Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

Second Time is a Charm: The Impact of Correcting

Missed Exam Questions on Student Learning


1
Christiane Schroeter
California Polytechnic State University
San Luis Obispo, CA 93407

V. Steven Green2
Arkansas State University
Jonesboro, AR 72401

Erin Bess3
California Polytechnic State University
San Luis Obispo, CA 93407

Abstract studying behavior, which may jeopardize the efficacy


This study determined the learning benefit of of exams as a tool to help students learn course
correcting missed exam questions. The results show material. Thus, there is a need to explore whether
that in addition to exams being an assessment tool, student learning could be enhanced by modifying the
they can also be used as a tool for student learning. traditional lifecycle of exams.
The availability of this information will provide help Alternative teaching methods may provide some
considering design, development, and improvement benefits to students' learning. Haskett (2001)
of traditional assessment methods for student explored many alternative teaching methods, such as
learning. reducing the number of lectures throughout the
quarter, face to face evaluations rather than conven-
tional testing, resubmission of work until a desired
Introduction and Objectives
grade is received, and oral presentations with
One of the missions of the university is to guide literature reviews instead of term papers. Students'
students in learning specific fundamental principles response to resubmitting their work was mostly
to enhance life-long learning. Assessment methods, positive, which suggests that this approach could be a
such as exams, should test the student's understand- successful teaching method. Light (1990) interviewed
ing of the material and provide feedback to students thousands of students to determine the qualities of
and professors (McKeachie, 2002). However, given the best courses they had taken at the university. In
that exams provide an impetus for students to quickly his study, students expressed that one of the charac-
study the class material right before the assessment teristics of the highest ranked courses includes “the
date, exams often only evaluate the student's knowl- opportunity to revise and improve their work before
edge at the time the exam is given and regrettably, it receives a final grade, thereby learning from their
often fail to be a learning tool. The typical lifecycle of mistakes in the process” (Light, 1990, pp. 8-9). This
an exam ends after it has been corrected by the finding is supported by Bain (2004) who suggests it is
professor and returned to the student. Risley (2007) important to give students multiple chances to
observed an exam lifecycle similar to our own teach- demonstrate their comprehension when administer-
ing experiences: 1) students take exam, 2) professor ing an exam.
grades exams, 3) professor returns graded exams, 4)
Many studies have examined the usefulness of
students look at their grade, see what they missed,
retaking exams, homework assignments, and quizzes
check to make sure the points were added correctly, 5)
to enhance learning (e.g. Bacon and Beyrouty, 1988;
students place the graded exam in their notebooks,
Haskett, 2001; Nickels and Uddin, 2003; Brye et al.,
maybe never to be looked at again until it is time to
2005; Risley, 2007). Results of these studies generally
study for the final exam, if the final exam is compre-
show that students do better on the makeup exams
hensive. Few students may take the time to deter-
regardless of whether the date of the makeup is
mine what they missed on a certain question and to
announced, they work in groups, or they can use their
re-work the problem or even correct their mistakes
notes. These studies also demonstrated some physio-
on the exam. Factors such as student procrastination,
logical benefit for the students who reported greater
social activities, part-time employment, and busy
inter-student cooperation. Bacon and Beyrouty
exam schedules frequently lead to last-minute
(1988) reported increased interest in the course while
1
Assistant Professor and contact author, Agribusiness Department; Phone: 805-756-5045; Fax: 805-756-5040; E-mail:
cschroet@calpoly.edu
2
Associate Professor
3
Former student

