auditory agnosia. epileptiform discharges. Such discharges of 1 to
All types of epilepsy have been associated 3 seconds in duration have been shown to
with autism (Gillberg, 1991). However, one re- briefly interfere with memory, language, and
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 503

academic performance including arithmetic, gression in Landau Kleffner syndrome is typi-


reading, and spelling (Aarts, Binnie, Smit, & cally later than in autism, and the regression is
Wilkins, 1984; Binnie, 1993; Kasteleijn-Nolst limited to language (an epileptic aphasia) and
Trenite et al., 1988; Shewmon & Erwin, 1988a, is not associated with social or behavioral
1988b; Siebelink, Bakker, Binnie, Kasteleijn- symptoms. In addition, the spike activity is
Nolst Trenite, 1988). Such episodes may be typically frequent and temporal rather than in-
exhibited in good performance on a class as- frequent and centro-temporal. On the basis of
signment punctuated by transient unexplained this analogy, however, the use of steroids and
errors that the child does not recall later, fol- other aggressive treatments for regression and
lowed by immediate resumption of good perfor- epileptiform activity was proposed in autism
mance. This same performance phenomenon can (Lewine et al., 1999; Stefanos, Grover, &
occur in the context of any task. Geller, 1995). However, there have been little
data to support the efficacy of steroids in ASD
Electroencephalograms in Autism associated with regression and epileptiform
EEGs and not much more for depakote or other
Epileptiform abnormalities on EEG are even anticonvulsants (Nass & Petrucha, 1990; Plio-
more frequent than epilepsy in the autism pop- plys, 1994; Tuchman, 2000). Certainly radical
ulation. The frequency of such abnormalities treatments such as subpial resection are not
increases with the number of EEGs obtained, recommended.
the inclusion of sleep in the recording, and the
duration of the recording. In one study in Other Potential Neurologic Conditions
which the mean number of recordings was four
and all recordings included sleep, 60.8% of the There are other aspects of brain function such
individuals with ASD had epileptiform activ- as sleep that have been relatively untouched by
ity (Kawasaki et al., 1997). If only the individ- research but deserve careful attention. Sleep
uals with ASD without clinical epilepsy were disorders are pervasive in ASD and are not con-
considered, then only 31% had an epileptiform fined to early childhood (Honomichl, Goodlin-
EEG. A lower prevalence of 21% epileptiform Jones, Burnham, Gaylor, & Anders, 2002).
EEGs was found in individuals with ASD in They lead to considerable stress among the chil-
recordings that included sleep, but the EEGs dren, parents, and siblings; yet we know little
varied in quality and number (Tuchman & about the neurobiologic and medical causes of
Rapin, 1997). This 21% included individuals this problem.
with clinical epilepsy. It is also notable in this
study that only 59% of the children with clini- CONCLUSION
cal epilepsy had epileptiform EEGs. It is not
clear what treatment, if any, is appropriate for Autism is a fascinating neurologic disorder
individuals with epileptiform EEGs and no ev- that appears to primarily affect neuronal orga-
idence of clinical seizure activity. nization events and to have a currently esti-
One particular issue with regard to epilepsy, mated onset in brain development no later than
EEG abnormalities, and autism has been in the 30 weeks gestation. Based on current data, it
context of regression. Tuchman and Rapin furthermore appears to be a neural systems
(1997) found no significant difference in the disorder that constrains information process-
occurrence of epilepsy between ASD children ing, in particular integrative processing and
presenting with regression and those present- circuitry. Imaging studies have demonstrated
ing with developmental delays. Using 23-hour a reduction in functional connectivity when
telemetry recordings, Tuchman, Jayakar, Yay- challenging social, language, and problem
lali, and Villalobos (1997) observed that 46% solving tasks are employed and intact connec-
of ASD children presenting with regression tivity when simpler tasks in areas of intact
had an epileptiform EEG. It has been sug- function are employed. Structural imaging
gested that some children with ASD and re- studies have demonstrated an increase in total
gression have Landau Kleffner syndrome or brain volume suggesting a premature accelera-
epileptic aphasia, despite clear differences be- tion in brain growth coinciding with the onset
tween these two syndromes. The onset of re- of symptoms. This increase appears largely to
504 Neurological and Medical Issues

arise from the outer zone of white matter that Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham,
contain fibers connecting adjacent regions of R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., et al. (1999).
cortex as well longer intra-hemispheric con- The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based
nections. Head circumference studies have whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuro-
been the largest source of data about brain Report, 10, 1647–1651.
Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social
growth as imaging studies remain limited by
cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,
cost, subject recruitment and cooperation. 11, 231–239.
FMRI studies though provide evidence of al- Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing
tered activation in gray matter suggesting emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12,
that it is a disorder of both cortical gray 169–177.
matter and its white matter projections. Ab- Adolphs, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Tranel, D. (2002).
normalities documented in sensorimotor inte- Impaired recognition of social emotions fol-
gration in studies of postural physiology and lowing amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive
the essentially universal abnormalities in Neuroscience, 14, 1264–1274.
praxis suggest that the neurobiologic abnor- Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2003). Differential
effects of developmental cerebellar abnormal-
mality is pervasive and broadly involves brain
ity on cognitive and motor functions in the
functions that require a high degree of inte-
cerebellum: An fMRI study of autism. Ameri-
gration. Clinical symptoms are most apparent can Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 262–273.
in those domains that place the highest Aminoff, M. J. (1992). Electrodiagnosis in clinical
demand on information integration (e.g., the neurology. New York: Churchill Livingston.
social, communication, and information pro- Arin, D. M., Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L.
cessing domains). The measurement of other (1991). The distribution of Purkinje cell loss
brain structures has largely been fraught with in the cerebellum in autism [Abstract]. Neurol-
controversial results due to small sample ogy, 41, 307.
sizes across the wide age and severity spec- Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Field, K., Sparks,
trum of autism. The presence of minor physi- B. F., & Singh, N. (2002). Effects of age on
brain volume and head circumference in
cal anomalies suggests the possibily of an
autism. Neurology, 59, 175–183.
early onset in brain development than the Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G.,
third trimester, though none of the identified Honeycutt, N. A., Augustine, A. M., Yates,
brain abnormalities found are typical of such K. O., et al. (1999). MRI volumes of amyg-
an early time. The identification of suscepti- dala and hippocampus in non-mentally re-
bility genes for autism will greatly aide in the tarded autistic adolescents and adults.
progress in understanding the pathophysiol- Neurology, 53, 2145–2150.
ogy of autism as it has done in Rett syndrome. Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton,
Such advances await the accrual of a pooled P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995).
sample of accurately diagnosed and charac- Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evi-
dence from a British twin study. Psychological
terized multiplex families and thus families
Medicine, 25, 63–78.
willing to participate.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Bolton, P., Le Couteur, A.,
& Rutter, M. (1993). Autism and megalen-
Cross-References cephaly. Lancet, 34, 1225–1226.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Jan-
Other aspects of the neurobiology of autism ota, I., Montgomery, M., et al. (1998). A clini-
are discussed in Chapters 16, 17, 19, and 20. copathologic study of autism. Brain, 121,
Pharmacological treatments are addressed in 889–905.
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1984). The brain
Chapter 44.
in infantile autism: A histoanatomic report.
Neurology, 34, 275. [abstract].
REFERENCES Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1985). His-
toanatomic observations of the brain in early
Aarts, J. P., Binnie, C. D., Smit, A. M., & Wilkins, infantile autism. Neurology, 35, 866–874.
A. J. (1984). Selective Cognitive Impairment Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1994). Neuro-
During Focal and Generalized Epileptiform anatomic observations of the brain in autism.
EEG Activity. Brain, 107, 293–308. In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 505

neurobiology of autism (pp. 119–145). Balti- et al. (1982). Computerized axial tomography
more: Johns Hopkins Press. in young autistic children. American Journal of
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1996). Observa- Psychiatry, 139, 510–512.
tions on the Purkinje cells in the cerebellar Carper, R. A., Moses, P., Tigue, Z. D., & Courch-
vermis in autism. Journal of Neuropathology esne, E. (2002). Cerebral lobes in autism:
and Experimental Neurology, 55, 613. Early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects.
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1997). Is autism Neuroimage, 16, 1038–1051.
a progressive process? Neurology, 48 (Suppl. Casanova, M. F., Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala,
2), A285. A. E., & Roy, E. (2002). Minicolumnar pathol-
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & Mc- ogy in autism. Neurology, 58, 428–432.
Carthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological stud- Ceponiene, R., Lepistö, T., Shestakova, A., Van-
ies of face perception in humans. Journal of hala, R., Alku, P., Nääntänen, R., et al. (2003).
Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551–565. Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment
Binnie, C. D. (1993). Significance and management in children with autism: They can perceive but
of transitory cognitive impairment due to sub- do not attend. Proceedings of the National
clinical EEG discharges in children. Brain De- Academy of Sciences, 100, 5567–5572.
velopment, 15, 23–30. Chen, W., Chen, Y., Chen, D., Chen, J., Yin, X., & Lin,
Blatt, G. I., Fitzgerald, C. M., Guptill, J. T., Q. (2003). Brainstem auditory evoked potentials
Booker, A. B., Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, in children with autistic disorder [Abstract].
M. L. (2001). Density and distribution of hip- Chinese Mental Health Journal, 17, 24–26.
pocampal neurotransmitter receptors in autism. Chiappa, K., & Gladstone, K. (1978). The limits of
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- normal variations in waves I through VII of
ders, 31, 537–543. the human brainstem auditory response. Neu-
Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory rology, 28, 402.
deficits in early infantile autism: Some simi- Chugani, D. C., Muzik, O., Rothermel, R., Behen,
larities to the amnestic syndrome. British Jour- M., Chakraborty, P., Mangner, T., et al.
nal of Psychology, 67, 73–87. (1997). Altered serotonin synthesis in the den-
Brambilla, P., Stanley, J. A., Sassi, R. B., Nicoletti, tatothalamocortical pathway in autistic boys.
M. A., Mallinger, A. G., Keshavan, M. S., Annals of Neurology, 42, 666–669.
et al. (2003). In vivo effects of lithium in Chung, M. K., Dalton, K. M., Alexander, A. L., &
dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex in healthy Davidson, R. J. (2004). Less white matter con-
human subjects: A 1H-MRS study. Bipolar centration in autism: 2D voxel-based mor-
Disorders (Suppl. 5), 35. phometry. Neuroimage, 23, 242–251.
Bruneau, N., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Gomot, M., Ciesielski, K. T., Allen, P. S., Sinclair, B. D., Pabst,
Adrien, J. L., & Barthélémy, C. (2003). Corti- H. F., Yanossky, R., & Ludwig, R. (1990). Hy-
cal auditory processing and communication in poplasia of cerebellar vermis in autism and
children with autism: Electrophysiological / childhood leukemia [Abstract]. Proceedings of
behavioral relations. International Journal of the 5th International Child Neurology Con-
Psychophysiology, 51, 17–25. gress, Tokyo, Japan, 650.
Bruneau, N., Roux, S., Adrien, J. L., & Barthélémy, Ciesielski, K. T., Courchesne, E., & Elmasian, R.
C. (1999). Auditory associative cortex dys- (1990). Effects of focused selective attention
function in children with autism: Evidence tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and
from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 normal individuals. Electroencephalography and
wave-T complex). Clinical Neuropsychology, Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 207–220.
110, 1927–1934. Cody, H., Pelphrey, K., & Piven, J. (2002). Struc-
Buchwald, J. S., Erwin, R., van Lancker, D., tural and functional magnetic resonance
Guthrie, D., Schwafel, J., & Tanguay, P. imaging of autism. International Journal of
(1992). Midlatency auditory evoked re- Developmental Neuroscience, 20, 421–431.
sponses: P1 abnormalities in adult autistic Cohen, N. J., Ryan, E., Hunt, C., Romine, L., Wsza-
subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical lek, T., & Nash, C. (1999). Hippocampal sys-
Neurophysiology, 84, 164–171. tem and declarative (relational) memory:
Campbell, M. B., Geller, B., Small, A. M., Petti, Summarizing the data from functional neu-
T. A., & Ferris, S. H. (1978). Minor physical roimaging studies. Hippocampus, 9, 83–98.
anomalies in young psychotic children. Ameri- Coleman, M. (1979). Studies of the autistic syn-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 573–575. dromes. In R. Katzman (Ed.), Congenital and
Campbell, M. S., Rosenbloom, S., Perry, R., acquired cognitive disorders (pp. 265–303).
George, A. E., Kricheff, I. I., Anderson, L., New York: Raven Press.
506 Neurological and Medical Issues

Conciatori, M., Stodgell, C. J., Hyman, S. L., (1994). Abnormality of cerebellar vermian
O’Bara, M., Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., et al. lobules VI and VII in patients with infantile
(2004). Association between the HOXA1 autism: Identification of hypoplastic and hy-
A218G polymorphism and increased head cir- perplastic subgroups with M. R. Imaging.
cumference in patients with autism. Biological American Journal of Roentgenology, 162,
Psychiatry, 55, 413–419. 123–130.
Cook, E. H., Lindgren, V., Leventhal, B. I., Courchesne, E., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press,
Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A., Shulman, C., G. A., Hesselink, J. R., & Jernigan, T. L.
et al. (1997). Autism or atypical autism in ma- (1988). Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lob-
ternally but not paternally derived proximal ules VI and VII in autism. New England Jour-
15q duplication. American Journal of Human nal of Medicine, 318, 1349–1354.
Genetics, 60, 928–934. Creasey, J., Rumsey, J. M., Schwartz, M., Duara,
Courchesne, E. (1978). Neurophysiological corre- R., Rapoport, J. L., & Rapoport, S. I. (1986).
lates of cognitive development: Changes in Brain morphometry in autistic men as mea-
long-latency event-related potentials from sured by volumetric computed tomography.
childhood to adulthood. Electroencephalogra- Archives of Neurology, 32, 669–672.
phy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 45, 468–482. Czapinski, P., & Bryson, S. E. (2003). Reduced fa-
Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. cial muscle movements in autism: Evidence for
(2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the dysfunction in the neuromuscular pathway?
first year of life in autism. Journal of the [Abstract]. Brain and Cognition, 51, 177–179.
American Medical Association, 290, 337–344. Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A Neuro-
Courchesne, E., Courchesne, R. Y., Hicks, G., & logical model for childhood autism. Archives
Lincoln, A. J. (1985). Functioning of the of Neurology, 35, 777–786.
brain-stem auditory pathway in non-retarded Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A., Panagiotides,
autistic individuals. Electroencephalography H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. (2002). Neural
and Clinical Neurophysiology, 61, 491–501. correlates of face and object recognition in
Courchesne, E., Elmasian, R., & Yeung-Courchesne, young children with autism spectrum disorder,
R. (1987). Electrophysiological correlates of developmental delay, and typical development.
cognitive processing: P3b and Nc, basic, clini- Child Development, 73, 700–717.
cal, and developmental research. In A. M. Hal- Dawson, G., Finley, C., Phillips, S., Galpert, L., &
liday, S. R. Butler, & R. Paul (Eds.), A textbook Lewy, A. (1988). Reduced P3 amplitude of the
of clinical neurophysiology (pp. 645–676). New event-related brain potential: Its relationship
York: Wiley. to language ability in autism. Journal of
Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Zic- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18,
cardi, R., Carper, R. A., Tigue, Z. D., et al. 493–504.
(2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005).
life in patients with autistic disorder. Neurol- Electrophysiological and behavioral studies of
ogy, 57, 245–254. face processing in autism: Understanding the
Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., Galambos, R., & Lin- nature of face processing impairments in
coln, A. J. (1984). Autism: Processing of novel autism. Developmental Neuropsychology. 27:3.
auditory information assessed by event-related De Fossé, L., Hodge, S. M., Makris, N., Kennedy,
brain potentials. Electroencephalog-raphy and D., Caviness, V., McGrath, L., et al. (2004).
Clinical Neurophysiology, 59, 238–248. Language-association cortex asymmetry in
Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A. J., Kilman, B. A., & autism and specific language impairment. An-
Galambos, R. (1985). Event-related brain po- nals of Neurology, 56, 757–766.
tential correlates of the processing of novel vi- Delong, G. R. (1978). A neuropsychological inter-
sual and auditory information in autism. pretation of infantile autism. In M. Rutter &
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism (pp. 207–218). New
ders, 15, 55–76. York: Plenum Press.
Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A. J., Yeung-Courchesne, Dunn, M., Vaughan, H., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzer, D.
R., Elmasian, R., & Grillon, C. (1989). Patho- (1999). Electrophysiological correlates of se-
physiologic findings in non-retarded autism mantic classification in autistic and normal
and receptive developmental language disor- children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16,
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental 79–99.
Disorders, 19, 1–17. Eccles, J. C., Iro, M., & Szentagothai, J. (1967).
Courchesne, E., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., The cerebellum as a neuronal machine. New
Press, G. A., Lincoln, A. J., Haas, R. H., et al. York: Springer.
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 507

Egaas, B., Courchesne, E., & Saitoh, O. (1995). Re- for brainstem involvement in infantile autism.
duced-size of corpus-callosum in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 24, 578–586.
Archives of Neurology, 52, 794–801. Gaffney, G. R., Kuperman, S., Tsai, L. Y., Minchin,
Elia, M., Ferri, R., Musumeci, S. A., Panerai, S., S., & Hassanein, K. M. (1987). Midsagittal
Bottitta, M., & Scuderi, C. (2000). Clinical magnetic resonance imaging of autism. British
correlates of brain morphometric features Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 831–833.
of subjects with low-functioning autistic disor- Galambos, R., & Hecox, K. E. (1978). Clinical ap-
der. Journal of Child Neurology, 15, 504–508. plications of the auditory brain stem response.
Erwin, R., van Lancker, D., Guthrie, D., Schwafel, Otolaryngological Clinics of North America,
J., Tanguay, P., & Buchwald, J. S. (1991). P3 11, 709–722.
responses to prosodic stimuli in adult autistic Garber, H. J., & Ritvo, E. R. (1992). Magnetic res-
subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical onance imaging of the posterior fossa in autis-
Neurophysiology, 80, 561–571. tic adults. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Escalante-Mead, P. R., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, 149, 245–247.
J. A. (2003). Abnormal brain lateralization in Garber, H. J., Ritvo, E. R., Chui, L. C., Griswold,
high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism V. J., Kashanian, A., & Oldendorf, W. H.
and Developmental Disorders, 33, 539–543. (1989). A magnetic resonance imaging study
Fatemi, S. H., Stary, J. M., Halt, A. R., & Real- of autism: Normal fourth ventricle size and
muto, G. R. (2001). Dysregulation of reelin absence of pathology. American Journal of
and Bcl-2 proteins in autistic cerebellum. Psychiatry, 146, 532–535.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Gepner, B., Mestre, D., Masson, G., & de Schonen,
ders, 31, 529–535. S. (1995). Postural effects of motion vision in
Ferri, R., Elia, M., Agarwal, N., Lanuzza, B., young autistic children. NeuroReport, 6,
Musumeci, S., & Pennisi, G. (2003). The mis- 1211–1214.
match negativity and the P3a components of Gharani, N., Benayed, R., Mancuso, V., Brzus-
the auditory event-related potentials in autis- towicz, L. M., & Millonig, J. H. (2004).
tic low-functioning subjects. Clinical Neuro- Association of the homeobox transcription
physiology, 114, 1671–1680. factor, ENGRAILED 2, 3, with autism spec-
Filipek, P. A. (1999). Neuroimaging in the develop- trum disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 9,
mental disorders: The state of the science. 474–484.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Gillberg, C. (1991). The treatment of epilepsy in
and Allied Disciplines, 40, 113–128. autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook, Disorders, 21, 61–67.
E. H., Dawson, G., Gordon, B. et al. (1999). Gillberg, C., Rosenthal, U., & Johansson, E. (1983).
The Screening and Diagnosis of Autistic Spec- Auditory brainstem responses in childhood
trum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel- psychosis. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
opmental Disorders, 29, 437–482. tal Disorders, 13, 181–195.
Filipek, P. A., Richelme, C., Kennedy, D. N., Gillberg, C., & Schaumann, H. (1983). Epilepsy
Rademacher, J., Pitcher, D. A., Zidel, S., et al. presenting as infantile autism? Two case stud-
(1992). Morphometric analysis of the brain in ies. Neuropaediatrics, 14, 206–212.
developmental language disorders and autism Goldberg, M., Landa, R., Lasker, A., Cooper, L., &
[Abstract]. Annals of Neurology, 32, 475. Zee, D. S. (2000). Evidence of normal cerebel-
Finley, W. W., Faux, S. F., Hutcheson, J., & Am- lar control of the vestibulo-ocular ref lex
stutz, L. (1985). Long-latency event-related (VOR) in children with high-functioning
potentials in the evaluation of cognitive func- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tion in children. Neurology, 35, 323–327. Disorders, 30, 519–524.
Fombonne, E., Roge, B., Claverie, J., Coury, S., & Goldberg, M., Lasker, A., Zee, D., Garth, E., Tien,
Fremolle, J. (1999). Microcephaly and macro- A., & Landa, R. (2002). Deficits in the initia-
cephaly in autism. Journal of Autism and De- tion of eye movements in the absence of a
velopmental Disorders, 29, 113–119. visual target in adolescents with high function-
Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y., & Gaeta, H. (2001). ing autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2039–2049.
The novelty P3: An event related brain poten- Gomot, M., Giard, M., Adrien, J., Barthelemy, C.,
tial (ERP) sign of the brain’s evaluation of & Bruneau, N. (2002). Hypersensitivity to
novelty. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, acoustic change in children with autism:
25, 355–373. Electrophysiological evidence of left frontal
Gaffney, G. R., Kuperman, S., Tsai, L. Y., & cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology, 39,
Minchin, S. (1988). Morphological evidence 548–577.
508 Neurological and Medical Issues

Goodin, D. S. (1992). Event-related (endogenous) Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Murakawa, K., Yoshi-
potentials. In M. J. Aminoff (Ed.), Electrodi- moto, T., Miyazaki, M., Harada, M., et al.
agnosis in clinical neurology (pp. 627–648). (1995). Development of the brainstem and
New York: Churchill Livingstone. cerebellum in autistic patients. Journal of
Goodin, D. S., Squires, K., & Starr, A. (1978). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 1–18.
Long latency event-related components of the Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Luu, C., Ha-
auditory evoked potential in dementia. Brain, zlett, E. A., Siegel, B. V., Lohr, J., et al.
101, 635–648. (1997). Decreased anterior cingulate gyrus
Greenfield, J. G. (1954). The spino-cerebellar metabolic rate in schizophrenia. American
degenerations. Springfield, Il: Charles C Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 682–684.
Thomas. Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Wei, T. C.,
Grillon, C., Courchesne, E., & Akshoomoff, N. Hof, P. R., Cartwright, C., Bienstock, C. A.,
(1989). Brainstem and middle latency audi- et al. (2000). Limbic circuitry in patients with
tory evoked potentials in autism and develop- autism spectrum disorders studied with
mental language disorder. Journal of Autism positron emission tomography and magnetic
and Developmental Disorders, 19, 255–269. resonance imaging. American Journal of Psy-
Gualtieri, C. T., & Adams, A. (1982). Minor chiatry, 157, 1994–2001.
physical anomalies in alcoholic and schizo- Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K.,
phrenic adults and hyperactive and autistic O’Brien, L. M., Lange, N., Bakardjiev, A.,
children. American Journal of Psychiatry, et al. (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex,
139, 640–643. subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes
Harcherik, D. F., Cohen, D. J., Ort, S., Paul, R., in autistic boys. Brain, 126, 1182–1192.
Shaywitz, B. A., Volkmar, F. R., et al. (1985). Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Makris, N., Filipek,
Computed tomographic brain scanning in four P. A., Kemper, T. L., Normandin, J. J., et al.
neuropsychiatric disorders of childhood. Amer- (2004). Localization of white matter volume
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 731–734. increase in Autism and Developmental Lan-
Hardan, A. Y., Jou, R. J., Keshavan, M. S., Varma, guage Disorder. Annals of Neurology, 55,
R., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Increased frontal 530–540.
cortical folding in autism: A preliminary MRI Hoeksma, M., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M., van En-
study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, geland, H. (2004). Processing capacity in
131, 263–268. children and adolescents with pervasive de-
Hardan, A. Y., Kilpatrick, M., Keshavan, M. S., & velopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
Minshew, N. J. (2003). Motor performance and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 341–354.
anatomic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI ) Holmes, G., & Stewart, T. G. (1988). On the con-
of the basal ganglia in autism. Journal of Child nection of the inferior olives to the cerebellum
Neurology, 18, 317–324. in man. Brain, 31, 125–137.
Hardan, A. Y., Minshew, N. J., & Keshavan, M. S. Holttum, J. R., Minshew, N. J., Sanders, R. S., &
(2000). Corpus callosum size in autism. Neu- Phillips, N. E. (1992). Magnetic resonance
rology, 55, 1033–1036. imaging of the posterior fossa in autism. Bio-
Hardan, A. Y., Minshew, N. J., Mallikarjuhn, M., & logical Psychiatry, 32, 1091–1101.
Keshavan, M. S. (2001). Brain volume in Honomichl, R. D., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., Burnham,
autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 16, M., Gaylor, E., & Anders, T. F. (2002). Sleep
421–424. patterns of children with pervasive develop-
Hashimoto, T., Murakawa, K., Miyazaki, M., mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Tayama, M., & Kuroda, Y. (1992). Magnetic- opmental Disorders, 32, 553–561.
Resonance-Imaging of the Brain Structures in Howard, M. A., Cowell, P. E., Boucher, J., Broks,
the Posterior-Fossa in Retarded Autistic- P., Mayes, A., Farrant, A., et al. (2000). Con-
Children. Acta Paediatrica, 81, 1030–1034. vergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evi-
Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Fujino, M. K., dence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism.
Miyazaki, M., & Kuroda, Y. (1989). Magnetic NeuroReport, 11, 2931–2935.
resonance imaging in autism: Preliminary re- Hsu, M., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Courchesne, E., &
port. Neuropediatrics, 20, 142–146. Press, G. A. (1991). Absence of pontine abnor-
Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Miyazaki, M., Mu- mality in infantile autism. Archives of Neurol-
rakawa, K., & Kuroda, Y. (1993). Brainstem ogy, 48, 1160–1163.
and cerebellar vermis involvement in autistic Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., &
children. Journal of Child Neurology, 8, Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation
149–153. and synchronization during sentences com-
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 509

prehension in high-functioning autism: Evi- ulation in autistic children. Journal of Autism


dence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, and Developmental Disorders, 12, 185–193.
1811–1821. Kuhl, P., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, S., Dawson,
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. processing of speech in preschool children
Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G. (1988). Ref lex with autism: behavioral and electrophysiologi-
seizures induced by intermittent light stimula- cal measures. Developmental Science, 8, 1.
tion. Advances in Neurology, 75, 99–121. Kurtzberg, D., Vaughan, H. G., Jr., Courchesne, E.,
Kawasaki, Y., Yokota, K., Shinomiya, M., Simizu, Friedman, D., Harter, M. R., & Putnam, L. E.
Y., & Niwa, S. (1997). Brief report: Elec- (1984). Brain and information: Event-related
troencephalographic paroxysmal activities in potentials. Annals of the New York Academy of
the frontal area emerged in middle childhood Science, 425, 300.
and during adolescence in a followup study of Kutas, M., Lindamood, T., & Hillyard, S. (1984).
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Word expectancy and event related brain po-
Disorders, 27, 605–620. tentials during sentence processing. In S. Ko-
Kemner, C., Oranje, B., Verbaten, M., van Enge- rnblum & J. Requin (Eds.), Preparatory states
land, H. (2002). Normal P50 gating in children and processes (pp. 217–238). Hillsdale, NJ:
with autism. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 63, Erlbaum.
214–217. Lainhart, J. E. (2003). Increased Rate of Head
Kemner, C., van der Gaag, R., Verbaten, M., & van Growth During Infancy in Autism. Journal of
Engeland, H. (1999). ERP differences among the American Medical Association, 290,
subtypes of pervasive developmental disor- 393–394.
ders. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 781–789. Lainhart, J. E., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Landa, R.,
Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., Santangelo, S. L., Coon, H., et al. (1997).
Camfferman, G., & van Engeland, H. (1994). Macrocephaly in Children and Adults With
Visual and somatosensory event-related brain Autism. Journal of the American Academy of
potentials in autistic children and three differ- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 282–290.
ent control groups. Electroencephalography Lee, M., Martin-Ruiz, C., Graham, A., Court, J.,
and Clinical Neurophysiology, 92, 225–237. Jaros, E., Perry, R., et al. (2002). Nicotinic re-
Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., ceptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex
Camfferman, G., & van Engeland, H. (1995). in autism. Brain, 125, 1483–1495.
Auditory event related brain potentials in Leigh, R. J., & Zee, D. S. (1991). The neurology of
autistic children and three different control eye movements (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: F. A.
groups. Biological Psychiatry, 38, 150–165. Davis.
Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Cuperus, J. M., Levitt, J. G., Blanton, R., Capetillo-Cunliffe, L.,
Camfferman, G., & van Engeland, H. (1998). Guthrie, D., Toga, A., & McCracken, J. T.
Abnormal saccadic eye movements in autistic (1999). Cerebellar vermis lobules VIII-X in
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental autism. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology
Disorders, 28, 61–67. and Biological Psychiatry, 23, 625–633.
Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. L. (1998). Neu- Lewine, J. D., Andrews, R., Chez, M., Patil, A. A.,
ropathology of infantile autism. Journal of Devinsky, O., Smith, M., et al. (1999). Magne-
Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, toencephalographic patterns of epileptiform
57, 645–652. activity in children with regressive autism
Kleiman, M. D., Neff, S., & Rosman, N. P. (1992). spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 104, 405–418.
The brain in infantile autism: Is the cerebel- Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Harms, L., & Allen,
lum really abnormal? Neurology, 2, 753–760. M. (1993). Contextual probability evaluation
Koczat, D., Rogers, S., Pennington, B., & Ross, R. autistic, receptive developmental language
(2002). Eye movement abnormality suggestive disorder, and control children: Event-related
of a spatial working memory deficit present in brain potential evidence. Journal of Autism
parents of autistic probands. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 37–58.
and Developmental Disorders, 32, 513–518. Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Harms, L., & Allen,
Kohen-Raz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. M. (1995). Sensory modulation of auditory
(1992). Postural control in children with stimuli in children with autism and receptive
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental developmental language disorder: Event-
Disorders, 22, 419–432. related brain potential evidence. Journal of
Kootz, J. P., Marinelli, B., & Cohen, D. J. (1982). Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25,
Modulation of response to environmental stim- 521–539.
510 Neurological and Medical Issues

Links, P. S., Stokwell, M., Abichandani, F., & Miles, J. H., & Hillman, R. E. (2000). Value of a
Simeon, J. (1980). Minor physical anomalies clinical morphology examination in autism.
in childhood autism: I. Their relationship to American Journal of Genetics, 91, 245–253.
pre- and perinatal complications. Journal of Minshew, N. J., & Dombrowski, S. M. (1994). In
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, vivo neuroanatomy of autism: Imaging studies.
228–273. In M. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The
Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, B. A., Lazar, neurobiology of autism (pp. 66–85). Balti-
N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., et al. more: Johns Hopkins University Press.
(2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in Minshew, N. J., Furman, J. M., Goldstein, G., &
autism: An fMRI study of spatial working Payton, J. B. (1990). The cerebellum in
memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840. autism: A central role or an epiphenomenon?
Manes, F., Piven, J., Vrancic, D., Nanclares, V., [Abstract]. Neurology, 40, 14.
Plebst, C., & Starkstein, S. E. (1999). An MRI Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., &
study of the corpus callosum and cerebellum Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological func-
in mentally retarded autistic individuals. Jour- tioning in non-mentally retarded autistic indi-
nal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro- viduals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
sciences, 11, 470–474. Neuropsychology, 14, 749–761.
Martin-Ruiz, C. M., Lee, M., Perry, R. H., Bau- Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J.
man, M., Court, J. A., & Perry, E. K. (2004). (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in
Molecular analysis of nicotinic receptor ex- autism: Profile of a complex information pro-
pression in autism. Molecular Brain Research, cessing disorder. Journal of the International
123, 81–90. Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.
Mason-Brothers, A., Ritvo, E. R., Guze, B., Mo, Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. (1999).
A., Freeman, B. J., Funderburk, S. J., et al. Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems
(1987). PC: Pre-, peri-, and postnatal factors but cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurol-
in 181 autistic patients from single and multi- ogy, 52, 917–922.
ple incidence families. Journal of the Ameri- Minshew, N. J., Sung, K., Jones, B. L., & Furman,
can Academy of Child and Adolescent J. M. (2004). Underdevelopment of the pos-
Psychiatry, 20, 39–42. tural control system in autism. Neurology, 63,
Maurer, R. G., & Demasio, A. R. (1982). Child- 2056–2061.
hood autism from the point of view of behav- Molloy, C., Dietrich, K., & Bhattacharya, A.
ioral neurology. Journal of Autism and (2003). Postural stability in children with
Developmental Disorders, 12, 195–205. autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Maziade, M., Merette, C., Cayer, M., Roy, M., Szat- and Developmental Disorders, 33, 643–652.
mari, P., Cote, R., et al. (2000). Prolongation of Murakami, J. W., Courchesne, E., Press, G. A.,
brainstem auditory-evoked responses in autis- Yeung-Courchesne, R., & Hesselink, J. R.
tic probands and their unaffected relatives. (1989). Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1077–1083. and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in
McAlonan, G. M., Daly, E., Kumari, V., Critchley, autism. Archives of Neurology, 46, 689–694.
H. D., van Amelsvoort, T., Suckling, J., et al. Murphy, D. G. M., Critchley, H. D., Schmitz, N.,
(2002). Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gat- McAlonan, G., van Amelsvoort, T., Robert-
ing in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain, 125, son, D., et al. (2002). Asperger syndrome—A
1594–1606. proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study
McClelland, R. J., Eyre, D. G., Watson, D., Calvert, of brain. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59,
G. J., & Sherrard, E. (1992). Central conduc- 885–891.
tion time in childhood autism. British Journal Nagy, E., & Loveland, K. (2002). Prolonged brain-
of Psychiatry, 160, 659–663. stem auditory evoked potentials: An autism-
McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Carver, specific or non-specific marker. Archives of
L., & Panagiotides, H. (2004). Event-related General Psychiatry, 59, 228–290.
brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal Nass, R., & Petrucha, D. (1990). Acquired aphasia
processing of faces in autism. Journal of Child with convulsive disorder: A pervasive devel-
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- opmental disorder variant. Journal of Child
plines 45(7), 1235–1245. Neurology, 5, 327–328.
Miles, J. H., Hadden, L. L., Takahashi, T. N., & Nelson, C. A. (1994). Neural correlates of recogni-
Hillman, R. E. (2000). Head Circumference Is tion memory in the first postnatal year of life.
an Independent Clinical Finding Associated In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human be-
With Autism. American Journal of Genetics, havior and the developing brain (pp. 269–313).
95, 339–350. New York: Guilford Press.
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 511

Nicolson, K., & Szatmari, P. (2003). Genetic and Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968b). Perceptual
neurodevelopmental inf luences in autistic dis- inconstancy in early infantile autism. Archives
order. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48, of General Psychiatry, 18, 76–98.
526–537. Ornitz, E., Ritvo, E. R., Brown, M., La Franchi, S.,
Norman, R. M. (1940). Cerebellar atrophy associ- Parmelee, T., & Walter, R. (1969). The EEG
ated with etat marbre of the basal ganglia. and rapid eye movements during REM sleep in
Journal of Neurological Psychiatry, 3, autistic and normal children. Electroen-
311–318. cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology,
Novick, B., Kurtzberg, A., & Vaughan, H. G., Jr. 26, 167–175.
(1979). An electrophysiologic indication of Ornitz, E. M., Ritvo, E. R., Panman, L. E., Lee,
defective information storage in childhood Y. H., Carr, E. M., & Walker, R. D. (1968).
autism. Psychiatry Research, 1, 101–108. The auditory evoked response in normal and
Novick, B., Vaughan, H. G., Jr., Kurtzberg, D., & autistic children during sleep. Electroenceph-
Simson, R. (1980). An electrophysiologic indi- alography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 25,
cation of auditory processing defects in 221–230.
autism. Psychiatry Research, 3, 107–114. Ornitz, E. M., & Walter, D. O. (1975). The effect
Nowell, M. A., Hackney, D. B., Muraki, A. S., & of sound pressure waveform on human brain-
Coleman, M. (1990). Varied MR appearance stem auditory evoked responses. Brain Re-
of autism: Fifty-three pediatric patients hav- search, 92, 490–498.
ing the full autistic syndrome. Magnetic Reso- Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
nance Imaging, 8, 811–816. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-
Nydén, A., Carlsson, M., Carlsson, A., & Gillberg, functioning autistic individuals: Relationship
C. (2004). Interhemispheric transfer in high- to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology
functioning children and adolescents with and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
autism spectrum disorders: A controlled pilot Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., &
study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu- Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abili-
rology, 46, 448–454. ties in autism and Tourette syndrome: An
Oades, R., Walker, M., Geffen, L., & Stern, L. information-processing approach. Journal of
(1988). Event related potentials in autistic and Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
healthy children on an auditory choice reac- Disciplines, 35, 1015–1032.
tion time task. International Journal of Psy- Palmen, S. J. M. C., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kemner,
chophysiology, 6, 25–37. C., Schnack, H. G., Durston, S., Lahuis, B. E.,
Ornitz, E. M., Atwell, C. W., Kaplan, A. R., & et al. (in press). Increased gray matter volume
Westlake, J. R. (1985). Brain-stem dysfunc- in medication naïve high-functioning children
tion in autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, with autism spectrum disorder. Psychological
42, 1018–1025. Medicine.
Ornitz, E. M., Brown, M. B., Mason, A., & Put- Palmen, S. J. M. C., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kemner,
nam, N. H. (1974). Effect of visual input on C., Schnack, H. G., Janssen, J., Kahn, R. S.,
vestibular nystagmus in autistic children. et al. (2004). Larger brains in medication
Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 369–375. naïve high-functioning subjects with Perva-
Ornitz, E. M., Forsythe, A. B., & de la Pena, A. sive Developmental Disorder. Journal of
(1973). The effect of vestibular and auditory Autism and Developmental Disorders.
stimulation on the rapid eye movements of Palmen, S. J. M. C., & van Engeland, H. (2004).
REM sleep in normal children. Electroen- Review on structural neuroimaging findings in
cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, autism. Journal of Neural Transmission, 111,
34, 379–390. 903–929.
Ornitz, E., Lane, S., Sugiyama, T., & de Traversay, J. Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou,
(1993). Startle modulation studies in autism. M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional capac-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ities in children with autism: Is there a general
ders, 23, 619–637. deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism
Ornitz, E. M., Mo, A., & Olson, S. T. (1980). Inf lu- and Developmental Disorders, 28, 467–478.
ence of click sound pressure direction on Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N., Reznick, J., Paul, G.,
brainstem responses in children. Audiology, Goldman, B., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scan-
19, 245–254. ning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968a). Neurophysio- Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.
logic mechanisms underlying perceptual incon- Perry, E. K., Lee, M. L. W., Martin-Ruiz, C. M.,
stancy in autistic and schizophrenic children. Court, J. A., Volsen, S. G., Merrit, J., et al.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 19, 22–27. (2001). Cholinergic activity in autism:
512 Neurological and Medical Issues

Abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal Raymond, G., Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L.
forebrain. American Journal of Psychiatry, (1996). Hippocampus in autism: A
158, 1058–1066. Golgi analysis. Acta Neuropathologica, 91,
Pierce, K., Muller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & 117–119.
Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs Ritvo, E. R., Freeman, B. J., Scheibel, A., Duong,
outside the fusiform “ face area” in autism: T., Robinson, H., Guthrie, D., et al. (1986).
Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124, Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of
2059–2073. four autistic subjects: Initial finding of the
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, N., & Andreasen, N. UCLA-NSAC Autopsy Research report. Amer-
(1996). Regional brain enlargement in autism: ican Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 862–866.
A magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal Ritvo, E. R., & Garber, H. J. (1988). Cerebellar hy-
of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- poplasia and autism [Letter]. New England
cent Psychiatry, 35, 530–536. Journal of Medicine, 319, 1152.
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., Havercamp, S., An- Ritvo, E. R., Ornitz, E. M., Eviatar, A., Markham,
dreasen, N. C., & Palmer, P. (1995). An MRI C. H., Brown, M. B., & Mason, A. (1969). De-
study of brain size in autism. American Jour- creased postrotatory nystagmus in early infan-
nal of Psychiatry, 152, 1145–1149. tile autism. Neurology, 19, 653–658.
Piven, J., Bailey, J., Ranson, B. J., & Arndt, S. Rodier, P. M., Bryson, S. E., & Welch, J. P. (1997).
(1997). An MRI study of the corpus callosum Minor malformation and physical measure-
in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, ments in autism: Data from Nova Scotia. Tera-
154, 1051–1056. tology, 55, 319–325.
Piven, J., Bailey, J., Ranson, B. J., & Arndt, S. Rodier, P. M., Ingram, J. L., Tisdale, B., Nelson, S.,
(1998). No difference in hippocampus volume & Roman, J. (1996). Embryological origins for
detected on magnetic resonance imaging in autism: Developmental anomalies of the cra-
autistic individuals. Journal of Autism and De- nial nerve nuclei. Journal of Comparative Neu-
velopmental Disorders, 28, 105–110. rology, 36, 351–356.
Piven, J., Nehme, E., Simon, J., Barta, P., Pearlson, Rojas, D. C., Bawn, S. D., Benkers, T. L., Reite,
G., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Magnetic reso- M. L., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Smaller left
nance imaging in autism: Measurement of the hemisphere planum temporale in adults with
cerebellum, pons, and fourth ventricle. Biolog- autistic disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 328,
ical Psychiatry, 31, 491–504. 237–240.
Plioplys, A. V. (1994). Autism: Electroencephalo- Rosenbloom, S., Campbell, M., & George, A. E.
gram abnormalities and clinical improvement (1984). High resolution CT scanning in infan-
with valproic acid. Archives of Pediatric and tile autism: A quantitative approach. Journal
Adolescent Medicine, 148, 220–222. of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
Polich, J., & Starr, A. (1984). Evoked potentials in 23, 72–77.
aging. In M. L. Albert (Ed.), Clinical neurol- Rosenblum, S. M., Arick, J. R., Krug, D. A.,
ogy of aging (p. 149). New York: Oxford Uni- Stubbs, E. G., Young, N. B., & Pelson, R. O.
versity Press. (1980). Auditory brainstem evoked responses
Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Brief report: in autistic children. Journal of Autism and De-
Neuropsychological testing of autistic children velopmental Disorders, 10, 215–225.
through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Rosenhall, U., Johansson, E., & Gillberg, C.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- (1988). Oculomotor findings in autistic chil-
ders, 4, 581–590. dren. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 102,
Prior, M. R., Tress, B., Hoffman, W. L., & Boldt, D. 435–439.
(1984). Computed tomographic study of chil- Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Brantberg, K., & Gill-
dren with classic autism. Archives of Neurol- berg, C. (2003). Autism and auditory brain
ogy, 41, 482–484. stem responses. Ear and Hearing, 24,
Ramoz, N., Reichert, J. G., Smith, C. J., Silver- 206–214.
man, J. M., Bespalova, I. N., Davis, K. L., Rossi, F. G., Okun, M., Yachnis, A., Quisling, R., &
et al. (2004). Linkage and association of the Triggs, W. (2002). Corticosteroid treatment of
mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier mitochondrial encephalomyopathies. Neurolo-
SLC25A12 gene with autism. American Jour- gist, 8, 313–315.
nal of Psychiatry, 16, 662–669. Rossi, P. G., Parmeggiani, A., Bach, V., Santucci,
Rapin, I. (1991). Autistic children: Diagnostic and M., & Visconti, P. (1995). EEG features and
clinical features. Pediatrics, 87, (Suppl.), epilepsy in patients with autism. Brain and
751–760. Development, 17, 169–174.
Neurologic Aspects of Autism 513

Rumsey, J. M., Grimes, A. M., Pikus, A. M., Squires, N., Squires, K., & Hillyard, S. A. (1975).
Duara, R., & Ismond, D. R. (1984). Auditory Two varieties of long-latency positive waves
brainstem responses in pervasive develop- evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in
mental disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 19, man. Electroencephalography and Clinical
1403–1418. Neurophysiology, 38, 387–401.
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neu- Starr, A., Sohmer, H., & Celesia, G. G. (1978).
ropsychological findings in high-functioning Some applications of evoked potentials to pa-
men with infantile autism, residual state. Jour- tients with neurological and sensory impair-
nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- ment. In E. Callaway, P. Tueting, & S. H.
chology, 10, 201–221. Koslow (Eds.), Event-related brain potentials
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neu- in man (pp. 155–196). San Francisco: Aca-
ropsychological divergence of high-level autism demic Press.
and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and De- Stefanos, G. A., Grover, W., & Geller, E. (1995).
velopmental Disorders, 20, 155–168. Case study: Corticosteroid treatment of lan-
Saitoh, O., Courchesne, E., Egaas, B., Lincoln, A. J., guage regression in pervasive developmental
& Schreibman, L. (1995). Cross-sectional area disorder. Journal of the American Academy of
of the posterior hippocampus in autistic pa- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34,
tients with cerebellar and corpus callosum ab- 1107–1111.
normalities. Neurology, 45, 317–324. Steg, J. P., & Rapoport, J. L. (1975). Minor physical
Saitoh, O., Karns, C. M., & Courchesne, E. anomalies in normal, neurotic, learning dis-
(2001). Development of the hippocampal for- abled, and severely disturbed children. Journal
mation from 2 to 42 years—MRI evidence of of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5,
smaller area dentata in autism. Brain, 124, 299–307.
1317–1324. Stevenson, R. E., Schroer, R. J., Skinner, C.,
Sears, L. L., Vest, C., Mohamed, S., Bailey, J., Ran- Fender, D., & Simensen, R. J. (1997). Autism
son, B. J., & Piven, J. (1999). An MRI study of and macrocephaly. Lancet, 349, 1744–1745.
the basal ganglia in autism. Progress in Neuro- Stockard, J. J., Stockard, J. E., & Sharbrough,
Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychia- F. W. (1978). Nonpathologic factors inf luenc-
try, 23, 613–624. ing brainstem auditory evoked potentials.
Seri, S., Cerquiglini, A., Pisani, F., & Curatolo, P. American Journal of EEG Technology, 18,
(1999). Autism in tuberous sclerosis: Evoked 177–209.
potential evidence for a deficit in auditory Strandburg, R. J., Marsh, J. T., Brown, W. S.,
sensory processing. Clinical Neurophysiology, Asarnow, R. F., Guthrie, D., & Higa, J. (1993).
110, 1825–1830. Event-related potentials in high-functioning
Shewmon, D. A., & Erwin, R. J. (1988a). The ef- adult autistics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic vi-
fect of focal interictal spikes on perception sual information processing tasks. Neuropsy-
and reaction time. I: General considerations. chologia, 31, 413–434.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuro- Student, M., & Sohmer, H. (1978). Evidence from
physiology, 69, 319–337. auditory nerve and brainstem evoked re-
Shewmon, D. A., & Erwin, R. J. (1988b). The ef- sponses for an organic brain lesion in children
fect of focal interictal spikes on perception with autistic traits. Journal of Autism and
and reaction time. II: Neuroanatomic speci- Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 13–20.
ficity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Student, M., & Sohmer, H. (1979). Erratum. Jour-
Neurophysiology, 69, 319–337. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9,
Siebelink, B. M., Bakker, D. J., Binnie, C. D., & 309.
Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G. A. (1988). Psy- Tanguay, P. E., & Edwards, R. (1982). Electrophys-
chological effects of subclinical epileptiform iological studies of autism: The whisper of the
EEG discharges in children. II: General intel- bang. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ligence tests. Epilepsy Research, 2, 117–121. Disorders, 12, 177–184.
Skoff, B. F., Mirsky, A. F., & Turner, D. (1980). Tanguay, P. E., Edwards, R. M., Buchwald, J.,
Prolonged brainstem transmission time in Schwafel, J., & Allen, V. (1982). Auditory
autism. Psychiatric Research, 2, 157–166. brainstem responses in autistic children.
Sparks, B. F., Friedman, S. D., Shaw, D. W., Ayl- Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 174–180.
ward, E. H., Echelard, D., Artru, A. A., et al. Taylor, M., McCarthy, G., Saliba, E., & Degio-
(2002). Brain structural abnormalities in vanni, E. (1999). ERP evidence of develop-
young children with autism spectrum disorder. mental changes in processing of faces. Clinical
Neurology, 59, 184–192. Neurophysiology, 110, 910–915.
514 Neurological and Medical Issues

Taylor, M. J., Rosenblatt, B., & Linschoten, L. Verbaten, M. N., Roelofs, J. W., van Engeland, H.,
(1982). Electrophysiological study of the audi- Kenemans, J. K., & Slangen, J. L. (1991). Ab-
tory system in autistic children. In A. Rothen- normal visual event-related potentials of autis-
berger (Ed.), Event-related potentials in tic children. Journal of Autism and
children (pp. 23–38). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Developmental Disorders, 21, 449–470.
Thivierge, J., Bedard, C., Cote, R., & Maziade, M. Vidal, C. N., DeVito, T. J., Hayashi, K. M., Drost,
(1990). Brainstem auditory evoked response D. J., Williamson, P. C., Craven-Thuss, B.,
and subcortical abnormalities in autism. Amer- et al. (2003). Detection and visualization of
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 1609–1613. corpus callosum deficits in autistic children
Townsend, J., Westerfield, M., Leaver, E., Makeig, using novel anatomical mapping algorithms.
S., Jung, T., Pierce, K., et al. (2001). Event- Proceedings of the International Society for
related brain response abnormalities in Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. Toronto,
autism: Evidence for impaired cerebello- Canada, May 10–16.
frontal spatial attention networks. Cognitive Vilensky, J. A., Demasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G.
Brain Research, 11, 137–145. (1981). Gait disturbances in patients with
Tsatsanis, K. D., Rourke, B. P., Klin, A., Volkmar, autistic behavior. Archives of Neurology, 38,
F. R., Cicchetti, D., & Schultz, R. T. (2003). 646–649.
Reduced thalamic volume in high-functioning Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Childhood disintegrative
individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry, disorder: Issues for DSM-IV. Journal of Autism
53, 121–129. and Developmental Disorders, 22, 625–642.
Tuchman, R. (2000). Treatment of seizure disorders Volkmar, F. R. (1994). Childhood disintegrative
and EEG abnormalities in children with disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism Clinics of North America, 3, 119–129.
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 485–489. Walker, H. A. (1977). Incidence of minor physical
Tuchman, R. F., Jayakar, P., Yaylali, I., & Villalo- anomaly in autism: Epilepsy. Journal of Autism
bos, R. (1997). Seizures and EEG findings in and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 165–176.
children with autism spectrum disorders. CNS Webb, S. J., Bernier, R., Panagiotides, H., Paul, M.,
Spectrums, 3, 61–70. & Dawson, G. (2003, April). Further evidence
Tuchman, R. F., & Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in for an abnormality in the temporal processing
pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures of faces in autism. Poster presented at the So-
and epileptiform electroencephalogram corre- ciety for Research in Child Development,
lates. Pediatrics, 99, 560–566. Tampa, FL.
Tuchman, R. F., & Rapin, I. (2002). Epilepsy in Webb, S. J., Bernier, R., Shook, J., Paul, M., &
autism. Neurology, 1, 352–358. Dawson, G. (2004, May). Electrophysiological
Tuchman, R. F., Rapin, I., & Shinnar, S. (1991a). evidence of delayed neural processing of faces
Autistic and Dysphasic Children. I. Clinical in children with autism. Paper presented at the
Characteristics. Pediatrics, 88, 1211–1218. International Meeting for Autism Research,
Tuchman, R. F., Rapin, I., & Shinnar, S. (1991b). Sacramento, CA.
Autistic and Dysphasic Children. II: Epilepsy. Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Panagi-
Pediatrics, 88, 1219–1225. otides, H. (2004). ERP evidence of atypical
Uther, M., Jansen, D. H. J., Huotilainen, M., face processing in young children with autism.
Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Näätänen, R. (2003). Mis- Manuscript in preparation.
match negativity indexes auditory temporal Wong, V., & Wong, S. N. (1991). Brainstem audi-
resolution: Evidence from event-related poten- tory evoked potential study in children with
tial (ERP) and event-related field (ERF) autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Devel-
recordings. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, opmental Disorders, 21, 329–340.
685–691. Zimmerman, M. C., Morgan, D. E., & Dubno, J. R.
van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Cammfferman, (1987). Auditory brainstem evoked response
G., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. characteristics in developing infants. Annals of
(2001). Eye movements, visual attention, and Otology, Rhinolology, and Laryngology, 96,
autism: A saccadic reaction time study using 291.
the gap and overlap paradigm. Biological Psy-
chiatry, 50, 614–619.
CHAPTER 19

Functional Neuroimaging Studies of


Autism Spectrum Disorders

ROBERT T. SCHULTZ AND DIANA L. ROBINS

As reviewed in Chapter 18 (“Neurologic As- be impaired for classic autistic symptoms to


pects of Autism”), there is a large and growing be present. Although there may be deficits in
body of evidence characterizing the patho- complex cognitive functions in many persons
physiology of the autism spectrum disorders with autism, those deficits are probably medi-
(ASDs). This chapter reviews the research lit- ated by deficits in general intelligence, such
erature that uses functional neuroimaging that it is difficult to see evidence for these
techniques to study the brain bases of the complex cognitive deficits in persons with
ASDs. Autism is defined on the basis of a se- autism who are average or above average in
lect set of behavioral disturbances that more or general intelligence. It remains a puzzle why
less map onto specific functional systems of autism is generally associated with impaired
the brain. The difficulties with social reci- intellectual functioning (i.e., why as many as
procity, communication, and restricted and 70% of cases have some degree of mental retar-
repetitive behaviors and interests that occur dation), and solving this puzzle may well shed
with autism suggest that the syndrome affects light on the basic mechanisms causing autism.
a functionally diverse and widely distributed All the evidence points to autism being a
set of neural systems. However, at the same highly heritable but complex genetic disorder,
time, not all brain systems are affected, and with locus and allelic heterogeneity (Rutter,
thus it would be a mistake to consider autism 2000; Veenstra-VanderWeele, Christian, &
as a disorder of general brain function. There Cook, 2004; Yonan et al., 2003). This genetic
can be many areas of spared function in heterogeneity, coupled with the gene-gene and
autism, for example, basic visual perceptual gene-environment interaction effects on brain
skills, as well as certain cognitive skills. In development, makes the autism phenotype in-
fact, autism is not incompatible with superior herently heterogeneous. Even where a set of
IQ; there are many very bright but very se- susceptibility genes is completely shared in
verely affected persons with autism, indicat- common by two individuals, we should expect
ing that general intellectual functions need not differences, sometimes marked, in phenotypic

This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(grants PO1 HD 03008, PO1 HD/ DC35482 and U54 MH066494-01) and by a grant from the James S.
McDonnell Foundation to the Perceptual Expertise. Ideas presented in this chapter are based in part on
many helpful discussions with our colleagues James Tanaka, Mathew State, Isabel Gauthier, Tom James,
Cheryl Klaiman, Ami Klin, and Fred Volkmar. Portions of this work appear in R. T. Schultz (in press), “De-
velopmental Deficits in Social Perception in Autism: The Role of the Amygdala and Fusiform Face Area,”
International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

515
516 Neurological and Medical Issues

expression for the two individuals. It is impor- context necessary to appreciate a set of func-
tant to note that whereas the same genes can tional findings. Moreover, computer-averaged
give rise to different phenotypic outcomes, phe- electroencephalography (EEG), as is done for
notypic variation must be completely mediated event-related potential (ERP) recordings,
by variation at the level of brain functioning might properly be considered a functional
(i.e., the endophenotype), and there exists a technique, but because EEG is a technique
very direct mapping of behavior to brain func- closely affiliated with the topic of neurologi-
tioning. Too much variation in the behavioral cal findings that is central to Chapter 18, its
phenotype of a study sample can complicate a derivative, ERPs, is also contained in that
neuroimaging study, where the goal is to define chapter. A newer technique, magnetoencepha-
a common set of brain characteristics that can lography (MEG), is not covered in Chapter 18
be linked to a set of cardinal features for the or in this chapter because it has rarely been
disorder. Yet, from a genetic point of view, it is used in studies of autism (though the inter-
probably unwise to place restrictions on the ested reader should see the work of Timothy P.
sample variability in the phenotype because Roberts and colleagues from the University of
doing so risks obfuscating what might be im- Toronto for some promising first studies using
portant and common genetic mechanisms. How- MEG to study the auditory system in autism).
ever, in neuroimaging research more generally, fMRI is far and away the most popular
and in this particular review of neural systems functional technique for investigating the pro-
that mediate autistic symptoms, we chose to re- cessing capacities of discrete brain areas and
strict our focus to those core features that are distributed brain systems. Compared to other
invariably present across the otherwise hetero- techniques, such as 15O-water PET and MEG,
geneous population. the equipment and expertise needed to per-
Functional neuroimaging procedures such form fMRI studies are widely available; nearly
as 15O-water positron emission tomography every academic medical center in the United
(PET) and functional magnetic resonance States and many other developed countries
imaging (fMRI ) are ideally suited for studying throughout the world have the basic equipment
in detail the separate neural systems that gov- to perform these studies. However, while this
ern select sensory and cognitive domains in practicality is important, fMRI also has sev-
relative isolation from other brain systems. eral other attributes that contribute to its pop-
Both techniques rely on the fact that when spe- ularity. Unlike PET, fMRI is noninvasive and
cific brain areas are engaged (more active rel- involves only minimal risk. 15O-water PET re-
ative to some baseline set of circumstances), quires the use of a radioactive tracer to map
there is an increase in blood flow to those brain blood flow (via the tagged water in
neural regions. Both 15O-water PET and fMRI blood) and hence the neural systems involved
techniques measure neural activity indirectly in specific processes. PET can also use a ra-
through the increased blood flow that is the dioactive tag on other ligands instead of water
brain’s response to local neural activity. to map glucose metabolism or to map aspects
Whereas conventional MRI and computed to- of neurotransmitter systems. This ability is its
mography (CT) allow imaging of the structure great advantage compared to fMRI, which can
of the brain and its components, functional measure only the hemodynamic response to lo-
techniques allow dynamic interrogation of calized neural activity, not metabolism or re-
the brain at work and allow specifying which ceptor density. Because most countries do not
systems are aberrant in autism and what cir- allow exposing children to radioactivity purely
cumstances (e.g., types of processing load) for research purposes, even though the levels
modulate these aberrations when compared to are small, PET is not available on a research
typically developing persons. Chapter 18 (this basis for use with minors. This is one reason
Handbook, this volume) reviews the literature that it has been used less frequently for the
on brain structure using MRI as well as post- study of autism. fMRI also offers a slight ad-
mortem work; thus, this work is not reviewed vantage in spatial resolution compared to
here in any detail and is mentioned only in PET—the smallest elements that can be re-
passing insofar as sometimes it provides the solved with fMRI are about 1 mm3, though in
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 517

practice, most studies employ a voxel resolu- series of auditory and verbal tasks using 15O-
tion of at least 3 mm3. fMRI is predicated on water PET. The persons with an ASD showed
the fact that the oxygenated and deoxygenated significantly less left lateralization compared
states of hemoglobin in the blood have differ- to controls in the perisylvian language areas
ent magnetic properties. The hemodynamic re- when listening to sentences. This finding has
sponse to local neural activity results in an now been replicated by Boddaert et al. (2003),
increase in the local concentration of oxyhe- who used 15O-water PET in five autistic adults
moglobin relative to deoxyhemoglobin. This is and eight comparison subjects while listening
detected with fMRI because deoxyhemoglobin to speechlike sounds and during a rest condi-
causes a local magnetic susceptibility that tion. As in the study by Müller and colleagues,
dampens the magnitude of signal in that re- the persons with an ASD showed less left lat-
gion; thus, active brain tissue provides locally eralization for the speechlike sounds. Both
stronger MRI signal values than less active re- studies point to deficiencies in the left tempo-
gions. The fMRI signal is referred to as the ral language regions as likely reasons underly-
blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal. ing the language difficulties found in autism.
This review is organized around the core These studies are also consistent with one re-
features of ASDs, namely, studies of (1) lan- cent sMRI study that showed reversed left-
guage and communication dysfunction, (2) so- right tissue asymmetry in language-related
cial dysfunction, and (3) repetitive behaviors cortices (Herbert et al., 2002).
and stereotypes, and, to a lesser extent, some More recently, Gervais et al. (2004) used
ancillary features of autism. Although there fMRI to study five persons with an ASD com-
have been some structural MRI (sMRI ) stud- pared to eight healthy controls. The ASD
ies that correlated magnitude of repetitive be- group failed to activate superior temporal sul-
haviors with volumetric measures of select cus (STS) voice-selective regions in response
brain parts, there have been no fMRI studies to vocal sounds compared to nonvocal sounds.
that have explored this third component of the The STS, however, showed normal activation
autism triad. Thus, we dispense with that as- patterns to nonvocal sounds, suggesting that
pect of the review at the outset. And although some more basic auditory processing deficit
studies of language are relatively common in was not responsible for the finding. They did
the normative fMRI and PET literature, there not report the abnormal hemispheric lateral-
has been surprisingly little work done in this ization found in prior studies, and their discus-
area in autism, so our review here is necessar- sion of the significance of these findings
ily short. The social deficits in autism, how- emphasized the difficulties that persons with
ever, have been studied rather frequently with an ASD have in processing socially relevant
functional neuroimaging approaches, which is auditory information. The ASD group ap-
probably a testament to the prevailing view peared to attend less to the vocal sounds, as
that these deficits are the core of autism after the fMRI they were able to recall signifi-
(Schultz, in press). This chapter, therefore, is cantly fewer of the vocal sounds ( but not the
devoted largely to reviewing the research on nonvocal sounds) compared to the controls.
the social-cognitive and social-perceptual Thus, the STS activation failure may be a sim-
processes in ASDs. ple failure to attend to the voices, something
that was rather automatic among the controls.
STUDIES OF COMMUNICATION It is not clear whether these differences might
AND LANGUAGE also be modulated by the group differences in
general intelligence, which were presumably
Even though behavioral studies of language large (the mean global IQ for the five ASD
and communication abilities in ASDs are cases was 81, and they had significantly fewer
rather common, there have been only five pub- years of education compared to the controls).
lished fMRI or PET studies focusing on lan- Just, Cherkassky, Keller, and Minshew
guage abilities in persons with an ASD. (2004) used fMRI to compare sentence com-
Müller and colleagues (1999) studied five prehension in a group of 17 persons with high-
high-functioning persons with autism during a functioning autism (HFA) compared to a
518 Neurological and Medical Issues

verbal IQ-matched control group (gender dis- Rapoport, 1988). Just and colleagues con-
tribution for each group was not specified). firmed these observations in the most exten-
The task was a two-choice sentence compre- sive testing of this hypothesis to date. Across
hension task (e.g., “ The cook thanked the fa- 186 regions of interest (ROI ) pairings, the
ther. Who was thanked? Cook—Father ”). The autism group had a reliable lower functional
ASD group showed significantly more activa- connectivity measurement (0.58 versus 0.61);
tion in the posterior left superior temporal in 79% of the ROI pairs, the average correla-
gyrus (Wernicke’s area) and less activation in tion in the autism group was less than that of
the left inferior frontal gyrus (Broca’s area) the control group. Ten of these ROI pairs were
compared to the control group. The authors significantly different between the groups, and
speculated that this pattern of results might in each case the autism group showed signifi-
mean that the autistic group engaged in more cantly less connectivity. These data and those
extensive analyses of the meanings of individ- of Castelli et al. and Horwitz et al. are consis-
ual words (via computations in Wernicke’s tent in pointing to deficits in synchronized
area) and less integration of the meanings of brain activity.
individual words into a coherent whole state- The measurement approach used by Just and
ment (in Broca’s area) and that this pattern is colleagues (2004) cannot make any strong
consistent with their tendency toward hyper- statements about the meaning of these correla-
lexia with poorer conceptual comprehension. tions. It is possible that any pair of nodes may
They did not find a significant difference be more or less synchronized in their BOLD
in lateralization of activation between the signal across time because of a host of common
two groups. In a somewhat related study, influences on them, with the possibility of no
Turkeltaub et al. (2004) studied the neural direct relationship between them. Friston et al.
basis of hyperlexia in a 9-year-old boy with (1997) distinguished this type of functional
pervasive developmental disorder-not other- connectivity from what they term ef fective
wise specified (PDD-NOS) who was reading connectivity, which is a stronger form of the
at a 15-year-old level and found greater left hypothesis that the regions have a direct influ-
Broca’s and left Wernicke’s activation com- ence on each other. One means of assessing ef-
pared to age and reading level control groups. fective connectivity is to look for significant
This boy did not demonstrate the frontal- increases or decreases in the magnitude of the
posterior imbalance shown by the group stud- correlation between two brain areas due to a
ied by Just and colleagues. However, a differ- change in task demands. Friston and colleagues
ent finding might have been expected because have called this correlation a psychophysical
the task differed (covert reading without a interaction (PPI ), such that during one type of
comprehension query). The difference might processing task the correlation is greater than
also reflect something specific to hyperlexia. during another. When someone tests for a PPI
In addition, Just and colleagues (2004) ex- between two nodes that are known to be in-
plored the correlation between the fMRI time volved in a specific function and finds a task-
series data for select pairs of regions of inter- related modulation of that function, this is
est, for example, between the average time believed to be a stronger argument for a direct
course for Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas. functional relationship between the nodes (see
These types of correlations have been used in Pasley, Mayes, & Schultz, 2004, for an example
the literature as an index of “ functional con- concerning face perception).
nectivity” (Friston, Frith, Liddle, & Fracko- It will be important to confirm these obser-
wiak, 1993). Just and colleagues describe the vations suggestive of reduced functional con-
correlations as suggesting “synchronization” nectivity with additional data and with tests
between brain areas. Prior work with 15O- employing task-dependent modulation of the
water PET in two studies has suggested that magnitude of the correlation in a hypothesized
persons with autism have reduced synchro- direction. Nevertheless, the existing fMRI and
nization during a theory of mind (ToM) task PET data provide exciting models of the nature
(Castelli, Frith, Happe, & Frith, 2002) and of the dysfunction in the autistic brain. More-
during rest (Horwitz, Rumsey, Grady, & over, these functional findings are consistent
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 519

with the sMRI data pointing to aberrations in contact; have abnormal emotional intonations
the volume of the cerebral white matter across in their voice and speech; have marked impair-
development. This topic is covered more exten- ment in the use of multiple nonverbal behav-
sively in Chapter 18, but briefly, recent data iors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression,
suggest a possible overgrowth of white matter body postures, and gestures to regulate social
during the first years of life in autism interaction; and do not spontaneously seek to
(Courchesne et al., 2001; Herbert et al., 2003) share enjoyment, interests, or achievements
that is followed by a selective reduction in spe- with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing,
cific white matter tracks by later adolescence bringing, or pointing out objects of interest).
and adulthood (Berthier, Bayes, & Tolosa, From a neuropsychological perspective, the
1993; Egass, Courchesne, & Saitoh, 1995; prevailing belief is that these aberrant social
Hardan, Minshew, & Keshavan, 2000; Manes behaviors are due to deficits in (1) social per-
et al., 1999; Piven, Saliba, Bailey, & Arndt, ception, for example, reading facial expres-
1997). This all might make particular sense in sions; (2) social cognition, for example,
the context of the overall increase in brain size perspective taking and ToM; and (3) social mo-
found in autism, especially at the youngest tivation. Thus, functional brain imaging studies
ages. We have argued elsewhere (Schultz, Ro- seek to study these underlying processes be-
manski, et al., 2000) that one consequence of cause they are thought to directly mediate the
an increased brain size in the ASDs will be a aberrant or absent behaviors that define autism.
reduction of interconnectivity because theo- This section reviews fMRI and PET studies that
retic models of brain size growth suggest that relate to this set of impairments, as well as
the degree of tissue connectivity must be re- some of the lesion literature that supports the
duced with increasing brain size (Ringo, functional imaging data.
1991). Such a reduction in neural integration
would also be consistent with one influential Face Perception Deficits
theory that attributes autistic symptoms to a
lack of “central coherence” (Frith, 1989), a Face perception can be subdivided into two
cognitive processing style that makes integra- general types: (1) recognition of person iden-
tion of parts into wholes problematic. It will be tity via the structural features of the face and
interesting to see the result of the many diffu- (2) recognition of internal affective state of
sion tensor-imaging studies currently under- another individual, independent of his or her
way at autism neuroimaging labs in several identity, via the shape of individual features
places throughout the United States and Eu- and changes in their relative distance from one
rope. Diffusion tensor imaging should add im- another during the expression. Both sets of
portant information on the nature of the white skills are necessary for successful functioning
matter and possibly on the nature of specific within a social group; it is important to be able
fiber tracks within the autistic brain. to quickly differentiate friends, enemies, and
potential mates prior to any interpersonal en-
STUDIES OF SOCIAL PERCEPTION counter. Moreover, for the development and
AND SOCIAL COGNITION maintenance of social relationships, it is es-
sential to successfully perceive and compre-
Current conceptualization of the social hend the changes in facial countenance that
deficits as embodied by the nomenclature of reflect the internal state of others. These two
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Men- sets of skills, while related, nonetheless ap-
tal Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; Ameri- pear to be mediated by somewhat different
can Psychiatric Association, 1994) entails an neural systems.
emphasis on absent or deficient behaviors im- Individuals with an ASD are selectively
portant for social relatedness. The diagnostic impaired in their ability to recognize face
criteria of DSM-IV describe social behavioral identity (Boucher & Lewis, 1992; Braverman,
deficits in several areas. Persons with autism Fein, Lucci, & Waterhouse, 1989; Davies,
fail to develop peer relationships appropriate Bishop, Manstead, & Tantam, 1994; de
to their developmental level; have poor eye Gelder, Vroomen, & Van der Heide, 1991;
520 Neurological and Medical Issues

Deruelle, Rondan, Gepner, & Tardif, 2004; Functional Neuroimaging Studies


Grelotti, Gauthier, & Schultz, 2002; Hauck, of Social Perception
Fein, & Maltby, 1999; Hobson, 1986a, 1986b;
Hobson, Ouston, & Lee, 1988b; Joseph & In the past 5 to 10 years, there has been a
Tanaka, 2002; Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, sharp increase in interest in using functional
& Cohen, 2002; Klin et al., 1999; Langdell, neuroimaging to understand the neural sys-
1978; Tantam, Monaghan, Nicholson, & Stir- tems that mediate social perception and so-
ling, 1989; Weeks & Hobson, 1987). Although cial cognition in normative as well as autistic
faces may appear to be quite different from populations. Of these areas, person identity
one another, features of the face and their via the face is probably best understood and
placement are remarkably uniform compared probably engages the most restricted set of
to those of other common objects, and thus our neural tissue.
skill in discriminating faces is more precisely A small region on the underside of the tem-
developed than for other types of object per- poral lobe, along the lateral extent of the mid-
ception (Diamond & Carey, 1986). The im- dle portion of the fusiform gyrus (FG), shows
pairment in face perception among those with selectivity (i.e., enhanced activation) for faces
autism is specific to faces because they are not compared to other complex objects (Kan-
impaired with other types of complex objects wisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997; Kanwisher
such as buildings or furniture (Grelotti et al., & Wojciulik, 2000; Puce, Allison, Gore, &
2002; Schultz, in press). We have hypothesized McCarthy, 1995); this area has been termed
that persons with an ASD fail to develop the the fusiform face area (FFA; Kanwisher et al.,
same level of expertise for faces as typically 1997). Anatomically, the middle portion of the
developing persons because of inadequately FG is split along its rostral-caudal extent by a
motivated attention to faces across develop- shallow, midfusiform sulcus. In fMRI, the
ment, which leads to less actual experience center of activation in face perception tasks is
with faces (Grelotti et al., 2002; Schultz, in typically offset toward the lateral aspect of the
press; Schultz, Gauthier, et al., 2000; Schultz, FG, in the right hemisphere (Haxby et al.,
Romanski, & Tsatsanis, 2000). Persons with 1999). Group composites show right hemi-
an ASD are also impaired in their ability to sphere activations to be larger than left; in
perceive, label, and show comprehension of fa- fact, individual subjects may or may not show
cially expressed emotions (Capps, Yirmiya, & left FG activation during face perception
Sigman, 1992; Dawson, Hill, Spencer, tasks. It is not completely clear what special
Galpert, & Watson, 1990; Fein, Lucci, Braver- computations are carried out in this region
man, & Waterhouse, 1992; Hobson, 1986a, compared to neighboring tissue such as the
1986b; Hobson & Lee, 1989; Hobson, Ouston, medial FG, lingual gyrus, inferior temporal
& Lee, 1988a, 1988b; Jaedicke, Storoschuk, & gyrus, and the lateral occipital gyrus. Most ac-
Lord, 1994; MacDonald et al., 1989; McGee, counts, however, hypothesize that the FFA is
Feldman, & Chernin, 1991; Snow, Hertzig, & particularly tuned to holistic and configural
Shapiro, 1987; Yirmiya, Sigman, Kasari, & processing, but attempts to demonstrate this
Mundy, 1992). On the surface, deficits in fa- have not always been successful (Yovel & Kan-
cial expression perception might appear to wisher, 2004).
have the most relevance for the social deficits The FFA is one of the two primary social
in autism. This may indeed prove to be true, perceptual areas. The other is the STS, a brain
but, at this time, deficits in person identity area known to be important for interpreting
from the face are better understood, especially dynamic social signals, such as direction of
concerning the functional neuroanatomy of eye gaze, gestures, facial expression, and other
autism. Because this literature is too large to “changeable” aspects of the face and body
cover adequately in detail in this chapter on (Adolphs, 2003; Allison, Puce, & McCarthy,
neuroimaging, the interested reader is referred 2000; Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2000).
to several of the most recent papers and re- There are no published fMRI studies of social
views on face perception and autism (e.g., perception implicating the STS in autism,
Deruelle et al., 2004; Grelotti et al., 2002; though there have been a number of confer-
Klin et al., 2002; Schultz, in press). ence presentations suggesting that this region
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 521

is clearly underactive during social perceptual arise from top-down processes than bottom-
tasks (e.g., Robins, Hunyadi, & Schultz, 2004). up processes.
There are, however, published reports of the Hypoactivity of the FFA has now been
morphology of the STS being altered in autism replicated by nine other labs (Aylward,
(Boddaert et al., 2004; Waiter et al., 2004); Bernier, Field, Grimme, & Dawson, 2004;
thus, it seems clear that this region will have Critchley et al., 2000a; Curby, Schyns, Gos-
an important role in developing models of the selin, & Guthier, 2003; Davidson et al., 2004;
autistic deficits in the “social brain.” Hall, Szechtman, & Nahmias, 2003; Hubl
In the first functional neuroimaging study et al., 2003; Pierce, Müller, Ambrose, Allen, &
of face perception among people with an ASD, Courchesne, 2001; Piggot et al., 2004; Wang,
we showed that the FFA was hypoactive in a Dapretto, Hariri, Sigman, & Bokheimer,
mixed group of 14 persons with autism and 2004). The compensatory activation of object
Asperger syndrome compared to two indepen- selective areas during face perception among
dent samples of 14 control participants persons with an ASD has only clearly been
(Schultz, Gauthier, et al., 2000). Because a replicated once (Hubl et al., 2003), and our
typical set of fMRI data for a single person own replication work (Schultz, 2004), with a
will have more than 50,000 individual data sample size three times larger than any other
points (image voxels), comparing fMRI data study, also fails to find reliable differences in
between people and between groups is statisti- that region. Critchley and colleagues demon-
cally complicated because multiple compar- strated hypoactivation of the FFA in a group
isons greatly inflate the risk of falsely of nine adult males with a clinical diagnosis of
identifying voxels as significantly different either autism (n = 2) or Asperger syndrome (n
between data sets by chance. This problem has = 7), using an active face perception task re-
long been recognized and is a very active area quiring the participants to categorize faces as
of research (see Jezzard, Matthews, & Smith, expressive or neutral. Pierce and colleagues
2001, for a complete discussion of this topic). also used an active perceptual task involving
To deal with this problem, we employed a gender discrimination of neutral faces in a
strategy of first identifying the FFA region in sample of six adults with autism. Hubl et al.
one sample of controls and then using that FFA showed FFA hypoactivation in seven adult
definition in an independent sample of 14 con- males with autism using both a gender dis-
trols compared to our group of 14 persons with crimination and a neutral versus expressive
an ASD. We also reversed this process and de- discrimination task. Aylward et al. examined
fined the FFA in the second control group and FFA activation to familiar and unfamiliar
then used that definition in a comparison of faces in a group of 11 persons with an ASD as
ASD and the first control group. In this way compared to 10 healthy controls; the FFA was
we were able to show selective underactivity hypoactive to only the unfamiliar faces. Hall
of the FFA region, which differed somewhat and colleagues used 15O-water PET in a group
between control samples in its precise location of eight high-functioning males with autism as
and extent; if this hypoactivation were a spuri- compared to eight healthy male controls dur-
ous finding, we would not expect it to be sig- ing an emotion recognition task and showed
nificant in both analyses. We also showed hypoactivation of the FFA, as well as other
compensatory overactivity of the neighboring deficits. Wang and colleagues used two tasks
tissue in the inferior temporal gyrus, an area in a group of 12 males. In one task, partici-
that was selectively active during the object pants had to select the facial expression that
differentiation task. matched the facial expression of the target
Hadjikhani, Chabris, et al. (2004) used face from two alternatives. In the second task,
fMRI to study the organization of the early vi- participants had to pair a verbal label with a
sual cortex in autism and found it to be intact, facial expression. Hypoactivation of the FFA
with a normal ratio between central and pe- was found only in the purely perceptual task,
ripheral visual field representation. Although perhaps because of the overall increase in the
requiring replication, this finding suggests face perceptual load in that condition relative
that the differences in higher-level percep- to the verbal labeling condition. Piggot and
tion, such as face perception, more likely colleagues were the first to use a sample of all
522 Neurological and Medical Issues

children, 14 boys with an ASD. This was a rent review), he showed greater than normal
companion to the Wang et al. study in that both FFA activation to HSF faces and hypoactiva-
used the same task methodology, and it is inter- tion to LSF faces.
esting that both found hypoactivation only Finally, using active face discrimination
under task conditions devoid of verbal stimula- tasks in two new samples of persons with an
tion. It is impossible to know without simulta- ASD (total ASD n = 44), we have replicated the
neous eye tracking data how the verbal labels FFA hypoactivation effect using neutral face
may have affected attention and FFA activa- pictures in one study and expressive faces in the
tion, but it is significant that both studies were second (Schultz et al., under review). Impor-
sensitive to the hypoactivation of the FFA tantly, we showed in both samples a significant
effect in ASDs only under the pure face condi- correlation to degree of face expertise, such that
tion. Attentional effects were explicitly exam- those with better scores on a standardized test
ined by Davidson et al. (2004), who showed of face perception outside the magnet showed
hypoactivation of the FFA across two samples more FFA activation during fMRI, regardless of
of males with an ASD (n’s = 14 and 17), com- group membership (i.e., ASD or controls).
pared to samples of controls of an equal size. Moreover, degree of social impairment as mea-
However, posterior regions of the FG showed sured by the Autism Diagnostic Observation
strong attentional effects, in that activation Schedule (ADOS) social domain also correlated
was increased significantly when participants with degree of FFA hypoactivation, such that
focused on the eye region as opposed to other the more socially impaired participants had less
aspects of the display. FFA activation to faces.
Grelotti et al. (2005) studied an 11-year-old Counting our two new samples, the two
boy with autism who was expert at distin- case studies, and the two samples included
guishing a novel class of objects known as within Davidson et al. (2004), there are now 15
Digimon (digital monsters), cartoon charac- reports of FFA hypoactivation with a total
ters of Japanese origin. This Digimon “expert ” sample size of 157 persons with an ASD and a
was compared to another boy with autism and combined control sample of 167. There are
a healthy control during several tasks involving also now two reported failures to find hypoac-
Digimon discrimination as well as face and tivation of the FFA in ASD. Pierce, Haist,
common object discrimination. Both boys with Sedaghat, and Courchesne (2004) used famil-
autism showed hypoactivation of their FFA to iar and unfamiliar faces in an fMRI study of 8
faces, but the Digimon expert showed en- adult males with autism and 10 healthy control
hanced activation to the Digimon images, sup- males. Like Aylward et al. (2004) and Grelotti
porting an expertise model of FFA functions. et al. (in press), Pierce et al. found signifi-
In their detailed case study of face percep- cantly greater activation to familiar as com-
tion in a young adult with ASD, Curby and pared to unfamiliar faces in the autism sample.
colleagues (2003) used fMRI to map out Whereas controls showed more FFA activation
FFA responses to different spatial frequency to unfamiliar faces than did the males with
ranges. Any visible object can be described in autism, this difference failed to reach statisti-
terms of its different spatial frequency compo- cal significance. It is not clear why this study
nents. The high spatial frequencies (HSFs), failed to confirm the group difference in FFA
that is, the sharp changes in brightness (the engagement to unfamiliar faces, but it is note-
edges), are especially important for individual worthy that the means were in the direction re-
feature identification. Low spatial frequencies ported by other studies, and thus the results of
(LSFs) capture information about the spatial this study might simply be due to insufficient
configuration of the features. Curby and col- statistical power (especially because the study
leagues showed that this individual had the ex- used an event-related design, which is much
pected hypoactivation of the FFA to broad lower in statistical power than the block de-
spatial frequency faces (i.e., unfiltered im- signs used by other studies; Birn, Cox, & Ban-
ages). However, consistent with the literature dettini, 2002). However, it is also noteworthy
on deficits in holistic processing and a bias to- that the fMRI task employed in this study did
ward features (see Schultz, in press, for a cur- not demand person identification processes,
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 523

but rather entailed a button press to each oc- any prior study (note that not all studies report
currence of a female face. As Grill-Spector, image size) and is about four times larger than
Knouf, and Kanwisher (2004) have nicely face pictures that we have employed in our
demonstrated, the FFA is involved in both per- studies. Images greater than about 3 to 5 de-
son detection (e.g., the gender task of Pierce grees of visual angle cannot be viewed in their
et al.) and person identification (i.e., differen- entirety (foveated) without eye movements.
tiating unique individuals), and this latter pro- Moreover, smaller images effectively empha-
cess may significantly bolster FFA activation size LSFs because details will be blurred. Re-
above and beyond levels achieved with simple calling that Curby et al. (2003) found greater
person or gender detection. Moreover, it may FFA activation to HSF faces in their case
be the FFA’s contributions to person identifi- study, the relative emphasis on HSFs might
cation processes that are important for differ- also have been an important influence to the
entiating controls and those with an ASD. results of Hadjikhani and colleagues. This pos-
Another interesting contribution of the study sibility is especially intriguing.
by Pierce and colleagues is the demonstration In light of the majority of findings indicat-
that personally meaningful faces (friends and ing that the FG has some specific role in ASDs,
family) modulate FFA engagement; this is con- studies are now beginning to appear examin-
sistent with the argument that inputs from the ing its morphology by way of high-resolution
amygdala on affective salience are critical for sMRI. Waiter and colleagues (2004) studied
amplifying FFA activation and engendering 16 males with an ASD between the ages of 12
proficient face perceptual processes (Schultz, and 20 and compared their brain structure to a
in press; Vuilleumier, Richardson, Armony, group of 16 healthy controls using an auto-
Driver, & Dolan, 2004). mated procedure known as voxel brain mor-
The second report failing to show hypoacti- phology (VBM). They found about a dozen
vation of the FFA in ASD was by Hadjikhani, brain areas that were specifically enlarged
Joseph, and colleagues (2004). They studied in the ASD group, consistent with the findings
11 adult males with an ASD (six autism, four of overall brain enlargement that have been
Asperger, and one PDD-NOS) compared to 10 reported multiple times (see Chapter 18,
adult males; notably, the control sample was this Handbook, this volume). Their second
significantly younger (mean age of 26 years strongest finding involved a specific enlarge-
versus 36 years for the participants with an ment of the right FG, with the location of the
ASD). This failure to find hypoactivation of peak size difference consistent with peak co-
the FFA might be attributable to the particu- ordinates found in fMRI studies of the FFA.
lars of this study, including the use of a pas- Moreover, we have now measured the structure
sive viewing of faces as the “ task ” during of the FG in a sample of 110 males with an
fMRI recording (all other studies to date used ASD compared to 103 male controls, with a
an active task to monitor attention and ensure large age range (from age 5 to 55 years) with a
that all participants were actively processing mean of about 17 years for the two groups
the faces), a sample that was much older than combined. We find bilateral enlargement of
those of the other studies, a slice thickness the fusiform, as well as overall brain enlarge-
that varied between participants, and, proba- ment (Schultz et al., in preparation). In the en-
bly most important, a sample of patients who tire group, the fusiform enlargement is not
were, on average, less socially impaired com- significant after accounting for the whole
pared to samples from other studies. Given brain enlargement; however, when the two
that we have reported a significant relation- groups are divided at the median age of 14.9
ship between degree of FFA activation and de- years of age, we find significant enlargement in
gree of social impairment, this seems to be the the older group of ASD versus the controls, even
most likely factor contributing to the reported when controlling for overall brain size, and a
activations (Schultz, 2004; Schultz et al., positive correlation with age only in the ASD
under review). However, Hadjikhani et al. also group. This suggests some aberrant growth pro-
used pictures of faces that subtended 20 de- cess extending into later adolescence and early
grees of visual angle; this is much larger than adulthood. Thus, the functional abnormalities
524 Neurological and Medical Issues

of the FFA may have demonstrable structural specific role in empathy (Leslie, Johnson-
underpinnings and longer-term causal influ- Frey, & Grafton, 2004) and imitation learning
ences on the structure of the brain itself. (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), both of which
would be relevant to the deficits found in
Functional Neuroimaging Studies autism. Indeed, the involvement of the frontal
of Social Cognition and temporal lobe cortices seems very likely,
as a number of resting blood flow studies have
Data emerging over the past several years from shown that these lobes are hypoactive in
neuroimaging studies, human lesion studies, autism (reviewed in Boddaert & Zilbovicius,
and animal studies suggest a working model of 2002). These functional findings are consis-
the social brain composed of a diverse set of tent with the structural findings in children
frontal, limbic, and temporal lobe circuitry. In showing the frontal and temporal lobes to be
addition to its primary role in social percep- the most deviant in terms of abnormal enlarge-
tion, the FFA appears to be involved in select ment (Carper, Moses, Tigue, & Courchesne,
aspects of social cognition. Three studies em- 2002). Moreover, there are a number of case
ploying visual ToM type of tasks have now studies of temporal lobe lesions causing autis-
shown the FFA to be active during social judg- ticlike sequelae (Gillberg, 1986, 1991; Hoon
ments in the absence of any presentation of a & Reiss, 1992) and one report suggesting that
face or facelike object (Castelli, Happe, Frith, tuberous sclerosis preferentially results in
& Frith, 2000; Martin & Weisberg, 2003; autism when the lesions cluster in the tempo-
Schultz, Klin, Grelotti, Herrington, & Volk- ral lobes (Bolton & Griffiths, 1997).
mar, 2003). One interpretation of the FG’s ac- Within the frontal lobes, the orbital and
tivity during social-cognitive tasks is that it medial PFCs are most often implicated in so-
actively codes and stores social semantic cial functions (Bechara, Tranel, Damasio, &
knowledge (Schultz, Grelotti, et al., 2003). In Damasio, 1996; Brothers, 1990; Damasio,
this context, the FFA’s low activity level dur- 1996). The orbital and medial PFCs have
ing face perception in individuals with an dense reciprocal connections with medial tem-
ASD may then in part reflect a paucity of so- poral areas, forming a system for regulating
cial ideation, as well as deficits in face percep- emotional processes (Carmichael & Price,
tion, both of which would have adverse 1995; Price, Carmichael, & Drevets, 1996).
consequences for social interactions (Schultz, Nonhuman primate studies have documented
in press; Schultz, Grelotti, et al., 2003). Con- abnormal social responsivity and loss of social
sistent with the proposed role for the FG in so- position within the group following lesions to
cial cognition, Kriegstein and Giraud (2004) orbital and medial PFCs (Bachevalier &
showed FG activation (at coordinates typical Mishkin, 1986; Butter, McDonald, & Snyder,
of activation during face perception) during a 1969). Involvement of orbital and medial PFCs
familiar voice recognition task. This might in social cognition is also consistent with find-
suggest some involuntary/automatic activation ings of ToM task deficits among neurological
of social semantic content stored in this region patients with bilateral orbital and medial PFC
when forced to identify persons by their voices lesions (Stone, Baron-Cohen, & Knight, 1998).
alone, or it may have been driven by visual im- Functional imaging studies have repeatedly
agery of the person heard speaking. suggested that dorsomedial prefrontal cortex
In addition to these social perceptual pro- (DMPFC) is a critical substrate for “social
cessing nodes, the brain areas most often im- cognition,” that is, for thinking about others’
plicated by functional neuroimaging and lesion thoughts, feelings, and intentions (Adolphs,
studies as important to social functions in- 2003; Castelli et al., 2002; Schultz, Grelotti,
clude the amygdala, aspects of the orbital pre- et al., 2003). Function of this prefrontal region
frontal cortex (PFC), the medial PFC (outside appears to be disturbed in persons with an
the cingulate region), and the temporal poles ASD. A PET study, for example, reported re-
(see Adolphs, 2003; Schultz, Grelotti, et al., duced dopaminergic activity in the DMPFC of
2003, for recent reviews). In addition, aspects autistic subjects (Ernst, Zametkin, Matochik,
of the inferior frontal convexity may have a Pascualvaca, & Cohen, 1997). Reduced glu-
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 525

cose metabolism has also been reported in a The Role of the Amygdala
subdivision of the anterior cingulate gyrus in
persons with autism engaged in a verbal Of the specific brain systems implicated in the
memory task (Haznedar et al., 2000). A pilot pathobiology of autism, none has attracted
15
O-water PET study of Asperger syndrome more attention than the limbic system, espe-
using a ToM task showed specific engagement cially the amygdala and functionally related
of the medial PFC, except that the center of cortical regions (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al.,
activation was displaced below and anterior 1999, 2000; Schultz, in press; Schultz, Roman-
in patients compared with controls (Happé ski, et al., 2000). A number of findings using
et al., 1996). More recently, Castelli et al. different measurement approaches implicate
(2002) showed reduced dorsomedial PFC ac- limbic dysfunction in ASDs, including post-
tivation in ASDs during an adaptation of Hei- mortem examination of the cytoarchitechton-
der and Simmel’s (1944) social attribution ics of limbic tissue of persons who were on
task, involving ToM skills. the autism spectrum, lesion studies in humans
and animals, and functional neuroimaging
Frontal Lobe Findings Concerning studies of healthy controls as well as persons
Cognitive and Motor Systems with an ASD.
The amygdaloid complex is a small,
The dorsal-lateral PFC is critical for working almond-shaped structure located deep in the
memory and executive functioning. One re- medial temporal lobe. Although initially the
cent fMRI study of 11 high-functioning autis- amygdala was conceptualized as a single
tic subjects compared to 6 healthy controls anatomical structure, it is actually composed
showed that subjects with autism had signifi- of more than a dozen nuclei, each with its own
cantly less task-related activation in dorsolat- afferent and efferent connections, neurochem-
eral PFC (Brodmann area 9/46) in comparison ical makeup, and cytoarchitecture (Amaral
with healthy subjects during a spatial working & Price, 1984; Amaral, Price, Pitkanen, &
memory task (Luna et al., 2002). These data Carmichael, 1992). Afferents to the lateral nu-
support the neuropsychological literature that cleus of the amygdala include frontal, cingu-
finds deficits in executive function in the late, insular, and temporal neocortex as well as
ASDs (see Chapter 22, this Handbook, this subcortical regions, such as nuclei of the thala-
volume). mus. The lateral nucleus acts as the sensory in-
Müller and colleagues have studied the terface for the amygdala (Amaral et al., 1992;
motor system in autism with fMRI. Most re- LeDoux, Cicchetti, Xagoraris, & Romanski,
cently, they (Müller, Kleinhans, Kemmosu, 1990). Cortical projections from the amygdala
Pierce, & Courchesne, 2003) reported, in a target virtually all regions of the temporal and
sample of eight male autistic patients and occipital lobes important for visual processing
eight comparison subjects, variable and scat- (Amaral & Price, 1984), as well as multiple re-
tered representation along the lateral convex- gions of the PFC, most notably the orbital PFC
ity of the frontal and parietal lobes during a and the medial wall of the PFC, including the
visually cued motor sequencing task. They anterior cingulate (Carmichael & Price, 1995;
suggest that this representation could be Price et al., 1996). Thus, the amygdala has a
caused by an early-onset disturbance in the reciprocal set of connections with the tempo-
cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways in autism. ral cortex as well as orbital and medial PFCs.
Allen, Müller, and Courchesne (2004) and In this way, the amygdala is centrally posi-
Allen and Courchesne (2003) conducted two tioned and capable of modulating and inter-
fMRI studies targeting cerebellar functions in preting the emotional significance of data
autism. Both employed a sample of eight autis- processed in the perceptual cortices as well
tic participants and eight matched control par- as assisting with the integration of emotion
ticipants during different motor and attention and cognition for decision making and action
tasks. Results suggest abnormal increases in in the frontal cortices (Adolphs, 2003; Alli-
cerebellar activity during the motor tasks but son et al., 2000; Hoffman & Haxby, 2000;
not during the attention tasks. Vuilleumier et al., 2004).
526 Neurological and Medical Issues

The healthy amygdala supports the auto- Schumann et al. (2004) suggested an inter-
matic processing of emotional information, and action between age and amygdala volume in
it can be engaged independently of attention individuals with an ASD, such that at the earli-
(Pasley et al., 2004, but see Pessoa, McKenna, est ages, the amygdala is larger than typical,
Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002) below the level but its growth plateaus prematurely and by
of conscious awareness in an obligatory fashion. adolescence there is not significant size differ-
In this way it may normally activate social ence compared to healthy controls.
schemas (Heberelein & Adolphs, 2004; Schultz, Animal models using nonhuman primates
Grelotti, et al., 2003). The amygdala plays a suggest that abnormalities in the development
critical role in emotional arousal, assigning sig- of the amygdala may play a particularly im-
nificance to environmental stimuli and mediat- portant role in the development of autistic
ing emotional learning (Gaffan, Gaffan, & symptomatology. Bilateral damage of the
Harrison, 1988; LeDoux, 1996). Damage to the amygdala shortly after birth can produce pat-
amygdala causes impairment in recognizing fa- terns of behavior similar to those seen in
cial expression (Adolphs, 1999; Calder et al., autism, such as social isolation, lack of eye
1996), detecting social faux pas (Stone, Baron- contact, impaired facial expression, and
Cohen, Young, Calder, & Keane, 2003), judging motor stereotypes (Bachevalier, 1994). How-
trustworthiness (Adolphs et al., 1998), and at- ever, similar lesions made in adulthood fail to
tributing social intentions (Heberelein & produce these behaviors (Emery et al., 2001).
Adolphs, 2004). It is most interesting that the early postnatal
Postmortem examination of the brains of lesions do not produce autistic characteristics
persons with autism finds consistent evidence immediately; rather, these features emerge
for abnormalities in size, density, and den- with age and experience, suggesting that le-
dritic arborization of neurons in the limbic sion impacts the animal’s ability to learn,
system, including the amygdala, hippocampus, perhaps by altering their behavior in such a
septum, anterior cingulate, and mammillary way that they obtain different experiences.
bodies (Bauman & Kemper, 1994; Kemper & Over the first years of development, faulty
Bauman, 1998). Findings indicate a stunting of early social and emotional learning appears
neuronal processes and increased neuronal to culminate in the presentation of autistic-
packing density, suggesting a curtailment of like symptoms, a developmental course not
normal development. dissimilar from autism. Bachevalier’s mon-
Several studies have now found hypoactiva- keys also showed signs of frontal lobe abnor-
tion of the amygdala in autism during tasks in- malities later in life (Bertolino et al., 1997;
volving the perception of facial expressions Saunders, Kolachana, Bachevalier, & Wein-
and during ToM-type tasks (Baron-Cohen berger, 1998). While an attractive model, one
et al., 1999; Castelli et al., 2002; Critchley recent attempt to replicate these findings
et al., 2000b; Pierce et al., 2001; Wang et al., with three neonatally lesioned macaque mon-
2004). New data suggest that the amygdala’s keys failed to recreate the autistic type of be-
role in these social-cognitive and perceptual haviors found by Bachevalier and colleagues
processes might largely be one of mediating (Prather et al., 2001). At age 8 to 9 months,
physiological arousal (e.g., see Anderson & the monkeys were attentive to social commu-
Sobel, 2003). Thus, hypoactivation of the nications but nevertheless showed a complex
amygdala in autism may reflect nonspecific pattern of changed social behaviors that in-
task effects, such as less interest in or reduced cluded increased fear during dyadic social in-
emotional arousal by task stimuli. This view is teractions (Amaral et al., 2003; Prather et al.,
consistent with social motivation hypotheses 2001). Current speculation about these dis-
of autism (e.g., Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, parate results centers on methodological dif-
& Rinaldi, 1998; Klin, Jones, Schultz, & Volk- ferences (Amaral et al., 2003; Prather et al.,
mar, 2003). MRI studies of amygdala volume, 2001). This area is clearly very important,
however, have produced many conflicting re- and more studies, with larger samples, are
sults (for a review, see Sweeten, Posey, now needed to clarify the effects of early
Shekhar, & McDougle, 2002). Most recently, amygdala damage.
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 527

CONCLUSION cerebral white matter volume are being related


to fMRI data suggesting decreased functional
Functional neuroimaging studies, particularly connectivity. This type of integration across
fMRI studies of social cognition, emotion, and types of data is essential in the near future,
social perception, hold great promise in defin- else the field risks being overwhelmed by iso-
ing the neural systems most aberrant in lated findings and information that cannot eas-
autism. Since the last edition of the Handbook ily be integrated into a functional model of the
of Autism, there has been a dramatic upswing brain bases of the ASDs.
of research activity in this area, and judging
from the large number of studies out this year
on social perception and social cognition, this Cross-References
area of research promises to provide a great
Other aspects of the neurobiology of autism
deal of important data for understanding
are discussed in Chapters 16, 17, and 18. As-
autism in the years to come. Studies using per-
sociated medical conditions and pharmacolog-
sons with an ASD are still relatively uncom-
ical treatments are addressed in Chapters 20
mon, and existing studies continue to grapple
and 44, respectively, and related issues in
with experimental designs and issues particu-
communication and neuropsychology, in Chap-
lar to studying children and adults with, on av-
ters 12, 32, and 22 through 25.
erage, lower cognitive ability and reduced
ability to comply with what can be a somewhat
difficult study environment. REFERENCES
Studies to date are making progress in un-
derstanding the language and social skill Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the
deficits that are defining features of the ASDs. human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3,
469–479.
In particular, there has been substantial prog-
Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of
ress from research studying aspects of face
human social behaviour. Nature Reviews, 4,
perception, which highlights a role for the FG, 165–178.
amygdala, and select aspects of the STS in Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998).
autism. More work needs to be done on social The human amygdala in social judgment. Na-
cognition, including empathy and ToM skills, ture, 393(6684), 470–474.
but preliminary data implicate orbital and me- Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2003). Differential
dial PFCs, as well as the amygdala. There is effects of developmental cerebellar abnormal-
an expectation in the research community that ity on cognitive and motor functions in the
mirror neuron networks in the lateral-inferior cerebellum: An fMRI study of autism. Ameri-
frontal cortex will prove to be highly relevant can Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 262–273.
Allen, G., Müller, R. A., & Courchesne, E. (2004).
to understanding autism, but work here is just
Cerebellar function in autism: Functional
beginning. There have been just a few studies magnetic resonance image activation during a
on language functioning, with mixed results. simple motor task. Biological Psychiatry,
However, abnormalities in hemispheric later- 56(4), 269–278.
ality and in the relative workload of frontal Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). So-
and temporal-parietal language areas appear cial perception from visual cues: Role of the
to be likely. STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7),
FMRI studies will have to be augmented 267–278.
and integrated with other types of approaches, Amaral, D. G., Bauman, M. D., Capitanio, J. P.,
including sMRI, diffusion tensor imaging, and Lavenex, P., Mason, W. A., Mauldin-Jourdain,
M. L., et al. (2003). The amygdala: Is it an es-
electrophysiological approaches. Multimodal
sential component of the neural network for
and systems level investigations, including
social cognition? Neuropsychologia, 41(4),
studies of effective connectivity, are clearly 517–522.
the way of future research, and these ap- Amaral, D. G., & Price, J. L. (1984). Amygdalo-
proaches will undoubtedly yield a much cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca
greater understanding of pathobiology of this fascicularis). Journal of Comparative Neurol-
complex disorder. Already, abnormalities in ogy, 230, 465–496.
528 Neurological and Medical Issues

Amaral, D. G., Price, J. L., Pitkanen, A., & tal mesial temporo-limbic lesions: a proton
Carmichael, S. T. (1992). Anatomical organi- magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging
zation of the primate amygdaloid complex. In study. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 740–748.
J. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiolog- Birn, R., Cox, R., & Bandettini, P. (2002). Detec-
ical aspects of emotion, memory and mental tion versus Estimation in Event-Related fMRI:
dysfunction (pp. 1–66). New York: Wiley-Liss. Choosing the Optimal Stimulus Timing. Neu-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag- roimage, 15, 252–264.
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J. B.,
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M. C.,
Anderson, A. K., & Sobel, N. (2003). Dissociating et al. (2003). Perception of complex sounds:
intensity from valence as sensory inputs to Abnormal pattern of cortical activation in
emotion. Neuron, 39(4), 581–583. autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160,
Aylward, E., Bernier, R., Field, K., Grimme, A., & 2057–2060.
Dawson, G. (2004). Normal activation of Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervais, H., Good,
fusiform gyrus in adolescents and adults with C. D., Bourgeois, M., Plumet, M. H., et al.
autism during viewing of familiar, but not un- (2004). Superior temporal sulcus anatomical
familiar, faces. Paper presented at the CPEA / abnormalities in childhood autism: A voxel-
STAART annual meeting, Bethesda, Maryland. based morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage,
Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe struc- 23(1), 364–369.
tures and autism: A review of clinical and ex- Boddaert, N., & Zilbovicius, M. (2002). Functional
perimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32, neuroimaging and childhood autism. Pediatric
627–648. Radiology, 23, 1–7.
Bachevalier, J., & Mishkin, M. (1986). Visual Bolton, P. F., & Griffiths, P. D. (1997). Association
recognition impairment follows ventromedial of tuberous sclerosis of temporal lobes with
but not dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in autism and atypical autism. Lancet, 349,
monkeys. Behavioural Brain Research, 20(3), 392–395.
249–261. Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., recognition in relatively able autistic children.
Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
S. C. R. (2000). The amygdala theory of 33(5), 843–859.
autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re- Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse,
views, 24, 355–364. L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., with pervasive developmental disorders. Jour-
Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
et al. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal 19, 301–316.
and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for
Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898. integrating primate behavior and neurophysi-
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1994). Neu- ology in a new domain. Concepts Neuroscience,
roanatomic observations of the brain in autism. 1, 27–151.
In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The Butter, C. M., McDonald, J. A., & Snyder, D. R.
neurobiology of autism (pp. 119–145). Balti- (1969). Orality, preference behavior, and rein-
more: Johns Hopkins Press. forcement value of nonfood object in monkeys
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, with orbital frontal lesions. Science, 164,
A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically 1306–1307.
to anticipated future outcomes following dam- Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Perrett,
age to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, D. I., Hodges, J. R., & Etcoff, N. L. (1996). Fa-
215–225. cial emotion recognition after bilateral amyg-
Berthier, M. L., Bayes, A., & Tolosa, E. S. (1993). dala damage: Differentially severe impairment
Magnetic resonance imaging in patients with of fear. Cognitive Neuropsycology, 13,
concurrent Tourette’s disorder and Asperger’s 699–745.
syndrome. Journal of the American Academy Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1992). Un-
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, derstanding of simple and complex emotions
633–639. in nonretarded children with autism. Journal
Bertolino, A., Saunders, R. C., Mattay, V. S., of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33,
Bachevalier, J., Frank, J. A., & Weinberger, 1169–1182.
D. R. (1997). Altered development of pre- Carmichael, S. T., & Price, J. L. (1995). Limbic
frontal neurons in rhesus monkeys with neona- connections of the orbital and medial pre-
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 529

frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. Journal of Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., &
Comparative Neurology, 363, 615–641. Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges between
Carper, R. A., Moses, P., Tigue, Z. D., & Courch- young autistic children and their mothers. Jour-
esne, E. (2002). Cerebral lobes in autism: nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 335–345.
Early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., & Ri-
Neuroimage, 16, 1038–1051. naldi, J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates
Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith U. (2002). of early symptoms of autism. Child Develop-
Autism, Asperger’s syndrome and brain mech- ment, 69(5), 1276–1285.
anisms for the attribution of mental states to de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., & Van der Heide, L.
animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849. (1991). Face recognition and lip reading in
Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. autism. European Journal of Cognitive Psy-
(2000). Movement and mind: A functional chology, 3, 69–86.
imaging study of perception and interpretation Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B. & Tardif, C.
of complex intentional movement patterns. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing
Neuroimage, 12, 314–325. in children with Autism and Asperger syn-
Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Zic- drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental
cardi, R., Carper, R. A., Tigue, Z. D., et al. Disorders, 34(2), 199–210.
(2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are
life in patients with autistic disorder. Neurol- and are not special: An effect of expertise.
ogy, 57, 245–254. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
Critchley, H. D., Daly, E. M., Bullmore, E. T., 115, 107–117.
Williams, S. C., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robert- Egaas, B., Courchesne, E., & Saitoh, O. (1995).
son, D. M., et al. (2000). The functional neu- Reduced-Size of Corpus-Callosum in Autism.
roanatomy of social behaviour: Changes in Archives of Neurology, 52, 794–801.
cerebral blood f low when people with autistic Emery, N. J., Capitanio, J. P., Mason, W. A.,
disorder process facial expressions. Brain, Machado, C. J., Mendoza, S. P., & Amaral,
123, 2203–2212. D. G. (2001). The effects of bilateral lesions
Critchley, H., Daly, E., Phillips, M., Brammer, M., of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions
Bullmore, E., Williams, S., et al. (2000). Ex- in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav-
plicit and implicit neural mechanisms for pro- ioral Neuroscience, 115, 515–544.
cessing of social information from facial Ernst, M., Zametkin, A. J., Matochik, J. A., Pas-
expressions: A functional magnetic resonance cualvaca, D., & Cohen, R. M. (1997). Reduced
imaging study. Human Brain Mapping, 9(2), medial prefrontal dopaminergic activity in
93–105. autistic children. Lancet, 350, 638.
Curby, K. M., Schyns, P. G., Gosselin, F., & Gau- Fein, D., Lucci, D., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse,
thier, I. (2003). Face-selective fusiform activa- L. (1992). Comprehension of affect in context
tion in Asperger’s syndrome: A matter of tuning in children with pervasive developmental dis-
to the right (spatial) frequency. Poster pre- orders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
sented at Cognitive Neuroscience, New York. chiatry, 33, 1157–1167.
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hy- Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J.,
pothesis and the possible functions of the pre- Rolls, E., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysio-
frontal cortex. Proceedings of the Royal logical and modulatory interactions in neu-
Society of London. Series B, Biological Sci- roimaging. Neuroimage, 6, 218–229.
ences, 351, 1413–1420. Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F., & Frack-
Davidson, R., Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., owiak, R. S. (1993). Functional connectivity:
Alexander, A. L., Gernsbacher, A. M., & The principal-component analysis of large
Goldsmith, H. H. (2004, May). Functional and (PET) data sets. Journal of Cerebral Blood
structural substrates of af fective processing in Flow and Metabolism, 13(1), 5–14.
autism: Reframing the origins of fusiform hy- Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.
poactivation. Paper presented at the third an- Oxford, England: Blackwell.
nual meeting of the International Meeting for Gaffan, E. A., Gaffan, D., & Harrison, S. (1988).
Autism Research (IMFAR), Sacramento, CA. Disconnection of the amygdala from visual
Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Tan- association cortex impairs visual-reward asso-
tam, D. (1994). Face perception in children ciation learning in monkeys. Journal of Neuro-
with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal science, 8, 3144–3150.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(6), Gervais, H., Belin, P., Boddaert, N., Leboyer, M.,
1033–1057. Coez, A., Sfaello, I., et al. (2004). Abnormal
530 Neurological and Medical Issues

cortical voice processing in autism. Nature Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Wei, T. C.,
Neuroscience, 7(8), 801–802. Hof, P. R., Cartwright, C., Bienstock, C. A.,
Gillberg, C. (1986). Onset at age 14 of a typical et al. (2000). Limbic circuitry in patients with
autistic syndrome: A case report of a girl with autism spectrum disorders studied with
herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Autism positron emission tomography and magnetic
and Developmental Disorders, 16, 369–375. resonance imaging. American Journal of Psy-
Gillberg, C. (1991). The treatment of epilepsy in chiatry, 157, 1994–2001.
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R. (2004). Impaired
Disorders, 21, 61–67. spontaneous anthropomorphizing despite in-
Grelotti, D., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). tact perception and social knowledge. Pro-
Social interest and the development of cortical ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
face specialization: What autism teaches us of the United States of America, 101(19),
about face processing. Developmental Psy- 7487–7491.
chobiology, 40, 213–225. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental
Grelotti, D., Klin, A. J., Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, study of apparent behavior. American Journal
P., Cohen, D. J., Gore, J. C., et al. (2005). of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259.
fMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus and Herbert, M. R., Harris, G. J., Adrien, K. T.,
amygdala to cartoon characters but not to Ziegler, D. A., Makris, N., Kennedy, D., et al.
faces in a boy with autism. Neuropsychologia, (2002). Abnormal asymmetry in language as-
43(5), 373–385. sociation cortex in autism. Annals of Neurol-
Grill-Spector, K., Knouf, N., & Kanwisher, N. ogy, 52, 588–596.
(2004). The fusiform face area subserves face Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K.,
perception, not generic within-category identi- O’Brien, L. M., Lange, N., Bakardjiev, A.,
fication. Nature Neuroscience, 7(5), 555–562. et al. (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex,
Hadjikhani, N., Chabris, C. F., Joseph, R. M., Clark, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes
J., McGrath, L., Aharon, I., et al. (2004). Early in autistic boys. Brain, 126, 1182–1192.
visual cortex organization in autism: An fMRI Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child’s ap-
study. NeuroReport, 15(2), 267–270. praisal of expressions of emotion: A further
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., Chabris, study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
C. F., Clark, J., Steele, S., et al. (2004). Acti- atry, 2, 671–680.
vation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals Hobson, R. P. (1986b). The autistic child’s ap-
with autism spectrum disorder view faces. praisal of expressions of emotion. Journal
Neuroimage, 22, 1141–1150. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27,
Hall, G. B. C., Szechtman, H., & Nahmias, C. 321–342.
(2003). Enhanced salience and emotion recog- Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related
nition in autism: A PET Study. American Jour- and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evi-
nal of Psychiatry, 160, 1439–1441. dence from the British Picture Vocabulary
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Jo- Scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental
hansson, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (1996). ‘The- Disorders, 19, 601–623.
ory of mind’ in the brain: Evidence from a Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988a).
PET scan Study of Asperger’s syndrome. Neu- Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating
roReport, 8, 197–201. faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18,
Hardan, A. Y., Minshew, N. J., & Keshavan, M. S. 911–923.
(2000). Corpus callosum size in autism. Neu- Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988b).
rology, 55, 1033–1036. What’s in a face? The case of autism. British
Hauck, M., Fein, D., & Maltby, N. (1999). Memory Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.
for faces in children with autism. Child Neu- Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct
ropsychology, 4, 187–198. representations of eye gaze and identity in the
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. distributed human neural system for face per-
(2000). The distributed human neural system ception. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 80–84.
for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sci- Hoon, A. H., & Reiss, A. L. (1992). The mesial-
ence, 4, 223–233. temporal lobe and autism: Case report and
Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Clark, V. P., review. Developmental Medicine and Child
Schouten, J. L., Hoffman, E. A., & Martin, A. Neurology, 34, 252–259.
(1999). The effect of face inversion on activity Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., Grady, C. L., &
in human neural systems for face and object Rapoport, S. I. (1988). The cerebral metabolic
perception. Neuron, 22(1), 189–199. landscape in autism: Intercorrelations of re-
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 531

gional glucose utilization. Archives of Neurol- Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An ap-
ogy, 45, 749–755. proach to the study of autism. Journal of Child
Hubl, D., Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., Lanfer- Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268.
mann, H., Federspiel, A., Strik, W., et al. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New
(2003). Functional imbalance of visual path- York: Simon & Shuster.
ways indicates alternative face processing LeDoux, J. E., Cicchetti, P., Xagoraris, A., & Ro-
strategies in autism. Neurology, 61(9), manski, L. M. (1990). The lateral amygdaloid
1232–1237. nucleus: Sensory interface of the amygdala in
Jaedicke, S., Storoschuk, S., & Lord, C. (1994). fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 10,
Subjective experience and causes of affect in 1062–1069.
high-functioning children and adolescents Leslie, K. R., Johnson-Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T.
with autism. Development and Psychopathol- (2004). Functional imaging of face and hand
ogy, 6, 273–284. imitation: Towards a motor theory of empathy.
Jezzard, P., Matthews, P. M., & Smith, S. M. (2001). Neuroimage, 21(2), 601–607.
Functional MRI: An introduction to methods. Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., Garver, B. A., Lazar,
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. N. A., Thulborn, K. R., Eddy, W. F., et al.
Joseph, R. M., & Tanaka, J. R. (2002). Holistic and (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in
part-based face recognition in children with autism: An fMRI study of spatial working
autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840.
chiatry, 44, 529–542. Macdonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., Le
Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Conteur, A., Evered, C., et al. (1989). Recog-
Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and nition and expression of emotional cues by
synchronization during sentences comprehen- autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child
sion in high-functioning autism: Evidence of Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 865–877.
underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821. Manes, F., Piven, J., Vrancic, D., Nanclares, V.,
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. Plebst, C., & Starkstein, S. E. (1999). An MRI
(1997). The fusiform face area: A module in study of the corpus callosum and cerebellum
human extrastriate cortex specialized for the in mentally retarded autistic individuals. Jour-
perception of faces. Journal of Neuroscience, nal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro-
17, 4302–4311. sciences, 11, 470–474.
Kanwisher, N., & Wojciulik, E. (2000). Visual at- Martin, A., & Weisberg, J. (2003). Neural foundations
tention: Insights from brain imaging. Nature for understanding social and mechanical con-
Reviews Neuroscience, 1(2), 91–100. cepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 575–587.
Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. L. (1998). Neu- McGee, G. G., Feldman, R. S., & Chernin, L.
ropathology of infantile autism. Journal of (1991). A comparison of emotional facial dis-
Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, play by children with autism and typical
57, 645–652. preschoolers. Journal of Early Intervention,
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. R. 15, 237–245.
(2003). The enactive mind: From actions to Müller, R. A., Behen, M. E., Rothermel, R. D.,
cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Chugani, D. C., Muzik, O., Mangner, T. J.,
Transactions of the Royal Society, Series, B., et al. (1999). Brain mapping of language and
358, 345–360. auditory perception in high-functioning autis-
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., Volkmar, F., & tic adults: A PET study. Journal of Autism and
Cohen, D. J. (2002). Visual fixation patterns Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 19–31.
during viewing of naturalistic social situations Müller, R. A., Kleinhans, N., Kemmotsu, N.,
as predictors of social competence in individu- Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2003). Abnor-
als with autism. Archives of General Psychia- mal variability and distribution of functional
try, 59, 809–816. maps in autism: An FMRI study of visuomotor
Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, learning. American Journal of Psychiatry,
D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A 160(10), 1847–1862.
normed study of face recognition in autism Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T.
and related disorders. Journal of Autism and (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unper-
Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508. ceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neu-
Kriegstein, K. V., & Giraud, A. L. (2004). Distinct ron, 42, 163–172.
functional substrates along the right superior Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Unger-
temporal sulcus for the processing of voices. leider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of
Neuroimage, 22(2), 948–955. emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings
532 Neurological and Medical Issues

of the National Academy of Science, 99, Schultz, R. T. (2004). Functional brain imaging in
11458–11463. autism. Paper presented at the annual CPEA /
Pierce, K., Haist, F., Sedaghat, F., & Courchesne, STAART meeting, Bethesda, Maryland.
E. (2004). The brain response to personally Schultz, R. T. (in press). Developmental deficits in
familiar faces in autism: Findings of fusiform social perception in autism: The role of the
activity and beyond. Brain, 127, 2703–2716. amygdala and fusiform face area. Interna-
Pierce, K., Müller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & tional Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.
Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin A., Fulbright, R.,
outside the fusiform “ face area” in autism: Anderson, A., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Ab-
Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124, normal ventral temporal cortical activity
2059–2073. during face discrimination among individuals
Piggot, J., Kwon, H., Mobbs, D., Blasey, C., Lot- with autism and Asperger’s syndrome.
speich, L., Menon, V., et al. (2004). Emotional Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–340.
attribution in high-functioning individuals Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Hunyadi, E., Herring-
with autistic spectrum disorder: A functional ton, J. B., Klin, A., Klaiman, C., et al. (under
imaging study. Journal of the American Acad- review). Toward an understanding of fusiform
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(4), face area activation in autism spectrum condi-
473–480. tions.
Piven, J., Saliba, K., Bailey, J., & Arndt, S. (1997). Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Kleinman,
An MRI study of autism: The cerebellum re- J., Van der Gaag, C., Marois, R., et al. (2003).
visited. Neurology, 49, 546–551. The role of the fusiform face area in social
Prather, M. D., Lavenex, P., Mauldin-Jourdain, cognition: Implications for the pathobiology of
M. L., Mason, W. A., Capitanio, J. P., Men- autism. Philosophical Transactions of the
doza, S. P., et al. (2001). Increased social fear Royal Society, Series, B., 358, 415–427.
and decreased fear of objects in monkeys with Schultz, R. T., Klin, A., Grelotti, D. J., Herrington,
neonatal amygdala lesions. Neuroscience, J., & Volkmar, F. (2003, October). fMRI stud-
106(4), 653–658. ies of the fusiform face area in autism spectrum
Price, J. L., Carmichael, S. T., & Drevets, W. C. disorders. Paper presented at the annual meet-
(1996). Networks related to the orbital and ing of the Society for Psychophysical Re-
medial prefrontal cortex; a substrate for emo- search, Chicago, IL.
tional behavior? Progress in Brain Research, Schultz, R. T., Romanski, L., & Tsatsanis, K.
107, 523–536. (2000). Neurofunctional models of autistic
Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. disorder and Asperger’s syndrome: Clues from
(1995). Face-sensitive regions in human ex- neuroimaging. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, &
trastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger’s syndrome
Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 1192–1199. (pp. 179–209). New York: Plenum Press.
Ringo, J. L. (1991). Neuronal interconnection as a Schultz, R. T., Win, L., Jackowski, A., Klin, A.,
function of brain size. Brain, Behavior, and Staib, L., Papademetris, X., et al. (in prepara-
Evolution, 38, 1–6. tion). Brain morphology in autism spectrum
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror- disorders: An MRI study.
neuron system. Annual Review of Neuro- Schumann, C. M., Hamstra, J., Goodlin-Jones,
science, 27, 169–192. B. L., Lotspeich, L. J., Kwon, H., Buonocore,
Robins, D. L., Hunyadi, E. T., & Schultz, R. T. M. H., et al. (2004). The amygdala is enlarged
(2004, May). Unique brain activations to inte- in children, but not adolescents, with autism;
gration of audio-visual emotional cues. Paper the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages. Jour-
presented at the 3rd annual meeting of the In- nal of Neuroscience, 24, 6392–6401.
ternational Meeting for Autism Research Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., & Shapiro, T. (1987).
(IMFAR), Sacramento, CA. Expression of emotion in young autistic chil-
Rutter, M. (2000). Genetic studies of autism: From dren. Journal of the American Academy of
the 1970s into the millennium. Journal of Ab- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836–838.
normal Child Psychology, 28, 3–14. Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T.
Saunders, R. C., Kolachana, B. S., Bachevalier, J., (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of
& Weinberger, D. R. (1998). Neonatal lesions mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
of the medial temporal lobe disrupt prefrontal 10(5), 640–656.
cortical regulation of striatal dopamine. Na- Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Young, A. W.,
ture, 393, 169–171. Calder, A. J., & Keane, J. (2003). Acquired
Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorders 533

theory of mind impairments in patients with (2004). A voxel-based investigation of brain


bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, structure in male adolescents with autistic
41, 209–220. spectrum disorder. Neuroimage, 22(2),
Sweeten, T. L., Posey, D. J., Shekhar, A., & Mc- 619–625.
Dougle, C. J. (2002). The amygdala and re- Wang, T. A., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A. R., Sigman,
lated structures in the pathophysiology of M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2004). Neural corre-
autism. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Be- lates of facial affect processing in children
havior, 71, 449–455. and adolescents with autism spectrum disor-
Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stir- der. Journal of the American Academy of Child
ling, J. (1989). Autistic children’s ability to and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(4), 481–490.
interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Weeks, S., & Hobson, R. (1987). The salience of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, facial expression for autistic children. Journal
623–630. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28,
Turkeltaub, P. E., Flowers, L., Verbalis, A., Mi- 137–151.
randa, M., Gareau, L., & Eden, G. F. (2004). Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., & Mundy,
The neural basis of hyperlexic reading: An P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-
fMRI Case Study. Neuron, 41, 11–25. functioning children with autism. Child Devel-
Veenstra-VanderWeele, J., Christian, S. L., & opment, 63, 150–160.
Cook, E. H. (2004). Autism as a paradigmatic Yonan, A. L., Palmer, A. A., Smith, K. C., Feld-
complex genetic disorder. Annual Review of man, I., Lee, H. K., Yonan, J., et al. (2003).
Genomics and Human Genetics, 5, 379–405. Bioinformatic analysis of autism positional
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., candidate genes using biological databases and
Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Distant inf lu- computational gene network prediction.
ences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical ac- Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 2, 303–320.
tivation during emotional face processing. Yovel, G., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Face percep-
Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1271–1278. tion: Domain specific, not process specific.
Waiter, G. D., Williams, J. H., Murray, A. D., Neuron, 44(5), 889–898.
Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., & Whiten, A.
CHAPTER 20

Medical Aspects of Autism

PAULINE A. FILIPEK

It is now unequivocal that neurobiological dys- likely to be causal factors of autism, including
function is causative in autism. Although a fragile X syndrome (FXS), bilateral deafness,
number of disorders have been potentially as- cerebral palsy, multiple congenital abnormali-
sociated with autism, the extent and nature of ties, and chromosomal anomalies. About 3.8%
these associations have traditionally been the had other medical concerns that were consid-
subject of much debate (Rutter, 1996). Wing ered less likely to be etiologic factors. The
and Gould (1979) found relatively lower rates overall rate of medical conditions, 11.9%, is
of known medical problems in their autistic similar to the rate found in a study of medical
sample relative to nonautistic subjects (17% conditions in twins with autism (12.9%; A.
versus 71%, respectively). Further, disorders Bailey et al., 1991). Although some found that
such as phenylketonuria (PKU) and tuberous IQ is not related to medical risk (Steffenburg,
sclerosis were found only in the nonautistic 1991), others found more medical conditions
group. Tuchman, Rapin, and Shinnar (1991) among autistic persons at lower IQ levels. For
compared groups of children with autism to example, in an epidemiologic study of autism,
those with developmental language disorder Ritvo et al. (1990) demonstrated that medical
and found similar rates of medical conditions, conditions were more frequent in persons with
about 5%, across groups. Similarly, Fombonne severe mental retardation, which is consistent
and du Mazaubrun (1992) noted that autistic with other reports (Rutter, Bailey, Bolton, &
children and those with special educational Le Couteur, 1994; Wing & Gould, 1979). The
needs did not differ in the frequency of most possibility of finding any associated medical
medical conditions, including congenital condition rises with increasing degrees of
rubella or chromosomal abnormalities. Of mental retardation—approaching 50% among
note, the autistic group was significantly less persons at the severe and profound levels of
likely to have Down syndrome or cerebral cognitive dysfunction (Scott, 1994). More re-
palsy, and all cases of neurofibromatosis and cent studies corroborate the findings of these
PKU were found in the nonautistic group. earlier studies (Barton & Volkmar, 1998;
In a series of studies, Bolton et al. (1991) Challman, Barbaresi, Katusic, & Weaver, 2003;
and Rutter, Bailey, Bolton, and Le Couteur Fombonne, du Mazaubrun, Cans, & Grandjean,
(1993) conducted extensive evaluations on 151 1997; C. Gillberg & Billstedt, 2000; Kielinen,
individuals with autism and found that 8.1% of Rantala, Timonen, Linna, & Moilanen, 2004;
these cases showed medical conditions that were Lauritsen, Mors, Mortensen, & Ewald, 2002;

This work was funded by grants from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD (RO1 HD 26554,
PO1 HD 35458, and RO1 NS 35896) and from the Children and Families Commission of Orange County,
CA. The author wishes to specifically thank Joseph H. Donnelly, MD; Jenifer Juranek, PhD; J. Jay Gargus,
MD, PhD; Teri M. Book, MSN, CPNP; and Laurie Ann Lennon, MS-CCC, for their insightful reviews and
comments. This chapter is dedicated to the memory of Doris A. Allen, EdD.

