Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 24

Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright © 2000 by

2000, Vol. 4, No. 3, 255–277 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

A Rose by Any Name? The Values Construct


Meg J. Rohan
School of Psychology
University of New South Wales

Definitional inconsistency has been epidemic in values theory and research. An ab-
breviated review of values-related theory and research is provided, and 5 aspects of
the values construct that may have contributed to this inconsistency and the resulting
lack of synthesis are discussed. A proposal for the process by which value priorities in-
fluence attitudinal and behavioral decisions also is outlined. Attitudinal and behav-
ioral decisions are shown to be traceable to personal value priorities, although the
link is indirect. The importance of 4 constructs in this process is highlighted. In the
past, personal value systems, social value systems, worldviews, and ideologies each
may have been given the generic label values.

“When I use a word,” Humpty Dumpty said, in rather a construct), it does not explain why enthusiastic atten-
scornful tone, “it means just what I choose it to tion to the values construct has not been revived now
mean—neither more nor less.” that there is a willingness to discuss and investigate
“The question is,” said Alice, “whether you can other latent constructs such as schemas (e.g., Reich &
make words mean different things.” Weary, 1998) and working models (e.g., Mikulincer,
“The question is,” said Humpty Dumpty, “which is
1998). Or, does the values construct exist in contempo-
to be master—that’s all.” (Carroll, 1865/1966, p. 185)
rary research under other names?
The status of values theory and research suffers be-
Important theorists in a variety of fields have em-
cause the word values is open to abuse and overuse by
phasized the importance of people’s value priorities in
nonpsychologists and psychologists alike. For exam-
understanding and predicting attitudinal and behav-
ple, consider politicians’ (and others’) moaning about
ioral decisions. For example, Gordon Allport (1961)
the erosion of family values. What do they mean by fam-
suggested that value priorities were the “dominating
ily values? People—including psychologists, anthro-
force in life” (p. 543) because they directed all of a per-
pologists, political scientists, and sociologists—seem
son’s activity toward their realization. Elsewhere,
to use the word values in Humpty Dumpty fashion: They
Allport (1955) berated psychologists for failing to con-
make it mean just what they choose it to mean.
sider that people’s value priorities influence their per-
However, the problem is not new. Adler (1956), for
ception of reality (p. 89). Allport’s reprimand remains
example, suggested that as a result of definitional con-
relevant even now because value theory and research
fusion, the “emphasis on values has slowed down the
are at the fringe of the field. For example, no discus-
advancement of the social sciences rather than fur-
sion of value theory appears in a sample of introduc-
thered it” (p. 279). One popular strategy for settling
tory social psychology and personality textbooks
confusion is to invent new names for the construct.
published in this decade (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert,
Clyde Kluckhohn (1951), whom Levitin (1968) de-
1997; Baron & Byrne, 1997; Burger, 1997; Carducci,
scribed as having offered one of the most comprehen-
1998; Cloninger, 1996; Hewstone, Stroebe, &
sive analyses of the values construct, described the
Stephenson, 1996; Liebert & Liebert, 1998; Myers,
result of this strategy:
1996; Pervin, 1996; E. R. Smith & Mackie, 1995). Al-
though Allport’s enthusiasm for the construct lost its Reading the voluminous, and often vague and diffuse,
influence with the rise of behaviorism (behaviorists literature on the subject in the various fields of learn-
would have looked with disfavor at this unobservable ing, one finds values considered as attitudes, motiva-
tions, objects, measurable quantities, substantive areas
Preparation of this article was supported in part by an Australian of behavior, affect-laden customs or traditions, and re-
Research Council Small Grant to Meg Rohan. Thanks to Mark lationships such as those between individuals, groups,
Zanna, David A. Kenny, Felicia Pratto, and Shalom Schwartz for objects, events. (C. K. M. Kluckhohn, 1951, p. 390)
their insightful comments and suggestions.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Meg J. Rohan, School of
Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, M. B. Smith (1969) also bemoaned the “proliferation of
Australia. E-mail: m.rohan@unsw.edu.au. concepts akin to values” that were labeled, for example,

255
ROHAN

as core attitudes or sentiments, preferences, cathexes, the goodness or badness of the stimuli in their environ-
and valences (p. 98). D. T. Campbell (1963) provided a ments (a “drive to evaluate,” Festinger, 1954; see also
list of 76 concepts that included value, attitude, and mo- Pratto, 1994). Norman Feather’s (1996) comment sheds
tive to illustrate that “superficially quite dissimilar ter- light on what may be taking place in the valuing process:
minologies may be describing essentially the same “We relate possible actions and outcomes within partic-
facts and processes” (pp. 100–101). D. T. Campbell ular situations to our value systems, testing them against
(1963) suggested that the common characteristic of our general conceptions about what we believe is desir-
these concepts was that each could be viewed as coordi- able or undesirable in terms of our own value priorities”
nators of behavior. However, conceptualizing the ab- (p. 224). Perhaps as a result of the lack of theoretical
stract, trans-situational, implicit nature of these funda- (and empirical) attention to the valuing process itself,
mental coordinators of behavior is difficult. programs designed to change people’s value priorities
The purpose of this article is to review briefly work (e.g., the value self-confrontation method; Rokeach,
in the area of values, to propose a definition of the val- 1973) have met with limited success, and long-term
ues construct that distinguishes it from other related changes are disappointingly rare (e.g., see Kristiansen
constructs, and to propose a process by which value & Hotte, 1996). Investigation of the valuing process
priorities coordinate people’s attitudinal and behav- may benefit from work such as Tetlock’s (1986) exami-
ioral decisions. In tracing the link between value prior- nations of ideological reasoning and work on the pro-
ities and decisions, I highlight the importance of two cessing of information (e.g., the heuristic–systematic
constructs—worldviews and ideologies—that are of- model and the elaboration likelihood model; see Eagly
ten labeled as values. Before presenting this proposal, I & Chaiken, 1993).
discuss aspects of the values construct that are at the
heart of definitional diversity and confusion. Value as a noun. A dilemma that early values
theorists and researchers faced was whether values
(noun) should be investigated from the perspective of
At the Heart of the Confusion? Five the entity being evaluated (e.g., “How much value does
Aspects of the Values Construct the entity have?”) or from the perspective of the person
doing the valuing (e.g., “What does this person value?”;
Aspect 1: Nouns and Verbs see Feather, 1975, p. 3, for a discussion of this point).
However, this issue essentially has been settled: Con-
Use of the word value as a noun is recorded in the temporary values theorists investigate the values con-
Compact Oxford English Dictionary (1991) as early as struct from the perspective of the person who evaluates
1303, to refer to the fairness and equivalence of the the entities in his or her environment, and they seek to
amount of a commodity in an exchange, and in 1398 to measure people’s priorities on various values in an ef-
mean a standard of estimation. Use of value as a verb is fort to understand the underlying motivations of peo-
registered at a similar time, to describe the act of ap- ple’s responses to their environments (see Rohan &
praising the worth—in terms of its appropriateness for Zanna, in press).
exchange—of a commodity. However, its meaning An aid to people’s constant evaluation of the stimuli
was later expanded to incorporate more abstract ex- in their environments (value as a verb) would be a cogni-
changes and standards. Thomas and Znanieki tive structure in which information about past evalua-
(1918/1958) focused on this latter meaning in their fa- tions could be collected (see Bargh, Chaiken, Govender,
mous The Polish Peasant work. & Pratto, 1992). This information, if organized, then
could serve as a kind of analogical principle to use in
Value as a verb. The use of value as a verb im- evaluating and imbuing meaning to newly encountered
plies that some higher level evaluation has taken place. objects and events. Humans’ ability to use analogy to
When people say that they value (verb) a thing, person, imbue meaning and coherence to their experiences is
action, or activity, they are expressing a deeper meaning highly developed. Indeed, some cognitive scientists
associated with that entity. So, they do not simply like (e.g., Holyoak & Thagard, 1995, 1997; Thagard & Shel-
the entity; they feel that it is good (the meaning of good is ley, in press) have considered it so much a part of human
discussed later) and relates to or somehow expresses experience that they have used computer simulations to
their underlying values (noun). The link between peo- demonstrate the ease with which analogies are used. Be-
ple’s liking for an entity and their value priorities has cause these analogical principles are relevant across sit-
been demonstrated empirically (see Feather, 1995; see uations and time, they may be what are generally
also Feather, 1982s). referred to as values (noun). The values construct de-
Little specific attention has been paid to the valuing scribed this way, then, seems not unlike schemata that
process, but it has been suggested, for example, that peo- were defined by Bartlett (1932) as being “active organi-
ple chronically and effortlessly engage in ascertaining zations of past experience” (p. 201).

256
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

Schwartz and Bilsky (1987, 1990) found that five (1968) suggested that value priorities occupy central
features of the values construct are recurrently men- positions in cognitive networks of attitudes and beliefs.
tioned in the values literature: that the values con- Feather (1971, 1980, 1999) also clearly described the
struct concerns (a) beliefs, (b) desirable end states or cognitive structure status of value systems and de-
behaviors, (c) trans-situational guides, (d) selection scribed them as abstract structures or associative net-
and evaluation of behavior and events, and (e) the works. However, he highlighted an important
relative ordering of beliefs, desirable end states or be- feature—that the networks are linked to the affective
havior, or guides. These features are all consistent system. As a result, these abstract structures—“orga-
with the suggestion that the value system is a stable nized summaries of experience”—provide “continuity
meaning-producing superordinate cognitive structure. and meaning under changing environmental circum-
Considering its analogical nature, the value system stances” (Feather, 1980, p. 249). Silvan Tomkins’s
may provide the basic architecture of what has been (e.g., 1979) script theory seems relevant. Tomkins sug-
referred to as the “narrative mode” of human under- gested that from the earliest weeks of life, humans store
standing that deals in “human or human-like intention “scenes” containing at least one affect and one object of
and action and the vicissitudes and consequences that that affect (see Carlson, 1981). These scenes are col-
mark their course” (Bruner, 1986, p. 13; see also lected into “scripts” so that sense can be made of the re-
McAdams, 1999). The value system also may pro- lations among various scenes.
vide organization for what Hazel Markus (1977) and An assumption implicit in discussions of affective
her colleagues (e.g., Fong &Markus, 1982) have in- links to value systems is that people will be motivated to
vestigated as self-schemata. engage in situations that are similar to other situations
It is a well-supported suggestion that value systems that resulted in positive affect (or an absence of negative
are cognitive structures, and often it is implied in value affect), to be with people who enable positive affect (or
definitions (see Table 1 for a selection of definitions) minimize negative affect), and to behave in ways that
and in explanations about the function of value sys- will produce positive affect (or reduce negative affect).
tems. For example, this suggestion is implied in These motivations may then produce what Schwartz
Allport’s (1961) contention that value systems were and Bilsky (1987, 1990) described as the motivational
“schemata of comprehensibility” (p. 544) and in C. K. goals that underlie value priorities—people may have
M. Kluckhohn’s (1951) answer to the question “Why preferences for particular types of emotion (and there is
are there values?”: Because without value systems “in- some indirect evidence for this; see work on self-regula-
dividuals could not get what they want and need from tory focus by Higgins, e.g., 1997). For example, accord-
other individuals in personal and emotional terms, nor ing to Schwartz and Bilsky (e.g., 1987) the defining goal
could they feel within themselves the requisite mea- of self-direction values is independent thought and ac-
sure of order and unified purpose” (p. 400). Others tion; thinking and behaving in independent ways may
have stated it more clearly. For example, Rokeach provide feelings that can be described as positively

Table 1. A Selection of Values Definitions


Theorist Definition

Lewin (1952, p. 41) Values influence behavior but have not the character of a goal (i.e., of a force field). For
example, the individual does not try to “reach” the value of fairness, but fairness is “guiding ”
his behavior. It is probably correct to say that values determine which types of activity have a
positive and which have a negative valence for an individual in a given situation. In other
words, values are not force fields but they “induce ” force fields. That means values are
constructs that have the same psychological dimension as power fields.
C. K. M. Kluckhohn (1951, p. 395) A value is a conception, explicit or implicit, distinctive of an individual or characteristic of a
group, of the desirable that influences the selection from available modes, means, and ends of
actions.
Heider (1958, p. 223) We shall use the term value as meaning the property of an entity (x has values) or as meaning a
class of entities (x is a value) with the connotation of being objectively positive in some way.
Rokeach (1973, p. 5) A value is an enduring belief that a specific mode of conduct or end-state of existence is
personally or socially preferable to an opposite or converse mode of conduct or end-state of
existence.
Feather (1996, p. 222) I regard values as beliefs about desirable or undesirable ways of behaving or about the
desirability or otherwise of general goals.
Schwartz (1994, p. 21) I define values as desirable transsituational goals, varying in importance, that serve as guiding
principles in the life of a person or other social entity.
Schwartz (1999, p. 24) I define values as conceptions of the desirable that guide the way social actors (e.g., organizational
leaders, policy-makers, individual persons) select actions, evaluate people and events, and
explain their actions and evaluations.