NACTA Journal • June 2010 21


Second Time

others observed increased communication between tors. Specifically, the classes were agricultural science
student and professor (Longer et al., 1987), decreased and marketing classes at Arkansas State University
anxiety (Brye et al., 2005), and perceived, enhanced in Jonesboro, Arkansas and California Polytechnic
learning (Bacon and Beyrouty, 1988; Longer et al., State University, San Luis Obispo, California. In this
1987; Brye et al., 2005). study, a written survey was distributed that consisted
Though many studies have documented the of questions regarding demographics, perceptions
effects of student learning through student self about the opportunity to correct missed exam
evaluation with questionnaires and results on questions, and perceptions about personal learning
makeup exams, few studies have looked at the long achievements. All students were given the option to
term effect on student learning. complete the survey without any incentive to partici-
Allowing students to look at material twice and to pate. Table 1 provides an overview of the time of the
receive feedback regarding the material that has been data collection and the course distribution. Overall,
submitted allows for a better quality work to be data was collected in 13 courses over two years. The
turned in later in the semester/quarter. Students are data collection began in the spring of 2006 at
interested in knowing their mistakes and, if given the Arkansas State University and in the fall of 2007 at
opportunity, almost three-fourths of students will California Polytechnic State University and it
resubmit better work (Haskett, 2001). Students often continued through the spring of 2008 at both univer-
perform better in a class where they have a better sities.
perspective of the outcome
and a better attitude Table 1. Course Distribution and Time of Data Collection
regarding their grade Non-Regrading Group (n=128)
Regrading Group
Time of data (n=190)
(Risley, 2007). ass Class
collection Total class Total class
One alternative to the Respondents Respondents
enrollment enrollment
traditional exam lifecycle Agricultural Fall 2007 - - 34 41
would be to allow the Economics
student to review the graded Agricultural Spring 2007 - - 11 12
Marketing
exam material, correct their
missed exam questions, and Spring 2006 21 22 - -
return them to the professor Agricultural
Statistics Fall 2006 - - 29 35
for regrading. Although
some studies have Spring 2007 - - 25 35
addressed the usefulness of
correcting missed exam obal Agricultural Fall 2007
Agricultural
Global - - 34 44
Marketing
Marketing
questions (Risley, 2007), to Spring 2008 33 34 - -
our knowledge, no study has
Spring 2006 - - 18 21
a s s e s s e d t h e l e a r n i n g Soil Fertility
benefits of this alternative Spring 2008 20 27 - -
exam lifecycle as deter-
Fall 2006 31 40 - -
mined by final exam grades.
The objectives of this Spring 2007 - - 13 14
study were to (1) identify Soil Science
student perceptions of Fall 2007 - - 26 28
correcting missed exam Spring 2008 23 23 - -
questions, (2) compare the
Total number of students 128 146 190 230
learning benefits of this
alternative method to the Response rate 88% 83%
traditional exam lifecycle
that ends with the professor
returning the exam, and to (3) evaluate benefits vs. To carry out a complete investigation of the issue,
cost to the instructor with regard to allowing stu- this study was administered with two different
dents to correct missed exam questions. The avail- student samples, each containing a different version
ability of this study's information will be useful in of the survey. The two versions of the survey were not
considering design, development and improvement of modified over time, in order to preserve a constant
traditional assessment methods for student learning. environment for the data collection. In addition, all
students were exposed to the same course materials
and each sample was provided with similar semes-
Materials and Methods ter/quarter exams. Lastly, to the extent possible, the
Study Design instructors tried to minimize any alteration in their
Given that the overall population of this study teaching styles. These measures were implemented
was college students, the sample consisted of stu- to control for any variability in external factors over
dents enrolled in classes taught by the two investiga- time and to allow a cross-comparison of the survey