534
Medical Aspects of Autism 535

Shevell, Majnemer, Rosenbaum, & Abraham- disorders and syndromes are now recognized
owicz, 2001; Skjeldal, Sponheim, Ganes, Jel- to be behaviorally on the autistic spectrum. As
lum, & Bakke, 1998). specific genotypes are identified within the
In summary, traditional studies have found spectrum of autism and related conditions, it
variable rates of medical conditions in autism, is likely that many, if not most, cases will be
ranging from 5% to 33%. The first edition of related to a specific medical (genetic) disorder
this text (1987) reviewed associated medical or syndrome.
conditions in the chapter titled, “Neurologic The original chapter title, “Medical Condi-
Functioning” (pp. 133–147). The accompany- tions Associated with Autism,” must now be
ing table (p. 138) listed almost 40 disorders revised to include both vantage points. It has
that had been reported in only “one or more” therefore been updated to “Medical Aspects of
(p. 137) cases of autism, but the text detailed, Autism” and is addressed as two, now comple-
very briefly, less than a dozen of these, includ- mentary, topics: (1) medical signs and symp-
ing then-newly described Rett syndrome (Hag- toms in children presenting with autism and
berg, Aicardi, Dias, & Ramos, 1983). In (2) comorbid autism in children presenting
contrast, the second edition (1997) included a with specific disorders or syndromes.
chapter dedicated to associated medical condi- This seemingly unconventional approach
tions (pp. 388–407), which selectively fo- incorporates presumably rarer disorders whose
cused on only four syndromes, two commonly prevalence may be marginal within an autistic
co-occurring with autism (FXS and tuberous population; for example, many children pre-
sclerosis complex [TSC]) and two with seem- senting with TSC are autistic, but few children
ingly uncommon associations with autism presenting with autism have comorbid TSC.
(Down and Williams syndromes). Particularly in medical settings where experi-
ence with ASDs is less common, there has
AUTISM AND MEDICAL CONDITIONS: been a tendency for medical specialists to
VIEWING THE RELATIONSHIP FROM focus less on behavior and more on specific
BOTH SIDES OF A SEMANTIC COIN signs and symptoms. As a result, many chil-
dren with the classic hallmarks of autism in
It would appear, at first glance, that this corre- addition to their other medical diagnoses may
sponding chapter in this third edition could be not be correctly diagnosed and thereby served.
titled either “Medical Conditions Associated For example, Howlin, Wing, and Gould (1995)
with Autism” or “Autism Associated with eloquently championed the importance of rec-
Medical Conditions.” Medical conditions can ognizing autism specifically in children with
refer either to broader classes of medical signs Down syndrome. Although autism diagnoses
and symptoms or to specific disorders and are typically made in the preschool years, they
syndromes. Additionally, the term medical noted much later ages of autistic diagnoses in
condition does not specify the presumed popu- Down syndrome cases, as well as in all cases
lation under study, for example, a cohort with of Down reported in the literature (range from
a specific medical disorder or a cohort with 7 years to adulthood). This “diagnostic over-
autism. The premise traditionally used for sig- shadowing” of sorts creates unnecessary stress
nificant “associated medical conditions” has for families and prevents them from using sup-
been of specific disorders or syndromes occur- ports and interventions available to families
ring within populations of autistic individuals. with an autistic child. Even though most of the
Since the first edition was published in following disorders and syndromes are uncom-
1987, a wealth of information about autism mon in samples of individuals with autism,
has emerged, much of it initially anecdotal they should always be considered in the range
(e.g., case reports) but increasingly empirical. of diagnostic possibilities.
Concurrently, the concept of autism is evolv- This chapter begins with a brief outline of
ing from the singular autistic disorder into the the appropriate medical evaluation for individ-
pleural autistic spectrum disorders (ASDs). As uals with an ASD, then discusses the medical
a result of this ongoing ontogeny, in tandem symptoms commonly seen in autism and the
with rapid genetic progress, more and more specific disorders presenting with an autistic
536 Neurological and Medical Issues

behavioral phenotype. A resource list for par- weight by adulthood (Bauman & Kemper,
ents and professionals is provided at the end of 1997). This phenomenon of large head size in
the chapter. autistic children is readily acknowledged, and
barring lateralizing signs on the remainder of
THE MEDICAL EVALUATION the examination, routine neuroimaging work-
IN AUTISM* up for the finding of a large head alone in
autism is not warranted. Several reports also
The medical evaluation in autism consists of a show a higher prevalence of microcephaly in
careful physical and neurologic examination autism, which is associated with abnormal
with selected laboratory testing. physical morphology, medical disorders, lower
IQ, and seizures (e.g., Fombonne et al., 1999;
Physical and Neurologic Examination Miles et al., 2000).
Sensorimotor function is commonly prob-
The head circumference in autistic children is lematic in autistic individuals and most severe
larger than is found in typically developing in those with lower cognitive function (Noter-
children (Aylward, Minshew, Field, Sparks, & daeme, Mildenberger, Minow, & Amorosa,
Singh, 2002; A. Bailey et al., 1995; Bolton et al., 2002; Rapin, 1996b; S. J. Rogers, Bennetto,
1994; Courchesne, Carper, & Akshoomoff, McEvoy, & Pennington, 1996). Sensory issues
2003; Davidovitch, Patterson, & Gartside, 1996; are very common, particularly sensory seeking,
Fidler, Bailey, & Smalley, 2000; Fombonne, oral sensitivity, and low endurance (Baranek,
Roge, Claverie, Courty, & Fremolle, 1999; 1999; Baranek, Foster, & Berkson, 1997;
Ghaziuddin, Zaccagnini, Tsai, & Elardo, 1999; Bernabei, Fenton, Fabrizi, Camaioni, & Peruc-
C. Gillberg & de Souza, 2002; Lainhart et al., chini, 2003; Watling, Deitz, & White, 2001).
1997; Miles, Hadden, Takahashi, & Hillman, Sensory-processing abilities are aberrant in
2000; Woodhouse et al., 1996). The same has 42% to 88% of autistic individuals and include
been noted with postmortem brain weights (A. preoccupation with sensory features of ob-
Bailey et al., 1993, 1998; Bauman, 1992, 1996; jects, over- or under-responsiveness to envi-
Bauman & Kemper, 1994, 1997). Only a small ronmental stimuli, or paradoxical responses to
proportion of autistic children have frank sensory stimuli (Kientz & Dunn, 1997). Hy-
macrocephaly/megalencephaly, but the distri- potonia was noted in about 25% of 176 autistic
bution of the measures is clearly shifted up- children and in 33% of 110 nonautistic men-
ward with the large majority falling above the tally retarded children, while spasticity was
50 percentile with the mean approximately at found in less than 5% of either group (exclu-
the 75 percentile (A. Bailey et al., 1995; sionary criteria for this sample included the
Bolton et al., 1994; Courchesne et al., 2003; presence of lateralizing gross motor findings;
Davidovitch et al., 1996; Filipek, Richelme, Rapin, 1996b). Motor apraxia was noted in al-
et al., 1992; Lainhart, 2003; Lainhart et al., most 30% of autistic children with normal
1997; Rapin, 1996b; Woodhouse et al., 1996). cognitive function, in 75% of retarded autistic
Some investigators have noted that the large children, and in 56% of a nonautistic retarded
head circumference correlates with higher IQ control group (Mari, Castiello, Marks, Mar-
(Filipek, Richelme, et al., 1992; Miles et al., raffa, & Prior, 2003; Rapin, 1996b; J. H.
2000). The large head circumference is not Williams, Whiten, & Singh, 2004). The pres-
necessarily present at birth but may appear in ence of observed motor stereotypies was noted
early to mid-childhood with increased rates of in over 40% of autistic children (in contrast to
growth (Lainhart et al., 1997; Mason-Brothers a much higher prevalence by parental report)
et al., 1987; Mason-Brothers et al., 1990). It and in over 60% of those with low IQ, but in
also appears that the head circumference is only 13% of the nonautistic control group
normal by adolescence and adulthood (Ayl- (Rapin, 1996b). Hand or finger mannerisms,
ward et al., 2002), as is postmortem brain body rocking, or unusual posturing is reported

* Portions of this section are taken with permission from Filipek et al. (1999, 2000).
Medical Aspects of Autism 537

in 37% to 95% of individuals and often mani- lence of pica in this group can result in high
fests during the preschool years (Lord, 1995; rates of substantial and often recurrent expo-
Rapin, 1996b; S. J. Rogers et al., 1996). sure to lead and, possibly, other metals (Shan-
In a large longitudinal study of autistic non & Graef, 1997). Several studies report the
children, over 6% also had a sibling with neurobehavioral effects and behavioral toxic-
autism (Rapin, 1996a). The overall recurrence ity of lead and its potential clinical relevance
risk estimate for ideopathic autism—the per- in patients with autism. Mean blood lead con-
cent chance that a younger sibling will also de- centration was notably higher in 18 children
velop the disorder—varies from about 3% to with autism than in 16 nonautistic children or
7% (A. Bailey, Phillips, & Rutter, 1996; in 10 normal siblings; 44% of the autistic and
Bolton et al., 1994; Piven et al., 1990; Ritvo, psychotic children had blood lead levels
Jorde, et al., 1989; Smalley, Asarnow, & greater than two standard deviations above the
Spence, 1989). However, there are gender dif- mean for normal controls (Cohen, Johnson, &
ferences to this risk estimate: If the first autis- Caparulo, 1976). In three of six reported cases
tic child is male, the recurrence risk estimate of lead poisoning in children with autism, de-
ranges from about 4% to 7%, but if female, the velopmental deviance seemed to have been
recurrence risk estimate ranges from 7% to present before the possible impact of lead toxi-
14.5% (Jorde et al., 1991; Ritvo, Jorde, et al., city, while in two, the lead poisoning may have
1989). The risk of having a second autistic contributed to the onset or acceleration of de-
child, therefore, is approximately 50-fold higher velopmental symptomatology (Accardo, Whit-
than in the general population. These risk esti- man, Caul, & Rolfe, 1988). A more recent
mates are based on the older prevalence rates chart review found that 17 children with
of approximately 4 per 10,000 and, therefore, autism were treated for plumbism over a 6-
cannot reflect the fact that many families year period from 1987 to 1992. When com-
choose not to have more natural children sub- pared with a randomly selected group of 30
sequent to receiving a diagnosis of autism. Re- children without autism who were treated dur-
gardless, it is the physician’s responsibility to ing the same interval, the children with autism
inform parents of this recurrence risk when a were significantly older at diagnosis and had a
child is diagnosed with autism. longer period of elevated blood lead levels dur-
ing treatment; 75% were subsequently reex-
Definitive Hearing Test posed despite close monitoring, environmental
inspection, and either lead hazard reduction or
Every child presenting with a receptive lan- alternative housing (Shannon & Graef, 1997).
guage deficit should receive a definitive hear- Therefore, all children with delays or who are
ing test. Audiologic assessment should occur at risk for autism should have a periodic lead
early in the differential diagnostic process and screen until the pica disappears (Centers for
use a battery of tests including behavioral au- Disease Control and Prevention, 1997; Shan-
diometric measures, assessment of middle ear non & Graef, 1997).
function, and electrophysiologic procedures
(American Speech-Language-Hearing Associ- Karyotype and DNA Analysis for
ation, 1991). If audiology cannot be performed Fragile X
adequately, brainstem-evoked responses should
be performed (Filipek, Accardo, et al., 2000; The newer cytogenetic methods of karyotyping
Filipek et al., 1999). and molecular analyses for FXS and the impli-
cations of a cytogenetic or molecular diagnosis
Lead Level for other family members justify their routine
inclusion in the diagnostic evaluation of a child
Children with developmental delays who with autism (American College of Medical Ge-
spend an extended period in the oral-motor netics: Policy Statement, 1994; A. Bailey, 1994;
stage of play (where everything goes into their Bauer, 1995; Dykens & Volkmar, 1997; Rutter,
mouths) are at increased risk for lead toxicity, Bailey, Simonoff, & Pickles, 1997; Rutter et al.,
especially in certain environments. The preva- 1994; Schaefer & Bodensteiner, 1992).
538 Neurological and Medical Issues

Metabolic Testing Shinnar, 1991). However, some neurologists


are routinely performing sleep EEGs on autis-
A wide range of biochemical determinations tic children at diagnosis and are finding subtle
have been performed in urine, blood, and cere- abnormalities in many, often localized to the
brospinal fluid in an attempt to identify a spe- temporal lobes. It is unclear whether the chil-
cific metabolic abnormality in individuals with dren with the abnormalities are those who
autism. Included are studies of inborn errors in would eventually develop clinical seizures,
amino acid, carbohydrate, purine, peptide, and and these findings along with the potential
mitochondrial metabolism, as well as toxico- benefits of valproate therapy need to be sys-
logical studies. The reported co-occurrence of tematically evaluated.
autistic-like symptoms in individuals with in-
born errors of metabolism has led to consider- Neuroimaging Studies
ation of screening tests as part of the routine
assessment of patients with severe develop- A review of the many neuroimaging reports in
mental impairment (Steffenburg, 1991). How- autism noted a very low prevalence of focal le-
ever, the percentage of children with autism sions or other abnormalities, none of which lo-
who prove to have an identifiable metabolic dis- calized consistently to be more than
order is probably less than 5% (Dykens & Volk- coincidental findings (Filipek, Kennedy, &
mar, 1997; Rutter et al., 1994, 1997). Most of Caviness, 1992). In a subsequent study using
the biochemical analyses are useful at present magnetic resonance imaging (MRI ), the preva-
only as research tools in the ongoing effort to lence of lesions in autistic children was equal
understand the biology of autism. to that in the normal control volunteers (Fil-
Metabolic testing or consultation clearly is ipek, Richelme, et al., 1992). However, corti-
indicated by a history of lethargy, cyclic vom- cal migration malformations have been
iting, early seizures, dysmorphic or coarse reported on MRI in a small number of high-
features, mental retardation, or, if mental re- functioning autistic or Asperger subjects, in-
tardation cannot be excluded, questionable new- cluding polymicrogyria, schizencephaly, and
born screening or birth out of the United States macrogyria, without collective preference for
because of the potential absence of newborn a particular lobe or hemisphere (Berthier,
screening and maternal public health measures. 1994; Berthier, Starkstein, & Leiguarda,
As recommended by the American College of 1990; Piven et al., 1990). It is unclear whether
Medical Genetics, selective metabolic testing these findings of cortical dysplasias are more
should be initiated only in the presence of sug- prevalent in autism than is currently recog-
gestive clinical and physical findings (Curry nized, as another study of 63 developmentally
et al., 1997). However, as described later in the disabled children did not note dysplasias (Fil-
section titled Mitochondrial Disorders, recent ipek, Richelme, et al., 1992). Regardless, un-
findings may ultimately lead to future recom- less lateralized findings are present on
mendations for screenings of lactate, pyruvate, neurological examination, conventional clini-
ammonia, and free and total carnitine (Fil- cal computed tomography (CT) or MRI scans
ipek, Juranek, Nguyen, Cummings, & Gargus, are not indicated in the routine diagnostic eval-
in press). uation of autism or any of the developmental
disorders. Positron emission tomography
Electroencephalography (PET) and single photon emission computer-
ized tomography (SPECT) are presently used
The association among electroencephalogram only as research tools and are not indicated in
(EEG) abnormalities, seizures, and develop- the diagnostic evaluation of autism.
mental regression is described in Chapter 18
(this Handbook, this volume). An adequate Tests of Unproven Value
EEG should be performed with prolonged sleep
to Stages III and IV in any child who presents There is inadequate evidence to support rou-
with suspicion of developmental regression tine clinical testing of individuals with autism
(Dykens & Volkmar, 1997; Tuchman, 1995; for hair analysis for trace elements (Gentile,
Tuchman & Rapin, 1997; Tuchman, Rapin, & Trentalange, Zamichek, & Coleman, 1983;
Medical Aspects of Autism 539

Shearer, Larson, Neuschwander, & Gedney, mild obstetrical complications independent of


1982; Wecker, Miller, Cochran, Dugger, & maternal age or parity, which makes a causal
Johnson, 1985), celiac antibodies (Pavone, Fi- relationship unlikely. Specifically, no associa-
umara, Bottaro, Mazzone, & Coleman, 1997), tions were found between autism and maternal
allergies (in particular, food allergies for factors, such as vaginal bleeding, infection,
gluten, casein, candida and other molds; Lu- diabetes, toxemia, use of pitocin, age, or
carelli et al., 1995), immunological or neuro- prior abortions (Bolton, Murphy, Macdonald,
chemical abnormalities (Cook, Perry, Dawson, Whitlock, Pickles, et al., 1997; Cryan, Byrne,
Wainwright, & Leventhal, 1993; Singh, War- O’Donovan, & O’Callaghan, 1996; Fein et al.,
ren, Averett, & Ghaziuddin, 1997; Yuwiler 1997; Gale, Ozonoff, & Lainhart, 2003; Ghaz-
et al., 1992), micronutrients such as vitamin iuddin, Shakal, & Tsai, 1995; Piven et al.,
levels (Findling et al., 1997; LaPerchia, 1987; 1993; Rapin, 1996a). There were also no asso-
Tolbert, Haigler, Waits, & Dennis, 1993), in- ciations noted between autism and gestational
testinal permeability studies (D’Eufemia age, forceps or caesarian delivery, neonatal
et al., 1996), stool analysis, urinary peptides depression, need for intensive care or mechan-
(Le Couteur, Trygstad, Evered, Gillberg, & ical ventilation, neonatal seizures, or pro-
Rutter, 1988), thyroid function (Cohen, Young, longed neonatal hospitalization (Bolton,
Lowe, & Harcherik, 1980; T. Hashimoto et al., Murphy, Macdonald, & Whitlock, 1997; Piven
1991), or erythrocyte glutathione peroxidase et al., 1993; Rapin, 1996a).
(Michelson, 1998). More recent studies simply add more con-
flicting data to the debate. Juul-Dam, Town-
MEDICAL SIGNS AND SYMPTOMS send, and Courchesne (2001) noted that their
ASSOCIATED WITH AUTISM autism cohort had a higher incidence of uter-
ine bleeding, a lower incidence of maternal
The more common signs and symptoms associ- vaginal infection, and less maternal use of
ated with autism include perinatal factors, contraceptives when compared with the gen-
hearing loss, food and gastrointestinal prob- eral population; the pervasive developmental
lems, immunologic abnormalities and sleep disorder–not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS)
disorders. cohort showed a higher incidence of hyper-
bilirubinemia. The authors concluded that in-
Perinatal Factors terpretation of these data “is difficult, as the
specific complications with the highest risk of
Early studies indicated that autism may be as- autism represented various forms of pathologic
sociated with increased but mild obstetrical processes with no presently apparent unifying
risk factors (Bryson, Smith, & Eastwood, feature” (p. E63). Wilkerson, Volpe, Dean,
1988; Deykin & MacMahon, 1980; Finegan & and Titus (2002) noted that different perinatal
Quarrington, 1979; Folstein & Rutter, 1977a, factors and maternal medical conditions con-
1977b; C. Gillberg & Gillberg, 1983; Levy, tributed to the risk of autism: prescriptions
Zoltak, & Saelens, 1988; Lord, Mulloy, Wen- during pregnancy, length of labor, viral infec-
delboe, & Schopler, 1991; Mason-Brothers tion, abnormal presentation at delivery, low
et al., 1987, 1990; Nelson, 1991; Piven et al., birthweight, maternal urinary infection, high
1993; Tsai, 1987). However, the strong influ- temperatures, and depression. In a population
ence of maternal parity/reproductive stoppage study in Sweden, Hultman, Sparen, and Cnat-
accounted for differences in at least two of tingius (2002) reported yet additional factors
these studies (Lord et al., 1991; Piven et al., as being associated with the risk of autism:
1993) and was not necessarily appropriately daily cigarette smoking, maternal birth out-
considered in the others. Subsequently, Zam- side Europe or North America, caesarean de-
brino, Balottin, Bettaglio, Gerardo, and Lanzi livery, being small for gestational age, Apgar
(1995) found that the obstetrical optimality scores below 7, and congenital malformations.
score was lower only in those autistic children In contrast, Klug, Burd, Kerbeshian, Benz, and
with central nervous system (CNS) damage; Martsolf (2003) examined parental, prenatal,
and Bolton, Murphy, Macdonald, Whitlock, and perinatal risk factors and found that the
Pickles, et al. (1997) noted an increase of only cohort with autism was quantitatively less
540 Neurological and Medical Issues

influenced by 15 specific risk markers than mended by the practice parameters for screen-
were fetal alcohol and sudden infant death syn- ing and diagnosing autism (Filipek, Accardo,
dromes; only low-magnitude risk markers et al., 2000; Filipek et al., 1999), audiological
( low birthweight, child malformations, and evaluations or evoked potentials should be per-
low level of maternal education) were mildly formed on all children with autism so that, if
but significantly elevated for autism, produc- indicated, appropriate referrals can be made
ing odds ratios of less than 2.4. Again, differ- for aural habilitation.
ing diagnostic methods for autism and
differing risk factor assessments can account Feeding Disturbances and
for at least some of the discrepancies noted. Gastrointestinal Problems
Only one report has examined the incidence
of autism in survivors of neonatal intensive Feeding habits and food preferences of chil-
care units (NICU; Matsuishi et al., 1999). In dren with autism are traditionally unconven-
this study, 90% of almost 6,000 NICU sur- tional and were even at one time considered as
vivors born between 1983 and 1987 were fol- part of the diagnostic indicators (Ahearn, Cas-
lowed neurodevelopmentally at 2 to 3 and 5 tine, Nault, & Green, 2001; Ritvo & Freeman,
years of age. Eighteen were diagnosed with 1978). However, this specific aspect of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental constellation of atypical behaviors has not re-
Disorders, third edition, revised (DSM-III-R; ceived much formal study. Ahearn et al. (2001)
American Psychiatric Association, 1987) studied 30 children with autism using the pro-
autistic disorder. The only risk factor identi- cedures developed by Munk and Repp (1994)
fied of the 28 factors examined was meconium for classifying feeding problems in the devel-
aspiration syndrome. The mean incidence for opmentally disabled. Just over half of the sam-
autism in the NICU survivors was 34 per ple lived at home with their parents, the
10,000, which is more than twice that found in remainder lived in community group homes for
two previous studies in the same geographic the disabled, and all attended the same private
region in Japan (Matsuishi et al., 1987; Ohtaki educational and treatment program. More than
et al., 1992). However, note that these refer- half showed low levels of food acceptance,
enced epidemiological studies were performed with 13% refusing all foods presented to them.
more than 15 years ago, during the same time However, the authors acknowledged caution in
frame as those performed by Ritvo, Freeman, interpreting these results, as there were no
et al. (1989) and do not reflect current preva- comparison groups of either typically develop-
lence rates. ing children or those with other developmental
disabilities. Field, Garland, and Williams
Hearing Loss (2003) also noted food selectivity by type
(62%) and by texture (31%); all three children
Many children diagnosed with autism are first with food refusal (12%) also had gastroe-
described by parents as acting “as if deaf.” sophageal reflux. Food selectivity “ by type”
However, the vast majority of children with was significantly higher for children with
autism are found to have normal hearing func- autism, and food refusal and oral motor prob-
tion. Rosenhall, Nordin, Sandstrom, Ahlsen, lems were significantly lower than found in
and Gillberg (1999) performed audiological children with other developmental disorders.
evaluations on 199 children and adolescents Bowers (2002) performed an audit of referrals
with autism and found that pronounced to pro- of autistic children to a dietetic service over a
found bilateral hearing loss or deafness was 3-month period and found that, despite selec-
present in 3.5% of all cases, a prevalence tive food preferences in 46%, the majority of
greater than that seen in the general popula- children had intakes that met or exceeded di-
tion but similar to that seen in individuals with etary reference values.
mental retardation. However, the rate of hear- Although there have been reports of gas-
ing loss in this study was equivalent across all trointestinal (GI ) complaints in children with
levels of cognitive functioning. In contrast, hy- autism dating back more than 30 years (e.g.,
peracusis was commonly found and affected Goodwin, Cowen, & Goodwin, 1971; Walker-
almost 20% of the autistic sample. As recom- Smith & Andrews, 1972), such problems have
Medical Aspects of Autism 541

become a significant focus of study in recent symptoms in autistic children is not as com-
years. Lightdale, Siegel, and Heyman (2001) mon as the GI literature might suggest. Taylor
surveyed 500 parents of autistic children; al- et al. (2002) noted an 8% rate of chronic con-
most 50% reported loose stools or frequent di- stipation, which is similar to that estimated for
arrhea. In an epidemiologic study, Fombonne the general childhood population (Loening-
and Chakrabarti (2001) found that 19% of Baucke, 1998). Black, Kaye, and Jick (2002)
children with autism reported GI symptoms, performed a nested case-control study in the
with constipation identified in 9%. Molloy and United Kingdom and found that both 9% of
Manning-Courtney (2003) found that, of 137 children with autism and 9% of children with-
children with autism diagnosed with the out autism had a history of GI complaints, pro-
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- ducing an odds ratio of 1.0 (no effect) for
Generic (ADOS-G; Lord et al., 2000), 24% autism with GI complaints. Peltola et al.
had a history of at least one chronic GI symp- (1998) also noted no association of ASD and
tom and 17% had diarrhea; they found no asso- GI symptoms over a 14-year period in Finland.
ciation between GI symptoms and autistic DeFelice et al. (2003) found no relationship
regression. between autism and GI immune dysregulation
Some reports from gastroenterologists have by investigating intestinal cytokines; in fact,
stated that GI symptoms occur in 46% to 84% intestinal levels of interleukin (IL)-6 and IL-8
of children with autism (Horvath, Papadim- were lower in patients with ASD than in age-
itriou, Rabsztyn, Drachenberg, & Tildon, matched controls. Whiteley (2004, p. 9) also
1999; Horvath & Perman, 2002a, 2002b). noted that “only a minority” of participants
However, in these studies, most of the autistic with autism in their study showed some bowel
samples had been referred to the gastroen- problems.
terologists for preexisting GI complaints, thus A. J. Wakefield et al. (1998) first reported
limiting the generalizability of the data. Afzal an apparent link among GI disease, develop-
et al. (2003) noted moderate or severe consti- mental regression, and the measles-mumps-
pation to be more frequent in the autistic group rubella (MMR) vaccine in 10 autistic children.
referred for GI symptoms than in control sub- These authors published over a dozen addi-
jects with abdominal pain (36% versus 10%); tional studies apparently supporting their ini-
over 50% had moderate to severe recto-sigmoid tial report (Ashwood et al., 2003; Furlano
loading than did controls (24%). Milk con- et al., 2001; Kawashima et al., 2000; O’Leary,
sumption was the strongest predictor of consti- Uhlmann, & Wakefield, 2000; Torrente et al.,
pation in the autistic group; stool frequency, 2002; A. J. Wakefield, 1999, 2002, 2003; A. J.
gluten consumption, soiling, and abdominal Wakefield & Montgomery, 1999; A. J. Wake-
pain were not predictive of constipation. field et al., 2000, 2002; J. Wakefield, 2002).
Sandler et al. (2000) hypothesized that, in While the ensuing international controversy
children with “regressive”-onset autism who and the direct effect of these studies on the
had antecedent antibiotic exposure followed by drop in the rates of immunization of children
diarrhea, endogenous intestinal microflora is beyond the scope of this chapter, it should be
might be disrupted by neurotoxin-producing noted that the majority of these authors have
bacteria. Eleven children received a trial of recently retracted their initial interpretation
vancomycin, leading to only short-term im- that there is a causal connection between the
provement of autistic symptomatology in 8 of onset of autistic symptoms and the MMR vac-
the 10 children. There was no control group in cine (Murch et al., 2004).
this study, and the raters were not blinded as It was of great interest when a case series
to the hypotheses being tested. Finegold et al. claimed that three autistic patients with GI
(2002) went on to investigate intestinal mi- complaints had dramatic improvement in the
croflora and found a higher prevalence of core symptoms of autism after receiving the
clostridia in the stools of children with autism hormone secretin as part of a diagnostic en-
than in control children, all of whom were pre- doscopy (Horvath et al., 1998; Larsen, 1998).
sumably referred for GI procedures. Subjective improvement was noticed, particu-
Kuddo and Nelson (2003) reviewed the lit- larly in areas of eye contact, alertness, and
erature and noted that the frequency of GI language capacities. Subsequently, 15 empiric
542 Neurological and Medical Issues

studies were performed and found no positive quiring that parents hold them, lie down with
effects of either porcine or human recombi- them, or sit beside their bed; all family mem-
nant secretin on autistic symptomatology bers go to bed at the same time; or curtains or
(Carey et al., 2002; Chez et al., 2000; Coniglio bedclothes be positioned in a certain way. If
et al., 2001; Corbett et al., 2001; Dunn-Geier these routines are not performed exactly, the
et al., 2000; Kern, Van Miller, Evans, & result is usually a tantrum or other angry out-
Trivedi, 2002; Levy et al., 2003; Lightdale, burst. As might be expected, only the autistic
Hayer, et al., 2001; Molloy et al., 2002; Owley children studied always followed their bedtime
et al., 2001; Roberts et al., 2001; Robinson, routine (Patzold et al., 1998), and the presence
2001; Sponheim, Oftedal, & Helverschou, 2002; of sleep problems was significantly associated
Unis et al., 2002); in fact, in one study, autistic with parental stress (Richdale, Francis, Gavidia-
symptoms worsened (Robinson, 2001). Payne, & Cotton, 2000). Schreck, Mulick, and
Smith (2004) suggested that both the quantity
Immune Factors and quality of sleep per night predicted overall
autism scores, as measured by the Gilliam
Interest in the potential relationship between Autism Rating Scale (GARS; Gilliam, 1995),
the immune system and autism arises given the social skills deficits, and stereotypic behav-
various cases reported in which infections iors. According to Wiggs and Stores (1996), it
(and possibly altered immune response) are as- is uncertain whether sleep disorders in chil-
sociated with the development of autism (Mar- dren with autism cause daytime maladaptive
chetti, Scifo, Batticane, & Scapagnini, 1990; behaviors, simply allow them to continue, or
Menage et al., 1992; Singh, Warren, Odell, worsen preexisting problems.
Warren, & Cole, 1993; Warren, Margaretten, Hering, Epstein, Elroy, Iancu, and Zelnik
Pace, & Foster, 1986). However, the few stud- (1999) compared the results of parental ques-
ies conducted have yielded inconsistent or con- tionnaires with electronic movement activity
tradictory findings and are discussed in recent recordings (actigraphy) in three groups of chil-
review articles (Hornig & Lipkin, 2001; Kor- dren: Group 1: autistic children whose parents
vatska, Van de Water, Anders, & Gershwin, reported sleep difficulties, Group 2: autistic
2002; Krause, He, Gershwin, & Shoenfeld, children whose parents did not report sleep
2002). difficulties, and Group 3: typically developing
children. The initial questionnaires showed
Sleep Disturbances that 50% of children in Group 1 had sleep dis-
orders versus only 20% in Group 2 and none in
Sleep disturbances have been a recognized Group 3. When sleep was quantified using
feature of autism for over 25 years, particu- actigraphy, there were no differences in pat-
larly abnormalities in sleep-wake cycles terns of sleep between Groups 1 and 2 except
(Hoshino, Watanabe, Yashima, Kaneko, & Ku- for more early morning awakenings in Group
mashiro, 1984; Inanuma, 1984; Ornitz, 1985; 1. These findings support the need for objec-
Tanguay, Ornitz, Forsythe, & Ritvo, 1976; see tive study methodologies in these samples.
also Didde & Sigafoos, 2001; Richdale, 1999; Diomedi et al. (1999) compared polysomno-
and Stores & Wiggs, 1998, for reviews). Stud- graph parameters in adult autistic individuals,
ies find that the majority of children with who demonstrated a significant reduction of
autism have sleep problems, often severe, and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, increased
usually involving extreme sleep latencies, interspersed wakefulness, and increased num-
lengthy nighttime awakenings, shortened night ber of awakenings with reduction of sleep effi-
sleep, and early morning awakenings (Hon- ciency relative to normal controls (see Harvey
omichl, Goodlin-Jones, Burnham, Gaylor, & & Kennedy, 2002, for a comprehensive review
Anders, 2002; Patzold, Richdale, & Tonge, of polysomnography in autism and other devel-
1998; Richdale & Prior, 1995; Schreck & opmental disabilities). Elia et al. (2000) noted
Mulick, 2000; Wiggs & Stores, 2004). Chil- that the total time in bed and total sleep time
dren with autism also have more unusual and were significantly lower in autistic individu-
obligatory bedtime routines, for example, re- als, who also had a higher density of muscle
Medical Aspects of Autism 543

twitches, which correlated with some psycho- cated in the subependymal regions and in the
logical indices of autism from the Psychoedu- cortex (Braffman & Naidich, 1994; Harrison
cational Profile-Revised (Schopler, Reichler, & Bolton, 1997; Truhan & Filipek, 1993).
Bashford, Lansing, & Marcus, 1990) and the These tumors are variably expressed, resulting
Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; in a phenotype that ranges from only mild skin
Schopler, Reichler, & Rochen-Renner, 1988). manifestations to severe mental retardation
Thirumalai, Shubin, and Robinson (2002) and intractable epilepsy (Curatolo et al., 2002;
identified REM sleep behavior disorder in al- Kandt, 2003; Short, Richardson, Haines, &
most half of 11 autistic children studied. Usu- Kwiatkowski, 1995). Between 50% and 60%
ally seen in elderly males, the diagnostic of affected individuals are mentally retarded
criteria include movements of the body or and 80% have seizures; those with mental re-
limbs associated with dreaming (REM), po- tardation invariably have seizures (Gomez,
tentially harmful sleep behavior, dreams that Sampson, & Whittemore, 1999).
appear to be acted out, and sleep behavior that TSC is an autosomal dominant disorder
disrupts sleep continuity (American Sleep caused by mutations in either of two genes:
Disorders Association, 1997, pp. 177–180). TSC1 on chromosome 9q34 producing hamartin
Additional studies on this topic are needed be- and TCS2 on chromosome 16p13.3 producing
cause pharmacologic treatment may amelio- tuberin (Curatolo et al., 2002; OMIM™, 2000).
rate some of the effects of this specific sleep It has been puzzling that two separate geno-
disorder. types could be associated with apparently
identical phenotypes. It is now known that
AUTISM ASSOCIATED WITH SPECIFIC hamartin and tuberin must bind together into a
DISORDERS OR SYNDROMES protein complex to regulate mTOR (mammalian
target of rapamycin) in a critical signaling
Autism is now associated with many more pathway to control cell size and proliferation
specific disorders or syndromes than previ- (Lewis, Thomas, Murphy, & Sampson, 2004;
ously known, many more than the tradition- McManus & Alessi, 2002; for a review, see
ally recognized tuberous sclerosis and fragile Kwiatkowski, 2003).
X syndrome (Table 20.1). The phenotypes of TSC1 and TSC2 have
been considered to be identical in character.
Tuberous Sclerosis Complex However, recent studies indicate that there
may be differences in severity between the
TSC is a neurocutaneous disorder that affects two genotypes. TSC1 accounts for 85% to 90%
as many as 1 in 6,000 to 10,000 individuals of familial cases, while TSC2 is responsible
and is characterized by benign tumors ( hamar- for 70% of sporadic cases. It also appears that
tomas) and nongrowing lesions ( hamartias) in individuals with TSC2 may more likely be
the brain and in many other organs such as the mentally retarded than those with TSC1 (P. J.
skin, kidneys, eyes, heart, and lungs (Curatolo, de Vries & Bolton, 2000; A. C. Jones et al.,
Verdecchia, & Bombardieri, 2002; Kandt, 1997, 1999; van Slegtenhorst et al., 1999;
2003). Depigmented macules (shaped like an Zhang et al., 1999) and, therefore, more likely
ash leaf; Fitzpatrick, 1991) are usually the to have severe seizures. Lewis et al. (2004)
first sign of the disease, which are often visu- noted that the presence of a TSC2 mutation
alized only with the use of an ultraviolet Wood carried a significantly higher risk of low IQ,
light. Facial angiofibroma, formerly called autistic disorder, and infantile spasms than a
adenoma sebaceum, and shagreen patches over TSC1 mutation. The odds ratio of low IQ in
the lower back are also characteristic but often TSC2 has been reported as 2.44 (P. J. de Vries
do not appear until late childhood or early ado- & Bolton, 2000) and 3.5 (Lewis et al., 2004),
lescence (Webb, Clarke, Fryer, & Osborne, the latter adjusted for a history of infantile
1996). The major intracerebral lesions are the spasms. It would make sense, therefore, that
tubers, which consist of histogenic malforma- individuals with TSC1 with a potentially
tions of both neuronal and glial elements with milder phenotype would be more likely to re-
giant heterotopic cells, characteristically lo- produce and contribute to familial lines of
544 Neurological and Medical Issues

Table 20.1 Other Syndromic Associations


47, XYY Abrams & Pergament, 1971
Nielsen, Christensen, Friedrich, Zeuthen, & Ostergaard, 1973
Nicolson, Bhalerao, & Sloman, 1998
Weidmer-Mikhail et al., 1998
CHARGE association (coloboma, heart defect, Fernell et al., 1999
choanal atresia, retarded growth and
development, genital hypoplasia, ear anomalies
Chromosome 2q37 deletion syndrome Ghaziuddin & Burmeister, 1999
Smith et al., 2001
Chromosome 13 deletion syndrome Steele, Al-Adeimi, Siu, & Fan, 2001
Smith et al., 2002
Chromosome 22q13 terminal deletion syndrome Prasad et al., 2000
Cohen syndrome Chandler, Moffett, Clayton-Smith, & Baker, 2003
Cornelia de Lang syndrome Berney, Ireland, & Burn, 1999
Pediatric epilepsy syndromes Besag, 2004
Infantile spasms/ West syndrome Askalan et al., 2003
Aristaless-related homeobox gene (ARX) Stromme, Mangelsdorf, Scheffer, & Gecz, 2002
syndrome Sherr, 2003
Fetal alcohol syndrome Aronson, Hagberg, & Gillberg, 1997
FG syndrome (mental retardation, large head, Ozonoff, Williams, Rauch, & Opitz, 2000
imperforate anus, congenital hypotonia, and
partial agenesis of corpus callosum)
Goldenhar syndrome Landgren, Gillberg, & Stromland, 1992
Histidinemia Kotsopoulos & Kutty, 1979
Hypomelanosis of Ito Akefeldt & Gillberg, 1991
Zappella, 1993
Pascual-Castroviejo et al., 1998
Infections Congenital Chess, 1971
Stubbs, 1978
Yamashita, Fujimoto, Nakajima, Isagai, & Matsuishi, 2003
Acquired Ghaziuddin, Al-Khouri, & Ghaziuddin, 2002
Ghaziuddin, Tsai, Eilers, & Ghaziuddin, 1992
C. Gillberg, 1991
C. Gillberg, 1986
Domachowske et al., 1996
Joubert syndrome Ozonoff, Williams, Gale, & Miller, 1999
Raynes, Shanske, Goldberg, Burde, & Rapin, 1999
Kleinfelter’s syndrome Kielinen et al., 2004
Lesch-Nyhan syndrome Nyhan, James, Teberg, Sweetman, & Nelson, 1969
Neurofibromatosis Type 1 C. Gillberg & Forsell, 1984
P. G. Williams & Hersh, 1998
Fombonne et al., 1997
Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome Tierney et al., 2001
Goldenberg, Chevy, Bernard, Wolf, & Cormier-Daire, 2003
Thalidomide embryopathy Stromland, Nordin, Miller, Akerstrom, & Gillberg, 1994
Stromland, Philipson, & Andersson Gronlund, 2002
Turner syndrome Skuse et al., 1997
El Abd, Patton, Turk, Hoey, & Howlin, 1999
Donnelly et al., 2000
Medical Aspects of Autism 545

TSC (A. C. Jones et al., 1997; Lewis et al., tion pattern, no FXS protein (FMRP) is pro-
2004). However, the jury is still out with re- duced and people are fully affected with FXS
spect to differential representation of autism (Maddalena et al., 2001). Prevalence of the
in the TSC1 and TSC2 genotypes. full syndrome is 2.4 per 10,000 in males and
Autistic symptoms were first described in 1.6 per 10,000 in females; prevalence of the
patients with TSC a decade before Kanner’s premutation carrier status is 12.3 per 10,000
classic delineation of infantile autism (Critch- in males and 38.6 per 10,000 in females (Dom-
ley & Earl, 1932). These early noted symptoms browski et al., 2002).
included stereotypies, absent or abnormal FXS is associated with numerous distinc-
speech, withdrawal, and impaired social inter- tive neuropsychological deficits, which are not
actions. TSC has since been strongly associ- analogous with the overall cognitive impair-
ated with autism, and estimates range from ment (Bennetto & Pennington, 2002; Loesch,
17% to over 65% of individuals with TSC who Huggins, & Hagerman, 2004). Loesch et al.
are also autistic, identified more frequently in (2004) reviewed the correlations between
those with mental retardation, most commonly FMRP depletion and deficits of cognitive and
with epilepsy (Curatolo et al., 1991; I. Gill- executive function on both genders with FXS
berg, Gillberg, & Ahlsen, 1994; Gutierrez, using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-
Smalley, & Tanguay, 1998; Harrison & III (WISC-III, Wechsler, 1997). The Digit
Bolton, 1997; Hunt & Shepherd, 1993; Kandt, Span and Symbol Search subtests in both sexes
2003; Riikonen & Simell, 1990). The reverse, and Picture Arrangement subtest in females
the number of autistic individuals with TSC, showed that subjects were particularly af-
has been estimated between 0.4% and 4% in fected by FMRP depletion, suggesting deficits
epidemiological studies (C. Gillberg, Steffen- in the cognitive constructs of processing speed,
burg, & Schaumann, 1991; Lotter, 1967; Ritvo short-term memory, and attention. Although
et al., 1990; Smalley, Tanguay, Smith, & females are usually less affected than males
Gutierrez, 1992). This rate increases to 8% to because of the presence of the second unaf-
14% in autistic subjects with epilepsy (C. Gill- fected X chromosome (Loesch et al., 2004),
berg, 1991; Riikonen & Amnell, 1981). 50% to 70%, nonetheless, demonstrated sig-
nificant cognitive deficits (B. B. de Vries
Fragile X et al., 1996), which correlated with the ratio of
cells in which the normal X chromosome is ac-
As the most common inherited cause of mental tivated (B. B. de Vries et al., 1996; Riddle
retardation, FXS is second only to Down syn- et al., 1998; Tassone et al., 2000) and FMRP
drome in terms of a known chromosomal cause levels (Mazzocco, Kates, Baumgardner, Fre-
of mental retardation. FXS is characterized by und, & Reiss, 1997).
macroorchidism, large protruding ears, and Premutation carriers, both male and fe-
moderate to severe mental retardation (for re- male, have traditionally been thought to be en-
cent reviews, see Hagerman, 2002; Kooy, tirely unaffected. It is surprising, however,
Willemsen, & Oostra, 2000; and Willemsen, that a premutation phenotype has recently
Oostra, Bassell, & Dictenberg, 2004). In over been recognized. Hagerman et al. (2001) and
99% of the cases, this syndrome is caused by Jacquemont et al. (2003, 2004) reported a syn-
an unstable amplification (excessive triplet drome of progressive intention tremor, cere-
repetition) of cytosine (C) and guanine (G) bellar ataxia, executive function deficits, and
within the FMR1 gene on chromosome Xq27.3 brain atrophy (FXTAS) in asymptomatic older
(Verkerk et al., 1991). The range of ∼5 to ∼44 males with the FXS premutation status. Addi-
CGG triplet repeats is considered to be a nor- tional documented signs include short-term
mal finding, ∼45 to 54 repeats is considered memory loss, cognitive decline, parkinsonism,
the intermediate “gray zone,” and ∼55 to 200 peripheral neuropathy, proximal muscle weak-
repeats is considered the premutation state ness, and autonomic dysfunction. The late onset
producing carriers who may or may not be af- of this syndrome is due to an age-related pene-
fected. Above a threshold of approximately trance, giving such carriers an age-adjusted
200 to 230 repeats with an abnormal methyla- 13-fold increased risk of combined intention
546 Neurological and Medical Issues

tremor and gait ataxia (Jacquemont et al., population had FXS, and Einfeld, Molony, and
2004). Most recently, Hagerman et al. (2004) Hall (1989) found comparable rates of autism
reported five female premutation carriers with in appropriately matched groups of FXS and
symptoms of FXTAS, but none demonstrated non-fragile XFXS males. Summarizing data
the dementia noted in the males with FXTAS. across 40 studies, Fisch (1992) found virtually
In addition, females with the premutation had identical pooled proportions of FXS in autistic
an increased (∼20%) incidence of premature males and in mentally retarded males in gen-
ovarian failure and early menopause, which eral. These studies suggested that autism and
has not been reported in females with the full FXS indeed may co-occur, but the prevalence
FXS mutation (Murray, 2000; Sherman, of these cases is much lower than originally
2000). thought, and that FXS is not a major etiologic
The early descriptions of FXS focused on factor in autism.
fully affected males and their many autistic The debate continues, with data that sup-
features. These features included poor eye port both sides of the argument. O. Hashimoto,
contact; language delay, perseveration, and Shimizu, and Kawasaki (1993) noted no asso-
echolalia; self-injurious behaviors; motor ciation between FXS and autism in their co-
stereotypies (e.g., hand flapping and body hort. Klauck et al. (1997) found that 139 of
rocking); hypersensitivity to auditory stimuli 141 patients with autism were negative for the
or environmental change; tactile defensive- full syndrome. Only one multiplex family ac-
ness; preoccupations with a narrow range of counted for the positive FXS testing in their
stimuli; and poor social relating (August & cohort: the mother with a premutation, one fe-
Lockhart, 1988; Borghgraef, Fryns, Dielkens, male autistic child who was heterozygous, and
Pyck, & Van den Berghe, 1987; Fryns, Jacobs, two male children with full mutations—one
Kleczkowska, & Van den Berghe, 1984; C. autistic with mental retardation and one with
Gillberg, Persson, & Wahlstrom, 1986; Hager- normal cognition and mild learning disabili-
man, Jackson, Levitas, Rimland, & Braden, ties. Maes, Fryns, Van Walleghem, and Van
1986; Meryash, Szymanski, & Gerald, 1982). den Berghe (1993) described males with FXS
In the previous edition of this text (1997), over as having stereotypic movements, disturbing
30 studies were noted describing the preva- language patterns, social avoidance reactions,
lence of autism in FXS and vice versa (Dykens and eccentric peculiarities, but also showing
& Volkmar, 1997). The prevalence rates for social openness and sensitivity; however, there
FXS in autistic samples ranged from 0 to 16%, was no difference in the general levels of autis-
with a median of about 4%, while the preva- tic behaviors between the mentally retarded
lence rates of autism in fragile XFXS samples males with and without FXS. Turk and Gra-
varied considerably, from 5% to 60%. Some ham (1997) reported that their FXS cohort
researchers asserted that their prevalence also did not demonstrate more autism than the
rates of FXS and autism—16% to 20%—far comparison group with idiopathic mental re-
exceeded the 4.5% to 7% of severely and tardation; however, both groups demonstrated
mildly retarded males with FXS, concluding more autism diagnoses (∼70% to 80%) than
that FXS was strongly associated with autism did a second comparison group with Down
(Blomquist et al., 1985; Fisch, Cohen, Jenkins, syndrome (∼30%). They concluded that FXS
& Brown, 1988; C. Gillberg & Wahlstrom, demonstrates a characteristic autistic-like
1985). In contrast, others claimed that their phenotype of communication and stereotypic
∼3% to 5% prevalence rates of autism and disturbances with delayed echolalia, repetitive
fragile XFXS were no higher than the rate of speech, and hand flapping.
FXS among mentally retarded males (Payton, D. B. Bailey, Hatton, Mesibov, Ament, and
Steele, Wenger, & Minshew, 1989; Watson Skinner (2000), using the CARS (Schopler
et al., 1984); therefore, the argument was that et al., 1988), reported that FXS boys without
FXS should not increase the risk of autism autism showed a relatively flat developmental
above and beyond the risk associated with profile in contrast to the varied, uneven pro-
mental retardation. Subsequent work sup- files and more severe cognitive difficulties
ported this latter position. A. Bailey et al. seen in FXS with autism; FXS without autism
(1993) found that only 1.6% of their autistic also demonstrated more social competence
Medical Aspects of Autism 547

and temperaments that were similar to typi- so that variability in mode of diagnosis should
cally developing children. They subsequently no longer be a confounding factor in this debate.
noted that the expression of the FXS protein
(FMRP) accounted for less variance in devel- Down Syndrome
opmental level in FXS than did the comorbid-
ity of autism, suggesting that autism in FXS Down syndrome is the most common chromo-
may come from a second hit predisposing to somal cause of mental retardation, originally
autism (D. B. Bailey, Hatton, Skinner, & Mesi- occurring in approximately 1 in 800 live births
bov, 2001; also postulated by Feinstein & (Hook, 1982); more recently, with available
Reiss, 1998). options for prenatal diagnosis and elective ter-
S. J. Rogers, Wehner, and Hagerman (2001) mination, it has decreased to approximately 1
used the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised in 1,000 live births (Bell, Rankin, & Donald-
(ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) son, 2003; Iliyasu, Gilmour, & Stone, 2002;
and the ADOS-G (Lord et al., 2000) to evalu- Olsen, Cross, & Gensburg, 2003). Although
ate FXS in comparison to non-FXS autistic once considered implausible, the comorbidity
and other developmentally disabled children of autism and Down syndrome is not rare
and reported two FXS subgroups: One-third of (Bregman & Volkmar, 1988; Ghaziuddin,
the FXS group met the stringent criteria for 1997, 2000; Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Ghaziuddin,
autism and were very similar to the non-FXS 1992; Howlin et al., 1995; Wakabayashi, 1979;
autistic cohort, while the remaining two-thirds Wing & Gould, 1979). In fact, Down’s origi-
of the FXS group were not autistic and were nal phenotypic description (Down, 1887/1990,
very similar to the group with other develop- pp. 6–7) of Mongolism certainly gives cre-
mental disabilities. Philofsky, Hepburn, Hayes, dence to the concept that comorbidity of Down
Hagerman, and Rogers (2004), also using the syndrome and autism has always existed:
ADOS and ADI for autism diagnosis, reported
that the FXS/autism cohort was more impaired These children have always great power of imita-
in nonverbal cognition and receptive and ex- tion and become extremely good mimics . . . I have
pressive language relative to the FXS children; known a ventriloquist to be convulsed with laugh-
receptive language function was similarly poor ter between the first and second parts of his enter-
in children with autism, regardless of FXS sta- tainment on seeing a Mongolian patient mount the
platform, and hearing him grotesquely imitate the
tus. Kau et al. (2004), again using the ADI,
performance with which the audience had been en-
noted that the FXS/autism cohort was more tertained. They have a strong sense of the ridicu-
cognitively impaired and demonstrated more lous; this is indicated by their humorous remarks
aberrant behaviors but, notwithstanding, was and the laughter with which they hail accidental
less impaired in the reciprocal social domain falls, even of those to whom they are most attached.
than the autistic cohort without FXS. The au- Another feature is their great obstinacy—they can
thors proposed a Social Behavior Profile only be guided by consummate tact. No amount of
(SBP) as a distinct subphenotype of FXS, coercion will induce them to do that which they
which may share mechanisms with autism. have made up their minds not to do. Sometimes
The extent of association of autism and they initiate a struggle for mastery, and the day
previous will determine what they will or will not
FXS is still unknown. As stated by Mazzocco
do on the next day. Often they will talk to them-
et al. (1998):
selves, and they may be heard rehearsing the dis-
. . . despite the specificity of autistic behavior putes which they think will be the feature of the
among fragile X males and females, these behav- following day. They in fact, go through a play in
iors (a) are not seen in all children with the disor- which the patient, doctor, governess, and nurses
der, ( b) range in severity across individuals with are the dramatis personae—a play in which the pa-
the disorder, and (c) may be seen among individu- tient is represented as defying and contravening the
als with fragile X regardless of whether they meet wishes of those in authority.
DSM criteria for autistic disorder. (p. 326)
In epidemiological studies, the prevalence
This issue may be resolved with current of Down syndrome in individuals with autism
studies that are using the gold standard autism ranges from none to 16.7% (see Fombonne,
diagnostic instruments (ADOS-G and ADI-R) 2003, for review). Large-scale studies that
548 Neurological and Medical Issues

screened samples with Down syndrome for anxiety (Gath & Gumley, 1986; Myers &
autism found relatively low rates of autism, Pueschel, 1991; Pueschel, Bernier, & Pezzullo,
ranging from 1.0% to 2.2%. However, other se- 1991). Further, adults with Down syndrome
ries have reported that as many as 10% of sub- are particularly vulnerable to Alzheimer-type
jects with Down syndrome also meet criteria dementia (Bush & Beail, 2004).
for autism (Ghaziuddin, 1997; Ghaziuddin
et al., 1992; C. Gillberg et al., 1986; Lund, Williams-Beuren Syndrome
1988; Wing & Gould, 1979).
Howlin et al. (1995) eloquently championed Williams-Beuren syndrome (WBS) is a rare
the importance of recognizing autism in chil- disorder first described over 40 years ago
dren with Down syndrome. Although autism (Beuren, Apitz, & Harmjanz, 1962; J. C.
diagnoses are typically made in the preschool Williams, Barratt-Boyes, & Lowe, 1961) and
years, they noted later ages of autistic diag- caused by a microdeletion on chromosome
noses in all cases reported in the literature 7q11.23 that includes the gene for elastin
(range from 7 years to adulthood). This singu- (OMIM™, 2000). Persons with WBS often
lar diagnostic view creates unnecessary stress show a distinctive cognitive profile, hyperacu-
for families and prevents them from using sup- sis, supravalvular aortic stenosis, hypercal-
ports and interventions available to families cemia, and characteristic facial features
with an autistic child. described as “elfin-like” (Bellugi, Lichten-
Reasons for the lack of recognition of autis- berger, Mills, Galaburda, & Korenberg, 1999;
tic signs in Down syndrome are unclear. The Osborn, Harnsberger, Smoker, & Boyer, 1990;
stereotyped personality of individuals with Pober & Dykens, 1996). Although WBS is
Down syndrome is outgoing, affectionate, thought to affect about 1 in 20,000 people
easygoing, placid, cheerful, highly social, and (Pober & Dykens, 1996), the most recent epi-
verbal—like “Prince Charming” (Gibbs & demiological study in Finland noted a preva-
Thorpe, 1983; Menolascino, 1965). Some moth- lence of 1 in 7,500 individuals (Stromme,
ers describe their children with Down syn- Bjornstad, & Ramstad, 2002). The association
drome with a wide range of personality features between WBS and autism has not yet been
(C. Rogers, 1987; Wishart & Johnston, 1990). widely studied, and there are only a few cases
While some children are easygoing, others are of comorbidity formally reported in the litera-
more active and distractible, with difficult ture (C. Gillberg & Rasmussen, 1994; Reiss,
temperaments (see Ganiban, Wagner, & Cic- Feinstein, Rosenbaum, Borengasser-Caruso,
chetti, 1990, for a review). Yet, children with & Goldsmith, 1985). Individuals with WBS
comorbid Down syndrome and autism are very were almost twice as likely to be diagnosed
different from other children with Down syn- with a psychiatric disorder characterized by
drome, demonstrating classic deficits in socia- anxiety, preoccupations, wandering, being
bility, immediate and delayed echolalia, poor overaffectionate, and seeking attention, and
developmental progress in communication having difficulty with interpersonal interac-
skills, motor stereotypies and ritualistic be- tions, with sleep disorders, and hyperacusis,
haviors or interests, and adaptive behaviors. than were controls who were matched for de-
Even though autism may not be common in gree of cognitive deficit (Einfeld, Tonge, &
Down syndrome, it should be considered in the Florio, 1997).
range of diagnostic possibilities for all individ- WBS and autism have traditionally been
uals with this syndrome. thought to show opposing patterns of cognitive
Rates of other psychiatric disorders are low strength and weakness. By definition, individ-
for persons with Down syndrome, even as uals with autism have poor verbal and nonver-
compared to groups with other types of devel- bal communication skills (see Chapter 12, this
opmental delay (Collacott, Cooper, & Mc- Handbook, this volume). In contrast, despite
Grother, 1992; Grizenko, Cvejic, Vida, & significant early language delay, many individ-
Sayegh, 1991; Myers & Pueschel, 1991). Some uals with WBS have been described as show-
children, however, may be prone to attentional ing relative sparing of expressive language and
difficulties, overactivity, oppositionality, and linguistic functioning, including high-level
Medical Aspects of Autism 549

syntax and semantics (Bellugi, Marks, Bihrle, individuals with WBS have “preserved lin-
& Sabo, 1988), storytelling and narrative en- guistic and social skills.”
richment strategies involving affective prosody Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan (2000) re-
and a sense of drama (Reilly, Klima, & Bel- ported on the dissociation of the social-
lugi, 1990), and a reliance on stereotypic, cognitive and social-perceptual components of
adult phrases (Udwin & Yule, 1990). However, theory of mind in WBS, with relative sparing
nonverbal, perceptual skills are typically weak of the latter. Children with WBS did poorly on
in WBS, often with marked difficulties in false-belief understanding (social-cognition)
visual-spatial processing, especially integrat- tasks but were able to provide mental-age-
ing details into a whole. Yet, certain visual- appropriate explanations for another person’s
spatial skills seem well preserved even within behaviors and to discriminate and match facial
this area of deficit. In particular, persons with expressions of emotion (social-perceptual
WBS generally excel on facial perception and tasks). Laws and Bishop (2004) demonstrated
recognition tasks (Bellugi, Wang, & Jernigan, that children with WBS indeed have difficul-
1994; Udwin & Yule, 1991). They often look ties with social relationships and a semantic-
intently at the faces of both strangers and fa- pragmatic language disorder (described by
miliar people (Bellugi, Bihrle, Neville, Do- some as “loquaciousness”), particularly with
herty, & Jernigan, 1992; Bertrand, Mervis, inappropriate initiation of conversation and
Rice, & Adamson, 1993), although they solve the use of stereotyped conversation. They pro-
face-processing tasks by different cognitive duce less coherent narratives and conversation
processes (Grice et al., 2001). (For reviews, despite having syntactic abilities equivalent to
see Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai, & St. normal controls, and they score low for conver-
George, 2000; and W. Jones et al., 2000.) sation rapport. They also have a restricted
Recently, investigators have more specifi- range of interests, specialized factual knowl-
cally characterized atypical language develop- edge, and usual vocabulary. The authors
ment in WBS. Mervis (1999) noted that (2004, p. 45) even suggested that:
referential language precedes referential point-
ing in WBS, and the developmental vocabulary Far from representing the polar opposite of autism,
spurt occurs prior to spontaneous exhaustive as suggested by some researchers, Williams syn-
sorting, the opposite of what is seen in typical drome would seem to share many of the character-
language development. Toddlers with WBS istics of autistic disorder.
also do not spontaneously use the pointing ges-
ture in free-play situations. Laing et al. (2002) Further research in WBS will elucidate
reported that despite superficially good social whether the extent of shared characteristics
skills, children with WBS were deficient at would enable official inclusion on the autistic
both initiating and responding to triadic inter- spectrum.
actions (e.g., child-interlocutor-object), which
are essential for instrumental and declarative Mitochondrial Disorders
joint attention and for referential uses of lan-
guage; they did show proficiency at dyadic in- Coleman and Blass (1985) first reported an as-
teractions (e.g., face to face), however. It has sociation of lactic acidosis with autism over
been suggested that children with WBS may 20 years ago, which was corroborated by Las-
be less interested in objects and more inter- zlo, Horvath, Eck, and Fekete (1994). Lom-
ested in faces than typical children (Bertrand bard (1998) postulated a mitochondrial
et al., 1993). The WBS group was also im- etiology for autism based on, among other
paired on the comprehension and production things, his unpublished anecdotal observations
of referential pointing, despite vocabulary lev- of carnitine deficiency. Functional neuroimag-
els higher than those of typically developing ing methodologies have also related autism
children of the same mental age, which could and deficient energy metabolism in the brain
not be explained on the basis of fine motor im- (Chugani, Sundram, Behen, Lee, & Moore,
pairments (Laing et al., 2002). The authors 1999; Levitt et al., 2003; Minshew, Goldstein,
thereby challenged the published claims that Dombrowski, Panchalingam, & Pettegrew,
550 Neurological and Medical Issues

1993). Graf et al. (2000) reported a family Mitochondrial diseases are probably a rare and in-
with a group of neurologic disorders associ- significant cause of pure autism; however, evidence
ated with the mitochondrial DNA G8363A is accumulating that . . . mitochondrial disorders
transfer ribonucleic acid (RNA)Lys mutation; can present with autistic features. Most patients
of four family members affected, one child will present with multisystem abnormalities (espe-
cially neurologic) associated with autistic behav-
was also severely autistic. Ramoz et al. (2004)
ior. (p. 381)
reported linkage and association of the mito-
chondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier SLC25A12 Nevertheless, because our knowledge of mi-
gene with autism. Filiano, Goldenthal, Rhodes, tochondrial function and dysfunction is
and Marin-Garcia (2002) reported a group of presently expanding exponentially and concur-
12 children presenting with hypotonia, in- rently with our knowledge of the neurobiology
tractable epilepsy, autism, and developmental and genetics of autism, further research is in-
delay (HEADD syndrome), who demonstrated dicated to elucidate the validity and extent of
reduced levels in specific mitochondrial respi- mitochondrial dysfunction in individuals with
ratory activities encoded by mitochondrial autism.
DNA, with a majority also showing mitochon-
drial structural abnormalities. Pons et al. Isodicentric Chromosome 15q Syndrome
(2004) recently reported five patients with
autism who had concurrent A3243G mito- A chromosomal duplication syndrome found in
chondrial (mt)DNA mutations and mtDNA de- autism involves the proximal long arm of chro-
pletion syndromes. This mutation typically mosome 15q11-q13 (IDIC 15). The duplica-
causes mitochondrial encephalopathy, lactic tion is usually maternally inherited and involves
acidosis, seizures, hearing loss, and strokes the area roughly corresponding to the Prader-
(MELAS syndrome) with ragged red fibers in Willi/Angelman critical region (PWACR) of ap-
skeletal muscle. Four of these five patients proximately four million base pairs. The
were ascertained because a maternal relative additional genetic material may be interstitial
was identified with the mutation, not because (within one chromosome 15, producing
they presented with symptoms consistent with 46,XY) and may or may not be inverted, pro-
a mitochondrial disorder. Clark-Taylor and ducing a trisomy (three copies) of 15q11-q13.
Clark-Taylor (2004) reported a child with Or, the additional material may form a sepa-
autism who also had an abnormal acyl-carnitine rate marker chromosome (47,XY), producing a
profile with elevations of unsaturated fatty- tetrasomy (four copies) of this region. Al-
acid metabolites C14:1 and C14:2 and ammo- though the prevalence of duplications of the
nia and alterations of tricarboxylic acid cycle PWACR is estimated to be similar to that of
energy production. Filipek et al. (in press) deletions in this region, 115,000 (Mohandas
reported that free and total carnitine and et al., 1999), the phenotype of the duplication
pyruvate were significantly reduced while am- syndrome has become appreciated only within
monia, lactate, and alanine levels were consid- the past 8 to 10 years.
erably elevated in 100 autistic children. The This syndrome is one of the most frequent
relative carnitine deficiency in these patients, of the currently identifiable chromosomal dis-
accompanied by slight elevations in lactate orders associated with autism, occurring in
and significant elevations in alanine and am- between 1% and 4% of autistic individuals
monia levels, is suggestive of mild mitochondr- (Browne et al., 1997; Konstantareas & Homa-
ial dysfunction, and the authors hypothesized tidis, 1999; Schroer et al., 1998). The clinical
that a mitochondrial defect might be the origin phenotype in autism is highly variable, ranging
of the carnitine deficiency in these autistic from profound psychomotor retardation to nor-
children. mal nonverbal cognitive scores (Filipek,
Lerman-Sagie, Leshinsky-Silver, Watem- Smith, et al., 2000). Rineer, Finucane, and
berg, and Lev (2004) reviewed the literature Simon (1998) noted that, of 29 individuals
on the association of autism and mitochondrial with IDIC 15, 20 met criteria for autism using
disorders: the GARS (Gilliam, 1995); those autistic IDIC
Medical Aspects of Autism 551

15 differed from the GARS autistic norming tion to high-resolution karyotype in all cases
group only on having better social function as of autism to detect duplication of 15q (Keller
measured by the social interaction subscale, et al., 2003; Yardin et al., 2002). In addition to
which corresponds to the anecdotal experience its association with autism, Longo et al.
of this author and other investigators (Cather- (2004) reported isodicentric 15q11-q13 dupli-
ine Lord, personal communication). More than cations in 3 of 63 (4.7%) patients with Rett
100 individuals with autism and this chromo- syndrome in addition to the MECP2 deletions.
somal anomaly have been reported in the liter-
ature to date (Baker, Piven, Schwartz, & Patil, Angelman / Prader-Willi Syndromes
1994; Battaglia et al., 1997; Bolton et al.,
2001; Borgatti et al., 2001; Bundey, Hardy, Described as “sister imprinting disorders”
Vickers, Kilpatrick, & Corbett, 1994; Cheng, (Cassidy, Dykens, & Williams, 2000), Angel-
Spinner, Zackai, & Knoll, 1994; Cook et al., man and Prader-Willi (PWS) syndromes are
1997; Estecio, Fett-Conte, Varella-Garcia, each the result of either a deletion or uni-
Fridman, & Silva, 2002; Fantes et al., 2002; parental disomy (UPD) in the PWACR of chro-
Flejter et al., 1996; C. Gillberg, Steffenburg, mosome 15 (see Clayton-Smith & Laan, 2003,
Wahlstrom, et al., 1991; Gurrieri et al., 1999; for a review). Angelman syndrome, coined the
Hotopf & Bolton, 1995; Hou & Wang, 1998; “ happy puppet syndrome” (Bower & Jeavons,
Keller et al., 2003; Konstantareas & Homa- 1967), presents with severe motor and intellec-
tidis, 1999; Lauritsen, Mors, Mortensen, & tual retardation, ataxia, hypotonia, epilepsy,
Ewald, 1999; Ludowese, Thompson, Sekhon, absence of speech, and unusual “ happy” facies
& Pauli, 1991; Mann et al., 2004; Mao & Jalal, (OMIM™, 2000). Evidence is strong that the
2000; Moeschler, Mohandas, Hawk, & Noll, gene for Angelman syndrome is the E6-associ-
2002; Rausch & Nevin, 1991; Repetto, White, ated protein ubiquitin-protein ligase gene
Bader, Johnson, & Knoll, 1998; Rineer et al., (UBE3A), which suggests that Angelman syn-
1998; Sabry & Farag, 1998; Schroer et al., drome is the first recognized example of a ge-
1998; Silva, Vayego-Lourenco, Fett-Conte, netic disorder of the ubiquitin-dependent
Goloni-Bertollo, & Varella-Garcia, 2002; Un- proteolytic pathway in humans (Kishino, La-
garo et al., 2001; Webb et al., 1998; Weidmer- lande, & Wagstaff, 1997; OMIM™, 2000).
Mikhail, Sheldon, & Ghaziuddin, 1998; Steffenburg, Gillberg, Steffenburg, and
Wisniewski, Hassold, Heffelfinger, & Higgins, Kyllerman (1996) reported that all four chil-
1979; Wolpert et al., 2000; Woods, Robinson, dren with Angelman syndrome ascertained in
Gardiner, & Roussounis, 1997; Yardin et al., a population study met behavioral criteria for
2002). autism. Trillingsgaard and Stergaard (2004)
Filipek et al. (2003) reported mitochondrial found that 13 of 16 children with Angelman
dysfunction in two autistic children with met ADOS-G (Lord et al., 2000) criteria for
isodicentric 15q syndrome. Both had unevent- an ASD; however, the authors noted that
ful perinatal courses, normal EEGs and MRI autism might have been overdiagnosed in their
scans, moderate motor delay, pronounced sample because of the extremely low cognitive
lethargy when ill, severe hypotonia, and mod- levels of the children with Angelman. C. A.
est lactic acidosis. On muscle mitochondrial Williams, Lossie, and Driscoll (2001) noted
enzyme assays, each had pronounced mito- that some children with ASD may be misdiag-
chondrial hyperproliferation and a partial res- nosed with Angelman, particularly with nega-
piratory chain block most parsimoniously tive genetic testing for Angelman. Thompson
placed at the level of complex III, suggesting and Bolton (2003) reported one case of Angel-
candidate gene loci for autism within the man syndrome and paternal UPD and de-
PWACR that affect pathways influencing mi- scribed the milder Angelman symptomatology
tochondrial function. associated with UPD as including a lack of
Some investigators have recently questioned autistic features.
whether fluorescent in situ hybridization PWS is characterized by obesity, muscular
(FISH) studies should be performed in addi- hypotonia, mental retardation, short stature,
552 Neurological and Medical Issues

hypogonadotropic hypogonadism, and small than nonverbal, despite almost universal se-
hands and feet. It appears that PWS results vere early language delay (Bearden et al.,
from UPD or deletion of the paternal copies of 2001; Niklasson et al., 2001; Wang, Woodin,
the imprinted small nuclear ribonucleoprotein Kreps-Falk, & Moss, 2000; Woodin et al.,
polypeptide N (SNRPN) and necdin genes and 2001). A marked deficit in visuospatial mem-
possibly others as well (OMIM™, 2000). Velt- ory has been documented in these children,
man et al. (2004) found that maternal UPD producing the described mathematics disabili-
cases of PWS would be more likely to exhibit ties. In addition to the selective deficit in visu-
ASD than would cases with deletions in the ospatial memory, Bearden et al. (2001) found a
PWACR. Therefore, the extent of the associa- dissociation between visuospatial and object
tions of Angelman and PWS with autism re- memory and noted the similarity of the VCFS
mains unclear, particularly the differential cognitive profile with WBS (Bearden, Wang,
effects of UPD as compared with deletions of & Simon, 2002).
the responsible genes. Kozma (1998) was the first to report co-
morbid autism in VCFS, with associated se-
Velocardiofacial Syndrome vere mental retardation. Niklasson et al.
(2001; Niklasson, Rasmussen, Oskarsdottir, &
Shprintzen, Goldberg, Young, and Wolford Gillberg, 2002) found that more than 30% of
(1981) first described velocardiofacial syn- their VCFS subjects were also autistic, 50%
drome (VCFS), which is characterized by had “autistic traits,” and more than 50% had
cleft palate, cardiac malformations (usually a mental retardation; only 6% of their sample
ventricular septal defect), typical facies had a normal IQ and were free of neuropsychi-
(tubular nose, narrow palpebral fissures, and atric disorders. Scherer, D’Antonio, and
retruded jaw), learning disabilities and/or men- Rodgers (2001) noted a sparse vocabulary and
tal retardation, microcephaly, short stature, pattern of sound types and very low mean bab-
CNS vascular malformations, and seizures bling length relative to other communication
(Coppola, Sciscio, Russo, Caliendo, & Pas- measures, differing qualitatively and quantita-
cotto, 2001; OMIM™, 2000; Perez & Sulli- tively from that found in Down syndrome.
van, 2002; Roubertie et al., 2001). VCFS is Glaser et al. (2002) noted uniquely lower recep-
now known to be caused by a microdeletion on tive language function relative to expressive
chromosome 22q11.2. It is also known as language ability; they also found parent-of-
CATCH 22 and chromosome 22q11 deletion origin effects, with those with a deletion of
syndromes, and its prevalence is estimated at 1 paternal origin scoring higher on language
per 4,000 (Bassett & Chow, 1999). measures than those with a deletion of mater-
There is an extremely high prevalence of nal origin.
neuropsychiatric disorders in VCFS involving
over 50% of the reported cases. Gothelf and Möbius Syndrome
colleagues reported that 16% to 25% will de-
velop psychotic disorder by adolescence; the Möbius syndrome maps to chromosome
prevalence of schizophrenia in VCFS is 25 13q12.2-q13 and is characterized by brainstem
times that of the general population (Gothelf maldevelopment resulting in congenital unilat-
& Lombroso, 2001; Gothelf, Presburger, Levy, eral or bilateral paresis of the facial (7th) cra-
et al., 2004; Gothelf, Presburger, Zohar, et al., nial nerve. There is variable involvement of
2004). Up to 40% meet criteria for attention other cranial nerves, usually the abducens
deficit / hyperactivity disorder, and 33% for (6th), but also possibly the trigeminal (5th),
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Over half of glossopharyngeal (9th), or hypoglossal (12th).
the cases in some series were mentally re- There is associated mental retardation, orofa-
tarded (Niklasson, Rasmussen, Oskarsdottir, cial and limb malformations, and muscu-
& Gillberg, 2001). loskeletal defects (Möbius, 1888; OMIM™,
The most characteristic behavioral pheno- 2000).
type is that of a nonverbal learning disorder, Several reports noted the co-occurrence of
with verbal IQ scores significantly greater autism and Möbius syndrome (C. Gillberg &
Medical Aspects of Autism 553

Winnergard, 1984; Larrandaburu, Schuler, found to have untreated PKU (Lowe, Tanaka,
Ehlers, Reis, & Silveira, 1999; Ornitz, Guthrie, Seashore, Young, & Cohen, 1980; Moreno
& Farley, 1977), while others described diffi- et al., 1992). In contrast, other studies have
culties in communication, social interactions, found essentially no significant abnormalities
and maladaptive behaviors without specific di- in metabolic tests in autistic individuals (John-
agnoses of autism (Giannini, Tamulonis, Gian- son, Wiersema, & Kraft, 1974; Perry, Hansen,
nini, Loiselle, & Spirtos, 1984; Meyerson & & Christie, 1978; Pueschel, Herman, & Gro-
Foushee, 1978). In an early report, C. Gillberg den, 1985). In the study by Lowe et al. (1980),
and Steffenberg (1989) noted autistic behav- the autistic symptoms in the children with PKU
iors in about 40% of individuals with Möbius improved after initiation of dietary therapy.
syndrome. One autistic child, whose brainstem Rutter et al. (1997) stated that because un-
neuropathology noted virtual absence of neu- treated PKU is very rare, it must be an even
rons in the facial nerve nucleus, was also de- rarer cause of autism. However, reliance on
scribed as having little facial expression and newborn screening programs alone may give a
may have also had Möbius or a similar syn- false sense of security, particularly in regions
drome (Rodier, Ingram, Tisdale, Nelson, & with large immigrant populations. A 4-year-
Romano, 1996). old child, born in the Middle East and diag-
Johansson et al. (2001) found an ASD in nosed with autism by both a child psychiatrist
40% of their cohort with Möbius syndrome, and child neurologist in the United States, pre-
using the ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994), with men- sented with undiagnosed PKU after her new-
tal retardation in one-third of the subjects. born brother was identified on routine newborn
Bandim, Ventura, Miller, Almeida, and Costa screen (Gargus & Filipek, n.d.). In addition,
(2003) used the CARS (Schopler et al., 1988) despite extremely strict dietary control of his
to diagnose autism in one-third of their cohort; PKU and frequent normal serum phenylalanine
the average CARS score for the autistic indi- levels when followed in metabolic clinic, the
viduals was 40.4, in the severe range, while the younger child also met Diagnostic and Statisti-
average for the nonautistic individuals was cal Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition
18.4. Stromland et al. (2002) reported comor- (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association,
bid autism in 24% of their cohort with Möbius, 1994), criteria for Asperger syndrome (Fil-
using the ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994). ipek, unpublished observation). Baieli, Pavone,
Although Möbius syndrome has an identi- Meli, Fiumara, and Coleman (2003) reported
fied genetic locus, there have been reports of that 2 of 35 individuals with classic PKU who
an association of Möbius after in utero expo- were diagnosed late in infancy ( before new-
sure to misoprostol, a prostaglandin analogue born screening became common) met criteria
used to prevent and treat GI ulceration from for autism, using the ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994)
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory medications and the CARS (Schopler et al., 1988); none of
(Pastuszak et al., 1998); misoprostol is also the 62 children identified by newborn screen-
available over the counter in some countries ing and on dietary treatment met criteria for
and used to self-induce abortions (Gonzalez autism. In addition, in the group of 144 with
et al., 1998). mild hyperphenylalanemia due to causes other
than classic PKU, one boy had Asperger syn-
Phenylketonuria drome with normal IQ, and one retarded child
with tetrahydropterin deficiency met criteria
Autism has been associated with several in- for autism. Again, in a sample of individuals
born errors of metabolism, primarily PKU with autism, finding undiagnosed and untreated
(Folstein & Rutter, 1988; Friedman, 1969; Mi- PKU is rare; however, in a sample of individuals
ladi, Larnaout, Kaabachi, Helayem, & Ben with PKU, up to 5% may be autistic.
Hamida, 1992; R. S. Williams, Hauser, Pur-
pura, DeLong, & Swisher, 1980). Almost half Congenital Blindness and Deafness
of one cohort with PKU had autistic sympto-
matology (Bliumina, 1975), and 2% to 5% of Autistic symptomatology has been anecdotally
autistic children in two other cohorts were associated with congenital blindness (CB) for
554 Neurological and Medical Issues

decades; in some studies, up to 30% of chil- CB without autism, there were significantly
dren with CB were also described as being more “autistic features” than seen in matched,
autistic (Chase, 1972; Fraiberg, 1977; Fraiberg sighted children. Brown et al. (1997) and Hob-
& Freedman, 1964; Keeler, 1958; Norris, son et al. (1999) compared congenitally blind
Spaulding, & Bordie, 1957; Wing, 1969; as (of various etiologies) and sighted autistic
cited in reviews by Cass, 1998; Hobson & children and noted remarkably similar clinical
Bishop, 2003; Hobson, Lee, & Brown, 1999). features. The mean CARS score (Schopler
S. J. Rogers and Newhart-Larson (1989) re- et al., 1988) was 27.8 (without Item VII, visual
ported a diagnosis of autism in all five boys responsiveness, scored) for the CB children
studied with Leber’s congenital amaurosis. with autism. The authors’ clinical impression
Ek, Fernell, Jacobson, and Gillberg (1998) was that blind autistic children were less se-
found that 56% of premature babies with verely impaired than sighted autistic children;
retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) had both none were abnormal in listening response
autistic disorder and mental retardation, and, (Item VIII ), but most were markedly abnor-
of those, one-third had coexistent cerebral mal in body (IV) and object use (V). There-
palsy. In comparison, only 14% of those with fore, they noted the close similarities and
hereditary retinal disease had autistic disor- possible subtle distinctions between the two
der. Janson (1993) postulated that, in blind autistic groups. Hobson and Bishop (2003)
children with ROP, a behavior pattern of unre- went on to evaluate 18 CB children between 4
sponsitivity and stereotypic object manipula- and 8 years of age who did not meet DSM-IV
tion emerges between 12 and 30 months to (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) cri-
distinguish autistic and nonautistic children teria for autism and had an IQ > 55; teacher
with CB. Msall et al. (2004) followed children impressions were used to divide the sample
with ROP at ages 5 and 8 years and found that into “more social” (MS, N = 9) and “less so-
23% had epilepsy; 39%, cerebral palsy; and cial” (LS, N = 9) groups. In the MS group, the
44%, learning disabilities. Of the children highest CARS score was 15.5 ( lowest possible
with no or minimal light perception or totally score is 14 without rating Item VII ), and no in-
detached retinas bilaterally, 9% were autistic, dividual item was rated higher than 0.5 above
as compared with only 1% of those with more normal. In contrast, in the LS group, the
favorable visual status. CARS scores ranged from 17.5 to 27.5 (mean
Cass, Sonksen, and McConachie (1994) re- 22.3 + 3.6). Four of the subjects had Leber’s
ported that, of an entire sample of over 600 congenital amaurosis and were all placed into
congenitally blind children of differing etiolo- the LS group; almost half of the subjects had
gies, only 17% demonstrated no evidence of ROP and were spread across both groups.
additional disabilities and were developing The comorbidity of autism and congenital
normally at age 16 months when first studied. blindness has received relatively meager atten-
Subsequently, 31% had a regression in their tion in the autism research literature. Diagno-
development at between 16 and 27 months of sis of autism in children with CB is
age; children who regressed tended to have particularly difficult. As Cass (1998) asked:
disorders of CNS/optic nerve/retina while
children who did not regress had a purely opti- . . . distinguishing normal from abnormal social-
cal cause for their blindness (e.g., congenital communication development in children with visual
cataracts or glaucoma). The more “central” impairment is an even more complex problem. Is it
pathophysiology of the blindness in the regres- possible to use diagnostic tools more firmly rooted
sion cohort was subsequently confirmed by in ICD-10 criteria such as the Autism Diagnostic
Interview (ADI ) and the Autism Diagnostic Obser-
neuroimaging studies; the children with devel-
vation Schedule (ADOS; Le Couteur et al., 1989;
opmental regression had more CNS lesions
Lord, 1991)? Again, there are major problems with
than those who did not regress (Waugh, Chong, this approach since these instruments focus (en-
& Sonksen, 1998). tirely appropriately for diagnosis in the sighted) on
Brown, Hobson, Lee, and Stevenson (1997) highly visual dependent behaviors such as referen-
reported that almost half of their sample with tial eye gaze, eye gaze for social purposes, protode-
CB met criteria for autism and that, even in clarative pointing and symbolic play, all of which
Medical Aspects of Autism 555

are either delayed or absent in normally developing autistic) is often delayed, remediation is often
children with visual impairment. (p. 129) suboptimal and ineffective.
Roper, Arnold, and Monteiro (2003) evalu-
Gense and Gense (1994) tried to develop ated deaf autistic, deaf learning disabled, and
guidelines, using an educational approach, for hearing autistic children. There were no dif-
children with CB and autism, but to date no ferences across the groups in the age at which
pragmatic approach to autistic children with parents first suspected a developmental or
CB has been developed. Hobson et al. (1999) hearing problem or when the hearing deficit
proposed several theoretical questions that was diagnosed. However, the deaf autistic chil-
need to be addressed on a larger scale to for- dren were first diagnosed with deafness at a
mally investigate the associations between mean of 1 year of age (range 6 months to 21⁄ 2
autism and CB: years), but not diagnosed with autism until a
mean of 15 years of age (range 5 to 16 years)
(a) Is the syndrome of autism in blind children to despite parental suspicions averaging 7 months
be clearly demarcated from autism-like clinical of age (range 2 to 18 months). In contrast, the
manifestations in nonautistic blind children, given hearing autistic children were diagnosed at a
that there appears to be a gradation in the number, mean of 71⁄ 2 years of age (range 4 to 11 years),
quality, and severity of abnormalities shown by albeit late since their parents’ suspicions aver-
different children? ( b) How far is it appropriate to aged 18 months of age (range 3 months to 5
consider each of the clinical manifestations as years). There were no differences in the cur-
autistic-like, when such abnormalities might arise rent levels of autistic behaviors demonstrated
on the basis of quite different psychopathological
by the deaf or hearing autistic groups, which is
mechanisms? (c) When blind children present with
a constellation of clinical features and a picture
consistent with the previous findings of Gar-
that approximates to the syndrome of early child- reau, Barthelemy, and Sauvage (1984). The
hood autism, is this picture distinguishable from authors also noted no discriminating charac-
that of autism in sighted children? If it is, might the teristics of the deaf autistic individuals that
distinguishing features afford insight into the de- would have facilitated earlier recognition of
velopmental psychopathology of autism itself ? the autistic symptoms. Therefore, early recog-
(p. 46) nition of hearing impairment in autistic chil-
dren and of autism in deaf children is essential
The incidence and prevalence of hearing for the provision of an appropriate intervention
impairment in children is 11 to 12 per 10,000 strategy for these children (Ewing & Jones,
(Boyle et al., 1996; Kubba, MacAndie, Ritchie, 2003).
& MacFarlane, 2004), and the rate steadily in-
creases with age (Boyle et al., 1996). The co- Fetal Anticonvulsant / Valproate Syndrome
morbidity of hearing impairment and autism
may be higher than expected (Gordon, 1991; Although the syndrome was described earlier
Jure, Rapin, & Tuchman, 1991). Jure et al. (Chessa & Iannetti, 1986; DiLiberti, Farndon,
(1991) performed a chart review of 46 children Dennis, & Curry, 1984; Paulson & Paulson,
diagnosed as deaf and autistic; nearly 20% had 1981), Ardinger et al. (1988) confirmed that
normal or near-normal nonverbal cognitive the multiple congenital anomalies and devel-
function, and only 20% had severe mental re- opmental delay noted in infants exposed to val-
tardation. The severity of the autistic behav- proic acid (VPA) in utero represented a
iors correlated with the level of cognitive definitive fetal valproate syndrome (FVS). The
impairment but not to the level of hearing loss. clinical features include craniofacial, cardio-
In almost 24%, the diagnosis of comorbid vascular, urinary tract, genital, digital, and
autism did not occur for over 4 years after the respiratory anomalies, and meningomyelocele.
diagnosis of deafness; and in another 22%, the Up to 90% have developmental delay.
diagnosis of hearing impairment was delayed Several subsequent papers reported autism
for many years after the diagnosis of autism. in children with FVS (Bescoby-Chambers,
Because the diagnosis of the comorbid condi- Forster, & Bates, 2001; Christianson,
tion (e.g., autism in the deaf or deafness in the Chesler, & Kromberg, 1994; Moore et al.,
556 Neurological and Medical Issues

2000; Samren, van Duijn, Christiaens, Hof- with the medical aspects of autism. When a
man, & Lindhout, 1999; G. Williams et al., child with an ASD is seen, providers need to
2001; P. G. Williams & Hersh, 1997). Chris- consider all potential associated medical dis-
tianson et al. (1994) first reported FVS in two orders and syndromes, both relatively common
sibling pairs. In the first family, the dose of and rare. They also need to consider associated
valproate was halved when the first pregnancy signs and symptoms that the family will con-
was confirmed and phenytoin was added; in front, such as sleeping and feeding distur-
the second pregnancy, valproate was continued bances. In addition, when a provider sees a child
at the mother’s usual dose. Although both with a rare syndrome or disorder, the child’s be-
children demonstrated many of the classic dys- havioral phenotype must be considered: Does
morphic findings associated with fetal anti- this child have autism or another neurobehav-
convulsant syndrome (epicanthal folds, ioral disorder? These deliberations will improve
synophrys, upturned nasal tip with anteverted the recognition of autism and the role of associ-
nares, and long philtrum), the older child was ated medical factors and ultimately best serve
developmentally normal while the younger the children and their families.
child was classically autistic with additional
craniofacial anomalies. The authors suggested Cross-References
that there may be a valproate dosage effect in
FVS, which was corroborated by Samren et al. Issues of diagnosis are addressed in Chapters 1
(1999). to 6; genetic factors are discussed in Chapter
Four of the 57 children with fetal anticon- 16; neurobiological aspects of the disorder are
vulsant syndromes reported by Moore et al. discussed in Chapter 18.
(2000) were reported to have autism (two ex-
posed to VPA alone, one to VPA and pheny- RECOMMENDED READING LIST FOR
toin, and one to carbamazepine and diazepam). PARENTS AND PROFESSIONALS*
Two additional children were diagnosed with
Asperger syndrome (one exposed to VPA and Batshaw, Mark, M. D. (2002). Children with dis-
one to VPA, phenytoin, and a benzodiazepine). abilities (5th ed.). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Eight additional cases were reported (Bescoby- A very well-written medical book on developmen-
Chambers et al., 2001; G. Williams et al., tal disabilities.
2001; P. G. Williams & Hersh, 1997), one with Durand, V. M. (1998). Sleep better! A guide to im-
Asperger syndrome and seven who met DSM- proving sleep for children with special needs.
IV or International Classification of Diseases Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
(ICD-10) criteria for autistic disorder. Provides practical strategies for encouraging chil-
dren with special needs to sleep.

CONCLUSION Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook


Jr., E. H., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., et al.
Studies of relatively strictly defined autistic (1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic
disorder have generally revealed low rates of spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and De-
velopmental Disorders, 29, 437–482.
medical conditions that might be associated
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek,
with autism; the broadened view of ASDs forces
G. T., Cook, Jr., E. H., Dawson, G., et al.
us to revisit this issue. This chapter provided a (2000). Practice parameter: Screening and di-
summary of the medical aspects of this com- agnosis of autism: Report of the Quality Stan-
plex disorder from complementary perspec- dards Subcommittee of the American
tives, reinforcing the complexity of the ASDs Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurol-
and strengthening the bridge between evidence- ogy Society. Neurology, 55(4), 468–479.
based medicine and clinical application. Background paper and current recommendations
Providers and investigators in all clinical and for physicians and other professionals for screen-
research disciplines should become familiar ing, diagnosis, and the medical evaluation of autism.

* Portions of this section were taken with permission from Volkmar & Wiesner (2004).
Medical Aspects of Autism 557

Available to download at www.forockids.org/en A well-written introduction to fragile X syndrome,


/forockids.php?page=12. written for parents and families.
Freeman, J., Vining, E. P. J., & Pillas, D. J. (2002).
Seizures and Epilepsy in Childhood: A Guide. REFERENCES
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
A parent-oriented guide to seizure disorders. Abrams, N., & Pergament, E. (1971). Childhood
psychosis combined with XYY abnormalities.
Immunization Safety Review Committee. (2001).
Journal of Genetic Psychology; Child Behav-
Immunization Safety Review: Measles-Mumps-
ior, Animal Behavior, and Comparative Psy-
Rubella Vaccine and Autism. Institute of Medi-
chology, 118, 13–16.
cine. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Accardo, P., Whitman, B., Caul, J., & Rolfe, U.
Immunization Safety Review Committee. (2001).
(1988). Autism and plumbism. A possible as-
Immunization Safety Review: Thimerosal Con-
sociation. Clinical Pediatrics, 27(1), 41–44.
taining Vaccines and Neurodevelopmental Dis-
Afzal, N., Murch, S., Thirrupathy, K., Berger, L.,
orders. Institute of Medicine. Washington,
Fagbemi, A., & Heuschkel, R. (2003). Consti-
DC: National Academy Press.
pation with acquired megarectum in children
Immunization Safety Review Committee. (2004).
with autism. Pediatrics, 112(4), 939–942.
Immunization Safety Review: Vaccines and
Ahearn, W. H., Castine, T., Nault, K., & Green, G.
Autism. Institute of Medicine. Washington,
(2001). An assessment of food acceptance in
DC: National Academy Press.
children with autism or pervasive developmen-
Reviews the evidence on MMR and mercury- tal disorder-not otherwise specified. Journal
containing vaccines and autism. Available from of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(5),
www.nap.edu. 505–511.
National Academy of Science. (2001). Report of the Akefeldt, A., & Gillberg, C. (1991). Hypome-
Committee on Educational Interventions for lanosis of Ito in three cases with autism and
Autistic Children. Washington, DC: National autistic-like conditions. Developmental Medi-
Academies Press. cine and Child Neurology, 33(8), 737–743.
American College of Medical Genetics: Policy
An excellent source of information on recom-
Statement. (1994). Fragile X Syndrome: Diag-
mended educational interventions for children with
nostic and carrier testing. American Journal of
autism, including language, physical /occupational,
Medical Genetics, 53, 380–381.
and social cognition therapies.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
Powers, M. D. (2000). Children with Autism: A Par- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ent’s Guide (2nd ed.). Bethesda, MD: Wood- ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
bine House. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
An excellent resource for parents, full of useful in- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
formation on a range of issues. ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Sleep Disorders Association. (1997). In-
Volkmar, F. R., Cook, E. H. J., Pomeroy, J., Real-
ternational classification of sleep disorders,
muto, G., & Tanguay, P. (1999). Practice para-
revised: Diagnostic and coding manual.
meters for autism and pervasive developmental
Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders
disorders. Journal of the American Academy of
Association.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(Suppl.
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
12), 32S–54S.
(1991). Guidelines for the audiologic assess-
Professional guidelines for psychiatric assessment ment of children from birth through 36 months
of autism. of age. (Committee on Infant Hearing.) Ameri-
Volkmar, F. R., & Wiesner, L. A. (2004). Health- can Speech-Language-Hearing Association,
care for Children on the Autism Spectrum: A 33(Suppl. 5), 37–43.
Guide to Medical, Nutritional, and Behavioral Ardinger, H. H., Atkin, J. F., Blackston, R. D.,
Issues. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House. Elsas, L. J., Clarren, S. K., Livingstone, S.,
et al. (1988). Verification of the fetal val-
A very well-written and highly informative book
proate syndrome phenotype. American Journal
for parents and professionals.
of Medical Genetics, 29(1), 171–185.
Weber, J. D. (2000). Children with Fragile X Syn- Aronson, M., Hagberg, B., & Gillberg, C. (1997).
drome: A Parent’s Guide. Bethesda, MD: Attention deficits and autistic spectrum prob-
Woodbine House. lems in children exposed to alcohol during
558 Neurological and Medical Issues

gestation: A follow-up study. Developmental perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and


Medicine and Child Neurology, 39(9), Psychiatry, 37, 89–126.
583–587. Bailey, D. B., Jr., Hatton, D. D., Mesibov, G.,
Ashwood, P., Anthony, A., Pellicer, A. A., Torrente, Ament, N., & Skinner, M. (2000). Early devel-
F., Walker-Smith, J. A., & Wakefield, A. J. opment, temperament, and functional impair-
(2003). Intestinal lymphocyte populations in ment in autism and fragile X syndrome.
children with regressive autism: Evidence for Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
extensive mucosal immunopathology. Journal ders, 30(1), 49–59.
of Clinical Immunology, 23(6), 504–517. Bailey, D. B., Jr., Hatton, D. D., Skinner, M., &
Askalan, R., Mackay, M., Brian, J., Otsubo, H., Mesibov, G. (2001). Autistic behavior, FMR1
McDermott, C., Bryson, S., et al. (2003). protein, and developmental trajectories in
Prospective preliminary analysis of the devel- young males with fragile X syndrome. Journal
opment of autism and epilepsy in children with of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2),
infantile spasms. Journal of Child Neurology, 165–174.
18(3), 165–170. Baker, P., Piven, J., Schwartz, S., & Patil, S.
August, G., & Lockhart, L. (1988). Familial autism (1994). Duplication of chromosome 15q11–13
and the fragile-X chromosome. Journal of in two individuals with autistic disorder. Jour-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(2), nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
197–204. 24(4), 529–535.
Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Field, K., Sparks, Bandim, J. M., Ventura, L. O., Miller, M. T.,
B. F., & Singh, N. (2002). Effects of age on Almeida, H. C., & Costa, A. E. (2003).
brain volume and head circumference in Autism and Mobius sequence: An exploratory
autism. Neurology, 59(2), 175–183. study of children in northeastern Brazil. Ar-
Baieli, S., Pavone, L., Meli, C., Fiumara, A., & quivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 61(2A), 181–185.
Coleman, M. (2003). Autism and phenylke- Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A
tonuria. Journal of Autism and Developmental retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor
Disorders, 33(2), 201–204. and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age.
Bailey, A. (1994). Physical examination and med- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
ical investigation. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & ders, 29, 213–224.
L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychi- Baranek, G. T., Foster, L. G., & Berkson, G.
atry: Modern approaches (3rd ed., pp. 79–93). (1997). Tactile defensiveness and stereotyped
Oxford, England: Blackwell Press. behaviors. American Journal of Occupational
Bailey, A., Bolton, P., Butler, L., Le Couteur, A., Therapy, 51, 91–95.
Murphy, M., Scott, S., et al. (1993). Preva- Barton, M., & Volkmar, F. (1998). How commonly
lence of the fragile X anomaly amongst autis- are known medical conditions associated with
tic twins and singletons. Journal of Child autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 673–688. Disorders, 28(4), 273–278.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, Bassett, A. S., & Chow, E. W. (1999). 22q11 dele-
P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). tion syndrome: A genetic subtype of schizo-
Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evi- phrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 46(7),
dence from a British twin study. Psychological 882–891.
Medicine, 25, 63–77. Battaglia, A., Gurrieri, F., Bertini, E., Bellacosa,
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Pickles, A., A., Pomponi, M. G., Paravatou-Petsotas, M.,
Yuzda, E., Schmidt, D., et al. (1991). Obstet- et al. (1997). The inv dup(15) syndrome: A
ric and neurodevelopmental data from the clinically recognizable syndrome with altered
British Twin Study of Autism. Psychiatric Ge- behavior, mental retardation, and epilepsy.
netics, 2, S7A /1. Neurology, 48(4), 1081–1086.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Bolton, P., Le Couteur, A., Bauer, S. (1995). Autism and the pervasive devel-
Rutter, M., & Harding, B. (1993). Autism and opmental disorders: Part 1. Pediatrics in Re-
megalencephaly. Lancet, 341, 1225–1226. view, 16(4), 130–136.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Jan- Bauman, M. L. (1992). Neuropathology of autism.
ota, I., Montgomery, M., et al. (1998). A clini- In A. B. Joseph & R. R. Young (Eds.), Move-
copathological study of autism. Brain, 121, ment disorders in neurology and psychiatry
899–905. (pp. 664–668). Boston: Blackwell Scientific
Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Publications.
Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, ge- Bauman, M. L. (1996). Brief report—Neu-
netic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological roanatomic observations of the brain in perva-
Medical Aspects of Autism 559

sive developmental disorders. Journal of Bernabei, P., Fenton, G., Fabrizi, A., Camaioni, L.,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(2), & Perucchini, P. (2003). Profiles of sensori-
199–203. motor development in children with autism
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1994). Neuro- and with developmental delay. Perceptual &
anatomic observations of the brain in autism. Motor Skills, 96(3, Pt. 2), 1107–1116.
In M. L. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The Berney, T. P., Ireland, M., & Burn, J. (1999). Be-
neurobiology of autism (pp. 119–145). Balti- havioural phenotype of Cornelia de Lange syn-
more: Johns Hopkins University Press. drome. Archives of Disease in Childhood,
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (1997). Is autism 81(4), 333–336.
a progressive process? Neurology, 48(Suppl.), Berthier, M. L. (1994). Corticocallosal anomalies
A285. in Asperger’s syndrome [Letter]. American
Bearden, C. E., Woodin, M. F., Wang, P. P., Moss, E., Journal of Roentgenology, 162, 236–237.
McDonald-McGinn, D., Zackai, E., et al. Berthier, M. L., Starkstein, S. E., & Leiguarda, R.
(2001). The neurocognitive phenotype of the (1990). Developmental cortical anomalies in
22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Selective deficit in Asperger’s syndrome: Neuroradiological find-
visual-spatial memory. Journal of Clinical and ings in two patients. Journal of Neuropsychia-
Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(4), 447–464. try and Clinical Neurosciences, 2, 197–201.
Bell, R., Rankin, J., & Donaldson, L. J. (2003). Bertrand, J., Mervis, C. B., Rice, C. E., & Adam-
Down’s syndrome: Occurrence and outcome in son, L. (1993). Development of joint atten-
the north of England, 1985–99. Paediatric tion by a toddler with Williams syndrome.
Perinatal Epidemiology, 17(1), 33–39. Paper presented at the Gatlinburg Conference
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Neville, H., Doherty, S., & on Research and Theory in Mental Retarda-
Jernigan, T. (1992). Language, cognition, and tion and Developmental Disabilities, Gatlin-
brain organization in a neurodevelopmental burg, TN.
disorder. In M. R. Gunnar & C. A. Nelson Besag, F. M. (2004). Behavioral aspects of pedi-
(Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuro- atric epilepsy syndromes. Epilepsy & Behav-
science. The Minnesota symposia on child psy- ior, 5(Suppl. 1), S3–S13.
chology (Vol. 24, pp. 201–232). Hillsdale, NJ: Bescoby-Chambers, N., Forster, P., & Bates, G.
Erlbaum. (2001). Foetal valproate syndrome and autism:
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & Additional evidence of an association. Develop-
St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43(12),
profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pat- 847.
tern of strengths and weaknesses [Special Beuren, A. J., Apitz, J., & Harmjanz, D. (1962).
issue]. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Supravalvular aortic stenosis in association
12(Suppl. 1), 7–29. with mental retardation and a certain facial
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Gal- appearance. Circulation, 26, 1235–1240.
aburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridg- Black, C., Kaye, J. A., & Jick, H. (2002). Relation
ing cognition, the brain and molecular of childhood gastrointestinal disorders to
genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. autism: Nested case-control study using data
Trends in Neurosciences, 22(5), 197–207. from the U. K. General Practice Research
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A., & Sabo, H. Database. British Medical Journal, 325(7361),
(1988). Dissociation between language and 419–421.
cognitive functions in Williams syndrome. In Bliumina, M. G. (1975). A schizophrenia-like vari-
D. Bishop & K. Mogfont (Eds.), Language de- ant of phenylketonuria. Zhurnal nevopatologii i
velopment in exceptional circumstances psikhiatrii, 75(10), 1525–1529.
(pp. 177–189). London: Churchill Livingstone. Blomquist, H., Bohman, M., Edvinsson, S., Gill-
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., & Jernigan, T. L. (1994). berg, C., Gustavson, K., Holmgren, G., et al.
Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsycho- (1985). Frequency of the fragile X syndrome
logical profile. In S. H. Broman & J. Grafman in infantile autism. A Swedish multicenter
(Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in develop- study. Clinical Genetics, 27(2), 113–117.
mental disorders: Implications for brain func- Bolton, P., Dennis, N. R., Browne, C. E., Thomas,
tion (pp. 23–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. N. S., Veltman, M. W., Thompson, R. J., et al.
Bennetto, L., & Pennington, B. F. (2002). Neu- (2001). The phenotypic manifestations of in-
ropsychology. In R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hager- terstitial duplications of proximal 15q with
man (Eds.), Fragile X syndrome: Diagnosis, special reference to the autistic spectrum dis-
treatment and research (pp. 3–109). Balti- orders. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
more: Johns Hopkins University Press. 105(8), 675–685.
560 Neurological and Medical Issues

Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Murphy, M., Scott, S., Browne, C. E., Dennis, N. R., Maher, E., Long,
Yuzda, E., Whitlock, B., et al. (1991). Genetic F. L., Nicholson, J. C., Sillibourne, J., et al.
findings and heterogeneity in autism. Psychi- (1997). Inherited interstitial duplications of
atric Genetics, 2, S7A /2. proximal 15q: Genotype-phenotype correla-
Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., tions. American Journal of Human Genetics,
Goode, S., Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case- 61(6), 1342–1352.
control family history study of autism. Journal Bryson, S. E., Smith, I. M., & Eastwood, D. (1988).
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(5), Obstetrical suboptimality in autistic children.
877–900. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Bolton, P., Murphy, M., Macdonald, H., & Whit- Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 418–422.
lock, B. (1997). Obstetric complications in Bundey, S., Hardy, C., Vickers, S., Kilpatrick,
autism: Consequences or causes of the condi- M. W., & Corbett, J. A. (1994). Duplication of
tion? Journal of the American Academy of the 15q11–13 region in a patient with autism,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(2), epilepsy and ataxia. Developmental Medicine
272–281. and Child Neurology, 36, 736–742.
Bolton, P., Murphy, M., Macdonald, H., Whitlock, Bush, A., & Beail, N. (2004). Risk factors for de-
B., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (1997). Obstetric mentia in people with down syndrome: Issues
complications in autism: Consequences or in assessment and diagnosis. American Journal
causes of the condition? Journal of the Ameri- of Mental Retardation, 109(2), 83–97.
can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia- Carey, T., Ratliff-Schaub, K., Funk, J., Weinle, C.,
try, 36, 272–281. Myers, M., & Jenks, J. (2002). Double-blind
Borgatti, R., Piccinelli, P., Passoni, D., Dalpra, L., placebo-controlled trial of secretin: Effects on
Miozzo, M., Micheli, R., et al. (2001). Rela- aberrant behavior in children with autism.
tionship between clinical and genetic features Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
in “inverted duplicated chromosome 15” pa- ders, 32(3), 161–167.
tients. Pediatric Neurology, 24(2), 111–116. Cass, H. (1998). Visual impairment and autism.
Borghgraef, M., Fryns, J., Dielkens, A., Pyck, K., Autism: Journal of Research and Practice, 2(2),
& Van den Berghe, H. (1987). Fragile (X) syn- 117–138.
drome: A study of the psychological profile in Cass, H., Sonksen, P. M., & McConachie, H. R.
23 prepubertal patients. Clinical Genetics, (1994). Developmental setback in severe vi-
32(3), 179–186. sual impairment. Archives of Diseases in Child-
Bower, B. D., & Jeavons, P. M. (1967). The “ happy hood, 70(3), 192–196.
puppet ” syndrome. Archives of Diseases in Cassidy, S. B., Dykens, E., & Williams, C. A.
Childhood, 42(223), 298–302. (2000). Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes:
Bowers, L. (2002). An audit of referrals of children Sister imprinted disorders. American Journal
with autistic spectrum disorder to the dietetic of Medical Genetics, 97(2), 136–146.
service. Journal of Human Nutrition and Diet, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1997,
15(2), 141–144. November). Screening young children for lead
Boyle, C. A., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Doernberg, poisoning: Guidance for state and local public
N. S., Holmgreen, P., Murphy, C. C., & Schen- health of ficials. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Dis-
del, D. E. (1996). Prevalence of selected devel- ease Control and Prevention-National Center
opmental disabilities in children 3–10 years of for Environmental Health.
age: The Metropolitan Atlanta Developmental Challman, T. D., Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., &
Disabilities Surveillance Program, 1991. MMWR Weaver, A. (2003). The yield of the medical
Surveillance Summaries, 45(SS-2), 1–14. evaluation of children with pervasive develop-
Braffman, B., & Naidich, T. P. (1994). The phako- mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel-
matoses: Part I. Neurofibromatosis and tuber- opmental Disorders, 33(2), 187–192.
ous sclerosis. Neuroimaging Clinics of North Chandler, K. E., Moffett, M., Clayton-Smith, J., &
America, 4, 299–324. Baker, G. A. (2003). Neuropsychological as-
Bregman, J., & Volkmar, F. (1988). Autistic social sessment of a group of UK patients with Cohen
dysfunction and Down syndrome. Journal of syndrome. Neuropediatrics, 34(1), 7–13.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Chase, J. (1972). Retrolental fibroplasia and autis-
Psychiatry, 18(3), 343–354. tic symptomatology. New York: American
Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. Foundation for the Blind.
(1997). Are there “autistic-like” features in Cheng, S. D., Spinner, N. B., Zackai, E. H., &
congenitally blind children? Journal of Child Knoll, J. H. (1994). Cytogenetic and molecular
Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(6), 693–703. characterization of inverted duplicated chro-
Medical Aspects of Autism 561

mosomes 15 from 11 patients. American Jour- controlled trial of single-dose intravenous se-
nal of Human Genetics, 55(4), 753–759. cretin as treatment for children with autism.
Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children with congeni- Journal of Pediatrics, 138(5), 649–655.
tal rubella. Journal of Autism and Childhood Cook, E. H., Jr., Lindgren, V., Leventhal, B. L.,
Schizophrenia, 1(1), 33–47. Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A., Shulman, C.,
Chessa, L., & Iannetti, P. (1986). Fetal valproate et al. (1997). Autism or atypical autism in ma-
syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genet- ternally but not paternally derived proximal
ics, 24(2), 381–382. 15q duplication. American Journal of Human
Chez, M. G., Buchanan, C. P., Bagan, B. T., Ham- Genetics, 60, 928–934.
mer, M. S., McCarthy, K. S., Ovrutskaya, I., Cook, E. H., Jr., Perry, B. D., Dawson, G., Wain-
et al. (2000). Secretin and autism: A two-part wright, M. S., & Leventhal, B. L. (1993). Re-
clinical investigation. Journal of Autism and ceptor inhibition by immunoglobulins: Specific
Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 87–94. inhibition by autistic children, their relatives,
Christianson, A. L., Chesler, N., & Kromberg, J. G. and control subjects. Journal of Autism and De-
(1994). Fetal valproate syndrome: Clinical and velopmental Disorders, 23, 67–78.
neuro-developmental features in two sibling Coppola, G., Sciscio, N., Russo, F., Caliendo, G., &
pairs. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu- Pascotto, A. (2001). Benign idiopathic partial
rology, 36(4), 361–369. seizures in the velocardiofacial syndrome: Re-
Chugani, D. C., Sundram, B. S., Behen, M., Lee, port of two cases. American Journal of Medical
M. L., & Moore, G. J. (1999). Evidence of al- Genetics, 103(2), 172–175.
tered energy metabolism in autistic children. Corbett, B., Khan, K., Czapansky-Beilman, D.,
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Brady, N., Dropik, P., Goldman, D. Z., et al.
Biological Psychiatry, 23(4), 635–641. (2001). A double-blind, placebo-controlled
Clark-Taylor, T., & Clark-Taylor, B. E. (2004). Is crossover study investigating the effect of
autism a disorder of fatty acid metabolism? porcine secretin in children with autism. Clin-
Possible dysfunction of mitochondrial beta- ical Pediatrics, 40(6), 327–331.
oxidation by long chain acyl-CoA dehydroge- Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N.
nase. Medical Hypotheses, 62(6), 970–975. (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the
Clayton-Smith, J., & Laan, L. (2003). Angelman first year of life in autism. Journal of the
syndrome: A review of the clinical and genetic American Medical Association, 290(3),
aspects. Journal of Medical Genetics, 40(2), 337–344.
87–95. Critchley, M., & Earl, C. J. (1932). Tuberous sclero-
Cohen, D. J., Donnellan, A. M., & Paul, R. (1987). sis and allied conditions. Brain, 55, 311–346.
Handbook of autism and pervasive developmen- Cryan, E., Byrne, M., O’Donovan, A., &
tal disorders. Oxford, England: Wiley. O’Callaghan, E. (1996). A case-control study
Cohen, D. J., Johnson, W. T., & Caparulo, B. K. of obstetric complications and later autistic
(1976). Pica and elevated blood lead level in disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
autistic and atypical children. American Jour- Disorders, 26, 453–460.
nal of Diseases of Children, 130, 47–48. Curatolo, P., Cusmai, R., Cortesi, F., Chiron, C.,
Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Handbook of Jambaque, I., & Dulac, O. (1991). Neuropsy-
Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders chiatric aspects of tuberous sclerosis. Annals
(2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. of the New York Academy of Sciences, 615,
Cohen, D. J., Young, J. G., Lowe, T. L., & 8–16.
Harcherik, D. (1980). Thyroid hormone in Curatolo, P., Verdecchia, M., & Bombardieri, R.
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Devel- (2002). Tuberous sclerosis complex: A review
opmental Disorders, 10(4), 445–450. of neurological aspects. European Journal of
Coleman, M., & Blass, J. P. (1985). Autism and lac- Paediatric Neurology, 6(1), 15–23.
tic acidosis. Journal of Autism and Develop- Curry, C. J., Stevenson, R. E., Aughton, D., Byrne,
mental Disorders, 15(1), 1–8. J., Carey, J. C., Cassidy, S., et al. (1997). Eval-
Collacott, R., Cooper, S., & McGrother, C. (1992). uation of mental retardation: Recommenda-
Differential rates of psychiatric disorders in tions of a Consensus Conference: American
adults with Down’s syndrome compared with College of Medical Genetics. American Jour-
other mentally handicapped adults. British nal of Medical Genetics, 72(4), 468–477.
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 671–674. Davidovitch, M., Patterson, B., & Gartside, P.
Coniglio, S. J., Lewis, J. D., Lang, C., Burns, T. G., (1996). Head circumference measurements in
Subhani-Siddique, R., Weintraub, A., et al. children with autism. Journal of Child Neurol-
(2001). A randomized, double-blind, placebo- ogy, 11, 389–393.
562 Neurological and Medical Issues

DeFelice, M. L., Ruchelli, E. D., Markowitz, J. E., Down, J. L. (1990). On some of the mental af fecta-
Strogatz, M., Reddy, K. P., Kadivar, K., et al. tions of childhood and youth. Oxford, England:
(2003). Intestinal cytokines in children with MacKeith Press. (Original work published
pervasive developmental disorders. American 1887)
Journal of Gastroenterology, 98(8), 1777–1782. Dunn-Geier, J., Ho, H. H., Auersperg, E., Doyle,
D’Eufemia, P., Celli, M., Finocchiaro, R., Pacifico, D., Eaves, L., Matsuba, C., et al. (2000). Ef-
L., Viozzi, L., Zaccagnini, M., et al. (1996). fect of secretin on children with autism: A
Abnormal intestinal permeability in children randomized controlled trial. Developmental
with autism. Acta Paediatricaica, 85(9), Medicine and Child Neurology, 42(12),
1076–1079. 796–802.
de Vries, B. B., Wiegers, A. M., Smits, A. P., Dykens, E. M., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Medical
Mohkamsing, S., Duivenvoorden, H. J., Fryns, conditions associated with autism. In D. J.
J. P., et al. (1996). Mental status of females Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of
with an FMR1 gene full mutation. American autism and pervasive developmental disorders
Journal of Human Genetics, 58(5), 1025–1032. (2nd ed., pp. 388–410). New York: Wiley.
de Vries, P. J., & Bolton, P. F. (2000). Genotype- Einfeld, S., Molony, H., & Hall, W. (1989). Autism
phenotype correlations in tuberous sclerosis. is not associated with the fragile X syndrome.
Journal of Medical Genetics, 37(5), E3. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 34(2),
Deykin, E. Y., & MacMahon, B. (1980). Pregnancy, 187–193.
delivery, and neonatal complications among Einfeld, S., Tonge, B. J., & Florio, T. (1997). Be-
autistic children. American Journal of Diseases havioral and emotional disturbance in indi-
of Children, 134(9), 860–864. viduals with Williams syndrome. American
Didde, R., & Sigafoos, J. (2001). A review of the Journal on Mental Retardation, 102(1),
nature and treatment of sleep disorders in in- 45–53.
dividuals with developmental disabilities. Re- Ek, U., Fernell, E., Jacobson, L., & Gillberg, C.
search in Developmental Disabilities, 22(4), (1998). Relation between blindness due to
255–272. retinopathy of prematurity and autistic spec-
DiLiberti, J. H., Farndon, P. A., Dennis, N. R., & trum disorders: A population-based study. De-
Curry, C. J. (1984). The fetal valproate syn- velopmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 40(5), 297–301.
19(3), 473–481. El Abd, S., Patton, M. A., Turk, J., Hoey, H., &
Diomedi, M., Curatolo, P., Scalise, A., Placidi, F., Howlin, P. (1999). Social, communicational,
Caretto, F., & Gigli, G. L. (1999). Sleep abnor- and behavioral deficits associated with ring X
malities in mentally retarded autistic subjects: turner syndrome. American Journal of Medical
Down’s syndrome with mental retardation and Genetics, 88(5), 510–516.
normal subjects. Brain and Development, Elia, M., Ferri, R., Musumeci, S. A., Del Gracco,
21(8), 548–553. S., Bottitta, M., Scuderi, C., et al. (2000).
Domachowske, J. B., Cunningham, C. K., Cummings, Sleep in subjects with autistic disorder: A
D. L., Crosley, C. J., Hannan, W. P., & Weiner, neurophysiological and psychological study.
L. B. (1996). Acute manifestations and neuro- Brain and Development, 22(2), 88–92.
logic sequelae of Epstein-Barr virus encephali- Estecio, M. R. H., Fett-Conte, A. C., Varella-Garcia,
tis in children [see comments]. Journal of M., Fridman, C., & Silva, E. (2002). Molecular
Pediatric Infectious Disease, 15(10), 871–875. and cytogenetic analyses on Brazilian youths
Dombrowski, C., Levesque, S., Morel, M. L., Rouil- with pervasive developmental disorders. Jour-
lard, P., Morgan, K., & Rousseau, F. (2002). nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Premutation and intermediate-size FMR1 alle- 32(1), 35–41.
les in 10572 males from the general popula- Ewing, K. M., & Jones, T. W. (2003). An educa-
tion: Loss of an AGG interruption is a late tional rationale for deaf students with multiple
event in the generation of fragile X syndrome disabilities. American Annals of the Deaf,
alleles. Human Molecular Genetics, 11(4), 148(3), 267–271.
371–378. Fantes, J. A., Mewborn, S. K., Lese, C. M.,
Donnelly, S. L., Wolpert, C. M., Menold, M. M., Hedrick, J., Brown, R. L., Dyomin, V., et al.
Bass, M. P., Gilbert, J. R., Cuccaro, M. L., (2002). Organisation of the pericentromeric
et al. (2000). Female with autistic disorder and region of chromosome 15: At least four partial
monosomy X (Turner syndrome): Parent-of- gene copies are amplified in patients with a
origin effect of the X chromosome. American proximal duplication of 15q. Journal of Med-
Journal of Medical Genetics, 96(3), 312–316. ical Genetics, 39(3), 170–177.
Medical Aspects of Autism 563

Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Green, et al. (1992). Morphometric analysis of the
L., Morris, R., et al. (1997). Pitocin induction brain in developmental language disorders and
and autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, autism. Annals of Neurology, 32, 475.
154, 438–439. Filipek, P. A., Smith, M., Gargus, J. J., Bocian, M.,
Feinstein, C., & Reiss, A. L. (1998). Autism: The Masser-Frye, D. S., & Spence, M. A. (2000).
point of view from fragile X studies. Journal of Evidence of mitochondrial dysfunction in
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(5), autistic patients with 15q inv dup [Abstract].
393–405. Annals of Neurology, 48, 542.
Fernell, E., Olsson, V. A., Karlgren-Leitner, C., Findling, R. L., Maxwell, K., Scotese-Wojtila, L.,
Norlin, B., Hagberg, B., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Huang, J., Yamashita, T., & Wiznitzer, M.
Autistic disorders in children with CHARGE (1997). High-dose pyridoxine and magnesium
association. Developmental Medicine and administration in children with autistic disor-
Child Neurology, 41(4), 270–272. der: An absence of salutary effects in a
Fidler, D. J., Bailey, J. N., & Smalley, S. L. (2000). double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Jour-
Macrocephaly in autism and other perva- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
sive developmental disorders. Developmental 27(4), 467–478.
Medicine and Child Neurology, 42(11), Finegan, J.-A., & Quarrington, B. (1979). Pre-,
737–740. peri- and neonatal factors and infantile
Field, D., Garland, M., & Williams, K. (2003). autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Correlates of specific childhood feeding prob- chiatry, 20, 119–128.
lems. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, Finegold, S. M., Molitoris, D., Song, Y., Liu, C.,
39(4), 299–304. Vaisanen, M. L., Bolte, E., et al. (2002). Gas-
Filiano, J. J., Goldenthal, M. J., Rhodes, H., & trointestinal microf lora studies in late-onset
Marin-Garcia, J. (2002). Mitochondrial dys- autism. Clinical Infection and Disease,
function in patients with hypotonia, epilepsy, 35(Suppl. 1), S6–S16.
autism and developmental delay: HEADD Fisch, G. (1992). Is autism associated with the
Syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, fragile X syndrome? American Journal of Med-
435–439. ical Genetics, 43(1/2), 47–55.
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, Fisch, G., Cohen, I., Jenkins, E., & Brown, W.
G. T., Cook Jr, E. H., Dawson, G., et al. (1988). Screening developmentally disabled
(2000). Practice parameter: Screening and di- male populations for fragile X: The effect of
agnosis of autism: Report of the Quality Stan- sample size. American Journal of Medical Ge-
dards Subcommittee of the American netics, 30(1/2), 655–663.
Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurol- Fitzpatrick, T. B. (1991). History and significance
ogy Society. Neurology, 55(4), 468–479. of white macules, earliest visible sign of
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook tuberous sclerosis. Annals of the New York
Jr, E. H., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., et al. Academy of Science, 615, 26.
(1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic Flejter, W. L., Bennett-Baker, P. E., Ghaziuddin,
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and De- M., McDonald, M., Sheldon, S., & Gorski,
velopmental Disorders, 29, 437–482. J. L. (1996). Cytogenetic and molecular analy-
Filipek, P. A., Juranek, J., Nguyen, M. T., Cum- sis of inv dup(15) chromosomes observed in
mings, C., & Gargus, J. J. (in press). Relative two patients with autistic disorder and mental
carnitine deficiency in autism. Journal of retardation. American Journal of Medical Ge-
Autism and Developmental Disorders. netics, 61, 182–187.
Filipek, P. A., Juranek, J., Smith, M., Mays, L. Z., Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977a). Genetic inf lu-
Ramos, E. R., Bocian, M., et al. (2003). Mito- ences and infantile autism. Nature, 265(5596),
chondrial dysfunction in autistic patients with 726–728.
15q inverted duplication. Annals of Neurology, Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977b). Infantile
53(6), 801–804. autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Jour-
Filipek, P. A., Kennedy, D. N., & Caviness, V. S., nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(4),
Jr. (1992). Neuroimaging in child neuropsy- 297–321.
chology. In I. Rapin & S. Segalowitz (Eds.), Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1988). Autism: Familial
Handbook of neuropsychology: Vol. 6 Child aggregation and genetic implications. Journal
neuropsychology (pp. 301–329). Amsterdam: of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(1),
Elsevier Science. 3–30.
Filipek, P. A., Richelme, C., Kennedy, D. N., Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of
Rademacher, J., Pitcher, D. A., Zidel, S. Y., autism and other pervasive developmental
564 Neurological and Medical Issues

disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and ence to Down’s syndrome. British Journal of
Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 365–382. Psychiatry, 149, 156–161.
Fombonne, E., & Chakrabarti, S. (2001). No evi- Gense, M. H., & Gense, D. J. (1994). Identifying
dence for a new variant of measles-mumps- autism in children with blindness and visual
rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics, 108(4), E58. impairments. Review, 26(2), 55–62.
Fombonne, E., & du Mazaubrun, C. (1992). Preva- Gentile, P. S., Trentalange, M. J., Zamichek, W., &
lence of infantile autism in four French re- Coleman, M. (1983). Brief report: Trace ele-
gions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric ments in the hair of autistic and control chil-
Epidemiology, 27(4), 203–210. dren. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Fombonne, E., du Mazaubrun, C., Cans, C., & Disorders, 13(2), 205–206.
Grandjean, H. (1997). Autism and associated Ghaziuddin, M. (1997). Autism in Down’s syn-
medical disorders in a French epidemiological drome: Family history correlates. Journal of In-
survey. Journal of the American Academy of tellectual Disability Research, 41(Pt. 1), 87–91.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(11), Ghaziuddin, M. (2000). Autism in Down’s syn-
1561–1569. drome: A family history study. Journal of Intel-
Fombonne, E., Roge, B., Claverie, J., Courty, S., & lectual Disability Research, 44(Pt. 5), 562–566.
Fremolle, J. (1999). Microcephaly and macro- Ghaziuddin, M., Al-Khouri, I., & Ghaziuddin, N.
cephaly in autism. Journal of Autism and De- (2002). Autistic symptoms following herpes
velopmental Disorders, 29(2), 113–119. encephalitis. European Child & Adolescent
Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the blind. Lon- Psychiatry, 11(3), 142–146.
don: Souvenir. Ghaziuddin, M., & Burmeister, M. (1999). Deletion
Fraiberg, S., & Freedman, D. (1964). Studies in the of chromosome 2q37 and autism: A distinct
ego development of the congenitally blind. subtype? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 19, Disorders, 29(3), 259–263.
113–169. Ghaziuddin, M., Shakal, J., & Tsai, L. (1995). Ob-
Friedman, E. (1969). The “autistic syndrome” and stetric factors in Asperger syndrome: Compar-
phenylketonuria. Schizophrenia, 1, 249–261. ison with high-functioning autism. Journal of
Fryns, J., Jacobs, J., Kleczkowska, A., & Van den Intellectual Disability Research, 39(Pt. 6),
Berghe, H. (1984). The psychological profile 538–543.
of the fragile X syndrome. Clinical Genetics, Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., Eilers, L., & Ghaziud-
25(2), 131–134. din, N. (1992). Brief report: Autism and her-
Furlano, R. I., Anthony, A., Day, R., Brown, A., pes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Autism
McGarvey, L., Thomson, M. A., et al. (2001). and Developmental Disorders, 22(1), 107–113.
Colonic CD8 and gamma delta T-cell infiltra- Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1992).
tion with epithelial damage in children with Autism in Down’s syndrome: Presentation and
autism. Journal of Pediatrics, 138(3), 366–372. diagnosis. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re-
Gale, S., Ozonoff, S., & Lainhart, J. (2003). Brief search, 36(5), 449–456.
report: Pitocin induction in autistic and Ghaziuddin, M., Zaccagnini, J., Tsai, L., & Elardo,
nonautistic individuals. Journal of Autism and S. (1999). Is megalencephaly specific to
Developmental Disorders, 33(2), 205–208. autism? Journal of Intellectual Disability Re-
Ganiban, J. M., Wagner, S., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). search, 43(Pt. 4), 279–282.
Temperament and Down syndrome. In D. Cic- Giannini, A. J., Tamulonis, D., Giannini, M. C.,
chetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Loiselle, R. H., & Spirtos, G. (1984). Defec-
Down syndrome: A developmental perspective tive response to social cues in Mobius’ syn-
(pp. 63–100). New York: Cambridge Univer- drome. Journal of Nervous Mental Disorders,
sity Press. 172(3), 174–175.
Gargus, J. J., & Filipek, P. A. (n.d.). Diagnosis of Gibbs, M., & Thorpe, J. (1983). Personality stereo-
PKU in a young autistic child secondary to type of noninstitutionalized Down syndrome
routine screening of a newborn sibling. Unpub- children. American Journal of Mental Defi-
lished observation. ciency, 87(6), 601–605.
Garreau, B., Barthelemy, C., & Sauvage, D. (1984). Gillberg, C. (1986). Onset at age 14 of a typical
A comparison of autistic symptoms with and autistic syndrome. A case report of a girl with
without associated neurological problems. herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Autism
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 369–375.
ders, 14, 105–111. Gillberg, C. (1991). The treatment of epilepsy in
Gath, A., & Gumley, D. (1986). Behaviour prob- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
lems in retarded children with special refer- Disorders, 21(1), 61–77.
Medical Aspects of Autism 565

Gillberg, C., & Billstedt, E. (2000). Autism and cardiofacial syndrome (deletion 22q11.2).
Asperger syndrome: Coexistence with other Journal of Pediatrics, 140(6), 753–758.
clinical disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandi- Goldenberg, A., Chevy, F., Bernard, C., Wolf, C., &
navica, 102(5), 321–330. Cormier-Daire, V. (2003). Clinical character-
Gillberg, C., & de Souza, L. (2002). Head circum- istics and diagnosis of Smith-Lemli-Opitz
ference in autism, Asperger syndrome, and syndrome and tentative phenotype-genotype
ADHD: A comparative study. Developmental correlation: Report of 45 cases. Archives of
Medicine and Child Neurology, 44(5), 296–300. Pediatrics, 10(1), 4–10.
Gillberg, C., & Forsell, C. (1984). Childhood psy- Gomez, M. R., Sampson, J., & Whittemore, V.
chosis and neurofibromatosis: More than a (1999). The tuberous sclerosis complex. Ox-
coincidence? Journal of Autism and Develop- ford, England: Oxford University Press.
mental Disorders, 14(1), 1–8. Gonzalez, C. H., Marques-Dias, M. J., Kim, C. A.,
Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I. C. (1983). Infantile Sugayama, S. M., Da Paz, J. A., Huson, S. M.,
autism: A total population study of reduced et al. (1998). Congenital abnormalities in
optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal pe- Brazilian children associated with misoprostol
riod. Journal of Autism and Developmental misuse in first trimester of pregnancy. Lancet,
Disorders, 13, 153–166. 351(9116), 1624–1627.
Gillberg, C., Persson, E., & Wahlstrom, J. (1986). Goodwin, M. S., Cowen, M. A., & Goodwin, T. C.
The autism-fragile-X syndrome (AFRAX): A (1971). Malabsorption and cerebral dysfunc-
population-based study of ten boys. Journal tion: A multivariate and comparative study of
of Mental Deficiency Research, 30(Pt. 1), autistic children. Journal of Autism and Child-
27–39. hood Schizophrenia, 1(1), 48–62.
Gillberg, C., & Rasmussen, P. (1994). Brief report: Gordon, A. G. (1991). Co-occurrence of deafness
Four case histories and a literature review of and infantile autism. American Journal of Psy-
Williams syndrome and autistic behavior. chiatry, 148(11), 1615.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Gothelf, D., & Lombroso, P. J. (2001). Genetics of
ders, 24(3), 381–393. childhood disorders: XXV. Velocardiofacial
Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, S. (1989). Autistic be- syndrome. Journal of the American Academy
haviour in Moebius syndrome. Acta Paediatri- of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(4),
caica Scandinavica, 78(2), 314–316. 489–491.
Gillberg, C., Steffenburg, S., & Schaumann, H. Gothelf, D., Presburger, G., Levy, D., Nahmani, A.,
(1991). Is autism more common now than ten Burg, M., Berant, M., et al. (2004). Genetic,
years ago? British Journal of Psychiatry, 158, developmental, and physical factors associated
403–409. with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in
Gillberg, C., Steffenburg, S., Wahlstrom, J., Gill- patients with velocardiofacial syndrome. Amer-
berg, I. C., Sjostedt, A., Martinsson, T., et al. ican Journal of Medical Genetics, 126B(1),
(1991). Autism associated with marker chromo- 116–121.
some. Journal of the American Academy of Child Gothelf, D., Presburger, G., Zohar, A. H., Burg, M.,
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(3), 489–494. Nahmani, A., Frydman, M., et al. (2004).
Gillberg, C., & Wahlstrom, J. (1985). Chromosome Obsessive-compulsive disorder in patients
abnormalities in infantile autism and other with velocardiofacial (22q11 deletion) syn-
childhood psychoses: A population study of 66 drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
cases. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu- 126B(1), 99–105.
rology, 27(3), 293–304. Graf, W. D., Marin-Garcia, J., Gao, H. G., Pizzo,
Gillberg, C., & Winnergard, I. (1984). Childhood S., Naviaux, R. K., Markusic, D., et al. (2000).
psychosis in a case of Moebius syndrome. Neu- Autism associated with the mitochondrial
ropediatrics, 15(3), 147–149. DNA G8363A transfer RNA(Lys) mutation.
Gillberg, I., Gillberg, C., & Ahlsen, G. (1994). Journal of Child Neurology, 15(6), 357–361.
Autistic behaviour and attention deficits in Grice, S. J., Spratling, M. W., Karmiloff-Smith,
tuberous sclerosis: A population-based study. A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., de Haan, M., et al.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, (2001). Disordered visual processing and os-
36(1), 50–56. cillatory brain activity in autism and
Gilliam, J. E. (1995). Gilliam Autism Rating Scale Williams syndrome. Neuroreport: For Rapid
(GARS). Austin, TX: ProEd. Communication of Neuroscience Research,
Glaser, B., Mumme, D. L., Blasey, C., Morris, 12(12), 2697–2700.
M. A., Dahoun, S. P., Antonarakis, S. E., et al. Grizenko, N., Cvejic, H., Vida, S., & Sayegh, L.
(2002). Language skills in children with velo- (1991). Behaviour problems of the mentally
566 Neurological and Medical Issues

retarded. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Hering, E., Epstein, R., Elroy, S., Iancu, D. R., &
36(10), 712–717. Zelnik, N. (1999). Sleep patterns in autistic
Gurrieri, F., Battaglia, A., Torrisi, L., Tancredi, children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
R., Cavallaro, C., Sangiorgi, E., et al. (1999). Disorders, 29(2), 143–147.
Pervasive developmental disorder and Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (2003). The pathogen-
epilepsy due to maternally derived duplica- esis of autism: Insights from congenital blind-
tion of 15q11-q13. Neurology, 52(8), ness. Philosophical transactions of the Royal
1694–1697. Society of London. Series B, Biological sci-
Gutierrez, G. C., Smalley, S. L., & Tanguay, P. E. ences, 358(1430), 335–344.
(1998). Autism in tuberous sclerosis complex. Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism
ders, 28(2), 97–103. and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 45–56.
Hagberg, B., Aicardi, J., Dias, K., & Ramos, O. Honomichl, R. D., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., Burnham,
(1983). A progressive syndrome of autism, de- M., Gaylor, E., & Anders, T. F. (2002). Sleep
mentia, ataxia, and loss of purposeful hand patterns of children with pervasive develop-
use in girls: Rett’s syndrome: Report of 35 mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel-
cases. Annals of Neurology, 14(4), 471–479. opmental Disorders, 32(6), 553–561.
Hagerman, R. (2002). The physical and behavioral Hook, E. (1982). The epidemiology of Down syn-
phenotype. In R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hager- drome. In S. M. Pueschel & J. Rynders (Eds.),
man (Eds.), Fragile X syndrome: Diagnosis, Down syndrome: Advances in biomedicine and
treatment and research (pp. 3–109). Balti- the behavioral sciences. Cambridge, MA: Ware
more: Johns Hopkins University Press. Press.
Hagerman, R., Jackson, A., Levitas, A., Rimland, Hornig, M., & Lipkin, W. I. (2001). Infectious and
B., & Braden, M. (1986). An analysis of immune factors in the pathogenesis of neurode-
autism in fifty males with the fragile X syn- velopmental disorders: Epidemiology, hypothe-
drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, ses, and animal models. Mental Retardation
23(1/2), 359–374. and Developmental Disabilities Research Re-
Hagerman, R., Leavitt, B. R., Farzin, F., Jacque- view, 7(3), 200–210.
mont, S., Greco, C. M., Brunberg, J. A., et al. Horvath, K., Papadimitriou, J. C., Rabsztyn, A.,
(2004). Fragile-X-associated tremor/ataxia Drachenberg, C., & Tildon, J. T. (1999). Gas-
syndrome (FXTAS) in females with the FMR1 trointestinal abnormalities in children with
premutation. American Journal of Human Ge- autistic disorder. Journal of Pediatrics, 135(5),
netics, 74(5), 1051–1056. 559–563.
Hagerman, R., Leehey, M., Heinrichs, W., Tassone, Horvath, K., & Perman, J. A. (2002a). Autism and
F., Wilson, R., Hills, J., et al. (2001). Intention gastrointestinal symptoms. Current Gastroen-
tremor, parkinsonism, and generalized brain terology Reports, 4(3), 251–258.
atrophy in male carriers of fragile X. Neurol- Horvath, K., & Perman, J. A. (2002b). Autistic dis-
ogy, 57(1), 127–130. order and gastrointestinal disease. Current
Harrison, J. E., & Bolton, P. F. (1997). Annotation: Opinion in Pediatrics, 14(5), 583–587.
Tuberous sclerosis. Journal of Child Psychol- Horvath, K., Stefanatos, G., Sokolski, K. N., Wach-
ogy and Psychiatry, 38, 603–614. tel, R., Nabors, L., & Tildon, J. T. (1998). Im-
Harvey, M. T., & Kennedy, C. H. (2002). proved social and language skills after secretin
Polysomnographic phenotypes in developmental administration in patients with autistic spec-
disabilities [Special issue]. International Jour- trum disorders. Journal of the Association of
nal of Developmental Neuroscience, 20(3/5), Academic Minority Physicians, 9(1), 9–15.
443–448. Hoshino, Y., Watanabe, H., Yashima, Y., Kaneko,
Hashimoto, O., Shimizu, Y., & Kawasaki, Y. M., & Kumashiro, H. (1984). An investigation
(1993). Brief report: Low frequency of the on sleep disturbance of autistic children. Folia
fragile X syndrome among Japanese autistic psychiatrica et neurologica japonica, 38(1),
subjects. Journal of Autism and Developmental 45–51.
Disorders, 23, 201–209. Hotopf, M., & Bolton, P. (1995). A case of autism
Hashimoto, T., Aihara, R., Tayama, M., Miyazaki, associated with partial tetrasomy 15. Journal
M., Shirakawa, Y., & Kuroda, Y. (1991). Re- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25,
duced thyroid-stimulating hormone response 41–49.
to thyrotropin-releasing hormone in autistic Hou, J. W., & Wang, T. R. (1998). Unusual features
boys. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu- in children with inv dup(15) supernumerary
rology, 33(4), 313–319. marker: A study of genotype-phenotype corre-
Medical Aspects of Autism 567

lation in Taiwan. European Journal of Pedi- et al. (1999). Comprehensive mutation analy-
atrics, 157(2), 122–127. sis of TSC1 and TSC2-and phenotypic correla-
Howlin, P., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1995). The recog- tions in 150 families with tuberous sclerosis.
nition of autism in children with Down syn- American Journal of Human Genetics, 64(5),
drome—implications for intervention and some 1305–1315.
speculations about pathology. Developmental Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly,
Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 406–414. J., Lincoln, A., et al. (2000). II. Hypersociabil-
Hultman, C. M., Sparen, P., & Cnattingius, S. ity in Williams syndrome [Special issue].
(2002). Perinatal risk factors for infantile Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.
autism. Epidemiology, 13(4), 417–423. 1), 30–46.
Hunt, A., & Shepherd, C. (1993). A prevalence Jorde, L. B., Hasstedt, S. J., Ritvo, E. R., Mason-
study of autism in tuberous sclerosis. Journal Brothers, A., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., et al.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, (1991). Complex segregation analysis of
323–339. autism. American Journal of Human Genetics,
Iliyasu, Z., Gilmour, W. H., & Stone, D. H. (2002). 49, 932–938.
Prevalence of Down syndrome in Glasgow, Jure, R., Rapin, I., & Tuchman, R. F. (1991).
1980–96—The growing impact of prenatal di- Hearing-impaired autistic children. Develop-
agnosis on younger mothers. Health Bull, mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33(12),
60(1), 20–26. 1062–1072.
Inanuma, K. (1984). Sleep-wake patterns in autis- Juul-Dam, N., Townsend, J., & Courchesne, E.
tic children (Summary in English). Japanese (2001). Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, factors in autism, pervasive developmental
25, 205–217. disorder-not otherwise specified, and the gen-
Jacquemont, S., Hagerman, R. J., Leehey, M., eral population. Pediatrics, 107(4), E63.
Grigsby, J., Zhang, L., Brunberg, J. A., et al. Kandt, R. S. (2003). Tuberous sclerosis complex
(2003). Fragile X premutation tremor/ataxia and neurofibromatosis type 1: The two most
syndrome: Molecular, clinical, and neuroimag- common neurocutaneous diseases. Neurologic
ing correlates. American Journal of Human Clinics, 21, 983–1004.
Genetics, 72(4), 869–878. Kau, A. S., Tierney, E., Bukelis, I., Stump, M. H.,
Jacquemont, S., Hagerman, R. J., Leehey, M. A., Kates, W. R., Trescher, W. H., et al. (2004).
Hall, D. A., Levine, R. A., Brunberg, J. A., Social behavior profile in young males with
et al. (2004). Penetrance of the fragile X- fragile X syndrome: Characteristics and speci-
associated tremor/ataxia syndrome in a premu- ficity. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
tation carrier population. Journal of the Ameri- 126A(1), 9–17.
can Medical Association, 291(4), 460–469. Kawashima, H., Mori, T., Kashiwagi, Y.,
Janson, U. (1993). Normal and deviant behavior in Takekuma, K., Hoshika, A., & Wakefield, A.
blind children with ROP. Acta Ophthalmolog- (2000). Detection and sequencing of measles
ica (Suppl. 210), 20–26. virus from peripheral mononuclear cells from
Johansson, M., Wentz, E., Fernell, E., Stromland, patients with inf lammatory bowel disease and
K., Miller, M. T., & Gillberg, C. (2001). Autis- autism. Digestive Diseases and Sciences, 45(4),
tic spectrum disorders in Mobius sequence: A 723–729.
comprehensive study of 25 individuals. Devel- Keeler, W. R. (1958). Autistic patterns and defec-
opmental Medicine and Child Neurology, tive communication in blind children with
43(5), 338–345. retrolental fibroplasia. In H. P. Hoch & J.
Johnson, R. J., Wiersema, V., & Kraft, I. A. (1974). Zubin (Eds.), Psychopathology of communi-
Hair amino acids in childhood autism. Journal cation (pp. 64–83). New York: Rune and
of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4(2), Stratton.
187–188. Keller, K., Williams, C., Wharton, P., Paulk, M.,
Jones, A. C., Daniells, C. E., Snell, R. G., Bent-Williams, A., Gray, B., et al. (2003).
Tachataki, M., Idziaszczyk, S. A., Krawczak, Routine cytogenetic and FISH studies for
M., et al. (1997). Molecular genetic and phe- 17p11/15q11 duplications and subtelomeric
notypic analysis reveals differences between rearrangement studies in children with autism
TSC1 and TSC2 associated familial and spo- spectrum disorders. American Journal of Med-
radic tuberous sclerosis. Human Molecular Ge- ical Genetics, 117A(2), 105–111.
netics, 6, 2155–2161. Kern, J. K., Van Miller, S., Evans, P. A., &
Jones, A. C., Shyamsundar, M. M., Thomas, M. W., Trivedi, M. H. (2002). Efficacy of porcine se-
Maynard, J., Idziaszczyk, S., Tomkins, S., cretin in children with autism and pervasive
568 Neurological and Medical Issues

developmental disorder. Journal of Autism Kuddo, T., & Nelson, K. B. (2003). How common
and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), are gastrointestinal disorders in children with
153–160. autism? Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 15(3),
Kielinen, M., Rantala, H., Timonen, E., Linna, 339–343.
S. L., & Moilanen, I. (2004). Associated med- Kwiatkowski, D. J. (2003). Tuberous sclerosis:
ical disorders and disabilities in children with From tubers to mTOR. Annals of Human Ge-
autistic disorder: A population-based study. netics, 67(Pt. 1), 87–96.
Autism: Journal of Research and Practice, 8(1), Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsodl, M.,
49–60. Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., et al. (2002).
Kientz, M. A., & Dunn, W. (1997). A comparison of Atypical development of language and social
the performance of children with and without communication in toddlers with Williams syn-
autism on the Sensory Profile. American Jour- drome. Developmental Science, 5(2), 233–246.
nal of Occupational Therapy, 51, 530–537. Lainhart, J. E. (2003). Increased rate of head
Kishino, T., Lalande, M., & Wagstaff, J. (1997). growth during infancy in autism. Journal of
UBE3A / E6-AP mutations cause Angelman’s the American Medical Association, 290(3),
syndrome. Nature Genetics, 15, 70–73. 393–394.
Klauck, S. M., Munstermann, E., Bieber-Martig, Lainhart, J. E., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Landa, R.,
B., Ruhl, D., Lisch, S., Schmotzer, G., et al. Santangelo, S. L., Coon, H., et al. (1997).
(1997). Molecular genetic analysis of the Macrocephaly in children and adults with
FMR-1 gene in a large collection of autistic autism. Journal of the American Academy of
patients. Human Genetics, 100(2), 224–229. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 282–290.
Klug, M. G., Burd, L., Kerbeshian, J., Benz, B., & Landgren, M., Gillberg, C., & Stromland, K.
Martsolf, J. T. (2003). A comparison of the ef- (1992). Goldenhar syndrome and autistic be-
fects of parental risk markers on pre- and peri- haviour. Developmental Medicine and Child
natal variables in multiple patient cohorts with Neurology, 34(11), 999–1005.
fetal alcohol syndrome, autism, Tourette syn- LaPerchia, P. (1987). Behavioral disorders, learn-
drome, and sudden infant death syndrome: An ing disabilities and megavitamin therapy. Ado-
enviromic analysis. Neurotoxicol Teratol, 25(6), lescence, 22(87), 729–738.
707–717. Larrandaburu, M., Schuler, L., Ehlers, J. A., Reis,
Konstantareas, M. M., & Homatidis, S. (1999). A. M., & Silveira, E. L. (1999). The occur-
Chromosomal abnormalities in a series of chil- rence of Poland and Poland-Moebius syn-
dren with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism dromes in the same family: Further evidence
and Developmental Disorders, 29(4), 275–285. of their genetic component. Clinical Dysmor-
Kooy, R. F., Willemsen, R., & Oostra, B. A. (2000). phology, 8(2), 93–99.
Fragile X syndrome at the turn of the century. Larsen, J. (1998, October 7). Could new therapy
Molecular Medicine Today, 6(5), 193–198. cure autism? [Segment in television series.]
Korvatska, E., Van de Water, J., Anders, T. F., & Dateline, NBC Television.
Gershwin, M. E. (2002). Genetic and immuno- Laszlo, A., Horvath, E., Eck, E., & Fekete, M.
logic considerations in autism. Neurobiology of (1994). Serum serotonin, lactate and pyruvate
Disease, 9(2), 107–125. levels in infantile autistic children [Letter].
Kotsopoulos, S., & Kutty, K. M. (1979). Histidine- Clinical Chimica Acta, 229(1/2), 205–207.
mia and infantile autism. Journal of Autism Lauritsen, M., Mors, O., Mortensen, P. B., &
and Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 55–60. Ewald, H. (1999). Infantile autism and associ-
Kozma, C. (1998). On cognitive variability in velo- ated autosomal chromosome abnormalities: A
cardiofacial syndrome: Profound mental retar- register-based study and a literature survey.
dation and autism. American Journal of Medical Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Genetics, 81(3), 269–270. 40(3), 335–345.
Krause, I., He, X. S., Gershwin, M. E., & Shoen- Lauritsen, M., Mors, O., Mortensen, P. B., &
feld, Y. (2002). Brief report: Immune factors Ewald, H. (2002). Medical disorders among
in autism: A critical review. Journal of Autism inpatients with autism in Denmark according
and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 337–345. to ICD-8: a nationwide register-based study.
Kubba, H., MacAndie, C., Ritchie, K., & MacFar- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
lane, M. (2004). Is deafness a disease of ders, 32(2), 115–119.
poverty? The association between socio- Laws, G., & Bishop, D. (2004). Pragmatic language
economic deprivation and congenital hearing impairment and social deficits in Williams
impairment. International Journal of Audiol- syndrome: A comparison with Down’s syn-
ogy, 43(3), 123–125. drome and specific language impairment. In-
Medical Aspects of Autism 569

ternational Journal of Language and Commu- Loesch, D. Z., Huggins, R. M., & Hagerman, R. J.
nications Disorders, 39(1), 45–64. (2004). Phenotypic variation and FMRP levels
Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., in fragile X. Mental Retardation and Develop-
Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989). mental Disabilities Research Review, 10(1),
Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized 31–41.
investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism Lombard, J. (1998). Autism: A mitochondrial dis-
and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387. order? Medical Hypotheses, 50(6), 497–500.
Le Couteur, A., Trygstad, O., Evered, C., Gill- Longo, I., Russo, L., Meloni, I., Ricci, I., Ariani, F.,
berg, C., & Rutter, M. (1988). Infantile Pescucci, C., et al. (2004). Three Rett patients
autism and urinary excretion of peptides and with both MECP2 mutation and 15q11–13 re-
protein-associated peptide complexes. Jour- arrangements. European Journal of Human Ge-
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, netics, 12(8), 682–685.
18(2), 181–190. Lord, C. (1991). Methods and measures of behavior
Lerman-Sagie, T., Leshinsky-Silver, E., Watem- in the diagnosis of autism and related disor-
berg, N., & Lev, D. (2004). Should autistic ders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14,
children be evaluated for mitochondrial disor- 69–80.
ders? Journal of Child Neurology, 19(5), Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds re-
379–381. ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child
Levitt, J. G., O’Neill, J., Blanton, R. E., Smalley, Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1365–1382.
S., Fadale, D., McCracken, J. T., et al. (2003). Lord, C., Mulloy, C., Wendelboe, M., & Schopler,
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic E. (1991). Pre- and perinatal factors in high-
imaging of the brain in childhood autism. Bio- functioning females and males with autism.
logical Psychiatry, 54(12), 1355–1366. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Levy, S., Souders, M. C., Wray, J., Jawad, A. F., ders, 21(2), 197–209.
Gallagher, P. R., Coplan, J., et al. (2003). Chil- Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr.,
dren with autistic spectrum disorders. I: Com- Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000).
parison of placebo and single dose of human The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-
synthetic secretin. Archives of Disease in Generic: A standard measure of social and
Childhood, 88(8), 731–736. communication deficits associated with the
Levy, S., Zoltak, B., & Saelens, T. (1988). A com- spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and De-
parison of obstetrical records of autistic and velopmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.
nonautistic referrals for psychoeducational Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994).
evaluations. Journal of Autism and Develop- Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A re-
mental Disorders, 18(4), 573–581. vised version of a diagnostic interview for
Lewis, J. C., Thomas, H. V., Murphy, K. C., & caregivers of individuals with possible perva-
Sampson, J. R. (2004). Genotype and psycho- sive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism
logical phenotype in tuberous sclerosis. Jour- and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
nal of Medical Genetics, 41(3), 203–207. Lotter, V. (1967). Epidemiology of autistic condi-
Lightdale, J. R., Hayer, C., Duer, A., Lind-White, tions in young children: Some characteristics
C., Jenkins, S., Siegel, B., et al. (2001). Ef- of the children and their parents. Social Psy-
fects of intravenous secretin on language and chiatry, 1, 163–173.
behavior of children with autism and gastroin- Lowe, T. L., Tanaka, K., Seashore, M. R., Young,
testinal symptoms: A single-blinded, open- J. G., & Cohen, D. J. (1980). Detection of
label pilot study. Pediatrics, 108(5), E90. phenylketonuria in autistic and psychotic chil-
Lightdale, J. R., Siegel, B., & Heyman, M. B. dren. Journal of the American Medical Associ-
(2001). Gastrointestinal symptoms in autistic ation, 243(2), 126–128.
children. Clinical Perspectives in Gastroen- Lucarelli, S., Frediani, T., Zingoni, A. M., Fer-
terology, 1, 56–58. ruzzi, F., Giardini, O., Quintieri, F., et al.
Loening-Baucke, V. (1998). Constipation in chil- (1995). Food allergy and infantile autism.
dren. New England Journal of Medicine, Panminerva Medica, 37, 137–141.
339(16), 1155–1156. Ludowese, C. J., Thompson, K. J., Sekhon, G. S., &
Loesch, D. Z., Huggins, R. M., Bui, Q. M., Taylor, Pauli, R. M. (1991). Absence of predictable
A. K., Pratt, C., Epstein, J., et al. (2003). Ef- phenotypic expression in proximal 15q dupli-
fect of fragile X status categories and FMRP cations. Clinical Genetics, 40(3), 194–201.
deficits on cognitive profiles estimated by ro- Lund, J. (1988). Psychiatric aspects of Down’s syn-
bust pedigree analysis. American Journal of drome. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 78(3),
Medical Genetics, 122A(1), 13–23. 369–374.
570 Neurological and Medical Issues

Maddalena, A., Richards, C. S., McGinniss, M. J., autistic disorder in neonatal intensive care
Brothman, A., Desnick, R. J., Grier, R. E., unit survivors. Journal of Autism and Develop-
et al. (2001). Technical standards and guide- mental Disorders, 29(2), 161–166.
lines for fragile X: The first of a series of Mazzocco, M. M., Kates, W. R., Baumgardner,
disease-specific supplements to the Standards T. L., Freund, L. S., & Reiss, A. L. (1997).
and Guidelines for Clinical Genetics Labora- Autistic behaviors among girls with fragile X
tories of the American College of Medical Ge- syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
netics. (Quality Assurance Subcommittee of tal Disorders, 27(4), 415–435.
the Laboratory Practice Committee) Genetics Mazzocco, M. M., Pulsifer, M., Fiumara, A.,
in Medicine, 3(3), 200–205. Cocuzza, M., Nigro, F., Incorpora, G., et al.
Maes, B., Fryns, J. P., Van Walleghem, M., & Van (1998). Autistic behaviors among children
den Berghe, H. (1993). Fragile-X syndrome with fragile X or Rett syndrome: Implications
and autism: A prevalent association or a mis- for the classification of pervasive developmen-
interpreted connection? Genetic Counseling, 4, tal disorder. Journal of Autism and Develop-
245–263. mental Disorders, 28(4), 321–328.
Mann, S. M., Wang, N. J., Liu, D. H., Wang, L., McManus, E. J., & Alessi, D. R. (2002). TSC1-
Schultz, R. A., Dorrani, N., et al. (2004). Su- TSC2: A complex tale of PKB-mediated S6K
pernumerary tricentric derivative chromo- regulation. Nature Cell Biology, 4(9),
some 15 in two boys with intractable epilepsy: E214–216.
Another mechanism for partial hexasomy. Menage, P., Thibault, G., Martineau, J., Herault, J.,
Human Genetics,115(2), 104–111. Muh, J., Barthelemy, C., et al. (1992). An IgE
Mao, R., & Jalal, S. M. (2000). Characteristics of mechanism in autistic hypersensitivity. Biologi-
two cases with dup(15) (q11.2-q12): One of cal Psychiatry, 31(2), 210–212.
maternal and one of paternal origin. General Menolascino, F. (1965). Psychiatric aspects of
Medicine Journal, 2(2), 131–135. mongolism. American Journal of Mental Defi-
Marchetti, B., Scifo, R., Batticane, N., & ciency, 69, 653–660.
Scapagnini, U. (1990). Immunological signifi- Mervis, C. B. (1999). The Williams syndrome cog-
cance of opiod peptide dysfunction in infantile nitive profile: Strengths, weaknesses, and in-
autism. Brain Dysfunction, 3(5/6), 346–354. terrelations among auditory short-term
Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & memory, language, and visuospatial construc-
Prior, M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp move- tive cognition. In E. Winograd & R. Fivush
ment in children with autism spectrum disor- (Eds.), Ecological approaches to cognition: Es-
der. Philosophical transactions of the Royal says in honor of Ulric Neisser (pp. 193–227).
Society of London. Series B, Biological sci- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ences, 358(1430), 393–403. Meryash, D., Szymanski, L., & Gerald, P. (1982).
Mason-Brothers, A., Ritvo, E. R., Guze, B., Mo, Infantile autism associated with the fragile-X
A., Freeman, B. J., Funderburk, S. J., et al. syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
(1987). Pre-, peri-, and postnatal factors in tal Disorders, 12(3), 295–301.
181 autistic patients from single and multiple Meyerson, M. D., & Foushee, D. R. (1978). Speech,
incidence families. Journal of the American language and hearing in Moebius syndrome: A
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, study of 22 patients. Developmental Medicine
26(1), 39–42. and Child Neurology, 20(3), 357–365.
Mason-Brothers, A., Ritvo, E. R., Pingree, C., Pe- Michelson, A. M. (1998). Selenium glutathione
tersen, P. B., Jenson, W. R., McMahon, W. M., peroxidase: Some aspects in man. Journal of
et al. (1990). The UCLA-University of Utah Environmental Pathology, Toxicology, and On-
epidemiologic survey of autism: Prenatal, cology, 17(3/4), 233–239.
perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics, Miladi, N., Larnaout, A., Kaabachi, N., Helayem,
86(4), 514–519. M., & Ben Hamida, M. (1992). Phenylke-
Matsuishi, T., Shiotsuki, Y., Yoshimura, K., Shoji, tonuria: An underlying etiology of autistic
H., Imuta, F., & Yamashita, F. (1987). High syndrome. A case report. Journal of Child Neu-
prevalence of infantile autism in Kurume City, rology, 7(1), 22–23.
Japan. Journal of Child Neurology, 2(4), Miles, J. H., Hadden, L. L., Takahashi, T. N., &
268–271. Hillman, R. E. (2000). Head circumference is
Matsuishi, T., Yamashita, Y., Ohtani, Y., Ornitz, an independent clinical finding associated
E., Kuriya, N., Murakami, Y., et al. (1999). with autism. American Journal of Medical Ge-
Brief report: Incidence of and risk factors for netics, 95(4), 339–350.
Medical Aspects of Autism 571

Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Dombrowski, S. M., Murray, A. (2000). Premature ovarian failure and
Panchalingam, K., & Pettegrew, J. W. (1993). the FMR1 gene. Seminars in Reproductive
A preliminary 31P MRS study of autism: Evi- Medicine, 18(1), 59–66.
dence for undersynthesis and increased degra- Myers, B., & Pueschel, S. (1991). Psychiatric dis-
dation of brain membranes. Biological orders in persons with Down syndrome. Jour-
Psychiatry, 33, 762–773. nal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 179(10),
Möbius, P. J. (1888). Über Angeborenen doppel- 609–613.
seitige Abducens-Facialis-Lähmung. Münch- Nelson, K. B. (1991). Prenatal and perinatal factors
ener Medizinische Wochenschrift, 35, 91–94, in the etiology of autism. Pediatrics, 87(5, Pt.
108–111. 2), 761–766.
Moeschler, J. B., Mohandas, T. K., Hawk, A. B., & Nicolson, R., Bhalerao, S., & Sloman, L. (1998).
Noll, W. W. (2002). Estimate of prevalence of 47,XYY karyotypes and pervasive develop-
proximal 15q duplication syndrome. Ameri- mental disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychi-
can Journal of Medical Genetics, 111(4), atry, 43(6), 619–622.
440–442. Nielsen, J., Christensen, K. R., Friedrich, U.,
Mohandas, T. K., Park, J. P., Spellman, R. A., Fil- Zeuthen, E., & Ostergaard, O. (1973). Child-
iano, J. J., Mamourian, A. C., Hawk, A. B., hood of males with the XYY syndrome. Jour-
et al. (1999). Paternally derived de novo inter- nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia,
stitial duplication of proximal 15q in a patient 3(1), 5–26.
with developmental delay. American Journal of Niklasson, L., Rasmussen, P., Oskarsdottir, S., &
Medical Genetics, 82(4), 294–300. Gillberg, C. (2001). Neuropsychiatric disor-
Molloy, C. A., & Manning-Courtney, P. (2003). ders in the 22q11 deletion syndrome. General
Prevalence of chronic gastrointestinal symp- Medicine Journal, 3(1), 79–84.
toms in children with autism and autistic spec- Niklasson, L., Rasmussen, P., Oskarsdottir, S., &
trum disorders. Autism: Journal of Research Gillberg, C. (2002). Chromosome 22q11 dele-
and Practice, 7(2), 165–171. tion syndrome (CATCH 22): Neuropsychiatric
Molloy, C. A., Manning-Courtney, P., Swayne, S., and neuropsychological aspects. Developmental
Bean, J., Brown, J. M., Murray, D. S., et al. Medicine and Child Neurology, 44(1), 44–50.
(2002). Lack of benefit of intravenous syn- Norris, M., Spaulding, P., & Bordie, F. (1957).
thetic human secretin in the treatment of Blindness in children. Chicago: University of
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Chicago Press.
Disorders, 32(6), 545–551. Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., &
Moore, S. J., Turnpenny, P., Quinn, A., Glover, S., Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor
Lloyd, D. J., Montgomery, T., et al. (2000). A deficits in children with autism and children
clinical study of 57 children with fetal anti- with a specific speech and language disorder.
convulsant syndromes. Journal of Medical Ge- European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
netics, 37(7), 489–497. 11(5), 219–225.
Moreno, H., Borjas, L., Arrieta, A., Saez, L., Pras- Nyhan, W. L., James, J. A., Teberg, A. J., Sweetman,
sad, A., Estevez, J., et al. (1992). Clinical het- L., & Nelson, L. G. (1969). A new disorder of
erogeneity of the autistic syndrome: A study purine metabolism with behavioral manifesta-
of 60 families. Investigacion Clinica, 33(1), tions. Journal of Pediatrics, 74(1), 20–27.
13–31. Ohtaki, E., Kawano, Y., Urabe, F., Komori, H.,
Msall, M. E., Phelps, D. L., Hardy, R. J., Dobson, Horikawa, M., Yamashita, Y., et al. (1992).
V., Quinn, G. E., Summers, C. G., et al. (2004). The prevalence of Rett syndrome and infantile
Educational and social competencies at 8 autism in Chikugo District, the southwestern
years in children with threshold retinopathy of area of Fukuoka prefecture, Japan. Journal of
prematurity in the CRYO-ROP multicenter Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(3),
study. Pediatrics, 113(4), 790–799. 452–454.
Munk, D. D., & Repp, A. C. (1994). Behavioral as- O’Leary, J. J., Uhlmann, V., & Wakefield, A. J.
sessment of feeding problems of individuals (2000). Measles virus and autism. Lancet,
with severe disabilities. Journal of Applied Be- 356(9231), 772.
havior Analysis, 27(2), 241–250. Olsen, C. L., Cross, P. K., & Gensburg, L. J.
Murch, S. H., Anthony, A., Casson, D. H., Malik, (2003). Down syndrome: Interaction between
M., Berelowitz, M., Dhillon, A. P., et al. culture, demography, and biology in determin-
(2004). Retraction of an interpretation. Lancet, ing the prevalence of a genetic trait. Human
363(9411), 750. Biology, 75(4), 503–520.
572 Neurological and Medical Issues

OMIM™. (2000). Online Mendelian Inheritance in Payton, J., Steele, M., Wenger, S., & Minshew, N.
Man—OMIMJ . Baltimore: McKusick-Nathans (1989). The fragile X marker and autism in
Institute for Genetic Medicine, Johns Hopkins perspective. Journal of the American Academy
University, Bethesda, MD: National Center of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(3),
for Biotechnology Information, National Li- 417–421.
brary of Medicine. Available from http://www Peltola, H., Patja, A., Leinikki, P., Valle, M.,
.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/omim. Davidkin, I., & Paunio, M. (1998). No evi-
Ornitz, E. M. (1985). Neurophysiology of infantile dence for measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine-
autism. Journal of the American Academy of associated inf lammatory bowel disease or
Child Psychiatry, 24(3), 251–262. autism in a 14-year prospective study [Letter].
Ornitz, E. M., Guthrie, D., & Farley, A. H. (1977). Lancet, 351(9112), 1327–1328.
The early development of autistic children. Perez, E., & Sullivan, K. E. (2002). Chromosome
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre- 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: DiGeorge and ve-
nia, 7, 207–229. locardiofacial syndromes. Current Opinion in
Osborn, A. G., Harnsberger, H. R., Smoker, W. R., Pediatrics, 14(6), 678–683.
& Boyer, R. S. (1990). Multiple sclerosis in Perry, T. L., Hansen, S., & Christie, R. G. (1978).
adolescents: CT and MR findings. American Amino compounds and organic acids in CSF,
Journal of Roentgenology, 155, 385–390. plasma, and urine of autistic children. Biologi-
Owley, T., McMahon, W., Cook, E. H., Laulhere, cal Psychiatry, 13(5), 575–586.
T., South, M., Mays, L. Z., et al. (2001). Multi- Philofsky, A., Hepburn, S. L., Hayes, A., Hager-
site, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of man, R., & Rogers, S. J. (2004). Linguistic and
porcine secretin in autism. Journal of the cognitive functioning and autism symptoms in
American Academy of Child and Adolescent young children with fragile X syndrome.
Psychiatry, 40(11), 1293–1299. American Journal of Mental Retardation,
Ozonoff, S., Williams, B. J., Gale, S., & Miller, 109(3), 208–218.
J. N. (1999). Autism and autistic behavior in Piven, J., Berthier, M. L., Starkstein, S. E., Nehme,
Joubert syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, E., Pearlson, G., & Folstein, S. E. (1990). Mag-
14(10), 636–641. netic resonance imaging evidence for a defect
Ozonoff, S., Williams, B. J., Rauch, A. M., & of cerebral cortical development in autism.
Opitz, J. O. (2000). Behavior phenotype of FG American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 734–739.
syndrome: Cognition, personality, and behav- Piven, J., Gayle, J., Chase, G. A., Fink, B., Landa,
ior in eleven affected boys. American Journal R., Wzorek, M. M., et al. (1990). A family his-
of Medical Genetics, 97(2), 112–118. tory study of neuropsychiatric disorders in the
Pascual-Castroviejo, I., Roche, C., Martinez- adult siblings of autistic individuals. Journal
Bermejo, A., Arcas, J., Lopez-Martin, V., Ten- of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
dero, A., et al. (1998). Hypomelanosis of ITO: cent Psychiatry, 29(2), 177–183.
A study of 76 infantile cases. Brain and Devel- Piven, J., Simon, J., Chase, G. A., Wzorek, M.,
opment, 20(1), 36–43. Landa, R., Gayle, J., et al. (1993). The etiol-
Pastuszak, A. L., Schuler, L., Speck-Martins, C. E., ogy of autism: Pre-, peri- and neonatal factors.
Coelho, K. E., Cordello, S. M., Vargas, F., Journal of the American Academy of Child and
et al. (1998). Use of misoprostol during preg- Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1256–1263.
nancy and Mobius’ syndrome in infants. New Pober, B. R., & Dykens, E. M. (1996). Williams
England Journal of Medicine, 338(26), syndrome: An overview of medical, cognitive,
1881–1885. and behavioral features. Child and Adolescent
Patzold, L. M., Richdale, A. L., & Tonge, B. J. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5(4),
(1998). An investigation into sleep character- 929–943.
istics of children with autism and Asperger’s Pons, R., Andreu, A. L., Checcarelli, N., Vila,
Disorder. Journal of Paediatrics and Child M. R., Engelstad, K., Sue, C. M., et al. (2004).
Health, 34(6), 528–533. Mitochondrial DNA abnormalities and autis-
Paulson, G. W., & Paulson, R. B. (1981). Terato- tic spectrum disorders. Journal of Pediatrics,
genic effects of anticonvulsants. Archives of 144, 81–85.
Neurology, 38(3), 140–143. Prasad, C., Prasad, A. N., Chodirker, B. N., Lee, C.,
Pavone, L., Fiumara, A., Bottaro, G., Mazzone, D., Dawson, A. K., Jocelyn, L. J., et al. (2000).
& Coleman, M. (1997). Autism and celiac dis- Genetic evaluation of pervasive developmental
ease: Failure to validate the hypothesis that a disorders: The terminal 22q13 deletion syn-
link might exist. Biological Psychiatry, 42(1), drome may represent a recognizable pheno-
72–75. type. Clinical Genetics, 57(2), 103–109.
Medical Aspects of Autism 573

Pueschel, S., Bernier, J., & Pezzullo, J. (1991). Be- Richdale, A., & Prior, M. R. (1995). The sleep/
havioural observations in children with wake rhythm in children with autism. European
Down’s syndrome. Journal of Mental Defi- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4, 175–186.
ciency Research, 35(6), 502–511. Riddle, J. E., Cheema, A., Sobesky, W. E., Gard-
Pueschel, S., Herman, R., & Groden, G. (1985). ner, S. C., Taylor, A. K., Pennington, B. F.,
Brief report: Screening children with autism et al. (1998). Phenotypic involvement in fe-
for fragile-X syndrome and phenylketonuria. males with the FMR1 gene mutation. Ameri-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- can Journal of Mental Retardation, 102(6),
ders, 15(3), 335–338. 590–601.
Ramoz, N., Reichert, J. G., Smith, C. J., Silver- Riikonen, R., & Amnell, G. (1981). Psychiatric dis-
man, J. M., Bespalova, I. N., Davis, K. L., orders in children with earlier infantile
et al. (2004). Linkage and association of the spasms. Developmental Medicine and Child
mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier Neurology, 23(6), 747–760.
SLC25A12 gene with autism. American Jour- Riikonen, R., & Simell, O. (1990). Tuberous sclero-
nal of Psychiatry, 161(4), 662–669. sis and infantile spasms. Developmental Medi-
Rapin, I. (1996a). Historical data. In I. Rapin cine and Child Neurology, 32(3), 203–209.
(Ed.), Preschool children with inadequate com- Rineer, S., Finucane, B., & Simon, E. W. (1998).
munication: Developmental language disorder, Autistic symptoms among children and young
autism, low IQ (pp. 58–97). London: Mac- adults with isodicentric chromosome 15.
Keith Press. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81(5),
Rapin, I. (1996b). Neurological examination. In I. 428–433.
Rapin (Ed.), Preschool children with inade- Ritvo, E., & Freeman, B. J. (1978). National soci-
quate communication: Developmental lan- ety for autistic children definition of the syn-
guage disorder, autism, low IQ (pp. 98–122). drome of autism. Journal of Autism and
London: MacKeith Press. Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 162–167.
Rausch, L. A., & Nevin, N. C. (1991). Duplication Ritvo, E., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., Mason-
of 15q11.2–15q13 in five cases with different Brothers, A., Jorde, L., Jenson, W. R., et al.
phenotypes. Journal of Medical Genetics, 28, (1989). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemi-
573–574. ologic survey of autism: Prevalence. American
Raynes, H. R., Shanske, A., Goldberg, S., Burde, Journal of Psychiatry, 146(2), 194–199.
R., & Rapin, I. (1999). Joubert syndrome: Ritvo, E., Jorde, L. B., Mason-Brothers, A., Free-
Monozygotic twins with discordant pheno- man, B. J., Pingree, C., Jones, M. B., et al.
types. Journal of Child Neurology, 14(10), (1989). The UCLA-University of Utah epi-
649–654. demiologic survey of autism: Recurrence risk
Reilly, J., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once estimates and genetic counseling. American
more with feeling: Affect and language in Journal of Psychiatry, 146(8), 1032–1036.
atypical populations. Development and Ritvo, E., Mason-Brothers, A., Freeman, B., Pin-
Psychopathology, 2(4), 367–391. gree, C., Jenson, W., McMahon, W., et al.
Reiss, A. L., Feinstein, C., Rosenbaum, K. N., (1990). The UCLA-University of Utah epi-
Borengasser-Caruso, M. A., & Goldsmith, demiologic survey of autism: The etiologic
B. M. (1985). Autism associated with role of rare diseases. American Journal of Psy-
Williams syndrome. Journal of Pediatrics, chiatry, 147(12), 1614–1621.
106(2), 247–249. Roberts, W., Weaver, L., Brian, J., Bryson, S.,
Repetto, G. M., White, L. M., Bader, P. J., Johnson, Emelianova, S., Griffiths, A. M., et al. (2001).
D., & Knoll, J. H. (1998). Interstitial duplica- Repeated doses of porcine secretin in the
tions of chromosome region 15q11q13: Clini- treatment of autism: A randomized, placebo-
cal and molecular characterization. American controlled trial. Pediatrics, 107(5), E71.
Journal of Medical Genetics, 79(2), 82–89. Robinson, T. W. (2001). Homeopathic Secretin in
Richdale, A. (1999). Sleep problems in autism: autism: A clinical pilot study. British Homeo-
Prevalence, cause, and intervention. Develop- pathic Journal, 90(2), 86–91.
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41(1), Rodier, P. M., Ingram, J. L., Tisdale, B., Nelson, S.,
60–66. & Romano, J. (1996). Embryological origin for
Richdale, A., Francis, A., Gavidia-Payne, S., & autism: Developmental anomalies of the cra-
Cotton, S. (2000). Stress, behaviour, and sleep nial nerve motor nuclei. Journal of Compara-
problems in children with intellectual disabil- tive Neurology, 370, 247–261.
ity. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Rogers, C. (1987). Maternal support for the
Disability, 25, 147–161. Down’s syndrome stereotype: The effect of
574 Neurological and Medical Issues

direct experience on the condition. Journal of abnormalities in the offspring. Annals of Neu-
Mental Deficiency Research, 31, 271–278. rology, 46(5), 739–746.
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Penning- Sandler, R. H., Finegold, S. M., Bolte, E. R.,
ton, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in Buchanan, C. P., Maxwell, A. P., Vaisanen,
high-functioning adolescents with autism M. L., et al. (2000). Short-term benefit from
spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, oral vancomycin treatment of regressive-onset
2060–2073. autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 15(7),
Rogers, S. J., & Newhart-Larson, S. (1989). Char- 429–435.
acteristics of infantile autism in five children Schaefer, G. B., & Bodensteiner, J. B. (1992). Eval-
with Leber’s congenital amaurosis. Develop- uation of the child with idiopathic mental re-
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 31(5), tardation. Pediatric Clinics of North America,
598–608. 39(4), 929–943.
Rogers, S. J., Wehner, E. A., & Hagerman, R. Scherer, N. J., D’Antonio, L. L., & Rodgers, J. R.
(2001). The behavioral phenotype in fragile (2001). Profiles of communication disorder in
X: Symptoms of autism in very young chil- children with velocardiofacial syndrome:
dren with fragile X syndrome, idiopathic Comparison to children with Down syndrome.
autism, and other developmental disorders. General Medicine Journal, 3(1), 72–78.
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pe- Schopler, E., Reichler, R., Bashford, A., Lansing,
diatrics, 22(6), 409–417. M. D., & Marcus, L. M. (1990). Individualized
Roper, L., Arnold, P., & Monteiro, B. (2003). Co- assessment and treatment for autistic and de-
occurrence of autism and deafness: Diagnostic velopmentally disabled children: Vol. 1. Psy-
considerations. Autism: Journal of Research choeducational Profile Revised. Austin, TX:
and Practice, 7(3), 245–253. ProEd.
Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Sandstrom, M., Ahlsen, Schopler, E., Reichler, R., & Rochen-Renner, B.
G., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Autism and hearing (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale
loss. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological
orders, 29(5), 349–357. Services.
Roubertie, A., Semprino, M., Chaze, A. M., Rivier, Schreck, K. A., & Mulick, J. A. (2000). Parental re-
F., Humbertclaude, V., Cheminal, R., et al. port of sleep problems in children with autism.
(2001). Neurological presentation of three pa- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
tients with 22q11 deletion: CATCH 22 syn- ders, 30(2), 127–135.
drome. Brain Development, 23(8), 810–814. Schreck, K. A., Mulick, J. A., & Smith, A. F.
Rutter, M. (1996). Autism research: Prospects and (2004). Sleep problems as possible predictors
priorities. Journal of Autism and Developmen- of intensified symptoms of autism. Research in
tal Disorders, 26(2), 257–275. Developmental Disabilities, 25(1), 57–66.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A. Schroer, R. J., Phelan, M. C., Michaelis, R. C.,
(1993). Autism: Syndrome of definition and Crawford, E. C., Skinner, S. A., Cuccaro, M.,
possible genetic mechanisms. In R. Plomin & et al. (1998). Autism and maternally derived
G. E. McLearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture and aberrations of chromosome 15q. American
psychology. Washington, DC: American Psy- Journal of Medical Genetics, 76, 327–336.
chological Association. Scott, S. (1994). Mental retardation. In M. Rutter,
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A. E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and ado-
(1994). Autism and known medical conditions: lescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (3rd
Myth and substance. Journal of Child Psychol- ed., pp. 618–646). Oxford, England: Black-
ogy and Psychiatry, 35, 311–322. well Scientific Publications.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Simonoff, E., & Pickles, A. Shannon, M., & Graef, J. W. (1997). Lead intoxica-
(1997). Genetic inf luences and autism. In D. J. tion in children with pervasive developmental
Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of disorders. Journal of Toxicology-Clinical Toxi-
autism and pervasive developmental disorders cology, 34, 177–182.
(2nd ed., pp. 370–387). New York: Wiley. Shearer, T. R., Larson, K., Neuschwander, J., &
Sabry, M. A., & Farag, T. I. (1998). Chromosome Gedney, B. (1982). Minerals in the hair and
15q11–13 region and the autistic disorder nutrient intake of autistic children. Journal of
[Letter]. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(1),
search, 42(Pt. 3), 259. 25–34.
Samren, E. B., van Duijn, C. M., Christiaens, G. C., Sherman, S. L. (2000). Premature ovarian failure
Hofman, A., & Lindhout, D. (1999). Anti- in the fragile X syndrome. American Journal of
epileptic drug regimens and major congenital Medical Genetics, 97(3), 189–194.
Medical Aspects of Autism 575

Sherr, E. H. (2003). The ARX story (epilepsy, men- chromosome 13q12:>q13 in a patient with
tal retardation, autism, and cerebral malfor- autism and auditory processing deficits. Cyto-
mations): One gene leads to many phenotypes. genetic and Genome Research, 98(4), 233–239.
Current Opinions in Pediatrics, 15(6), 567–571. Sponheim, E., Oftedal, G., & Helverschou, S. B.
Shevell, M. I., Majnemer, A., Rosenbaum, P., & (2002). Multiple doses of secretin in the treat-
Abrahamowicz, M. (2001). Etiologic yield of ment of autism: A controlled study. Acta Pae-
autistic spectrum disorders: A prospective diatrica, 91(5), 540–545.
study. Journal of Child Neurology, 16(7), Steele, M. M., Al-Adeimi, M., Siu, V. M., & Fan,
509–512. Y. S. (2001). Brief report: A case of autism
Short, M., Richardson, E., Haines, J., & Kwiatkow- with interstitial deletion of chromosome 13.
ski, D. (1995). Clinical, neuropathological and Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
genetic aspects of the tuberous sclerosis com- ders, 31(2), 231–234.
plex. Brain Pathology, 5(2), 173–179. Steffenburg, S. (1991). Neuropsychiatric assess-
Shprintzen, R. J., Goldberg, R. B., Young, D., & ment of children with autism: A population-
Wolford, L. (1981). The velo-cardio-facial based study. Developmental Medicine and
syndrome: A clinical and genetic analysis. Pe- Child Neurology, 33(6), 495–511.
diatrics, 67(2), 167–172. Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C. L., Steffenburg, U., &
Silva, A. E., Vayego-Lourenco, S. A., Fett-Conte, Kyllerman, M. (1996). Autism in Angelman
A. C., Goloni-Bertollo, E. M., & Varella- syndrome: A population-based study. Pedi-
Garcia, M. (2002). Tetrasomy 15q11-q13 iden- atric Neurology, 14, 131–136.
tified by f luorescence in situ hybridization in a Stores, G., & Wiggs, L. (1998). Abnormal sleep
patient with autistic disorder. Arquivos de patterns associated with autism. Autism: Jour-
neuro-psiquiatria, 60(2-A), 290–294. nal of Research and Practice, 2, 157–169.
Singh, V., Warren, R., Averett, R., & Ghaziuddin, Stromland, K., Nordin, V., Miller, M., Akerstrom,
M. (1997). Circulating autoantibodies to neu- B., & Gillberg, C. (1994). Autism in thalido-
ronal and glial filament proteins in autism. Pe- mide embryopathy: A population study. Devel-
diatric Neurology, 17(1), 88–90. opmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
Singh, V., Warren, R., Odell, J., Warren, W., & 36(4), 351–356.
Cole, P. (1993). Antibodies to myelin basic Stromland, K., Philipson, E., & Andersson Gron-
protein in children with autistic behavior. lund, M. (2002). Offspring of male and female
Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 7(1), 97–103. parents with thalidomide embryopathy: Birth
Skjeldal, O. H., Sponheim, E., Ganes, T., Jellum, defects and functional anomalies. Teratology,
E., & Bakke, S. (1998). Childhood autism: The 66(3), 115–121.
need for physical investigations. Brain and De- Stromland, K., Sjogreen, L., Miller, M., Gillberg,
velopment, 20(4), 227–233. C., Wentz, E., Johansson, M., et al. (2002).
Skuse, D. H., James, R. S., Bishop, D. V., Coppin, Mobius sequence—a Swedish multidiscipline
B., Dalton, P., Aamodt-Leeper, G., et al. study. European Journal of Paediatric Neurol-
(1997). Evidence from Turner’s syndrome of ogy, 6(1), 35–45.
an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cogni- Stromme, P., Bjornstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K.
tive function. Nature, 387(6634), 705–708. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams
Smalley, S., Asarnow, R., & Spence, M. A. (1989). syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(4),
Autism and genetics: A decade of research. 269–271.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 953–961. Stromme, P., Mangelsdorf, M. E., Scheffer, I. E., &
Smalley, S., Tanguay, P., Smith, M., & Gutierrez, Gecz, J. (2002). Infantile spasms, dystonia,
G. (1992). Autism and tuberous sclerosis. and other X-linked phenotypes caused by mu-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- tations in Aristaless related homeobox gene,
ders, 22(3), 339–355. ARX. Brain and Development, 24(5), 266–268.
Smith, M., Escamilla, J. R., Filipek, P., Bocian, Stubbs, E. G. (1978). Autistic symptoms in a child
M. E., Modahl, C., Flodman, P., et al. (2001). with congenital cytomegalovirus infection.
Molecular genetic delineation of 2q37.3 dele- Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophre-
tion in autism and osteodystrophy: Report of a nia, 8(1), 37–43.
case and of new markers for deletion screening Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A com-
by PCR. Cytogenetics and Cell Genetics, ponential view of theory of mind: Evidence
94(1/2), 15–22. from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76(1),
Smith, M., Woodroffe, A., Smith, R., Holguin, S., 59–89.
Martinez, J., Filipek, P. A., et al. (2002). Mo- Tanguay, P. E., Ornitz, E. M., Forsythe, A. B., &
lecular genetic delineation of a deletion of Ritvo, E. R. (1976). Rapid eye movement
576 Neurological and Medical Issues

(REM) activity in normal and autistic children Tuchman, R. (1995). Regression in pervasive devel-
during REM sleep. Journal of Autism and opmental disorders: Is there a relationship
Childhood Schizophrenia, 6(3), 275–288. with Landau-Kleffner Syndrome? Annals of
Tassone, F., Hagerman, R. J., Taylor, A. K., Mills, Neurology, 38, 526.
J. B., Harris, S. W., Gane, L. W., et al. (2000). Tuchman, R., & Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in
Clinical involvement and protein expression in pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures
individuals with the FMR1 premutation. and epileptiform electroencephalogram corre-
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 91(2), lates. Pediatrics, 99, 560–566.
144–152. Tuchman, R., Rapin, I., & Shinnar, S. (1991).
Taylor, B., Miller, E., Lingam, R., Andrews, N., Autistic and dysphasic children. II: Epilepsy.
Simmons, A., & Stowe, J. (2002). Measles, Pediatrics, 88(6), 1219–1225.
mumps, and rubella vaccination and bowel Turk, J., & Graham, P. (1997). Fragile X syndrome,
problems or developmental regression in chil- autism and autistic features. Autism: Journal
dren with autism: Population study. British of Research and Practice, 1, 175–197.
Medical Journal, 324(7334), 393–396. Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1990). Expressive language
Thirumalai, S. S., Shubin, R. A., & Robinson, R. of children with Williams syndrome. American
(2002). Rapid eye movement sleep behavior Journal of Medical Genetics (Suppl. 6), 108–114.
disorder in children with autism. Journal of Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1991). A cognitive and be-
Child Neurology, 17(3), 173–178. havioural phenotype in Williams syndrome.
Thompson, R. J., & Bolton, P. F. (2003). Case re- Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu-
port: Angelman syndrome in an individual ropsychology, 13(2), 232–244.
with a small SMC(15) and paternal uni- Ungaro, P., Christian, S. L., Fantes, J. A., Mutiran-
parental disomy: A case report with reference gura, A., Black, S., Reynolds, J., et al. (2001).
to the assessment of cognitive functioning and Molecular characterisation of four cases of in-
autistic symptomatology. Journal of Autism trachromosomal triplication of chromosome
and Developmental Disorders, 33(2), 171–176. 15q11-q14. Journal of Medical Genetics,
Tierney, E., Nwokoro, N. A., Porter, F. D., Freund, 38(1), 26–34.
L. S., Ghuman, J. K., & Kelley, R. I. (2001). Unis, A. S., Munson, J. A., Rogers, S. J., Goldson,
Behavior phenotype in the RSH/Smith-Lemli- E., Osterling, J., Gabriels, R., et al. (2002). A
Opitz syndrome. American Journal of Medical randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled
Genetics, 98(2), 191–200. trial of porcine versus synthetic secretin for
Tolbert, L., Haigler, T., Waits, M. M., & Dennis, T. reducing symptoms of autism. Journal of the
(1993). Brief report: Lack of response in an American Academy of Child and Adolescent
autistic population to a low dose clinical trial Psychiatry, 41(11), 1315–1321.
of pyridoxine plus magnesium. Journal of van Slegtenhorst, M., Verhoef, S., Tempelaars, A.,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, Bakker, L., Wang, Q., Wessels, M., et al.
193–199. (1999). Mutational spectrum of the TSC1 gene
Torrente, F., Ashwood, P., Day, R., Machado, N., in a cohort of 225 tuberous sclerosis complex
Furlano, R. I., Anthony, A., et al. (2002). patients: No evidence for genotype-phenotype
Small intestinal enteropathy with epithelial correlation. Journal of Medical Genetics,
IgG and complement deposition in children 36(4), 285–289.
with regressive autism. Molecular Psychiatry, Veltman, M. W., Thompson, R. J., Roberts, S. E.,
7(4), 375–382, 334. Thomas, N. S., Whittington, J., & Bolton, P. F.
Trillingsgaard, A., & Stergaard, O. (2004). Autism (2004). Prader-Willi syndrome—a study com-
in Angelman syndrome: An exploration of co- paring deletion and uniparental disomy cases
morbidity. Autism: Journal of Research and with reference to autism spectrum disorders.
Practice, 8(2), 163–174. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Truhan, A. P., & Filipek, P. A. (1993). Magnetic 13(1), 42–50.
resonance imaging: Its role in the neuroradio- Verkerk, A. J., Pieretti, M., Sutcliffe, J. S., Fu,
logic evaluation of neurofibromatosis, tuber- Y. H., Kuhl, D. P., Pizzuti, A., et al. (1991).
ous sclerosis, and Sturge-Weber syndrome. Identification of a gene (FMR-1) containing a
Archives of Dermatology, 129, 219–226. CGG repeat coincident with a breakpoint clus-
Tsai, L. Y. (1987). Pre-, peri- and neonatal factors ter region exhibiting length variation in fragile
in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov X syndrome. Cell, 65(5), 905–914.
(Eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism: Cur- Volkmar, F. R., & Weisner, L. A. (2004). Health-
rent issues in autism (pp. 179–189). New York: care for Children on the Autism Spectrum: A
Plenum Press. Guide to Medical, Nutritional, and Behavioral
Medical Aspects of Autism 577

Issues (pp. 348–362). Bethesda, MD: Wood- in a survey of 75 autistic males. New England
bine House. Journal of Medicine, 310(22), 1462.
Wakabayashi, S. (1979). A case of infantile autism Waugh, M. C., Chong, W. K., & Sonksen, P. (1998).
associated with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Neuroimaging in children with congenital dis-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9(1), orders of the peripheral visual system. Devel-
31–36. opmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
Wakefield, A. J. (1999). MMR vaccination and 40(12), 812–819.
autism. Lancet, 354(9182), 949–950. Webb, D. W., Clarke, A., Fryer, A., & Osborne,
Wakefield, A. J. (2002). Enterocolitis, autism and J. P. (1996). The cutaneous features of tuber-
measles virus. Molecular Psychiatry, 7(Suppl. ous sclerosis: A population study. British Jour-
2), S44–S46. nal of Dermatology, 135(1), 1–5.
Wakefield, A. J. (2003). Measles, mumps, and Webb, T., Hardy, C. A., King, M., Watkiss, E.,
rubella vaccination and autism. New England Mitchell, C., & Cole, T. (1998). A clinical,
Journal of Medicine, 348(10), 951–954. cytogenetic and molecular study of ten
Wakefield, A. J., Anthony, A., Murch, S. H., Thom- probands with supernumerary inv dup (15)
son, M., Montgomery, S. M., Davies, S., et al. marker chromosomes. Clinical Genetics,
(2000). Enterocolitis in children with develop- 53(1), 34–43.
mental disorders. American Journal of Gas- Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
troenterology, 95(9), 2285–2295. Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: Psy-
Wakefield, A. J., & Montgomery, S. M. (1999). chological Corporation.
Autism, viral infection and measles-mumps- Wecker, L., Miller, S. B., Cochran, S. R., Dugger,
rubella vaccination. The Israel Medical Associ- D. L., & Johnson, W. D. (1985). Trace element
ation Journal, 1(3), 183–187. . concentrations in hair from autistic children.
Wakefield, A. J., Murch, S. H., Anthony, A., Lin- Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 29(Pt.
nell, J., Casson, D. M., Malik, M., et al. 1), 15–22.
(1998). Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, Weidmer-Mikhail, E., Sheldon, S., & Ghaziuddin,
non-specific colitis, and pervasive develop- M. (1998). Chromosomes in autism and re-
mental disorder in children. Lancet, lated pervasive developmental disorders: A
351(9103), 637–641. cytogenetic study. Journal of Intellectual Dis-
Wakefield, A. J., Puleston, J. M., Montgomery, ability Research, 42(Pt. 1), 8–12.
S. M., Anthony, A., O’Leary, J. J., & Murch, Whiteley, P. (2004). Developmental, behavioural
S. H. (2002). Review article: The concept of and somatic factors in pervasive developmen-
entero-colonic encephalopathy, autism and tal disorders: Preliminary analysis. Child:
opioid receptor ligands. Alimentary Pharma- Care, Health, and Development, 30(1), 5–11.
cology Therapeutics, 16(4), 663–674. Wiggs, L., & Stores, G. (1996). Severe sleep distur-
Wakefield, J. (2002). New centers to focus on autism bance and daytime challenging behaviour in
and other developmental disorders. Environ- children with severe learning disabilities.
mental Health Perspectives, 110(1), A20–A21. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40
Walker-Smith, J., & Andrews, J. (1972). Alpha-1- (Pt. 6), 518–528.
antitrypsin, autism, and coeliac disease. Wiggs, L., & Stores, G. (2004). Sleep patterns and
Lancet, 2(7782), 883–884. sleep disorders in children with autistic spec-
Wang, P. P., Woodin, M. F., Kreps-Falk, R., & trum disorders: Insights using parent report
Moss, E. M. (2000). Research on behavioral and actigraphy. Developmental Medicine and
phenotypes: Velocardiofacial syndrome: Dele- Child Neurology, 46(6), 372–380.
tion 22q11.2. Developmental Medicine and Wilkerson, D. S., Volpe, A. G., Dean, R. S., &
Child Neurology, 42(6), 422–427. Titus, J. B. (2002). Perinatal complications as
Warren, R. P., Margaretten, N. C., Pace, N. C., & predictors of infantile autism. International
Foster, A. (1986). Immune abnormalities in Journal of Neuroscience, 112(9), 1085–1098.
patients with autism. Journal of Autism and Willemsen, R., Oostra, B. A., Bassell, G. J., &
Developmental Disorders, 16(2), 189–197. Dictenberg, J. (2004). The fragile X syndrome:
Watling, R. L., Deitz, J., & White, O. (2001). Com- From molecular genetics to neurobiology.
parison of Sensory Profile scores of young Mental Retardation and Developmental Dis-
children with and without autism spectrum abilities Research Review, 10(1), 60–67.
disorders. American Journal of Occupational Williams, C. A., Lossie, A., & Driscoll, D. (2001).
Therapy, 55(4), 416–423. Angelman syndrome: Mimicking conditions
Watson, M., Leckman, J., Annex, B., Breg, W., and phenotypes. American Journal of Medical
Boles, D., Volkmar, F., et al. (1984). Fragile X Genetics, 101(1), 59–64.
578 Neurological and Medical Issues

Williams, G., King, J., Cunningham, M., Stephan, Woodhouse, W., Bailey, A., Rutter, M., Bolton, P.,
M., Kerr, B., & Hersh, J. H. (2001). Fetal val- Baird, G., & Le Couteur, A. (1996). Head cir-
proate syndrome and autism: Additional evi- cumference in autism and other pervasive de-
dence of an association. Developmental Medicine velopmental disorders. Journal of Child
and Child Neurology, 43(3), 202–206. Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 665–671.
Williams, J. C., Barratt-Boyes, B. G., & Lowe, J. B. Woodin, M., Wang, P. P., Aleman, D., McDonald-
(1961). Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circula- McGinn, D., Zackai, E., & Moss, E. (2001).
tion, 24, 1311–1318. Neuropsychological profile of children and
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A adolescents with the 22q11.2 microdeletion.
systematic review of action imitation in autis- General Medicine Journal, 3(1), 34–39.
tic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Woods, C. G., Robinson, M., Gardiner, C., & Rous-
Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299. sounis, T. (1997). Four cases of the condition
Williams, P. G., & Hersh, J. H. (1997). A male with described by Bundey with autism and a mater-
fetal valproate syndrome and autism. Develop- nal 15q11–13 duplication [Abstract]. Ameri-
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 39(9), can Journal of Human Genetics, 61, A117.
632–634. Yamashita, Y., Fujimoto, C., Nakajima, E., Isagai,
Williams, P. G., & Hersh, J. H. (1998). Brief re- T., & Matsuishi, T. (2003). Possible associa-
port: The association of neurofibromatosis tion between congenital cytomegalovirus in-
type 1 and autism. Journal of Autism and De- fection and autistic disorder. Journal of Autism
velopmental Disorders, 28(6), 567–571. and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 455–459.
Williams, R. S., Hauser, S. L., Purpura, D. P., De- Yardin, C., Esclaire, F., Laroche, C., Terro, F.,
Long, G. R., & Swisher, C. N. (1980). Autism Barthe, D., Bonnefont, J. P., et al. (2002).
and mental retardation: Neuropathologic stud- Should the chromosome region 15q11q13 be
ies performed in four retarded persons with tested systematically by FISH in the case of an
autistic behavior. Archives of Neurology, 37, autistic-like syndrome? Clinical Genetics,
749–753. 61(4), 310–313.
Wing, L. (1969). The handicaps of autistic chil- Yuwiler, A., Shih, J. C., Chen, C. H., Ritvo, E. R.,
dren—a comparative study. Journal of Child Hanna, G., Ellison, G. W., et al. (1992). Hy-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 10(1), 1–40. perserotoninemia and antiserotonin antibodies
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments in autism and other disorders. Journal of
of social interaction and associated abnormal- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(1),
ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica- 33–45.
tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Zambrino, C. A., Balottin, U., Bettaglio, E., Ger-
Disorders, 9(1), 11–29. ardo, A., & Lanzi, G. (1995). Obstetrical sub-
Wishart, J., & Johnston, F. (1990). The effects of optimality in children with autism: An Italian
experience on attribution of a stereotyped sample [Letter]. Journal of Autism and Devel-
personality to children with Down’s syn- opmental Disorders, 25, 553–555.
drome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, Zappella, M. (1993). Autism and hypomelanosis of
34(5), 409–420. Ito in twins. Developmental Medicine and
Wisniewski, L., Hassold, T., Heffelfinger, J., & Child Neurology, 35(9), 826–832.
Higgins, J. V. (1979). Cytogenetic and clinical Zhang, H., Nanba, E., Yamamoto, T., Ninomiya,
studies in five cases of inv dup(15). Human H., Ohno, K., Mizuguchi, M., et al. (1999).
Genetics, 50(3), 259–270. Mutational analysis of TSC1 and TSC2 genes
Wolpert, C. M., Menold, M. M., Bass, M. P., Qum- in Japanese patients with tuberous sclerosis
siyeh, M. B., Donnelly, S. L., Ravan, S. A., complex. Journal of Human Genetics, 44(6),
et al. (2000). Three probands with autistic 391–396.
disorder and isodicentric chromosome 15.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96(3),
365–372.
SECTION IV

THEORETICAL
PERSPECTIVES

Theory is an attempt to understand and inte- novel situations, or to intuitively understand


grate observable phenomena. In turn, theories what the other person knows, wants, and ex-
focus the attention of researchers and clini- pects from a query or social action. However
cians on particular types of data. New method- intellectually gifted an individual with autism
ologies generate new data, and challenge may be, there are difficulties in spontaneous,
theories; in turn, intellectual curiosity and novel, and naturalistic social interactions.
theoretical questions lead investigators to per- Such individuals may be stymied by subtle
form new studies and create new methods. humor; by when and how to repair the common
Autism has been a testing ground for theories breakdowns, ambiguities, and miscommunica-
regarding every aspect of human behavior, in- tions of ordinary discourse; and by the com-
cluding language, social interaction, and af- plex subplots of typical social relationships.
fective development. In turn, studies of autism Gifted as they may be in work with computers
have led to new observations that require new or explicit sciences, for individuals with
or amended theories about the preconditions autism the intuitive algebra of belief, knowl-
and course of normal development. The major edge, desire, and intent may remain wholly or
domains of behavioral and psychological diffi- in part elusive. And the perplexity and confu-
culties of individuals with autism and similar sion that they feel in regard to the internal
conditions have been recognized throughout lives of others are mirrored to a great extent in
the world since the time of Kanner’s original similarly baffling feelings in regard to their
descriptions. These include a particular pro- minds. Early in the life of a child with autism,
file of cognitive functioning with relative the parents become aware, sometimes gradu-
sparing of some areas that call on visual-per- ally and then with a sense of shock, that some-
ceptual abilities (exemplified by the Block De- thing is going wrong at the heart of social
sign tests of the Wechsler intelligence relations. They initially may think their child
batteries) and profound difficulties with activ- is deaf or may worry about her language skills;
ities that require social judgment (as shown in they then sense and can describe that she is not
difficulties in understanding the plot line of socially present in the way that other children
the Picture Arrangement tasks in the same are. Children’s social abilities and interests
scales). The presence of specific neuropsycho- are represented in the most subtle and moving
logical profiles has motivated a major stream ways: calming down in the parents’ arms; an-
of research and provides a rich source of theo- ticipating and enjoying the approach, touch,
retical speculation about the underlying basis and hug of someone offering affection; paying
of autism. In general, the difficulties of indi- attention to what interests another and hoping
viduals with autism are most apparent when to focus the other’s attention on something
they are called upon to understand implicit wished for or especially attractive; playing
motives and intentions in social situations, to and working alongside a peer; expressing dis-
use abstract reasoning and apply concepts to like, annoyance, and anger in ways that make

579
580 Theoretical Perspectives

the point without harming self or others; cop- the formation of flexible relations and areas of
ing with depression and anxiety, often with ability. All these psychological functions are
the help of others; falling in love with parents closely tied in with basic intellectual compe-
and then with others, and, as happens, falling tence in complex manners. A developmental
out of love, mourning a loss, and making new approach to understanding the more sophisti-
friendships. The developmental line from cated mental difficulties shown by individuals
first relations to mature relationships is the with autism must start with consideration of
personal novel written by each individual, IQ, mental age, or underlying cognitive capac-
with fewer or more subplots. For individuals ity. These intellectual abilities may be con-
with autism, this saga is markedly more re- ceptualized as a single, basic, general level of
stricted than for most typically developing intelligence (as in classical formulations of a
persons. The task of theory is to understand g factor in intelligence) or as multiple, only
at what point, and for what reasons, the devel- partially correlated types of intelligences.
opmental pathway from being cared for to From whatever theoretical perspective, it is
becoming capable of passionate love is dis- important to recognize that some aspects of
rupted. Other behavioral processes that are the psychological problems of individuals with
dysfunctional in autism include the capacity autism are shared with other individuals
to engage in imaginative and creative play, to with cognitive and adaptive problems (those
find pleasure in diverse activities, to be able with mental retardation) while others seem
to move from one topic (interest, activity, distinctive. Thus, patterns or subtypes of so-
hobby) to another, over time and in different cial-behavioral functioning (e.g., Wing and
situations, to be able to appreciate that others Gould, 1979) may, in part, be related to gen-
may not share one’s passionate interest or eral intellectual level (Volkmar, Cohen, Breg-
preoccupation. Thus, individuals with autism man, Hooks, & Stevenson, 1989). However, IQ
tend to be narrow in their interests and focus alone does not predict the full range of impair-
and to be obsessively preoccupied by details ments or the specific pattern of impairment or
or their own hobbies (train schedules, sports competence. The most distinctively human
statistics, historical events). Their perceptual competence is the ability to use language—to
abilities, perseverance in the face of obsta- ask for things; to plan; to fantasize; to engage
cles, and single-mindedness may lead to per- in abstract thinking; to share thoughts and
sonal achievements (such as remarkable feelings in speech; to read, write, and trans-
mastery of a body of knowledge) and voca- late; to argue, compromise, deceive, seduce,
tional advancement (in fields that call for and insult; to record our histories in mind and
hard work, care, honesty, memory, detail, and on paper. Babies in utero hear their mothers’
predictability, while being suited to the indi- voices and respond to sounds. From the very
vidual’s intellectual level), but often isolate first months of life, children engage in commu-
them from other people. nication that will eventually be encoded by
Of course, there are broad variations and language. They understand words and phrases
blurring between “normal” and “atypical.” and soon have a small vocabulary; around 18
Traits such as obsessiveness and preoccupa- months or so, they begin to spout words like
tion with one’s favorite sports team or histori- weeds and become active language users.
cal epoch are not restricted to individuals with From then on, language and communication
autism; on the other hand, there are individu- through speech affects every domain of mental
als with autism spectrum disorders who have and behavioral life. Standing back from all
wonderful and genuine talents (including that is known about autism, surely the most
artistic talents) that reveal imagination and salient fact would be that up to 30% to 40% of
creativity, a special way of seeing the world individuals with strictly defined autism are
and conveying it to others. Theories are mute. Those that do speak exhibit a range of
needed to explain the etiological relations that communication difficulties, in prosody, narra-
exist between difficulties in imagination and tive skills, and the social use of language.
restriction of interest and other areas of diffi- Some of these communicative difficulties were
culty, including the modulation of affect and already apparent to Kanner; others have been
Theoretical Perspectives 581

clearly defined over the past decades. Since clumsiness, or executive dysfunction. Eventu-
communication is so deeply encoded biologi- ally, there will be further clarification of the
cally and so relevant to all aspects of social neurobiological templates underlying domains
functioning, it is natural to place heavy em- of behavior, and the interconnections among
phasis on communication in the pathogenesis systems that lead to emerging social, intellec-
of autism. Are these, however, at the core? Do tual, and communicative competencies. The
they reflect some deeper or more proximal dis- rapid technological and theoretical advances
turbance in socialization? In the history of of many biomedical and behavioral fields—
theories in the field of autism, there has been a developmental neuroscience, neuro-imaging
tendency to highlight one domain at the ex- of the brain function, cognitive sciences, to
pense of others. Of course, in science, theories name a few—will surely lead to new data con-
are generally meant to explain only a circum- cerning normal development and impairments
scribed set of data, and one should not ask too in autism and associated disorders. These data
much of a good theory. In the field of autism, will generate new theories and, we hope, pro-
the breadth of data is enormous. There are vide increasingly comprehensive and useful
many domains of functioning that are im- understanding of development in autism in-
paired, and there are areas that are relatively cluding the connections across domains of
intact; behavior and psychological functioning function and sensitive points for therapeutic
change over the course of development; there is intervention.
enormous heterogeneity in clinical severity, in-
telligence, adaptive functioning, and presum- REFERENCES
ably in etiology. One should not expect any
single theory to do justice to all of this. A use- Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., Bregman, J. D.,
ful theory, such as one that relates aspects of Hooks, M. Y., & Stevenson, J. M. (1989). An
autistic social dysfunction to impairments in examination of social typologies in autism.
acquisition of a theory of how the mind of oth- Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(1), 82–86.
ers operates, can be pushed beyond its limits if
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
asked to explain why the majority of individu- of social interaction and associated abnormal-
als with autism suffer from intellectual disabil- ities in children: epidemiology and classifica-
ity. For example, a theory aimed at problems in tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
communication may, or may not, have any rele- Disorders, 9(1), 11–29.
vance to understanding stereotypies, motor
CHAPTER 21

Problems of Categorical Classification Systems

LORNA WING

Some system of classification of developmental shown in the way that the ideas put forward
and behavioral disorders is essential for clini- have tended to appear, disappear, and some-
cal work and research. The problem besetting times reappear in a different form, still with-
all attempts to produce reliable and valid diag- out a final resolution.
nostic categories for autism and related disor-
ders is the continuing lack of any independent Before the Twentieth Century
biological or psychological markers. In the ab-
sence of such markers, classification systems There are a few detailed accounts, written be-
have used various aspects of clinical history, fore the twentieth century, of individuals with
patterns of behavior, and psychological func- behavior closely resembling that seen in autistic
tions. Each of these is comprised of many dif- spectrum disorders (Frith, 1989; Houston &
ferent elements, manifested in many different Frith, 2000; Lane, 1977; Wing, 1997a). As far
ways, partially or completely independent of as can be ascertained, no one suggested any con-
each other. It is not surprising that the cate- nection between such individuals with strange
gories suggested have a number of drawbacks. behavior until Henry Maudsley (1867) made
This chapter examines the weaknesses of the what was perhaps the first step toward a classi-
categorical systems for diagnostic classifica- fication. He grouped children with bizarre, dis-
tion published to date. It also proposes a classi- turbed behavior under the label “insane.” He
fication based on multiple dimensions. The suggested a number of subgroups but his de-
steps in the argument will be as follows: First, scriptions are couched in such theoretical terms
the history of the development of concepts of that it is difficult or impossible to relate them
autism is described. Second, the classification to observable clinical pictures. There are hints
problems caused by the frequent co-occurrence that some of the children he discussed had autis-
of autistic conditions with other developmental tic spectrum disorders, particularly those in the
disorders is outlined. Third, the many anom- subgroup he labeled “instinctive insanity.”
alies of the past and present international sys- Maudsley also described children with catatonic
tems of classification are considered. Finally, a features, which he referred to as “cataleptoid
dimensional approach to classification that is insanity.”
being used in clinical work and in research is
presented. The 1900s to 1950s

THE HISTORY OF CONCEPTS In the first half of the twentieth century, a


OF AUTISM number of authors attempted to define syn-
dromes among children regarded as having psy-
There have been many attempts to define spe- choses. De Sanctis (1906) and Heller (see
cific diagnostic subgroups among autism and Hulse, 1954) described children who seemed to
related conditions. The difficulty of the task is develop normally for up to 3 or 4 years and

583
584 Theoretical Perspectives

then lost language, social, and other skills. De Rutter gave detailed descriptions of each of
Sanctis (1908) also wrote about children he these criteria. (See Chapter 16, by M. Rutter,
referred to as having dementia precoccissima this volume.)
catatonica. Earl (1934) used the term catatonia Asperger (1944, 1991) described children
with reference to adolescents with bizarre pat- with a behavior pattern he called autistic
terns of movement in an institution for those psychopathy. The main characteristics were
with mental retardation. Much of the behavior (1) social approaches that were inappropriate,
he described was characteristic of children one-sided, egocentric; (2) a narrow, limited
with severe autism. Mahler (1952) was con- range of special interests pursued with inten-
cerned with children with a pattern of behavior sity; (3) good expressive language used mainly
she called symbiotic psychosis. They clung to to engage in monologues on their special inter-
their caregivers without real feeling and had ests; (4) poor motor coordination; and (5) a
other disturbances of communication and be- marked lack of common sense. Despite the dif-
havior. These authors are rarely referred to ferences from Kanner’s group, careful reading
now. However, echoes of their descriptions of of Asperger’s lengthy descriptions shows that
the children they studied, including the simi- many of the features found in early infantile
larities between autistic behavior and catatonic autism were also present in Asperger’s group
features, can be found in more recent writings (Wing, 1981a, 1991, 2000). Asperger’s ideas,
on subgrouping of autistic spectrum disorders, originally published in German, did not become
to be discussed later in this chapter. well known in English-speaking countries until
The two writers among this group who did the 1980s (Frith, 1991; Wing, 1981a). Wing
eventually become recognized internationally suggested the term Asperger’s syndrome in
were Kanner (1943) and Asperger (1944, 1991). preference to autistic psychopathy.
Kanner wrote vivid descriptions of children Some writers used the term childhood
with an unusual pattern of behavior he labeled schizophrenia for conditions that were closely
early infantile autism. These children, from similar to those given the various labels men-
birth or very early in life, (1) were aloof and in- tioned above (Bender, 1947; Despert, 1938;
different to other people; (2) were mute or had Goldfarb, 1974). Bleuler (1911) coined the
echolalic, repetitive speech they did not use for label schizophrenia for a disorder in adults. He
communication; (3) had an “anxiously obses- used the term autism to refer to the turning
sive desire for the maintenance of sameness” in away from the external environment into a
their own repetitive routines; (4) were fasci- world of fantasy. He considered this was the re-
nated with objects that they did not use for the sult of the emotional flattening and the thought
purposes for which they were designed; and (5) disorder characteristic of schizophrenia. Kan-
had “good cognitive potential” shown in perfor- ner borrowed the terms autism and autistic for
mance tests or unusual rote memory. Kanner his group of socially aloof children, though he
and Eisenberg (1956) considered that social acknowledged that there were differences be-
aloofness and resistance to change in repetitive tween the social problems in the two conditions
routines that were elaborate in form were the (Kanner, 1965). At first he believed that early
essential diagnostic criteria. If these two were infantile autism was “a unique and separate
present, they believed that the rest would also condition” (Kanner, 1943). Later, however,
be found. In their experience, this clinical pic- Kanner (1949) quoted from a letter Despert
ture began from birth or became manifest dur- wrote to him. She argued that social withdrawal
ing the first 2 years of life. Later, Rutter (1978) was the crucial feature of schizophrenia regard-
argued against the use of Kanner and Eisen- less of the age when it began. She suggested
berg’s two criteria, particularly because lan- that communication impairment, insistence on
guage impairments were not mentioned. He sameness, fear of noise, and so on, as seen in
suggested three criteria for autism: (1) im- early infantile autism, were all symptoms of
paired social relationships; (2) impairment of schizophrenic social withdrawal. This letter
language and prelanguage skills; and (3) insis- made Kanner lean toward the view that early in-
tence on sameness, by which was meant a vari- fantile autism probably was the earliest form of
ety of stereotyped behaviors and routines. childhood schizophrenia, though he later ex-
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 585

pressed doubts about this formulation as being tive disorder and one had clear-cut organic
too simplistic (Kanner, 1965). features. Because so few children with this age
Anthony (1958) discussed the ideas on of onset were found, this subgroup was not in-
childhood psychosis put forward during this cluded in the study.) There were significant
period of time. He pointed out that these syn- differences between the early and late onset
dromes, named after their authors, overlapped groups, not only in their clinical pictures but
to such an extent that they could not properly also in their cognitive abilities and family, ge-
be differentiated. netic, and social factors. The early onset group
most clearly resembled Kanner’s early infan-
The 1960s tile autism. The late onset group had the fea-
tures and family histories similar to that of
A more scientific approach to the study of schizophrenia in adults.
childhood psychoses began to emerge in the This study was influential in the move to-
1960s (Wing, 1997a) when a group of psychia- ward using the term autism for the early onset
trists, psychologists, and pediatricians met group and away from the diagnosis of child-
under the chairmanship of Creak (1964). Their hood schizophrenia. Rutter (1972) noted the
original remit was to clarify the diagnosis of confusion previously surrounding the diagnosis
childhood psychosis, but they eventually de- of childhood schizophrenia. He concluded that,
cided to label the clinical picture they were in the way it had been used in the past, this di-
defining schizophrenic syndrome of childhood. agnosis had ceased to have any scientific mean-
They agreed on a list of nine diagnostic points ing or communicative value. Rutter used the
for this condition. In effect, the nine points term schizophrenic psychosis in childhood to
covered virtually the same clinical features refer to the adult-type condition beginning in
as Kanner’s description of early infantile middle or late childhood. He differentiated this
autism, except they did not specify age of from infantile autism beginning in the first 3
onset. It is worth noting that, under the heading years of life.
of “distortion in motility patterns” they in- Another important event in this decade was
cluded “immobility as in katatonia” (i.e., cata- that Lotter (1966) completed the first epidemi-
tonia). Although unsatisfactory in many ways, ological study of autism in the former English
the list of nine points was the first attempt to county of Middlesex. He identified, in a series
lay down specific criteria for diagnosing psy- of increasingly stringent screening steps, those
chosis in young children. children who had Kanner and Eisenberg’s
Among others, Despert (1938), Bender (1956) main criteria of social aloofness and
(1947), and Anthony (1958) had, in previous elaborate repetitive routines.
decades, identified three groups among chil-
dren with psychoses. They classified them by The 1970s
age of onset: (1) very early onset, (2) onset
between 3 and 5 years, and (3) onset in middle During the 1970s, Wing and Gould (1979) car-
or late childhood. The different authors gave ried out an epidemiological study among
different names to these three subgroups de- children known to have any kind of special
pending on their theoretical orientation. Kolvin needs living in the former London borough of
and his colleagues were the first workers to Camberwell, in the United Kingdom. The aim
undertake systematic research into the corre- of the study was to identify children under 15
lates of age of onset (Kolvin, 1971; Kolvin, years of age on December 31, 1970, who had
Ounsted, Humphrey, & McNay, 1971). They any of the features described as occurring in
studied 80 children admitted to hospital as in- autism. In contrast to Lotter, Wing and Gould
patients, during the course of the 1960s, for did not begin with preset diagnostic criteria.
intensive assessment of their psychoses. Forty- They were concerned to discover how the indi-
seven had an onset before 3 years, and 33 had vidual features found in autistic disorders were
an onset between 5 to 15 years. (The authors manifested and distributed among the pop-
found only three children with onset between ulation of children they studied and how
3 and 5 years. One had a progressive degenera- they related to each other. They found that
586 Theoretical Perspectives

impairments of social interaction, communica- cial impairments, and peculiarities of verbal


tion, and imagination were very likely to occur and nonverbal communication, despite ade-
together and to be associated with a narrow quate expressive speech. Gillberg and his col-
repetitive range of activities and/or interests. leagues, and Tantam also included narrow and
This was referred to as the triad of impair- intense interests, and clumsiness.
ments. Their more innovative (and controver- Wolff (1995) described her series of stud-
sial) finding was that each of these features ies, beginning in the 1960s, of a group of chil-
could occur in a wide range of manifestations, dren she originally suggested had schizoid
of which those in Kanner’s autism formed only personality disorder of childhood. She now be-
a small proportion. In particular, they found lieves that they represent the most able end of
that impairment of social interaction could be Asperger’s syndrome.
shown in the following ways: (1) aloofness and Rourke and Tsatsanis (2000) discussed non-
indifference to others, (2) passive acceptance verbal learning disabilities, also known as de-
of approaches from others, and (3) active- velopmental learning disabilities of the right
but-odd approaches to others. The findings led hemisphere. These authors noted the consider-
to the development of the concept of the autis- able overlap with Asperger’s syndrome. They
tic spectrum of disorders. (This was originally emphasized the need for further research to
referred to as the autistic continuum; Wing, clarify the nature of the relationship between
1988; Wing & Gould, 1979). The word spec- these disorders.
trum is preferred because, unlike continuum, it Rapin and Allen (1983) suggested that dis-
does not imply a smooth transition from one order of semantic and pragmatic aspects of
end to the other. By analogy with the color language, although an integral part of autism
spectrum, it does imply a range of clinical pic- and related disorders, could occur without
tures that differ from each other but have an the rest of the autistic picture. Bishop (2000)
underlying unity. The spectrum included the argued that children with this disorder could
most severe to the subtlest manifestations of fall between the diagnostic boundaries of
the triad. autistic spectrum disorders and developmental
Support for the concept of a wide spectrum language disorders. Wing and Gould (1979)
of autistic disorders has appeared, for exam- had previously described children who were
ple, in a genetic study of families by Rutter chatty, but whose speech was inappropriate in
and his colleagues (Bolton et al., 1994). They the social context. These authors placed the
provided evidence for broadening the pheno- children in the autistic spectrum and referred
typic definition beyond the criteria for autism to them as “active-but-odd” in social inter-
and other pervasive developmental disorders. action (see later in this chapter). The close
C. Gillberg (1992) suggested that autism be re- relationship between semantic pragmatic dis-
garded as a subclass among a very wide group order and autism was emphasized by Lister
of disorders of empathy. He considered that Brook and Bowler (1992).
this approach would have value for both re- In 1983, Newson introduced her concept
search and clinical work. The implications of of a pattern of behavior she referred to as
these findings for classification are discussed pathological demand avoidance syndrome
later in this chapter. (PDA) (Newson, 1983; Newson, Le Maréchal,
& David, 2003). She emphasized the chil-
The 1980s Onward dren’s resistance to carrying out the ordinary
functions of everyday life, such as dressing
In the 1980s, attention turned to disorders in oneself, or obeying direct commands to com-
the wider autistic spectrum. Asperger (1944, plete tasks. They sometimes reacted to such
1991) did not lay down specific criteria for demands with rage and panic. Newson de-
his syndrome but, subsequently, others have scribed their inappropriate social interaction,
done so (Ehlers & Gillberg, 1993; C. Gillberg, which tended to be “ill-judged, labile, ambigu-
Gillberg, Rastam, & Wentz, 2001; Szatmari, ous, without depth.” They could slip from lov-
Brenner, & Nagy, 1989; Tantam, 1988). Al- ing to violent behavior for no apparent reason.
though details differed, they all included so- Other features listed were marked passivity in
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 587

infancy, poor motor coordination, apparent mality are not clearly demarcated. Some
lack of awareness of their own social identity, disorders cause particular diagnostic problems
acting out being another person, real or fic- because they share features with the autistic
tional, with complete conviction, retelling fan- spectrum. Autistic spectrum disorders and
tasies as if they were true, obsessive interest in an associated condition may co-exist but only
another person, and blaming other people if the latter be recognized. Alternatively, an
anything went wrong. Much of the behavior autistic spectrum disorder may be the only con-
of these children was destructive or harmful dition present but can be misdiagnosed as
to others, including harassment of individuals. one of the associated conditions. It can be diffi-
The children seemed to enjoy other people’s cult to decide the most appropriate diagnostic
distress. Possibly, they found extremes of formulation.
emotional response easier to read than the
usual more subtle nonverbal aspects of com- Disorders Beginning in Childhood
munication. Newson considered this disorder
to be a pervasive developmental disorder but All the disorders that begin in childhood
not part of the autistic spectrum, despite the can co-exist with each other and with an autis-
finding that the prevalence of autism among tic spectrum disorder. The disorders likely
the siblings was as high as in typical autistic to cause diagnostic problems and the reasons
disorder. A few of the active-but-odd children for confusion among these disorders are dis-
in Wing and Gould’s (1979) study showed the cussed next.
same characteristics. There are also similari-
Generalized Mental Retardation
ties to Mahler’s (1952) description of the
clinical picture she referred to as “symbiotic Retardation of cognitive, language, and motor
psychosis.” Newson found that her syndrome skills from any cause can occur with or without
was more commonly seen in girls. Wolff and autism. Down’s syndrome is usually associated
McGuire (1995) followed up girls they had di- with appropriate sociability and good nonver-
agnosed as schizoid personality disorder of bal communication, but an autistic spectrum
childhood and noted that they were more likely disorder can occur even in association with
than the boys to be antisocial and delinquent. this condition (Howlin, Wing, & Gould, 1995;
One of the case descriptions was particularly Wakabayashi, 1979; Wing & Gould, 1979).
reminiscent of Newson’s syndrome. Accurate diagnosis may be a problem if a
Cohen, Paul, and Volkmar (1986) discussed person’s mental age is too low for pretend play
children with disorders that appeared to be on a to have developed—that is, below 20 months
spectrum between pervasive developmental dis- (Wing, Gould, Yeates, & Brierley, 1977). Peo-
order and specific developmental disorder. They ple at this mental level often have simple
coined the term multiplex developmental disor- stereotypies, such as body rocking or finger
der. They suggested three major diagnostic fea- flicking, but when their social responsiveness is
tures: (1) impairment in regulation of affective observed, a differential diagnosis can be made
state and anxiety, (2) impairment of social be- even at this level of function. The diagnosis is
havior and social sensitivity, and (3) impaired worth making because it has practical implica-
cognitive processing, including confusion be- tions for treatment and management. Some sim-
tween reality and fantasy, paranoid preoccupa- ple stereotypies can occur in young children
tions and over engagement with fantasy figures. with mental retardation whose mental age is
There are obvious similarities to Newson’s above 20 months. If the children do not have
pathological demand-avoidance syndrome. autism, these behaviors do not dominate their
activity pattern, and social responses and pre-
ASSOCIATED CONDITIONS CAUSING tend play appropriate to their mental age will be
DIAGNOSTIC PROBLEMS evidenced.
Developmental Language Disorders
The borderlines that separate the autistic
spectrum, other developmental disorders, some Disorders that affect receptive and/or expres-
psychiatric conditions, and even eccentric nor- sive language can occur alone or with other
588 Theoretical Perspectives

disorders, including the autistic spectrum con- they are able to concentrate on the repetitive
ditions. To differentiate developmental lan- activities of their own choosing. Conversely,
guage disorders from autism and related children diagnosed as having attention deficit /
conditions, clinicians must observe the social hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often have fea-
relationships, imaginative activities, and the de- tures of autistic spectrum disorders (Clark,
sire to communicate as shown in the use of ges- Feehan, Tinline, & Vostanis, 1999). The two
ture and other nonverbal methods (Wing, 1969). disorders can occur together and accurate diag-
Rutter and his colleagues (Bartak, Rutter, & nostic formulation can be difficult.
Cox, 1975, 1977; Cox, Rutter, Newman, & Bar-
Deficits of Attention, Motor Control,
tak, 1975) studied young children with severe
and Perception
developmental language disorder (SDLD) in-
cluding impairment of receptive language, and C. Gillberg, Rasmussen, Carlström, Svenson,
compared them with children with autism. Both and Waldenström (1982), in an epidemiological
groups had nonverbal IQs of 70 or above. They study of 6-year-old children in Gothenborg,
found clear differences between the groups on Sweden, identified a group of 14 children
the variables mentioned earlier, and they noted (1.2% of the population not mentally retarded)
the presence of deviant as well as delayed lan- who had a combination of severe motor clumsi-
guage in the group with autism. There was, ness and attention deficit disorder. In later pa-
however, some overlapping of the clinical symp- pers, this condition was referred to as deficits
toms, and there were a small number of children of attention, motor control and perception (the
with features of both groups. Cantwell, Baker, DAMP syndrome). I. C. Gillberg and Gillberg
Rutter, and Mawhood (1989) followed the chil- (1989) found that 8 of the 14 children had
dren into middle childhood and Rutter, Maw- autistic-like traits. One of the eight had autis-
hood, and Howlin (1992) extended the study tic disorder (DSM-III-R definition), three met
into early adult life. In these follow-up studies, the criteria for Asperger’s syndrome as de-
marked differences between the groups re- fined by the authors, based on Asperger’s
mained. However, those with SDLD tended to (1944, 1991) original description, and four had
have limited social relationships and this lasted many of the features of this syndrome.
into early adult life, despite improvement in the When deficits of attention, motor control,
individual’s conversational language. A further and perception are present, they can mask an
follow-up at age 23 to 24 (Mawhood, Howlin, & associated autistic spectrum disorder. There-
Rutter, 2000) showed continuing social and lan- fore, careful assessment of the total behavior
guage difficulties in the SDLD group and more pattern is essential so that all the elements are
overlap with the group with autism. detected.
Developmental Coordination Disorder Tourette’s Syndrome
Some children are particularly clumsy in gross The tics, compulsive shouting and swearing,
or fine motor movements or both. This problem and the echoing of words, sounds and actions
is characteristic of the subgroups described by that can occur in Tourette’s Syndrome (Shapiro,
Asperger (1944, 1991) and by Newson (1983; Shapiro, Brown, & Sweet, 1978) resemble the
Newson et al., 2003). However, such clumsiness same phenomena occurring in autism. This dis-
can occur alone or in association with other order can occur together with autistic spectrum
developmental disorders (Green et al., 2002). disorders (Kerbeshian & Burd, 1986; Realmuto
The diagnosis of an associated autistic spec- & Main, 1982).
trum disorder has to be made on the develop-
Hearing Impairments
mental history and observation of the pattern
of behavior, especially the quality of the social Hearing loss is often suspected in young chil-
interaction. dren who exhibit speech delay associated with
autistic spectrum disorders. Rutter and Lock-
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
yer (1967) noted that this concern had been
Many children with autistic spectrum disorders present for one-third of their sample of chil-
are restless and distractible, though typically dren with autism. The presence of the triad of
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 589

impairments (Wing & Gould, 1979), espe- children with autistic spectrum disorders (Cass,
cially the lack of nonspoken methods of com- 1998).
munication, points to a diagnosis of an autistic
spectrum disorder. However, hearing impair- Neuropsychiatric Conditions in Adults
ments can occur along with autistic disorders.
They can be difficult to diagnose because Any of the psychiatric conditions that usually
individuals with autism commonly ignore begin in adult life can occur along with autis-
some sounds, especially speech that they can- tic spectrum disorders. The commonest of
not comprehend. Careful investigation is im- these are the affective disorders, especially
portant because poor hearing can exacerbate anxiety and depression (Wright, 1982). Some
social and communication problems in chil- clinical features found in certain psychiatric
dren with autism. conditions resemble those found in autism.
These conditions can pose problems of differ-
Visual Impairments
ential diagnosis.
Impairments of social interaction, communica-
Catatonia
tion, and imagination and stereotypies including
eye poking, rocking, and hand-flapping have Acute catatonia is diagnosed on the combina-
been described in a substantial minority of tion of (1) mutism, (2) absence of voluntary
children with severe congenital visual impair- movement, and (3) maintenance of imposed
ments. This pattern of behavior sometimes oc- postures. However, a number of authors have
curs as part of a developmental setback in the described a range of clinical phenomena they
second or third year of life in some children consider to be catatonic features (Bush, Fink,
with severe visual impairments who were ini- Petrides, Dowling, & Francis, 1996; Joseph,
tially thought to be of normal cognitive poten- 1992; Rogers, 1992). A large number of these
tial (Cass, 1998; Cass, Sonksen, & McConachie, features (for example, stereotyped movements,
1994). These authors noted that the develop- tiptoe walking, echolalia, echopraxia, odd
mental impairments were strongly associated hand postures) are also seen in autistic disor-
with the severity of the visual loss. The most se- ders, especially in younger and more severely
vere congenital visual impairments are known disabled children. A severe exacerbation of
to be associated with central nervous system catatonic features, sufficient to cause major
disorders, such as congenital rubella. It is often problems with carrying out everyday activi-
difficult to estimate how much of the develop- ties, can occur in some people with autistic
mental problem is due to the visual impairment spectrum disorders. Motor problems seen in
and how much to the neuropathology. Chess Parkinsonism, such as freezing in the course
(1971), in a study of children with congenital of an action, or hesitation in crossing a thresh-
rubella, found a strikingly high prevalence of old, are frequently part of the same picture
autism, using Kanner’s strict criteria, and of (Realmuto & August, 1991). Wing and Shah
“partial autism.” On follow-up at 8 to 9 years of (2000) found that 17% of people with autistic
age, Chess (1977) found, among those with spectrum disorders age 15 and over when re-
autism or partial autism diagnosed at 2.5 to 5 ferred to a diagnostic center had marked cata-
years, some had recovered, some improved, and tonic and Parkinsonian features. They also
some remained autistic. Three children had be- found a significant association between pas-
come autistic and one partially autistic since sivity in social interaction and the later devel-
first seen. The results strongly suggested that opment of catatonic features.
the autistic behavior followed from the effects The occurrence of catatonic phenomena
of the virus on the central nervous system rather has probably added to the confusion between
than the sensory impairments. The relationship autistic spectrum disorders and schizophre-
of such impairments to autistic conditions is of nia. Although it can occur in schizophrenia,
considerable interest. Some visually impaired catatonia can also be associated with a variety
children experience a period of apparent nor- of neurological and psychiatric conditions
mality before a setback, and the timing of this (Rogers, 1992). It is particularly characteris-
setback is very similar to that described in some tic of postencephalitic states, as described in
590 Theoretical Perspectives

considerable detail by Sacks (1982). The diag- behaviors dating from childhood. This sub-
nosis of associated autistic spectrum disorder group tended to have a very poor outcome. The
has to be made on the developmental history results suggested that they required the type of
and the complete clinical picture. therapeutic approach that is appropriate for an
autistic spectrum disorder.
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Schizoid Personality Disorder and
Some of the phenomena seen in obsessive-
Schizotypal Personality Disorder
compulsive disorder, or in obsessive-compulsive
personality disorder, including the urge to There is a marked overlap among the DSM-IV
count and manipulate numbers, to carry criteria for these disorders and those for As-
out the same action over and over again, or perger’s syndrome. Unlike the labels schizoid
fearfully to avoid particular situations, have or schizotypal personality, the diagnosis of
obvious similarities to the repetitive routines autistic spectrum disorder is useful because it
of people with autism (Bejerot, Nylander, & helps the individual, the parents, and others in-
Lindstrom, 2001). Baron-Cohen (1989) dis- volved to understand the underlying impair-
cussed the use of the term obsessions to de- ments. Furthermore, it has implications for
scribe the repetitive behavior typical of education and treatment and the voluntary
autistic disorders. He considered the term in- autism societies provide reference groups for
appropriate because the subjective phenomena parents and for the individuals concerned.
of resistance to the repetitive activities could
Schizophrenia
not be discerned in autism. Overlap of the fea-
tures of obsessive-compulsive disorder and The reasons for separating autistic spectrum
autistic spectrum disorders, especially in disorders from schizophrenia of the adult type
high-functioning adults with autism, can ob- occurring in childhood were discussed previ-
scure the diagnosis of an autistic spectrum ously. The social impairment and odd speech
disorder unless a detailed developmental his- and behavior that are characteristic of autistic
tory of the person is obtained (Szatmari, spectrum disorders have been (and still are)
1991; Szatmari, Bartolucci, Bremner, Bond, sometimes confused with adult schizophrenia
& Rich, 1989; Thomsen, 1994). (Nylander & Gillberg, 2001). This is most
The similarities and differences among likely to happen if the person concerned is re-
autism, catatonia, obsessional disorders, ferred to psychiatric services for the first time
Parkinson’s disease, Tourette’s syndrome, in adolescence or adult life, especially if a de-
and encephalitic encephalopathy raise inter- tailed developmental history is not available.
esting questions concerning the possible site Among the 371 people seen at the British
and nature of the neuropathology and neuro- National Autistic Society’s diagnostic and
chemistry (Damasio & Maurer, 1978). assessment center whose records have been an-
alyzed (unpublished data) there were 78 ado-
Anorexia Nervosa
lescents and adults with autistic spectrum
Severe eating problems are common in chil- disorders (over 15 years old with enough ex-
dren with autistic spectrum disorders. Among pressive speech to describe symptoms). Of
371 children and adults with autistic spectrum these, only one had had an episode of typical
disorders seen at the British National Autistic schizophrenic delusions and auditory halluci-
Society’s diagnostic and assessment center, nations. In addition, 6% had had episodes of
whose records were analyzed, one-third had a bizarre behavior, in some cases accompanied
history of bizarre food fads causing a severely by visual hallucinations of a concrete kind,
restricted diet (unpublished data). C. Gillberg such as a bus emerging from the bathroom wall
and Rastam (1992) found that a minority or a freight train running across the floor.
among 51 teenagers with anorexia nervosa had These episodes were in response to stress and
autistic-like conditions. Wentz (2000) fol- resolved when the stress was removed. As
lowed up this group for 10 years and con- mentioned, the association of autistic disor-
firmed that 16% had persistent impairment of ders with catatonia has also caused confusion
social interaction and obsessive-compulsive with schizophrenia.
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 591

Psychosocial Deprivation Diseases (ICD) and the American Psychiatric


Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
Children deprived from the earliest years of ual of Mental Disorders (DSM), have reflected
social interaction and opportunities for learn- changing ideas of autism and related disorders.
ing can be withdrawn, may be delayed in de- The evolution of these systems illustrates both
veloping speech, and may show stereotyped the advances in understanding that have oc-
movements. If the deprivation is gross and curred in the past three decades and the confu-
prolonged, they may function as severely re- sions and conflicts of concepts that remain.
tarded. If they are of potentially normal cogni- The chapter on mental disorders in ICD-8
tive ability, recovery tends to be rapid once the (World Health Organization [WHO], 1967)
environment is improved (Clarke & Clarke, mentioned only infantile autism—and this only
1976; Koluchova, 1972, 1976). A child who is as an atypical form of schizophrenia. No diag-
autistic may have the added disadvantage of a nostic criteria were given.
poor environment, but the coincidence should Ten years later, this same chapter in ICD-9
not be taken to imply a causal connection. (World Health Organization, 1977) included a
Rutter et al. (1999) studied children adopted section titled “Psychoses with Origins Spe-
into U.K. families from extremely physically cific to Childhood.” In a notable change from
and emotionally deprived environments in the eighth edition, schizophrenia of adult type
Roumanian orphanages. These authors found occurring in childhood was specifically ex-
that, at 4 years old, 6% had autistic-like pat- cluded from the list of so-called childhood
terns of behavior and a further 6% had isolated psychoses.
autistic features. There were differences from Three years after ICD-9, the third edition of
typical autism, especially the improvement in the DSM, referred to as DSM-III (American
the adopted children seen at follow-up at 6 Psychiatric Association, 1980) was published,
years of age, so the authors used the term This introduced the term pervasive develop-
“quasi-autism” for this clinical picture. The re- mental disorders (PDD) as a general category,
lationship with typical autism remains to be thus acknowledging the shift in the concept
explored. of autism from a psychiatric to a develop-
mental disorder. There were two main sub-
Borderlines of Normality groups; (1) infantile autism with onset before
30 months and (2) childhood onset pervasive
The normal variation of human behavior encom- developmental disorder with onset after 30
passes people who collect objects, people who months but before 12 years. A category of in-
have circumscribed interests, and people who fantile autism, residual state was also included
are not particularly sociable or adept in social for those with a history of infantile autism but
interaction. As pointed out by Asperger (1944, who no longer met the full criteria. The criteria
1991), artists and scientists need a capacity to for infantile autism were, in brief: (1) social im-
lose themselves at times in their own special pairment, (2) language impairment, (3) resis-
fields to the exclusion of all else. The border- tance to change or attachments to objects, and
lines of normality and the autistic spectrum (4) absence of symptoms of schizophrenia
overlap, sometimes blurring the edge where nor- (delusions, hallucinations, etc.).
mal variation ends and pathology begins. The Denckla (1986) chaired a working party
theoretical issues are of great interest but, in that considered possible revisions of the
practice, differential diagnosis is of importance DSM-III criteria. The report emphasized that
only when the individuals concerned, or their the generally accepted criteria for autism and
families, experience problems and need help. related disorders, namely social and communi-
cation impairment and repetitive, stereotyped
INTERNATIONAL SYSTEMS OF activities, could occur in widely varying de-
CLASSIFICATION grees of severity. The working party recom-
mended that the subtlest forms of social and
Successive editions of the World Health Or- communication impairments and verbal and ab-
ganization’s International Classification of stract repetitive routines should be recognized
592 Theoretical Perspectives

as part of the range of pervasive developmental order were the same as for DSM-III-R infantile
disorders. Age of onset was excluded as a autism. Lists of manifestations were also given.
defining feature. The revised version of DSM- There were 12 of these, four for each main cri-
III, known as DSM-III-R (American terion. The content of these overlapped with
Psychiatric Association, 1987) was influenced those in DSM-III-R but differed in detail and no
by these views (Waterhouse, Wing, Spitzer, & clinical examples were given. The arrangement
Siegal, 1992). This revision kept the general in developmental order in DSM-III-R was also
category of PDD but the subgroups were lost in DSM-IV, though no reason was given.
labeled autistic disorder and pervasive devel- In contrast to DSM-III-R, age of onset for autis-
opmental disorder not otherwise specified tic disorder was included again, this time being
(PDD-NOS). The main criteria for autistic under 3 years. The tenth revision of the Inter-
disorder were the same as those for DSM-III national Classification of Diseases, ICD-10
infantile autism, but age of onset and absence (WHO, 1993) had closely similar subgroups
of schizophrenic symptoms were no longer in- and research criteria but included atypical
cluded. For each main criterion, a list of dif- autism. This would be classified with PDD-
ferent manifestations, with clinical examples, NOS in DSM-IV. Both systems allowed PDD-
was given, adding up to 16 items in total. A NOS/atypical autism to be diagnosed if social
minimum number and distribution of items or communication impairment or repetitive ac-
had to be present for diagnosis of each of the tivities were present. Social impairment was not
two subgroups. The list of manifestations for obligatory. ICD-10 also included a subgroup la-
each criterion was arranged in order so that beled overactive disorder associated with men-
those more likely to be seen in younger tal retardation and stereotyped movements, for
or more disabled people were earlier in each which one of the criteria was absence of social
list. This version was criticised for being over- impairment of the autistic type.
inclusive (Volkmar, Cicchetti, & Bregman, The American Psychiatric Association
1992). It was found that more children who (2000) published DSM-IV-Text Revision (TR)
were severely or profoundly mentally retarded in which the descriptive texts accompanying
were diagnosed as having autism than when the diagnostic criteria were revised but the cri-
ICD-10 criteria were used. This was consid- teria were left unaltered apart from one small
ered by the critics to be inappropriate on but significant change to the criteria for PDD-
the grounds that the social impairment and NOS. Instead of the criteria being social im-
other features of autism in many severely or pairment or communication impairment or
profoundly retarded children was not “ true stereotyped behavior as in DSM-IV, the first or
autism.” Since there are no objective criteria in this list was changed to and. This meant that
for true autism, it could be argued that, regard- social impairment was now necessary for the
less of etiology, there are good clinical reasons diagnosis of all of the subgroups of pervasive
for grouping together all those with the triad developmental disorders in DSM-IV-TR, thus
of impairments. The arrangement of the DSM- making it similar to the concept of the autistic
III-R diagnostic criteria in developmental spectrum. At the time of writing, ICD-10 still
order could have facilitated studies of how dif- has the or . . . or format in the criteria for
ferent clinical patterns were related to cogni- atypical autism.
tive ability.
The fourth version, DSM-IV (American Psy- Problems of the DSM-IV/ICD-10
chiatric Association, 1994) also retained the System of Classifications
overall category of PDD but introduced new
subgroups, in effect moving away from the con- This brief history indicates that the authors of
cept of the autistic spectrum. These were (1) the two international classification systems
autistic disorder, (2) Rett’s disorder, (3) child- have responded to research findings differenti-
hood disintegrative disorder, (4) Asperger’s ating autism from schizophrenia (Anthony,
disorder, and (5) pervasive developmental dis- 1958; Kolvin, 1971; Kolvin et al., 1971; Rut-
order not otherwise specified (PDD NOS). The ter, 1972). They have recognized the appropri-
three main behavioral criteria for autistic dis- ateness of regarding autism as one of a wider
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 593

range of developmental disorders. It is less 2000; Wing, 1981a, 1991, 2000). In effect, the
clear that the changes between editions have DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for Asperger’s
represented advances in the specifying of disorder are the same as for autistic disorder
diagnostic categories within the pervasive de- apart from the criteria relating to language,
velopmental disorders. The bases for distin- early development and cognitive ability. This
guishing categories have included age of onset, does not accord with the way the diagnosis is
type of onset, etiology, level of ability, and used in clinical practice, which is more in line
current clinical picture. The advantages and with Asperger’s own descriptions of his syn-
disadvantages of grouping in each of these cat- drome (Eisenmajer et al., 1996). There may be
egories are now considered. justification for defining a subgroup with so-
cial impairment but with typical development
Age of Onset
of language and adaptive skills up to 3 years of
Age of onset as a diagnostic criterion for autis- age (though none has yet been published), but
tic disorder has progressed through the differ- this should not be called Asperger’s syndrome.
ent editions of DSM and ICD from before 30 Since, in clinical work, the clinical picture
months to before 3 years, being temporarily is of prime importance, there is a strong case
discarded on the way. As discussed above, to be made for dropping age of onset as a crite-
Kolvin and his colleagues demonstrated that rion. In the international classification sys-
age of onset was very closely associated with tems the close but by no means absolute link
the clinical picture. However, the correlation between age of onset and clinical picture can
was not absolute and there was some blurring of be noted. The importance of careful investiga-
the borderlines between subgroups. Specifying tion where age of onset is different from the
a limited age range within which onset can usual pattern can be emphasised, without mak-
occur leads to various problems. Volkmar, ing age of onset a criterion.
Stier, and Cohen (1985) pointed out that the
Type of Onset
term age of recognition was more appropriate
than age of onset because the early signs of ab- In childhood disintegrative disorder both age
normal development may not be detected by of onset and type of onset are included among
parents. Identifying age of onset for those seen the DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria. Catastrophic
for the first time as adolescents or adults can be loss of at least two adaptive skills has to have
difficult. Parents may not remember or may be occurred after a period of normal development
unavailable. There are occasional cases of typi- lasting at least 2 years. This picture is rare
cally autistic behavior beginning well after 3 (Fombonne & Chakrabarti, 2001). In a small
years due, for example, to a virus encephalitis minority of cases of this kind, it becomes ap-
(Ghaziuddin, Tsai, Eilers, & Ghaziuddin, 1992; parent over time that the cause is a progressive
C. Gillberg, 1986, 1991). Parents, at least those neurological disorder (Corbett, Harris, Taylor,
in the United Kingdom, are puzzled and dis- & Trimble, 1977). In the majority, no further
satisfied if their child is diagnosed as having deterioration occurs after the initial loss of
PDD-NOS or atypical autism (or worse still, skills and the clinical picture from then on is
“autistic features”) especially since, in some like that in autistic disorder in children of
educational districts, such diagnoses may make similar levels of ability. Volkmar (1992) con-
a child ineligible for the type of education he or cluded from a case review that there was some
she requires. support for the validity of this diagnostic con-
The criteria for Asperger’s disorder de- cept on grounds of onset, course, and progno-
mand typical development of various skills, in- sis. Volkmar also mentioned clinical features
cluding language and curiosity, up to 3 years as differentiating the two disorders but, in the
of age. However, some older children, adoles- light of his description, it is difficult to see
cents and adults with the behavior pattern de- why. He did note that there could be problems
scribed by Asperger (1944, 1991) have of differential diagnosis. From the parents’
histories dating from infancy that are identical point of view, the term disintegrative suggests
to those with typical autistic disorder a continuing deterioration, with all that im-
(Leekam, Libby, Wing, Gould, & Gillberg, plies for the child’s future care. This did not
594 Theoretical Perspectives

occur in most of the children who had been better in the high-functioning group. The au-
given this diagnosis and who were reviewed by thors discussed the possibility of differences
Volkmar. in etiology between these two IQ groups but
the question is still unresolved. Lotter (1974),
Etiology
Wing and Gould (1979), and Wing (1981b)
In the great majority of cases, the exact cause found that the severity of the impairment of
of an autistic spectrum disorder is unknown, social interaction was related to IQ. These
although there is good evidence for the im- findings point to the importance of IQ as
portance of genetic factors (Bolton et al., one of the factors in diagnostic formulations,
1994). Only one subgroup of the pervasive de- though not as a diagnostic criterion, for indi-
velopmental disorders in DSM-IV and ICD-10 viduals with autistic spectrum disorders.
is based on etiology—that is Rett’s syndrome
Current Clinical Picture
(Hagberg, Aicardi, Dias, & Ramos, 1983).
Children with this neurological condition may As noted, for clinical work, the clinical picture
go through a stage in early childhood when they is the most important aspect of a diagnostic for-
meet diagnostic criteria for autism (C. Gillberg, mulation. DSM-IV and ICD-10 give detailed
1989; Tsai, 1992). After a few years, some of clinical criteria for various subgroups. How-
these become sociable and affectionate. There ever, the use of the varied mixture of age of
are a variety of other conditions that are some- onset, type of onset, etiology, level of ability
times or often associated autistic spectrum dis- together with clinical picture as the basis for
orders, such as tuberose sclerosis (Hunt & classification has produced many anomalies.
Dennis, 1987; Hunt & Shepherd, 1993), infan-
tile spasms (Taft & Cohen, 1971), the Fragile X A MULTIDIMENSIONAL
anomaly (Bailey et al., 1993; Meryash, Szy- DIAGNOSTIC FORMULATION
manski, & Park, 1982), and Williams’ syn-
drome (C. Gillberg & Rasmussen, 1994; Udwin, The precise relationship between the overt clin-
Yule, & Martin, 1987). It is not at all clear why ical picture and the underlying neuropsychol-
Rett’s syndrome should be selected as a sub- ogy and neuropathology is as yet unknown. It is
group while the rest are excluded. therefore more logical to classify the clinical
picture separately from other features such as
Level of Ability
level of ability and physical etiology if known.
Absence of delay in cognitive development is In this section of the chapter, a multidimen-
one of the criteria for Asperger’s disorder. sional diagnostic formulation that the author
This is despite the fact that Asperger noted the and colleagues have found useful in clinical
clinical picture he described could be found in practice is described. Its value is that it commu-
individuals with cognitive ability in the mildly nicates to other professionals, and the families
or even severely retarded range (Asperger, involved, the essentials of the clinical picture
1944, 1991; Frith, 1991). The subgroup that is and the needs of the child or adult concerned.
found only in ICD-10, overactive disorder as- A semi-structured interview schedule, the
sociated with mental retardation and stereo- Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Social and
typed movements, has IQ of less than 50 as one Communication Disorders (DISCO) has been
of its criteria. As mentioned previously, another designed to collect information, systemati-
criterion is the absence of social impairment of cally, concerning developmental history and
the autistic type, so it is difficult to understand present clinical picture. It is completed, in as-
why this subgroup was included in the category sociation with psychological observation and
of pervasive developmental disorders. testing, for the clinical assessment of individu-
Bartak and Rutter (1976) compared chil- als with autistic spectrum disorders and other
dren with autism who had intelligence quo- developmental conditions. The present author
tients (IQs) of 69 and below ( low functioning) and colleagues have used it in the assessment
with those with IQs of 70 and above ( high of over 700 children and adults. An analysis
functioning). The greatest difference was in of the data from 200 of these has been pub-
prognosis in adult life, which was significantly lished (Leekam, Libby, Wing, Gould, & Tay-
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 595

lor, 2002; Wing, Leekam, Libby, Gould, & that stage (Klin, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 1992;
Larcombe, 2002). From this clinical experi- Osterling & Dawson, 1994; Ricks, 1975, 1979;
ence, it has become obvious that the multidi- Ricks & Wing, 1975).
mensional approach to classification is much Research into theory of mind and its rela-
more in line with clinical reality than is any tionship to skill in social interaction also em-
categorical system. phasizes the importance of social impairment
The first step is to establish, from the de- in autistic spectrum disorders (Bowler, 1994;
velopmental history and pattern of behavior, Frith, 1989; Happé, 1999). Klin, Jones,
whether the person concerned has the triad of Schultz, Volkmar, and Cohen (2002) discussed
impairments, especially impairment of social the need for a more precise characterization
interaction. Then the diagnostic formulation and quantification of the social disorder,
includes the following dimensions: which they considered to be the core impair-
ment in autism.
1. Type of social impairment (aloof, passive, As Szatmari (1992) pointed out, at this
active-but-odd) stage, the issue regarding classification within
2. Pattern of skills and disabilities (verbal, the autistic spectrum may be its value for clin-
performance, self-care, motor, forward ical practice, education, and research rather
planning ability, and level of independence) than any absolute concept of validity. The use-
3. Etiology, if known fulness of Wing and Gould’s (1979) division of
4. Co-existing conditions, such as epilepsy, social interaction into the aloof, passive, and
ADHD, and so on active-but-odd subgroups has received some
5. The social situation of the family and the support from studies by Castelloe and Dawson
individual (1993), Borden and Ollendick (1994), Eaves,
Ho, and Eaves (1994), C. Gillberg (1992),
Only one clinical feature is included—that and Volkmar, Cohen, Bregman, Hooks, and
is, the quality of the impaired social inter- Stevenson (1989) even though the studies var-
action. Why should this one feature be se- ied in the degree to which each of the three
lected as the key when there are so many other groups differed from the others. Each sub-
features to be found in autistic spectrum disor- group tends to be associated with particular
ders? It would be theoretically possible to sub- kinds of clinical pictures. These are described
group on, for example, language problems, or next, with the variations associated with dif-
reactions to sensory stimuli, or motor coordi- ferent levels of cognitive ability and the rela-
nation, or types of stereotyped activities, or tionships to the DSM-IV classification.
any other aspect or combination that can be Wing and Gould (1979) found approxi-
defined. Such subgroups would overlap in their mately half of the fully mobile children with
clinical features but would not be identical. IQs under 70 that they studied were aloof at
Any of them might be of interest for research. the time of interview, one quarter were passive
The quality of social interaction was chosen and one quarter active-but-odd. From experi-
for purely pragmatic reasons. Impairment in ence with clinic referrals, the proportions of
this aspect of behavior is associated with im- passive and active-but-odd would have been
pairments of communication and imagination. much higher if children with IQs of 70 and
It has particularly marked effects on the whole above had been included, but no exact figures
life of the individuals concerned. are available.
No claim is made that this subgrouping has
validity in terms of the neuropathology, or that The Aloof Group
it is the only or best system possible. In the au-
thor’s experience, however, it has proved use- This group corresponds most closely to the
ful in planning education, management, and the popular image of autism, so the diagnosis is
provision of services. Impairments of two-way less likely to be missed than in the other sub-
social interaction and nonverbal communica- groups. It comprises children and adults who
tion can be detected even in the first year of a are most cut off from social contact. They may
child’s life, if the condition is manifested at become agitated when in close proximity to
596 Theoretical Perspectives

others. They usually reject unsolicited physi- Inappropriate or socially embarrassing be-
cal or social contact, although they may, for a havior is very common. This includes temper
brief time, enjoy rough physical play. Some tantrums, aggression, destructiveness, restless-
individuals in this group approach other per- ness, and screaming. These may occur in re-
sons to obtain food or other needs. They may sponse to interference with repetitive routines.
seek the physical comfort of sitting on a lap or The underlying problem is lack of understand-
being cuddled. Once satisfied, they move away ing of instructions and of the rules of social be-
abruptly and without a backward glance. They havior. Those who can speak may repeatedly
tend to avoid eye contact, though brief glances make inappropriate, even obscene, remarks in a
are common and intense staring may occur oc- loud voice in public as well as at home. Some,
casionally. Their social aloofness is particu- whether or not they have speech, may grunt,
larly marked with age peers. It tends to be less bellow, or otherwise vocalize in socially unac-
obvious with well-known people, especially ceptable ways.
parents. However, at least in the early years, On standardized psychological tests, visuo-
the signs of normal attachment behavior are spatial skills tend to be better than verbal
minimal or absent (Churchill & Bryson, 1972; skills (Wing, 1981b). Wing and Gould (1979)
Wing, 1969; Wolff & Chess, 1964). found that 78% of the aloof children in their
Understanding and use of both verbal and study had nonverbal IQs under 50 and 92% had
nonverbal communication are severely im- language comprehension ages under 20 months.
paired; in the most disabled of this group, A small minority of people who are aloof in so-
they can be virtually absent. Those who do cial interaction have a special skill in, for ex-
speak often show immediate and delayed ample, drawing, fitting and assembling tasks,
echolalia, pronoun reversal, and/or idiosyn- or arithmetical calculations. They may show
cratic use of words or phrases as described by surprisingly good rote memory for visual or
Kanner (1946). verbal material or music. Some perform at such
There is little or no evidence of the develop- a high level in their special skill in contrast to
ment of imagination. Play tends to be confined their severe retardation in other areas that the
to the manipulation of objects. Repetitive term autistic savant has been applied to them
behavior may be seen in simple or complex (Hermelin, 2001; O’Connor & Hermelin, 1988;
motor stereotypies, fascination with simple Rimland, 1965; Treffert, 1989). Aloof children
sensory stimuli, lining up objects, insisting on with higher levels of ability are those most
complex routines for particular activities such likely to show elaborate repetitive routines and
as preparing to go to bed or following precisely therefore to fit Kanner and Eisenberg’s (1956)
the same route to familiar places. two essential criteria. This classic picture is
In their early years, their tendency to walk rare because aloofness is significantly associ-
on tiptoe, their springy gait, and their rapid ated with severe or profound retardation. In
movements may make the aloof children ap- contrast, elaborate repetitive routines with ob-
pear graceful. As adolescence approaches, jects are much more likely to be seen in chil-
posture and gait tend to become more obvi- dren with moderate or mild levels of mental
ously ill coordinated and odd. Unusual reac- retardation.
tions to sensory stimuli may also be seen, Aloofness and indifference to others are
especially in the early years. These include ig- most likely to persist throughout childhood
noring, being distressed by, or becoming un- and into adult life in those who are severely or
usually fascinated with, sound, light, heat, profoundly mentally retarded. The clinical
cold, touch, pain, vibration, or kinesthetic sen- picture in children who are aloof in their early
sations, including self-spinning or watching years but have higher levels of ability may
things that spin. Lack of response to pain can change in middle or later childhood to become
lead to diagnostic problems in physical illness. identical with that described by Asperger.
It may also be a factor in repetitive self-injury They may become passive or even active-but-
that may be seen in some aloof severely re- odd in social interaction. However, there are
tarded children. Responses to sensory stimuli some people of good ability who become inde-
may be paradoxical—for example, covering of pendent and high achievers as adults but re-
the eyes on hearing a loud sound. main aloof all their lives.
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 597

An associated identifiable neuropathology show less intense resistance to interference.


is more often found in the history or present Some have elaborate routines, including elabo-
state of severely or profoundly retarded aloof rate repetitive activities with objects. In the
people than in those who have higher levels of more able passive persons, routines may take
ability (Wing & Gould, 1979). the form of circumscribed interests that re-
Those in the aloof group may fit the DSM-IV quire rote memorization of masses of facts
categories of autistic disorder, PDD-NOS, or about a chosen subject, though with little un-
childhood disintegrative disorder. Asperger’s derstanding of the real meaning. Some copy
disorder is a less likely diagnosis. actions of characters from books or the televi-
sion, perhaps enacting lengthy scenarios with
The Passive Group amazing accuracy but little or no understand-
ing of their meaning. This can be mistaken for
Children in this subgroup may not be diag- pretend play, but careful observation shows
nosed in their early years. The main signs of that the activity is mostly solitary, derivative,
impairment in children are absence of sponta- and repetitive.
neous social interaction and poor nonverbal Other characteristics of the aloof group, in-
communication, rather than indifference to cluding stereotyped movements and odd re-
others and overtly strange and difficult behav- sponses to sensory stimuli, may be found in
ior, which are so noticeable in the aloof group. passive children. These features are usually
Children and adults in this group do not make less marked or absent, especially after the
spontaneous social approaches, except to ob- early years. Some are poorly coordinated in
tain their needs. However, they accept other’s gross motor skills. Passive children are likely
approaches without protest and even with some to be the best behaved and the most easily
appearance of enjoyment. They can be led to managed of all the autistic groups.
join in games and activities organized by oth- They tend to have higher levels of ability
ers although they typically take a passive role, than the aloof group and some are in the aver-
such as that of the baby in the game of mothers age or superior range. The majority perform
and fathers. They are able to copy other peo- better on visuospatial skills than on verbal
ple’s actions, but without full understanding. skills, but some have higher scores on language
Their eye contact is usually poor. tests. Some have one or more special skills at a
Speech is often better developed than in very high level, meriting the label of autistic
the aloof group, but there is usually poor into- savant.
nation and the characteristic lack of inter- The prognosis in adult life for the passive
personal communication for pleasure. Some group tends, overall, to be better than for the
have abnormalities of speech as in the aloof aloof children. By later childhood or adoles-
group. Others have large vocabularies and even cence, some have become active-but-odd in be-
good grammar although the content is mostly havior. Despite the generally more amenable
repetitive and confined within a narrow range behavior in childhood, some of those in the
of subjects. Passive individuals have little ap- passive group may become very difficult in
preciation of subtle verbal jokes, but they may periods of stress or pressure, especially in ado-
enjoy slapstick humor, and simple childish play lescence. This change is sometimes dramatic
on words. Understanding and use of nonverbal and long lasting. Another problem that is sig-
communication is equally impaired. nificantly more likely to occur in the passive
Imaginative play may be absent, or they may group in adolescence or early adult life is the
simply copy other children’s activities—for ex- exacerbation of catatonic features (Wing &
ample, bathing and feeding dolls. This play Shah, 2000). These features sometimes remit
lacks spontaneity and inventiveness, and re- but can remain a problem over many years.
mains repetitive and limited in form. Occa- However, the most able people in the passive
sionally, a passive person will show echopraxia group may adapt to the demands of adult life.
of gestures equivalent to echolalia of speech Some may even become high achievers in areas
(Attwood, 1984). related to their special skills. Level of ability,
Passive individuals have repetitive routines as in all autistic spectrum disorders, is of
but, compared to the aloof group, they usually major importance in relation to outcome.
598 Theoretical Perspectives

They are less likely than the aloof group to They may not use gestures, especially those in-
have an associated identifiable neuropathology dicating feeling and emotions (Attwood, 1984)
(Wing & Gould, 1979). but some in this group make exaggerated and
Among the DSM-IV diagnostic categories, inappropriate movements of face and limbs
those in the passive group could fit the cate- when speaking. Their eye contact is also inap-
gories of autistic disorder, PDD-NOS, or As- propriate, staring too hard at times but looking
perger’s disorder. They are unlikely to fit the away when it would be socially correct to meet
criteria for childhood disintegrative disorder. the gaze of the conversational partner. People
in this group have, in particularly marked form,
The Active-but-Odd Group the impairment of the pragmatic aspects of
communication that are common to all autistic
Children and adults who make spontaneous ap- spectrum disorders (Lister Brook & Bowler,
proaches to others, but in a peculiar, naive, and 1992).
one-sided fashion, comprise the active-but-odd Like some passive children, many in this
group. These individuals seek to indulge their group have repetitive, stereotyped pseudo-
circumscribed interests by talking at another pretend play. They build and rebuild the same
person or by asking questions but not for the imaginary system of roads and bridges, or they
pleasure of reciprocal social interaction. The pretend to be the same animal or the same
active approaches may be so persistent—per- inanimate object such as a train (Wing et al.,
haps accompanied by physical clinging—that 1977). Some children act out scenes from their
these people are boring, unwelcome, or even favorite videos, which they watch repetitively.
distressing to their unwilling listeners. Diagno- Their re-enactment is remarkable for the accu-
sis and classification are particularly difficult racy with which they copy the fragments cho-
for this group. In some cases, the autistic fea- sen, the repetitiveness and absence of any
tures are fairly obvious, despite the active so- imaginative embroidery, and the lack of com-
cial approaches. In others, different aspects of prehension or even interest in the meaning.
behavior, such as poor motor coordination, in- Some invent their own imaginary worlds,
appropriate use of language, or high anxiety, which may be very elaborate but have the char-
may capture attention, so that the autistic char- acteristic rigidity and repetitiveness.
acteristics are overlooked. As in the other two groups, some active-
Compared to the aloof and passive groups, but-odd children have repetitive routines
speech tends to be greater in quantity but, at involving objects. Others have more abstract
least in the early years, may be characterized circumscribed interests, including time-
by delay and by the same abnormalities as pre- tables, calendars, genealogy of royal families,
viously described. Some individuals, however, physics, astronomy, particular birds or ani-
have no delay in speech development and can mals or even specific people. Repetitive ask-
use correct grammar and employ large vocabu- ing of the same series of questions, regardless
laries even at a young age. Repetitiveness, of the answers received, is also characteristic.
long-windedness, and lack of ease with collo- In their early years, some display motor
quial turns of phrase are characteristic of even stereotypies and unusual responses to sensory
the best speakers in the group. Hurtig, Ensrud, stimuli. These may fade with increasing age.
and Tomblin (1982), examining the conversa- Children in this group frequently have prob-
tions of active-but-odd children with adults, lems of motor coordination and have an odd,
described how the children used questions as immature gait and posture. Some have been
conversational openers without prefacing their described as puppet-like in their movements.
inquiries with social conventions, such as a Unlike children in the aloof group, those who
greeting. This behavior generally seemed very are active-but-odd in social interaction tend to
odd to the conversational partner. be wary of balancing and climbing.
Active-but-odd individuals also have impair- Behavior problems are common. Irrelevant
ments in the nonverbal aspects of communica- remarks and repetitive questioning can include
tion. Vocal intonation is monotonous or has socially inappropriate themes such as physical
strange inflections and poor volume control. abnormalities, details of other people’s personal
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 599

lives, or sex and violence. Odd approaches to groups in different environments (Lord, 1984)
others easily turn to pestering and then to tem- and at different ages. Wing (1988) found in an
per tantrums and physical aggression. They may epidemiological study that 14% of the 42 fully
show oversensitivity to any perceived criticism. mobile children with autistic spectrum disor-
A small minority get into trouble with the law ders, who were aloof in their early years had,
(Wing, 1981a, 1997b) because they lack under- by later childhood or adolescence, become pas-
standing of social rules. Asperger (1944, 1991) sive or active-but-odd. However, among the 37
described one small subgroup of people with his fully mobile children who were passive or
syndrome, who would fit into the active-but-odd active-but-odd in their early years, 14% be-
group, who appeared to take delight in mali- came socially aloof as they grew older. Almost
cious acts against others. Newson’s group with all the children in this study had varying de-
pathological demand avoidance syndrome also grees of mental retardation. In contrast, 39
shows this type of behavior. (65%) of 60 children and adults referred to a
Most but not all members of this group have clinic, who had overall IQs of 70 or above and
higher levels of skill than those who are aloof, who had been aloof in early childhood, had be-
and some are in the average or superior range. come passive or active-but-odd with increasing
Some active-but-odd people have verbal scores age. None of the 70 referrals with this level of
that are equal to or higher than their perfor- ability who had been passive or active-but-odd
mance scores on Wechsler IQ batteries, but the had become aloof (unpublished data). Diag-
subtests on which they do well depend more nostic formulations for individuals need to
on rote memory than on reasoning ability. change over time in response to changes in the
Prognosis is varied. Some make good prog- person concerned.
ress and become more appropriate in social in-
teraction as adults. As with the passive group, CONCLUSION
the most able of those who as children were
active-but-odd in social interaction may adapt The so-called syndromes into which autistic
to the demands of adult life and some become spectrum disorders have been divided are not
high achievers in their own special fields of in- unique and separate conditions. They are best
terest. Most difficulties of adaptation occur in understood in the context of the full range of
those whose social behavior is inappropriate, developmental disorders right up to the bor-
especially if they have the characteristics of derline of normality.
the pathological demand avoidance group de- There are major problems with the past and
scribed by Newson. current classification systems based on cate-
In their study, Wing and Gould (1979) gories, which to a large extent overlap with
found that, of the three groups, the active-but- each other. No objective measures have been
odd were least likely to have an associated found to test the validity of the diagnostic cate-
identifiable neuropathology. gories used in existing classification systems.
Among the diagnostic categories in DSM-IV The categories have not proved helpful in pre-
and ICD-10, the active-but-odd group would be scribing type of education, behavior manage-
most likely to show the criteria for Asperger’s ment, medication, or other treatment. They are
disorder or PDD-NOS. Some possibly might useful in comparative research only if their re-
meet the criteria for autistic disorder. liability is enhanced by increasing the details
of the specifications, thereby narrowing the
The Need for Flexibility categories, and by training the workers involved
(Le Couteur et al., 1989). But, however neces-
This classification of the impairments of so- sary this is for comparing the results of differ-
cial interaction found in the spectrum of autis- ent studies, it has the effect of excluding those
tic disorders should not be regarded as rigid. who do not fit neatly into the categories. Thus,
The borderlines between the groups are ill it artificially endorses the categories used.
defined, lending weight to the view that they The multidimensional approach suggested
are part of a spectrum of related conditions. groups together clinical pictures that have in
One person can show the behavior of different common the triad of impairments whatever the
600 Theoretical Perspectives

underlying cause. The justification for this is American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnos-
on the clinical grounds that the presence of the tic and statistical manual of mental disorders
triad is the most important factor determining (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
an individual’s needs, whatever other condition Anthony, E. J. (1958). An experimental approach
is present (Wing, 2001). The lumping together to the psychopathology of childhood autism.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 21,
for a clinical diagnostic formulation does not
211–225.
prevent researchers from splitting off special Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autistischen psychopa-
groups they want to investigate, as long as they then” im kindersalter [Autistic psychopathy in
give clear descriptions of the group they select. childhood]. Archives fur Psychiatrie und Ner-
In any case, stating only, for example, that the venkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.
children being studied have DSM-IV autistic Asperger, H. (1991). Autistic psychopathy in child-
disorder is not sufficient information to allow hood. In U. Frith (Ed. & Trans.), Autism and
comparison with other studies. Asperger syndrome (pp. 37–92). Cambridge,
A multidimensional approach of this kind is England: Cambridge University Press.
a practical way to cope with the current in- Attwood, A. (1984). The gestures of autistic chil-
dren. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-
complete state of knowledge concerning autis-
versity of London, London, England.
tic spectrum disorders and the difficulties of
Bailey, A., Bolton, P., Butler, L., Le Couteur, A.,
defining the borderlines with other develop- Murphy, M., Scott, S., et al. (1993). Preva-
mental and neuropsychiatric conditions. It has lence of the Fragile X anomaly amongst autis-
to be acknowledged that it would be difficult tic twins and singletons. Journal of Child
to incorporate into a standardized method Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 673–688.
of classification such as DSM-IV or ICD-10. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Do autistic children have
Categories are much easier to deal with in a obsessions and compulsions? British Journal of
theoretical system. The problem is that, in the Clinical Psychology, 28, 193–200.
real world of clinical practice, when trying to Bartak, L., & Rutter, M. (1976). Differences be-
help people with autistic spectrum disorders, tween mentally retarded and normally intelli-
gent autistic children. Journal of Autism and
neatly defined categories are of very little use.
Childhood Schizophrenia, 6, 109–120.
There may come a time when the relationship Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A compar-
between neuropathology, psychology, and ative study of infantile autism and specific de-
overt behavior is fully understood. Until then, velopmental receptive language disorder: I. The
it is likely that problems of classification will children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126,
continue unresolved. 127–145.
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). A com-
parative study of infantile autism and specific
Cross-References developmental receptive language disorder:
III. Discriminant function analysis. Journal
Approaches to categorical classification are of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7,
discussed in Chapters 1 through 7; assessment 297–302.
issues are addressed in Chapters 27 through 33; Bejerot, S., Nylander, L., & Lindstrom, E. (2001).
Chapter 47 discusses cross-cultural issues of Autistic traits in obsessive-compulsive dis-
classification as does Chapter 48. order. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 55,
169–176.
Bender, L. (1947). Childhood schizophrenia: A clin-
REFERENCES ical study of 100 schizophrenic children. Amer-
ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 17, 40–56.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag- Bishop, D. (2000). What’s so special about As-
nostic and statistical manual (3rd ed.). Wash- perger syndrome? The need for further explo-
ington, DC: Author. ration of the borderlands of autism. In A. Klin,
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag- F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), As-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- perger syndrome (pp. 231–253). New York:
ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Guilford Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag- Bleuler, E. (1911). Dementia praecox or the group
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- of schizophrenias (J. Zinkin, Trans.). New
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. York: International Universities Press.
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 601

Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Corbett, J., Harris, R., Taylor, E., & Trimble, M.
Goode, S., Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case- (1977). Progressive disintegrative psychosis of
control family history study of autism. Journal childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, Psychiatry, 17, 211–219.
877–900. Cox, A., Rutter, M., Newman, S., & Bartak, L.
Borden, M., & Ollendick, T. (1994). An examina- (1975). A comparative study of infantile
tion of the validity of social subtypes in autism and specific developmental receptive
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental language disorder: II. Parental characteristics.
Disorders, 24, 23–38. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 146–159.
Bowler, D. (1994). “ Theory of mind” in Asperger’s Creak, E. M. (1964). Schizophrenic syndrome in
syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and childhood: Further progress report of a work-
Psychiatry, 33, 877–894. ing party. Developmental Medicine and Child
Bush, G., Fink, M., Petrides, G., Dowling, F., & Neurology, 6, 530–535.
Francis, A. (1996). Catatonia: I. Rating scale Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neuro-
and standardised examination. Acta Psychi- logical model for childhood autism. Archives
atrica Scandinavica, 93, 129–136. of Neurology, 35, 777–786.
Cantwell, D., Baker, L., Rutter, M., & Mawhood, L. Denckla, M. B. (1986). New diagnostic criteria for
(1989). Infantile autism and developmental re- autism and related behavior disorders: Guide-
ceptive dysphasia: A comparative follow-up lines for research protocols. Journal of the
into middle childhood. Journal of Autism and American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
Developmental Disorders, 19, 19–32. chiatry, 25, 221–224.
Cass, H. (1998). Visual impairment and autism: De Sanctis, S. (1906). Sopra alcune varieta della
Current questions and future research. Autism: demenza precoce [Concening various kinds
International Journal of Research and Practice, of precocious dementia]. Rivista Sperimentale
2, 117–138. di Freniatria e di Medicina Legale, 32,
Cass, H., Sonksen, P., & McConachie, H. (1994). 141–165.
Developmental setback in severe visual im- De Sanctis, S. (1908). Dementia praecocissima cata-
pairment. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, tonica oder katatonie des fruheren kinder-
70, 192–196. salters? [Catatonic dementia praecox or
Castelloe, P., & Dawson, G. (1993). Subclassifica- catatonia of early childhood?]. Folia Neurobio-
tion of children with autism and pervasive de- logica, 2, 9–12.
velopmental disorder: A questionnaire based on Despert, J. C. (1938). Schizophrenia in children.
Wing’s subgrouping scheme. Journal of Autism Psychiatric Quarterly, 12, 366–371.
and Developmental Disorders, 23, 229–242. Earl, C. J. C. (1934). The primitive catatonic psy-
Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children with congeni- chosis of idiocy. British Journal of Medical
tal rubella. Journal of Autism and Childhood Psychology, 14, 230–253.
Schizophrenia, 1, 33–47. Eaves, L., Ho, H., & Eaves, D. (1994). Subtypes of
Chess, S. (1977). Follow-up report on autism in con- autism by cluster analysis. Journal of Autism
genital rubella. Journal of Autism and Child- and Developmental Disorders, 24, 3–22.
hood Schizophrenia, 7, 69–8l. Ehlers, S., & Gillberg, C. (1993). The epidemiol-
Churchill, D., & Bryson, C. Q. (1972). Looking ogy of Asperger syndrome: A total population
and approach behavior of psychotic and nor- study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
mal children as a function of adult attention or atry, 34, 1327–1350.
preoccupation. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 13, Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M. R., Leekam, S., Wing, L.,
171–177. Gould, J., Welham, M., et al. (1996). Compar-
Clark, T., Feehan, C., Tinline, C., & Vostanis, P. ison of clinical symptoms in autism and As-
(1999). Autistic symptoms in children with at- perger’s disorder. Journal of the American
tention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. European Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 50–55. 35, 1523–1531.
Clarke, A. M., & Clarke, A. D. B. (1976). Early ex- Fombonne, E., & Chakrabarti, S. (2001). No evi-
perience: Myth and evidence. London: Open dence for a new variant of measles-mumps-
Books. rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics, 108, e56.
Cohen, D. J., Paul, R., & Volkman, F. R. (1986). Is- Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.
sues in the classification of pervasive and Oxford, England: Blackwell.
other developmental disorders: Toward DSM- Frith U. (Ed.). (1991). Autism and Asperger syn-
IV. Journal of the American Academy of Child drome. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
Psychiatry, 25( 2), 213–220. versity Press.
602 Theoretical Perspectives

Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., Eilers, L., & Ghaziuddin, Happé, F. (1999). Theory of mind and self con-
N. (1992). Autism and herpes simplex en- sciousness: What is it like to be autistic? Mind
cephalitis. Journal of Autism and Developmen- and Language, 14, 82–89.
tal Disorders, 22, 107–114. Hermelin, B. (2001). Bright splinters of the mind.
Gillberg, C. (1986). Onset at age 14 of a typical London: Jessica Kingsley.
autistic syndrome: A case report of a girl with Houston, R., & Frith, U. (2000). Autism in history.
herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Autism Oxford, England: Blackwell.
and Developmental Disorders, 16, 369–376. Howlin, P., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1995). The recog-
Gillberg, C. (1989). The borderland of autism and nition of autism in children with Down syn-
Rett syndrome: Five case histories to highlight drome: Implications for intervention and some
diagnostic difficulties. Journal of Autism and speculations about pathology. Developmental
Developmental Disorders, 19, 545–560. Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 406–414.
Gillberg, C. (1991). Autistic syndrome with the Hulse, W. C. (1954). Dementia infantilis. Journal of
onset at age 31 years: Herpes encephalitis as a Nervous and Mental Diseases, 119, 471–477.
possible model for childhood autism. Develop- Hunt, A., & Dennis, J. (1987). Psychiatric disorder
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, among children with tuberous sclerosis. Devel-
920–924. opmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29,
Gillberg, C. (1992). Autism and autistic-like condi- 190–198.
tions: Subclasses among disorders of empathy Hunt, A., & Shepherd, C. (1993). A prevalence
(The Emmanuel Miller Memorial Lecture study of autism in tuberous sclerosis. Journal
1991). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23,
atry, 33, 813–842. 323–340.
Gillberg, C., Gillberg, C., Rastam, M., & Wentz, Hurtig, R., Ensrud, S., & Tomblin, J. B. (1982).
E. (2001). Asperger syndrome (and high- The communicative function of question pro-
functioning autism) Diagnostic Interview duction in autistic children. Journal of Autism
(ASDI ): A preliminary study of a new struc- and Developmental Disorders, 12, 57–69.
tured clinical interview. Autism: International Joseph, A. (1992). Catatonia. In A. Joseph & R.
Journal of Research and Practice, 5, 57–66. Young (Eds.), Movement disorders in neurology
Gillberg, C., & Rasmussen, P. (1994). Four case his- and neuropsychiatry (pp. 342–345). Oxford,
tories and a literature review of Williams’ syn- England: Blackwell.
drome and autistic behavior. Journal of Autism Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec-
and Developmental Disorders, 24, 381–393. tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Gillberg, C., Rasmussen, P., Carlström, G., Sven- Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical lan-
son, B., & Waldenström, E. (1982). Perceptual, guage in early childhood autism. American
motor and attentional deficits in six-year-old Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242–246.
children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Kanner, L. (1949). Problems of nosology and psy-
chiatry, 23, 131–144. chodynamics in early infantile autism. Ameri-
Gillberg, C., & Rastam, M. (1992). Do some cases of can Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 19, 416–426.
anorexia nervosa reflect underlying autistic-like Kanner, L. (1965). Infantile autism and the schizo-
conditions? Behavioural Neurology, 5, 27–32. phrenias. Behavioural Science, 10, 412–420.
Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Asperger Kanner, L., & Eisenberg, L. (1956). Early infantile
syndrome: Some epidemiological considera- autism, 1943–1955. American Journal of Or-
tions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- thopsychiatry, 26, 55–65.
atry, 30, 631–638. Kerbeshian, J., & Burd, L. (1986). Asperger’s syn-
Goldfarb, W. (1974). Growth and change of schizo- drome and Tourette syndrome. British Journal
phrenic children. New York: Wiley. of Psychiatry, 148, 731–735.
Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A., Henderson, L., Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., &
Huber, J., & Henderson, S. (2002). The severity Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying
and nature of motor impairment in Asperger’s the social phenotype in autism. American
syndrome: A comparison with specific develop- Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 895–908.
mental disorder of motor function. Journal of Klin, A., Volkmar, F., & Sparrow, S. (1992). Autis-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 655–668. tic social dysfunction: Some limitations of the
Hagberg, B., Aicardi, J., Dias, K., & Ramos, O. theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of Child
(1983). A progressive syndrome of autism, de- Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 861–876.
mentia, ataxia, and loss of purposeful hand use Koluchova, J. (1972). Severe deprivation in twins:
in girls: Rett’s syndrome. A report of 35 cases. A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Annals of Neurology, 14, 471–479. Psychiatry, 13, 107–114.
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 603

Koluchova, J. (1976). A report on the further devel- adult life: I. Cognitive and language outcomes.
opment of twins after severe and prolonged de- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
privation. In A. M. Clarke & A. D. B. Clarke 41, 547–559.
(Eds.), Early experience: Myth and evidence Meryash, D. L., Szymanski, L. S., & Park, G. S.
(pp. 56–66). London: Open Books. (1982). Infantile autism associated with the
Kolvin, I. (1971). Studies in the childhood psy- Fragile-X syndrome. Journal of Autism and
choses: I. Diagnostic criteria and classification. Developmental Disorders, 12, 295–302.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 381–384. Newson, E. (1983). Pathological demand-avoidance
Kolvin, I., Ounsted, C., Humphrey, M., & McNay, syndrome: Communication, 17, 3–8.
A. (1971). Studies in the childhood psychoses: Newson, E., Le Maréchal, K., & David, C. (2003).
II. The phenomenology of childhood psy- Pathological demand avoidance syndrome: A
choses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 118, necessary distinction within the pervasive de-
385–395. velopmental disorders. Archives of Diseases of
Lane, H. (1977). The wild boy of Aveyron. London: Childhood, 88, 595–600.
Allen & Unwin. Nylander, L., & Gillberg, C. (2001). Screening for
Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., autism spectrum disorders in adult psychiatric
Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989). out-patients: A preliminary report. Acta Psy-
Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized chiatrica Scandinavica, 103, 428–434.
investigator instrument. Journal of Autism and O’Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1988). Low intelli-
Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387. gence and special abilities. Journal of Child
Leekam, S., Libby, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Gill- Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 391–396.
berg, G. (2000). Comparison of ICD-10 and Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni-
Gillberg’s criteria for Asperger syndrome. tion of children with autism: A study of first
Autism: International Journal of Research and birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism
Practice, 4, 11–28. and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–258.
Leekam, S., Libby, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Taylor, Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental
C. (2002). The Diagnostic Interview for Social language disorders: Nosologic considerations.
and Communication Disorders: Algorithms for In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language,
ICD-10 childhood autism, and Wing and Gould reading and spelling (pp. 155–184). New York:
autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Academic Press.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 327–342. Realmuto, G. M., & August, G. J. (1991). Catatonia
Lister Brook, S., & Bowler, D. (1992). Autism by in autistic disorder: A sign of co-morbidity or
another name? Semantic and pragmatic im- variable expression? Journal of Autism and De-
pairments in children. Journal of Autism and velopmental Disorders, 21, 517–528.
Developmental Disorders, 22, 61–82. Realmuto, G. M., & Main, B. (1982). Coincidence
Lord, C. (1984). The development of peer relations of Tourette’s disorder and infantile autism.
in children with autism. In F. J. Morrison, C. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
Lord, & D. P. Keating (Eds.), Applied develop- ders, 12, 367–372.
mental psychology (pp. 165–229). New York: Ricks, D. M. (1975). Vocal communication in
Academic Press. pre-verbal normal and autistic children. In
Lotter, V. (1966). Epidemiology of autistic condi- N. O’Connor (Ed.), Language, cognitive
tions in young children: I. prevalence. Social deficits and retardation (pp. 75–80). London:
Psychiatry, 1, 124–137. Butterworths.
Lotter, V. (1974). Factors related to outcome in Ricks, D. M. (1979). Making sense of experience to
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Child- make sensible sounds. In M. Bullowa (Ed.),
hood Schizophrenia, 4, 263–277. Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal
Mahler, M. S. (1952). On child psychoses and communication (pp. 245–268). Cambridge:
schizophrenia: Autistic and symbiotic infan- Cambridge University Press.
tile psychoses. Psychoanalytic Study of the Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, com-
Child, 7, 286–305. munication and the use of symbols in normal
Maudsley, H. (1867). Insanity of early life. In H. and autistic children. Journal of Autism and
Maudsley (Ed.), The physiology and pathol- Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.
ogy of the mind (pp. 259–386). New York: Rimland, B. (1965). Infantile autism. London:
Appleton. Methuen.
Mawhood, I., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Rogers, D. (1992). Motor disorder in psychiatry:
Autism and developmental receptive language Towards a neurological psychiatry. Chichester,
disorder—a comparative follow-up in early England: Wiley.
604 Theoretical Perspectives

Rourke, B., & Tsatsanis, K. (2000). Non-verbal Treffert, D. (1989). Extraordinary people. New
learning disabilities and Asperger syndrome. York: Bantam Books.
In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow Tsai, L. (1992). Is Rett syndrome a subtype of per-
(Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 231–253). New vasive developmental disorders? Journal of
York: Guilford Press. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22,
Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia recon- 551–562.
sidered. Journal of Autism and Childhood Udwin, O., Yule, W., & Martin, N. (1987). Cogni-
Schizophrenia, 2, 315–337. tive abilities and behavioral characteristics of
Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. children with idiopathic infantile hypercal-
Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reap- cemia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
praisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 1–25). chiatry, 28, 297–310.
New York: Plenum Press. Volkmar, F. (1992). Childhood disintegrative disor-
Rutter, M., Anderson-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bre- der: Issues for DSM-IV. Journal of Autism and
denkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. Developmental Disorders, 22, 625–642.
(1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following se- Volkmar, F., Cicchetti, D., & Bregman, J. (1992).
vere early global privation. Journal of Child Three diagnostic systems for autism: DSM-III,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 537–550. DSM-III-R and ICD-10. Journal of Autism and
Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A five to fifteen Developmental Disorders, 22, 483–492.
year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: I. Volkmar, F., Cohen, D., Bregman, J., Hooks, M., &
Description of the sample. British Journal of Stevenson, J. (1989). An examination of social
Psychiatry, 113, 1169–1182. subtypologies in autism. Journal of the Ameri-
Rutter, M., Mawhood, L., & Howlin, P. (1992). Lan- can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychol-
guage delay and social development. In ogy, 28, 82–86.
P. Fletcher & D. Hall (Eds.), Specific speech Volkmar, F., Stier, D., & Cohen, D. (1985). Age of
and language disorders in children (pp. 63–78). recognition of pervasive developmental dis-
London: Whurr. order. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142,
Sacks, O. (1982). Awakenings (Rev. ed.). London: 1450–1452.
Pan Books. Wakabayashi, S. (1979). A case of infantile autism
Shapiro, A. K., Shapiro, E. S., Brown, R. D., & associated with Down’s syndrome. Journal of
Sweet, R. D. (1978). Gilles de la Tourette syn- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 31–36.
drome. New York: Raven Press. Waterhouse, L., Wing, L., Spitzer, R., & Siegal, B.
Szatmari, P. (1991). Asperger’s syndrome: Diagno- (1992). Pervasive developmental disorders:
sis, treatment, and outcome. Psychiatric Clin- From DSM-III to DSM-III R. Journal of Autism
ics of North America, 14, 81–93. and Developmental Disorders, 22, 525–550.
Szatmari, P. (1992). The validity of autistic spec- Wentz, E. (2000). Ten-year outcome of anorexia ner-
trum disorders: A literature review. Journal vosa with teen-age onset. Gothenberg: Gothen-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, berg University, Sweden.
583–600. Wing, L. (1969). The handicaps of autistic chil-
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R. S., Bond, dren: A comparative study. Journal of Child
S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 1–40.
functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism Wing, L. (1981a). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical
and Developmental Disorders, 19, 213–226. account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.
Szatmari, P., Brenner, R., & Nagy, J. (1989). As- Wing, L. (1981b). Language, social and cognitive
perger syndrome: A review of clinical fea- impairments in autism and severe mental re-
tures. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, tardation. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
554–560. tal Disorders, 11, 31–44.
Taft, L. T., & Cohen, H. J. (1971). Hypsarrhythmia Wing, L. (1988). The continuum of autistic char-
and childhood autism: A clinical report. Jour- acteristics. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov
nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, (Eds.), Diagnosis and assessment in autism
327–336. (pp. 91–110). New York: Plenum Press.
Tantam, D. (1988). Asperger’s syndrome. Journal Wing, L. (1991). Asperger syndrome and Kanner’s
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, autism. In U. Frith (Ed. & Trans.), Autism and
245–255. Asperger syndrome (pp. 93–121). Cambridge
Thomsen, P. (1994). Obsessive-compulsive disor- England: Cambridge University Press.
der in children and adolescents. A review of Wing, L. (1997a). Asperger’s syndrome: Manage-
the literature. European Child and Adolescent ment requires diagnosis. Journal of Forensic
Psychiatry, 3, 138–158. Psychiatry, 8, 253–257.
Problems of Categorical Classification Systems 605

Wing, L. (1997b). The history of ideas on autism: Wolff, S. (1995). Loners: The life path of unusual
Legends, myths and reality. Autism: Interna- children. London: Routledge.
tional Journal of Research and Practice, 1, Wolff, S., & Chess, S. (1964). A behavioral study
13–23. of schizophrenic children. Acta Psychiatrica
Wing, L. (2000). Past and future research on Scandinavica, 40, 438–466.
Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, Wolff, S., & McGuire, R. J. (1995). Schizoid person-
& S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome ality in girls: A follow-up study. What are the
(pp. 418–432). New York: Guilford Press. links with Asperger’s syndrome? Journal of
Wing, L. (2001). The autistic spectrum (pp. 45–56). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 793–818.
Berkeley, CA: Ulysses. World Health Organization. (1967). Manual of the
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments international statistical classification of dis-
of social interaction and associated abnormal- eases, injuries and causes of death (8th ed.,
ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica- Vol. 1). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental World Health Organization. (1977). Manual of the
Disorders, 9, 11–29. international statistical classification of dis-
Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S., & Brierley, L. eases, injuries and causes of death (9th ed.,
(1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally re- Vol. 1). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
tarded and in autistic children. Journal of Child World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10
Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 167–178. classification of mental and behavioral disor-
Wing, L., Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Gould, J., & ders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva,
Larcombe, M. (2002). The Diagnostic Inter- Switzerland: Author.
view for Social and Communication disorders: Wright, E. C. (1982). The presentation of mental ill-
Background, inter-rater reliability and clinical ness in mentally retarded adults. British Journal
use. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- of Psychiatry, 141, 496–502.
try, 43, 307–325.
Wing, L., & Shah, A. (2000). Catatonia in autistic
spectrum disorders. British Journal of Psychi-
atry, 176, 357–362.
CHAPTER 22

Executive Functions

SALLY OZONOFF, MIKLE SOUTH, AND SHERRI PROVENCAL

Executive function (EF) is the cognitive con- Initial Studies


struct used to describe goal-directed, future-
oriented behaviors thought to be mediated The first empirical investigation of the EFs
by the frontal lobes (Duncan, 1986), including of people with autism was done by Rumsey
planning, inhibition of prepotent responses, (1985), who administered the Wisconsin Card
flexibility, organized search, self-monitoring, Sorting Test (WCST), a measure of cognitive
and use of working memory (Baddeley, 1986; flexibility, to adult men with high-functioning
Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Pennington, 1994). Ex- autism. Relative to a sample of typical adults
ecutive dysfunction has been found in both matched on age, individuals with autism
individuals with autism and their family mem- demonstrated significant perseveration, sort-
bers, across many ages and functioning levels, ing by previously correct rules, despite feed-
on many different instruments purported to back that their strategies were incorrect. In a
measure executive function. This chapter sum- later study, Rumsey and Hamburger (1990)
marizes this still growing literature. We ex- demonstrated that this perseveration was not a
plore a number of issues that have emerged in general consequence of learning or develop-
the EF literature as the field has matured, in- mental disorders, as WCST impairment was
cluding the developmental trajectory of EF, specific to an adult sample with autism and
its relation to other cognitive abilities and fea- was not apparent in matched controls with se-
tures of autism, and its association with other vere dyslexia.
neurodevelopmental disorders. We conclude the Prior and Hoffmann (1990) were the first re-
chapter with thoughts about future research di- search team to administer the WCST to a pedi-
rections, including new findings that executive atric sample with autism. Like adults with
dysfunction may be familial, and suggestions autism, the children in this study made signifi-
for remediation. cantly more perseverative errors than matched
controls. They also performed significantly less
well than controls on the Milner Maze Test,
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN AUTISM: demonstrating deficits in planning and diffi-
A LITERATURE REVIEW culty learning from mistakes. The authors noted
that the autistic group “perseverated with mal-
Executive difficulties have been found in em- adaptive strategies, made the same mistakes re-
pirical investigations of people with autism peatedly, and seemed unable to conceive of a
for as long as EF tasks have been included in strategy to overcome their difficulties” (p. 588).
studies. Initial investigations were published The results of another study using the
two decades ago and the domain continues to WCST with individuals with high-functioning
be actively studied today. In this section, we autism were particularly interesting because
first review early studies of EF in autism and deficits were found relative to a control group
then present new issues that have emerged in with attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder
more recent investigations. (ADHD) and conduct disorder (Szatmari, Tuff,

606
Executive Functions 607

Finlayson, & Bartolucci, 1990). As discussed More Recent Studies of Executive


later in this chapter, executive dysfunction Function and Autism: Component
may be associated with these syndromes as Process Analyses
well (e.g., Chelune, Ferguson, Koon, & Dickey,
1986; Lueger & Gill, 1990). Despite this con- Executive function is a multidimensional
servative choice of control group, participants construct. The category includes a number of
with autism still made significantly more skills (flexibility, planning, inhibition, organi-
perseverative errors and completed fewer cate- zation, self-monitoring, goal-setting, working
gories on the WCST than the comparison sam- memory) that appear to be, to some extent, dis-
ple. Ozonoff, Pennington, and Rogers (1991) sociable. The tasks used in initial studies of
replicated this finding using a control group EF in autism were relatively imprecise, typi-
composed of children with learning and atten- cally measuring several executive operations,
tion difficulties. They found not only signifi- with no method to examine variance in indi-
cantly more perseveration by the group vidual skills. For example, the WCST, the
with autism but also significantly fewer fail- most widely used measure of EF in autism, is
ures to maintain set than the control group, a generally considered a test of cognitive flexi-
variable logically and conceptually opposite bility, but other operations also appear to be
that of perseveration. Performance on another required for successful performance, includ-
EF measure, the Tower of Hanoi, a test of plan- ing attribute identification, categorization,
ning, correctly predicted diagnosis in 80% of working memory, inhibition, selective atten-
subjects, while other neuropsychological vari- tion, and encoding of verbal feedback (Bond &
ables (e.g., theory of mind, memory, emotion Buchtel, 1984; Dehaene & Changeux, 1991;
perception, spatial abilities) predicted group Ozonoff, 1995; Perrine, 1993; van der Does &
membership at no better than chance levels. van den Bosch, 1992). To perform well on this
Following the sample longitudinally, Ozonoff task, subjects must be able to discriminate
and McEvoy (1994) found that deficits on among stimuli, classify them according to ab-
the Tower of Hanoi and WCST were stable over stract principles, inhibit previously reinforced
a 2.5-year period. EF abilities not only did responses, sustain attention to appropriate at-
not improve during the follow-up interval tributes of compound stimuli, and use verbal
but also showed a tendency to decline relative feedback, provided in the context of a social
to controls over time. Shu, Lung, Tien, and interaction, to change their behavior. When an
Chen (2001) reported significant deficits on individual receives a poor score on the WCST,
WCST performance in a sample of 26 Tai- it is difficult to determine which cognitive op-
wanese children with autism, relative to erations were responsible.
matched controls. Since these children were In the previous edition of this Handbook,
raised in a completely different culture and en- Ozonoff (1997) explored the utility of an infor-
vironment than the Western children who mation processing approach for examining indi-
participate in most EF studies, the authors sug- vidual components of EF. The information
gested that executive dysfunction may be a processing approach focuses on understanding
core impairment in autism. the sequence of mental operations involved in
In a review of the EF literature, Pennington the performance of cognitive tasks (e.g., infor-
and Ozonoff (1996) reported that 13 of the 14 mation input, encoding, transformation, selec-
studies existing at the time of publication tion, retrieval, and output). The information
demonstrated impaired performance on at processing perspective is not a specific model
least one EF task in autism, including 25 of or theory; rather, it is a broad framework
the 32 executive tasks used across those em- for understanding cognition. It provides rela-
pirical studies. The magnitude of group dif- tively theory-independent methods and spe-
ferences tended to be large, with an average cific experimental paradigms for understanding
effect size (Cohen’s d) across all studies of complex behavior (Anderson & Bower, 1973;
.98, marked by especially large effect sizes Ingram, 1989). Thus, a variety of cognitive
for the Tower of Hanoi (d = 2.07) and the models and constructs can be articulated and
WCST (d = 1.04). tested from within this framework. A central
608 Theoretical Perspectives

methodologic strategy of the information pro- the components of EF and determine which
cessing approach is component process analysis are impaired in autism.
(Farah, 1984; Friedrich & Rader, 1996). The Ozonoff and McEvoy (1994), for example,
goal of component process analysis is decompo- designed a novel Go-NoGo task to examine
sition of complex cognitive functions into the component skills that appear important to
elementary operations that appear to underlie WCST performance. The task consisted of
them, the time course and relationship of these three test conditions with a hierarchy of pro-
component processes to each other, and the in- cessing demands: (1) A “neutral inhibition”
ternal representations, schemas, or codes they condition required subjects to respond to a
act on (Friedrich & Rader, 1996). The compo- neutral stimulus while simultaneously inhibit-
nent process approach has been used for many ing responses to another neutral stimulus (this
years in the fields of experimental psychology condition required no shifting of cognitive
and cognitive neuropsychology. This perspec- set); (2) a “prepotent inhibition” condition re-
tive has relevance for elucidating cognitive quired inhibition of a previously reinforced,
mechanisms of autism as well. The component well-learned response; and (3) a “ flexibility”
process approach permits more detailed explo- condition necessitated frequent shifting from
ration of the specific role of EF in cognitive one response pattern to another, placing higher
and social development in both normal and ab- demands on cognitive flexibility. Individuals
normal populations and may facilitate research with autism performed as well as controls
into more precise interventions for individuals when inhibiting neutral responses but were
with autism spectrum disorders. This section moderately impaired when inhibiting prepo-
summarizes recent research examining the tent responses and very deficient in shifting
component processes of EF most closely related their response set. Interpretation of these re-
to autism. Later in the chapter, the component sults was complicated, however, by the con-
process approach is applied to executive reme- founding of the inhibition and flexibility
diation efforts. conditions. Specifically, the prepotent inhibi-
tion condition also required flexibility (i.e.,
Flexibility and Inhibition
when shifting from the response pattern re-
Two EFs that are conceptually linked but often quired in the neutral inhibition condition to
postulated to be separable are inhibition and the new response mode necessitated by the
flexibility. Some researchers have argued that prepotent inhibition condition). Because the
autism involves a primary deficit in inhibitory two constructs were not measured indepen-
control (Russell, Jarrold, & Henry, 1996; Rus- dently, it was difficult to determine which
sell, Saltmarsh, & Hill, 1999; Turner, 1997, cognitive operation, inhibition or flexibility,
1999), while others stress the central role of contributed more to the poor performance of
flexibility impairments in autism (Ozonoff & the prepotent inhibition condition.
Jensen, 1999). Many tasks confound the two Ozonoff and Strayer (1997) conducted a
cognitive operations and, even using a compo- second study that isolated inhibition and flexi-
nent process approach, it is difficult to design bility operations more completely. Two inhibi-
tasks that cleanly measure one or the other tion tasks were administered to a group of
process. For example, the act of shifting atten- high-functioning children with autism and a
tion has been postulated to require both inhibi- matched sample of typically developing chil-
tion of attention to previously relevant stimuli dren. In the Stop-Signal measure (Logan,
and movement of attention to new stimuli. 1994; Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984), subjects
Similarly, changing response patterns appears were engaged in a simple task in which they
to require both inhibition of the previous categorized words as animals or objects by
motor program and switching to a new motor pressing keys on a two-choice response box.
program (Luria, 1966; Sandson & Albert, On a subset of trials, an auditory signal was
1984). By extension, flexibly shifting cogni- presented to indicate that responses to the pri-
tive set (as in the WCST) would appear to con- mary task should be inhibited on that trial.
found these two processes as well, a problem Thus, this task measured the ability to control
that has complicated recent work to untangle a voluntary motor response and did not require
Executive Functions 609

any flexibility. The Negative Priming task Task (ID/ ED) is a computerized set-shifting
(Tipper, 1985) measured cognitive (rather task that measures flexibility while control-
than motor) inhibitory mechanisms (Neill, ling for other cognitive processes that might
Lissner, & Beck, 1990). Participants saw a be important to task performance. A series
five-letter string (e.g., TVTVT) and were of compound stimuli composed of colored
asked to judge whether the second and fourth shapes and lines is presented. Participants
letters were the same or different. On some tri- learn, through trial and error with computer-
als, the target stimuli ( letters 2 and 4) were generated feedback, to respond to the shape;
the same as the distractor stimuli ( letters 1, 3, the line is effectively an irrelevant dimension.
and 5) from the immediately preceding trial. It Once training to the shape is complete, the ne-
has been demonstrated that when distractors cessity to perform two kinds of shifts takes
from previous trials become targets on subse- place. In the first intradimensional shift, new
quent trials, performance is slower and less shapes and lines are introduced, but shape re-
accurate than if the stimuli had not been previ- mains the relevant response dimension. In
ously seen (Tipper, 1985). This disruption the later extradimensional shift, the contin-
in performance, termed the negative priming gencies change, with the line becoming the
ef fect, is thought to be due to the costs of ac- salient stimulus and the previously trained
tively inhibiting attention to the stimulus shape now irrelevant. Only the extradimen-
when it was a distractor in earlier trials. Thus, sional shift requires conceptual flexibility
a weak negative priming effect indicates defi- (i.e., shifting from one concept or cognitive
cient cognitive inhibition (Neill et al., 1990). set to another); the intradimensional shift re-
Ozonoff and Strayer (1997) found that sub- quires only perceptual flexibility, or shifting
jects with autism were unimpaired, relative to from one exemplar to another exemplar within
age- and IQ-matched normal controls, on both the same cognitive set (e.g., shape). This task
tests of inhibition. On the Stop-Signal task, no is functionally similar to the category shifts
group differences were evident in the likeli- required by the WCST, but conceptually sim-
hood of responding on signal trials (i.e., when pler and with multiple manipulations built in
responses should have been withheld). On the to control for other sources of impairment,
Negative Priming task, both groups demon- such as inhibitory dysfunction or discrimina-
strated an intact negative priming effect and tion learning deficits. Experiments on mar-
there were no significant group differences in moset monkeys with prefrontal lesions suggest
the magnitude of this effect. That is, when dis- that both orbital and lateral regions of pre-
tractors on one trial became targets on subse- frontal cortex are involved in the extradimen-
quent trials, the act of previously ignoring sional shift (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996).
these stimuli slowed reaction time and in- Three recent investigations have used
creased error rate to a similar extent in both the CANTAB ID/ ED subtest with individuals
groups. Thus, across tasks measuring both with autism spectrum disorders (Hughes,
motor and cognitive components of inhibition, Russell, & Robbins, 1994; Ozonoff et al., 2004;
the inhibitory control of the autistic group was Turner, 1997). Relative to matched controls
similar to that of matched typically developing with mental retardation, individuals with
controls. This effect was recently replicated autism and mental retardation demonstrated in-
using a different negative priming paradigm tact performance during the early phases of the
(Brian, Tipper, Weaver, & Bryson, 2003). task measuring discrimination learning, in-
Consistent results have been found by other hibitory control, and intradimensional shifting,
research teams employing different paradigms but impairment at the extradimensional shift
as well. Several groups have used a test from (Hughes et al., 1994). The authors concluded
the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Auto- that deficits in flexibility “rather than low level
mated Battery (CANTAB; Robbins et al., motoric inhibition” were most prominent on
1998) to tease apart the relative contributions this task (p. 488). Turner replicated the extradi-
of flexibility and inhibition in the perfor- mensional shifting deficit in individuals with
mance deficits of individuals with autism. autism and mental retardation, but not in par-
The Intradimensional-Extradimensional Shift ticipants with autism of normal IQ, although
610 Theoretical Perspectives

small sample size and low power may have directed. On valid cue trials, one of the boxes
contributed to this result. In the most recent is brightened, followed by presentation of the
study, the ID/ ED subtest was administered target in that box. On invalid cue trials, one of
to 79 participants with autism and 70 well- the boxes is brightened, followed by presenta-
matched typical controls recruited from seven tion of the target in the opposite box. On neu-
universities that are part of the Collaborative tral trials, both boxes brighten, rendering the
Programs of Excellence in Autism (CPEA) net- cue uninformative. Typically, a validity effect
work (Ozonoff et al., 2004). Significant group is obtained, in which targets are processed
differences were found in ID/ ED performance, more quickly on valid than on neutral trials
with the autism group showing intact intradi- and more slowly on invalid than on neutral tri-
mensional shifting, but deficits in extradimen- als. Using this paradigm, Wainwright-Sharp
sional shifting, relative to controls. Deficits and Bryson (1993) found no validity effect for
were found in both lower and higher IQ individ- subjects with autism when the cue was pre-
uals with autism across the age range of 6 to sented very briefly (100 msec) but a robust va-
47 years. lidity effect when the cue was presented for
Another form of flexibility that has longer duration (800 msec). This suggested
been investigated in autism is attention shift- that the participants with autism took longer
ing. Courchesne, Akshoomoff, and Ciesielski than controls to disengage attention from the
(1990; Courchesne et al., 1994) designed a fixation cross and move it to the location indi-
task that examined the shifting of attention cated by the cue. Very similar results were ob-
between sensory modalities. Subjects were tained by others using the same paradigm
told to monitor one modality (either auditory (Casey, Gordon, Mannheim, & Rumsey, 1993),
or visual) until an oddball target was detected reinforcing the suggestion that the disen-
and then shift to the other modality to find gage/move component of attention is dysfunc-
targets. “False alarm” errors occurred when tional in autism.
subjects failed to disengage from the first Rinehart and colleagues also found deficits
modality and inappropriately continued to on an attention-shifting task in a group of boys
respond to old targets, while “misses” oc- with high-functioning autism, compared to
curred when subjects failed to quickly move typically developing controls matched on age,
or reengage attention in the new channel, re- IQ, and sex (Rinehart, Bradshaw, Moss, Br-
sulting in failure to detect new targets. Aver- ereton, & Tonge, 2001). They used a global-
age IQ adults with autism performed as well local task, in which stimuli were large (global)
as typical controls in the first phase of the digits composed of smaller ( local) digits. Tar-
task, which required no shifting. Performance gets could appear at either the global or local
was more than six standard deviations below level, necessitating shifting attention between
that of controls, however, during the phase stimulus levels on a trial-by-trial basis. The
that required rapid alternation of attention group with autism was significantly slower to
between auditory and visual channels. Results find global targets when the previous stimulus
suggested that the deficit of the group with was processed at the local level, suggesting de-
autism was primarily during the disengage lays in shifting between processing levels.
operation, as evidenced by a high false alarm These studies, in aggregate, suggest that
rate but a normal miss rate. operations that require flexibility, including
Another paradigm that has been used to both shifting of cognitive set and shifting of at-
study attention shifting in individuals with tentional focus, are impaired in individuals
autism is the visuospatial orienting task of Pos- with autism, while inhibitory functions appear
ner (1980). In this task, two boxes are posi- relatively more intact. A recent study suggested
tioned on either side of a central fixation cross that cognitive flexibility was a particularly
on a computer screen. Targets appear in one of good predictor of outcome (Berger, Aerts,
the two boxes, and subjects are instructed to van Spaendonck, Cools, & Teunisse, 2003), fur-
respond to them as quickly as possible. A visu- ther highlighting its potential significance to
ospatial cue is presented just before the target autism. Specifically, it was found that perfor-
appears, indicating where attention should be mance on a set-shifting task was better able
Executive Functions 611

than tasks in other cognitive domains to predict different from the digit of either one or two
social understanding and social competence in previous trials) than on tasks of nonverbal
high-functioning adolescents and adults with working memory (a box search task, with
autism. penalties for returning to locations that had
already been examined, and a spatial span
Working Memory
task). This prediction was not borne out, and
Another component of EF that has been the group with autism performed as well as
explored in people with autism is working both comparison groups on all tasks, despite
memory. This term refers to the ability to having a nonsignificant but still substantial
maintain information in an activated, online IQ disadvantage of approximately two-thirds
state to guide cognitive processing (Baddeley, of a standard deviation.
1986). Initial interest in working memory Thus, there is mixed evidence for working
in autism was driven by studies of perfor- memory as an impaired component of EF. This
mance on Tower tasks (Tower of Hanoi, Tower has prompted some reconsideration of Tower
of London), which, as reviewed earlier, is tasks as measures of working memory. A task
typically poor in individuals with autism. analysis performed by Goel and Grafman
Tower tasks are thought to measure planning (1995) suggests that Tower tasks measure plan-
and, thus, at least intuitively, should require ning functions less than they might appear, but
working memory (e.g., maintaining a repre- are instead primary measures of the ability to
sentation of a potential move online while resolve goal-subgoal conflicts. Tower tasks
considering its consequences). Bennetto, Pen- often require participants to perform moves
nington, and Rogers (1996) found that adoles- that appear, at a superficial level, to be incor-
cents and adults with high-functioning autism rect or opposite the goal state. Failure to appre-
were significantly impaired relative to age- ciate this results in poorer task performance
and IQ-matched controls on several tests and lower planning efficiency scores, but for
of verbal working memory (counting and reasons more conceptually related to flexibil-
sentence span tasks), while they performed ity than to working memory. It is not currently
similarly to controls on tests of declarative clear whether working memory is a specific
memory function, such as rote short-term, difficulty for people with autism, and more re-
verbal long-term, and recognition memory. search is needed.
In contrast, other studies have failed to find
working memory deficits in autism. In an in- Section Summary
vestigation by Russell and colleagues, a group
with both autism and mental retardation did not This body of research begins to clarify the na-
differ from matched controls on three measures ture of executive dysfunction in autism. While
of verbal working memory capacity, a dice- tasks employed in initial research, such as the
counting task, an odd-man-out task, and a sen- WCST, suggested impairments in flexibility,
tence span test (Russell et al., 1996). Similarly, they were relatively imprecise measures that
a case report of an individual with autism and confounded a number of executive processes.
mental retardation demonstrated deficits in Further work has refined our ability to exam-
flexibility but normal working memory (Mot- ine specific executive components and their
tron, Peretz, Belleville, & Rouleau, 1999). And respective associations with autism. This work
no group differences were found in a higher suggests that inhibitory control and possibly
functioning sample, relative to matched com- working memory are relatively spared func-
parison groups with Tourette’s syndrome and tions, while mental flexibility of a variety of
typical development, on three tasks of working types (set shifting, attention shifting) appears
memory in a third study (Ozonoff & Strayer, compromised (Hill & Russell, 2002; Hughes,
2001). One hypothesis of this study was that 2002; Ozonoff & Jensen, 1999). As the af-
performance would be more impaired on a task fected components of EF in autism have been
of verbal working memory (an n-back task, in clarified, a number of additional interesting
which participants had to identify whether the issues have emerged from the EF literature in
digit on the computer screen was the same as or recent years.
612 Theoretical Perspectives