257
ROHAN

valenced, high arousal (e.g., excited, elated). The defin- 798), empirical focus on the values construct has
ing goal of security values is stated as safety, harmony, become somewhat obscured. To allow the values con-
and stability of society, of relationships, and of self; be- struct a chance to come back into the limelight, I pro-
having in ways that enable such safety, harmony, and se- pose that the term attitude is used only for evaluations
curity may provide a feeling that can be described as of specific entities. The term values then can be re-
positively valenced, low arousal (e.g., calm, relaxed; served for discussions of abstract trans-situational
see Feldman Barrett & Russell, 1998, for an affect struc- guides. Already, this seems to be the convention in dis-
ture model in which the distinction between high and cussions of what are labeled value-expressive attitudes
low arousal is described). (e.g., see Maio & Olson, 2000a, 2000b).
Feather’s (e.g., 1999) suggestion that some value
types may be relatively undifferentiated with a limited Summary. Used as a verb, value refers to the pro-
network of associations, whereas others have a high de- cess of ascertaining the merit of an entity with reference
gree of differentiation with a complex network of asso- to an abstract value system structure. Used as a noun,
ciations, may provide direction for further research into value refers to the result of this process. These value
the development of values and value change. The im- judgments may be formed or amended when people en-
portance of particular value types may be driven by re- counter new entities or existing judgments are chal-
peated confirmation of particular entity–outcome lenged. Rather than use attitude to refer to evaluations of
sequences. Repeated disconfirmation of such se- either specific or more abstract entities, I propose that
quences may stimulate modification, and the provision attitude is reserved for describing evaluations of spe-
of experiences (either direct or vicarious) may build up cific entities. In view of the conceptualization of the
the entity–outcome sequences in less elaborated value value system as an affectively charged cognitive struc-
types. ture, more attention to affect value system links seems
warranted.
Values versus attitudes. When the values con-
struct is viewed in terms of an abstract meaning-produc-
ing cognitive structure, the divide between value priori- Aspect 2: Values, Value Types, Value
ties and evaluations of specific entities seems wide Priorities, and Value Systems
indeed. However, people not only use the words “I
value” in talking about their evaluations of specific or Not only is the word values used in reference to
tangible entities, they also use them in describing their people’s value priorities and the organization of those
evaluations of abstract trans-situational guides. For ex- value priorities, their value systems, it is also used to
ample, people not only may say “I value that ring” but describe judgments and categories of judgments. For
also may say “I value security.” The problem is that se- example, broad-mindedness is a judgment that con-
curity can be labeled as a value, but it seems inappropri- cerns acceptance of diversity, and self-direction refers
ate to label a person’s attachment to a ring as a value. The to a category of judgments that concern independence
term attitude may provide a temporary solution to the and free thinking.
problem of the abstractness or specificity of people’s The ensuing confusion not only leads to misunder-
judgments. Allport (e.g., 1935), as well as others (e.g., standings and misinterpretations but also obscures an
see Eagly & Chaiken, 1993), used the word attitude to important assumption that has been characteristic of all
describe specific judgments as well as abstract judg- value theories and for which there is now empirical sup-
ments that could be labeled as values. However, atti- port: Although people differ in terms of their value pri-
tudes that have some kind of value-related implication orities, the structure of the human value system is
are often discussed in terms of having an “ego defen- universal (e.g., Schwartz, 1992, 1994, 1996). That is,
sive” function (e.g., Katz, 1960; Ostrom & Brock, 1968, people differ only in terms of the relative importance
1969; Sherif & Cantril, 1947; M.B. Smith, Bruner, & they place on a set of universally important value types.
White, 1956; see also Johnson & Eagly, 1990). Work by The assumption that value system structure is uni-
David Sears and his colleagues on symbolic racism versal may be lost in phrases such as “people attach
(e.g., Sears & Kinder, 1985) and work by Mark Zanna great importance to their values” (Maio & Olson,
and his colleagues (e.g., Esses, Haddock, & Zanna, 1998, p. 294) that are meant to describe people’s ten-
1993; Zanna, Haddock, & Esses, 1990; see also Biernat, dency to defend their value priorities. It also may be
Vescio, Theno, & Crandall, 1996) in which the violation lost in discussions of how children acquire values (i.e.,
of symbolic beliefs was found to be an important factor how children’s value priorities undergo change; see
in prejudiced attitudes can be viewed as another per- Grusec & Kuczynski, 1997).
spective on the value-related implications of attitudes. A review of value theories is presented next to pro-
Although the term attitude has remained popular vide an overview of past and contemporary focus on
(e.g., see the much-quoted passage in Allport, 1935, p. the value system, value types, and value priorities.

258
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

Early value theories. In general, early theorists itation and awareness of oneself. Solitude and sociality
focused on individual differences in the organization of together are both necessary in the good life. (Morris,
some universally relevant set of human features. For 1956, p. 16)
example, Alexander Shand (1896, 1914) proposed a
theory of character in which different configurations in In measurement terms, Morris can be viewed as being
the organization of sentiments (a concept somewhat ahead of his time—his approach has been labeled the
consistent with the values construct) resulted in differ- prototype approach and it has been used, for example,
ences in people’s attitudinal and behavioral responses in assessing attachment style (e.g., see Griffin &
to the world. Eduard Spranger (1928), a philosopher Bartholomew, 1994), parenting style (Rohan & Zanna,
who also focused on organization, suggested that six at- 1996), and self-esteem (Rohan, 2000). The logic of this
titudes (i.e., value types) were present in everyone in approach is that people in general (both novices and ex-
different proportions with one dominating. So, for ex- perts) primarily understand the world by assessing di-
ample, Spranger suggested that for the self-affirming verse configurations of characteristics and comparing
rhetorician, political value priorities dominated, this assessment with a prototype (see Broughton,
whereas economic value priorities dominated the prac- Boyes, & Mitchell, 1993; Cantor, Smith, French, &
tical type. Mezzich, 1980; Mayer & Bower, 1986; Setterlund &
Spranger’s (1928) work inspired the first (1931) Niedenthal, 1993).
version of the Study of Values instrument (Allport,
Vernon, & Lindzey, 1960). This instrument provided
an indication of the relative priorities people placed on Rokeach’s (1973) value theory. Milton Rokeach
the six value types by measuring the effect of people’s (1973)—who has been accorded the major credit for
value priorities on their answers to questions. The providing an impetus for values research after behav-
Study of Values instrument was one of the most popu- iorism’s heyday (see Mayton, Ball-Rokeach, & Loges,
lar measures of human value priorities for many years. 1994)—used a somewhat different approach to mea-
Also guided by the assumption that a value system surement. He named values, briefly explained their
contains a finite number of universally relevant value meaning, and asked people to arrange the value words
types on which people place relative importance, Mor- “in order of importance to YOU, as guiding principles
ris (1956) presented people with 13 ways to live and in YOUR life” (e.g., Rokeach, 1973, p. 27). There were
asked them to rate each of the descriptive paragraphs to two types of value words in the list: goals (terminal val-
show how much they liked or disliked each of them ues) and modes of conduct (instrumental values). The
(see Dempsey & Dukes, 1966, for a shortened, revised list of goals included such things as a “comfortable life
version). Morris (1956) found that five general value (a prosperous life)” and “self-respect (self-esteem),”
types were contained in the “ways to live” descriptions and the mode of conduct list included such things as
(see pp. 32–34): social restraint and self-control, en- “broad-minded (open-minded),” “forgiving (willing to
joyment and progress in action, withdrawal and pardon others),” and “helpful (working for the welfare
self-sufficiency, receptivity and sympathetic concern, of others)” (see Rokeach, 1973, pp. 359–361). Respon-
and self-indulgence (or sensuous enjoyment). Each dents then arranged the list of value words in terms of
way seems to describe the implications of a high prior- the relative importance they placed on them. The set of
ity on one value for priorities on other values. For ex- values named was created on the basis of intuition (see
ample, the following way can be viewed as a Rokeach, 1973, p. 30) and was meant to be a reasonably
description of the effects that a high priority on hedo- comprehensive sample of possible human values.
nistic values has for other value priorities: However, Braithwaite and Law (1985) identified four
omissions in this list: values relating to “physical devel-
Life is something to be enjoyed—sensuously enjoyed, opment and well-being (e.g., physical fitness, good
enjoyed with relish and abandonment. The aim in life health),” “individual rights (e.g., privacy, dignity),”
should not be to control the course of the world or soci- “thriftiness (e.g., care with money, taking advantage of
ety or the lives of others, but to be open and receptive to opportunities),” and “carefreeness (acting on impulse,
things and personas, and to delight in them. Life is more spontaneity).” Nevertheless, Rokeach’s (1973) list of
a festival than a workshop or a school for moral disci- value words was produced with the assumption that “all
pline. To let oneself go, to let things and persons affect
men everywhere possess the same values to different
oneself, is more important than to do—or to do good.
Such enjoyment, however, requires that one be
degrees” (p. 3). Incidentally, Schwartz (e.g., 1992) was
self-centered enough to be keenly aware of what is hap- unable to find support for the usefulness of the termi-
pening and free for new happenings. So one should nal–instrumental distinction.
avoid entanglements, should not be too dependent on Since its development, the Rokeach Value Survey
particular people or things, should not be self-sacrific- (Rokeach, 1973) has been perhaps the most popular
ing; one should be alone a lot, should have time for med- method of measuring value priorities. Unfortunately,

259
ROHAN

no theory about the underlying value system structure seem that openness to change is somehow better than
was proposed, and therefore the Rokeach Value Sur- conservation) but also may direct attention to the myr-
vey is essentially a list of unconnected value words. iad of ways in which these motivations can be ex-
Without a theory about underlying value system struc- pressed. For example, the funky-looking new wave
ture, it is impossible to understand the consequences of aromatherapist who adheres religiously to his or her re-
high priorities on one value type for priorities on other gime of essential oil applications may well be moti-
value types (see Schwartz, 1996, for further discussion vated by a focus on organization rather than on
of the problems that result when an integrated system opportunity (the conservation end of the openness to
of value priorities is not considered). Attention to the change–conservation dimension); the osteopath who
consequences of one value priority for other value pri- relishes in his or her status as a miracle worker may be
orities surely is critical to understanding patterns of re- strongly motivated by his or her focus on individual
sponses that seem, at first glance, to be unrelated. Such outcomes rather than on social context outcomes (the
attention may be important for understanding consis- self-enhancement end of the self-enhancement–self-
tency in response patterns. Response consistency al- transcendence dimension).
ready has received a huge amount of theoretical and The focus on the individual–social context outcomes
empirical attention (e.g., see Aronson, 1969, 1992; dimension (i.e., the self-enhancement–self-transcen-
Festinger, 1957). dence dimension) may reflect people’s beliefs about hu-
man nature (see Wrightsman, 1991): People who have a
Schwartz’s (e.g., 1992) value theory. Schwartz greater focus on social context outcomes than on indi-
and Bilsky (e.g., 1987, 1990) developed a theory about vidual outcomes may believe that humans are essen-
value system structure by focusing on the motivational tially good, whereas people who have a greater focus on
concern embodied in each value. They revived the as- individual outcomes than on social context outcomes
sumption that people differ only in the relative impor- may be less positive about others’ essential goodness
tance they place on a universally important set of value (Schwartz has made similar suggestions, e.g., personal
types, and because they focused on the motivational communication, April 3, 1999). Indirect support for this
goal each value type embodies, the implications of pri- suggestion was provided by de St. Aubin (1996), who
orities on one value type for priorities on others within found that people who were highly humanistic were
an integrated system could be proposed. more likely to report high priorities on values that
In a revision of the original theory, Schwartz (1992) Schwartz (e.g., 1992) classified as relevant to the social
suggested that two motivational dimensions structure context outcomes end of the individual–social context
the value system. These are cast in terms of conflicts, outcomes dimension (e.g., broad-mindedness, world of
and the two dimensions can be understood in terms of beauty; see Table 2).
two fundamental human problems that need to be The focus on the opportunity–organization dimen-
solved (Rohan, 1998; see also F. R. Kluckhohn & sion (i.e., the openness to change–conservation dimen-
Strodtbeck, 1961, who suggested there were five fun- sion) seems conceptually similar (or even identical) to
damental human problems). One dimension, labeled the Big Five personality factor Openness to Experi-
openness to change–conservation, relates to the con- ence as described and discussed by McCrae (1996).
flict between being motivated “to follow their own in- According to McCrae, this factor is best understood
tellectual and emotional interests in unpredictable and “as a fundamental way of approaching the world that
uncertain directions” or “to preserve the status quo and affects not only internal experience but also interper-
the certainty it provides in relationships with close oth- sonal interactions and social behavior” (p. 323). The
ers, institutions, and traditions” (Schwartz, 1992, p. focus on the opportunity–organization dimension also
43). The second dimension, labeled self-enhance- may relate to temperament (Rohan, 1998; see also
ment–self-transcendence, relates to the conflict be- Kochanska & Thompson, 1997, who discuss its role in
tween concern for the consequences of own and children’s internalization of parental values). For ex-
others’ actions for the self and concern for the conse- ample, on the basis of their response to novelty, Kagan
quences of own and others’ actions in the social con- and his colleagues (e.g, Kagan, Reznick, & Gibbons,
text. Ten value types are arranged along these two 1989) identified what seems to be a stable tempera-
dimensions (see Table 2 for definitions and representa- ment style; it may be expected that those who focus on
tive values for each type): power, achievement, opportunity will be less anxious in response to novelty
hedonism, stimulation, self-direction, universalism, than will those who focus on organization. Links be-
benevolence, conformity, tradition, and security. tween the focus on the opportunity–organization di-
In Figure 1, the location of these value types along mension and what Carol Dweck and her colleagues
the two dimensions is shown. New labels for these two (e.g., Dweck & Leggett, 1988) described in terms of
dimensions are provided. Use of these labels not only incremental self-theories (personal attributes are rela-
may avoid evaluative misinterpretation (e.g., it may tively malleable) and entity self-theories (personal at-

