Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

Review of General Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 17, No. 1, 28 –39 1089-2680/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030030

Origins of Modern Ideas on Love and Loss:


Contrasting Forerunners of Attachment Theory

Margaret S. Stroebe John Archer


Utrecht University and University of Groningen University of Central Lancashire

In this article we examine some origins of John Bowlby’s attachment theory, a highly influential
scientific approach to love and loss in contemporary society. Although some potential influences have
been well-documented, others have either received no recognition or have failed to have an impact. We
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

focus specifically on three of Bowlby’s predecessors, exploring how these were differentially influential
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

on his work. The first of these, Charles Darwin, was amply endorsed by Bowlby, both in terms of the
adaptive background to his theory and more specifically in relation to Darwin’s study of the emotions
associated with grief. The second, Alexander Shand, was recognized as important but is cited little and
omitted from the central issue of the resolution of grief. The third, Bertrand Russell, formulated ideas on
attachment and separation before Bowlby, and possibly contributed to the intellectual forces that
influenced him too. To our knowledge, Russell’s work was not cited by Bowlby, despite the fact that it
contained the seeds of many of Bowlby’s ideas on attachment. It remains unclear whether this was
because he had not read Russell or through omission; there is no definitive evidence either way. Tracing
these historical origins illustrates how theory development involves a process of integration and selection,
how even radical theories are rooted in previous scholarship, and how it can take decades for inspiring
ideas to develop into full-blown, well-tested, theories.

Keywords: attachment, theory, grief, bereavement, historical origins

Modern ideas on love (attachment) and loss (grief) are those that scrutiny. In particular, he advocated replacing psychoanalytically
have taken the study of these topics from the realm of clinical based speculations with a body of knowledge derived largely from
speculation to that of empirically tested theory. They include the empirical research evidence. As he put it: “as a body of theory
substantiation of attachment theory principles (see, e.g., Mi- psychoanalysis has suffered from a number of crippling handi-
kulincer & Shaver, 2008), derived from the earlier writings of caps” (Bowlby, 1980b, p. 650). He went on to state that, as a result
Bowlby (1961, 1969/1982); and the empirical study of grief (e.g., of deriving its material from patients’ “inevitably biased ac-
Bonanno, & Keltner, 1997; Stroebe, & Stroebe, 1991), based on counts,” “hypotheses, unchecked by independent data, have run
Bowlby’s (1973, 1980a) later work on permanent as well as wild” (p. 650). By the close of the last century, attachment theory
temporary losses of attachment. had evolved into what Cassidy and Shaver (1999) described as
The origins of most of the current ideas that formed the basis of “one of the broadest, most profound, and most creative lines of
empirical studies in this area are to be found in Bowlby’s work research in 20th-century psychology” (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999,
(Bowlby, 1961, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980b), together with that of Preface; p. x).
two of his colleagues, Mary Ainsworth and Colin Murray Parkes, In developing his ideas, Bowlby obviously had a number of
who developed the study of attachment security and styles (e.g., intellectual influences, and many of their contributions are dis-
Ainsworth, & Bowlby, 1954; Bowlby, Ainsworth, Boston & cussed extensively in his three-volume work on attachment and
Rosenbluth, 1956), and grief (Bowlby & Parkes, 1970; Parkes, loss (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980a). Other sources of influ-
2006). Although a clinician, whose training was rooted in psycho- ence include those from his personal life, ranging from his early
analysis (van der Horst & van der Veer, 2010), Bowlby saw the childhood separation experiences to the formative impact of his
need to develop a body of theory that could be subject to empirical work with troubled young people in early adulthood (Holmes,
1993). They also involve the historical and societal context in
which he developed his ideas (see van Dijken, van der Veer, van
Izendoorn, & Kuijpers, 1998; Newcombe & Lerner, 1982; van der
This article was published Online First February 4, 2013. Horst, 2011). But it was in close collaboration with Ainsworth and
Margaret S. Stroebe, Department of Clinical & Health Psychology, Parkes that Bowlby derived the basic tenets of attachment theory.
Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands and Department of Clinical
Subsequent ideas about grief came from diverse fields, including
Psychology & Experimental Psychopathology, University of Groningen,
Groningen, The Netherlands; John Archer, School of Psychology, Univer-
cybernetics, information processing, developmental psychology,
sity of Central Lancashire, Preston, Lancashire, United Kingdom. and ethology (Bretherton, 1992; van der Horst, 2011). In formu-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Margaret lating the essentially biological nature of these processes, he in-
S. Stroebe, Department of Clinical & Health Psychology, Utrecht Univer- corporated, in particular, the contributions of Darwin and the
sity, Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: M.S.Stroebe@UU.NL European ethologists, and also U.S. comparative psychologists. Of
28
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 29

course, psychoanalytic writings— on loss in particular—were used spired—selectively— by scholarship that is around at a particular
extensively to develop his theory. period in history. Before embarking on our discussion of these
Bowlby had personal contact with a broad range of scholars who three influences, we provide a brief outline the main tenets of
were also influential in forming his ideas, including those associ- Bowlby’s theory of attachment and loss.
ated with the British Psychoanalytical Society (Holmes, 1993). He
worked closely with James Robertson, documenting and filming
patterns of reactions of hospitalized children to separation from Attachment Theory
their parents (Bowlby & Robertson, 1952; see van der Horst & van The core ideas embedded in Bowlby’s work (Bowlby, 1953a,
der Veer, 2009). In the early 1950s he developed a friendship with 1969/1982, 1973, 1980a) are many and complex. We risk over-
Konrad Lorenz (van der Horst, 2011), and subsequently developed simplifying them in a short summary, but brief descriptions of
close collaboration with the British ethologist Robert Hinde, who some concepts are necessary for developing our discussion of their
provided “generous and stern guidance” (Bowlby, 1980b, p. 650), origins. Attachment can be defined simply as an emotional con-
and who joined Bowlby’s seminars at the Tavistock Clinic. Here nection to someone, evidenced by proximity seeking, feelings of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

“Freudian and Kleinian analysts, Hullian and Skinnerian learning security in the persons’ presence, and protest on separation from
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