EMERGING ISSUES suggested that these tasks may have been less
developmentally appropriate for the sample.
As the study of EF in autism has continued, However, in another investigation by the same
several new issues have arisen, suggesting that research team (Griffith, Pennington, Wehner, &
the story is not as simple as it first appeared. Rogers, 1999), studying even younger children
with autism (mean age = 4 years, 3 months),
Developmental Course of there were no differences in performance on any
Executive Dysfunction of eight executive tasks (including the spatial
reversal task), compared to a developmentally
An important question related to the contri- delayed group matched on chronological age and
bution of executive processes to autism centers both verbal and nonverbal mental age. Based on
on when deficits emerge. Historically, based limited normative data (Diamond et al., 1997),
primarily on work with adult patients, the both groups performed at levels lower than
frontal lobes were assumed to become function- expected for their mental age, suggesting that
ally mature only in adolescence; however, both EF impairment at this age may not be autism-
developmental research and animal models specific, but rather a function of general devel-
have shown that this brain region is operational, opmental delay. Likewise, in a larger study (n =
remarkably capable, and adaptable throughout 72) of even younger children with autism (mean
development (Duncan, 2001; Hughes & Gra- age = 3 years, 8 months), Dawson et al. (2002)
ham, 2002). EF research on children and ani- reported no significant differences on six EF
mals necessarily requires adapting tasks to tasks (again including spatial reversal), relative
appropriate levels; the resulting creativity and to developmentally delayed and typically devel-
simplicity have been very beneficial for compo- oping control groups matched on mental age.
nent process research (Dawson et al., 2002; This work raises the possibility that differ-
Diamond, Prevor, Callender, & Druin, 1997; ential EF deficits emerge with age and are not
Hughes & Graham, 2002). present (at least relative to other samples with
Two research groups have tested age-related delayed development) early in the preschool
EF development in very young children with range. Whether this is because of a general
autism. The first investigation to examine EFs deficit, common to developmental delay, or
in preschool-age children with autism was con- whether there is no delay in autism early on is
ducted by McEvoy, Rogers, and Pennington not certain. Since EFs are just beginning to de-
(1993). They used several developmentally sim- velop during the early preschool period in all
ple measures of prefrontal function that were children, a relative lack of variance across
first designed for use with nonhuman primates groups may explain this apparent developmen-
and human infants (Diamond & Goldman- tal discontinuity. Differences in the way EF is
Rakic, 1986). Their sample included young measured at different ages may also contribute
children with autism (mean age = 5 years, 4 to this finding. The executive tests that have
months) and matched developmentally delayed been administered to very young children with
and typically developing control groups. In the autism do not require the same use of arbitrary
spatial reversal task, an object was hidden in rules that those given to older individuals do.
one of two identical wells outside the subject’s If arbitrary rule use is central to the EF per-
vision. The side of hiding remained the same formance deficits of autism (Biro & Russell,
until the subject successfully located the object 2001), the discontinuity between earlier and
on four consecutive trials, after which the side later development may be due simply to mea-
of hiding was changed to the other well. Thus, surement differences.
successful search behavior required flexibility Further work, particularly longitudinal re-
and set shifting. Significant group differences search, is needed to examine when during devel-
were found, with the children with autism opment specific executive difficulties emerge
making more perseverative errors than children and what their developmental precursors may
in either the mental- or chronological-age- be. It has been argued, for example, that execu-
matched groups. However, no group differences tive dysfunction is secondary to (and thus dri-
were evident on three other EF measures. It was ven by) other earlier appearing symptoms, a
Executive Functions 613

topic to which we turn next. This timing argu- reported that performance on executive tasks
ment has at times been used to determine which was significantly correlated with measures of
cognitive or psychological processes are core or social interaction, including joint attention, in
“primary” to autism. Emergence early in devel- preschool-age autism and control groups. This
opment does not necessarily indicate primacy, was an intriguing finding, as joint attention in-
nor must development of an impairment over tuitively would appear to require rapid attention
time imply that it is secondary. If autism is a shifting and, by extension, intact EF. In fact, an
disorder with multiple core deficits, as many re- EEG study indicated that activity in the left
searchers suspect, then it is plausible that dif- frontal region was associated with the initiation
ferent impairments may come online at different of joint attention in typical infants (Mundy,
points in development. Card, & Fox, 2000). Also examining very young
children with autism, Griffith et al. (1999)
Relationship to Other Cognitive replicated the finding of significant correlations
Impairments and Symptoms of Autism between joint attention and EFs. In contrast,
Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, and Rinaldi (1998;
The relationship among EFs, other cognitive Dawson et al., 2002) did not find a relationship
and social-cognitive processes, and the devel- between joint attention and executive perfor-
opment of autism is complex and has been mance in their young sample of children with
explored in several recent studies. The ex- autism, but instead found an association with a
planatory power of executive dysfunction to memory task, the delayed nonmatch to sample
autism would be greatest if individual differ- task. And Swettenham et al. (1998) found that
ences in EF predicted variations in other im- young children with autism have more difficulty
pairments or in symptoms of autism. In this shifting attention between social than nonsocial
section, we discuss the relationships among stimuli, suggesting that the impairment is not
EFs, social-cognitive processes, language, in- simply in the shifting process, but interacts with
telligence, and autistic symptoms. the nature of the stimulus.
The relationship between executive dys-
Executive Function and Social Processes
function and theory of mind impairment has
Prefrontal cortex appears to be involved not only also been studied. Ozonoff et al. (1991) found
in EF performance but also in the regulation significant correlations between performance
of social behavior, emotional reactions, and so- on EF and false belief tasks, and this finding
cial discourse (Dennis, 1991; Grattan, Bloomer, has since been replicated several times (see
Archambault, & Eslinger, 1990; Price, Daffner, Perner & Lang, 1999, for a review). Several ex-
Stowe, & Mesulam, 1990; Stuss, 1992), so rela- planations for this association have been pro-
tionships among these skills are not unexpected. posed: (1) the deficits are independent modular
Bennetto et al. (1996) provide a coherent model cognitive operations that are parallel central
of how executive deficits could lead to social impairments of autism, (2) one ability is a pre-
difficulties in autism, as effective social inter- requisite for the other, so that deficits in one
action depends on the ability to hold a vari- cause deficits in the other, (3) both are driven
able stream of context-specific information in by a third shared impairment, or (4) both share
mind, including subtle verbal and nonverbal cues, similar neural underpinnings (Hughes, 2001;
then plan and respond to this ever-changing Ozonoff, 1995; Rutter & Bailey, 1993).
stream appropriately and flexibly. As men- One way to examine these possibilities is to
tioned earlier, significant correlations between experimentally manipulate task requirements
performance on set-shifting tasks and social to see which ability contributes more to suc-
understanding tasks have been found (Berger cess or failure on the task. An early study of
et al., 2003), as have relationships between EF this type was performed by Russell and col-
and adaptive behavior (Gilotty, Kenworthy, leagues, using a task originally designed to ex-
Sirian, Black, & Wagner, 2002). amine strategic deception ability (Russell,
One of the first studies to document a rela- Mauthner, Sharpe, & Tidswell, 1991). Children
tionship between social and executive processes with autism were taught to play a game in
was conducted by McEvoy et al. (1993), who which they competed with an experimenter for
614 Theoretical Perspectives

a piece of candy. The candy was placed in one theory of mind development is a consequence
of two boxes with windows that revealed the of improvements in executive control (Perner,
contents of the box to subjects, but not to the Kain, & Barchfeld, 2002). Perner and Lang
experimenter. The objective of the task was to (2002), having, in their minds, discounted the
“ fool” the experimenter into looking for the strictly executive account of false belief and
candy in the empty box. It was explained that other mentalizing skills, concluded that there
the strategy of pointing to the empty box would was still no clear explanation to account for the
be successful in winning the candy, whereas remarkably strong relationship between theory
pointing to the box that contained the chocolate of mind and executive tasks.
would result in losing it. Even after many tri- Indeed, the story was not so simple and the
als, however, the participants with autism were opposite hypothesis, that some level of social
unable to point to the empty box, despite the awareness is necessary for EF, has also re-
consequences of this strategy. Russell et al. ceived support. In the WCST, for example,
first attributed these results to a perspective- feedback is provided by the examiner after
taking deficit that caused an inability to engage each card is sorted; successful set shifting re-
in deception. In a follow-up study, however, quires using this feedback to alter behavior. If,
Hughes and Russell (1993) demonstrated that however, feedback supplied in a social context
significant group differences remained even is less salient or more difficult to process for
after the element of deception was removed people with autism, they may perform poorly
from the task. Subjects were simply instructed on EF tasks for primarily social reasons. A few
to point to the empty box to get the candy. Even studies have contrasted performance on execu-
with no opponent present, the children with tive tests when they are administered in the
autism persisted in using the inappropriate traditional manner—by human examiners—to
strategy. Hughes and Russell attributed this performance when they are administered by
pattern of performance to a deficit in disengag- computer. Ozonoff (1995) reported that the
ing from the object and using internal rules to WCST was significantly easier for individuals
guide behavior. This behavior is similar to the with autism when it was given by computer,
perseveration, inappropriate strategy use, and with group differences considerably smaller in
stimulus overselectivity documented on the the computer administration than the human
WCST and other tasks. administration conditions. In the group with
The work of Russell and Hughes led the way autism, the number of perseverations was cut
for several other studies that explored the hy- in half on the computerized version of the task,
pothesis that some degree of executive control while performance did not differ across condi-
is necessary for successful performance on tions in the typically developing control group
theory of mind tasks and, by extension, for the (Ozonoff, 1995). Two other research teams
development of theory of mind (Moses, 2001; have found similar facilitation of performance
Russell, Saltmarsh, et al., 1999). In one of the on computer-administered executive tasks rela-
most recent studies of this kind, Perner and tive to standard ( human) administration (Grif-
Lang (2002) reported a pair of large studies of fith, 2002; Pascualvaca, Fantie, Papageorgiou,
typically developing preschool children in & Mirsky, 1998). This suggests that the format
which the executive account of false belief was of the executive task, particularly the nature of
explicitly tested. As in previous studies, the the feedback (social versus nonsocial), may
false belief performance was strongly corre- have a much greater impact on performance for
lated (r = .65) with performance on a simple people with autism than has previously been
card sorting task. However, against explicit appreciated. This may even help explain the ap-
predictions from the executive theory, the card parent developmental discontinuity of perfor-
sorting task was just as strongly correlated (r = mance on executive tests discussed earlier. In
.65) with a verbal explanation task that was not the tasks used with very young children with
dependent on executive abilities. Perner and autism (Griffith et al., 1999), feedback is sup-
colleagues also found intact theory of mind, plied by the apparatus itself (e.g., a tangible re-
but deficient EFs, in young children at risk of ward under a cup or in a well) and not by a
ADHD, arguing against the theory that later human. However, other studies have found ex-
Executive Functions 615

ecutive deficits in individuals with autism ample, “If the question is about me, and if it is
when the task was fully computerized and all asking what the contents really are, then the
feedback was provided by machine, with no so- answer is a pencil” versus “If the question is
cial interaction required, as in the CANTAB about someone else, and if it is asking what the
studies reported earlier (Ozonoff et al., 2004). contents are thought to be, then the answer is
And computerized tasks, in constraining the candy.” Thus, a critical skill to successful so-
problem space, reduce the opportunity for rule lution of this false-belief task is embedded use
violations and thus may be less sensitive to par- of if-then rules (Frye et al., 1995).
ticular types of executive deficits (Brophy, The Tower of Hanoi is a standard EF mea-
Taylor, & Hughes, 2002). Thus, it has been dif- sure in which subjects must sequentially move
ficult to tease apart the relative primacy of EF disks among pegs to duplicate a goal state
and mentalizing or other social deficits in the determined by the experimenter. To receive a
chain of cognitive impairments that are in- high score on this task, subjects must predict
volved in autism. intermediate disk configurations produced by
Another hypothesis is that EF and theory different potential moves, consider their impli-
of mind abilities share similar cognitive un- cations for future disk configurations, and eval-
derpinnings, and impairments in this basic uate their utility toward eventual attainment of
cognitive function drive both deficits. At a su- the goal state (Harris, 1993). Embedded rules,
perficial level of analysis, EF and theory of applied recursively, must again be used, for ex-
mind appear rather dissimilar. Focusing on the ample, “If I move the blue disk to peg 3, then
content of the domains may, however, obscure it will leave peg 1 open for the yellow disk, thus
similarities that exist at a process level of freeing up the red disk.” This component pro-
analysis. Several writers have suggested that cess analysis suggests that Tower tasks are
the operations involved in successful EF primary measures of rule-based reasoning and
and theory of mind task performance are sim- recursive rule use, as much as or more than they
ilar (Carlson, Moses, & Breton, 2002; Hughes, are measures of working memory (Goel &
1998; Perner & Lang, 1999). Grafman, 1995). Furthermore, this analysis
The Smarties task is a standard false-belief suggests that theory of mind and EF tasks,
measure (Perner, Frith, Leslie, & Leekam, which appear rather different at the content
1989). Subjects are shown a box of Smarties level, may be similar at the process level. Tasks
(similar to M&M’s) and asked what it con- in both domains appear to require recursive or
tains. After a response is given, the box is sequential analysis of information and embed-
opened to reveal that it holds a pencil. Subjects ded rule use (Frye et al., 1995; Hughes & Rus-
are then asked to predict what the next subject, sell, 1993). The focus is not on what type of
who has never seen the box, will think it con- information is processed but on how it is
tains. A pass is scored if the subject responds, processed. Other impairments of autism that
“Candy.” An analysis of this task (following appear different at the macroanalytic, surface,
Frye, Zelazo, & Palfai, 1995) suggests that or content level may be related at the microana-
successful performance requires consideration lytic or process level.
of two mental perspectives and two types of The last hypothesis regarding the relation-
cognitive judgments. The subject must attend ship between EF and theory of mind is that
to two different perspectives about the con- both abilities are subserved by neural net-
tents of the box—his or her own perspective works in the same brain regions (Ozonoff
and that of the other person—and must also et al., 1991). Imaging studies have, in fact,
make two types of judgments—what is thought provided support for this hypothesis, both con-
to be in the box and what is really in the box. firming the role of frontal cortex in EFs (Baker
As Frye et al. explain, subjects must employ et al., 1996; Dias et al., 1996) and demonstrat-
two recursive if-then rules to solve the problem ing the involvement of frontal cortex in networks
correctly. Using only one or the other rule will that are activated during social-cognition tasks
result in an incorrect answer. Successful per- (Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Fletcher et al.,
formance requires that the rules be considered 1995; Happé et al., 1996; Stone, Baron-Cohen,
in an embedded and sequential manner, for ex- & Knight, 1998). Component process research
616 Theoretical Perspectives

has to date been very useful for suggesting and be responsible for the stereotyped, repetitive
disproving theories and should continue to be behaviors of autism spectrum disorders. Further,
fruitful over the next decade, particularly in she hypothesized that different components of
combination with the new possibilities af- EF would be associated with different types of
forded by functional imaging techniques. repetitive behavior. In support of this, she re-
ported that perseveration on a set-shifting task
Relation to Language and Intelligence
was correlated with more primitive stereo-
The contribution of language development to typed behaviors, such as hand flapping, while
EF ability has also received significant re- impoverished generativity was correlated with
search attention. Hughes (1996) reported a higher level repetitive behaviors such as cir-
simple set-shifting deficit in preschool chil- cumscribed interests. In contrast, South,
dren with autism, who had significant diffi- Ozonoff, and McMahon (under review) found
culty imitating a simple hand gesture after no significant correlations between any cate-
being primed with a different hand movement. gory of repetitive behavior and any EF vari-
She suggested that the deficit arose from a able. For example, the correlations between the
failure to use language to control thoughts and number of perseverations on the WCST and
behavior. Russell, Jarrold, and Hood (1999) various forms of repetitive behavior ranged
explored this idea by devising an EF task that from a low of r = −.03 for lifetime history of
had no arbitrary or novel rules to follow and circumscribed interests to a high of r = .16 for
another that required only verbal response. lifetime history of unusual obsessions with
There were no differences in performance be- objects. This sample was significantly older
tween groups with autism, mixed developmen- (mean age = 14 years) and more intellectually
tal delay, and typical development. The capable (mean VIQ = 111) than Turner’s sam-
authors theorized that the deficient perfor- ple, so direct comparisons are difficult and
mance on EF tests of people with autism further research is clearly needed.
arises primarily from failure to verbally en-
code rules and use them to drive behavior. Specificity to Autism
When no such ability was required, perfor-
mance was predicted to be unimpaired. No review of the EF hypothesis would be
Liss et al. (2001) gave a battery of EF tests complete without discussion of the so-called
to children with high-functioning autism and a “discriminant validity” problem (Pennington,
control group of children with developmental Bennetto, McAleer, & Roberts, 1996). For a
language disorders. The only group difference, causal mechanism to have explanatory power,
more perserverative errors on the WCST by it should be relatively specific to the dis-
the autism group, disappeared when verbal IQ order it is intended to explain (Pennington &
(VIQ) was statistically controlled. Likewise, Ozonoff, 1991). Yet, difficulties in EF are
significant relationships between EF perfor- seen in a wide variety of disorders, including
mance and measures of autism symptom sever- ADHD (Chelune et al., 1986), conduct disorder
ity and adaptive behavior disappeared when (Lueger & Gill, 1990), early treated phenylke-
controlling for IQ. Liss et al. concluded that EF tonuria (Welsh, Pennington, Ozonoff, Rouse,
deficits, while common in autism, are likely a & McCabe, 1990), obsessive-compulsive dis-
function of more general cognitive impairments order (OCD; Head, Bolton, & Hymas, 1989),
and should not be considered causal. Ozonoff Tourette’s syndrome (Bornstein, 1990), and
has also identified significant contributions of schizophrenia (Axelrod, Goldman, Tompkins,
IQ to EF performance in people with autism & Jiron, 1994; Beatty, Jocic, Monson, &
(Miller & Ozonoff, 2000; Ozonoff & McEvoy, Katzung, 1994). If deficits in EF generally dis-
1994; Ozonoff & Strayer, 2001). tinguish “normal” from “abnormal,” but are
not specific indicators that distinguish one syn-
Relation to Repetitive Behavior
drome from another, their explanatory power is
Turner (1997, 1999) has suggested that execu- diminished.
tive dysfunction (e.g., perseveration, deficient The discriminant validity problem may be
inhibitory control, impaired generativity) may “more apparent than real” (Hughes, 2001),
Executive Functions 617

however. Once the large construct of EF is dence of distinct EF profiles in autism and
parsed into more unitary and functionally other neurodevelopmental disorders.
independent cognitive operations through
component process analyses, it appears that Section Summary
different neurodevelopmental disorders are
associated with different profiles of strength Although there remain many important ques-
and weakness in EF. For example, as discussed tions to be answered, research on the compo-
earlier, evidence suggests that inhibitory nent processes of executive dysfunction in
function may be intact in individuals with autism has made many gains since the publi-
autism (Ozonoff & Strayer, 1997). In con- cation of the previous edition of this Hand-
trast, performance on inhibition paradigms book. Robust findings of EF deficits in older
such as the Negative Priming and Stop-Signal children and adults with autism, relative to
tasks is deficient in both adults with schizo- appropriate clinical and normal controls, have
phrenia and children with attention problems been tempered by the discovery of more com-
(Beech, Powell, McWilliam, & Claridge, plex patterns of EF development in very
1989; Schachar & Logan, 1990). Other disso- young children with autism. It is likely that
ciations are apparent in the domain of atten- some important turning point is missed by
tion. The ability to sustain attention appears children with autism sometime during the
intact in autism, with several studies finding late preschool age; nonetheless, the specifics
unimpaired performance on the Continuous of this developmental milestone are not yet
Performance Test (Buchsbaum et al., 1992; elucidated. There are indications of signifi-
Casey et al., 1993; Garretson, Fein, & Water- cant correlations between EF abilities and
house, 1990). Deficits in sustained attention core symptoms of autism, beginning with
are a cardinal feature of ADHD (Douglas & very early social impairments and continuing
Peters, 1979) and are also prominent in throughout childhood and adulthood, but the
Tourette’s syndrome (Comings & Comings, causal directions and specific nature of these
1987), schizophrenia (Bellak, 1994; Cornblatt relationships are unknown. This section also
& Keilp, 1994), and other disorders. Autism reviewed associations between EF and other
does appear to involve difficulty shifting at- important characteristics of autism, including
tention, while this skill appears unimpaired in metacognitive mentalizing ability (e.g., the-
children with ADHD. Studies using Posner’s ory of mind), IQ, language, and repetitive
(1980) paradigm have demonstrated difficul- behaviors.
ties in the disengage and move operations in
children with autism (Casey et al., 1993; FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Courchesne et al., 1994; Wainwright-Sharp &
Bryson, 1993), but not children with ADHD In the next section, we explore exciting future
(Swanson et al., 1991). Finally, selective asso- directions for research on EF and autism.
ciations of executive deficits with specific
disorders have been demonstrated using Executive Function and Families: Are
CANTAB. On this battery’s tower task, the Executive Function Deficits Part of the
Stockings of Cambridge, individuals with Broader Autism Phenotype?
autism show clearly deficient performance
(Ozonoff et al., 2004), while medicated chil- Evidence exists that autism is inherited in a
dren with ADHD (Kempton, Vance, Luk, complex polygenic fashion, with up to a dozen
Costin, & Pantelis, 1999) and adults with genes possibly involved (Pickles et al., 1995;
OCD (Purcell, Maruff, Kyrios, & Pantelis, Risch et al., 1999). There is much interest in
1998) perform comparably to typical controls. identifying the multiple susceptibility loci in-
Thus, considering EF as a multidimensional volved in causing the disorder. Research sug-
rather than a unitary construct has helped ob- gests that what is inherited is not autism itself,
tain more precision in the nature of the dys- but an extended set of difficulties that are
function associated with autism and has milder than but qualitatively similar to autism
provided preliminary dissociations and evi- (Bailey et al., 1998; Piven & Palmer, 1997).
618 Theoretical Perspectives

This broader autism phenotype, as it has come with autism demonstrated weaknesses in EF
to be known, has been found in 15% to 45% of and memory but strengths in phonological de-
family members of people with autism in dif- coding and visual-spatial function relative to
ferent samples (Bailey et al., 1998). New re- probands with dyslexia. The very same cogni-
search, summarized in this section, suggests tive profiles were found in both parents and sib-
that cognitive difficulties, including executive lings of the probands, with family members
dysfunction, may be part of the broader autism of children with autism demonstrating signifi-
phenotype. It is hoped that specification of in- cantly inferior executive and memory functions,
termediate phenotypes may one day assist in but significantly superior reading and visual-
gene localization efforts (Piven, 2001). Exam- spatial skills, relative to the family members of
ining the profiles of cognitive strengths and children with dyslexia. Only 6% of the dyslexia
weaknesses in first-degree relatives of individ- families included at least one member with ex-
uals with autism also provides an alternative ecutive dysfunction, while 94% of the autism
strategy for identifying the core cognitive im- families did. Thus, while executive dysfunction
pairments of autism (Hughes, 2001). is not the only cognitive difficulty that appears
Ozonoff, Rogers, Farnham, and Pennington to be part of the inherited phenotype and that
(1993) found that siblings of probands with may be useful in gene localization efforts, its
autism performed significantly less well than clear presence in family members, across multi-
siblings of children with reading disabilities on ple studies and laboratories, highlights its cen-
the Tower of Hanoi, while the two groups per- trality to autism.
formed equally on theory of mind tasks.
Hughes, Plumet, and Leboyer (1999) found defi- Remediation of Executive Function
cient extradimensional shifting on CANTAB’s
ID/ ED task in siblings of children with autism Another new direction in EF research is inter-
but not siblings of children with either delayed vention. The component process approach
or typical development. The siblings of children to identifying relationships between specific
with autism also performed more poorly than cognitive deficits and behavioral symptoms of
the comparison siblings on CANTAB’s tower autism has clear implications for treatment. Al-
task once their significantly better performance most no empirical work has been done on reme-
on a spatial working memory subtest was statis- diation of executive deficits in autism, but
tically controlled. there is a large literature on cognitive remedia-
Similar deficits in EF have been reported tion of other disorders that may prove relevant
for parents of children with autism, using tasks to autism. Defined broadly, cognitive remedia-
of set shifting and planning (Hughes, Leboyer, tion is a systematic approach to teaching indi-
& Bouvard, 1997; Piven & Palmer, 1997), al- viduals to overcome cognitive deficits arising
though there are complex relationships with from brain dysfunction (Task Force on Head
sex and clinical diagnosis. In one study, execu- Injury, 1984). It involves the identification of
tive performance in the autism parent group specific neuropsychological deficits and the
was significantly positively correlated with design and implementation of a treatment pro-
pretest clinical impressions of social oddity gram to retrain and/or compensate for deficits.
(Hughes et al., 1997), consistent with findings Typically, cognitive remediation is only part
discussed earlier of significant correlations of a more comprehensive program that includes
between set shifting and social behavior. other treatment modalities such as psychother-
Other cognitive difficulties have also been apy and organized social activities (Butler,
reported in family members, including weak 1998). Typical targets of cognitive remediation
central coherence (Happé, Briskman, & Frith, are memory, attention, motivation, language,
2001) and impaired theory of mind (Baron- and EFs. In this section, we describe the poten-
Cohen & Hammer, 1997). One study examined a tial of cognitive remediation to address the
variety of neuropsychological functions in chil- executive deficits of autism described in the
dren diagnosed with either autism or dyslexia literature and highlight some key issues that
and all their parents and siblings (Ozonoff, will need to be addressed before this approach
McMahon, Coon, & Lainhart, 2002). Probands will be of utility for persons with autism.
Executive Functions 619

with brain dysfunction, Kerns, Eso, and Thom-


Cognitive Remediation in Pediatric son (1999) modified APT to start at a more
Populations basic level and make tasks more child-friendly.
Much of what we know about the effectiveness Materials were made more interesting and en-
of cognitive remediation is based on studies gaging, and stimuli were changed from abstract
of adults with acquired brain injury or schizo- symbols to more familiar concepts, such as
phrenia (Bell, Bryson, Greig, Corcoran, & human features ( hair, sex, clothing), family re-
Wexler, 2001; Butler & Namerow, 1988; Gi- lationships (siblings, grandparents), and house-
anutsos, 1991; Glisky & Schacter, 1989; Kurtz, hold items. In a small study of the efficacy of
Moberg, Gur, & Gur, 2001; Levine et al., 2000; this program—called Pay Attention!—children
Parente & Anderson-Parente, 1991; Pilling with ADHD in the treatment group performed
et al., 2002; Wehman et al., 1989; Wood & significantly better than matched controls on
Fussy, 1990). One of the more widely utilized several measures of attention and academic
approaches is the Attention Process Training performance.
(APT) program (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1986). Butler and Copeland (2002) developed a
APT was originally developed for adults with comprehensive cognitive rehabilitation pro-
brain injury. It contains modules for improving gram to remediate attention and executive
focused attention (the ability to respond to spe- processes in children suffering neurological im-
cific sensory information in the environment), pairments secondary to treatment for cancer
sustained attention (the ability to maintain (e.g., cranial irradiation). Their 20-session
a behavioral response over time), selective at- program included APT, training in metacogni-
tention (the ability to maintain a response in tive strategies, individual cognitive-behavioral
the presence of distracting or competing stim- therapy focused on self-monitoring and self-
uli), alternating attention (the ability to shift coaching skills, and games and activities to pro-
focus of attention and demonstrate mental mote generalization of new skills. A pilot study
flexibility), and divided attention (the ability found that children in the treatment group im-
to respond simultaneously to more than one proved their performance on measures of atten-
task or stimulus). The efficacy of APT has been tion significantly more than no-treatment
demonstrated in adults with brain injury controls (Butler & Copeland, 2002). No group
(Mateer, 1992; Mateer, Sohlberg, & Youngman, differences were found on academic tests, how-
1990; Sohlberg & Mateer, 1987) and schizo- ever, suggesting generalization to other cogni-
phrenia (Kurtz et al., 2001). tive skills was not obtained. Nevertheless, this
The clinical application of cognitive comprehensive therapeutic approach may be
remediation techniques to pediatric popula- helpful in other populations of cognitively im-
tions is fairly new. Several investigators have paired children, including children and adoles-
used the APT training package with some cents with autism spectrum disorders.
success with children with ADHD (Semrud-
Cognitive Remediation and Autism
Clikeman, Teeter, Parle, & Conner, 1995;
Stevenson, Whitmont, Bornholt, Livesey, & At first glance, the literature on the cognitive
Stevenson, 2002; Williams, 1989) and trau- remediation of autism appears sparse. How-
matic brain injury (Thomson, 1994). Theoreti- ever, many comprehensive early treatment
cal approaches underlying adult rehabilitation models, including applied behavioral analysis
programs, however, may not fully apply to pe- and Treatment and Education of Autistic and
diatric remediation efforts (Parente & Her- Related Communication-handicapped CHil-
rmann, 1996). The developing brain is not as dren (TEACCH) programs, fit into the broad-
well understood as the adult brain, and devel- est category of cognitive remediation. These
opmental neuroscientists are just beginning to approaches aim to improve skills within spe-
uncover relationships between brain structure cific cognitive domains (e.g., expressive and
and function in the typically developing brain. receptive language, visual-spatial abilities) by
Recognizing the need to address both de- breaking down complex behaviors into basic
velopmental issues and the limited awareness components through a component process task
and compromised reasoning skills of children analysis. Skill components are then taught in a
620 Theoretical Perspectives

hierarchical manner, with repeated practice. mon to other pediatric populations, including
The TEACCH program also places much em- the poor fit that results from application of
phasis on the development of compensatory adult rehabilitation models to children, as noted
strategies, such as visual schedules, to address earlier. Other challenges may be unique to
cognitive deficits, which is another hallmark autism spectrum disorders. Functional organi-
of cognitive remediation programs. zation in the brain of individuals with a devel-
There are, however, few papers specifically opmental disorder may be dissimilar from that
addressing EF remediation for people with of the typically developing brain. And most
autism (Ozonoff, 1998). Bock (1994) reported cognitive remediation programs have only a
an intervention that trained four children with small component devoted to the particular cog-
autism on a tridimensional sorting task. Partic- nitive difficulties of autism (e.g., shifting and
ipants learned to sort the same set of objects divided attention, EFs), suggesting that any ef-
into three different categories (color, shape, forts to use these packages with autism will
number). This required shifting cognitive set require substantial modification or tailoring.
from one attribute of an object to another and is Most cognitive rehabilitation programs are
thus analogous to the WCST and the ID/ ED based on the assumption that the cognitive
subtest of CANTAB. After training, children processes being trained were previously estab-
were tested using a different set of objects lished and then damaged. Techniques are aimed
(cans of food) that they were asked to sort into at “retraining” and strengthening neural con-
new categorical sets ( brand, size, food type). nections through massed practice in order to re-
The study found that training on the categoriza- store mental functions and processing speed.
tion task increased sorting ability on the un- This assumption likely does not apply to a
trained item set and that gains were maintained developmental disorder like autism, and its
two months postintervention (Bock, 1994). impact on the efficacy of the approach is not
A report of a program called REHABIT sug- known. Another issue is the problem of general-
gested it may have efficacy for children with ization of learned skills; as Butler and
autism (Jepsen & von Thaden, 2002). This pro- Copeland (2002) found, lack of generalization
gram was developed to teach a variety of cogni- is not unique to children with autism. This im-
tive skills, including EFs, attention, memory, poses serious challenges to professionals devel-
language, and academic achievement. Adoles- oping and implementing cognitive remediation
cents with mixed diagnoses, including autism, programs for individuals with autism spectrum
other developmental delays, and acquired neu- disorders, who often have severe problems gen-
rological insults, were matched in a pairwise eralizing gains to other settings, materials, and
fashion on diagnosis, age, sex, and IQ and ran- teachers. Many cognitive remediation programs
domly assigned to either the REHABIT treat- include some form of individual psychotherapy,
ment group or the education-as-usual group. usually focused on increasing awareness of
Pre- or posttest comparisons suggested signifi- deficits and teaching self-monitoring and com-
cant group by time interaction effects, with pensatory strategies. Due to the limited self-
adolescents in the treatment group demonstrat- awareness and insight of most people with
ing significantly more improvement than con- autism spectrum disorders, however, the role
trols in planning, simultaneous processing, that psychotherapy should play in the cognitive
reading, and adaptive behavior (e.g., indepen- remediation of autism is not clear. In summary,
dent functioning, prevocational skills, self- cognitive remediation approaches may eventu-
direction, responsibility). While small sample ally prove useful in improving the EFs of people
size precluded diagnosis-specific analyses, the with autism, but they will require modifica-
authors state that significant gains were made tions and careful thought to develop techniques
across diagnostic categories. Thus, it appears appropriate for this population. This is an excit-
that the subgroup of children with autism who ing new direction for future EF research.
participated in this study benefited from this
type of cognitive (including executive) remedi- CONCLUSION
ation (Jepsen & von Thaden, 2002).
Cognitive remediation of autism is faced In this chapter, we reviewed the empirical lit-
with many of the same challenges that are com- erature on EF in autism, from initial studies
Executive Functions 621

finding large group differences to more recent Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bull-
work specifying the nature of the affected more, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., et al.
component processes. An eventual goal of this (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and
research is to identify the executive profile or autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Jour-
fingerprint of autism, which may someday nal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.
Beatty, W. W., Jocic, Z., Monson, N., & Katzung,
assist in diagnosis, contribute to gene localiza-
V. M. (1994). Problem solving by schizo-
tion efforts, and improve remediation tech- phrenic and schizoaffective patients on the
niques. We also explored issues that have Wisconsin and California Card Sorting Tests.
emerged as EF research has matured. With new Neuropsychology, 8, 49–54.
studies have come new questions, most still Beech, A., Powell, T., McWilliam, J., & Claridge,
unanswered, about the developmental course of G. (1989). Evidence of reduced “cognitive in-
executive dysfunction and its relationship to hibition” in schizophrenia. British Journal of
other symptoms of autism and to other neu- Clinical Psychology, 28, 109–116.
rodevelopmental disorders. Finally, this chap- Bell, M., Bryson, G., Greig, T., Corcoran, C., &
ter presented new research on the familiality of Wexler, B. E. (2001). Neurocognitive enhance-
ment therapy with work therapy: Effects on
executive dysfunction and the promise of newly
neuropsychological test performance. Archives
developed remediation techniques. In the years
of General Psychiatry, 58, 763–768.
since the previous edition of this Handbook, we Bellak, L. (1994). The schizophrenic syndrome and
have learned a great deal, only to find out how attention deficit disorder: Thesis, antithesis, and
much more there is yet to know about this com- synthesis? American Psychologist, 49, 25–29.
plex disorder and its many manifestations. Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
(1996). Intact and impaired memory functions
Cross-References in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.
Berger, H. J. C., Aerts, F. H. T. M., van Spaen-
Other aspects of development and behavior are donck, K. P. M., Cools, A. R., & Teunisse, J. P.
reviewed in Chapters 8 to 15; psychological as- (2003). Central coherence and cognitive shift-
ing in relation to social improvement in high-
sessment is discussed in Chapter 29, and other
functioning young adults with autism. Journal
theoretical perspectives on autism are pro- of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
vided in Chapters 21 and 23 through 26. 25, 502–511.
Biro, S., & Russell, J. (2001). The execution of arbi-
REFERENCES trary procedures by children with autism. De-
velopment and Psychopathology, 13, 97–110.
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human as- Bock, M. A. (1994). Acquisition, maintenance, and
sociative memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. generalization of a categorization strategy by
Axelrod, B. N., Goldman, R. S., Tompkins, L. M., children with autism. Journal of Autism and
& Jiron, C. C. (1994). Poor differential perfor- Developmental Disorders, 24, 39–51.
mance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in Bond, J. A., & Buchtel, H. A. (1984). Comparison
schizophrenia, mood disorder, and traumatic of the Wisconsin card sorting test and the Hal-
brain injury. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol- stead category test. Journal of Clinical Psy-
ogy, and Behavioral Neurology, 7, 20–24. chology, 40, 1251–1255.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, Bornstein, R. A. (1990). Neuropsychological perfor-
England: Clarendon Press. mance in children with Tourette’s syndrome.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Jan- Psychiatry Research, 33, 73–81.
ota, I., Montgomery, M., et al. (1998). A clini- Brian, J. A., Tipper, S., Weaver, B., & Bryson, S.
copathological study of autism. Brain, 121, (2003). Inhibitory mechanisms in autism spec-
889–905. trum disorders: Typical selective inhibition of
Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, location versus facilitated perceptual process-
C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. ing. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
(1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: try, 44, 552–560.
A PET study of the Tower of London task. Brophy, M., Taylor, E., & Hughes, C. (2002). To go
Neuropsychologia, 34, 515–526. or not to go: Inhibitory control in “ hard to
Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Parents of manage” children. Infant and Child Develop-
children with Asperger syndrome: What is the ment, 11, 125–140.
cognitive phenotype? Journal of Cognitive Neu- Buchsbaum, M. S., Siegel, B. V., Wu, J. C., Hazlett,
roscience, 9, 548–554. E., Sicotte, N., Haier, R., et al. (1992). Attention
622 Theoretical Perspectives

performance in autism and regional brain rocognitive function and joint attention ability
metabolic rate assessed by positron emission in young children with autism spectrum disor-
tomography. Journal of Autism and Develop- der versus developmental delay. Child Devel-
mental Disorders, 22, 115–125. opment, 73, 345–358.
Butler, R. W. (1998). Attentional processes and Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (1991). The Wis-
their remediation in childhood cancer. Medical consin Card Sorting Test: Theoretical analysis
and Pediatric Oncology, 1, 75–78. and modeling in a neuronal network. Cerebral
Butler, R. W., & Copeland, D. R. (2002). Atten- Cortex, 1, 62–79.
tional processes and their remediation in chil- Dennis, M. (1991). Frontal lobe function in child-
dren treated for cancer: A literature review hood and adolescence: A heuristic for assess-
and the development of a therapeutic ap- ing attention regulation, executive control, and
proach. Journal of the International Neuropsy- the intentional states important for social dis-
chological Society, 8, 115–124. course. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7,
Butler, R. W., & Namerow, N. S. (1988). Cognitive 327–358.
retraining in brain injury rehabilitation: A Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1986).
critical review. Journal of NeuroRehabilitation Comparative development in human infants
and Neural Repair, 2, B1–B5. and infant rhesus monkeys on cognitive func-
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). tions that depend on prefrontal cortex. Society
How specific is the relation between executive of Neuroscience Abstracts, 12, 742.
function and theory of mind? Contributions of Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., &
inhibitory control and working memory. Infant Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cogni-
and Child Development, 11, 73–92. tive deficits in children treated early and con-
Casey, B. J., Gordon, C. T., Mannheim, G. B., & tinuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society
Rumsey, J. M. (1993). Dysfunctional attention for Research in Child Development, 62, 1–205.
in autistic savants. Journal of Clinical and Ex- Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996).
perimental Neuropsychology, 15, 933–946. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of attentional
Chelune, G. J., Ferguson, W., Koon, R., & Dickey, and affective shifts. Nature, 380, 69–72.
T. O. (1986). Frontal lobe disinhibition in At- Douglas, V. I., & Peters, K. G. (1979). Toward a
tention Deficit Disorder. Child Psychiatry and clearer definition of the attentional deficit of
Human Development, 16, 221–234. hyperactive children. In G. A. Hale & M.
Comings, D. E., & Comings, B. G. (1987). A con- Lewis (Eds.), Attention and cognitive develop-
trolled study of Tourette syndrome: Attention ment (pp. 173–247). New York: Plenum Press.
deficit disorders, learning disorders, and school Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganization of behaviour
problems. American Journal of Human Genet- after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsy-
ics, 41, 701–741. chology, 3, 271–290.
Cornblatt, B. A., & Keilp, J. G. (1994). Impaired Duncan, J. (2001). Frontal lobe function and the
attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of control of visual attention. In J. Braun & C.
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, Koch (Eds.), Visual attention and cortical cir-
31–46. cuits (pp. 69–88). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Courchesne, E., Akshoomoff, N. A., & Ciesielski, Farah, M. J. (1984). The neurological basis of men-
K. (1990). Shifting attention abnormalities in tal imagery: A componential analysis. Cogni-
autism: ERP and performance evidence. Jour- tion, 18, 245–272.
nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- Fletcher, P. C., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C.,
chology, 12, 77. Dolan, R. J., & Frackowiak, R. (1995). Other
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J. P., Akshoomoff, minds in the brain: A functional imaging study
N. A., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A., of theory of mind in story comprehension.
Murakami, J. W., et al. (1994). A new finding Cognition, 57(2), 109–128.
in autism: Impairment in shifting attention. In Friedrich, F., & Rader, S. (1996). Component pro-
S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical cess analysis in experimental and clinical neu-
cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: ropsychology. In M. Marirish & J. Moses (Eds.),
Implications for brain function (pp. 101–137). Theoretical foundations of clinical neuropsy-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. chology for clinical practitioners (pp. 59–79).
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., & Ri- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
naldi, J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory
of early symptoms in autism. Child Develop- of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive
ment, 69, 1276–1285. Development, 10, 483–527.
Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., Garretson, H. B., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L.
McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neu- (1990). Sustained attention in children with
Executive Functions 623

autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Self versus other. Infant and Child Develop-
Disorders, 20, 101–114. ment, 11, 159–170.
Gianutsos, R. (1991). Cognitive rehabilitation: A Hughes, C. (1996). Planning problems in autism at
neuropsychological specialty comes of age. the level of motor control. Journal of Autism
Brain Injury, 5, 353–368. and Developmental Disorders, 26, 99–107.
Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Sirian, L., Black, D. O., Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in
& Wagner, A. E. (2002). Adaptive skills and preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and
executive function in autism spectrum disor- verbal ability. British Journal of Developmen-
ders. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 241–248. tal Psychology, 16, 233–253.
Glisky, E. L., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Extending Hughes, C. (2001). Executive dysfunction in
the limits of complex learning in organic am- autism: Its nature and implications for the
nesia: Computer training in a vocational do- everyday problems experienced by individu-
main. Neuropsychologia, 27, 107–120. als with autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman,
Goel, V., & Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The devel-
lobes implicated in planning functions? Inter- opment of autism: Perspectives from theory
preting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neu- and research (pp. 255–275). Mahwah, NJ:
ropsychologia, 33, 623–642. Erlbaum.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of primate Hughes, C. (2002). Executive functions and devel-
prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior opment: Emerging themes. Infant and Child
by representational memory. In V. B. Mount- Development, 11, 201–209.
castle, F. Plum, & S. R. Geiger (Eds.), Hand- Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring exec-
book of physiology: The nervous system utive functions in childhood: Problems and so-
(pp. 373–417). Bethesda, MD: American lutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
Physiological Society. 7, 131–142.
Grattan, L. M., Bloomer, R., Archambault, F. X., & Hughes, C., Leboyer, M. H., & Bouvard, M. (1997).
Eslinger, P. J. (1990). Cognitive and neural un- Executive function in parents of children with
derpinnings of empathy. Clinical Neuropsy- autism. Psychological Medicine, 27, 209–220.
chologist, 4, 279. Hughes, C., Plumet, M.-H., & Leboyer, M. (1999).
Griffith, E. M. (2002). Manipulating feedback nor- Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: In-
malizes perseveration in individuals with creased prevalence of executive dysfunction
autism. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, and superior spatial span amongst siblings of
University of Denver, Colorado. children with autism. Journal of Child Psy-
Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., chology and Psychiatry, 40, 705–718.
& Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s
young children with autism. Child Develop- difficulty with mental disengagement from an
ment, 70, 817–832. object: Its implications for theories of autism.
Happé, F., Briskman, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Ex- Developmental Psychology, 29, 498–510.
ploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994).
Weak “central coherence” in parents and sib- Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism.
lings of children with autism: I. Experimental Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.
tests. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- Ingram, R. E. (1989). Information processing as a
atry, 42, 299–307. theoretical framework for child and adolescent
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., psychiatry. In M. H. Schmidt & H. Remschmidt
Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (1996). The- (Eds.), Needs and prospects of child and adoles-
ory of mind in the brain: Evidence from a PET cent psychiatry (pp. 25–36). Cambridge, MA:
scan study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroRe- Hogrefe & Huber.
port, 8, 197–201. Jepsen, R. H., & von Thaden, K. (2002). The effect
Harris, P. (1993). Pretending and planning. In S. of cognitive education on the performance of
Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. students with neurological developmental dis-
Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: abilities. NeuroRehabilitation, 17, 201–209.
Perspectives from autism (pp. 228–246). Ox- Kempton, S., Vance, A., Luk, E., Costin, J., & Pan-
ford, England: Oxford University Press. telis, C. (1999). Executive function and atten-
Head, D., Bolton, D., & Hymas, N. (1989). Deficit tion deficit hyperactivity disorder: Stimulant
in cognitive shifting ability in patients with medication and better executive function per-
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psy- formance in children. Psychological Medicine,
chiatry, 25, 929–937. 29, 527–538.
Hill, E. L., & Russell, J. (2002). Action memory Kerns, K. A., Eso, K., & Thomson, J. (1999). Inves-
and self-monitoring in children with autism: tigation of a direct intervention for improving
624 Theoretical Perspectives

attention in young children with ADHD. De- Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Belleville, S., & Rouleau, N.
velopmental Neuropsychology, 16, 273–295. (1999). Absolute pitch in autism: A case study.
Kurtz, M. M., Moberg, P. J., Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. Neurocase, 5, 485–501.
(2001). Approaches to cognitive remediation of Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG corre-
neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia: A lates of the development of infant joint atten-
review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Re- tion skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36,
view, 11, 197–210. 325–338.
Levine, B., Robertson, I. H., Claire, L., Carter, G., Neill, W. T., Lissner, L. S., & Beck, J. L. (1990).
Hong, J., Wilson, B. A., et al. (2000). Rehabili- Negative priming in same-different matching:
tation of executive functioning: An experimen- Further evidence for a central locus of
tal-clinical validation of Goal Management inhibition. Perception and Psychophysics, 48,
Training. Journal of the International Neuropsy- 398–400.
chological Society, 6, 299–312. Ozonoff, S. (1995). Executive functions in autism.
Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learn-
Morris, R., et al. (2001). Executive functioning ing and cognition in autism (pp. 199–219).
in high-functioning children with autism. Jour- New York: Plenum Press.
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive func-
261–270. tion in autism and other disorders. In J. Rus-
Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit sell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder
thought and action: A user’s guide to the stop- (pp. 179–211). New York: Oxford University
signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr Press.
(Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, mem- Ozonoff, S. (1998). Assessment and remediation of
ory and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego, executive dysfunction in autism and Asperger
CA: Academic Press. syndrome. In E. Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, & L.
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome and high-
On the ability to inhibit simple and choice re- functioning autism (pp. 263–289). New York:
action time responses: A model and a method. Plenum Press.
Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Ozonoff, S., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R.,
Perception and Performance, 10, 276–291. Klin, A., McMahon, W. M., et al. (2004). Per-
Lueger, R. J., & Gill, K. J. (1990). Frontal-lobe cogni- formance on CANTAB subtests sensitive to
tive dysfunction in conduct disorder adolescents. frontal lobe function in people with autistic
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 696–706. disorder: Evidence from the CPEA network.
Luria, A. R. (1966). The higher cortical functions Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
in man. New York: Basic Books. ders, 34, 139–150.
Mateer, C. A. (1992). Systems of care for post- Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive
concussive syndrome. In L. J. Horn & N. D. function profiles in three neurodevelopmental
Zasler (Eds.), Rehabilitation of post-concussive disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
disorders (pp. 143–160). Philadelphia: Hanley tal Disorders, 29, 171–177.
& Belfus. Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudi-
Mateer, C. A., Sohlberg, M. M., & Youngman, P. nal study of executive function and theory of
(1990). The management of acquired attention mind development in autism. Development and
and memory disorders following mild closed Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.
head injury. In R. Wood (Ed.), Cognitive reha- Ozonoff, S., McMahon, W. M., Coon, H., & Lainhart,
bilitation in perspective (pp. 68–96). London: J. (2002). Neuropsychological profiles of autism
Taylor & Francis. and dyslexia families. Paper presented at the an-
McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. nual meeting of the American Academy of Child
(1993). Executive function and social commu- and Adolescent Psychiatry, San Francisco.
nication deficits in young autistic children. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, (1991). Executive function deficits in high-
34, 563–578. functioning autistic individuals: Relationship
Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (2000). The external va- to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology
lidity of Asperger disorder: Lack of evidence and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
from the domain of neuropsychology. Journal of Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., Farnham, J. M., & Pen-
Abnormal Psychology, 109, 227–238. nington, B. F. (1993). Can standard measures
Moses, L. J. (2001). Executive accounts of theory- identify subclinical markers of autism? Jour-
of-mind development. Child Development, 72, nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
688–690. 23, 429–441.
Executive Functions 625

Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory card sorting task? Infant and Child Develop-
function in nonretarded children with autism. ment, 11, 93–105.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Perrine, K. (1993). Differential aspects of concep-
ders, 27, 59–77. tual processing in the Category Test and
Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evi- Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clini-
dence of intact working memory in autism. cal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- 461–473.
ders, 31, 257–263. Pickles, A., Bolton, P. F., Macdonald, H., Bailey,
Parente, R., & Anderson-Parente, J. (1991). Re- A., Le Couteur, A., Sim, C. H., et al. (1995).
training memory: Techniques and applications. Latent-class analysis of recurrence risks for
Houston, TX: CSY Publishing. complex phenotypes with selection and mea-
Parente, R., & Herrmann, D. (1996). Retraining surement error: A twin and family history
cognition: Techniques and applications. study of autism. American Journal of Human
Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Press. Genetics, 57, 717–726.
Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou, Pilling, S., Bebbington, P., Kuipers, E., Garety, P.,
M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional capac- Geddes, J., Martindale, B., et al. (2002). Psy-
ities in children with autism: Is there a general chological treatments in schizophrenia: II.
deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism Meta-analyses of randomized controlled trails
and Developmental Disorders, 28, 467–478. of social skills training and cognitive remedi-
Pennington, B. F. (1994). The working memory func- ation. Psychological Medicine, 32, 783–791.
tion of the prefrontal cortices: Implications for Piven, J. (2001). The broad autism phenotype: A
developmental and individual differences in complementary strategy for molecular genetic
cognition. In M. M. Haith, J. Benson, R. studies of autism. American Journal of Med-
Roberts, & B. F. Pennington (Eds.), Future- ical Genetics, 8, 34–35.
oriented processes in development (pp. 243– Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in
289). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. parents from multiple-incidence autism fami-
Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & lies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and try, 38, 1011–1021.
working memory: Theoretical and measurement Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quar-
issues. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32,
Attention, memory, and executive function 3–25.
(pp. 327–348). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Price, B. H., Daffner, K. R., Stowe, R. M., & Mesu-
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1991). A neuro- lam, M. M. (1990). The comportmental learn-
scientific perspective on continuity and dis- ing disabilities of early frontal lobe damage.
continuity in developmental psychopathology. Brain, 113, 1383–1393.
In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Prior, M. R., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Neuropsy-
symposium on developmental psychopathol- chological testing of autistic children through
ogy: Volume III. Models and integrations an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal
(pp. 117–159). Rochester, NY: University of of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20,
Rochester Press. 581–590.
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive Purcell, R., Maruff, P., Kyrios, M., & Pantelis, C.
functions and developmental psychopatholo- (1998). Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-
gies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- compulsive disorder: A comparison with unipo-
try, 37, 51–87. lar depression, panic disorder, and normal
Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. controls. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55,
(1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s the- 415–423.
ory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communi- Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Br-
cation. Child Development, 60, 689–700. ereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). A deficit
Perner, J., Kain, W., & Barchfeld, P. (2002). Execu- in shifting attention present in high-function-
tive control and higher-order theory of mind in ing autism but not Asperger’s disorder.
children at risk of ADHD. Infant and Child Autism: Journal of Research and Practice, 5,
Development, 11, 141–158. 67–80.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of the- Risch, N., Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L., Nouri, N.,
ory of mind and executive control. Trends in Hinds, D., Hallmayer, J., et al. (1999). A
Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337–334. genomic screen of autism: Evidence for a
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2002). What causes 3-year- multilocus etiology. American Journal of
olds’ difficulty on the dimensional change Human Genetics, 65, 493–507.
626 Theoretical Perspectives

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, retarded autistic children. Autism: Journal of
B. J., Lawrence, A. D., McInnes, L., et al. Research and Practice, 5, 165–174.
(1998). A study of performance on tests from Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1986). Attention
the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe process training (APT). Puyallup: Washington
dysfunction in a large sample of normal volun- Association for Neuropsychological Research
teers: Implications for theories of executive and Development.
functioning and cognitive aging. Journal of the Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1987). Effec-
International Neuropsychological Society, 4, tiveness of an attention training program.
474–490. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu-
Rumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving ropsychology, 9, 117–130.
in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W. M. (under
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, review). Repetitive behavior in the high-
15, 23–36. functioning autism spectrum: Phenotypic char-
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). acterization and relationship to cognitive func-
Neuropsychological divergence of high-level tioning.
autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism Stevenson, C. S., Whitmont, S., Bornholt, L.,
and Developmental Disorders, 20, 155–168. Livesey, D., & Stevenson, R. J. (2002). A cog-
Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Henry, L. (1996). Working nitive remediation programme for adults with
memory in children with autism and with mod- attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Aus-
erate learning difficulties. Journal of Child tralian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 673–686. 36, 610–616.
Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T.
intact executive capacities in children with (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of
autism: Implications for the core executive mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10,
dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism 640–656.
and Developmental Disorders, 29, 103–112. Stuss, D. T. (1992). Biological and psychological
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, development of executive functions. Brain and
T. (1991). The “ windows task ” as a measure of Cognition, 20, 8–23.
strategic deception in preschoolers and autis- Swanson, J. M., Posner, M., Potkin, S. G., Bon-
tic subjects. British Journal of Developmental forte, S., Youpa, D., Fiore, C., et al. (1991).
Psychology, 9, 331–349. Activating tasks for the study of visual-spatial
Russell, J., Saltmarsh, R., & Hill, E. (1999). What attention in ADHD children: A cognitive
do executive factors contribute to the failure anatomic approach. Journal of Child Neurol-
on false belief tasks by children with autism? ogy, 6, S119–S127.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T.,
40, 859–868. Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The
Rutter, M., & Bailey, A. (1993). Thinking and rela- frequency and distribution of spontaneous at-
tionships: Mind and brain (some reflections on tention shifts between social and nonsocial
theory of mind and autism). In S. Baron-Cohen, stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and
H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Un- nonautistic developmentally delayed infants.
derstanding other minds: Perspectives from Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
autism (pp. 481–504). Oxford, England: Oxford 39, 747–753.
University Press. Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, A. J., & Bar-
Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of tolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s Syndrome and
perseveration. Neuropsychologia, 22, 715–732. autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the
Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity American Academy of Child and Adolescent
and inhibitory control in normal development Psychiatry, 29, 130–136.
and childhood psychopathology. Developmen- Task Force on Head Injury. (1984). Standards for
tal Psychology, 26, 710–720. cognitive rehabilitation. Erie, PA: American
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Teeter, P. A., Parle, N., & Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine.
Conner, R. T. (1995). Innovative approaches Thomson, J. (1994). Rehabilitation of high school-
for working with children with ADHD. Paper aged individuals with traumatic brain injury
presented at the annual conference of the through utilization of an attention training pro-
American Educational Research Association, gram. Journal of the International Neuropsy-
San Francisco. chological Society, 1, 149.
Shu, B. C., Lung, F. W., Tien, A. Y., & Chen, B. C. Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect:
(2001). Executive function deficits in non- Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quar-
Executive Functions 627

terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, Wehman, P., Kreutzer, J., Sale, P., West, M., Mor-
571–590. ton, M., & Diambra, J. (1989). Cognitive im-
Turner, M. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunc- pairment and remediation: Implications for
tion account of repetitive behavior in autism. employment following traumatic brain injury.
In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive dis- Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 4,
order (pp. 57–100). New York: Oxford Univer- 66–75.
sity Press. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., Ozonoff, S., Rouse,
Turner, M. (1999). Repetitive behavior in autism: A B., & McCabe, E. R. B. (1990). Neuropsychol-
review of psychological research. Journal of ogy of early-treated phenylketonuria: Specific
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 839–849. executive function deficits. Child Development,
van der Does, A. W., & van den Bosch, R. J. (1992). 61, 1697–1713.
What determines Wisconsin Card Sorting per- Williams, D. J. (1989). A process-specific training
formance in schizophrenia? Clinical Psychol- program in the treatment of attention deficits
ogy Review, 12, 567–583. in children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Wainwright-Sharp, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1993). University of Washington, Seattle.
Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning Wood, R. L., & Fussy, L. (1990). Cognitive rehabili-
people with autism. Journal of Autism and De- tation in perspective. London: Taylor & Francis.
velopmental Disorders, 23, 1–13.
CHAPTER 23

Empathizing and Systemizing in


Autism Spectrum Conditions

SIMON BARON-COHEN, SALLY WHEELWRIGHT, JOHN LAWSON, RICHARD GRIFFIN,


CHRIS ASHWIN, JAC BILLINGTON, AND BHISMADEV CHAKRABARTI

Autism is diagnosed when a child or adult statistical definition average but is hardly high
has abnormalities in a triad of behavioral do- functioning.
mains: social development, communication, By the 1990s, interest had shifted to study-
and repetitive behavior/obsessive interests ing the truly high-functioning strata of the
(American Psychological Association [APA], autistic spectrum: those whose IQs were close
1994; World Health Organization [WHO], to 100 or above. This strata would have in-
1994). In the 1960s and 1970s, many of the cluded those with superior IQ, that is, those
children with autism who were studied by cog- whose IQ was higher than 2 standard devia-
nitive developmentalists also had comorbid tions above the population mean (Baron-
learning difficulties (i.e., below-average intel- Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson,
ligence) and language delay (Frith, 1970; Her- 1997; Frith, 1991; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen,
melin & O’Connor, 1970; Wing, 1976). An 1997; Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti, &
average IQ of 60 was not uncommon in samples Rourke, 1995; Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson, &
studied during that period. Bartolucci, 1990). Since we know that IQ is a
strong predictor of outcome in autism (Rutter,
AUTISM SPECTRUM CONDITIONS: 1978), it is important to take IQ into account.
LOW, MEDIUM, AND HIGH- Asperger syndrome (AS) was first de-
FUNCTIONING SUBGROUPS scribed by Asperger (1944). The descriptions
of the children he documented overlapped
In the 1980s, cognitive developmentalists considerably with the accounts of childhood
began to focus on what was then called high- autism (Kanner, 1943). Little was published
functioning autism (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & on AS in English until relatively recently
Frith, 1985, 1986). Such children might be (Frith, 1991; Wing, 1981). Current diagnostic
better described as medium-functioning be- practice recognizes people with AS as meet-
cause although they had IQs within the average ing the same criteria as for high-functioning
range, their IQ fell within two standard devia- autism (HFA) but with no history of language
tions from the population mean of 100. Be- delay and no cognitive delay. That is, as a tod-
cause one standard deviation is 15 points, dler, the individual was speaking on time (i.e.,
anyone with an IQ above 70 would still have single words by age 2 and/or phrase speech by
been included in this band. An IQ of 71 is by 3 years old) and has had a mental age in line

We are grateful for support from the MRC (United Kingdom), the James S. McDonnell Foundation, and the
Isaac Newton Trust during the period of this work. Parts of this chapter are reprinted from elsewhere
(Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Griffin, Lawson, & Hill, 2002).

628
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 629

with his or her chronological age (i.e., an IQ in relative to mental age. These deficits can
the normal range). Although some studies occur by degrees. The term empathizing en-
have claimed a distinction between AS and compasses the following earlier terms: theory
HFA (Klin et al., 1995), the majority of stud- of mind, mind reading, and taking the inten-
ies have not demonstrated many, if any, signif- tional stance (Dennett, 1987).
icant differences. Empathizing involves two major elements:
This background into autism and intelli- (1) the ability to attribute mental states to self
gence is important because it reveals that over and others as a natural way to understand
the past 40 years, there has been a major shift agents (Baron-Cohen, 1994a; Leslie, 1995;
in research strategy. When studying the cogni- Premack, 1990) and (2) having an emotional
tive development of autism, one strategy (and reaction that is appropriate to the other per-
one we focus on here) is to identify the deficits son’s mental state. In this sense, it includes
or talents that are present in all three subgroups what is normally meant by the term theory of
( low, medium, and high functioning). In this mind (the attributional component), but it goes
way, we can characterize necessary, core char- beyond to include having some affective reac-
acteristics of people on the autism spectrum tion (e.g., sympathy).
and test whether a cognitive theory can account The first of these elements, the mental state
for such core features. At the same time, we can attribution component, has been widely dis-
clarify those associated characteristics that cussed in terms of being an evolved ability,
may occur more frequently than chance but given that the universe can be broadly divided
may not lie in this core. The list of associated into two kinds of entities: those that possess
( but not universal) characteristics is very long intentionality and those that do not (Brentano,
and includes the following: language delay, 1970). The mental state attribution component
learning disability, self-injury, clumsiness, at- is effectively judging whether this is the sort
tention deficit / hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), of entity that might possess intentionality. In-
epilepsy, gastrointestinal inflammation, hyper- tentionality is defined as the capacity of some-
lexia, and nonright-handedness. We suggest that thing to refer or point to things other than
the core characteristics comprise two triads: itself. A rock cannot point to anything. It just
is. In contrast, a mouse can “look ” at a piece
Triad A: Social difficulties, communica- of cheese, it can “ want ” the piece of cheese, it
tion difficulties, and difficulties in imagin- can “ think ” that this is a piece of cheese, and
ing other people’s minds so on. Essentially, agents have intentionality,
whereas nonagents do not.
Triad B: Strong, narrow obsessional in-
This means that when we observe agents
terests, repetitive behavior, and “islets of
and nonagents move, we construe their motion
ability”
as having different causes (Csibra, Gergely,
Biro, Koos, & Brockbanck, 1999; Gelman &
This new view builds on the concept of the Hirschfield, 1994). Agents can move by self-
triad but extends it into two triads (Wing & propulsion, which we naturally interpret as
Gould, 1979). In the next sections, we look at driven by their goals and desires, while nona-
some different cognitive theories to see how gents can reliably be expected not to move un-
well they can account for these two triads of less acted on by another object (e.g., following
characteristics. a collision). Note that mental state attribution
is quite broad, because it includes not just
THE MINDBLINDNESS/ EMPATHIZING attribution of beliefs, desires, intentions,
THEORY thoughts, and knowledge, but also perceptual
or attentional states and all of the emotions
The mindblindness theory of autism (Baron- (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, & Golan,
Cohen, 1995) and its extension into empathiz- submitted; Griffin & Baron-Cohen, 2002).
ing theory (Baron-Cohen, 2002) propose that The second empathizing element, the af-
in autism spectrum conditions, there are fective reaction component, is closer to what
deficits in the normal process of empathizing, we ordinarily refer to with the English word
630 Theoretical Perspectives

empathy. Thus, we not only attribute a mental looking and, by 14 months, strive to establish
state to the agent in front of us (e.g., the man joint attention (Butterworth, 1991; Hood,
“ thinks” the cake is made of soft, creamy Willen, & Driver, 1997; Scaife & Bruner,
chocolate) but also anticipate his or her emo- 1975). By 14 months, they also start to pro-
tional state (the man will be disappointed duce and understand pretense (Bates, Benigni,
when he bites into it and discovers it is hard Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979;
and stale), and we react to his or her emotional Leslie, 1987). By 18 months, they begin to
state with an appropriate emotion ourselves show concern at the distress of others (Yir-
(we feel sorry for him). Empathizing thus es- miya et al., 1992). By 2 years old, they begin to
sentially allows us to make sense of the behav- use mental state words in their speech (Well-
ior of other agents we are observing and man & Bartsch, 1988).
predict what they might do next and how they Empathizing develops beyond early child-
might feel. And it allows us to feel connected hood and continues to develop throughout the
to other agents’ experience and respond appro- life span. These later developments include:
priately to them.
• Attribution of the range of mental states to
The Normal Development of Empathizing self and others, including pretense, decep-
tion, and belief (Leslie & Keeble, 1987)
Empathizing develops from human infancy • Recognizing and responding appropriately
(Johnson, 2000). In the infancy period, it in- to complex emotions, not just basic ones
cludes: (Harris, Johnson, Hutton, Andrews, &
Cooke, 1989)
• Being able to judge whether something is • Linking mental states to action, including
an agent (Premack, 1990) language, and, therefore, understanding
• Being able to judge whether another agent and producing pragmatically appropriate
is looking at you (Baron-Cohen, 1994b) language (Tager-Flusberg, 1993)
• Being able to judge whether an agent is ex- • Making sense of others’ behavior, predict-
pressing a basic emotion (Ekman, 1992) ing it, and even manipulating it (Whiten,
and, if so, what type 1991)
• Engaging in shared attention, for example, • Judging what is appropriate in different so-
by following gaze or pointing gestures cial contexts, based on what others will
(Mundy & Crowson, 1997; Scaife & think of our own behavior
Bruner, 1975; Tomasello, 1988) • Communicating an empathic understanding
• Showing concern or basic empathy at an- of another mind
other’s distress or responding appropri-
ately to another’s basic emotional state Thus, by 3 years old, children can under-
(Yirmiya, Sigman, Kasari, & Mundy, stand relationships between mental states such
1992) as “seeing leads to knowing” (Pratt & Bryant,
• Being able to judge an agent’s goal or basic 1990). By 4 years old, they can understand that
intention (Premack, 1990) people can hold false beliefs (Wimmer &
Perner, 1983). By 5 to 6 years old, they can un-
Empathizing can be identified and studied derstand that people can hold beliefs about be-
from at least 12 months of age (Baron-Cohen, liefs (Perner & Wimmer, 1985). By 7 years
1994a; Premack, 1990). Thus, infants show old, they begin to understand what not to say
dishabituation to actions of agents who ap- to avoid offending others (Baron-Cohen,
pear to violate goal directedness (Gergely, O’Riordan, Jones, Stone, & Plaisted, 1999).
Nadasdy, Gergely, & Biro, 1995; Rochat, Mor- With age, mental state attribution becomes in-
gan, & Carpenter, 1997). They also expect creasingly more complex (Baron-Cohen, Jol-
agents to emote (express emotion), and they liffe, et al., 1997; Happé, 1993). The little
expect consistency across modalities ( between cross-cultural evidence that exists suggests a
face and voice; Walker, 1982). They are also similar picture in very different cultures (Avis
highly sensitive to where another person is & Harris, 1991).
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 631

These developmental data have been inter- Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, &
preted in terms of an innate module being part Plumb, 2001; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, &
of the infant cognitive architecture. This has Jolliffe, 1997) or on age-appropriate screening
been dubbed a theory of mind mechanism instruments such as the Empathy Quotient
(ToMM; Leslie, 1995). But as we have sug- (EQ; Baron-Cohen, Richler, Bisarya, Gu-
gested, empathizing also encompasses the runathan, & Wheelwright, 2003; Baron-Cohen
skills that are needed for normal reciprocal so- & Wheelwright, 2004) or the Friendship and
cial relationships (including intimate ones) Relationship Quotient (FQ; Baron-Cohen &
and in sensitive communication. Empathizing Wheelwright, 2003).
is a narrowly defined domain, namely, under-
standing and responding to people’s minds. THE EMPATHIZING-SYSTEMIZING
Deficits in empathizing are referred to as de- THEORY
grees of mindblindness.
A deficit in empathizing might account for
Empathizing in Autism Spectrum Triad A—the social and communication abnor-
Conditions malities that are diagnostic of autism—and it
could even account for difficulties in imagin-
Since the first test of mindblindness in chil- ing other people’s mental states. However,
dren with autism (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), such a deficit has little if anything to con-
there have been more than 30 experimental tribute to our understanding of Triad B—
tests. The vast majority of these have revealed repetitive behavior, obsessions, and the islets
profound impairments in the development of of ability. Thus, our view of autism is now
their empathizing ability. These tests are re- broader and suggests that alongside empathiz-
viewed elsewhere (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Baron- ing deficits, a different process is intact or
Cohen, Tager-Flusberg, & Cohen, 1993) but even superior. This process is what we call sys-
include deficits in the following: temizing (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003).

• Joint attention (Baron-Cohen, 1989c) Systemizing


• Use of mental state terms in language
(Tager-Flusberg, 1993) Whereas we think of empathizing as the drive
• Production and comprehension of pretense to identify and respond affectively to agents’
(Baron-Cohen, 1987; Wing & Gould, 1979) mental states to understand and predict the be-
• Understanding that “seeing leads to know- havior of that agent, we think of systemizing as
ing” (Baron-Cohen & Goodhart, 1994; the drive to analyze and build systems to un-
Leslie & Frith, 1988) derstand and predict the behavior of nonagen-
• Distinguishing mental from physical enti- tive events. Systems are all around us in our
ties (Baron-Cohen, 1989a; Ozonoff, Pen- environment and fall into at least six classes:
nington, & Rogers, 1990)
• Making the appearance-reality distinction 1. Technical (e.g., machines and tools)
(Baron-Cohen, 1989a) 2. Natural (e.g., biological and geographical
• Understanding false belief (Baron-Cohen phenomena)
et al., 1985) 3. Abstract (e.g., mathematics or computer
• Understanding beliefs about beliefs (Baron- programs)
Cohen, 1989b) 4. Social (e.g., a business or a football league)
• Understanding complex emotions (Baron- 5. Motoric (e.g., a juggling technique or a
Cohen, 1991) Frisbee throw)
• Showing concern at another’s pain (Yir- 6. Organizable (e.g., a collection, a taxonomy,
miya et al., 1992) or a list)

Some children and adults with AS show their The way we make sense of any of these sys-
empathizing deficits only on age-appropriate tems is not in terms of mental states, but rather
adult tests (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, et al., 1997; in terms of underlying rules and regularities.
632 Theoretical Perspectives

Systemizing involves an initial analysis of the motion of the planets (astronomy), and classi-
system down to its lowest level of detail to fication of lizards (taxonomy).
identify potentially relevant parameters that Experimental studies converge on the same
may play a causal role in the behavior of the conclusion: Children with autism have not only
system. These parameters are then systemati- an intact intuitive physics but also an acceler-
cally observed or manipulated one by one, and ated or superior development in this domain
their effects on the whole system are noted. (relative to their empathizing and relative to
That is, systemizing entails an analysis of their mental age, both verbal and nonverbal).
input-operation-output relationships. Once For example, using a picture-sequencing para-
the operations on inputs are identified and digm, children with autism performed signifi-
checked, the output of the system becomes to- cantly better than mental age-matched controls
tally predictable. in sequencing physical-causal stories (Baron-
Cohen et al., 1986). Two studies found children
Systemizing in Autism Spectrum with autism showed superior understanding of
Conditions a camera (Leekam & Perner, 1991; Leslie &
Thaiss, 1992). In two direct tests of intuitive
Are people with autism intact or even superior physics in children and adults with AS (Baron-
at systemizing? We know from clinical de- Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al., 2001; Law-
scriptions of children with autism that they are son, Baron-Cohen, & Wheelwright, 2004),
typically fascinated by machines (the paragon people with AS were found to be functioning
of nonintentional systems). Parents’ accounts at a normal or even superior level relative to
(Hart, 1989; Lovell, 1978; Park, 1967) are a controls. Finally, using the Systemizing Quo-
rich source of such descriptions. Typical tient (SQ), it was found that adults with AS
examples include extreme fascinations with scored higher than controls (Baron-Cohen
electricity pylons, burglar alarms, vacuum et al., 2003).
cleaners, washing machines, video players,
trains, planes, and clocks. Sometimes, the ma- Family Studies of Empathizing
chine that is the object of the child’s obsession and Systemizing
is quite simple (e.g., the workings of drain-
pipes or the design of windows). Our survey of Family studies add to this picture. Parents of
obsessions in children with autism substanti- children with AS also show mild but signifi-
ated this clinical observation that their preoc- cant deficits on an adult mind-reading task (on
cupations tend to cluster in the area of systems the adult version of the Reading the Mind in
(Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). the Eyes test). This task mirrors the deficit in
Children with an autism spectrum condi- empathizing seen in patients with autism or
tion who have enough language, such as is seen AS (Baron-Cohen & Hammer, 1997b; Baron-
in children with AS, may be described as hold- Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al., 2001). This
ing forth, like a “little professor,” on their familial resemblance at the cognitive level is
favorite subject or area of expertise, often fail- assumed to reflect genetic factors, since
ing to detect that their listener may have long autism and AS appear to have a strong herita-
since become bored of hearing more on the ble component (Bailey et al., 1995; Bolton
subject. The apparently precocious systematic et al., 1994; Folstein & Rutter, 1977; Le Cou-
understanding, while being relatively oblivi- teur et al., 1996).
ous to their listener’s level of interest, We should also expect that parents of chil-
suggests that their systemizing might be out- dren with autism or AS would be overrepre-
stripping their empathizing skills in develop- sented in occupations in which possession of
ment. The anecdotal evidence includes an superior systemizing is an advantage, while a
obsession with not just machines (technical deficit in empathizing would not necessarily
systems) but also other kinds of systems. be a disadvantage. A clear occupation for such
Examples of their interest in natural systems a cognitive profile is engineering. A study of
include obsessions with the weather (meteorol- 1,000 families found that fathers and grandfa-
ogy), the formation of mountains (geography), thers (patri- and matrilineal) of children with
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 633

autism or AS were more than twice as likely to viations from the mean. The scale of the dia-
work in the field of engineering, compared to gram is less important than the principle un-
fathers and grandfathers of children with derlying it.
other disabilities (Baron-Cohen, Wheel- We have used the terms Brain Type B (Bal-
wright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997). In- anced), Brain Type E (Empathizing), Brain
deed, 28.4% of children with autism or AS had Type S (Systemizing), to describe the three
at least one relative (father and/or grandfa- basic brain types that are generated from this
ther) who was an engineer. Related evidence model. These all fall within two standard devi-
comes from a survey of students at Cambridge ations from the mean on both dimensions. We
University, studying either sciences (physics, have also shown on the graph the extremes of
engineering, or mathematics) or humanities Brain Types S and E. The terms describe the
(English or French literature). When asked discrepancy between the empathizing score
about family history of a range of psychiatric and the systemizing score. In the Balanced
conditions (schizophrenia, anorexia, autism, Brain, there is no difference between scores
Down syndrome, or manic depression), the (i.e., E = S). In Brain Type E, empathizing is
students in the science group showed a sixfold one or two standard deviations higher than
increase in the rate of autism in their families, systemizing (i.e., E > S). In the Extreme Brain
and this increase was specific to autism Type E, this discrepancy is greater than two
(Baron-Cohen et al., 1998). standard deviations (i.e., E >> S). In Brain
Type S, systemizing is one or two standard de-
Plotting Empathizing and Systemizing viations higher than empathizing (i.e., S > E).
For the Extreme Brain Type S, this discrep-
If empathizing and systemizing are indepen- ancy is greater than two standard deviations
dent dimensions, it is possible to plot on or- (i.e., S >> E).
thogonal axes possible scores from possible The key point is the discrepancy between
tests assessing these two abilities. Figures the scores rather than the absolute scores
23.1 and 23.2 provides a visual representation themselves. For example, someone could score
of this model of the relationship between em- two standard deviations above the mean on
pathizing and systemizing. It suggests appro- empathizing (a very high score), but if the
priate labels for different possible patterns of score was three standard deviations above the
scores. The axes show number of standard de- mean on systemizing, he or she would be

Social Communication Imagining others'


sensitivity sensitivity thoughts and feelings

Empathizing

Islets of Obsessions Repetitive


ability with systems behavior

Systemizing

Figure 23.1 Explaining the core characteristics of autism spectrum conditions in terms of empathizing and
systemizing.
634 Theoretical Perspectives

Empathizing
number of pieces of evidence are consistent
with the extreme male brain (EMB) theory of
+3 autism. First, regarding empathizing measures,
females score higher than males on tests of un-
+2 derstanding faux pas, and people with AS score
even lower than unaffected boys (Baron-Cohen,
+1
Wheelwright, Stone, & Rutherford, 1999; Law-

Systemizing
son et al., 2004). Second, girls make more eye
contact than boys (Lutchmaya, Baron-Cohen, &
-3 -2 -1 0 +1 +2 +3
Raggett, 2002), and children with autism make
even less eye contact than unaffected boys
-1 (Swettenham et al., 1998). Third, girls tend to
pass false belief tests slightly earlier than boys
-2 (Happé, 1995), and children with autism are
even later to pass false belief tests. Finally,
-3 women score slightly higher than men on the
Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, and adults
with AS or high-functioning autism score even
Key lower than unaffected men (Baron-Cohen, Jol-
Type B (E = S) liffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997). There
are also established sex differences in system-
Type E (E > S)
izing, males tending to score higher on tests of
Type S (E < S) folk physics, map use, and mental rotation, for
Extreme Type E example (Kimura, 1999), and people with
autism being at least intact if not superior on
Extreme Type S
these tasks (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Sc-
ahill, Lawson, & Spong, 2001; Baron-Cohen
Figure 23.2 Empathizing and systemizing associa-
et al., 2003; Lawson et al., 2004).
tions. Note: Axes show standard deviations from the
mean. This model of the independence of em-
pathizing and systemizing also predicts the
described as having Brain Type S. Thus, the existence of very high-functioning individuals
key issue is possible asymmetries of ability. with AS, who may be extreme high achievers
Evidence from sex difference research in domains such as mathematics and physics—
(Kimura, 1992) suggests that Brain Type S is equivalent to Nobel Prize winners even—but
more commonly found in males, while Brain who have deficits in empathizing. Some case
Type E is more frequent in females. For this studies are beginning to identify such very
reason, we can also use the terminology Fe- high-functioning individuals (Baron-Cohen,
male Brain and Male Brain types as synonyms Wheelwright, et al., 1999).
for Brain Types E and S, respectively. One re-
sult that is consistent with this idea is that OTHER MODELS OF COGNITIVE
human neonates, 1 day old, show a sex differ- DEVELOPMENT IN AUTISM
ence: Female babies look longer at a human
face than a mechanical mobile, while male ba- In this final section, we briefly summarize
bies show the opposite pattern of preferences some other cognitive developmental theories
(Connellan, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Ba’tki, of autism because they are important alterna-
& Ahluwalia, 2001). tives against which to consider the empathiz-
ing-systemizing theory.
THE EXTREME MALE BRAIN
THEORY Executive Function Theory

Autism has been described as the extreme of People with autism spectrum conditions show
the male brain (Asperger, 1944; Baron-Cohen, repetitive behavior, a strong desire for rou-
2002; Baron-Cohen & Hammer, 1997a). A tines, and a need for sameness. The only cogni-
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 635

tive account that has attempted to explain this previously documented, namely, weak central
aspect of the syndrome is the executive dys- coherence (Frith, 1989; Happé, 1996). Weak
function theory (Ozonoff, Rogers, Farnham, & central coherence refers to the individual’s
Pennington, 1994; Pennington et al., 1997; preference for local detail over global pro-
Russell, 1997b). This theory paints an essen- cessing. This has been demonstrated in
tially negative view of this repetitive behavior, terms of an autistic superiority on the Em-
assuming that it is a form of frontal lobe perse- bedded Figures Task (EFT) and the Block
veration or inability to shift attention. Design Subtest (Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen,
We recognize that some forms of repetitive 1997; Shah & Frith, 1983, 1993). It has also
behavior in autism such as stereotypies (e.g., been demonstrated in terms of an autistic
twiddling the fingers rapidly in peripheral vi- deficit in integrating fragments of objects and
sion) are likely to be due to executive deficits. integrating sentences within a paragraph (Jol-
Moreover, we recognize that, as people with liffe & Baron-Cohen, 2001a; Jolliffe &
autism who have additional learning disabili- Baron-Cohen, 2001b). The faster and more
ties are tested, executive deficits are more accurate performance on the EFT and Block
likely to be found (Russell, 1997a). But the Design subtest has been interpreted as evi-
fact that it is possible for people with AS to dence of good segmentation skills and supe-
have no demonstrable executive dysfunction rior attention to detail. The latter has also
while still having deficits in empathizing and been demonstrated on visual search tasks
talents in systemizing suggests that executive (Plaisted, O’Riordan, & Baron-Cohen,
dysfunction cannot be a core feature of autism 1998a, 1998b).
spectrum conditions. Our view of systemizing certainly em-
The executive account has also traditionally braces aspects of the central coherence (CC)
ignored the content of repetitive behavior. The theory. For example, systemizing requires as a
emphasizing-systemizing (E-S) theory, in con- first stage an excellent attention to detail,
trast, draws attention to the fact that much identifying parameters that may then be
repetitive behavior involves the child’s obses- tested for their role in the behavior of the sys-
sional or strong interests with mechanical sys- tem under examination. So, both the E-S the-
tems (e.g., light switches or water faucets) or ory and the CC theory predict excellent
other systems that can be understood in terms attention to detail. However, the E-S and CC
of rules and regularities. Rather than these be- theories also make opposite predictions when
haviors being a sign of executive dysfunction, it comes to an individual with autism being
they may reflect the child’s intact or even su- able to understand a whole system. The E-S
perior development of his or her systemizing. theory predicts that people with autism, faced
The child’s obsession with machines and sys- with a new system to learn, will learn it faster
tems and what is often described as his or her than someone without autism, as long as there
“need for sameness” in attempting to hold the are underlying rules and regularities that can
environment constant might be signs that the be discovered. Moreover, they will readily
child is a superior systemizer. The child might grasp that a change of one parameter in one
be conducting mini-experiments in his or her part of the system may have distant effects on
surroundings in an attempt to identify physi- another part of the system. Thus, if the task is
cal-causal or other systematic principles un- a constructional one (e.g., building a model
derlying events. plane), they will be able to grasp that changing
One possibility is that the strong drive to the thickness of the wings may cause the plane
systemize seen in autism spectrum conditions to land at a steeper angle. This kind of reason-
may underlie the Triad B features (repetitive ing clearly involves good central coherence of
behavior, obsessional or narrow interests, and the system. What is being understood is the
the islets of ability). relationship between one parameter and one
distal outcome. In contrast, the CC theory
Central Coherence Theory should predict that they should fail to under-
stand whole (global) systems or the relation-
It could be argued that good systemizing ships between parts of a system. This has not
skills are simply an expression of an anomaly been tested.
636 Theoretical Perspectives

SUMMARY Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). The autistic child’s the-


ory of mind: A case of specific developmental
This chapter has reviewed both the early mind- delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
blindness theory of autism and the more recent atry, 30, 285–298.
extensions of the empathizing-systemizing Baron-Cohen, S. (1989c). Perceptual role-taking
theory and the extreme male brain theory of and protodeclarative pointing in autism.
autism. The first of these extensions addresses British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
7, 113–127.
a problem that the early theory had, namely,
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Do people with autism
the need to also account for the obsessional understand what causes emotion? Child Devel-
features of autism. The second of these may opment, 62, 385–395.
help explain the marked sex ratio in autism Baron-Cohen, S. (1994a). How to build a baby that
and throw light on the biological basis of can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in
autism (Lutchmaya & Baron-Cohen, 2002). mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cogni-
Both of these extensions lead to new predic- tive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 13,
tions when contrasted with other cognitive de- 513–552.
velopmental theories of this condition and Baron-Cohen, S. (1994b). The mindreading sys-
illustrate some of the progress that is being tem: New directions for research. Current Psy-
chology of Cognition, 13, 724–750.
made in this part of the field of developmental
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay
psychopathology.
on autism and theory of mind. Boston: MIT
Press/ Bradford Books.
Cross-References Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain
theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Aspects of social development are discussed 6, 248–254.
in Chapters 11 and 26; attention and percep- Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S.,
tion are discussed in Chapter 13. Issues of Short, L., Mead, G., Smith, A., et al. (1998).
Does autism occur more often in families of
emotional developmental are discussed in
physicists, engineers, and mathematicians?
Chapter 15. Autism, 2, 296–301.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Goodhart, F. (1994). The “see-
REFERENCES ing leads to knowing” deficit in autism: The
Pratt and Bryant probe. British Journal of De-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag- velopmental Psychology, 12, 397–402.
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997a). Is autism
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. an extreme form of the male brain? Advances
Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopa- in Infancy Research, 11, 193–217.
then” im Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997b). Parents of
und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136. children with Asperger syndrome: What is the
Avis, J., & Harris, P. (1991). Belief-desire reason- cognitive phenotype? Journal of Cognitive
ing among Baka children: Evidence for a uni- Neuroscience, 9, 548–554.
versal conception of mind. Child Development, Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., &
62, 460–467. Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, of theory of mind: Evidence from very high
P., Simmonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). functioning adults with autism or Asperger
Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evi- Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and
dence from a British twin study. Psychological Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.
Medicine, 25, 63–77. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985).
Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Perception in autistic Does the autistic child have a “ theory of
children. In D. Cohen (Ed.), Handbook of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986).
New York: Wiley. Mechanical, behavioural and intentional un-
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). Are autistic children be- derstanding of picture stories in autistic chil-
haviourists? An examination of their mental- dren. British Journal of Developmental
physical and appearance-reality distinctions. Psychology, 4, 113–125.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone,
ders, 19, 579–600. V., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 637

sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V., &
children and children with Asperger syn- Rutherford, M. (1999). A mathematician, a
drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental physicist, and a computer scientist with As-
Disorders, 29, 407–418. perger syndrome: Performance on folk psy-
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gu- chology and folk physics test. Neurocase, 5,
runathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The 475–483.
Systemising Quotient (SQ): An investigation of Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C.,
adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Func- Bolton, P., & Goodyer, I. (1997). Is there a
tioning Autism and normal sex differences. link between engineering and autism? Autism:
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, An International Journal of Research and Prac-
Series B [Special issue], 358, 361–374. tice, 1, 153–163.
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L.,
(Eds.). (1993). Understanding other minds: & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and commu-
Perspectives from autism: Oxford University nication from 9–13 months: Correlational
Press. findings. In E. Bates (Ed.), The emergence of
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (1999). Ob- symbols: Cognition and communication in in-
sessions in children with autism or Asperger fancy. New York: Academic Press.
syndrome: A content analysis in terms of core Bolton, P., MacDonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P.,
domains of cognition. British Journal of Psy- Goode, S., Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case-
chiatry, 175, 484–490. control family history study of autism. Journal
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35,
Friendship Questionnaire (FQ): An investiga- 877–900.
tion of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Brentano, F. (1970). Psychology from an empirical
Functioning Autism, and normal sex differ- standpoint. London: Routledge, and Kegan
ences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Paul.
Disorders, 33, 509–517. Butterworth, G. (1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The of joint visual attention. In A. Whilen (Ed.),
Empathy Quotient (EQ). An investigation of Natural theories of mind. Oxford, England.
adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Func- Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S.,
tioning Autism, and normal sex differences. Ba’tki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2001). Sex differ-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ences in human neonatal social perception. In-
ders, 34, 163–175. fant Behavior and Development, 23, 113–118.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., &
Lawson, J., & Hill, J. (2002). The exact mind: Brockbanck, M. (1999). Goal attribution with-
Empathising and systemising in autism spec- out agency cues: The perception of “pure rea-
trum conditions. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Hand- son” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 253–284.
book of cognitive development: Blackwells. Dennett, D. (1987). The intentional stance. Cam-
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., & bridge, MA: MIT Press/ Bradford Books.
Golan, O. (submitted). A Taxonomy of Human Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion:
Emotions. An old controversy and new findings. In V.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Bruce, A. Cowey, et al. (Eds.), Processing the
Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the facial image. New York: Clarendon Press/Ox-
Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A ford University Press.
study with normal adults, and adults with As- Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: A
perger Syndrome or High-Functioning autism. genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Psycholology and Psychiatry, 18, 297–321.
42, 241–252. Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. normal and autistic children: I. Immediate re-
(1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evi- call of auditory sequences. Journal of Abnor-
dence from normal adults and adults with mal Psychology, 76, 413–420.
autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cogni- Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.
tion, 4, 311–331. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Frith, U. (1991). Autism and Asperger’s syndrome.
Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001). Are intuitive Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
physics and intuitive psychology independent? Gelman, S., & Hirschfield, L. (1994). Mapping the
Journal of Developmental and Learning Disor- mind. Cambridge: Press Syndicate, University
ders, 5, 47–78. of Cambridge.
638 Theoretical Perspectives

Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Gergely, C., & Biro, S. Klin, A., Volkmar, F., Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., &
(1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 Rourke, B. (1995). Validity and neuropsycho-
months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193. logical characterization of Asperger syn-
Griffin, R., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The inten- drome: Convergence with nonverbal learning
tional stance: Developmental and neurocognitive disabilities syndrome. Journal of Child Psy-
perspectives. In A. Brook & D. Ross (Eds.), chology and Psychiatry, 36, 1127–1140.
Dennett beyond philosophy: Cambridge Uni- Lawson, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S.
versity Press. (2004). Empathising and systemising in adults
Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and with and without Asperger Syndrome. Journal
theory of mind in autism: A test of Relevance of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34,
Theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119. 301–310.
Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability Le Couteur, A., Bailey, A., Goode, S., Pickles, A.,
in the theory of mind task performance of sub- Robertson, S., Gottesman, I., et al. (1996). A
jects with autism. Child Development, 66, broader phenotype of autism: The clinical
843–855. spectrum in twins. Journal of Child Psychol-
Happé, F. (1996). Studying weak central coherence ogy and Psychiatry, 37, 785–801.
at low levels: Children with autism do not suc- Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic
cumb to visual illusions: A research note. child have a metarepresentational deficit?
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Cognition, 40, 203–218.
37, 873–877. Leslie, A. (1987). Pretence and representation: The
Harris, P., Johnson, C. N., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., origins of “ theory of mind.” Psychological Re-
& Cooke, T. (1989). Young children’s theory view, 94, 412–426.
of mind and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, Leslie, A. (1995). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core
3, 379–400. architecture and domain specificity. In L.
Hart, C. (1989). Without reason. New York: Harper Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Domain speci-
& Row. ficity in cognition and culture. New York:
Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1970). Psychologi- Cambridge University Press.
cal experiments with autistic children. Lon- Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children’s
don: Pergamon Press. understanding of seeing, knowing, and believ-
Hood, B., Willen, J., & Driver, J. (1997). An eye- ing. British Journal of Developmental Psychol-
direction detector triggers shifts of visual ogy, 6, 315–324.
attention in human infants. Unpublished man- Leslie, A., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month old
uscript, Harvard University. infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25,
Johnson, S. (2000). The recognition of mentalistic 265–288.
agents in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Leslie, A., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity
4, 22–28. in conceptual development: Evidence from
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people autism. Cognition, 43, 225–251.
with autism or Asperger’s syndrome faster Lovell, A. (1978). In a summer garment. London:
than normal on the Embedded Figures Task? Secker and Warburg.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Lutchmaya, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). Human
38, 527–534. sex differences in social and non-social look-
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001a). A test of ing preferences at 12 months of age. Infant Be-
central coherence theory: Can adults with haviour and Development, 25, 319–325.
high-functioning autism or Asperger Syn- Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Raggett, P.
drome integrate fragments of an object. Cogni- (2002). Foetal testosterone and eye contact in
tive Neuropsychiatry, 6, 193–216. 12 month old infants. Infant Behavior and De-
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001b). A test of velopment, 25, 327–335.
central coherence theory: Can adults with Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention
high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome and early social communication. Journal of
integrate objects in context? Visual Cognition, Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
8, 67–101. 653–676.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affec- Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990).
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Are there emotion perception deficits in
Kimura, D. (1992, September). Sex differences in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psy-
the brain. Scientific American, 119–125. chology and Psychiatry, 31, 343–363.
Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S., Farnham, J., & Pennington,
MA: MIT Press. B. (1994). Can standard measures identify
Empathizing and Systemizing in Autism Spectrum Conditions 639

subclinical markers of autism? Journal of Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T.,
Autism and Developmental Disorders. Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The
Park, C. (1967). The siege. London: Hutchinson. frequency and distribution of spontaneous at-
Pennington, B., Rogers, S., Bennetto, L., Griffith, tention shifts between social and non-social
E., Reed, D., & Shyu, V. (1997). Validity test stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and
of the executive dysfunction hypothesis of non-autistic developmentally delayed infants.
autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Executive function- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9,
ing in autism. Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- 747–753.
versity Press. Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, M., & Bartolucci,
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome and autism:
Mary thinks that . . . ” Attribution of second- Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the Ameri-
order beliefs by 5–10 year old children. Jour- can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, try, 29, 130–136.
437–471. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals
Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. about the understanding of minds in children
(1998a). Enhanced discrimination of novel, with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-
highly similar stimuli by adults with autism Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding
during a perceptual learning task. Journal of other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, England: Oxford University Press.
765–775. Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint-attentional
Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. processes in early language acquisition. Lan-
(1998b). Enhanced visual search for a con- guage Sciences, 10, 69–88.
junctive target in autism: A research note. Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal perception of ex-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, pressive behaviours by human infants. Journal
39, 777–783. of Experimental Child Psychology, 33,
Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children un- 514–535.
derstand that looking leads to knowing (so Wellman, H., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young chil-
long as they are looking into a single barrel). dren’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition, 30,
Child Development, 61, 973–983. 239–277.
Premack, D. (1990). The infant’s theory of self- Whiten, A. (1991). Natural theories of mind. Ox-
propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16. ford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about be-
Young infants’ sensitivity to movement infor- liefs: Representation and constraining func-
mation specifying social causality. Cognitive tion of wrong beliefs in young children’s
Development, 12, 537–561. understanding of deception. Cognition, 13,
Russell, J. (Ed.). (1997a). Autism as an executive 103–128.
disorder. Oxford, England: Oxford University Wing, L. (1976). Early childhood autism. Oxford:
Press. Pergamon Press.
Russell, J. (1997b). How executive disorders can Wing, L. (1981). Asperger syndrome: A clinical ac-
bring about an inadequate theory of mind. In J. count. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–130.
Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder. Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. of social interaction and associated abnormal-
Rutter, M. (1978). Language disorder and infantile ities in children: Epidemiology and classifica-
autism. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treat- Disorders, 9, 11–29.
ment. New York: Plenum Press. World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10
Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for classification of mental and behavioral disor-
joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, ders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic
253, 265–266. guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P.
autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psy- (1992). Empathy and cognition in high func-
chology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620. tioning children with autism. Child Develop-
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic indi- ment, 63, 150–160.
viduals show superior performance on the
block design test? Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.
CHAPTER 24

The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism

FRANCESCA HAPPÉ

Many of the chapters in this volume offer ac- “central coherence.” Central coherence is the
counts of the core deficits that characterize term she coined for the everyday tendency to
autism spectrum disorders. The social and process incoming information in context for
communicative difficulties and the rigid pat- gist—pulling information together for higher
tern of behavior and interests are the focus level meaning, often at the expense of memory
of current theories postulating deficits in “ the- for detail. For example, as Bartlett’s classic
ory of mind,” executive functions, and so forth. work showed, the gist of a story is easily re-
What such deficit accounts fail to explain, called, while the surface form is effortful to
however, is why people with autism show not retain and quickly lost (Bartlett, 1932). Global
only deficits but also striking assets in certain processing also predominates over local pro-
areas. Savant skills, in recognized areas such cessing in at least some aspects of perception;
as music, art, calculation, and memory, are 10 we see the whole rather than the parts (Kimchi,
times more common in people with autism than 1992; Navon, 1977). The preference for inte-
in others with intellectual disabilities, occur- gration and global processing also character-
ring in approximately 1 in 10 individuals with izes young typically developing children and
autism (Miller, 1999). If skills outside these individuals with (nonautistic) intellectual dis-
areas are included, such as doing jigsaw puz- ability—who (unlike those with autism) show
zles remarkably well, the great majority of an advantage recalling organized versus jum-
people with autism might be said to have a spe- bled material (Hermelin & O’Connor, 1967).
cific and surprising talent. Even the child who Indeed, research suggests that global process-
is extremely distressed by minute changes to a ing may predominate even in infants as young
familiar room (undetectable to most people) as 3 months (Bhatt, Rovee-Collier, & Shyi,
shows an unusual skill, albeit with upsetting 1994; Freedland & Dannemiller, 1996).
results. How can we account for these assets, Frith suggested that this aspect of human
which current deficit theories of autism appear information processing is disturbed in autism
unable to explain? and that people with autism show detail-
One current theory of autism proposes a dif- focused processing in which features are per-
ferent, rather than merely deficient, mind at ceived and retained at the expense of global
the center of autism. Frith (1989), prompted by configuration and higher level meaning. Clin-
a strong belief that assets and deficits in ically, children and adults with autism often
autism might have one and the same origin, show a preoccupation with details and parts,
proposed that autism is characterized by weak while failing to extract gist or see “ the big

I would like to thank the people with autism spectrum disorders, their families, and caregivers who have
helped us with our research. The ideas presented here were developed with Uta Frith and my research team
at the SGDP Research Centre and have been discussed in part in Happé (1999).

640
The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 641

picture.” Kanner, in his original writings on the usual benefit from meaning in memory
autism, commented on the tendency for frag- tests. Thus, while control subjects recalled
mentary processing in relation to the chil- sentences far better than unconnected word
dren’s characteristic resistance to change: strings, this advantage was greatly dimin-
“. . . a situation, a performance, a sentence is ished in the autism group. This work and sub-
not regarded as complete if it is not made up sequent replications (Tager-Flusberg, 1991;
of exactly the same elements that were pres- but see Lopez & Leekam, 2003) suggest that
ent at the time the child was first confronted people with autism do not make use of either
with it ” (Kanner, 1943). Indeed, Kanner saw semantic relations (same category versus as-
as a universal feature of autism the “inability sorted words) or grammatical relations (sen-
to experience wholes without full attention to tences versus word lists) in memory. Weak
the constituent parts,” a description akin to coherence is also demonstrated by good ver-
Frith’s notion of weak central coherence. batim but poor gist memory for story material
One of the most positive aspects of Frith’s (Scheuffgen, 1998) and poor inference, dis-
notion of central coherence is the ability to ambiguation, and construction of narrative
explain patterns of excellent and poor perfor- (Dennis, Lazenby, & Lockyer, 2001; Jolliffe,
mance with one cognitive postulate. Weak 1998; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999; Norbury
central coherence predicts relatively good per- & Bishop, 2002).
formance where attention to local information Frith and Snowling (1983) used homo-
(i.e., relatively piecemeal processing) is ad- graphs (words with one spelling, two mean-
vantageous, but poor performance on tasks re- ings, and two pronunciations) to examine use
quiring the recognition of global meaning or of preceding sentence context to derive mean-
integration of stimuli in context. The central ing and determine pronunciation; for example,
coherence account of autism, then, is better “In her eye there was a big tear,” “In her dress
characterized in terms of cognitive style than there was a big tear.” If people with autism
cognitive deficit. have weak central coherence at this level, then
reading a sentence may, for them, be akin to
WEAK CENTRAL COHERENCE: reading a list of unconnected words, and sen-
EVIDENCE AT THREE LEVELS tence context will not be built up to allow
OF PROCESSING meaning-driven disambiguation. In the origi-
nal studies and subsequent replications with
In recent years, the notion that children with high-functioning children and adults (Happé,
autism show weak central coherence has re- 1997; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999), individ-
ceived increasing interest and prompted a uals with autism spectrum disorder failed to
rapidly growing number of studies. Detail- use preceding sentence context to determine
focused processing has been demonstrated the pronunciation of homographs. These find-
at several levels (reviewed later). The division ings bring to mind Kanner’s description of his
into verbal-semantic, visuo-spatial construc- original cases: “. . . the children read monoto-
tional, and perceptual levels for the purpose nously, and a story . . . is experienced in unre-
of this review is largely for convenience. lated portions rather than in its coherent
An interesting issue for future research con- totality” (Kanner, 1943). This finding is par-
cerns possible high-level or top-down effects ticularly interesting, in that people with
on even apparently peripheral perceptual autism (at these levels of intelligence) clearly
processes (Coren & Enns, 1993). are able to read for meaning when explicitly
required to do so. Indeed, when instructed in
Verbal-Semantic Coherence reading for meaning, group differences on the
homograph task disappeared (Snowling &
Some of the earliest empirical evidence that Frith, 1986). It seems, then, that weak central
influenced Frith’s notion of weak central co- coherence characterizes the spontaneous ap-
herence came from the groundbreaking stud- proach or automatic processing preference of
ies by Hermelin and O’Connor (1967), who people with autism and is thus a cognitive
showed that people with autism do not derive “style” best captured in open-ended tasks.
642 Theoretical Perspectives

Visuo-Spatial Constructional Coherence Mottron and colleagues also situate the


mechanism for weak coherence effects at the
An elegant demonstration of weak coherence level of perception. Their “enhanced perceptual
was given by Shah and Frith (1993), who functioning framework ” posits overdeveloped
showed that the well-documented facility of low-level perception and atypical relationships
people with autism on the standard Wechsler between low- and high-level processing (see
Block Design task is due specifically to seg- Mottron & Burack, 2001). They cite in favor
mentation abilities. A sizable advantage was of their proposal, but also compatible with
gained from presegmentation of designs for Plaisted et al.’s account, the finding that people
typically developing and intellectually im- with autism show enhanced local processing
paired groups but was not observed in individu- and intact global processing of musical stimuli
als with autism, suggesting that the latter (Heaton, 2003; Heaton, Hermelin, & Pring,
processed the design in terms of its constituent 1998; Mottron, Peretz, & Menard, 2000).
blocks. Individuals with autism, both low- and While such findings may be at odds with the
high-functioning, also excel at the Embedded original description of weak coherence, they
Figures Test (EFT), in which a small shape are in keeping with the suggestion (see preced-
must be found within a larger design (Jolliffe & ing discussion of homograph reading) that weak
Baron-Cohen, 1997; Shah & Frith, 1983). coherence is a cognitive style or bias; that is,
Autistic weak coherence has also been demon- that global processing is possible for people
strated in studies showing good recognition of with autism, but local processing is preferred in
objects from detail despite poor integration of open-ended tasks.
object parts (Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 2001) Other evidence of good feature processing
and detail-by-detail drawing style (Booth, or local bias at the perceptual level includes
Charlton, Hughes, & Happé, in press; Mottron reduced benefit from canonical pattern in
& Belleville, 1993) with facility for copying dot counting (Jarrold & Russell, 1997), unusu-
even globally incoherent (“impossible”) figures ally high occurrence of absolute pitch (Heaton,
(Mottron, Belleville, & Ménard, 1999). Hermelin, & Pring, 1998), reduced suscep-
tibility to visually induced motion (Gepner,
Perceptual Coherence Mestre, Masson, & de Schonen, 1995), a
reduced McGurk effect (i.e., less influence
In recent years, there has been a renewal of in- from visual to auditory speech perception;
terest in perceptual processes in autism. In par- DeGelder, Vroomen, & Van der Heide, 1991),
ticular, Plaisted and colleagues have suggested and mixed findings regarding susceptibility
that the mechanism underlying weak coherence to visual illusions (Happé, 1996; Ropar &
effects may operate at the perceptual level and Mitchell, 1999). It is notable that autobiograph-
specifically lies in enhanced discrimination ical accounts of autism often describe frag-
and reduced generalization (see Plaisted, mented perception (Gerland, 1997).
2001). Plaisted hypothesizes that people with
autism process features held in common be- Negative Findings
tween objects relatively poorly and process
features unique to an object (those that dis- In general, then, people with autism are distin-
criminate items) relatively well. This is thought guished from age- and ability-matched com-
to underlie the pattern of superior visual search parison groups in showing relative attention to
(O’Riordan, Plaisted, Driver, & Baron-Cohen, parts and relative inattention to wholes. People
2001), superior discrimination learning of with autism do appear to integrate the proper-
highly confusable patterns (Plaisted, O’Riordan, ties of a single object (e.g., color and form in a
& Baron-Cohen, 1998), and poor prototype ex- visual search task; Plaisted et al., 1998) and to
traction (Plaisted, O’Riordan, Aitken, & Kill- process the meaning of individual words (in
cross, submitted; but see also Klinger & Stroop tasks; Eskes, Bryson, & McCormick,
Dawson, 2001), demonstrated by Plaisted, 1990; Frith & Snowling, 1983) and objects (in
O’Riordan, and their colleagues in an elegant memory tasks; Ameli, Courchesne, Lincoln,
series of studies. Kaufman, & Grillon, 1988; Pring & Hermelin,
The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 643

1993). It seems to be in connecting words or Deficits in theory of mind were considered


objects that coherence is weak, although Lopez just one consequence of weak central coher-
and Leekam (2003) have shown that straight- ence: Understanding social interaction, and
forward priming from a word or scene (or extracting the higher level representation of
perhaps local elements of the scene) does occur thoughts underlying behavior, was seen as the
in autism. There have also been findings pinnacle of coherent processing and gist ex-
directly counter to those predicted by the weak traction. On this account, people with autism
coherence hypothesis. Ozonoff, Strayer, were socially impaired because they were un-
McMahon, and Filloux (1994) and Mottron, able to derive high-level meaning, necessary
Burack, Stauder, and Robaey (1999) failed to for development and use of theory of mind.
find the predicted local advantage using the Subsequently, Frith and Happé (1994) modi-
well-known Navon (1977) hierarchical figures; fied this view and proposed as a working hy-
asked to report about large letters composed pothesis that weak central coherence and
of smaller letters, people with autism showed impaired theory of mind were independent
the normal tendency to process the global form facets of autism (for discussion, see Happé,
first and with greater interference from global 2000). However, it is likely that these two as-
to local levels. However, this paradigm is pects of autism interact, and failure to inte-
known to be sensitive to small changes of grate information in context may contribute to
methodology (Kimchi, 1992); more recently, everyday life social difficulties. Featural pro-
Plaisted, Swettenham, and Rees (1999) have cessing may play a part in certain social im-
found evidence of local advantage and interfer- pairments: Piecemeal processing of faces, for
ence from local to global stimuli in a condition example (as reflected in reduced decrement
where participants with autism were required from inversion in face recognition tests; Hob-
to divide attention between local and global son, Ouston, & Lee, 1988), may hamper emo-
levels but not in a selective attention task. An- tion recognition (McKelvie, 1995).
other counterfinding comes from Brian and There is evidence that some people with
Bryson (1996), who found normal effects of autism who pass false belief tests still show
meaning in a modified EFT, although they weak central coherence. For example, theory of
failed to find the well-replicated superiority on mind task performance is related to perfor-
standard EFT, and it is unclear whether differ- mance on the Comprehension subtest of the
ent results would have been obtained with Wechsler scales (commonly thought to require
groups matched on IQ. Overall, the evidence to pragmatic and social skill) but not to perfor-
date would appear to support the notion that mance on the Block Design subtest (Happé,
processing of details and features is somewhat 1994), thought to be a marker of central coher-
superior in autism, while the data on impair- ence. Weak coherence seems to characterize
ments of global or configural processing are people with autism regardless of their theory of
less clear. What is certain is that the notion of mind ability in studies using perceptual (visual
central coherence requires further refinement illusions; Happé, 1996) and verbal tasks ( ho-
and can only benefit from the alternative ac- mograph reading; Happé, 1997). Jarrold, But-
counts and suggestions for underlying mecha- ler, Cottington, and Jimenez (2000), however,
nisms now emerging in the field. found evidence of an inverse relation between
ability to ascribe mental states to faces (inter-
CENTRAL COHERENCE AND preted as tapping theory of mind) and segmen-
DEFICIT ACCOUNTS tation ability (interpreted as evidence of weak
coherence; Shah & Frith, 1983). They found
There are relatively few studies of the relation that performance on Baron-Cohen’s “Eyes
between coherence and those key abilities task ” and speed on the EFT were significantly
thought to be impaired in autism, such as the- negatively correlated in a sample of under-
ory of mind or executive function. Central co- graduates. In addition, in a group of children
herence was at first proposed to account for with autism, false belief task performance was
the theory of mind impairment as well as for negatively correlated with EFT performance,
nonsocial assets and deficits (Frith, 1989). with the correlation reaching significance
644 Theoretical Perspectives

once verbal mental age was partialled out. Lon- solute pitch. Takeuchi and Hulse (1993) con-
gitudinal studies would be necessary, however, clude from a review of the research on typical
to establish possible developmental causal rela- development that absolute pitch could be
tions between coherence and theory of mind learned by most children before about 6 years
(for further discussion, see Happé, 2001). of age, after which “a general developmental
There have been two investigations of the shift from perceiving individual features to
possible relation between coherence and execu- perceiving relations among features makes
tive functions. Teunisse, Cools, van Spaendonck, [absolute pitch] difficult or impossible to ac-
Aerts, and Berger (2001) tested coherence and quire” (p. 345). If people with autism show a
shifting ability in high-functioning adoles- pervasive and persistent local processing bias,
cents with autism. They found weak coherence this would explain the high frequency of ab-
and poor shifting to be more common in the solute pitch and the superior ability to learn
autism group than among comparison typi- note-name mappings at later ages.
cally developing participants, but neither was In the domain of graphic talent, it also ap-
universal. Performance on the two types of pears that the extraordinary skill of some indi-
measure was unrelated and did not correlate viduals with autism may reflect a detail-focused
with symptom severity or social ability. Booth processing style. Mottron and Belleville (1993)
et al. (2003) examined directly the possible present a case study of an artist with autism
role of one executive function, planning, on whose productions are characterized by pro-
global / local processing in a drawing task. ceeding from one contiguous detail to the next,
They compared boys with autism spectrum rather than the more usual sketching of outline
disorders and boys with attention-deficit / followed by details. On a number of tasks (e.g.,
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as well as a copying of impossible figures), this man showed
typically developing comparison group, all fragmented perception and a bias toward
matched on age and IQ. A drawing task requir- local processing. Pring, Hermelin, and Heavey
ing planning ahead (to add a requested internal (1995), who tested part-whole processing (using
element), showed the predicted planning modified Block Design tasks) in children with
deficits in both clinical groups, while analysis autism and normally developing children with
of drawing style showed that piecemeal draw- and without artistic talent, conclude that there
ing (e.g., starting with features, drawing detail is “a facility in autism for seeing wholes in
to detail) was characteristic of the autism terms of their parts, rather than as unified
group only. Performance on the executive gestalts” (p. 1073), and that this ability may be
function and central coherence elements of the a general characteristic of individuals with an
task did not correlate in the clinical groups, aptitude for drawing, with or without autism.
and the authors conclude that weak coherence Thus, a natural focus on features may be a pre-
is not common to all groups with executive disposing factor for talent in both music and art.
dysfunction and that poor planning cannot ex-
plain detail focus in autism. CENTRAL COHERENCE AND
NORMAL VARIATION IN COGNITIVE
COHERENCE AND SAVANT SKILLS STYLE

Weak central coherence, then, may be a cogni- Since weak central coherence gives both advan-
tive style capable of explaining autistic assets, tages and disadvantages, it is possible to think
as well as deficits, in experimental tasks. It of this balance ( between preference for parts
may also be able to explain the high rate of sa- versus wholes) as akin to a cognitive style—a
vant skills among people with autism ( but for style that may vary in the normal population.
an alternative account, see Mottron & Burack, There may be a normal distribution of cognitive
2001). In the area of musical talent, Heaton style from “ weak ” central coherence (preferen-
et al. (1998) have shown that musically naive tial processing of parts, e.g., good proofread-
children with autism are significantly better ing), to “strong” (preferential processing of
than matched controls at learning labels for wholes, e.g., good gist memory). There is some
individual pitches—the ability underlying ab- disparate evidence of normal individual differ-
The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 645

ences in local-global processing from infancy results fit with work by Baron-Cohen and col-
(Stoecker, Colombo, Frick, & Allen, 1998), leagues, showing that fathers of children with
through childhood (Chynn, Garrod, Demick, & autism are fast at the EFT (Baron-Cohen &
DeVos, 1991), and in adulthood (Marendaz, Hammer, 1997) and overrepresented in pro-
1985). Sex differences have been reported on fessions such as engineering (Baron-Cohen,
tasks thought to tap local-global processing Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer,
(Kramer, Ellenberg, Leonard, & Share, 1996), 1997; but see counterargument by Jarrold &
although studies have typically confounded Routh, 1998). However, while Baron-Cohen
type ( local /global) and domain (visuo-spatial / et al. explain their results in terms of superior
verbal) of processing. The possibility of sex “ folk physics” (intuitive understanding of
differences in coherence is intriguing in rela- physical systems) or, more recently, superior
tion to autism, which shows a very high male to “systemizing” (Baron-Cohen, 2002), the hy-
female ratio, especially at the high-ability end pothesis of weak central coherence predicts
of the spectrum. Might the normal distribution that people with autism and their relatives
of coherence in males be shifted toward weak will be characterized by expertise only with
coherence and featural processing? At the ex- those mechanical (and nonmechanical) sys-
treme weak coherence end of the continuum tems where detail focus is an advantage. Weak
may lie an area of increased risk for autism— central coherence also stretches beyond the
individuals who fall at this extreme end of the visuo-spatial domain and predicts piecemeal
continuum of cognitive style may be predis- processing in verbal tasks (see earlier discus-
posed to develop autism if unlucky enough to sion), not easily accounted for by superior folk
suffer the additional deficits (e.g., impaired physics or systemizing.
theory of mind, executive dysfunctions) appar-
ent in this disorder. CONCLUSION

CENTRAL COHERENCE AND THE Many challenges remain to the central coher-
EXTENDED PHENOTYPE OF AUTISM ence account, not least to specify the cognitive
and neural mechanisms for coherence. Should
As a cognitive style, rather than deficit, weak we think of a single, central mechanism inte-
central coherence is an interesting contender for grating information from diverse modules/
an aspect of autism that is transmitted geneti- systems for higher level meaning/configura-
cally and characterizes the relatives of individu- tion? Or should coherence be thought of as a
als with autism. We compared cognitive style property of each subsystem, a setting for the
in parents of boys with autism, with dyslexia, relative precedence of global versus local pro-
or without developmental disorder (Happé, cessing, repeated throughout the brain? We are
Briskman, & Frith, 2001). The parents, and es- currently exploring individual differences in
pecially fathers, of the children with autism coherence across and within a number of do-
showed significantly superior performance on mains to establish whether, for example, de-
tasks favoring local processing: They excelled at gree of coherence in a verbal task predicts
the EFT, at (unsegmented) block design, and at degree of coherence in a visuo-spatial task.
accurately judging visual illusions. They were Neuropsychological lesion and brain imag-
also more likely than other fathers to give local ing studies may also give clues to the unitary
completions to sentence stems such as, “ The or distributed basis of central coherence. The
sea tastes of salt and . . . ?,” “pepper.” In all right hemisphere has long been implicated in
these respects, they resembled individuals with global, integrative, and context-sensitive pro-
autism, but for these fathers their detail- cessing. Individuals with acquired right hemi-
focused cognitive style was an asset, not a sphere damage show deficits on visuo-spatial
deficit. Performance on the tests of coherence constructional tasks, maintaining details but
also related strongly to self-ratings of everyday missing global configuration (Robertson &
preferences and abilities in detail-focused areas Lamb, 1991). Discourse also becomes piece-
but not to self-ratings of social skills and inter- meal in such patients, with difficulties in inte-
ests (Briskman, Happé, & Frith, 2001). These grating verbal information and extracting gist
646 Theoretical Perspectives

(Benowitz, Moya, & Levine, 1990). Func- eralizations. Cohen (1994) has presented a
tional imaging work, too, suggests a role for computational model of autism, in which lack
right hemisphere regions in configural pro- of generalization results from an increase
cessing. Fink et al. (1997), using fMRI, found in units—an intriguing example of how com-
right lingual gyrus activation during attention putational analyses may interact with neu-
to global aspects of a hierarchical figure (e.g., roanatomical data and psychological theory to
an H made up of Ss) and left inferior occipital help solve the puzzle of autism. It is intriguing
activation during local focus. Electrophysio- to think that the cognitive style of weak coher-
logical (ERP) studies, too, suggest right hemi- ence in autism, with its attendant assets and
sphere activity during global versus local deficits, might result from an “embarrassment
tasks (Heinze, Hinrichs, Scholz, Burchert, & of riches” at the neural level.
Mangun, 1998). Since people with autism
show piecemeal processing, as well as repeti- Cross-References
tive stereotyped behavior (normally sup-
pressed by regions in the right hemisphere; Other theoretical perspectives are provided in
Brugger, Monsch, & Johnson, 1996), it is Chapters 21 to 23 and 25 to 26; developmental
tempting to look for the origins of autism in aspects of autism are addressed in Chapters 8
right hemisphere anomalies. However, there is to 10.
relatively little conclusive evidence of local-
ized and specific structural damage. At least REFERENCES
one brain imaging study to date has found
right hemisphere abnormalities in (three) in- Ameli, R., Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A., Kaufman,
dividuals with a high-functioning form of A. S., & Grillon, C. (1988). Visual memory
autism, Asperger syndrome (McKelvey, Lam- processes in high-functioning individuals with
bert, Mottron, & Shevell, 1995). However, ev- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 18, 601–615.
idence of damage in limbic, frontal, and
Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Field, K., Sparks,
cerebellar regions has also been reported, and
B. F., & Singh, N. (2002). Effects of age on
it is by no means clear which anomalies are brain volume and head circumference in
specific and universal to autism. autism. Neurology, 59, 175–183.
It is unlikely, however, that autism will Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain
prove to be the result of damage confined theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
to one brain region, and the very notion of cen- 6, 248–254.
tral coherence suggests diffuse differences in Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Parents of
brain organization. One intriguing finding, in children with Asperger syndrome: What is the
this respect, is that some individuals with cognitive phenotype? Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 548–554.
autism have larger or heavier brains than do
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C.,
comparison groups, with increased cell pack-
Bolton, P., & Goodyer, I. (1997). Is there a
ing in several areas (Aylward, Minshew, Field, link between engineering and autism? Autism:
Sparks, & Singh, 2002; Piven et al., 1995). Journal of Research and Practice, 1, 101–109.
Recent work suggests an early, transient Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in ex-
period of brain overgrowth, due perhaps to an perimental and social psychology. Cambridge,
abnormal acceleration of postnatal growth England: Cambridge University Press.
processes or failure of the usual pruning Benowitz, L. I., Moya, K. L., & Levine, D. N.
(Courchesne, Carper, & Akshoomoff, 2003). (1990). Impaired verbal reasoning and con-
This brain growth dysregulation appears to structional apraxia in subjects with right hemi-
result in reduced functional connectivity (e.g., sphere damage. Neuropsychologia, 23, 231–241.
Bhatt, R. S., Rovee-Collier, C., & Shyi, G. C. W.
Just, Cherkassky, Keller, & Minshew, in
(1994). Global and local processing of inci-
press). Processing with excess neurons may re- dental information and memory retrieval at 6
sult in a failure to process information for gist: months. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
If there is the capacity for registering and re- chology, 57, 141–162.
calling each exemplar, perhaps there would Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., & Happé, F.
be no drive to extract prototypes or make gen- (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and ex-
The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 647

ecutive dysfunction: Planning drawing in Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.
Autism and ADHD. Philosophical Transactions Oxford: Blackwell.
of the Royal Society, 358(1430), 387–392. Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond
Brian, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1996). Disembedding “ theory of mind.” Cognition, 50, 115–132.
performance and recognition memory in Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for
autism / PDD, Journal of Child Psychology and meaning and reading for sound in autistic and
Psychiatry, 37, 865–872. dyslexic children. Journal of Developmental
Briskman, J., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2001). Ex- Psychology, 1, 329–342.
ploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Gepner, B., Mestre, D., Masson, G., & de Schonen,
Weak “central coherence” in parents and sib- S. (1995). Postural effects of motion vision
lings of children with autism. II. Real-life in young autistic children. NeuroReport, 6,
skills and preferences. Journal of Child Psy- 1211–1214.
chology and Psychiatry, 42, 309–316. Gerland, G. (1997). A real person: Life on the out-
Brugger, P., Monsch, A. U., & Johnson, S. A. side (J. Tate, Trans.). London: Souvenir Press.
(1996). Repetitive behavior and repetition Happé, F. G. E. (1994). Wechsler IQ profile and
avoidance: The role of the right hemisphere. theory of mind in autism: A research note.
Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 21, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
53–56. 35, 1461–1471.
Chynn, E. W., Garrod, A., Demick, J., & DeVos, E. Happé, F. G. E. (1996). Studying weak central co-
(1991). Correlations among field dependence- herence at low levels: Children with autism do
independence, sex, sex-role stereotype, and age not succumb to visual illusions, a research
of preschoolers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
73, 747–756. try, 37, 873–877.
Cohen, I. L. (1994). An artificial neural network Happé, F. G. E. (1997). Central coherence and the-
analogue of learning in autism. Biological Psy- ory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in
chiatry, 36, 5–20. context. British Journal of Developmental Psy-
Coren, S., & Enns, J. T. (1993). Size contrast as a chology, 15, 1–12.
function of conceptual similarity between test Happé, F. G. E. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit
and inducers. Perception and Psychophysics, or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sci-
54, 579–588. ences, 3, 216–222.
Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. Happé, F. G. E. (2000). Parts and wholes, meaning
(2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the and minds: Central coherence and its relation
first year of life in autism. Journal of the to theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H.
American Medical Association, 290, 337–344. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under-
DeGelder, B., Vroomen, J., & Van der Heide, L. standing other minds: Perspectives from autism
(1991). Face recognition and lip-reading in and developmental cognitive neuroscience
autism. European Journal of Cognitive Psy- (pp. 203–221). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
chology, 3, 69–86. versity Press.
Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). Happé, F. G. E. (2001). Social and non-social de-
Inferential language in high-functioning chil- velopment in autism: Where are the links. In
dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Devel- J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R.
opmental Disorders, 31, 47–54. Zelazo (Eds.), Perspectives on development in
Eskes, G. A., Bryson, S. E., & McCormick, T. A. autism (pp. 237–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(1990). Comprehension of concrete and ab- Happé, F. G. E., Briskman, J., & Frith, U. (2001).
stract words in autistic children. Journal of Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism:
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, Weak “central coherence” in parents and sib-
61–73. lings of children with autism: I. Experimental
Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, tests. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J. atry, 42, 299–307.
(1997). Neural mechanisms involved in the Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and dis-
processing of global and local aspects of hier- embedding in autism. Journal of Child Psy-
archically organized visual stimuli. Brain, chology and Psychiatry, 44, 543–551.
120, 1779–1791. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism
Freedland, R. L., & Dannemiller, J. L. (1996). and pitch processing: A precursor for savant
Nonlinear pattern vision processes in early in- musical ability. Music Perception, 15, 291–305.
fancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, Heinze, H. J., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Burchert,
21–32. W., & Mangun, G. R. (1998). Neural mechanisms
648 Theoretical Perspectives

of global and local processing: A combined global-local perceptual bias. Neuropsychology,


PET and ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neu- 10, 402–407.
roscience, 10, 485–498. Lopez, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children
Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1967). Remembering with autism fail to process information in con-
of words by psychotic and subnormal children. text? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
British Journal of Psychology, 58, 213–218. try, 44, 285–300.
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, T. (1988, Novem- Marendaz, C. (1985). Global precedence and field
ber). What’s in a face? The case of autism. dependence: Visual routines? Cahiers de Psy-
British Journal of Psychology, 79(4), 441–453. chologie Cognitive, 5, 727–745.
Jarrold, C., Butler, D. W., Cottington, E. M., & McKelvey, J. R., Lambert, R., Mottron, L., &
Jimenez, F. (2000). Linking theory of mind Shevell, M. I. (1995). Right-hemisphere dys-
and central coherence bias in autism and in the function in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of
general population. Developmental Psychol- Child Neurology, 10, 310–314.
ogy, 36, 126–138. McKelvie, S. J. (1995). Emotional expression
Jarrold, C., & Routh, D. A. (1998). Is there really a in upside-down faces: Evidence for con-
link between engineering and autism? A reply figurational and componential processing.
to Baron-Cohen et al., Autism, 1997 1(1), British Journal of Social Psychology, 34,
101–109. Autism: Journal of Research and 325–334.
Practice, 2, 281–289. Miller, L. K. (1999). The savant syndrome: Intel-
Jarrold, C., & Russell, J. (1997). Counting abilities lectual impairment and exceptional skill. Psy-
in autism: Possible implications for central co- chological Bulletin, 125, 31–46.
herence theory. Journal of Autism and Devel- Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of per-
opmental Disorders, 27, 25–37. ceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject
Jolliffe, T. (1997). Central coherence dysfunction in with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and
autistic spectrum disorder. Unpublished doc- Cognition, 23, 279–309.
toral dissertation, University of Cambridge. Mottron, L., Belleville, S., & Ménard, A. (1999).
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by
with autism and Asperger syndrome faster graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarchization or
than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? working memory deficit? Journal of Child
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–755.
38, 527–534. Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of perceptual functioning in the development of
central coherence theory; linguistic process- autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yir-
ing in high-functioning adults with autism or miya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development
Asperger’s syndrome: Is local coherence im- of autism: Perspectives from theory and re-
paired? Cognition, 71, 149–185. search (pp. 131–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). A test of Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., &
central coherence theory: Can adults with high- Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing
functioning autism or Asperger’s syndrome in- among high-functioning persons with autism.
tegrate fragments of an object? Cognitive Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40,
Neuropsychiatry, 6, 193–216. 203–211.
Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and
Minshew, N. J. (in press). Cortical activation global processing of music in high-functioning
and synchronization during sentence compre- persons with autism: Beyond central coher-
hension in high-functioning autism: Evidence ence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
of underconnectivity. Brain. atry, 41, 1057–1065.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affec- Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The prece-
tive contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. dence of global features in visual perception.
Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.
and the global / local paradigm: A critical re- Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. M. V. (2002). Inferen-
view. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38. tial processing and story recall in children with
Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype for- communication problems: A comparison of spe-
mation in autism. Development & Psychopath- cific language impairment, pragmatic language
ology, 13, 111–124. impairment and high-functioning autism. Inter-
Kramer, J. H., Ellenberg, L., Leonard, J., & Share, national Journal of Language and Communica-
L. J. (1996). Developmental sex differences in tion Disorders, 37, 227–251.
The Weak Central Coherence Account of Autism 649

O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36,
Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search 1065–1076.
in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Robertson, L. C., & Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsy-
Human Perception and Performance, 27, chological contribution to theories of part /
719–730. whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23,
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & 299–330.
Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (1999). Are individuals
in autism and Tourette syndrome: An informa- with autism and Asperger’s syndrome suscep-
tion processing approach. Journal of Child tible to visual illusions? Journal of Child Psy-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1032. chology and Psychiatry, 40, 1283–1293.
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., Havercamp, S., Scheuffgen, K. (1998). Domain-general and domain-
Andreasen, N. C., & Palmer, P. (1995). An specific deficits in autism and dyslexia. Un-
MRI study of brain size in autism. American published doctoral dissertation, University of
Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1145–1149. London.
Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in
autism: An alternative to weak central coher- autistic children: A research note. Journal of
ence. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yir- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.
miya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic indi-
of autism: Perspectives from theory and re- viduals show superior performance on the
search (pp. 149–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Block Design task? Journal of Child Psychol-
Plaisted, K. C., O’Riordan, M. A. F., Aitken, ogy and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.
M. R. F., & Killcross, A. S. (submitted). Cater- Snowling, M., & Frith, U. (1986). Comprehension in
gorisation in autism: Evidence for a reduced “ hyperlexic” readers. Journal of Experimental
prototype ef fect. Child Psychology, 42, 392–415.
Plaisted, K. C., O’Riordan, M. A. F., & Baron- Stoecker, J. J., Colombo, J., Frick, J. E., & Allen,
Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a J. R. (1998). Long- and short-looking infants’
conjunctive target in autism: A research note. recognition of symmetrical and asymmetrical
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, forms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
39, 777–783. ogy, 71, 63–78.
Plaisted, K. C., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in
Children with autism show local precedence in the free recall of autistic children: Further evi-
a divided attention task and global precedence dence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal of
in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742. Takeuchi, A. H., & Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute
Pring, L., & Hermelin, B. (1993). Bottle, tulip and pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 345–361.
wineglass: Semantic and structural picture Teunisse, J.-P., Cools, A. R., van Spaendonck,
processing by savant artists. Journal of Child K. P. M., Aerts, F. H., & Berger, H. J. (2001).
Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1365–1385. Cognitive styles in high-functioning adoles-
Pring, L., Hermelin, B., & Heavey, L. (1995). cents with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism
Savants, segments, art and autism. Journal and Developmental Disorders, 31, 55–66.
CHAPTER 25

Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental


Models of Autism

PETER MUNDY AND COURTNEY BURNETTE

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder char- Wetherby, Chapter 36). Nevertheless, rela-


acterized by the early onset of a robust distur- tively little attention has focused on the emer-
bance of social and communicative development gent theoretical and empirical interface
(Bailey, Philips, & Rutter, 1996; Kanner, between research on joint attention distur-
1943/1973; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz, & bance and neurodevelopment in autism. The
Klin, 2004). Because the nature of this disor- aim of this chapter is to provide a discussion of
der is very complex, it will likely require sig- this vital topic.
nificant broadening of the current boundaries Most broadly, joint attention refers to the
of behavioral neuroscience before sufficient capacity of young children to coordinate their
knowledge is available to ameliorate the im- visual attention with a social partner. This ca-
pairments of individuals with autism. This ex- pacity unfolds between 6 and 18 months in
pansion is well underway and is exciting to typical development and is exemplified by the
witness. From animal models to intervention ability to follow the line of visual regard of a
studies, from metabolic genetics and neu- social partner or to initiate episodes of shared
roimaging to the identification of early behav- attention with eye contact and gestures such as
ioral manifestations, the syndrome of autism is showing (see Figure 25.1). Children with
being examined from multiple perspectives to autism display a robust disturbance in these
piece together a veridical picture of the true and related social-orienting skills. This distur-
nature of this disorder. bance is problematic because joint attention
One important piece of the picture has been skills provide a fulcrum around which much of
revealed over the past 20 years by studies on social learning and self-organization revolves
the nature of early social-communication im- in the first years of life (Baldwin, 1995). It
pairments in children with autism. In this may be especially important to recognize that
chapter, we discuss a fundamental facet of this one of the more pernicious aspects of joint at-
work that has revolved around research on tention impairment is the early onset of a dra-
joint attention impairment in children with matic reduction in the tendency of children
autism. Many aspects of this research have with autism to initiate episodes of social shar-
been reviewed elsewhere (Charman, 1998; ing with other people (Mundy, 1995). A reduc-
Leekam & Moore, 2001; Mundy & Crowson, tion in the tendency of young children with
1997), including several chapters in this Hand- autism to initiate critical social behaviors may
book (e.g., Chawarska, Chapter 8; Prizant & be singularly important because developmen-

The preparation of this paper was supported, in part, by NICHD R01 HD38052 and a Florida State Department
of Education grant to the University of Miami Center for Autism and Related Disabilities (P. Mundy, PI ).

