Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 22

Rethinking stereotype reliance

Author(s): Nichole M. Bauer


Source: Politics and the Life Sciences, 32(1):22-42.
Published By: Association for Politics and the Life Sciences
https://doi.org/10.2990/32_1_22
URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.2990/32_1_22

BioOne (www.bioone.org) is a nonprofit, online aggregation of core research in the biological, ecological, and
environmental sciences. BioOne provides a sustainable online platform for over 170 journals and books published
by nonprofit societies, associations, museums, institutions, and presses.
Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Web site, and all posted and associated content indicates your acceptance of
BioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/page/terms_of_use.
Usage of BioOne content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non-commercial use. Commercial inquiries
or rights and permissions requests should be directed to the individual publisher as copyright holder.

BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit publishers, academic institutions, research
libraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to critical research.
Rethinking stereotype reliance
Understanding the connection between female candidates and gender
stereotypes
Nichole M. Bauer
Department of Political Science
Indiana University, Bloomington
1100 E. Seventh Street
Bloomington, IN 47405
nmbauer@indiana.edu

ABSTRACT. Increasing numbers of female candidates are running for Congress in American national elections.
Despite the rise in female candidates running for office, women are not significantly increasing their presence
in the House and Senate. A much hypothesized influence over the electoral fates of female candidates is the
role of gender stereotypes. However, political science scholars have struggled to pinpoint the effect of
stereotypes on vote choice, if there is any effect. This essay compares the way social psychology and political
science scholars theoretically, conceptually and empirically test for gender stereotype influence over
evaluations of female candidates and politicians. Differences emerge in the theoretical assumptions made in
the two disciplines, the types of measures used in research, and the empirical tests conducted to demonstrate
the presence or absence of stereotypes in evaluations of women. The discussion explores how scholars
studying female candidates and politicians can integrate insights from social psychology to clarify the role of
stereotypes in candidate evaluation and choice.

Key words: Gender stereotypes, female candidates, women in politics, gender prejudice, stereotype
activation, stereotype reliance, counter-stereotypic information

A
record-setting 98 women won a seat to emotion: dangerous.’’1 Such behavioral and perceptual
Congress in 2012. Despite this recent surge limitations may indeed make it difficult for female
of successful female candidates, women re- candidates to run effective and successful campaigns.
main underrepresented at all levels of elected office. Conventional wisdom hints that gender stereotypes
Among other explanations, gender stereotypes have create a performative bind for women to be resolute
been identified as a possible contributing factor. and caring at the same time. The roots of this bind rest
Gender stereotypes might hinder the electoral success with the assumption that stereotypes are automatically
of female candidates because they are at odds with the and unavoidably relied on to evaluate female candi-
behavioral expectations people have for political dates. However, empirical evidence showing gender
candidates. One Washington Post article describes the stereotypes are to blame for the electoral struggles of
delicate balancing act female candidates face, ‘‘Female female candidates yields no clear conclusions. Some
candidates traverse a narrow path, avoiding behaviors studies find gender stereotypes help female candi-
that might give rise to stereotypes: Be firm, but not dates,2,3 other studies find they reduce support for
angry. Be compassionate, but not weepy. Too much female candidates,4,5 and yet another body of research
shows gender stereotypes have no discernible effect on
doi: 10.2990/32_1_22 support for female candidates.6,7 Given these contrast-

22 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

ing findings, a question arises: Just, what role do stereotypes. Next, the essay reviews the divergent
gender stereotypes play in electoral evaluations of foundational assumptions made in political behavior
female candidates? Mixed results point to the need for and social psychology regarding the automatic versus
clarification regarding when and how gender stereo- controlled nature of stereotypes before discussing
types factor into the voter decision-making calculus. differences in empirical approaches. In the final
One reason for the empirical murkiness rests with sections, I identify several areas for new and innovative
the theoretical premises and empirical methods polit- research before discussing the implications of stereo-
ical science scholars use to test for the influence of types on female candidates.
gender stereotypes. Political behavior studies assume The study of gender and elections is at a critical
gender stereotypes always and automatically influence juncture, as political scientists struggle to pinpoint why
evaluations of female candidates.7 Huddy and Capelos women have yet to achieve gender parity in the House,
outline the prevailing logic: Senate, state legislatures, and even in executive
positions. Explanations for women’s political under-
Voters hold gender stereotypes that depict women as
representation include such factors as high barriers to
more nurturing than aggressive, logical or ambitious.
entry in electoral politics and a shortage of willing
Gender stereotypes of women are at odds with voters’
candidates.10,11,12,13,14,15,16 Indeed, there are many
expectations of politicians as tough, ambitious, and
factors that influence whether women run for office
strategic individuals. Thus, citizens will vote against a
and whether they succeed. Yet, the recent increases in
woman politician for being overly nice and insufficient-
women running for office suggest many of these
ly tough (p. 29).7
obstacles to the ballot are diminishing.17,18 More
Undergirding this logic is the assumption stereotypes women are not only running for Congress, more
inevitably doom the campaigns of female candidates. female candidates are running in competitive and
Assuming individuals always use gender stereotypes to well-funded campaigns—yet gender parity is still out
make choices about female candidates means many of reach. Research on institutional barriers is important
scholars do not test for the presence of stereotypes; but offers a limited perspective regarding the many
rather, scholars focus on discerning the effects of factors influencing the electoral success of female
stereotypes. candidates. The underrepresentation of women in
Contrasting the approach of political scientists is elected office is incomplete without an examination
work in social psychology testing the process of how of the individual-level choice process regarding female
stereotypes come to influence judgments. Social psy- candidates. The changing dynamics of the electoral
chology research does not start with the assumption of environment facing women in recent elections, such as
automatic stereotype reliance.8 Rather, social psychol- the increasing ideological diversity of the candidate
ogists start with the premise that stereotype reliance pool, illustrates the need for fresh insights into how
depends on a number of contextual factors, including individuals make electoral choices about female
characteristics of the decision-making environment and candidates.
the decision-maker.9 Research in this field consistently
shows stereotypes to influence perceptions of women in
a controlled and conditional manner. The process of
Literature selection process
relying on stereotypes to evaluate women can be quite A large body of research exists on gender stereotypes
complex, however—and not inevitable. in the behavior, psychology, and communication
This paper compares how political behavior and subfields of political science; indeed, reviewing every
social psychology scholars approach the study of piece of research written on this topic would be
gender stereotypes, and the implications for under- difficult. An extensive review of the political behavior,
standing evaluations of female candidates. The next gender, and campaigns literature was conducted to
section discusses the methodology for identifying the identify the relevant studies for this analysis. For the
appropriate and relevant research in both fields. The social psychology literature, the review took a slightly
following section outlines a definition of gender different approach. This review started by identifying

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 23


Bauer

recent research on gender stereotypes and meta- frequent reference. 24 Next, a list of terms was
analyses of existing literature in this area. developed for searching databases housing social
Systematically investigating how political science psychology journals. The terms used to find articles
scholars theoretically approach the study of stereotypes included ‘‘automatic’’ and ‘‘stereotypes,’’ ‘‘gender
and empirically measure these forces is the focal point stereotypes,’’ ‘‘automatic’’ and ‘‘gender stereotypes,’’
of this review. A search for relevant research relied on ‘‘stereotypes’’ and ‘‘prejudice,’’ and ‘‘women’’ and
databases such as JSTOR and EBSCOhost and used the ‘‘leaders’’ and ‘‘stereotypes.’’ Another way articles were
following terms: ‘‘gender’’ and ‘‘stereotypes,’’ ‘‘gender identified was to rely on meta-analyses published in
stereotypes,’’ and ‘‘female candidates’’ and ‘‘stereo- social psychology journals that focused on stereotypes.
types.’’ The determinants of article selection included Bargh and Blair each conducted a meta-analysis of
whether the researchers aimed to explain evaluations,
social psychology research on automatic and controlled
perceptions, and voting behaviors for female candi-
stereotypes and this list provided a helpful starting
dates. The final set of articles includes both survey and
point.8,25,26 Much of the research on stereotypes in
experimental research. Table 1 includes an entry for all
social psychology discusses stereotypes broadly, includ-
the articles included in this analysis, the article’s
ing how gender stereotypes, racial stereotypes, and age
approach to stereotypes, and the general findings.
based stereotypes might influence behavior. For these
Important to note is that this literature review does
not encompass the broader voting behavior literature; articles, any focus was limited to the sections devoted
however, much of the research on gender stereotypes to gender stereotypes.
notes that the voting behavior research tends to
overlook the role of candidate gender as an influence
Defining gender stereotypes
over electoral choice. Several canonical works on
voting behavior, including The American Voter, Voting, Generally, stereotypes are shared beliefs about the
and An Economic Theory of Democracy, do not attributes and behaviors of individuals based on their
consider candidate gender as an influence.19,20,21 These membership in groups defined by a singular character-
studies focused on vote choice for presidential candi- istic such as race, gender, or age.23 Most everyone can
dates, all of whom at that time were men; female identify stereotypes associated with women, different
candidates for other levels of office were rare. As Dolan racial groups, or age cohorts. Whether right or wrong,
explains, ‘‘One of the major reasons for the lack of stereotypes are part of shared cultural knowledge, and
information on public support for women candidates is stereotypes, such as those about women, even span
that, until recently, the number of women candidates in cultural boundaries. This section develops a definition
American politics was small, so there were too few
of gender stereotypes and outlines how stereotypes
cases to study systematically’’ (p. 4).22 After determin-
operate as categorization tools to differentiate between
ing how stereotypes are used to evaluate female
men and women and, moreover, how stereotypes may
candidates, a next step is to revisit the broader voting
set behavioral expectations for women.
behavior literature to incorporate candidate gender
Stereotypes about women are multidimensional
into models of candidate choice.
constructs reflecting beliefs about traditional gender
Identifying the relevant social psychology research
followed a slightly different approach. The literature roles, behaviors, and traits.27,28,29 As definitional
on stereotypes in this discipline is truly voluminous. boundaries, gender role stereotypes relegate women
The first step in this research selection process was to to positions in the home as mothers, wives, and
identify studies cited in political science research. caregivers.30,31 Stereotypic traits associated with wom-
Recent political science research depends on psychol- en include compassion, warmth, and emotional sensi-
ogy to define gender stereotypes. For example, many tivity.32,33 Work in social psychology has uncovered
rely on Hamilton and Sherman’s definition of stereo- several consistent findings about how individuals come
types as culturally shared ideas individuals have about to learn about stereotypes as general ideas and how
the appropriate roles and behaviors for women.23 these ideas permeate cultural conceptions about
Allport’s research on racial stereotypes is another women.