22 NACTA Journal • June 2010


Second Time

questions between both samples. To our knowledge, incentive for making corrections was that students
no previous study has collected data on this issue with could earn up to half of the points missed on the exam,
a comparative control sample. based on the correctness of their re-submitted
The two samples consisted of 190 students answers. All participants in the regrading sample
enrolled in the regrading sample, and 128 students in were informed that the exam solutions would be
the non-regrading sample (control group). A 'regrad- discussed in class once the regraded exams were
ing survey' was distributed to all students enrolled in returned, but that until then, no answers would be
the sample classes at the end of the semester/quarter posted. It was up to each student to decide upon
after they had completed the class, and had the option which and how many answers they would choose to
to correct their graded semester/quarter exams for rework. Students were not allowed to mark their
additional points. Similarly, a 'non-regrading survey' graded exam copies and had to submit their reworked
was also distributed to students in the non-regrading answers on separate sheets of paper stapled to the
sample asking similar questions as the regrading original exam. Students were required to resubmit
sample. Students in the non-regrading sample were their exams by the next class period; late exams were
not offered the regrading option and were not told not accepted. The instructors then regraded the
anything about resubmission for additional points. submitted answers, where a correct reworked
On the regrading survey, students were asked to rate solution obtained full credit, i.e. half of the original
the effectiveness of correcting missed exam questions points of that exam question, while incomplete
on their learning, whereas the non-regrading survey answers could earn some partial credit. For example,
explored the students' opinions of whether they if a student completely missed an exam question that
learned from their mistakes on exams. All students was worth 10 points on the original exam, but would
were asked about their study habits to determine resubmit the correct solution to the missed exam
whether they had used their semester/quarter exam question during the regrading period, he/she could
as a learning tool to study for the final exam. Student earn up to an additional five points. Incorrect
learning was measured subjectively by assessing answers did not get any points, but a student could
students' perceptions about their learning of the not lose any of the points from the original exam.
material. They also could not earn additional points for already
Some students in the non-regrading sample correctly solved answers from their original exams.
would ask about the regrading option because word All of the reworked points were summed and added to
traveled from previous quarters/semesters, but they the students' original exam score. After the regrading
were still not given the option. Risley (2007) observed period, the exams were returned to the students and
the same “word-of-mouth-effect.” all of the answers were discussed in class or posted. At
Overall, the response rate shown in Table 1 the end of the semester/quarter, all exams and
shows the distribution of the students across the regraded answers were collected.
different courses over time. The number of students Risley (2007) offers his students identical
in each class and the response rate is included in the conditions for reworking exams, while Nickels and
table to show that a high response rate was received Uddin (2003) awarded 80% of the points lost.
from each sample and that the student enrollment Instructors may choose to alter details such as
was similar across groups. amount of additional points offered or resubmission
In addition to student feedback, professors time frame according to their needs.
participating in this regrading option provided The non-regrading sample served as the control
reflections of their experiences. These reflections group since their semester/quarter exams were based
were completely open and were not guided by any on the traditional lifecycle of an exam. The survey
specific questions. questions for the non-regrading sample consisted of
hypothetical questions regarding the option to
Data Collection resubmit their exams for additional points. The
questions on each survey were similar to allow for
Students were placed into the regrading or non-
comparison between the two groups, i.e. the non-
regrading groups based on the class in which they
regrading and regrading group, respectively.
were enrolled and the semester/quarter that they
were taking the class. An entire class for the semes-
ter/quarter was either a regrading or non-regrading Data Analysis
sub-sample. This study employed six types of variable groups:
Students who were enrolled in the regrading 1) demographics and study habits, 2) method of
sample were told at the beginning of the quar- correcting missed exam questions, 3) motivation for
ter/semester that they would be offered the option to correcting missed exam questions, 4) test anxiety, 5)
correct their semester/quarter exams and submit post-exam learning, 6) benefits and cost to the
them for regrading. This procedure was not offered instructor with regard to allowing students to correct
for the final exams due to time constraints. These missed exam questions.
students wrote their semester/quarter exam, All students who attended classes that utilized
received back the graded exam, and then had the the regrading treatment were combined as the
opportunity to resubmit the missed questions. The “regrading sample,” while the other students were
NACTA Journal • June 2010 23
Second Time