theorists, a Piagetian, psychiatric social workers, and an antipsy- this attachment figure. Early theorists were interested in the in-
chiatrist” joined together to search for a framework to understand fant’s attachment to its mother, the need for such a connection
the mother-infant relationship (Stevenson-Hinde, 2007, p. 337). being a fundamental part of human experience (Ainsworth, Blehar,
Furthermore, even though he was convinced that he was develop- Waters, & Wall, 1978). Affect regulation was understood to be the
ing novel scientific truth in forming attachment theory, Bowlby force impelling proximity seeking during periods of separation
acknowledged that others thought along similar lines. In particular, (anxiety) from the attachment figure. The clinging of the infant to
he spoke of Donald Winnicott (1896 –1971), pediatrician, psychi- the mother was regarded as serving the biological purpose of
atrist, sociologist, and psychoanalyst, who wrote about early emo- keeping the infant close and thereby increasing its chance of
tional development in the 1940s and was particularly recognized survival (originally in an environment beset by predators). Infants
for his concept of the “good enough mother,” which refers to maintain proximity to their caregiver, they stay nearby and safe,
maternal sensitivity, a concept that was compatible with attach- and this provides a source of comfort and protection from threat-
ment theory principles (Fonagy, 1999). Bowlby acknowledged ening experiences. As such, attachment provides a secure base (the
Winnicott in the following way: “the integrating function of the feeling of safety provided by an attachment figure, see Ainsworth,
unique mother-figure is one the importance of which I believe can 1982; Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1988) for the infant to explore out-
hardly be exaggerated; in this I am at one with Winnicott who has wardly, and the possibility of returning to this safe haven (i.e., the
constantly emphasized it” (Bowlby, 1958, p. 370). place to seek reassurance when distressed). Thus, the infant is
In this article, while recognizing these many influences on biologically disposed to use the caregiver as a haven of safety
Bowlby’s work, we focus on the intellectual contributions of three while exploring the environment. Infants need to know that the
earlier authors to Bowlby’s ideas on attachment and loss, so as to caregiver is dependable, in the sense of being there when needed,
highlight the contrasting nature of their contributions. The first, providing a sense of secure attachment, a solid base from which to
Charles Darwin, was both cited and fully used by Bowlby; the explore the world.
second, Alexander Shand, was initially cited approvingly, yet not Secure attachment is reflected in normal distress when the care-
used in formulating the central aspects of Bowlby’s theory of grief; giver leaves, and pleasure on her return. However, there are also
and the third, Bertrand Russell, had ideas very similar in outline to patterns of insecure attachment, which—importantly—are linked
the key concepts of attachment theory and yet was not cited at all. to inconsistent treatment, neglect, and/or rejection by the care-
We begin with Charles Darwin, whose contributions were to giver. As such, the style of attachment of the caregiver has a major
provide an evolutionary background for attachment, and to explore impact on that of the infant/child. There is a certain degree of
the emotional expression of grief in The Expression of the Emo- concordance, cross-generation transmission of patterns of attach-
tions in Man and Animals. Alexander Shand was a British psy- ment (e.g., George & Solomon, 1999; Rosenstein & Horowitz,
chologist, well known during the earlier part of the last century, 1996). Three insecure forms of attachment have been identified
who wrote about grief in his book The Foundations of Character. (e.g., Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main & Solomon, 1986). Ambivalent
The third source is Bertrand Russell’s The Conquest of Happiness. (preoccupied) attachment is characterized by clinging when the
We examine the latter in most detail, because, while Bowlby caregiver leaves, and rejection and anger on return. Avoidant
claimed to recognize the contributions of Darwin and Shand, to the (dismissing) infants stay calm when the mother leaves, but avoid
best of our knowledge, Russell is not mentioned by Bowlby or his and reject her on return. Disorganized (disoriented) infants display
associates, and yet his ideas bear a striking similarity to funda- variable behavior on such occasions, showing contradictory and
mental tenets of attachment theory. However, it is important to sometimes apparently disoriented behavior.1 Although there are
note that it is not our purpose to argue that Bowlby should have long-term effects of early attachment insecurity experiences on
given credit to Russell. The difference between Russell’s propo- mental health (e.g., they are associated with psychopathology,
sitions based on his experience and observations (in one book
chapter), and Bowlby’s systematic formulation and painstaking
1
development of his ideas into a broader, far more elaborate theory Later, rather than styles, dimensions of avoidance and anxiety in (adult)
relationships were described and these were either related to working
(culminating in a three volume work), would not speak for this. models of the self and of others (e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), or
Rather, in tracing each of the above-mentioned historical origins, to attachment-related avoidance and attachment-related anxiety (Mi-
our aim is to illustrate how innovative theorizing may be in- kulincer & Shaver, 2007).
30 STROEBE AND ARCHER

such as personality disorders), it is also important to note that environment. In particular, Bowlby argued that the primary func-
insecure attachment styles are considered adaptive in the sense that tion of the attachment bond between offspring and parent was
they enable children to cope in the face of less-than-optimal protection from predators, especially important when there is a
parental circumstances (Shorey & Snyder, 2006). long period of infant dependence (Bowlby, 1969, pp. 275–278).
Not surprisingly, given such long-term connections, the idea of This is likely to be correct, from what is now known about the
continuity or stability in patterns of attachment across the life span is danger that predators posed to early hominins, and indeed to
fundamental to attachment theory. Childhood attachment styles per- humans today (Hart & Sussman, 2005).
sist with reasonable stability and influence adult behavior, although Bowlby did not address the more puzzling question of why, from
they can also change, being influenced by relationship experiences an evolutionary perspective, there should be insecure forms of attach-
and/or (traumatic) life events (Feeney, 1999). Just as security of ment, which he viewed as pathological consequences of certain forms
attachment influences the development and maintenance of adult of parental behavior. Their frequency, and relative consistency in
relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987), so does it affect the ending of different cultures, both raise the question of why such apparently
such relationships, when a loved one dies. For example, securely pathological forms should be so common. It was only later, when
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attached individuals are generally able to cope and come to terms with evolutionary theorists applied the concept of alternative evolutionary
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

their bereavement more easily than insecurely attached individuals strategies—those that would be differentially adaptive in different
(Fraley & Bonanno, 2004; Parkes, 2006). According to attachment environments—to the study of attachment that this issue was ad-
theory, loss involves a set of sequential reactions, essentially protest, dressed. At present, the two main theories are that insecure attach-
searching and despair, forming a stage or phase view of loss (Bowlby, ments are adaptive either as reproductive strategies under certain
1980a; Bowlby & Parkes, 1970). Underlying these grief reactions is frequently occurring environmental conditions (Belsky, Steinberg, &
the process of grief work, an effortful struggle to confront thoughts Draper, 1991), or as differential responses to danger that will collec-
and feelings relating to the deceased, to come to terms with the fact tively advantage the group to which they belong (Ein-Dor, Mi-
that the loved one is no longer present. kulincer, Doron, & Shaver, 2010).
The body of empirical evidence supporting the attachment the- How it is that the apparently maladaptive process of grief could
ory principles described above has expanded enormously across have arisen by natural selection has proved a puzzle for many re-
recent decades (see, e.g., Cassidy & Shaver, 2008; Mikulincer & searchers (Archer, 1999, pp. 59– 64). Bowlby answered this question
Shaver, 2007). For example, Waters, Hamilton and Weinfeld in the following way: grief forms only a very small part of the much
(2000) presented three long-term longitudinal studies relevant to more common separation reactions, whose adaptive value is apparent.
the hypothesis that adolescent/adult representations of attachment He stated: “In the course of our evolution, it appears, our instinctual
derive (in part) from attachment security in infancy. Like others equipment has come to be so fashioned that all losses have been
(e.g., Cassidy, 2000; Fraley & Shaver, 2000), Waters et al. (2000) assumed to be retrievable and are responded to accordingly.”
concluded that there was much evidence supporting this stability (Bowlby, 1961, p. 333). Thus the mechanism controlling these reac-
of attachment hypothesis. Discontinuity in attachment representa- tions is of ancient origin and not sufficiently flexible to apply to one
tions was related to negative life events and circumstances, as case and not the other. This view was reiterated subsequently in the
attachment theory also maintains. last part of Bowlby’s trilogy (Bowlby (1980a, p. 91).
Darwin’s writings on grief are to be found in his book on the
Charles Darwin’s Influence on Bowlby expression of emotions (Darwin, 1872/1904). Three aspects are
noteworthy: first, detailed descriptions of the expressions associ-
There were two aspects of Darwin’s work that influenced Bowl-
ated with grief, and in particular weeping; second, descriptions of
by’s thinking. The first is evolution by natural selection (Darwin,
similar expressions in several monkeys and apes, thus locating
1859/1911) as a basis for understanding the origin and scope of
grief in the biological heritage of the human species; and third the
attachment and loss; the second is Darwin’s specific writings on
dual reactions associated with grief, which formed a basis for
grief (Darwin, 1872/1904), particularly his description of the dual
phase or stage descriptions of grief and separation reactions.
nature of the grief process, on which Bowlby based his stage or
Bowlby (1961, p. 333) used Darwin’s description of crying in
phase view of loss.
both separated infants and bereaved adults to suggest that both are
Bowlby (1973, pp. 449 –53) used Darwin’s writings on evolu-
activated by the same system, “an ancient instinctual response.” As
tion by natural selection (Darwin, 1859/1911) to underpin his
a psychoanalyst he was particularly interested in this link with the
conception of attachment and separation, and to contrast this
childhood separation response, because it resonated with his two
approach with Freud’s ideas which were not evolutionarily based
previous articles on separation and loss in young infants (Bowlby,
(Bowlby, 1973, pp. 50, 105). Bowlby (1973, p. 106) stated that the
1960a, 1960b). Bowlby (1961, p. 338) credited Parkes with alert-
paradigm he was using to understand separation reactions was
ing him to Darwin’s work on grief, some 10 years before Parkes’
derived from “current evolution theory” and he set out several
(1972) book on grief was published.2 It is not clear whether it was
aspects that follow from this, mainly derived from ethological
awareness of Darwin’s writings that enabled Bowlby (1961), and
theory, in which he had been interested for some time (Bowlby,
independently Pollock (1961), to view the mainly anecdotal ac-
1953b; see van der Horst, 2011, pp. 75–102).
counts of grief in animals as part of a wider picture informed by
In developing his conception of attachment, Bowlby (1969/
1982) made a clear distinction (following Tinbergen, 1951/1969) evolutionary thinking. The common reactions shown by human
between causation (the motivational system) and evolutionary
function: this referred to the consequences of attachment that had 2
This is to be found in Bowlby’s foreword to the first edition of Parkes
led to its perpetuation by natural selection in the human ancestral (1972).
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 31