24 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

Table 1. Political science research on gender stereotypes.


Study Approach to stereotypes Method Measures of stereotypes Overall findings
Schneider and Bos, Tests content of stereotypes Experimental Trait associations Expectations for female
forthcoming for female politicians politicians are not in line
with gender stereotypes
Schneider, forthcoming Influence of campaign rhetoric Experimental Issue associations Female candidates who use
on application of gender counter-stereotypic rhetoric
stereotypes are evaluated highly on
traditionally masculine and
feminine issues
Bos, 2011 How stereotypes influence Observational Trait and issue competency Delegates who receive
vote choices among measures information about female
delegates at nominating candidates from secondhand
conventions sources are more likely to
evaluate female candidates
negatively
Brooks, 2011 Evaluations of behaviors of Experimental Trait associations Stereotypes did not affect
male and female politicians evaluations of female
(measures baseline candidates and individuals
stereotype expectations) did not have gender
stereotypic expectations
Hayes, 2011 Interaction of party and Observational Trait associations Gender stereotyping is limited
gender stereotypes and news in the face of partisan
content stereotypes
analysis
Dolan, 2010 Influence of gender stereotypes Observational Issue and trait associations; Stereotypes, asked about
on support for female attitudes toward gender generally, help female
candidates balance candidates
Fridkin and Kenney, Influence of gender stereotypes Observational Trait and issue ratings Female Senate incumbents
2009 on support for female rated highly on masculine
Senate incumbents and feminine traits and
issues
Fridkin et al., 2009 Influence of gender stereotypes Experimental No direct measures; measures Stereotypes protect female
on support for female perception of candidate as candidates who are targeted
candidates who are targeted honest and likely to be a by negative ads
by negative ads leader
Sanbonmatsu and Interaction of party and Observational Issue competency ratings Gender stereotyping overrides
Dolan, 2009 gender stereotypes the influence of partisan
stereotypes
Falk and Kenski, 2006 Influence of national political Observational No stereotype measures Female candidates receive less
climate valuing masculine support during war
traits on support for female
candidates
Dolan, 2004 The role of gender stereotypes Observational Issue attributions and Candidate sex found to have a
in determining electoral differences in support for limited role on voting
support for women female versus male decisions; significant
candidates variation in year-to-year
support for female
candidates
Lawless, 2004 Stereotypic inferences and Observational Traits and issues People prefer candidates with
female candidate issue masculine traits, thus gender
ratings stereotypes hurt female
candidates
Gordon et al., 2003 Influence of gender stereotypes Experimental Trait ratings of candidates as Female candidates not harmed
on female candidates who caring, trustworthy, and by gender stereotypes when
use negative ads knowledgeable they attack on masculine
issues
Herrnson et al., 2003 Whether gender stereotypes Observational Issue ratings Stereotypes help women
restrict female candidate
campaign strategy
Matland and King, Interaction of gender and Experimental General candidate Gender stereotypes override
2003 partisan stereotypes qualification questions partisan stereotypes for
Democratic voters
Huddy and Capelos, Interaction of party and Experimental Trait associations Stereotypes, in the presence of
2002 gender stereotypes through other cues, have no effect
a parallel processing model

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 25


Bauer

Table 1. Political science research on gender stereotypes (cont.).


Study Approach to stereotypes Method Measures of stereotypes Overall findings
Sanbonmatsu, 2002 Effect of gender stereotypes on Experimental Issue competency, belief Female voters are more
support for female stereotypes supportive of female
candidates candidates
Smith and Fox, 2001 Gender bias in vote choice Observational Vote choice No gender bias detected in
analysis vote choice
Koch, 2000 Stereotype use as heuristics Survey Ideology associations Female candidates are
perceived as more liberal
Fox and Smith, 1998 Gender bias Experimental Vote choice Female candidates suffer at
the ballot box; stereotypes
blamed—though not
measured
McDermott, 1998 Stereotypes as an Observational Perception of candidate Female candidates perceived
informational heuristic ideology as liberal
McDermott, 1997 Stereotypes as an Experimental Perception of candidate Female candidates perceived
informational heuristic ideology as liberal
Kahn 1994 Effect of gender differences in Experimental Overall candidate viability Stereotypes hurt female
news media coverage of and news candidates as the level of
male and female candidates content office increases
analysis
Huddy and Terkildsen, Do individuals value Experimental Trait associations Masculine traits preferred for
1993 masculine or feminine traits candidates
in candidates
Huddy and Terkildsen, Origins of issue gender Experimental Trait associations Female candidates portrayed
1993 stereotypes with masculine traits
evaluated highly on those
traits
Alexander and The attribution of masculine Observational Trait attributions and gender Stereotype harm on female
Anderson, 1993 and feminine traits to role ideology scale candidates depends on
female candidates gender role attitudes of
voters
Kahn, 1992 Effect of stereotypic press Experimental Issue and trait associations Stereotypes are used to
coverage on male and and news differentiate between male
female candidates content and female candidates
analysis
Leeper, 1991 Gender prejudice in candidate Experimental Trait and issue associations Female candidate rated
evaluation unviable as a contender,
gender stereotypes implied
as the cause
Rosenwasser et al., Sexism/gender bias Experimental Gendered task ratings Female candidates rated
1987 poorly at the tasks expected
of politicians
Kaid et al., 1984 Stereotype use in candidate Experimental Trait measures Female candidates rated
evaluation highest when they rely on
counter-stereotypic
strategies
Sigelman and Sigelman, Gender bias Experimental Vote choice measures No evidence of gender bias
1982 found
Sapiro, 1981 Gender bias Experimental Issue competency ratings Female candidate rated poorly
on all dimensions of issue
competency

Stereotypes can be useful in impression formation.34 providers and disciplinarians.35 Once individuals learn
Individuals rely on stereotypes to learn about appro- to associate women with stereotypic behaviors and
priate social norms and behaviors. Individuals learn traits, repeated exposure to stereotypic individuals,
about stereotypes from parents, significant adult either through personal interactions or media repre-
figures, the media, and public representations of sentations, reinforces the stereotype. Thus, the stereo-
women that reinforce the categorization of people into type socialization process is self-reinforcing:
gender-based groups.23 By the time children begin individuals learn to expect women will embody a set
school, they can make stereotypic associations between of characteristics, many women do embody these
women as nurturers and caregivers and fathers as characteristics, and this cyclical process confirms the

26 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

pre-existing beliefs and thus reinforces stereotypic leadership roles that might require them to behave in
expectations. counter-stereotypic ways.
Stereotypes are also categorization tools that distin- The content of descriptive and prescriptive stereo-
guish between men and women; they differentiate types are essentially the same, but the way individuals
between the traits individuals expect men and women use these mental representations to form judgments
to embody. Divided into agentic and communal differs. Descriptive stereotypes may emerge in judg-
characteristics, stereotypes distinguish the appropriate ment after observing the behavior of a woman. For
roles and behaviors for women relative to men. instance, an individual might see a female politician cry
Communality reflects the role of women as caregivers and then rely on descriptive stereotypes to explain her
and agency refers to the traditional leadership roles of behavior. In this instance, descriptive stereotypes reflect
men. The origins of this distinction lie in the biological widespread knowledge of gender stereotypes. The
and evolutionary roles of women as denizens of the distinction between descriptive and prescriptive stereo-
home and men as hunters outside the home. Commu- types is nuanced, but conceptually important for
nal stereotypes are family oriented and promote political behavior research. Gender stereotypes func-
nurturing behaviors for women.36 Agentic qualities tion more as prescriptions for behavior than descriptive
are those most valued in leaders, such as assertiveness accounts or representations, and when used this way
and outspokenness.37 Research on agentic and com- may constrain the behaviors of women.
munal stereotypes finds them to align closely with The prescriptive versus descriptive distinction can
masculine and feminine stereotypes.38,39 help scholars understand how individuals use stereo-
types to develop expectations for women and how
Conceptualizing gender stereotypes individuals use stereotypes to evaluate actual behaviors
Stereotypes about women, in particular, have both of women. Brooks identifies a tension between
descriptive and prescriptive components.6,33 First, descriptive stereotypes as beliefs defining what women
gender stereotypes are descriptive because they provide are and prescriptive stereotypes defining acceptable
a ‘‘standard of comparison in construing the impli- behaviors for women—and how this tension can create
cations of. . .behavioral information’’ for men and a dilemma for women in politics.6 Prescriptively,
women (p. 676).40 Descriptive stereotypes refer to stereotypes can have negative effects for women who
observable traits exhibited by woman and those by behave in counter-stereotypic ways. The political
men—women are more compassionate and men are climate demands female candidates break with gender
tougher.41 The emphasis on descriptive stereotypes is stereotypes to run successful campaigns and be
that they come from observable characteristics: ‘‘gen- effective legislators in office. Candidates are expected
der stereotypes consist of beliefs about the character- to exhibit toughness, aggressiveness, and assertiveness,
istics women do possess, whereas the prescriptive but these are counter-stereotypic characteristics for
component consists of beliefs about the characteristics women.7 This creates a dilemma for female candidates.
women should possess’’ (p. 666).42 Female candidates may choose to break stereotypic
Prescriptive stereotypes specify the behavioral ex- expectations, but this path is fraught with risks—
pectations for women. Prentice and Carranza explain female candidates may suffer a backlash for being
that stereotypic prescriptions for women reflect ‘‘tra- unfeminine. Alternatively, female candidates can con-
ditional emphases on interpersonal sensitivity, niceness, form to stereotypic expectations and risk negative
modesty, and sociability’’ (p. 275).33 Prescriptive evaluations as viable political contenders. The exact
stereotypes lead to the expectation, before actual effect of stereotypes on female candidates depends on
observation of behaviors and characteristics, that how and when stereotypes form candidate evaluations.
women behave in a stereotype-consistent manner. Stereotypes are gender differentiation tools: they can
Women who are not emotional might even be explain observed behaviors of women and lead
perceived negatively because they failed to conform individuals to develop expectations for the behaviors
to stereotypic expectations.43 Prescriptive gender ste- of women. However, just because people know about
reotypes have the greatest potential for limiting the gender stereotypes does not mean such ideas will
success of women attempting to take on nontraditional always be salient and relied upon in judgment. Indeed,