260
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

Table 2. Value Types, Definitions, and Representative Values (See, e.g., Schwartz, 1992, 1996)
Value Types and Definitions Representative Values

Power: Social Status and Prestige, Control or Dominance Social power: Control over others, dominance.
Over People and Resources. Authority: The right to lead or command.
Wealth: Material possessions, money.

Achievement: Personal Success Through Demonstrating Success: Achieving goals.


Competence According to Social Standards. Capability: Competence, effectiveness, efficiency.
Ambition: Hard work, aspirations.
Influence: Have an impact on people and events.

Hedonism: Pleasure and Sensuous Gratification for Oneself. Pleasure: Gratification of desires.
Enjoyment in life: Enjoyment of food, sex, leisure, and so on.

Stimulation: Excitement, Novelty, and Challenge in Life. Daringness: Adventure-seeking, risktaking.


A varied life: Filled with challenge, novelty, change.
An exciting life: Stimulating experiences.

Self-Direction: Independent Thought and Action-Choosing, Creativity: Uniqueness, imagination.


Creating, Exploring. Freedom: Freedom of action and thought.
Independence: Self-reliance, self-sufficiency.
Curiosity: Interest in everything, exploration.
Choose own goals: Select own purposes.

Universalism: Understanding, Appreciation, Tolerance, and Broadminded: Tolerant of different ideas and beliefs.
Protection for the Welfare of all People and for Nature. Wisdom: A mature understanding of life.
Social justice: Correcting injustice, care for the weak.
Equality: Equal opportunity for all.
A world at peace: Free of war and conflict.
A world of beauty: Beauty of nature and the arts.
Unity with nature: Fitting into nature.
Protecting the environment: Preserving nature.

Benevolence: Preservation and Enhancement of the Welfare Helpful: Working for the welfare of others.
of People With Whom One is in Frequent Personal Contact. Honesty: Genuineness, sincerity.
Forgivingness: Willingness to pardon others.
Loyalty: Faithful to my friends, group.
Responsibility: Dependable, reliable.

Tradition: Respect, Commitment, and Acceptance of the Humility: Modesty, self-effacement.


Customs and Ideas That Traditional Culture or Religion Acceptance of my portion in life: Submission to life’s circumstances.
Provide the Self. Devotion: Hold to religious faith and belief.
Respect for tradition: Preservation of time-honored customs.
Moderate: Avoiding extremes of feeling or action.

Conformity: Restraint of Actions, Inclinations, and Impulses Politeness: Courtesy, good manners.
Likely to Upset or Harm Others and Violate Social Obedience: Dutiful, meet obligations.
Expectations or Norms. Self-discipline: Self-restraint, resistance to temptation.
Honor parents and elders: Showing respect.

Security: Safety, Harmony, and Stability of Society, of Family security: Safety for loved ones.
Relationships, and of Self. National security: Protection of my nation from enemies.
Social order: Stability of society.
Cleanliness: Neatness, tidiness.
Reciprocation of favors: Avoidance of indebtedness.

tributes are relatively fixed) also could be explored. Identification of the underlying value system struc-
Further, the promotion versus prevention self-regula- ture allowed Schwartz (1992) to specify the relations
tory focus proposed by Tory Higgins (1997) also among the value types in the value system: People’s pri-
seems somewhat consistent with the focus on the op- orities on adjacent value types will be similar, whereas
portunity–organization dimension (see Rohan & maximum differences in priorities will occur when
Zanna, 1998, for a discussion of the links between value types are opposite each other. For example, be-
value priorities and self-regulatory focus). cause human rights activists are likely to have high pri-

261
ROHAN

Figure 1. Location of 10 value types in a two-dimensional space.

orities on equality values—these are representative of the two most important and two least important value
the universalism value type—they are likely also to words to provide an anchor for ratings) of 44 value
have high priorities on self-direction because universal- words (see Table 2) that relate to the 10 value types in the
ism and self-direction are adjacent in the system. Fur- value system.
ther, because the power value type is in direct opposition
to universalism, activists’ lowest value priorities are Summary. To avoid confusion, use of the terms
likely to be on power. Thus, activists’ very negative re- value priority, value type, and value system is encour-
action to a show of police strength and power at a street aged to clarify the assumption (which has theoretical
rally could be predicted. support) that all humans have a value system that con-
According to Schwartz (1996), value priorities are tains a finite number of universally important value
responses to “three universal requirements of human types, but differ in terms of the relative importance they
existence: biological needs, requisites for coordinated place on each of these value types—people’s value pri-
social interaction, and demands of group survival and orities—can be kept clearly in mind.
functioning” (p. 2). As mentioned earlier, this com- The Schwartz value theory (1992) is a contribution to
ment highlights the assumption that has been charac- understanding not only the components of the human
teristic of value theories: All of the value types in the value system but also how people differ in terms of the
human value system are important in some way to hu- dynamic organization of value priorities on the 10 value
man functioning—otherwise, why would they be part types contained in the value system. Value-neutral la-
of the universally shared value system structure? The bels for the two motivational dimensions described in
relative importance people place on each value type re- the Schwartz theory as structuring the value sys-
flects their choices about what they are prepared to lose tem—focus on individual outcomes–focus on social
a little of to gain a little more of something else. For ex- context outcomes, and focus on opportunity–focus on
ample, although all people are likely to see the positive organization—were proposed, and links between these
aspects of adhering to traditional standards, the struc- dimensions and other constructs were highlighted.
ture of the value system (see Figure 1) is such that to
have higher priorities on a tradition value type means Aspect 3: Value Priorities Are a Function of
lower priorities on stimulation and hedonism value What Type of Judgments?
types: less excitement in exchange for predictability.
Measurement of value priorities is in the style of the Braithwaite and Scott (1991) cast this problem in
Rokeach Value Survey (Rokeach, 1973). In the terms of the controversy about whether value priorities
Schwartz Value Inventory (Schwartz, 1996), respon- are conceptions of the desirable (what people ought to
dents are asked to rate the importance (after choosing do) and the desired (what people want to do). They

262
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

suggested that this question had been settled, and there it is clear that we neither strive for, nor will, neither
was some consensus that value priorities concerned the want, nor desire anything because we judge it to be
desired rather than the desirable. However, Schwartz good; on the contrary, we judge something to be good
and Bilsky’s (1987) value definition can be viewed in because we strive for it, will it, want it, and desire it”
terms of both the desired and the desirable: They de- (Postulate IX, p. 160).
scribed the values construct in terms of the “cognitive Spinoza’s wisdom leads to a suggestion for the inte-
representations of three types of universal human re- gration of the ought and want aspects of value priori-
quirements: biologically based needs of the organism, ties in a way that reference to disputable notions of
social interactional demands for interpersonal coordi- good can be avoided. Often in early research, what was
nation, and social institutional demands for group wel- good and moral was assumed to be consensual (e.g.,
fare and survival” (p. 551). Thus, people’s value see Peck & Havinghurst, 1960, who focused on char-
priorities can be viewed in terms of people doing what acter development). Roger Brown (1965) provided an
they ought because they want to survive in their social example in which people generally might not agree
environments. But, do people want to do more than about the goodness and badness of particular types of
simply survive? The question of what value priorities people: He reported that in 1938, a psychologist (E. R.
are judgments of seems to require further attention. Jaensch), who was also a Nazi supporter, discovered
an ideal type (the J-Type) who would make a good
Values as guides for survival. If people’s value Nazi party member because he (and presumably she)
priorities are viewed in terms of survival in the social would “recognize that human behavior is fixed by
environment (i.e., in terms of satisfying requirements blood, soil and national tradition” (Brown, 1965, p.
of existence), it is difficult to explain why people will 478) and an “anti-type” who would be an irritant to the
ignore their personal safety to behave in ways they feel Nazi cause as a result of liberal views that the environ-
are consistent with their value priorities. For example, ment and education were the determinants of behavior.
consider the person with high priorities on benevolence
or universalism (see Table 2) who, as discussed earlier, Value priorities as guides to “best possible
can be understood in terms of a greater focus on social living.” The ought and want aspects can be integrated
context outcomes than on individual outcomes. She or if value priorities are viewed as evidencing the dynamic
he may do as the unidentified passenger did in 1982 organization of judgments about the capacity of entities
when he altruistically passed the helicopter winch to (i.e., things, people, actions, activities) to enable best
others so that they could escape the freezing Washing- possible living. This means (consistent with arguments
ton River after their plane crashed (Kelly, 1982): pro- made by many theorists and researchers) that people are
vide others with a means of survival to his or her own not simply driven to satisfy their basic survival needs
detriment. Or, consider people who embrace a religion (e.g., see Lee, 1948) or to avoid pain (e.g., see Higgins,
and likely have high priorities on tradition (see 1997, 1998) but are driven to live as pleasantly and pro-
Schwartz, 1992), who would rather die than give up ductively as possible. This proposal is grounded in the
their religion. Patrick Henry’s proclamation, “Give me Aristotelian (Aristotle, circa 350 BCE/1980) wisdom
liberty or give me death,” provides another example. that eudaimonia is the ultimate human goal. The Greek
When people give up their lives to uphold their value word eudaimonia has been misleadingly translated as
priorities, they make exceptionally strong statements “happiness.” However, by eudaimonia, Aristotle meant
that without this entity, life is not worth living. Value human flourishing (i.e., actualizing potential) rather
priorities surely are more than survival guides. than mere positive affect. This point was taken up by
Waterman (1993), who translated eudaimonia as “per-
Values as guides for goodness. Returning to the sonal expressiveness,” linked it to optimal psychologi-
notion that value priorities concern the “ought” rather cal functioning, and successfully distinguished between
than “want,” value priorities may provide guides for two conceptions of happiness: Aristotle’s eudaimonia
goodness. The ability to live by such guides rather than and hedonic enjoyment. According to Aristotle, flour-
simple drives has been discussed at length as the feature ishing is the ultimate goal towards which all human ac-
that separates animals from humans (e.g., see Tomkins, tion is directed (see Nicomachean Ethics, Book 1, Sec-
1962), and some believe that humans essentially strive tion VII, Aristotle, circa 350 BCE/1980). Value
to be moral or ethical (e.g., Aronson, 1992; Kagan, priorities can provide guides for living the best way pos-
1999). Therefore, value priorities provide the princi- sible. The value system, therefore, can be understood as
ples for moral and ethical living (e.g., Hart, 1962). concerning the aspects of the environment to which hu-
However, judgments of goodness are idiosyncratic mans must pay attention. This is consistent with the
(e.g., see Ichheiser, 1949; Kendler, 1993). In 1644, idea, discussed earlier, that the two motivational dimen-
Spinoza (1644/1985) explained the idiosyncratic na- sions structuring the value system concern the two fun-
ture of the judgment of goodness: “From all this, then, damental human problems humans must solve.