and nonhuman mammals, and the similarities between grief and 1995; Stroebe, Stroebe, Schut, Zech, & van den Bout, 2002) and
separation reactions, led to their subsequently being viewed as theoretical formulations (e.g., Archer, 2008; Bonanno & Kaltman,
common biological reactions with the same underlying cause. 1999; Stroebe & Schut, 1999), so that its central premise is now
A more direct influence on Bowlby’s theorizing about attach- doubtful.
ment and loss stems from two passages in which Darwin (1872/ In this section we consider an alternative source known to
1904, pp. 79 – 81, 181–183) described the general effects of grief Bowlby, and whose ideas on the resolution of grief resonate more
in terms of two reactions: one involves violent and frantic move- with modern empirical evidence than those of Freud. Bowlby
ments, including weeping and anger, and the other—following on warmly endorsed the contributions of the British psychologist
from this—is a passive reaction, involving feelings of sadness and Alexander Shand to the subject of grief, stating that he “delineates
despair. Bowlby (1973) makes the same distinction when he writes most of the features of grief as we now know them” (Bowlby,
of the separation reactions of a child, as follows: 1980a, pp. 24–25). Both here and earlier (Bowlby, 1961) he also
stated that Shand’s book “deserves to be better known.” He added
The way he behaves follows a typical sequence. At first he protests
vigorously and tries by all the means available to him to recover his that “Shand’s. . .ideas are strongly endorsed and given a central
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mother. Later he seems to despair of recovering her but none the less position” (Bowlby, 1980a, p. 27). Yet, apart from one quote
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

remains preoccupied with her and vigilant for her return (p. 46). regarding the central importance of anger in grief (p. 29), and a
short quote a further two pages on (pp. 31–2), remarking on the
Bowlby added a further phase to this sequence: “Later he seems complexity of grief, Shand is not mentioned in any of the remain-
to lose interest in his mother and to become emotionally detached ing 408 pages of Bowlby’s book.3 Nor is he cited by Parkes (1972,
from her.” (p. 46). The three words in italics formed the basis of 1986, 1996). Thus Shand’s writings on grief were known by
his phase or stage view of separation, set out previously (Bowlby, Bowlby, and apparently approved of, yet are only cited briefly in
1960a, 1961): in this later excerpt, we can see their similarity with developing his ideas.
the two passages in Darwin (1872/1904), when he contrasted the Alexander Shand was an influential early British psychologist
initial distress reaction with a subsequent one characterized by best known for the three editions of his book The Foundations of
sadness and despair. In the first of these passages, Darwin (1872/ Character (Shand, 1914, 1920, 1927). In his obituary, C.S. Myers
1904, pp. 79) wrote the following: “When a mother suddenly loses (1936) wrote: “This standard work is so well known and appreci-
her child, sometimes she is frantic with grief, and must be con- ated by all students of the subject that its admirable and enduring
sidered to be in an excited state.” After elaborating the character- qualities need not be stressed here.” Yet his work was little known
istics of the “excited state” he wrote: “As soon as the sufferer is
in Bowlby’s time and remains little known today. One part of the
fully conscious that nothing can be done, despair and deep sorrow
book sets out “the laws of sorrow,” in modern terms a series of
takes the place of frantic grief” (p. 80).
empirically testable hypotheses about grief. Shand described the
Bowlby later added another phase, the initial shock and disbe-
main features of grief and its individual variations, and other
lief, to form four phases of grief (Bowlby & Parkes, 1970; Bowlby,
aspects that we know from modern research, and illustrated them
1980a). Although this view of grief has been influential among
with reference to the writings of the great philosophers, poets,
practitioners, empirical evidence indicates a more flexible process
playwrights, and novelists, there being no empirical research re-
involving the intermingling of aspects of the distress and despair
lating to them at the time of writing.
reactions (Archer, 1999, pp. 97–100). Nevertheless the existence
Before we consider Shand’s “laws of sorrow,” it is worth noting
of an active stress reaction and a more passive longer-term one to
that he also commented on the function of grief in the following
both separation and loss is well established, whether it is sequential
or intermingled. way: “It is the great function of sorrow in love, through its
In summary, the main influence of Darwin on Bowlby’s con- tendencies of attraction and restoration, to establish a durable bond
ception of attachment and loss lies in the location of these pro- with the object. For the bond which joy alone forms with an object
cesses in the evolved repertoire of the species, having arisen by would in its absence be quickly dissolved, were there no sorrow to
natural selection, and showing similarities to reactions found in reinforce it” (Shand, 1914, p. 333). Shand’s comments are similar
nonhuman animals. A secondary influence is Darwin’s two- to a view expressed by Darwin (1843), in a letter to his cousin,
process characterization of grief, which formed the basis of Bowl- whose wife had died in childbirth. Darwin wrote: “grief is the
by’s description of both separation and grief. necessary price for having been born with . . . such feelings” (i.e.,
love for his wife). This view was not developed by Bowlby or
Parkes, but was taken up in later writings on grief, by Badcock
Alexander Shand and the Laws of Sorrow (1990), and Archer (1999, 2001), who stated that grief is a by-
Despite the important influence of Darwin, Bowlby’s primary product of “mechanisms whose primary function is to maintain
source in formulating his ideas about grief was Freud’s Mourning social relationships that are crucial for fitness” (Archer, 2001, p.
and Melancholia (Freud, 1917/1957), in which it is argued that 269). Without the mental pain associated with separation or loss,
grief is an active process taking up time and energy and that it relationships would assume an “out of sight, out of mind” char-
serves the function of detaching the person from painful memories acter.
of the deceased. This process was later termed “grief work,” and it
became accepted as being essential to the resolution of grief 3
Shand appears in the index for pages 131 and 132 but is not referred to
(Archer, 1999, pp. 108–129). Empirical challenges to this view on either page: this is likely to have been a misprint for pages 31–32, where
began with the study by Stroebe and Stroebe (1991) and continued he is mentioned but not indexed. Altogether there are five index entries for
with further studies (e.g., Bonanno, Keltner, Holen, & Horowitz, Shand, whereas there are 22 for Freud.
32 STROEBE AND ARCHER