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 27


Bauer

stereotypes can serve many purposes, but they have an stereotype automaticity assumption—and the tenuous-
ambiguous role in electoral politics. The next section ness of this assumption.51 The authors conducted
contrasts how scholars in political science and social several experiments varying the type of gender stereo-
psychology study stereotypes. Understanding the the- typic information given to participants. Not surpris-
oretical differences in how these disciplines approach ingly, providing individuals with gender stereotypic
this area can offer news insights into the role of information about women led these individuals to
stereotypes in politics. make stereotypic judgments about women. However,
the authors do not measure the evaluations of women
absent stereotypic information. The author’s do show
Divergent assumptions: Stereotypes as that trait stereotypic information activates broader
automated versus controlled processes stereotypes about behaviors, but the baseline state of
stereotype reliance is not considered. One possible
Stereotypes are ubiquitous cultural constructs defin-
explanation for their findings rests with the use of a
ing the appropriate social roles and behaviors for
stimulus providing subjects with only stereotypic
women.3,23 Both political science and social psychol-
information. Providing subjects with counter-stereo-
ogy began the study of stereotypes from the same
typic information or measuring baseline stereotypes
observation that stereotypes are part of shared cultural
could have led to very different evaluations of women.
knowledge. This ubiquity led to an assumption in both
Bargh and colleagues challenged the assumption of
fields of stereotype reliance as automatic and inevita-
stereotype automaticity in arguing that automatic
ble. Social psychology has revised this assumption and
processes should have four characteristics: they should
now characterizes stereotypes as regulated and con-
be unintentional, uncontrollable, the individual must
text-dependent processes.44,45,46 This difference in how
be unaware of using stereotypes, and they must be
political science and social psychology approach
cognitively efficient.8 Testing whether the process of
stereotypes emerged because social psychologists em- stereotype reliance possesses these four traits found
braced a more direct focus on clarifying the process of little evidence to support the conclusion that stereo-
stereotype reliance while political science is more types are automatic but instead pointed to stereotype
concerned with the effects of stereotypes on political reliance as a conditional process. Stereotypes have
judgments. become more complex in part because cultural norms
Prior to the 1990s, social psychology posited that promoting gender egalitarianism have made individu-
individuals are hardwired to make stereotypic associ- als more aware of the potential influence of stereotypes
ations between certain personality traits and members on judgment. Awareness of stereotypes and social
of stereotyped groups.47 As Devine and Sharp explain, desirability pressure may lead some to control and even
‘‘many theorists have assumed that stereotype activa- suppress the use of stereotypes in judgment.52 For the
tion occurred effortlessly when people come in contact automatic stereotype assumption to hold, stereotypic
with members of the stereotyped group’’ (p. ideas would have to be top of mind at almost all times,
61).46,48,49,50 Research illustrating the ubiquity of ready to apply to every member of a stereotyped group
stereotypes as universal concepts is used to justify that individuals might encounter. Such a sequence of
assumptions about stereotype reliance. If ‘‘everyone judgment would be cognitively exhausting.
knows’’ that women are expected to be emotionally Early research in social psychology measured stereo-
sensitive, then encountering a woman should activate type application rather than stereotype activation—an
this shared knowledge. This logic parallels how the approach similar to how current work in political
stereotype automaticity assumption is justified in science investigates stereotypes. The assumption of
political science research. Studies in social psychology stereotype automaticity prevailed at first and scholars
prior to the 1990s built empirical findings on this sought to determine how these presumed ideas
critical assumption of stereotype reliance as automatic influenced judgments. Stereotypes were thought to be
and inevitable. characterized by ‘‘high resistance to change and
Deaux and Lewis’ study on how gender stereotypes invariance across context and strategy’’ (p. 244).25 If
influence judgments about women illustrates the stereotypes are deeply engrained associations, then

28 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

most people should categorize and evaluate individuals women senators.’’2 Favorable evaluations of female
based on their membership to a stereotyped group in a incumbents compared to male incumbents serves as
similar manner. In other words, there should be little evidence of stereotype reliance in this investigation.
variation in experimental studies asking individuals to This approach overlooks the malleability of gender
categorize people based on a visible stereotype; stereotypes and how exogenous factors might lead
however, empirical tests of stereotype application respondents to evaluate female incumbents more
reveal a lot of inconsistency in stereotype strength favorably. The connection Fridkin and Kenney make
among subjects.53 between general knowledge of stereotypes among the
Research on voting behavior and the fate of female public and reliance on stereotypes in candidate
candidates has not moved away from this assumption evaluation represents the approach to stereotypes
of automatic stereotypes. Research on heuristics, which undertaken by much of the political behavior litera-
shows that gender operates as a basic informational ture.
cue in voting, contributes to the reliance on the Testing the automaticity of stereotypes in social
assumption of automatic activation. Other judgmental psychology showed the incredible flexibility of these
shortcuts, such as partisanship, allow individuals to supposedly rigid associations. Reviews of experimental
make inferences about a political candidate, issue, or research in social psychology journals identified a
scenario from a compact cue.54,55,56,57,58 Gender in number of studies that directly tested the automaticity
particular is a rich inference cue providing information of stereotypes and found stereotypes are not uncon-
about a female candidate’s partisanship, ideology, trollable associations individuals make upon encoun-
feminist orientation, and issue priorities.59,60,61 Using tering members of stereotyped groups.25 A literature
gender to associate female candidates with Democratic review in social psychology by Blair identified 14
partisanship and a liberal ideology led some scholars to studies on gender stereotypes and found high evidence
assume stereotypes were responsible for this effect. of stereotype malleability in all of the studies. Blair’s
Feminine stereotypes associate women with such review identified five factors conditioning the use of
traits as compassion and caring for the disadvantaged stereotypes, including the motives of the perceiver,
and are hypothesized to lead individuals to connect strategies intended to counter stereotypes, the level of
issues like education and childcare with female attention in judgment tasks, the configuration of
candidates—issues that reinforce the role of women stimulus cues, and the characteristics of the member
as mothers. However, the heuristic effects of gender of the stereotyped group. Over the last decade, research
may reflect the Democratic party membership of many in social psychology has moved away from assuming
female candidates who ran for political office at the automatic stereotypes to testing the conditions of
national, state, and local levels in the 1980s and 1990s stereotype activation and application.
more than it reflects stereotypes.32 Indeed, many of the If stereotypes are not automatically activated when
traits and issues associated with women align with individuals encounter a member of a stereotyped
principles of the Democratic party—an association that group, then when and how does stereotype activation
could be due to gender stereotypes or partisan take place? Kunda and Spencer elaborate on the
stereotypes.62 difference between the activation and application of
Fridkin and Kenney apply the assumption of stereo- stereotypes observing that, ‘‘stereotype activation is the
types to a study on evaluations of female candidates. extent to which a stereotype is accessible in one’s mind,
The authors justify the assumption explaining that, and stereotype application is the extent to which one
‘‘We expect that the incumbent’s gender will influence uses a stereotype to judge a member of a stereotyped
people’s impressions of men and women senators group’’ (p. 522).63 In other words, stereotype activa-
running for reelection because of the prevalence of tion reflects the cognitive accessibility of stereotypes
gender stereotypes among citizens’’ (p. 303). Without and application reflects whether and how individuals
this assumption, not tested in this study, it is difficult to use these stereotypes in judgment. Contextual factors
accurately conclude that ‘‘the gender of the candidate can activate stereotypes by increasing or decreasing
had a direct influence on overall evaluations of the their cognitive accessibility. Passing a woman on the
sitting senators, producing more favorable views of street is unlikely to trigger stereotypes, but working

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 29


Bauer

under a female manager may activate stereotypes. This rate the effectiveness of a female senator might rely on
activation scenario resembles a priming process. A information about the bills she has sponsored to
contextual factor, which in political communication evaluate her effectiveness as opposed to her gender.
may take the form of news coverage or campaign Stereotype application depends, first, on stereotype
advertising, can make a stereotypic cue highly salient. activation. After activation, characteristics of the
With repeated exposure, stereotype constructs become decision-making or evaluative task and characteristics
cognitively accessible frameworks that individuals can of the individual decision-maker determine stereotype
then use in forming impressions of political leaders. application. Absent the need to evaluate or make a
For stereotype application to occur, an individual judgmental decision about a woman, feminine stereo-
must be required to make a judgment about a types will likely not be applied. The electoral context
stereotyped target, a stage that occurs after stereotype contains a variety of motivational factors that might
activation. As Blair and Banaji explain, ‘‘stereotypes increase the likelihood of reliance on stereotypes,
cannot be used in judgment if those stereotypes have including whether an individual plans to vote. A citizen
not been previously activated by situational living in a state with a female Senate candidate may
cues. . .Hence, stereotype activation is a necessary receive stereotypic information, thus activating stereo-
though insufficient step in stereotyping’’ (p. 1142).64 types, but this particular individual may not be
Activated stereotypes not used in judgment will motivated to vote in the election. Because this person
dissipate after the contextual prime that activated the does not plan to make a choice about the stereotyped
stereotype dissipates. Modeling stereotype reliance as a
women, he or she will not apply the activated
process requires measuring both stages of stereotype
stereotype.
use.
Kunda and Sinclair test for motivation as a factor
Stereotype activation can depend on an individual’s
determining the reliance on stereotypes in judgment.9
comprehension goals, level of attention to a member of
In particular, they examined whether students receiving
a stereotyped group, and levels of motivation in
positive feedback from a female instructor were
stereotyping.53,63,65 Comprehension goals refer to the
motivated to evaluate the instructor positively and
extent individuals care about the accuracy of their
therefore had no need for stereotypes in their evalua-
evaluations, their motivation in making a decision, and
tion. Conversely, they tested whether students who
the amount of information they have about the
received negative criticism from their female instructor
stereotyped target. Strong partisans who never vote
would be motivated to evaluate her negatively and
against their preferred party may have low compre-
hension goals in an election involving a stereotyped therefore relied on stereotypes to support the negative
woman. The strong partisan only needs information evaluative outcome. Motivated reasoning predicts that
about the candidate’s party identification, not the stereotype reliance is highly conditional and arises
candidate’s gender, to make a decision. when individuals perceive a woman as behaving in a
For a cue to trigger stereotypes, an individual must ‘‘disagreeable’’ manner. Stereotypes are then used to
pay attention to and be aware of the cue. Referring to a evaluate the woman in light of her disagreeable
female candidate as motherly in a campaign ad might behavior. Stereotyping as a byproduct of motivated
trigger stereotypes, but if an individual does not pay reasoning supports the conclusion that stereotype
attention to the ad, then stereotype activation does not reliance is strategic on the part of the individual
occur. Competing stereotype cues might decrease the decision-maker.
level of attention an individual pays to a single A variety of motivations can influence stereotype use
stereotype cue. For example, individuals might not in judgment. These include an individual’s concern
apply or even have feminine stereotypes activated in with affirming self-worth, how much an individual
the content of an African-American woman because cares about making an accurate evaluation of a
the cue of race might override gender. Situational member of a stereotyped group, conformance to social
factors such as the amount of differentiating informa- desirability pressures that discourage stereotyping, or
tion individuals have about a stereotype target can also the need to distinguish between two otherwise equal
influence stereotype application. Individuals asked to choices.9,63,65 Sometimes individuals have multiple and