treated as the control group. We analyzed the data by included questions about students' ability to retain
comparing the survey responses between the treat- the class material after the exam in order to assess
ment and the control group. Descriptive statistics the benefits of the alternative exam lifecycle for
were compared with regard to the questions about student learning.
demographics and study habits. For all other ques- Lastly, the non-regrading group was asked
tions, independent t-tests were used to determine the whether they would take the opportunity to correct
difference between the groups' answers. mistakes if they thought it would enhance their
Regarding demographics, several variables may learning of the material. The regrading survey
impact student learning such as age, gender, marital employed a similar situation and asked if they
and employment status, year in school, and the received no points for making corrections on their
university attended. In addition, the survey assessed exam, would they still have taken the time to make
school responsibilities such as number of hours corrections to their exam. All students had the
worked per week, credit hours/ units enrolled, and opportunity to make corrections to learn from their
how far from campus the student lived. In order to mistakes, but only the students in the regrading
assess the general study habits of the students, treatment had the extra incentive of turning back
questions about the number of hours studied for the their exam to be regraded and gain back points
course in which the survey was given, were included. missed on the semester/quarter exams.
Questions about major, age, and year in school
may allow for comparisons regarding maturity and Results and Discussions
knowledge level. State of residence and proximity of Demographics and Study Habits
the students' residence to their particular school were A comparison of demographics and study habits
evaluated to allow for comparison between Arkansas is shown in Table 2. First, we compare demographics,
State University students and California Polytechnic such as age, gender, standing, and residence during
State University students. the semester/quarter, between both groups. The table
The survey also included questions about shows that the average age was very similar in the
whether the students took advantage of the opportu- regrading and the non-regrading groups, with 22
nity to correct missed exam questions. We assessed years in the regrading group and 23 years in the non-
the methods of correction by asking the students regrading group. The breakdown by age shows that
whether they corrected the exam questions by both the regrading group and non-regrading group
working with other students, visiting the professor consisted of mostly older students with the majority
during office hours, or using books and/or notes. of students in both groups being 21 years and older.
Students were allowed to select more than one Gender distribution was skewed towards the male
method of correction in their answers. We combined population, with 76% of the non-regrading group and
the answer categories “fully agree” and “agree” in 68% of the regrading group being male. The majority
this question, as these were indicative of students
who employed this method.
In addition, the data Table 2. Demographics and Study Habits of the Non-Regrading and Regrading Groups
analysis assessed whether Variable Categories Non-Regrading Group
(n=128)
Regrading Group
(n=190)
the opportunity for regrad- Demographics
ing eased test anxiety or Age 18 1% 3%
altered the students' study 19 6% 7%
20 12% 17%
efforts for the exam. A 21 27% 24%
comparison was made to 22 22% 21%
determine the change in the 23+ 23% 28%
anxiety levels of students Average 23 22
who were allowed to correct Gender Male 76% 68%
Female 24% 32%
their exams compared to the Standing Freshmen 2% 9%
anxiety levels of the control Sophomore 17% 9%
group. When students are Junior 37% 37%
less anxious about taking an Senior 41% 42%
exam, a more relaxed and Graduate 3% 1%
positive learning environ- Average distance to School 19 Miles 15 Miles
ment is established. Study habits
We a l s o c o l l e c t e d Number of credit hours during the semester/ quarter 14 15
subjective learning mea- Average number of hours worked per week 26.5 21.6
sures, such as their percep-
tions regarding the extent of Number of hours studied per week 2.6 2.9
what they learned from the Typically do assigned readings 39% 42%
mistakes they made on the
Class required for major 89% 90%
exams. Both surveys