A summary of the so-called “Laws of sorrow” is presented in Shand (1914) noted that grief tended to evoke anger, writing that
Table 1. Shand’s “laws” described several of the central features of “The tendency of sorrow to arouse anger under certain conditions
grief later articulated by Bowlby and Parkes, although Shand is appears to be part of the fundamental constitution of the mind” (p.
only cited in relation to two issues, the urge to regain the lost 347). Bowlby (1980a, p. 29) cited this statement, noting the central
loved-one, and anger as a part of the grief process. In addition, he place of anger in Shand’s writings on grief, although Shand is not
noted the importance of the strength of the attachment for gener- mentioned again in the many subsequent references to anger by
ating the grief reaction, and identification with the deceased. He Bowlby, nor in the chapter on anger in Parkes’ book.
also described important sources of individual differences, and In most cases, there are similarities between Shand’s “laws of
contextual influences on grief, such as the additional distress when sorrow” (see Table 1) and Bowlby’s later writings on grief, two of
death is sudden, and the importance of social support. which are acknowledged, and others unremarked. There is, in
All of these topics have their parallels in Bowlby’s and Parkes’ addition, one crucial difference between their writings. Shand
writings. Bowlby (1980, p. 27) endorsed Shand’s contribution to distinguished between (1) disclosing thoughts and feelings asso-
understanding “the complex motivation present in situations evok- ciated with grief to others, which he stated is beneficial, and (2) the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ing grief” (italics in original), in particular the need to seek ways expression of negative emotions, which he stated is not. These two
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of being reunited with the deceased. Shand (1914, p. 323) had features have been viewed together as components of “grief work”
described the way that a bereaved person is attracted to the lost in the formulation derived from Freud. However, modern research
loved-one and seeks to maintain all that remains of their former has clearly demonstrated that they are distinct in the way that
union. Bowlby gave prominence to the urge to search for in his Shand indicated (see Table 1).
account of grief, naming the active phase of grief as “the phase of The theory of grief work holds that a resolution of grief (some-
yearning and searching for the lost figure” (Bowlby, 1980a, p. 86). thing that typically occurs in most bereaved people over time) can
Parkes (1972) devoted a chapter to “searching” in his book on only be achieved by confronting thoughts and feelings associated
bereavement, and he viewed the urge to be reunited with the with the loss, and actively working through these over time (Ar-
deceased as central to the grief process and the key to understand- cher, 1999, pp. 119 –125; Stroebe, 1992–3; Stroebe & Stroebe,
ing the motivation behind the feelings and actions of bereaved 1991). It follows from this that avoiding expression of the emo-
people. tions associated with grief should be detrimental to progress to-

Table 1
A Selection of Shand’s Laws of Sorrow

Lawa Comment Modern sources

58. When a primary desire is frustrated, sorrow Grief is proportional to the strength of attachment to Archer (1999, pp. 165–167)
tends to occur, in proportion to the strength the lost person or object.
of the desire Grief is viewed as a form of frustration – of an Parkes (1972, pp. 74–76); Klinger (1975)
important biological system involving emotional
attachments
59. The grieving person is attracted to the Grief involves an active searching component, Parkes (1972, pp. 57–76); Bowlby (1980a,
loved object and strives to maintain the which compels people to seek out associations pp. 86–100); Archer (1999, pp. 76–86)
former union with the lost loved-one
60. The grieving person tends to seek to restore Grief also involves identification with the lost Parkes (1972, pp. 77–84); Archer (1999, pp.
the lost loved-one as they were loved-one, and seeking to be reunited 84–89)
67/68. Grief is intensified when the loss is Unexpected and untimely deaths produce more Parkes (1981)
sudden and unexpected, and lessened when it problems for bereaved
is foreseen Study showing more intense grief where there is Parkes & Weiss (1983); Schultz, Boerner, &
little warning of the death. Further studies Herbert (2008, pp. 278–280)
complicated by methodological issues
69. Grief lessened by emotional support Social support associated with better adjustment and McIntosh et al. (1993); Vanderwerker &
lower risk of prolonged grief Prigerson (2003)
72. Grief lessened by being disclosed Disclosure was viewed as part of “grief work.” Pennebaker & O’Heeron (1984); Pennebaker
Where it has been studied separately, it has been et al. (2001)
shown to be beneficial: it is likely to involve
cognitive restructuring
73. Grief is less painful and intense when Expression of emotion viewed as beneficial as part Bonanno Keltner, Holen & Horowitz (1995);
controlled of “grief work.” Where it has been studied Bonanno & Keltner (1997)
separately, control or avoidance of emotions has
been shown to be beneficial
74. Grief is greater if the cause of the loss is Concerns attributions of blame: grief is often Archer (1999, pp. 72–74);
attributed to the self or the loved-one associated with self-blame
75. Grief tends to arouse anger Reported frequently in studies of grief; An Archer (1999, pp. 70–72); Bowlby (1980a,
important part of the formulation of grief by p. 29); Parkes (1972, pp. 100–105)
Bowlby and Parkes
78. Extreme grief tends to destroy the capacity Grief associated with loss of interest in general and Shuchter & Zisook (1993)
to enjoy other activities enjoyed before specific activities, and difficulty concentrating
a
The “laws” are paraphrased from longer statement, and the term grief is used instead of sorrow. The laws of sorrow are those described by Shand, 1920.
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 33

ward resolution. Bonanno et al. (1995) operationalized avoidance Security in Attachment


of emotion by measuring the mismatch between people’s reported
emotional state and their degree of autonomic arousal (via heart As noted earlier, central to attachment theory was the notion of
rate). Bereaved people with high physiological arousal but low “security,” in a relational or affectionate sense of the word. This
also formed a core feature of Russell’s theme of affection, when he
reported emotion (i.e., avoiders), measured at 6 months postloss,
stated that “the absence of affection gives . . . a sense of insecu-
subsequently showed fewer signs of grief and fewer somatic
rity” (p. 176). He elaborated this elsewhere:
symptoms when measured 8 months later: this is the opposite to
what would be expected from grief work theory. It suggests that The better sort of affection corresponds to the feeling of the man
Shand was correct when he stated that the expression of negative whose ship is secure, the less excellent sort corresponds to that of the
emotions is associated with a poorer outcome. Bonanno and Kelt- shipwrecked swimmer. The first of these kinds of affection is only
ner (1997) measured negative emotional expression directly, by possible in so far as a man feels safe, . . . the latter kind, on the
observing facial expressions of bereaved people, again 6 months contrary, is caused by the feeling of insecurity. (Russell, 1930, pp.
after their loss. They found—as Shand had stated—that expressing 181–182).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

negative emotions at this time was associated with more pro- Russell also described something very close to attachment the-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nounced grief later on (at 14 and 25 months). This occurred even ory’s concept of a “secure base,” a basic notion in attachment
when people’s initial distress was taken into account, so that they theory, that also underpins the typology of attachment set out by
were both absolutely and relatively worse off than people who Ainsworth (1979) and subsequently applied to adult relationships
showed fewer negative emotional expressions. (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994; Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Russell
These two studies clearly support Shand’s view that the expres- (1930) described it as follows:
sion of negative emotions leads to poorer—not better— outcomes.
He distinguished this from disclosing to others thoughts and feel- [B]ehind . . . external interests there is the feeling that he will be
ings associated with grief, which was viewed as beneficial, con- protected from disaster by parental affection. The child for whom for
sistent with grief work theory. Evidence from studies of the dis- any reason parental affection is withdrawn is likely to become timid
and unadventurous . . . and no longer able to meet the world in a mood
closure of previously undisclosed traumatic events indicates that
of gay exploration (p. 178).
this leads to better physical health and well being (Pennebaker,
Zech, & Rimé, 2001). Studies of parents bereaved by sudden infant Like Bowlby, Russell described both the giving and receiving of
death (Lepore, Ragan, & Jones, 2000; McIntosh, Silver, & Wort- feelings of security in attachment, between caregiver and child,
man, 1993) indicate some support for the view that disclosure to and within adult relationships. For example, he says: “I have been
others is beneficial, as Shand (1914) suggested. speaking . . . of the affection of which the person is the object. I
It appears that Bowlby did not notice the distinction Shand made wish now to speak of the affection that a person gives” (Russell,
between emotional expression and disclosure. Why this was the 1930, p. 181). These were “reciprocally life-giving” (p. 182).
case is not clear, because he was familiar with the source and—as In sum, the notions of security of attachment, the secure base,
noted above— did state that it would be given a “central position” the impact of the caregiver’s attachment pattern on the formation
in his work. As indicated above, most of Bowlby’s understanding of attachment in the young child, all core elements of attachment
of grief did parallel Shand’s “laws of sorrow” (see Table 1)—with theory, resonate with Russell’s earlier exposition.
this one important exception.
Short- and Long-Term Consequences of (In)security
Love and Attachment From Russell to Bowlby Consistent with Bowlby’s theory, attachment security (associ-
We now turn to a source that is not cited by Bowlby, and yet ated with fearlessness) is also said by Russell to serve a protective
contains descriptions of a number of the fundamental propositions function (both short- and long-term), because such a person “will
of attachment theory (Bowlby (1953a, 1969/1982). To someone pass unscathed through many difficult situations in which a timid
with a deep interest in Bowlby’s theory, it came as a great surprise man would come to grief” (Russell, 1930, p. 177). The protective
to find a chapter in Russell’s (1930) The Conquest of Happiness in function of a secure attachment that developed early on, as Russell
which these propositions are described. They are to be found in the maintains, parallels Bowlby’s claim about the immediate and
chapter on Affection. Clearly, the objectives of the two writers longer-term effects of insecurity, for example: “what occurs in the
were different; Russell’s was to examine the roots of (un)happi- earliest months and years can have deep and long-lasting effects”
ness, Bowlby’s to examine the relationship between parental care (Bowlby, 1953a, p. 15).
and mental health. Yet the features that are common to both Russell also linked security more specifically to well-being and
authors are quite extensive, including the core notions of security, mental health, following the theme of his whole volume, which
deprivation, and the fundamental influence of the nature of early concerned happiness: “Those who face life with a feeling of
attachment bonds on later wellbeing. In both, the emphasis is on security are much happier than those who face it with a feeling of
maternal care (and the male child).4 Neither Russell in the 1930s insecurity” (Russell, 1930, p. 176). The consequences of attach-
nor Bowlby in the 1950s focused on permanent loss through death: ment insecurity for mental ill health developed into a major line of
as indicated in the previous sections, this came later in the third of argument in Bowlby’s work, for example when he wrote of “the
Bowlby’s trilogy on attachment and loss (Bowlby, 1980a). We
now describe how Russell formulated these similar ideas and relate 4
Not only the topic, but the language of their times is evident in both
them to the attachment theory propositions. accounts.
34 STROEBE AND ARCHER