30 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

conflicting decision-making goals. As Kunda and constant influence over perceptions of female candi-
Spencer explain: dates, their effects should be fairly consistent. Female
candidates who adhere to stereotypes should receive
People who would usually refrain from stereotyping
positive evaluations along dimensions reflecting stereo-
because they are motivated to avoid prejudice may
types, such as likability ratings. By contrast, female
nevertheless apply a stereotype if they are sufficiently
candidates who engage in counter-stereotypic cam-
driven by self-enhancement goals that can be satisfied
paign strategies should face negative evaluations for
by stereotyping. . .[A]nd people who would usually breaking with stereotypes. Gordon and colleagues
apply a stereotype to further comprehension may tested whether female candidates who engaged in
nevertheless refrain from applying it when they become negative advertising, a counter-stereotypic campaign
motivated to avoid prejudice (p. 532).63 tactic for female candidates, suffered from breaking
In some cases, the external decision-making environ- with expectations.70 The study did not find that female
ment may strengthen an individual’s strategic goals and candidates received a boost by going negative on
influence stereotype application. The goals, decision- masculine issues. Conversely, Hitchon and colleagues
making context, and level of control individuals have found female candidates did indeed suffer when they
over the activation and application of stereotypes leads relied on negativity and the authors attributed this
to the conclusion that stereotype reliance upon effect to gender stereotypes.43 Not explicitly testing for
the activation of stereotypes points to an empirical and
encountering a woman, or female candidate, is not
theoretical gap in these studies because stereotypes
automatic.
might not have been the driving force behind these
Assuming stereotypes always matter for female
evaluations.
candidates means many political science scholars do
Counter-stereotypic expectations for women are, in
not test for how different factors influence stereotype
a broader sense, a major factor limiting the activation
activation and instead focus on stereotype application.
of stereotypes in social psychology research. The
The stereotype automaticity assumption leads to two
political environment is inherently counter-stereotypic
sets of hypotheses in political science research about
for women. Candidates, politicians, and political
how stereotypes influence the electoral fortunes of
operatives are expected to embody such traits as
female candidates. Stereotypes either provide individ-
confidence, emotional distance, and strategic behav-
uals with an evaluative tool with which to judge female
ior.7,32 This suggests that voters might not always
candidates, or they can constrain the perceived range of expect female candidates to conform to gender
acceptable behavior of female candidates. Under both stereotypes—and that female candidates might not
of these processes, there is an assumption that gender suffer from breaking with stereotypes. Determining
stereotypes shape evaluations of female candidates. what expectations individuals have for female candi-
Thus, stereotypes become a rubric used to judge dates, and how these expectations relate to stereotypes,
women running for office.66,67,68,69 Two competing is critical to understanding the influence of stereotypes
hypotheses emerge from the premise that individuals in candidate evaluations and electoral choice.
expect women to conform to stereotypes. First, Brooks offers experimental evidence for the notion
stereotypic women should receive a boost for meeting that stereotype activation is not automatic when
voter expectations and counter-stereotypic women individuals encounter female candidates. Measuring
should be subject to a backlash for failing to meet whether subjects held baseline stereotype expectations
expectations.43 Second, stereotypic women should be about female candidate emotionality revealed that
punished because of the stark mismatch between individuals do ‘‘hold strongly gendered stereotypes
gender stereotypes and political norms, and counter- about the emotionality of ordinary people, but they do
stereotypic women should be rewarded because they fit not seem to hold them about political candidates’’ (p.
more with the political ideal. 603).6 Schneider and Bos probed the automaticity of
Problematic in this chain of logic about the influence gender stereotypes in politics with an experiment
of stereotypes is that research on gender stereotypes designed to measure whether individuals hold baseline
yields no consistent findings. If gender stereotypes are a assumptions about female candidates that align with

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 31


Bauer

stereotypes.71 If stereotypes are an ever-present mental the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false memory
representation that individuals automatically turn to, paradigm. Comparing the measurement approaches
then expectations expressed for female candidates between the two disciplines illustrates innovative
should reflect these gender stereotypes. The study measurement methods political science scholars can
relied on a technique designed to measure the baseline incorporate in their research.
stereotype content of a particular category of individ- A common method used in social psychology, the
uals, in this case female politicians (as opposed to just IAT requires individuals to make rapid, first impression
women generally). Interestingly, participants in the associations between stereotype primes and a member
study did not automatically attribute traditional of a stereotyped group. This test specifically measures
women’s traits, such as warmth or emotional sensitiv- implicit stereotypes. The prime can take the form of
ity, to female politicians. This evidence only suggests photos or adjectives but is usually a small piece of
stereotypes may not always be in play for female information flashing on a computer screen.72 For
candidates but does call into question the assumption gender, the IAT might ask individuals to classify words
of stereotype automaticity. such as ‘‘gentle’’ or ‘‘assertive’’ as either masculine or
The fundamental difference between political science feminine. Administering the IAT test before an
and social psychology concerning gender stereotypes is experimental treatment can show how rapidly subjects
that the former tests the effects of stereotypes and the use stereotypes to classify words or phrases as
latter tests the process of stereotype reliance. The aims masculine or feminine. At its base, the IAT measures
of social psychology research speak more directly to individual knowledge about stereotypes, but its power
the broader goal of uncovering the processes and comes in the ability to measure response times. Faster
cognitive tools individuals use in a variety of decision- response times indicate automatic gender stereotypes
making contexts. Conversely, political scientists have a and slower response times indicate more complex
more narrow concern with delineating how stereotypes cognitive processes at work. Rudman and Glick used
the IAT to assess the type of backlash women who
shape political outcomes. This key difference between
adopt agentic attributes receive.73 The IAT measured
understanding a process and understanding an out-
the content of prescriptive stereotypic expectations that
come is one reason why the two disciplines make such
research participants had for women and found that
different assumptions about stereotype reliance. Turn-
people in general expect women to be nice. Another
ing to methods, measures, and analytical techniques
benefit of the IAT is that it asks individuals to quickly
employed in stereotype research in each discipline lends
click through prompts, and the rapid response format
further insight into how social psychology and political
can minimize social desirability pressures because
science differ in the study of stereotypes.
respondents do not have the opportunity to edit or
over-think their responses.
Methodological approaches to stereotypes A drawback of the IAT approach is lack of
agreement on what qualifies as a ‘‘fast’’ response time.
Social psychology models stereotype reliance as a Baseline response times are usually set relative to the
multistage process that at a minimum involves two speed of other respondents. The longer it takes an
steps: activation and then application, whereas the individual to make a connection between a woman
political science literature assumes stereotype activa- and, say, the trait of sensitivity, the more this may
tion, and only tests for application. Just as these two either reflect that (a) the stereotype is not very strong,
disciplines hold different assumptions about the (b) individuals are suppressing stereotypes, or (c)
process of stereotype reliance, they also employ stereotype activation requires additional informa-
different methods to measure the presence and effect tion.74 Adapting the IAT in survey research may not
of stereotypes. This section reviews the measurement of prove practical for political scientists; however, the IAT
stereotypes in empirical research. Social psychology is adaptable to experimental design. A possible
utilizes a variety of tools to estimate stereotype approach is to use the IAT prior to a survey
activation, including the Implicit Association Test administration to determine whether respondents rely
(IAT), the Go/No Go Association Task (GNAT), and on feminine traits or political traits when evaluating a