24 NACTA Journal • June 2010


Second Time

of students in both groups were upper classmen with possible as well and the students were able to indicate
the number of freshman students being below 10% all the methods that they used. Both the regrading
for both groups. This distribution of academic group and the non-regrading group assessed their
standing is similar to the sample in Nickels and methods of correcting missed exam questions. Figure
Uddin (2003), which focused their data collection on 1 shows the various methods employed by the non-
sophomore and junior/senior level classes. regrading and the regrading groups and the percent-
Regarding the student's residence during the age of students who used each of these methods. For
semester/quarter, most students indicated that they the 'another student showed me' and 'I visited the
do not live on campus, as the average commuting professor for assistance' options, there was no
distance to campus was 19 miles in the non-regrading significant difference between the two groups in the
group and 15 miles in the regrading group. percentage of students who used these methods for
In order to assess the general study habits of the correcting missed exam questions. For the other
students, questions about the number of credit hours methods, the t-tests showed a significant difference
during the semester/ quarter, number of hours between the non-regrading and the regrading group.
worked, and number of hours studied were included. In the non-regrading group, 50% used the
In addition, the survey asked whether the student book/notes, whereas in the regrading group, 90% of
typically reads the assigned class material and students utilized this method to correct their missed
whether the class is required for their major. Table 2 exam questions (t (207) =6.96***, p<4.464-11). In the
shows that respectively, the average number of credit non-regrading group, 49% of the students stated that
hours per semester/ quarter was similar in both they worked together with other students to figure
groups, with 14 and 15 credit hours per semester/ out what they solved incorrectly on the exam, while
quarter. The number of
hours worked was compara-
ble between both groups, 100%
with a mean of 26 hours 90%
worked per week in the non-
regrading group and a mean 80%
of 22 hours worked per week 70%
in the regrading group.
Results showed that the 60%
minority of students did the 50%
required readings in the
class, with 39% of the 40%
students in the non- 30%
regrading group and 42% of
the students in the regrad- 20%
ing sample reportedly doing
10%
the required reading. The
majority of students in both 0%
samples stated that the class Used book/notes Worked together Students showed me Visited professor Copied from other
with other students duringofficehours students' exams
they were enrolled in was
required for their major,
Non-Regrading Regrading
with 89% in the non-
regrading sample and 90%
in the regrading sample. Method of Correction Non-Regrading Group Regrading Group Independent t-test
Used book/notes M=3.27, SD=1.81 M=4.41, SD=2.10 t (207)=6.96***, p<4.464-11
This is similar to the sample Worked together with M=3.16, SD=1.63 M=3.59, SD=1.56 t (255)=2.31***, p<0.022
by Nickels and Uddin other students
(2003), where 83% of the Students showed me M=3.09, SD=1.56 M=3.27, SD=1.54 NS, t (261)=0.99, p<0.323
Visited professor during M=2.24, SD=1.59 M=2.40, SD=1.55 NS, t (274)=0.88, p<0.382
students stated that the office hours
class was required for their Copied from other M=2.25, SD=1.51 M=1.81, SD=1.38 t (246)=2.59***, p<0.010
major. students’ exams
Strongly Agree=5, Agree=4, Somewhat agree/disagree=3, Disagree=2, Strongly disagree=1, No opinion=0
M= Mean, SD=Standard Deviation
Method of Correcting Figure 1 a and b: Non-Regrading Group vs. Regrading Group: Distribution of Methods of Correcting Missed
Missed Exam Exam Questions (a, top) and Descriptive Statistics of Methods of Correction (b, bottom); Students were
allowed to select more than one response.
Questions
The survey included
questions about how students corrected their missed this percentage was 59% in the regrading group
exam questions, such as working with other students, (t (255) =2.31***, p<0.022). In addition, in the non-
visiting the professor during office hours, or using the regrading group, 47% asked other students to show
book/ notes. A combination of these techniques was them how to correct what they solved incorrectly on