terrible damage which may be done to a child’s personality by ing the overanxious interpersonally dependent, clinging attempts
deprivation” (Bowlby, 1953a, p. 15). to stay close that is characteristic of this style, with references to
The general link between childhood experience and later adjust- “desperate efforts” and wanting to “win affection” (Ainsworth,
ment was explicit and fundamental in both Russell’s and Bowlby’s Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Ainsworth, 1979). There seems in
writings. Russell (1930) started his chapter on affection by exam- this example also to be an understanding of concordance in at-
ining the causes of unhappiness, linking this with feelings of being tachment style between caregiver and child, as is assumed in
unloved in the following way: attachment theory (e.g., George & Solomon, 1999).
Russell went on to postulate continuity in the style of attachment
A man may have the feeling of being unloved for a variety of reasons across the life span with respect to attachment style, as proposed
. . . he may in childhood have had to accustom himself to receiving
by Bowlby (1953a, 1969): “The habits of mind formed in early
less love than fell to the share of other children. [He] . . . may take
various attitudes as a result. He may make desperate efforts to win
years are likely to persist through life” (Russell, 1930, p. 179).
affection, probably by means of exceptional acts of kindness (Russell, Even more striking, Russell made the connection with adult ro-
1930, p. 175). mantic attachment styles, an extension that was only developed
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

within the attachment theory perspective in the 1980s (e.g., Fraley


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This resonates with attachment theory’s basic notion that the & Shaver, 2000; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994; Hazan & Shaver,
capacity for love and sympathy in adult life has its roots in early 1987). Russell continued his description of the dependently at-
childhood, expressed by Bowlby (1953a) in a variety of ways, such tached child as follows: “Many people when they fall in love look
as “deprivation of mother-love in early childhood, can have a for a little haven of refuge from the world. . . . [Men] seek from
far-reaching effect on the mental health and personality develop- their wives what they obtained formerly from an unwise mother”
ment of human beings” (p. 18); and “the quality of the parental (Russell, 1930, p. 179).
care which a child receives in his earliest years is of vital impor- It cannot be said that the styles of attachment are worked out at
tance for his future mental health” (p. 11); and “essential for all precisely in Russell’s writing. However, there is notable cor-
mental health is that the infant and young child should experience respondence between his statements and attachment theory’s pos-
a warm, intimate, and continuous relationship with his mother” tulation of both the secure and insecure ambivalent (dependent)
(p. 11). styles. Furthermore, the notions of concordance between caregiver
Russell (1930) also noted the long-term effects on character of and child in attachment style, continuity of style across the life
early childhood deprivation when he stated the following: “Very span, and even the claim that type of attachment influences the
frequently . . . previous misfortunes in childhood have produced nature of a romantic relationship all seem to be reflected by
defects of character which are the cause of failure to obtain love in Russell, just as they are fundamental to attachment theory.
later years” (p. 180). Compare this with “the prolonged deprivation
of a young child of maternal care may have grave and far-reaching
From Russell to Bowlby:
effects on his character and so on the whole of his future life”
(Bowlby, 1953a, p. 50).
The Possibility of Theoretical Origins
Thus, there are notable similarities in the writings of Bowlby Possibly, Bowlby came to propose the ideas compared above
and Russell regarding their views on the protective function of quite independently, or through a third theorist known to both
secure attachment and risks of insecure attachment, both in infancy Bowlby and Russell. But how likely is it that Bowlby knew about
and childhood and for later mental health and personality devel- Russell’s work? As we probed the personal backgrounds of Rus-
opment. The nature of attachment to the primary caregiver and sell and Bowlby to try to answer this question, it became evident
childhood experience in relation to this person (with respect to that there were some remarkable similarities in their backgrounds,
separation and deprivation) were understood by both writers to be a common intellectual heritage and even geographical proximity,
critical. features which could combine to make personal and academic
acquaintance a possibility. On the other hand, we discovered vast
differences between these two men, which may have contributed to
Attachment Styles a lack of recognition on Bowlby’s part. In this section, we outline
A central tenet of attachment theory is that the style of the biographical features in Russell’s and Bowlby’s lives, to indicate
caregiver (mother) impacts on that of the infant. This was devel- the likelihood of Bowlby’s knowledge of Russell’s work.
oped by Bowlby (1953a) throughout his monograph, and taken up
again in the second of his trilogy on attachment and loss (Bowlby, Personal Backgrounds: Formative Childhood
1973, chapter 15). Again, we find parallels in Russell’s work: Experiences
The affection given must be itself robust . . . the timid mother or Both Russell and Bowlby were born into upper-class British
nurse, who is perpetually warning children against disasters that may families of national renown (Russell in 1872; Bowlby in 1907):
occur . . . may produce in them a timidity equal to her own, and may they could in many ways both be considered privileged. This alone
cause them to feel that they are never safe in her immediate neigh-
would increase the likelihood of mutual awareness and perhaps
borhood. To the unduly possessive mother this feeling on the part of
a child may be agreeable: she may desire his dependence upon herself
contact. Russell inherited the title of Lord (Earl) from his grand-
more than his capacity to cope with the world (Russell, 1930, p. 179). father (who outlived his father); Bowlby’s father was the baronet
Sir Anthony Bowlby, an eminent surgeon. However, the events of
This and other statements go some way toward identifying the Russell’s childhood and upbringing differed in many ways from
insecure preoccupied style of attachment, coming near to describ- Bowlby’s. Russell’s parents died when he was very young, and he
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 35

was brought up by his grandmother, his grandfather dying a few Trinity. Bowlby was an undergraduate there from 1925 to 1928,
years after Russell was placed in their care (Clark, 1975). Despite beginning with the Natural Science Tripos, Part 1, covering a wide
having a very liberal political attitude his grandmother was de- variety of subjects. He excelled, winning prizes and gaining a first
scribed by Clark (1975) as being religiously conservative, strict class honors degree (Holmes, 1993). He was a medical student
and old fashioned in her upbringing of Russell. He did not attend who concentrated on both the natural and the moral sciences,
school, being educated instead by governesses and tutors. psychology and—linking him to Russell—philosophy (Berry,
By contrast, Bowlby was brought up in his parents’ home. 2005; van Dijken et al., 1998). Both men were wide-ranging
Features noted by one of his biographers, Holmes (1993), regard- thinkers who, in the style of education at Cambridge, came into
ing his upbringing include the presence of nurses and governesses, contact with scholars from different disciplines. Those with whom
limited access to his mother (a disciplinarian with a strict regime), Russell had contact were as diverse as Joseph Conrad, D.H.
and a rather remote father. Like Russell, he experienced separa- Lawrence, Wittgenstein, Trotsky, and Einstein. Likewise, we men-
tion, being parted from his family for weeks at a time from a tender tioned a similar variety of contacts and influences on Bowlby
age (his biographers give different ages, 7 or somewhat older), earlier. Russell was not only highly controversial and outspoken,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attending boarding school, as was then typical for this class of he was also blind to counterarguments at times (Clark, 1975), a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