263
ROHAN

People’s failure to distinguish between wants and priorities on self-direction. Though this may be a uni-
survival needs may reflect their value priorities. So, by versal human requirement, people’s value priorities
saying “I need” rather than “I want,” a person may be may dictate the primacy of relatedness over other hu-
expressing that an object is somehow related to his or man requirements. So, for example, the need for relat-
her high priorities on a particular value type. Consider, edness may be primary for those who have high
for example, “I need a new car.” People with high pri- priorities on value types motivated by a focus on social
orities on the power value type (defined in terms of a context outcomes but less important for those who
motivation toward social status and prestige; see Table have high priorities on value types motivated by a fo-
2) may see a new car as absolutely necessary to main- cus on individual outcomes. For the latter people, the
tain their social superiority. To these people, losing need for power may be primary (see Baumeister, 1998,
their social superiority would mean feeling unable to who discussed the needs for “belongingness” and
live the best way possible. power as fundamental interpersonal motives).
Although people may be motivated to live the best If value priorities are understood as guides to best
way possible, a multitude of personal and environmen- possible living that provide a way to order require-
tal constraints mean that people do not always behave ments and desires, it is easy to explain why people in
in ways consistent with their value priorities. Further- the same environment differ in terms of their value
more, because people generally use what has been de- priorities if value systems concern “universal human
scribed as the “fast and frugal” satisficing strategy— requirements” (Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987) or if value
labeled the “take the best strategy” (Gigerenzer & systems concern conceptions of what is desirable that
Goldstein, 1996)—they may not always make deci- have been learned in the social environment. Though
sions that lead to best possible living. People’s evalua- they share an environment, particular configurations
tions of their progress in negotiating their social of experiences and personal attributes will result in
environment in a way that enables them to adhere to variations in people’s views of what best possible liv-
their value priorities, that is, their progress in living the ing means to them; this will be reflected in their
best way possible, can be understood as the evaluation value priorities that evidence the dynamic organiza-
that underlies self-esteem (see Rohan, 2000, in which tion of people’s judgments about the capacity of enti-
self-esteem is described as resulting from an estimate ties to enable best possible living. Furthermore, value
of personal progress in living the best way possible). priorities are likely to change as people’s judgments
are amended in response to changes in circumstances
Ordering the importance of requirements and and personal attributes (e.g., skill level). For exam-
desires. From this perspective, the value system can ple, consider the finding that people tend to become
be viewed as providing a way to order which require- more conservative when they become parents (e.g.,
ments or desires are more or less important to best pos- see Altemeyer, 1988). For a parent, best possible liv-
sible living. So, for example, people who have high pri- ing now may include having a happy and successful
orities on tradition and conformity value types can be child, and planning for this happiness and success
viewed as having judged that best possible living may mean that the importance of value types moti-
means that personal desires are less important than the vated by a focus on organization (tradition, confor-
requirements of being a cooperative group member. In mity, security) will be increased.
contrast, people who have high priorities on the hedo-
nism value type can be viewed as having judged that Summary. In response to the confusion about
best possible living means that personal desires are the nature of the judgments that give rise to value pri-
more important than are the requirements of group orities, it was proposed that judgments concern the
membership. capacity of entities to enable best possible living.
Psychologists have described humans’ psychologi- Best possible living is viewed in Aristotelian terms
cal requirements at length. For example, Aronson and was explained to mean flourishing or personal
(1992) suggested that people strive to view themselves expressiveness—not just surviving (Aristotle, circa
in three ways: as competent, predictable and consis- 350 BCE/1980). This view builds on the suggestion
tent, and moral. Other basic human needs have been that people’s value priorities will change in response
discussed. For example, the need for relatedness al- to changes in their environments: As people’s cir-
ways has been a popular topic (e.g., Baumeister & cumstances change, so too will their judgments about
Leary, 1995; Bowlby, 1969). What these things mean what best possible living means to them. This view is
to people, and the primacy of each, are likely to be dic- consistent with the consensus described by
tated by people’s value priorities. For example, moral- Braithwaite and Scott (1991) that personal value pri-
ity to a person who has high priorities on tradition may orities have to do with the desired, or what people
mean following traditional practice, whereas it may want, rather than with the desirable, or what people
mean being true to oneself to someone who has high ought to do.

264
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

Aspect 4: Personal, Social, or (James, 1890/1950, p. 330). However, people may have
Cultural Value Systems? more than one social value system—they may have per-
ceptions of the value systems of all people and groups
People have not only their own value system but per- with whom they interact. However, in view of people’s
ceptions of others’ value systems, and groups (e.g., ability to categorize (e.g., Cantor & Mischel, 1979), it is
clubs, religious congregations, corporations, societies, likely that people will have a limited number of
cultures) can be described in terms of the values they en- prototypical social value systems that will allow catego-
dorse or promote. Whereas people’s own value system rization of their perceptions of others’ value priorities.
and their perceptions of others’ value systems are Fourth, if both personal and social value systems exist,
intrapsychic cognitive structures, descriptions of then attitudinal and behavioral decisions may be traced
groups’ value systems are not. Although a distinction is to either system. This is discussed further later.
often made between personal value systems and de- Some of the confusion about the difference between
scriptions of groups’ value systems (cultural value sys- people’s personal value systems and their perceptions
tems), there has been little or no attention to the of others’ value priorities—people’s social value sys-
distinction between two intrapsychic value systems, tems—stems from the tendency to discuss people’s
what will be referred to here as personal and social value stable tendencies to deal with others in their social en-
systems. Descriptions of the values that groups endorse vironments in particular ways as social values (e.g.,
can be understood as being ideological value systems. Beggan & Allison, 1994; Liebrand & Dehue, 1996;
The difficulty in conceptualizing these value systems is McClintock, 1978; Rokeach, 1973; Van Lange, Otten,
beyond the scope of this article, but suffice it to say that De Bruin, & Joireman, 1997). Schwartz (1992) pro-
there is no consensus about whether to understand these vided evidence that such tendencies can be understood
in terms of the average of the group members’ personal in terms of what has been labeled the focus on individ-
value priorities or, for example, group leaders’ or other ual–social context outcomes dimension of the personal
significant members’ beliefs about what the groups’ value system.
value priorities should be (see Rokeach, 1979, for a dis- The strength and replicability of the finding that
cussion). In focus here are intrapsychic value systems. personal value priorities influence perceptions and be-
Making a distinction between two intrapsychic value havior (see Postman, Bruner, & McGinnies, 1948, for
systems, the personal and the social, introduces new an early study) has been discussed at length (e.g.,
complications. Attention to at least four of these is re- Allport, 1955). Distinguishing between personal and
quired. First, a layer of complication is added to the social value priorities is grounded in the assumption
problem of definition. Whereas personal value systems that not only will personal value priorities influence
can be defined as judgments of the capacity of entities to perceptions and behavior, but so too will social value
enable best possible living, what are social value sys- priorities influence perceptions and behavior. Indirect
tems? It is proposed that social value systems contain evidence for this assumption comes from studies that
people’s perceptions of others’ judgments about best show people who identify with groups often behave
possible living or functioning, that is, others’ (e.g., other similarly in response to a stimulus. For example, per-
people, groups, institutions, cultures) value priorities. haps the Dartmouth and Princeton football fans’ foot-
Social value systems, therefore, organize people’s per- ball club social value priorities were highly salient and
ceptions of others. Second is the issue of whether per- therefore influenced their perceptions of the football
sonal and social value systems are similarly or game made famous by Hastorf and Cantril (1954). Per-
differently structured. Rules of parsimony—and re- haps salient ethnic group social value priorities also
search (e.g., Schwartz, 1999; Schwartz & Sagiv, produced the finding (Vallone, Ross, & Lepper, 1985)
1995)—support the proposal that personal and social that pro-Arab and pro-Israeli television viewers saw
value systems will be structured similarly. Furthermore, different things in the same programs.
if personal value system structure is universal (although People’s social value systems may have idiosyn-
value priorities differ, the underlying structure is the cratic aspects when they do not involve actual individ-
same for all people; see earlier), then others’ value prior- uals. For example, the way people perform role-related
ities surely also would be organized according to this behavior may relate to idiosyncratic aspects of their so-
universal structure. Third, what does the personal ver- cial value systems. For example, one person’s percep-
sus social value system distinction mean for the number tions of a prison guard’s value priorities may differ
of intrapsychic value systems? It seems reasonable to from another’s, and these differences are likely to be
propose that people have only one personal value sys- reflected in different role-playing behavior. Not all the
tem. Logically, it would be impossible for people to guards in Zimbardo’s prison study (Zimbardo, Haney,
have more than one personal value system if they are to Banks, & Jaffe, 1982) behaved similarly. Work in the
feel that their thoughts “do not fly about loose, but seem area of intercultural stereotyping also is relevant to the
each to belong to some one thinker and not to another” idiosyncratic nature of social value systems. For exam-

265
ROHAN

ple, Sidanius, Pratto, and Bobo (1996) showed that reconciliation may be total adherence to personal value
there were identifiable individual differences in peo- priority standards. Diary study methods (see Wheeler &
ple’s beliefs about Black people; social value priorities Reis, 1991) may be most useful for examining this prob-
are likely to have inspired these beliefs. lem, as suggested by a pilot study (Rohan & Harris,
When social value systems involve actual individu- 1999) using this methodology. It was found, for exam-
als, accuracy and clarity become an issue. Relevant to ple, that people’s personal value priorities were related
this issue is Grusec and Goodnow’s (1994) work in to the relative number of occasions in which their own,
which children’s accurate or inaccurate perceptions of others’, or both own and others’ value priorities were
parental value priorities and other messages influenced viewed as being an issue: Although all participants re-
subsequent behavior (see also Grusec, 1997). Inaccu- ported more instances in which both own and others’
racy or confusion about social value systems may be im- value priorities were involved, participants motivated
portant to understanding social anxiety (e.g., see Leary both by a focus on organization and social context out-
& Kowalski, 1995, who suggest that lack of knowledge comes (high priorities on benevolence, tradition, and
about social rules will lead to social anxiety). Social in- conformity) reported the greatest number.
telligence (e.g., Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987) also may re- Research in the area of self-presentation may be
late to social value system accuracy and clarity. useful to investigations of the daily reconciliation be-
If personal and social value systems exist, then peo- tween personal and social value priorities. Two main
ple must decide whether to behave in line with others’ kinds of motivations for seeking to present particular
expectations—consistent with social value priori- information or images about self to others have been
ties—or in line with their own value priorities. There is proposed (see Baumeister, 1982): as a means to gain
no shortage of research showing that people use infor- practical and material rewards or to claim an identity.
mation about others’ needs, desires, and expectations in Whereas the first can be understood as a delayed-grati-
regulating their behavior (e.g., Moretti & Higgins, fication reconciliation solution (behaving according to
1999), and the notion that people need to reconcile satis- social value priorities for later personal value priority
faction of their own value priorities with conformity to satisfaction), the second (often observed in studies on
others’ value priorities is not a new issue. For example, reactance) can be understood in terms of a personal
Allport (1955) acknowledged the problem. He dis- value priority reconciliation solution.
cussed the difference between the “tribal” and the “per- Past research into “value fit” has acknowledged the
sonal” and suggested that although “opportunistic benefits associated with compatibility between per-
modes of adjusting” (p. 39) are dictated by the social sonal and social value priorities (e.g., see Bills, 1952;
(tribal) environment, individuals develop other stan- Feather, 1975; Furnham & Bochner, 1986; Triandis,
dards of conduct for themselves. Allport (1955) sug- 1990). The difficulty of conceptualizing and measuring
gested that attempting to reconcile these two modes of what were labeled earlier as ideological value systems is
becoming, the tribal and the personal, was a lifelong an important factor in interpreting such research. In re-
process. Permanent reconciliation between the tribal cent research (Rohan & Maiden, 2000), a modified
and the personal may take the form of what has been de- Schwartz Value Inventory was used to measure teach-
scribed as internalization—people then have similar ers’ perceptions of their school’s ideological value sys-
value priorities to those of an important social group, tem (i.e., their school-related social value priorities),
such as the family or a religious organization (e.g., see and it was shown that the fit between teachers’ personal
Deci & Ryan, 1995; Emler, Ohana, & Dickinson, 1990; value priorities and their social value priorities (using
Goodnow, 1990; Rohan, 1998; Rohan & Zanna, 1998). within-subject correlations) strongly predicted, for ex-
Theory and research in which it is suggested that per- ample, reported stress, job commitment, and satisfac-
sonal autonomy is important for psychological health tion. Neither an index of fit constructed on the basis of
(e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1995) implies an optimal reconcilia- personal value priorities and the school’s value priori-
tion for daily conflicts between personal and social ties according to the school principal nor an index con-
value priorities: Behave according to personal value pri- structed on the basis of fit between personal value
orities. However, for some (e.g., those with high priori- priorities and the average of all teachers’ social value
ties on the conformity value type), behaving in ways that priorities could match this prediction.
are in line with others’ value priorities is in line with There is an unmistakable similarity between the no-
their own value priorities. Highest priorities on a partic- tion that people need to reconcile personal and social
ular value type may be associated with a particular opti- value priorities and the concept of agendas (individual,
mal reconciliation. For example, for the person with interpersonal, relationship, group) discussed by Snyder
highest priorities on hedonism, optimal reconciliation and Cantor (1998) in their proposal that a functionalist
may be something like “whatever gives me the most strategy could be used productively to understand per-
pleasure” (or, perhaps, the least pain). However, for sonality and social behavior. Use of a values approach
people who have highest priorities on power, optimal will provide a way to understand the structure of the