650
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 651

Figure 25.1 Examples of (a) responding to joint attention bids, ( b) initiating joint attention with a point, and
(c) initiating joint attention with alternating gaze. Source: “A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early So-
cial Communication Scales (ESCS)”, by P. Mundy, A. Hogan, and P. Doehring, 1996, available from
http://yin.psy.miami.edu:80/Child/ Pmundy/manual.html; and “Assessing Interactional Competencies: The Early
Social Communication Scales,” by J. M. Seibert, A. E. Hogan, and P. C. Mundy, 1982, Infant Mental Health Jour-
nal, 3, pp. 244–245.

tal theory suggests that a large part of early bance may be viewed as associated with a ro-
ontology hinges on experience, including the bust disturbance in the early tendency of
experiences children create for themselves young children with autism to initiate social-
through their own actions (Cicchetti & orienting and sharing with others (Dawson,
Tucker, 1994; Gottlieb & Halpern, 2002; Pi- Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998;
aget, 1952). More recently, theory also has Mundy, 1995). This behavioral disturbance
begun to suggest that not only do infants play reduces the flow of social information to the
a role in creating critical experiences for child to such an extent that it contributes to
themselves, but also a failure to create these subsequent disorganization in the neural, as
self-generated social experiences may con- well as behavioral, development of these chil-
tribute to suboptimal neurodevelopmental dren (Dawson, Webb, et al., 2002; Klin, War-
outcomes (cf. Greenough, Black, & Wallace, ren, Schultz, & Volkmar, 2003; Mundy &
1987). We have attempted to incorporate Crowson, 1997; Mundy & Neal, 2001). We re-
some of these important ideas into our own view elements of this coactive model of the
efforts to understand the significance of joint neurodevelopmental disturbance of autism
attention disturbance in autism. The result is later in this chapter.
a coactive model of development (Gottlieb & The observation that autism is character-
Halpern, 2002) that suggests there may be a ized by a deficit in the initiation of joint atten-
complex interplay between early behavior dis- tion with others may also be especially
turbance (i.e., symptoms of autism) and sub- important as we attempt to understand the
sequent neurodevelopmental pathology in brain systems that play a role in this syndrome.
autism. In particular, joint attention distur- Currently, much of the brain-behavior research
652 Theoretical Perspectives

and theory on the social impairments of tems for the self-initiation of social behaviors
autism emphasizes the study of the perception and cognition and how these dorsal systems
of social behavior rather than systems involved relate to ventral social-perceptual systems
in the initiation of social behavior (Baron- constitutes a goal of the highest order in the
Cohen et al., 2000; Carver & Dawson, in press; current field of research on autism (cf. Frith
Critchley et al., 2000). This emphasis is not & Frith, 1999, 2001). Indeed, it is important
necessarily misplaced because individuals to recognize that we do not yet clearly under-
with autism display deficits in social percep- stand the degree to which the initiation of
tion (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Hobson, social behaviors serves to organize social per-
1993; Langdell, 1978; Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, ception (or vice versa). Detailed knowledge
& Yirmiya, 1992). Moreover, the interpreta- of this topic may be critical to understanding
tion of research on the neurodevelopment of the atypical neurodevelopment of autism.
social perception in autism is supported by a In this chapter, we take a small step in this
rich corpus of data on the brain systems that direction by reviewing research that links joint
are involved in the perception of social behav- attention development and its disturbance in
iors in primates and humans (Adolphs, 2001; autism to the DMFC system. We also provide a
Brothers, 1990; Elgar & Cambell, 2001; discussion of the potential links between re-
LeDoux, 1989). However, as noted earlier, search on the DMFC and brain systems in-
autism is marked not only by social-perceptual volved in social perception. Finally, we
or social-information processing difficulties attempt to link the coactive model of the neu-
but also by impairments in the spontaneous rodevelopmental disturbance of autism and re-
generation and expression of social behaviors search on the DMFC. To provide a foundation
and cognition (U. Frith, 1989; Klin et al., for these discussions, we begin with a brief
2003; Leslie, 1987; Minshew et al., 2002; overview of joint attention disturbance in
Mundy, 1995). Therefore, in addition to re- autism.
search on the neural systems involved in social
perception, neurodevelopmental studies of the JOINT ATTENTION AND SOCIAL
systems involved in the self-initiation of social IMPAIRMENT IN AUTISM
behavior may be of great importance for re-
search on autism. As is well known, Kanner (1943/1973) first
It may be instructive to recognize that the noted that the pathognomonic feature of autism
brain systems involved in initiating social be- was the “children’s inability to relate them-
havior may not be identical to those involved selves in the ordinary way to people and situa-
in the perception of social behavior. For exam- tions” because “ these children have come into
ple, several papers have emphasized the the world with an innate inability to form the
importance of ventral “social brain” brain usual biologically provided affective contact
systems in perception of social behaviors and with people, just as other children come into
the social pathology of autism. These brain the world with innate physical or intellectual
systems include the orbitofrontal cortex, tem- handicaps” (Kanner, 1943/1973, pp. 42–43). It
poral cortical areas including the superior is less well known that in the three decades
temporal sulcus (STS) and superior temporal following Kanner’s and Asperger’s (1944)
gyrus (STG), and subcortical areas such as the identification of the syndrome, very little em-
amygdala (Adolphs, 2001; Bachvalier, 1994; pirical or theoretical work was devoted to
Baron-Cohen et al., 2000; Brothers, 1990). In defining the nature of the fundamental social
contrast, when the tendency to initiate social impairments that afflict these children
behaviors, such as joint attention bids or (Howlin, 1978). One result of this paucity of
social-cognitive problem solving is studied, re- information was a relatively impoverished di-
search suggests that a more dorsal, medial- agnostic system. The statement that children
frontal cortical (DMFC) system may be with autism display “a pervasive lack of re-
involved in autism (U. Frith & Frith, 1999, sponsiveness to others” (e.g., American Psy-
2001; Mundy, 2003). Thus, understanding the chiatric Association, 1980) was the only
functional neurodevelopment of dorsal sys- descriptor of the social deficits associated
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 653

with autism until the late 1980s (e.g., Ameri- tures for the sharing experiences with others
can Psychiatric Association, 1987). This de- (Kasari, Sigman, Mundy, & Yirmiya, 1990;
scriptor painted a broad but inaccurate picture Mundy et al., 1986). In previous work, we ar-
of the social behavior of these children. It de- gued that joint attention disturbance in autism
scribed only the most aloof subgroup of chil- was central to what Kanner described as the
dren with autism and contributed substantially “children’s inability to relate themselves in
to an underestimation of the prevalence of the ordinary way to people and situations”
autism (see Wing & Potter, 2002, for a related (Mundy & Sigman, 1989).
discussion). Indeed, it was only with the publi- The capacity for joint attention begins to
cation of the most recent nosology (e.g., Amer- emerge by 6 months of age (Scaife & Bruner,
ican Psychiatric Association, 1994) that there 1975) and takes several different forms, each
are sufficiently well-articulated diagnostic of which may be reliably measured in infants
criteria to begin to provide a clear and compre- and young children. One behavior involves in-
hensive description of the social impairments fants’ ability to follow the direction of gaze,
of autism. head turn, and/or pointing gesture of another
The observation that early social-communi- person (Scaife & Bruner, 1975). This behavior
cation disturbance in autism is exemplified by is called responding to joint attention skill
a robust failure to adequately develop joint at- (RJA; Mundy et al., 2003; Seibert, Hogan, &
tention skills (Curcio, 1978; Loveland & Mundy, 1982). Another type of skill involves
Landry, 1986; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & infants’ use of eye contact and/or deictic ges-
Sherman, 1986; Wetherby & Prutting, 1984) tures (e.g., pointing or showing) to sponta-
has contributed to the improved description of neously initiate coordinated attention with a
the social deficits of autism (Mundy & Crow- social partner. This type of protodeclarative
son, 1997; Ozonoff & South, 2001). As noted act (Bates, 1976) is referred to as initiating
previously, the term joint attention skills refers joint attention (IJA; Mundy et al., 2003; Seib-
to the capacity of individuals to coordinate or ert et al., 1982). These behaviors, especially
share attention with a social partner regarding IJA, appear to serve social functions as the
an object or event. This capacity in infancy goal, and reinforcement for these behaviors
may involve only the social coordination of seems to relate simply to the value of sharing
overt aspects of visual attention, as when a experience with others (Bates, 1976; Mundy,
toddler shows a toy to a parent (Carpenter, 1995). Social attention coordination may also
Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998; Rheingold, Hay, & be used for imperative purposes (Bates,
West, 1976). However, with development, joint 1976). Infants and young children may use eye
attention skills in older children and adults contact and gestures to initiate attention coor-
also play a role in the social coordination of dination with another person to elicit aid in
covert aspects of attention, as when social obtaining an object or event. This may be re-
partners coordinate attention vis-à-vis psycho- ferred to as a proto-imperative act (Bates,
logical phenomena, such as ideas, intentions, 1976) or initiating behavior requests (IBR;
or emotions (Bretherton, McNew, & Beeghly- Mundy et al., 2003). This type of attention co-
Smith, 1981; Tomasello, 1999). Thus, the reg- ordination serves a less social function, inso-
ulation and sharing of overt visual attention in far as it is employed as part of an instrumental
early development is thought to contribute (in goal of obtaining a desired object or event
a manner we do not yet fully understand) to (Bates, 1976; Mundy, 1995).
the subsequent development of the capacity Joint attention skill acquisition is a major
to socially share aspects of cognition later in milestone of early development (Bakeman &
development. Adamson, 1984), in part, because these skills
Joint attention skill deficits in children assist infants in organizing social information
with autism involve a robust and early-onset to facilitate their own learning and develop-
disturbance in the tendency to share or coor- ment. In language learning, for example, par-
dinate overt visual attention with a social ents do not sit with their infants in structured
partner. It is manifest in an attenuation of the situations to teach vocabulary word by word.
functional use of eye contact, affect, and ges- Rather, much of early language acquisition
654 Theoretical Perspectives

takes place in unstructured or incidental social- strumental purposes (see Charman, 1998;
learning situations where: (1) the parent pro- Leekam & Moore, 2001; Mundy & Crowson,
vides a learning opportunity by referring to a 1997, for reviews).
new object or event in the environment, but (2) The self-organizing function of joint atten-
the infant may need to discriminate among a tion in autism may be illustrated with findings
number of stimuli in the environment in order from a recent important study by Bono and
to focus on the correct object /event and ac- Sigman (in press). In this study, 29 children
quire the new word-object-event association. with autism were followed longitudinally be-
Thus, the infant is confronted with the possi- tween approximately 4 and 5 years of age.
bility of committing a referential mapping Data on the amount of time per week children
error or focusing on the wrong stimuli during were in structured interventions were col-
incidental word learning opportunities (Bald- lected, as were data on joint attention abilities
win, 1995). To resolve this problem, the infant using the Early Social-Communication Scales
may attend to and process the direction of gaze (ESCS; Mundy et al., 2003) and data from
of the parent (i.e., use RJA skill) to limit the standardized language assessments. The re-
number of potential stimuli they need to at- sults revealed that across this 1-year period,
tend to, thereby increasing the likelihood of a both IJA and RJA were significantly related to
correct word learning experience (Baldwin, language gains. Alternatively, amount of inter-
1995). Similarly, when the infant initiates a vention was only weakly related to language
bid for joint attention, the responsive care- gains across the 1-year interval. However, sig-
giver may follow the child’s line of regard and nificant conditional intervention effects were
take advantage of the child’s focus of atten- observed such that more time in structured in-
tion to provide a new word in a context that tervention was associated with significant lan-
maximizes the learning opportunity (cf. guage gains for children with better-developed
Tomasello, 1995). Joint attention skills assist RJA skills. Thus, measures of joint attention
infants in organizing social information input may be a marker of individual differences in
and avoiding referential mapping errors in intervention responsivity among children with
these situations (Baldwin, 1995). Hence, joint autism. One possible interpretation of this
attention may be regarded as an early develop- finding is that differences in RJA skills re-
ing self-organizing facility that is critical to flected differences in the ability of children
much of subsequent social and cognitive de- with autism to self-organize information in so-
velopment (e.g., Baldwin, 1995; Bruner, 1975; cial learning situations and that this skill con-
Mundy & Neal, 2001). tributes to their capacity to benefit from early
Children with autism, unfortunately, dis- intervention.
play robust levels of impairments in the ten- In addition to reflecting a self-organizing
dency to initiate and respond to joint attention disturbance, joint attention deficits in autism
bids. This impairment contributes to a signifi- may reflect impairments in the social-cognitive
cant deficit in the capacity for early social capacity to represent another person’s per-
learning. Observations suggest that joint atten- spectives (Leslie & Happé, 1989), as well as a
tion disturbance may be manifest in children disturbance in the social motivation to ap-
with autism as early as between 12 and 18 proach or orient to social partners (Mundy,
months of age (Osterling & Dawson, 1994; 1995). Joint attention deficits in children with
Swettenham et al., 1998). Theoretically, from autism, however, should not be confused with
early in development, children with autism processes associated with attachment because
display deficits in types of social behaviors children with autism display atypical, but
that ordinarily serve to organize and facilitate clear, signs of attachment (Sigman & Mundy,
subsequent social and communicative develop- 1989; see also Pierce, Frank, Farshad, &
ment. It is interesting, though, that this deficit Courchesne, 2001). Moreover, attachment does
in early social-communication skill is not per- not appear to be strongly related to joint atten-
vasive as children with autism display only tion skills in children with autism or typical
modest evidence of IBR impairments on mea- development (Capps, Sigman, & Mundy, 1994;
sures of social attention coordination for in- Crowson, Mundy, Neal, & Meyer, 2003).
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 655

Although young children with autism dis- social impairment of autism in a current nosol-
play deficits in both IJA and RJA skills, the ogy (American Psychiatric Association, 2000,
impairment in RJA appears to remit to a signif- p. 75). Thus, many of the current autism diag-
icant degree with development (Leekam & nostic and screening instruments include mea-
Moore, 2001; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, sures of joint attention (Baron-Cohen et al.,
1994). The impairment in IJA, however, re- 1996; Charman, 1998; Lord et al., 1999;
mains in older children (Baron-Cohen, 1995). Stone, Coonrod, & Ousley, 2000). The gold
Research also suggests that symptom intensity standard Autism Diagnostic Observation
(Mundy et al., 1994) and symptom course, Schedule (Lord et al., 1999) even reflects the
such as the tendency to initiate interaction notion of a developmental dissociation in joint
with peers in later childhood and adolescence attention. Measures used for diagnosis with
(Lord, Floody, Anderson, & Pickles, 2003; the youngest children (Module 1) include both
Sigman & Ruskin, 1999), are related to indi- IJA and RJA assessments, while Module 2 de-
vidual differences in IJA, but not RJA impair- signed for developmentally more advanced
ment among young children with autism. A children includes only the IJA measures in the
dissociated pattern of IJA and RJA development diagnostic scores.
is also observed in typical development and
may occur because IJA and RJA reflect differ- JOINT ATTENTION, SOCIAL
ent integrations of neurodevelopmental, social- ORIENTING, AND AUTISM
cognitive, and social-emotional processes
(Mundy, Card, & Fox, 2000; Mundy & Will- Given its central role in the phenotype of
oughby, 1998). autism, it is not surprising that considerable
IJA reflects the tendency to spontaneously effort over the past 20 years has been di-
initiate social attention coordination behavior, rected toward understanding the development
whereas RJA is a measure of the tendency to of joint attention. Most models of joint atten-
respond to another person’s signal to shift at- tion disturbance, indeed most models of
tention. Hence, IJA may be more affected by autism, approach the social symptoms of the
executive and social-motivation processes in- syndrome from a relatively linear and deter-
volved in the generation and self-initiation of ministic perspective. These models view be-
behavioral goals than RJA (Mundy, 1995; havioral symptoms of the syndrome, such as
Mundy & Willoughby, 1998; Mundy et al., joint attention disturbance, as the end point of
2000). In particular, IJA appears to involve the a unidirectional process. This process starts
tendency to spontaneously initiate episodes of with some form of genotypic atypicality that
sharing the affective experience of an object leads directly to neurodevelopmental anom-
or event with a social partner (Mundy, Kasari, alies, which, in turn, are unerringly expressed
& Sigman, 1992). Indeed, a significant compo- as abnormal social behavior (Minshew, John-
nent of IJA disturbance in autism may be ex- son, & Luna, 2001). For example, social behav-
plained in terms of an attenuation of the ior disturbance in autism has been viewed as an
tendency to initiate episodes of shared positive end-point outcome of “core” neurodevelopmen-
affect with a social partner (Kasari et al., tal deficits in a social-cognitive module
1990). However, a recent report has failed to (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Leslie & Happé, 1989),
replicate this finding, suggesting the need for executive functions (McEvoy, Rogers, & Pen-
more research on this important topic (Plou- nington, 1993), or cerebellar contributions to
sia, 2002). attention control (Courchesne et al., 1994).
This literature has led to the instantiation However, an understanding of atypical, as well
of joint attention disturbance, and especially as typical development, may benefit from a
IJA disturbance, as a cardinal symptom of less linear and deterministic perspective (Cic-
autism. For example, a “lack of spontaneous chetti & Tucker, 1994). “Epigenetic,” “rela-
seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or tional,” or “coactive” models of causality
achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack suggest that biological and environmental ex-
of showing, bringing or pointing out objects of perience interact over time and maturation to
interest)” is now one of four symptoms of the yield developmental disturbance (Gottlieb &
656 Theoretical Perspectives

Halpern, 2002). Moreover, “experience” as a ment to relevant aspects of the environment


causal influence on development includes not (Bahrick & Lickliter, 1999; Karmiloff-Smith,
only external sources of stimulation but also 1995). These predispositions provide a “pre-
forms of stimulation that the individual ac- paredness with which human infants come to
tively generates through self-initiated interac- the task of learning” (Tomasello, 1999, p. 305)
tions with the world (Piaget, 1952; Scarr, and a starting point around which subsequent
1992). Thus, rather than end points in patho- brain and behavior development organizes. In
logical process, it may be especially important particular, infants may display a predisposi-
to understand how the early onset of impair- tion toward social information processing
ments in major milestones of social develop- (Blass, 1999; Cummins & Cummins, 1999). A
ment potentially contributes to the subsequent disturbance of such a bias in autism may result
unfolding of the full syndrome of autism from imbalances in general aspects of early
across the first years of life. It may be espe- perception and information processing (Mot-
cially instructive to consider the potential de- tron & Burack, 2001) or aspects of perception
velopmental impact of an early disturbance of that are specific to social information process-
the self-organizing functions of joint attention. ing (e.g., Adolphs, 2001). In any event, a criti-
To understand this developmental impact, cal assumption of our social-orienting model
consider the notion that joint attention distur- has been that joint attention skill deficits in
bance may be part of a broader social-orienting children with autism reflect a disturbance in
impairment in autism. The term social-orienting the predilection to spontaneously orient to and
impairment has been introduced to the field by process social information that is normally
the seminal work of Dawson et al. (1998), who manifest in the first years of life (Mundy,
observed that children with autism may dis- 1995; Mundy & Sigman, 1989).
play a more robust orienting deficit to social Results of several studies suggest social-
rather than nonsocial sounds. However, the no- orienting and joint attention skills are related
tion that children with autism display a deficit and that impairments in these domains may be
in orienting to social stimuli has a long history manifest very early in children with autism.
in research on autism. For example, it can be For example, 20-month-old infants who were
discerned in various forms in models of subsequently diagnosed with autism at 42
autism that emphasize impairments in the months have been observed to display far less
first year of life in cerebellar processes social orienting, or spontaneous gaze shifts
(Courchesne et al., 1994) or in the biological between objects and people, than did control
reward mechanisms that serve to promote so- infants (Swettenham et al., 1998). Measuring
cial behavior (Mundy, 1995; Panksepp, 1979). spontaneously alternating gaze between an ob-
The latter impairments may be related to a dis- ject and a person is also a core component of
turbance in the early onset of orbital and/or the assessment of IJA skill. In fact, it was the
more dorsal medial-frontal contributions to type of behavior that best discriminated chil-
orienting and learning (Dawson, Munson, dren with autism from comparison children in
et al., 2002; Mundy, 2003; Mundy et al., our original joint attention study (Mundy
2000), as well as problems in the perception or et al., 1986).
processing of affect and behavioral contingen- Other research also speaks to the commonal-
cies (Dawson & Lewy, 1989; Hobson, 1993). ity and very early onset of social-orienting and
All these models embrace the supposition that joint attention disturbance in autism. The liter-
a social-orienting impairment may reflect an ature on normal development indicates that
initial or core aspect of pathology that has forms of social-orienting and joint attention
ramifications for the subsequent development skill development emerge between 6 and 12
of social, cognitive, and even neurological dis- months of age (Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998;
turbance in autism. see Moore & Dunham, 1995, for review). In re-
Our own version of social-orienting impair- search on autism, studies of family videotape
ment in autism is based in part on the assump- records suggest that by 12 months of age, chil-
tion that in the first year of life, there are dren with autism may display evidence of a
predispositions that guide attention deploy- disturbance in joint attention and social ori-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 657

enting (Osterling & Dawson, 1994). Measures outcomes of adolescent children with autism
of joint attention skills have also contributed to (Sigman & Ruskin, 1999), as well as how
the very early identification of autism at 18 well children with autism process nonverbal
months of age in a sample of 16,000 children social-affective information (Dissanayake,
(Baron-Cohen et al., 1996). Even earlier social- Sigman, & Kasari, 1996).
orienting measures such as parent reports of eye
contact, showing interest in others, reacting NEURAL PLASTICITY, SOCIAL
when spoken to, and laughing to others may ORIENTING, AND JOINT ATTENTION
serve to facilitate early identification as early
as 14 months (Willensen-Swinkel et al., 2002). How do joint attention and related early social-
Several other studies provide further evi- orienting impairments play a role in the etiol-
dence for a basic social-orienting disturbance ogy of autistic developmental pathology? The
in autism. Klin (1991) has reported that the microgenetic theory of pathology suggests that
typical preference for speech and speech-like understanding the developmental nature and
sounds, usually displayed by infants in the first timing of symptoms may be of critical impor-
months of life, was not present in any of the tance if the complete basis of pathology is to be
children with autism observed by him. It was, understood (Brown, 1994). This may be the
however, present in all of the developmentally case with respect to the developmental nature
delayed matched controls observed in this and timing of joint attention and social-orient-
study. As noted earlier, Dawson et al. (1998) ing disturbance in autism. Thus, another criti-
examined the degree to which children with cal feature of our model is based on theory that
autism, Down syndrome, or normal develop- suggests early experience drives a substantial
ment oriented (displayed a head turn) toward portion of postnatal brain development.
social stimuli (clapping hands or calling the Several researchers have suggested that
child’s name) and to nonsocial stimuli (playing since the normal environment reliably pro-
a musical jack-in-the-box or shaking a rattle). vides species members with certain invariable
The results indicated that the children with types of stimulation and experience, many
autism displayed deficits in orienting to both mammalian species have evolved neural mech-
types of stimuli. Their failure to orient to so- anisms that take advantage of the consistency
cial stimuli, however, was significantly more of experience to shape and organize neural
impaired than their orienting to nonsocial development (e.g., Brown, 1994; Changeux &
stimuli. Furthermore, individual differences Danchin, 1976; Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Got-
in difficulty with social orienting, but not tlieb & Halpern, 2002; Greenough et al.,
object-orienting, were significantly related to 1987). One research group has described this
a measure of joint attention among the chil- process as experience-expectant neural devel-
dren with autism. Additional efforts from this opment (Greenough et al., 1987). Experience-
group have also shown that joint attention is expectant process in neural development
directly related to language acquisition, and involves a “readiness of the brain” to receive
social-orienting measures are indirectly re- specific types of information from the envi-
lated to language through their association ronment (Black, Jones, Nelson, & Greenough,
with joint attention development in 3- to 4- 1998). This assumption parallels the notion in
year-old children with autism (Dawson et al., developmental research and theory that there
2004). Research also suggests that a social- are predispositions that guide attention and
orienting factor may reflect a symptom clus- learning early in infancy (Bahrick & Lickliter,
ter assessed with the Childhood Autism 1999; Karmiloff-Smith, 1995; Tomasello,
Rating Scale (CARS; Stella, Mundy, & Tuch- 1999). One aspect of this readiness is an initial
man, 1999). Processes involved in individual overproduction of potential neural connections
differences in joint attention measures of so- in the brain. Research on cortical development
cial orienting have displayed long-term conti- suggests that the number of synaptic connec-
nuity with processes involved in adaptive tions between neurons increases dramatically
outcomes. Individual differences in early for several years postnatally, especially in the
joint attention predict the social and cognitive first 12 to 24 months of life. Subsequently,
658 Theoretical Perspectives

average brain volume, as measured in terms processes in neurological development cur-


of synaptic density, gradually decreases (see rently stems almost exclusively from research
Huttenlocher, 1994, for review). This decrease on sensory development, it is likely that other
in brain volume involves the process of culling aspects of human neurobehavioral development
the early proliferation of synaptic neural con- are also affected by experience-expectant
nections through the effects of experience into processes. In this aspect of their discussion,
a more efficient and functional system of con- Greenough et al. make two comments that are
nections (Brown, 1994; Changeux & Danchin, potentially relevant to understanding the im-
1976; Gottlieb & Halpern, 2002; Greenough pact of a joint attention /social-orienting im-
et al., 1987; Huttenlocher, 1994). Distinctive pairment in autism. They suggested that some
aspects of environmental stimulation encoun- early experience-expectant effects may in-
tered by each member of a species (e.g., pat- volve self-organizing processes whereby “some
terned visual stimulation, speech sounds, types of ‘expected’ experience may rely
social-affective exchanges) may promote largely on the infant to produce them”
species-specific functional neurodevelopment (p. 545). They also suggested that infant pre-
during early sensitive periods of development verbal social communication interactions may
(Black et al., 1998). Many of these sensitive provide an example of the “active participation
periods are thought to occur in the first few of the infant in acquiring and organizing expe-
years of life (Greenough et al., 1987). Typi- rience” that provides necessary and critical
cally, activated or functional synapses are re- experience-expectant information in early
tained, while those that are not activated by stages of human development (Greenough
stimulation degenerate. Consequently, varia- et al., 1987, p. 553; see Gottlieb & Halpern,
tion in the environment and stimulus input 2002; McWhinney, 1998, for related discus-
during an early sensitive period of neural plas- sions). Infants’ tendency for early social ori-
ticity may lead to fundamental effects on enting and to ultimately engage in numerous
physiological, morphological, and functional episodes of social attention coordination, or
aspects of central nervous system development joint attention, may make a contribution that is
that lay a foundation for future typical or atyp- critical to experience-expectant processes that
ical neurobehavioral development (Black et al., serve to organize social neurodevelopment
1998; Greenough et al., 1987). (Mundy & Neal, 2001). Moreover, in children
If there is a robust failure of early infor- with autism, a disruption of social orienting
mation input into developing neural subsys- and joint attention may lead to an impoverish-
tems, then a decrease in synapse elimination ment of critical forms of social information
may occur, leaving a persistent and abnormal input that exacerbates atypical social neurode-
organization of neural structure (Greenough velopment through an attenuation of the typi-
et al., 1987). For example, Huttenlocher cal experience-expectant process (Mundy &
(1994, pp. 139–141) reviewed studies that Crowson, 1997; Mundy & Neal, 2001; see Fig-
suggested that the early blockade of neuro- ure 25.2).
muscular activity in animals, through curare In our initial discussion of this possibility
administration or forelimb restraint, leads to (Mundy & Sigman, 1989), we noted that a fail-
significantly more (albeit less usefully orga- ure of joint attention development may serve to
nized) synaptic connections in the motor cor- isolate the infant with autism from the typical
tex. Thus, early in development, a significant pattern of social exchange, contributing to
impairment in the input to and/or output from something akin to a primate isolation effect
brain systems may result in a stable, diffuse, (Kraemer, 1985). Primate isolation syndrome
and overabundant pattern of connections that is a behavioral response to attenuated early so-
renders the system functionally atypical. Con- cial interactive opportunities that leads to
sequently, the development of behaviors based some symptoms that have been observed in
on this diffuse and overabundant system may children with autism, such as stereotypies. We
be substantially less than optimal. subsequently revised this notion to suggest that
Greenough et al. (1987) also noted autism may be characterized by an initial neu-
that, while evidence of experience-expectant ropathological process (INP), which leads to
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 659

Neurobehavioral
disorganization

Social
Initial
orient/joint Less
neurological
attention social input
deficit
deficit

Less Greater
neural culling brain volume

Figure 25.2 A coactive model of organism-environment interaction in the neurobehavioral development of


autism in the first 6 years of life. Adapted from “Joint Attention and Early Communication: Implications for In-
tervention with Autism,” by P. Mundy and M. Crowson, 1997, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
653–675; and “Neural Plasticity, Joint Attention and a Transactional Social-Orienting Model of Autism,” by P.
Mundy and R. Neal, 2001, International Review of Mental Retardation, 23, pp. 139–168.

less than optimal social-orienting behavior in 1993). For example, during showing, infants
the first months of life (Mundy & Crowson, have the opportunity to monitor their own ex-
1997). The INP may involve a deficit in neural perience of an object (e.g., enjoyment), while
systems that contribute to social reward sensi- also observing the response of a social partner
tivity (Mundy, 1995; Panksepp, 1979) or other (e.g., their direction of gaze and affect) to
processes that may affect social orienting both the object and their own behavior. Thus,
(e.g., Courchesne et al., 1994; Dawson & self-initiated bids for joint attention provide a
Lewy, 1989). Indeed, the model is not about the rich opportunity for infants to compare infor-
cause of the INP. Rather, it raises the hypothe- mation about a social partner’s awareness and
ses that the social behavior symptoms caused responses to the displayed object with their
by the INP may themselves contribute to the own (Bates, 1976). This kind of self-other
etiology of the subsequent neurodevelopmental comparison of experience with respect to a
pathology of autism. Specifically, this model third object or referent provides information
illustrated the possibility that a robust attenu- that makes a vital contribution to the develop-
ation of social orienting in the first months of ment of the capacity of infants to simulate the
life could, in turn, contribute to secondary mental states of others. Simulation theory sug-
neurological disturbance (SND) in autism gests that individuals use their awareness of
(Mundy & Crowson, 1997). their own current or past mental processes to
One assumption of this model is that so- analyze and estimate the intentions of others
cial-orienting and joint attention behaviors cre- (Gallese & Goldman, 1998; Stich & Nichols,
ate a vital and unique source of social 1992). That is, developmentally, people learn
information that is necessary for typical to use self-knowledge, derived from context-
experience-expectant social neurodevelop- specific self-monitoring, to extrapolate and
ment. In particular, episodes of joint attention, make inferences about the covert psychologi-
especially those initiated by the child, are cal processes that contribute to the behaviors
thought to provide unique information for in- of other people in related contexts. In infancy,
fants. This involves the integration of joint attention, as well as other behaviors such
proprioceptive information on the actions and as imitation, provides fundamental opportuni-
intentions of the self with exteroceptive infor- ties to practice and develop the ability to sim-
mation from observations of the actions and ulate the mental states of others (Meltzoff &
intentions of others, in reference to some third Gopnik, 1993; Mundy et al., 1993). Thus, an
object or event (Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, attenuation of joint attention deprives children
660 Theoretical Perspectives

with autism of the practice of self and other dedicated systems necessary for the typical
social information processing that may be crit- development and function of social-cognitive
ical to the stimulation of neural systems in- modules (Mundy, 1995). This presents a com-
volved in social-cognitive development plementary but different view than typical
(Mundy, 1995, 2003; Mundy & Neal, 2001; ToM models, which emphasize critical errors
Mundy et al., 1993). within the specific functional parameters of
Another assumption inherent to this model one or another module but do not consider pos-
is that the contribution of the SND to the de- sible errors in the developmental processes
velopmental processes involved in autism will that may give rise to the modules themselves.
be reduced to the degree early intervention in-
creases the tendency of the young child with THE SOCIAL-ORIENTING MODEL
autism to process social information (see AND BRAIN VOLUME IN AUTISM
Mundy & Crowson, 1997, for discussion). In-
deed, a model of autism that incorporates a We have briefly reviewed theory and evidence
disturbance of experience-expectant processes on early neural plasticity that suggests that
may assist in understanding intervention pro- proliferation of potential synaptic connections
cess and may assist us in understanding the between neurons leads to an increase in brain
common observation that earlier intervention volume in the first 12 to 24 months followed
may work better than later intervention and by a gradual decrease in brain volume in part
that more intervention may work better than due to experience-expectant processes of
less (Black et al., 1998). culling understimulated or underutilized con-
The coactive component of the social- nections (Greenough et al., 1987; Hutten-
orienting model is not so much an alternative locher, 1994). We have also suggested that an
to other models of autism as it is a comple- attenuation of social information processing
ment or extension of extant models. For exam- and experience-expectant processes early in
ple, it is possible that a social-orienting the life of children with autism may con-
disturbance, and subsequent disruption of tribute to a disruption of this typical pattern
experience-expectant neural development, is of neural plasticity and development (Mundy
an important part of the disturbance of the de- & Neal, 2001).
velopment of social-cognitive modules envi- One of the more interesting and consistent
sioned in the theory of mind (ToM) model of findings in neuroanatomical research is that
autism (Mundy, 1995). A basic notion of the many individuals with autism display larger
latter is that there has been an evolution to- than average brain volumes (Hardan, Minshew,
ward increasing specialization of central Mallikarjuhn, & Keshavan, 2001; Piven et al.,
nervous systems to support complex social- 1995), at least across the first 6 years of devel-
cognitive and social-communication functions opment (Aylward, Minshew, Field, Sparks, &
(Baron-Cohen, 1995; Brothers, 1990; Cos- Singh, 2002). Moreover, recent evidence sug-
mides, 1989; Humphrey, 1976; Whiten & gests that level of impairment may be posi-
Byrne, 1988). If this notion is accurate, it is tively associated with brain volume in autism
also plausible that there has been an evolution (Akshoomoff et al., 2004). However, the cur-
of experience-expectant neurodevelopmental rent neuroanatomical findings in research on
processes that provide a foundation for modu- autism are often inconsistent. Null findings
lar social-cognitive development. Indeed, and even decreased regional brain volumes
research with sensory-impaired children sug- among individuals with autism have also been
gests that sufficient social input is required for noted in the literature (Aylward et al., 1999;
typical ToM development, measured by false Haznedar et al., 1997). Variations among stud-
belief tasks (Peterson & Siegal, 1995). Thus, ies complicate this type of research but may be
in the modular terms of the ToM model, a fail- expected because: (1) The power of these stud-
ure of early experience in social interactions ies is often low due to small sample sizes, (2)
may yield a disturbance of early information consensus has yet to be reached on uniform
processing. This failure contributes to a dis- imaging and data analysis methods to be used
turbance in the neurological development of across studies, and (3) there is a need to con-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 661

trol for comparison group differences in so- tosis processes gone awry (Akshoomoff,
matic, developmental, or cognitive status. Pierce, & Courchesne, 2002). However, human
Nevertheless, in a seminal magnetic reso- development does not necessarily occur as an
nance imaging (MRI ) study that controlled for unerring response to unfolding biological pro-
individual differences in height and nonverbal cess. Rather, it may be the outcome of complex
IQ, Piven et al. (1995) reported male autistic organism-environment interactions. Therefore,
individuals displayed significantly greater understanding developmental disorders may
total brain volume than controls. This differ- require an epigenetic and coactive perspective
ence was not just due to greater ventricle vol- that emphasizes organism-environment inter-
ume but also reflected greater brain tissue actions in understanding this aspect of autistic
volume. In a second report, Piven, Arndt, Bai- pathology (Gottlieb & Halpern, 2002; Gree-
ley, and Andreasen (1996) examined MRI data nough et al., 1987). Moreover, it may also be
from 35 autistic and 36 comparison research important to understand that some critical
participants, also controlling for height and components of the environment ensue from the
nonverbal IQ. They again observed larger child’s own behavior, especially in social de-
brain volumes in male but not female partici- velopment, where the learning environment
pants with autism. Furthermore, significant primarily involves children’s active participa-
enlargement was observed for the temporal, tion in interactions with social partners. Thus,
parietal, occipital, but not frontal lobes of a robust disturbance of early social proclivities
these individuals. In a third report, these au- may dramatically change the social environ-
thors focused on an examination of cerebellar ment of the child and lead to a chronic disrup-
anomalies in the sample of 35 people with tion of fundamental organism-environment
autism and 35 controls (Piven, Saliba, Bailey, interactions during a sensitive period of devel-
& Arndt, 1997). They observed no decrease in opment. We think it is important to consider
the posterior lobules of the cerebellum, but did the hypothesis that early arising behavioral
observe a significantly higher total cerebellar deficits in social orienting and joint attention
volume than in the comparison group. More re- lead to a critical impoverishment in the first
cently, others have also observed higher total years of social information input, which con-
cerebellar, cortical, and basal ganglia volumes tributes to the course of atypical neurodevel-
(Courchesne et al., 2001; Hardan, Minshew, opment in autism (see Figure 46.2). To the
Harenski, & Keshavan, 2001; Hardan, Min- degree that an attenuation of social environ-
shew, Mallikarjuhn, et al., 2001; Sears et al., mental input makes a major contribution to
1999). the excess brain volume phenomena in autism,
These findings are consistent with observa- it may be difficult to identify biological
tions of higher autopsy brain weight, as well as markers of atypical processes related to
above average head circumference in autism synaptogenesis and/or apoptosis. This coac-
(Bailey et al., 1996). It is important to note tive hypothesis of atypical neurodevelopment
that brain enlargement may not be a general also serves to emphasize why studies of early
feature of mental retardation or developmental identification and behavioral intervention
disorders. Children with mental retardation, may be so important. According to this
but not autism, have been observed to display model, very early behavioral intervention
lower total brain volumes than comparison in- may serve to not only ameliorate existing lev-
dividuals (e.g., Hamano, Iwasaki, Kawashima, els of disturbance but also prevent or attenu-
& Takita, 1990). Thus, in the aggregate, these ate the subsequent neurodevelopmental
studies are consistent with the notion that disturbance that potentially arises from early
autism, unlike other developmental disorders, impoverished social interactions in the first
may be characterized by macroencephaly. years of life (see Mundy & Crowson, 1997;
The nature of the processes that lead to in- Mundy & Neal, 2001).
creased brain volume in autism is not clear. Although individuals with autism typically
Many researchers currently conceptualize this display a deficit in joint attention and social
phenomenon singularly in terms of genetic, orienting, they also display significant individ-
neurotransmitter, neural migration, and apop- ual differences in joint attention development
662 Theoretical Perspectives

that are related to their developmental out- been directly examined with imaging and elec-
comes (Mundy, Sigman, Kasari, 1990; Sigman trophysiological methods, both dorsal-cortical
& Ruskin, 1999). If, as we suspect, individual and ventromedial brain activation correlates
differences in joint attention reflect the differ- have been observed.
ences in the degree to which impoverished so- Perhaps the first study of brain behavior re-
cial orienting plays a role in the atypical lations and joint attention development was
development of children, individual differ- conducted at UCLA by Caplan et al. (1993),
ences in early social orienting and joint atten- who studied the behavioral outcome of 13 in-
tion measures may be correlated with or fants who underwent hemispherectomies in an
predict the course of volumetric brain growth attempt to treat their intractable seizure disor-
in samples of children with autism. With ad- ders. The ESCS (Mundy et al., 2003) were
vances in early identification (Willensen- used to assess the postsurgical development of
Swinkel et al., 2002), testing of this important joint attention and related behaviors among
hypothesis may become possible within the these children. Positron emission tomography
next few years. Furthermore, an even stronger (PET) data were gathered prior to surgical in-
test of this model may be provided in interven- tervention. These data indicated that meta-
tion studies. That is, according to this model, bolic activity in the frontal hemispheres,
ameliorating the social-orienting disturbance especially the left frontal hemisphere, pre-
early in the lives of children with autism may dicted the development of IJA skill in this sam-
be expected to directly impact neurodevelop- ple. However, neither RJA skill nor IBR skill
mental organization and volumetric indices of was observed to relate to any of the PET in-
brain growth in children with autism (Mundy dices of cortical activity. Moreover, metabolic
& Neal, 2001). activity recorded from other brain regions was
not significantly associated with joint atten-
JOINT ATTENTION AND BRAIN tion or other social-communication skills in
BEHAVIOR RESEARCH this study. These regions included orbital,
parietal, superior temporal, middle temporal,
Given the foregoing literature, it is reasonable inferior temporal, mesial temporal, lateral oc-
to assume that understanding the brain systems cipital, and mesial occipital. Thus, frontal ac-
involved in joint attention development may tivity appeared to be specifically related to the
provide clues to critical aspects of the neurobe- development of the tendency to spontaneously
havioral pathology of autism (Mundy & Neal, initiate social attention coordination with oth-
2001; Mundy et al., 2000). Current brain be- ers to share experience.
havior research and theory on autism empha- A post-hoc explanation of the frontal con-
size the importance of investigating the role of nection to IJA was offered in a later paper
areas related to the ventromedial prefrontal (Mundy, 1995). A frontal and left lateralized
cortex, medial temporal cortex, and STS in- system emerges in infancy by 10 months of age
volved in the perception of social action and that plays a role in the executive and emotional
the valence of social stimuli (e.g., Dawson, processes associated with approach tenden-
Munson, Estes, Osterling, McPartland, et al., cies. These approach tendencies are involved
2002; Kawashima et al., 1999). Recall, though, in positive social affiliative behaviors (Fox,
that social orienting and joint attention distur- 1991). Mundy suggested that the IJA impair-
bance in autism may involve systems involved ment in autism may reflect a disturbance in
in self-monitoring and the self-initiation or the emergence of this left frontal “social-
generation of behavior as well as those involved approach” system. Based on earlier work
in the perception of the behaviors of a social (Panksepp, 1979), an impairment in IJA was
partner (Mundy et al., 1993). It may be impor- hypothesized to reflect a developmental dis-
tant to broaden inquiry to include the study of turbance in frontally mediated processes in-
brain systems involved in the social initiations volved in assigning positive reward value to
as well as social perceptions. Indeed, when the social stimuli. Impairment in these frontally
brain-behavior correlates of initiating versus mediated processes leads to a relative insensi-
responding to joint attention measures have tivity to the social reward value of social inter-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 663

actions and an attenuation of the motivation to sponse to the gaze direction of a social part-
emphasize social information processing early ner (Kawashima, Sugiura, Kato, Nakamura,
in life. Consequently, social-cognitive and Hatano, et al., 1999; Kingstone, Friesen, &
social-behavioral development in children with Gazzaniga, 2000; see Vaughan & Mundy, in
autism is marginalized (Mundy, 1995). press, for review). However, eye contact and,
To begin to test aspects of this model, especially, gaze aversion have also been ob-
Mundy et al. (2000) examined the hypothesis served to activate components of the DMFC
that EEG activity in a left lateralized, frontal- (Calder et al., 2002). Thus, a frontal contribu-
cortical system would be a significant corre- tion to RJA should not be ruled out on the basis
late of IJA development in typical infants. of this one study. Perhaps the use of alternative
EEG and joint attention data using the ESCS RJA-related paradigms (Hood, Willen, & Dri-
were collected on 32 infants at 14, 16, and 18 ver, 1998), as well as different age groups from
months of age. The EEG data were collected those previously studied (Mundy et al., 2000),
while infants were involved in attending to a will shed light on this issue in future research.
nonsocial stimulus (i.e., balls moving about in The observations of Mundy et al. (2000)
a cage). The results indicated that individual suggest that a dual process or multiple system
differences in 18-month IJA were predicted by model of neurodevelopmental disturbance in
a complex pattern of 14-month EEG activity joint attention in autism may be useful to con-
in the 4–6Hz band that included indices of left sider. A dual parietal and frontal model of
medial-frontal EEG and left occipital activa- joint attention would be consistent with theory
tion, as well as indices of right central and on typical attention development (Posner &
right occipital deactivation. Although the lo- Petersen, 1990), as well as evidence of both
cation of the generators of the EEG data could parietal and frontal contributions to orienting
not be definitively determined in this study, impairments in autism (Townsend et al.,
the frontal correlates of IJA reflected activity 2001). Moreover, a dual process model of joint
derived from electrodes positioned at F3 of the attention is consistent with observations of a
10/20 placement system (Jasper, 1958). In dissociation between IJA and RJA impair-
terms of cortical coordinates, these electrodes ments in the development of children with
were positioned on infants above a point of autism (Leekam & Moore, 2001; Mundy et al.,
confluence of Brodmann’s areas 8 and 9 of the 1994). The dual process model may also have
DMFC of the left hemisphere (Martin, 1996). implications for current neuropsychological
This area includes aspects of the frontal eye research on joint attention in autism.
fields and supplementary motor cortex in- Two studies have suggested that both IJA
volved in visual attention control. Theory on and RJA involve common inhibitory processes
attention development (e.g., Posner & Pe- that, in comparative studies, tend to be associ-
tersen, 1990) suggested that, in addition to ated with dorsolateral cortical activity (Grif-
DMFC activity, data from these electrodes fith, Pennington, Wehner, & Rogers, 1999;
likely reflected activity in the anterior cingu- McEvoy et al., 1993). Alternatively, a seminal
late (AC), a subcortical structure ventral to study conducted by Dawson, Munson, et al.
cortical areas 8/9 (Martin, 1996). (2002) led to the observation that joint atten-
Alternatively, neither RJA nor IBR mea- tion ability in children with autism appears to
sures were associated with the pattern of EEG be significantly correlated with a delayed
activity that was associated with IJA (Mundy nonmatch to sample (DNMS) measure associ-
et al., 2000). However, RJA assessed at 18 ated with activity of a temporal-ventromedial
months was predicted by EEG indices of left frontal circuit, rather than a dorsolateral
parietal activation and right parietal deactiva- frontal system. Dawson et al. suggested that
tion at 14 months of age. This observation was the DNMS task provided a measure of the
consistent with research that suggests parietal children’s sensitivity to shifts in reward con-
areas specialized for spatial orienting and at- tingencies. However, the latent variable re-
tention, perhaps along with temporal systems flecting joint attention used in structural
specialized for processing gaze, contribute to equations modeling by the Dawson, Munson,
the human capacity to shift attention in re- et al. study was composed of two measures of
664 Theoretical Perspectives

RJA to one measure of IJA and may have been Several significant observations emerged
referenced to one of the RJA measures. Thus, from this study. First, no significant correla-
it was not clear whether these results were ap- tions were observed between any of the 14-
plicable to RJA, IJA, or both types of skills. month EEG data and 18-month IBR pointing.
Subsequently, a study by Nichols, Fox, and Alternatively, in the 3–6 Hz band, there were
Mundy (in press) attempted to replicate and four significant correlations of 14-month EEG
extend Dawson, Munson, et al.’s observation power and 18-month IJA pointing (r = −.55 to
in a study of typically developing infants. This −.62, ps < .01). These correlations indicated
study used separate measures of IJA and RJA that bilateral activity recorded above DMFC
and observed that infant DNMS performance sites at 14 months was associated with more
was related to IJA but not RJA development. IJA pointing at 18 months. These correlations
Moreover, this study also included a measure involved electrodes that were placed above
of self-recognition to examine the hypothesis cortical regions corresponding to Brodmann’s
that self-monitoring functions associated with areas 8, 9, and 6. Henderson et al. also ana-
the DMFC would also be associated with IJA lyzed data from the 6–9 Hz band, which re-
(Mundy, 2003). The results of the study sup- vealed 15 significant correlations with IJA
ported this hypothesis and suggested that mul- pointing (r = −.60 to −.78, ps < .01). Again, bi-
tiple functions (e.g., sensitivity to reward lateral activity corresponding to the previously
contingencies and self-monitoring), supported identified dorsal medial-frontal sites were the
by ventromedial and dorsomedial cortical sys- strongest predictors of IJA pointing at 18
tems, may contribute to IJA but not necessarily months. It is interesting that in the 6–9 Hz
RJA development. Thus, it may be important to bandwidth, data from sites corresponding to
consider a dual or multiple process model of temporal, orbitofrontal, as well as dorsolateral
neurodevelopmental disturbance in joint atten- activity at 14 months also predicted IJA point-
tion skills as we attempt to better understand ing at 18 months.
the basis of autistic impairment in this domain These observations are extremely impor-
of development. tant for a number of reasons. First, the bilat-
The dual process model of IJA and RJA, and eral nature of the Henderson et al. (2002)
especially the putative relations between a findings suggest that Mundy’s (1995) model
medial-frontal system and IJA, requires addi- emphasizing processes associated with left
tional substantiation. A critical study was pro- frontal functions and IJA may, at best, be in-
vided by Henderson, Yoder, Yale, and complete. Nevertheless, these results do pro-
McDuffie (2002), who also used the ESCS to vide support for the hypothesis that DMFC
examine the EEG correlates of joint attention processes play an important role in IJA devel-
in twenty-seven 14- to 18-month-old typically opment (Mundy et al., 2000). As previously
developing infants. However, to improve the noted, the specific DMFC areas of involve-
spatial resolution of their data, this research ment observed in the studies by Mundy et al.
group used a high-density array of 64 elec- and Henderson et al. correspond to aspects of
trodes. In addition they reasoned that, since both the frontal eye fields and supplementary
the total ESCS scores for measures of IJA and motor cortex associated with the control of
related behaviors that had been used in the saccadic eye movement and motor movement
Mundy et al. (2000) study were composites of planning (e.g., Brodmann’s area 8/9; Martin,
several items, the exact nature of the behaviors 1996). It may be tempting to suggest that
involved in associations with EEG activity these associations simply reflect the motor
were unclear. To address this issue, Henderson control of eye movements and/or gestural be-
et al. compared the EEG correlates of only two haviors that are intrinsic to IJA behavior.
behaviors: infants’ self-initiated pointing to However, the simple elegance of the Hender-
share attention regarding their observation of son et al. study controls for this possible inter-
an active mechanical toy (IJA pointing), versus pretation. The motor movements involved in
self-initiated pointing to elicit aid in obtaining IJA pointing and IBR pointing are virtually
an out of reach object (IBR pointing). identical. Therefore, a neuromotor explana-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 665

tion of the different cortical correlates of IJA (C. Frith & Frith, 1999; U. Frith & Frith,
and IBR appears unlikely. Instead, since IJA 2001).
pointing and IBR pointing serve different so-
cial-communicative functions, it is reasonable BRAIN-BEHAVIOR RESEARCH AND
to assume that the difference in EEG corre- THEORY OF MIND
lates of these infant behaviors also reflects
differences in the neurodevelopmental sub- In one of the first studies of its kind, Fletcher
strates of these functions. et al. (1995) observed that the performance of
Another important aspect of the results of six typical adult men on the ToM stories was
the Henderson et al. (2002) study is that they associated with PET indices of increased
suggest baseline activity in other cortical blood flow in an area of the left medial-frontal
areas such as dorsolateral, orbitofrontal, and gyrus corresponding to Brodmann’s area 8 rel-
temporal cortex may be involved in IJA. The ative to their performance on the “physical
latter observations are especially intriguing as stories.” Goel, Grafman, Sadato, and Hallett
they are consistent with the hypothesis that (1995) also observed that only tasks involving
IJA development reflects an integration of inferences about other people’s minds elicited
dorsal-cortical functions with ventral “social PET activation of a distributed set of neural
brain” facilities noted in other research (Daw- networks characterized by prominent activa-
son, Munson et al., 2002). We return to the tion of the left medial-frontal lobe and left
possible nature of this integrated activity later medial-frontal gyrus. These authors concluded
in this chapter. that, when inferential reasoning depends on
The EEG methodology of the Mundy et al. constructing a mental model about the beliefs
(2000) and Henderson et al. (2002) studies and intentions of others, activation of the
were insufficiently precise to be indicative of DMFC is required. Goel, Gold, Kapur, and
the specific cortical systems involved in joint Houle (1997) also observed that while general
attention. Therefore, it is important to con- inferential reasoning processes also seem to
sider additional data on brain behavior corre- involve frontal activation, this activation may
lates in social-cognitive development. Joint be centered on more dorsolateral areas of the
attention development has long been theoreti- frontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 46) rather
cally linked to subsequent ToM development in than the more dorsal-medial areas 8/9 associ-
research on typical development (Bretherton ated with social cognition.
et al., 1981), as well as in research on the na- Since studies have often used stories, or
ture of autism (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Mundy verbal stimuli, language-related processes may
et al., 1993). Charman et al. (2000), for exam- have affected the functional localization of
ple, have observed that, after controlling for ToM skills in these studies. To address this
differences in typical 20-month-olds’ IQ and possibility, Gallagher et al. (2000) used func-
language development, an IJA measure of al- tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI ) to
ternating gaze at 20 months was a significant examine brain activity in both verbal-ToM sto-
predictor of ToM performance at 44 months. ries and nonverbally presented ToM tasks that
This alternating gaze measure was the same involved the processing of visually presented
measure that most readily identified autism in cartoons. The results indicated that the bilat-
20-month-olds (Swettenham et al., 1998) and eral brain activation correlates of both tasks
was similar to the IJA measure that best dis- displayed considerable overlap, specifically in
criminated autism and control samples in our the paracingulate area of the DMFC. The
initial studies (Mundy et al., 1986). Thus, data paracingulate area (Brodmann’s area 32)
indicating that ToM skill development is asso- refers to a subcortical frontal structure that
ciated with DMFC functioning would provide forms the ventral border between the DMFC
important indirect support for the hypothesis (Brodmann’s area 8/9, superior and middle
that the DMFC may contribute to joint atten- frontal gyri) and the AC of the limbic system
tion development. This type of association be- (especially Brodmann’s area 24). Schultz,
tween the DMFC and ToM have been reported Romanski, and Tsatsanis (2000) have also
666 Theoretical Perspectives

reported a study that employed nonverbal ToM text of reading and solving ToM stories but did
task presentation called the Social Attribution display activity in an immediately adjacent
Task (SAT; Klin, 2000). In this task, a brief area. This pattern distinguished the people
sequence of geometric forms moving across a with Asperger disorder from controls. Some-
blank white background was presented to eight what different findings emerged in a related
typical adult participants. People viewing the fMRI study by Baron-Cohen et al. (1999),
SAT tend to anthropomorphize the movement which assessed the ability of groups to infer
sequence of geometric forms and describe it in emotional states from pictures of eyes. This
terms of intentional, animate behavior. Pre- social-cognitive assessment method revealed
sumably, this is a fundamental component of that activity in part of the “social brain” net-
ToM skill (Klin, 2000). Schultz et al. reported work, involving orbitofrontal cortex, the STG,
that processing of this task recruited bilateral and the amygdala, was involved in ToM pro-
activation of the DMFC (Brodmann’s area 9) cessing. Moreover, significant differences
in their typical sample. Related research has were found between the Asperger and typical
demonstrated that people with autism spec- samples in this pattern of task-related brain
trum disorders do not tend to anthropomor- action. In addition, activation of the left and
phize the movement sequence on the SAT right DMFC was also observed to be a specific
(Klin, 2000). component of ToM task performance in this
An important control condition was also in- study. However, unlike the data from Happé
cluded in a study by Sabbagh and Taylor et al. (1996), the Asperger sample did not dif-
(2000). Using evoked response potentials fer from controls in task-related activation of
(ERP) and a dense EEG electrode array (128 this cortical area. It was apparent that the typ-
sites), they presented university students with ical controls in Baron-Cohen et al. (1999) dis-
a paradigm that compared false-belief ToM played evidence of bilateral medial-frontal
task performance with an analogous nonsocial activation on ToM tasks. Alternatively, the
task. The latter involves thinking about the people with Asperger disorder displayed evi-
contents held within a camera (i.e., picture) dence of unilateral left medial-frontal activa-
instead of the false belief held within some- tion, but no evidence of right medial-frontal
one’s mind, as in a ToM task (see Leslie & activation in association with the ToM tasks
Thaiss, 1992). Sabbagh and Taylor observed (see Tables 3 and 4, Baron-Cohen et al., 1999).
significantly greater ERP data from the left Thus, there may have been a medial-frontal
dorsolateral and dorsomedial cortex in the group difference in this study that was not de-
ToM false belief task (e.g., ERP from elec- tected by the analyses provided.
trode sites approximately above BA 9/10/46), Russell et al. (2000) have also employed a
rather than in the nonsocial camera task in ToM measure known as the “eyes” task (Baron-
their sample. Cohen et al., 1999) in an fMRI study that
Thus, although some imaging and case examined the neural metabolic activation pat-
studies have observed associations between terns associated with ToM in individuals af-
ToM performance and orbital activity, rather fected by schizophrenia. The schizophrenic
than dorsal medial-frontal activity (Bach, participants made more errors on this measure
Happé, Fleminger, & Powell, 2000; Baron- of attributions of mental state than did the
Cohen et al., 1999), the link between ToM per- controls. Moreover, the controls displayed rel-
formance and the DMFC currently is the most atively more activity in the medial-frontal
consistent finding in the relevant literature lobe (Brodmann’s area 45/9) in association
(U. Frith & Frith, 2001). Moreover, in addi- with ToM task performance relative to the in-
tion to basic studies, applied research with dividuals with schizophrenia. Again, though,
clinical samples points to the involvement of the group differences were not limited to the
the DMFC in ToM performance. DMFC, but also included ventral “social
In a PET study of autism spectrum disor- brain” components of the left inferior-frontal
ders, Happé et al. (1996) reported that five gyrus (Brodmann’s areas 44/45/47) and the
adults with Asperger disorder did not display left middle- and superior-temporal gyri (Brod-
activity in the medial-frontal gyrus in the con- mann’s areas 21/22).
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 667

Complementing these group comparison of significant findings was revealed for areas
data are observations that suggest there may of the AC (Brodmann’s area 24 and 24’). Volu-
be dopaminergic activity in the DMFC of chil- metric data indicated that the autism group
dren with autism (Ernst, Zametkin, Matochik, displayed smaller brain volume in the right AC
Pascualvacae, & Cohen, 1997). A recent region, especially Brodmann’s area 24’ rela-
voxel-based morphometric study with 28 high- tive to the control sample. The autism sample
function children and adults with autism also also displayed hypometabolism in the right AC
indicated increased gray matter density in the cortex relative to controls. The Asperger sub-
AC and left superior frontal gyrus, as well as sample displayed left AC hypometabolism
the left inferior parietal lobe and right frontal relative to controls. This hypometabolism was
lobe subgyral regions (Hardan, Yorbik, Min- not observed in more ventral portions of the
shew, Diwadkar, & Keshavan, 2002). Recall AC (Brodmann’s area 25). Finally, in the
that the first three of these brain regions cor- autism sample, metabolism in left Brod-
respond to the dorsal medial-frontal area mann’s area 24 was correlated with the social
(Brodmann’s areas 8/9/24) that has been im- interaction, verbal communication, and non-
plicated in joint attention and social-cognitive verbal communication scores on the Autism
processing. Diagnostic Interview (ADI ), and metabolism
There are also at least two individual dif- in Brodmann’s area 24’ was correlated with
ference studies that emphasize the potentially the ADI social interaction scores in the
important role of the DMFC in autistic social autism sample. Thus, consistent with the no-
symptom presentation. Ohnishi et al. (2000) tion that the MFC/AC system may be integral
used PET to examine the associations between to the development of joint attention and so-
regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and cial cognition, these studies provide evidence
symptom presentation in children with autism that activity in this system may be related to
and IQ-matched controls. Symptom presenta- social symptom presentation in autism.
tion was measured using factor-based scale In summary, theory suggests that infant
scores for the Impairments in Communication joint attention and later social-cognitive mea-
and Social Interaction scale and the Obsessive sures may reflect common processes (e.g.,
Desire for Sameness scale from the CARS Bretherton et al., 1981; Wellman, 1993) and
(Schopler, Reichlet, DeVellis, & Daly, 1980). sources of disturbance in autism (Baron-
The results indicated that the children with Cohen, 1995; Mundy et al., 1986). Recent
autism displayed decreased baseline rCBF rel- research indicates that common neuropsycho-
ative to controls in the superior temporal gyrus logical functions of the DMFC/AC may play a
(BA 22), left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45), role in IJA, ToM, and related social impair-
and left medial prefrontal cortex (BA 9/10). ments in individuals with autism. At present,
Moreover, less activity in the latter area though, the functional resolution of the data is
(DMFC, BA 9/10) was reportedly correlated inexact, especially those from the joint atten-
with CARS factor scores indicative of more tion studies. Thus, the degree to which this ap-
disturbance on the Impairments in Communi- parent commonality across tasks and measures
cation and Social Interaction factor-based actually involves the same functional units
scale. Alternatively, rCBF in the right hip- within the DMFC/AC system is not clear.
pocampus and the amygdala was reportedly Moreover, current data also raise the possibil-
correlated with the Obsessive Desire for ity that DMFC processes contribute to both
Sameness factor-scaled score. joint attention and ToM skill in conjunction
In another study, Haznedar et al. (2000) used with processes associated with ventral “social
PET and MRI coregistration to examine the hy- brain” systems (e.g., Henderson et al., 2002;
pothesis that the amygdala and hippocampus Russell et al., 2000) that may be involved in
would display metabolic rate and morphometric the perception and analysis of the social be-
differences in 17 high-functioning individuals haviors of others. An argument could also be
with autism relative to typical controls. The made that other brain systems, such as cere-
results, however, revealed few differences in bellar contributions to the attention regula-
these areas. Alternatively, a consistent pattern tion functions of the DMFC/AC, may also be
668 Theoretical Perspectives

involved (Courchesne et al., 1994). Ultimately, complex contributes to the planning and exe-
it may be necessary to adopt a complex sys- cution of self-initiated, goal-directed behav-
tems approach (e.g., Bressler, 1995; Miller & ior. The DMFC/AC system also appears to
Cohen, 2001) in attempts to fully understand play a role in the self-monitoring of goal-
the dynamic neural systems involved in these directed behaviors. Goal-directed behaviors
behavior domains. Prior to such an inclusive refer to a range of activities, from control of
and dynamic level of analysis, a better under- overt behavior such as saccades in visual ori-
standing of the component processes involved enting, to the more covert mental activity in-
in the system will be necessary. However, volved in generating or operating on mental
there has been little detailed recognition of representations (cognition). Self-monitoring,
what the DMFC/AC component may bring to in part, refers to the evaluation of whether
such a system (e.g., Adolphs, 2001; Dawson, goal-directed behavior does or does not lead to
Munson, et al., 2002). This may be a gap in our reward (e.g., Amador, Schlag-Rey, & Schlag,
collective research efforts with autism that re- 2000; Busch, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Ferstl &
quires reconsideration. von Cramon, 2001). Related to these facilities
is the role the DMFC/AC complex plays in the
THE ROLE OF THE DORSAL MEDIAL- maintenance of representations of multiple
FRONTAL CORTEX IN SOCIAL AND goals in working memory. The DMFC/AC is
NONSOCIAL BEHAVIOR also involved in the capacity to flexibly switch
between goal representations (e.g., Birrell &
What processes and functions of the Brown, 2000; DiGirolamo et al., 2001; Koech-
DMFC/AC complex may make it important to lin, Basso, Peirini, Panzer, & Grafman, 1999),
social development? How do these functions as well as the DMFC/AC role in the appraisal
develop? Are they specific to social behavior? of the valence of stimuli and the generation or
Is an impairment in DMFC/AC social func- modulation of emotional responses to stimuli
tions integral to the pathogenesis of autism? (e.g., Fox & Davidson, 1987; Lane, Fink,
Is there a primary developmental impairment Chua, & Dolan, 1997; Teasdale et al., 1999).
of the DMFC/AC system in autism, or are The foregoing, in all likelihood, is a nonex-
functions in the DMFC/AC complex dis- haustive list of DMFC/AC functions. Never-
rupted in autism secondary to neurodevelop- theless, it is important to note that even in this
mental deficits in “ upstream” cerebellar constrained view, there are functional charac-
mechanisms or ventral-brain mediated social teristics of the DMFC/AC system that may
information perception and processing? Does provide a unifying bridge between theories of
impairment of the DMFC lead to a critical autism that emphasize impairments in basic
organism-environment disturbance in autism cognitive functions and those that emphasize
(e.g., the early infancy tendency to initiate specific social-cognitive or social-emotional
social orienting), and does such a disturbance impairments. For example, in their recent ele-
contribute to subsequent neurobehavioral dis- gant work, Minshew et al. (2001) have argued
turbance in autism? that autism involves a selective disorder of
These and related questions may occupy complex information processing. This disorder
the efforts of many people over the next few of complex information processing is report-
years of research on autism. While definitive edly manifest, at least in higher functioning
answers to these questions are not close at individuals, as a fundamental impairment in
hand, a wealth of information is emerging on concept formation. This involves the capacity
the functions of the DMFC and AC, which may to spontaneously initiate a strategy for elimi-
guide inquiry in this arena. Moreover, several nating alternatives, and the strategy needs to
hypotheses concerning the specific social- be monitored and changed in accordance with
cognitive, as well as nonsocial functions of the experience of success or failure while process-
DMFC, have been generated, and these inter- ing the solution (Minshew et al., 2002). Given
sect with current theory on autism. the current functional analysis of the DMFC
The DMFC and AC may play a central role briefly outlined earlier, it seems reasonable to
in several processes that are related to an exec- suggest that the DMFC/AC system may play a
utive system. In particular, the DMFC/AC role in this aspect of cognitive disturbance in
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 669

autism. Moreover, it is interesting that Min- impairment among high-functioning children


shew et al. (2001) suggest that, at its most with autism (Burnette et al., in press). Hence,
basic level, the disorder of complex informa- a better understanding of the relations among
tion processing may be associated with im- weak central coherence, DMFC functions, and
pairments in neocortical systems involved in social-cognitive disturbance in autism may be
overt saccade and occulomotor control. Brod- a useful and integrative goal for future studies.
mann’s area 8/9 of the DMFC/AC system in- The executive functions of the DMFC/AC
cludes portions of the frontal eye fields and is may also play several specific roles in social
integral to saccadic and occulomotor control and social-cognitive impairments in autism.
(Martin, 1996). Impairments in the DMFC/AC facility for
Another recently developed cognitive/per- self-monitoring, as well as maintaining multi-
ceptual model of autism revolves around the ple goals and representations (Birrell &
weak central coherence (WCC) hypothesis Brown, 2000; DiGirolamo et al., 2001; Koech-
(U. Frith & Happé, 1994). Briefly, WCC in lin et al., 1999), may conceivably be essential
autism reflects a bias toward processing stim- to the representation decoupling and tagging
ulus details. Alternatively, holistic stimulus mechanism that Leslie (1987) suggested un-
processing, which involves integration of mul- derlies metarepresentational processes that
tiple dimensions of information (central coher- may be impaired in autism. IJA may also in-
ence), is more difficult for people with autism. volve the capacity to shift attention between
Hence, they often have difficulty with the social and nonsocial goals and representations
types of gestalt, inferential, contextually (Mundy et al., 1986, 2000). Hence, impair-
bound information processing that is neces- ment in this facility of the DMFC/AC may also
sary to adaptive social information processing, be involved in joint attention disturbance in
such as in face processing or the processing of autism.
pragmatic aspects of communication. One in- From another perspective, though, it may be
dication of weak central coherence is the diffi- useful to consider the proposition that, as part
culty verbal children with autism have on of early social development, some of the gen-
homograph tasks that demand processing of eral executive facilities of the DMFC/AC be-
the entire context of the sentence to interpret come redescribed as specific “social-executive
the correct meaning of a word, such as, “a tear functions.” These may arise, in part, from the
in her eye” versus “a tear in her dress” (Bur- self-monitoring and self-initiating facilities of
nette et al., in press; Happé, 1997). It is inter- the DMFC/AC. The hypothesis that the
esting that wholistic or inferential text DMFC/AC plays an integral role in self-
interpretation has recently been observed to be monitoring stems from several findings (Craik
associated with left medial-frontal activation et al., 1999; C. Frith & Frith, 1999; U. Frith &
in an MRI study (Ferstl & von Cramon, 2001). Frith, 2001). Prominent here is research that
Indeed, Ferstl and von Cramon suggest that has led to the observation that, when people
the “ frontomedian area [of the cortex] has a make erroneous saccadic responses in an at-
function for the self-initiation of a cognitive tention deployment task, there is a negative de-
process in the context of tasks that require the flection in the stimulus and response locked
active utilization of the individual’s back- ERP called the error-related negativity (ERN;
ground knowledge” (p. 338). This function de- Busch et al., 2000; Luu, Flaisch, & Tucker,
scribed by Ferstl and von Cramon appears to 2000). Source location suggests the ERN em-
have much in common with the nature of cen- anates from an area proximal to the AC (e.g.,
tral coherence. Ferstl and von Cramon also re- Luu et al., 2000). Observations of the ERN
late this capacity for the self-initiation of suggest that there are specific cell groups
background-dependent cognition directly to within the DMFC/AC that are not only active
the capacity for successful performance on in initiating a behavioral act, such as orienting
ToM measures. Indeed, in our own research, to a stimulus, but also distinct cell groups in-
we have recently observed that poorer perfor- volved in processing the positive or negative
mance on a homographs task, indicative of outcome of the response behavior (i.e., accu-
WCC, was related to poor performance on the racy and reinforcement information; e.g.,
ToM task or evidenced greater social-cognitive Busch et al., 2000; Stuphorn, Taylor, & Schall,
670 Theoretical Perspectives

2000). Thus, cell groups within the DMFC/AC Previous theory and research on social re-
come to serve as part of a supervisory atten- ward sensitivity and social-orienting distur-
tion system (SAS; Norman & Shallice, 1986) bance in autism (Dawson et al., 1998; Dawson,
that functions to guide behavior, especially at- Munson, et al., 2002; Grelotti et al., 2002)
tention deployment, depending on the motiva- have emphasized the possible contributions of
tional context of the task (Amador et al., 2000; the orbitofrontal cortex or subcortical struc-
Busch et al., 2000; Luu et al., 2000). tures such as the amygdala (e.g., de-Haan, Pas-
Robert Schultz and coworkers at Yale calis, & Johnson, 2002; Tremblay & Schultz,
(Schultz et al., 2000) have begun to consider 1999; Wantanabe, 1999). The literature re-
this functional role of the DMFC/AC in social viewed herein, though, suggests that it may be
behavior and its impairment in autism. In one useful to expand this focus to include contri-
scenario, Schultz et al. suggested that im- butions from the DMFC/AC complex. The
paired information flow from the amygdala to logic here is twofold. First, infant measures of
the DMFC may attenuate the tendency for so- IJA provide one operationalization of the ten-
cial stimuli to acquire their normal valence dency to spontaneously social orient to an in-
causing social processing difficulties. The na- teractive partner (Mundy & Neal, 2001), and
ture of these social processing difficulties is there is now empirical evidence to directly link
not yet well defined, though this research team this tendency with DMFC activity (Henderson
has suggested that they might include face et al., 2002; Mundy et al., 2000). Second, and
processing disturbance in autism, which, in equally important, the areas of the DMFC as-
turn, contributes to impairments in the typi- sociated with IJA (Brodmann’s area 8/9) over-
cal development of social-cognitive facilities lap aspects of the frontal eye fields and
(Grelotti, Gauthier, & Schultz, 2002). supplementary motor cortex. These cortical
Similar hypotheses were raised in attempts areas may be important to consider in under-
to understand the nature of joint attention and standing processes that hamper the tendency of
social-orienting disturbance in autism (Daw- children with autism to look appropriately or
son et al., 1998; Dawson, Munson, et al., 2002; suf ficiently often at people because they are
Mundy, 1995; Mundy & Neal, 2001). These re- vital to regulating attention deployment through
searchers have suggested that: (1) frontal and the active integration of the context in which re-
temporal /amygdala circuits that mediate rein- ward occurs with the planning and control of
forcement and emotional /motivational goal saccades or visual orienting (Amador et al.,
guidance contribute to a bias to attend to so- 2000; Luu et al., 2000; Stuphorn et al., 2000).
cial stimuli in infancy, and (2) a disturbance in There is also some evidence that dopamin-
this bias, from the neonatal period onward, ergic projections to the AC play a role in the
plays a fundamental role in developmental dis- mediation of reward-related activity (Allman,
turbance of social behavior and social cogni- Hakeem, Erwin, Nimchinsky, & Hof, 2001).
tion in autism. As noted in earlier sections of Moreover, Allman et al. note two characteris-
this chapter, an early onset of this disturbance tics of the AC that make this brain region of
hypothetically leads to a robust disturbance in particular interest in understanding joint at-
social orienting in autism and a robust attenu- tention and pathology in autism. First, they
ation of the flow of social information to the present evidence to suggest that spindle cell
developing child. The resulting impoverish- formations in the AC may be a novel special-
ment of social information could be suffi- ization of neural circuitry found only in great
ciently severe to disrupt experience-expectant apes and humans. Interestingly, joint attention
neurodevelopmental processes (see Greenough facilities and related social-cognitive ability
et al., 1987) and contribute to the subsequent may also be unique to apes and humans
disorganization and impairment of brain and (Tomasello, 1999). Allman et al. also suggest
behavior systems including those that subserve that these spindle cells appear to emerge post-
social-emotional and social-cognitive skill de- natally, at about 4 months of age, and their de-
velopment (Dawson, 1994; Loveland, 2001; velopment may be affected by environmental
Mundy & Crowson, 1997; Mundy & Neal, factors (Allman et al., 2001, pp. 109–112).
2001; Mundy & Sigman, 1989). The timing of the emergence of spindle cell sys-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 671

tems of the AC suggest they have the potential volved in the development and control of atten-
to be involved in experience-expectant, as tion (Carper & Courchesne, 2000; Townsend
well as experience-dependent, coactive neu- et al., 1996, 2001). Thus, a major goal of re-
rodevelopmental process. A corollary is that search on autism is to resolve this issue and ex-
this characteristic of the spindle cell system amine the possible complex interplay among
of the AC may be important to consider in the DMFC/AC complex, orbitofrontal and
exploring the type of coactive, organism envi- amygdala functions, and cerebellar input in
ronment interactive model of autistic pathol- the development of attention regulation in peo-
ogy we have outlined here and elsewhere ple affected by this disorder (see Mundy,
(Mundy & Neal, 2001). 2003; Vaughan & Mundy, in press, for related
One challenge to the notion that the discussions).
DMFC/AC complex plays a role in the early In addition to its role in social attention im-
onset of social-orienting disturbance in autism pairment, the DMFC/AC may play another re-
is that frontal occulomotor control of attention lated and critical role in the social disturbance
deployment may be relatively late to develop in of autism. C. Frith and Frith (1999; U. Frith &
infancy (Johnson, Posner, & Rothbart, 1991). Frith, 2001) have suggested that the supervi-
However, recent research suggests that by 3 to sory attention system of the DMFC that
4 months, “ the cortical eye fields are actively engages in self-monitoring of attention deploy-
involved in the prospective control of saccades ment also develops the capacity to represent
and visual attention” (Canfield & Kirkham, the actions, goals, intentions, and emotions of
2001, p. 207). Further inquiry into the mecha- the self (see also Craik et al., 1999). Further-
nisms and early development of this system in more, Frith and Frith (1999; U. Frith & Frith,
relation to early social attention and informa- 2001) argue that, as the DMFC comes to par-
tion processing may hold a key to a deeper un- ticipate in the development of representations
derstanding of the pathogenesis of autism, as of the self, it also integrates information from
well as typical social development. Future the ventral social brain perception system
studies may find the ERN paradigm to be use- (e.g., STS) that provides information about
ful in infant studies, as well as studies of the the goal-directed behaviors and emotions of
cortical control of attention to social and others. This integrative activity may be facili-
nonsocial stimuli in children affected by tated by the abundance of connections be-
autism spectrum disorders. It may also be im- tween the DMFC and the STS, as well as the
portant to begin to explore the role that spindle orbitofrontal cortex in primates (Morecraft,
cell formation may play in typical develop- Guela, & Mesulam, 1993). Moreover, it may
ment, as well as in the atypical case of autism. be useful to think of this facility for the inte-
Another issue is that it is not completely gration of proprioceptive self-action informa-
clear if social orienting, in particular, is im- tion with exteroceptive information on the
paired in autism. Although a social-orienting actions and behaviors of others as another
disturbance may be a robust phenomenon in emergent social-executive facility of the
children with autism (Dawson et al., 1998; DMFC/AC. Ultimately, this DMFC/AC social-
Klin, 1991), a more general impairment in ori- executive function may serve to compare and
enting to nonsocial stimuli is apparent as well integrate the actions of self and the actions of
(Dawson et al., 1998; Townsend, Harris, & others (Frith & Frith, 2001), perhaps utilizing
Courchesne, 1996). Moreover, some research the DMFC/AC facility for the maintenance of
suggests that social orienting and social- representation of multiple goals in working
emotional processing disturbances in autism memory. This integration gives rise to the ca-
may not be as pervasive as theory would sug- pacity to infer the intentions of others by
gest (Pierce et al., 2001; Sigman et al., 1992; matching them with representations of self-
Warreyn & Roeyers, 2002). Instead, autism initiated actions or intentions (cf. Leslie,
may be characterized by a general rather than 1987). Once this integration begins to occur in
socially specific orienting disturbance that the DMFC/AC, a fully functional, adaptive
arises from impairment in a complex cerebel- human social-cognitive system emerges with
lar, parietal, and frontal axis of systems in- experience (C. Frith & Frith, 1999; U. Frith &
672 Theoretical Perspectives

Frith, 2001). Thus, it may be very important to and complex interactive skill, albeit through
better understand the developmental links be- poorly understood processes, has been sug-
tween temporal systems involved in the percep- gested as an essential component of the gene-
tion of social behavior of others (e.g., Adolphs, sis of social-cognitive disturbance in autism
2001) and this more dorsal system for self- (Hobson, 1993; Meltzoff & Gopnik, 1993;
monitoring and self-other comparison (U. Frith Mundy et al., 1993). Some have described this,
& Frith, 2001). In terms of joint attention de- and related phenomena, as a fundamental dis-
velopment and autism, it may be especially im- turbance in the capacity for children with
portant to understand how information gleaned autism to engage in shared experience or inter-
about others in RJA (presumably via the tem- subjectivity (Hobson, 1993; Mundy & Hogan,
poral /parietal other monitoring system) feeds 1994). These observations, in conjunction
into and affects the development of the DMFC with the theoretical analysis by C. Frith and
self-other monitoring system putatively in- Frith (1999; U. Frith & Frith, 2001), lead to
volved in IJA. the hypothesis that the activity of the
In a recent essay, we attempted to begin to DMFC/AC complex may well be integral to
address this topic by suggesting that the this function. Stated more forthrightly, al-
timely and early onset of RJA, and related be- though not yet well recognized in the relevant
havior development, may be an important literature (e.g., Trevarthen & Aitken, 2001), it
stimulant for typical IJA and DMFC social- may well be that DMFC/AC complex may
executive development (Vaughan & Mundy, in make an important contribution to the neuro-
press). Furthermore, as we noted earlier in functional platform from which the essential
this chapter, episodes of joint attention, espe- human capacity for intersubjectivity springs.
cially those initiated by the child, provide a C. Frith and Frith’s (1999; U. Frith &
context for infants to integrate proprioceptive Frith, 2001) model also has parallels with sim-
information on the actions and intentions of ulation theory as applied to social cognition
self with exteroceptive information on the ac- (e.g., Stich & Nichols, 1992). As noted earlier,
tions and intentions of others, in reference to simulation theory suggests that individuals use
some third object or event (Mundy et al., their awareness (i.e., representations) of their
1993). Recall the example that, during the act own mental processes to simulate and analyze
of showing, infants have the opportunity to the intentions of others (Gallese & Goldman,
monitor their own experience (e.g., emotions) 1998). Gallese and Goldman have also dis-
and their behavior directed toward an object, cussed the possible role of mirror neurons in
while observing the response of a social part- the social-cognitive simulation facility of the
ner (e.g., their direction of gaze and affect) to DMFC/AC. Mirror neurons are a specific class
both the object and the infant’s behavior. This of motor neurons that are activated both by
interaction provides an opportunity for the in- particular actions performed by an individual
fant to process some information about a so- and when the individual observes a similar ac-
cial partner’s awareness and responses to the tion performed by another person. According
displayed object as well as the showing (shar- to Gallese and Goldman, the motor and premo-
ing) behavior of the child (cf. Bates, 1976). tor cortex adjacent to or overlapping with the
Thus, self-initiated bids for joint attention DMFC is rich in mirror neurons (see Rizzolatti
may provide infants an important if not & Arbib, 1998). Too little is yet known about
unique opportunity to learn about the internal the nature and distribution of mirror neurons to
psychological processes of the self and, per- provide an extended discussion here. Neverthe-
haps, of others as well. less, further inquiry into the relations among
Theoretically, engagement in this process mirror neurons, social cognition, and the DMFC
within joint attention episodes facilitates may be useful (Gallese & Goldman, 1998). In
social-cognitive development, as well as social- particular, there may be a link between mirror
emotional attunement in typical development neurons and imitative behavior. Since imitation
(Mundy, Kasari, & Sigman, 1992, 1993; constitutes a domain of impairment in autism
Mundy & Willoughby, 1998; Stern, 1985). A that has been theoretically linked to social and
failure in the development of this fundamental social-cognitive disturbance in autism (Melt-
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 673

zoff & Gopnik, 1993; Rogers & Pennington, early onset of difficulty in the self-initiation
1991), it may be judicious to explore the tri- of action (e.g., in social orienting or IJA) in in-
partite link among imitation, social cognition, fancy and eventually is manifest in difficulty
and the functions of the DMFC/AC in research in the self-initiation of aspects of social cogni-
on the nature of autism. tion (Frith & Frith, 1999), as well as the self-
There are many other important implica- initiation of aspects of nonsocial cognition
tions for research on autism to be drawn from (Minshew et al., 2001). Moreover, it may be
the sagacious and potentially seminal synthe- useful to consider this conjecture in the con-
sis provided by Uta and Chris Frith. One of text of the observation that one common goal
these was alluded to at the beginning of this of intervention protocols with people with
chapter. The basic idea is that a component of autism seems to be to increase their tendency
information that is necessary for adequate to self-initiate adaptive goal-directed action in
social-cognitive development derives from social, as well as nonsocial, situations.
self-monitoring of self-initiated actions in so-
cial situations (C. Frith & Frith, 1999). More- CONCLUSION
over, an impairment in the early tendency to
initiate social behaviors may be especially The study of autism presents an enormously
problematic for the development of children complex puzzle. Unfortunately, several critical
with autism because it disrupts their capacity pieces of the puzzle seem to be missing. One
for social action, which ultimately is the foun- of these pieces may involve the role of coactive
dation of social self-knowledge requisite to organism-environment interactive processes
social-cognitive development (Mundy, 1995; wherein deficits in the early social behavior
Mundy & Neal, 2001). It may be useful to repertoire of children with autism contribute
adopt something akin to a Piagetian view of to a disturbance in social experience that is so
development. Among his many brilliant contri- robust as to compromise subsequent neurologi-
butions, Piaget (1952) helped us understand cal and behavioral development. If so, our ef-
that early cognitive development derived in forts may need to be redoubled with respect to
large part from the actions infants took on ob- the development of early identification and in-
jects in their world. Indeed, a major compo- tervention methodologies. Another piece that
nent of cognitive development was explained in may be missing in our attempt to attain a
terms of the redescription of overt action (sen- veridical view of the etiology of autism may
sorimotor schemes) to covert mental represen- involve the role of the DMFC/AC complex in
tations of action in the sensorimotor period cognitive and social-cognitive development.
(i.e., in infancy). Unfortunately, Piaget did not This role may be embodied in a fundamental
speak as directly or as completely to social de- disturbance in the capacity to self-initiate, or-
velopment as he did to cognition applied to ganize, and monitor behavior and cognition.
solving object-related problems in the world. The DMFC/AC complex may be integral to
Nevertheless, his constructivist model of de- social-orienting disturbance and the coactive
velopment may be equally applicable to social organism-experience model of autism we have
development. That is, it is plausible that the in- attempted to outline in this chapter. More-
fant’s capacity to initiate action in social in- over, a DMFC/AC system impairment may
teraction (e.g., in orienting to a social partner be central to difficulties that people with
or showing a toy to a social partner) and to autism appear to display in intersubjectivity
note social reactions to self-initiated action and social-cognitive development, as well as
constitute a major early building block of the development of other complex cognitive
social-cognitive development (see Braten, processes. Finally, it may be instructive to
1998). Hence, early difficulty in organizing note that impairment of the DMFC/AC com-
and initiating social action may play as signif- plex reportedly produces a symptom profile
icant a role in the pathogenesis of autism that includes apathy, inattention, dysregulation
(Mundy & Neal, 2001). Indeed, it may be use- of autonomic functions, variability in pain
ful to consider a common developmental path sensitivity, akinetic mutism, and emotional in-
of impairment in autism that begins with the stability (see Busch et al., 2000). This profile
674 Theoretical Perspectives

has obvious commonalities with characteris- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diag-
tics of people affected by autism. The observa- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
tion of related functional commonalities led to ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC:
the proposal of an influential neurological American Pyschiatric Association.
model some 25 years ago, which also empha- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diag-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
sized the role of the DMFC/AC complex in
ders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American
autism (Damasio & Maurer, 1978). When it Psychiatric Association.
was proposed, the model of Damasio and Mau- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diag-
rer was not easily open to empirical investiga- nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
tion. Currently, though, the tools are at hand ders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC:
and inquiry into the neurodevelopmental role American Psychiatric Association.
of the DMFC/AC complex in the pathogenesis Asperger, H. (1944). Die Autistischen psychopa-
of autism has once again become an important then [Autistic psychopathy in childhood].
goal of future research. Kindesalter: Archiv Für Psychiatrie und
Nevenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.
Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Field, K., Sparks,
Cross-References B. F., & Singh, N. (2002). Effects of age on
brain volume and head circumference in
Development of infants and young children autism. Neurology, 59, 175–183.
with autism is discussed in Chapter 8, social Aylward, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Hon-
development is addressed in Chapter 11, as- eycutt, N., Augustine, A., Yates, K., et al.
pects of attention and perception are reviewed (1999). MRI volumes of amygdala and hip-
in Chapter 13. A convergent theoretical per- pocampus in the non-mentally retarded autis-
spective is provided in Chapter 26. tic adolescents and adults. Neurology, 53,
2145–2150.
Bach, L., Happé, F., Fleminger, S., & Powell, J.
REFERENCES (2000). Theory of mind: Independence of
executive function and the role of the frontal
Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cortex in acquired brain injury. Cognitive-
cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, Neuropsychiatry, 5, 175–192.
11, 231–239. Bachvalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe struc-
Akshoomoff, N. A., Lord, C., Lincoln, A., Cour- tures and autism: A review of clinical and ex-
chesne, R., Carper, R., Townsend, J. P., et al. perimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32,
(2004). Outcome classification of preschool 627–648.
children with autism spectrum disorders Bahrick, L., & Lickliter, R. (1999). Intersensory
using MRI brain measures. Journal of the redundancy guides attentional selectivity and
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 43, perceptual learning in infancy. Developmental
349–357. Psychology, 36, 190–201.
Akshoomoff, N. A., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. Bailey, A., Philips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996).
(2002). The neurobiological basis of autism Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, ge-
form a developmental perspective. Develop- netic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological
ment and Psychopathology, 14, 613–634. perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and
Allman, J., Hakeem, A., Erwin, J., Nimchinsky, E., Psychiatry, 37, 89–126.
& Hof, P. (2001). The anterior cingulate: The Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating
evolution of the interface between emotion attention to people and objects in mother-
and cognition. Annals of the New York Acad- infant and peer-infant interaction. Child De-
emy of Sciences, 935, 107–117. velopment, 55, 1278–1289.
Amador, N., Schlag-Rey, M., & Schlag, J. (2000). Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link
Reward predicting and reward detecting neu- between joint attention and language. In
ronal activity in the primate supplementary C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint atten-
eye field. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, tion: Its origins and role in development
2166–2170. (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diag- Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cam-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor- bridge, MA: MIT Press.
ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Swettenham,
Psychiatric Association. J., Nightingale, N., Morgan, K., et al. (1996).
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 675

Psychological markers in the detection of Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to lan-


autism in infancy in a large population. British guage: A psychological perspective. Cogni-
Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 158–163. tion, 3, 255–287.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Bullmore, E., Wheel- Burnette, C., Mundy, P., Meyer, J., Sutton, S.,
wright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. (2000). Vaughan, A., & Charak, D. (in press). Weak
Social intelligence in the normal and autistic central coherence and its relation to theory of
brain: An fMRI study. Neuroscience and mind and anxiety in autism. Journal of Autism
Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 355–364. and Related Disorders.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bull- Busch, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. (2000). Cognitive
more, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., et al. and emotional inf luences in the anterior cin-
(1999). Social intelligence in the normal and gulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4,
autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Jour- 214–222.
nal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898. Calder, A., Lawrence, A., Keane, J., Scott, S.,
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acqui- Owen, A., Christoffels, I., et al. (2002). Read-
sition of performatives. New York: Academic ing the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia,
Press. 40, 1129–1138.
Birrell, J., & Brown, V. (2000). Medial-frontal cor- Canfield, R., & Kirkham, N. (2001). Infant cortical
tex mediates perceptual attention set shifting development and the prospective control of
in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, saccadic eye movements. Infancy, 2, 197–211.
4320–4324. Caplan, R., Chugani, H., Messa, C., Guthrie, D.,
Black, J., Jones, T., Nelson, C., & Greenough, W. Sigman, M., Traversay, J., et al. (1993). Hemi-
(1998). Neuronal plasticity and the developing spherectomy for early onset intractable
brain. In N. Alessi (Ed.), The handbook of child seizures: Presurgical cerebral glucose metabo-
and adolescent psychiatry: Vol. 4. Varieties of lism and postsurgical nonverbal communica-
development (pp. 31–53). New York: Wiley. tion patterns. Developmental Medicine and
Blass, E. (1999). The ontogeny of human infant Child Neurology, 35, 582–592.
face recognition: Orogustatory, visual and Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). At-
social inf luences. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early so- tachment security in children with autism. De-
cial cognition: Understanding others in the velopmental Psychopathology, 6, 29–261.
first months of life (pp. 35–66). Mahwah, NJ: Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998).
Erlbaum. Social cognition, joint attention, and commu-
Bono, M., & Sigman, M. (in press). Relations nicative competence from 9 to 15 months of
among joint attention, amount of intervention, age. Monographs of the Society for Research in
and language gain in early autism. Journal of Child Development, 63(4, Serial No. 255),
Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1–142.
Braten, S. (1998). Infant learning by altercentric Carper, R., & Courchesne, E. (2000). Inverse cor-
participation: The reverse of egocentric obser- relation between frontal lobe and cerebellum
vation in autism. In S. Braton (Ed.), Intersub- sizes in children with autism. Brain, 123,
jective communication and emotion in early 836–844.
ontogeny (pp. 105–124). Cambridge, England: Carver, L., & Dawson, G. (in press). Development
Cambridge University Press. and neural basis of face recognition in autism.
Bressler, S. (1995). Large scale cortical networks Molecular Psychiatry.
and cognition. Brain Research Reviews, 20, Changeux, J., & Danchin, A. (1976). Selective sta-
288–304. bilization of developing synapses as a mecha-
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. nism for specification of neuronal networks.
(1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in Nature, 264, 705–712.
gestural and verbal communication: When do Charman, T. (1998). Specifying the nature and
infants acquire a theory of mind. In M. E. course of the joint attention impairment in
Lamb & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cog- autism in the preschool years: Implications for
nition (pp. 333–373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. diagnosis and intervention. Autism: Interna-
Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for tional Journal of Research and Practice, 2,
integrating primate behavior and neurophysi- 61–79.
ology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuro- Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J.,
science, 1, 27–51. Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing
Brown, J. (1994). Morphogenesis and mental pro- joint attention, imitation, and play infancy pre-
cess. Development and Psychopathology, 6, cursors to language and theory of mind. Cog-
551–564. nitive Development, 15, 481–498.
676 Theoretical Perspectives

Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development autism fail to orient to naturally-occurring so-
and self-regulatory structures of the mind. De- cial stimuli. Journal of Autism and Develop-
velopment and Psychopathology, 6, 553–550. mental Disorder, 28, 479–485.
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Dawson, G., Munson, J. A., Estes, A., Osterling,
Has natural selection shaped how humans rea- J. A., McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002).
son? Studies with the Wason selection task. Neurocognitive function and joint attention
Cognition, 31, 187–276. ability in young children with autism spec-
Courchesne, E., Karns, C., Davis, H., Ziccardi, R., trum disorder versus developmental delay.
Carper, R., Tigue, Z., et al. (2001). Unusual Child Development, 73, 345–358.
brain growth patterns in early life in patients Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abott, R., Osterling, J. A.,
with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurol- Munson, J. A., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early
ogy, 57, 245–254. social attention impairments in autism: Social
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J. P., Akshoomoff, N. A., orienting, joint attention and attention to dis-
Yeung-Courchesne, G., Murakami, J. W., Lin- tress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.
coln, A., et al. (1994). A new finding: Impair- Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G. D., Dager,
ment in shifting attention in autistic and S., Friedman, S., Aylward, E. H., et al.
cerebellar patients. In E. Broman & E. Grafman (2002). Defining the broader phenotype of
(Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in develop- autism: Genetic, brain, and behavioral per-
mental disorder: Implications for brain function spectives. Developmental Psychopathology,
(pp. 101–137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 14(3), 581–611.
Craik, F., Moroz, T., Moscovich, M., Stuss, D., de-Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. (2002).
Winocur, G., Tulving, E., et al. (1999). In Specialization of neural mechanisms underly-
search of the self: A positron emission tomog- ing face recognition in human infants. Journal
raphy study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34. of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 199–202.
Critchley, H., Daly, E., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., DiGirolamo, G., Kramer, A., Barad, V., Cepeda,
Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., et al. N., Weissman, D., Milham, M., et al. (2001).
(2000). The functional neuroanatomy of social General and task specific frontal lobe recruit-
behavior: Changes in the cerebral blood f low ment in older adults during executive
when people with autistic disorder process fa- processes: A fMRI investigation of task-
cial expressions. Brain, 123, 2203–2212. switching. NeuroReport, 12, 2065–2071.
Crowson, M., Mundy, P., Neal, R., & Meyer, J. Dissanayake, C., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1996).
(2003). Joint attention and developmental vul- Long-term stability of individual differences
nerability in infants with insecure attachment. in the emotional responsiveness of children
Manuscript submitted for publication. with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Cummins, D., & Cummins, R. (1999). Biological Psychiatry, 36, 1–8.
preparedness and evolutionary explanation. Elgar, K., & Cambell, R. (2001). Annotation: The
Cognition, 73, B37–B53. cognitive neuroscience of face recognition:
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and Implications for developmental disorders.
communication in mute autistic children. Jour- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6,
nal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 705–717.
282–292. Ernst, M., Zametkin, J., Matochik, J., Pascualvaca,
Damasio, A., & Maurer, R. (1978). A neurological D., & Cohen, R. (1997). Low medial pre-
model for childhood autism. Archive of Neurol- frontal dopaminergic activity in autistic chil-
ogy, 35, 777–786. dren. Lancet, 350, 638.
Dawson, G. (1994). Development of emotional ex- Ferstl, E., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The role of
pression and emotional regulation in infancy: coherence and cohesion in text comprehen-
Contributions of the frontal lobe. In G. Daw- sion: An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive
son & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and Brain Research, 11, 325–340.
the developing brain (pp. 346–378). New York: Fletcher, P., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S., Dloan,
Guilford Press. R., Frackowiak, R., et al. (1995). Other minds
Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, attention, in the brain: A functional imaging study of
and the social-emotional impairments of indi- “ theory of mind” in story comprehension.
viduals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Cognition, 57, 109–128.
Autism, nature, diagnosis, and treatment Fox, N. (1991). It’s not left, it’s right: Electroen-
(pp. 49–74). New York: Guilford Press. cephalograph asymmetry and the development
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J. A., Ri- of emotion. American Psychologist, 46,
naldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with 863–872.
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 677

Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1987). EEG asymmetry in ory of mind” in the brain: Evidence from a
ten-month-old infants in response to approach PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neu-
of a stranger and maternal separation. Devel- roReport, 8, 197–201.
opmental Psychology, 23, 233–240. Hardan, A., Minshew, N. J., Harenski, K., & Ke-
Frith, C., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds: A shavan, M. (2001). Posterior fossa magnetic
biological basis. Science, 286, 1692–1695. resonance imaging in autism. Journal of the
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Oxford, England: Blackwell. Psychiatry, 40, 666–672.
Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2001). The biological basis Hardan, A., Minshew, N. J., Mallikarjuhn, M., &
of social interaction. Current Directions in Keshavan, M. (2001). Brain volume in autism.
Psychological Science, 10, 151–155. Journal of Child Neurology, 16, 421–424.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond Hardan, A., Yorbik, O., Minshew, N. J., Diwadkar,
“ theory of mind.” Cognition, 50, 115–132. V., & Keshavan, M. (2002, November). A voxel
Gallagher, H., Happé, F., Brunswick, P., Fletcher, based morphometry study of gray matter in
P., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Reading the autism. Paper presented at the International
mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR), Or-
of “ theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal lando, FL.
tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21. Haznedar, M., Buchsbaum, M., Metzger, M., Soli-
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons mando, A., Spiegle-Cohen, J., & Hollander,
and the simulation theory of mind-reading. E. (1997). Anterior cingulate gurus volume
Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493–501. and glucose metabolism in autistic dis-
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). order. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154,
The seats of reason? An imaging study of de- 1047–1050.
ductive and inductive reasoning. NeuroReport, Haznedar, M., Buchsbaum, M., Wei, T., Hof, P.,
8, 1305–1310. Cartwright, C., Bienstock, C., et al. (2000).
Goel, V., Grafman, J., Sadato, N., & Hallett, M. Limbic circuitry in patients with autism
(1995). Modeling other minds. NeuroReport, 6, spectrum disorders studied with positron
1741–1746. emission tomography and magnetic resonance
Goldman-Rakic, P. (1987). Development of cortical imaging. American Journal of Psychiatry,
circuitry and cognitive function. Child Devel- 157, 1994–2001.
opment, 58, 601–622. Henderson, L., Yoder, P., Yale, M., & McDuffie, A.
Gottlieb, G., & Halpern, C. (2002). A relational (2002). Getting the point: Electrophysiological
view of causality in normal and abnormal de- correlates of protodeclarative pointing. Inter-
velopment. Development and Psychopathology, national Journal of Developmental Neuro-
14, 421–436. science, 20, 449–458.
Greenough, W., Black, J., & Wallace, C. (1987). Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development
Experience and brain development. Child De- of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
velopment, 58, 539–559. Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998).
Grelotti, D., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. (2002). So- Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention
cial interest and the development of cortical in human infants. Psychological Science, 9,
face specialization: What autism teaches us 131–134.
about face processing. Developmental Psy- Howlin, P. (1978). The assessment of social behav-
chobiology, 40, 213–225. ior. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Ed.), Autism:
Griffith, E., Pennington, B., Wehner, E., & Rogers, A reappraisal of concepts and treatment
S. (1999). Executive functions in young chil- (pp. 63–69). New York: Plenum Press.
dren with autism. Child Development, 70, Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intel-
817–832. lect. In P. Bateson & R. Hinde (Eds.), Growing
Hamano, K., Iwasaki, N., Kawashima, K., & points in ethology (pp. 303–317). London:
Takita, H. (1990). Volumetric quantification Cambridge University Press.
of brain volume in children using sequential Huttenlocher, P. (1994). Synaptogenesis in the
CT scans. Neuroradiology, 32, 300–303. human cerebral cortex. In G. Dawson & K.
Happé, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and brain de-
mind in autism: Reading homographs in con- velopment (pp. 137–152). New York: Guilford
text. British Journal of Developmental Psychol- Press.
ogy, 15, 1–12. Jasper, H. (1958). The 1020 international electrode
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Jo- system. EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10,
hansson, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (1996). “ The- 371–375.
678 Theoretical Perspectives

Johnson, M., Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. (1991). Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An ap-
Components of visual orienting in early in- proach to the study of autism. Journal of Child
fancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268.
looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive LeDoux, J. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interac-
Neuroscience, 3, 335–344. tions in the brain. Cognition and emotion, 3,
Kanner, L. (1973). Childhood psychosis: Initial 267–289.
studies and new insights. New York: Wiley. Leekam, S., & Moore, C. (2001). The development
(Original work published 1943) of joint attention in children with autism. In J.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1995). Annotation: The ex- Barack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo
traordinary cognitive journey from foetus (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspec-
through infancy. Journal of Child Psychology tives from theory and research (pp. 105–130).
and Psychiatry, 36, 1293–1313. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The
(1990). Affective sharing in the context of origins of “ theory of mind.” Psychological Re-
joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, view, 94, 412–426.
and mentally retarded children. Journal of Leslie, A., & Happé, E. F. (1989). Autism and os-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, tensive communication: The relevance of
87–100. metarepresentation. Development and Psycho-
Kawashima, R., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., Nakamura, pathology, 1, 205–212.
A., Hatano, K., Ito, K. et al. (1999). The Leslie, A., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity
human amygdala plays an important role in in conceptual development: Neuropsychologi-
gaze monitoring: A PET Study. Brain, 122, cal evidence from autism. Cognition, 43,
779–783. 225–251.
Kingstone, A., Friesen, C. K., & Gazzaniga, M. Lord, C., Floody, H., Anderson, D., & Pickles, A.
(2000). Ref lexive joint attention depends on (2003, April). Social engagement in very young
lateralized cortical functions. Psychological children with autism: Dif ferences across con-
Science, 11, 159–166. texts. Poster presented at the Society for Re-
Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children’s listening search in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
preferences in regard to speech: A possible Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leven-
characterization of the symptoms of social thal, B., DiLavore, P., et al. (1999). The
withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Develop- Autism Diagnostic Observations Schedule-
mental Disorders, 21, 29–42. Generic: A standard measure of social and
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing meaning to ambiguous communication deficits associated with
visual stimuli in higher functioning autism autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribu- and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
tion Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Loveland, K. (2001). Toward an ecological model
Psychiatry, 41, 831–846. of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yir-
Klin, A., Warren, J., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. R. miya, & P. Zelazao (Eds.), The development of
(2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to autism: Perspectives from theory and research
cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical (p. 17–37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Transaction of the Royal Society of London, Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention
10, 1–16. and language in autism and developmental lan-
Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Peirini, P., Panzer, S., & guage delay. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior mental Disorders, 16, 335–349.
prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, Luu, P., Flaisch, T., & Tucker, D. (2000). Medial-
399, 148–151. frontal cortex in action monitoring. Journal of
Kraemer, G. (1985). Effects of differences in early Neuroscience, 20, 464–469.
social experience on primate neurobiological- Martin, J. (1996). Neuroanatomy: Text and Atlas
behavioral development. In M. Reite & T. (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fields (Eds.), The psychobiology of attachment McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, R. (1993).
and separation (pp. 135–161). New York: Aca- Executive function and social communication
demic Press. deficits in young autistic children. Journal of
Lane, R., Fink, G., Chua, P., & Dolan, R. (1997). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563–578.
Neural activation during selective attention to McWhinney, B. (1998). Models of the emergence of
subjective emotional responses. NeuroReport, language. Annual Review of Psychology, 49,
8, 3969–3972. 199–227.
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 679

Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of the Abridged Early Social Communication
imitation in understanding persons and devel- Scales (ESCS). (Available from the University of
oping a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Miami Psychology Department, Coral Gables,
Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under- Florida; pmundy@miami.edu)
standing the minds of others: Perspectives from Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1994). Intersubjectivity,
autism (pp. 335–366). New York: Oxford Uni- joint attention and autistic developmental
versity Press. pathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.),
Miller, E., & Cohen, J. (2001). An integrative the- Rochester symposium of developmental psycho-
ory of prefrontal cortex functioning. Annual pathology: Vol. 5. A developmental perspective
Reviews of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. on the self and its disorders (pp. 1–30). Hills-
Minshew, N. J., Johnson, C., & Luna, B. (2001). The dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
cognitive and neural basis of autism: A disorder Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1996). A
of complex information processing and dys- preliminary manual for the abridged Early
function of neocortical systems. International Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Avail-
Review of Mental Retardation, 23, 111–137. able from http://yin.psy.miami.edu:80/Child
Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). / Pmundy/manual.html.
Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Joint
between concept formation and concept iden- attention, affective sharing, and intersubjec-
tification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334. tivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 15,
Moore, C., & Dunham, D. (1995). Joint attention: 377–381.
Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (2001). Neural plasticity,
NJ: Erlbaum. joint attention and a transactional social-
Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Follow- orienting model of autism. International Re-
ing the direction of gaze and language devel- view of Mental Retardation, 23, 139–168.
opment in six month olds. Infant Behavior and Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). Specifying the
Development, 21, 373–377. nature of the social impairment in autism. In
Morecraft, R., Guela, C., & Mesulam, M. (1993). G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: New perspectives on
Architecture of connectivity within the diagnosis, nature, and treatment (pp. 3–21).
cingulo-frontal-parietal neurocognitive net- New York: Guilford Press.
work for directed attention. Archives of Neu- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A
rology, 50, 279–283. longitudinal study of joint attention and lan-
Mottron, L., & Burack, J. (2001). Enhanced per- guage development in autistic children. Jour-
ceptual functioning in the development of nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, 20, 115–128.
& P. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1993). The
autism: Perspectives from theory and research theory of mind and joint attention deficits in
(pp. 131–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg,
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social- & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other
emotional approach behavior in children with minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 181–203).
autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
63–82. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint
Mundy, P. (2003). The neural basis of social im- attention, developmental level, and symptom
pairments in autism: The role of the dorsal presentation in young children with autism. De-
medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate velopment and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.
system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T.
chiatry, 44, 793–809. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism:
Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG corre- The contribution of nonverbal communication
lates of the development of infant joint atten- measures. Journal of Child Psychology and
tion skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, Psychiatry, 27, 657–669.
325–338. Mundy, P., & Willoughby, J. (1998). Nonverbal
Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention communication, affect, and social emotional
and early communication: Implications for in- development. In A. Wetherby, S. Warren, & J.
tervention with autism. Journal of Autism and Reichle (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic
Developmental Disorders, 27, 653–675. communication: Preintentional to intentional
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., and presymbolic to symbolic (pp. 111–134).
Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A Manual for Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
680 Theoretical Perspectives

Nichols, K., Fox, N., & Mundy, P. (in press). Neu- Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. (1998). Language
rocognitive functions of initiating joint atten- within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21,
tion in toddlers. Infancy. 188–194.
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to ac- Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoretical
tion: Willed and automatic control of behavior. approach to the deficits in infantile autism.
In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 635–652.
(Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation Russell, T., Rubia, K., Bullmore, E., Soni, W.,
(pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press. Suckling, J., Brammer, M., et al. (2000). Ex-
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Hashimoto, T., Kunihiro, ploring the social brain in schizophrenia: Left
T., Nishikawa, M., Uema, T., et al. (2000). prefrontal underactivation during mental state
Abnormal regional cerebral blood f low in attribution. American Journal of Psychiatry,
childhood autism. Brain, 123, 1838–1844. 157, 2040–2042.
Osterling, J. A., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recog- Sabbagh, M., & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural corre-
nition of children with autism: A study of first lates of theory of mind: An event related po-
birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism tential study. Psychological Science, 11, 46–50.
and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257. Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for
Ozonoff, S., & South, M. (2001). Early social de- joint visual attention in the infant. Nature,
velopment in young children with autism: The- 253, 265–266.
oretical and clinical implications. In G. Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the
Bremmer & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell hand- 1990’s: Development and individual differ-
book of infant development (pp. 565–588). Ox- ences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.
ford, England: Blackwell. Schopler, E., Reichlet, R., DeVellis, R., & Daly, K.
Panksepp, J. (1979). A neurochemical theory of (1980). Toward objective classification of
autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 2, 174–177. childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating
Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, con- Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Develop-
versation and theory of mind. Journal of Child mental Disorders, 10, 91–103.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459–474. Schultz, R., Romanski, L., & Tsatsanis, K. (2000).
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in chil- Neurofunctional models of autistic disorder
dren. New York: Norton. and Asperger syndrome: Clues from neu-
Pierce, K., Frank, H., Farshad, S., & Courchesne, roimaging. In A. Klin, F. Volkmar, & S. Spar-
E. (2001, November). Brain activity to mother row (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 172–209).
and other familiar faces: Evidence for socio- New York: Guilford Press.
emotional responding in autism. Paper pre- Sears, L., Cortney, V., Somaia, M., Bailey, J., Bon-
sented at the International Meeting for Autism nie, J., & Piven, J. (1999). An MRI study of
Research (IMFAR), San Diego, CA. basal ganglia in autism. Progress in Neuro-
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., & Andreasen, N. Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychia-
(1996). Regional brain enlargement in autism: try, 23, 613–624.
A magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C.
of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- (1982). Assessing interactional competencies:
cent Psychiatry, 35, 530–536. The Early Social Communication Scales. In-
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., Havercamp, S., An- fant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244–245.
dreasen, N., & Palmer, P. (1995). An MRI Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N.
study of brain size in autism. American Jour- (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of
nal of Psychiatry, 152, 1145–1149. others by autistic, mentally retarded and nor-
Piven, J., Saliba, K., Bailey, J., & Arndt, S. (1997). mal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.
An MRI study of autism: The cerebellum re- Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attach-
visited. Neurology, 49, 546–551. ments in autistic children. Journal of the
Plousia, M. (2002). Affective expressions during American Academy of Child and Adolescent
joint attention interactions with an adult: The Psychiatry, 28, 74–81.
case of autism. Journal of the Hellenic Psycho- Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and
logical Society, 9, 9–21. change in the social competence of children
Posner, M., & Petersen, S. (1990). The attention with autism, down syndrome, and developmen-
system of the human brain. Annual Review of tal delay. Monographs of the Society for Re-
Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. search in Child Development, 64(Serial No.
Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). 256), 1–108.
Sharing in the second year of life. Child Devel- Stella, J., Mundy, P., & Tuchman, R. (1999). Social
opment, 47, 1148–1158. and non-social factors in the Childhood
Joint Attention and Neurodevelopmental Models of Autism 681

Autism Rating Scales. Journal of Autism and Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. (2001). Infant inter-
Developmental Disorders, 29, 307–317. subjectivity: Research, theory and clinical ap-
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the in- plications. Journal of Child Psychology and
fant. New York: Basic Books. Psychiatry, 42, 3–48.
Stich, S., & Nichols, S. (1992). Folk psychology: Vaughan, A., & Mundy, P. (in press). Neural sys-
Simulation versus tacit theory. Mind and Lan- tems and the development of gaze following
guage, 7, 29–65. and related joint attention skills. In R. Flom,
Stone, W., Coonrod, E., & Ousley, O. (2000). Brief K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.), The ontogeny of
report: Screening Tool for Autism in Two- gaze processing in infants and children. Mah-
years-olds (STAT): Development and prelimi- wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
nary data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R., &
Disorders, 30, 607–612. Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive devel-
Stuphorn, V., Taylor, T., & Schall, J. (2000). Per- opmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychol-
formance monitoring by the supplementary ogy and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.
eye field. Nature, 408, 857–860. Wantanabe, M. (1999). Neurobiology: Attraction is
Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., relative not absolute. Nature, 398, 661–663.
Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The Warreyn, P., & Roeyers, H. (2002, November).
frequency and distribution of spontaneous at- Joint attention, social referencing and request-
tention shifts between social and nonsocial ing abilities in young children with autism spec-
stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and trum disorder. Paper presented at the
nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. International Meeting for Autism Research
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, (IMFAR). Orlando, FL.
39, 747–753. Wellman, H. (1993). Early understanding of mind:
Teasdale, J., Howard, R., Cox, S., Ha, Y., Brammer, The normal case. In S. Baron-Cohen, H.
M., Williams, S., et al. (1999). Functional MRI Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under-
of the cognitive generation of affect. American standing other minds: Perspectives from autism
Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 209–215. (pp. 40–58). Oxford, England: Oxford Univer-
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cog- sity Press.
nition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Wetherby, A., & Prutting, C. (1984). Profiles of
attention: Its origins and role in development communicative and cognitive-social abilities
(pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. in autistic children. Journal of Speech and
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of Hearing Research, 27, 367–377.
human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Whiten, A., & Byrne, R. (1988). Tactical deception
University Press. in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11,
Townsend, J., Harris, N., & Courchesne, E. (1996). 233–273.
Visual attention abnormalities in autism: De- Willensen-Swinkel, S., Dietz, F., Nober, E., van
layed orienting to location. Journal of the In- Daalen, H., van Engeland, H., & Buitlaar, J.
ternational Neuropsychological Society, 2, (2002, October). A population based study on
541–550. early detection of autism at age 14 months
Townsend, J., Westerfield, M., Leaver, E., Makeig, in the Netherlands. Paper presented at the In-
S., Tzyy-Ping, J., Pierce, K., et al. (2001). ternational Meeting for Autism Research
Event-related brain response abnormalities in (IMFAR). Orlando, FL.
autism: Evidence for impaired cerebello- Wing, L., & Potter, D. (2002). The epidemiology of
frontal spatial attention networks. Cognitive autistic spectrum disorders: Is the prevalence
Brain Research, 11, 127–145. rising? Mental Retardation and Developmental
Tremblay, L., & Schultz, W. (1999). Relative re- Disabilities Research Review, 8, 151–161.
ward preference in primate orbitofrontal cor-
tex. Nature, 398, 704–708.
CHAPTER 26

The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition:


Lessons from Autism

AMI KLIN, WARREN JONES, ROBERT T. SCHULTZ, AND FRED R. VOLKMAR

One of the most intriguing puzzles posed by Their full scale IQs are within the normative
individuals with autism is the great discrep- range, whereas their mean age equivalent score
ancy between what they can do on explicit on the Interpersonal Relationships subdomain
tasks of social reasoning (when all of the ele- of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
ments of a problem are verbally given to them) (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) is 4 years.
and what they fail to do in more naturalistic These individuals have many cognitive, lin-
situations (when they need to spontaneously guistic, knowledge-based, and potentially use-
apply their social reasoning abilities to meet ful vocational assets, yet this social adaptive
the moment-by-moment demands of their daily score would suggest that if left to their own de-
social life; Klin, Schultz, & Cohen, 2000). vices in a challenging social situation, their
While even the most intellectually gifted indi- social survival skills or street smarts might be
viduals display deficits in some complex social equivalent to those of young children. Yet,
reasoning problems (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, many of these individuals are capable of a de-
Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997; Happé, 1994), gree of self-sufficiency that is much higher
some, particularly those without cognitive than 4 years. It is possible, however, that they
deficits, can solve such problems at relatively are able to achieve this level of independence
high levels (Bowler, 1992; Dahlgren & despite significant social disabilities by choos-
Trillingsgaard, 1996) without showing com- ing highly structured and regimented life rou-
mensurate levels of social adaptation. This tines that avoid novelty and the inherent
discrepancy is troublesome because while it is unpredictability of typical social life. In other
possible to teach them better social reasoning words, they may be able to constrain the in-
skills, such new abilities may have little im- evitable complexity of social life by setting
pact on their real-life social or communicative themselves a routine of rigid rules and habits,
competence (Hadwin, Baron-Cohen, Howlin, adhering very closely to this lifestyle in what
& Hill, 1997; Ozonoff & Miller, 1995). is typically a very solitary life.
There has been little systematic research to Some recent studies focused on responses
investigate the magnitude of this discrepancy. to naturalistic social situations suggest that
Nevertheless, an indicator of its size can be de- the discrepancy between performance on
rived from a sample of 40 older adolescents structured and naturalistic tasks may be even
and adults with autism followed in our center. greater than hitherto thought possible. Con-

This chapter is reprinted by permission of Oxford University Press.

682
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 683

(a) (b)

Figure 26.1 Focus on eyes versus mouth: Cut to shocked young man. (a) Focus on typically developing viewer.
( b) Focus of viewer with autism.

sider the following two examples from eye- autism is seen trying to gather information
tracking studies of normative-IQ adolescents from the young man’s mouth. The young
and adults with autism. In these experiments man’s mouth is slightly open but expression-
(Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen, less, and it provides few clues about what is
2002a, 2002b), eye-tracking technology allows happening in the scene.
researchers to see and measure what a person This discrepancy in viewing patterns is also
is visually focusing on when viewing complex seen in group data. Figure 26.2 plots the focus
social situations. This paradigm allows for an of eight normative-IQ adults with autism (in
appreciation of a person’s spontaneous reac- red) and eight age-, IQ-, and gender-matched
tions to naturalistic demands inherent in seek- typical controls (in blue; this is a subsample
ing meaning in what is viewed. In real-life from the data in Klin et al., 2002b) for one
social situations, many crucial social cues frame of this video sequence. This subsample
occur very rapidly. Failure to notice may lead is used here to visually illustrate the findings
to a general failure in assessing the meaning of obtained for the entire sample summarized
entire situations, thus precluding adaptive re- later. While typical viewers converge on the
actions to them. Figure 26.1 shows a still eye region, some individuals with autism con-
image of two characters from a movie: at left, verge on the mouth regions, whereas others’
a young man, and at right, a young woman. focus is peripheral to the face. When the vi-
Overlaid on the image are crosses that mark, sual fixation patterns were summarized for the
in green, the focus of a normative-IQ adult
with autism and, in yellow, the focus of a typi-
cal adult viewer matched for gender and IQ.
The boldest crosshairs mark each viewer’s
point of regard at the moment of this still,
while the gradated crosses reveal the path of
each viewer’s focus over the preceding five
frames. The image in this figure is a still from
a shot immediately following an abrupt cam-
era cut. In the preceding shot, a character
smashes a bottle in the right half of the frame
(where both viewers were focused). The cam-
era cuts to show the reaction of the young man
and woman, and both viewers respond imme-
diately. While the typical viewer responds di- Figure 26.2 Group data (N = 16) illustrating focus
rectly to the look of surprise and horror in the on eyes versus mouth. Viewers with autism: black
young man’s wide eyes, the viewer with crosses; typically developing viewers: white crosses.
684 Theoretical Perspectives

entire sample in this study (N = 30, 15 partici- percentage of overall viewing time focused on
pants in each group), individuals with autism, eyes and mouths.
relative to controls, focused twice as much These results contrast markedly with an-
time on the mouth region of faces and 21⁄ 2 other recent study of face scanning in autism
times less on the eye region of faces when (van der Geest, Kemner, Verbaten, & van En-
viewing dynamic social scenes. There was vir- geland, 2002), in which participants showed
tually no overlap in the distributions of visual normative visual fixation patterns when view-
fixation patterns across the two groups of par- ing photographs of human faces relative to con-
ticipants. Figure 26.3 presents these data as a trols. The difference between the two studies
was that while in the latter investigation partic-
ipants were presented with static pictures of
faces, in the former study participants were
presented with dynamic (i.e., video) depictions
100 100 of social interactions, coming perhaps closer to
replicating a more naturalistic social situation
(i.e., we almost never encounter static depic-
tions of faces in our daily social interactions).
90 90
In such more “spontaneous situations,” the de-
viation from normative face-scanning patterns
in autism seems to be magnified. And the mag-
80 80 nitude of this deviation is put in context if we
appreciate the fact that preferential looking at
the eyes rather than at the mouths of an ap-
70 70 proaching person has been shown in infants as
young as 3 months of age (Haith, Bergman, &
Moore, 1979).
A second example from the same eye-track-
60 60
ing studies (Klin et al., 2002a) focuses on a
developmental skill that emerges and is fully
per cent time (%)

operational by the time a child is about 12 to


50 50 14 months of age. It involves the joint-attention
skill of following a pointing gesture to the tar-
get indicated by the direction of pointing
40 40 (Mundy & Neal, 2000). Pointing, like many
other nonverbal social cues, can both modify
and further specify what is said. For effective
30
communication exchange, verbal and nonver-
30
bal cues need to be quickly integrated. Figure
26.4 shows a scene from a movie in which the
young man inquires about a painting hanging
20 20 on a distant wall. In doing so, he first points to
a specific painting on the wall and then asks
the older man (who lives in the house), “Who
10 10 did the painting?” While the verbal request is
more general (as there are several pictures on
the wall), the act of pointing has already spec-
0
ified the painting in which the young man is
0
mouth eyes
interested. The figure shows the visual scan-
Figure 26.3 Group data (N = 30) summarizing vi-
ning paths of the adult viewer with autism (in
sual fixation patterns on eyes versus mouth. Black green) and the typical viewer (in yellow). As
bars: viewers with autism; white bars: typically de- shown in Figure 26.4 and more clearly in the
veloping viewers. schematic renditions underneath, the viewer
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 685

(a)

(b) (c)

Figure 26.4 Scanning patterns in response to social visual versus verbal cues. Viewers with autism: black trace
in (a) and ( b); typically developing viewer: white trace in (a) and (c).

with autism does not follow the pointing ges- man. In contrast, the viewer with autism uses
ture but instead waits until he hears the ques- primarily the verbal cue, neglecting the non-
tion and then appears to move from picture to verbal gesture and, in doing so, resorts to a
picture without knowing which one the conver- much more inefficient pursuit of the refer-
sation is about. The typical viewer (in yellow) enced painting. When the viewer with autism
follows the young man’s pointing immediately, was later questioned in an explicit fashion
ending up, very deliberately, on the correct about whether he knew what the pointing ges-
( large) picture. Hearing the question, he then ture meant, he had no difficulty defining the
looks to the older man for a reply and back to meaning of the gesture. Yet, he failed to apply
the young man for his reaction. The visual this knowledge spontaneously when viewing
path he follows clearly illustrates his ability to the scene from the movie.
use the nonverbal gesture to immediately in- That normative-IQ adolescents and adults
spect the painting referenced by the young with autism fail to display normative reactions
686 Theoretical Perspectives

exhibited by typical young children does not • Instead of assuming a social environment
mean that their ability to function in the world that consists of a pregiven set of definitions
is at this very early stage of development. and regularities and a perceiving social
Rather, it raises the possibility that these indi- agent (e.g., a child) whose mind consists of
viduals learn about the social world in a differ- a pregiven set of cognitive capacities that
ent manner. What is this developmental path is can solve problems as they are explicitly
of both clinical and research importance. Col- presented to it, this framework proposes an
lectively, the various examples presented here active mind that sets out to make sense of
suggest a need to explain the discrepancy be- the social environment and that changes it-
tween performance on structured and explicit self as a result of this interaction (Mead,
as against naturalistic and spontaneous tasks 1924).
and, in so doing, to explore what might be a • Moving from a focus on abstracted compe-
unique social developmental path evidenced in tencies (what an organism can do), this
autism. This chapter contends that theories of framework focuses on the adaptive func-
the social dysfunction in autism need to ad- tions that are subserved by these competen-
dress both of these phenomena. Traditionally, cies (i.e., how an agent engages in the
theories of social-cognitive development have process of acquiring such competencies in
relied on a framework delineated by computa- the first place; Klin et al., 2000).
tional models of the mind and of the brain • Moving away from a focus on cognition,
(Gardner, 1985), which focus on abstracting this framework rekindles a once more
problem-solving capacities necessary to func- prominent role given to affect and predis-
tion in the social environment. The methodolo- positional responses in the process of so-
gies used typically employ explicit and often cialization (Damasio, 1999).
verbally mediated tasks to probe whether a • It shifts the focus of investigation from
person has these capacities. In real life, how- what can be called disembodied cognition,
ever, social situations rarely present them- or insular abstractions captured by compu-
selves in this fashion. Rather, the individual tational cognition (e.g., algorithms in a
needs to go about defining a social task as such digital computer) to embodied cognition, or
by paying attention to and identifying the rele- cognitive traces left by the action of an
vant aspects of a social situation prior to hav- organism on an environment defined by
ing an opportunity to use his or her available species-specific regularities and by a
social-cognitive problem-solving skills. Thus, species-specific topology of differential
to study more naturalistic social adaptation, salience (i.e., some things in the environ-
there may be some utility in using an alterna- ment are more important than others).
tive theoretical framework that centers around
a different set of social-cognitive phenomena Of particular importance in this framework is
such as people’s predispositions to orient to the premise that agents may vary in what they
salient social stimuli, to naturally seek to im- are seeking in the environment, resulting in
pose social meaning on what they see and highly disparate “mental representations” of
hear, to differentiate what is relevant from the world that they are interacting with (Clark,
what is not, and to be intrinsically motivated 1999; Varela, Thompson, & Rosch, 1991).
to solve a social problem once such a problem This process, in turn, leads to individual varia-
is identified. The framework presented in this tion in neurofunctional specialization because
chapter is called enactive mind (EM) to high- more prominence is given in this framework to
light the central role of motivational predispo- the notion of the brain as a repository of expe-
sitions to respond to social stimuli and a riences (LeDoux, 2002); that is, our brain be-
developmental process in which social cogni- comes who we are or experience repeatedly.
tion results from social action. Specifically, the EM approach is offered as
The emphases of the EM framework differ an avenue to conceptualize phenomena deemed
from those in computational models in a num- essential for understanding social adaptation,
ber of ways: and which are typically not emphasized in re-
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 687

search based on computational models of the sider a multitude of elements that are more or
social mind. These phenomena include the less important depending on the context of the
need to consider the complexity of the social situation and the person’s perceptions, de-
world, the very early-emerging nature of a sires, goals, and ongoing adjustment. Success-
multitude of social adaptive mechanisms, and ful adaptation requires from a person a sense
how these mechanisms contextualize the emer- of relative salience of each element in a situa-
gence of social cognition, as well as important tion, preferential choices on the basis of prior-
temporal constraints on social adaptation. Our ities learned through experience, and further
formulation of the EM framework is primarily moment-by-moment adjustments. For example,
based on Mead’s Darwinian account of the if we were to represent the skills of driving a
emergence of mind (Mead, 1924), the work of car successfully, we could define the driving
Searle (1980) and Bates (1976, 1979) as to the domain as involving wheels, roads, traffic
underlying functions of communication, the lights, and other cars, but this domain is
philosophy of perception of Merleau-Ponty hardly complete without encompassing a host
(1962), and, particularly, on a framework for of other factors including attention to pedes-
cognitive neuroscience outlined by Varela and trians (sometimes but not always), driving reg-
colleagues (1991), from which the term enac- ulations ( but these can be overridden by safety
tive mind is borrowed. Excellent summaries of factors), local customs (in some cities or coun-
psychological and neurofunctional aspects of tries more than others), variable weather con-
this framework have been provided by Clark ditions, signals from other drivers, and so on.
(1999) and Iacoboni (2000a). Some of the This rich texture of elements defines the
views proposed here have long been part of “ background” of knowledge necessary to solve
discussions contrasting information process- problems in the driving domain. Similarly, the
ing and ecological approaches to every aspect social domain consists of people with age,
of the mind, including attention and sensori- gender, ethnic, and individual differences;
motor integration, memory and language, facial and bodily gestures; language and
among other psychological faculties (e.g., Gib- voice/prosodic cues in all of their complexity
son, 1963; Neisser,1997). and context-dependent nature; posture; physi-
cal settings and social props; situation-specific
THE SOCIAL WORLD AS AN conventions, among a host of other factors.
OPEN-DOMAIN TASK Successful driving or social adaptation would
require more than knowing a set of rules—at
In the EM approach, a fundamental difference times referred to as knowing that—it would
between explicit and naturalistic social tasks rather require knowing how, or a learning pro-
is captured in the distinction between closed cess that is based on the accumulation of expe-
domains and open domains of operation riences in a vast number of cases that result in
(Winograd & Flores, 1986). Research para- being able to navigate the background environ-
digms based on computational models of the ment according to the relative salience of each
social mind often reduce the social word to a of the multitude of elements of a situation and
set of pregiven rules and regularities that can the moment-by-moment emerging patterns that
be symbolically represented in the mind of a result from the interaction of the various ele-
young child. In other words, the social world is ments. In autism, one of the major limitations
simplified into a closed-task domain, in which of available teaching strategies, including
all essential elements to be studied can be forms of social skills training (e.g., Howlin,
fully represented and defined. This is justified Baron-Cohen, & Hadwin, 1999), is the diffi-
in terms of the need to reduce the complexity culty in achieving generalization of skills or
of the social environment into a number of eas- how to translate a problem-solving capacity
ily tested problem-solving tasks. In contrast, learned in a closed-domain environment
the EM approach embraces the open-ended na- (e.g., therapeutic methods relying on explicit
ture of social adaptation. The social world as rules and drilling) into a skill that the person
an open-task domain implies the need to con- avails himself or herself of in an open-domain
688 Theoretical Perspectives

environment (e.g., a naturalistic social situa- These principles imply, however, that the
tion). This may also be the reason that individ- surrounding environment will be enacted or
uals with autism have difficulty in recreated differently on the basis of differ-
spontaneously using whatever social-cognitive ences in predispositions to respond in a certain
skills they may have learned through explicit way (Maturana & Varela, 1973). In autism,
teaching. Incidentally, driving is an equally our eye-tracking illustrations are beginning to
challenging task to individuals with autism. show what this social landscape may look like
In the EM approach, the child “enacts the from the perspective of individuals with this
social world,” perceiving it selectively in condition. Consider, for example, the illustra-
terms of what is immediately essential for so- tion in Figure 26.5, showing the point of regard
cial action, whereas mental representations of (signaled by the white cross in the center of
that individualized social world arise from re- the green circle) of a normative-IQ adult with
peated experiences resulting from such per- autism who is viewing a romantic scene.
ceptually guided actions (Varela et al., 1991). Rather than focusing on the actors in the fore-
In this way, the surrounding environment is re- ground, he is foveating on the room’s light
duced to perceptions that are relevant to social switch on the left. In Figure 26.6, a 2-year-old
action, a great simplification if we are to con- boy with autism is viewing a popular Ameri-
sider the richness of what is constantly avail- can children’s show. His point of regard on the
able for an agent to hear, see, and otherwise video frame presented as well as his scanpath
experience. Similarly, the mental representa- immediately before and after that frame (seen
tions (i.e., social cognition) available for the in green on the right corner of the picture) in-
child to reason about the social environment dicate that rather than focusing on the protago-
are deeply embedded in the child’s history of nists of the show and their actions, this child is
social actions, thus constituting a tool for so- visually inspecting inanimate details on the
cial adaptation. Thus, there are two principles shelves. By enacting these scenes in this man-
underlying the EM approach to naturalistic so- ner, it is likely that, from the perspective of
cial situations as open-domain tasks. First, the the two viewers with autism, the scenes are no
vast complexity of the surrounding environ- longer social scenes, however clear their social
ment is greatly simplified in terms of a differ- nature might be to a typical viewer. It is also
ential topology of salience that separates likely that if these viewers were explicitly
aspects of the environment that are irrelevant asked or prompted to observe the social scenes
(e.g., light fixtures, a person turned away) and perform a task about them, they might be
from those that are crucially important (e.g., able to fare much better. The fact that they did
someone staring at you). Second, this topology not orient to the essential elements in the
of salience is established in terms of perceptu- scene, however, suggests that were they to be
ally or cognitively guided actions subserving part of such a situation, their adjustment to the
social adaptation. environmental demands (e.g., fit in the ongo-

Figure 26.5 Adult viewer with autism (white cross Figure 26.6 Toddler viewer with autism (white
circled in black): Focus on nonessential inanimate cross circled in white): Focus on nonessential inani-
details. mate details.
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 689

ing play taking place between the two child greatly reduced in children with autism. To
protagonists) would be greatly compromised. limit the discussion to early social orientation
skills, we consider only infants’ reactions to
DEVELOPMENTAL ELEMENTS human sounds and faces. The human voice ap-
IN THE EMERGENCE OF MENTAL pears to be one of the earliest and most effec-
REPRESENTATIONS tive stimuli conducive of social engagement
(Alegria & Noirot, 1978; Eimas, Siqueland,
Computational models of the social mind Jusczyk, & Vigorito, 1971; Eisenberg, 1979;
make use of cognitive constructs that could Mills & Melhuish, 1974), a reaction that is not
help a child navigate successfully the social observed in autism (Adrien et al., 1991, 1993;
environment (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1995). Klin, 1991, 1992; Osterling & Dawson, 1994;
There is less emphasis on how these con- Werner, Dawson, Osterling, & Dinno, 2000).
structs emerge within a broader context of In fact, the lack of orientation to human
early social development, which is a justifi- sounds (e.g., when the infant hears the voice of
able way of modeling the more specific, tar- a nearby adult) has been found to be one of the
geted social-cognitive skills. In contrast, the most robust predictors of a later diagnosis of
EM approach depends on this broader discus- autism in children first seen at the age of 2
sion of early social predispositions to justify years (Lord, 1995). In the visual modality,
the need to consider complex social situations human faces have been emphasized as one of
in terms of a differential topology of salience. the most potent facilitators of social engage-
In other words, why should some aspects ment (Bryant, 1991). For example, 2-day-olds
of the environment be more salient than look at their mother rather than at another
others? To address this question, we need unknown woman (I. W. R. Bushnell, Sai, &
to outline a set of early social reactions that Mullin, 1989), 3-month-olds focus on the more
may precede and accompany the emergence emotionally revealing eye regions of the face
of social-cognitive skills. (Haith et al., 1979), and 5-month-olds are
In the EM approach, the perceptual makeup sensitive to very small deviations in eye gaze
of typical human infants is seen as consisting during social interactions (Symons, Hains, &
of a specific set of somatosensory organs that Muir, 1998) and can match facial and vocal ex-
are constantly seeking salient aspects of the pressions on the basis of congruity (Walker,
world to focus on, particularly those that have 1982). In autism, a large number of face per-
survival value. To invoke the notion of survival ception studies have shown deficits and abnor-
value implies the notion of adjustment to or ac- malities in such basic visual social processing
tion on the environment. In this context, the situations (Hobson, Ouston, & Lee, 1988; Klin
gravitation toward and engagement of et al., 1999; Langdell, 1978), which, inciden-
cospecifics is seen as one of the important sur- tally, were not accompanied by failure in de-
vival functions. Thus, social stimuli are seen velopmentally equivalent tasks in the physical
as having a higher degree of salience than (nonsocial) domain. For example, one study
competing inanimate stimuli (e.g., Bates, demonstrated adequate visual processing of
1979; Klin et al., 2000). The possibility that in buildings as against faces (Boucher & Lewis,
autism the relative salience of social stimuli 1992). Another study asked children with
might be diminished (e.g., Dawson, Meltzoff, autism to sort people who varied in terms of
Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998; Klin, age, sex, facial expressions of emotion, and the
1989) could be the basis for a cascade of devel- type of hat that they were wearing (Weeks &
opmental events in which a child with this con- Hobson, 1987). In contrast to typical children
dition fails to enact a relevant social world, who grouped pictures by emotional expres-
thus failing to accrue the social experiences sions, the participants with autism grouped
hypothesized in the EM approach to be the the pictures by the type of hat the people were
basis for social-cognitive development. wearing. Such studies indicated not only ab-
A large number of social predispositions normalities in face processing but also prefer-
have been documented in the child develop- ential orientation to inanimate objects, a
ment literature, some of which appear to be finding corroborated in other studies (Dawson
690 Theoretical Perspectives

et al., 1998). In a more recent study (Dawson affect (Tronick, Cohn, & Shea, 1986) and ap-
et al., 2002), children with autism failed to ex- propriately to the emotional content of praise
hibit differential brain event-related potentials or prohibition (Fernald, 1993). From very
to familiar versus unfamiliar faces, but they early on, they expect contingency between
did show differences relative to familiar ver- their actions and those of their partners
sus unfamiliar objects. (Tarabulsy, Tessier, & Kappas, 1996). Fewer
While computational models of the social developmental phenomena have demonstrated
mind are often modular in nature (e.g., Leslie, this effect more clearly than studies using the
1987), that is, certain aspects of social func- “still-face paradigm” (Tronick, Als, Adam-
tioning could be preserved while others were son, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978). When mothers
disrupted, the EM approach ascribes impor- who have previously been stimulating their
tance to early disruptions in sociability be- babies in a playful fashion withdraw the
cause of its central premise that normative smiles and vocalization and assume a still
social cognition is embedded in social percep- face, infants as young as 2 to 3 months first
tion and experience. This principle states that make attempts to continue the interaction but
social perception is perceptually guided social then stop smiling, avert their gaze, and may
action, and social-cognitive processes emerge protest vigorously (Field, Vega-Lahar, Scafidi,
only from recurrent sensorimotor patterns that & Goldstein, 1986; Gusella, Muir, & Tronick,
allow action to be perceptually guided ( hence 1988). One study of the still-face effect in-
the notion of embodied cognition; Varela et al., volving children with autism has failed to doc-
1991). The radical assumption of this frame- ument this normative pattern of response
work, therefore, is that it is not possible to dis- (Nadel et al., 2000).
entangle cognition from actions and that if this In summary, in the EM approach early so-
happened (e.g., a child was taught to perform a cial predispositions are thought to create the
social-cognitive task following an explicit drill basis and the impetus for the subsequent emer-
rather than acquiring the skill as a result of re- gence of mental representations, which be-
peated social engagement and actions), the cause of their inseparability from social action
given skill would represent a disembodied cog- (i.e., they are embodied), retain their adaptive
nition, or a reasoning skill that would not re- value. Infants do not build veridical models of
tain its normative functional value in social the social world on the basis of universals or
adaptation (Markman & Dietrich, 2000). For context-invariant representations. Rather, their
example, an infant may be attracted to the face models or expectations of the world follow
of his or her mother, seeking to act on it. In the their salience-guided actions on an ever-
context of acting on it, the infant learns a great changing environment that needs to be coped
deal about faces and mothers, although this with in an adaptive, moment-by-moment, and
knowledge is a function of the child’s active context-dependent manner (Engel, Fries, &
experiences with that face, which may include Singer, 2001).
learning of contingencies (e.g., vocal sounds
and lip movements go together; certain voice CONTEXTUAL ELEMENTS IN THE
inflections go with certain face configura- EMERGENCE OF MENTAL
tions such as smiles and frowns) and that REPRESENTATIONS
these contingencies have pleasurable value
(thus leading to approach or an attempt at The classical computation model in cognitive
reenactment of the situation) or unpleasurable science assumes that cognitive processes are
value (thus leading to withdrawal). Studies of rule-based manipulations of symbols repre-
infants’ early social development have shown senting the outside environment (e.g., Newell,
that they not only are sensitive to affective 1991). Similarly, computational models of the
salience but also act on that salience through social mind build on the notion that to operate
reactions that are appropriate to emotional socially is to execute algorithms involving
signals (Haviland & Lelwica, 1987). They mental representations (e.g., Baron-Cohen,
react negatively to their mothers’ depressed 1994). In contrast, the EM approach raises the
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 691

nontrivial question of how a representation ac- come from having a body with various sensori-
quires meaning to a given child, the so-called motor capacities,” and “perception and action
mind-mind problem (Jackendoff, 1987). The are fundamentally inseparable in lived cogni-
question is: What is the relationship between tion” (Varela et al., 1991, p. 173). An artificial
computational states (e.g., manipulation of separation of cognition from the other ele-
mental representations) and a person’s experi- ments would render the given cognitive con-
ence of the real-life referent of the computa- struct a “mental ghost ” once again. We can
tional state? How do we go from having a exemplify the inseparability of cognition and
representation of a person’s intention to expe- action through Held and Hein’s (Held, 1965;
riencing that intention by reacting to it in a Held & Hein, 1963) classic studies of percep-
certain way? In the computer world, we do tual guidance of action. They raised kittens in
know where the meaning of the computational the dark and exposed them to light only under
algorithms comes from, namely, the program- controlled conditions. One group of kittens
mer. But how do mental representations ac- was allowed to move around normally, but
quire meaning to a developing child? In each of them was harnessed to a carriage that
autism, individuals often acquire a large num- contained a second group of kittens. While the
ber of symbols and symbolic computations groups shared the same visual experience,
that are devoid of shared meaning with others; the second group was entirely passive. When
that is, the symbols do not have the meaning to the kittens were released after a few weeks of
them that they have to typical children. Exam- this treatment, members of the first group (the
ples are hyperlexia (reading decoding skills one that moved around) behaved normally,
go unaccompanied by reading comprehension; whereas members of the second group (the one
Grigorenko et al., 2002), echolalia and that was passively carried by the others) be-
echopraxia (echoing of sounds or mimicry of haved as blind, bumping into objects and
movements; Prizant & Duchan, 1981; Rogers, falling off edges. These experiments illustrate
1999), metaphoric language (e.g., neologisms, the point that meaningful cognition of objects
words used in idiosyncratic ways; Lord & (i.e., the way we see them and adjust to them)
Paul, 1997), and prompt-dependent social ges- cannot be formed by means of visual extrac-
tures, routines, or scripts (e.g., waving bye-bye tion alone; rather, there is a need for percep-
without eye contact, staring when requested to tual processes to be actively linked with action
make eye contact), among many others. While to guide further action on these objects. Stud-
it is difficult to conceive of a dissociation be- ies of adaptation of disarranged hand-eye
tween knowing a symbol and acting on it (e.g., coordination in humans (Held & Hein, 1958),
knowing the meaning of the pointing gesture tactile vision substitution in blind humans
and spontaneously turning the head when (Bach-y-Rita, 1983), and neural coding of
somebody is pointing somewhere), this actu- body schema in primates (Iriki, Tanaka, &
ally happens in autism, as shown in Figure Iwamura, 1996), among others (see Iacoboni,
26.3 and the other earlier examples. We know 2000b), support this point. A striking example
that children with autism can learn associa- is provided in a study (Aglioti, Smania, Man-
tively (e.g., a symbol becomes paired with a fredi, & Berlucchi, 1996) of a patient with
referent). This happens, for example, in vocab- right brain damage who denied the ownership
ulary instruction using simple behavioral tech- of her left hand and of objects that were worn
niques. But one of the big challenges for these by her left hand (e.g., rings). When the same
children is often to pair a symbol with the objects were worn by the right hand, the pa-
adaptive action subsumed by the symbol tient recognized them as her own. In infancy
(Wetherby, Prizant, & Schuler, 2000). research, a wide range of phenomena, from
In the EM approach, symbols or cognition haptic and depth perception (E. W. Bushnell
in general have meaning to the child using & Boudreau, 1993) to Piagetian milestones
them because they are “embodied actions” (Thelen, Schoener, Scheier, & Smith, 2001),
(Clark, 1999; Johnson, 1987), meaning that have begun to characterize developmental
“cognition depends upon the experiences that skills as “perception-for-action” systems,
692 Theoretical Perspectives

while neuroimaging studies have shown over- or a lethal predator). A central example of such
lapping brain circuitry subserving action ob- systems is the ability to perceive certain pat-
servation and action generation (Blakemore & terns of movement as biological motion. This
Decety, 2001). system allows humans, as well as other
Perception-for-action systems are particu- species, to discern motion of biological forms
larly relevant to a discussion of social adapta- from motion occurring in the inanimate envi-
tion. Consider the skill of imitation, one of the ronment. In the wild, an animal’s survival
major deficits in autism (Rogers, 1999). It is would depend on its ability to detect approach-
interesting that while children with autism ing predators and predict their future actions.
have great difficulty in learning through imi- In humans, this system has been linked to the
tation, they do exhibit a great deal of mirror- emergence of the capacity to attribute inten-
ing or copying behaviors, both vocally (e.g., tions to others (Frith & Frith, 1999). The
echoing what other people say) and motori- study of biological motion has traditionally
cally (e.g., making the same gesture as another used Johansson’s (1994) paradigm of human
person), which, however, are typically devoid motion display. In his work, the motion of the
of the function that these behaviors serve to living body is represented by a few bright
typical people displaying them. One hypothe- spots describing the motions of the main
sis derived from the EM approach would pre- joints. In this fashion, the motion pattern is
dict that this curious discrepancy originates dissociated from the form of people’s bodies.
from the aspect of the typical person’s action The moving presentation of this set of bright
that is most salient in the child’s perception. spots evokes a compelling impression of basic
Whereas typical children may see a waving human movements (e.g., walking, running,
gesture as a motion embedded in the act of dancing) as well as of social movements (e.g.,
communication or emotional exchange, chil- approaching, fighting, embracing). Figure 26.7
dren with autism may dissociate the motion illustrates a series of static images of the
from the social context, focusing on the salient human form rendered as point-light anima-
physical facts and thus repeating the gesture in tions. The phenomenon studied by Johansson,
a mechanical fashion, not unlike what a typical however, can be fully appreciated only when
child might do in a game of imitating meaning- the display is set in motion.
less gestures or what a neonate might do when Using this paradigm, several studies have
protruding his or her tongue in response to documented adults’ abilities to attribute gen-
seeing an adult doing so (Meltzoff & Moore, der, emotions, and even personality features to
1977). This hypothesis originates from the no- these moving dots (Dittrich, Troscianko, Lea,
tion that while perception for action may & Morgan, 1996; Koslowski & Cutting, 1978).
occur in the absence of social engagement Even 3-month-old infants are able to discrimi-
(e.g., in neonates), in typical infants sometime nate between the moving dots depicting a
around the middle of their second year of life, walking person and the same dot display mov-
imitation is much more likely to serve social ing randomly (Fox & McDaniel, 1982). The
engagement and social learning than to occur presence of this ability at such a young age, as
outside the realm of social interaction, as in well as its presence in other species including
autism. Supporting this hypothesis is a series monkeys (Oram & Perret, 1994) and birds
of studies in which, for example, 18-month-old (Regolin, Tommasi, & Vallortigara, 2000), and
infants were exposed to a human or a mechani- the demonstrated singularity of biological mo-
cal device attempting to perform various ac- tion relative to other forms of motion from
tions. The children imitated the action when it the perspective of the visual system (Neri,
was performed by the human model, but not
when it was performed by the mechanical de-
vice (Meltzoff, 1995).
Perception-for-action systems are of par-
ticular interest in the context of survival abili- Figure 26.7 Series of static images of the human
ties (e.g., responding to a threatening person form rendered as point-light displays.
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 693