English society. Both Russell’s and Bowlby’s biographers sur- characteristic that may not have appealed to Bowlby, given his
mised that having to endure these separation experiences had rigorous approach to acquiring empirical knowledge— but perhaps
lasting influences on their academic and personal lives (Clark, making Russell hard for him to ignore.
1975; Holmes, 1993). They may have been oriented toward think- Furthermore, Russell was highly prolific; his writings (which
ing how experiences in early life, and in particular separations and covered both scholarly and popular books and articles, mainly on
lack of maternal affection, could influence the person’s character mathematics and philosophy) could hardly have been overlooked
and ability to form relationships throughout life. by Bowlby. Russell had written books and articles on philosoph-
By all accounts, they developed into very different personalities. ical topics before Bowlby’s time: we noted above that Bowlby’s
Clark (1975) described Russell as follows: academic interests and studies covered philosophy; it would seem
likely that, given that he was a Trinity College scholar, Russell’s
[T]he quintessential man, the bundle of contradictions passionately ded- work would have been assigned or brought to his notice during his
icated to intellect, at times carrying the rational argument to irrational
studies. Would he, though, have read The Conquest of Happiness?
extremes, the natural-born emotional adventurer for ever hampered by
orphaned youth and too early marriage. . . . [A] man of epic proportions,
Perhaps: the content of the book would surely have interested him,
struggling through a lifetime beset with frustration and near-disaster; in given that it delved into the causes of unhappiness and happiness.
youth the constitutional sceptic, in old age the sometimes splendid figure For an avid reader and broad scholar such as Bowlby, it would not
with courage never to submit or yield (pp. 9 –10). have taken much time or effort: It provided a short and readable
guide as to how to live a happy life. This would have been in line
We know that he was also prone to bouts of depression and with Bowlby’s interests in social and clinical issues and it is a
sometimes haunted by suicidal thoughts (Clark, 1975). source that could hardly be ignored. This book became one of the
Bowlby, equally dedicated to intellect and independent-minded most famous on popular philosophy ever written, selling three
like Russell, was, by contrast, blessed with an inner calm that million copies in the first four years.5
would stand him in good stead throughout his life (Holmes, 1993). Although it seems likely that Bowlby was aware of Russell’s
Holmes goes on to describe a man in some ways very different publications in general, and even The Conquest of Happiness in
from Russell: particular, the question arises as to whether he would have been
sympathetic to Russell’s work in this domain. We do not know.
A . . . capacity to reconcile divergent elements is to be found in his
personality which, although remarkably coherent and consistent, con-
Curiously, though, a search of the Web turned up the following
tained many contradictory aspects: reserved, yet capable of inspiring comment in the Editor’s Notes for the Journal of the Bertrand
great affection; quintessentially “English” and yet thoroughly cosmopol- Russell Archives in 1978, “the following persons recently visited
itan in outlook; conventional in manner yet revolutionary in spirit; . . . an or researched in the Russell Archives: . . . John Bowlby” (Black-
explorer of the psyche who mistrusted the purely subjective; . . . an enfant well, 1978, p. 4). Assuming this to be the same John Bowlby,
terrible who was always slightly formal. (Holmes, 1993, pp. 30–31) perhaps his interest was indeed aroused much later in life.

Beyond Academics: Their Later Life Experiences and


Later Development: Academic Backgrounds
Interests
A most striking link between Russell and Bowlby is that they
were both at Trinity College, Cambridge, United Kingdom, al- A rather remarkable incident is described by van der Horst and
though Russell studied there a generation before Bowlby. Russell van der Veer (2009), describing how Russell’s wife, Patricia,
entered Trinity College as an undergraduate in 1890, he was shared Bowlby’s concern for hospitalized children separated from
appointed lecturer in philosophy there in 1899, was dismissed from their parents. She had been asked (but refused) to leave her
the College in 1916 (following antiwar protests), but offered 7-year-old son, who had developed a high temperature, alone in
reinstatement in 1919. His links with Trinity remained for decades hospital. This incident led her to publish letters in The Lancet in
(he was made a fellow for life in the late 1940s); he retained a love 1945, vehemently opposing the visiting rules of hospitals: “I feel
for the College, even though this was more like a stormy affair at
times, across the decades of his life (Clark, 1975). Thus, he would 5
According to recent advertizing for a modern interpretation of the
have been a very visible figure in the years that Bowlby was at volume (Phillips, 2010).
36 STROEBE AND ARCHER

very strongly that when children are patients in hospitals some Conclusions
members of their family should be allowed to remain with them
whenever this is possible” (P. Russell, 1945; in van der Horst & Bowlby’s work has had an enormous impact on the psychology
van der Veer, 2009). The letters were subject to considerable of human relationships, their development, long-term impact, and
the consequences of their loss. Initially they caused a sensation and
discussion and debate. One can probably assume that Bertrand
were considered to be revolutionary. For these reasons, it is inter-
Russell, an adoring and deeply concerned father (see Clark, 1975),
esting to enquire about forerunners of Bowlby’s thinking on love
shared these ideas on hospitalization common to both his wife and
and loss. As we have shown, Darwin and Shand were cited as
Bowlby (e.g., 1953a).
important contributors to Bowlby’s work. Darwin was both ac-
Russell and Bowlby had other common interests besides their
knowledged and several aspects of his overall theory and specific
academic ones. They were both actively involved in education,
writings on grief were explicitly used by Bowlby to develop his
particularly for disadvantaged young persons. Russell founded his
ideas on grief. Alexander Shand’s work was treated differently:
own school, Beacon Hill School, catering for children from broken
despite initially acknowledging him as a major influence on his
homes (Clark, 1975). He also lectured and wrote on education.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

thinking, Bowlby (1980a) hardly cited him in relation to the


Bowlby took up a position in a school for maladjusted children
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

