Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/archger

Does negative information about aging influence older adults’ physical T


performance and subjective age?

Manon Marqueta, , Mohamed Boutaayamoub,c, Cédric Schwartzc, Médéa Locquetd,
Olivier Bruyèred, Jean-Louis Croisierc,e, Stéphane Adama
a
Psychology of Aging Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
b
INTELSIG Laboratory, Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
c
Laboratory of Human Motion Analysis, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
d
Department of Public Health, Epidemiology and Health Economics, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
e
Department of Motricity Sciences, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study investigated the way negative stereotypes influence older adults’ physical performance and how old
Stereotype threat they feel mentally and physically. Sixty-four older adults aged 65 years and older performed different physical
Self-perceptions of aging tasks using a 3D optoelectronic system under a low or high stereotype threat condition. Self-perceptions of aging
Balance were considered as a moderator of the effects of threat. Overall, the effects of threat on physical performance
Walking
were mostly not significant across tasks. However, threat condition influenced older adults' mental subjective
Subjective age
age after they had performed the physical tests; people in the high-threat condition felt closer to their chron-
ological age. Threat also influenced participants' physical subjective age, and this effect was moderated by self-
perceptions of aging. More precisely, participants in the high-threat condition felt 7% physically older than their
chronological age when they had more negative self-perceptions, while participants in the low-threat condition
felt 13% younger. No differences emerged for participants who had more positive self-perceptions. The present
findings suggest that performing physical tests under stereotype threat might worsen older people’s subjective
experience of their own aging by making them feel older.

1. Introduction of confirming negative stereotypes and, consequently, underperform on


stereotype relevant tasks (Steele & Aronson, 1995). Even if older adults
In modern industrialized societies, the increasing proportion of are stereotyped as cognitively and physically incompetent (e.g.,
older people is associated with an idea of aging focused deeply on de- Hummert, 2011), research on stereotype threat has mostly investigated
cline. Even if older people are perceived as warm by younger in- the effects of stereotype threat on cognitive abilities (for a review, see
dividuals, they are often considered as physically and cognitively inept Lamont, Swift, & Abrams, 2015). To our knowledge, only two studies
(Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008). According to Levy (2003, 2009), these (Horton, Baker, Pearce, & Deakin, 2010; Swift, Lamont, & Abrams,
negative stereotypes about aging are internalized across the lifespan 2012) examined stereotype threat effects on older adults’ motor skills.
and become self-relevant as people grow older (i.e., self-perceptions of Swift et al. (2012) found that a measure of handgrip strength and
aging: SPA), which may affect their mental and physical health. In persistence was impaired among older people when they were told that
particular, a number of long-term follow-up studies have shown that the purpose of the research was to see whether older people performed
holding negative SPA at baseline are associated with more physical various tasks differently when compared with young people. On the
health problems and an increased-mortality risk over time (for a review, contrary, Horton et al. (2010) found no effect of stereotype threat (i.e.,
see Westerhof et al., 2014). For example, Sargent-Cox, Anstey, and reading an article confirming decline of memory and physical abilities
Luszcz (2012) found that poor SPA were associated with a steep decline during aging) on measures of grip strength and flexibility. One possible
of balance, gait speed, and ability to rise from a chair in older adults explanation for this difference may be that while participants’ grip
over a 16-year period. strength was measured directly after the manipulation in the first study,
Not only can SPA affect physical performance, but stereotype threat there was a delay between the manipulation and the measures of the
may also play a role. Stereotype threat appears when people feel a risk dependent variables in the second one. Stereotype threat effects are