Morrone, & Burr, 1998) suggest that this is a side-up animation matching the sound effects
highly conserved and unique system that of the social interaction, we were able to test
makes possible the recognition of movements the child’s ability to impose mental represen-
of others in order to move toward or away from tations of human movement interactions on the
them. Several neuroimaging studies have sin- ambiguous visual stimuli. This paradigm is il-
gled out the superior temporal sulcus as an im- lustrated in Figure 26.8. Our preliminary data
portant structure involved in the perception of for eleven 2-year-old toddlers, 5 with a diagno-
biological motion (Grezes et al., 2001; Gross- sis of autism and 6 typical children, are given
man et al., 2000), a region also associated with in Figure 26.9. Overall, the typically develop-
basic survival reactions such as evaluating fa- ing toddlers demonstrated a marked prefer-
cial expressions and/or direction of eye gaze ence for the right-side-up figure (83% of total
(Puce, Allison, Benton, Gore, & McCarthy,
1998). A PET study attempting to separate de-
contextualized human motions (point-light
displays depicting a hand bringing a cup to a
person’s mouth) from what can be seen as a
more naturalistic human motion (a person
dancing) showed that the perception of the lat-
ter also implicated limbic structures such as , ,
accompanying audio: pat
the amygdala (Bonda, Petrides, Istry, &
Evans, 1996). This finding is consistent with a
perception-for-action system that not only per-
ceives to act but also is embedded in an ap-
proach/withdrawal, affective-based context
(Gaffan, Gaffan, & Harrison, 1988). audio:
,
a
,
Given the fundamental and adaptive nature
of perception of biological motion, we would
expect this system to be intact in even very
disabled children. One study has shown the
system to be intact in children with profound
spatial deficits and a degree of mental retarda- , ,
audio: cake
tion (Jordan, Reiss, Hoffman, & Landau,
2002). In contrast, our own preliminary data
suggest that this system may be compromised
in young children with autism. We used Jo-
hansson point-light displays to depict a series
, ,
of social approaches that are part of typical ex- audio: pat
periences of young children (e.g., an animated
adult trying to attract the attention of a young
toddler, “pat-a-cake,” “peek-a-boo”). Scenes
were presented in two formats simultaneously,
one on each of the two horizontal halves of a
audio:
, a,
computer screen. The scenes were identical
except that one was right side up and the other
was upside down. The child heard the corre-
sponding sound effects of that social scene
(e.g., the verbal approach of an adult). The ex-
periment followed a visual preference para- , ,
audio: cake
digm in which the child looked at one of the
two scenes presented. By requiring the child to Figure 26.8 Cross-modal matching task with social
choose between an upside-down and a right- animation stimuli.
694 Theoretical Perspectives

83%
(a)
56%
44%

17%
15 saccades
Figure 26.9 Percent of total viewing time spent on
upright versus inverted figures. Black bars; toddlers 23 saccades
with autism; white bars: typically developing toddlers.

viewing time versus 17% for the upside-down


display), while the toddlers with autism
showed a pattern closer to a random choice
(56% versus 44%). We also analyzed initial
fixations and final fixations (defined by the
figure the child was focusing on at the end of
the animation) as a rudimentary look at how (b)

understanding of the animation’s content Figure 26.10 Initial and final fixation data and num-
might progress during viewing and recorded ber of saccades between upright and inverted figures.
the number of times the toddlers with autism (a) Initial fixation: toddlers with autism 40% upright,
60% inverted: typically developing toddlers 50% up-
shifted their focus from the upright to the in-
right, 50% inverted. ( b) Final fixation: toddlers with
verted figure, relative to typically developing autism 50% upright, 50% inverted; typically develop-
controls. These results are depicted graphi- ing toddlers 79% upright, 21% inverted. Number of
cally in Figure 26.10. While typically develop- saccades between upright and inverted figures; tod-
ing toddlers and toddlers with autism both dlers with autism 23 saccades min-1, typically develop-
exhibited initial fixations at chance or near- ing toddlers 15 saccades min-1.
chance levels, the typically developing infants
were focused on the upright figure at the end
of more than three-fourths of all trials, while 1991). This stance is justified when a given
the toddlers with autism remained at chance task is explicit and fully defined. However, in
level. Of similar interest are group differences naturalistic situations, there are important
in the pattern of shifting between the upright temporal constraints in social adaptation be-
and inverted figures. Toddlers with autism cause failure to detect an important but fleet-
shifted more frequently than typically devel- ing social cue or a failure to detect temporal
oping toddlers, a trend suggestive of increased relationships between two social cues may
difficulty in adequately understanding either lead to partial or even misleading comprehen-
of the two displays. If corroborated in larger sion of the situation, which may in turn lead to
studies, this finding would point to a major ineffective adjustment to the situation. For ex-
disruption in a highly conserved skill that is ample, if the viewer of a scene fails to monitor
thought to be a core ability underlying social a nonspeaker in a social scene who is clearly
engagement and, subsequently, the capacity to embarrassed by what another person is saying,
attribute intentionality to others. the viewer is unlikely to correctly identify the
meaning of that situation (Klin et al., 2002a).
TEMPORAL CONSTRAINTS ON In this way, the EM approach sees social adap-
MODELS OF SOCIAL ADAPTATION tation along the same principles currently
being considered in research of “embodied
Computational models of the mind place less vision” (Churchland, Ramachandran, & Se-
emphasis on the temporal unfolding of the cog- jnowski, 1994). This view holds that the task of
nitive processes involved in a task (Newell, the visual system is not to generate exhaustive
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 695

mental models of a veridical surrounding envi- cial adaptive reactions to split-second environ-
ronment but to use visual information to per- mental demands with moment-by-moment dis-
form real-time, real-life adaptive reactions. regard of the vast majority of the available
Rather than creating an inner mirror of the visual stimulation. Such a child is ready to play
outside world to formulate problems and then the social game. For individuals with autism,
to solve them ahead of acting on them, vision however, the topology of salience, defined as
is seen as the active retrieval of useful infor- the “ foveal elicitation” of socially relevant
mation as it is needed from the constantly stimuli (as exemplified in our eye-tracking il-
present and complex visual environment. From lustrations and in studies of preferential atten-
the organism’s adaptive perspective, the tion to social versus nonsocial entities; see
topology of salience of this visual tapestry, earlier discussion), is much flatter. If viewed in
from light reflections to carpet patterning, to this light, the social worlds enacted by individ-
furniture and clothing, to mouths and eyes, is uals with autism and by their typical peers may
far from flat. We would be overwhelmed and be strikingly different.
paralyzed by its richness if we were to start
from a position of equal salience to every as- SOCIAL COGNITION AS
pect of what is available to be visually in- SOCIAL ACTION
spected. Rather, we actively retrieve aspects
of the visual environment that are essential for The radical assumption made in the EM ap-
quick, adaptive actions by foveating on se- proach is that mental representations as de-
quential locations where we expect to find scribed in computational models of the mind
them. These expectations are generated by a are proxies for the actions that generated them
brain system dedicated to salience (a lion en- and for which they stand (Lakoff & Johnson,
tering the room is more important than the 1999; Thelen & Smith, 1994; Varela et al.,
light switch next to the door) and an ever more 1991). This counterintuitive view can be
complex (going from infancy to adulthood) traced back to Mead’s (1924) account of the
understanding of the context of the situation, social origins of mind. Mead saw the emer-
the so-called top-down approach to vision gence of mind as the capacity of an individual
(Engel et al., 2001). to make a “gesture” (e.g., bodily sign, vocal
A pertinent example of this view of vision sound) that means to the other person seeing
is Clark’s (1999, p. 346) analysis of a baseball or hearing it the same as for the person making
game in which an outfielder positions himself it. The meaning of the gesture, however, is in
or herself to catch a fly ball: the reaction of the other. A gesture used in
this way becomes a symbol, that is, something
It used to be thought that this problem required that stands for the predicted reaction of the
complex calculations of the arc, acceleration and other person. Once a child has such a symbolic
distance of the ball. More recent work, however, gesture, he or she can then uphold it as a repre-
suggests a computationally simpler strategy
sentation for the reaction of the social partner,
(McBeath, Shaffer, & Kaiser, 1995). Put simply,
thus being able to take a step back from the
the fielder continually adjusts his or her run so that
the ball never seems to curve toward the ground, but immediate experience and then to contemplate
instead appears to move in a straight line in his or alternatives of action using such symbols as
her visual field. By maintaining this strategy, the proxies for real actions. In the EM approach,
fielder should be guaranteed to arrive in the right the fact that the emergence and evolution of a
place at the right time to catch the ball. (p. 346) symbol are tied to actions of adaptation, which
in turn are immersed in a context of so-
Piaget (1973) provided similar examples from matosensory experiences, salience, and per-
children’s play, and Zajonc (1980) provided ceptually guided actions, makes the symbol a
similar examples from intersubjective adapta- proxy for these elements of the action. When
tion. Consistent with these examples, the EM we uphold and manipulate symbols in our
approach considers the “social game” to be not mind, therefore, we are also evoking a network
unlike the outfielder’s effort. A typical toddler of experiences resulting from a life history of
entering a playroom pursues a sequence of so- actions associated with that symbol.
696 Theoretical Perspectives

This view connecting social cognition with tation of our face recognition results in
social action is of help in our attempt to ex- autism. The FG was not selectively activated
plore possible reasons that accomplishments in when individuals with autism were looking at
social reasoning in individuals with autism are faces because they were not experts on faces.
not accompanied by commensurate success in In contrast, typically developing individuals
social action. Consider an example from re- have a lifetime to develop this expertise, a re-
search on face perception. While face recogni- sult of a very large number of recurrent experi-
tion deficits are very pronounced in young ences of focusing on and acting on other
children with autism (Klin et al., 1999), the people’s faces beginning in very early infancy.
size of this deficit is much smaller in older and As previously described, faces have little
more cognitively able adolescents (Celani, salience to young children with autism and
Battacchi, & Arcidiacono, 1999). The possibil- would thus represent a much less frequent tar-
ity that older individuals might perform such get of recurrent actions necessary to produce
tasks using atypical strategies relative to their expertise.
peers was investigated in our recent fMRI If this interpretation is correct, were indi-
study of face recognition in autism (Schultz viduals with autism asked to perform a visual
et al., 2000) in which normative-IQ individuals recognition task using stimuli on which they
with autism and controls were presented with had expertise, we might observe FG activa-
face versus object recognition tasks. In con- tion. Preliminary results supportive of this
trast to controls for whom face processing was suggestion were obtained in an fMRI study of
associated with fusiform gyrus (FG) activa- an individual with autism whose expertise
tion, in individuals with autism, face process- area is Digimon characters (a large series of
ing was associated with activation in inferior cartoon figures; Grelotti et al., in press).
temporal gyrus (ITG) structures, an activation Of interest, fMRI activations for Digimon
pattern that was obtained for controls when characters in this individual with autism also
they were processing objects. These results in- included the amygdala, suggesting salience-
dicated that individuals with autism did not rely driven rewards associated with the characters.
on the normal neural substrate during face Results such as these are beginning to delin-
perception (Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, eate a developmental profile of functional
1997) but rather engaged brain areas that were brain maturation in autism in which hardwired
more important to nonface, object processing social salience systems are derailed from very
(Haxby et al., 1999). In other words, they failed early on, following a path marked by seeking
to treat faces as a special form of visual stimu- physical entities (not people) and repeatedly
lus, treating them instead as ordinary objects. enacting them and thus neglecting social expe-
It would be tempting from these results to riences (Klin et al., 2002a). This hypothesis is
hypothesize that a circumscribed area of the consistent with the notion of functional brain
brain—namely, the FG—and the mechanism it development as “an activity-dependent pro-
represents—namely, perception of face iden- cess” that emphasizes the infancy period as a
tity—were causally related to autism. Given window of maximal plasticity (Johnson,
the centrality of face perception in interper- 2001). An interesting line of research support-
sonal interactions, this would be a plausible ing this hypothesis is the case of people with a
theory of autism. However, other recent stud- period of visual deprivation early in postnatal
ies (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier, Tarr, life due to bilateral congenital cataracts. Al-
Anderson, Skudlarski, & Gore, 1999) have though early surgical correction was associ-
suggested that the FG is not necessarily the ated with rapid improvement of visual acuity,
brain site for face recognition, appearing in- deficits in configural processing of faces re-
stead to be a site associated with visual exper- mained even after many years postsurgery
tise, so that when a person becomes an expert (Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent,
on a given object category (e.g., Persian car- 2001; Maurer, Lewis, Brent, & Levin, 1999).
pets), selective activation of the FG occurs Configural processing of a class of visual
when the person is looking at an instance of stimuli (e.g., faces) represents a developmen-
that object. This notion suggests a reinterpre- tal shift from processing an object from its
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 697

parts to processing objects in a Gestalt man- of social cues including language exchange,
ner (Tanaka, Kay, Grinnell, Stansfield, & voice/prosody cues, facial and bodily gestures,
Szechter, 1998), which, in turn, is a mark of posture, and body movements, among many
the acquisition of perceptual expertise (Dia- others. These cues are embedded in a complex
mond & Carey, 1986; Gauthier & Nelson, visual and auditory setting, with some physical
2001). Thus, studies of early visual depriva- stimuli being relevant to the social events (i.e.,
tion seem to highlight the effects of reduced representing specific social contexts—e.g., a
early visual enactment of a class of visual cafeteria—or specific “props”—e.g., a cos-
stimuli on later, automatic, and more efficient tume worn by one of the students) and other
ways of processing that class of stimuli. physical stimuli being entirely irrelevant (e.g.,
Returning to the fMRI example in which in- light switches or fixtures, number of doors,
dividuals with autism treated faces as objects detailing in the walls). Such situations are
(Schultz et al., 2000), it is of considerable in- challenging because there is hardly any aspect
terest that all participants could perform rela- of the social event that is explicitly defined.
tively well on the behavioral task of face Faced with a highly complex and ambiguous
recognition. They could correctly match faces, social display that demands a reaction (e.g.,
albeit using a strategy that differed markedly where to sit down, how to insert themselves in
from controls. Thus, an analysis of results on an unfolding social event), they need to make
the behavioral task by itself would have un- sense of what they see and hear by imposing
veiled no significant differences between the social meaning onto essential social aspects of
two groups. Yet, we might consider the behav- the situation (e.g., facial expressions) while ig-
ioral impact of failing to process faces as a noring irrelevant stimuli (e.g., light fixtures).
special class of objects. Most people are able To study how difficult it might be for indi-
to recognize possibly thousands of faces very viduals to make sense of such a situation, we
quickly, whereas their ability to recognize, for can use an experimental metaphor that mea-
example, pieces of luggage is much more lim- sures a person’s spontaneous tendency to im-
ited. Thus, some of us are likely to mistake our pose social meaning on ambiguous visual
bags when coming to pick them up from a lug- stimuli. More specifically, it measures how
gage carousel at the airport, but we are very salient the social meaning of an array of am-
unlikely to mistake our mother-in-law rushing biguous visual stimuli is to a viewer and how
to greet us from the surrounding crowd. socially relevant the viewer’s thinking is when
The point illustrated in this example is the making an effort to make sense of the presented
importance of developmental and contextual visual stimuli. The paradigm involves the pre-
aspects of social development in making social- sentation of a classic animation in which geo-
cognitive accomplishments into tools of social metric shapes move and act like humans
action. Temporal constraints on social adapta- (Heider & Simmel, 1944; Figure 26.11). Typi-
tion require skills to be displayed sponta- cal viewers immediately recognize the social
neously and quickly, without the need for an nature of the cartoon and provide narratives
explicit translation of the requirements to be
met in a given social task. There is a need to
seek socially relevant information and to
maintain online, as it were, a continuous pro-
cess of imposing social meaning to what is
seen. This comes easily and effortlessly to
typical individuals. In contrast, the most chal-
lenging task in the daily lives of individuals
with autism involves the need to adjust to com-
monplace, naturalistic social situations. Con-
sider, for example, an adolescent with autism
entering a high school cafeteria. There is usu-
ally an array of interrelated social events tak- Figure 26.11 Screen shot showing cast of characters
ing place, each one consisting of a vast amount from Heider and Simmel’s (1944) cartoon.
698 Theoretical Perspectives

that include a number of social attributions in- mental states to others, the act of adjusting to
volving relationships portrayed there (e.g., social demands imbues social-cognitive ac-
being a bully, being a friend), the meaning of complishments with their functional value. It
specific actions (e.g., trapping, protecting), is in this light that the preceding examples sug-
and attributions of mental states (e.g., being gest that in autism there is a breakdown in the
shy, thinking, being surprised) to the charac- process through which social-cognitive skills
ters. In contrast, cognitively able adolescents and social action become inseparable.
and adults with autism have great difficulty in
doing so. In one study (Klin, 2000), they were, CONCLUSION
on average, able to recognize only one-fourth
of the social elements deemed essential to un- This chapter began with an intriguing puzzle
derstanding the plot of the story. A large pro- posed by normative-IQ individuals with
portion of them limited their narratives to autism: How can they learn so much about the
faithful descriptions of the geometric events world yet still be unable to translate this
depicted in the cartoon but without any social knowledge into real-life, social adaptive ac-
attributions. This was surprising considering tions? A framework different from the prevail-
that an inclusionary condition in this study re- ing computational models of social-cognitive
quired participants to pass a relatively ad- development was offered—enactive mind—as
vanced social reasoning task (a second-order a way of exploring this puzzle. This framework
theory of mind task; Tager-Flusberg & Sulli- is based on the emerging embodied cognitive
van, 1994). Thus, these individuals’ ability to neuroscience. EM views cognition as embed-
solve explicit social-cognitive problems was no ded in experiences resulting from a body’s ac-
assurance that they would use these skills tions on salient aspects of its surrounding
spontaneously. Some were unable to make any environment. Social cognition is seen as the
social attribution at all. Yet, such spontaneous experiences associated with a special form of
attributions of intentionality to these geomet- action, namely, social interaction. These expe-
ric cartoons have been documented in infants riences are tools of social adaptation that can
(Gergely, Nadasdy, Csibra, & Biro, 1995) and be abstracted in the form of symbols and used
even primates (Uller & Nichols, 2000). Some to reason about social phenomena, although
of the individuals with autism did, however, they retain their direct connection to the com-
make a meaningful effort to make sense of the posite of enactive experiences that originated
cartoon, but in doing so provided entirely irrel- and shaped them over the lifetime of the child.
evant attributions, explaining the movements In autism, the EM approach proposes the
of the geometric shapes in terms of physical hypothesis that the preceding process is de-
meaning (e.g., magnetic forces), not social railed from its incipience because the typical
meaning. Translated into a task of social ad- overriding salience of social stimuli is not pres-
justment to a naturalistic setting like the high ent. In its place is a range of physical stimuli,
school cafeteria, the results of this study which attracts the child’s selective attention,
would suggest that some of these individuals leading into a path of ever-greater specializa-
might have no access to the social cues (not tion in things rather than people. Individuals
even noticing them), whereas others might with autism are capable of acquiring language
search for causation relationships in the wrong and concepts and even a vast body of informa-
domain, namely, physical rather than social. tion on people. But these tools of thought are
To impose social meaning on an array of vi- acquired outside the realm of active social en-
sual stimuli is an adaptive reaction displayed gagement and the embodied experiences predi-
by typical children from infancy onward at an cated by them. In a way, they possess what is
ever-increasing level of complexity. This spon- typically the rooftop of social development.
taneous skill is cultivated in countless hours of However, this rooftop is freestanding. The con-
recurrent social engagement. From discerning structs and definitions are there, but their foun-
the meaning of facial expressions and detect- dational experiences are not. The EM approach
ing human motion and forms of human action, contends that without the set of embodied so-
to attributing intentionality and elaborate cial-cognitive tools required to produce mo-
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 699

ment-by-moment, social adaptive reactions in Bach-y-Rita, P. (1983). Tactile vision substitution:


naturalistic social situations, social behavior Past and future. International Journal of Neu-
becomes truncated, slow, and inefficient. roscience, 19, 29–36.
A corollary of this hypothesis is that indi- Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that
can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in
viduals with autism learn about people in a
mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cogni-
way that departs from the normative processes tive, 13(5), 513–552.
of social development. The fact that cogni- Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay
tively able individuals with autism are able to on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge,
demonstrate so much social-cognitive under- MA: MIT Press.
standing in some situations is as interesting as Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., &
the fact that they fail to make use of these Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test
skills in other situations. To study possible of theory of mind: Evidence from very high
compensatory paths and the degrees to which functioning adults with autism or Asperger
syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and
they help these individuals to achieve more in-
Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.
dependence is as important a research en- Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acqui-
deavor as to document their social-cognitive sition of pragmatics. New York: Academic
failures. But to do so, there will be a need to go Press.
beyond results on explicit tasks. There will be Bates, E. (1979). On the evolution and develop-
a need to both explore more in depth the atyp- ment of symbols. In E. Bates (Ed.), The
ical processes used by these individuals to emergence of symbols: Cognition and commu-
perform explicit tasks and increase our arsenal nication in infancy (pp. 1–32). New York:
of methodologies capable of studying social Academic Press.
Blakemore, S.-J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the
adaptation in more naturalistic settings (Klin
perception of action to the understanding of
et al., 2002a). intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2,
561–567.
Cross-References Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Ostry, D., & Evans, A.
(1996). Specific involvement of human pari-
A review of social development is provided in etal systems and the amygdala in the percep-
Chapter 11; relevant developmental concepts tion of biological motion. Journal of
are reviewed in Chapters 12, 14, and 15; and Neuroscience, 15(1), 3737–3744.
related theoretical perspective is described in Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face
recognition in relatively able autistic children.
Chapters 22 to 25.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
33(5), 843–859.
REFERENCES Bowler, D. M. (1992). “ Theory of Mind” in As-
perger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychol-
Adrien, J., Faure, M., Perrot, A., Hameury, L., ogy and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877–893.
Garreau, B., Barthelemy, C., et al. (1991). Bryant, P. E. (1991). Face to face with babies. Na-
Autism and family home movies: Preliminary ture, 354, 19.
findings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Bushnell, E. W., & Boudreau, J. P. (1993). Motor
Disorders, 21, 43–49. development and the mind: The potential role
Adrien, J., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., Perot, A., of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects
Hameury, L., Larmande, C., et al. (1993). of perceptual development. Child Develop-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and ment, 64(4), 1005–1021.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 617–625. Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989).
Aglioti, S., Smania, N., Manfredi, M., & Berlucchi, Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face.
G. (1996). Disownership of left hand and ob- British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
jects related to it in a patient with right brain 7, 3–15.
damage. NeuroReport, 8, 293–296. Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L.
Alegria, J., & Noirot, E. (1978). Neonate orienta- (1999). The understanding of the emotional
tion behavior towards human voice. Interna- meaning of facial expressions in people with
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 1, autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
291–312. Disorders, 29, 57–66.
700 Theoretical Perspectives

Churchland, P. S., Ramachandran, V. S., & Se- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting
jnowski, T. J. (1994). A critique of pure vision. minds: A biological basis. Science, 286(5445),
In C. Koch & J. L. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale 1692–1695.
neuronal theories of the brain: Computational Gaffan, E. A., Gaffan, D., & Harrison, S. (1988).
neuroscience (pp. 23–60). Cambridge, MA: Disconnection of the amygdala from visual as-
MIT Press. sociation cortex impairs visual-reward associ-
Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? ation learning in monkeys. Journal of
Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 345–351. Neuroscience, 8, 3144–3150.
Dahlgren, S. O., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). The- Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: A his-
ory of mind in non-retarded children with tory of the cognitive revolution (chapter 5).
autism and Asperger’s syndrome. A research New York: Basic Books.
note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- Gauthier, I., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). The develop-
try, 37, 759–763. ment of face expertise. Current Opinion in
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Neurobiology, 11(2), 219–224.
Body and emotion in the making of conscious- Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. (1997). Becoming a
ness. New York: Harcourt. “greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for
Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagi- face recognition. Vision Research, 37,
otides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. 1673–1681.
(2002). Neural correlates of face and object Gauthier, I., Tarr, M., Anderson, A., Skudlarski,
recognition in young children with autism P., & Gore, J. (1999). Activation of the middle
spectrum disorder, developmental delay and fusiform face area increases with expertise in
typical development. Child Development, recognizing novel objects. Nature Neuro-
73(3), 700–717. science, 2, 568–573.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S.
J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12
fail to orient to naturally occurring social months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.
stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Gibson, J. J. (1963). The useful dimension of sensi-
Disorders, 28(6), 479–485. tivity. American Psychologist, 18, 1–15.
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Gauthier,
are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal I., Skudlarski, P., Cohen, D. J., et al. (in
of Experimental Psychology, 115, 107–117. press). FMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus
Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E., & Mor- and amygdala to cartoon characters but not
gan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from faces in a boy with autism. Neuropsychologia.
dynamic point-light displays represented in Grezes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Mar-
dance. Perception, 25, 727–738. tin, C., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2001).
Eimas, P., Siqueland, E., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. Does perception of biological motion rely on
(1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, specific brain regions? Neuroimage, 13(5),
171, 303–306. 775–785.
Eisenberg, R. (1979). Stimulus significance as a Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., Pauls, D. L., Senft, R.,
determinant of infant responses to sound. In Hooper, C., & Volkmar, F. R. (2002). A de-
E. B. Thomas (Ed.), Origins of infant’s social scriptive study of hyperlexia in a clinically re-
responsiveness. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ferred sample of children with developmental
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., & Singer, W. (2001). Dy- delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental
namic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony Disorders, 32(1), 3–12.
in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neu- Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens,
roscience, 2, 704–716. D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000).
Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: In- Brain areas involved in perception of biologi-
fant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar cal motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, science, 12(5), 711–720.
64, 657–674. Gusella, J. L., Muir, D. W., & Tronick, E. Z.
Field, T., Vega-Lahar, N., Scafidi, F., & Goldstein, (1988). The effect of manipulating maternal
S. (1986). Effects of maternal unavailability behavior during an interaction on 3- and 6-
on motion-infant interactions. Infant Behavior month olds’ affect and attention. Child Devel-
and Development, 9, 473–478. opment, 59, 1111–1124.
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill,
biological motion by human infants. Science, K. (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have
218, 486–487. an effect on the ability to develop conversa-
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 701

tion in children with autism? Journal of Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biologi-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), cal motion and a model for its analysis. Per-
519–537. ception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1979). Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The
Eye contact and face scanning in early in- bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and rea-
fancy. Science, 198(4319), 853–855. son. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of Johnson, M. (2001). Functional brain development
mind: Understanding of story characters’ in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2,
thoughts and feelings by able autistic, men- 475–483.
tally handicapped, and normal children and Jordan, H., Reiss, J. E., Hoffman, J. E., & Landau,
adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental B. (2002). Intact perception of biological mo-
Disorders, 24(2),: 129–154. tion in the face of profound spatial deficits:
Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The in- Williams syndrome. Psychological Science,
duced affect response: 10-week-old infants’ 13(2), 162–167.
responses to three emotional expressions. De- Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. (1997).
velopmental Psychology, 23, 97–104. The fusiform face area: A module in human
Haxby, J., Ungerleider, L., Clark, V., Schouten, J., extrastriate cortex specialized for face per-
Hoffman, E., & Martin, A. (1999). The effect ception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17,
of face inversion on activity in human neural 4302–4311.
systems for face and object perception. Neu- Klin, A. (1989). Understanding early infantile
ron, 22(1), 189–199. autism: An application of, G. H. Mead’s the-
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental ory of the emergence of mind. L . S. E. Quar-
study of apparent behavior. American Journal terly, 3(4), 336–356.
of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259. Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic Children’s listening
Held, R. (1965). Plasticity in sensory-motor sys- preferences in regard to speech: A possible
tems. Scientific American, 213(5), 84–94. characterization of the symptom of social
Held, R., & Hein, A. (1958). Adaptation of dis- withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Develop-
arranged hand-eye coordination contingent mental Disorders, 21(1), 29–42.
upon re-afferent stimulation. Perceptual and Klin, A. (1992). Listening preferences in regard to
Motor Skills, 8, 87–90. speech in four children with developmental
Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced disabilities. Journal of Child Psychology and
stimulation in the development of visually Psychiatry, 33(4), 763–769.
guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to am-
Physiological Psychology, 56(5), 872–876. biguous visual stimuli in higher functioning
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s autism and Asperger syndrome: The social at-
in a face? The case of autism. British Journal tribution task. Journal of Child Psychology
of Psychology, 79, 441–453. and Psychiatry, 41, 831–846.
Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., &
Teaching children with autism to mind-read. Cohen, D. (2002a). Defining and quantifying
New York: Wiley. the social phenotype in autism. American
Iacoboni, M. (2000a). Attention and sensorimotor Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895–908.
integration: Mapping the embodied mind. In Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., &
A. W. Toga & J. C. Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain Cohen, D. J. (2002b). Visual fixation patterns
mapping: The systems (pp. 463–490). San during viewing of naturalistic social situations
Diego, CA: Academic Press. as predictors of social competence in individu-
Iacoboni, M. (2000b). Mapping human cognition: als with autism. Archives of General Psychia-
Thinking, numerical abilities, theory of try, 59(9), 809–816.
mind, consciousness. In A. W. Toga & J. C. Klin, A., Schultz, R., & Cohen, D. (2000). Theory
Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain mapping: The sys- of mind in action: Developmental perspec-
tems (pp. 523–534). San Diego, CA: Aca- tives on social neuroscience. In S. Baron-
demic Press. Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen
Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Cod- (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspec-
ing of modified body schema during tool use tives from developmental neuroscience (2nd
by macaque postcentral neurons. NeuroReport, ed., pp. 357–388). Oxford, England: Oxford
7, 2325–2330. University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the com- Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti,
putational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A
702 Theoretical Perspectives

normed study of face recognition in autism pathology. In L. Glidden (Ed.), International


and related disorders. Journal of Autism and review of research in mental retardation (Vols.
Developmental Disorders, 29(6), 497–507. 2–3, pp. 141–168). New York: Academic
Koslowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1978). Recog- Press.
nizing the sex of a walker from point-lights Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical
mounted on ankles: Some second thoughts. implications of joint attention deficits in
Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 459. autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1,
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the 173–183.
f lesh. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Nadel, J., Croue, S., Mattlinger, M.-J., Canet, P.,
Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An ap- Hudelot, C., Lecuyer, C., et al. (2000). Do
proach to the study of autism. Journal of Child children with autism have expectancies about
Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268. the social behavior of unfamiliar people? A
LeDoux, J. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains be- pilot study using the still face paradigm.
come who we are. New York: Viking Penguin. Autism, 4(2), 133–146.
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Neisser, U. (1997). The future of cognitive science:
Brent, H. P. (2001). Early visual experience An ecological approach. In D. M. Johnson &
and face processing. Nature, 410, 890. C. E. Erneling (Eds.), The future of the cogni-
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretence and representation: The tive revolution (pp. 247–260). New York: Ox-
origins of “ theory of mind.” Psychological Re- ford University Press.
view, 94, 412–426. Neri, P., Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (1998). See-
Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year olds re- ing biological motion. Nature, 395(6705),
ferred for possible autism. Journal of Child 894–896.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(8), 1365–1382. Newell, A. (1991). Unified theories of cognition.
Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and commu- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
nication in autism. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar Oram, M. W., & Perrett, D. I. (1994). Response of
(Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive de- anterior superior temporal polysensory (STPa)
velopmental disorders (pp. 195–225). New neurons to “ biological motion” stimuli. Jour-
York: Wiley. nal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(2), 99–116.
Markman, A. N., & Dietrich, E. (2000). Extending Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recogni-
the classical view of representation. Trends in tion of children with autism: A study of first
Cognitive Science, 4, 470–475. birthday home video tapes. Journal of Autism
Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. G. (1973). De and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.
máquinas y seres vivos. Santiago, Chile: Edito- Ozonoff, S., & J. N. Miller. (1995). Teaching the-
rial Universitaria. ory of mind: A new approach to social skills
Maurer, D., Lewis, T. L., Brent, H. P., & Levin, training for individuals with autism. Journal of
A. V. (1999). Rapid improvement in the acuity Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(4),
of infants after visual input. Science, 286, 415–433.
108–110. Piaget, J. (1973). The affective unconscious and the
McBeath, M., Shaffer, D., & Kaiser, M. (1995). cognitive unconscious. Journal of the American
How baseball outfielders determine where to Psychoanalytic Association, 21, 249–261.
run to catch f ly balls. Science, 268, 569–573. Prizant, B., & Duchan, J. (1981). The functions of
Mead, G. H. (1924). Mind, self, and society. immediate echolalia in autistic children. Jour-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. nal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(3),
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the inten- 241–249.
tion of others: Re-enactment of intended acts Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., & Mc-
by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psy- Carthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation
chology, 31, 838–850. in humans viewing eye and mouth movements.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation Journal of Neuroscience, 18(6), 2188–2199.
of facial and manual gestures by human Regolin, L., Tommasi, L., & Vallortigara, G.
neonates. Science, 198, 75–78. (2000). Visual perception of biological motion
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of per- in newly hatched chicks as revealed by an im-
ception. London: Routledge & K. Paul. printing procedure. Animal Cognition, 3(1),
Mills, M., & Melhuish, E. (1974). Recognition of 53–60.
mother’s voice in early infancy. Nature, 252, Rogers, S. (1999). An examination of the imitation
123–124. deficits in autism. In J. Nadel & G. Butter-
Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (2000). Neural plasticity, worth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy: Cambridge
joint attention and autistic developmental studies in cognitive perceptual development
The Enactive Mind—From Actions to Cognition: Lessons from Autism 703

(pp. 254–283). New York: Cambridge Univer- American Academy of Child and Adolescent
sity Press. Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J., & Shea, E. (1986). The
Anderson, A., Volkmar, F. R., et al. (2000). transfer of affect between mothers and in-
Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity fants. In T. B. Brazelton & M. W. Yogman
among individuals with autism and Asperger (Eds.), Af fective development in infancy
syndrome during face discrimination. Archives (pp. 11–25). Norwood, NL: Ablex.
of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–340. Uller, C., & Nichols, S. (2000). Goal attribution in
Searle, J. (1980). Speech act theory and pragmatics. chimpanzees. Cognition, 76, B27–34.
Boston: D. Reidel. Van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N.,
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). & van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of
Vineland adaptive behavior scales, expanded children with pervasive developmental disor-
edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance der toward human faces: A fixation time
Service. study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
Symons, L. A., Hains, S. M. J., & Muir, D. W. atry, 43(4), 1–11.
(1998). Look at me: Five-month-old infants’ Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The
sensitivity to very small deviations in eye- embodied mind: Cognitive science and human
gaze during social interactions. Infant Behav- experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
ior and Development, 21(3), 531–536. Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal perception of ex-
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). A sec- pressive behaviors by human infants. Journal of
ond look at second-order belief attribution in Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 514–535.
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Weeks, S., & Hobson, R. (1987). The salience of
Disorders, 24(5), 577–586. facial expression for autistic children. Journal
Tanaka, J. W., Kay, J. B., Grinnell, E., Stansfield, of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(1),
B., & Szechter, L. (1998). Face recognition in 137–151.
young children: When the whole is greater Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, H.
than the sum of its parts. Visual Cognition, (2000). Recognition of autism spectrum disor-
5(4), 479–496. der before one year of age: A retrospective
Tarabulsy, G. M., Tessier, R., & Kappas, A. (1996). study based on home videotapes. Journal of
Contingency detection and the contingent orga- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2),
nization of behavior in interactions: Implica- 157–162.
tions for socioemotional development in Wetherby, A. M., Prizant, B. M., & Schuler, A. L.
infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 25–41. (2000). Understanding the nature of communi-
Thelen, E., Schoener, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, cation and language impairments. In A. M.
L. B. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A Wetherby & B. M. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spec-
field theory of infant perseverative reaching. trum disorders: A transactional developmental
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 1–86. perspective (pp. 109–142). Baltimore: Paul H.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). A dynamic systems Brookes.
approach to the development of cognition and Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding
action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. computers and cognition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Publications.
Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Prefer-
to entrapment between contradictory messages ences need no inferences. American Psycholo-
in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the gist, 35(2), 151–175.
Author Index

Aamodt-Leeper, G., 433, Adrien, J., 229, 232, 240, Alberto, P. A., 903, 907, Almeida, M. C., 890, 1011,
544 366, 394, 479, 481, 908, 913 1030, 1031, 1130
Aarts, J. P., 503 689, 708, 712, 751, Alberts, A. S., 464 Almes, M. J., 136
Abbassi, V., 460 134 Albin, R. W., 822, 824, Almond, P., 719, 731, 733,
Abbeduto, L., 961 Adrien, K. T., 355, 517 825, 903, 1124 741, 747, 750, 762,
Abe, T., 44, 47, 140 Aerts, F. H., 372, 610, 613, Albus, K. E., 181 1004, 1005, 1006,
Abell, F., 371, 495, 497, 644 Aldred, S., 463 1008, 1199
1305 Afzal, N., 541 Aldridge, M. A., 384, 389 Almqvist, F., 1238
Abichandani, F., 501 Agarwal, N., 479, 481, 482 Alegria, J., 689 Alper, S., 868, 911
Abott, R., 657 Aghajanian, G. K., 456 Aleman, D., 552 Alpern, D. G., 836
Abou-Issa, H., 462 Aglioti, S., 691 Alen, R., 432 Alpern, G. D., 384, 1011
Abraham, A., 463 Agran, M., 868, 912 Alessandri, M., 1004, 1030, Alpern, M., 1126, 1127
Abrahamowicz, M., 535 Aharon, I., 521 1089 Alpert, C. L., 883, 884, 890
Abrams, M., 141 Ahearn, W. H., 540 Alessandri, S. M., 149 Alpert, M., 348
Abrams, N., 544 Ahlsen, G., 51, 185, 540, Alessi, D. R., 543 Als, H., 690
Abramson, R. K., 208, 269, 545, 832 Alessi, N., 27, 189 Altemeier, W., 320, 321,
754 Ahluwalia, J., 634 Alexander, A. L., 494, 521, 355, 707
Accardo, P., 172, 187, 491, Aicardi, J., 126, 135, 154, 522 Althaus, M., 177, 268
492, 499, 537, 540, 535, 594 Alexander, D., 1142 Althouse, R. B., 916
708, 709, 712, 713, Aihara, R., 461, 539 Alexiou, C., 1215, 1217 Altmark, L., 433
724, 782, 841, 1124, Aimi, J., 464 Alinsanski, S., 324 Amador, N., 668, 670
1139, 1310 Ainsworth, M. D. S., 416, Al-Khouri, I., 544 Aman, M., 189, 762, 838,
Achenbach, T. M., 747, 1223 Alkin, M. C., 1034 839, 1102, 1103, 1104,
748, 799 Ainsworth, M. S., 238, 322 Alku, P., 481, 482 1109, 1110, 1111,
Ackerman, A. B., 1142 Aircardi, J., 431 Allan, J., 204, 206, 215 1310
Ackerman, L., 252 Aisemberg, P., 27, 839, Allard, A., 267 Amanullah, S., 324
Acredolo, L. P., 336 847, 1107 Allen, D., 30, 97, 325, 410, Amaral, D. G., 525, 526,
Adams, A., 233, 237, 238, Aitken, K., 319, 325, 673 539, 616, 1306 732, 1306
318, 320, 321, 386, Aitken, M. R. F., 642 Allen, D. A., 30, 132, 169, Ambrose, J., 261, 497, 521,
501, 934 Aizenstein, M. L., 456 175, 586, 1307, 1310 526, 1306
Adams, A. N., 819, 821, Akefeldt, A., 544 Allen, D. M., 369 Ambrosini, P., 762
823 Akerley, M. S., 1069 Allen, D. S., 251 Ameli, R., 642
Adams, A. V., 373 Akerstrom, B., 544 Allen, G., 261, 497, 498, Ament, N., 546
Adams, C., 175, 335, 352 Akiskal, H. S., 266 521, 525, 526, 832, Ames, T., 1092
Adams, L., 385, 836, 847 Akkerhuis, G. W., 455, 456, 1306 Amin, T., 429
Adams, M. J., 886, 1143 458, 459 Allen, J., 227, 229, 288, Aminoff, M. J., 476
Adams, P., 189, 1102, Akshoomoff, N., 367, 368, 293, 335, 645, 713, Amir, R. E., 21, 77, 78,
1104, 1112 439, 477, 478, 500, 714, 741, 962, 1304 130, 136, 138, 431,
Adams, P. B., 739, 1102, 536, 610, 617, 646, Allen, K., 967 432
1107 655, 656, 659, 660, Allen, L., 759 Amnell, G., 545
Adams, P. I., 189 661, 668, 832, 889, Allen, M., 24, 97, 103, 104, Amorosa, H., 177, 536
Adams, T., 889 1014, 1305 107, 369, 374, 375, Amos, P., 151, 153
Adams, W. B., 464 Akyerli, C., 130 479, 480 Amstutz, L., 479
Adamson, L., 235, 318, Al-Adeimi, M., 544 Allen, P. S., 499 Anastasi, A., 822
319, 336, 369, 549, Alaghband-Rad, J., 181 Allen, V., 476 Anastasopoulos, G., 1215
653, 690 Alar, N., 1013, 1022 Allison, D. B., 900 Andelman, A. S., 904
Adkins, W. N., 144 Alarcón, M., 432, 433, 438, Allison, T., 483, 520, 525, Anders, T. F., 29, 503, 542,
Adoh, T. O., 842 439, 754 693 1312
Adolphs, R., 250, 497, 520, Albano, A. M., 181, 182 Allman, J., 670 Andersen, P. H., 435
524, 525, 526, 652, Albers, R. J., 345 Allolio, B., 462 Andersen-Wood, L., 180,
656, 668, 672 Albert, M. L., 608 Almeida, H. C., 553 418

I•1
I•2 Author Index

Anderson, A., 419, 519, Arcas, J., 544 Ashwood, P., 438, 541 Baieli, S., 553
520, 521, 525, 696, Archambault, F. X., 613 Askalan, R., 544 Bailey, A., 5, 7, 8, 14, 25,
697 Archer, L., 93, 95, 110, Asperger, H., 8, 24, 89, 91, 26, 28, 51, 65, 76, 92,
Anderson, A. E., 885, 888 213, 252, 317, 719, 93, 94, 95, 105, 108, 93, 96, 107, 108, 170,
Anderson, A. K., 526 734, 735, 736, 743, 109, 118, 169, 186, 189, 210, 216, 239,
Anderson, A. W., 170, 191, 845 187, 209, 248, 298, 291, 293, 304, 325,
889 Archer, P., 712 305, 348, 374, 584, 341, 346, 347, 355,
Anderson, C., 152, 824 Arcidiacono, L., 413, 696 586, 588, 591, 593, 365, 375, 376, 419,
Anderson, D., 655 Arcus, D., 983 594, 599, 628, 634, 425, 426, 427, 429,
Anderson, G. M., 453, 454, Ardinger, H. H., 555 652, 981, 983, 1247, 432, 434, 436, 437,
455, 456, 457, 458, Ardlie, K. G., 431 1279, 1304, 1306 438, 439, 442, 488,
459, 460, 461, 462, Ariani, F., 551 Assemany, A. E., 1061 489, 500, 534, 536,
1108, 1208 Arick, J., 476, 719, 731, Aston, M., 304 537, 538, 546, 553,
Anderson, I., 1283 733, 741, 747, 750, Atkin, J. F., 555 594, 613, 617, 618,
Anderson, J. L., 1124 762, 1004, 1005, 1006, Attwood, A., 249, 250, 323, 632, 650, 661, 722,
Anderson, J. M., 371 1007, 1008, 1009, 411, 597, 598, 735 732, 740, 754, 763,
Anderson, J. R., 607 1012, 1023, 1199 Attwood, T., 113, 761, 980, 777, 834, 1305
Anderson, L., 189, 462, Arin, D. M., 489 981, 982, 983, 984 Bailey, D., 932
492, 1102, 1104, 1112 Armenteros, J., 839, 1102, Atwell, C. W., 484 Bailey, D. B., 1034, 1035,
Anderson, M., 257, 258, 1107 Audero, M. A., 1232 1038
345, 353, 960 Armony, J. L., 523, 525 Auerbach, J. G., 183 Bailey, D. B., Jr., 546, 547
Anderson, S., 267, 899, Armstrong, D., 139, 140, Auerbach, S., 454 Bailey, J., 123, 492, 494,
929, 936, 938, 949, 143, 145 Auersperg, E., 542, 1112 496, 498, 499, 519,
1036 Arndt, S., 5, 107, 123, 214, Aughton, D., 538 646, 660, 661
Anderson, T., 1177 288, 289, 427, 492, August, G., 30, 427, 546, Bailey, J. N., 536
Anderson-Parente, J., 619 493, 494, 496, 498, 589, 838 Bailey, J. S., 1126, 1127,
Anderson-Wood, L., 436, 499, 519, 646, 660, Augustine, A., 371, 497, 1128
591 661, 741, 1076, 1077 660 Bailey, K. J., 249
Andersson, L., 425 Arnold, L. E., 762, 1102, Auranen, M., 432, 433 Bailey, M. E., 130, 138
Andersson Gronlund, M., 1103, 1104, 1109, Avdi, E., 1058 Bailey, N., 493
544, 553 1110 Averett, R., 539 Bailey, V., 260
Ando, H., 251, 252, 289, Arnold, M., 953 Avikainen, S., 389 Baird, C., 342, 349, 947,
317 Arnold, M. E., 27, 838 Avis, J., 630 961
Andrasik, F., 1143 Arnold, M. S., 1036, 1044, Awad, S., 962 Baird, G., 31, 48, 52, 54,
Andreasen, N., 179, 492, 1045, 1132 Axelrod, B. N., 616 57, 60, 105, 227, 229,
493, 499, 646, 660, Arnold, P., 555, 886 Ayers, K., 432, 433 231, 232, 233, 234,
661 Arnold, S., 76, 79, 80, 82, Ayllon, T., 1125 235, 237, 248, 266,
Andrellos, P. J., 845 1312 Aylward, E., 165, 170, 171, 315, 320, 342, 349,
Andresen, J., 144 Aron, M., 152 172, 191, 371, 493, 369, 384, 410, 432,
Andretta, M., 1093 Aronson, A., 336 496, 497, 498, 501, 434, 438, 439, 536,
Andreu, A. L., 550 Aronson, M., 544 521, 522, 536, 646, 588, 613, 634, 652,
Andrews, C. E., 64 Arora, R. C., 455 651, 660, 755 654, 655, 656, 657,
Andrews, G., 630 Arora, T., 1034 Aylward, G. P., 709, 710, 665, 707, 708, 709,
Andrews, J., 540 Arrieta, A., 553 711 713, 714, 715, 716,
Andrews, N., 435, 541 Artru, A. A., 170, 172, Ayres, A. J., 387, 835, 836, 717, 730, 732, 733,
Andrews, R., 503 493, 496, 497, 498, 844, 890 734, 741, 754, 755,
Angrist, B., 838, 839, 847 501 Ayres, J. A., 1280, 1281 759, 760, 763, 832,
Angus, J. W. S., 838, 848 Arvanitis, L. A., 1105 Azen, S. P., 849 837, 847, 947, 961,
Annell, A.-L., 1240 Arvidsson, T., 48, 62 Azmitia, E. C., 454 1307
Anneren, G., 109 Asano, E., 752 Azrin, N. H., 905, 1125, Bakardjiev, A., 493, 494,
Annex, B., 546 Asano, F., 47 1126, 1134 496, 497, 498, 519,
Ansari, D., 549 Asarnow, R., 181, 183, 481, 1305
Ansermet, F., 80, 82 537 Babb, S., 208, 269 Bakay, B., 464
Antalffy, B., 140, 431, 432 Asendorpf, J. B., 386 Bacchelli, E., 130, 432 Bakeman, R., 319, 336, 653
Anthony, A., 435, 438, Asgharian, A., 708, 724, Bach, L., 666 Baker, B., 962
541 1056 Bach, V., 502 Baker, G. A., 544
Anthony, E. J., 585, 592 Ashburner, J., 371, 495, Bachevalier, J., 371, 524, Baker, L., 9, 335, 343, 588
Antolovich, M., 1058 497, 1305 526, 652 Baker, M. J., 254, 913
Antonarakis, S. E., 552 Ashden, B., 324 Bachmann-Andersen, L., Baker, P., 551
Anvret, M., 130, 138 Ashenden, B., 288, 298 50 Baker, S., 615, 665
Anzalone, M., 367, 939, Asher, S. R., 341 Bach-y-Rita, P., 691 Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.
980, 981, 983, 1310 Ashford, L., 31, 227, 232, Bacon, A. L., 325, 410 J., 322
Apitz, J., 548 233, 707, 708, 721, Baddeley, A. D., 606, 611 Bakke, S., 535
Apolito, P. M., 906 734 Bader, P. J., 551 Bakker, D. J., 503
Applebee, A., 813 Ashley-Kioch, A., 432 Badner, J. A., 432 Bakker, L., 543
Applegate, H., 463 Ashley-Koch, A., 32, 432, Baer, D., 823, 826, 883, Baldessarini, R. J., 1103
Apter, A., 299 433, 439, 754 915, 950, 1006, 1130 Baldwin, D. A., 236, 337,
Aquaviva, C., 78 Ashman, S. B., 709 Bagan, B. T., 542 650, 654
Aram, R., 369 Ashwal, S., 172, 187, 537, Bagenholm, A., 1074 Baldwin, J. M., 382
Aras, T., 107 540, 709, 841, 1124, Bagnato, S. J., 713 Baldwin, S., 1244
Arbelle, S., 171, 833, 1011 1139, 1310 Bagwell, C. L., 177 Baldwin, V. L., 1076
Arbib, M., 389, 673 Ashwin, C., 525, 652 Bahrick, L., 656, 657 Ball, J., 745, 750
Author Index I•3

Balla, D., 83, 225, 238, 439, 461, 497, 524, Basso, G., 668, 669 Beisler, J. M., 790
315, 316, 682, 754, 525, 526, 590, 613, Basu, S., 432 Beitchman, J. H., 335
755, 793, 800, 804, 615, 618, 628, 629, Bates, E., 149, 224, 235, Bejerot, S., 102, 590
845 630, 631, 632, 633, 320, 336, 337, 340, Bekkering, H., 355, 389
Ballaban-Gil, K., 76, 79, 634, 635, 636, 641, 630, 653, 659, 673, Belcher, R., 207, 298, 302,
80, 82, 202, 204, 205, 642, 645, 652, 654, 687, 689, 800 1078, 1091, 1093
206, 212, 213, 214, 655, 656, 657, 660, Bates, G., 555, 556 Belchic, J. K., 1032, 1033
288, 289, 290, 291, 665, 666, 667, 682, Bates, P. E., 1092 Beldjord, C., 77, 138, 432
292, 293, 296, 298, 687, 689, 690, 707, Bat-Haee, M. A., 152 Belfiore, P., 901
299, 1312 708, 709, 713, 714, Ba’tki, A., 634 Belicohenko, P., 137
Ballantyne, A. O., 100, 715, 716, 724, 730, Battacchi, M. W., 413, 696 Belin, P., 517
835, 836 732, 733, 734, 739, Battaglia, A., 551 Belin, T. R., 1108
Ballenger, J., 1282 741, 754, 755, 763, Batticane, N., 542 Bell, M., 619
Balottin, U., 539 795, 832, 837, 838, Baudonniere, P., 386 Bell, R., 547, 1061
Baltaxe, C., 256, 257, 336, 847, 872, 873, 947, Bauer, S., 537 Bellacosa, A., 551
346, 348, 352, 353, 961, 1306, 1307, 1308 Bauman, K. E., 825 Bellak, L., 617
393, 962, 1306 Barratt, P., 991 Bauman, M., 490, 1305 Bellamy, G. T., 1095
Bambara, L., 929 Barratt-Boyes, B. G., 548 Bauman, M. D., 526, 1306 Belleville, S., 611, 642, 644
Bandettini, P., 522 Barrett, A. M., 371 Bauman, M. L., 371, 372, Bellgrove, M. A., 104
Bandim, J. M., 553 Barrett, P. R., 461 463, 488, 489, 490, Bellugi, U., 260, 548, 549
Bangs, T., 148 Barrett, S., 432 492, 496, 526, 536, Belmaker, R. H., 463
Banks, B., 1088 Barretto, A., 904 832, 1014, 1108, 1282, Belsito, K. M., 1111
Banks, P. D., 1088 Barron, J. L., 189, 459, 1305 Bemporad, J., 183, 189,
Bannerman, D. J., 1098 460, 462 Baumberger, T., 257, 345, 324, 1015
Banon, J., 462 Barry, L. M., 869 960 Bemporad, M. L., 1281
Barad, V., 668, 669 Barry, R. J., 268 Baumeister, A., 1141, 1183 Benaroya, S., 253, 392
Barak, Y., 1220 Barshop, B. A., 464 Baumgardner, T. L., 269, Benavidez, D. A., 833
Barale, F., 1305 Barta, P., 107, 492, 499, 545 Benayed, R., 433, 474
Baran, J. A., 844 768 Baumgartner, P., 324, 415 Bence, R., 744
Baranek, G., 172, 187, 226, Bartak, L., 8, 237, 256, Bauminger, N., 323, 415, Bencsath, A., 463
234, 236, 387, 398, 265, 292, 343, 345, 416, 419, 1020, 1021, Bender, L., 8, 167, 584, 585
491, 492, 499, 536, 349, 462, 588, 594, 1034 Ben Hamida, M., 553
537, 540, 707, 708, 1201, 1244, 1306 Bawn, S., 355, 493, 495 Ben-Hur, T., 255
709, 712, 713, 724, Barth, A., 75, 77 Baxter, M., 209, 295 Benigni, L., 149, 630
782, 831, 832, 833, Barthe, D., 551 Bayes, A., 108, 519 Benjamin, E., 1239
834, 837, 841, 843, Barthélémy , C., 229, 232, Bayley, N., 83, 791 Benkers, T., 355, 493, 495
844, 848, 849, 870, 366, 373, 394, 432, Bazhenova, O., 336 Benner, A., 433
1124, 1139, 1310 459, 479, 481, 517, Beadle-Brown, J., 214 Bennett-Baker, P. E., 551
Barbaresi, W. J., 534 542, 555, 689, 712, Beail, N., 548 Bennetto, L., 234, 237, 369,
Barber, M., 44 751, 834, 1208 Bean, J., 542 370, 371, 372, 384,
Barbetta, P., 991, 1032 Bartlett, F. C., 640 Beard, C. M., 51 385, 387, 388, 389,
Barbieri, F., 243 Bartolucci, G., 77, 78, 92, Bearden, C. E., 552 536, 537, 545, 611,
Barcellos, L. F., 431 93, 94, 104, 109, 169, Beatty, W. W., 616 613, 616, 635, 836,
Barchfeld, P., 614 170, 172, 173, 174, Beaty, T., 141 837
Barcus, M. J., 1091, 1092 183, 186, 187, 202, Beauchamp, K., 870 Benowitz, L. I., 646
Barkley, R. A., 28, 1061, 203, 204, 206, 207, Beaumanoir, A., 80, 82, Bentin, S., 483, 693
1062 210, 211, 256, 257, 175 Benton, D., 1112
Barlow, A., 97 258, 288, 295, 297, Bebbington, P., 619 Bent-Williams, A., 551
Barlow, D., 826 298, 299, 343, 345, Bebko, J. M., 749, 903, Benz, B., 539
Barnard, L., 189 372, 373, 375, 427, 1072 Berant, M., 552
Barnes, T. R., 848 437, 590, 606, 607, Bechara, A., 524 Berberich, J. P., 882, 883
Barnett, A., 588 628, 731, 734, 753, Beck, J. C., 432 Berelowitz, M., 541
Barnett, C., 832, 833 754 Beck, J. L., 609 Berg, C. J., 266
Barnett, J. Y., 27, 839, 847, Barton, L. E., 904 Becker, L., 456 Berg, M., 129
1107 Barton, M., 229, 534, 713, Becker, M. A., 464 Berg, W., 912, 916
Barnhill, G., 354, 374, 375, 716, 717, 1306 Becker, W. C., 1126 Berger, H. J., 372, 610,
872, 876, 1018, 1020 Barton, R. M., 373 Beckett, C., 180, 418, 436, 613, 644
Barnhill, L. J., 1105 Barton, S., 204, 206, 215, 591 Berger, L., 541
Baroff, G., 291 288, 293, 320, 335, Bedair, H. S., 1103 Berger, M., 1244
Baron-Cohen, S., 27, 31, 384, 835, 836, 962, Bedard, C., 476, 477 Bergman, T., 317, 684, 689
48, 52, 54, 57, 60, 64, 1011, 1304 Bedrosian, J., 806 Bergstrom, T., 1023
88, 95, 101, 104, 105, Bartsch, K., 630 Beech, A., 617 Berinlinger, M., 184
118, 185, 186, 190, Bashe, P. R., 113 Beeghly, M., 324 Berk, R. A., 846
191, 227, 229, 231, Bashford, A., 543, 757, Beeghly-Smith, M., 653, Berkell, D. E., 1091, 1093
232, 233, 234, 235, 1055, 1060, 1064, 665, 667 Berker, M., 130
236, 237, 253, 254, 1069, 1176, 1178, 1216 Beery, K., 794, 844, 847 Berkson, G., 536, 833, 834
258, 259, 261, 266, Basile, V. C., 900 Beh, M. B., 1282 Berlin, I., 209
299, 318, 320, 325, Bass, L., 1034 Behar, D., 266 Berlucchi, G., 691
336, 342, 352, 353, Bass, M. P., 434, 544, 551, Behen, M., 454, 490, 517, Bernabei, P., 536, 1221,
368, 369, 373, 384, 754 549, 752 1223
385, 393, 394, 397, Bassell, G. J., 545 Behrmann, M., 956 Bernal, J., 299
399, 410, 419, 434, Bassett, A. S., 552 Beidel, D. C., 181, 182 Bernard, C., 544
I•4 Author Index

Bernard, S., 435 Birmaher, B., 27, 189, 1110 Bodfish, J. W., 265, 267, Botting, N., 759, 760
Bernard-Opitz, V., 259, Birn, R., 522 838, 839, 847, 848, Bottitta, M., 494, 499, 542,
351, 871, 873, 876, Birnbrauer, J. S., 1070 1112 1307
902, 956 Biro, S., 612, 629, 630, 698 Boggs, S. R., 1060, 1062 Boucher, J., 237, 252, 254,
Berney, T. P., 544 Birrell, J., 668, 669 Bohman, I. L., 46, 1243 324, 344, 350, 369,
Bernier, J., 548 Bisarya, D., 439, 631, 632, Bohman, M., 46, 546, 1243 370, 371, 385, 393,
Bernier, R., 342, 432, 483, 634 Boiron, M., 751 394, 395, 396, 399,
521, 522 Bishop, D., 353, 433, 519, Boksenbaum, N., 183 412, 488, 497, 499,
Bernstein, D., 249 549, 586 Boldt, D., 492 519, 689, 836, 847,
Bernstein, G. A., 461, 462 Bishop, D. V., 32, 33, 169, Boler, J. B., 462 962, 1307
Berry-Kravis, E. M., 455 174, 175, 176, 335, Boles, D., 546 Boudreau, J. P., 691
Berryman, J., 269, 914 544 Bolhofner, K., 181 Boullin, D. J., 455
Bertenthal, B., 693 Bishop, D. V. M., 73, 80, Bolick, T., 978, 981 Boulton, D., 27
Berthier, M. L., 107, 108, 82, 97, 98, 353, 428, Bolte, E., 541 Bourbeau, P. E., 1095
519, 537, 538 641, 722, 740, 753, Bölte, S., 294, 521 Bourgeois, M., 521
Bertini, E., 551 759, 760, 810, 812, Bolton, D., 616 Bourland, G., 151
Bertolino, A., 526 813 Bolton, P., 5, 8, 26, 28, 51, Bouvard, M., 189, 326,
Bertoninci, J., 337 Bishop, M., 32, 418, 554 57, 65, 76, 107, 108, 459, 462, 618, 1112
Bertrand, J., 49, 52, 57, 60, Bishop, P., 1112 170, 171, 185, 208, Bove, F., 49, 52, 57, 60,
185, 549 Bissette, G., 142 269, 295, 425, 426, 185
Berument, S. K., 427, 722, Bisson, E., 432 427, 428, 429, 430, Bowcock, A. M., 431
763 Bitsika, V., 1058, 1064, 434, 436, 438, 439, Bower, B. D., 551
Besag, F. M., 544 1065, 1069 442, 489, 500, 524, Bower, C., 127, 434
Besalel, V. A., 905 Björck, P. O., 46 534, 536, 537, 538, Bower, G. H., 607
Bescoby-Chambers, N., Bjorevall, G. B., 1177 539, 543, 545, 546, Bower, T. G., 384, 389
555, 556 Bjornstad, P. G., 548 551, 552, 586, 594, Bowers, L., 540
Bespalova, I. N., 433, 474, Black, A., 1076 617, 632, 633, 645, Bowers, M. B., Jr., 456,
550 Black, C., 541 724, 1305 457, 1103
Betancur, C., 433, 434 Black, D. O., 613 Bombardieri, R., 543 Bowlby, J., 322
Bettaglio, E., 539 Black, D. W., 212 Bond, J. A., 607 Bowler, D., 105, 175, 186,
Bettelheim, B., 256, 1089, Black, G., 130, 139 Bond, S., 169, 186, 187, 189, 325, 369, 370,
1195, 1243 Black, J., 651, 657, 658, 202, 203, 204, 206, 371, 586, 595, 598,
Bettison, S., 832, 1200 660, 661, 670 207, 210, 211, 288, 682, 1182
Beuren, A. J., 548 Black, M., 321 295, 297, 298, 299, Bowman, E. P., 108
Beversdorf, D. Q., 371 Black, S., 436, 551 590 Bowman, L., 128, 908
Beyer, J., 1182 Blackston, R. D., 555 Bonda, E., 693 Box, M., 951
Beyer, K. S., 130, 432, 433 Blake, D. T., 1308 Bondoux, D., 462 Boyd, S. G., 144
Beyer, S., 207 Blakemore, S.-J., 692 Bondy, A., 708, 885, 891, Boyer, R. S., 548
Bezemer, M., 30, 183 Blanc, R., 394 929, 940, 955, 956, Boyle, C., 49, 52, 57, 60,
Bhalerao, S., 544 Bland, L., 463 1036 63, 185, 555, 1070,
Bhangoo, R. K., 181 Blanton, R., 499, 549, Bonforte, S., 617 1123
Bhatt, R. S., 640 1305 Bonham, J. R., 145 Boyle, M. H., 24, 110, 174,
Bhattacharya, A., 486 Blasey, C., 170, 185, 521, Bonnefont, J. P., 551 733
Bhaumik, S., 1108 552 Bonnet-Brilhault, F., 479 Braconnier, A., 459
Bibby, P., 897, 1061, 1064 Blasi, F., 130, 432 Bonnie, J., 661 Braddock, B. A., 150
Bible, G. H., 1126 Blass, E., 656 Bono, M., 654 Braddock, S. R., 150
Bice, T. L., 251, 292, 294, Blass, J. P., 549 Bonora, E., 432, 433 Braden, M., 546
316 Blatt, G. J., 463, 490 Bonvillian, J., 836, 955 Bradford, Y., 438, 754
Bick, P., 189 Bleger, J., 1229 Booker, A. B., 463, 490 Bradshaw, J. L., 104, 610,
Bieber-Martig, B., 546 Blehar, M., 238, 322, 416, Bookheimer, S., 170, 173, 836, 1308
Bieber Nielsen, J., 130, 1223 521, 526 Brady, M., 901, 968
132, 137, 138 Bleuler, E., 7, 89, 584 Boomsma, D. I., 434 Brady, N., 542, 1112
Biederman, J., 28, 177, Blew, P., 1030 Boon, F., 1108 Braestrup, C. B., 462
183, 189, 208, 1109, Blischak, D., 802 Booth, R., 642, 644 Braffman, B., 543
1111 Bliumina, M. G., 553 Boozer, H., 1126, 1128 Brain, P. F., 1112
Bienenstock, B., 29 Block, J., 653, 654, 662 Borden, M., 30, 595 Brambilla, P., 491, 500,
Bienstock, C., 496, 497, Blomquist, H., 546 Border, M., 1004 1305
525, 667 Bloom, A. S., 267 Bordie, F., 554 Brammer, M., 373, 419,
Bienvenu, T., 78, 130, 138, Bloom, D., 183 Borengasser-Caruso, M. A., 521, 525, 526, 615,
432 Bloom, F. E., 183 548 652, 666, 667, 668
Bihrle, A., 549 Bloom, K., 317 Borgatti, R., 551 Brandon, P. K., 901
Bijou, S. J., 823 Bloom, L., 336, 337 Borghgraef, M., 546 Brandt, B. B., 870
Billingsly, F., 826 Bloomer, R., 613 Borjas, L., 553 Branford, D., 1108
Billstedt, E., 534 Bober, S. J., 849 Bornholt, L., 619 Brannon, M. E., 1127, 1128
Bilu, Y., 1185 Bobrove, P., 1127, 1148 Bornstein, M. H., 236 Brantberg, K., 478
Bimbela, A., 884, 1060, Bocian, M., 544, 550, 551 Bornstein, R. A., 616 Brase, D. A., 142
1061 Bock, G., 434, 441, 443 Borsook, T. K., 886 Brasic, J., 27, 762, 839,
Binkoff, J. A., 885, 1089 Bock, M. A., 620 Bosch, G., 118, 414 847, 1107
Binnie, C. D., 503 Boddaert, N., 517, 521, Bosch, S., 870, 876 Brask, B. H., 46, 52
Binstock, T., 435 524 Bothuyne, S., 236, 237, Brass, M., 389
Bird, A., 431, 440 Boddington, E., 1283 389, 390 Braten, S., 673
Bird, H., 732, 762 Bodensteiner, J. B., 537 Bottaro, G., 539 Braun, T., 432, 438, 754
Author Index I•5

Braunstein, P., 348 Brookner, F., 183 642, 643, 719, 720, Burton, D., 33, 95, 100,
Bravaccio, C., 80, 265, 266, Brooks, C., 846 731, 734, 735, 750, 110, 167, 290, 294,
494 Brooks, R., 317 753, 754, 832, 835, 295, 300, 303
Braverman, M., 455, 458, Brophy, M., 615 836, 837, 1014 Busch, G., 668, 669, 670,
519, 520, 1306 Brophy, S. L., 315 Brzustowicz, L. M., 433, 673
Brawley, E. R., 1130 Brothers, L., 326, 524, 652, 474 Buschbacher, P., 932, 934,
Brayne, C., 57, 185, 724 660 Bucci, J. P., 1108 940
Brazelton, T. B., 690 Brotherson, M. J., 1092, Buchan, K. A., 886 Bush, A., 548
Breakefield, X. O., 455, 1093 Buchanan, C. P., 541, 542 Bush, G., 589
458 Brothman, A., 545 Buchsbaum, M., 367, 496, Bushnell, E. W., 691
Bréart, G., 61 Brough, S., 1058 497, 525, 617, 660, Bushnell, I. W. R., 689
Breaux, A. M., 1110 Browder, D. M., 865, 866, 667 Butera, G., 872
Bredenkamp, D., 180, 418, 867, 868, 869, 877 Buchtel, H. A., 607 Butler, B., 255
436, 591 Brown, A., 541 Buchwald, J., 476, 478, 481 Butler, C., 44
Breen, C. G., 904, 991, Brown, B., 145 Buckley, J., 1076, 1094 Butler, D. W., 643
1031 Brown, C. S., 233, 238, Buckley, N. K., 1126 Butler, E., 24, 120, 835,
Breg, W., 546 251, 316, 835 Budd, C. M., 911 846
Bregman, J., 16, 17, 18, 50, Brown, E., 234, 235, 236, Budden, S., 134, 135, 142, Butler, I., 141, 142
177, 547, 580, 592, 651, 656, 657, 670, 144, 147, 149 Butler, L., 429, 536, 546,
595, 749, 1105 671, 689, 707, 799, Buechel, C., 518 594
Brehm, S., 1067, 1177 832 Buell, J., 967 Butler, R. W., 618, 619, 620
Breinlinger, M., 46 Brown, F., 900, 929, 941, Buervenich, S., 130, 138 Butter, C. M., 524
Brelsford, K., 261, 324, 1004 Buffington, D., 959 Butterworth, G., 549, 630
385, 412 Brown, J., 336, 657, 658 Buggey, T., 871, 876, 886 Butterworth, J., 1092
Bremner, R., 92, 93, 94, Brown, J. M., 542 Bui, Q. M., 569 Buxbaum, J. D., 429, 432,
109, 169, 186, 187, Brown, J. R., 32, 1193 Buican, B., 1109 433, 439, 753, 754
202, 203, 204, 206, Brown, K. A., 819, 823 Buitelaar, J., 5, 30, 166, Buxhoeveden, D. P., 107,
207, 210, 211, 256, Brown, K. M., 1126, 1127 169, 171, 182, 183, 474, 488, 1305
288, 295, 297, 298, Brown, L., 818, 885, 1090 189, 190, 249, 269, Buysse, A., 105, 177
299, 590 Brown, M., 484 318, 319, 412, 460, Buysse, V., 1034, 1035, 1038
Brendlinger, J., 908 Brown, R., 337, 340, 390, 657, 662, 723, 1103, Buznikov, G. A., 454
Brenner, R., 586 396, 418, 436, 554, 1111 Byl, N. N., 1308
Brent, H. P., 696 555, 805 Bukelis, I., 547 Bymaster, F. P., 1105
Brentano, F., 629 Brown, R. D., 588 Buktenica, N., 794, 844, Byrd, R., 289, 291, 296
Brereton, A., 104, 610, 836, Brown, R. L., 551 847 Byrne, J., 538
1061, 1065, 1199 Brown, S., 236, 459 Bullmore, E., 373, 419, Byrne, M., 539
Bresnick, B., 712 Brown, V., 668, 669 521, 525, 526, 615, Byrne, R., 383, 660
Bressler, S., 668 Brown, W., 546, 967, 968 652, 666, 667 Bzoch, K., 83, 800
Bretherton, I., 149, 324, Brown, W. H., 991 Bullock, T. M., 1282
630, 653, 665, 667 Brown, W. S., 481 Bumin, G., 153 Cabeza, R., 372
Breton, C., 615 Browne, C. E., 550, 551 Bundey, S., 551 Cacioppo, J. T., 383, 384,
Brian, J., 542, 544, 609, Brubaker, J., 128 Bundy, A. C., 845 836
643 Brucke, T., 139, 141, 142 Buonocore, M. H., 108, Cain, D. H., 236
Bricker, D., 712, 1003 Bruggeman, R., 1103 526 Cairns, R. B., 313
Bridge, J., 27, 189, 1110 Brugger, P., 646 Burack, J., 206, 234, 238, Caison, W., 758
Brierley, L., 46, 52, 321, Bruininks, R., 794, 835, 251, 317, 368, 642, Calamari, G., 251, 416, 417
392, 396, 587, 598 846 643, 644, 656, 1068, Calamari, J. E., 905
Briesmeister, J. M., 1060, Brun, P., 260 1182 Calder, A., 526, 663
1062, 1065 Brunberg, J. A., 545, 546 Burchert, W., 646 Calhoun, S. L., 93, 294,
Brigance, A., 712 Bruneau, N., 459, 479, Burd, L., 27, 28, 46, 52, 55, 374
Brion-Meisels, S., 795 481 71, 75, 77, 101, 135, Caliendo, G., 552
Briskman, J., 618, 645 Bruner, J., 226, 236, 259, 539, 588, 838, 1111 Calkins, S., 256, 257, 342,
Brissie, J., 31, 227, 232, 260, 319, 337, 353, Burde, R., 544 345, 346, 960
233, 707, 708, 721, 382, 391, 630, 653, Burford, B., 127 Callender, G., 612
734 654, 946, 1181 Burg, M., 552, 1128 Callesen, K., 105
Bristol, M., 889, 926, 1061, Brunswick, P., 665 Burgio, L. D., 908 Callias, M., 1059
1064, 1068, 1070, Bruttini, M., 78, 130 Burgoine, E., 108, 345 Calligaro, D. O., 1105
1074, 1075 Bryan, A. A., 235, 707 Burke, C., 1177 Calvert, G. J., 477
Britten, K., 823, 887, 890 Bryan, R. N., 141 Burke, J. C., 821, 887, 890, Calzada, E., 177
Britton, L. N., 908 Bryant, P., 630, 689 1055, 1160 Camaioni, L., 149, 536,
Brockbank, M., 318, 629 Bryson, C. Q., 596 Burke, M., 800 630
Brodkin, E. S., 1107, Bryson, G., 619 Burlew, S. B., 869 Camarata, M. N., 959
1108 Bryson, S., 24, 31, 44, 47, Burmeister, M., 544 Camarata, S., 959, 1049
Broks, P., 497, 499 77, 78, 100, 101, 110, Burn, J., 544 Cambell, R., 652
Brondino, M. J., 1063 170, 172, 173, 174, Burnette, C., 669 Cameron, K., 1243
Bronen, R., 107, 108, 375, 184, 187, 190, 237, Burnham, M., 503, 542 Cameron, M. J., 902
977, 979, 982, 998 268, 269, 291, 294, Burns, T. G., 542, 1112 Cameron, S., 909
Bronicki, G. J., 1078, 1092, 295, 317, 320, 343, Burns, T. L., 212 Camfferman, G., 480, 481,
1093 344, 368, 384, 387, Buron, K. D., 1016 482, 485
Bronkema, J., 1092 388, 389, 427, 437, Burr, D. C., 692, 693 Campbell, B., 902
Brook, J. S., 429 500, 501, 539, 542, Bursztejn, C., 459, 1208 Campbell, H. A., 747, 750
Brook, S. L., 175 544, 609, 610, 617, Burt, D. B., 1088, 1093 Campbell, J. O., 886
I•6 Author Index

Campbell, M., 13, 18, 19, Carper, R., 489, 492, 493, Cavallaro, C., 551 384, 385, 389, 393,
45, 72, 93, 174, 175, 499, 500, 501, 519, Cavallaro, M., 225, 234, 397, 410, 434, 613,
179, 184, 189, 212, 524, 536, 646, 660, 236, 243 634, 650, 654, 655,
232, 233, 234, 248, 661, 671, 1305 Caviness, V., 495 656, 665, 707, 708,
323, 348, 460, 461, Carr, E., 346 Caviness, V., Jr., 538 709, 713, 714, 715,
462, 492, 730, 732, Carr, E. G., 819, 821, 822, Cayer, M., 31, 170, 172, 716, 717, 730, 732,
734, 739, 740, 750, 823, 824, 883, 885, 477, 478, 731, 735, 733, 734, 741, 754,
838, 839, 840, 847, 897, 898, 899, 902, 754 755, 832, 837, 847,
984, 1102, 1104, 1107, 910, 911, 916, 926, Cazden, C., 340 947, 961, 1011, 1017,
1112, 1221 961, 966, 1030, 1033, Celani, G., 413, 696 1068, 1307
Campbell, M. B., 501 1088, 1089 Celesia, G. G., 476 Charney, D. S., 181
Campbell, M. S., 492 Carr, E. M., 484 Celiberti, D. A., 890, 1089 Charney, R., 256
Campbell, P., 1133 Carr, J., 819, 821, 823, 908, Celli, M., 539 Charnov, E. K., 147, 148
Campbell, R., 236, 911 1076, 1077 Cepeda, N., 668, 669 Chase, G., 170, 195, 197,
Campbell, S., 1036 Carrie, A., 77, 130, 138 Ceponiene, R., 481, 482 295, 427, 428, 537,
Candela, P., 872 Carrington, S., 1013 Cernerud, L., 1057, 1074, 538, 539, 808, 809
Candland, D. K., 6 Carrow-Woolfolk, E., 807 1076 Chase, J., 427, 554
Canet, P., 690 Carson, R. E., 454 Cernichiari, E., 436 Chatterjee, D., 453, 454
Canfield, R., 671 Carson, W. H., 1106 Cerniglia, L., 883, 912, Chawarska, K., 31, 32, 224,
Cannon, B., 949 Carswell, H. W., 849 1004, 1009 232, 233, 234, 317,
Cannon, M., 181 Carter, A., 103, 106, 111, Cerquiglini, A., 479, 481 318, 319, 325, 326,
Cans, C., 47, 52, 61, 534, 170, 177, 238, 252, Cervetti, M., 871, 876, 886 350, 773, 946, 960,
544 294, 316, 412, 735, Cesaroni, L., 831, 832, 961
Cantor, R. M., 438, 439, 772, 773, 782, 793, 1014, 1284 Chaze, A. M., 552
754 800, 803 Cestone, V., 888 Cheadle, J. P., 130, 132
Cantwell, D., 9, 168, 335, Carter, B., 1068 Chabane, N., 517, 521 Checcarelli, N., 550
343, 588 Carter, C., 254, 929, 930, Chabris, C. E., 521 Cheema, A., 545
Caparulo, B. K., 342, 456, 932, 934, 937, 940, Chabris, C. F., 521 Chelly, J., 77, 138, 432
457, 459, 537, 744 941, 952, 953, 954, Chadsey-Rusch, J., 1091 Chelune, G. J., 607, 616
Capelle, P., 107 1005, 1006, 1049 Chadwick-Denis, A., 345 Cheminal, R., 552
Capetillo-Cunliffe, L., Carter, G., 619 Chadwick-Dias, A., 257, Chen, B. C., 607
499 Cartwright, C., 496, 497, 960 Chen, C. H., 539
Capitanio, J. P., 526, 1306 525, 667, 1111 Chakrabarti, S., 49, 52, 54, Chen, D., 478
Caplan, R., 183, 354, 662 Caruso, M. A., 185 55, 57, 60, 61, 64, 171, Chen, J., 478
Capps, L., 238, 258, 259, Carver, L., 171, 235, 483, 175, 185, 204, 291, Chen, N. C., 1103
260, 322, 353, 415, 652, 690, 709 434, 435, 541, 593, Chen, R., 64, 261, 324, 412
520, 654 Casanova, M., 107, 142, 740, 753 Chen, S. H. A., 902
Capute, A., 800 474, 488, 1305 Chakraborty, P., 490 Chen, W., 478
Caputo, J. N., 905 Casat, C. D., 1110, 1111 Challman, T. D., 534 Chen, Y., 478
Caputo, V., 758 Casavant, T. L., 432 Chamberlain, B., 250, 415 Cheng, R., 432, 433
Carbonari, C., 455, 458 Cascella, P., 799, 961 Chambers, C. T., 833, 834 Cheng, S. D., 551
Carbone, V., 964, 965 Casella, C., 243 Chambers, D., 747, 750 Cherkassky, V., 355, 487,
Card, J., 613, 655, 656, Casey, B. J., 367, 368, 610, Chambers, W. W., 1282 517, 518, 646
662, 663, 664, 665, 617 Chambless, D. L., 1059 Chernin, L., 324, 520
669, 670 Casey, F. G., 903, 904 Chaminade, T., 419 Chernoff, R., 1062
Cardon, L. R., 431, 438 Casey, S., 907 Chance, P., 291 Cherro Aguerre, M., 1233
Caretto, F., 542 Caspi, A., 181, 439, 443 Chandler, K. E., 544 Cherry, K. E., 251, 267
Carey, J. C., 538 Cass, H., 127, 145, 554, 589 Chandler, L., 967, 968, Chesler, N., 555, 556
Carey, S., 520, 697 Cassanova, M. F., 140 991, 1031 Chess, S., 7, 50, 436, 544,
Carey, T., 542, 1112 Cassidy, S., 538, 551 Changeux, J., 607, 657, 658 589, 596
Carlson, C. F., 1060 Casson, D. H., 541 Channon, S., 1017 Chessa, L., 555
Carlson, D. C., 267, 1108 Casson, D. M., 435, 541 Chapman, M., 1176 Chevy, F., 544
Carlson, J., 821, 897, 911, Castellani, P. J., 1089 Chapman, R., 340, 805, 806 Chez, M., 503, 542
916, 926 Castelli, F., 518, 524, 525, Chapman, S., 909 Chheda, R. L., 144
Carlson, M., 849 526 Chapman, T. F., 181, 182 Chiappa, K., 476
Carlson, S. M., 615 Castelloe, P., 30, 292, 595, Chappell, P. B., 1111 Chiat, S., 256, 347
Carlsson, A., 495 818, 820 Chappell, P. D., 456 Chick, J., 97, 169, 178,
Carlsson, M., 495 Castells, S., 457, 463 Charak, D., 669 179, 184, 187
Carlström, G., 588 Castiello, U., 536 Charlop, M. H., 257, 819, Chicz-Demet, A., 459, 460,
Carmichael, S. T., 524, Castine, T., 540 823, 886, 903, 904, 462
525 Castle, J., 180, 418, 436, 908, 950, 953, 995, Chidambi, G., 415
Carney, R. M., 432 591 1004, 1009, 1061, 1129 Childress, D., 5, 107, 170,
Caro-Martinez, L., 237, Catalano, R. A., 1312 Charlop-Christy, M. H., 173, 427
320, 351, 962 Cataldo, M., 819, 825, 912 871, 873, 884, 885, Childs, J., 234
Caron, A. J., 317 Cater, J., 1105 887, 956 Childs, K. E., 1032
Caron, M. J., 185 Cathcart, K., 264, 758, 890, Charlton, R., 642, 644 Chiles, M., 260, 549
Caron, R. F., 317 1050, 1061, 1064 Charman, T., 28, 31, 48, Chin, H. H., 259
Carpenter, M., 236, 336, Cattaneo, V., 243 52, 54, 57, 60, 105, Chin, H. Y., 873, 876
386, 389, 630, 653, Cattell, P., 147 127, 227, 229, 231, Chin, Y., 833, 834, 849
884, 885, 956, 978, Caul, J., 537 232, 233, 234, 235, Chiron, C., 545
979 Caulfield, M., 903 237, 248, 266, 315, Cho, S. C., 455
Carpentieri, S., 251 Cautela, J., 269, 914, 1016 320, 342, 349, 369, Chock, P., 1138, 1141
Author Index I•7

Chodirker, B. N., 544 Clark, B. S., 44, 47, 184, 537, 539, 553, 561, 1060, 1107, 1124,
Chong, W. K., 554 343 580, 587, 689, 696, 1134, 1139, 1310
Choroco, M. C., 462, 839, Clark, F., 849 707, 719, 720, 744, Cook, I., 368, 372, 373
840 Clark, H. B., 1128 749, 772, 782, 783, Cook, K. T., 872, 876,
Choudhury, M. S., 1105 Clark, J., 267, 521 789, 793, 820, 832, 1015, 1016, 1020
Chouinard, S., 185 Clark, L., 84 961, 962, 984, 1107, Cooke, T., 630
Chow, E. W., 552 Clark, M. L., 1064 1108, 1121, 1224, Cools, A. R., 372, 610, 613,
Christen, H. J., 464 Clark, P., 205 1225, 1281 644
Christensen, K. R., 544 Clark, T., 268, 588 Cohen, H. J., 594 Coon, H., 73, 77, 78, 368,
Christiaens, G. C., 556 Clark, V., 520, 696 Cohen, I., 30, 251, 335, 372, 373, 489, 500,
Christian, L., 914, 1016 Clarke, A., 543 460, 461, 546, 646 501, 536, 609, 610,
Christian, S. L., 515, 551 Clarke, A. D. B., 591 Cohen, J., 429, 668 615, 617, 618
Christian, W. P., 1128 Clarke, A. M., 591 Cohen, M., 1095 Coonrod, E., 31, 227, 229,
Christianson, A. L., 555, Clarke, D., 101, 209, 210, Cohen, N. J., 496 232, 233, 655, 707,
556 265, 295 Cohen, P., 182, 429 708, 712, 717, 718,
Christie, R. G., 462, 553 Clarke, S., 820, 1032 Cohen, R., 524, 525, 667 721, 734
Christodoulou, J., 126 Clarkson, T. W., 435 Cohen, S., 929 Cooper, J. O., 1004, 1009
Christoffels, I., 663 Clark-Taylor, B. E., 550 Cohen, Y., 1218, 1219 Cooper, K. J., 868
Christopher, B., 1089 Clark-Taylor, T., 550 Cohn, J., 690 Cooper, L., 484
Christopher, J. A., 241 Clarren, S. K., 555 Coid, J., 462 Cooper, R., 151, 153
Christopher, W., 1089 Claverie, J., 439, 500, 501, Coker, S. B., 145 Cooper, S., 548
Christophersen, E. R., 1059 536 Colarusso, R. P., 844 Cope, C. A., 432, 433, 439
Chritodoulou, J., 143 Clayton-Smith, J., 130, Cole, C. L., 1030 Cope, H. A., 32, 754
Chua, P., 668 139, 544, 551 Cole, P., 542 Copeland, D. R., 619, 620
Chuba, H., 352, 353, 810, Cleary, J., 759 Cole, T., 551 Coplan, J., 542, 800
962 Clegg, J., 436 Coleman, M., 26, 51, 127, Coplin, J. W., 234, 385,
Chugani, D. C., 454, 490, Clement, S., 1064, 1065, 128, 130, 185, 212, 392
517, 549, 752 1075 248, 249, 261, 429, Coppihn, B., 433, 544
Chugani, H., 140, 454, 662 Clements-Baartman, J., 958 455, 460, 461, 464, Coppola, G., 552
Chui, L. C., 499 Close, D., 181 488, 499, 538, 539, Corbett, B., 542, 1112
Chun, M., 520, 696 Cloud, D., 197, 295, 427, 549, 553, 1055, 1212 Corbett, J., 75, 101, 210,
Chung, M. C., 1056, 1057 808, 809 Colette, J. W., 461 265, 266, 408, 551,
Chung, M. K., 494 Cnattingius, S., 539 Collacott, R., 548 593
Chung, S. Y., 209 Cobo, C., 1235 Collerton, D., 142 Corcoran, C., 619
Chung, Y. B., 900, 1183 Cochran, S. R., 539 Collier-Crespin, A., 762, Cordello, S. M., 553
Church, C., 324 Cocuzza, M., 547 1102, 1109, 1111 Cordisco, L. K., 1035,
Churchill, D., 384, 596, Cody, H., 491 Collis, G., 412 1045, 1047
836, 1011 Coe, D., 720, 745, 749, Colman, H., 189 Core, A. J., 849
Churchland, P. S., 694 751, 833, 1029, 1030 Colombo, J., 645 Coren, S., 641
Chynn, E. W., 645 Coelho, K. E., 553 Comings, B. G., 617, 838 Corliss, M., 962
Cialdella, P., 47, 52 Coello, A. R., 868 Comings, D. E., 136, 617, Cormier-Daire, V., 544
Cianchetti, C., 1103 Coez, A., 517 838 Cornblatt, B. A., 617
Ciaranello, R. D., 29, 453 Coffey-Corina, S., 481, Conciatori, M., 494 Cornelius, C., 344
Ciccehetti, D. V., 173 482 Condon, S. O., 183 Cornford, M. E., 140
Cicchetti, D., xv, 16, 17, Cohen, C., 205 Cone, J. D., 826 Corona, R., 833, 1011
18, 20, 24, 30, 33, 83, Cohen, D., 17, 106, 191, Coniglio, S. J., 542, 1112 Cort, C., 967
96, 97, 103, 104, 170, 234, 236, 248, 249, Conley, R. W., 1094 Cortesi, F., 545
173, 177, 183, 225, 250, 275, 317, 343, Connellan, J., 634 Cortney, V., 661
234, 238, 247, 251, 348, 350, 352, 375, Connelly, R., 64 Cosmides, L., 660
275, 315, 316, 326, 412, 593, 595, 631, Conner, R., 619, 912 Costa, A. E., 553
345, 367, 374, 375, 682, 683, 684, 686, Connor, R., 1034, 1112 Costall, A., 253, 395, 833,
412, 498, 520, 548, 689, 694, 696, 699, Conroy, M., 967, 968, 991 849
592, 628, 651, 655, 804, 813, 832, 839 Constantino, J., 32, 95, Coster, W., 841, 845
682, 689, 696, 719, Cohen, D. F., 750 315, 440, 746, 753, Costin, J., 617
720, 736, 738, 749, Cohen, D. J., xv, xviii, xix, 754 Cote, R., 476, 477, 478
750, 754, 755, 777, 8, 9, 15, 16, 17, 18, 21, Conti-Ramsden, G., 759, Cotgrove, A., 1283
779, 783, 784, 789, 25, 27, 29, 30, 50, 71, 760 Cottet-Eymard, J. M., 459
793, 800, 804, 845, 72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 79, Cook, C., 61, 316, 317 Cottington, E. M., 643
1062 80, 93, 94, 101, 104, Cook, E., 45, 348, 655 Cotton, S., 542
Cicchetti, P., 525 108, 169, 170, 175, Cook, E. H., 27, 228, 233, Cottrel, D., 1283
Ciesielski, K., 268, 368, 178, 180, 181, 182, 439, 453, 454, 455, Couchesne, R., 429
480, 499, 610, 1014 185, 209, 210, 211, 456, 474, 491, 492, Coulter, L., 958
Cimbis, M., 130 223, 224, 238, 251, 499, 515, 542, 732, Courchesne, E., 24, 31, 97,
Cipani, E., 908 256, 267, 293, 312, 752, 757, 762, 1112 104, 107, 141, 172,
Cirignotta, F., 152 315, 316, 318, 323, Cook, E. H., Jr., 169, 170, 261, 266, 268, 367,
Cisar, C. L., 1032 324, 335, 342, 343, 172, 187, 196, 326, 368, 371, 375, 476,
Claire, L., 619 346, 349, 350, 353, 459, 537, 539, 540, 477, 478, 479, 480,
Clare, A. J., 151 387, 388, 412, 428, 541, 547, 551, 708, 482, 489, 492, 493,
Claridge, G., 617 453, 454, 455, 456, 709, 712, 713, 724, 494, 496, 497, 498,
Clark, A., 142, 145, 686, 457, 458, 459, 460, 731, 739, 753, 754, 499, 500, 501, 519,
687, 691, 695, 710, 461, 463, 478, 485, 756, 757, 761, 782, 521, 522, 524, 525,
711 486, 492, 520, 522, 808, 841, 1055, 1059, 526, 536, 539, 610,
I•8 Author Index

Courchesne, E. (Continued) Crosbie, J., 183 341, 349, 366, 375, Davidson, R., 494, 521,
617, 642, 646, 654, Crosland, K. A., 821 408, 682, 833, 834 522, 668
655, 656, 659, 661, Crosley, C. J., 544 Dahlgren-Sandberg, A., Davies, B., 961
668, 671, 832, 837, Cross, P. K., 547 103, 375 Davies, S., 438, 519, 541
889, 1014, 1282, 1305, Crossley, S. A., 322, 707, Dahlin, G., 1243 Davis, B., 953, 1044, 1132,
1306 1057 Dahlstrom, B., 261 1141
Courchesne, R., 439, 474, Croue, S., 690 Dahoun, S. P., 552 Davis, C. A., 901
476, 477, 479, 480, Crowson, M., 170, 171, Dairoku, H., 318 Davis, H., 489, 492, 493,
551, 660 236, 427, 536, 537, Dake, L., 963, 985, 996 499, 501, 519, 661
Court, J., 463, 490 586, 594, 630, 632, Dale, P., 337, 800 Davis, J. M., 848
Courty, S., 439, 536 650, 651, 653, 654, Dales, L., 435 Davis, K. A., 608
Courvoisie, H. E., 1105 658, 659, 660, 661, Daley, T., 1004 Davis, K. L., 433, 474, 550
Coury, S., 500, 501 670, 709, 1306 Dalpra, L., 551 Davis, R. L., 436
Couvert, P., 77, 78, 130, 138 Crucian, G. P., 371 Dalrymple, N., 267, 301, Davis, S., 315, 431, 746,
Couzin, J., 1283 Cryan, E., 539 916, 985, 996, 997, 753, 1073
Covington, C., 151 Csibra, G., 317, 549, 629, 1023 Dawson, A. K., 544
Cowan, C., 81 698 Dalton, K. M., 494, 521, Dawson, G., 5, 30, 31, 113,
Cowan, W. B., 608 Cuccara, M. L., 208 522 165, 171, 172, 187,
Cowdery, G. E., 819, 825, Cuccaro, M., 269, 429, 432, Dalton, P., 433, 544 191, 224, 225, 226,
907 434, 544, 550, 551, Daly, E., 107, 419, 493, 233, 234, 235, 236,
Cowell, P. E., 497, 499 753, 754 521, 526, 652 237, 238, 251, 252,
Cowen, M. A., 540 Cueva, J., 212, 839, 1107 Daly, K., 289, 667, 721, 268, 294, 316, 318,
Cowen, P. S., 1063 Cullain, R. E., 269 744, 748 319, 320, 321, 324,
Cox, A., 8, 31, 33, 48, 52, Cummings, C., 538, 550 Daly, T., 799, 874, 875, 326, 341, 342, 366,
54, 57, 60, 95, 100, Cummings, D. L., 544 884, 885, 899, 929, 368, 372, 373, 374,
105, 110, 167, 213, Cummins, D., 656 930, 932, 933, 934, 375, 384, 385, 386,
214, 227, 229, 231, Cummins, R., 656 937, 939, 941, 950, 387, 388, 396, 397,
232, 233, 234, 235, Cunningham, C. E., 1060, 1035, 1036, 1045, 398, 409, 415, 429,
237, 266, 290, 292, 1064 1046, 1132, 1133, 1140 480, 481, 482, 483,
294, 295, 300, 303, Cunningham, C. K., 544 Damasio, A., 367, 373, 491, 492, 499, 520,
320, 342, 343, 345, Cunningham, L. J., 1064 387, 488, 524, 526, 521, 522, 526, 537,
349, 352, 369, 384, Cunningham, M., 556 590, 674, 686, 838, 539, 540, 595, 609,
410, 434, 588, 613, Cunningham, P. B., 1063 839, 840 610, 612, 613, 615,
634, 652, 654, 655, Cuperus, J. M., 177, 480, Damasio, H., 524 617, 642, 651, 652,
656, 657, 665, 707, 481, 482, 485 Dambrosia, J. M., 462 654, 656, 657, 659,
708, 709, 713, 714, Curatolo, P., 479, 481, 542, Damiani, V. B., 1076 662, 663, 665, 668,
715, 716, 730, 732, 543, 545 Danchin, A., 657, 658 670, 671, 689, 690,
733, 734, 741, 754, Curby, K. M., 521, 522, Daneshvar, S., 871, 873 707, 708, 709, 712,
755, 763, 832, 837, 523 D’Angiola, N., 257, 353 713, 724, 755, 757,
847, 947, 961, 1306, Curcio, C., 909 Daniells, C. E., 543, 545 782, 793, 799, 800,
1307 Curcio, F., 236, 320, 385, Daniels, J., 431 818, 820, 832, 833,
Cox, N. J., 733 386, 653 Danielsson, B., 48, 62 834, 837, 841, 846,
Cox, R., 522, 720, 721, Curen, E. L., 189 Danko, C. D., 968, 990, 934, 942, 946, 959,
741, 748, 749 Curin, J. M., 459 991, 1057, 1061, 1064, 962, 987, 998, 1003,
Cox, S., 668 Curran, C., 905 1074 1004, 1008, 1078,
Coyle, J. T., 142 Curry, C. J., 538, 555 Dannals, R. F., 142 1124, 1139, 1310, 1311
Coyne, P., 875, 1019, 1020, Curtis, M., 1016 Dannemiller, J. L., 640 Dawson, J. E., 267
1022, 1023 Cusmai, R., 545 Dant, C. C., 429 Dawson, M., 109, 201, 203,
Craig, J., 253, 259 Cutler, B., 997, 998 D’Antonio, L. L., 552 204, 206
Craig, K. D., 833, 834 Cutrer, P. S., 234, 385, D’Anuono, P., 80 Day, H. M., 821, 911
Craighero, L., 524 392 Daoud, A. S., 130 Day, J. R., 821
Craik, F., 669, 671 Cutting, J. E., 692 Da Paz, J. A., 553 Day, R., 438, 541
Craney, J. L., 181 Cvejic, H., 548 Dapretto, M., 170, 173, Deal, A., 932
Craven-Thuss, B., 494 Cycowicz, Y., 481, 482 521, 526 Dean, A., 488, 489, 536,
Crawford, E. C., 550, 551 Czapansky-Beilman, D., Darby, J. K., 84 617, 618
Crawford, S., 1017 542, 1112 Darcy, M., 1030, 1033 Dean, R. S., 539
Creak, E. M., 585, 1243 Czapinski, P., 500 Darley, F., 336 Deb, S., 100
Creak, M., 201, 204 Czarkowski, K. A., 1108 Darling, R. B., 1060, 1064, Debbaudt, D., 299
Creasey, J., 492 1068 de Bildt, A., 170, 412, 454,
Creedon, M., 832 D’Adamo, P., 78 Darrow, M. A., 916 520, 689, 696, 753,
Crimmins, D. B., 824, 903, Dadds, M. R., 1060 Dartnall, N., 367, 374, 789 789
910 Daffner, K. R., 613 D’Ateno, P., 871 DeBoer, M., 1004
Crispino, L., 1282 Dager, S., 165, 171, 191, Dattilo, J., 890 DeCasper, A., 319
Critchley, H., 107, 419, 651, 755 Davanzo, P. A., 1108 Decety, J., 419, 692, 693
493, 521, 526, 652 Daggett, J., 889 Davey, M., 101 de Chaffoy de Courcelles,
Critchley, M., 545 Dahl, E. K., 29, 182 David, C., 586, 588 D., 1103
Crites, D. L., 93 Dahl, K., 9 David, F. J., 833, 843 DeCoste, D., 801
Crocker, W. T., 909 Dahl, N., 109 Davidkin, I., 541 Decouf le, P., 49, 52, 57,
Croen, L., 44, 49, 59, 61, Dahl, V., 212 Davidovitch, M., 49, 73, 80, 60, 185
63, 184, 291, 434, 462 Dahle, E. J., 138, 432 536, 1312 Deeney, T., 845
Cronin, P., 189, 839, 872, Dahlgren, S. O., 105, 227, Davids, A., 744 DeFelice, M. L., 541
1310 232, 234, 325, 329, Davidson, L., 142 De Fossé, L., 495
Author Index I•9

DeGangi, G. A., 846 Denzler, B., 1176 Dietz, F., 657, 662 Donnellan-Walsh, A., 1008
Degangi, G. A., 980, 982 Deonna, T., 80, 82 Dietz, P. E., 299 Donnelly, M., 693
de Gelder, B., 324, 519 Depastas, G., 139, 145 Diggs, C. C., 149 Donnelly, S. L., 432, 433,
DeGelder, B., 324, 642 de Plá, E. P., 1232 DiGirolamo, G., 668, 669 434, 439, 544, 551,
De Giacomo, A., 223, 227, DeRaad, A., 916 DiLalla, D. L., 73, 75, 224, 753, 754
235, 1056, 1057 Derby, K. M., 819, 823, 225, 227, 721, 1051 Donovan, L., 1232
Degiovanni, E., 483 916 DiLavore, P., 45, 110, 196, Dooley, J. M., 130
de Gomberoff, L., 670 de Roux, N., 130, 138 214, 224, 228, 233, Dore, J., 340
de Haan, M., 549, 670 Derrick, A., 32, 169, 173, 248, 326, 341, 342, Dorrani, N., 78, 551
Dehaene, S., 607 754, 755, 757 343, 348, 349, 541, Dorsey, M. F., 825
Dehart, D. B., 454 Deruelle, C., 261, 519, 520 547, 551, 655, 709, Doryon, Y. D., 834
Deibert, A. N., 1126 De Sanctis, S., 583, 584, 712, 730, 731, 732, Doss, S., 910
Deitz, J., 366, 536, 834, 1221 734, 739, 740, 753, Dougherty, J. M., 1177
843 de Schonen, S., 486, 642 756, 757, 780, 781, Douglas, V. I., 617
De Jonge, R. C., 183 DesLauriers, A. M., 1060 808, 1124, 1143, 1216 Doussard-Roosevelt, J.,
de Kogel, K., 249, 318, Desnick, R. J., 545 DiLeo, D., 875 336, 832
319 Desombre, H., 751 DiLiberti, J. H., 555 Dover, R., 734, 748, 749,
Delaney, M. A., 1105 de Souza, L., 536 DiMarco, R., 956 753
de la Pena, A., 484 Despert, J. C., 584, 585 Dimiceli, S., 170, 315, 440, Dowd, M., 32, 731, 733,
Delatang, N., 394 Despert, J. L., 167 754 754, 755
Delbello, M. P., 181 Desrochers, M. N., 915 di Nemi, S. U., 1305 Dowling, F., 589
DeLeon, I. G., 824 Desser, P. R., 1018 Dinh, E., 73, 77, 78 Down, J. L., 547
Delgado, C., 653, 654, 662 DeStefano, F., 436 Dinno, H., 689 Down, M., 439
Del Gracco, S., 542, 1307 DeStefano, L., 1091, 1092 Dinno, N., 225, 398, 708 Doyle, D., 542, 1112
del Junco, D., 144, 145 de Traversay, J., 478 Dio Bleichmar, E., 1229 Doyle, S., 293, 804
Dellve, L., 1057, 1074, Detweiler, J. B., 1182 Diomedi, M., 542 Dozier, C. L., 819, 821, 823
1076 D’Eufemia, P., 539 Dirlich-Wilhelm, H., 888, Drachenberg, C., 541
del Medico, V., 1110 Deutsch, C. K., 493, 494, 1058, 1129 Dragich, J., 78, 138
Delmolino, L., 290, 297 496, 497, 498, 519, DiSalvo, C. A., 870 Drasgow, E., 967
DeLoache, J. S., 395 1305 Dissanayake, C., 174, 213, Dratman, M. L., 744
DeLong, G. R., 108, 270, Deutsch, G. K., 1308 251, 322, 657, 707, Dreux, C., 459
368, 371, 488, 553, Devaney, J., 130, 138, 139, 833, 1057, 1199 Drevets, W. C., 524, 525
1107, 1305 148, 151 Dissanayake, M., 1011 Drew, A., 105, 233, 234,
DeLong, M. R., 142 DeVellis, R., 289, 667, 721, Dittrich, W. H., 692 235, 237, 320, 342,
DeLong, R., 428 744, 748 Diwadkar, V., 667 349, 369, 384, 410,
Delon-Martin, C., 693 DeVet, K. A., 1062 Dix, T., 1057 613, 634, 654, 656,
Delprato, D., 883, 884, 937, Devinsky, O., 503 Dixon, J. M., 909 665, 708, 730, 732,
951 DeVito, T. J., 494 Dixon, P., 716 832, 837, 947, 961
Delquadri, J., 264, 991, DeVos, E., 645 Dixon, S. D., 81 Drinkwater, S., 1033
1032 de Vries, B. B., 78, 545 Djukic, A., 82 Driscoll, D., 551
Demasio, A. R., 488 de Vries, H., 249, 318, 319 Dloan, R., 665 Driver, J., 318, 523, 525,
DeMaso, D. R., 1108 de Vries, P. J., 543 Dobbin, A. R., 900 630, 642, 663
Demchak, M., 1033 DeWeese, J., 1112 Dobson, V., 554 Dropik, P., 542, 1112
de Mendilaharsu, S. A., Dewey, D., 387, 388, 1057, Dodds, J., 712 Drost, D. J., 494
1229 1074, 1077 Dodman, T. N., 1281 Druin, D. P., 612
Demet, E. M., 459, 460, Dewey, M., 1014, 1021 Dodson, S., 148 Drury, I., 144
462 Dewey, W. L., 142 Doehring, P., 315, 320, Druse, M., 454
Demeter, K., 146 de Wied, D., 460 651, 800 D’Souza, I., 429
Demick, J., 645 Deykin, E. Y., 51, 76, 539, Doernberg, N., 57, 63, 555, Duara, R., 476, 477, 492,
DeMyer, B., 962 1305 1070, 1123 1015
DeMyer, M. K., 9, 204, Dharmani, C., 82 Doherty, S., 549 Dubno, J. R., 476
206, 215, 288, 293, Dhillon, A. P., 541 Doke, L. A., 1154 Duchan, J., 255, 257, 346,
296, 301, 320, 321, Dhossche, D., 463 Dolan, C. V., 434 691, 961
335, 384, 385, 387, Diambra, J., 619 Dolan, R., 518, 523, 525, Ducharme, J. M., 268, 907
453, 835, 836, 839, Diament, D. M., 1060 615, 646, 668 Duer, A., 542
882, 1011, 1056, 1068, Diamond, A., 612 Dolgoff-Kaspar, R., 1111 Duffy, K., 1088, 1278
1069, 1072, 1075, Diamond, R., 520, 697 Doll, E. A., 745 Dugan, E., 251, 1030, 1032
1077, 1089, 1198, Dias, K., 126, 154, 431, Domachowske, J. B., 544 Dugas, M., 189, 1112
1304 535, 594 Dombrowski, C., 545 Dugger, D. L., 539
DeMyer, W., 204, 206, 215, Dias, R., 609, 615 Dombrowski, S. M., 491, Duivenvoorden, H. J., 545
288, 293, 335, 384, Dick, J., 1220 496, 549 Duker, P. C., 867, 898, 899,
836, 962, 1011 Dickens, B. F., 462 Domenech-Jouve, J., 459 903, 1127
Denckla, M. B., 98, 591, Dickey, T. O., 607, 616 Domingue, B., 997, 998 Duku, E., 24, 110, 174
839 Dickinson, H., 759 Domino, E. F., 848 Dulac, O., 545
Dennett, D., 629 Dictenberg, J., 545 Donahoe, P. A., 1044, Duley, J. A., 464
Dennis, J., 594 Didde, R., 542 1132, 1141 du Mazaubrun, C., 47, 52,
Dennis, M., 613, 641 Didden, R., 898 Donahue, P., 953 61, 534, 544
Dennis, N. R., 550, 551, Didonato, R. M., 147, 148 Donaldson, L. J., 547 Duncan, J., 606, 612
555 Dielkens, A., 546 Donnellan, A., xviii, xix, Dunham, D., 656
Dennis, T., 539 Dietrich, E., 690 149, 561, 866, 902, Dunlap, G., 264, 820, 823,
Denny, R., 968, 1030 Dietrich, K., 486 903, 904, 905, 906, 865, 866, 869, 899,
Deno, S., 205 Dietz, C., 723 907, 934, 966 900, 906, 912, 916,
I • 10 Author Index

Dunlap, G. (Continued) Edwards, D., 1060, 1062 Endicott, J. E., 15 Fadiga, L., 255, 389
932, 934, 940, 1003, Edwards, J. G., 1283 Ene, M., 97, 104, 107, 375 Fagbemi, A., 541
1004, 1032, 1033, Edwards, M., 48, 52, 54, Eng, D., 141, 142 Fagen, J. A., 147
1058, 1060, 1092, 61, 63 Engel, A. K., 690, 695 Faherty, C., 867
1124, 1129 Edwards, R., 476 Engel, W., 138 Fahey, K., 169, 181
Dunn, D. W., 463 Edwards, S., 804 Engelstad, K., 550 Fahum, T., 299
Dunn, J. K., 129, 139, 140, Edwards-Brown, M., 463 English, K., 969, 990, 991 Falco, C., 265, 266
143 Eeg-Olofsson, O., 130 Enns, J., 234, 368, 641 Falco, R., 1004, 1005,
Dunn, L., 147, 790, 804 Eens, A., 1103 Ensrud, S., 257, 353, 598 1006, 1007, 1008,
Dunn, M., 30, 169, 251, Egaas, B., 494, 496, 499, Ephrimidis, B., 1058, 1069 1009, 1012
342, 369, 479, 482, 519 Epperson, N., 1108 Falcone, J. F., 1105
539, 616, 1306, 1307, Egel, A., 264, 820, 823, Epstein, J., 569 Falfai, T., 325
1311, 1313 1030, 1037, 1126, 1138 Epstein, R., 542 Fan, Y. S., 544
Dunn, W., 268, 366, 536, Egelhoff, J. C., 430 Erdem, E., 107 Fann, W. E., 848
832, 833, 834, 842, Egger, J., 143 Erel, O., 1058 Fantes, J. A., 551
843, 981, 983 Ehlers, J. A., 553 Erfanian, N., 914 Fantie, B. D., 367, 368,
Dunn-Geier, J., 542, 1112 Ehlers, S., 53, 102, 103, Erickson, C., 267 498, 614
Dunst, C., 83, 932 105, 227, 232, 234, Erickson, K., 154, 155, Farag, T. I., 551
Duong, T., 489 329, 352, 375, 525, 937, 941 Farah, M. J., 388, 608
Dupes, M., 462 586, 615, 666, 724, Erikson, K., 802, 804 Faranda, N., 253
DuPre, E. P., 177 834, 1244 Eriksson, I., 76, 142 Faraone, S. V., 177, 183,
Durand, V., 819, 823, 824, Eikeseth, S., 239, 254, 870, Ermer, J., 268, 833 208
903, 910, 911, 926 874, 897, 1061, 1064 Ernst, M., 189, 524, 525, Farbotko, M., 1064, 1065
Durbach, M., 1034 Eikseth, S., 984 667, 1112 Farley, A. H., 227, 316,
Durkin, K., 105, 352 Eilers, L., 544, 548, 593 Erwin, E. J., 941 553, 832, 834, 837
Durnik, M., 1177 Eimas, P., 689 Erwin, J., 670 Farley, I., 142
Durston, S., 493, 499 Einfeld, S., 546, 548, 1197, Erwin, R., 478, 481, 503 Farmer-Dougan, V., 903
Duruibe, V. A., 462 1198, 1199 Escalante-Mead, P. R., 496 Farndon, P. A., 555
Dussault, W. L. E., 1125 Eisenberg, L., 201, 204, Escamilla, J. R., 544 Farnham, J., 618, 634, 635
Duvner, T., 102 215, 224, 293, 584, Eschler, J., 31, 73, 317, 708 Farrant, A., 497, 499
Duyme, M., 459, 461 585, 596 Esclaire, F., 551 Farrell, P., 205
Dwyer, J. J., 270 Eisenberg, R., 689 Eskes, G. A., 344, 642 Farrington, C. P., 48, 54,
Dwyer, J. T., 108 Eisenberg, Z. W., 1102 Eslinger, P. J., 613 60, 435
Dyer, K., 821, 823, 824, Eisenmajer, R., 24, 93, 167, Eso, K., 619 Farroni, R., 317
900, 910, 1128 172, 348, 593 Espin, G., 205 Farroni, T., 318
Dyken, P., 76 Ek, U., 554 Estecio, M. R. H., 551 Farshad, S., 654, 671
Dykens, E., 187, 211, 551, Ekdahl, M. M., 901 Estes, A., 5, 31, 171, 233, Faruque, F., 141
719, 720, 750, 794 Ekman, P., 630 234, 375, 388, 429, Farzin, F., 546
Dykens, E. D., 169, 182 El Abd, S., 544 612, 613, 656, 657, Fassbender, L. L., 149
Dykens, E. M., 26, 537, Elardo, S., 536 662, 663, 665, 668, Fatemi, S. H., 463, 490
538, 546, 548, 1055 Elbert, J. C., 863, 1181 670 Faure, M., 689, 834
Dykens, M., 429 Elchisak, M. A., 458 Estevez, J., 553 Faux, S. F., 479
Dyomin, V., 551 Eldevik, S., 239, 254, 870, Estill, K., 902 Favell, J. E., 819, 1130,
Dyson, L. L., 1074 874, 984 Etcoff, N. L., 526 1141, 1144, 1151,
Dziurawiec, S., 317 Elgar, K., 652 Evans, A., 693 1183
Elia, M., 479, 481, 482, Evans, I. M., 820, 915, 916, Favot-Mayaud, I., 48, 54,
Earl, C. J., 545, 584 494, 499, 542, 1307 1004 60, 435
Eastman, L. E., 909 Elizur, A., 1220 Evans, P. A., 542 Faw, G. D., 913, 1126
Eastwood, D., 539 Ellaway, C., 126, 143 Evans-Jones, L. G., 73 Fay, W., 255, 256, 346,
Eaves, D., 29, 595, 833 Ellenberg, L., 645 Everaard, T., 109 348, 961
Eaves, L., 542, 595, 1112 Elliot, G. R., 708 Everall, I. P., 101 Federspiel, A., 521
Eaves, L. C., 29, 262, 288, Elliott, C. D., 789 Everard, M., 1014 Fee, M., 914
290, 291, 833 Elliott, G., 21, 31, 73, 314, Everard, T., 201, 203, 204, Fee, V., 720, 745, 749, 751,
Eaves, L. J., 432, 438 317, 335 206 1029, 1030
Eaves, R. C., 720, 746, 747, Elliott, M. J., 349 Evered, C., 107, 124, 412, Feehan, C., 268, 588
750 Elliott, R. O., Jr., 900 462, 520, 539 Fehm, L., 181, 182
Ebert, M. H., 458 Ellis, D., 100, 968 Eviatar, A., 484 Feigenbaum, J. D., 373
Ebstein, R. P., 433, 463 Ellis, H., 97, 100, 317, Eviatar, Z., 260 Fein, D., 29, 30, 169, 229,
Eccles, J. C., 489 375 Ewald, H., 185, 188, 430, 238, 251, 323, 325,
Echelard, D., 170, 172, 493, Ellis, W., 207 453, 534, 551 344, 367, 369, 410,
496, 497, 498, 501 Ellison, D., 1068, 1074 Ewell, M. D., 1126, 1127 462, 519, 520, 539,
Eck, E., 549 Ellison, G. W., 539 Ewens, W. J., 431 616, 617, 713, 716,
Ecker, C. L., 843 Elmasian, R., 24, 268, 368, Ewing, K. M., 555 717, 833, 1306, 1307
Eddy, M., 885, 1089 479, 480, 482, 499 Exner, J. E., 794 Fein, G. G., 795
Eddy, W. F., 373, 485, 496, Elroy, S., 542 Eyberg, S. M., 1060, 1062 Feinberg, E., 1182
525 Elsas, L. J., 555 Eyre, D. G., 477 Feinberg, J., 463
Edelson, S. M., 832 Elwin, B., 1243 Feineis-Matthews, S., 521
Eden, G. F., 518 Emelianova, S., 542 Fabrizi, A., 536, 1221 Feinstein, C., 30, 169, 185,
Edminson, P. D., 462 Emerson, E., 1245 Fabry, P. L., 1138 251, 323, 461, 462,
Edvinsson, S., 546 Emery, N. J., 526 Facchinetti, F., 142, 143 539, 547, 548, 616,
Edwards, A., 48, 52, 54, 61, Emilsson, B., 1177 Factor, D. C., 17, 749, 762 1306
63, 266, 408 Enayati, A., 435 Fadale, D., 549 Feitelson, D., 398
Author Index I • 11

Fekete, M., 549 Finlayson, M., 104, 372, 427, 428, 434, 435, Freedman, D. X., 454, 455,
Felce, D., 904 373, 375, 628 438, 439, 500, 501, 456, 459, 1195
Feldman, C., 259, 260, 352, Finley, C., 480 534, 536, 541, 544, Freeman, B., 38, 47, 63,
353 Finley, W. W., 479 547, 593, 740, 741, 184, 234, 251, 292,
Feldman, H. M., 1110, Finocchiaro, R., 539 753, 773, 1056, 1057, 294, 299, 316, 321,
1111, 1112 Finucane, B., 550, 551 1175, 1308 455, 461, 489, 500,
Feldman, I., 515 Fiona, J. S., 185 Fonagy, P., 1283 534, 536, 537, 539,
Feldman, R. S., 324, 520, Fiore, C., 617 Fong, L., 290, 1075, 1093 540, 545, 724, 741,
890, 899, 953, 1011, Firestone, P., 1142 Fong, P. L., 257, 1089 745, 750, 751, 753,
1030, 1031, 1130 First, M., 10, 200 Fonlupt, P., 693 755, 762, 872
Fellous, M., 433 Fisch, G., 251, 546 Fontanesi, J., 147, 148 Freeman, K. A., 871, 887
Fender, D., 501 Fischer, K. J., 870 Foote, R., 1060, 1062 Freeman, N. L., 17, 749
Fenske, E. C., 875, 1029 Fischer, M. L., 350 Ford, T., 49, 60 Freeman, S. F. N., 1034
Fenson, L., 337, 800 Fish, B., 183 Forehand, R., 1060 Freeman, S. K., 963, 985,
Fenton, G., 536 Fishel, P., 320, 321, 355, Forman, D. R., 386 996
Ferguson, W., 607, 616 707 Forsberg, P., 48, 62 Freeman, T., 1068, 1074
Fermanian, J., 751 Fisher, J. R., 1044, 1046 Forsell, C., 430, 544 Freidlander, B., 1093
Fernald, A., 336, 690 Fisher, M., 1034 Forster, P., 555, 556 Freitag, C. M., 754
Fernald, R. D., 326, 327 Fisher, P., 762 Forsythe, A. B., 484, 542 Freitag, G., 249, 882
Fernandez, M., 320, 321, Fisher, S. E., 432, 438, Foshee, T. J., 1126, 1127 Fremolle, J., 439, 500, 501,
355, 707 1307 Foss, J., 462 536
Fernandez, P., 7, 436 Fisher, W., 27, 28, 46, 52, Foster, A., 542 Freund, L. S., 269, 544,
Fernell, E., 458, 544, 553, 55, 71, 75, 128, 135, Foster, L. G., 536, 833, 545
554 152, 838, 907, 908, 834 Friberg, L., 140
Ferrante, L., 758 909, 912, 1111 Foster, S. L., 826 Frick, J. E., 645
Ferrara, J. M., 916 Fisman, S., 93, 95, 213, Foushee, D. R., 553 Fridman, C., 551
Ferrari, M., 251, 818 252, 719, 734, 735, Fowler, A. E., 345 Friedman, D., 479, 481,
Ferrari, P., 459, 461, 1208 736, 743, 845, 1068, Fox, A., 1031 482
Ferri, R., 479, 481, 482, 1074 Fox, J., 968 Friedman, E., 463, 553
494, 499, 542, 1307 Fitzgerald, C. M., 463, 490 Fox, L., 932, 934, 940, Friedman, S., 165, 170,
Ferris, S., 461, 501 Fitzgerald, M., 46, 437, 1003, 1004 171, 172, 191, 493,
Ferruzzi, F., 539 463, 754 Fox, M., 1307 496, 497, 498, 501,
Ferster, C. B., 882, 1089 Fitzgerald, P. M., 150 Fox, N., 373, 613, 655, 656, 651, 755
Ferstl, E., 668, 669 Fitzpatrick, T. B., 543 662, 663, 664, 665, Friedrich, F., 608
Fett-Conte, A. C., 551 Fiumara, A., 539, 547, 553 668, 669, 670 Friedrich, U., 544
Fewell, R. R., 399, 846 Flach, F. F., 460 Fox, R., 692 Fries, P., 690, 695
Fey, M., 959, 966 Flagler, S., 1069 Fox, S., 905 Friesen, C. K., 663
Fiamenghi, G. A., 383 Flaisch, T., 669, 670 Foxx, R. M., 910, 913, 915, Friesen, D., 746
Fico, C., 265, 266 Flannery, K. B., 823, 900 916, 1126, 1134 Friston, K., 371, 495, 497,
Fidler, D. J., 536 Flejter, W. L., 551 Frackowiak, R., 371, 495, 518, 1305
Field, D., 540 Fleming, J. E., 733 497, 518, 615, 646, Frith, C., 171, 518, 524,
Field, K., 493, 501, 521, Fleming, N., 130, 132 665, 1305 525, 526, 615, 646,
522, 536, 646, 660 Fleminger, S., 666 Fraiberg, S., 347, 396, 417, 652, 665, 666, 669,
Field, L. L., 431 Fletcher, J., 260 554 671, 672, 673, 692
Field, T., 253, 690 Fletcher, K. E., 27, 456 Fraknoi, J., 744 Frith, J., 323
Fifer, W., 319 Fletcher, P., 105, 525, 615, Frances, A. J., 11 Frith, U., 88, 89, 105, 109,
Figueroa, R. G., 905 665, 666, 804 Francis, A., 542, 589 169, 174, 186, 255,
Filiano, J. J., 550 Flodman, P., 544 Francis, K., 951 259, 263, 265, 299,
Filipek, P., 172, 187, 342, Flood, W. A., 267 Francke, U., 21, 77, 78, 326, 346, 348, 368,
489, 491, 492, 493, Floody, H., 655 130, 136, 431 411, 518, 519, 524,
494, 499, 536, 537, Flores, F., 687 Francks, C., 438 525, 526, 583, 584,
538, 540, 543, 544, Florio, T., 548 Frank, H., 654, 671 594, 595, 615, 618,
550, 551, 553, 708, Flowers, L., 518 Frank, J. A., 526 628, 631, 632, 635,
709, 712, 713, 722, Floyd, J., 904 Frankenburg, F. R., 1103 640, 641, 642, 643,
724, 752, 782, 841, Flynn, B. M., 1094 Frankenburg, W. K., 710, 645, 652, 665, 666,
1124, 1139, 1305, Fogassi, L., 255, 389 711, 712 669, 671, 672, 673,
1307, 1310 Foley, D., 886 Frankhauser, M., 459, 1111 692, 1016, 1244
Fill, C., 138 Foley, S. M., 1092 Franz, D. N., 430 Fritz, M. S., 44, 57, 61
Filloux, F., 368, 372, 498, Folio, M. R., 846 Franzini, L. R., 1183 Frolile, L. A., 1078
643 Folstein, S., 28, 107, 170, Fraser, W., 44, 48, 59, 62, Frost, J. D., 144, 145
Findling, R. L., 539, 1109 195, 208, 270, 291, 100, 177 Frost, L., 708, 885, 891,
Fine, J., 256, 257, 258 427, 429, 430, 432, Fratta, A., 1103 929, 940, 955, 956,
Fine, M. J., 1060 433, 436, 439, 440, Frazier, J., 181, 208 1036
Finegan, J.-A., 539 453, 492, 499, 537, Frea, W. D., 267, 886, 897, Frydman, M., 552
Finegold, S. M., 541 538, 539, 553, 632, 911, 912, 913, 1033 Frye, D., 325, 615
Finestack, L., 966 754, 768 Fredericks, H. D., 1076 Frye, V., 1006
Fink, B., 170, 427, 537, Fombonne, E., 27, 42, 43, Frediani, T., 539 Fryer, A., 543
538 47, 49, 52, 53, 54, 55, Fredrika, M., 731, 750 Fryer, S. L., 108
Fink, G., 518, 646, 668 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 75, Freed, J., 27, 189, 1110 Fryman, J., 387, 837
Fink, M., 589 102, 171, 175, 185, Freedland, R. L., 640 Fryns, J., 545, 546
Finlayson, A. J., 174, 183, 204, 223, 227, 235, Freedman, D., 554 Fu, Y. H., 545
606, 607 269, 291, 316, 317, Freedman, D. G., 322 Fueki, N., 140
I • 12 Author Index

Fuentes, F., 708, 949, 1004, Gardiner, J. M., 369, 370, Gergely, G., 629, 630, 698 366, 375, 408, 425,
1032 371 Gerhardt, P., 899, 1089, 429, 430, 431, 432,
Fujiede, T., 460 Gardiner, M., 438, 754 1093, 1095 433, 434, 457, 459,
Fujikawa, H., 100 Gardner, A., 1060 Gericke, G. S., 1103 461, 462, 476, 478,
Fujimoto, C., 544 Gardner, H., 686, 1180 Gerland, G., 201, 642 484, 495, 502, 524,
Fujino, M. K., 499 Gardner, J. M., 1126 German, M., 459, 1111 525, 534, 536, 539,
Fujita, T., 254 Gardner, M. F., 844, 847 Gerner, R. H., 460 540, 544, 545, 546,
Fukuda, T., 138 Gardner, Medwin, D., 145 Gernsbacher, A. M., 521, 548, 551, 552, 553,
Fukuyama, Y., 127 Gardner, S. C., 545 522 554, 586, 588, 590,
Fulbright, A., 419 Gareau, L., 518 Gernsbacher, M., 952 593, 594, 595, 615,
Fulbright, R., 170, 191, Garelis, E., 456, 457 Gershon, B., 1032 666, 713, 714, 720,
519, 520, 521, 525, Garety, P., 619 Gershon, E. S., 432 724, 735, 736, 741,
696, 697, 889 Garfin, D., 720, 721, 741, Gershwin, M. E., 542, 1312 743, 748, 749, 750,
Fuller, K., 1034 748, 749, 926, 978 Gerstein, L., 24, 99, 352 751, 753, 756, 760,
Fuller, S. P., 1088, 1093 Garfinkel, B. D., 458, 459, Gervais, H., 517, 521 832, 833, 834, 1055,
Fullerton, A., 875, 1022, 460 Geschwind, D., 432, 433, 1074, 1212, 1243,
1023 Garfinkle, A. N., 889, 969, 438, 439, 754 1244, 1305, 1306
Funderburk, S. J., 500, 536, 1031 Geurts, H. M., 373 Gillberg, G., 593
539 Gargus, J. J., 538, 550, 553 Gharani, N., 433, 474 Gillberg, I., 28, 62, 92, 93,
Funk, J., 542, 1112 Garland, M., 540 Ghaziuddin, M., 24, 27, 50, 94, 102, 107, 108, 113,
Fuqua, R. W., 870, 876 Garman, M., 804 91, 93, 97, 99, 100, 185, 425, 461, 539,
Furlano, R. I., 438, 541 Garmezy, N., 14, 249 101, 102, 103, 120, 545, 551, 588
Furman, J. M., 484, 486 Garofalo, E., 144 187, 209, 210, 295, Giller, E. L., 455, 458
Furman, L., 961 Garreau, B., 373, 459, 555, 299, 344, 352, 374, Gillham, J. E., 170, 238
Furniss, F., 269 689, 751, 834 375, 536, 539, 544, Gillham, J. G., 316
Furukawa, H., 227, 834, Garretson, H. B., 367, 617 547, 548, 551, 593, Gilliam, J. E., 542, 550,
1224 Garrison, H. L., 991, 1031 734, 835, 846 720, 721, 751, 752
Fussy, L., 619 Garrod, A., 645 Ghaziuddin, N., 24, 27, 50, Gilliam, T. C., 438, 439,
Fyer, A. J., 181, 182 Garth, E., 485 91, 93, 100, 101, 102, 754
Gartside, P., 536 120, 209, 210, 295, Gillies, S., 201, 204
Gabbay, U., 1220 Garver, B. A., 485, 496, 525 299, 344, 544, 547, Gillis, B., 1074
Gable, A., 435 Garver, K. E., 373 548, 593, 734 Gillott, A., 269
Gabler-Halle, D., 900, 1183 Garvey, J., 967 El-Ghoroury, N. H., 254 Gillum, H., 959
Gabrielli, W. F., 1108 Gath, A., 548 Ghuman, J. K., 544 Gilmour, W. H., 547
Gabriels, R., 542, 1112 Gauthier, I., 170, 191, 419, Giannini, A. J., 553 Gilotty, L., 613
Gaddes, J., 350 519, 520, 521, 522, Giannini, M. C., 553 Gingell, K., 266, 408
Gademan, P. J., 183 523, 525, 670, 696, Gianutsos, R., 619 Ginsberg, G., 256, 257, 258
Gaeta, H., 481, 482 697, 889 Giard, M., 481 Gioia, G. A., 373, 794
Gaffan, D., 526, 693 Gavidia-Payne, S., 542 Giardini, O., 539 Giovanardi Rossi, P., 296
Gaffan, E. A., 526, 693 Gaviria, P., 1231, 1232 Gibbs, B., 1071 Girard, M., 77, 138
Gaffney, G. R., 494, 499 Gayle, J., 170, 195, 197, Gibbs, M., 258, 261, 548 Girardi, M. A., 1112
Gagnon, E., 907 295, 427, 537, 538, Gibson, J. J., 687 Giraud, A. L., 524
Gajzago, C., 27 539, 808, 809 Giedd, J., 181 Girolametto, L., 806, 958,
Galaburda, A., 548 Gaylor, E., 503, 542 Gigli, G. L., 542 959
Galambos, R., 476, 477, Gaylord-Ross, R., 904 Gigrand, K., 916 Giron, J., 130, 138, 139,
479, 480, 482 Gazdag, G. E., 889 Gil-Ad, I., 1220 148
Galant, K., 1003 Gazzaniga, M., 663 Gilbert, J. R., 434, 544 Giros, B., 433
Gale, S., 539, 544 Gecz, J., 544 Gilchrist, A., 33, 95, 100, Gispen-de Wied, C. C., 183
Galef, B. G., 383 Geddes, J., 189, 619 110, 167, 213, 214, Givner, A., 1126
Gallagher, C., 958 Gedney, B., 539 290, 294, 295, 300, Glad, K. S., 250, 261
Gallagher, H., 665 Geffen, L., 479 303, 352, 521, 523 Gladstone, K., 476
Gallagher, J. J., 1061, 1070 Geffken, G. R., 251, 316 Gill, H., 130, 132 Glahn, T. J., 1138, 1141
Gallagher, P. A., 1088 Geller, A., 453 Gill, K. J., 607, 616 Glasberg, B., 290, 297,
Gallagher, P. R., 542 Geller, B., 181, 501 Gill, M. J., 1089 1074, 1075
Gallese, V., 255, 389, 659, Geller, E., 455, 458, 503 Gillam, R., 813 Glascoe, F. P., 710, 712
673 Gelman, S., 629 Gillberg, C., 11, 15, 18, 21, Glaser, B., 552
Galpert, L., 318, 324, 386, Gena, A., 261 24, 26, 28, 47, 48, 49, Glassman, M., 321
409, 480, 520, 946 Genazzani, A., 142, 143 51, 53, 54, 60, 62, 63, Glasson, E. J., 434
Gamache, T. B., 152, 155 Gendrot, C., 78 76, 77, 92, 93, 94, 100, Glaze, D. G., 129, 139, 141,
Gane, L. W., 545 Gensburg, L. J., 547 101, 102, 103, 105, 142, 143, 144, 145,
Ganes, T., 535 Gense, D. J., 555 107, 108, 109, 113, 150
Ganiban, J. M., 548 Gense, M., 555, 1004, 127, 132, 134, 142, Gleason, J. R., 800
Ganz, J. B., 869, 913 1005, 1006 143, 149, 165, 171, Glennon, S., 801
Gao, H. G., 550 Gentile, P. S., 538 177, 184, 185, 186, Glennon, T. J., 842
Garand, J., 889, 985, 996 George, A. E., 492 187, 199, 202, 204, Glenwick, D. S., 1055,
Garbarino, H., 1229 Gepner, B., 261, 486, 519, 205, 212, 213, 215, 1057
Garber, H. J., 499, 1107 520, 642, 839 227, 229, 232, 234, Glesne, H., 886
Garber, M., 831, 832, 1014, Geracioti, T. D., 456 248, 249, 261, 265, Glick, L., 73, 80, 1312
1284 Gerald, P., 546 270, 288, 289, 290, Glick, M., 187, 211, 794
Garcia, J., 251, 1030, 1031 Gerard, J. D., 912 291, 292, 293, 295, Glisky, E. L., 619
Gardener, P., 871, 876, 886 Gerardo, A., 539 296, 297, 299, 304, Glousman, R., 455
Gardiner, C., 551 Gergely, C., 630 329, 341, 349, 352, Glover, S., 555, 556
Author Index I • 13

Gluncic, V., 454 Goode, J., 434, 441, 443 548, 580, 585, 586, Greenspan, S., 832, 889,
Gobbini, M. I., 520 Goode, S., 100, 108, 170, 587, 589, 593, 594, 890, 957, 966, 967
Göbel, D., 46, 184 171, 202, 205, 206, 595, 596, 597, 598, Greenspan, S. I., 873, 929,
Gochman, P., 183 207, 210, 212, 215, 599, 629, 631, 734, 932, 936, 939, 940,
Goel, V., 611, 615, 665 216, 288, 290, 292, 735, 736, 741, 743, 941, 1061, 1140, 1310
Goetz, L., 883, 884 293, 295, 296, 297, 745, 749, 755, 756, Greenspan, S. J., 1003
Goh, H., 907 298, 299, 304, 335, 833, 1189, 1243, 1245, Greer, R., 253, 947, 948
Golan, O., 629 342, 352, 426, 427, 1305, 1306 Greig, T., 619
Gold, B., 665 437, 536, 537, 586, Gould, M., 9, 91, 165, 168, Greiner, N., 956
Gold, J. R., 744 594, 632, 732, 733, 732, 762 Grelotti, D., 520, 521, 522,
Gold, M. W., 1095 753, 754, 756, 820, Gould, T. D., 376 523, 524, 526, 670,
Gold, N., 1074 1088 Goulet, P., 258 696
Gold, V. J., 882 Goodhart, F., 631 Goutieres, F., 129, 135 Grenot-Scheyer, M., 988
Goldberg, B., 296 Goodin, D. S., 479 Grace, A. A., 1306 Gresham, F. M., 874, 1182
Goldberg, J., 170, 173, 190 Goodlin-Jones, B. L., 108, Grady, C., 518, 1015 Grether, J., 44, 49, 59, 61,
Goldberg, M., 463, 484, 503, 526, 542 Graef, J. W., 537 63, 184, 291, 434, 462
485 Goodman, R., 49, 60, 62 Graf, W. D., 550 Grezes, J., 693
Goldberg, P., 1075, 1077 Goodman, W., 27, 178, Graff, J. C., 1074 Grice, P., 806
Goldberg, R. B., 552 1034, 1107, 1111, 1282 Grafman, J., 611, 615, 665, Grice, S. J., 370, 371, 549
Goldberg, S., 544 Goodman, Y., 1185 668, 669 Grier, R. E., 545
Goldberg, T. E., 140, 263 Goodson, S., 31 Grafton, S. T., 524 Griffin, C., 1058
Goldberg, W., 342 Goodwin, M. S., 540 Graham, A., 463, 490, 612 Griffin, J. C., 1127
Golden, C. J., 844 Goodwin, T. C., 540 Graham, J. M., 150 Griffin, R., 628, 629
Golden, F., 1126 Goodwyn, S., 336 Graham, L., 1013 Griffith, E., 102, 234, 372,
Goldenberg, A., 544 Goodyer, I., 632, 645 Graham, P., 79, 546 388, 612, 613, 614,
Goldenthal, M. J., 550 Gopnik, A., 320, 337, 383, Grandin, T., 201, 261, 295, 635, 663, 984
Goldfarb, W., 348, 584 390, 659, 672, 673, 366, 374, 832, 939, Griffiths, A. M., 542
Goldfried, M. R., 1181 836 1013, 1014, 1015, Griffiths, M., 109
Goldin, L. R., 431 Gordon, A. G., 555 1016, 1017, 1077, Griffiths, P. D., 430, 524
Goldman, A., 659, 673 Gordon, B., 301, 491, 492, 1088, 1178, 1184, Grigorenko, E., 263, 354,
Goldman, B., 412, 483 499, 537, 540, 708, 1185, 1276, 1277, 438, 691
Goldman, D., 431, 542, 712, 713, 724, 782 1278, 1280, 1281, Grigsby, J., 545
1112 Gordon, C. T., 27, 169, 1311 Grillon, C., 268, 477, 478,
Goldman, R. S., 616 181, 367, 368, 610, Grandjean, H., 47, 52, 61, 479, 480, 482, 642
Goldman-Rakic, P., 373, 617, 1106 534, 544 Grill-Spector, K., 523
606, 612, 657 Gordon, E. K., 458 Grant, J., 236 Grimes, A. M., 476, 477
Goldsmith, B., 185, 548 Gordon, K. E., 130 Grant, S. B., 148, 149, 152, Grimme, A., 521, 522
Goldsmith, H. H., 521, 522 Gordon, M., 953 153 Grinager, A. N., 733
Goldson, E., 542, 1112 Gordon, O. T., 267 Grattan, L. M., 613 Grinnell, E., 697
Goldstein, G., 97, 103, 144, Gordon, R., 708, 949, 1032, Graubard, P. S., 1126 Griswold, D., 354, 375,
257, 263, 264, 267, 1034, 1036, 1044, Gray, B., 551 1018
268, 295, 344, 367, 1045, 1132 Gray, C., 354, 869, 889, Griswold, V. J., 499
368, 369, 371, 372, Gordon-Williamson, G., 912, 985, 988, 995, Grizenko, N., 548
373, 387, 484, 487, 1310 996, 998, 1021, 1022 Groden, G., 553
497, 549, 652, 660, Gore, J., 520, 522, 693, 696 Gray, D. E., 1057, 1058, Groden, J., 269, 869, 914,
668, 767, 835, 1306 Gorman-Smith, D., 914 1059, 1068, 1075 984, 1016
Goldstein, H., 336, 394, Gorski, J. L., 551 Gray, K. M., 265, 266 Groen, A., 239, 949, 1048
887, 947, 950, 951, Gosling, A., 1110 Graziano, A. M., 1060 Grøholt, B., 1243
955, 967, 968, 969, Gossage, L., 966 Greco, C. M., 546 Groom, J. M., 1034
990, 991, 1004, 1011, Gosselin, F., 521, 522, 523 Greden, J., 27, 100, 295 Groothues, C., 180, 418,
1030, 1031, 1032, 1033 Gothelf, D., 552 Green, D., 588 436, 591
Goldstein, J. M., 1105 Goto, T., 127 Green, G., 540, 891, 936, Gross, M. M., 315
Goldstein, M., 142 Gotoh, H., 141 987, 1003, 1005, 1006, Grossman, E., 693
Goldstein, S., 690 Gottesman, I., 108, 170, 1007, 1008 Grossman, J., 106, 412,
Goloni-Bertollo, E. M., 551 341, 346, 347, 376, Green, J., 33, 95, 100, 110, 800, 803
Golse, B., 225, 234, 236 425, 426, 434, 437, 167, 213, 214, 229, Grounds, A., 101, 299
Gomberoff, M., 1230 500, 536, 632, 754 290, 294, 295, 300, Grover, W., 503
Gomes, A., 369 Gottfries, J., 139 303, 352, 713, 716, Grubber, J., 753
Gomez, J., 320 Gottlieb, D., 105 717 Gruber, B., 1010
Gomez, M. R., 51, 543 Gottlieb, G., 651, 655, 656, Green, L., 462, 539 Gruber, C., 83, 746, 753
Gommeren, W., 1103 657, 658, 661 Greenberg, D. A., 429, 434 Gruber, H. E., 464
Gomot, M., 479, 481 Goudreau, D., 183, 238, Greenberg, J., 958 Grufman, M., 177
Goncu, A., 383 251, 315, 783, 793 Greene, B. F., 1126, 1128 Grynfeltt, S., 261
Gonen, N., 1220 Goudy, K., 1149 Greene, L. W., 461 Gsodl, M., 549
Gonzaga, A. M., 1112 Goujard, J., 61 Greene, N. M., 458 Gualtieri, C. T., 501
Gonzalez, C. H., 553 Gould, J., 16, 24, 28, 30, Greene, R., 177, 1109 Guela, C., 671
Gonzalez, N. M., 189, 1112 33, 46, 49, 50, 52, 53, Greenfeld, D., 201, 205, Guenin, K. D., 267, 1109
Gonzalez-Lopez, A., 251, 93, 94, 102, 167, 172, 215, 247, 293, 296 Guenther, S., 253
1030, 1031 184, 214, 248, 249, Greenfield, J. G., 489 Gueorguieva, R., 1108
Good, A., 883, 912, 1004, 265, 313, 317, 321, Greenhill, L., 189, 1109 Guerin, P., 751
1009 323, 324, 348, 392, Greenough, W., 651, 657, Guilloud-Bataille, M., 432,
Good, C. D., 521 396, 534, 535, 547, 658, 660, 661, 670 462
I • 14 Author Index