266
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

agendas and will provide a way to understand how value language reflects human capacity for
well-being is influenced by the satisfaction of each type metacognition (e.g., see Mischel, 1998). This meta-
of agenda or only one type of agenda to the detriment of cognition not only means that people are capable of talk-
others. In short, the values approach may serve as a uni- ing about their value priorities but also means that peo-
fying foundation for carrying out Snyder and Cantor’s ple are able to use value language to argue for one
suggestions for investigation. attitudinal or behavioral decision over another. Indeed,
Perelman (1982) suggested, based on Aristotle’s wis-
Summary. To settle the confusion generated by dom, that convincing arguments require reference to the
the existence of different types of value systems, in dis- value system. As discussed later, arguments that contain
cussions of people’s own value priorities it should be value priority associations are referred to as ideologies.
specified that personal value priorities are at issue; in Humans’ capacity for metacognition concerning
discussions of people’s perceptions of others’ (e.g., their value priorities is critical for measuring value pri-
other individuals, groups, institutions, societies) value orities. Because the way people cognitively represent
priorities, it should be specified that social value priori- their value priorities is shared (although value priorities
ties are at issue; and when descriptions of the value pri- differ, the structure of the value system is universal), it
orities endorsed or promoted by groups are discussed, seems unsurprising that the language of values is shared
they should be identified as ideological value systems. (see Schwartz, 1996, p. 2). However, individual differ-
Personal and social value systems are located within ences are likely to exist in the ease with which people
the person (i.e., are both intrapsychic value systems). can (or want to) think and talk about their personal and
Although people will have only one personal value sys- social value priorities; that is, their metacognition con-
tem, they are likely to have more than one social value cerning their value priorities may differ. People who
system. Social value systems will have the same struc- suffer from a lack of what has been called self-concept
ture as the personal value system. clarity (J. D. Campbell, 1990) and self-certainty
A major issue that the personal–social distinction (Baumgardner, 1990) may suffer from difficulty in
highlights is how people reconcile what they want with thinking and talking about their personal value priori-
what others want, and whether optimal reconciliation ties. (See also Fazio & Powell, 1997, who showed that
is related to people’s personal value priorities requires “attitude accessibility” was related to students’ adjust-
consideration. Investigations of this issue could use di- ment to college—attitude accessibility is likely to be
ary study methods and strategies suggested by psy- linked to the ease with which people can think and talk
chologists who promote a functionalist approach to about value priorities.) Rokeach (1973) alerted re-
understanding personality and social behavior. searchers to this problem when he suggested that com-
pletion of his value survey was “highly projective in
Aspect 5: Value Systems, Worldviews, nature, somewhat like the Rorschach or the Thematic
and Ideologies Apperception Test” (p. 27). Whether difficulty in re-
porting their personal value priorities is related to a ha-
Not only is the word values often used to refer to bitual tendency to settle daily personal–social value
cognitive structures that now have been labeled as system conflicts (discussed earlier) in favor of social
value systems, but it also has been applied to people’s value priorities should be considered.
conscious beliefs about the way the world is or should Perhaps the developers of the early value priority
be, as well as to the value-laden constructions people measures were very aware that not all people would find
use when deliberating about, justifying, or promoting it easy to think or talk about their value priorities. In the
their attitudinal or behavioral decisions. To settle the early measures, personal value priorities were indirectly
resulting confusion, I propose the following guidelines measured by asking people about their attitudes or prob-
so that the values construct can be distinguished from able behavior (e.g., the study of values; Allport et al.,
two other related constructs: identify focus on the cog- 1960) or by asking them about their preferred ways of
nitive structure in terms of value systems (or value living (e.g., Morris, 1956). Because direct questions are
types, or value priorities), use the term worldviews to asked about value priorities, Rokeach’s (1973) and
describe people’s conscious beliefs about the world Schwartz’s (1992) value inventories, as well as a new
that are a function of their value priorities, and use the measurement tool designed to measure priorities ac-
term ideologies to describe value-laden constructions cording to the Schwartz (1992) value theory (Oishi,
people use in or after their decision making. Schimmack, Diener, & Suh, 1998), seem to take less ac-
count of this problem.
A separate issue: Use of value language. A sep- Schwartz and his colleagues have almost completed
arate issue from naming and separately investigating the testing a new questionnaire for measuring personal
three very different constructs is the ease with which value priorities that requires less abstract thought (Sha-
people use the language of values. People’s facility with lom Schwartz, personal communication, March 24,

267
ROHAN

2000). Respondents are asked to compare themselves ments will contain either implicit or explicit reference to
with individuals who are described in terms of what is value priorities. Thus, value priorities still guide attitu-
important to them. The descriptions are theoretically dinal and behavioral decisions because they enable the
linked to values that differ in terms of the two value sys- decision to be framed in a particular way (see Pratto, in
tem dimensions. For example, to measure priorities re- press; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; see also Conover &
lated to the conformity value type, male respondents are Feldman’s 1984 discussion of the use of schemas in
asked to rate their similarity to a person who is described thinking through political issues).
in the following way: “It is important to him to be polite However, people use value priority-based argu-
to other people all the time. He believes he should al- ments not only in deliberating about their decisions but
ways show respect to his parents and to older people.” also for self-justification purposes (see Aronson et al.,
To reduce demands even further, another strategy may 1997, who linked self-justification with the mainte-
be to ask respondents questions that relate directly to the nance of self-esteem; recall that earlier, self-esteem
underlying motivational dimensions. For example, to was described as resulting from an estimate of progress
find out whether people are more focused on individual in living the best way possible, i.e., according to per-
or social context outcomes, they could be asked, sonal value priorities). Both pre- and postdecisional
“Which do you think is better: to be respected or appre- value priority-relevant arguments can be labeled as
ciated? Which do you think is worse: to be a selfish per- ideologies (see Pratto, in press). The label ideology
son or a dependent person?” To find out whether people seems appropriate for these constructions that are
are more focused on opportunity or organization, they learned—and maintained—in the social environment
could be asked, “Which would be better in the long run: even though, as a terminology, it has about as much
to be consistent or be open to new experiences? Which definitional diversity as the term values (e.g., see
would be worse: to have limited options or have no McLellan, 1986, p. 1: “Ideology is the most elusive
plans?” This would enable identification of people in concept in the whole of social science”).
terms of the quadrant location of their highest value pri- Rokeach (1973) described people’s postdecision
orities with minimum decision making. use of ideologies (though not using that term) and sug-
Wolfgang Bilsky (1998a, 1998b), with whom gested that this enabled a person to “end up smelling
Schwartz (e.g., Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987) developed himself, and being smelled by others, like a rose” (p.
the original version of his value theory, also has dis- 20). Rokeach (1973) expanded:
cussed people’s abilities to think about their value prior-
ities. Bilsky (1998a, 1998b) suggested that the The language of values is an ingenious language admi-
motivational concerns that underlie value priorities can rably suited to the enlightenment of all kinds of self-in-
terest, whether enlightened or unenlightened, selfish
be characterized according to an implicit–explicit con-
or altruistic. It permits rational justification of self-in-
tinuum. So, for example, to the extent that value priori-
terest, and, insofar as it is necessary, the language of
ties are easily accessible to consciousness, they are values can also be employed as an Aesopian language
explicit motives. He also suggested that the implicit–ex- to permit rationalized justification of self-interest. (p.
plicit distinction may allow integration of the values and 168)
motivation literatures.
Because ideologies contain value priority associations,
Value systems and ideologies. Demonstrations people will be able to feel they are making the best deci-
that people’s value priorities influence their perceptions sion and will be able to convince themselves and others
(e.g., Postman et al., 1948) support the widely held as- that they are good, moral, or ethical, that is, behaving
sumption that the operation of people’s value systems in according to a set of principles. (Morality from this
guiding their behavior often occurs effortlessly, without point of view has been referred to as enlightened
conscious awareness. However, more conscious self-interest, see Ramm, 1998; see Kristiansen & Hotte,
thought is likely when making a decision that involves 1996 for a discussion of the value language use in
an entity that is not easily categorized in a way that en- self-evaluations of morality; see Kristiansen & Zanna,
ables swift analogical reasoning from value priorities, 1988, 1994, for discussion of value language use in jus-
when a choice is between whether to behave in accord tifying social and intergroup attitudes; and see Pratto,
with one’s own value priorities or another’s value prior- in press, for a discussion of “legitimizing ideologies.”)
ities, or when the reconciliation process involves one’s Whether the same ideology will be used in deliberating
own and more than one other’s dissimilar value priori- about and in explaining, justifying, or promoting the
ties (see Wegner & Bargh, 1998, for a review of auto- decision may depend on whether the context of deci-
matic and controlled behavior theory and research). In sion making changes. The appropriate ideology in one
such situations, people are likely to argue for one course context may not be appropriate in another.
of action over another. Because, as mentioned earlier, Because the rhetoric in ideologies can be manipu-
good arguments need reference to value priorities, argu- lated to connect almost any thing, person, action, or ac-