description of grief, and sources of individual differences, and he


(which led to his publication of Forty-Four Juvenile Thieves
ignored a key aspect of his work in relation to the resolution of
[Bowlby, 1944] in 1944), before continuing his studies in medical
grief, in favor of Freud’s writings on grief. In retrospect, this led
school. In his own words, this alerted him “to a possible connec-
to some misleading concepts about the resolution of grief persist-
tion between prolonged deprivation and the development of a
ing until they were empirically tested and found wanting (e.g.,
personality apparently incapable of making affectional bonds and,
Archer, 1999, 2008; Stroebe & Stroebe, 1991; Wortman & Silver,
because immune to praise and blame, prone to repeated delinquen-
1989).
cies” (Bowlby, 1981, p. 2).
Bertrand Russell had thought along similar lines to those elaborated
Bowlby is likely to have been aware of Russell for other in Bowlby’s writings on infant–parent attachment and its conse-
reasons. Russell was extremely eminent, in many different re- quences for later emotional security. In contrast to the above forerun-
spects, not just for his writings and lectures, culminating in the ners, we noted an absence of any reference to Russell in Bowlby’s and
receipt of the Nobel Prize for Literature in 1950. He was a highly his colleagues’ work, despite the fact that the excerpts from Russell’s
visible, highly controversial public figure, deeply concerned about The Conquest of Happiness indicated a close conceptual parallel with
social issues and world affairs and with close links to the govern- Bowlby’s thinking. The difference between the two sources lies in the
ment, with whom he was sometimes far from popular, and whom degree of elaboration of the ideas. There is disparity between noting
he openly criticized (and vice versa). He also outraged the more that something is likely to be the case and regarding it as sufficiently
Victorian segment of society for his moral statements, for exam- important to spend years of one’s life working out the consequences
ple, by endorsing sex before marriage. through scholarship and research.
Bowlby too was highly renowned within academic spheres and From this perspective it should be clear that we do not see this
way beyond, and it seems not unlikely that Russell’s attention, in historical quest as signifying a lack of appreciation for the origi-
turn, would have been drawn to his work. Bowlby had enormous nality and impact of Bowlby’s work, nor do we want to detract
societal influence, from his early report for the World Health from his immense contribution, but rather, our quest is to put his
Organization on maternal care and children’s mental health work in the context of thoughts that were around at the time. It
(Bowlby, 1951), which was rewritten as the best-selling Penguin would indeed be surprising if similar ideas were not raised before
book Child Care and the Growth of Love (Bowlby, 1953a), Bowlby formulating the ideas into a coherent and extended theory
through to his monumental trilogy Attachment and Loss (1969, and providing further (albeit— by today’s standards—preliminary)
1973, 1980) and beyond. empirical support for them. From this perspective, Russell made
Perhaps one domain where Russell and Bowlby differed most more limited, less well-integrated, propositions on the basis of
notably was in their attitudes and involvement in the activities of common sense, inspired simply by his own experience and obser-
war. Russell had been imprisoned for being a conscientious ob- vation. He did not pursue them academically, or take them where
jector during the 1st World War. Later, he was prominent in the they could have led him.
nuclear disarmament movement and he also protested actively It is interesting to speculate whether Bowlby was aware of
against the Vietnam War. By contrast, Bowlby had attended Dart- Bertrand Russell’s writing on this topic. We have only circum-
mouth Naval Academy. Although he decided early on that the stantial evidence that he may have been: As noted above, Russell
navy was not for him, he served in the army in the 2nd World War, was, like Bowlby, at Trinity College, Cambridge for a period of
having volunteered in 1940. Again, we cannot tell whether these time, although a generation before. He was of such fame that a
apparent differences in attitudes to war caused antipathy on Bowl- young academic attending later might have read his work (partic-
by’s part toward Russell. ularly given the intellectual curiosity and concern with societal
In conclusion, given the visibility that both men had, combined affairs that they had in common). On the other hand, not only was
with their academic and societal proximity, it remains puzzling there the generation difference, but Russell was in a different field
that neither man cited the other, nor do we have any indications of from Bowlby, being an eminent and controversial philosopher,
their paths crossing, despite their overlapping life spans (Russell while Bowlby was studying medicine. Whatever the case may be,
died in 1970; Bowlby in 1990). Most of all, one might have van der Horst and van der Veer’s (2009) statements about the
expected reference to The Conquest of Happiness in Bowlby’s eclectic nature of Bowlby’s theorizing help place such specula-
work. tions in perspective:
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 37

Being a theorist by nature of training, Bowlby carefully weighed the Bonanno, G. A., & Kaltman, S. (1999). Toward an integrative perspective
evidence and tried to integrate the findings into a coherent framework. on bereavement. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 760 –776. doi:10.1037/
He was quite systematic about using evidence from different fields; 0033-2909.125.6.760
even when seemingly coincidental snippets of information came his Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of emotion and
way. If they fit the larger picture, and he found additional validation, the course of bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106,
he incorporated them. In areas that were not his specialty, Bowlby 126 –137. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.126
consulted others about the value of new ideas he had come across. Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, M. J. (1995). When
Bowlby took evidence from other fields and investigators, but he avoiding unpleasant emotions might not be such a bad thing: Verbal-
melded it into a coherent theory. Constantly rewriting and polishing autonomic response dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement.
his manuscripts, he incorporated other people’s ideas into this frame- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 975–989. doi:
work. Whereas he always gave them credit initially, after a while 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.975
these new ideas blended into his own. It was this eclectic approach Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and home
that would eventually become Bowlby’s trademark (pp. 158 –159). life. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 19 –52; 107–127.
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva, Switzerland:
It is also important to emphasize the limited scope of Russell’s
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

World Health Organization.


statements on this topic, the fact that a lot of basics about attach-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bowlby, J. (1953a). Child care and the growth of love. Harmondsworth,


ment theory, such as its biological origins in evolutionary theory UK: Penguin.
and research on imprinting, the experimental work of Harlow, and Bowlby, J. (1953b). Critical phases in the development of social responses
the detailed description of working models were not found in in man and other animals. New Biology, 14, 25–32.
Russell’s writing. As mentioned earlier, for these and other aspects Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. The
of his theorizing Bowlby drew inspiration from a variety of other International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350 –373.
scholars and sources, contributing to the development of attach- Bowlby, J. (1960a). Separation anxiety. The International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 41, 89 –113.
ment theory into one of the leading theoretical syntheses in the
Bowlby, J. (1960b). Grief and mourning in infancy and early childhood.
history of psychology.
The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 15, 9 –52.
Bowlby, J. (1961). Processes of mourning. International Journal of Psy-
References choanalysis, 42, 317–340.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.).
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Infant-mother attachment. American Psychol-
New York, NY: Basic Books.
ogist, 34, 932–937. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.932
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1982). Attachment: Retrospect and prospect. In C. M.
anger. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The place of attachment in human
Bowlby, J. (1980a). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression.
behaviour (pp. 3–30). London, UK: Tavistock.
New York, NY: Basic Books.
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
Bowlby, J. (1980b). By ethology out of psychoanalysis: An experiment in
attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum. interbreeding. Animal Behaviour, 28, 649 – 656. doi:10.1016/S0003-
Ainsworth, M. D., & Bowlby, J. (1954). Research strategy in the study of 3472(80)80125-4
mother-child separation. Courrier Centre International de l’Énfance, 4, Bowlby, J. (1981). Perspective: A contribution by John Bowlby. Bulletin of
105–131. the Royal College of Psychiatrists, 5, 2– 4. doi:10.1192/pb.5.1.2
Archer, J. (1999). The nature of grief: The evolution and psychology of Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy
reactions to loss. London, UK: Routledge. human development. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Archer, J. (2001). Grief from an evolutionary perspective. In M. Stroebe, Bowlby, J., Ainsworth, M., Boston, M., & Rosenbluth, D. (1956). The
R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereave- effects of mother-child separation: A follow-up study. British Journal of
ment research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 263–283). Wash- Medical Psychology, 29, 211–247. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1956
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10436- .tb00915.x
011 Bowlby, J., & Parkes, C. M. (1970). Separation and loss within the family.
Archer, J. (2008). Theories of grief: Past, present and future perspectives. In E. J. Anthony & C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child and his family (pp.
In M. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook 197–216). New York, NY: Wiley.
of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and inter- Bowlby, J., & Robertson, J. (1952). A two-year-old goes to hospital.
vention (pp. 45– 65). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso- Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 46, 425– 427.
ciation. Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and
Badcock, C. (1990). Oedipus in evolution: A new theory of sex. Oxford, Mary Ainsworth. Developmental Psychology, 28, 759 –775. doi:
UK: Blackwell. 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. (1991). Attachment styles among young Cassidy, J. (2000). Adult romantic attachments: A developmental perspec-
adults: A test of a 4-category system. Journal of Personality and Social tive on individual differences. Review of General Psychology, 4, 111–
Psychology, 61, 226 –244. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226 131. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.4.2.111
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (1999/2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory,
interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary practice and clinical applications. 1st/2nd revised edition. New York,
theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647– 670. doi:10.2307/ NY: Guilford.
1131166 Clark, R. (1975). The life of Bertrand Russell. Harmondsworth, UK:
Berry, D. (2005). John Bowlby: Understanding his shadow. Lecture pre- Penguin Books.
sented at the Harvard Graduate School of Education, November, 18, Darwin, C. (1843). Letter to W. D. Fox [25 March]. In F. Burkhardt & S.
2005. Smith (Eds.), The correspondence of Charles Darwin Vol. 2 (pp. 352–
Blackwell, K. (1978). Editor’s notes. The Journal of the Bertrand Russell 353). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Archives, 29 –32, 2– 4. Darwin, C. (1859/1911)). On the origin of species. London, UK: Murray.
38 STROEBE AND ARCHER