Guisa Cruz, V. M., 1231 Halit, H., 549 1035, 1036, 1038, Harms, L., 479, 480
Gumley, D., 548 Hall, D. A., 545, 546 1044, 1045, 1046, Harnsberger, H. R., 548
Gunter, H. L., 97, 375 Hall, G. B. C., 521 1047, 1049, 1132, 1144 Harper, J., 17, 214, 288,
Gupta, N., 74, 77, 78, 175 Hall, I., 299 Hanefeld, F., 127, 129, 130, 289, 460, 741, 1076,
Guptill, J. T., 463, 490 Hall, L. J., 267 131, 132, 138 1077
Gur, R. C., 619 Hall, L. M., 459, 460, 461, Hanfeld, F., 464 Harper, L. V., 1061
Gur, R. E., 619 462, 1208 Hanks, S. B., 150, 151, Harper, V. N., 847, 848
Guralnick, M. J., 1034 Hall, N., 369 152, 153, 154 Harrell, R., 912
Gurevich, N., 209 Hall, R. V., 264, 1125, Hanley, H. G., 454, 455, Harrington, B., 1016
Gurling, H. M. D., 429 1126 456 Harrington, H., 181
Gurney, J. G., 44, 57, 61 Hall, S. K., 255 Hanline, M., 1003 Harrington, R., 434, 438,
Gurrieri, F., 551 Hall, T., 953, 1043, 1050 Hanna, G., 539 442, 710, 711
Gurunathan, N., 439, 631, Hall, W., 546 Hannah, G. T., 1128 Harris, D. B., 795
632, 634 Hallahan, D. P., 1073 Hannan, T., 319 Harris, F., 967, 1130
Gusella, J. L., 690 Hallberg, L. R. M., 1057, Hannan, W. P., 544 Harris, G. J., 355, 517
Gustavson, K., 546 1074, 1076 Hannequinn, D., 258 Harris, J. C., 142, 263
Gustein, S. E., 1020 Halle, J., 264, 819, 822, Hannesdóttir, H., 1238 Harris, L. L., 1003
Guthrie, D., 227, 316, 354, 883, 884, 900, 967, Hanrahan, J., 1034 Harris, N., 368, 671
478, 481, 489, 499, 1183 Hans, S. L., 183 Harris, P., 383, 394, 395,
553, 662, 745, 750, Hallett, M., 665, 839 Hanselmann, H., 1239, 396, 399, 615, 630
832, 834, 837, 1305 Halliday, M. A. K., 337 1240 Harris, R., 593
Guthrie, R. D., 467 Halligan, P. W., 646 Hansen, C., 130, 132, 137, Harris, S. L., 239, 257,
Gutierrez, E., 526 Hallin, A., 212, 739 138 290, 297, 708, 818,
Gutierrez, G., 51, 545, 769 Hallmayer, J., 426, 428, Hansen, N., 1238, 1239 820, 870, 872, 873,
Gutknecht, L., 432 432, 433, 434, 437, Hansen, P. M., 905 890, 949, 953, 1004,
Gutstein, S., 929, 932, 936, 617, 754 Hansen, S., 462, 553 1029, 1030, 1032,
940, 941, 958, 967, Halloran, W., 1092 Hanson, D. R., 425 1033, 1034, 1035,
992 Hallum, E., 212 Hanson, E., 97, 104, 107, 1036, 1038, 1044,
Guy, J., 431 Halpern, C., 651, 655, 656, 375, 723 1045, 1046, 1047,
Guze, B., 500, 536, 539 657, 658, 661 Hanson, R., 267 1049, 1055, 1060,
Gwirtsman, H., 460 Halsey, N. A., 1312 Hanson, S., 1074 1061, 1071, 1074,
Halt, A. R., 463, 490 Hanzerl, T. E., 823 1077, 1132, 1144
Ha, Y., 668 Haltiwanger, J., 845 Happé, E. F., 654, 655 Harris, S. W., 545
Haas, R., 141, 144, 145, Hamanaka, T., 255 Happé, F., 105, 106, 518, Harrison, D. J., 154
147, 148, 154, 367, Hamano, K., 661 524, 525, 526, 595, Harrison, J. E., 543, 545
499 Hamberger, A., 143, 462 615, 618, 630, 634, Harrison, S., 526, 693
Habers, H., 967 Hamburger, J. D., 267 635, 642, 643, 644, Harrower, J., 868, 929, 930,
Hackney, D. B., 499 Hamburger, S. D., 27, 181, 645, 665, 666, 669 932, 934, 937, 940,
Hadano, K., 255 263, 372, 388, 487, Happé, F. G., 253, 258, 941, 954, 1005, 1006,
Haddad, C., 147 606, 1016, 1106 326, 368, 640, 641, 1033, 1049
Hadden, L. L., 501, 536 Hamdan-Allen, G., 187, 642, 643, 644, 645, Harry, B., 988
Haddon, M., 1311 251, 270 682, 709, 730, 731, Hart, B. M., 883, 950, 951,
Hadjez, J., 459 Hamel, B. C., 78 832, 962 967, 1130, 1144
Hadjikhani, N., 521 Hameury, L., 229, 232, Hara, H., 325, 834 Hart, C., 632
Hadwin, J., 259, 261, 682, 240, 366, 689, 708, Harada, M., 172, 499 Hart, L. A., 438
687, 872, 873 712, 751, 834 Harbauer, H., 1240, 1241 Harter, M. R., 479
Haeggloef, B., 288 Hamill, D., 804 Harcherik, D., 456, 458, Harteveld, E., 183
Hafeman, C., 1074 Hamilton, R., 901 460, 492, 539 Hartlep, P., 1125, 1126
Hagberg, B., 48, 53, 54, 76, Hamilton-Hellberg, C., 457 Harchik, A. E., 916, 1098 Hartlep, S., 1125, 1126
126, 128, 129, 130, Hammeke,T. A., 844 Hardan, A., 297, 493, 494, Hartley, J., 353
131, 132, 136, 139, Hammer, J., 618, 632, 634, 498, 499, 519, 660, Hartung, J., 800
142, 143, 144, 148, 645, 1308 661, 667, 1305 Harvey, M. T., 542
154, 431, 535, 544, Hammer, M. S., 542 Harden, A., 144 Hasegawa, M., 140, 141
594 Hammer, S., 78, 435 Harder, S. R., 823 Hasegawa, T., 318
Hagerman, R., 545, 546, Hammes, J. G. W., 384, 392 Hardin, M. T., 838, 848 Hashimoto, O., 546
547, 844, 1110 Hammill, D., 844 Harding, B., 139, 488, 489, Hashimoto, T., 172, 461,
Hagie, C., 1021 Hammond, J., 207 536, 546, 617, 618, 499, 539, 667
Hagiwara, R., 985, 996 Hammond, M., 1034, 1060, 1305 Hass, R., 140
Hagiwara, T., 354, 374, 1062 Hardy, C., 551 Hassanein, K. M., 494
375, 869, 1018 Hamre-Nietupski, R., 1090 Hardy, R. J., 554 Hassold, T., 551
Häglöf, B., 204, 213 Hamre-Nietupski, S., 818, Hare, D., 212 Hasstedt, S. J., 537
Hagopian, L. P., 907 1097 Harel, B., 251 Hastings, R. P., 1055, 1070
Haier, R., 367, 617 Hamstra, J., 526 Harenski, K., 660, 661 Hatano, K., 662, 663
Haigler, T., 539 Hanaoka, S., 135, 136 Hari, R., 389 Hatfield, E., 383, 384, 836
Haines, J., 438, 543, 754 Hand, M., 291 Haring, N. G., 1010 Hattab, J., 463
Hainline, L., 317 Handen, B. L., 189, 906, Haring, T. G., 886, 904, Hattersley, J., 1244
Hains, S. M. J., 317, 689 1110, 1111, 1112 991, 1031, 1143 Hatton, D., 546, 547, 833,
Haist, F., 522 Handleman, J. S., 239, 257, Hariri, A., 170, 173, 521, 834, 849
Haith, M. M., 317, 684, 708, 817, 819, 820, 526 Hattox, S. E., 458, 459
689 870, 873, 949, 953, Harlow, H. F., 1304 Hauck, J. A., 387, 388
Hakeem, A., 670 1003, 1004, 1029, Harlow, M., 1304 Hauck, M., 323, 520
Haley, S., 845 1030, 1032, 1034, Harmjanz, D., 548 Hauser, E. R., 429, 754
Author Index I • 15

Hauser, S. L., 553, 1305 Hendrich, B., 431 Higley, J. D., 454 Hoffman, J. E., 693
Havercamp, S., 492, 499, Hendry, C. N., 23, 1312 Higurashi, M., 141, 142 Hoffman, T., 1023
646, 660, 661 Hendry, S. N., 80 Hile, M. G., 915 Hoffman, W., 388, 492
Haviland, J. M., 261, 690 Henggeler, S. W., 1063 Hilgenfeld, T., 354 Hoffmann, W., 64, 487,
Havlak, C., 151 Henighan, C., 147, 149 Hill, A. E., 75 606, 1016
Hawk, A. B., 550, 551 Heninger, G. R., 456, 1107 Hill, D., 318, 324, 387, Hofman, A., 556
Hawkridge, S. M., 1103 Hennessy, M. J., 154 409, 520, 838, 946 Hofmeister, A. M., 916
Haxby, J., 520, 525, 696 Hennick, J. M., 1089 Hill, E., 608, 611, 614, 837 Hogan, A., 315, 320, 651,
Hay, D. F., 653 Henriksen, K. F., 130, 132, Hill, J., 190, 628, 629, 631, 653, 654, 662, 663,
Hayashi, K. M., 494 137, 138 632, 1088 673, 800
Hayashi, T., 463, 1226 Henry, D., 842 Hill, K., 259, 261, 682 Hogan, K., 233, 235, 236,
Hayden, D., 957 Henry, L., 234, 370, 608, Hill, S. D., 415, 467 238, 251, 316, 708,
Hayden-Chumpelik, D., 611 Hill, W., 428 712, 734, 799, 801,
951 Henry, R. R., 30 Hiller, S., 454 833, 834, 835, 947,
Hayek, G., 143 Hepburn, S., 31, 227, 232, Hillman, R., 60, 435, 439, 1007, 1011
Hayek, Y., 142 233, 235, 236, 237, 501, 536 Holden, J., 437
Hayer, C., 542, 713, 717, 238, 251, 316, 321, Hills, J., 545 Holdgrafer, M., 45, 228,
718 342, 366, 367, 384, Hillyard, S., 480, 482 323, 426, 554, 599,
Hayes, A., 547 385, 386, 387, 388, Hinds, D., 426, 432, 433, 740, 753, 832
Hays, S., 746, 753 547, 707, 708, 721, 617 Hole, K., 462
Haywood, H. C., 872 734, 753, 757, 799, Hingtgen, J. N., 9, 384, Hole, W. T., 461
Hazlett, E., 367, 496, 617 801, 833, 834, 835, 385, 387, 453, 836, Holguin, S., 544
Haznedar, M., 496, 497, 837, 843, 847, 947, 1011 Holland, J. G., 1125
525, 660, 667 1007, 1011 Hington, J. N., 1198 Hollander, C. S., 460, 461
He, X. S., 542 Herault, J., 542, 1208 Hinrichs, H., 646 Hollander, E., 432, 433,
Head, D., 616 Herbert, D. A., 139 Hippler, K., 24, 91, 96, 97 437, 439, 660, 754,
Heal, L., 207 Herbert, J. T., 1094 Hirasawa, K., 141 1111
Heap, I., 914 Herbert, M., 355, 489, 493, Hirata, J., 28 Holliday-Willey, L., 201,
Heap, J., 1017 494, 496, 497, 498, Hirsch, R. P., 710, 711 203
Hearsey, K., 302, 867, 932, 517, 519, 1061, 1305 Hirsch, S. R., 1105 Hollis, C., 436
937, 940, 1043, 1049, Herbert-Jackson, E., 1143 Hirschfield, L., 629 Holm, V. A., 127
1050, 1067, 1177, 1178 Herbison, J., 953, 1043, Hirstein, W., 459 Holmans, J., 431
Heaton, P., 642, 644 1050 Hittner, J. B., 900, 906 Holmes, A. S., 1095
Heavey, L., 5, 8, 25, 61, 93, Hering, E., 542 Hitzeman, R., 462 Holmes, D. L., 870, 899,
316, 317, 419, 427, Herman, B., 459, 462, 1112 Hjelmquist, E., 234, 375, 1089, 1093, 1095
442, 644 Herman, R., 553 1306 Holmes, G., 489
Heberlein, A. S., 526 Hermelin, B., 255, 263, Ho, H., 29, 262, 288, 290, Holmes, J. P., 267, 1105
Hebert, J. M., 433 323, 388, 411, 596, 291, 542, 595, 833, Holmes, M., 431
Heckaman, K., 968, 1030 628, 640, 641, 642, 1112 Holmes, N., 214, 790, 1076,
Hecox, K. E., 476 644 Hobbs, N., 12 1077
Hedrick, D., 83, 804 Heron, T. E., 1004, 1009 Hobson, P., 324, 781 Holmgreen, P., 555
Hedrick, J., 551 Herrington, J., 522, 523, Hobson, R., 32, 227, 237, Holmgren, G., 546
Heemsbergen, J., 352, 426, 524, 526 253, 256, 260, 261, Holroyd, S., 739
732, 733, 753, 756, Herrmann, D., 619 344, 347, 353, 384, Holt, K. D., 1061, 1070
820 Hersen, M., 826 386, 389, 390, 396, Holthausen, H., 143
Heffelfinger, J., 551 Hersh, J., 241, 544, 556 407, 408, 411, 412, Holttum, J. R., 499, 909
Hef lin, J., 929, 930 Hersov, L., 107, 124, 1244 413, 414, 415, 416, Holtzman, G., 49, 73, 80,
Hef lin, L. J., 1032 Hertzig, M., 17, 184, 249, 417, 418, 436, 520, 1312
Heiberg, A., 147, 149 261, 324, 409, 456, 554, 555, 643, 656, Homatidis, S., 335, 550,
Heider, F., 525, 697 458, 459, 460, 462, 672, 689, 731, 836 551, 749
Heimann, M., 384, 385, 386 520, 707, 749, 1208 Hoch, H., 253, 868 Hommer, D., 454
Heimann, S. W., 1105 Hervas, A., 48, 59, 62 Hock, M. L., 901 Honda, H., 47, 435
Hein, A., 691 Herzing, L. B. K., 429 Hodapp, R., 1120 Honeycutt, N., 371, 497,
Heinrichs, W., 545 Hess, C., 1092 Hoder, E. L., 455 660
Heinze, H. J., 646 Hess, L., 833, 834, 849 Hodgdon, L., 984 Hong, D., 189, 839, 1310
Helayem, M., 553 Hesselink, J. R., 367, 371, Hodge, S. E., 434 Hong, J., 619
Held, M., 131 499, 1282, 1305 Hodge, S. M., 495 Honomichl, R. D., 503, 542
Held, R., 691 Hetherington, H., 143 Hodges, J. R., 526 Hood, B., 234, 318, 616,
Heller, A., 1092 Hetzroni, O., 149 Hodgson, S., 1307 630, 663
Heller, T., 8, 70, 71, 72, Heuschkel, R., 541 Hoehler, F. K., 462 Hoogstrate, J., 44, 49, 59,
175, 1212, 1239 Heward, W. L., 1004, 1009 Hoeksma, M., 480 184, 291
Hellgren, L., 28, 425 Hewett, F., 948 Hoey, H., 544 Hook, E., 547
Hellings, J. A., 821, 1108 Heyes, C., 383, 389 Hof, P., 496, 497, 525, 667, Hooks, M., 345, 580, 595
Hellreigel, C., 349 Heyman, M. B., 541 670 Hoon, A. H., 524
Helps, S., 1059 Heyne, L. A., 870 Hofacker, N., 143 Hooper, C., 263, 691
Helverschou, S. B., 542 Hicks, G., 476, 477, 479, Hoff buhr, K., 130, 138, Hooper, J., 746
Hemmeter, M. L., 889, 890 480 139, 148, 151 Hooper, K., 905
Hemphill, L., 353 Higa, J., 481 Hoffman, E., 151, 696 Hooper, S., 833, 834, 849
Hemsley, R., 1244 Higgins, G., 143 Hoffman, E. A., 520, 525 Hoover, J. A., 886
Henderson, L., 588, 664, Higgins, J. N., 430 Hoffman, E. L., 228, 231, Hopkins, B., 1033
665, 667, 670 Higgins, J. V., 551 319, 409 Hopkins, J. M., 1030, 1031
Henderson, S., 588, 835 Higgins, K., 322 Hoffman, E. P., 136 Hopkins, K. R., 1094
I • 16 Author Index

Horikawa, M., 540 Hughes, A., 804 Idziaszczyk, S., 543, 545 Jackson, S., 177, 268, 747,
Horne, W. C., 453, 454 Hughes, C., 253, 267, 326, Ierodiakonou, C., 1215 748
Horner, R. D., 1126, 1130, 368, 372, 373, 387, Iivanainen, M., 296, 297 Jacobi, D., 5, 107, 170, 173,
1143 609, 611, 612, 613, Iliyasu, Z., 547 427
Horner, R. H., 821, 822, 614, 615, 616, 618, Ilmoniemi, R. J., 481 Jacobs, B., 140
823, 824, 825, 897, 642, 644, 836, 837, Imuta, F., 46, 540 Jacobs, H., 884, 899, 1004,
898, 899, 900, 903, 868, 959, 1094 Inaba, Y., 435 1129, 1140
911, 934, 1010 Hughes, D., 813 Inanuma, K., 542 Jacobs, J., 546
Hornig, M., 542 Hughes, J. R., 461, 462 Incorpora, G., 547 Jacobs, M. M., 429
Hornsey, H., 838 Hughes, S. O., 255 Inge, K. J., 1088 Jacobsen, G., 1282
Hornykiewicz, O., 142 Hulse, S. H., 644 Ingenito, G. G., 1106 Jacobsen, K., 129, 147, 149
Horrigan, J. P., 1105 Hulse, W. C., 583 Ingenmey, R., 257 Jacobson, J., 252
Horsley, J. A., 1126 Hulshoff Pol, H. E., 493, Ingersoll, B., 886, 887, 888, Jacobson, L., 554, 901
Horvath, E., 549 499 889, 890 Jacquemont, S., 545, 546
Horvath, K., 541, 1112 Hultman, C. M., 539 Inglis, A., 335 Jaedicke, S., 344, 520
Horwitz, B., 518 Humbertclaude, V., 552 Ingram, D., 151 Jaeken, J., 464
Hosack, K., 1092 Humphrey, M., 585, 592 Ingram, J. L., 489, 553, 836 Jaenisch, R., 440
Hoshika, A., 541 Humphrey, N., 660 Ingram, R. E., 607 Jahr, E., 239, 254, 870,
Hoshino, K., 17 Humphreys, L., 1060 Innocent, A. J., 904 874, 984
Hoshino, Y., 46, 52, 71, 79, Humphry, R., 849 Inoue, M., 261 Jakobsson, G., 227, 232,
227, 455, 456, 459, Hundert, J., 1033 Insel, T., 266, 326, 327, 329, 425, 834
460, 461, 463, 542, Hunnisett, E., 236 462, 1111, 1306 Jalal, S. M., 551
707, 834, 1224, 1225, Hunt, A., 51, 545, 594 Iovannone, R., 1092 Jamain, S., 433
1249 Hunt, C., 496 Ireland, M., 544 Jambaque, I., 545
Hotopf, M., 551 Hunt, F. M., 713 Ireton, H., 712 James, A. L., 268
Hou, J. W., 551 Hunt, J. M., 83, 148 Ireys, H. T., 1062 James, J. A., 544
Hough, M. S., 258 Hunt, J. McV., 792 Iriki, A., 691 James, J. E., 1060
Houghton, J., 1092, 1093 Hunt, P., 254, 883, 884 Iro, M., 489 James, M., 609
Houle, S., 665 Hunter, K., 128, 146, 155 Irvin, L. K., 1092 James, R., 433, 544
Houlihan, D., 901 Huntley, M., 800 Isaacs, C., 427 James, V., 60
Houston, R., 583 Hunyadi, E., 521, 523 Isagai, T., 544 James, W., 234
Houwink-Manville, I., 138 Huotilainen, M., 481 Isager, T., 50, 75, 76, 212, Jamieson, B., 394, 708,
Howard, M., 497, 499, 1088 Huppke, P., 131, 138 296, 1238, 1239 967, 1030, 1046
Howard, R., 668 Hurkens, E., 171, 186, 187 Isarangkun, P. P., 462 Jamison, D. L., 79
Howie, P. M., 106 Hurley, C., 886, 911, 1033 Ishige, K., 46, 52 Jan, M. M. S., 130
Howlin, P., 24, 30, 33, 50, Hurst, J. A., 432, 1307 Ishii, T., 1224 Janicki, M. P., 1089
100, 110, 113, 170, Hurtig, R., 257, 353, 598 Ishikawa, A., 127 Janisse, J., 752
174, 185, 202, 203, Hurwic, M., 456, 457, 463 Ishikawa, N., 135, 136 Jankovic, J., 134, 150, 838,
204, 205, 206, 207, Huson, S. M., 553 Ishikawa, T., 1094 839
208, 210, 211, 212, Hutcheson, H., 438, 754 Ishizaka, Y., 1225 Janner, J. P., 905
213, 214, 215, 216, Hutcheson, J., 479 Ismond, D. R., 476, 477 Janney, R., 868
255, 259, 261, 288, Hutchinson, J. M., 1128 Isquith, P. K., 373, 794 Janols, L. O., 109
290, 292, 293, 294, Hutchinson, T., 319, 799, Isserlin, M., 1239 Janosky, J. E., 1112
295, 296, 297, 298, 942, 946, 947, 978, Itard, J., 167 Janota, I., 488, 489, 536,
299, 302, 304, 335, 1007 Itkonnen, T., 901 617, 618
342, 343, 345, 348, Hutt, C., 268 Ito, K., 389, 662, 663 Jansen, D. H. J., 481
349, 351, 373, 412, Hutt, S. J., 268 Ittenbach, R. F., 1312 Jansen, L. M., 183
436, 520, 535, 544, Huttenlocher, J., 337 Ivancic, M. T., 908, 1126, Janson, U., 554
547, 548, 587, 588, Huttenlocher, P., 658, 660, 1143 Janssen, J., 493, 499
652, 682, 687, 708, 708 Iversen, P., 459 Janssen, P. A., 1103
724, 732, 760, 872, Hutton, D., 630 Iverson, L. L., 453 Janusonis, S., 454
873, 875, 961, 1055, Hutton, J., 202, 205, 206, Iverson, S. D., 453 Janzen, J., 1004, 1016,
1056, 1057, 1064, 210, 212, 213, 215, Ivey, M., 75, 77 1017
1078, 1088, 1244 216, 290, 292, 293, Iwamura, Y., 691 Jarast, R., 1230
Hoyson, M., 394, 708, 870, 295, 296, 297, 298, Iwasaki, N., 661 Jarman, P. H., 1128
953, 967, 968, 1030, 299, 335, 342 Iwata, B. A., 152, 155, 819, Jaros, E., 463, 490
1046 Hutton, S., 1088 822, 824, 825, 901, Jarrold, C., 234, 254, 344,
Hoza, B., 177 Huws, J. C., 261 907, 908, 910, 1126, 350, 370, 393, 394,
Hresko, W., 804 Hviid, A., 44, 49, 52, 57, 1127, 1128, 1143 395, 396, 399, 608,
Hsu, M., 499 435 Iyama, C. M., 129 611, 616, 642, 643,
Hsu, W. L., 342 Hwang, B., 253, 959 Izaguirre, J., 838 645, 1307
Huang, C. T., 389 Hwang, C. P., 1055 Izard, C. E., 409, 411 Jarvis, K., 342
Huang, J., 539, 1307 Hyman, S., 134, 146, 152, Izeman, S. G., 1133 Jaselskis, C., 27, 1107
Huang, Y. W., 458 155, 436, 494, 1312 Jasinowski, C., 902
Hubbard, C. R., 145 Hymas, N., 616 Jackendoff, R., 691 Jasper, H., 663
Huber, H., 1092 Jackowski, A., 523 Jasper, S., 253, 872, 1064,
Huber, J., 588 Iacoboni, M., 28, 389, 687, Jackson, A., 186, 546 1074
Hubl, D., 521 691 Jackson, C. T., 323 Jatlow, P., 1108
Hudelot, C., 690 Iacono, T., 884 Jackson, D., 1125 Jawad, A. F., 542
Hudson, A., 1200 Iancu, D. R., 542 Jackson, J., 849 Jayakar, P., 503
Huffman, L. R., 261 Iannetti, P., 555 Jackson, R. K., 9, 385, 387, Jayne, D., 911
Huggins, R. M., 545, 569 Iarocci, G., 1182 453, 1198 Jeavons, P. M., 551
Author Index I • 17

Jefferson, G., 912 Jones, M. B., 170, 173, 185, Kamp, L. N. J., 1233 Kashiwagi, Y., 541
Jelliffe, L. L., 462 187, 427, 437, 537, Kamps, D., 251, 264, 872, Kassorla, I. C., 882
Jellinger, K., 139, 140 754, 1058 886, 887, 913, 956, Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D.
Jellum, E., 535 Jones, P. B., 107 991, 1030, 1031, 1032 G., 503
Jenkins, E., 546 Jones, R., 261, 630, 831 Kanafani, N., 60, 435 Kates, W. R., 269, 545, 547
Jenkins, S., 542 Jones, T., 555, 657, 658, Kanai, T., 458 Kato, T., 389, 662, 663
Jenks, J., 542, 1112 660 Kancki, M., 46, 52 Katsovich, L., 1103, 1106
Jennings, P., 960 Jones, V., 387 Kandt, R. S., 543, 545 Katusic, S. K., 534
Jennings, W. B., 414 Jones, W., 13, 105, 106, Kane, J. M., 1106 Katz, K., 964
Jensen, A. R., 434 191, 234, 236, 248, Kaneko, M., 455, 456, 460, Katz, M., 183
Jensen, C. M., 1096 249, 250, 260, 275, 461, 463, 542, 1224, Katz, N., 834
Jensen, J., 458, 459, 460, 313, 325, 326, 368, 1225, 1249 Katz, R. C., 870
608, 611 373, 375, 376, 412, Kaneko, S., 1225 Katzung, V. M., 616
Jensen, V., 177, 949 520, 526, 549, 595, Kaneko, W., 883, 884, 912, Kau, A. S., 547
Jensen, W. R., 47, 63 683, 684, 694, 696, 937 Kauffman, J. M., 1073
Jenson, W., 461, 534, 536, 699 Kanes, S. J., 1105 Kauffmann, C., 267
539, 540, 545, 909, Jonson, C., 299 Kanner, L., xv, 6, 13, 17, Kaufman, A. S., 642, 788,
916 Jonveaux, P., 130, 138 26, 32, 33, 74, 75, 79, 789
Jepsen, R. H., 620 Jordan, H., 352, 426, 427, 90, 91, 97, 167, 201, Kaufman, B., 958
Jerdonek, M. A., 130 428, 693, 732, 733, 204, 206, 210, 212, Kaufman, N. L., 788, 789
Jerger, J., 844 753, 756, 820 214, 215, 224, 233, Kaufman, S., 141
Jerger, S., 844 Jordan, R., 256, 322, 371, 249, 256, 288, 295, Kavanagh, M. E., 453, 455,
Jernigan, T., 367, 371, 499, 397, 399, 1011 296, 298, 299, 305, 459
549, 1282, 1305 Jorde, L., 47, 63, 537, 540 312, 324, 327, 336, Kavanaugh, R. D., 394, 395
Jezzard, P., 521 Jorgensen, E., 462 344, 346, 347, 374, Kawano, Y., 540
Jick, H., 59, 61, 435, 541 Joseph, A., 589, 838 406, 407, 419, 425, Kawasaki, Y., 502, 503,
Jimenez, F., 643 Joseph, R., 234, 260, 291, 441, 500, 584, 585, 546, 1225
Jimerson, D. C., 458 336, 368, 372, 373, 596, 628, 641, 650, Kawashima, H., 541
Jiron, C. C., 616 439, 520, 521, 609, 652, 742, 744, 750, Kawashima, K., 47, 661
Joanette, Y., 258 610, 615, 617 831, 871, 925, 962, Kawashima, R., 389, 662,
Jocelyn, L. J., 544 Joseph, S., 210, 265 1011, 1178, 1180, 663
Jocic, Z., 616 Jou, R. J., 493 1194, 1203, 1233, Kay, J. B., 697
Joe, C., 336 Jouve, J., 459 1239, 1266, 1277, 1304 Kaye, J., 59, 61, 435, 541
Johannesson, T. M., 109 Joy, H., 991 Kanodia, R., 463 Kayihan, H., 153
Johansson, E., 476, 484 Juhrs, P., 207, 298, 302, Kanwisher, N., 520, 523, Kaysen, D., 169, 181
Johansson, G., 692 1091, 1093 696 Kazak, S., 5, 426, 427
Johansson, M., 48, 62, 105, Jukes, E., 201, 204 Kao, B. T., 1064, 1065, Kazdin, A. E., 821, 822,
109, 352, 525, 553, Jundell, I., 1239 1074 897, 915, 1062
615, 666 Jung, T., 483, 493 Kaplan, A. R., 484 Keane, J., 526, 663
John, A., 734, 753, 757 Juranek, J., 538, 550, 551 Kaplan, B., 320 Keaveney, L., 180
Johnson, C., 655, 668, 669, Jure, R., 555, 832 Kaplan, D., 27, 839, 847, Keeble, S., 630
673, 1149 Jusczyk, P., 689 1107 Keel, J. H., 207, 298, 302
Johnson, C. N., 630 Just, M., 355, 487, 517, Kaplan, J. E., 1090 Keeler, W. R., 417, 554
Johnson, C. R., 189, 267, 518, 646 Kaplan, M., 832 Keer, A. M., 127
268, 367, 368, 369, Juul-Dam, N., 539 Kappas, A., 690 Keetz, A., 884, 950
372, 373, 1110, 1111 Kapucu, O., 107 Kehres, J., 258
Johnson, D., 97, 551 Kaabachi, N., 553 Kapur, S., 665, 1103 Keilitz, L., 1143
Johnson, E., 1055, 1070 Kaczmarek, L., 969, 990, Karantanos, G., 1215 Keillor, J. M., 371
Johnson, J., 875, 906, 912 991, 1030 Karapurkar, T., 57, 63, Keilp, J. G., 617
Johnson, L., 253, 899 Kadesjö, B., 48, 53, 54, 184 1070, 1123 Keith, R. W., 844
Johnson, M., 317, 318, 670, Kadivar, K., 541 Kardash, A., 17 Keliher, C., 905
671, 691, 695, 696 Kafantaris, V., 839, 840 Karlgren-Leitner, C., 544 Keller, K., 551
Johnson, R. J., 462, 553 Kagan, J., 983 Karmiloff-Smith, A., 236, Keller, T., 355, 487, 517,
Johnson, S., 630, 1004, Kahn, A. A., 460 549, 656, 657 518, 646
1005, 1006 Kahn, J. V., 149 Karns, C., 489, 492, 493, Kellet, K., 885, 956
Johnson, S. A., 646 Kahn, R. S., 183, 493, 499 496, 499, 501, 519, Kelley, L. A., 1108
Johnson, S. B., 863, 1181 Kain, W., 614 661 Kelley, M., 251, 252, 257,
Johnson, W. D., 539 Kaiser, A., 883, 884, 889, Karsh, K. G., 819, 916 258, 259, 260, 316,
Johnson, W. T., 456, 457, 890, 950 Karumanchi, S., 459, 1111 317, 352, 353
537 Kaiser, M., 695 Karumanchi, V., 459, 1111 Kelley, R. I., 544
Johnson-Frey, S. H., 524 Kalachnik, J. E., 823, 848 Kasari, A., 946 Kelly, K. M., 336
Johnston, C., 153 Kaland, N., 105 Kasari, C., 233, 236, 239, Kelly, L., 916
Johnston, F., 548 Kalaria, R. N., 443 260, 261, 318, 319, Kem, D. L., 189, 1106
Johrs, P., 1078 Kales, A., 458 320, 323, 324, 325, Kema, I. P., 454
Jolliffe, T., 105, 201, 295, Kalish, B. I., 744, 750 369, 409, 410, 411, Kemmerer, K., 956, 991
628, 630, 631, 634, Kalmanson, B., 1310 415, 416, 520, 630, Kemmotsu, N., 525
635, 641, 642, 682, Kalmijn, M., 460, 461 631, 652, 653, 655, Kemner, C., 171, 183, 478,
1284 Kalsher, M. J., 819, 825 657, 659, 660, 662, 480, 481, 482, 485,
Jones, A. C., 543, 545 Kaminsky, L., 1057, 1074, 665, 671, 673, 707, 493, 499, 684
Jones, B. L., 486 1077 946, 1007, 1020, 1034 Kemp, D., 821, 897, 911,
Jones, G., 323 Kamio, Y., 371, 1225 Kashanian, A., 499 916, 926, 1088
Jones, J. P. R., 212 Kamoshita, S., 135, 136 Kashiwagi, H., 1223 Kemp, S., 793
I • 18 Author Index

Kempas, E., 432, 433 Kincaid, D., 1092 522, 523, 524, 525, Koenig, K., 977, 1105,
Kemper, T. L., 371, 372, King, B. H., 1108, 1111 526, 595, 609, 610, 1106, 1108
463, 488, 489, 490, King, J., 183, 556 615, 617, 628, 650, Koga, Y., 100
492, 493, 494, 496, King, L., 1268 651, 652, 657, 666, Kohen-Raz, R., 387, 388,
526, 536, 1014, 1305 King, M., 460, 551 671, 682, 683, 684, 485, 486, 832, 839
Kempton, S., 617 King, N., 1061, 1065 686, 689, 691, 694, Kohler, F., 886, 937, 938,
Kendall, T., 1283 Kingstone, A., 663 696, 697, 698, 699, 939, 940, 968, 990,
Kendler, K. S., 432, 438 Kinney, E. M., 871 730, 732, 734, 735, 991, 1129, 1130
Kenemans, J. K., 482 Kinsbourne, M., 1067, 1177 736, 739, 740, 750, Kohler, R., 953
Kennedy, C. H., 267, 542, Kirby, B. L., 113 772, 773, 777, 779, Kohn, A. E., 267, 1109
886, 901, 904, 1143 Kirchner, D., 806 782, 784, 789, 793, Kohn, Y., 299
Kennedy, D., 355, 494, 495, Kirk, K. S., 1111 800, 803, 809, 810, Kokkinaki, T., 383
517, 536, 538 Kirk, U., 793 813, 820, 832, 835, Kolachana, B. S., 526
Kenworthy, L., 373, 613 Kirkham, N., 671 889, 946, 960, 961, Kolevzon, A., 753
Keogh, M., 30, 335 Kirkpatrick-Sanchez, S., 962, 977, 979, 980, Koller, R., 301
Keogh, W., 149 909, 910 982, 983, 984, 998, Kolmen, B. K., 1112
Kerbeshian, J., 27, 28, 46, Kirwin, P. D., 456 1102, 1106, 1109, Kolodny, E., 77, 140
52, 55, 71, 75, 77, 101, Kisacky, K. L., 886, 887 1121, 1221 Kologinsky, E., 885, 1089
135, 539, 588, 838, Kishino, T., 551 Klinger, L. G., 369, 371, Koluchova, J., 591
1111 Kissel, R. C., 1128 374, 642 Kolvin, I., 8, 9, 15, 17, 70,
Kerdelhue, B., 459, 462 Kita, M., 73, 74, 749, 1312 Klinger, M. R., 369, 371 79, 179, 180, 585, 592
Kern, J. K., 542 Kitahara, H., 127 Klitz, W., 431 Komatsu, F., 1225
Kern, L., 820, 823, 865, Kitsukawa, K., 904 Kluckman, K., 454 Komori, H., 540
866, 869 Kitt, C. A., 142 Klug, M. G., 539 Komoto, J., 28
Kern, R. A., 1110 Kjelgaard, M., 342, 343, Klykylo, W. M., 457, 462 Kondo, I., 138
Kern, S. J., 436 344, 350 Knight, R. T., 524, 615, Koning, C., 323, 348
Kern-Dunlap, L., 820, 1032 Kjellgren, G., 48, 62 1306 Konkol, O., 149
Kerns, K. A., 619 Kjoerholt, K., 154 Knivsberg, A. M., 462 Konstantareas, M., 301,
Kerr, A., 127, 136, 137, Klaiman, C., 523 Knobler, H., 459 335, 550, 551, 749,
139, 147, 148, 151, Klare, G. R., 1143 Knobloch, H., 50 1072
153 Klauck, S. M., 130, 432, Knoll, J. H., 551 Koon, R., 607, 616
Kerr, B., 556 433, 546 Knott, F., 1057, 1077 Koop, S., 461
Kerr, M. M., 1030 Kleczkowska, A., 546 Knouf, N., 523 Koos, O., 629
Kerwin, R. W., 107 Kleefstra, T., 78 Knowles, T., 267 Kootz, J. P., 478
Keshavan, M., 491, 493, Kleiman, K., 806 Knox, S. H., 845 Kooy, R. F., 545
494, 498, 499, 500, Kleiman, M. D., 499 Kobagasi, S., 1281 Kopin, I. J., 458
519, 660, 661, 667 Klein, D. F., 181, 182 Kobayashi, R., 73, 74, 80, Kopp, J., 886
Keskr, J., 909 Klein, K., 31, 105, 227, 100, 202, 204, 206, Koppenhaver, D., 154, 155,
Kestenbaum, C., 183 232, 233, 234, 266, 212, 213, 214, 288, 802, 804
Ketelaars, C., 753 707, 708, 733, 734, 289, 291, 293, 296, Korein, J., 460, 461
Ketelsen, D., 1031 741, 754, 755 298, 1224, 1225, 1312 Korenberg, J. R., 548
Key, E., 1239 Klein, L. S., 253, 392 Kobayashi, S., 886 Korf, J., 456
Keyes, J., 909 Kleinhans, N., 525 Koburn, A., 1108 Korkman, M., 793
Keymeulen, H., 177 Kleinman, J., 521, 522, Koch, D., 251, 416, 417 Korn, S., 7
Khan, K., 542, 1112 523, 524, 526, 716 Kochanska, G., 386 Korvatska, E., 542, 1312
Khoromi, S., 140 Klevstrand, M., 155 Kocoglu, S. R., 130 Korzilius, H., 898
Kielinen, M., 48, 534, 544 Klicpera, C., 24, 91, 96, 97 Koczat, D., 485 Kosen, R. L., 1183
Kientz, M., 366, 536, 832, Kliewer, C., 1031 Koechlin, E., 668, 669 Koshes, R. J., 269
834, 843, 981, 983 Klima, E. S., 549 Koegel, B. L., 1180 Koshimoto, T., 1225
Kienzl, E., 141, 142 Klin, A., xix, 5, 7, 13, 14, Koegel, L., 874, 875, 883, Koslowski, L. T., 692
Kiernan, W. E., 1088, 1093 18, 19, 20, 24, 25, 28, 886, 900, 911, 912, Kotsopoulos, S., 462, 544
Kiesewetter, K., 128 30, 31, 32, 33, 41, 45, 913, 926, 929, 930, Kozinetz, C. A., 136
Kilgore, E., 1108 72, 74, 88, 91, 92, 93, 932, 934, 937, 940, Kozloff, M. A., 1055, 1060,
Kilifarski, M., 429, 432, 95, 96, 97, 99, 100, 941, 950, 951, 952, 1065
433, 439, 754 102, 103, 104, 105, 953, 954, 961, 978, Kozma, C., 552
Killcross, A. S., 642 106, 107, 108, 109, 1004, 1005, 1006, Kraemer, G., 658
Killian, W., 139, 142, 145 110, 111, 113, 124, 1009, 1025, 1033, Kraemer, H., 21, 29, 314,
Kilman, A., 103, 369, 374 170, 173, 174, 175, 1049, 1061, 1064, 335, 437, 733, 754,
Kilman, B. A., 24, 477, 179, 184, 186, 191, 1183 1307
479, 480, 482 213, 224, 225, 227, Koegel, R., 264, 268, 821, Kraft, I. A., 462, 553
Kiln, A., 73 232, 233, 234, 236, 823, 874, 875, 882, Krageloh-Mann, I., 140
Kilpatrick, M., 498, 551 238, 239, 248, 249, 883, 884, 886, 887, Kraijer, D., 251, 753
Kilsby, M., 207 250, 251, 256, 263, 888, 889, 890, 900, Kramer, A., 668, 669
Kim, C. A., 553 268, 269, 275, 293, 906, 911, 912, 913, Kramer, J. H., 645
Kim, J., 100, 101, 208, 269, 294, 303, 313, 316, 926, 929, 930, 932, Kramer, M., 17
1014 317, 318, 319, 321, 934, 937, 940, 941, Kramer, N., 138
Kim, K., 889 323, 325, 326, 341, 950, 951, 952, 953, Krams, M., 371, 495, 497,
Kim, S.-J., 429, 439 343, 348, 350, 352, 954, 978, 1004, 1005, 1305
Kimberg, D. Y., 388 353, 354, 365, 367, 1006, 1009, 1033, Krantz, P., 261, 298, 299,
Kimbrough-Oller, D., 800 368, 372, 373, 374, 1049, 1055, 1058, 354, 867, 869, 871,
Kimchi, R., 640, 643 375, 376, 412, 419, 1060, 1061, 1064, 873, 875, 883, 884,
Kimura, D., 633, 634 498, 519, 520, 521, 1129, 1160, 1183 887, 913, 950, 953,
Author Index I • 19

959, 968, 985, 995, Kuriya, N., 540 Landesman-Dwyer, S., 1245 Laushey, K. M., 1032
1004, 1009, 1010, Kurland, L. T., 51 Landgren, M., 544 Lavenex, P., 526, 1306
1029, 1030, 1035, Kuroda, Y., 461, 499, 539 Landis, B. H., 458 LaVigna, G. W., 902, 903,
1044, 1045, 1046, Kurosawa, M., 435 Landis, H. D., 459 904, 905, 906, 907,
1047, 1061, 1127, Kurtz, M. M., 619 Landon-Jimenez, D., 30, 966, 1076
1129, 1132, 1143 Kurtz, P. F., 903, 904, 908, 335 Lavin, D. R., 795
Krasny, L., 73, 77, 78 909 Landrus, R., 758, 1176, Law, P. A., 1111
Kratochwill, T. R., 1059 Kurtzberg, A., 480, 482 1216 Lawlor, B. A., 437, 754
Krause, I., 542 Kurtzberg, D., 479, 480 Landry, R., 832 Lawrence, A., 609, 663
Krauss, M. W., 214, 288, Kurtzer, D., 479, 482 Landry, S., 236, 249, 251, Lawrence, L. G., 208, 269
289, 290, 293, 295, Kustan, E., 1106 254, 255, 256, 320, Lawry, J., 968
297, 323, 324, 1055, Kustanovich, V., 1307 346, 351, 653, 947 Laws, G., 549
1059, 1075, 1077 Kusunoki, T., 1225 Landsdown, R., 201 Lawson, J., 628, 632, 634
Kravits, T., 251, 886, 887, Kutas, M., 482 Lane, H., 583 Lawson, W., 201, 203
913, 956, 991, 1030, Kutty, K. M., 462, 544 Lane, J. B., 143 Layton, T., 948, 949, 955,
1031 Kwiatkowski, D., 543 Lane, R., 668 957
Krawczak, M., 543, 545 Kwiatkowski, J., 813 Lane, S., 478 Lazar, N. A., 373, 485, 496,
Krawiecki, N., 76 Kwon, H., 108, 170, 185, Lanfermann, H., 521 525
Kream, J., 456, 458 521, 526 Lang, B., 613, 614, 615 Lazenby, A. L., 641
Kregel, J., 1089, 1091, Kwon, J., 325, 410, 652, Lang, C., 542, 1112 Le, L., 871, 885, 887
1092, 1093, 1094 671, 707 Lang, J., 20, 779 Lea, S. E., 692
Kreitner, R., 1127 Kyllerman, M., 551 Langdell, T., 324, 384, 392, Leach, D. J., 1070
Kremer-Sadlik, T., 868 Kyrios, M., 617 412, 413, 520, 652, 689 Leaf, R., 864, 868, 870,
Kreppner, J. M., 180 Lange, N., 355, 493, 494, 874, 875, 891, 929,
Kreps-Falk, R., 552 Laan, L., 551 496, 497, 498, 519, 1004
Kresch, L. E., 27, 178 Laccone, F., 131, 138 1305 League, R., 83, 800
Kreutzer, J., 479, 482, 619 Lachar, D., 1110, 1111 Langer, S. N., 824, 899 Leary, M. R., 387, 838
Kricheff, I. I., 492 Lachman, S. R., 908 Langstrom, B., 458 Leaver, E., 483, 493, 663,
Kriegstein, K. V., 524 Lafargue, R. T., 27, 839, Langworthy-Lam, K., 838 671
Kristiansson, M., 299 847, 1107 Lanquetot, R., 899 Leavitt, B. R., 546
Kristoff, B., 708, 949, LaFluer, B., 130, 138, 139, Lansdown, R., 295 Lebel, L. A., 1106
1004, 1032, 1034 148 Lansing, M., 543, 757, Lebiedowska, M. K., 839
Kroeker, R., 266 La Franchi, S., 484 1176 LeBlanc, L., 885, 906,
Kromberg, J. G., 555, 556 Lagercrantz, H., 139 Lanuzza, B., 479, 481, 482 956
Krug, D. A., 476, 719, 731, Lagomarcino, T., 1089, Lanzi, G., 539 Leboyer, M., 189, 326, 434,
733, 741, 747, 750, 1092, 1094 LaPerchia, P., 539 459, 462, 517, 618,
762, 1004, 1005, 1006, Lahey, M., 337, 813 Lappalainien, R., 143 1112
1007, 1008, 1009, Lahuis, B. E., 493, 499 Larcombe, M., 595, 745, Leckman, J., 27, 456, 459,
1012, 1023, 1199 Lai, C. S., 432, 1307 755 462, 546, 719, 720,
Kruger, A., 237, 318, 337, Lai, Z., 260, 549 Larmande, C., 240, 689, 750, 838, 848, 1103,
369, 383 Laing, E., 549 708, 751 1111, 1306
Kubba, H., 555 Lainhart, J., 28, 73, 77, 78, Larnaout, A., 553 Le Couteur, A., 5, 8, 25,
Kuddo, T., 541 208, 210, 269, 270, Laroche, C., 551 26, 28, 33, 45, 51, 65,
Kudo, S., 78 427, 428, 439, 489, Laron, Z., 1220 76, 93, 95, 101, 107,
Kugiumutzakis, G., 383 500, 501, 536, 539, 618 Larrandaburu, M., 553 108, 110, 167, 170,
Kuhaneck, H. M., 842 Laitenen, R., 823 Larrieu, J. A., 177 213, 214, 227, 228,
Kuhl, D. P., 545 Lake, R., 459 Larsen, F. W., 203, 204, 232, 248, 265, 290,
Kuhl, P., 224, 225, 481, Lakesdown, R., 1284 206, 212, 213 294, 315, 323, 341,
482, 833, 834, 837, Lakoff, G., 695 Larsen, J., 541 342, 346, 347, 348,
846, 962 Lal, S., 456, 457 Larson, K., 539 412, 419, 425, 426,
Kuhr, R., 1064, 1065, 1075 Lalande, M., 551 Larsson, E. V., 886 427, 429, 434, 436,
Kuipers, E., 619 Lalli, J. S., 822, 901, 907 Lasker, A., 484, 485 437, 438, 439, 442,
Kulesz, A., 740 Lam, A. Y., 429 Laski, K. E., 950, 953, 462, 489, 500, 520,
Kulldorff, M., 64 Lam, M. K., 758 1004, 1009, 1061, 1129 534, 536, 537, 538,
Kulomaki, T., 389 Lamabrecht, L., 1124 Lassen, N. A., 140 539, 546, 547, 553,
Kumar, A., 140 Lamb, J., 432, 433, 438 Laszlo, A., 549 554, 594, 599, 617,
Kumari, V., 107, 493 Lamb, M. R., 645 Latif, R., 137 632, 712, 722, 731,
Kumashiro, H., 227, 460, Lambert, R., 107, 646 Lattimore, J., 1128 734, 740, 753, 754,
463, 542, 834, 1224, Lambrecht, L., 196, 228, Lauder, J. M., 454 775, 779, 832, 1124,
1225 233, 348, 541, 547, Laulhere, T., 342, 542, 752, 1143, 1215, 1305
Kumazawa, T., 1281 551, 655, 712, 731, 757, 762, 1112 Lecuyer, C., 690
Kumra, S., 183 739, 753, 756, 757, Launay, J., 459, 462, 1112 L’Ecuyer, S., 167, 169, 757
Kunce, L. J., 213 808 Laurent, A., 367, 926, 927, Ledda, M. G., 1103
Kunihiro, T., 667 Lampe, M., 844 928, 929, 932, 933, Leddet, I., 751
Kuoch, H., 869 Landa, R., 170, 173, 195, 935, 936, 937, 938, LeDoux, J., 525, 526, 652,
Kuperman, S., 212, 494, 197, 295, 352, 427, 939, 940, 941, 942, 686
499 428, 439, 484, 485, 978, 979, 980, 981, Lee, A., 237, 256, 260, 261,
Kupretz, S. S., 456 489, 500, 501, 536, 982, 983, 986, 989, 344, 347, 384, 389,
Kurita, H., 72, 73, 74, 76, 537, 538, 539, 808, 990, 995, 996, 997 390, 396, 411, 412,
79, 80, 167, 173, 224, 809, 979, 1111 Lauritsen, M., 185, 188, 413, 414, 415, 416,
270, 342, 749, 1224, Landaburu, H. J., 908 430, 435, 453, 534, 418, 436, 520, 554,
1311, 1312 Landau, B., 693 551 555, 689, 836
I • 20 Author Index

Lee, C., 154, 544 Leventhal, B., 45, 348, 655 Lincoln, A., 97, 100, 104, Lockyer, L., 8, 201, 204,
Lee, D., 268 Leventhal, B. I., 474 107, 260, 375, 474, 205, 206, 210, 213,
Lee, E. B., 31, 227, 232, Leventhal, B. L., 169, 170, 549, 551, 642, 655, 215, 247, 292, 293,
233, 707, 708, 721, 196, 228, 233, 326, 656, 659, 660, 668, 294, 296, 588, 641,
734 429, 439, 455, 456, 835, 836 962
Lee, E. W. H., 209 459, 539, 541, 547, Lincoln, A. E., 24 Loder, R. T., 154
Lee, H. K., 515 551, 712, 731, 732, Lincoln, A. J., 103, 183, Loening-Baucke, V., 541
Lee, J., 454 739, 753, 754, 756, 268, 367, 369, 374, Loesch, D. Z., 545, 569
Lee, M., 463, 490, 549 757, 761, 808, 1107, 476, 477, 479, 480, Loew, L. H., 321
Lee, S. S. J., 130 1124 482, 494, 496, 499 Lofgren, D. P., 183
Lee, T., 643 Levesque, S., 545 Lindamood, T., 482 Loftin, J., 1180
Lee, Y. H., 484 Levi, G., 1221, 1222, 1223 Lindauer, S. E., 821 Logan, G. D., 608, 617
Leehey, M., 545, 546 Levin, A. V., 696 Lindback, T., 462 Lohr, J., 496
Leekam, S., 24, 93, 94, Levin, C. G., 1003 Lindblom, R., 51, 227, 232, Loiselle, R. H., 553
167, 172, 236, 318, Levin, J., 253 329, 834 Lombard, J., 549
348, 593, 594, 595, Levin, L., 821, 897, 916, Lindem, K., 183 Lombroso, P. J., 136, 138,
615, 632, 641, 643, 926, 1044, 1046, 1064, Lindenbaum, R. H., 51 552
650, 654, 655, 663, 1074 Linder, T. W., 846 Long, E. S., 824
735, 736, 745, 755, Levine, B., 619 Lindgren, V., 474, 551 Long, F. L., 550
756 Levine, D. N., 646 Lindhout, D., 556 Long, J. S., 882, 887
Leenaars, A. A., 97 Levine, M., 1021 Lindsay, R. L., 1102, 1109, Long, S., 966, 1180
Leevers, H. J., 396, 399 Levine, R. A., 545, 546 1111 Longaker, T., 1021
Lefebre, D., 912 Levinson, D., 1219 Lindsay, W. R., 914 Longhi, E., 549
Legacy, S. M., 907 Levitas, A., 546 Lindstrom, E., 590 Longo, I., 78, 130, 551
Le Grand, R., 696 Levitt, J. G., 499, 549, Lind-White, C., 542 Lonigan, C. J., 863, 1181
Leibenluft, E., 181 1305 Linetsky, E., 255 Lonnerholm, G., 462
Leiber, B., 127 Levy, D., 552 Ling, A. H., 318 Loomis, M. E., 1126
Leigh, R. J., 484 Levy, E. R., 1307 Ling, D., 318 Loos, L., 888, 1004, 1005,
Leiguarda, R., 107, 538 Levy, R., 1126, 1128 Lingam, R., 435, 541 1006, 1007, 1008,
Leinikki, P., 541 Levy, S., 539, 542 Lingham, S., 464 1009, 1012, 1058, 1129
Leininger, L., 101, 187 Levy, S. E., 1312 Link, C. G., 1105 Lopez, A. G., 991
Leiter, R. G., 83, 790 Levy, S. M., 1088, 1095 Links, P. S., 501 Lopez, B., 236, 318, 641,
Lekman, A., 139, 142 Lewin, S., 127 Linna, S. L., 48, 534, 544 643
LeLaurin, K., 1130 Lewine, J. D., 503 Linnell, J., 435, 541 Lopez-Martin, V., 544
Lelord, G., 459, 751, 1208 Lewis, C., 1057, 1077 Linscheid, T., 460 Lopreiato, J., 436
Lelwica, M., 690 Lewis, H., 394, 708, 1050, Linschoten, L., 476 Lord, C., 5, 7, 13, 14, 18,
Lemanek, K., 320, 321, 1051 Lipkin, W. I., 542 19, 29, 31, 32, 45, 72,
355, 408, 707, 732 Lewis, J. C., 543, 545 Lisch, S., 27, 546 75, 92, 93, 96, 110,
Le Maréchal, K., 586, 588 Lewis, J. D., 542, 1112 Lison, C. A., 886 169, 170, 174, 175,
Lembrecht, L., 45, 326 Lewis, K. R., 134, 1088, Liss, M., 251, 616 179, 184, 186, 196,
Lempert, H., 341 1093 Lissner, K., 1017 202, 203, 204, 205,
Lempp, R., 1240, 1241 Lewis, M., 149, 265, 267, Lissner, L. S., 609 206, 210, 214, 215,
Lenane, M., 169, 181, 183 319, 838, 839, 847, Lister Brook, S., 586, 598 216, 223, 224, 226,
Lennon, S., 27, 189, 1110 848 Little, J., 794 227, 228, 229, 231,
Lennox, C., 903 Lewis, S. E., 228, 231, 319, Little, L., 250 232, 233, 234, 238,
Lennox, D. B., 914 409 Littleford, C., 321, 384, 239, 248, 251, 252,
Lenoir, P., 229, 232, 240, Lewis, T. L., 696 385, 386, 708, 836, 265, 288, 289, 290,
366, 689, 708, 712, Lewis, V., 237, 324, 370, 837 291, 292, 293, 294,
751, 834 393, 394, 395, 412, Littlejohns, C. S., 101, 210, 295, 297, 298, 304,
Lensing, P., 1112 519, 689 265 312, 315, 316, 317,
Leonard, B., 991, 1032 Lewy, A., 235, 268, 320, Littlewood, R., 870 322, 323, 324, 325,
Leonard, H., 127, 143 480, 656, 659, 959 Litwiller, M., 1108 326, 335, 336, 341,
Leonard, J., 645 Leysen, J. E., 1103 Liu, C., 541 342, 343, 344, 346,
Leonard, M. A., 253 Lhermitte, F., 255 Liu, D. H., 551 348, 349, 350, 351,
Lepistö, T., 481, 482 Li, J., 48, 54, 60, 435 Liu, J., 432, 433, 438, 439, 352, 354, 365, 369,
Lerman-Sagie, T., 550 Libby, S., 24, 93, 94, 322, 754 375, 376, 385, 408,
Lerner, M., 1109 397, 399, 593, 594, Livesey, D., 619 409, 426, 429, 438,
Lerner, Y., 459 595, 735, 736, 745, Livingstone, S., 555 439, 520, 537, 539,
Lese, C. M., 551 755, 756, 1011 Livni, E., 1220 541, 547, 551, 553,
Lesesne, C., 1062 Lichtenberger, L., 548, 549 Lloyd, D. J., 555, 556 554, 599, 650, 655,
Leshinsky-Silver, E., 550 Lickliter, R., 656, 657 Lloyd, K. G., 142 660, 689, 691, 707,
Leslie, A., 186, 254, 259, Liddell, M. B., 443 Loban, W., 340 708, 709, 712, 721,
368, 393, 394, 615, Liddle, P. F., 518 Lobato, D. J., 1064, 1065, 722, 723, 730, 731,
628, 629, 630, 631, Lieb-Lundell, C., 150, 151, 1074 732, 733, 734, 739,
632, 652, 654, 655, 153 Lobo, S., 48, 59, 62 740, 741, 749, 750,
666, 669, 671, 690 Liebowitz, M. R., 181, 182 Locascio, J. J., 462, 839, 753, 754, 755, 756,
Leslie, K. R., 524 Lieu, T. A., 436 840 757, 759, 761, 763,
Lettick, A. L., 301 Light, J., 956 Locke, P., 264, 803 775, 777, 778, 779,
Letts, C., 804 Lightdale, J. R., 541, 542 Lockhart, L., 546 780, 781, 784, 792,
Lev, D., 550 Likins, M., 916 Lockshin, S. B., 1035, 793, 802, 808, 818,
LeVasseur, P., 869, 984 Lin, Q., 478 1043, 1044, 1046, 820, 832, 834, 889,
Leven, L. I., 453 Linarello, C., 1029, 1030 1047, 1130, 1138 926, 946, 947, 955,
Author Index I • 21

960, 961, 962, 967, Lugaresi, E., 152 Maertens, P., 463 Manjiviona, J., 33, 173,
969, 978, 980, 984, Luiselli, J. K., 902 Maes, B., 546 174, 373, 387, 835
988, 1004, 1005, 1030, Luk, E., 617 Maestrini, E., 130, 430, Mank, D., 1089, 1094,
1031, 1033, 1036, Luk, F. L., 209 432, 438 1095
1071, 1076, 1077, Luna, B., 185, 373, 484, Maestro, S., 225, 234, 236, Mankoski, R., 32, 432, 439,
1124, 1132, 1143, 485, 496, 498, 525, 243 731, 733, 754, 755
1177, 1215, 1216, 1221 655, 668, 669, 673 Magill-Evans, J., 232, 323, Manly, J. T., 1062
Lord, J., 1096 Lund, C. E., 848 348 Mann, J. J., 453
Lorge, I., 348 Lund, D., 1125 Magito-McLaughlin, D., Mann, L. H., 1133
Lorimer, P. A., 869, 913 Lund, J., 548 897 Mann, N. A., 321
Losche, G., 224, 237 Lundahl, A., 886 Magliano, J. P., 354 Mann, S. M., 551
Losh, M., 259, 260, 353 Lunetta, K. L., 431 Magnussen, P., 432, 433 Mannheim, G. B., 27, 267,
Lossie, A., 551 Lung, F. W., 607 Magnusson, K., 1243 367, 368, 610, 617,
Loth, F. L., 171, 186, 187 Lunsky, Y. J., 301 Magnusson, P., 5, 49, 55, 1106
Lotspeich, L., 108, 170, Luria, A. R., 608 59 Manning, A., 964
185, 315, 426, 432, Lurier, A., 1110, 1111 Magnússon, P., 753 Manning, J. T., 461
433, 437, 440, 453, Lutchmaya, S., 634, 636 Magnusson, P. K. E., 432, Manning-Courtney, P., 541,
521, 526, 617, 754 Luteijn, E., 177, 268, 747, 433 542
Lotter, V., 42, 44, 46, 52, 748 Maher, E., 550 Mannion, K., 1093
201, 204, 205, 206, Luteijn, F., 747, 748 Maher, J., 264 Mannuzza, S., 181, 182
210, 212, 215, 288, Luthert, P., 488, 489, 500, Maher, K. R., 460 Manolson, H., 958
293, 296, 434, 545, 536, 546, 617, 618, Mahler, M., 9, 584, 587, Mansell, J., 1245
585, 594, 1076, 1243 1305 1243 Manstead, A. S. R., 519
Lou, H., 140, 144 Lutz, J., 1239 Mahoney, W., 77, 78, 170, Mantel, N., 64
Lovaas, N. W., 253 Lutzker, J. R., 886, 911 172, 173, 174, 190, Mao, R., 551
Lovaas, O., 926, 929, 932, Luu, C., 496 437, 731, 734, 753, Marans, S., 804
933, 937, 939, 940, Luu, P., 668, 669, 670, 673 754 Marans, W. D., 74, 107,
941 Luyster, R., 342 Mailloux, Z., 835, 836, 108, 111, 721, 735,
Lovaas, O. I., 155, 189, Lynch, N., 189, 462 846, 847 772, 773, 782
253, 268, 288, 346, Lyon, G. R., 981 Main, B., 27, 588, 838 Marchand-Martella, N.,
708, 873, 874, 875, Lyon, M. F., 138 Maiti, A. K., 832 1023
876, 882, 883, 884, Majnemer, A., 535 Marchant, R., 1244
887, 889, 891, 915, Maag, J. W., 458, 459, 903 Makeig, S., 483, 493, 663, Marchetti, B., 542
948, 961, 966, 1003, MacAndie, C., 555 671 Marcin, C., 1231
1004, 1005, 1006, Macdonald, H., 170, 171, Makita, K., 71 Marcus, B. A., 906, 908
1008, 1012, 1029, 412, 426, 427, 434, Makris, N., 355, 489, 493, Marcus, J., 183
1035, 1038, 1043, 438, 442, 520, 534, 494, 495, 517, 1305 Marcus, L., 290, 351, 543,
1047, 1048, 1049, 536, 537, 539, 586, Malecha, M., 267 721, 749, 757, 962,
1051, 1055, 1060, 594, 617, 632 Maley, A., 428 1004, 1005, 1036,
1061, 1089, 1132, MacDonald, R. B., 900 Maley, K., 1031 1055, 1056, 1060,
1141, 1142, 1180, MacDonald, R. F., 824, 899 Malhotra, A. K., 431 1061, 1064, 1065,
1181, 1284, 1310 MacDuff, G. S., 298, 299, Malhotra, S., 74, 76, 77, 1066, 1067, 1068,
Lovaas, O. L., 346 875, 913, 1127 78, 175 1069, 1070, 1071,
Love, S. R., 833, 914 MacDuff, M. T., 913, 1046, Malicki, D. M., 138, 432 1073, 1075, 1132,
Loveland, K., 236, 248, 1061 Malik, M., 435, 541 1176, 1177, 1216
249, 251, 252, 254, Mace, F. C., 822, 901 Malin-Ingvar, M., 80, 82 Marcy, S., 150, 153
255, 256, 257, 258, MacFarlane, M., 555 Malkmus, D., 1092 Marendaz, C., 645
259, 260, 261, 275, Machado, C. J., 526 Mallikarjuhn, M., 493, 498, Marfo, K., 934
316, 317, 320, 324, Machado, N., 438, 541 499, 661 Margaretten, N. C., 542
346, 347, 350, 351, Macintosh, K. E., 174, 213 Mallinger, A. G., 491, 500 Margerotte, G., 1177
352, 353, 354, 385, Mack, K. K., 177 Malone, R. P., 839, 840, Margolin, R. A., 1015
399, 412, 478, 653, Mackay, M., 544 1102, 1105 Mari, F., 78
670, 833, 947, 1110, MacLean, C. J., 432 Malott, R. W., 1125, 1126 Mari, M., 536
1111 MacLean, J., 77, 78, 170, Maltby, N., 520 Marinelli, B., 478
Lovell, A., 632 173, 187, 1058 Malvy, J., 751 Marin-Garcia, J., 550
Lovett, K., 1177 Maclean, J. E., 172, 173, Mamelle, N., 47, 52 Markham, C. H., 484
Lovett, P., 1177 174, 427, 437, 731, Mamourian, A. C., 550 Markle, S. M., 1125
Lowe, J. B., 548 734, 753, 754 Mancina, C., 886, 887 Markman, A. N., 690
Lowe, T. L., 50, 460, 463, MacLean, W. E., Jr., 1183 Mancuso, V., 433, 474 Markowitz, J., 541, 1089,
539, 553 Macleay, A., 460 Mandalawitz, M., 1004 1095
Lubetsky, M., 189, 909, Macleod, H. A., 127 Mandel, D., 849 Markowitz, P., 1306
1110, 1111 MacMahon, B., 51, 76, 539, Mandelbaum, D. E., 1307 Marks, D., 536
Lucarelli, S., 539 1305 Mandell, D. S., 1312 Marks, R. E., 1110
Lucci, D., 29, 30, 519, 520 MacMillan, D. L., 874, Mandlawitz, M. R., 1125, Marks, S., 549, 1021
Luce, S., 891, 906, 910, 1182 1173 Markusic, D., 550
936, 1003, 1008, 1030, MacNaughton, N., 136 Manes, F., 494, 499, 519 Marlow, A. J., 430, 438
1128 MacPhie, I. L., 438 Manfredi, M., 691 Marois, R., 521, 522, 523,
Lucki, I., 453 Maddalena, A., 545 Mangelsdorf, M. E., 544 524, 526
Ludlow, L. H., 845 Madsen, C. H., 1126, 1127 Mangiapanello, K., 871 Marques-Dias, M. J., 553
Ludowese, C. J., 551 Madsen, K. E., 435 Mangner, T., 490, 517 Marquis, J., 347, 897
Ludwig, R., 499 Madsen, K. M., 44, 49, 52, Mangun, G. R., 646 Marraffa, C., 536
Lueger, R. J., 607, 616 57, 435 Manikam, R., 1029, 1030 Marriage, K., 101
I • 22 Author Index

Mars, A., 49, 52, 57, 60, Maughan, A., 1062 1004, 1009, 1010, McGee, J., 946, 947, 955,
185 Mauldin-Jourdain, M. L., 1029, 1030, 1035, 967, 969
Marsden, D. D., 838 526, 1306 1044, 1045, 1046, McGhee, D. E., 1110
Marsh, J. T., 481 Maurer, D., 696 1047, 1061, 1127, McGill, P., 1245
Marsh, R. D., 1105 Maurer, R., 367, 387, 488, 1129, 1130, 1132, 1143 McGill-Evans, J., 845
Marshall, A. M., 883, 884 590, 674, 837, 838, McClelland, R. J., 477 McGillivary, L., 813
Marshall, J. C., 646 839, 840 McComas, J. J., 253 McGimsey, J., 819, 1183
Martella, R. C., 1023 Maurice, C., 891, 936, McConachie, H., 554, 589 McGinniss, M. J., 545
Martens, B. K., 915 1003, 1008 McConnachie, G., 821, McGoldrick, M., 1068
Martin, A., 268, 269, 520, Mauthner, N., 613 897, 916, 926 McGonigle, J. J., 909, 1107
524, 696, 762, 1102, Mavrolefteros, G., 181 McConnell, S., 324, 967, McGough, J., 189, 839,
1105, 1106, 1108, Mavropoulou, S., 1217 968, 991, 1031 1307, 1310
1109 Mawhood, L., 30, 33, 202, McCormick, M. C., 435 McGrath, A. M., 870, 876
Martin, G. L., 1125 203, 204, 206, 207, McCormick, T. A., 344, McGrath, J., 844
Martin, J., 431, 663, 664, 210, 214, 216, 290, 642 McGrath, L., 32, 495, 521,
669 292, 293, 294, 295, McCoy, J. F., 1125 731, 733, 754, 755
Martin, N., 594, 819, 821, 298, 299, 302, 343, McCracken, J., 182, 189, McGrother, C., 548
823, 897, 1061, 1064 348, 349, 351, 352, 208, 295, 428, 499, McGuffin, P., 431
Martindale, B., 619 426, 436, 588, 732, 549, 839, 1102, 1103, McGuire, L., 832, 833
Martineau, J., 240, 459, 733, 753, 756, 760, 1104, 1305, 1310 McGuire, R. J., 210, 212,
542, 689, 708, 751, 820, 961 McCullough, K. M., 886 587
1208 Mawson, D., 101, 299 McCune-Nicholich, L., 391, McHale, S., 351, 820, 962,
Martinez, J., 544 Maxwell, A. P., 541 393 1057, 1068, 1074
Martinez, M., 432 Maxwell, K., 539 McDaniel, C., 692 McInnes, L., 609
Martinez-Bermejo, A., 544 Maybery, M., 428 McDermott, C., 544 McIntosh, D. E., 1061
Martin-Ruiz, C., 463, 490 Mayer, M., 812 McDermott, J., 520, 696 McIntosh, D. N., 844
Martinsson, T., 551 Mayes, A., 497, 499 McDonald, J. A., 524 McKay, B., 1103
Martsolf, J. T., 539 Mayes, L., 234, 236, 317, McDonald, M., 551 McKean, T., 1089, 1281
Maruff, P., 617 318, 345, 518, 526, McDonald-McGinn, D., McKeegan, G. F., 902
Maruki, K., 141 739, 1014 552 McKellop, J. M., 430
Maruyama, G., 205 Mayes, S. D., 93, 294, 374 McDonnell, J., 264, 1015 McKelvey, J. R., 107, 646
Masaaki, I., 140 Maynard, J., 543 McDonough, L., 237, 253, McKelvie, S. J., 643
Maslin-Cole, C., 238, 322, Mays, L. Z., 542, 551, 752, 384, 393, 395 McKenna, K., 169, 181
416, 417, 1057 757, 762, 1112 McDougle, C., 27, 178, McKenna, M., 526
Mason, A., 484 Mazaleski, J. L., 901, 907 189, 211, 267, 454, McKenna, P. J., 848
Mason, D., 883, 950, 1046 Maziade, M., 476, 477, 478 456, 526, 821, 1102, McKissick, F. C., 251, 415
Mason, S. S., 903 Mazure, C., 1103 1103, 1104, 1105, McLaughlin-Cheng, E., 252
Mason, W. A., 526, 1306 Mazziotta, J. C., 140, 389 1106, 1107, 1108, McLennan, J. D., 251
Mason-Brothers, A., 47, Mazzocco, M. M., 269, 1109, 1282 McMahon, R., 1060, 1062
63, 299, 461, 500, 534, 545, 547 McDowell, L., 908 McMahon, W., 368, 372,
536, 537, 539, 540, Mazzoncini, B., 1223 McDuff, M. T., 1004, 1009 433, 437, 461, 498,
545 Mazzone, D., 539 McDuffie, A., 664, 665, 534, 536, 539, 542,
Masser-Frye, D. S., 550 McAfee, J., 872, 982, 667, 670 545, 609, 610, 615,
Masson, G., 486, 642 992 McEachin, J., 708, 864, 616, 617, 618, 643,
Masters, J. C., 744 McAleer, O., 616 868, 870, 874, 875, 722, 752, 754, 757,
Mateer, C. A., 619 McAlonan, G., 107, 493 891, 915, 926, 929, 762, 1111, 1112
Matese, M. J., 833 McArthur, A. J., 144 1004, 1048 McManus, E. J., 543
Matey, L., 1035, 1043, McArthur, D., 235 McEvoy, M. A., 968, 991, McMorrow, M. J., 910
1044, 1046, 1047, McAtee, M. L., 897 1030, 1033 McNally, R. J., 905
1130, 1138 McAuliffe, S., 1110 McEvoy, R., 234, 237, 255, McNay, A., 585, 592
Matochik, J., 524, 525, 667 McBeath, M., 695 257, 258, 259, 260, McNerney, E., 874, 875,
Matson, J., 251, 267, 719, McBride, P. A., 453, 456, 320, 346, 352, 353, 926, 941, 978, 1049
720, 721, 745, 749, 458, 459, 460, 461, 372, 384, 385, 387, McNew, S., 653, 665, 667
750, 751, 833, 909, 462, 1208 388, 536, 537, 607, McPartland, J., 5, 31, 113,
912, 914, 915, 951, McBurnett, K., 177 608, 612, 613, 616, 171, 233, 234, 235,
1029, 1030 McCabe, A., 335 655, 663, 836, 837 375, 388, 483, 612,
Matsuba, C., 542, 1112 McCabe, E. R. B., 616 McFadden, T., 813 613, 656, 662, 663,
Matsuda, H., 667 McCallon, D., 720, 721, McFarland, M., 1062 665, 668, 670, 690,
Matsuishi, T., 46, 140, 540, 741, 748, 749 McGann, A., 145 1078
544 McCann, J., 1306 McGarvey, L., 541 McPhedran, P., 455
Matsuno, D., 455 McCarthy, G., 483, 520, McGee, G., 324, 520, 799, McQuade, R. D., 1106
Matsuoka, K., 886 525, 693 874, 875, 883, 884, McQuivey, C., 264
Mattay, V. S., 526 McCarthy, K. S., 542 885, 887, 890, 899, McSwain-Kamran, M.,
Matthews, A. L., 819, 823 McCarthy, P., 46 903, 929, 930, 932, 1107
Matthews, N. J., 369, 371 McCaughrin, W., 207, 1094 933, 934, 937, 938, McSweeny, J., 256, 293,
Matthews, P. M., 521 McCauley, J. L., 429 939, 940, 941, 950, 343, 348, 352, 813,
Matthews, W. S., 251 McCellan, J., 191 953, 1004, 1011, 1030, 962
Matthys, W., 177 McClannahan, L., 261, 298, 1031, 1035, 1036, McTarnaghan, J., 997, 998
Mattison, R. E., 335 299, 354, 867, 869, 1045, 1046, 1125, McWhinney, B., 658
Mattlinger, M. J., 260, 690 871, 873, 875, 883, 1127, 1129, 1130, McWilliam, J., 617
Maturana, H. R., 688 884, 887, 913, 950, 1132, 1133, 1140, Mead, G., 633, 686, 687,
Maudsley, H., 583 959, 968, 985, 995, 1143 695
Author Index I • 23

Medeiros, J., 902 546, 547, 749, 758, Miller, N., 871, 1015, 1016 Möbius, P. J., 552
Meek, J. L., 456 883, 915, 932, 937, Miller, S. B., 539 Mock, M. A., 1021
Meek, M., 147, 149 940, 1043, 1048, 1049, Miller, S. L., 1308 Modahl, C., 342, 462, 544,
Mees, H., 948, 1125 1050, 1055, 1060, Millichap, J. G., 75, 135 833
Meinberg, D. L., 871, 886 1064, 1066, 1067, Millonig, J. H., 433, 474 Moes, D., 262, 264, 897
Meinhold, P. M., 903, 909 1069, 1073, 1075, Mills, D., 548, 1055 Moeschler, J. B., 551
Meir, D., 459, 463 1076, 1077, 1089, Mills, J., 821 Moffett, M., 544
Meiselas, K. D., 838, 839, 1176, 1177, 1178, Mills, J. B., 545 Moffitt, T. E., 181, 439,
847 1179, 1182, 1184, 1216 Mills, J. I., 1160 443
Meisels, S. J., 709 Messa, C., 662 Mills, M., 322, 689 Mohamed, S., 498
Melbye, M., 435 Messenheimer, J. A., 1111 Mills Costa, N., 1228 Mohandas, T. K., 550, 551
Melero-Montes, M., 61, Messer, D., 322, 397, 399, Millstein, R., 208 Mohkamsing, S., 545
435 1011 Milner, B., 720 Moilanen, I., 48, 534, 544
Melhuish, E., 322, 689 Mestre, D., 486, 642, 839 Milstein, J. P., 886, 995 Molenaar, P. C. M., 434
Meli, C., 553 Mesulam, M., 98, 613, 671 Milstien, S., 141 Molina, B. S., 177
Melin, L., 719, 731, 750, Metz, J. R., 883 Miltenberger, R. G., 820, Molitoris, D., 541
883, 884 Metzger, M., 660 823, 907, 914 Moller-Nielsen, A., 105
Mellerup, T., 462 Meundi, D. B., 457 Milton, R., 1095 Molloy, C., 486, 541, 542
Melmer, G., 429 Mewborn, S. K., 551 Minassian, S. L., 1307 Molony, H., 546
Melnick, M., 434 Meyer, D. J., 1074 Minchin, S., 494, 499 Monaco, A. P., 430, 432
Melnyk, A. R., 145 Meyer, E. C., 932, 980, Minderaa, R., 171, 186, Monaghan, L., 520
Melone, M. B., 301 981, 982 187, 455, 456, 458, Mondloch, C. J., 696
Meloni, I., 78, 130, 551 Meyer, J., 372, 652, 654, 459, 747, 748 Monsch, A. U., 646
Meltzer, H., 49, 60, 1103 668, 669 Minow, F., 177, 536 Monson, N., 616
Meltzoff, A., 171, 224, Meyer, K. A., 267 Minshew, N., 97, 103, 181, Montagna, P., 152
225, 233, 234, 235, Meyer, L., 988 185, 257, 263, 264, Montague, J., 127, 147
236, 237, 320, 326, Meyer, L. H., 870, 915, 267, 268, 295, 344, Montan, N., 1031
337, 383, 384, 385, 916, 1031, 1034 355, 367, 368, 369, Montegar, C. A., 1126,
388, 390, 396, 397, Meyer, L. S., 1044, 1046 371, 372, 373, 387, 1127
483, 526, 613, 651, Meyer, M., 438, 810 484, 485, 486, 487, Monteiro, B., 555
656, 657, 659, 670, Meyer, R. N., 1078 491, 493, 494, 496, Montero, G., 209
671, 672, 673, 689, Meyerson, M. D., 553 497, 498, 499, 501, Monteverde, E., 1107
690, 692, 707, 799, Micali, N., 171 517, 518, 519, 525, Montgomery, J., 321
832, 833, 834, 836, Michael, J., 819, 823, 963, 536, 546, 549, 646, Montgomery, M., 488, 489,
837, 846, 962 964, 1125 652, 655, 660, 661, 536, 617, 618
Menage, P., 542 Michaelis, R., 140, 550, 667, 668, 669, 673, Montgomery, S. M., 438,
Ménard, A., 642 551 767, 832, 835, 1306 541
Menard, E., 326, 642 Michal, N., 289, 749 Minter, M., 418 Montgomery, T., 555, 556
Menchetti, B. M., 1094 Micheli, E., 1065, 1178 Miotto, E. C., 373 Monuteaux, M. C., 177
Mendelson, L., 1310 Micheli, R., 551 Miozzo, M., 551 Moodley, M., 136
Mendez, M. F., 255 Michelman, J. D., 27, 209 Miranda, M., 518 Moon, M. S., 1093
Mendilaharsu, C., 1229 Michelotti, J., 248 Miranda-Linne, F., 719, Moore, A., 1056, 1057,
Mendoza, S. P., 526 Michelson, A. M., 539 731, 750, 883, 884 1064
Menolascino, F., 548 Michie, A., 914 Mirenda, P., 149, 255, 256, Moore, C., 236, 318, 650,
Menold, M. M., 429, 432, Mick, E., 177 802, 803, 804, 866, 654, 655, 656, 663
433, 434, 439, 544, Miedaner, J., 152 869, 884, 885, 934, Moore, D., 414
551, 754 Miladi, N., 553 937, 941, 951 Moore, G. J., 549
Menon, V., 170, 185, 521 Milberger, S., 183 Mirsky, A. F., 367, 368, Moore, J., 432
Menscher, S., 911 Mildenberger, K., 177, 536 476, 498, 614 Moore, K. M., 463
Merckelbach, H., 758 Milders, M., 236 Mises, R., 1207 Moore, M., 317
Merette, C., 31, 170, 172, Miles, J., 60, 435, 439, 501, Mishkin, M., 524 Moore, M. J., 684, 689
477, 478, 731, 735, 754 536 Missliwetz, J., 139, 145 Moore, M. K., 383, 692
Merewether, F. C., 348 Miles, S., 107, 352, 353, Mistry, J., 383 Moore, N. A., 1105
Merhar, S., 1312 809, 810, 962 Misumi, K., 47 Moore, R. J., 1090
Merikangas, K., 431 Miles, T., 101 Mitchell, C., 551 Moore, S. C., 1092, 1095
Merjanian, P., 371 Milham, M., 668, 669 Mitchell, J., 151, 153, 461, Moore, S. J., 555, 556
Merleau-Ponty, M., 687 Militerni, R., 80, 265, 266, 462 Moore, V., 31
Merlino, J., 907 494 Mitchell, P., 642 Moore, W., 1076
Mermelstein, R., 351 Miller, B., 267 Mitomo, M., 141 Moraine, C., 78
Merrin, D. J., 336 Miller, D. E., 744 Mittenberger, R. G., 899 Morales, M., 656, 1021
Merrit, J., 463, 490 Miller, E., 48, 54, 60, 435, Mittler, P., 201, 204 Mordzinski, C., 740
Merritt, A., 144, 145 541, 668 Miyake, Y., 73, 74, 76, 80, Morecraft, R., 671
Merry, S. N., 1110 Miller, J., 24, 33, 93, 103, 749, 1312 Morel, M. L., 545
Merson, R., 962 104, 174, 186, 213, Miyazaki, M., 172, 461, Moreno, H., 553
Mervis, C. B., 549 258, 340, 373, 375, 499, 539 Morey, J., 44
Meryash, D., 546, 594 544, 616, 682, 731, Mizuguchi, M., 543 Morford, M., 459
Merzenich, M. M., 1308 735, 736, 805, 1056 Mlele, T., 291 Morgan, C. N., 291
Mesaros, R. A., 149 Miller, L. J., 789, 844, 846 Mlika, A., 61 Morgan, D., 692
Mesibov, G., 113, 188, 189, Miller, L. K., 640 Mo, A., 476, 500, 536, 539 Morgan, D. B., 916
207, 213, 264, 266, Miller, M., 544, 553 Moak, J. P., 129, 143 Morgan, D. E., 476
289, 290, 292, 298, Miller, M. L., 901 Mobbs, D., 170, 185, 521 Morgan, K., 545, 652, 655,
299, 301, 302, 324, Miller, M. T., 553 Moberg, P. J., 619 657, 713, 714, 763
I • 24 Author Index

Morgan, R., 630 Muhle, R., 1309 536, 539, 546, 594, Nash, C., 496
Morgan, S. B., 234, 251, Muir, D. W., 317, 689, 690 1110 Nash, S., 227, 408
305, 316, 385, 392, Mukhlas, M., 47 Murphy, W. D., 1143 Nasr, A., 291
1069 Mulder, E., 454, 753 Murray, A., 521, 523, 546 Nass, R., 503
Morgan, T., 31, 224, 232 Mulick, J. A., 542, 752, Murray, D. S., 542 Nation, K., 354
Morgan, V., 693 903, 909 Murray, P. J., 1110, 1111 Nault, K., 540
Mori, T., 541 Mullen, E. M., 791 Murray, R. M., 181 Nave, G., 1023
Morishita, R., 138 Müller, R. A., 261, 497, Murrie, D. C., 299 Naviaux, R. K., 550
Moroz, T., 669, 671 517, 521, 525, 526, Murtaugh, M., 207 Navon, D., 640, 643
Morrelli-Robbins, M., 1003 1306 Musiek, F. E., 844 Naylor, S. T., 27, 178, 456,
Morren, J., 898, 903 Mullin, J. T., 689 Musumeci, S., 479, 481, 1107, 1108
Morressy, P. A., 1183 Mullins, J. L., 914, 1016 482, 494, 499, 542, Nazzi, T., 337
Morrier, M., 799, 874, 875, Mulloy, C., 539 1307 Neal, A. R., 709
885, 929, 930, 932, Mumme, D. L., 552 Mutirangura, A., 551 Neal, R., 651, 654, 658,
933, 934, 937, 939, Mundy, P., 7, 185, 233, Muzik, O., 454, 490, 517, 659, 660, 661, 662,
941, 950, 1035, 1036, 235, 236, 237, 238, 752 670, 671, 673, 684,
1045, 1046, 1132 239, 251, 253, 254, Mycke, K., 1057, 1058, 833
Morris, J., 518 260, 261, 315, 318, 1074 Neale, M. C., 432
Morris, M., 462, 552, 319, 320, 321, 322, Myer, E., 142 Needleman, R., 344
1127 324, 349, 369, 392, Myers, B., 548 Neef, N. A., 1030, 1126,
Morris, N., 1126, 1128 394, 409, 411, 416, Myers, M., 542, 1112 1138, 1143
Morris, R., 30, 325, 369, 520, 613, 630, 631, Myers, R. M., 170, 315, Neff, N. H., 456
373, 410, 539, 616, 650, 651, 652, 653, 440, 754 Neff, S., 499
772, 773 654, 655, 656, 657, Myklebust, H. R., 97 Nehme, E., 107, 492, 499,
Morrison, L., 251, 1031 658, 659, 660, 661, Myles, B., 113, 354, 366, 537, 538, 768
Morrone, M. C., 692, 693 662, 663, 664, 665, 374, 375, 867, 868, Neighbor, G., 693
Mors, O., 185, 188, 534, 667, 669, 670, 671, 869, 872, 876, 913, Neill, W. T., 609
551 672, 673, 684, 702, 985, 986, 996, 1015, Neisser, U., 687
Mortensen, E. L., 105 707, 709, 721, 724, 1016, 1018, 1020, Neisworth, J. T., 713, 871
Mortensen, P. B., 185, 188, 738, 750, 800, 833, 1021, 1022 Nelson, C., 482, 657, 658,
534, 551 915, 946, 947, 962, Myrianthopoulos, N. C., 660, 697
Mortimer, A. M., 848 1007, 1306 434 Nelson, D. S., 7, 51, 296,
Mortimore, C., 628, 630, Munk, D. D., 540, 900 1111
631, 634, 682 Munson, J., 5, 31, 171, 226, Nääntänen, R., 481, 482 Nelson, E. C., 27
Morton, J., 317 233, 234, 236, 316, Nabors, L., 541, 1112 Nelson, J. E., 27, 1106
Morton, M., 619 319, 375, 387, 388, Nacewicz, B. M., 521, 522 Nelson, K., 337, 977
Mortweet, S. L., 1059 429, 542, 612, 613, Nadasdy, Z., 630, 698 Nelson, K. B., 436, 462,
Moscovich, M., 669, 671 656, 657, 662, 663, Nadel, J., 253, 260, 386, 539, 541
Moser, H., 126, 127, 128, 665, 668, 670, 708, 690 Nelson, K. E., 959
132, 140, 142, 144, 757, 1112, 1311 Nader, R., 833, 834 Nelson, L. G., 544
147 Munson, R., 826 Nagai, Y., 325, 834, 1225 Nelson, M., 1034
Moser-Richters, M., 322 Munstermann, E., 546 Nagamitsu, S., 462 Nelson, S., 489, 553, 836
Moses, L. J., 614, 615 Murakami, A., 367, 368 Naganuma, G., 152 Nemanov, L., 433
Moses, L. M., 130 Murakami, J., 141, 367, Nagell, K., 336, 653 Nemeroff, C. B., 142
Moses, P., 489, 524 499, 610, 617, 655, Nagy, E., 478 Neri, P., 692, 693
Mosier, C., 383 656, 659, 668, 832, Nagy, J., 93, 97, 101, 586 Ness, K. K., 44, 57, 61
Moss, E., 552 889 Nahmani, A., 552 Neuman, C. J., 415
Moss, S. A., 610 Murakami, Y., 540 Nahmias, C., 521 Neuringer, A., 871
Motil, K. J., 145 Murakawa, K., 172, 499 Naidich, T. P., 543 Neuschwander, J., 539
Motile, K. J., 139 Muraki, A. S., 499 Naidu, S., 126, 127, 128, Neville, H., 549
Mottron, L., 107, 185, 234, Murata, S., 456, 459, 460, 130, 132, 134, 136, Nevin, N. C., 551
326, 368, 611, 642, 461, 1225, 1249 137, 140, 141, 142, New, E., 17, 184, 749
643, 644, 646, 656, Murata, T., 73, 74, 80, 100, 144, 146, 147, 151 Newbury, D. F., 432, 438
1182 202, 204, 206, 212, Naik, B., 1108 Newcorn, J. H., 453
Motulsky, A. G., 434 213, 214, 288, 289, Naitoh, H., 140 Newell, A., 690, 694
Mountain-Kimchi, K., 103, 291, 293, 296, 298, Nakahato, M., 455 Newell, K. M., 847, 848
374, 375 1312 Nakajima, E., 544 Newhart-Larson, S., 417,
Mouren-Simeoni, M. C., Muratori, F., 225, 234, 236, Nakamura, A., 389, 662, 554
517 243 663 Newhouse, L., 964
Mouridsen, S. E., 50, 75, Murch, S., 435, 438, 541 Nakamura, H., 255 Newman, B., 871, 886
76, 203, 204, 206, 212, Muris, P., 758 Nakamura, K., 389 Newman, H. A., 462
213, 296 Murphy, C., 57, 63, 555, Nakane, A., 463, 1226 Newman, S., 588
Moya, K. L., 646 883, 912, 1004, 1009, Nakayasu, N., 270 Newschaffer, C. J., 44, 57,
Moyes, F. A., 835 1070, 1123 Nakhoda-Sapuan, S., 871 61
Moyes, R. A., 872 Murphy, D. G. M., 107, 493 Nalin, A., 142 Newsom-Davis, I. C., 1059
Mozes, T., 171 Murphy, D. L., 459 Namerow, N. S., 619 Newson, E., 109, 201, 203,
Msall, M. E., 554 Murphy, G., 214 Nanba, E., 543 204, 206, 586, 588
Mudford, O. C., 897, 1061, Murphy, K. C., 543, 545 Nanclares, V., 494, 499, Newton, J. S., 822
1064 Murphy, L. B., 398 519, 740 Ney, J., 387
Muenz, L. R., 369, 487 Murphy, M., 126, 127, 144, Narasaki, M., 127 Nguyen, M. T., 538, 550
Muglia, P., 1103 147, 208, 269, 295, Narayan, M., 457 Nichols, K., 664
Müh, J. P., 432, 459, 542 428, 429, 434, 534, Naruse, H., 461, 463, 1226 Nichols, S., 659, 673, 698
Author Index I • 25

Nicholson, H., 520 Nowell, M. A., 499 O’Leary, M., 142 388, 396, 397, 398,
Nicholson, J. C., 550 Numato, Y., 463 Olivari, C., 1232 429, 526, 542, 595,
Nicholson, R., 183 Nunn, K., 80 Oliver, J., 241 612, 613, 651, 654,
Nicolao, P., 130, 138 Nurmi, E. L., 429 Ollendick, T., 30, 595, 656, 657, 662, 663,
Nicoletti, M. A., 491, 500 Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., 253 1059 665, 668, 670, 671,
Nicoll, D., 434 Nwokoro, N. A., 544 Oller, D., 235, 319 689, 707, 708, 799,
Nicolson, K., 491 Nyberg, L., 372 Oller, K., 349, 962 800, 832, 833, 834,
Nicolson, R., 544 Nydén, A., 103, 234, 299, Olley, G., 953, 962 837, 846, 942, 946,
Nie, J., 73, 77, 78 352, 375, 495, 748, Olley, J. G., 351, 863, 866, 962, 987, 998, 1003,
Niedermeyer, E., 144, 151 1306 867, 868, 876, 1003, 1004, 1008, 1112,
Niehaus, D., 266 Nye, J., 837 1176, 1310 1311
Nielsen, J., 140, 144, 544, Nyhan, W. L., 464, 544 Olsen, C. L., 547 Östman, O., 212
1107 Nyholt, D. R., 432, 433 Olson, G., 317 Ostrosky, M., 884, 967,
Nielson, J., 1282 Nylander, L., 590 Olson, L. M., 429 968, 991, 1031
Niemann, G., 140 Nzeocha, A., 905 Olson, S. T., 476 Ostry, D., 693
Nientimp, E. G., 1030 Olsson, B., 132, 134, 135, Oswald, D. P., 720, 870
Nietupski, J., 818, 1090, Oades, R., 479 431 Otsubo, H., 544
1097 Oakes, P., 261 Olsson, I., 1305 Ottinger, D. R., 394
Nightingale, N., 105, 227, O’Bara, M., 494 Olsson, M. B., 1055 Oudakker, A. R., 78
229, 652, 655, 657, Oberfield, R., 838, 839, Olsson, V. A., 544 Ounsted, C., 268, 585, 592
713, 714, 763 847 O’Malley Cannon, B., 938, Ousley, O., 228, 229, 231,
Nigro, F., 547 Oberlander, T. F., 833, 834 1036 233, 235, 236, 238,
Nigro, M. A., 144 O’Brien, D. J., 462, 1306 O’Neill, D. K., 253 251, 301, 316, 319,
Nihei, K., 140 O’Brien, D. W., 1282 O’Neill, J., 201, 549 321, 384, 385, 386,
Niimi, M., 47 O’Brien, G., 189, 299, 300 O’Neill, M., 831 409, 655, 708, 717,
Nijhof, G. J., 189 O’Brien, L. M., 493, 494, O’Neill, R. E., 257, 822, 718, 734, 799, 801,
Niklasson, L., 552 496, 497, 498, 519, 823, 824, 825, 887, 835, 836, 837, 947,
Nimchinsky, E., 670 1305 890, 900, 903, 906, 1007, 1011
Ninomiya, H., 543 O’Brien, M., 1143 911, 912, 1061 Ouston, J., 260, 261, 389,
Nir, I., 459 O’Brien, R. A., 455, 458 Ong, B., 24, 167, 172 412, 413, 520, 643,
Nishikawa, M., 667 O’Callaghan, E., 539 Onizawa, J., 455 689
Nissen, G., 1240, 1241 Ochocka, J., 1096 Oosterlaan, J., 373 Ouvrier, R., 80
Niwa, S., 502, 503, 1225 Ochs, E., 868 Oostra, B. A., 545 Overall, J. E., 1102, 1112
Njardvik, U., 251 O’Connor, N., 255, 263, Opitz, J. M., 127, 128 Ovrutskaya, I., 542
Njio, L., 30, 183 388, 596, 628, 640, Opitz, J. O., 544 Owely, T., 722
Nober, E., 657, 662 641 Oram, M. W., 692 Owen, A., 609, 615, 663
Noebels, J., 431, 432 O’Connor, T., 180, 430 Oranje, B., 478 Owen, L., 127
Noemi, C., 1230 Oda, H., 254 O’Reilly, B., 1093 Owen, M. J., 443
Nohria, C., 428 Oda, S., 47 O’Riordan, M., 630, 635, Owen, V., 1004
Noin, I., 345 Odell, J., 542 642 Owley, T., 542, 752, 757,
Noirot, E., 689 O’Dell, M., 883, 950, 952, Ornitz, E., 27, 209, 227, 762, 1112
Nolin, T., 257, 960 1004, 1009 316, 366, 367, 455, Ownby, J. B., 1033
Noll, W. W., 551 Odom, S. L., 394, 967, 968, 476, 478, 484, 488, Ozanne, A. E., 148, 149
Nomura, Y., 127, 140, 141, 991, 1011, 1029, 1030, 540, 542, 553, 832, Ozbayrak, K. R., 107
142 1031, 1033 834, 837, 981 Ozonoff, S., 13, 24, 33, 73,
Norbury, C., 33, 175, 176, O’Donovan, A., 539 Orr, S., 366 77, 78, 93, 102, 103,
353, 641, 722, 740, Odor, S., 1021 Orsini, D. L., 30 104, 105, 113, 115,
753, 810, 812, 813 O’Driscoll, G., 183, 189 Orsmark, C., 109 174, 186, 213, 234,
Nordin, V., 205, 212, 288, Oduwaiye, K. A., 842 Orsmond, G., 214, 288, 238, 251, 258, 261,
289, 290, 292, 293, Oelsner, R. R., 1229 289, 290, 293, 295, 262, 264, 268, 322,
295, 297, 304, 478, Oetting, J. B., 347 297, 323, 324, 1055, 324, 326, 350, 368,
540, 544, 720, 748, Offord, D. R., 733 1059, 1075, 1077 370, 372, 373, 375,
749, 750, 751, 753, 832 Oftedal, G., 542 Ort, S., 492, 838, 848 388, 391, 413, 416,
Nordquist, V., 968, 1030 Oftelie, A., 267 Ortegon, J., 258, 261, 324, 417, 487, 498, 539,
Norlin, B., 544 Ogdie, M. N., 1307 412 544, 607, 608, 609,
Norman, D., 670 Ogle, P. A., 1074 Ortiz, E., 253 610, 611, 613, 614,
Norman, R. M., 489 Ohashi, Y., 47 Osada, H., 76, 80 615, 616, 617, 618,
Normandin, J. J., 489, 493, Ohnishi, T., 667 Osaki, D., 953, 1043, 1050 620, 631, 634, 635,
494, 1305 Ohno, K., 543 Osann, K., 342 643, 653, 682, 731,
Norris, C., 890 Ohno, Y., 455, 459, 460 Osborn, A. G., 548 735, 736, 758, 786,
Norris, M., 554 Ohta, M., 325, 384, 834 Osborne, J. P., 543 836, 837, 890, 984,
Norris, R. G., 1089 Ohtaki, E., 540 Osborne, P., 1110 1050, 1056, 1057,
North, W. G., 1111 Ohtani, Y., 540 Oshima, M., 1225 1061, 1064, 1078
Northup, J., 912, 916 Ohura, T., 461 Oshima, N., 463
Norton, J., 204, 206, 215, Okada, F., 460 Oskarsdottir, S., 552 Pabst, H. F., 499
288, 293, 320, 335, Oke, N. J., 912, 990, 991, Osnes, P. G., 827 Pace, G. M., 152, 155, 819,
835, 962, 1304 1032 Ostergaard, O., 544 825, 907
Noterdaeme, M., 177, 536 Oksanen, H., 296, 297 Osterling, J., 5, 31, 171, Pace, N. C., 542
Nouri, N., 426, 432, 433, Okuda, K., 261 224, 225, 226, 233, Pacifico, L., 539
617 Okun, M., 502 234, 235, 236, 237, Padden, S., 481, 482
Novick, B., 479, 480, 482 Oldendorf, W. H., 499 316, 319, 341, 366, Padeliadou, S., 1217
Novotny, S., 1111 O’Leary, J. J., 438, 541 375, 384, 385, 387, Page, J., 385, 836, 847
I • 26 Author Index

Page, T., 464, 1126, 1128, Parrish, J. M., 1126, 1138 Pedersent, C. B., 435 Perry, T. L., 462, 553
1143 Parsons, M. B., 1033 Peeters, T., 1177 Persson, B., 297, 758,
Paine, C., 212 Partington, J., 929, 932, Pefiesson, R. S., 1018 1177
Painter, K. M., 151 933, 937, 939, 941, Pei, F., 225, 234, 236 Persson, E., 177, 546, 548
Paisey, T. J., 905 963, 964 Peirini, P., 668, 669 Perucchini, P., 536
Palermo, M. T., 265, 266 Partington, M. W., 455 Pelham, W., 1109 Pescucci, C., 551
Palfai, T., 615 Parush, S., 834 Pelham, W., Jr., 177 Peselow, E. D., 838, 839,
Palferman, S., 5, 8, 25, 93, Pasamanick, B., 50 Pelios, L., 898, 903 847
419, 427, 442 Pascalis, O., 670 Pellicer, A. A., 541 Pessoa, L., 526
Palm, A., 143 Pascotto, A., 552 Pelphrey, K., 412, 483, 491 Peters, A., 346
Palmen, S. J. M. C., 491, Pascual-Castroviejo, I., 544 Pelson, R. O., 476 Peters, K. G., 617
493, 499 Pascualvaca, D., 367, 368, Peltola, H., 541 Petersen, B. P., 909
Palmer, A., 301, 432, 433, 498, 524, 525, 614, Pelton, G. H., 267 Petersen, P. B., 433, 437,
515, 1071, 1075 667 Pencarinha, D., 208 536, 539
Palmer, D. S., 1034 Pasley, B. N., 518, 526 Pence, A. R., 1034 Petersen, S., 663
Palmer, F., 800 Passante, N., 746, 753 Pencrinha, D., 269 Peterson, B., 27, 754, 1103
Palmer, L. J., 431 Passingham, R., 371, 495, Penge, R., 1223 Peterson, C., 660, 968, 991,
Palmer, P., 5, 107, 170, 497, 1305 Pennington, B., 324, 372, 1089
173, 182, 214, 269, Passoni, D., 551 373, 375, 485, 631, Peterson, F. M., 915
288, 289, 295, 427, Pasto, L., 1182 634, 635, 663, 673 Peterson, R. E., 455
428, 492, 499, 617, Pastuszak, A. L., 553 Pennington, B. E., 236 Peterson, R. F., 883
618, 646, 660, 661, Patel, M. R., 819, 821, 823 Pennington, B. F., 33, 104, Pethick, S., 337
741, 1076, 1077 Patel, S. T., 462 105, 185, 234, 237, Petit, E., 1208
Palumbo, D., 1109 Patil, A. A., 503 261, 262, 268, 320, Petkovic, Z. B., 459
Palumbo, L. W., 902 Patil, S., 188, 551 326, 350, 368, 369, Petrides, G., 589
Pan, J. W., 143 Patja, A., 541 370, 371, 372, 375, Petrides, M., 693
Panagiotaki, G., 549 Patja, K., 296, 297 383, 384, 385, 386, Petropoulos, M. C., 48, 54,
Panagiotides, H., 171, 235, Patta, P. C., 462 387, 388, 389, 391, 60, 435
483, 690 Pattavina, S., 1023 413, 487, 536, 537, Petrucha, D., 503
Panchalingam, K., 549 Patterson, B., 536 545, 606, 607, 611, Pettegrew, J. W., 549
Pancsofar, E. L., 1091 Patterson, E. T., 1127 612, 613, 614, 615, Petterson, B., 434
Pandit, B. S., 48, 52, 54, Patterson, G., 1061 616, 618, 836, 837, Petti, T. A., 501
61, 63 Patton, M. A., 544 1050, 1306 Petty, L. K., 27, 209
Panerai, S., 494, 499, 758 Patzold, L. M., 542 Pennington, R., 655, 663, Peze, A., 386
Panksepp, J., 461, 656, 659, Paul, G., 412, 483 1030 Pezzullo, J., 548
662, 1112 Paul, M., 483 Pennisi, G., 479, 481, 482 Pfiffner, L. J., 177
Panman, L. E., 484 Paul, R., 17, 30, 31, 71, 79, Peppe, S., 1306 Phelan, M. C., 550, 551
Pantelis, C., 617 224, 232, 233, 234, Peraino, J. M., 1088 Phelps, D. L., 554
Panyan, M., 1126, 1127, 238, 251, 256, 293, Percy, A., 127, 128, 129, Phelps, L. A., 1091
1128 315, 316, 318, 319, 130, 131, 132, 134, Phelps, M. E., 140
Panzer, S., 668, 669 323, 335, 340, 341, 135, 136, 138, 139, Philbrick, L., 1003, 1004
Papademetris, X., 523 342, 343, 346, 348, 140, 141, 142, 143, Philippart, M., 129, 130,
Papadimitriou, J. C., 541 349, 350, 352, 353, 144, 145, 150, 151, 135, 140, 144
Papageorgiou, M., 367, 492, 561, 587, 691, 152 Philippe, A., 432, 433,
368, 498, 614 707, 783, 793, 801, Perdue, S., 183 462
Papageorgiou, V., 1216, 802, 804, 809, 810, Pereira, J., 136 Philips, W., 650, 661
1217 813, 832, 946, 960, Perel, J. M., 457, 463 Philipson, E., 544, 553
Papanikolau, E., 427 961, 962, 966 Peretz, I., 326, 611, 642 Phillips, M., 521, 526
Papanikolau, K., 5, 426, 427 Pauli, R. M., 551 Perez, E., 483, 552 Phillips, N. E., 499
Paradis, T., 899 Paulk, M., 551 Perez, J., 1305 Phillips, S., 480
Parano, E., 432, 433 Pauls, D., 24, 27, 41, 91, Pergament, E., 544 Phillips, W., 318, 355, 537,
Paravatou-Petsotas, M., 551 92, 95, 96, 102, 104, Perkins, J., 1142 707, 834
Paredes, S., 886, 887 109, 110, 124, 263, Perloff, B. F., 882, 883 Philofsky, A., 547
Parente, R., 619 691, 735, 736, 1111 Perman, J. A., 541 Piaget, J., 149, 224, 234,
Parham, L. D., 835, 836, Paulson, G. W., 555 Perner, J., 105, 395, 613, 236, 321, 382, 391,
843, 846, 847 Paulson, R. B., 555 614, 615, 630, 632 392, 395, 656, 673,
Park, C., 632, 1076, 1185, Paunio, M., 541 Perot, A., 689 695
1311 Pavone, L., 539, 553 Perou, R., 1062 Piazza, C. C., 128, 152,
Park, G. S., 594 Pavone, P., 432, 433 Perrett, D., 236, 266, 389, 908, 909, 912
Park, J., 550, 1129 Payton, J., 369, 484, 487, 521, 523, 526, 692 Piazza, K., 134, 146
Park, R. J., 430 546 Perrine, K., 607 Picardi, N., 353
Parker, D., 251, 265, 267, Pearce, P., 958, 959 Perrot, A., 229, 232, 240, Piccinelli, P., 551
839, 847, 1031 Pearl, G., 768 366, 689, 708, 712, Piccolo, E., 873, 874
Parker, J., 1056, 1072 Pearlman-Avnion, S., 260 751, 834, 1208 Pichal, B., 105
Parks, B. T., 903 Pearlson, G., 107, 492, 499, Perry, A., 147, 749, 762, Pichichero, M. E., 436
Parks, D. R., 886, 913 537, 538 1076 Pickens, D., 693
Parks, S. L., 312, 730, 744 Pearson, D., 258, 261, 324, Perry, B. D., 539 Pickett, J., 212, 293, 295,
Parle, N., 619 385, 412, 1110, 1111 Perry, D., 27, 209, 295 296, 297
Parmeggiani, A., 296, 502 Pearson, G. S., 169, 182 Perry, E. K., 463, 490 Pickles, A., 5, 31, 32, 108,
Parmelee, T., 484 Pearson, J., 189 Perry, R., 77, 177, 189, 170, 171, 180, 208,
Parr, J. R., 432, 433, 438 Peck, C., 934 463, 490, 492, 1102, 228, 269, 295, 335,
Parrett, J., 886, 887, 913 Peck, S. M., 819, 823 1104 341, 342, 343, 346,
Author Index I • 27

385, 408, 426, 427, Ploof, D. L., 1110 Prather, F., 83 Quach, H., 433
428, 430, 434, 437, Plotkin, S., 455 Prather, M. D., 526 Quarrington, B., 539
534, 536, 537, 538, Plousia, M., 655 Pratt, C., 105, 352, 569, Quemada, N., 1207
539, 553, 586, 594, Plumb, I., 190, 631, 632 630 Quilitch, H. R., 1127, 1128
617, 632, 655, 722, Plumet, M. H., 326, 521, Preece, D., 1177 Quill, K., 259, 936, 966,
730, 731, 732, 734, 618 Prego Silva, L. E., 1229 990, 1021
740, 741, 753, 754, Pober, B. R., 548 Premack, D., 629, 630 Quinn, A., 555, 556
763 Pocock, S. J., 429 Presberg, J., 1108 Quinn, G. E., 554
Pickrel, S. G., 1063 Poe, M. D., 833, 843 Presburger, G., 552 Quinn, J., 936
Pickup, G. J., 171 Poirier, K., 78 Press, G. A., 141, 367, 371, Quintana, H., 27, 189, 1110
Picton, T. W., 844 Poland, J., 113 499, 610, 617, 832, Quintana-Murci, L., 433
Pierce, C., 1148 Polatkoff, S., 189 889, 1282, 1305 Quintieri, F., 539
Pierce, K., 250, 261, 266, Poldrack, R. A., 1308 Prevor, M. B., 612 Quisling, R., 502
483, 493, 497, 521, Polich, J., 479 Pribor, E. F., 27 Qumsiyeh, M. B., 551
522, 525, 526, 654, Poline, J. B., 517 Price, B. H., 613
661, 663, 671, 837, Polinsky, R. J., 458 Price, D. L., 142 Rabsztyn, A., 541
886, 887, 890, 913, Pollard, J., 234 Price, J. L., 524, 525 Racine, Y., 733
959, 990, 991, 1004, Pomerleau, O., 1127, Price, L. H., 27, 189, 267, Racusi, G., 772, 773
1011, 1030, 1031, 1306 1148 456, 821, 1107, 1108, Rademacher, J., 494, 536,
Pierce, S., 343, 345 Pomeroy, J., 782, 1055, 1111, 1282 538
Pierce, T., 322 1059, 1060, 1124, Price, R., 693 Rader, S., 608
Pieretti, M., 545 1134, 1139 Primavera, E., 934 Rafaelsen, O. J., 462
Pieropan, K., 886 Pommer, D. A., 1127 Prince, S., 269, 914 Raggett, P., 634
Pietila, J., 1307 Pomponi, M. G., 551 Pring, L., 642, 644 Ragland, E. U., 1030
Piggot, J., 170, 185, 521 Pond, R., 800, 804 Prior, J., 427, 428 Rahav, M., 1218
Piha, J., 1238 Ponnet, K., 105 Prior, M., 24, 27, 33, 93, Rahbar, B., 251, 292, 294,
Pikus, A. M., 476, 477 Pons, R., 550 167, 172, 173, 174, 316
Pilling, S., 619 Pontius, W., 1304 185, 261, 320, 348, Raiford, K. L., 32, 429,
Pillotto, R. F., 136 Porat, S., 1218 373, 387, 388, 459, 432, 433, 439, 754
Pilowsky, T., 171, 433, 734, Porges, S., 336, 832 460, 487, 492, 536, Rainville, C., 185
748, 749, 753 Porparino, M., 1182 542, 593, 606, 744, Raivio, K. O., 464
Pincus, H. A., 11 Porter, F. D., 544 835, 1016, 1201 Rakic, P., 454
Pine, D. S., 181 Porterfield, P. J., 1143 Pritchard, J. K., 426, 432 Ramachandran, V. S., 459,
Pingree, C., 47, 63, 184, Posar, A., 296 Prizant, B., 255, 257, 319, 694
299, 461, 534, 536, Posey, D. J., 189, 267, 454, 346, 691, 713, 758, Ramberg, C., 352
537, 539, 540, 545 526, 1106, 1108, 1109 759, 773, 777, 778, Ramos, E. R., 551
Pinker, S., 340 Posner, K., 1109 799, 800, 890, 926, Ramos, O., 126, 154, 431,
Pinter, E., 901 Posner, M., 610, 617, 663, 927, 928, 929, 930, 535, 594
Pinter-Lalli, E., 822 668, 669, 670, 671, 932, 933, 935, 936, Ramoz, N., 433, 474, 550
Pinto, M. C. B., 1230 673 937, 938, 939, 940, Ramsden, S., 130, 139
Pinto-Martin, J. A., 1312 Potenza, M. N., 1105 941, 942, 946, 947, Ramstad, K., 548
Pisani, F., 479, 481 Potkin, S. G., 617 961, 978, 979, 980, Randall, P., 1056, 1072
Pitcher, D. A., 494, 536, Potter, C. A., 952 981, 982, 983, 984, Randolf, B., 368
538 Potter, D., 653 985, 986, 987, 989, Randolph, B., 234
Pitkanen, A., 525 Potter, M., 991 990, 995, 996, 997, Rank, B., 9, 16
Pitts-Conway, V., 886, 904, Potucek, J., 956, 991 1005, 1007, 1008, Rankin, J., 547
1143 Poulsen, C., 959 1009, 1016 Ranson, B. J., 494, 496,
Piven, J., 5, 107, 123, 170, Poulson, C. L., 261, 871 Proctor-Williams, K., 959, 498, 499
173, 182, 188, 195, Poustka, A., 130, 432 966 Rantala, H., 534, 544
197, 214, 250, 269, Poustka, F., 27, 294 Pronovost, W., 348, 813 Rao, J. M., 177
288, 289, 295, 412, Povinelli, D., 317 Provence, S., 9, 29, 182, Rao, N., 758
427, 428, 439, 453, Powell, D. R., 264 183 Rapagna, S., 1034
483, 489, 491, 492, Powell, J., 48, 52, 54, 61, Prutting, C., 351, 653, 806, Rapin, I., 30, 65, 76, 79,
493, 494, 496, 498, 63, 666 947 80, 82, 97, 175, 202,
499, 500, 501, 519, Powell, S., 322, 371, 397, Przybeck, T., 746, 753 204, 205, 206, 212,
536, 537, 538, 539, 399, 1011 Puce, A., 483, 520, 525, 213, 214, 224, 288,
551, 617, 618, 646, Powell, T., 617, 1074 693 289, 290, 291, 292,
660, 661, 741, 768, Power, R., 1064 Puchalski, C., 396 293, 295, 296, 298,
808, 809, 1058, 1076, Power, T., 264 Pueschel, S., 548, 553 299, 385, 387, 388,
1077, 1307 Powers, J., 458 Puleston, J. M., 438, 541 491, 502, 503, 534,
Pizzo, S., 550 Powers, L., 1060, 1063, Pulsifer, M., 547 536, 537, 538, 539,
Pizzuti, A., 545 1068, 1093 Pumariega, A. J., 267 544, 555, 586, 832,
Placidi, F., 542 Powers, M., 113, 817, 818, Purcell, R., 617 1305, 1307, 1309,
Plaisted, K., 630, 635, 642, 819, 820, 826, 1037 Purdy, W. C., 456, 459 1311, 1312
643 Powers, S., 1183 Purisch, A. D., 844 Rapoport, J. L., 27, 169,
Planer, D., 255 Pozdol, S. L., 712, 718 Purpura, D. P., 553, 1305 179, 181, 202, 203,
Plebst, C., 494, 499, 519, Pozner, A., 207 Putnam, L. E., 479 204, 206, 207, 208,
740 Prasad, A. N., 544 Putnam, N. H., 484 209, 210, 251, 266,
Plesner, A.-M., 435 Prasad, C., 544 Putnam, S., 32, 731, 733, 288, 290, 293, 294,
Pliner, C., 31, 73, 317, 708 Prasher, V., 209, 295 754, 755 295, 297, 298, 299,
Plioplys, A. V., 503 Prassad, A., 553 Pyck, K., 546 316, 492, 501, 1015,
Plomin, R., 431 Prather, E., 804 Pyles, D. A. M., 1128 1106
I • 28 Author Index

Rapoport, J. T., 267 1175, 1176, 1177, 207, 210, 211, 288, 1133, 1141, 1143,
Rapoport, M. D., 344, 352 1199, 1216 295, 297, 298, 299, 1144, 1148, 1151,
Rapoport, S. I., 492, 518 Reichlet, R., 667 590 1154
Rapoport, S. L., 1015 Reid, D., 956, 1010, 1033, Richard, E., 868 Ritchie, K., 555
Rapson, R. L., 383, 384, 1126, 1127, 1128, Richards, B. S., 154 Ritvo, A., 741, 745, 762
836 1138 Richards, C. S., 545 Ritvo, E., 38, 47, 63, 184,
Rashkis, S., 458 Reid, K., 804 Richards, H., 62 234, 251, 292, 294,
Rasing, E., 867, 899 Reid, M. J., 1060, 1062 Richardson, A. J., 438 299, 316, 366, 367,
Rasmussen, C., 813 Reif, W. E., 1127 Richardson, E., 543 453, 455, 458, 461,
Rasmussen, P., 548, 552, Reik, W., 440 Richardson, J., 145 484, 488, 489, 499,
588, 594 Reilly, J., 260, 549 Richardson, M. P., 523, 525 500, 534, 536, 537,
Rast, J., 901 Reilly, S., 145 Richdale, A., 459, 460, 542 539, 540, 542, 545,
Rastam, M., 101, 113, 432, Reimers, T., 912 Richelme, C., 494, 536, 538 741, 745, 750, 762
586, 590, 743, 760 Reinecke, D. R., 871, 886 Richler, J., 342, 439, 631, Ritvo, R., 251, 292, 294,
Rastamm, M., 109 Reinhartsen, D. B., 870 632, 634 299, 316
Raste, Y., 190, 631, 632 Reis, A. M., 553 Richman, D. M., 819, 823 Rivera, C. M., 253
Ratering, E., 758 Reis, K., 1051 Richman, G. S., 825, 1128 Rivier, F., 552
Ratey, J. J., 189 Reiss, A. L., 141, 185, 269, Richters, M. M., 169, 180 Rivinus, T. M., 79
Ratliff-Schaub, K., 542, 429, 524, 545, 547, Ricks, D. M., 324, 349, Rizzolatti, G., 255, 389,
1112 548 392, 410, 595 524, 673
Ratner, H. H., 237, 318, Reiss, J. E., 693 Ricks, M., 415 Rizzolio, F., 78
369, 383 Reiss, M. L., 1128 Riddle, J. E., 545 Roache, J. D., 1110, 1111
Ratzoni, G., 299 Reite, M., 355, 493, 495 Riddle, J. I., 1141, 1144, Robaey, P., 643
Rauch, A. M., 544 Reitzel, J.-A., 103 1151 Robb, S. A., 144
Rausch, L. A., 551 Remschmidth, H., 1241 Riddle, M. A., 838, 848 Robbins, F. R., 820, 899,
Ravan, S. A., 32, 432, 433, Remy, P., 373 Riederer, P., 139, 141, 142, 900, 916, 1003, 1058,
439, 551, 754 Renda, Y., 130 145 1129
Ravitz, A., 459 Rende, R., 17, 71, 79, 324, Riegelman, R. K., 710, 711 Robbins, L. A., 1015, 1016
Rawitt, R., 1111 707 Riguet, C. B., 253, 392 Robbins, T. W., 368, 372,
Ray, B., 962 Renner, B. R., 720, 734, Riikonen, R., 143, 545 609, 615
Rayfield, R., 1094 748, 749, 818, 820, Rimland, B., 268, 312, 455, Roberts, A. C., 609, 615
Raymond, G., 488 1176, 1199 546, 596, 733, 744, Roberts, B., 956
Raynes, H. R., 544 Renner, H., 144 832, 1014, 1089, 1195 Roberts, J., 317, 345
Realmuto, G., 782 Renner, P., 369, 371 Rinaldi, J., 233, 234, 235, Roberts, M., 1060
Realmuto, G. M., 27, 458, Repacholi, B., 150, 153 236, 237, 384, 385, Roberts, R. J., 616
459, 460, 588, 589, Repetto, G. M., 551 388, 396, 397, 526, Roberts, S. B., 432
838, 1124, 1134, 1139 Repp, A. C., 540, 819, 900, 613, 651, 656, 657, Roberts, S. E., 552
Realmuto, G. R., 463, 490 904, 915, 916, 934 670, 671, 689, 707, Roberts, W., 542
Reaney, S., 967, 968, 1031 Rescorla, L., 182, 183, 345, 799, 832 Robertson, D., 107, 419,
Reaven, J., 953, 1043, 748, 799, 800 Rincover, A., 268, 1089, 493, 521, 526, 652
1050 Restall, G., 845 1129 Robertson, I. H., 619
Recasens, C., 462 Rett, A., 8, 75, 126, 127, Rineer, S., 550, 551 Robertson, J., 32, 167, 169,
Reddy, K. P., 541 129, 132, 134, 135, Rinehart, N. J., 104, 610, 173, 754, 755, 757
Reddy, V., 253, 395, 833, 139, 142, 144, 145, 836 Robertson, K. D., 440
849 147, 154, 431 Ring, A., 1220 Robertson, L. C., 645
Redmond, D. E., Jr., 458 Revecki, D., 45 Ring, B., 326 Robertson, M., 628, 630,
Redwood, L., 435 Revell, W. G., 1094 Ring, H., 373, 419, 525, 631, 634, 682, 838
Reed, D., 635 Revicki, D., 75 526, 615, 652, 666 Robertson, S., 45, 108, 170,
Reed, H. K., 897, 898, 899 Reynell, J., 83, 800 Ringdahl, J. E., 904 228, 323, 426, 437,
Reed, J., 1245 Reynolds, J., 551 Ringman, J. M., 838, 839 554, 599, 632, 740,
Rees, L., 462, 643 Reynolds, N. J., 1130 Ringo, J. L., 519 753, 754, 832
Reeser, R., 1010 Reznick, J., 337, 412, 483, Rinner, L., 1015, 1016 Robins, D. L., 229, 521,
Reeve, C. E., 863, 866, 867, 983 Riordan, M. R., 1128 713, 716, 717
868, 876, 899 Reznick, S., 800 Rios, P., 45, 170, 171, 228, Robins, E., 15
Reeve, E., 458 Rheinberger, A., 1032 323, 412, 426, 427, Robinson, C., 1284
Reeve, S. A., 886 Rheingold, H. L., 653 520, 536, 537, 554, Robinson, H., 489
Reeves, D., 897, 1061, 1064 Rhodes, H., 550 586, 594, 599, 632, Robinson, J. F., 90
Regnier, M. C., 1129 Rhodes, L. E., 1095 740, 753, 832, 1017 Robinson, M., 551
Regolin, L., 692 Rhodes, P. H., 436 Risch, N., 170, 315, 426, Robinson, R., 543
Rehm, R., 268, 889 Riccardi, V. M., 127, 136, 431, 432, 433, 440, Robinson, S., 902, 1069
Reichelt, K. L., 462 141, 142, 152 617, 754 Robinson, T., 201, 295, 542
Reichert, J., 429, 432, 433, Ricci, I., 551 Risi, S., 45, 110, 196, 214, Rochat, P., 395, 398, 630
439, 474, 550, 754 Rice, C., 57, 63, 549, 1070, 223, 224, 228, 233, Roche, C., 544
Reichle, J., 149, 885, 910, 1123 248, 326, 342, 343, Rochen-Renner, B., 543,
926, 939 Rice, M., 149, 345 348, 541, 547, 551, 546, 553, 554
Reichle, N. C., 297, 301, Rice, M. A., 144, 145 655, 709, 712, 731, Rodger, R., 719, 720, 750
302, 1075, 1076, 1256 Rice, M. L., 345, 347 739, 753, 756, 757, Rodgers, J. R., 552
Reichler, R., 289, 543, 546, Rice, M. L. A., 145 778, 779, 780, 781, Rodier, P., 385, 436, 489,
553, 554, 720, 721, Rich, B., 50, 75, 76, 212, 808, 1124, 1143 501, 553, 836
732, 734, 744, 748, 296 Risley, T. R., 883, 903, Rodrigue, J. R., 234, 251,
749, 757, 758, 818, Rich, S., 169, 186, 187, 948, 950, 951, 1006, 316, 385, 392
820, 1055, 1066, 1067, 202, 203, 204, 206, 1125, 1126, 1130, Rodrigues, L., 61, 316, 317
Author Index I • 29

Rodriguez, A., 288, 298, Romine, L., 496 Roy, M., 31, 170, 172, 291, 213, 214, 215, 216,
324 Romski, M., 956 477, 478, 731, 735, 754 227, 228, 232, 237,
Roeder-Gordon, C., 1078, Ronald, A., 1239 Roy, P., 180 288, 290, 292, 293,
1092, 1093 Rondan, C., 519, 520 Royer, J., 251, 1030 294, 295, 296, 297,
Roelofs, J. W., 482 Roosa, J., 996 Royers, H., 105 298, 299, 312, 313,
Roeyers, H., 177, 236, 237, Root, R., 206 Rubia, K., 666, 667 314, 315, 318, 322,
373, 389, 390, 671, Ropar, D., 642 Rubin, C., 149 323, 324, 335, 336,
1057, 1058, 1074 Roper, L., 555 Rubin, E., 31, 224, 232, 341, 342, 343, 344,
Rogan, P., 1088 Rosch, E., 686, 687, 688, 367, 375, 773, 926, 345, 346, 347, 348,
Rogé, B., 439., 500, 501, 690, 691 927, 928, 929, 932, 349, 351, 352, 355,
536 Rose, G. D., 900 933, 935, 936, 937, 373, 408, 412, 418,
Roger, H., 435 Rose, K., 1183 938, 939, 940, 941, 425, 426, 427, 428,
Rogers, C., 548 Rosenbaum, J. L., 317 942, 977, 978, 979, 429, 430, 432, 434,
Rogers, D., 589, 848 Rosenbaum, K., 185, 548 980, 981, 982, 983, 435, 436, 438, 439,
Rogers, M. F., 1021 Rosenbaum, P., 535 986, 987, 989, 990, 441, 442, 443, 453,
Rogers, R. D., 615 Rosenberg, E., 1307 995, 996, 997, 998, 455, 462, 489, 500,
Rogers, S., 13, 29, 32, 33, Rosenberg, L. E., 462 1005, 1008 515, 520, 534, 536,
73, 75, 104, 105, 185, Rosenberger-Diesse, J., 464 Rubin, M., 1105 537, 538, 539, 546,
224, 225, 227, 229, Rosenblatt, B., 476 Ruble, L. A., 267, 301 547, 553, 554, 584,
234, 236, 237, 238, Rosenbloom, L., 73, 75 Ruchelli, E. D., 541 585, 588, 591, 592,
261, 316, 317, 320, Rosenbloom, S., 492 Rucker, R. E., 1091 594, 599, 613, 628,
321, 322, 324, 336, Rosenblum, J. F., 1004, Rude, H. A., 1030, 1031 632, 650, 661, 731,
342, 350, 355, 366, 1005, 1008 Ruhl, D., 546 732, 740, 741, 753,
367, 368, 369, 370, Rosenblum, S. M., 476 Rumeau-Rouquette, C., 61 756, 760, 763, 775,
371, 372, 373, 375, Rosenhall, U., 478, 484, Rumsey, J., 27, 179, 181, 779, 780, 781, 783,
383, 384, 385, 386, 540, 832 202, 203, 204, 206, 820, 832, 834, 1088,
387, 388, 389, 394, Rosenkranz, J. A., 1306 207, 208, 209, 210, 1240, 1244
396, 397, 413, 416, Rosen-Sheidley, B., 429, 251, 263, 266, 268, Rutter, M. J., 110
417, 419, 485, 487, 430, 432, 433, 436, 288, 290, 293, 294, Rutter, M. L., 180, 189,
493, 495, 536, 537, 440, 453 295, 297, 298, 299, 247, 248, 249, 255,
542, 547, 554, 607, Rosenthal, U., 476 316, 344, 352, 367, 256, 265, 269, 707,
611, 612, 613, 614, Rosman, N. P., 499, 1305 368, 372, 388, 476, 708, 712, 722, 731,
615, 618, 631, 634, Ross, A., 800 477, 487, 492, 518, 732, 733, 734, 740,
635, 655, 663, 673, Ross, D., 457, 462, 948 606, 610, 617, 839, 753, 756, 961, 962,
691, 692, 707, 708, Ross, G. S., 398 1015, 1016 1124, 1143, 1201,
709, 716, 721, 723, Ross, R., 485 Runco, M. A., 821 1215, 1216, 1244,
734, 753, 757, 833, Rossetti, L., 800 Ruoppila, I., 296, 297 1305, 1306
834, 836, 837, 843, Rossi, F. G., 502 Rusch, F., 207, 1089, 1091, Ryan, E., 496
847, 863, 874, 939, Rossi, P. G., 502 1092, 1094, 1095 Ryan, R., 102, 800
947, 950, 952, 953, Rotatori, A. F., 261 Ruskin, E., 235, 236, 237, Rydelius, P.-A., 1238, 1241
957, 959, 967, 987, Roth, D., 1096 253, 323, 342, 394, Rydell, A.-M., 1243
1003, 1004, 1008, Rothbart, M., 671 397, 398, 655, 657, Rydell, P., 255, 256, 777,
1043, 1050, 1051, Rothermal, R., 454 662, 946, 1020, 1030 778, 890, 926, 927,
1057, 1112, 1181, Rothermel, R., 490, 517, Russell, J., 234, 344, 350, 928, 929, 930, 932,
1182, 1193 752 368, 370, 372, 373, 933, 934, 937, 938,
Rogers-Warren, A., 826, Rothman, S., 788 388, 608, 609, 611, 939, 940, 941, 961,
883, 884, 951 Rotholtz, D., 264 612, 613, 614, 615, 978, 979
Rogoff, B., 383 Rotholz, D. A., 906 616, 635, 642, 836, Ryerse, C., 110, 317
Rogosch, F. A., 326 Roubertie, A., 552 837
Roid, G. H., 844, 846 Roubertoux, P., 459, 461 Russell, T., 666, 667 Sabbagh, M., 258, 666
Roid, G. M., 789 Rouillard, P., 545 Russo, A., 1124 Saberski, L., 456
Rojahn, J., 266, 909, 1110 Rouleau, N., 611 Russo, D. C., 1129 Sabo, H., 549
Rojas, D., 355, 493, 495 Roulet, E., 80, 82 Russo, F., 552 Sabry, M. A., 551
Rojas, J., 656 Rourke, B., 24, 33, 96, 97, Russo, L., 551 Sachdev, P., 839
Rolando, S., 147 103, 104, 173, 234, Russo, M., 77 Sacks, O., 590, 1283
Roley, S., 835, 836, 846, 375, 498, 586, 628, Russon, A. E., 383 Sadato, N., 665
847 736, 985, 995 Rutgers, A. H., 322 Saeger, K., 455
Rolfe, U., 537 Rouse, B., 616 Rutherford, J. B., Jr., 903 Saelens, T., 539
Rolider, A., 908, 909 Rousseau, F., 545 Rutherford, M., 88, 191, Saelid, G., 462
Rolls, E., 518 Roussounis, T., 551 321, 396, 397, 634 Saemundsen, E., 5, 49, 55,
Roman, J., 489 Routh, D. A., 645 Ruttenberg, B. A., 744, 750 59, 753
Romanczyk, R., 929, 936, Routsoni, C., 1215 Rutter, M., 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, Saez, L., 553
938 Roux, S., 229, 232, 366, 11, 12, 14, 15, 17, 18, Safir, M. P., 73, 80, 1312
Romanczyk, R. G., 254, 394, 479, 712, 751, 21, 23, 26, 28, 30, 31, Safran, J. S., 1015
874, 899, 901, 916, 834 32, 33, 34, 45, 51, 65, Saha, A. R., 1106
949, 1035, 1043, 1044, Rovee-Collier, C., 640 70, 72, 73, 76, 78, 79, Sahakian, B. J., 609
1046, 1047, 1130, 1138 Rovner, L., 909 81, 91, 93, 95, 97, 107, Sahl, R., 297
Romanczyk, R. L., 819, 823 Rowe, A., 373 108, 110, 165, 167, Sai, F., 689
Romano, J., 553, 836 Rowland, D., 526 168, 169, 170, 175, Sainato, D., 968, 1031
Romanski, L., 372, 520, Rowlett, R., 1107 180, 201, 202, 203, Saito, Y., 460
525, 665, 670 Roy, E., 107, 474, 488, 204, 205, 206, 207, Saitoh, O., 367, 368, 494,
Romick, K. S., 908 1305 208, 209, 210, 212, 496, 499, 519, 1305
I • 30 Author Index

Sakaguchi, M., 1225 Scahill, V., 634, 838 Schmitz, N., 107, 493 Schultz, R., 13, 24, 91, 92,
Sakuragawa, N., 140 Scaife, M., 630, 653 Schmotzer, G., 546 95, 96, 102, 104, 106,
Sale, P., 619 Scalise, A., 542 Schnack, H. G., 493, 499 109, 110, 129, 138,
Saliba, E., 483 Scambler, D., 229, 716, Schnecker, K., 141, 142 143, 144, 145, 150,
Saliba, J. R., 109 723 Schneider, H. C., 897 191, 234, 236, 248,
Saliba, K., 123, 519, 661 Scanlon, C., 1004 Schneiderman, G., 455 249, 250, 275, 313,
Salisbury, D. M., 16, 80, 82 Scapagnini, U., 542 Schneier, F. R., 181, 182 325, 326, 375, 412,
Salthammer, E., 154 Scarborough, H. S., 345 Schoener, G., 691 419, 432, 526, 595,
Saltmarsh, R., 608, 614 Scariano, M. M., 1014, Scholz, M., 646 650, 651, 652, 665,
Salvia, J., 713 1077, 1277, 1280, 1281 Schopler, D., 354 670, 682, 683, 684,
Salzberg, C. L., 912 Scarr, S., 656 Schopler, E., 10, 14, 17, 18, 686, 689, 694, 696,
Sam, I. L., 140 Sceery, W. R., 27, 202, 45, 64, 73, 75, 79, 169, 699, 735, 736, 977,
Samdperil, D. L., 1016 203, 204, 206, 207, 186, 188, 202, 203, 979, 982, 998
Sampen, S., 1125 208, 209, 210, 251, 204, 206, 212, 213, Schultz, R. A., 551
Sampson, J., 543, 545 266, 288, 290, 293, 215, 227, 238, 251, Schultz, R. B., 107, 108
Samren, E. B., 556 294, 295, 297, 298, 264, 289, 292, 293, Schultz, R. J., 136, 138,
Samson, Y., 373 299, 316, 344, 352 294, 295, 297, 298, 139, 145
Samuels, M., 151, 153 Schaapveld, M., 903 299, 302, 324, 336, Schultz, R. T., 5, 7, 14, 92,
Sanchez, L., 739, 839, 1107 Schachar, R., 183, 617 369, 453, 539, 543, 96, 105, 170, 191, 239,
Sandberg, A., 234, 1306 Schacter, D. L., 619 546, 553, 554, 667, 325, 365, 368, 372,
Sanders, C., 253 Schaefer, C. E., 1060, 1062, 707, 720, 721, 732, 373, 375, 376, 498,
Sanders, G., 461 1065 733, 734, 741, 744, 518, 519, 520, 521,
Sanders, J. L., 305 Schaefer, G. B., 537 748, 749, 757, 758, 522, 523, 524, 525,
Sanders, M. R., 1060, 1062 Schaeffer, B., 882, 883 792, 818, 820, 889, 526, 696, 697, 777,
Sanders, R. S., 499 Schaffer, B., 289, 749, 758, 926, 932, 937, 940, 889, 1103
Sandler, A. D., 1112 1176, 1216 953, 978, 1004, 1005, Schultz, W., 670
Sandler, R. H., 541 Schaffer, R., 319, 323 1036, 1043, 1048, Schulz, D. W., 1106
Sandman, C. A., 189, 459, Schain, R., 454, 455, 456, 1049, 1050, 1055, Schulz, S. C., 438, 463
460, 461, 462 1195 1059, 1060, 1061, Schumann, C. M., 108, 526
Sandow, D., 1094 Schall, J., 669, 670 1064, 1065, 1066, Schuntermann, P., 1056,
Sandson, J., 608 Schanen, C., 78, 138 1067, 1068, 1069, 1061, 1069
Sandstrom, M., 540, 832 Scharre, J., 832 1070, 1071, 1074, Schuster, J. W., 868
Sanford, M., 733 Schatz, A. M., 100, 367, 1075, 1076, 1077, Schwab-Stone, M., 9, 10,
Sangiorgi, E., 551 835, 836 1089, 1132, 1175, 17, 26
Sankar, D. V. S., 464 Schatz, J., 187, 251 1176, 1177, 1178, Schwafel, J., 476, 478, 481
Santangelo, S., 170, 173, Schaumann, H., 47, 49, 60, 1179, 1180, 1182, Schwandt, W. L., 886
489, 500, 501, 536 62, 227, 232, 329, 502, 1183, 1186, 1199, Schwartz, I., 969, 988,
Santarcangelo, S., 910 545, 834 1216, 1310 1030, 1031
Santogrossi, J., 885, 951 Scheffer, I. E., 544 Schouten, J., 520, 696 Schwartz, L. S., 1128
Santos, C. W., 1110, 1111 Scheibel, A., 140, 489 Schreck, K. A., 542, 752 Schwartz, M., 130, 132,
Santucci, M., 502 Scheier, C., 691 Schreibman, L., 236, 237, 137, 138, 492
Sapuan, S., 956 Schell, R., 819 250, 253, 261, 264, Schwartz, S., 551
Sarlo-McGarvey, N., 147 Schellenberg, G. D., 165, 268, 346, 384, 393, Schyns, P. G., 521, 522, 523
Sarne, Y., 459, 462, 1252 171, 191, 651, 755 394, 395, 494, 496, Scifo, R., 542
Sarokoff, R. A., 871 Schendel, D., 44, 49, 52, 499, 821, 823, 867, Sciscio, N., 552
Sarria, E., 320 57, 435, 555 882, 883, 884, 886, Scotese-Wojtila, L., 539
Sartorius, N., 17 Schepis, M., 956, 1033 887, 888, 889, 890, Scott, F. J., 57, 254, 724
Sasagawa, E., 254 Scherer, N. J., 552 912, 913, 916, 937, Scott, J. P., 1112
Sasaki, M., 325, 834, 1176, Schermer, A., 1031 950, 953, 959, 961, Scott, S., 429, 534, 536,
1177 Schery, T., 800 966, 990, 991, 1004, 546, 594, 663
Sasaki, N., 141 Scheuffgen, K., 641 1005, 1006, 1007, Scotti, J. R., 915
Sassi, R. B., 491, 500 Schicor, A., 458 1009, 1011, 1030, Scourfield, J., 48, 59, 62
Sasso, G., 819, 823, 872, Schiel, W., 143 1031, 1032, 1055, Scragg, P., 101, 299
912, 916, 1030, 1031 Schiller, M., 322 1058, 1060, 1061, Scriver, C. R., 462
Sasson, N., 412, 483 Schindler, M. K., 315 1129, 1160, 1310 Scuderi, C., 494, 499, 542,
Sattler, J. M., 787, 792 Schlag, J., 668, 670 Schroeder, G. L., 883 1307
Satz, P., 30 Schlag-Rey, M., 668, 670 Schroeder, M., 431 Seal, B., 836, 955
Saulnier, C., 777, 784, 793 Schlattmann, P., 64 Schroer, R. J., 251, 501, Searle, J., 687
Saunders, K., 64 Schleichkorn, J., 153 550, 551 Sears, L., 241, 250, 267,
Saunders, R. C., 526 Schleien, S. J., 870 Schroth, G., 140 498, 661
Sauvage, D., 555, 751, Schloss, P. J., 911 Schroth, P., 745, 750 Sears, S. P., 467
1208 Schlosser, R., 802 Schuh-Wear, C. L., 1127, Seashore, M. R., 50, 463,
Savedra, J.,Perman, J., 134, Schmahmann, J. D., 1305 1128 553
146 Schmall, B., 454 Schuler, A., 254, 348, 691, Secord, W., 807
Sayegh, L., 548 Schmeidler, J., 437, 753, 773, 777, 926, 927, Sedaghat, F., 522
Sayi, A., 130 754 936, 938, 939, 941, Seeger, T. F., 1106
Scacheri, C., 130, 138, 139, Schmidek, M., 813 961, 966, 967, 968, Seeman, P., 1103
148 Schmidt, A. W., 1106 979, 980, 981, 983, Segawa, M., 127, 140, 141,
Scafidi, F., 690 Schmidt, B., 142 984, 985, 988, 990, 142
Scahill, L., 268, 269, 1102, Schmidt, D., 534 995, 997 Segebarth, C., 693
1103, 1104, 1105, Schmidt, M., 1241 Schuler, L., 553 Seibert, J., 653, 654, 662
1106, 1108, 1109 Schmidt, S., 800 Schulman, M., 30 Seielstad, M., 431
Author Index I • 31