NACTA Journal • June 2010 25


Second Time

the exam, while there were 49% who employed this non-regrading and the regrading group, with the
method in the regrading group (NS, t (261) =0.99, regrading group being significantly opposed to
p<0.323). correcting missed exam questions if points were not
Of all students in the non-regrading sample, 25% given for making these corrections. Thus, the
visited the professor during office hours, while 32% incentive to obtain more points through regrading
took advantage of this opportunity in the regrading was an important motivator. More students in the
group (NS, t (274) =0.88, p<0.382). While overall the regrading sample took the time to correct missed
traffic during office hours significantly increased exam questions when they were rewarded with
during a “regrading” semester/quarter, it was not additional points for doing so, as opposed to the
required to meet with the instructor. This differs students in the non-regrading sample who were not
from Haskett (2001) who required his students to rewarded directly with points. Of the students in the
meet with the instructor to review their mistakes regrading group, only 27% said that they would have
between resubmissions of their work. Given the corrected their exams if no points were awarded.
diversity in learning styles, students in our study When asked the similar question, 91% of the non-
were free to choose which method of correction best regrading group said that they would correct a missed
suited themselves. exam question if they felt it would enhance their
Interestingly, a greater percentage of students in learning of the material. There was no mention of
the non-regrading group than in the regrading group extra points with this question given to the non-
stated that they copied the correct solutions from regrading group.
other students, with about 25% vs. 14%, respectively Our findings show that it is important to provide
(t (246) =2.59***, p<0.010). additional points as a reward for correcting missed
Thus, our survey suggests that the regrading exam questions, especially given the short turn-
activity supports active and collaborative learning, around time, in order to increase participation and
since the majority of the students used book/notes reap the learning benefits. Nickel and Uddin (2003)
and worked together to solve the questions they awarded up to 80% of the points lost. In their study,
missed on the exams. Bacon and Beyrouty (1988) put some students still felt that there was not enough
a twist on this concept by having students retake incentive when their original scores were high or,
exams in groups of two or three, thus allowing the similarly, when reworking was too time-consuming.
students to learn from each other. According to Light
(2001), it is vital to organize interactive relationships Test Anxiety
around the academic work in order to be a successful Both student samples had positive perceptions
college student. Teamwork is something that is about having the opportunity to correct missed exam
valuable in a working environment and employers questions and felt it would put them more at ease
look for employees who are willing to work together during the exam. Figures 3a and 3b show that 71% of
and use resources such as
books or notes to solve
problems. 7%
2% 16%

27%
Motivation for
Correcting Missed
Exam Questions
Figures 2a and 2b show
the students' motivation for
correcting missed exam
questions in the non-
regrading and regrading
g r o u p s , r e s p e c t i v e l y.
Students tended to be
idealistic in what would
motivate them to make test 57%
91%
corrections, yet realistically,
they appear to be more
concerned about their Yes No No opinion
grades than they are about
learning. a. Non-Regrading Group: b. Regrading Group:
I would take the opportunity to correct missed exam If points were not given for making corrections to
The independent t-test questions, if I thought it would enhance my learning of missed exam questions, I probably still would have
revealed a significance the material (Yes=2, No=1, No opinion=0, M=1.84, corrected the mistakes (Yes=2, No=1, No opinion=0,
SD=0.53). M=1.11, SD=0.65).
difference (t (298) =10.80***,
p<3.567-23) in the average Figure 2a and b: Non-Regrading vs. Regrading Group: Motivation for Correcting Missed Exam Questions, t
motivation between the p<3.567-23
(298) =10.80***,