Darwin, C. (1872/1904). The expression of the emotions in man and Parkes, C. M., & Weiss, R. S. (1983). Recovery from bereavement. New
animals. London, UK: Murray. doi:10.1037/10001-000 York, NY: Basic Books.
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., Doron, G., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). The Parkes, C. M. (1972). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life. London,
attachment paradox: How can so many of us (the insecure ones) have no UK: Tavistock.
adaptive advantages? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 123– Parkes, C. M. (1986). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life (2nd ed.).
141. doi:10.1177/1745691610362349 London, UK: Tavistock.
Feeney, J. (1999). Adult romantic attachment and couple relationships. In Parkes, C. M. (1996). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life (3rd ed.).
J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, re- London, UK: Tavistock.
search, and clinical applications (pp. 355–377). New York, NY: Guil- Parkes, C. M. (2006). Love and loss: The roots of grief and its complica-
ford. tions. London, UK: Routledge.
Fonagy, P. (1999). Psychoanalytic theory from the viewpoint of attachment Pennebaker, J. W., & O’Heeron, R. C. (1984). Confiding in others and
theory and research. In J. Cassidy, & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of illness rate among spouses of suicide and accidental-death victims.
attachment: Theory, practice and clinical applications (pp. 595– 624). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 473– 476. doi:10.1037/0021-843X
New York, NY: Guilford. .93.4.473
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Fraley, R. C., & Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Attachment and loss: A test of Pennebaker, J. W., Zech, E., & Rimé, B. (2001). Disclosing and sharing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

three competing models on the association between attachment-related emotion: Psychological, social, and health consequences. In M. Stroebe,
avoidance and adaptation to bereavement. Personality and Social Psy- R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereave-
chology Bulletin, 30, 878 – 890. doi:10.1177/0146167204264289 ment research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 517–543). Wash-
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theo- ington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10436-
retical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered ques- 022
tions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132–154. doi:10.1037/1089- Phillips, T. (2010). The conquest of happiness: A modern-day interpreta-
2680.4.2.132 tion of a self-help classic. Oxford, UK: Infinite Ideas Limited.
Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), Pollock, G. H. (1961). Mourning and adaptation. International Journal of
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Psychoanalysis, 42, 341–361.
Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 239 –260). London: Hogarth Press & Institute of Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. (1996). Adolescent attachment and
Psychoanalysis. (Original work published 1917) psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64,
George, C., & Solomon, J. (1999). Attachment and caring: The caregiving 244 –253. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.244
behavioural system. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Russell, B. (1930). The conquest of happiness. London, UK: George Allen
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 649 – 670). & Unwin.
New York, NY: Guilford. Schultz, R., Boerner, K., & Herbert, R. S. (2008). Caregiving and bereave-
Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: ment. In M. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.),
Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 430 – 445. doi: and intervention (pp. 265–285). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.430 ical Association.
Hart, D., & Sussman, R. W. (2005). Man the hunted: Primates, predators, Shand, A. F. (1914). The foundations of character. London, UK: Mac-
and human evolution. Boulder, CO: Westview. millan.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an Shand, A. F. (1920). The foundations of character (2nd ed.). London, UK:
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Macmillan.
511–524. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511 Shand, A. F. (1927). The foundations of character (3rd ed.). London, UK:
Holmes, J. (1993). John Bowlby and attachment theory. London, UK: Macmillan.
Routledge. Shorey, H., & Snyder, C. (2006). The role of adult attachment styles in
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement psychopathology and psychotherapy outcomes. Review of General Psy-
from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25. doi:10.1037/h0076171 chology, 10, 1–20. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.1.1
Lepore, S. J., Ragan, J. D., & Jones, S. (2000). Talking facilitates Shuchter, S. R., & Zisook, S. (1993). The course of normal grief. In M. S.
cognitive-emotional processes of adaptation to an acute stressor. Journal Stroebe, W. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 499 –508. doi:10.1037/0022- Theory, research and intervention (pp. 23– 43). New York, NY: Cam-
3514.78.3.499 bridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511664076.003
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure-disoriented/ Stevenson-Hinde, J. (2007). Attachment theory and John Bowlby: Some
disorganized attachment pattern. In T. Brazelton & M. Yogman (Eds.), reflections. Attachment & Human Development, 9, 337–342. doi:
Affective development in infancy (pp. 95–124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 10.1080/14616730701711540
McIntosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). Religion’s role Stroebe, M. S. (1992–3). Coping with bereavement: A review of the grief
in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a child. work hypothesis. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 26, 19 – 42.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 812– 821. doi: doi:10.2190/TB6U-4QQC-HR3M-V9FT
10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.812 Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. A. W. (1999). The dual process model of coping
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Struc- with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197–
ture, dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford Press. 224. doi:10.1080/074811899201046
Myers, C. S. (1936). Dr. A. F. Shand, 1858 –1936 [Obituary]. British Stroebe, M., & Stroebe, W. (1991). Does “grief work” work? Journal of
Journal of Psychology, 26, 323–324. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 479 – 482. doi:10.1037/0022-
Newcombe, N., & Lerner, J. (1982). Britain between the wars: The his- 006X.59.3.479
torical context of Bowlby’s theory of attachment. Psychiatry: Journal Stroebe, M., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., Zech, E., & van den Bout, J. (2002).
for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 45, 1–12. Does disclosure of emotions facilitate recovery from bereavement?
Parkes, C. M. (1981). Anticipatory grief. British Journal of Psychiatry, Evidence from two prospective studies. Journal of Consulting and
138, 183a. doi:10.1192/bjp.138.2.183a Clinical Psychology, 70, 169 –178. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.169
ORIGINS OF MODERN IDEAS ON LOVE AND LOSS 39

Tinbergen, N. (1951/1969). The study of instinct. New York, NY: Oxford van Dijken, S., van der Veer, R., van Izendoorn, M., & Kuijpers, H. (1998).
University Press. Bowlby before Bowlby: The sources of an intellectual departure in
van der Horst, F. C. P., & van der Veer, R. (2009). Changing attitudes psychoanalysis and psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral
towards the care of children in hospital: A new assessment of the Sciences, 34, 247–269. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6696(199822)34:
influence of the work of Bowlby and Robertson in Britain, 1940 –1970. 3⬍247::AID-JHBS2⬎3.0.CO;2-N
Attachment & Human Development, 11, 119 –142. doi:10.1080/ Waters, E., Hamilton, C., & Weinfeld, N. (2000). The stability of attach-
14616730802503655 ment security from infancy to adolescence and early adulthood: General
van der Horst, F., & van der Veer, R. (2010). The ontology of an idea: John introduction. Child Development, 71, 678 – 683. doi:10.1111/1467-8624
Bowlby and his contemporaries on mother-child separation. History of .00175
Psychology, 13, 25– 45. doi:10.1037/a0017660 Wortman, C. B., & Silver, R. C. (1989). The myths of coping with loss.
van der Horst, F. (2011). John Bowlby – from psychoanalysis to ethology: Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 349 –357. doi:
Unravelling the roots of attachment theory. Chichester, UK: Wiley- 10.1037/0022-006X.57.3.349
Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9781119993100
Vanderwerker, L. C., & Prigerson, H. G. (2004). Social support and Received June 27, 2012
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

technological connectedness as protective factors in bereavement. Jour- Revision received August 13, 2012
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nal of Loss and Trauma, 9, 45–57. doi:10.1080/15325020490255304 Accepted August 16, 2012 䡲

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!


Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be
notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

Вам также может понравиться