32 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

female candidate. Identifying a female candidate with participants from allocating additional cognitive re-
more political traits than feminine traits might offer sources to the original word set, thus giving a better
evidence that feminine stereotypes are not in play for baseline estimation of stereotype activation. This
women in politics. method draws on concepts similar to the IAT but does
Another straightforward method of measuring ste- not measure response times to the tasks. The method’s
reotype activation is the Go/No-Go Association Task simplicity makes it easily altered to experimental
(GNAT).75 This test asks respondents to make associ- research and survey designs as well. A criticism is that
ations between a concept and an attribute. Unlike the the initial presentation of words can prime stereotypes,
IAT, the GNAT does not require the use of contrasting thus leading participants to falsely remember feminine
attributes and categories. For instance, an IAT for words not in the original word set. Not explicitly
gender asks individuals to sort attributes as masculine identifying the category the words belong to may avoid
or feminine, but the GNAT can center on a single sensitizing research participants.
concept such as ‘‘womanhood.’’ To measure the A drawback of these activation tests is that they may
implicit nature of gender stereotypes about women, be better suited for experimental research than surveys
the words presented in the task would all relate to (although survey experiments are becoming increas-
gender stereotypes with ‘‘distractor’’ words included in ingly common). Setting up an IAT for a survey is fairly
the mix. The participant then indicates, by hitting a straightforward as many survey platforms put timers
space bar on a computer, whether a word fits the on questions, but building this into a nonexperimental,
concept or not. Rather than categorizing attributes as cross-sectional study may be cumbersome. Moreover,
masculine or feminine, participants only have to these methods all require measurement before presen-
determine whether the attribute relates to stereotypes tation of a more complex stimulus. Administering an
about women. The number of correct categorizations implicit test of automatic stereotype activation in a pre-
and the time taken to make these associations test/post-test experimental design is possible but
determines if an attitude or idea is implicit. Steffens researchers would have to take precaution to ensure
and Jelenec used the GNAT to test the automaticity of the pre-test does not prime subjects before presenting
gender stereotypes about the math abilities of women the stimulus. Building in a week or two delay between
in school age children. 76 The concept given to the pre-test and presentation of the stimuli would limit
participants was math and the attributes related to priming effects from the initial stereotype activation
boys and girls. Essentially, the students classified males measurement. However, this method may prove
or females as better at math. The author’s used difficult to implement in studies with opt-in samples
participant response times to distinguish between from platforms such as Amazon’s Mechanical Turk
awareness and endorsement of the stereotype-based (MTurk) or the Cooperative Congressional Election
belief that women do not excel at math. Study (CCES)—although asking for email addresses
Another method is the Deese-Roediger-McDermott does enable follow-up requests.81 The benefit of these
(DRM) false memory paradigm.77,78 With this ap- methods is that they allow for a more precise
proach, participants receive a list of words associated measurement of automatic processes.
with a concept, such as femininity. After viewing the As mentioned, political science scholars measure
words, participants take a ‘‘surprise’’ memory test stereotypes based on their application. There are three
including words in the original set and words related to dominant measurement techniques: differences in vote
the relevant concept. In the case of gender, for instance, choice preferences between male or female candidates,
the original word set might have included soft, warm, differences in issue expertise attributions between male
and empathetic. Distractor terms added into the and female candidates or leaders, and differences in
surprise memory test might include emotional or trait attributions. The first approach measures differ-
sensitive. Identifying emotional or sensitive as part of ences in preference for a hypothetical male or a female
the original word set, when they were not, might candidate in experiments or surveys. In this research,
indicate higher levels of stereotype activation com- ‘‘gender stereotypes’’ are used to explain any differenc-
pared to those who do not make this identification.79,80 es in candidate preferences, though other factors may
Introducing the memory test as a surprise prevents drive preferences between male and female candi-

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 33


Bauer

dates.82 Studies with observational data recording the and characteristics, deem men more competent at
vote share received by male and female candidates is legislating around issues of national security and
another method. These studies tend to find little military crises, and contend that men are superior to
difference between male and female vote share and women at addressing the new obstacles generated by
conclude gender bias is no problem for female September 11, 2001’’ (p. 480).17 However, this
candidates.83 However, as Anzia and Berry point out, conclusion may be overblown given that on four of
this measurement approach overlooks the challenges the six issue attributions, most respondents selected the
female candidates might face on the campaign trail to neutral response option. Of course, Lawless’ finding
win over voters.84 Moreover, some experimental that respondents who placed masculine issues such as
research does conclude that gender stereotypes are an terrorism at the top of the issue agenda and believed
obstacle for female candidates. Fox and Smith present- male candidates best at that issue did express lower
ed subjects with a hypothetical male or female support for female candidates—but this does not
candidate and found that fewer subjects expressed characterize the majority of the sample. Testing the
support for the female candidate compared to the male salience of gender stereotypes and then linking these
candidate.5 Understanding whether gender stereotypes beliefs to evaluations of female candidates might offer
are an obstacle for female candidates is a first step to more information about what types of individuals are
determining the challenges female candidates must most likely to rely on stereotypes.
surmount on the campaign trail. Trait attributions are the third approach to measur-
A second method political science scholars use to ing gender stereotypes in political science. These
measure stereotypes is the issue attribution technique. questions tend to follow the same format as the issue
Here, researchers ask participants in a survey or attribution questions, and when there is a neutral
experiment to indicate whether a male or female response option, it is usually the modal choice. In one
candidate is stronger on a given issue.3,4,17 The list of study about reliance on stereotypes to evaluate female
issues include traditional women’s issues, such as candidates, respondents indicated they perceived nei-
education or health care, masculine issues, such as ther candidate more likely to embody three of the four
terrorism or military affairs, and gender-neutral issues, traits tested.3 This finding is not unique. That so many
such as agricultural policy. Differences in the number subjects do not stereotype female candidates on
of respondents choosing the female, male, or neither questions about political issue competencies or trait
candidate (politician) as stronger on an issue serves as attributions suggests the process of stereotype reliance
the basis for determining if gender stereotypes have a is at the very least complex. If stereotypes automati-
harmful or helpful impact on female candidates. In one cally influenced candidate perceptions, then female
study by Leeper, respondents were not provided with a candidates should consistently be identified with
neutral response category.4 The study asked subjects to stereotypic traits and issues.
rate the candidate they saw in their experimental Trait measures may offer more insight into stereo-
condition (either a male or a female), and then used a t- type activation than stereotype effects. Survey research
test to assess statistically significant differences in mean can easily employ these types of questions by asking
ratings. Gender differences emerged in just three of the respondents if they expect female candidates to display
nine issues. This is not overwhelming evidence of a trait such as ‘‘aggressiveness,’’ and then measure the
stereotype reliance in candidate evaluations, and response with a scale with one end anchored by ‘‘no
indicates that stereotypes might not be the evaluative aggression’’ on the low end and ‘‘high aggression’’ on
tool individuals rely on to distinguish between male the high end. This differs from approaches asking
and female candidates. respondents to indicate whether men or women are
Survey based research asking subjects to select a more likely to display a trait, which may be measuring
male candidate, female candidate, or neither as stereotype knowledge more than stereotype reliance.
strongest on an issue tends to yield the highest Trait scales can also establish whether individuals hold
proportion of responses in the neutral category. A stereotypic expectations for female candidates. Brooks
study by Lawless concludes that gender stereotypes are uses this approach with a question about candidate
a bane because ‘‘citizens prefer men’s leadership traits emotionality to measure baseline stereotype expecta-

34 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

tions.6 Her study asks respondents to rate how much Determining whether individuals have stereotypic or
they expect the candidate in question to be emotional/ counter-stereotypic expectations for female candidates
unemotional on a scale of 1–7. Brooks asks these offers a starting point for future research. Blair has
questions before the experimental manipulation to found that counter-stereotypic expectations result in
measure baseline candidate emotionality and finds reduced stereotype activation—an outcome that might
individuals do not have stereotypic expectations for have implications for female candidates.25 Several
female candidates. Allowing for a time lag between the political science studies do examine how the campaign
initial measure of stereotypes, introduction of a context can activate or suppress gender stereotypes.
stimulus, and subsequent measure of stereotype reli- Kahn has undertaken several investigations of gender
ance is a simple method to test for changes in differences in the types of news coverage male and
stereotype activation; if stereotypes are always activat- female candidates receive.69,85 Female candidates
ed, there should be little change in stereotype measures. receive news coverage highlighting their personal and
Research in political science utilizes a variety of family lives, along with their electoral viability.
methods to test stereotype application, but many of the Moreover, women tend to receive less coverage overall.
empirical results suggest stereotypes function in a Other studies find that female candidates are more apt
conditional manner. Several processes may be at work to receive coverage discussing their clothing choices
in existing political science research on gender stereo- and personal appearance over their professional
types. First, providing research participants with a qualifications86—a type of news coverage with the
neutral response option could cue social desirability capacity to activate stereotypes. A number of other
concerns. The neutral option may appear as the safe studies have measured differences in the quantity and
choice because researchers cannot infer gender bias. quality of media coverage received by female candi-
But selecting neutral could suggest that individuals are dates, the news media’s reliance on stereotypic traits,
ambivalent about whether a male or female candidate and whether female candidates play to stereotypes on
is strongest on an issue or they may not have enough websites or in campaign ads.66,69,87,88 Hayes coded
information to make an informed judgment. Another newspaper articles during an election for whether they
depicted candidates in light of masculine or feminine
possibility is that stereotypes are not strong enough
traits.89 Merging the news coverage data with survey
considerations to influence whether a respondent feels
data allowed Hayes to conclude that stereotypic news
a male or female candidate is strongest on an issue.
coverage did not shift perceptions of female candidates
Examining the effects of stereotypes without investi-
away from the dominant cue of partisanship.
gating the process of stereotype reliance makes it
Measuring baseline expectations at the individual
difficult to understand how these mental representa-
level is another approach to understanding the
tions might be constraining the political opportunities
influence of stereotypes. One possible research design
for women.
would be to conduct a survey measuring stereotypes
before a campaign season begins in earnest, then
Avenues for new research on gender, analyze how stereotypes emerge in campaign coverage,
stereotypes, and politics and again measure stereotypes in the closing weeks of
the campaign. Including an implicit association test or
Applying the insights and theoretical framework go/no go task association in a large-scale survey such as
from social psychology to political science research can the Cooperative Congressional Election Survey may
yield a rich and fruitful research agenda. Several ways not be a practical approach for measuring baseline
political science scholars can better understand gender stereotypes, but there are several scales designed to
stereotypes include: treating stereotypes as a condi- measure stereotypes based on the implicit stereotype
tional influence, using stereotype measures to better research. Rudman and colleagues created a stereotype
understand gender prejudice, exploring how parallel battery based on the implicit association test re-
versus competing stereotypes influence decision-mak- search.74 The scale asks subjects to rate a stereotype
ing, and turning to more nuanced measurement target along a series of trait dimensions: strong-weak,
techniques to test for stereotype influence. harsh-lenient, hard-soft, cold-warm, and distant-car-