268
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

tivity to value priorities (i.e., to what constitutes best Value systems and worldviews. Solomon Asch
possible living), an ideology can be made relevant to a (1952) discussed the importance of knowing about the
wide variety of situations by virtue of the value priority “conscious mode” in which things appeared to people
associations contained within it, and the attitudinal and (pp. 64–65). Feather (1971) recognized that this con-
behavioral decisions that result will depend on features scious mode was distinct from value systems, and he de-
of the situation in which it is applied (e.g., see Pratto, scribed it as a perceived structure that represented the in-
Tatar, & Conway-Lanz, 1999, who suggested that ide- formation immediately present in the environment. The
ologies may have different implications for different term worldview can be used to distinguish between this
social groups). However, the manipulation of the rhet- conscious mode and the cognitive structure that has
oric in ideologies means that values are often viewed been described as the value system. Although the term
as “remarkably slippery social constructions that take worldview also is not without definitional diversity (see
on different meanings over time and across political Mannheim, 1936/1972), it seems the most appropriate
cultures” (Tetlock, Peterson, & Lerner, 1996, p. 34). because it is defined as “contemplation of the world,
But, it is ideologies, not value priorities, that are re- view of life” (Compact Oxford English Dictionary,
markably slippery. 1991, p. 2340).
Ideologies may contain a large number of value as- How do value systems relate to people’s world-
sociations or a few. For example, a noblesse oblige ide- views? Parsons (1951) suggested an inescapable link
ology—those with more should help those with between people’s personal value priorities and the way
less—contains implications for universalism value pri- they viewed the world. Early research in which peo-
orities but no obvious implication for stimulation value ple’s (personal) value priorities influenced their per-
priorities (see Pratto, Stallworth, & Conway-Lanz, ceptions (e.g., Postman et al., 1948) supported the
1999, who examined use of this ideology in legitimiz- suggestion that people’s worldviews directly evidence
ing “guns” rather than “butter” social policies). In con- their personal value systems. Because of this strong
trast, political ideologies may contain a greater number link, the value system structure could be used to guide
of links (e.g., see Braithwaite, 1994, 1997; Rokeach, investigations of people’s worldviews. Beliefs relating
1973). Whether the structure of ideologies can be to each value type should be found.
mapped onto the value system will depend on the num- There has been some research into the relation be-
ber of value system associations: Whereas broader ide- tween personal value priorities and worldviews. For ex-
ologies may map onto value system structure, ample, Altemeyer (1998) examined the relation
narrower ideologies will not. between people’s worldviews and their personal value
Tetlock (1986; see also Billig, 1991; Billig et al., systems. He found, consistent with expectations and
1988) focused on ideologies people use in decision earlier research (e.g., Rohan & Zanna, 1996), that the
making. His value pluralism model of ideological rea- strongest relations between the right-wing authoritarian
soning (as well as the revised model; Tetlock, Peterson, worldview and the personal value system concerned
& Lerner, 1996) can be used in systematic investiga- priorities on tradition and conformity value types. He
tions of the value priority-related reasoning connected also found that the strongest relation between the social
to attitudinal and behavioral decisions. Tetlock et al. dominance orientation (SDO) worldview (see Pratto, in
(1994) used the model to demonstrate, for example, that press; Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle, 1994) and
political ideologies differ in the degree to which they ac- the personal value system concerned priorities on the
knowledge conflicts among important value priorities, power value type. Altemeyer (1998) observed that
and thus the extent to which ideologies contain complex
trade-off reasoning. Whether this model is relevant to
understanding how people reconcile personal and social Right-wing authoritarians fear that authority and con-
ventions are crumbling so quickly that civilization will
value priorities can be considered.
collapse and they will be eaten in the resulting jungle.
In other research, Tetlock and his colleagues are in-
In contrast, high SDOs already see life as “dog eat
vestigating “taboo trade-offs” (e.g., Fiske & Tetlock, dog” and—compared with most people—are deter-
1997; Tetlock, Kristel, Elson, Green, & Lerner, 2000)— mined to do the eating. (p. 75)
trade-offs that people do not like to think about or dis-
cuss openly (e.g., trade-offs between friends and its
monetary cost). In addition, these researchers are inves- How very different, then, is the conscious mode in
tigating the success of ideologies that do or do not ac- which things appear to right-wing authoritarians and to
knowledge conflicts between value priorities. For people who have a strong SDO as a result of their per-
example, Tetlock (in press) found that to the extent that a sonal value priorities. For each, it is clear that best pos-
politician acknowledges value conflict (i.e., has en- sible living has a very different meaning (i.e., they have
gaged in more complex trade-off reasoning), she or he is very different value priorities), and this controls the
trusted and respected less (the “traitor effect”). way each views the world.

269
ROHAN

I mentioned earlier that if personal value priorities are or should be. Thus, people’s worldviews constitute
evidence judgments about the capacity of entities to their version of actual or potential realities. An ideol-
enable best possible living, then personal value priori- ogy is a rhetorical association or set of associations be-
ties will change when circumstances (or personal at- tween things, people, actions, or activities and best
tributes) change. Changes in circumstances could be possible living. Because value systems structure judg-
understood not only in terms of physical circumstances ments about the capacity of entities to enable best pos-
but also in terms of the people contained in one’s social sible living, ideologies will contain either implicit or
environments. Constant interaction with people who explicit reference to value priorities.
have different personal value priorities may change
people’s beliefs about the world; changes in people’s The Process
beliefs about the world will be reflected in changes to
their personal value priorities. If personal value priorities are “intimately bound up
with a person’s sense of self” (Feather, 1992, p. 112)
Summary. I propose the following guidelines so and are “a type of personality disposition” (Bilsky &
that the values construct can be distinguished from two Schwartz, 1994, p. 178), then it seems logical to sug-
other related constructs: Use the term value system if gest that all attitudinal and behavioral decisions ulti-
the cognitive structures are in focus, use the term mately should be traceable to personal value priorities
worldviews if investigating people’s beliefs about the (see Figure 2). That is, personal value priorities cause
way the world is or should be that are a function of their decisions. People intuitively may operate under the as-
value priorities, and use the term ideologies to describe sumption that personal values cause decisions, and this
value-laden linguistic constructions that are used in or assumption may explain the (possibly) universal pro-
after decision making. hibition against hypocrisy (see Aronson, Fried, &
In investigations of worldviews, beliefs that evi- Stone, 1991, who use this prohibition to change behav-
dence each value type should be found because the ior). Thus, in Figure 2, the personal value system
way people view their worlds is a function of their per- stands as the superordinate structure.
sonal value priorities. In contrast, ideologies will differ People’s personal value systems cause people to
in terms of the number of value associations that can be view the world in a particular way. However, as men-
identified. An ideology only will have links to value tioned, constant interaction with people who have dif-
priorities—either personal or social—by virtue of the ferent personal value priorities may change people’s
references (implicit or explicit) to values contained in beliefs about the world; changes in people’s beliefs
that ideology. about the world will be reflected in changes to value
priorities. In Figure 2, there are double arrows between
What Does It All Mean for the personal value systems, worldviews, and social value
Values–Attitudinal and Behavioral systems to reflect this potential. This arrangement
Decision Link? takes account of Rokeach’s (1973) comments that “a
major advantage gained in thinking about a person as a
To bring together the points made regarding the po- system of values rather than a cluster of traits is that it
tentially confusing aspects of the values construct that becomes possible to conceive of his undergoing
relate to understanding how value priorities cause atti- change as a result of changes in social conditions” (p.
tudinal and behavioral decisions, I now describe a pro- 21; see also Tomkins, 1966; Young, 1946).
posal for this process. First, definitions for constructs The most direct path from personal value priorities
relevant to the process are given. (through worldviews) to attitudinal and behavioral de-
A value is an implicit analogical principle con- cisions (see Figure 2) reflects the widely held and empir-
structed from judgments about the capacity of things, ically supported assumption that people’s personal
people, actions, and activities to enable best possible value priorities often guide their behavior effortlessly,
living. Value priorities evidence the dynamic organi- with little or no conscious awareness. Relevant to this is
zation of these principles. Value systems are integrated the hypothesis put forward by Schwartz (1996): Associ-
structures within which there are stable and predictable ations with “any outside variables” will “decrease
relations among priorities on each value type. monotonically as one moves around the circular struc-
Personal value systems concern people’s own judg- ture of value types in both directions from the most posi-
ments about the capacity of entities to enable best pos- tively associated value type to the least positively
sible living for themselves. Social value systems associated value type” (p. 6). Sagiv and Schwartz
concern people’s perceptions about others’ judgments (1995) found, for example, that interpersonal coopera-
concerning the capacity of entities to enable best possi- tion is related in the predicted way: Benevolence priori-
ble living, that is, others’ value priorities. A worldview ties were most strongly associated with interpersonal
is a collection of conscious beliefs about how things cooperation, and power priorities were least strongly as-

270
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

Figure 2. Proposed relations among personal and social value priorties, worldviews, ideologies, and
attitudinal and behavioral decisions.

sociated. Interpersonal cooperation behavior was cho- consistency” (e.g., Lavine, Thomsen, & Gonzales,
sen for study because the games that could be used to test 1997)—attitudes may be consistent with either per-
hypotheses “are constructed to tap behaviors that ex- sonal or social value systems.
press relatively pure motivations straightforwardly” When people use ideologies to help them make more
(Schwartz, 1996, p. 6). Research in which these types of complex decisions—those that require conscious
relatively pure motivations (i.e., those that are relatively thought—the ideologies may be in line with either per-
unaffected by situational influences and therefore social sonal or social value priorities. A path from the personal
value priorities) are investigated may be most relevant value system (through worldview) to a personal value
to the most direct path from personal value systems to at- system linked ideology element, as well as a path from a
titudinal or behavioral decisions. social value system to a social value system linked ide-
If people are thoroughly immersed in their interac- ology element, therefore, is included. Billig’s (1991)
tions with others, they may behave according to their discussion of discourse analysts who demonstrate peo-
social value priorities effortlessly. The path from so- ple’s ability to take on different “patterns of talk” is rele-
cial value systems to attitudinal decisions reflects this vant to the idea that people may use different ideologies
possibility. Perhaps relevant to this path is research in their decision making depending on whether personal
(Ybarra & Trafimow, 1998) in which priming of what or social value systems are salient. Somewhat similar is
was described as the collective self (perhaps, social Luker’s (1984) proposal that people differ in terms of
value priorities) produced different behavior than did the beliefs on which they base their attitudes. For exam-
priming of what was described as the private self (per- ple, Luker found that prolife activists based their abor-
haps, personal value priorities). That such priming pro- tion attitudes on statements such as the purpose of sex is
duced different behavior may be important to consider procreation, whereas prochoice activists based their
in discussions and investigation of “interattitudinal abortion attitudes on statements such as the purpose of

271
ROHAN

sex is to foster intimacy and experience pleasure. Al- promotion to alter decisions may underlie Maio and
though Seligman and Katz (1996) suggested that people Olson ’s (1998) findings: They found change in attitu-
may “construct value systems in the context of specific dinal decisions (in this case, reports of value priorities)
issues” (p. 55), they made a point related to Luker’s, when people provided reasons for their previously re-
namely that people associate particular value priorities ported attitudinal decisions.
with particular issues (see also Tourangeau, Rasinski, & The major implication of the proposed process by
D’Andrade, 1991, who found that students who had dif- which value priorities—both personal and social—
ferent opinions also framed the relevant issue in differ- cause attitudinal and behavioral decisions is that the
ent ways by linking the issue to different value priorities often-recorded failure to find strong value–attitude–
and premises). It is likely that when personal value pri- behavior relations (e.g., LaPiere, 1934; Wicker, 1969)
orities are salient, the ideology that “feels right” will be can be explained. In Figure 2, four possible paths from
one that contains links to important personal value pri- personal value systems to decisions are proposed. Each
orities; when social value priorities are salient, the ideol- of these paths may be associated with a different deci-
ogy that feels right will be one that contains links to sion, even though the underlying value systems are sta-
important social value priorities. ble. Specification of which path is being investigated
People may change their beliefs about the world (i.e., may enhance understanding and prediction of the
their worldview) if they behave in particular ways often value–attitude–behavior relation.
enough. This may be why “stateways can change folk-
ways” (see Aronson, 1995). Consider as a simple exam- General Summary
ple the change in attitudes and behavior regarding the
wearing of seat belts (for a weightier example, see In discussing five confusion-producing aspects of
Deutsch & Collins, 1951). There is now general consen- the values construct, I presented a selection of theory
sus that it is good to wear seat belts, although the ideolo- and research in which the term values was not used but in
gies people may use to justify their positive attitudes and which the construct under investigation seemed consis-
seat belt wearing behavior may differ depending on tent with it. I also described work of earlier values theo-
value priorities (e.g., high priorities on conformity: “be- rists, and a contemporary value theory, the Schwartz
cause it’s the law”; high priorities on security: “because (1992) value theory. Because it explains how people’s
it keeps me safe”). The arrow from attitudinal or behav- solutions to two of the most basic human problems in-
ioral decisions back to the worldview element was in- fluence their motivational focus, the Schwartz (1992)
cluded to reflect this type of worldview change theory can provide the structure for investigating the
possibility (Figure 2). The arrow from decision explana- fundamental coordinators of behavior.
tion, justification, promotion back to the worldview ele- Confusion can be reduced greatly if social theorists
ment was included for the same reason. and social scientists specify when they are talking about
Whether or not they made a decision with full people’s value priorities and value systems rather than
awareness, people are likely to use ideologies in ex- the category of judgment or set of judgments that can be
plaining to themselves or others why they made a described as values or value types. The term attitude,
particular decision, in justifying their decision, or in which sometimes has been used to describe a value pri-
promoting their decision. As mentioned, whether the ority, should be reserved for describing a specific evalu-
ideology used in deliberating about decisions is the ation of an entity. To distinguish it from the values
same one used in decision explanation, justification, construct, a worldview was described in terms of a per-
or promotion may depend on whether the deci- son’s conscious beliefs—which are a function of that
sion-relevant context (in terms of both physical cir- person’s value priorities—about the way the world is or
cumstances and people involved) remains constant. should be. Value priority-based arguments people use to
Therefore, in Figure 2 there is an element from attitu- help them make decisions as well as explain, justify, or
dinal and behavioral decision labeled “situationally promote their decisions were labeled as ideologies, and
appropriate ideology.” The only time decision mak- an ideology was described as a rhetoric association or
ing should not require use of an ideology is when set of associations between entities and living the best
people deny they had any choice: Involuntarily ex- way possible. Because judgments about how to live the
pressed attitudes or behaviors need not be explained best way possible are assumed to be what value systems
or justified (this has been demonstrated extensively in organize so analogical reasoning can be used to provide
dissonance research; e.g., Zanna & Cooper, 1974). meaning to experience and guide action, ideologies will
If people use a different ideology in the course of contain either implicit or explicit reference to value
explanation, justification, or promotion, they may priorities.
change the decision (the arrow from situationally ap- I proposed a distinction between personal and social
propriate ideology back to attitudinal or behavioral de- value systems. Both are intrapsychic structures, and
cision). The potential for explanation, justification, or social value systems contain organizations of people’s