Corresponding author. Present address: INSERM U1219 Bordeaux Research Centre for Population Health, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France.
E-mail address: mmarquet@uliege.be (M. Marquet).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2018.06.013
Received 17 January 2018; Received in revised form 22 June 2018; Accepted 25 June 2018
Available online 26 June 2018
0167-4943/ © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

more evident when the dependent variables are measured proximally to threat on physical subjective age may also be moderated by SPA. More
the manipulation (Lamont et al., 2015). precisely, this effect may be particularly true for people who perceive
These mixed findings highlight the need to conduct further research more negatively their physical functioning.
for a better understanding of the effects of stereotype threat on older Taking into account the previous results, the purpose of the current
adults’ physical abilities. Indeed, stereotype threat has been shown to study was to examine the effects of stereotype threat on older adults’
induce a prevention focus among older adults, resulting in cognitive physical performance, measured as objectively as possible, and on their
performance that is slow and cautious (Barber & Mather, 2013b; subjective age. Firstly, we hypothesized that older people under high
Popham & Hess, 2015). Therefore, it would be interesting to investigate threat would underperform in physical tests. More specifically, we
whether older adults under threat adopt a more cautious approach to hypothesized that stereotype threat would induce a prevention focus
physical tasks resulting in slower execution of these tasks. Moreover, if (Barber & Mather, 2013a): older adults exposed to negative age-related
participants are vigilant and try to control step-by-step their perfor- information would be more likely to adopt a conservative strategy
mance to ensure a positive outcome, this could be damaging in tasks when performing physical tasks, and this could disrupt their perfor-
(e.g., walking) that rely more on proceduralized strategies (i.e., auto- mance. Secondly, we expected that older people in a stereotype threat
matic strategies running with minimum intervention from working situation would feel older. Similarly to previous studies (e.g., Marquet
memory) (Beilock, Jellison, Rydell, McConnell, & Carr, 2006). et al., 2017; Uotinen, Rantanen, & Suutama, 2005), we included mea-
In addition, since stereotype threat and SPA have mostly been stu- sures of both mental and physical subjective age. We expected a greater
died separately, their joint influence on physical abilities should be influence of stereotypes on physical subjective age, that is, when there
further examined. A recent study focusing on memory abilities showed was a match between the activated stereotypes and the outcome do-
that older people were more vulnerable to stereotype threat if they had main (Kornadt & Rothermund, 2015; Marquet et al., 2017). We also
more negative SPA (Fernández-Ballesteros, Bustillos, & Huici, 2015). tested whether these effects were moderated by SPA (i.e., perceptions of
However, the question remains whether such results would be similar one’s own physical functioning during aging). We hypothesized that
in situations assessing physical abilities under stereotype threat. stereotype threat effects on physical performance and physical sub-
In parallel to these questions, the effects of stereotype threat on jective age would be more pronounced among participants with more
subjective age have been insufficiently studied. Subjective age reflects negative SPA.
the numeric age people feel like or view themselves (Stephan, Sutin, &
Terracciano, 2015b). Previous studies consistently found that while 2. Materials and methods
adolescents and young adults tend to feel older than their chronological
age, most middle-aged and older adults report feeling younger than 2.1. Participants
their chronological age by an average of 20% (e.g., Montepare &
Lachman, 1989; Rubin & Berntsen, 2006). Despite this trend, subjective Based on sample sizes and effect sizes from previous studies (Beilock
age is somewhat malleable and people can deviate from their chron- et al., 2006; Coudin & Alexopoulos, 2010; Hausdorff, Levy, & Wei,