26 NACTA Journal • June 2010


Second Time

students in the non-regrading group either fully Post-Exam Learning


agreed or agreed that the opportunity to correct their The additional time students spend on correcting
exam would have alleviated anxiety regarding test their exam and working on class material may be
taking, while 77% of the regrading group stated they beneficial, as this repeated exposure to the material
felt less test anxiety with the opportunity to correct may add to their learning. As figures 4a and 4b
their exams (t (304) =2.34**, p<0.020). These findings indicate, in the non-regrading sample, 38% felt they
build on the study of Brye et al. (2005) who reported learned “very much” from the mistakes they made on
decreased anxiety among his students. In the regrade the course exams, while 47% learned “some” from
study by Nickels and Uddin (2003), students stated these mistakes. In the regrading group, 88% of the
that even though they did not use the regrade students felt that correcting their tests was very
possibility, they felt it was nice to know that they had useful and caused them to learn from their mistakes,
the option, and it allowed them to be a little more and an additional 9% stated they experienced some
relaxed. learning benefit from it (t(168)= 7.72***, p<1.004-12).
Thus, this question clearly
5% 1%
2% 5% suggests that students feel
9% there is learning benefit to
11%
correcting missed exam
4%
questions.
5%
This is consistent with
previous studies in which
students indicated that the
13% regrade policy did result in
58% increased learning (Bacon
19% and Beyrouty, 1988; Nickels
68% and Uddin, 2003). It also
confirms the findings by
Haskett (2001), who
allowed students who were
Strongly agree Agree Somewhat agree/disagree Disagree Strongly disagree No opinion unhappy with their grades
to resubmit their term
a. Non-Regrading Group: b. Regrading Group: papers and abstracts as
The idea that I could make corrections to missed exam The idea that I could make test corrections to missed
questions would have put me more at ease during the exam questions put me more at ease during the exam many times as they wished
exam (Strongly Agree=5, Agree=4, Somewhat (Strongly Agree=5, Agree=4, Somewhat until they had obtained the
agree/disagree=3, Disagree=2, Strongly disagree=1, No agree/disagree=3, Disagree=2, Strongly disagree=1, No grade they wanted. Haskett
opinion=0, M=4.35, SD=1.16). opinion=0, M=3.99, SD=1.53).
describes this regrading
Figure 3a and b: Non-Regrading vs. Regrading Group: Test Anxiety, t (304)=2.34***, p<0.020 method as a powerful
teaching tool, since several
students showed significant
9% 1% 2% improvement in their
9%
6% writing ability after obtain-
ing feedback and resubmit-
ting their work. For exam-
38% ple, one student who
struggled with his writing
style on the early assign-
ments turned in later
papers that were of very
high quality on the first
attempt (Haskett, 2001).
47%
Benefits and Costs to
88%
Instructor
Regrading does require
Very much Some Not at all No opinion a heavy time commitment
a. Non-Regrading Group: b. Regrading Group: for both the professor and
Rate to what extent you learned from the mistakes you Rate the usefulness of correcting missed exam questions the students. Given that
made on the exams in this class (Very much=3, Some=2, (Very much=3, Some=2, Not at all=1, No opinion=0, this study has only been
Not at all=1, No opinion=0, M=2.15, SD=0.87). M=2.84, SD=0.51).
carried out in classes with
less than 50 students, the
Figure 4a and b: Non-Regrading vs. Regrading Group: Post-Exam Learning, instructor's added time
t (168)=7.72***, p<1.004-12