272
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

perceptions of others’ value priorities. Both can influ- Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The gen-
erality of the automatic attitude evaluation effect. Journal of
ence people’s attitudinal and behavioral decisions, and
Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893–912.
as Gordon Allport (1955) suggested, reconciliation be- Baron, R. A., & Byrne, D. (1997). Social psychology (8th ed.).
tween action to satisfy personal value priorities or con- Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
formity to social value priorities is likely to be a Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and so-
lifelong process. cial psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Personal and social value systems, worldviews, and Press.
Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phe-
ideology constructs were included in a proposal for the
nomena. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3–26.
process by which personal value priorities cause attitu- Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The interface between intrapsychic and in-
dinal and behavioral decisions. I offer the framework as terpersonal processes: Cognition, emotion, and self as adapta-
a starting point for researchers who, like Allport, believe tions to other people. In J. Cooper & J. M. Darley (Eds.), Attri-
that value systems are the fundamentally important con- bution and social interaction: The legacy of Edward E. Jones
struct for understanding and predicting people’s atti- (pp. 279–292). Washington, DC: American Psychological As-
sociation.
tudes and behavior, but who have found that the Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
confusion associated with the values area reduces the for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva-
potential for focused, systematic investigations. tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
For centuries, psychologists and others have sought Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself:
to understand the fundamental coordinators of human Self-certainty and self-affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 405–417.
behavior. The message embedded in this article is that
Beggan, J. K., & Allison, S. T. (1994). Social values. Encyclopedia of
by conceptualizing these fundamental coordinators as Human Behavior, 4, 253–262.
value systems, we may be able to synthesize and build Biernat, M., Vescio, T. K., Theno, S. A., & Crandall, C. C. (1996).
on the knowledge produced by generations of theorists Values and prejudice: Toward understanding the impact of
and researchers who have focused on this important American values on outgroup attitudes. In C. Seligman, J. M.
construct, whether or not they have used the problem- Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario symposium: The psy-
chology of values (Vol. 8, pp. 153–189). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
atic term values. rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Billig, M. (1991). Ideology and opinions: Studies in rhetorical psy-
References chology. London: Sage.
Billig, M., Condor, S., Edwards, D., Gane, M., Middleton, D., &
Adler, A. (1956). The value concept in sociology. American Journal Radley, A. R. (1988). Ideological dilemmas: A social psychol-
of Sociology, 62, 272–279. ogy of everyday thinking. London: Sage.
Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook Bills, R. E. (1952). The effect of a value on learning. Journal of Per-
of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 798–844). New York: Russell sonality, 21, 217–222.
& Russell. Bilsky, W. (1998a, January). Values and motives. Paper presented at
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psy- the International Research Workshop on Values: Psychological
chology of personality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Structure, Behavioral Outcomes, and Intergenerational Trans-
mission, Maale-Hachamisha, Israel.
Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Bilsky, W. (1998b). Values and motives. Unpublished manuscript,
Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität, Münster, Germany.
Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1960). Study of values.
Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Values and personality. Euro-
Manual and test booklet (3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
pean Journal of Personality, 8, 163–181.
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Braithwaite, V. (1994). Beyond Rokeach’s equality-freedom model:
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality.” In M. P.
Two-dimensional values in a one-dimensional world. Journal
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
of Social Issues, 50, 67–94.
30, pp. 47–92). San Diego: Academic.
Braithwaite, V. (1997). Harmony and security value orientations in
Aristotle. (1980). The Nicomachean ethics (D. Ross, Trans.). Oxford, political evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
England: Oxford University Press. (Original work published tin, 23, 401–414.
circa 350 BCE)
Braithwaite, V. A., & Law, H. G. (1985). Structure of human values:
Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current Testing the adequacy of the Rokeach Value Survey. Journal of
perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 250–263.
social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34). New York: Academic. Braithwaite, V. A., & Scott, W. A. (1991). Values. In J. P. Robinson,
Aronson, E. (1992). The return of the repressed: Dissonance theory P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personal-
makes a comeback. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 303–311. ity and social psychological attitudes (pp. 661–753). New York:
Aronson, E. (1995). The social animal. New York: Freeman. Academic.
Aronson, E., Fried, C., & Stone, J. (1991). Overcoming denial and in- Broughton, R., Boyes, M. C., & Mitchell, J. (1993). DIS-
creasing the intention to use condoms through the induction of tance-from-the-PROtotype (DISPRO) personality assessment
hypocrisy. American Journal of Public Health, 81, 1636–1638. for children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 60, 32–47.
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (1997). Social psychology Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press.
(2nd ed.). New York: Longman. Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA:
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Harvard University Press.
Prentice Hall. Burger, J. M. (1997). Personality. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

273
ROHAN

Campbell, D. T. (1963). Social attitudes and other acquired behav- tions and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp.
ioral dispositions. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a sci- 263–289). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
ence (Vol. 6, pp. 94–172). New York: McGraw-Hill. Feather, N. T. (1992). Values, valences, expectations, and actions.
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 109–124.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538–549. Feather, N. T. (1995). Values, valences, and choice: The influence of
Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). Personality and social intelli- values on the perceived attractiveness and choice of alterna-
gence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. tives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception. 1135–1151.
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol- Feather, N. T. (1996). Values, deservingness, and attitudes toward
ogy (Vol. 12, pp. 3–52). New York: Academic. high achievers: Research on tall poppies. In C. Seligman, J. M.
Cantor, N., Smith, E. E., French, R. de S., & Mezzich, J. (1980). Psy- Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario symposium: The psy-
chiatric diagnosis as prototype categorization. Journal of Ab- chology of values (Vol. 8, pp. 215–251). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
normal Psychology, 89, 181–193. rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Carducci, B. J. (1998). The psychology of personality: Viewpoints, Feather, N. T. (1999). Values, achievement, and justice: Studies in
research, and applications. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. the psychology of deservingness. New York: Kluwer Aca-
Carlson, R. (1981). Studies in script theory: I. Adult analogs of a demic/Plenum Publishers.
childhood nuclear scene. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Feldman Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipo-
chology, 40, 501–510. larity in the structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Carroll, L. (1966). Alice’s adventures in wonderland and through the Psychology, 74, 967–984.
looking glass. London: J. M. Dent & Sons. (Original work pub- Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hu-
lished 1865) man Relations, 7, 117–140.
Cloninger, S. C. (1996). Personality: Description, dynamics, and de- Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto,
velopment. New York: Freeman. CA: Stanford University Press.
The Compact Oxford English Dictionary (2nd ed.). (1991). London: Fiske, A. P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1997). Taboo trade-offs: Reactions to
Oxford University Press. transactions that transgress the spheres of justice. Political Psy-
Conover, P. J., & Feldman, S. (1984). How people organize the politi- chology, 18, 255–297.
cal world: A schematic model. American Journal of Political
Fong, G., & Markus, H. (1982). Self-schemas and judgments about
Science, 28, 95–126.
others. Social Cognition, 1, 191–204.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for
Furnham, A., & Bochner, S. (1986). Culture shock: Psychological
true self-esteem. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and
reaction to unfamiliar environments. London: Methuen.
self-esteem (pp. 31–49). New York: Plenum.
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and
Dempsey, P., & Dukes, W. (1966). Judging complex value stimuli:
frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Re-
An examination and revision of Morris’s “Paths of Life.” Edu-
view, 103, 650–669.
cational and Psychological Measurement, 26, 871–882.
Goodnow, J. J. (1990). The socialization of cognition: What’s in-
de St. Aubin, E. (1996). Personal ideology polarity: Its emotional
volved? In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cul-
foundation and its manifestation in individual value systems, re-
tural psychology. Essays on comparative human development
ligiosity, political orientation, and assumptions concerning hu-
(pp. 259–286). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
man nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
Press.
152–165.
Griffin, D. W., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). The metaphysics of mea-
Deutsch, M., & Collins, M. E. (1951). Interracial housing: A psycho-
surement. In K. Bartholomew (Ed.), Attachment processes in
logical evaluation of a social experiment. Minneapolis: Univer-
adulthood (pp. 17–52). Bristol, PA: Jessica Kingsley.
sity of Minnesota Press.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to Grusec, J. (1997). A history of research on parenting strategies and
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273. children’s internalization of values. In J. E. Grusec & L.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of
Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 3–22). New
Emler, N. P., Ohana, J., & Dickinson, J. (1990). Children’s represen- York: Wiley.
tations of social relations. In G. Duveen & B. Lloyd (Eds.), So- Grusec, J., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). The impact of parental discipline
cial representations and the development of knowledge (pp. methods on the child’s internalization of values: A
161–183). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental
Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Values, stereo- Psychology, 30, 4–19.
types, and emotions as determinants of intergroup attitudes. In Grusec, J. E., & Kuczynski, L. (Eds.). (1997). Parenting and chil-
D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and dren’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary
stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. theory. New York: Wiley.
137–166). New York: Academic. Hart, H. L. A. (1962). The concept of law. Oxford, England: Oxford
Fazio, R. H., & Powell, M. C. (1997). On the value of knowing one’s University Press.
likes and dislikes: Attitude accessibility, stress, and health in Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study.
college. Psychological Science, 8, 430–436. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.
Feather, N. T. (1971). Organization and discrepancy in cognitive Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New
structures. Psychological Review, 78, 355–379. York: Wiley.
Feather, N. T. (1975). Values in education and society. New York: Hewstone, M., Stroebe, W., & Stephenson, G. M. (1996). Introduc-
Free Press. tion to social psychology. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Feather, N. T. (1980). Values in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychol-
Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 247–294). New York: ogist, 52, 1280–1300.
Wiley. Higgins, E. T. (1998). From expectancies to worldviews: Regulatory
Feather, N. T. (1982). Human values and the prediction of action: An focus in socialization and cognition. In J. M. Darley & J. Cooper
expectancy-valence analysis. In N. T. Feather (Ed.), Expecta- (Eds.), Attribution and social interaction: The legacy of Edward