ological age in response to experiences situationally and socially asso- 1999; Swift et al., 2012), we recruited seventy-two older people aged
ciated with a certain normative age (Montepare, 2009). For example, 65 years and over. They agreed to participate in our study between
inducing an older subjective age can be achieved by simulating an age- April 2015 and February 2016. They were recruited on the basis of data
related decline. Older adults who experience visual disfluency during a collected during their last appointment in a larger ongoing longitudinal
reading task and who receive no explanation for the blurriness, feel study conducted among community-dwelling elderly subjects (Beaudart
older than participants who are told the text is blurry because of a et al., 2015). We contacted participants with good global cognitive
printing error and those who read clear text (Eibach, Mock, & Courtney, functioning (i.e., Mini-Mental State Examination score ≥24–26, de-
2010). Situations can also be manipulated to induce a younger sub- pending on participants’ education; Tombaugh & McIntyre, 1992),
jective age. Older adults who are led to believe that their test perfor- without walking assistance, who were not diagnosed as sarcopenic in
mance on a grip-strength task is higher than the performance of 80% of Beaudart et al.’ (2015) study (according to the European Working
their peers report feeling younger after the test and have better sub- Group on Sarcopenia in Older People; Cruz-Jentoft et al., 2010), and
sequent performance in the same task compared to older adults who who did not present a risk of falling (i.e., Timed Up and Go test
perform the same test with no feedback (Stephan, Chalabaev, Kotter- score < 14 s; Podsiadlo & Richardson, 1991, and Tinetti test score ≥24
Grühn, & Jaconelli, 2013). points; Tinetti, 1986).
Some results also suggest that age-related stereotypes, which are Eight participants were removed from the analyses: one reported
likely to be activated by cognitive testing, may influence subjective age having Parkinson disease, three had missing data on our measure of
because there are cues in the environment that remind older adults of SPA, and four reported a fall during the past 6 months, which had not
the expectation that they are not competent (e.g., one is expected to been specified earlier. According to G*Power 3.1 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang,
have memory problems because of aging). This in turn prevents them & Buchner, 2007) and given this sample size, the power to detect a
from maintaining a younger subjective age. For example, it has been medium to large effect size comparable to other studies (Beilock et al.,
shown that older people feel older after performing stereotyped tasks, 2006; Swift et al., 2012) in a multiple regression analysis varies be-
such as memory tests (Geraci, De Forrest, Hughes, Saenz, & Tirso, 2018; tween 1 – b = 0.71 and 1 – b = 0.98.
Hughes, Geraci, & De Forrest, 2013). Interestingly, performing a task In the end, 29 female and 35 male were included in our analyses.
described as a memory test is sufficient to create a stereotype threat Their age ranged from 65 to 90 years (M = 72.22, SD = 5.37) and on
situation (Rahhal, Hasher, & Colcombe, 2001). In line with these re- average they had completed 13 years education. Fifty-nine percent
sults, a recent study showed that when older people are worried about were married, 17% were widowed and the remainder were separated
confirming negative stereotypes concerning their memory abilities in a (13%) or single (11%).
stereotype threat situation, they feel mentally (i.e., cognitive vitality)
older (Marquet, Missotten, Dardenne, & Adam, 2017). Extending this 2.2. Procedure
logic to physical subjective age, we suggest that the activation of ne-
gative age stereotypes about physical performance in a stereotype This study was conducted in two parts described below.
threat situation may lead older people to feel physically older. More-
over, since there is some evidence that beliefs that age-related memory 2.2.1. Part 1 – longitudinal study
changes are inevitable predict the subjective aging effect over the First, during the last appointment scheduled in the context of the
course of a testing session (Geraci et al., 2018), the effect of stereotype longitudinal study, participants completed different questionnaires and