NACTA Journal • June 2010 27


Second Time

commitment due to regrading could prevent this correct missed exam questions and regrade exams,
alternative measure from working in a large class the benefits may outweigh the time commitment.
setting. However, given that this study only employed Students who were given the opportunity to re-
exams in short answer formats, which are quite time- submit their exam for regrading expressed that they
consuming to grade, larger classes could be accommo- were more likely to learn from their mistakes than
dated with the regrading option in a multiple choice the non-regrading group. Repeated exposure to
exam format. Thus, the method could work in a large material and the opportunity to re-submit exams that
classroom setting depending on the type of exam have already been graded may help students retain
chosen. Even if the students would solve a few exam information and learn from their mistakes, as
questions together in the classroom on the day the opposed to the traditional exam, which is routinely
exam would be returned, there would likely be an discarded after completion.
added learning benefit. Additionally, correcting missed exam questions
In addition, correcting missed exam questions is allows instructors to see where most students
not only beneficial to the students, professors may struggle with the material that is being taught. Thus,
learn from repeated exposure to students' work as teaching styles could be adjusted accordingly.
well. When a professor takes the time to review a Ultimately, this may lead to higher exam grades
student's exam for a second time, it allows for an during the next quarter, if the educator was success-
insight to which exam questions students struggle ful in conveying the material in an improved way. It
with and which ones they succeed at. Thus, reviewing could be argued though, that through regrading a
what students miss on their exams provides reliable mere inflation of the students' grades may occur.
feedback which allows professors to adjust their However, this was not confirmed by previous litera-
teaching styles accordingly. Another benefit to the ture, as Risley (2007) found that only about 18% of his
professor is the opportunity to incorporate more students who participated in a similar regrading
peer-learning activities since the students will activity received a higher semester/quarter course
already be familiar with one another. grade. Even if allowing students to resubmit their
Previous studies observed an improved student- work for more points does not affect their final course
instructor relationship (Longer et al., 1987; Risley, grade, the students' morale is increased and a better
2007; Nickels and Uddin, 2003). Risley (2007) found learning environment and student/teacher relation-
that the students felt better about the class, as they ship is established (Nickels and Uddin, 2003). In our
felt more fairly treated by the instructor. This was study, students overwhelmingly indicated a positive
observed in our study as well: Allowing students to attitude about the opportunity to make corrections to
correct their exams resulted in less apprehension missed exam questions for regrading.
regarding exam taking and resulted in a more relaxed This study leaves some questions open for future
classroom environment. research. As a preliminary study in this area, it is
Lastly, allowing students to gain more points by limited to college students at two universities in
regrading exams lessened the need and want for extra California and Arkansas. Expanding the geographic
credit activities which can cause stress and a large focus of the study to include college students on more
time commitment to the professor. U.S. states, or even other countries, would enrich the
findings. Further data collection may continue,
Summary where more detailed data on the students' time spent
This study constitutes a unique contribution to and method of regrading the exams will be collected.
the existing literature because it evaluates whether Additional information about student learning will
altering a traditional assessment tool can enhance be collected by repeating questions from the semes-
student learning. Our study shows that the opportu- ter/quarter exams on the final exam. Although
nity to correct their semester/quarter exams signifi- students indicated that they learned from their
cantly eased students' test anxiety, which would mistakes, an objective comparison of final exam
create a more positive learning environment. grades will need to be conducted to quantify whether
Correcting missed exam questions as an alternative their learning was realized and retained sufficiently
teaching method allowed for increased student long term to score well on the final exam. Given that
interaction, more positive attitudes regarding exam the regrading option is not offered on the final exams
taking and a better learning environment. This of either treatment groups, a comparison by final
alternative exam lifecycle also revealed students' exam grades could show additional insight into
affinity for using their books and notes to correct student learning.
answers and working together in groups in addition
to visiting the professor for assistance. Literature Cited
Furthermore, the survey suggests that the Bacon, R.K. and C.A. Beyrouty. 1988. Test retakes by
additional opportunity for students to review missed groups of students as a technique to enhance
exam questions may help to retain the information learning. Journal of Agronomic Education 17(2):
long term. This study shows that if professors and 99-101.
students are willing to put in the time and effort to Bain, K. 2004.What the best college teachers do.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
28 NACTA Journal • June 2010
Second Time

Brye, K.R., M.C. Savin, and E.E. Gbur. 2005. McKeachie, W.J. 2002. McKeachie's teaching tips.
Graduate-level science classes: How important 11th ed. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
are extra time, open notes, and book when Company.
evaluating students with calculation-oriented Nickels, K. and M. Uddin. 2003. The impact on
exams? NACTA Journal 49:2-9. student learning of resubmission of work and
Haskett, J. 2001. Integrating inquiry-based learning, flexible deadlines. Proceedings from the Annual
student feedback, and lecture in a science course. Meeting of the American Society for Engineering
Journal of Natural Resources and Life Science Education, Gulf-Southwest Annual Meeting,
Education 30: 23-26. Arlington, Texas, 19-21 March.
Light, R. 1990. The Harvard assessment seminars. Risley, J.M. 2007. Reworking exams to teach chemis-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Graduate try content and reinforce student learning.
School of Education and Kennedy School of Journal of Chemical Education 84: 1445-1447.
Government.
Longer, D.E., D.B. Marx, and D.W. Albers. 1987. The
influence of unannounced partial retesting on
learning and classroom attendance. Journal of
Agronomic Education 16:3-5.

NACTA Journal • June 2010 29


Copyright of NACTA Journal is the property of North American Colleges & Teachers of Agriculture and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Вам также может понравиться