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 35


Bauer

ing. Averaging these components produces an index Next, research is scant on how competing or parallel
estimating levels of stereotype activation. In this index, stereotypes work in political science. Democratic party
a higher score indicates a more feminine stereotypic stereotypes and gender stereotypes overlap, possibly
perception of the female candidate. Brooks has a indicative of a parallel processing model of stereotype
similar trait-based measure of stereotypes.6 Huddy and reliance. Hayes as well as Sanbonmatsu and Dolan test
Terkildsen’s scale asks participants to rate candidates for whether partisan stereotypes overpower gender
along a series of masculine and feminine trait stereotypes or vice versa, and each come to contrasting
dimensions, and differences in the ratings are used to conclusions.89,94 Developing a better understanding
construct a measure of stereotypes.32 These scales may about when different types of competing or overlap-
be most useful for measuring stereotype activation ping stereotypes come into play can clarify pertinent
before and after the introduction of a stimulus. questions about when gender stereotypes have an effect
Removing the assumption that stereotypes are on female candidates. The intersection of race and
automatic allows researchers to systematically test for gender stereotypes is a relatively untouched area of
possible conditions leading to stereotype activation and research. Philpot and Walton argue that for black
application. Bos examined how stereotypes influenced female candidates, racial and gender stereotypes will
the impressions of female candidates formed by mutually reinforce one another.95 Do other racial and
delegates at state nominating conventions.90 The study ethnic stereotypes have a competing influence on
showed that when delegates received information from female candidates? Other possibilities for exploring
secondhand sources, as opposed to information orig- how individuals react to candidates fitting into multiple
inating from the candidate, stereotypes were relied on stereotypes include looking at age and gender, race and
to form impressions. Other areas where stereotypes age and gender, and incorporating the influence of
might have a conditional influence include the tone of partisan stereotypes.
the campaign environment or reactions to a female A somewhat undeveloped area is the influence of
legislator’s behavior once elected to office. Do displays gender prejudice or sexism. Some research has shown
of anger and aggression, and other counter-stereotypic that levels of prejudice moderate stereotype reliance.46
behavior lead individuals to judge female leaders more Studying the intersection of gender stereotypes and
harshly than male leaders? Also, how do individuals prejudice in a political context is an area ripe with
judge female candidates who engage in leadership opportunity for new and innovative research.46 Gender
behavior conforming to stereotypes, such as engaging stereotypes are widely held sets of beliefs about the
in consensus building? typical traits or behaviors of women. For a variety of
Alternative analytical techniques can also provide a reasons, people sometimes make these simplistic
more concrete understanding of stereotypes. Stereotype associations between women and a prescribed set of
influence might be strongest as a mediating variable on characteristics. Often individuals use stereotypes as an
evaluations of female candidates in experimental informational heuristic and not to justify discrimina-
research. Krupnikov and Bauer adopt this approach tory attitudes or practices.59,60,61 Sexism, on the other
to understanding how stereotypes affect judgments of hand, reflects discrimination against women and here
female candidates who launch a negative advertising stereotypes are tools employed to justify an imbalanced
campaign.91 In this study, stereotypes only influenced power structure between women and men.96,97,98
candidate judgments under a narrow set of condi- Questions gauging levels of sexism include supporting
tions—when the female candidate and subject identi- women’s equity issues, favoring more gender parity in
fied with different political parties, and when the government, general support for feminist positions,
female candidate initiated the negativity. Here stereo- and expressing support for a future female presi-
types influenced evaluations of the female candidate as dent.82,99 None of these questions, however, addresses
a mediating variable. Imai and colleagues92 and the more complex issues of gender prejudice.
Malhotra and Krosnick93 demonstrate the ability of Glick and Fiske define sexism as a special case of
mediation analyses in their research, and these tech- prejudice marked by ambivalence toward women.
niques offer a more nuanced way to explore the Sexism has both a hostile and benevolent component.
conditionality of stereotype influence. The hostile component, the more blatant form of

36 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

sexism, is characterized by anger toward women and stereotypes at all would be difficult to detect with self-
the belief that women do not belong in the work- report scales because the outcome of low stereotype
force.96 Most people know and agree that hostile scores would be similar in both situations.
sexism is not acceptable. The benevolent element of
sexism appears to result in better treatment of women
but reflects paternalism and shifts the balance of power Discussion
in favor of men.96 Glick and Fiske define benevolent Women’s political underrepresentation is not a new
sexism as a ‘‘set of interrelated attitudes toward women phenomenon. Indeed, female politicians face unique
that are sexist in terms of viewing women stereotyp- challenges in the political arena. Empirical research has
ically and in restricted roles but that are subjectively yet to explain how individuals evaluate and make
positive in feeling tone (for the perceiver) and also tend
decisions about whether to vote for female candidates.
to elicit behaviors typically categorized as prosocial
This empirical gap is particularly striking considering
(e.g., helping) or intimacy seeking (e.g., self-disclo-
the robust body of research showing female legislators
sure)’’ (p. 461).96
have a distinct impact on perceptions of effective
Some people are comfortable expressing ‘‘benevolent
representation and on the substantive policy issues
sexist’’ attitudes because there is nothing immediately
considered by a legislature. Many of the benefits
negative that jumps out in these beliefs. Benevolent
afforded by having more women in positions of
sexism has the greatest potential to influence evalua-
political influence connect with gender stereotypes,
tions of female candidates, Glick and Fiske argue,
but it is not clear if these stereotypic outcomes shape
because it is subtle. Furthermore, people with benev-
voting decisions for female candidates.
olent sexist attitudes may sometimes vote for female
Among the substantive policy outputs women bring
candidates; those with hostile sexist attitudes are
to government are support for reproductive healthcare
unlikely to ever consider voting for a female candidate.
services for women and legislation designed to curb
Incorporating measures of sexism in research on vote
sexual harassment.103 Female legislators also provide
choice can clarify whether individuals hold biased
attitudes toward female candidates. important symbolic benefits to women as a group
Physiological measures offer another way scholars through floor statements, speeches, and cosponsorship
can gain a better understanding about the influence of activities.104,105 Anzia and Berry find female legislators
stereotypes and how individuals react to ‘‘nontradi- bring more federal dollars to their districts than male
tional’’ candidates. The ease of measuring changes in legislators, providing representative benefits not just to
heart rate, pupil dilation, and subtle changes on the women but all members of their district.84 The
surface of the skin can detect when and whether social presence of women in the legislature can substantially
desirability forces are at work.100,101 Biological mea- influence the items on the legislative agenda, policy
sures are increasingly common in communication, outcomes, and the tone of debate in government.
psychology, and political science research.102 Measur- However, voter perceptions of female candidates might
ing physiological reactions can offer more insight be limiting the opportunities for women to provide
about the automaticity of stereotypes and can more these policy benefits by winning elective office.
accurately assess when individuals suppress stereotypes Women comprise approximately half of the U.S.
than self-report measures. Increased heart rate and population, but constitute just 20 percent of Congress.
increased pupil dilation can indicate more thoughtful Increasing the number of women in elected office
processing indicative of stereotype suppression. Sup- creates a legislature more representative of the popu-
pression occurs when a stereotype is activated but then lation it serves. Descriptive representation occurs when
associated attitudes are suppressed to avoid appearing the legislature closely resembles the characteristics of
sexist or discriminatory. Those who suppress stereo- the electorate it represents. Normative democratic
typic responses might initially make a rapid association theorists argue a legislature becomes more legitimate
between a woman and gender stereotypes, but these when it resembles the demographic characteristics of
individuals will then take time to edit these associa- the population.106 Surveys have shown that women
tions. The act of suppressing stereotypes and not using want descriptive representation, which only comes

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 37


Bauer

from having more women in elected office.107,108,109 evaluations on support for women candidates,’’ Political
This desire among women to be represented by women Behavior, 2010, 32: 69–88.
is thought to reflect the sentiment that women feel 4. Mark Stephen Leeper, ‘‘The impact of prejudice on female
excluded from the political system, suggesting a lack of candidates: An experimental look at voter inference,’’
faith in the representational capacities of govern- American Political Quarterly, 1991, 19(2): 248–261.
ment.105 Evidence of a gender affinity effect, where 5. Richard L. Fox and Eric R.A.N. Smith, ‘‘The role of
women are most likely to support female candidates, candidate sex in voter decision-making,’’ Political Psycholo-
reinforces this point.110 Increasing the number of gy, 1998, 19(2): 405–419.
women in office leads to increased voter turnout, 6. Deborah Jordan Brooks, ‘‘Testing the double-standard for
political engagement, and higher levels of knowledge candidate emotionality: Voter reactions to the tears and anger
among women in the electorate.10,13,14,17,106 More- of male and female candidates,’’ Journal of Politics, 2011, 73:
over, studies find broad support among the population, 597–615.
including men and women, for increasing the number 7. Leonie Huddy and Teresa Capelos, ‘‘Gender stereotyping
of women in office.3 Considering the consistent level of and candidate evaluation: Good news and bad news for
support for female candidates among the general women politicians,’’ in The Social Psychology of Politics, eds.
Victor C. Ottati, R. Scott Tindale, John Edwards, Fred B.
public, it is puzzling that female candidates often face Bryant, Linda Heath, Yolanda Suarez-Balcazar, and Posavac
difficulties persuading voters. Emil J. (New York: Kluwer Publishers, 2002).
Many of the benefits female policymakers bring to
8. John A. Bargh, ‘‘The four horsemen of automaticity:
office reflect gender stereotypes; however, it is not clear Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social
if this legislative activity shapes individual perceptions cognition,’’ in The Handbook of Social Cognition, eds.
for female candidates and how these perceptions might Robert S. Wyer, Jr. and Srull Thomas K. (Hillsdale, NJ:
factor into voting decisions. Political science needs a Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1994).
firmer grasp on how external forces shape women’s 9. Ziva Kunda and Lisa Sinclair, ‘‘Motivated reasoning with
ability to be effective leaders within the legislature. stereotypes: Activation, application, and inhibition,’’ Psy-
Stereotypic perceptions may not be unique to the chological Inquiry, 1999, 10(1): 12–22.
campaign trail, as these perceptions may re-emerge in 10. Janet Clark, ‘‘Getting there: Women in political office,’’
the behavioral expectations for women in political Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
office. As leaders, female members of Congress must Science, 1991, 515: 63–76.
balance expectations to represent both their district 11. Kira Sanbonmatsu, ‘‘Political parties and the recruitment
and to represent the interests of women in general.105 of women to state legislatures,’’ Journal of Politics, 2002, 64:
Because of the ways female candidates must balance 791–809.
these expectations, the challenges they face on the 12. Rebecca E. Deen and Thomas H. Little, ‘‘Getting to the
campaign trail are likely to reemerge once elected. Even top: Factors influencing the selection of women to state
if women project images of political toughness, they legislatures,’’ State and Local Government Review, 1999, 31:
may still have to overcome stereotypic depictions in 123–134.
news coverage of their legislative accomplishments. 13. Carol Nechemias, ‘‘Geographic mobility and women’s
These obstacles pose a challenge for many female access to state legislatures,’’ Western Political Quarterly,
political leaders and may limit their electoral and 1985, 38: 119–131.
legislative success. 14. Joan Acke, ‘‘Inequality regimes gender, class, and race in
organizations,’’ Gender and Society, 2006, 20: 441–464.
References 15. Diane D. Blair and Jeanie R. Stanley, ‘‘Personal
1. ‘‘The rules for female candidates,’’ The Washington Post, relationships and legislative power: Male and female
November 7, 2007, Style, p. C01. perceptions,’’ Legislative Studies Quarterly, 1991, 16: 495–
507.
2. Kim L. Fridkin and Patrick J. Kenney, ‘‘The role of gender
stereotypes in U.S. Senate campaigns,’’ Politics and Gender, 16. Phillip Paolino, ‘‘Group-salient issues and group repre-
2009, 5: 301–329. sentation: Support for women candidates in the 1992 Senate
elections,’’ American Journal of Political Science, 1995, 39:
3. Kathleen Dolan, ‘‘The impact of gender stereotyped 294–313.