274
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

E. Jones (pp. 243–269). Washington, DC: American Psycho- Luker, K. (1984). Abortion and the politics of motherhood. Berkeley:
logical Association. University of California Press.
Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in cre- Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (1998). Values as truisms: Evidence and
ative thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books. implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1997). The analogical mind. American 294–311.
Psychologist, 52, 35–44. Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (2000). Emergent themes and potential
Ichheiser, G. (1949). Sociophysical and cultural factors in race rela- approaches to attitude function: The function-structure model of
tions. American Journal of Sociology, 54, 395–401. attitudes. In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate:
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Functions of attitudes (pp. 417–442). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Dover. (Original work published 1890) Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (1990). Involvement and persuasion: Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (2000). What is a “value-expressive” atti-
Types, traditions, and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 107, tude? In G. R. Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate:
375–384. Functions of attitudes (pp. 249–269). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Kagan, J. (1999). Three seductive ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
University Press. Mannheim, K. (1972). Ideology and utopia: An introduction to the
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and unin- sociology of knowledge. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
hibited types of children. Child Development, 60, 838–845. (Original work published 1936)
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Markus, H. R. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information
Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163–204. about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Kelly, J. (1982, January 25). We’re not going to make it. Time, 26–27. 35, 63–78.
Kendler, H. H. (1993). Psychology and the ethics of social policy. Mayer, J. D., & Bower, G. H. (1986). Learning and memory for per-
American Psychologist, 48, 1046–1053. sonality prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Kluckhohn, C. K. M. (1951). Values and value orientations in the the- ogy, 51, 473–492.
ory of action. In T. Parsons & E. Sils (Eds.), Toward a general Mayton, D. M. II, Ball-Rokeach, S. J., & Loges, W. E. (1994). Hu-
theory of action (pp. 388–433). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- man values and social issues: An introduction. Journal of Social
versity Press. Issues, 50, 1–18.
Kluckhohn, F. R., & Strodtbeck, F. (1961). Variations in value orien- McAdams, D. P. (1999). Personal narratives and the life story. In L.
tations. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson & Co. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
Kochanska, G., & Thompson, R. A. (1997). The emergence and de- and research (2nd ed., pp. 478–500). New York: Guilford.
velopment of conscience in toddlerhood and early childhood. In McClintock, C. G. (1978). Social values: Their definition, measure-
J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s ment, and development. Journal of Research and Development
internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory in Education, 12, 121–137.
(pp. 53–77). New York: Wiley. McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness.
Kristiansen, C. M., & Hotte, A. M. (1996). Morality and the self: Im- Psychological Bulletin, 120, 323–337.
plications for the when and how of value–attitude–behavior re- McLellan, D. (1986). Ideology. Milton Keynes, England: Open Uni-
lations. In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The versity Press.
Ontario symposium: The psychology of values (Vol. 8, pp. Mikulincer, M. (1998). Attachment working models and the sense of
77–105). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. trust: An exploration of interaction goals and affect regulation.
Kristiansen, C. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1988). Justifying attitudes by ap- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1209–1224.
pealing to values: A functional perspective. British Journal of Mischel, W. (1998). Metacognition at the hyphen of social-cognitive
Social Psychology, 27, 247–256. psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2,
Kristiansen, C. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). The rhetorical use of val- 84–86.
ues to justify social and intergroup attitudes. Journal of Social Moretti, M. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1999). Internal representations of
Issues, 50, 47–65. others in self-regulation: A new look at a classic issue. Social
LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, Cognition, 17, 186–208.
230–237. Morris, C. (1956). Varieties of human value. Chicago: University of
Lavine, H., Thomsen, C. J., & Gonzales, M. H. (1997). The develop- Chicago Press.
ment of interattitudinal consistency: The shared-consequences Myers, D. (1996). Social psychology (5th ed.). New York:
model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, McGraw-Hill.
735–749. Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1998). The mea-
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. (1995). Social anxiety. New York: surement of values and individualism–collectivism. Personality
Guilford. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1177–1189.
Lee, D. (1948). Are basic needs ultimate? Journal of Abnormal and Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (1968). A cognitive model of attitudi-
Social Psychology, 43, 391–395. nal involvement. In R. P. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire,
Levitin, T. (1968). Values. In J. P. Robinson & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), T. M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.),
Measures of social psychological attitudes (pp. 405–501). Ann Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook (pp. 373–383).
Arbor: University of Michigan Press, Survey Research Center, Chicago: Rand McNally.
Institute for Social Research. Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (1969). Cognitive bonding to central
Lewin, K. (1952). Constructs in field theory [1944]. In D. Cartwright values and resistance to a communication advocating change in
(Ed.), Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers policy orientation. Journal of Experimental Research in Per-
by Kurt Lewin (pp. 30–42). London: Tavistock. sonality, 4, 42–50.
Liebert, R. M., & Liebert, L. L. (1998). Liebert & Spiegler’s person- Parsons, T. (1951). The social system. London: Routledge & Kegan
ality: Strategies and issues (8th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Paul.
Brooks/Cole. Peck, R. F., & Havinghurst, R. J. (1960). The psychology of character
Liebrand, W. B. G., & Dehue, F. M. J. (1996). Social values. In A. S. development. New York: Wiley.
R. Manstead & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Blackwell encyclopedia of Perelman, C. (1982). The realm of rhetoric. Notre Dame: University
social psychology (pp. 609–614). Oxford, England: Blackwell. of Notre Dame Press.

275
ROHAN

Pervin, L. A. (1996). The science of personality. New York: Wiley. Schwartz, S. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and
Postman, L., Bruner, J. S., & McGinnies, E. (1948). Personal values contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19–45.
as selective factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and So- Schwartz, S. (1996). Value priorities and behavior: Applying a the-
cial Psychology, 43, 142–154. ory of integrated value systems. In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, &
Pratto, F. (1994). Consciousness and automatic evaluation. In P. M. M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario symposium: The psychology of
Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional values (Vol. 8, pp. 1–24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
influences in perception and attention (pp. 115–143). San sociates, Inc.
Diego, CA: Academic. Schwartz, S. (1999). A theory of cultural values and some implica-
Pratto, F. (in press). The puzzle of continuing group inequality: tions for work. Applied Psychology: An International Review,
Piecing together psychological, social, and cultural forces in so- 48, 23–47.
cial dominance theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in exper- Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a psychological struc-
imental social psychology (Vol. 31). San Diego, CA: Academic. ture of human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). So- chology, 53, 550–562.
cial dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a psychological struc-
social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social ture of human values: Extensions and cross-cultural replica-
Psychology, 67, 741–763. tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
Pratto, F., Stallworth, L. M., & Conway-Lanz, S. (1999). Social dom- 878–891.
inance orientation and the ideological legitimization of social Schwartz, S. H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture specifics in
policy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1853–1875. the content and structure of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Pratto, F., Tatar, D., & Conway-Lanz, S. (1999). Who gets what and Psychology, 26, 92–116.
why? Determinants of social allocations. Political Psychology, Sears, D. O., & Kinder, D. R. (1985). Whites’ opposition to busing:
20, 127–150. On conceptualizing and operationalizing group conflict. Jour-
Ramm, D. R. (1998). Consider the scientific study of morality. Amer- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1141–1147.
ican Psychologist, 53, 323–324. Seligman, C., & Katz, A. N. (1996). The dynamics of value systems.
Reich, D. A., & Weary, G. (1998). Depressives’ future event schemas In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario
and the social inference process. Journal of Personality and So- symposium: The psychology of values (Vol. 8, pp. 53–75).
cial Psychology, 74, 1133–1145. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Rohan, M. J. (1998, January). Value priorities, value systems, and the Setterlund, M. B., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1993). “Who am I? Why am I
value fingerprint: Reflections on development. Paper presented here?”: Self-esteem, self-clarity, and prototype matching. Jour-
at the International Research Workshop on Values: Psychologi- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 769–780.
cal Structure, Behavioral Outcomes, and Intergenerational Shand, A. F. (1896). Character and the emotions. Mind, 17, 203–226.
Transmission, Maale-Hachamisha, Israel. Shand, A. F. (1914). The foundations of character: Being a study of
Rohan, M. J. (2000). Describing self-esteem: The achievement-integ- the tendencies of the emotions and sentiments. London:
rity framework. Manuscript submitted for publication. Macmillan.
Rohan, M., & Harris, B. (1999). [Daily decisions: In line with per- Sherif, A. F., & Cantril, H. (1947). The psychology of ego-involve-
sonal or social value priorities?] Unpublished raw data. ments: Social attitudes and identifications. New York: Wiley.
Rohan, M. J., & Maiden, W. (2000). Investigations into “value fit.” Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup
Unpublished manuscript, University of New South Wales, Syd- theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cam-
ney, Australia. bridge University Press.
Rohan, M. J., & Zanna, M. P. (1996). Value transmission in families. Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., & Bobo, L. (1996). Racism, conservatism, af-
In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario firmative action, and intellectual sophistication: A matter of
symposium: The psychology of values (Vol. 8, pp. 253–276). principled conservatism or group dominance? Journal of Per-
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. sonality and Social Psychology, 70, 476–490.
Rohan, M. J., & Zanna, M. P. (1998). The “products of socialization”: Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (1995). Social psychology. New York:
A discussion of self-regulatory strategies and value systems. In Worth.
J. Cooper & J. M. Darley (Eds.), Attribution and social interac- Smith, M. B. (1969). Social psychology and human values: Selected
tion: The legacy of Edward E. Jones (pp. 279–292). Washing- essays. Chicago: Aldine.
ton, DC: American Psychological Association. Smith, M. B., Bruner, J. S., & White, R. W. (1956). Opinions and per-
Rohan, M. J., & Zanna, M. P. (in press). Values and ideologies. In A. sonality. New York: Wiley.
Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psy- Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1998). Understanding personality and so-
chology: Intraindividual processes (Vol. 1). Oxford, England: cial behavior: A functionalist strategy. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T.
Blackwell. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
Rokeach, M. (1968). Beliefs, attitudes, and values. San Francisco: (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 635–679). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Jossey-Bass. Spinoza, B. (1994). The Ethics III. In E. Curley (Ed. and Trans.), A
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Spinoza reader: The Ethics and other works. Princeton, NJ:
Press. Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1644)
Rokeach, M. (1979). From individual to institutional values: With Spranger, E. (1928). Types of men: The psychology and ethics of per-
special reference to the values of science. In M. Rokeach (Ed.), sonality (P. J. W. Pigors, Trans.). Halle, Germany: Max
Understanding human values: Individual and societal (pp. Niemeyer Verlag.
47–70). New York: Free Press. Tetlock, P. E. (1986). A value pluralism model of ideological reason-
Sagiv, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (1995). Value priorities and readiness ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 819–827.
for out-group social contact. Journal of Personality and Social Tetlock, P. E. (in press). Coping with trade-offs: Psychological con-
Psychology, 69, 437–448. straints and political implications. In S. Lupia, M. McCubbins,
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of val- & S. Popkin (Eds.), Political reasoning and choice. Berkeley:
ues: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In University of California Press.
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Tetlock, P. E., Armor, D., & Peterson, R. S. (1994). The slavery de-
(Vol. 24, pp. 1–65). San Diego: Academic. bate in antebellum America: Cognitive style, value conflict, and

276
THE VALUES CONSTRUCT

the limits of compromise. Journal of Personality and Social tive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of
Psychology, 66, 115–126. Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 733–746.
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. B.,Green, M. C., & Lerner, J. Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of
S. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, personal expressiveness (edudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment.
forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678–691.
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 853–870. Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in
Tetlock, P. E., Peterson, R. S., & Lerner, J. S. (1996). Revising the social life. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The
value pluralism model: Incorporating social content and context handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 446–496).
postulates. In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), New York: McGraw-Hill.
The Ontario symposium: The psychology of values (Vol. 8, pp.
Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life
25–51). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
events: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 59,
Thagard, P., & Shelley, C. P. (in press). Emotional analogies and ana-
339–354.
logical inference. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N.
Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cogni- Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of
tive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitudes objects. Jour-
Thomas, W. I., & Znanieki, F. (1958). The Polish peasant in Europe nal of Social Issues, 25, 41–78.
and America. New York: Dover. (Original work published Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Interpersonal trust and attitudes toward
1918) human nature. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S.
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect imagery consciousness (Vol. 1). New Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psycho-
York: Springer. logical attitudes (pp. 373–412). New York: Academic.
Tomkins, S. S. (1966). Affect and the psychology of knowledge. In S. Ybarra, O., & Trafimow, D. (1998). How priming the private self or
S. Tomkins & C. E. Izard (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and person- collective self affects the relative weights of attitudes and sub-
ality: Empirical studies (pp. 72–97). London: Tavistock. jective norms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
Tomkins, S. S. (1979). Script theory. In H. E. Howe, Jr., & R. A. 362–370.
Dienstbier (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 26, Young, K. (1946). Handbook of social psychology. London:
pp. 201–236). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., & D’Andrade, R. (1991). Attitude
Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). Dissonance and the pill: An attri-
structure and belief accessibility. Journal of Experimental So-
bution approach to studying the arousal properties of disso-
cial Psychology, 27, 48–75.
nance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29,
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and
703–709.
collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on moti-
vation (pp. 41–133). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Zanna, M. P., Haddock, G., & Esses, V. M. (1990, June). On the na-
Vallone, R. P., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media ture of prejudice. Paper presented at the Nags Head Conference
phenomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias on Stereotypes and Intergroup Relations, Kill Devil Hill, NC.
in coverage of the “Beirut Massacre.” Journal of Personality Zimbardo, P. G., Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Jaffe, D. (1982). The
and Social Psychology, 58, 582–592. psychology of imprisonment. In J. C. Brigham & L.
Van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., De Bruin, E. M. N., & Joireman, J. A. Wrightsman (Eds.), Contemporary issues in social psychology
(1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competi- (4th ed., pp. 230–235). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

277

Вам также может понравиться