182
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

realized different tasks that we used as baseline characteristics (for true purpose of the study and that any questions had been answered to
greater details about the procedure, see Beaudart et al., 2015). They their satisfaction. No participant refused to sign the second form nor
also answered a questionnaire that assessed their SPA during this ap- asked for their data to be removed from the study.
pointment. This allowed us not to administer this measure during the
second part of the study (which manipulated stereotypes threat) in 2.3. Measures from the longitudinal study
order to avoid suspicion among participants about the true nature of the
study. 2.3.1. Baseline clinical characteristics and physical performance
We considered different characteristics collected during the larger
2.2.2. Part 2 – experimental study study: anthropometric data (i.e., height, weight, body mass index),
During the second part of the study, all participants were tested number of drugs, self-reported comorbidities, activities of daily living
individually by a 24-year-old female experimenter in a quiet private (i.e., Katz; Katz, Ford, Moskowitz, Jackson, & Jaffe, 1963, Lawton;
room (Laboratory of Human Motion Analysis of the University of Lawton & Brody, 1970), cognitive function (i.e., Mini-Mental State
Liège). This part of the study was introduced as a study to investigate Examination; Tombaugh & McIntyre, 1992), level of depression (i.e.,
the mechanisms that underlie walking using a kinematic 3D analysis Geriatric Depression Scale; Yesavage et al., 1983), nutritional status
system. Participants gave their informed consent to take part in the (Guigoz, Vellas, & Garry, 1996), physical frailty (Fried et al., 2001),
study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of quality of life (i.e., SF-36; Ware & Sherbourne, 1992), tiredness
Liège. After giving consent, participants were asked to rate how old (Avlund, Kreiner, & Schultz-Larsen, 1993), grip strength and work as
they felt, both mentally and physically (baseline mental and physical well as risk of fall (i.e., Timed Up and Go test; Podsiadlo & Richardson,
subjective age), and then to provide socio-demographic data. 1991, and Tinetti test; Tinetti, 1986), and physical performance (i.e.,
Stereotype threat was then manipulated; 32 participants were ran- Short Physical Performance Battery test; Guralnik et al., 2000). We also
domly assigned to a low-threat condition and 32 to a high-threat con- assessed the time elapsed (in days) between the first (i.e., last ap-
dition. Participants in the low-threat condition were told only that the pointment as part of the larger longitudinal study) and second part of
aim of the study was to investigate the mechanisms that underlie the study (i.e., experimental study).
walking and to study their movements in 3D during different walking
tasks. In addition to this explanation, participants in the high-threat 2.3.2. SPA
condition were also told that the precise aim of the research was to SPA were measured using the French version of the Attitudes to
“study balance among people aged 65 years and older”. Participants in the Aging Questionnaire (Marquet et al., 2016). This questionnaire assessed
high-threat condition were also exposed to several additional pieces of attitudes toward the aging process as a personal experience from the
information to emphasize the idea that older adults suffer from balance perspective of older adults. For the 24 items of the questionnaire, re-
problems. In fact, they were invited to answer a short survey before spondents were asked to indicate their degree of agreement on a five-
performing the different physical tasks. This short survey gave parti- point Likert scale, where 1 reflects “strongly disagree” or “not at all
cipants negative information about balance abilities during aging. They true”, and 5 reflects “strongly agree” or “extremely true”. Even if this