38 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

17. Jennifer L. Lawless, ‘‘Women, war, and winning elec- types and the perception of male and female candidates,’’
tions: Gender stereotyping in the post-September 11th era,’’ American Journal of Political Science, 1993, 37: 119–147
Political Research Quarterly, 2004, 57: 479–490.
33. Deborah A. Prentice and Erica Carranza, ‘‘What women
18. Jennifer L. Lawless, Becoming a Candidate (New York: and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t
Cambridge University Press, 2012). have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes,’’
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2002, 26: 269–281.
19. Angus Campbell, Phillip E. Converse, Warren E. Miller,
and Donald E. Stokes, The American Voter (Chicago: 34. Mahzarin R. Banaji, Curtis Hardin, and Alexander J.
University of Chicago Press, 1960). Rothman, ‘‘Implicit stereotyping in person judgment,’’
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1993, 65, 272–
20. Bernard R. Berelson, Paul F. Lazarsfeld, and William N. 281.
McPhee, Voting: A Study of Opinion Formation in a
Presidential Campaign (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 35. Sara E. Hill and Ross Flom, ‘‘18- and 24-month-old’s
1954). discrimination of gender consistent and inconsistent activi-
ties,’’ Infant Behavior and Development, 30(1): 168–173.
21. Anthony Downs, An Economic Theory of Democracy
(New York: Harper & Row, 1957). 36. John T. Jost and Aaron C. Kay, ‘‘Exposure to benevolent
sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences
22. Kathleen A. Dolan, Voting for Women: How the Public for specific and diffuse forms of system justification’’ Journal
Evaluates Women Candidates (Boulder, CO: Westview Press, of Personality and Social Psychology, 2005, 88: 498–509.
2004).
37. Crystal L. Hoyt, Stefanie K. Johnson, Susan Elaine
23. David L. Hamilton and Steven J. Sherman, ‘‘Stereo- Murphy, and Kerry Hogue Skinnell, ‘‘The impact of blatant
types,’’ in Handbook of Social Cognition eds. Robert S. Wyer stereotype activation and group sex-composition on female
and Thomas K. Srull, Volume 2 (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, leaders,’’ Leadership Quarterly, 2010, 716–732.
1994), pp. 1–68.
38. Madeline E. Heilman and Tyler G. Okimoto, ‘‘Why are
24. Gordon Allport, The Nature of Prejudice (Reading, MA: women penalized for success at male tasks? The implied
Addison Wesley, 1954). communality deficit,’’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 2007,
25. Irene V. Blair, ‘‘The malleability of automatic stereotypes 91(1): 81–92.
and prejudice,’’ Personality and Social Psychology Review, 39. Jennifer L. Eberhardt and Susan T. Fiske, ‘‘Affirmative
2002, 6(3): 242–261. action in theory and implementation: Issues of power,
26. Anne M. Koenig, Alice H. Eagly, Abigail A. Mitchell, ambiguity, and gender versus race,’’ Basic and Applied Social
and Tina Ristikari, ‘‘Are leader stereotypes masculine? A Psychology, 1994, 15, 201–220.
meta-analysis of three research paradigms?’’ Psychological 40. Alan J. Lambert and Robert S. Wyer Jr. ‘‘Stereotypes and
Bulletin, 2011, 137(4): 616–642. social judgment: The effects of typicality and group
27. Amanda B. Diekman and Alice H. Eagly, ‘‘Stereotypes as heterogeneity,’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, 1990, 59(4): 676–691.
and future,’’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
41. Madeline E. Heilman, ‘‘Description and prescription:
1999, 26(10): 1171–1188.
How gender stereotypes prevent women’s ascent up the
28. Alice H. Eagly and Steven J. Karau, ‘‘Role congruity organizational ladder,’’ Journal of Social Issues, 2001, 57(4):
theory of prejudice toward female leaders,’’ Psychological 657–674.
Review, 2002, 109: 573–598.
42. Diana Burgess and Eugene Borgida, ‘‘Who women are,
29. Alice H. Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, ‘‘The advantages of who women should be: Descriptive and prescriptive gender
an inclusive definition of attitude,’’ Social Cognition, 2007, stereotyping in sex discrimination,’’ Psychology, Public
25: 582–602. Policy, and Law, 1999, 5(3): 665–692.

30. Laurie A. Rudman and Stephen E. Kilianski, ‘‘Implicit 43. Jacqueline Hitchon, Chang Chingching, and Rhonda
and explicit attitudes toward female authority,’’ Personality Harris, ‘‘Should women emote? Perceptual bias and opinion
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2000, 26: 1315. change in response to political ads for candidates of different
genders,’’ Political Communication, 1997, 14(1): 49–69.
31. Roger D. Masters, ‘‘Evolutionary biology and political
theory,’’ American Political Science Review, 1990, 84(1): 44. Galen V. Bodenhausen and C. Neil Macrae, ‘‘Stereotype
195–210. activation and inhibition,’’ in Stereotype Activation and
Inhibition, ed. Robert S. Wyer. Jr. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
32. Leonie Huddy and Nayda Terkildsen, ‘‘Gender stereo- Erlbaum Associates, 1998).

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 39


Bauer

45. John A. Bargh, ‘‘The cognitive monster: The case against presidential vote,’’ American Political Science Review,1989,
the controllability of automatic stereotype effects,’’ in Dual 83: 567–573.
Process Theories in Social Psychology, ed. Shelley Chaiken
and Yaacov Trope (New York: Guilford Press, 1999). 58. Michael B. Mackuen, Robert S. Erikson, and James A.
Stimson, ‘‘Peasants or bankers? The American electorate and
46. Patricia G. Devine, ‘‘Stereotypes and prejudice: Their the U.S. economy,’’ American Political Science Review, 1992
automatic and controlled components,’’ Journal of Personal- 86: 597–611.
ity and Social Psychology, 1989, 56: 680–690.
59. Monika L. McDermott, ‘‘Voting cues in low-information
47. Susan T. Fiske, ‘‘Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimi- elections: Candidate gender as a social information variable
nation,’’ in Handbook of Social Psychology, eds. Daniel T. in contemporary United States elections,’’ American Journal
Gilbert, Susan T. Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey, 4th ed. (New of Political Science, 1997, 41(1): 270–283.
York: McGraw-Hill, 1998), pp. 357–411.
60. Susan Welch, ‘‘Are women more liberal than men in the
48. Patricia G. Devine and Lindsay B. Sharp, ‘‘Automaticity U.S. Congress?’’ Legislative Studies Quarterly, 1985, 10(1):
and control in stereotyping and prejudice,’’ in Handbook of 125–134.
Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination, ed. Todd D.
Nelson, (New York: Psychology Press, 2009), pp. 61–88. 61. Jeffrey W. Koch, ‘‘Do citizens apply gender stereotypes to
infer candidates’ ideological orientations?’’ Journal of Poli-
49. Marilyn P. Brewer, ‘‘A dual process model of impression tics, 2000, 62(2): 414–429.
formation,’’ in Advances in Social Cognition, vol. 1, eds.
Thomas K. Srull and Robert S. Wyer, Jr. (Hillsdale, NJ: 62. John R. Petrocik, ‘‘Issue ownership in presidential
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988), pp. 1–36. elections, with a 1980 case study,’’ American Journal of
Political Science, 1996, 40(3): 825–850.
50. John F. Dovidio, Nancy Evans, and Richard B. Tyler,
‘‘Racial stereotypes: The contents of their cognitive repre- 63. Ziva Kunda and Steven J. Spencer, ‘‘When do stereotypes
sentations,’’ Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-
1986, 22(1): 22–37. based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and
application,’’ Psychological Bulletin, 2003, 129(4): 522–544.
51. Kay Deaux and Laurie L. Lewis, ‘‘Structure of gender
stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gen- 64. Irene V. Blair and Mahzarin BanajiR. , ‘‘Automatic and
der label,’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, controlled processes in stereotype priming,’’ Journal of
1984, 46(5): 991–1004. Personality and Social Psychology, 1996, 70(6): 1142–1163.