Seifer, R., 322 Sharpe, P. A., 152 212, 213, 214, 288, 236, 237, 238, 251,
Seiteberger, F., 139, 140 Sharpe, S., 613 289, 290, 291, 292, 253, 254, 258, 260,
Sejnowski, T. J., 694 Sharpley, C., 1058, 1064, 293, 296, 298, 299, 261, 318, 319, 320,
Sekhon, G. S., 551 1065, 1069 502, 534, 538, 1305, 321, 322, 323, 324,
Sekul, E. A., 127, 128, 129, Shattuck, P., 214, 288, 289, 1307, 1311, 1312 325, 335, 342, 350,
134, 135, 143, 144, 151 290, 293, 324 Shinohara, M., 140 369, 382, 385, 386,
Selfe, L., 795 Shavelle, R. M., 212, 293, Shinomiya, M., 502, 503, 392, 394, 397, 398,
Seligman, M., 1060, 1064, 295, 296, 297 1225 409, 410, 411, 415,
1068 Shaw, D. W., 170, 172, 493, Shiotsuki, M., 46 416, 520, 521, 526,
Selman, R. L., 795 496, 497, 498, 501 Shiotsuki, Y., 540 630, 631, 652, 653,
Seltzer, G. B., 906 Shaw, S., 261 Shipley-Benamou, R., 886 654, 655, 656, 657,
Seltzer, M. M., 214, 288, Shay, J., 839 Shirakawa, Y., 461, 539 658, 659, 660, 662,
289, 290, 293, 295, Shaywitz, B. A., 453, 455, Shirataki, S., 47, 1223 665, 667, 669, 670,
297, 323, 324, 1055, 456, 457, 459, 492, 744 Shoaf, S. E., 454 671, 673, 702, 707,
1059, 1075, 1077 Shea, E., 690 Shoemaker, J., 1128 724, 738, 740, 741,
Selvin, S., 44, 49, 59, 61, Shea, M. C., 822 Shoenfeld, Y., 542 750, 751, 753, 755,
63, 184, 291 Shea, V., 266, 292, 298, Shoji, H., 46, 540 757, 833, 836, 946,
Semenuk, G., 458 299, 301, 302, 1050, Shook, J., 483 961, 962, 1007, 1011,
Semprino, M., 552 1061, 1069, 1073, 1184 Shores, R., 968, 1030 1020, 1030
Semrud-Clikeman, M., 619 Shearer, D. D., 886, 990 Short, A., 17, 73, 79, 227, Sigurdardóttir, S., 753
Senft, R., 263, 691 Shearer, T. R., 539 707, 1048, 1050, 1182 Sikich, L., 1111
Senju, A., 318 Shedlorski, J. G., 467 Short, C. B., 336 Silahtaroglu, A., 130, 132,
Sergeant, J. A., 373 Sheehan, D. V., 1282 Short, L., 633 137, 138
Seri, S., 479, 481 Sheely, R., 958, 967 Short, M., 543 Silberg, J., 430
Serra, M., 171, 177, 186, Sheffield, V., 438, 754 Shoshan, Y., 926, 941, 1049 Siller, M., 254, 320, 335
187, 268 Sheikh, R. M., 1105 Showalter, D., 30 Sillibourne, J., 550
Sevcik, R., 956 Sheinkopf, S., 235, 319, Shprintzen, R. J., 552 Silva, A. E., 551
Sevenich, R., 823 349, 800, 948, 962, Shrader, C., 1021 Silva, E., 551
Sevin, B., 745, 749, 751, 1008, 1064 Shriberg, L., 256, 293, 343, Silveira, E. L., 553
951 Sheitman, B. B., 27, 839, 348, 352, 813, 962 Silver, E. J., 1062
Sevin, J., 719, 720, 721, 847, 1107 Shu, B. C., 607 Silver, M., 261
745, 749, 750, 751, Shekhar, A., 526 Shubin, R. A., 543 Silverman, J. M., 429, 432,
833, 951 Sheldon, J. B., 916, 1098 Shukla, S., 267 433, 437, 439, 474,
Seymour, P. A., 1106 Sheldon, S., 544, 551 Shulman, C., 224, 341, 342, 550, 753, 754
Seys, D. M., 1127 Shell, J., 1102, 1104 343, 474, 551, 730, Silverstein, M. L., 181
Sfaello, I., 517 Shellenberger, S., 980 732, 734, 740, 748, Sim, C. H., 617
Shabani, D. B., 267, 870 Shepard, B. A., 173 749, 753, 1176, 1178 Sim, L., 426
Shafer, K., 969, 990, 991, Shepherd, C., 51, 545, 594 Shulman, L., 76, 79, 80, 82, Simell, O., 545
1030 Shepherd, M., 14, 15, 17, 1312 Simensen, R. J., 251, 501
Shafer, M. S., 1030, 1088 1240 Shuster, A., 438 Simeon, J., 501
Shaffer, D., 14, 15, 17, 200, Sheppard, D. M., 1308 Shuster, L., 1281 Simeonsson, R. J., 351,
695, 762, 1240 Sheppard, V., 1112 Shyamsundar, M. M., 543 820, 962, 1057, 1068,
Shah, A., 101, 209, 210, Sherer, M., 886, 887, 888, Shyi, G. C. W., 640 1074
214, 290, 299, 589, 908 Shyu, V., 635, 844 Simion, F., 317, 318
597, 635, 642, 643, Sheridan, S. M., 1063 Sicotte, N., 367, 617 Simizu, Y., 502, 503
790, 838, 839, 840 Sherman, J. A., 883, 916, Siebelink, B. M., 503 Simmel, M., 525, 697
Shah, B., 189, 839, 1310 1098 Siegal, B., 348, 592, 730, Simmonds, H. A., 464
Shah, P., 1108 Sherman, M., 251, 321, 732, 734, 739, 740, 750 Simmonoff, E., 632
Shahbazian, M., 132, 137, 416, 417 Siegal, D. J., 263 Simmons, A., 373, 419,
138, 431, 432 Sherman, S. L., 546 Siegal, M., 660 435, 525, 526, 541,
Shahmoon-Shanok, R., 1310 Sherman, T., 7, 185, 233, Siegel, A., 1064 615, 652, 666
Shakal, J., 539 236, 237, 253, 254, Siegel, B., xix, 13, 16, 17, Simmons, H., 49, 60
Shaked, M., 1058 317, 318, 320, 321, 18, 19, 21, 29, 31, 45, Simmons, J., 962
Shallice, T., 670 369, 392, 394, 653, 72, 73, 93, 174, 175, Simmons, J. Q., 256, 346,
Shalock, R. L., 1096 656, 665, 667, 669, 179, 184, 232, 233, 882, 887, 1089, 1180,
Shamow, N. A., 897 707, 738 234, 248, 294, 314, 1306
Shannak, K., 142 Shernoff, E., 722, 752 317, 323, 335, 541, Simmons, J. Q., III., 393
Shannon, M., 537 Sherr, E. H., 544 542, 707, 708, 709, Simon, E. W., 550, 551
Shanske, A., 544 Sherrard, E., 477 713, 717, 718, 948, Simon, J., 107, 492, 499,
Shao, Y., 32, 429, 432, 433, Sherwin, A. C., 167, 460 984, 1008, 1056, 1069, 539, 768
439, 753, 754 Sheslow, B. V., 373 1221 Simon, N., 6
Shapiro, A. K., 588 Shestakova, A., 481, 482 Siegel, B. V., 496, 617 Simonoff, E., 108, 170, 425,
Shapiro, B., 800 Shevell, M. I., 107, 535, 646 Siegel, B. V., Jr., 367 426, 427, 430, 434, 438,
Shapiro, E. G., 44, 57, 61 Shewmon, D. A., 503 Siegel, D. J., 97, 103, 257, 441, 442, 500, 536, 537,
Shapiro, E. S., 588 Shields, W. D., 354 263, 264, 295, 367, 538, 553
Shapiro, H., 712 Shields-Wolfe, J., 1088 387, 487, 835, 1306 Simpson, G. M., 838, 848
Shapiro, T., 17, 184, 249, Shigley, R. H., 1055, 1060, Siegel, G., 293, 804, 883 Simpson, R., 113, 354, 374,
251, 261, 321, 324, 1064, 1069, 1178 Siegel, L., 1273 375, 867, 868, 869,
346, 409, 416, 417, Shih, J. C., 539 Sievers, P., 44, 57, 61 872, 907, 913, 929,
453, 520, 707, 749 Shimizu, Y., 47, 435, 546 Sigafoos, J., 542, 870 930, 985, 986, 996,
Sharbrough, F. W., 476, 479 Shinnar, S., 65, 76, 79, 80, Sigman, M., 7, 170, 173, 1018, 1021, 1022,
Share, L. J., 645 82, 202, 204, 205, 206, 185, 233, 234, 235, 1123, 1129
I • 32 Author Index

Sims, A., 167, 169, 757 Smith, C. E., 821, 897, 916 Sommerschild, H., 1243 Sponheim, E., 5, 48, 54, 55,
Simson, R., 479, 480 Smith, C. J., 429, 432, 433, Song, Y., 541 432, 535, 542, 735,
Sinclair, B. D., 499 437, 439, 474, 550, Soni, W., 666, 667 736, 748
Sinclair, J., 1022, 1284 753, 754 Sonksen, P., 554, 589 Sponseller, P. D., 150, 153,
Sinclair, L., 949 Smith, E. O., 141 Soper, H. V., 30, 900 154
Singer, G. H. S., 1060, Smith, I. M., 44, 47, 100, Soprano, A. M., 1228, 1230 Spradlin, J. E., 819, 822,
1063, 1068, 1092 184, 237, 291, 294, Sörensen, S. A., 50 883, 884
Singer, H. S., 137 295, 320, 384, 387, Sorgi, P., 189 Sprague, J. R., 822, 823,
Singer, J., 321, 323 388, 389, 539, 835, Souders, M. C., 542 824, 825, 903, 911,
Singer, W., 690, 695 836, 837 Sourkes, T. L., 456, 457 1010
Singh, N., 493, 501, 536, Smith, K., 515, 874 South, M., 213, 251, 375, Sprague, R. L., 847, 848
646, 660, 762, 915 Smith, L., 105, 147, 149, 542, 616, 653, 722, Spratling, M. W., 549
Singh, S., 74, 76 369, 691, 695, 849 731, 736, 752, 757, Springer, J., 749, 1072
Singh, T., 536 Smith, M., 51, 207, 503, 762, 1112 Spurkland, I., 1239
Singh, V., 539, 542 544, 545, 550, 551, Southall, D., 151, 153 Square-Storer, P., 957
Sinka, I., 804 769, 1078 Sowers, J., 1093 Squires, J., 710, 712
Siperstein, R., 31, 73, 79 Smith, M. A., 46 Sowinski, J., 434 Squires, K., 479, 480
Siponmaa, L., 299 Smith, M. D., 298, 302, Spagnola, M., 1062 Squires, N., 480
Siqueland, E., 689 902, 904, 905, 916, Sparen, P., 539 Squires, T., 261
Sirian, L., 613 1091, 1093 Sparks, B. F., 170, 172, Srinath, S., 457
Sirianni, N., 130, 136, 138, Smith, N. J., 435 493, 496, 497, 498, Srinivasan, M., 139
139, 148 Smith, P., 61, 254, 393, 501, 536, 646, 660 Sriram, N., 871, 956
Sirota, K. G., 868 395, 396, 399, 1307 Sparling, J. W., 224 St. George, M., 549
Sisson, L., 266, 967 Smith, P. G., 316, 317 Sparrevohn, R., 106 St. James, P. J., 258, 350
Sitter, S., 177 Smith, P. K., 394 Sparrow, S., 24, 234, 315, Stach, B. A., 147, 148
Siu, V. M., 544 Smith, R., 544, 1127, 1148 316, 367, 374, 595, Stackhaus, J., 1032
Sjogreen, L., 553 Smith, R. C., 848 628, 682, 777, 793, Stackhouse, T., 237, 321,
Sjöholm, E., 46 Smith, R. G., 901, 907 800, 804, 845 342, 384, 385, 386,
Sjöholm-Lif, E., 1243 Smith, S. M., 521 Sparrow, S. A., 315 387, 388, 837, 847
Sjostedt, A., 551 Smith, T., 155, 239, 253, Sparrow, S. S., 24, 25, 28, Stafford, N., 261
Skender, M., 136, 144 343, 708, 874, 915, 33, 83, 91, 96, 97, 103, Staghezza, B., 732
Skinner, B. F., 1006, 1125, 926, 929, 949, 951, 104, 107, 108, 111, Stahl, S. M., 454, 455, 456
1155 953, 987, 1004, 1007, 170, 173, 177, 183, Stahmer, A., 237, 253, 384,
Skinner, C., 501 1009, 1012, 1044, 186, 213, 225, 227, 393, 394, 395, 867,
Skinner, M., 546, 547 1048, 1058, 1132, 238, 251, 252, 294, 884, 886, 888, 889,
Skinner, S. A., 550, 551 1141 316, 324, 325, 375, 890, 913, 1004, 1009,
Skjeldal, O., 5, 48, 54, 55, Smits, A. P., 545 412, 520, 689, 696, 1011
129, 130, 131, 132, Smoker, W. R., 548 719, 720, 735, 736, Staib, L., 523
147, 149, 535 Snauwaert, D. T., 1092, 738, 750, 754, 755, Stanhope, S. J., 839
Skoff, B. F., 476 1093 772, 773, 777, 782, Stanley, J. A., 491, 500
Skotko, B. G., 154, 155 Snead, R. W., 1108 783, 784, 789, 793, Stansfield, B., 697
Skudlarski, P., 522, 696 Sneed, T. J., 1126 835 Stanton, A., 1074
Skuse, D. H., 433, 434, 544 Snell, M., 821, 868 Spaulding, P., 554 Stapells, D. R., 844
Slangen, J. L., 482 Snell, R. G., 543, 545 Spechler, L., 183 Stark, J. A., 1088, 1093
Slavik, B. A., 1014 Snidman, N., 983 Speck-Martins, C. E., 553 Starkstein, S. E., 107, 494,
Slifer, K. J., 825 Snow, M. E., 17, 184, 261, Spector, S., 458 499, 519, 537, 538
Slifer, M., 753 324, 409, 459, 460, Speed, W. C., 438 Starr, A., 476, 479
Sloan, J., 1057, 1068, 1074, 462, 520, 707, 749, Spellman, R. A., 550 Starr, E., 5, 426, 427
1075, 1076 1208 Speltz, M. L., 1029 Stary, J. M., 463, 490
Sloan, M. E., 1126, 1138 Snowling, M., 255, 263, Spence, A., 946 State, R. C., 27, 267,
Sloane, H. N., 1125 641, 642 Spence, M. A., 537, 550 1106
Sloman, L., 544 Snyder, D., 29, 30, 524 Spence, S. J., 432, 433 Stauder, J., 234, 368, 643
Slomka, G. T., 1107 Snyder, J., 521 Spencer, A., 318, 324, 409, Stedge, D., 27, 189, 1110
Slonims, V., 127, 248 Snyderman, M., 788 520, 740 Steege, M., 912, 916
Small, A. M., 189, 460, Soares, D., 740 Spencer, C., 431, 432 Steel, R., 335
461, 462, 501, 739, Soares, J. C., 1305 Spencer, E. K., 189, 838, Steele, M., 544, 546
1102, 1104, 1107, Sobel, N., 526 839, 840, 847 Steele, R., 204, 206, 215,
1112 Sobesky, W. E., 545 Spencer, E. S., 1112 288, 293, 962, 1304
Smalley, S., 51, 182, 185, Sobsey, D., 290, 1075, Spencer, T., 1109 Steele, S., 521
208, 295, 428, 430, 1093 Spengler, P., 914 Steenhuis, M. P., 177, 268
536, 537, 545, 549, Sofic, E., 139, 141, 142, Spengler-Schelley, M., 964 Steere, D. E., 1091
769, 1305, 1307 145 Spiegle-Cohen, J., 660 Steerneman, P., 758
Smania, N., 691 Sofronoff, K., 1064, 1065 Spielman, R. S., 431 Stefanatos, G., 541, 1112
Smári, J., 753 Sohlberg, M. M., 619 Spiker, D., 170, 315, 415, Stefanos, G. A., 503
Smedegaard, N., 1238, Sohmer, H., 476 426, 432, 433, 437, Steffenburg, S., 47, 49, 60,
1239 Sokol, D. K., 463 440, 617, 754 62, 108, 186, 199, 202,
Smeeth, L., 61, 316, 317 Sokol, M. S., 1112 Spinner, N. B., 551 204, 213, 215, 291,
Smit, A. M., 503 Sokolski, K. N., 541, 1112 Spirtos, G., 553 293, 296, 425, 534,
Smith, A., 542, 633, 902 Solimando, A., 660 Spitz, R., 317, 1304 538, 545, 551, 553,
Smith, B., 266, 371, 408, Solnick, J. V., 1089 Spitzer, R., 15, 16, 17, 45, 1305
1056, 1057 Solomon, O., 868 184, 592, 707, 708 Steffenburg, U., 129, 291,
Smith, C., 869, 926, 1062 Somaia, M., 661 Spong, A., 634 551
Author Index I • 33

Steffens, M., 235, 319, 349, 386, 408, 409, 655, Suckling, J., 107, 493, 666, 320, 342, 369, 384,
800, 962 707, 708, 712, 717, 667 410, 434, 613, 634,
Steg, J. P., 501 718, 721, 732, 734, Suddendorf, T., 266, 389 643, 652, 654, 655,
Stehli, A., 1281, 1284 799, 801, 833, 834, Sudhalter, V., 30, 335, 345 656, 657, 665, 707,
Stein, D. J., 266 835, 836, 837, 843, Sue, C. M., 550 708, 709, 713, 714,
Stein, M. T., 81 947, 962, 1007, 1011, Sugayama, S. M., 553 715, 716, 730, 732,
Stein, R. E. K., 1062 1056, 1068 Sugiura, M., 389, 662, 663 733, 734, 741, 754,
Steinberg, M., 169 Stoof, J. C., 1103 Sugiyama, T., 44, 47, 478, 755, 763, 832, 837,
Steiner, V., 800, 804 Stores, G., 542 1224 847, 947, 961, 1307
Steingard, R., 28, 189, Storey, K., 822, 824, 825, Sulkes, J., 1220 Swiderski, R. E., 1307
1108, 1111 903, 968, 990, 991 Sullivan, C. L., 870, 876 Swiezy, N., 267, 912, 1109
Steinhausen, H., 46, 184 Storm, K., 1095 Sullivan, K., 259, 260, 344, Swisher, C. N., 553
Steinhoff, K., 1109 Storoschuk, S., 31, 32, 335, 354, 549, 552, 698 Swisher, C. W., 1305
Steinschneider, M., 1306, 344, 408, 520, 731, Sullivan, M. W., 149 Switala, A. E., 107, 474,
1307 732, 740, 741, 753 Sullivan, N., 723 488, 1305
Steir, D., 169, 175 Stott, C., 632, 645 Sullivan, R. C., 1089, 1257, Sylvester, O., 462
Stella, J., 319, 657, 721, Stott, D. H., 835 1263 Symons, F. J., 265, 267,
724, 750, 800, 833, Stowe, J., 435, 541 Sulmont, V., 459, 461 839, 847
947 Stowe, M. J., 1124 Sulzer-Azaroff, B., 890, Symons, L. A., 689
Stellern, J., 794 Stowe, R. M., 613 953, 1011, 1030, 1031, Syrota, A., 373
Stellfeld, M., 435 Stowitschek, J. J., 912, 1130 Sytema, S., 753
Stenbom, Y., 132 1091 Summers, C. G., 554 Szatmari, P., 8, 13, 17, 18,
Stephan, M., 556 Strain, P., 708, 870, 897, Summers, J. A., 1078 19, 24, 31, 72, 77, 78,
Stephenson, J. B., 127, 136, 898, 899, 912, 937, Summers, S. A., 1092, 1093 88, 92, 93, 94, 95, 97,
147, 148 938, 939, 940, 953, Sundberg, C., 963, 964 100, 101, 103, 104,
Stergaard, O., 551 967, 968, 990, 991, Sundberg, M., 929, 932, 109, 110, 118, 169,
Stern, D., 225, 234, 236, 1004, 1011, 1030, 933, 937, 939, 941, 170, 172, 173, 174,
260, 319, 383, 673, 1031, 1035, 1045, 963, 964 175, 179, 183, 184,
836, 980 1046, 1047, 1057, Sundram, B. S., 549 185, 186, 187, 202,
Stern, L., 479 1061, 1064, 1074, Sung, K., 486 203, 204, 206, 207,
Sternbach, H., 460 1129, 1130 Sungum-Paliwal, R., 266, 210, 211, 213, 216,
Stevens, M., 1306, 1307 Strandburg, R. J., 481 408 232, 233, 234, 248,
Stevenson, C. L., 871 Stratton, J., 1017, 1018, Sungum-Paliwal, S. R., 48, 252, 256, 257, 258,
Stevenson, C. S., 619 1019 52, 54, 61, 63 269, 288, 295, 297,
Stevenson, J., 418, 454, Stratton, K., 435 Suratt, A., 832, 833 298, 299, 317, 323,
554, 580, 595, 838, 848 Stratton, M., 847 Surian, L., 258, 352 348, 372, 373, 375,
Stevenson, R. E., 501, 538 Strauss, D. J., 212, 293, Surratt, A., 912, 913, 951 427, 437, 477, 478,
Stevenson, R. J., 619 295, 296, 297 Susnjara, I. M., 459 491, 586, 590, 595,
Stewart, K., 984 Strayer, D. L., 368, 370, Sussman, F., 936, 958, 966 606, 607, 628, 719,
Stewart, M. A., 427 372, 498, 608, 609, Susuki, I., 141 730, 731, 732, 733,
Stewart, M. P., 1032 611, 616, 617, 643, Sutcliffe, J. S., 545 734, 735, 736, 739,
Stewart, T. G., 489 836 Sutton, K., 956, 1112 740, 743, 750, 753,
Stich, S., 659, 673 Streedback, D., 1127 Sutton, S., 669 754, 845, 984, 1014,
Stiebel, D., 1064 Streiner, D., 24, 93, 95, Suwa, S., 461 1058, 1066, 1074, 1221
Stier, D., 17, 72, 73, 79, 100, 101, 110, 174, Suzuki, H., 140 Szatmark, I., 45
223, 593, 707 213, 252, 269, 317, Svennerholm, B. A., 142 Szechter, L., 697
Stipek, D. J., 324, 415 343, 345, 719, 734, Svennerholm, L., 139, 457, Szechtman, H., 521
Stirling, J., 520 735, 736, 743, 845, 459 Szekely, B., 459, 462
Stockard, J. E., 476, 479 1014 Svenson, B., 588 Szekely, G., 1220, 1252
Stockard, J. J., 476, 479 Stremel-Campbell, K., Sverd, J., 209, 456, 457, Szentagothai, J., 489
Stoddart, K., 212 1076 463 Szivos, S. E., 207
Stodgell, C. J., 494 Striano, T., 395, 398 Swaab-Barneveld, H., 169, Szurek, S., 209
Stoecker, J. J., 645 Strik, W., 521 171, 412 Szymanski, E., 1091
Stoel-Gammon, C., 960 Strogatz, M., 541 Swanson, J., 617, 1109 Szymanski, L., 546, 594
Stoiber, K. C., 1059 Stromer, R., 871 Swartz, K. L., 838, 848
Stokes, D., 101 Stromland, K., 544, 553 Swayne, S., 542 Tableau, F., 459, 462
Stokes, P. E., 460 Stromme, P., 544, 548 Swe, A., 214, 288, 289, Tabuteau, F., 1112
Stokes, R., 950 Strong, C., 813 290, 293, 324 Tachataki, M., 543, 545
Stokes, T. F., 826, 827 Strouse, M. C., 916 Swedo, S. E., 266 Tachibana, R., 46, 52, 227,
Stokwell, M., 501 Strunk, P., 1240, 1241 Sweeney, J., 185, 373, 456, 455, 456, 459, 460,
Stone, D. H., 547 Strupp, K., 64 458, 484, 485, 496, 461, 834, 1224
Stone, J. L., 1307 Stubbs, E. G., 476, 544 498, 832 Tadevosyan, O., 439
Stone, K. R., 384, 389 Student, M., 476 Sweeney, M. H., 384, 389 Tadevosyan-Leyfer, O., 32,
Stone, R. L., 464 Stump, M. H., 547 Sweet, R. D., 588 731, 733, 754, 755
Stone, V., 88, 524, 526, Stuphorn, V., 669, 670 Sweeten, T. L., 526 Taft, L. T., 594
615, 630, 634, 1306 Sturmey, P., 60, 211, 719, Sweetland-Baker, M., 257, Tager-Flusberg, H., 104,
Stone, W., 31, 225, 227, 721, 745, 749, 750 912 106, 185, 234, 256,
228, 229, 231, 232, Stuss, D., 371, 613, 669, Sweetman, L., 544 257, 258, 259, 260,
233, 235, 236, 237, 671 Swettenham, J., 31, 48, 52, 291, 293, 336, 342,
238, 251, 316, 317, Subhani-Siddique, R., 542, 54, 57, 60, 105, 227, 343, 344, 345, 346,
319, 320, 321, 351, 1112 229, 231, 232, 233, 347, 348, 350, 353,
355, 366, 384, 385, Subramaniam, B., 141 234, 235, 237, 266, 354, 369, 373, 439,
I • 34 Author Index

Tager-Flusberg (Continued) Teague, L. A., 1107 Thurber, C., 259, 260, 348, Toth, S. L., 1062
549, 630, 631, 641, Teal, M. B., 730, 749 353 Touchette, P. E., 824, 899
698, 960, 962, 1306 Teasdale, J., 668 Thurm, A., 723 Tourrette, C., 394
Taiminen, T., 102 Tebbenkamp, K., 872, 876, Thvedt, J., 907 Towbin, K. E., 28, 116,
Taira, R., 1223 1020 Thyer, B. A., 905 169, 181, 182, 187,
Takagi, K., 1281 Teberg, A. J., 544 Tidhar, S., 433 189, 190
Takagi, N., 138 Teebi, A. S., 130 Tidmarsh, L., 53, 102 Townsend, J., 367, 368,
Takahashi, T., 60, 435, 501, Teeter, P. A., 619 Tidswell, T., 613 483, 493, 539, 610,
536 Teitelbaum, O., 387, 837 Tiegerman, E., 934 617, 655, 656, 659,
Takatsu, T., 455 Teitelbaum, P., 387, 837 Tien, A., 485, 607 660, 663, 668, 671,
Takekuma, K., 541 Temlett, J. A., 1103 Tierney, E., 544, 547, 762, 832, 889, 1305
Takesada, M., 461, 463, Tempelaars, A., 543 1102, 1103, 1104 Tpocu, M., 130
1226 Temple, E., 1308 Tigue, Z., 489, 492, 493, Trad, P. V., 249
Takeuchi, A. H., 644 Tendero, A., 544 499, 501, 519, 524, Tramer, M., 1239
Takeuchi, K., 262 ten Hove, F., 183 661 Tran, C. Q., 21, 77, 78, 130,
Takita, H., 661 Tentler, D., 109 Tildon, J. T., 541, 1112 136, 138, 431, 432
Talay-Ongan, A., 832, 833, Terenius, L., 76, 142, 462 Tilton, J. R., 321, 394 Tranel, D., 497, 524, 526
834, 843 Terpstra, J. E., 322 Timm, M., 968, 1030 Traphagen, J., 966
Tallal, P., 1308 Terro, F., 551 Timonen, E., 534, 544 Trasowech, J., 257, 819,
Tallis, J., 1228, 1230 Terzic, I. M., 459 Tines, J., 1094 823, 950, 953
Tamarit, J., 320 Terzic, J., 459 Tinline, C., 268, 588 Trauner, D. A., 100, 144,
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., 236 Tesch, D., 898, 903 Tint, G. S., 454 145, 367, 835, 836
Tamminga, C. A., 1106 Tessier, R., 690 Tipper, S., 609 Traversay, J., 662
Tamulonis, D., 553 Testa, M., 183 Tirosh, E., 49, 73, 80, 1 Travis, L., 961, 962, 1030
Tanaka, J. R., 520 Teunisse, J. P., 324, 372, 312 Treanor, J., 436
Tanaka, J. W., 697 610, 613, 644 Tisdale, B., 489, 553, 836 Trecker, A., 835, 849
Tanaka, K., 50, 463, 553 Thaiss, L., 632, 666 Titus, E., 455 Treffert, D., 46, 596
Tanaka, M., 691 Thal, D., 337, 800 Titus, J. B., 539 Tremblay, L., 670
Tancredi, R., 551 Tharp, R. G., 1126 Tobias, R., 431 Trentacoste, S. V., 1309
Tanguay, P., 32, 51, 167, Thelen, E., 691, 695, 849 Tobin, A., 83, 804 Trentalange, M. J., 538
169, 173, 181, 182, Themner, U., 177 Tobing, L. E., 1055, 1057 Trescher, W. H., 547
183, 208, 295, 428, Thibadeau, S., 1183 Toci, L., 261 Tress, B., 492
476, 478, 481, 542, Thibault, G., 542 Todd, A. W., 897, 898, 899 Trevarthen, C., 319, 325,
545, 751, 754, 755, Thiemann, K. S., 887, 1033 Todd, R. D., 32, 95, 315, 383, 673
757, 769, 782, 1124, Thirrupathy, K., 541 440, 746, 754 Trevathan, E., 127, 128,
1134, 1139 Thirumalai, S. S., 543 Toga, A., 499 132, 134
Tani, Y., 458 Thivierge, J., 31, 170, 172, Toichi, M., 371 Triggs, W., 502
Tankersley, M., 886, 887 476, 477, 731, 735, Tojo, Y., 318 Trillingsgaard, A., 105,
Tanoue, Y., 47 754 Tolbert, L., 539 325, 551, 682
Tantam, D., 92, 93, 94, 95, Thomas, G., 991 Tolkin-Eppel, P., 343 Trimble, M., 593
97, 100, 101, 107, 109, Thomas, H. V., 543, 545 Tolosa, E. S., 108, 519 Tripathi, H. L., 142
124, 179, 183, 184, Thomas, J., 1176 Tomasello, M., 236, 237, Trivedi, M. H., 542
203, 204, 206, 208, Thomas, M. A., 1092 318, 336, 337, 369, Trivedi, R., 140
210, 212, 295, 297, Thomas, M. W., 543 383, 390, 395, 398, Trivette, C., 932
299, 519, 520, 586, Thomas, N. S., 551, 552 630, 653, 654, 656, Tronick, E. Z., 690
735, 979, 983 Thomas, S., 839, 1246 657, 670, 978, 979 Tronick., 981
Tao, Kuo-Tai., 1203 Thompsen, W., 44 Tomblin, J. B., 257, 353, Troscianko, T., 692
Tao, L., 349 Thompson, A., 170, 173, 598 Troutman, A. C., 903, 907,
Tarabulsy, G. M., 690 190 Tomchek, S., 847 908, 913
Taras, M., 915, 938, 949 Thompson, E., 686, 687, Tomkins, S., 543 Truhan, A. P., 543
Tardieu, M., 77, 138 688, 690, 691 Tomlins, M., 427 Trupin, E. W., 1110
Tardif, C., 519, 520 Thompson, K. J., 551 Tommasi, L., 692 Trygstad, O., 462, 539
Tarr, M., 696 Thompson, P. M., 1305 Tommerup, N., 130, 132, Tryon, A. S., 886
Tassone, F., 545 Thompson, R. A., 322 137, 138 Tsai, L., 18, 24, 27, 28, 30,
Taubman, M., 886 Thompson, R. J., 551, 552 Tompkins, L. M., 616 33, 50, 91, 93, 101,
Tayama, M., 172, 461, 499, Thompson, S. J., 237, 253, Tonge, B., 104, 265, 266, 102, 120, 187, 210,
539 384, 393, 395 542, 548, 610, 836, 248, 266, 299, 344,
Taylor, A., 181, 342, 545, Thompson, T., 461, 462 1061, 1065, 1197, 373, 427, 434, 494,
569, 844 Thomsen, P., 101, 590 1198, 1199, 1201 499, 536, 539, 544,
Taylor, B., 48, 54, 60, 253, Thomson, J., 619 Toombs, K., 871, 876, 886 547, 548, 593, 594,
257, 435, 541, 868, Thomson, M., 438, 541 Toomey, K. E., 185 734, 790
870, 871, 872, 1044, Thorin, E., 1092 Topaloglu, H., 130 Tsai, M. C., 30
1046, 1064, 1074 Thorndike, E. L., 1006 Topcu, M., 130, 153 Tsatsanis, K., 367, 372,
Taylor, C., 594, 755, 756, Thorne, G. L., 1126 Tordjman, S., 459, 460, 374, 498, 520, 586,
870 Thorp, D. M., 394, 884, 461, 462, 1208 665, 670, 789, 985,
Taylor, E., 79, 593, 615 1009, 1011 Torrente, F., 438, 541 995
Taylor, G., 263 Thorpe, J., 548, 900 Torrisi, L., 551 Tsiouri, I., 947
Taylor, H. G., 295 Thorsen, P., 44, 49, 52, 57, Toruner, G. A., 130 Tsuang, M. T., 183
Taylor, J., 901, 903 435 Toth, K., 5, 31, 171, 233, Tsuruhara, T., 461
Taylor, M., 476, 483, 666 Thorson, N., 264 234, 375, 388, 612, Tu, J. B., 455
Taylor, N. D., 253, 392 Thulborn, K. R., 373, 485, 613, 656, 657, 662, Tuchman, R., 65, 76, 79,
Taylor, T., 669, 670 496, 525 663, 665, 668, 670 80, 82, 202, 204, 205,
Author Index I • 35

206, 212, 213, 214, Ungerleider, L., 520, 526, Vanhala, R., 432, 433, 481, Vidal, C. N., 494
224, 288, 289, 290, 696 482 Vieland, V. J., 1307
291, 292, 293, 295, Unis, A., 542, 846, 1112 van Hooff, J., 249, 318, 319 Vigorito, J., 689
296, 298, 299, 502, Urabe, F., 540 Van Houten, R., 257, 821, Viken, A., 129
503, 534, 538, 555, Usher, S., 908 902, 907, 908, 909 Vila, M. R., 550
657, 721, 724, 750, Usui, S., 28 van Ijzendoorn, M. H., 322 Vilensky, J. A., 488, 839,
832, 1305, 1307, 1311, Uther, M., 481 Van Kannen, D. P., 268 840
1312 Uyanik, M., 153 Van Krevelen, D., 90, 91, Villalobos, R., 503
Tucker, D., 247, 275, 651, Uzgiris, I. C., 83, 148, 383, 93, 94, 105, 1233 Villareal, L., 1231, 1232
655, 669, 670 386, 792 Van Lancker, D., 344, 478, Vincent, J., 429
Tucker, R. M., 1308 481 Vinet, M. C., 130, 138
Tudmay, A. J., 139 Vadasy, P. F., 1074 van Lange, N. D. J., 454 Vinters, H. V., 140
Tuff, L., 104, 174, 183, Vail, C., 1003 Van Miller, S., 542 Viozzi, L., 539
372, 373, 375, 437, Vaisanen, M. L., 541 Van Oost, P., 236, 237, Visconti, P., 502
606, 607, 628 Valdez-Menchaca, M., 1049 389, 390 Visscher, P. M., 434
Tulving, E., 370, 371, 669, Valdovinos, M. G., 821 van Ree, J. M., 460 Vitale, L. J., 90
671 Valle, M., 541 van Slegtenhorst, M., 543 Vitiello, B., 762
Tumuluru, R., 1110 Vallortigara, G., 692 van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Vogel, F., 434
Tunali, B., 257, 258, 259, Van Acker, R., 134, 148, 372, 610, 613, 644 Vogindroukas, I., 1216,
260, 264, 352, 353, 149, 151, 152, 153 Van Walleghem, M., 546 1217
1064 van Amelsvoort, T., 107, Varela, F., 686, 687, 688, Volden, J., 344, 354, 962
Tunali-Kotoski, B., 258, 419, 493, 521, 526, 690, 691 Volkmar, F. R., 5, 7, 8, 9,
261, 324, 354, 385, 652 Varella-Garcia, M., 551 10, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17,
412 van Bentem, P.-P. G., 455 Vargas, F., 553 18, 19, 20, 21, 23, 24,
Turanli, G., 130 van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. Vargha-Khadem, F., 432 25, 26, 27, 28, 30, 31,
Turbott, S. H., 1110 A., 322 Varilo, T., 432, 433 32, 33, 41, 45, 50, 51,
Turic, D., 431 van Bokhoven, H., 78 Varley, C. K., 1110 53, 71, 72, 73, 74, 75,
Turk, J., 544, 546 Van Bourgondien, M., 124, Varma, R., 493 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 88,
Turkeltaub, P. E., 518 292, 297, 301, 302, Vasa, S. F., 794 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96,
Turnbull, A. P., 897, 1078, 721, 749, 838, 1075, Vaughan, A., 663, 669, 671, 97, 99, 100, 101, 102,
1092, 1093, 1129 1076, 1256 672 103, 104, 105, 106,
Turnbull, H. R., 1078, Vance, A., 617 Vaughan, H., 479, 482 107, 108, 109, 110,
1092, 1124 Vance, H. B., 267 Vaughan, H., Jr., 479, 480, 111, 113, 124, 169,
Turner, D., 476 van Daalen, E., 723 482 170, 173, 174, 175,
Turner, L. M., 712, 718 van Daalen, H., 657, 662 Vayego-Lourenco, S. A., 177, 178, 179, 180,
Turner, M., 265, 608, 609, Van den Ban, E., 30, 183 551 181, 182, 184, 186,
616 Van den Berghe, G., 464 Vedora, J., 871 187, 189, 191, 200,
Turnpenny, P., 555, 556 Van den Berghe, H., 546 Veenstra-VanderWeele, J., 209, 210, 211, 213,
Turton, L., 1006 van den Bosch, R. J., 607 429, 439, 515 223, 224, 225, 227,
Tuten, H., 152 Van den Pol, R. A., 1143 Vega-Lahar, N., 690 232, 233, 234, 236,
Twachtman-Cullen, D., Van den Veyver, I. B., 21, Velting, O. N., 181, 182 238, 239, 248, 249,
293 77, 78, 130, 136, 137, Veltman, M. W., 551, 552 250, 251, 252, 263,
Twardosz, S., 968, 1030 138, 431, 432 Venezia, M., 653, 654, 662 268, 269, 275, 293,
Tyano, S., 1220, 1252 van der Does, A. W., 607 Venter, A., 186, 202, 203, 294, 296, 303, 312,
Tye, N. C., 1105 Van der Gaag, C., 521, 522, 204, 206, 215, 216, 313, 314, 315, 316,
Tyrano, S., 459, 462 523, 524, 526 289, 292, 293, 294, 317, 318, 319, 322,
Tzyy-Ping, J., 663, 671 Van der Gaag, J., 269 295, 297, 298, 335, 323, 324, 325, 326,
Van der Gaag, R., 5, 30, 733, 741, 749, 978, 343, 345, 348, 350,
Uchiyama, T., 435 166, 169, 171, 182, 1076, 1077 352, 353, 354, 365,
Uddenfeldt, U., 109 183, 187, 190, 412, Ventner, A., 354 367, 368, 373, 374,
Udenfriend, S., 455 482, 1111 Ventura, L. O., 553 375, 376, 387, 388,
Udwin, O., 549, 594 Van der Geest, J. N., 171, Verbalis, A., 518 412, 419, 428, 429,
Uema, T., 667 485, 684 Verbaten, M., 171, 183, 455, 456, 458, 459,
Uhlmann, V., 541 Van der Heide, L., 519, 642 478, 480, 481, 482, 485, 486, 491, 492,
Uller, C., 698 van der Hoeven, J., 269, 485, 684 498, 519, 520, 521,
Ullstadius, E., 384, 385, 1111 Verdecchia, M., 543 522, 523, 524, 525,
386 Van der Lely, H., 258, 352 Verhoef, S., 543 526, 534, 537, 538,
Ulvund, S., 369 van der Linden, C., 1103 Verkerk, A. J., 545 546, 547, 556, 561,
Umansky, R., 150, 151, 153 van der Linden, J. A., 183 Verma, N. P., 144 580, 587, 592, 593,
Underwood, L. A., 905 Van der Wees, M., 169, Vernon, S., 1032 595, 628, 650, 651,
Ungaro, P., 551 171, 412 Verrier, A., 61 652, 683, 684, 689,
Unger, D. D., 1088 Van de Water, J., 542, 1312 Verstraeten, T., 436 691, 694, 696, 697,
Unger, D. G., 264 van Duijn, C. M., 556 Verté, S., 373 699, 707, 719, 720,
Ungerer, J., 7, 185, 233, Vandvik, I. H., 1239 Vesala, H., 296, 297 721, 730, 732, 734,
234, 236, 237, 238, van Engeland, H., 30, 171, Vest, C., 498 735, 736, 739, 740,
251, 253, 254, 318, 177, 183, 189, 249, Vest, L., 336 747, 748, 749, 750,
320, 321, 322, 350, 318, 319, 460, 478, Vestal, C., 290, 295, 297, 753, 754, 761, 772,
369, 382, 385, 386, 480, 481, 482, 485, 1055, 1059, 1075, 1077 773, 777, 779, 782,
392, 394, 397, 398, 491, 493, 499, 657, Vestergaard, M., 44, 49, 52, 783, 784, 789, 793,
416, 653, 656, 665, 662, 684, 723 57, 435 794, 800, 803, 809,
667, 669, 707, 738, van Geert, P. L., 171, 186, Vickers, S., 551 810, 813, 820, 821,
740, 836 187 Vida, S., 548 822, 832, 835, 839,
I • 36 Author Index

Volkmar, F. (Continued) Walker, K., 1006 Watson, J. S., 150, 153 Weissman, D., 668, 669
889, 906, 908, 912, Walker, M., 479 Watson, L., 318, 324, 409, Weisz, C. L., 149
946, 960, 961, 962, Walker, P., 454, 915 520, 946 Weitzman, E., 958, 959
977, 979, 980, 982, Walker, R. D., 484 Watson, L. R., 833, 843 Weizman, A., 459, 462,
983, 984, 998, 1014, Walker, V. G., 236 Watson, L. S., 1126 1220, 1252
1055, 1059, 1060, Walker-Andrews, A. S., Watson, M., 546 Weizman, R., 459, 462,
1102, 1106, 1107, 261 Watson, P., 130, 139 1220, 1252
1109, 1111, 1124, Walker-Smith, J., 540, 541 Watson, R., 151 Welch, J. P., 501
1134, 1139, 1221, Wall, S., 238, 322, 416, Watts, E., 1021, 1031 Welham, M., 93, 348, 593
1224, 1225, 1281, 1223 Waugh, M. C., 554 Wellington, P., 833, 1011
1282, 1306 Wallace, B., 1093 Weatherly, J. J., 1003 Wellman, H., 630, 667
Volpe, A. G., 539 Wallace, C., 651, 657, 658, Weaver, A., 534 Wells, K. C., 1062
Volsen, S. G., 463, 490 660, 661, 670 Weaver, B., 609 Welsh, M. C., 616
Volterra, V., 149, 630 Waller, D., 459, 462, 1112 Weaver, L., 542 Wenar, C., 744, 750
von Cramon, D. Y., 668, Walter, A., 269 Webb, D. W., 543 Wendelboe, M., 539
669 Walter, D. O., 476 Webb, E., 44, 48, 59, 62 Wenger, S., 546
von Knorring, A.-L., 204, Walter, J., 440 Webb, P. J., 154 Wenk, G., 134, 142, 146
213, 288 Walter, R., 484 Webb, S., 165, 171, 191, Wentz, E., 113, 553, 586,
von Tetzchner, S., 129, Walter, S., 8, 172, 173, 235, 483, 651, 690, 590, 743, 760
147, 149, 155 174, 731, 734, 753 755 Wermuth, T. R., 1092
von Thaden, K., 620 Waltrip, C., 167, 169, 757 Webb, T., 137, 551 Werner, E., 225, 398, 689,
Vostanis, P., 266, 268, 408, Wan, M., 21, 77, 78, 130, Weber, A. M., 430 708
588, 1056, 1057, 1216, 136, 431 Weber, D., 1212, 1213 Werner, H., 320
1217 Wang, A. T., 170, 173 Weber, R. C., 900 Wernicke, C., 1232
Vourc’h, P., 432 Wang, J. J., 252, 316 Webster, D. R., 464 Werry, J., 82, 180, 191, 915
Vrancic, D., 494, 499, 519, Wang, J.-J., 294, 793 Webster, J., 254 Wert, B. Y., 871
740 Wang, L., 551 Webster-Stratton, C., 1060, Wertheimer, A., 1064,
Vroomen, J., 519, 642 Wang, N. J., 551 1061, 1062 1065, 1075
Vuilleumier, P., 523, 525 Wang, P. P., 549, 552 Wechsler, D., 545, 731, 788 Wesecky, A., 150, 153
Vukicevic, J., 21, 314, 335, Wang, Q., 543 Wecker, L., 539 Wessels, M., 543
708 Wang, T. A., 521, 526 Weed, K. A., 1004 West, B. J., 901
Vygotsky, L., 982 Wang, T. R., 551 Weeks, D. E., 430, 431 West, D., 914
Wannamaker, S. W., 870 Weeks, L., 127 West, M., 619, 653, 1094
Waage-Baudet, H., 454 Wanska, S., 340 Weeks, S., 261, 414, 520, Westby, C., 813
Wacher, D. P., 904 Want, S. C., 383 689 Westerfield, M., 483, 493,
Wachtel, R., 541, 800, Wantanabe, M., 670 Wehby, J., 968, 1030 663, 671
1112 Ward, M., 1013, 1022, Wehman, P., 619, 1088, Westlake, J. R., 484
Wacker, D., 819, 823, 912, 1078, 1091 1089, 1090, 1091, Wetherby, A., 235, 236,
916, 926, 939 Ward-O’Brien, D., 1107, 1092, 1093, 1094 319, 351, 352, 653,
Wadden, N., 719, 720, 750 1108 Wehmeyer, M., 151, 868 691, 707, 713, 758,
Wagner, A. E., 613 Ware, J., 723 Wehner, B., 237 759, 773, 777, 778,
Wagner, E. A., 467 Waring, R. H., 463 Wehner, E., 229, 234, 321, 799, 800, 890, 926,
Wagner, H. N., 142 Waring, S. C., 144 342, 366, 367, 372, 927, 928, 929, 930,
Wagner, S., 548, 867, 1130, Warren, J., 299, 651, 652 384, 385, 386, 387, 932, 933, 935, 936,
1149 Warren, R., 539, 542 388, 547, 612, 613, 937, 938, 939, 940,
Wagstaff, J., 551 Warren, S., 883, 884, 888, 614, 663, 716, 723, 941, 942, 946, 947,
Wahlberg, T., 261, 354 889, 930, 951 833, 834, 837, 843, 961, 978, 979, 980,
Wahlstrom, J., 429, 546, Warren, W., 542 847 981, 982, 983, 985,
548, 551 Warreyn, P., 671 Wei, T., 496, 497, 525, 667 986, 989, 990, 995,
Wainwright, J., 832 Warrington, E. K., 369, Weidenbaum, M., 1093 996, 997, 1007
Wainwright, L., 1307 371, 488 Weider, S., 957, 966, 967, Wetherley, A., 1007, 1008,
Wainwright, M. S., 539 Wassink, T. H., 188, 1307 1061 1009
Wainwright-Sharp, J. A., Watanabe, H., 542 Weidmer-Mikhail, E., 100, Wetzel, R. J., 1126
268, 368, 610, 617, Watanabe, M., 46, 52, 227, 209, 295, 544, 551 Wexler, B. E., 619
1014 455, 456, 460, 461, Weimer, A. K., 100, 367, Whalen, C., 236, 253, 867,
Waiter, G. D., 521, 523 834, 1224, 1249 835, 836 884, 886, 889
Waits, M. M., 539 Watanbe, Y., 458 Weinberger, D. R., 526 Wharton, P., 551
Wakabayashi, S., 251, 252, Watemberg, N., 550 Weiner, L. B., 544 Wheeler, M. A., 371
289, 317, 547, 587 Waterhouse, L., 16, 29, 30, Weinle, C., 542, 1112 Wheelwright, S., 48, 52,
Wakefield, A., 435, 438, 169, 238, 323, 325, Weintraub, A., 542, 1112 54, 57, 60, 88, 105,
541 344, 367, 410, 462, Weintraub, S., 98 190, 191, 227, 229,
Wakefield, J., 541 519, 520, 592, 617, Weir, F., 353 231, 373, 419, 434,
Wakstein, D., 348, 813 833, 1306 Weir, K., 16, 80, 82 439, 461, 525, 526,
Wakstein, M., 348, 813 Waters, E., 238, 322, 416, Weir, R., 346 615, 628, 629, 631,
Waldenström, E., 588 1223 Weisberg, J., 524 632, 633, 634, 645,
Waldo, M. C., 744 Watkins, J. M., 181, 183 Weisner, L. A., 556 652, 666, 709, 713,
Walker, A. S., 630, 689 Watkins, N., 1032 Weiss, E., 1108 714, 715, 716, 847,
Walker, D., 170, 173, 190, Watkiss, E., 551 Weiss, M. J., 872, 873, 874, 1307
264 Watling, R., 366, 536, 834, 1030 Whisnant, J. P., 51
Walker, H. A., 501 843, 846 Weissbach, H., 455 Whitaker, P., 991, 1061
Walker, H. M., 1126 Watson, D., 477 Weissman, B. A., 459, 462, Whitaker-Azmitia, P. M.,
Walker, J., 236 Watson, J. B., 1006 1252 454
Author Index I • 37

White, G. B., 1281 Williams, D. F., 438 749, 755, 756, 833, Woods, C. G., 551
White, L. M., 551 Williams, D. J., 619 838, 839, 840, 1014, Woods, J., 759
White, M. S., 1281 Williams, E., 253, 395, 1056, 1066, 1069, Woods, R. P., 389
White, O., 366, 536, 826, 833, 849 1175, 1182, 1189, Woodyatt, G. C., 148,
834, 843 Williams, G., 556 1197, 1216, 1243, 149
White, R. L., 1032 Williams, J., 227, 443 1244, 1245, 1246, Woolsey, J. E., 904
Whitehouse, D., 263 Williams, J. B. W., 17 1305, 1306 Worcester, J., 865, 866,
Whitehouse, P. J., 142 Williams, J. C., 548 Winking, D. L., 1093 869
Whitehouse, W., 48, 52, 54, Williams, J. H., 266, 389, Winklosky, B., 32, 731, Wozniak, J., 208
61, 63 521, 523, 536 733, 754, 755 Wray, J., 542
Whiteley, P., 541 Williams, J. M., 408 Winner, M., 992, 996 Wright, D. M., 1111
Whiten, A., 266, 389, 521, Williams, K., 80, 143, 540, Winnergard, I., 552, 553 Wright, E. C., 589
523, 536, 630, 660 1014, 1015 Winocur, G., 669, 671 Wright, H. H., 208, 269,
Whitley, F. P., 1143 Williams, M., 181, 800, Winograd, T., 687 754
Whitlock, B., 434, 534, 539 980 Winsberg, B. G., 456, 457, Wrozek, M., 427
Whitman, B., 537 Williams, N., 431 463 Wszalek, T., 496
Whitman, T. L., 1126, 1128 Williams, P. D., 1074 Winterling, V., 823, 865, Wu, J. C., 367, 617
Whitmont, S., 619 Williams, P. G., 241, 267, 869, 900 Wulf, G., 1014
Whitney, R., 905 544, 556 Wise, S., 690 Wyatt, R. J., 467
Whitney, T., 1020 Williams, R. E., 901 Wishart, J., 548 Wynn, J., 239, 949, 1048
Whittaker, C. A., 952 Williams, R. S., 553, 1305 Wisniewski, L., 551 Wzorek, M., 170, 195, 197,
Whittemore, V., 543 Williams, S., 17, 419, 521, Wittchen, H. U., 181, 182 295, 427, 428, 439,
Whittington, C. J., 1283 526, 652, 668 Wittenberger, M. D., 1110 489, 500, 501, 536,
Whittington, J., 552 Williams, S. C., 521, 526 Witt-Engerström , I., 76, 537, 538, 539, 808,
Wichart, I., 142 Williams, S. C. R., 525 127, 128, 129, 132, 809
Wickstrom, S., 394, 967, Williams, T., 1057, 1077 139, 142, 148, 431
969, 990 Williams, W. A., 454 Wiznitzer, M., 539, 1307 Xagoraris, A., 525
Widawski, M. H., 1108 Williamson, C. G., 939 Wohlfahrt, J., 44, 49, 52, Xiang, F., 130, 138
Widiger, T. A., 11 Williamson, G., 367, 980, 57, 435
Wiebe, M. J., 730, 749 981, 983 Wohlgemuth, D., 73 Yachnis, A., 502
Wieder, S., 832, 889, 890, Williamson, P. C., 494 Wohlleben, B., 46, 184 Yairi, E., 733
929, 932, 936, 939, Willis, B. S., 1126, 1127, Wojciulik, E., 520 Yale, M., 664, 665, 667,
940, 941, 1003, 1140, 1128 Wolchik, S. A., 903 670
1310 Willis, K. D., 152, 155 Wolery, M., 865, 868, 869 Yales, A., 459
Wiegers, A. M., 545 Willis, T. J., 903 Wolf, C., 544 Yamada, S., 140
Wiersema, V., 462, 553 Willoughby, J., 655, 673 Wolf, E. G., 744, 750 Yamamoto, J., 261, 262
Wieser, M., 142 Wilsher, C. P., 1110 Wolf, J., 323 Yamamoto, T., 455, 459,
Wiesner, L., 31, 224, 232 Wilson, B., 619, 867 Wolf, L., 296, 1068, 1074 460, 543
Wigal, T., 1109 Wilson, F., 93, 95, 100, Wolf, M., 821, 822, 825, Yamashita, F., 46, 540
Wiggs, L., 542 101, 110, 213, 252, 948, 967, 1006, 1125, Yamashita, I., 460
Wignyosumarto, S., 47 269, 317, 734, 735, 1126 Yamashita, T., 539
Wigram, T., 127 736, 743, 845, 1014 Wolf berg, P., 967, 968, Yamashita, Y., 140, 540,
Wiig, E., 807 Wilson, J., 79, 129, 139, 980, 990, 991 544
Wijsenbeek, H., 1220 464 Wolfe, B. E., 1181 Yamazaki, K., 460, 461,
Wilcken, B., 143 Wilson, R., 431, 545 Wolfensberger, W., 1246 463, 1223, 1226
Wilcox, B. L., 1124 Wilson, S., 82 Wolff, S., 33, 97, 101, 169, Yan, S., 455
Wilcoxen, A., 800 Wimmer, H., 105, 630 178, 179, 184, 187, Yang, X.-L., 1203
Wilder, D. A., 267, 870 Wimpory, D. C., 227, 408 210, 212, 586, 587, 596 Yankee, J., 833, 834, 849
Wilens, T., 1109 Win, L., 523 Wolffe, A. P., 440 Yano, S., 140
Wilgosh, L., 290, 1075, Winborn, L. C., 904 Wolford, L., 552 Yanossky, R., 499
1093 Windsor, J., 293, 804 Wollersheim, J. P., 915 Yardin, C., 551
Wilkerson, D. S., 539 Wing, L., 7, 16, 24, 28, 30, Wolpert, C. M., 32, 429, Yashima, Y., 46, 52, 227,
Wilkins, A. J., 503 33, 43, 46, 49, 50, 52, 432, 433, 434, 439, 456, 461, 463, 542,
Will, M., 1089 53, 62, 64, 88, 91, 92, 544, 551, 753, 754 834, 1224, 1225
Willems, H., 758 93, 94, 101, 102, 103, Wolraich, M. L., 1109 Yashinaga, K., 202, 204,
Willemsen, R., 545 106, 108, 109, 113, Wong, C. Y., 455 206, 212, 213, 214
Willemsen-Swinkels, 166, 167, 172, 178, Wong, D., 142, 428 Yates, A., 1111
S. H. N., 189, 723 184, 187, 208, 209, Wong, S. E., 904 Yates, K., 371, 497, 660
Willemsen-Swinkels, 210, 211, 214, 248, Wong, S. N., 476, 477 Yaylali, I., 503
S. W., 183 249, 250, 265, 290, Wong, V., 476, 477 Yeargin-Allsopp, M., 49,
Willen, J., 318, 630, 663 295, 297, 312, 313, Wood, F. B., 438 52, 57, 60, 63, 185,
Willensen-Swinkel, S., 657, 317, 321, 323, 324, Wood, K., 832, 833, 834, 555, 1070, 1123
662 344, 345, 348, 349, 843 Yeates, S., 46, 52, 321, 392,
Willey, L. H., 295 392, 396, 408, 534, Wood, R., 619, 1091 396, 587, 598
Williams, A. R., 1074 535, 547, 548, 554, Woodcock, M., 137 Yeoh, H. C., 455
Williams, B. J., 544, 722, 580, 583, 584, 585, Woodcock, T., 137 Yeung-Courchesne, G., 655,
752 586, 587, 588, 589, Woodhouse, J. M., 842 656, 659, 668
Williams, C., 462, 551 592, 593, 594, 595, Woodhouse, W., 439, 536 Yeung-Courchesne, R., 141,
Williams, D., 201, 832, 596, 597, 598, 599, Woodin, M., 552 268, 367, 368, 371,
1014, 1017, 1022, 600, 628, 629, 631, Woodroffe, A., 544 479, 480, 482, 499,
1284 653, 724, 734, 735, Woods, A., 124, 207, 298, 610, 617, 832, 889,
Williams, D. E., 909, 910 736, 741, 743, 745, 302 1282, 1305
I • 38 Author Index

Yilmaz, I., 153 Yoshinaga, K., 288, 289, Zahn, T., 181, 266, 268 Zhou, E. C., 454
Yin, X., 478 291, 293, 296, 298 Zajdemann, H., 335 Ziatas, K., 105, 352
Yirmiya, N., 171, 260, 261, Young, A. H., 189 Zajonc, R., 695 Ziccardi, R., 489, 492, 493,
318, 320, 324, 325, Young, A. W., 526 Zaki, M., 130 499, 501, 519, 661
409, 410, 411, 415, Young, D., 552, 1142 Zakian, A., 751 Zidel, S., 494, 536, 538
416, 433, 520, 630, Young, G. C., 97 Zalenski, S., 1029 Ziegler, A., 80, 82
631, 652, 653, 655, Young, H., 1004, 1005, 1006 Zalkin, H., 464 Ziegler, D. A., 489, 493,
671, 707, 724, 734, Young, J. G., 50, 453, 455, Zambito, J., 907 494, 496, 497, 498,
741, 748, 749, 750, 458, 459, 460, 463, Zambrino, C. A., 539 517, 519, 1305
751, 753, 755, 946, 539, 553 Zametkin, A., 181, 524, Ziegler, M. G., 459
1058, 1068 Young, J. I., 431, 432 525 Zigler, E., 206, 739, 1120
Ylisaukko-Oja, T., 432, Young, N. B., 476 Zametkin, J., 667 Zilber, N., 459
433 Young, S. N., 456, 457, 459 Zamichek, W., 538 Zilbovicius, M., 373, 524
Yntema, H. G., 78 Youngman, P., 619 Zanolli, K., 889 Zimbroff, D. L., 1106
Yoder, P., 235, 236, 386, Youpa, D., 617 Zappella, M., 131, 142, Zimerman, B., 181
664, 665, 667, 670, Yovel, G., 520 143, 149, 150, 153, Zimmerman, A. W., 1111
708, 734, 799, 801, Yu, C.-E., 429 544 Zimmerman, E. G., 27, 209
884, 888, 889, 947, Yudovin, S., 354 Zappert, J., 71 Zimmerman, I., 800, 804
948, 949, 950, 955, Yule, E., 1244 Zarcone, J. R., 821, 822, Zimmerman, M. C., 476
957, 1007, 1011 Yule, W., 346, 549, 594, 901, 907 Zimnitzky, B., 1108
Yokochi, K., 127 1244 Zarcone, T. J., 821 Zingoni, A. M., 539
Yokota, A., 234, 251, 292, Yusal, S., 739 Zarifian, E., 459 Zoghbi, H., 21, 77, 78, 127,
294, 316, 741, 745, 762 Yuva-Paylor, L. A., 431, Zee, D., 484, 485 130, 132, 134, 136,
Yokota, K., 502, 503, 1225 432 Zeilberger, J., 1125 137, 138, 139, 141,
Yokoyama, F., 456, 459, Yuwiler, A., 453, 455, 458, Zekan, L., 459 142, 144, 145, 152,
460, 1249 539 Zelazo, P., 325 431, 432, 1312
Yonan, A. L., 432, 433, 515 Yuzda, E., 108, 170, 500, Zelazo, P. D., 615 Zohar, A. H., 552
Yonan, J., 515 534, 536, 632 Zelazo, P. R., 1068 Zollinger, B., 1221
Yonclas, D. G., 235, 236, Yuzda, F. Y., 425, 426, 434 Zelnik, N., 542 Zoltak, B., 539
707 Zemke, R., 849 Zorn, S. H., 1106
Yorbik, O., 667 Zaccagnini, J., 536 Zercher, C., 254 Zubieta, J. K., 189
Yoshen, Y., 978 Zaccagnini, M., 539 Zetlin, A., 207 Zuddas, A., 1103
Yoshikawa, H., 140 Zackai, E., 551, 552 Zeuthen, E., 544 Al-Zuhair, A. G. H., 130
Yoshimoto, T., 172, 499 Zaeem, L., 432 Zhang, H., 440, 543 Zvi, J. C., 30
Yoshimura, I., 251, 252, Zager, D. B., 897 Zhang, L., 545 Zwaigenbaum, L., 77, 78,
289, 317 Zaharia, E. S., 1141 Zhang, X., 130 170, 173, 187, 190,
Yoshimura, K., 46, 540 Zahl, A., 261 Zhang, Z., 130, 138 427, 437
Subject Index

AAPEP. See Adolescent and Adult Psychoeducational outcome studies, 288–300


Profile (AAPEP) sexuality, 304
ABA. See Applied behavioral analysis (ABA) work, 304
ABC. See Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) Advocacy, 1063, 1064, 1078, 1308–1309
Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), 762 Aerobic exercise, antecedent, 900
Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS), 848 Affect, interpersonal coordination of, 409–410. See also
Academic skills, functional (NRC instructional Emotion(s)
priorities), 928 Affective development, 324–325
Academic supports, 1018–1020. See also School(s)/ Affective disorder, 428
school-based programs Affective symptoms, 27–28
Acculturation theory, 398 Ages and Stages Questionnaire, Second Edition (ASQ),
Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment, 712
748 Akathisia Ratings of Movement Scale (ARMS), 848
Achievement: Allergies, 539
academic, 294–295 (see also School(s)/school-based Aloof group, 249, 595–597
programs) Alpine Learning Group, 1044–1045
clinical assessment, 786 Amalgamatic nucleus, 1229
Acting-Out scale, 747 Ambulation (Rett syndrome), 153
Action level imitation, 383 Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), 1091
Action on objects, 384 Aminoacidophathies, 76. See also Phenylketonuria (PKU)
Active-but-odd group, 250, 586, 598–599 Amino acids and acetylcholine, 462–463
Adaptive Behavior Scale-School Version, American Amygdala, 497, 525–526
Association of Mental Deficiency (AAMD), 751 Anatomic studies, 1305. See also Neuroimaging studies
Adaptive functioning/ behavior, 114, 238, 251, 294–295, Angelman / Prader-Willi syndromes, 76, 135, 551–552
792–793, 1178 Animal models and studies of portmortem brain tissues,
Addison-Schilder disease, 76 440–441
ADHD. See Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Anorexia nervosa, 590
(ADHD) Antecedent interventions, 899–902
ADI-R. See Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) Antipsychotics, atypical, 1102–1106
Administrative organization (TEACCH), 1176 Anxiety, 100–101, 178, 208, 269, 428, 1013–1015,
Adolescent and Adult Psychoeducational Profile 1282–1283
(AAPEP), 743, 758 Appearance, physical, 1057–1058
Adolescents with autism, 288–305 Applied behavioral analysis (ABA), 949, 959, 964, 1005,
and average intelligence, 303 1006–1007, 1046, 1125–1126
clinical issues, 300–304 Apprenticeships, 1147
emotion, 411–414 Apraxia, 150–151, 387, 951
and mental retardation, 300–303 Argentina. See Latin America
outcome studies, 288–300 Aripiprazole, 1106
peer relationships, 303 Aristaless-related homeobox gene (ARX syndrome), 544
school, 303, 1013–1023 (see also School(s)/school-based ASIEP. See Autism Screening Instrument for Educational
programs) Planning (ASIEP-2)
ADOS. See Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Asperger syndrome, 88–118
(ADOS) alternative diagnostic concepts, 97–98
Adulthood, transition to, 875–876, 1022–1023 autism versus, 92
Adults with autism, 288–305 cachet of diagnosis, 1308
and average intelligence, 303–304 classification /diagnosis, 89–90, 91–95, 165, 166, 167,
college, 303–304 169
consultants on care for, 1137 criteria, 735–736, 1306
deterioration in, 213–214, 289–290 historical perspective, classification, 6, 8, 24–25,
jobs/work (see Employment) 90–96, 169
and mental retardation, 300–303 clinical assessment, 111–113
neuro-psychiatric conditions, 589–590 clinical features, 94, 98–102

I • 39
I • 40 Subject Index

Asperger syndrome (Continued) Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R):


differential diagnosis, 81, 173–174 defined/described, 753–755
epidemiology, 53, 54, 102 overview table, 743
future research directions, 109–111 usage/comparison, 176, 214, 228, 232, 289, 293, 315,
genetics, 108–109 326, 342, 712, 722, 734, 740, 743, 751, 760, 762,
ICD10 research diagnostic guidelines for, 93 780
instruments, 760–762 Autism Diagnostic and Observation Scale, 820
language, 335, 1306 Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), 326,
medical and neurobiological studies, 106–108 739, 740–741, 743, 753, 756–757, 760, 762, 780
natural history, 186–187 ADOS-G (Generic), 176, 214, 228, 551, 712, 722
neuropsychological studies, 102–104 PL-ADOS (Pre-Linguistic), 756
original clinical concept (Asperger’s), 89–90, 96–97 “Autism in schizophrenia” (1916 term), 89
outcomes, 109–110, 213 Autism Screening Instrument for Educational Planning
psychiatric disorders/conditions and, 28, 208, 209–210, (ASIEP-2), 1006
269 Autism Screening Questionnaire/Social Communication
social-cognitive studies, 104–106 Questionnaire (ASQ/SCQ), 714–715. See also Social
social skills interventions/curricula, 872 Communication Questionnaire (SCQ)
terminology (versus “autistic psychopathy”), 584 Autism Services Center (ASC), Huntington, West Virginia,
treatment and interventions, 113–116 1255–1264
undiagnosed, 1245 Autism specialists, preparation of. See Training
validity, 102–111, 872 (preparation of autism specialists)
versus HFA ( high-functioning autism), 91–92, 95–96, Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), 88, 535, 586
103–104, 628– 629 Autistic continuum, 586, 1233
Asperger Syndrome Diagnostic Interview (ASDI ), 113, Autistic disorder, 25, 535
743, 760–761 Autistic psychopathy, 584
Assessment, 705–706. See also Clinical evaluation in Aversive/nonaversive interventions, 915–916
autism spectrum disorders Avoidant personality disorder, 178, 179
Assessment of Basic Language and Learning Skills
(ABLLS), 929 Babbling, 91, 349
Attachment, 238, 322–323 Backhanded bullying, 1022
Attention, 28, 234–235, 367–369, 610, 1307. See also Joint BAEPs. See Brainstem auditory evoked potentials (BAEPs)
attention Barnes Akathisia Scale, 848
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), 265, Baron-Cohen’s “eyes task,” 643
614, 616– 617, 619, 629, 644, 1016 Basal ganglia, 497–498
differential diagnosis (PDD-NOS), 177 Bayley Scales of Infants Development-II, 791
executive function, 373 Beery Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor
stimulants, 1109–1111 Integration (VMI ), 794, 844
treatment studies, in developmentally disabled Behavior, development and, 221–222
populations, 1110–1111 Behavioral assessment (functional ecological approach),
Attention Process Training (APT) program, 619 817–827
Attributions, first-order/second-order, 105 characteristics, 817–821
Atypical antipsychotics, 1102–1106 domains, 821–825
Atypical autism (PDD-NOS), 6, 25 treatment plan development, 825–827
Atypical PDD, 16 Behavioral Assessment Scale of Oral Functions in Feeding,
Auditory brainstem response (ABR), 844 847
Auditory potentials: Behavioral competencies, and provider training, 1140,
brainstem evoked (exogenous), 476–478 1143–1146
cognitive, 479–482 hands-on training, 1143–1146
long latency, 478–479 operational definitions, 1144
middle latency, 478 Behavioral curricula, 874–875
Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) Behavioral Development Questionnaire, 820
strategies, 149, 801–803, 885, 941, 955–957 Behavioral difficulties:
Australia, 1197–1201 adolescents/adults, 290
Australian Scale for Asperger’s Syndrome (ASAS), 743, Asperger syndrome, 89–90
761 conduct disorder, 268
Austria and Germany, 1212–1215 deviance from normal, 14
Autism: public (stressors confronting families), 1058
classification /diagnosis (overview), 1–3 violent /criminal behavior, 101
diagnosis/classification, 1–3, 5–34 Behavioral interventions, 882–892, 897–917
differences in, 1283–1285 antecedent interventions, 899–902
differential diagnosis, 81, 94, 174–175 Asperger syndrome, 114–115
epidemic, 1308 augmentative and alternative communication strategies,
essence of, 419 885
history of concepts, 583–587 aversive and nonaversive, 915–916
versus mental retardation, 226 in the community, 916
past and future, 1304–1313 computer applications, 916–917
primary/secondary, 7–8 consequence-based interventions, 902–910
public policy perspectives, 1119–1121 differential efficacy of strategies, 914
subtyping, 28–31 extinction procedures, 907
theoretical perspectives, 579–581 functional communication training (FCT), 910–912
Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC), 714–715, 719–720, future directions, 887–892
723, 740–741, 742, 747, 750–751, 754, 762, 1204 interruption and redirection, 902–903
Subject Index I • 41

issues in (current and future), 914–917 Cardiff Acuity Test, 842


language and communication skills, 910–912 Cardiovascular, 459
maintenance and generalization, 887, 914–915 CARS. See Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)
naturalistic, 883–885 Catatonia, 589–590
versus nonbehavioral approaches, 890 CATCH 22 (chromosome 22q11 deletion syndrome), 552
noncontingent reinforcement, 907–908 Categorical definitions:
parents/families, 1061, 1062 of autism, approaches to, 14–25
pivotal response training, 883–885, 888, 954 (see also DSM-III, 15
Pivotal response training (PRT)) DSM-III-R, 16–17
punishment procedures, 908–910 DSM-IV and ICD-10, 17–18
reinforcement-based interventions, 903–907 DSM-IV field trial, 18–21
respondent conditioning procedures, 913–914 from ICD-9 to ICD-10, 17
self-management, 885–886, 913 problems with, 583– 600
skill acquisition, 910–913 Causality, models of, 655
social skills, 912–913 CBCL, 748
social validity, 916 Celiac antibodies, 539
stimulus change procedures, 902 Center-based programs, 1043–1047
strategies, 882–887 differences/similarities among programs, 1045–1047
structured, 883 specific programs, 1044, 1047
teacher variables, 888 strengths/ limitations, 1051–1052
video instruction, 886–887 terminology (mainstreaming, inclusion, integration),
Behavioral momentum, 901 1047
Behavioral studies, 1305–1307 Central coherence, 640– 646
Behavioral Summarized Evaluation-Revised (BSE-R) cognitive style, normal variation, 644– 645
(overview table), 742, 751 deficit accounts, 643– 644
Behavior Observation Scale (BOS), 745 evidence (at three levels of processing), 641– 643
Behavior Rating Instrument for Autistic and Atypical extended phenotype of autism, 645
Children (BRIAAC), 742, 744 savant skills, 644
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions social development, 326
(BRIEF), 794 theory, 635
Behavior Rating Scale (BRS), 791 weak central coherence (WCC) hypothesis, 669
Behavior Summarized Evaluation-Revised (BSE-R), 751 Central nervous system:
Biological studies/findings. See Genetics; Neuroimaging function, 474–488
studies structure, 488–500
Biological vulnerabilities, anxiety and, 269 Cerebellar hemispheres, 498–499
Blindness, congenital, 417–419, 553–555 Cerebellar vermis, 499
Block Design test, 635, 642, 643 Cerebellum, 498
Blood 5-HT, 454–455 Cerebral palsy, 50, 51
Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal, 517 Cerebrospinal f luid, 459
Body mapping, 387, 389 CHARGE association, 544
Body movements, imitation of, 384–385 Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT), 227, 229,
Brain. See also Central nervous system; Neuroimaging 713–716, 723, 741
studies: Checklist-based performance appraisals, 1152
animal models, portmortem studies, 440–441 Child-centered approaches, 959
extreme male brain (EMB) theory, 634 Child Development Inventories (CDIs), 712
systems, and initiating social behavior, perception of Childhood, disorders beginning in (conditions causing
social behavior, 645 diagnostic problems), 587–589
Type B ( balanced), 633– 634 Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS):
Type E (empathizing), 633– 634 defined/described, 720–721
Type S (systemizing), 633– 634 overview table, 714–715, 742
volume/weight, 488, 492–494 use of/comparisons, 543, 546, 553, 657, 667, 723,
Brain behavior: 734, 744, 745, 748–750, 758, 820, 1176, 1204,
correlates (play), 397 1227
joint attention and, 662– 665 validity, 714, 740–741, 742
theory of mind and, 665– 668 Childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), 70–84
Brainstem, 499 associated medical conditions, 76
Brainstem auditory evoked potentials (BAEPs), 476–478 autism and, 79–80
Brigance Screens, 712 behavioral and clinical features, 74–75
Broad descriptive orientation (enhancing communication case report, 83–84
abilities), 930 clinical features, 72–74
Broader autism phenotype. See Phenotype, broader autism course and prognosis, 75
Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency (BOTMP), definition, 71–72
794, 846, 847 development of diagnostic concept, 6, 17, 21–23, 25,
Bruxism, 902 70–72
Bullying, 1021–1022 differential diagnosis, 81–82, 175
Business community, developing active ties, 1097 epidemiology, 53–56, 75, 185
evaluation and management, 82–83
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery genetics, 77–79
(CANTAB), 609, 615, 618, 620 neurobiological findings and etiology, 75–77
Canada, 1201–1203 onset (age/characteristics), 72–74
Candidate genes ( leads/strategies), 433–434, 440 Rett syndrome and, 78, 81
“Can’t ” versus “ won’t ” dilemma, 1057 validity as diagnostic category, 79–81
I • 42 Subject Index

Childhood onset pervasive developmental disorder Communication abilities, enhancing, 925–942. See also
(COPDD), 15, 16, 71 Language interventions
Childhood psychosis, 1239–1240 approaches, 928–931
Childhood schizophrenia. See also Schizophrenia: behavioral problems, relation to, 926–928
differential diagnosis, 81–82 challenges, 927
terminology/classification, 9, 584 dimensions, critical, 931–941
Children of school age, with an autistic spectrum disorder, NRC guidelines and other tenets of practice, 927–928
247–276 priority of, in treatment, 925–926
developmental issues, 245–265 Communication assessment, 799–814
language and communication, 254–260 children with advanced language, 807–817
psychiatric and behavioral problems, 265–270 early linguistic communication, 804–807
Children’s Global Assessment Scale, 762 prelinguistic, 799–804
Children’s Psychiatric Rating Scale, 1107 Communication boards, 149
Children’s Social Behavior Questionnaire (CSBQ), 742, Communication Competence Checklist (CCC), 753
747, 748 Communication Intention Inventory, 801
Children’s Unit for Treatment and Evaluation (State Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scale (CSBS), 743,
University of New York at Binghamton), 1035, 1044, 758–759, 800–801
1047, 1130, 1131 Communicative Development Inventory, 800
China, 1203–1206 Community:
Chromosomal abnormalities and genetically determined behavioral interventions, 916
medical conditions, 429–430, 544. See also Genetics interpersonal supports for members of, 993–995
Cingulate cortex, 496 Community-integrated residential services for adults with
Citalopram, 1109 autism (working model, mother’s perspective),
Clancy Behavior Scale, 1204 1255–1264
Classification. See Diagnosis/classification components of a successful residential program,
Classroom structure. See Curriculum and classroom 1257–1264
structure historical perspective, 1256–1257
Clinical evaluation in autism spectrum disorders, 772–795 introduction, 1255–1256
comprehensive developmental approach, 773–776 Comorbidity:
diagnostic formulation and differential diagnosis, Asperger syndrome, 100–102
781–782 autism, 26
diagnostic work-up, 778–781 Competencies and coping (TEACCH principle),
medical workups, 782–783 1067–1068
psychological assessment, 777, 783–795 Component process analyses, 607– 611
speech, language, and communication, 777–778 Computational models, enactive mind (EM) framework
Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Milestone Scale, 800 versus, 686
Clinical neurology, 500–503 Computer applications, behavioral interventions, 916–917.
Clinical web sites, 305 See also Technology/computers
Clomipramine, 1106–1107 Conduct problems. See Behavioral difficulties
Clozapine, 1103 Congenital rubella. See Rubella
Cluster and factor analysis, 29–30 Consequence-based interventions, 902–910
Cluster reports (epidemiological studies), 64– 65 Contingencies or reinforcement, 827
Coactive model of organism-environment interaction, 659 Contingent electric shock as a punisher, 909
Cognition /cognitive: Continuum orientation, 930–931. See also
behavioral studies, 1306 Spectrum /continuum
executive function, 619– 620 Conversation. See also Communication; Language; Speech:
families, approach with, 1062, 1064 manner assessing communication, 806–807
outcomes (changes in ability), 205 skills (school-age children), 257–259, 873
profiles (neuropsychological characterstics), 374–375 Coordination disorder, developmental, 588
remediation, 619– 620 COPDD. See Childhood onset pervasive developmental
Rett syndrome, 146–148 disorder (COPDD)
skills, 872–873 Core characteristics, two triads, 629
strengths/challenges (school-based programs), Corpus callosum, 494–495
1016–1017 Cortical language areas, 495–496
style, central coherence and normal variation in, Cortisol secretion, 460
644– 645 Cost issues, 1143, 1169, 1170
theories (TEACCH philosophy/principles), 1178 Course of development, uneven /unusual (stressors
theory of mind and, 872–873 confronting families), 1056
Cohen syndrome, 544 Coworker training/support, 1097
Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (CPEA), Criminal justice system, autism and, 299–300
1196 Cronbach’s alphas, 747
College: Cross-cultural issues. See Cultural issues
adolescents/adults with autism, 303–304 Cross-model matching and body mapping, 389
student trainees/providers, 1137–1138 CSBQ. See Children’s Social Behavior Questionnaire
Colombia. See Latin America (CSBQ)
Communication: CSF 5-HIAA, 456–457
language (see Language) CSF HVA, 457
nonverbal, 235–236 CTRF, 748
preverbal, 235–236 Cues, multiple, 954
protoimperative/protodeclarative, 320 Cultural issues:
requestive, 320 assessment /diagnosis, 32
vocalizations, 235 cross-cultural programs, 1174–1187
Subject Index I • 43

Curriculum and classroom structure, 863–877 behavioral definition (versus biological), 1307
behavioral curricula, 874–875 categorical definitions of autism:
classroom structure, 866–867 approaches to, 14–25
cognitive skills, 872–873 problems with, 583– 600
comprehensive curricula, 873–875 classification issues, 9–13
inclusion settings, 867–868 controversies, 25–33
outcomes, 865–867 development of autism as diagnostic concept, 6–9
pivotal response training, 874 (see also Pivotal response diagnostic confusion, as stress to families, 1056
training (PRT)) diagnostic process, 1, 9
play skills, 870, 1011, 1012 (see also Play) dimensional versus categorical classification, 10–11
preschool curricula, 873–874 early diagnosis, 31–33, 232–233, 707–709, 1204
publishers, 864 etiologies and causes, 12
settings, 867–868 general issues in diagnosis of autistic spectrum
social skills, 868–872 disorders, 730–741
social validity, 876 history of concepts of autism, 583–587
transition to adulthood, 875–876 labels, 2, 10
Curriculum content (school-based programs), 1010–1012 misconceptions about, 10–11
communication skills (expressive/receptive language misuse of classification systems, 12
and spontaneous language), 1010, 1012 multidimensional diagnostic formulation, 594–599
daily routines, 1010, 1012 active-but-odd group, 250, 586, 598–599
instructional strategies and curriculum areas (STAR aloof group, 249, 595–597
program), 1012 passive group, 249–250, 597–598
play skills, 1011, 1012 “official” status, diagnostic systems, 10
preacademic skills, 1010–1011, 1012 pervasive developmental disorder (PDD), term
social interaction skills, 1011, 1012 introduced (DSM-III, 1980), 2
research, role of, 13–14
Daily Living Skills, 793 spectrum with fuzzy borders and overlaps, autism as,
Daily routines (school-based), 1010, 1012 1307–1308
DAMP (Disorders of Attention, Motor Control, and stigmatization, potential for, 12–13
Perception) syndrome, 28, 182–184, 588 theory, role of, 11
DAS. See Differential Abilities Scales (DAS) Diagnosis/classification systems, official, 2, 10,
DDDC. See Douglass Development Disabilities Center 591–594
(DDDC) Diagnostic instruments in autistic spectrum disorders,
Deafness, congenital, 553–555. See also Hearing 730–763
Declarative memory, 370 for Asperger syndrome, 760–762
Deficit acceptance, 1178 direct observation scales, 756–758
DeGangi-Berk Test of Sensory Integration, 846 first empirically developed rating scales and
Deictic terms, use of, 347 questionnaires, 744–745
Delaware Autistic Program, 1036 interviews, 753–756
Delay, deviance versus, 227–229, 386–387, 397–398, 776 measuring change in core behaviors, 761–762
Delayed nonmatch to sample (DNMS), 663 overview tables, 742–743, 787
Dementia infantilis, 8, 75 psychometric issues, 735
Dementia precoccissima catatonica, 584 rating scales, currently used, 748–752
Denmark. See Nordic nations recommended, 787
Denver Development Screening Test (DDST), 1204 related diagnostic and behavioral assessment
Denver Model at University of Colorado, 712, 953, instruments, 758–760
1050–1051 reliability, 736–740
Depression, 11, 100–101, 208 scales measuring core deficits, 746–748
Detection of Autism by Infant Sociability Interview validity, 740–741
(DAISI ), 227 Diagnostic interviews, 753–756
Deterioration in adulthood, 213–214, 289–290 Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication
Development, behavior and, 221–222 Disorders (DISCO), 594–595, 735–736, 743, 745,
Developmental, Individualized, Relationship-based (DIR) 755–756
model, 873, 929, 932, 934, 939, 957, 959 Diagnostic problems, associated conditions causing,
Developmental approach, 14 587–591
Developmental change, 31 anorexia nervosa, 590
Developmental continuum (principles of TEACCH), 1068 attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 588
Developmental delay, deviance versus, 227–229, 386–387, borderlines of normality, 591
397–398, 776 catatonia, 589–590
Developmental disorder(s): deficits of attention, motor control, and perception
autism as (principles of TEACCH), 1066 (DAMP syndrome), 588
language disorders, 175, 587–588 developmental coordination disorder, 588
learning disability of the right hemisphere, 98 developmental language disorders, 587–588
“other ” (problems of categorical classification disorders beginning in childhood, 587–589
systems), 587 eccentric normality, 587
pervasive (see Pervasive developmental disorder (PDD)) generalized mental retardation, 587
Developmental-pragmatic approaches/strategies, 947, hearing impairments, 588–589
953–955, 957–960, 966–967 neuro-psychiatric conditions in adults, 589–590
Developmental psychosis, 1233–1234 obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 590
Developmental regression, 30–31 personality disorders (schizoid and schizotypal), 590
Developmental sensitivity, 818 psychosocial deprivation, 591
Diagnosis/classification, 1–3, 5–34 schizophrenia, 590
I • 44 Subject Index

Diagnostic problems, associated conditions causing Early Social-Communication Scales (ESCS), 654, 662
(Continued) Eating problems, 847
Tourette’s syndrome, 588 Eccentric normality (problems of categorical classification
visual impairments, 589 systems), 587
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Echoes, 963, 965
(DSM) of the American Psychiatric Association, 2, Echolalia, 255–256, 346–347, 804, 805, 961, 964
71–72, 92, 184, 232, 592–594, 748 Ecological approach, functional ( behavioral assessment),
DSM-IV/ICD-10 systems as epistemological backbone of 817–827
this Handbook, 2 Ecological model of autism, experiential effects on pretend
problems of DSM-IV/ICD-10 system of classification, play, 398–399
592–594 Ecological /setting event interventions (versus immediate
autism, 14–25 antecedents), 899
definition of autism (in DSM-IV and ICD-10), 20–21 Ecological validity, 826, 827
definition of nonautistic PDDs, 21–25 Ecuador. See Latin America
DSM-III, 15 Education:
DSM-III-R, 16–17 Asperger syndrome, 986–998
DSM-IV and ICD-10, 17–18 as intervention (overview), 1310
DSM-IV field trial, 18–21 legal issues (see Legal issues, educating children with
Didactic approaches, 947, 959, 963, 1141–1142, 1143 autism)
Differential Abilities Scales (DAS), 787, 789 outcomes, 205–208
Differential diagnosis, 25 personal perspective (Grandin), 1276–1277
Asperger syndrome, 25 of public (press/ TV/ Internet), 1309
autistic disorder, 25 Rett, 154–155
childhood disintegrative disorder, 25, 81–82 Educational interventions:
involuntary movements, 847 with families, 1061, 1062
PDD-NOS, 25, 172–184 models of, 1043–1053 (see also Center-based programs;
Rett syndrome, 25, 132–135 Home-based programs; School(s)/school-based
Differential reinforcement procedures, 904–910 programs)
of Alternative Behavior (DRA / DRAlt), 904, 905, 906, social communication, 987–989
907 Education for All Handicapped Children Act of 1975, 867
of Incompatible Behavior (DRI ), 904, 905, 906, 907, Efficiency ( behavioral assessment), 822
908, 909 Effort ( behavioral assessment), 822
of Low Rates of Responding (DRL), 904, 906 Ego skin, 1230
of Other Behavior (DRO), 904–905, 906, 907, 910 Electroencephalograms (EEGs), 144–145, 503, 538, 663,
Difficult life events, 302–303 665, 1204
Dimensional versus categorical classification, 10–11 Elizabeth Ives School for Special Children (New Haven,
Direct observation scales, 756–758 CT), 1287
DISCO. See Diagnostic Interview for Social and Elementary and Secondary Education Act (No Child Left
Communication Disorders (DISCO) Behind), 863
Discourse management, 806, 808 Embarrassment, 415
Discrete trial training (DTI ), 883, 941, 948, 949, 959, Embedded Figures Task (EFT), 635, 642, 643, 645
1004, 1005, 1007–1009 Embodied cognition, 686, 690
Discriminant validity problem (EF hypothesis), 616– 617 Emergence of mind, 687
Disembodied cognition, 686, 690 Emotion(s), 406–420
Disintegrative PDDs, 33. See also Childhood disintegrative abstract (sibling’s perspective), 1271
disorder (CDD); Rett syndrome complex, 415–417
Disintegrative psychosis (Heller’s syndrome), 8, 15, 71, 135 conceptual issues, 406–408
DMFC/AC (dorsal medial-frontal cortex), 645, 663– 673 emotional expressiveness, 410–411
Dopamine, 457 Grandin (gradual change and my emotional life), 1283
Douglass Development Disabilities Center (DDDC), 1045, interpersonal coordination of affect, 409–410
1046, 1047, 1131, 1132 later childhood and adolescence, 411–414
Down syndrome, 50, 51, 235, 321, 345, 547–548 self-conscious, 414–417
DRA / DRI / DRL/ DRO. See Differential reinforcement social impairment in congenitally blind children and
procedures Romanian orphans, 417–419
Drawings, 794–795 stress/anxiety during adolescence, 1014–1015
DSM. See Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental systematic studies, early years, 408–409
Disorders (DSM) of the American Psychiatric Emotional assessment, 794–795
Association Emotional regulation, 934, 939, 980–983
Due process, 1167 Emotional support (families), 1062, 1064
Dyskinesia, 839, 848 Emotional vulnerability, supports for, 1015
Dyskinesia Identification System Condensed User Scale Empathizing:
(DISCUS), 848 in autism spectrum conditions, 631
Dyslexia, 263, 618, 645 mindblindness, 629– 631
Dysmorphic features (minor physical anomalies; MPAs), normal development of, 630– 631
501 theory of mind and, 629– 631
Dyspraxia, 387 Empathizing-systemizing theory, 631– 634
Empathy, defined, 629– 630
Early diagnosis, 31–33, 232–233, 707–709, 1204 Empirically supported treatments, 1038, 1059
Early inclusion, 1035–1038. See also Mainstream Employment, 298–299, 304, 1087–1098
Early infantile autism, 584 average intelligence and, 304
Early intervention, 238–239 entrepreneurial supports, 1094–1095
Early Language Milestone Scale, 800 family, role of, 1092–1093
Subject Index I • 45

historical overview, 1089 repetitive behavior, 616


legislation, 1089–1091 skills, 234, 325–326
mental retardation and, 301–302 social processes and, 613– 616
outcomes, 205–208 specificity to autism, 616– 617
personal perspective (Grandin), 1277–1278 working memory, 611, 788
poll results, 1088 Exercise, physical, 900, 1015
protected, 1313 Experience-expectant neural development, 657– 658
recommendations toward greater employability, Explicit memory, 370
1096–1097 Exploration and play, 236–237. See also Play
school-to-work transition process, 1091–1093 Extended school year, 1171
secure, 1095 Extinction procedures, 901, 907, 910
service models, 1093–1096 Extreme emotional dysregulation, 940
sheltered workshops, 1095 Extreme male brain (EMB) theory, 634
supported, 207, 302, 1093–1094 Eye(s):
Emulation, 383 “eyes task ” (Baron-Cohen), 643
Enactive mind (EM), 682– 699 gaze, 317–319, 962
computational models versus framework of, 686 pointing (augmentative communication systems), 149
emergence of mental representations, 689– 694 tracking/movement studies, 318, 483–485, 683– 684,
explanation of term, 686 688, 695
social cognition as social action, 695– 698
social development, 326 Faces, 149, 318, 385, 519–520, 696, 697
social world as open-domain task, 687– 689 Facilitated Communication (FC), 1185. See also
temporal constraints on models of social adaptation, Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC)
694– 695 strategies
Entrepreneurial supports, 1094–1095 Fads/unproven therapies (stresses on families), 1059
Environmental engineering/modifications, 899, 997 False alarm errors, 610
Epidemiology, 42– 65, 434 False-belief task, 615
Asperger’s syndrome, 53, 54 Family(ies), 1055–1079. See also Parent(s)
autism and social class, 64 advocacy training/support, 1063, 1064
autistic disorder, 46–49 behavioral approach, 1061, 1062
characteristics of autistic samples, 45–51 cognitive approach, 1062, 1064
childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), 53–56, 75 education approach, 1061, 1062
cluster reports, 64– 65 emotional support, 1062, 1064
design /selection of studies, 42–45 goals, 1060
gender, 46–49 implications, 1065–1066
immigrant status, ethnicity, social class, and other instrumental support, 1063, 1064
correlates, 62– 65 life cycle, autism and, 1068–1078
international (countries), 46–49 modalities, 1064–1065
Australia, 1197–1198 parent(s) as cotherapist(s), 1061, 1062
China, 1203–1204 patterns/studies, 90, 137, 426–429, 437–438, 617– 618,
Israel, 1219 632– 633
IQ, 46–49 principles, 1060–1061
pervasive developmental disorders, 51–56, 184–185 relationship enhancement methods, 1061–1063
Rett syndrome, 135–136 school-to-work transition process and, 1092–1093
time trends, 56– 62 social classes, 7, 64
Epigenetic mechanisms/models, 439–440, 655 stressors confronting, 1055–1059
Epilepsy, 50, 51, 82, 151, 295–296, 502–503, 544, 629 support, 989, 997–998, 1059–1066
Episodic memory, 370 targeted outcomes, 1061–1064
ERN (error-related negativity), 669 TEACCH approach (principles), 1066–1068
Erythrocyte glutathione peroxidase, 539 techniques, 1065
Escitalopram, 1109 FAPE standard (free appropriate public education),
ESCS. See Early Social-Communication Scales (ESCS) 1163–1164
Ethics (provider training), 1139 Feedback on provider work performance, 1127
Ethnicity, 62– 65 Feeding problems, 146, 540–542
Etiology, 12, 75–77, 136–139, 185–186, 594, 1229–1231 Fetal alcohol syndrome, 544
Evaluation of Sensory Processing (ESP), 843 Fetal anticonvulsant syndrome, 555–556
Evoked potentials, 475–483 Fetal valproate syndrome (FVS), 555–556
Executive function (EF), 606– 621 First words (normal language development), 337–340
coherence, 644 Floor Time, 873, 889, 957
component process analyses, 607– 611 Fluoxetine (Prozac), 1107, 1282–1283
deficits and broader autism phenotype, 617– 618 Fluvoxamine, 1107–1108
emerging issues, 612– 617 Foot deformities (Rett syndrome), 154
f lexibility and inhibition, 608– 611 Fragile X syndrome (FXS), 26, 50, 51, 185, 429–430, 442,
future directions, 617– 620 534, 537, 545–547
generativity hypothesis, 396 France, 1206–1212
imitation, 388 Frontal lobe findings, 525
language and intelligence, 616 Functional adjustment, 775–776
literature review, 606– 611 Functional analysis, 949
model of cognitive development in autism (theory of Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST), 824
mind), 634– 635 Functional Assessment and Curriculum for Teaching
neuropsychological characteristics, 372–374 Everyday Routines (FACTER), 1023
remediation, cognitive, 618– 620 Functional communication training (FCT), 910–912
I • 46 Subject Index

Functional ecological approach, 817–827 Head circumference, 500–501, 536


Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI ), 355. See Headstart, 1037
also Neuroimaging studies Hearing:
Functional routines (FR), teaching, 1004, 1005 assessment, 537, 842
Functioning: impairment, 50, 540, 553–555, 588–589
levels ( low, medium, high) autism spectrum conditions, sound sensitivity, 1281–1282
and theory of mind, 628– 629 Heller’s syndrome (disintegrative psychosis), 8, 15, 71,
specific areas of (infancy and early childhood), 233–238 135
Fusiform face area (FFA), 520–523 High probability ( high p) requests, 901
Fusiform gyrus (FG), 695 Hippocampus, 496–497
Future research directions, 109–111, 440–441, 617– 620, Histidinemia, 544
887–892, 914–917, 1311–1313 HIV, 76
Holistic orientation, 1178–1179
Gait, 500 Home-based programs, 1043, 1047–1049
Gangliosidoses, 76 pivotal response training (PRT) at University of
Gastrointestinal inf lammation, 629 California at Santa Barbara), 1049
GAUTENA Autism Society, 1237 strengths/ limitations, 1051
Gaze, 317–319, 962 Young Autism Project at UCLA, 1047–1049
Generalization and maintenance, treatment gains, 826, Home Life Checklist, 1023
887, 914–915, 928, 935–936 Homework /assignments, modifying, 1019
Generalized anxiety disorder, 178 Hydrotherapy (Rett syndrome), 153
Generativity hypothesis (play), 395–396 Hyperlexia, 263, 354, 629, 691
Genetic counseling, 441–443 Hypomelanosis of Ito, 544
Genetics, 425–444, 1309–1310 Hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis, 459, 460–461
animal models and studies of portmortem brain tissues, Hypothyroidism, 76
440–441 Hypotonia, 150
Asperger syndrome, 108–109
biological findings, 439 IBR skill. See Initiating behavior requests (IBR skill)
candidate gene strategies, 433–434 ICD-10. See International Statistical Classification of
childhood disintegrative disorder, 77–79 Diseases and Related Health Problems of the World
chromosomal abnormalities and genetically determined Health Organization (ICD/ICD-10)
medical conditions, 429–430 Iconic systems, 885
clinical implications, 441–443 Id, 1229
epidemiological findings, 434 IDEA (Individuals with Disabilities Education Act), 1090,
epigenetic mechanisms, 439–440 1091, 1124, 1161–1162, 1171
familial clustering, 437–438 Identity development, 1022
family studies, 426–429 If-then rules, embedded use of, 615
future directions, 440–441 IJA /RJA (initiating/responding to joint attention) skills,
genetic partitioning of autism, 436–440 654, 655, 662, 663, 664, 667, 672
genome-wide screens of sib pair samples, 432–433 I LAUGH model, 992, 996
leads for candidate genes, 440 Imitation, 320–321, 382–391, 400–401, 889
linkage evidence, 438–439 action level, 383
molecular genetics, 430–434 action on objects, 384
multiplex-singleton comparisons: developmental of body movements (intransitive acts), 384–385
regression, 438 cross-model matching and body mapping, 389
neurocognitive endophenotypes, 439 delay versus deviance, 386–387
nongenetic risk factors, 434–436 development and, 382–384
quantitative genetics, 425–429 developmental correlates, 385–386
quantitative trait loci (QTL) approaches, 440 emulation, 383
twin studies, 425–426 executive functions, 388
Germany and Austria, 1212–1215 kinesthesia, 388–389
Gesturing, protoimperative versus protodeclarative, 320 methodological issues, 390–391
Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS), 542, 714–715, model of interpersonal development, 383
721–722, 723, 742, 751–752 motor problems in autism, 387–388
Goldenhar syndrome, 544 neural mechanisms, 389–390
Grading/tests, modifying, 1019 oral-facial, 385
Grandin, Temple: possible mechanisms underlying imitation problem,
contributions to public education on autism, 1184–1186, 387–389
1311 praxis and body mapping, 387
personal perspective of autism, 1276–1285 reciprocal, 889
Greece, 1215–1218 response facilitation, 383
Group homes, 1312 social development, 320–321
Group work and class discussions, 1019 stimulus enhancement, 383
Guardianship (adolescents/adults), 301 symbolic content, 388
Guilt, 415 Immigrant status (epidemiological studies), 62– 65
Immune/immunological factors, 539, 542
Habilitation programs, 1183–1184 Impairments in Communication and Social Interaction
Hair analysis for trace elements, 538 scale, 667
Handedness, nonright-, 629 Incidental teaching, 951
Handicaps, Behavior, and Skills schedule (HBS), 214, 745 Inclusion. See Mainstream
Hand splinting, 152 Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn), 345
Hanen approach, 958, 959 Individualization, 1035, 1066–1067, 1178
Subject Index I • 47

Individualized Education Program (IEP), 1073, Interpersonal development, model of, 383
1164–1165, 1171 Interpersonal relationships versus relatedness, 417
Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP), 1164, 1168 Interpersonal supports (social communication), 989–995
Infant(s)/toddlers, autism in, 31–33, 223–240 for members of the community, 993–995
areas of functioning, 233–238 for peers, 990–992
autism versus mental retardation, 226 for teachers and professionals, 992–993
autism versus typical and developmentally delayed Interruption /redirection, 902–903
peers, 225–226, 230 Interventions/treatment, 859–861, 1310–1311
communication, 230 Asperger, 113–116
early diagnosis, 31–33, 232–233 behavioral approaches (see Behavioral interventions)
early intervention, 238–239 education as, 1310 (see also Educational interventions)
methodological limitations, 231 language/communication enhancement (see
social interaction, 230 Communication abilities, enhancing; Language
stereotypical behaviors and repetitive patterns, 230 interventions)
symptoms in first year of life, 223–227 PDD-NOS, 187–190
symptoms in second and third years, 227–231 psychopharmacologic, 1310 (see also
Infant(s)/toddlers with disabilities, services for: Psychopharmacology)
identification /assessment, 1168 treatment plan development, 825–827
Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP), 1164, 1168 Intestinal permeability studies, 539
services for ( legal issues), 1167–1169 Intradimensional-Extradimensional Shift Task (ID/ ED),
transition, 1168 609– 610, 618, 620
Infant Behavioral Summarized Scale (IBSS), 229 Intraverbals, 964, 965
Infant Behavior Summarized Evaluation (IBSE), 712 Intrinsic reinforcers, 947, 959
Infantile autism (terminology), 134, 314 In vivo protocol, 810, 811
Infantile spasms/ West syndrome, 544 Involuntary movements, 838–840, 847–848
Infant / Toddler Checklist, 713 Isodicentric chromosome 15q syndrome, 550–551
Infections, congenital /acquired, 76, 544. See also Rubella Israel, 1218–1221
Initial Communication Processes Scale, 800 Italy, 1221–1222
Initiating behavior requests (IBR skill), 653, 662, 664, 665
Initiation of communication, 954 Japan, 1223–1226
Instrument(s). See Diagnostic instruments Job carving, 1097
Instrumental support for families, 1063, 1064 Job duties, specification of (provider training), 1126
Intellectual disability, 8 Joint attention, 650– 674
Intellectualization of affect, 89 brain behavior research, 662– 668
Intelligence, autism and, 290–292, 628– 629, 1313 brain volume in autism, 660– 662
clinical assessment, 786, 787–792 deficits, 889, 1007
epidemiology, 46–49 defined, 650
outcome and, 215–216 initiating/responding skills (IJA /RJA), 654, 655, 662,
Intentional stance, 629 663, 664, 667, 672
Interests, circumscribed/all absorbing, 94, 100 neural plasticity and, 657– 660
International perspectives, 1193–1247 neurodevelopmental models of autism, 650– 674
Australia, 1197–1201 role of dorsal medial-frontal cortex in social and
Canada, 1201–1203 nonsocial behavior, 668– 673
China, 1203–1206 skills, 946
comparative analysis, 1193–1194, 1197 social communication, 979
France, 1206–1212 social development, 319–320
Germany and Austria, 1212–1215 social impairment and, 652– 655
Greece, 1215–1218 social orienting model, 655– 662
Israel, 1218–1221 theory of mind and, 665– 668
Italy, 1221–1222 Joubert syndrome, 544
Japan, 1223–1226
Korea, 1226–1228 Kanner’s description, early controversies, 6–8
Latin America, 1228–1233 Karyotype and DNA analysis for Fragile X, 537
Netherlands, 1233–1235 Kaufman-Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC-II ),
Spain, 1235–1238 789, 794
Sweden and other Nordic nations, 1238–1243 Kinesthesia (imitation), 388–389
United Kingdom, 1243–1247 Kleinfelter’s syndrome, 544
United States, 1194–1196 Knowledge:
International Statistical Classification of Diseases and examples of “need to know” orientation competencies
Related Health Problems of the World Health (provider training), 1139
Organization (ICD/ICD-10), 2, 10, 591–594. See also “ knowing that ” versus “ knowing how,” 687
Diagnosis/classification trainee skills and, 1138–1140
definition of autism (in DSM-IV and ICD-10), 20–21 Korea, 1226–1228
DSM-IV and ICD-10, 17–18
DSM-IV/ICD-10 systems as epistemological backbone of Labels, 2, 10
this Handbook, 2 Landau-Kleffner syndrome, 82, 135
from ICD-9 to ICD-10, 17 Language. See also Communication:
ICD-10 PDD classification (overactive disorder with acquisition (sibling’s perspective), 1266–1267
mental retardation), 6 behavioral interventions, 910–912
ICD10 research diagnostic guidelines, 93 deficits, 335, 427, 1306
problems of DSM-IV/ICD-10 system of classification, executive function, 616
592–594 expressive versus receptive, 961
I • 48 Subject Index

Language (Continued) Limbic system, 495–496, 497, 525–526


neuroimaging studies of, 517–519 Lipofuscinosis, 76
supports for comprehension (school-based), 1018 Litigation, parent-initiated, 1124–1125
Language/communication development, 335–356 Living arrangements (adolescents/adults), 297–298
Asperger syndrome, 94, 99–100, 115 Loss/death of parent, 1273–1274
in autism, 227, 292–294, 335–336, 341–356 Luria-Nebraska Neurosopsychological battery, 844
adolescents/adults, 292–294
articulation, 343–344 Mainstream, 1029–1039
babbling, 349 definition, 1029
characteristics of speech, 256 early education and beyond, 1037–1038
conversational skills, 257–259 early inclusion, 1035–1036
course and developmental change, 341–343 empirically supported interventions, 1038
deficits overlapping with other disorders, 335 individualized, comprehensive programming, 1035
echolalia, 255–256, 346–347 laws/regulations, 1120
infancy and early childhood, 227 models of inclusion, 1035
intonational peculiarities, 348 outcomes of inclusion, 1034–1035
language comprehension in autism, 349–351 peers, role of, 1037
language use, 351–354 peers initiation, 1030
narrative storytelling, 259–260 planning inclusion activities, 1037
personal pronouns, 256–267, 347 settings, 867–868
reading, 354–355 social skills, 1029–1033
school-age children, 254–260 statewide services, 1036–1037
suprasegmental aspects of language, 347–349 teachers’ and parents’ perspectives, 1033–1034
syntax and morphology, 345–346 terminology (mainstream /inclusion /integration), 1029
theories of origin, 355–356 training inclusion providers, 1129–1130
use of deictic terms, 347 Maintenance and generalization, 826, 887, 914–915, 928,
word use, 344–345 935–936
deficits in relatives, 427 Mands, 963, 965
in typical populations (non-autistic), 336–341 Manual signs, 955
Language Development Survey, 800 Mastery criterion (provider training), 1146–1147
Language disorder, autism versus (infancy and early Maternal drug/alcohol abuse, 436, 544
childhood), 227 Maternal hypothyroidism, 436
Language interventions, 946–970. See also Communication May Center for Early Childhood Education, 1036
abilities, enhancing Mean Length of Utterance (MLU), 345, 805
core communicative deficits: Measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccination, 435–436,
early language stage, 963–969 541. See also Rubella
methodological elements common to ABA approaches, Measurement, qualitative/quantitative, 935–936
949 MECP2 gene, 130–132, 136, 148. See also Rett syndrome
methods (three major approaches), 947, 959 Mediation, 1166
prelinguistic stage, 947–961 Medical conditions associated with autism, 7, 50, 543–556.
Latin America, 1228–1233 See also specific conditions
Law enforcement agencies, fostering awareness, 994–995 Angelman / Prader-Willi syndromes, 76, 135, 551–552
Lead level, 537 CATCH 22, 552
LEAP program (Learning Experiences, an Alternative cerebral palsy, 50
Program for Preschoolers and Their Parents), 940, congenital blindness and deafness, 553–555
953, 1035–1036, 1045, 1046–1047 congenital rubella, 50
Learning characteristics/styles, 114, 983–986, 1007–1008 Down syndrome, 50, 547–548
Learning/educational supports, 989, 995–997 epidemiological studies, 50–51
Least restrictive environment (LRE), 1165–1166 epilepsy, 50
Legal issues: fetal anticonvulsant /valproate syndrome, 555–556
classification, 9 fetal valproate syndrome (FVS), 555–556
educating children with autism, 1161–1172 Fragile X, 50, 545–547
FAPE standard (free appropriate public education), hearing deficits, 50
1163–1164 isodicentric chromosome 15q syndrome, 550–551
IDEA (Individuals with Disabilities Education Act), mitochondrial disorders, 549–550
1090, 1091, 1124, 1161–1162, 1171 Möbius syndrome, 552–553
Individualized Education Program (IEP), 1164–1165, neurofibromatosis, 50
1171 phenylketonuria (PKU), 50, 553
Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP), 1164, semantics (versus autism associated with medical
1168 conditions), 535
least restrictive environment (LRE), 1165–1166 syndromic associations, 544
employment for adolescents/adults with autism, 9, tuberous sclerosis, 50, 543–545
1089–1091 velocardiofacial syndrome (VCFS), 552
Americans with Disabilities Act, 1091 visual deficits, 50
Vocational Rehabilitation Act of 1973 (employment), Williams-Beuren syndrome (WBS), 548–549
1090–1091 Medical conditions associated with CDD, 76
Leigh disease, 76 Medical evaluation in autism, 536–539, 782–783
Leiter International Performance Scale Revised definitive hearing test, 537
(Leiter-R), 374, 787, 789, 844 electroencephalography, 538
Lesch-Nyhan syndrome, 544 karyotype and DNA analysis for Fragile X, 537
Levels of Narrative Development, 813 lead level, 537
Life skills, 1023 metabolic testing, 538
Subject Index I • 49

neuroimaging studies, 538 Multidisciplinary training (TEACCH component), 1177


physical and neurologic examination, 536–537 Multiple Complex (or Multiplex) Developmental Disorder
head circumference, 536 (MCDD), 182–184, 186
recurrence risk estimate for ideopathic autism, 537 Multiplex developmental disorder, 587
sensorimotor function, 536–537 Multiplex-singleton comparisons, 438
tests of unproven value, 538–539 Mutual regulation, 940
Medical signs/symptoms associated with autism, 539–543
Medications. See Psychopharmacology Naltrexone, 1111–1112
Memory, 369–372, 388, 611, 788, 1279 Narrative assessment, 810–813
Mental Development Index (MDI ), 791 Narrative Rubrics, 813
Mental representations (enactive mind), 689– 694 Narrative storytelling (school-age children), 259–260
Mental retardation: National Research Council (NRC) guidelines, 927–928
Asperger syndrome, 94 Natural environments (staff performance), 1130
autism and, 28, 51, 291, 300–303 Naturalistic approaches, 883–885, 947, 950, 959, 964–966
autism diagnosis more acceptable than, 1308 Natural Language Paradigm (NLP), 929, 930, 937, 940,
autism versus (infancy and early childhood), 226 941, 952
categorical classification systems, problems of, 587 Negative priming effect, 609
differential diagnosis (PDD-NOS), 176–177 NEPSY, 787, 793–794
difficult life events, 302–303 Netherlands, 1233–1235
guardianship, 301 Neural basis for autism (genetics), 443
ICD-10 PDD classification (overactive disorder with Neural mechanisms (imitation), 389–390
mental retardation), 6 Neurobiological factors, 75–77, 79, 140–141
lifestyle risk factors and, 297 Neurochemical abnormalities (tests of unproven value),
and mental retardation, 300–303 539
self-help skills, 300–301 Neurochemical alterations, Rett syndrome, 141–143
sexuality, 301 Neurochemical studies of autism, 453–465
work, 301–302 amino acids and acetylcholine, 462–463
Metabolic testing, 538 blood 5-HT, 454–455
Metachromatic leukodystrophy, 76 cardiovascular, 459
Metaphoric language, 691 cerebrospinal f luid, 459
Methylphenidate, 1110 CSF 5-HIAA, 456–457
Mexico. See Latin America CSF HVA, 457
Milieu teaching, 950–951, 959 dopamine, 457, 458
Miller Assessment for Preschoolers (MAP), 844, 846, 847 hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis, 459, 460–461
Mindblindness/empathizing theory, 629– 631 neuroendocrine studies of serotonergic functioning, 456
Mind-mind problem, 691 norepinephrine, 458–459
Mind reading, 629, 873 peptide research, 461–462
Minimal speech approach (MSA), 952, 959 plasma, 457–458, 459
Minor physical anomalies (MPAs), 501 purines and related compounds, 463–464
Mirtazapine, 1109 serotonin, 453–456
Mismatch negativity (MMN), 481 stress response systems in autism, 459
Mitochondrial deficits/disorders, 76, 549–550 thyroid hormone and TRH test, 460–461
MMR vaccine. See Measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) tryptophan metabolism, 455–456
vaccination urine, 459
Möbius syndrome, 552–553 urine 5-HIAA and 5-HT, 455
Modeling, 870 Neurocognitive endophenotypes, 439
Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT), Neurofibromatosis, 50, 51, 544
229, 714–715, 716–717. See also Checklist for Autism Neuroimaging studies, 515–527, 538
in Toddlers (CHAT) blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal, 517
Modified Rogers Scale, 848 communication and language, 355, 517–519
Molecular genetics, 430–434. See also Genetics face perception deficits, 519–520
Monozygotic twinning as risk factor, 434 fMRI, 516–517
Montgomery County Public School System Preschool for frontal lobe findings, 525
Children with Autism, 1037 fusiform face area (FFA), 520–523
Mood stabilizers, 1111 PDD-NOS, 171–172
More Than Words program, 958 role of amygdala, 525–526
Morphology, syntax and, 345–346 social cognition, 524–525
Mortality and causes of death, 212–213, 296–297 social perception, 520–524
Motivation, 954, 1231 Neurolipidosis, 76
Motivation Assessment Scale (MAS), 824 Neurological /medical issues (overview), 423–424
Motor. See also Sensory and motor features in autism: Neurologic aspects of autism, 473–504
apraxia, 500 central nervous system function, 474–488
Asperger syndrome, 94, 100 central nervous system structure, 488–500
assessments, 845–848 clinical neurology, 500–503
autism, 387–388 dysmorphic features (minor physical anomalies; MPAs),
clumsiness, 94 501
features, 834–835 electroencephalograms (EEGs) in autism, 503
imitation, 237–238 epilepsy in autism, 502–503
skills and praxis, 845–847 evoked potentials, 475–483
Motor Free Visual Perception Test, 844 head circumference, 500–501
Mullen, 787, 791 neurologic examination, 500
Multiaxial diagnostic approaches, 14 neuropathology, 488–490
I • 50 Subject Index

Neurologic aspects of autism (Continued) developmental course, 288–290


neuropsychologic profile, 486–488 education, 205–208, 294–295
oculomotor physiology, 483–485 employment, 205–208, 298–299
pathogenesis, theories of, 490–491 epilepsy, 295–296
postural physiology, 485–486 higher functioning individuals, and risk of psychiatric
structural brain imaging, 491–500 disturbance, 210–211
Neuropathology, 139–140, 488–490 of inclusion, 1034–1035
Neuropsychological assessment, 786, 793–794 intelligence, 290–292
Neuropsychological characteristics in autism and related language, 292–294
conditions, 365–376 living arrangements, 297–298
attention, 367–369 marriage, 299
cognitive profiles, 374–375 mortality/causes of death, 212–213, 296–297
executive function, 372–374 predictors of, 215–216
memory, 369–372 psychiatric disorders, 208–212, 295
sensory perception, 366–367 social, 201–204
Neuropsychological studies, 102–104, 171, 486–488, 645 suicide, 212
New situations (sibling’s perspective), 1269 victims of crime, 299
Nomothetic measures, 820
Noncontingent reinforcement, 907–908 Parent(s). See also Family(ies):
Nonright-handedness, 629 advocacy, 1308–1309
Nonverbal communication, 89, 235–236 associations (international), 1206, 1211–1212,
Nonverbal intelligence, 272 1214–1215, 1219, 1222–1223, 1228
Nonverbal learning disabilities (NLD), 97 collaboration with:
Nordic nations, 1238–1243 provider training, 1129
Norepinephrine, 458–459 TEACCH component, 1067, 1176, 1178
Normality: consent (education, legal issues), 1166
borderlines of (problems of categorical classification as cotherapists, 1061, 1062
systems), 591 death of, 1273–1274
eccentric, 587 involvement, 775, 936
physical appearance, 1057–1058 litigation (impact on provider training), 1124–1125
Nuclear autism, 184 pathogenesis ( history of autism), 7
Nutrition: reports, 225, 227, 408–409, 712
feeding problems, 146, 540–542 stressors confronting families, 1055–1059
growth patterns and (Rett syndrome), 145–146 training, 1061
Paroxetine, 1108
Observational assessments, 843–845 Passive group, 249–250, 597–598
Observational studies (emotion), 409 Pathogenesis, theories of, 490–491
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 178, 265, 275, 590 Pathological demand avoidance syndrome (PDA), 586
Obsessive Desire for Sameness, 667 Patty-cake, 320
Obstetrical complications/risk factors, 434–435, 539–540 Pay Attention! program, 619
Oculomotor physiology, 483–485 Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS), 846
“Official” status, diagnostic systems, 10 Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT/ PPVT-R /
Olanzapine, 1105 PPVT-III ), 147, 293, 790, 804
Onset: Pediatric epilepsy syndromes, 544. See also Epilepsy
age/type of, and problems of categorical classification Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory (PEDI ), 845
systems, 593 Pediatric Speech Intelligibility Test, 844
Asperger syndrome, 90, 94, 98–99 Peekaboo, 320
childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), 72–74 Peers:
criteria (autism versus Asperger syndrome), 92 interpersonal support for, 990–992
Operant conditioning procedures, early educational relationships with, 303, 323–324, 1020
applications of (provider training), 1125 role of, in interventions, 938–939, 968–969, 1037
Optimal arousal, 940 Peptide research, 461–462
Oral-facial imitations, 385 Perception-for-action systems, 691– 693
Oral-motor apraxia or dyspraxia, 500 Perceptual coherence, 642
Organization(s): Perceptual reasoning, 788
parent (international), 1206, 1211–1212, 1214–1215, Performance appraisal (provider training), 1128, 1144
1219, 1222–1223, 1228 Perinatal factors, 539–540
school-based, 1019–1020 Perseveration, 1231, 1307
Organizational behavior management (OBM), 1128 Personality, clinical assessment, 786
Organizational citizenship, 1139 Personality disorders:
Original narcissism, 1229 avoidant, 178, 179
Orthogonal movement disorder exams, 847 differential diagnosis (PDD-NOS), 178–180
Orthopedic aspects and intervention (Rett syndrome), schizoid, 97, 178, 179, 586, 590
150–154 schizotypal, 179, 590
Outcome(s), 201–217, 288–300 Personal perspectives, 1253
adaptive behavior, 294–295 mother (working model, community-integrated
adolescents/adults, 288–300 residential services for adults with autism),
Asperger syndrome, 109–110, 213 1255–1264
changes in recent years, 203–205 personal (Grandin), 1276–1285
cognitive ability, changes in, 205 sibling, 1265–1275
criminal justice system, autism and, 299–300 teacher, 1287–1303
deterioration in adulthood, 213–214 Personal pronouns, 256–267
Subject Index I • 51

Pervasive developmental disorder (PDDs): Plasticity, neural, 657– 660


conditions currently classified as (in ICD-10 and DSM- Play, 236–237, 382, 391–400
IV), 6 autism-specific findings in pretend play, 392
nonautistic, defined, 21–25 brain behavior correlates, 397
term introduced (DSM-III, 1980), 2 clinical evaluation, 795
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified curricula /teaching skills, 870, 928, 1011, 1012
(PDD-NOS), 165–191 deficient imaginative, 1307
Asperger syndrome and, 173–174, 735 delay versus deficit, 397–398
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) ecological model of autism, 398–399
versus, 177, 268 generativity hypothesis, 395–396
biological studies of, 169–172 impaired sensorimotor and functional play, 394–395
case of James, 274–275 intact symbolic abilities, 393–394
classification issues, 2, 6, 16–17, 25, 233, 275 methodological issues, 399–400
conceptual background, 167–169 normal development ( language and communication), 339
differential diagnosis, 172–184 sibling’s perspective, 1271
Asperger syndrome, 173–174 social development, 321–322
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), symbolic, 391–397
177 Positive Behavioral Support (PBS), 934–935
autistic disorder, 174–175 Postural physiology, 485–486
childhood disintegrative disorder, 175 Prader-Willi. See Angelman / Prader-Willi syndromes
childhood-onset conditions, 177–178 Pragmatic language impairment (PLI ), 98, 175–176
developmental language disorders, 175 Pragmatic Rating Scale (PRS), 808–810
generalized anxiety disorder, 178 Pragmatics, 339, 805–806, 962
mental retardation, 176–177 Praise, behavior-specific (provider training), 1127
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 178 Praxis, 387, 845–847
other developmental disorders, 175–176 Precedence rule, 92
other PDD entities, 172–175 Predictive value (negative/positive) of screening measures,
personality disorders, 178 711–712
pragmatic language impairment (PLI ), 175–176 Pre-Linguistic Autism Observation Schedule, 740
reactive attachment disorder, 180 Prelinguistic Communication Assessment, 801
schizophrenia, 180–181 Preschool curricula, 873–874
semantic-pragmatic disorder, 175 Preschool Language Scale, 800, 804
social phobias, 181–182 Pressure/squeeze machine, 1281, 1282
table, conditions to be considered, 172 Preverbal communication, 235–236
epidemiology, 53, 184–185 Pride, asocial form of, 415
etiology, 185–186 Princeton Child Developmental Institute (PCDI ), 873, 875,
multiple meanings, 168–169 1035, 1045, 1047, 1131, 1132
natural history, 186–187 Problem solving, 1016–1017, 1280
nosology, 166 Procedural reliability, 826, 827
self-injury, 189 Process, diagnostic, 1, 9
subgroups (two potential) within, 182–184 Processing speed, 788
treatment, 187–190 Professional relationships, family stress and, 1058–1059
Pervasive Developmental Disorder Problems Scale, 748 Professional training. See Training (preparation of autism
Pervasive Developmental Disorders Rating Scale (PDDRS), specialists)
742, 746–747 Profile scatter (clinical assessment), 775
Pervasive Developmental Disorders Screening Test PROMPT system, 951–953, 957, 959
(PDDST): Pronoun use, 347, 804–805, 961
Stage 1, 714–715, 717, 723 Prosody, 962
Stage 2, 714–715, 717–718, 723 Prosody Voice Screening Protocol (PVSP), 813–814
Pharmacotherapy. See Psychopharmacology Provider training. See Training (preparation of autism
Phenocopies, 436 specialists)
Phenomenological approach /systems, 12 Psychiatric conditions:
Phenotype, broader autism, 32–33, 116, 427, 428, adolescents/adults with autism, 295
617– 618, 645, 1058, 1308 categorical classification systems, problems of, 587
Phenotype versus genotype, 12 outcomes, 208–211
Phenylketonuria (PKU), 50, 51, 76, 136, 141, 553 personality disorders (see Personality disorders)
Phonics, learning to read with, 1284 psychopathology (Japan), 1224–1225
Phonology, 338 psychosis/phychotic, 210, 1229
Physical and neurologic examination, 536–537 risk of, and level of functioning, 210–211
Piagetian stages, 148 schizophrenia (see Schizophrenia)
Pica, 905–906 Psychoanalytic conceptions, 1229
Picture Arrangement, 788 Psychodynamic treatment approaches, 1239, 1242–1243
Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS), 155, Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP-R), 543, 743,
885, 891, 929, 955–957, 959, 964 757–758, 1227
Pivotal response training (PRT), 883–885, 888, 912, 929, Psychogenic theories/ hypothesis, 7, 1304–1305
930, 941, 953 Psychological assessment. See Clinical evaluation in autism
behaviors, 954 spectrum disorders
curriculum, 874 Psychomotor Development Index (PDI ), 791
defined, 1004 Psychopathy (terminology), 91
school-based, 1004, 1005 Psychopharmacology, 1102–1113
UCSB (UC at Santa Barbara), 1049 aggression, 906
Plasma, 457–458, 459 anxiety, 269
I • 52 Subject Index

Psychopharmacology (Continued) Respondent conditioning procedures, 913–914


Asperger syndrome, 116 Response facilitation (imitation), 383
atypical antipsychotics, 1102–1106 Responsibility/guilt (sibling’s perspective), 1265–1266
Japan, 1226 Rett syndrome, 126–156, 500
mood stabilizers, 1111 ambulation, 153
naltrexone, 1111–1112 apraxia-ataxia, 150–151
secretin, 1112 clinical characteristics, 133
need for research, 1310 clinical presentation and natural history, 126–129, 133
PDD-NOS, 189 cognitive and adaptive functioning, 146–148
personal perspective (Grandin), 1282–1283 communication abilities, 148–150
Rett syndrome, 143–145 definition, 21
self-injury, 906 diagnostic criteria, 129–130, 131, 132
serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs), 1106–1109, differential diagnosis features, 25, 81, 132–135
1282–1283 drug therapy, 143–145
single-mechanism intervention research, 1183 educational implications, 154–155
stimulants, 1109–1111 EEG characteristics, 145
Psychosis, 210, 1229 epidemiology, 135–136
Psychosocial deprivation (problems of categorical etiology, 136–139
classification systems), 591 feeding problems, 146
PTSD, 12 foot deformities, 154
Public policy perspectives, 1119–1121 genetics (MECP2 mutation), 78–79, 130–132, 431–432
Publishers, curricula, 864 growth patterns and nutrition, 145–146
Punishment procedures, 908–910 hydrotherapy, 153
Purines and related compounds, 463–464 ICD-10 and DSM-IV inclusion, 6, 8, 11
infantile autism compared to, 134
Qualitative versus quantitative approaches, 935 neuroanatomy, 140–141
Quality of care, defined, 1127 neurochemical alterations, 141–143
Quality of life improvements, 1134–1136 neurologic examination, 500
Quantitative genetics, 425–429, 440 neuropathology, 139–140
Quantitative trait loci (QTL), 440 orthopedic aspects and intervention, 150–154
Quetiapine, 1105 scoliosis, 154
spasticity, 153
Rainman, 1097, 1245, 1299, 1311 stages, 128–129, 133
Rapid motor imitation (RMI ) response approach, 948, 959 stereotypic hand movements, 127, 150, 151–153
Reactive attachment disorder (differential diagnosis, PDD- variants, 130–132
NOS), 180 Revised Knox Preschool Play Scale (PPS), 845
Reading, 354–355, 1284 Reynell Developmental Language Scales, 83, 800, 804
Real-Life Rating Scale (RLRS), 742 Rigidity, 1307
Reception by feature, function, and class (RFFC), 964 Rimland Diagnostic Form for Behavior Disturbed Children,
Receptive (sequence of teaching verbal behavior), 965 742–743, 744
Receptive-Expressive Emergent Language Scale (REEL), Risk estimate, recurrence (for ideopathic autism), 537
83, 800 Risk factors, nongenetic, 434–436
Recreation: Risperidone, 1103–1105
school-based, 1023 Ritvo-Freeman Real Life Rating Scale (RLRS), 745
sibling’s perspective, 1270–1271 RJA / IJA (initiating/responding to joint attention) skills,
sports, 994 654, 655, 662, 663, 664, 667, 672
Regional centers, 1176 Romanian children, 418–419
Register variation, 806, 808 Rorschach Inkblot Test, 794
REHABIT program, 620 Rosetti Infant Toddler Language Scale, 800
Reinforcement: Rubella:
assessment of natural communities of, 827 congenital, 7, 50, 51
behavioral interventions based on, 903–907 measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccination, 435–436
differential, 904–910 Rumination, treatment of, 901–902
of Alternative Behavior (DRA / DRAlt), 904, 905, 906,
907 Safety (provider training), 1139
of Incompatible Behavior (DRI ), 904, 905, 906, 907, Safety net for student, 1015
908, 909 Salaries, 1148
of Low Rates of Responding (DRL), 904, 906 Salience, topology of, 688, 695
of Other Behavior (DRO), 904–905, 906, 907, 910 Sameness, insistence on, 667, 871
Relationship development intervention (RDI ) model, 929, Savant skills, 354, 644
940, 958–959 SCAN (Screening Test for Auditory Processing Disorders),
Relationship enhancement methods, 1061–1063 844
Relaxation techniques, 913, 1016 Scanning patterns in response to social visual /verbal cues,
Reliability, 736–740 685
Repetitive behavior, 616, 635. See also Stereotypies SCERTS model. See Social Communication, Emotional
Research: Regulation, Transactional Support (SCERTS) model
future directions, 109–111, 440–441, 617– 620, Schizoid personality disorder, 97, 178, 179, 586, 590
887–892, 914–917, 1311–1313 Schizophrenia:
gap between clinical application and, 1181–1182, 1186 Asperger syndrome, 101, 187
role of, 13–14 classification:
Residential services, 1076, 1077, 1205, 1255–1264 historical perspective, 8, 15, 27, 70, 89, 584
Residual autism, 15–16 problems of categorical systems, 590
Subject Index I • 53

differential diagnosis, PDD-NOS, 179, 180–181 Self-monitoring (provider training), 1128


echolalia, 255 Self-organizing facility, 654
outcomes, 209–210 Self-regulation, 940, 954, 981
retrospective studies, 428 Self-support, 116
Schizophrenic psychosis in childhood, 585 Semantic memory, 370
Schizophrenic syndrome of childhood, 585, 1243 Semantic-pragmatic disorder, 97–98, 175
Schizotypal personality disorder, 179, 590 Semantics, 338
School(s)/school-based programs, 303, 1003–1023, 1044, Sensitivity/specificity, 710–711
1049–1051, 1312 Sensory conditions, behavioral assessment and, 824
academic supports, 1018–1020 Sensory Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), 843
applied behavioral analysis (ABA), 1005–1007 Sensory Integration and Praxis Test (SIPT), 844, 846, 847
cognitive strengths and challenges, 1016–1017 Sensory and motor features in autism, 536–537, 831–850
communication skills, 1010, 1012 clinical assessment, 840–848
curriculum content, 1010–1011 development, 831–840
daily routines, 1010, 1012 intervention considerations, 848–849
discrete trial training (DT), 1004, 1005, 1007–1009 motor assessments, 845–848
emotions, stress, anxiety during adolescence, 1014–1015 play, and, 394–395
functional routines (FR), 1004, 1005 questionnaires and structured interviews, 842–843
group work and class discussions, 1019 sensory assessment, 841–845
instructional techniques, 1008–1010 sensory differences, and school-based interventions,
learning characteristics of children with autism related 1013–1014, 1015
to curriculum needs, 1007–1008 sensory features, 831–834
life skills and recreation, 1023 sensory perception, 366–367
models, 1044, 1049–1051 sensory problems (Grandin), 1280–1282
Denver Model at University of Colorado, 1050–1051 Sensory Processing Assessment (SPA), 844
strengths/ limitations, 1052 Sensory Profile, 843
TEACCH, 1049–1050 Sensory Sensitivity Questionnaire-Revised, 843
organizations and time management, 1019–1020 Separation (sibling’s perspective), 1271–1273
pivotal response training, 1004, 1005 (see also Pivotal Sequenced Inventory of Communicative Development
response training (PRT)) (SICD), 83, 804
play skills, 1011, 1012 Serotonin, 453–456
preacademic skills, 1010–1011, 1012 Serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs), 1106–1109
problem solving, 1016–1017 Sertraline, 1108
progress in recent decades, 1312 Service-research interaction (TEACCH component),
sensory differences, 1013–1014, 1015 1175–1176
social interaction skills, 1011, 1012 Setback phenomenon, 223
social supports, 1020–1022 Severe developmental language disorder (SDLD), 588
strategies, 1004–1008, 1011–1013 Sex education, 1313
teaching areas, 1011–1013 Sex hormones, 461
transition to adulthood, 1022–1023 Sexual abuse allegations, 1185
visual thinking, 1017 Sexuality, 301, 304
School-age children. See Children of school age, with an Sheltered workshops, 1095
autistic spectrum disorder Shutdown, 940
School Function Assessment (SFA), 845 Sibling’s personal perspective, 1265–1275
School-to-work transition process, 1091–1093 Sib pair samples, genome-wide screens of, 432–433
Scoliosis, 154 Signing, manual, 885, 955
Screeners versus instruments, diagnostic, 112–113 Simpson-Angus Scale for Extrapyramidal Symptoms, 848
Screening measures (for autism in young children), Situations Options Choices Strategies and Stimulation
707–724 (SOCCSS) program, 996
autism-specific, 713–717 Skill acquisition /enhancement, 910–913, 1178
early identification of autism and, 707–709 Skill-based learning opportunities, 937–938
general characteristics, 709–712 Skinnerian categories of verbal behaviors, 963–964
level 1, 712–717 Sleep disorders/disturbances, 503, 542–543
level 2, 717–723 Small Wonders Preschool class, 1046
non-age-specific, 719–723 Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome, 544
nonspecific, 712–713 Social anxiety, 428
overview table, 714–715 (see also specific measures) Social Attribution Task (SAT), 666
positive and negative predictive value, 711–712 Social class, 7, 62– 65
psychometric characteristics, 710–712 Social cognition, 104–106, 519–525, 695– 698
retrospective analysis of home videotapes, 708 Social communication:
sensitivity and specificity, 710–711 atypical (stressors confronting families with autism),
Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds (STAT), 1057
714–715, 718–719, 723 high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome,
Scripts, 871 917–999
Secretin, 1112, 1183 core challenges, 978–983
Seizures, 7, 76, 144. See also Epilepsy educational programming, 986–998
Self-help skills, 300–301 emotional regulation, 980–983
Self-initiated actions, 673 environmental modifications, 997
Self-injurious behavior, 189, 901, 906, 909–910, family support, 989
1267–1268 family supports, 997–998
Self-knowledge, identity development and, 1022 interpersonal supports, 989–995
Self-management, 871–872, 885–886, 913 joint attention, 979
I • 54 Subject Index

Social communication (Continued) Stay-put requirement ( legal issues), 1167


learning and educational supports, 989, 995–997 Stereotyped movement disorder, 27
learning style differences, impact on intervention Stereotypies:
planning, 983–986 autism, 27, 230, 500, 635, 847
mutual regulation, 981 executive function and, 616, 635
self regulation, 981 versus involuntary movements (tics/dyskinesias), 847
symbol use, 979 research about, lack of, 1307
transactional supports, 989 Rett syndrome, 127, 150, 151–153
visual and organizational supports, 995–997 treatment, noncontingent reinforcement, 908
Social Communication, Emotional Regulation, Stigmatization, potential for, 12–13
Transactional Support (SCERTS) model, 929, Stimulants, 1109–1111
932–941, 959, 966, 988–989 Stimulus/stimuli, 383, 902, 1231
Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), 714–715, Story Structure Decision Tree, 813
722, 740, 753, 740 Strengths/needs, emphasis on, 820
Social development in autism, 312–327 Stress:
social dysfunction as diagnostic feature, 313–316 exercise for, 1015
specific social processes, 317–325 response systems (sympathetic/adrenomedullary and
studying social behavior in autism, 316–317 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function), 459
theoretical models of autistic social dysfunction, sources of, for families, 1055–1059
325–326 Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure (SNAP), 813
Social-emotional assessment, 794–795 Structural brain imaging (CNS function), 491–500
Social-emotional maladjustment (SEM), and Asperger Structured behavioral interventions, 883
syndrome, 114 Structured classrooms/teaching, 937, 1067, 1177, 1178.
Social impairment /functioning, 89, 94, 99, 230, 652– 655, See also Curriculum and classroom structure
1306 Structured descriptive assessment (SDA), 824
Social interventions, school-based, 1011, 1012, 1020–1021 Studies to Advance Autism Research and Treatment
Socialization (sibling’s perspective), 1268–1269 (STAART), 1196
Social orienting model, 655– 657, 660– 662 Subacute sclerosing panencephalitis, 76
Social outcomes, 201–204 Subtyping autism, 28–31, 440
Social perception, 519–526 Suicide, 208, 212
Social phobias (differential diagnosis, PDD-NOS), Supervisory attention system (SAS), 670
181–182 Supported employment, 207, 302, 1093–1094. See also
Social processes and executive function, 613– 616 Employment
Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), 742, 746 Symbiotic psychosis (Mahler), 8–9, 584
Social skills: Symbolic behavior, 947
behavioral interventions, 912–913 Symbolic content (imitation), 388
curricula, 868–872 Symbolic play. See Play
mainstream, 1029–1033 Symbol use (social communication), 979
personal perspective (Grandin), 1279 Syndromic associations, 544
Social stories, 869–870, 996 Syntax, 338, 345–346, 963
Social supports (school-based), 1020–1022 System(s):
Social validity, 876, 916 abstract, 631
Social world as an open-domain task (enactive mind), aided/unaided (AAC), 801
687– 689 motoric, 631
Son-Rise program, 958 natural, 631
Sound sensitivity, 1281–1282. See also Sensory and motor organizable, 631
features in autism social, 631
Spain, 740, 1235–1238 technical, 631
Spasticity (Rett syndrome), 153 Systemizing. See Empathizing-systemizing theory
Special Olympics, 994
Spectrum /continuum: Tact(s), 964, 965
of autistic disorders, 88 Tactile Defensiveness and Discrimination Test-Revised
of interventions, 1183–1184 (TDDT-R), 844
orientation, 930–931 Task analysis, 949
of professionals, 1183 TEACCH (Treatment and Education of Autistic and
of services, 1177 Related Communication Handicapped Children) at
of social roles, 1182–1183 University of North Carolina, 1049–1050, 1174–1180
Speech. See also Communication; Language: components, 1175–1177
assessment, 777–778 educational interventions, 1049–1050
characteristics (school-age children), 256 philosophy and principles, 1177–1179
minimal speech approach (MSA), 952 principles (working with families), 1066–1068
patterns of development, 335 professional training, 1131, 1132
as predictor of outcome (age 5), 216 related issues, 190, 207, 289, 302, 619– 620, 867, 889,
responsiveness to, 805 932–933, 953, 1036, 1037, 1043, 1055, 1061, 1064,
social (interest in), 319 1066–1068, 1070–1071, 1073, 1131, 1132, 1184,
Sports, 994 1237
Squeeze machine (Grandin), 1281, 1282 supported employment, 302
Stanford-Binet, 374 Teacher(s), 888, 948–950, 1033–1034
STAR program, 1005–1006, 1008, 1012 Teacher’s personal perspective (adult outcomes),
State and local educational agencies (SEA, LEA), 1162 1287–1303
Statewide services, 1036–1037 data collection, 1287–1288, 1303
Statistical approaches (subtyping), 29–30 implications, 1299–1302
Subject Index I • 55

students: Tower tasks, 373, 607, 615


Bill, 1288–1290 Train-and-hope strategy, 826
David, 1290–1291 Training (preparation of autism specialists), 1123–1155
Eric, 1297–1299 addressing current intervention trends, 1129–1130
Jimmy, 1292–1293 behavior analysis, 1125–1126
John, 1293–1295 behavior-specific praise, 1127
Karen, 1291–1292 best practices, 1130–1132
Polly, 1295–1296 big picture, 1139
Tom, 1296–1297 college student trainees/providers, 1137–1138
Teach Me Language, 959, 963–964 comprehensive systems, 1130–1132
Team sports, 994 consultants on adult care, 1137
Teasing/ bullying from peers, 1021–1022 demand for qualified personnel, 1124–1125
Technology/computers, 115–116, 871, 916–917, 955, didactic training, 1141–1142, 1143
1142 dissemination, 1139, 1142–1143
Temperament and Atypical Behavior Scale Screener (TABS distinctive features, 1139
Screener), 713 empirical evolution of best practice provider preparation,
Temporal analysis ( behavioral assessment), 824 1125–1128
Temporal constraints on models of social adaptation, ethics, 1139
694– 695 evaluating curriculum, 1153–1155
Test of Early Language Development, 804 feedback on work performances, 1127
Test of Playfulness (ToP), 845–846 hands-on training, behavioral competencies, 1143–1146
Test of Visual Motor Skills-Revised (TVMS), 847 inclusion providers, 1129–1130
Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (nonmotor) Revised, 844 job duties, specification of, 1126
Thalamus, 498 limitations of staff training research, 1132
Thalidomide, 436, 544 maintaining new skills, 1152–1153
Theoretical perspectives, 579–581 managing training system, 1153
categorical classification systems (problems), 583– 600 manuals, behavior modification, 1126
enactive mind, 682– 699 mastery criterion, 1146–1147
executive functions, 606– 621 methods most effective, 1141–1147
joint attention and neurodevelopmental models of autism, natural environments, 1130
650– 674 objectives, 1140, 1141
language/communication, theories of origin, 355–356 operant conditioning procedures, 1125
models of autistic social dysfunction (social operational definitions of behavior competencies,
development), 325–326 1144
role of theory in diagnosis/classification, 11 organizational behavior management (OBM), 1128
weak central coherence account of autism, 640– 646 organizational citizenship, 1139
Theory of mind, 628– 636 parent-professional relationships, 1129
attributional component-629 performance appraisals, 1128, 1144, 1152
austism versus Asperger, 104–106 primary group factors, 1132–1134
autism spectrum conditions, 628– 629 prioritizing according to immediate need, 1141
central coherence theory, 635 privileges as positive reinforcement, 1127–1128
cognitive development, 634– 635 professionalism, 1139
curricula, 872–873 quality of life, 1134–1136
empathizing-systemizing theory, 631– 634 research /development, 1126–1128
executive function and, 615 rewarding trainees/staff for exceptional performances,
executive function theory, 634– 635 1148–1150
extreme male brain (EMB) theory, 634 safety, 1139
impairment, 613 self-monitoring, 1128
joint attention, 660, 665– 668 sequencing dilemmas, 1141
mindblindness/empathizing theory, 629– 631 supervision, ongoing, 1152–1153
neuropyschological perspective, 519, 527 technological innovations, 1142
social development, 325 trainee considerations, 1136–1140
task, second-order, 698 trainer selection /preparation, 1150–1152
Williams-Beuren syndrome, 549 Walden Incidental Teaching Checklist, 1145
Thyroid hormone and TRH test, 460–461, 539 Walden Special Art Activities (SA) Checklist, 1146
Time management, 1019–1020 workshops/ lectures, 1126–1127
Time trends (epidemiological studies), 56– 62 Transactional supports, 989
approaches, 58 Triads, 586, 629
comparison of cross-sectional surveys, 59– 60 Tryptophan metabolism, 455–456
incidence studies, 61 Tuberous sclerosis (TS), 26, 50, 51, 135, 185, 442,
overview table, 57 543–545
referral statistics, 58–59 Turner syndrome, 354, 544
repeat surveys in defined geographic areas, 60– 61 Twin(s):
successive birth cohorts, 61 monozygotic twinning as risk factor, 434
Toddler Infant Motor Evaluation (TIME), 846 studies (genetics), 425–426
Toe walking, 500
Toilet training (sibling’s perspective), 1267 UCLA Young Autism Project, 1131, 1132, 1141, 1142
Tooth grinding, 902 United Kingdom, 1243–1247
Topology of salience, 688, 695 United States, 1194–1196
Touch sensitivity (Grandin), 1280–1281 Upright /inverted figures, 693– 694
Tourette’s syndrome/disorder, 27, 101, 255, 268, 354, 588, Urine, 455, 459, 539
838 Uzgiris-Hunt Ordinal Scale of Infant Development, 792
I • 56 Subject Index

Vacations (sibling’s perspective), 1270 Voice output communication aids (VOCAs), 925, 937, 955
Validity, 102–111, 740–741 Voluntary movements/praxis (sensory/motor), 835–838
Valproate syndrome, fetal (FVS), 555–556
Valproic acid, maternal use, 436 Walden model, 1045
Velocardiofacial syndrome (VCFS), 552 categorical orientation and, 929, 930
Ventricles, 499–500 communication enhancement, 929, 930, 940, 953
Verbal apraxia or apraxis of speech, 336 inclusion, 1035–1036
Verbal auditory agnosia, 502 preschool curricula, 873–874
Verbal behavior, 959, 963–964. See also Language professional training, 1131, 1132
Verbal communication, idiosyncrasies in, 89 Incidental Teaching Checklist, 1145
Verbal comprehension, 788 Special Art Activities (SA) Checklist, 1146
Verbal intelligence, 103 WCST. See Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
Verbal-semantic coherence (EF), 641 Weak central coherence (WCC) hypothesis, 640– 646, 669.
Victims of crime, 299 See also Central coherence
Video instruction, 886–887 Wechsler scales, 292, 374, 545, 643, 731, 787–789, 1204
Videotape analysis studies, 225–227, 708 Williams-Beuren syndrome (WBS), 548–549
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS), 83, 315, 316, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), 606– 609, 611, 614,
762, 787, 793, 800, 804, 845 620
Violent /criminal behavior, 101 Word use, 344–345, 805
Visual deficits, 50, 589 slowed down version of normal, 961
Visual-Motor Integration, Developmental (VMI ) test, 847 unusual and idiosyncratic, 962
Visual-Motor Skills-Revised-Upper Limits, 847 Working memory, 611, 788
Visual /somatosensory cognitive potentials, 482–483 Workshops/ lectures (provider training), 1126–1127
Visual supports (enhancing communication abilities),
936–937, 995–997 Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS), 848
Visual thinking, 1017, 1278–1280 Yale In Vivo Pragmatic Protocol, 810, 811
Visuo-spatial constructional coherence (EF), 642 Young Autism Project at UCLA, 1047–1049
Visuospatial orienting task, 610
Vocalizations (infancy and early childhood), 235 Ziprasidone, 1105–1106
Vocational Rehabilitation Act of 1973, 1090–1091 Zoloft, 1283
Vocational training, 116. See also Employment

Вам также может понравиться