were asked to judge four different posters with regard to different scale can be divided into three subscales, we only used the Physical
characteristics (e.g., clarity, attractiveness, esthetics). Three of the Change subscale (range: 8–40): a high score reflects a more positive
posters focused on balance (e.g., “How to keep one’s balance after 60?” view of one’s physical functioning (e.g., “My health is better than I
and “Arrange your home to resolve your balance disabilities”) and one expected for my age”) and of one’s experience of aging itself (e.g.,
on fall (e.g., “How to arrange one’s home to avoid falls?”). Participants “Growing older has been easier than I thought”).
were told that the purpose of the survey was to select the most relevant
poster to raise awareness of balance disorders and encourage home 2.4. Measures from the experimental study
modifications among older adults.
All the participants then completed different physical tasks in the 2.4.1. Socio-demographic characteristics
following order: 1) Timed Up and Go test, (2) Comfortable walking We assessed chronological age, educational level, sex, and marital
speed, (3) Rapid walking speed, (4) Tandem walking, (5) Crossing over status. Moreover, participants were asked whether they suffered from
two different obstacles, (6) One-leg standing test (twice), and (7) Grip osteoarthritis in the hands (responding, yes, severely; yes, mildly; yes,
strength and persistence. During the physical tasks (except grip strength very slightly or no, not at all, coded 1 to 4, respectively) (Swift et al.,
and persistence), participants were equipped with 3D active markers, 2012). They were also asked to report their subjective mental and
which allowed their kinematics to be followed and physical perfor- physical health (“How do you judge your current state of mental/
mance to be measured as objectively as possible. physical health?”) on 9-cm continuous scales with “very bad” and “very
All participants were given instructions for each test - i.e., what was good” as endpoints (Coudin & Alexopoulos, 2010). Finally, they re-
expected from them (e.g., walking at a comfortable pace along a line). ported whether they had experienced a sad or depressed mood during
Participants in the high-threat condition only were also reminded of the the last two weeks on a 10-cm scale ranging from “not at all” to “ex-
study aims before each test. They were informed at the beginning of tremely”.
each test of the type of balance that was measured (e.g., when getting
up from a chair, when walking slowly compared to rapidly, when 2.4.2. Motion analysis system and physical tasks
crossing an obstacle, in a static position) by the female experimenter. During the physical tasks described below (except grip strength and
For the grip strength and persistence measures, participants in the high- endurance), we used a three-dimensional optoelectronic system
threat condition were also told that muscular strength was assessed (Codamotion™, Charnwood Dynamics, Rothley, UK) to measure the
since it influences balance in older adults. movements. We tracked the 3D positions with active markers placed on
Following these tasks, all participants reported their mental and the volunteers and four Codamotion CX1 units. The acquisition rate was
physical subjective age for the second time. They also answered two equal to 200 Hz. Three markers were placed on each foot, three on each
questionnaires: one assessed their suggestibility, and the other assessed arm and two on the back. Markers on each heel and big toe (i.e., hallux)
their usual physical activity. were placed on the shoes. Markers on arms and the back were included
Finally, participants were debriefed verbally and in writing. They as part of separate investigations and will not be analyzed or discussed
signed a second consent form indicating that they were aware of the further. For some physical tasks (i.e., the Timed Up and Go and the one-