52. John F. Dovidio, Kerry Kawakami, and Samuel L. 65. Steven Fein and Steven J. Spencer, ‘‘Prejudice as self-
Gaertner, ‘‘Reducing contemporary prejudice: Combating image maintenance: Affirming the self through derogating
explicit and implicit bias at the individual and intergroup others,’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1997,
level,’’ in Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination, ed. Stuart 73: 31–44.
Oskamp (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
2000), pp. 137–163. 66. Costas Panagopolous, ‘‘Boy talk/girl talk: Gender differ-
ences in campaign communication strategies,’’ Women and
53. Laurie A. Rudman, Richard D. Ashmore, and Melvin L. Politics, 2004, 26: 131–155.
Gary, ‘‘‘Unlearning’ automatic biases: The malleability of
implicit prejudice and stereotypes,’’ Journal of Personality 67. Judith S. Trent and Teresa Sabourin, ‘‘Sex still counts:
and Social Psychology, 81(5): 856–868. Women’s use of televised advertising during the decade of the
80’s,’’ Journal of Applied Communication Research, 1993,
54. Paul M. Sniderman, Richard A. Brody, and Philip E. 21: 42.
Tetlock, Reasoning and Choice: Explorations in Political
Psychology (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1993). 68. Susan Gluck Mezey, ‘‘Does sex make a difference? A case
study of women in politics,’’ Western Political Quarterly,
55. Donald Green, Bradley Palmquist, and Eric Shickler, 1978, 31: 492–501.
Partisan Hearts and Minds (New Haven: Yale University
Press, 2002). 69. Kim Fridkin Kahn, ‘‘Gender differences in campaign
messages: The political advertisements of men and women
56. Paul Allen Beck, Russell J. Dalton, Steven Greene, and candidates for the U.S. Senate,’’ Political Research Quarterly,
Robert Huckfeldt, ‘‘The social calculus of voting: Interper- 1993, 46: 481–502.
sonal, media, and organizational influences on presidential
choices,’’ American Political Science Review, 2002, 96(1): 70. Ann Gordon, David M. Shafire, and Ann N. Crigler, ‘‘Is
57–73. negative advertising effective for female candidates? An
experiment in voters’ use of gender stereotypes,’’ Harvard
57. Robert S. Erikson, ‘‘Economic conditions and the International of Press/Politics, 2003, 8(3): 35–53.

40 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1


Rethinking gender stereotypes

71. Monica C. Schneider and Angela L. Bos, ‘‘Measuring 84. Sarah F. Anzia and Christopher R. Berry, ‘‘The Jackie
stereotypes of female politicians,’’ Political Psychology, (and Jill) Robinson effect: Why do congresswomen outper-
forthcoming. form congressmen?’’ American Journal of Political Science,
2011, 55(3): 478–493.
72. Anthony G. Greenwald, Debbie E. McGhee, and Jordan
L. K. Schwartz, ‘‘Measuring individual differences in implicit 85. Kim Fridkin Kahn, ‘‘Does being male help? An investi-
cognition: The Implicit Association Test,’’ Journal of Per- gation of the effects of candidate gender and campaign
sonality and Social Psychology, 1998, 74: 1464–1480. coverage on evaluations of U.S. Senate candidates,’’ Journal
of Politics, 1992, 54: 497–517.
73. Laurie A. Rudman and Peter Glick, ‘‘Feminized man-
agement and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden 86. Caroline J. Heldman, Susan J. Carroll, and Stephanie
costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle Olson, ‘‘‘She brought only a skirt’: Print media coverage of
managers,’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Elizabeth Dole’s bid for the Republican presidential nomi-
1999, 77: 1004–1010. nation,’’ Political Communication, 2005, 22: 315–335.

74. Laurie A. Rudman, Anthony G. Greenwald, and Debbie 87. Kim Fridkin Kahn and Edie N. Goldberg, ‘‘Women
E. McGhee, ‘‘Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit candidates in the news: An examination of gender differences
gender stereotypes: Self and in-group share desirable traits,’’ in U.S. Senate campaign coverage,’’ Public Opinion Quar-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2001, 27: 1164– terly, 1991, 55(2): 180–199.
1178.
88. Kathleen Dolan, ‘‘Do women candidates play to gender
75. Brian A. Nosek and Mahrazin Banaji, ‘‘The Go/No Go stereotypes? Do men candidates play to women? Candidate
Association Task,’’ Social Cognition, 2001, 19(6): 625–666. sex and issues priorities on campaign websites,’’ Political
Research Quarterly, 2005, 58(1): 31–44.
76. Melanie C. Steffens and Petra Jelenec, ‘‘Separating
implicit gender stereotypes regarding math and language: 89. Danny Hayes, ‘‘When gender and party collide: Stereo-
Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, typing in candidate trait attribution,’’ Politics & Gender,
but not for girls,’’ Sex Roles, 2011, 64: 324–335. 2011, 7: 133–165.

77. James Deese, ‘‘On the prediction of occurrence of 90. Angela L. Bos, ‘‘Out of control: Delegates’ information
particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall,’’ Journal of sources and perceptions of female candidates,’’ Political
Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58: 17–22. Communication, 2011, 28: 87–109.

78. Henry L. Roediger, III and Kathleen B. McDermott, 91. Yanna Krupnikov and Nichole M. Bauer, ‘‘The rela-
tionship between campaign negativity, gender, and campaign
‘‘Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented
context,’’ Political Behavior, forthcoming.
in lists,’’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, & Cognition, 1995, 21, 803–814. 92. Kosuke Imai, Luke Keele, Dustin Tingley, and Teppei
Yamamoto, ‘‘Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning
79. Alison P. Lenton, Irene V. Blair, and Reid Hastie,
about causal mechanisms from experimental and observa-
‘‘Illusions of gender: Stereotypes evoke false memories,’’
tional studies,’’ American Political Science Review, 2011,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2001, 37(1): 3–
104(4): 765–789.
14.
93. Neal Malhotra and Jon A. Krosnick, ‘‘Retrospective and
80. Irene V. Blair, Jennifer E. Ma, and Alison P. Lenton,
prospective performance assessments during the 2004 elec-
‘‘Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit
tion campaign: Tests of mediation and news media priming,’’
stereotypes through mental imagery,’’ Journal of Personality
Political Behavior, 2007, 29(2): 249–278.
and Social Psychology, 2001, 81(5): 828–841.
94. Kira Sanbonmatsu and Kathleen Dolan, ‘‘Do gender
81. Adam Berinsky, Gregory A. Huber, and Gabriel S. Lenz, stereotypes transcend party?’’ Political Research Quarterly,
‘‘Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: 2009, 62(3): 485–494.
Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk,’’ Political Analysis, 2012,
20(3): 351–368. 95. Tasha S. Philpot and Hanes Walton, Jr., ‘‘One of our
own: Black female candidates and the voters who support
82. Kira Sanbonmatsu, ‘‘Gender stereotypes and vote them,’’ American Journal of Political Science, 51(1): 49–62.
choice,’’ American Journal of Political Science, 2002, 46(1):
20–34. 96. Peter Glick and Susan T. Fiske, ‘‘The Ambivalent Sexism
Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism,’’
83. Richard L. Fox, ‘‘Congressional elections: Where are we Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1996, 70: 491–
on the road to gender parity?’’ in Gender and Elections, eds. 512.
Susan J. Carroll and FoxRicahrd L. (New York: Cambridge
University Press), pp. 97–116. 97. Lynda A. King and Daniel W. King, ‘‘Sex-role egalitar-

P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1 41


Bauer

ianism scale: Development, psychometric properties, and women’s issues bills than their male colleagues?’’ Legislative
recommendations for future research,’’ Psychology of Women Studies Quarterly, 1998, 23: 435–448.
Quarterly, 1997, 21: 71–87.
104. Michele L. Swers, The Difference Women Make: The
98. Maureen C. McHugh and Irene Hanson Frieze, ‘‘The Policy Impact of Women in Congress (Chicago: University of
measurement of gender-role attitudes: A review and com- Chicago Press, 2002).
mentary,’’ Psychology of Women Quarterly, 1997, 21: 1–16.
105. Kim Quaile Hill and Patricia A. Hurley, ‘‘Symbolic
99. Kathleen Dolan and Kira Sanbonmatsu, ‘‘Candidate speeches in the U.S. Senate and their representational
gender and experimental political science,’’ in the Cambridge implications,’’ Journal of Politics, 2002, 64: 219–231.
Handbook of Experimental Political Science, eds. James N.
Druckman, Donald P. Green, James H. Kuklinski, and Arthur 106. Hannah F. Pitkin, The Concept of Representation
Lupia (New York: Cambridge University Press, 2011). (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1967).
100. Peter K. Hatemi and Rose McDermott, ‘‘Disgust and 107. Cindy Simon Rosenthal, ‘‘The role of gender in
political preferences,’’ PS: Political Science and Politics, descriptive representation,’’ Political Research Quarterly,
forthcoming.
1995, 48: 599–611.
101. John E. Newhagen, ‘‘Beyond self-report: Using latency
108. Jane Mansbridge, ‘‘Should blacks represent blacks and
to respond to model the question answering process on web-
women represent women? A contingent ‘yes’,’’ Journal of
based public opinion surveys,’’ in the Sourcebook for Political
Communication Research, eds. Erik P. Bucy and R. Lance Politics, 1999, 61(3): 627–657.
Holbert (New York: Routledge, 2011).
109. Kira Sanbonmatsu, ‘‘Gender-related political knowl-
102. Douglas R. Oxley, Kevin B. Smith, John R. Alford, edge and the descriptive representation of women,’’ Political
Matthew V. Hibbing, Jennifer L. Miller, Mario Scalora, Peter Behavior, 2003, 25: 367–388.
K. Hatemi, and John R. Hibbing, ‘‘Political attitudes vary
with physiological traits,’’ Science, September 19, 2008, 321: 110. Kathleen Dolan, ‘‘Is there a ‘gender affinity effect’ in
1667–1670. American politics? Information, affect, and candidate sex in
U.S. House elections,’’ Political Research Quarterly, 2008,
103. Michele L. Swers, ‘‘Are women more likely to vote for 61: 79–89.

42 P OLITICS AND THE L IFE S CIENCES  S PRING 2013  VOL . 32, NO . 1

Вам также может понравиться