183
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

leg standing test), we also used force plates (Kistler™ type 9281EA, Kisle 2.4.3. Subjective age
AG, Switzerland) to compute physical performance parameters. The Participants were told “Many people feel older or younger than they
physical tasks were performed in the following order. actually are”. They were asked “What age do you feel now?” and were
The Timed Up and Go test is a measure of balance performance provided with two different lines to determine the age they felt men-
(Podsiadlo & Richardson, 1991) that provides, in seconds, the time tally (Mental subjective age; i.e., cognitive vitality: reasoning, lan-
taken by an individual to rise from a chair, walk three meters, turn, and guage, memory, attention, etc.) and physically (Physical subjective age;
return back to a sitting position. Higher scores indicate slower walking e.g., muscle strength, cardiovascular and pulmonary health).
speed. Participants had to draw a tick mark on each line that was 120 mm long
For the comfortable and rapid walking, participants were asked to and contained no markings other than two endpoint age labels (“0” on
walk straight forward at a comfortable pace and then as fast as possible the far left and “120” on the far right) (Hughes et al., 2013) Subjective
along a 14.3-meter line. The position of the markers placed on the heel age was determined by measuring the distance between the left end and
and toe of each foot were measured during each walking test. A signal- the participant’s tick mark in millimeters (1 mm = 1 year). Consistent
processing algorithm based on a piecewise linear fitting method with prior research (e.g., Eibach et al., 2010; Stephan, Sutin, Caudroit,
(Boutaayamou et al., 2014) was then applied to these data to compute & Terracciano, 2016), proportional discrepancy scores were calculated
different parameters based on approximately 3–4 strides (Boutaayamou by subtracting participants’ chronological age from their mental and
et al., 2015). Mean velocity (m/s), mean double support time (s), ca- physical subjective age, and these difference scores were divided by
dence (stride/s), mean minimum toe clearance (mm), and mean stride chronological age. A positive value indicates an older subjective age,
width (mm) were selected as gait performance parameters based on the whereas a negative value indicates a youthful subjective age. Baseline
literature (Chiba, Ebihara, Tomita, Sasaki, & Butler, 2005; Sturnieks, St subjective age measures (the same items administered at the beginning
George, & Lord, 2008; Verghese, Holtzer, Lipton, & Wang, 2009). These of the study) were used as covariates.
parameters represent different domains of spatiotemporal gait perfor-
mance (e.g., Hollman, McDade, & Petersen, 2011). 2.4.4. Suggestibility
During the tandem walking, subjects were required to walk heel-to- Participants were also evaluated for suggestibility levels using the
toe along a 8.3-meter line. They were told that precision, not velocity, short version of the Multidimensional Iowa Suggestibility Scale (Kotov,
matters. The mean inter-feet distance as well as the mean lateral dis- Bellman, & Watson, 2004), a 21-item self-reported measure of sug-
tance between the feet were computed with the same signal-processing gestibility that was translated in French. The scale includes five sug-
algorithm described in the previous paragraph. Higher scores indicate a gestibility subscales: Consumer Suggestibility, Persuadability, Physio-
decreased ability of maintaining medio-lateral stability. Note that data logical Suggestibility, Physiological Reactivity, and Peer Conformity.
for this test were missing for one participant because of a technical We used the total score (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86), which corresponds
problem with Codamotion during the acquisition of the data. to the sum of the five subscales (Range: 21–105).
Participants were asked to walk and cross over an obstacle, twice, at
a comfortable pace. The markers placed on the heels allowed us to
measure the time taken to cross the obstacle, which can be considered 2.4.5. Physical activity
as indicator of balance (Pan, Hsu, Chang, Renn, & Wu, 2016; Sturnieks Physical activity was measured with the Physical Activity self-
et al., 2008). The first obstacle was a box (14 cm height, 27.5 cm width, Administered Questionnaire (Vol et al., 2011). It is comprised of 22
and 33.5 cm length), and the second, a differently sized box (22.5 cm closed questions and divided into three subscales: usual daily activities,
height, 22.5 cm width, and 31 cm length). Obstacles were placed regular sport activities, and leisure activities. We considered the global
midway, and participants had to cross them lengthways without physical activity level, which is the sum of the three subscales (Range:
touching them. 3–15).
The one-leg standing test (for a review, see Michikawa, Nishiwaki,
Takebayashi, & Toyama, 2009) assessed static and standing balance. 2.5. Statistical analyses
Participants were asked to stand on their right leg if possible, and the
test was interrupted after 30 s. The time until the participant put the Data analyses were performed using SPSS 23 (IBM Corp, 2015) and
other foot down was used for the measurement. Compensatory arm Hayes’ PROCESS macro for SPSS v3.0.
movements were accepted during the test. The test was performed Preliminary independent sample t-tests (or Mann–Whitney U tests)
twice, and we considered the mean time the participants stood on their and Chi-squared analyses were first conducted on baseline character-
leg (Range: 0–30 s). istics and performance from the longitudinal study to determine if there
Finally, hand grip strength (in kilograms) and endurance (in sec- were any significant variations, and thus potential confounds, across
onds) were measured using a Saehan® type hand dynamometer since conditions. We also computed these analyses on demographic char-
strength has been shown to be lower in a stereotype threat condition acteristics collected during the experimental study (including chron-
(Swift et al., 2012). The measures were conducted with the participants ological age, mental and physical subjective age, gender, marital status,
sitting and holding the dynamometer at a 90-degree angle with fore- educational level, and self-rated health) as well as on SPA, suggest-
arms on a table. For the grip strength, participants were encouraged to ibility, and physical activity. The criterion for statistical significance
perform maximal contractions three times. Three measurements were was p < 0.05.
taken on each hand alternating between the left and right hand. The Multiple regressions were then conducted for physical tasks and
best score of the six trials was used in the analyses (Roberts et al., subjective age as the dependent variables and threat, SPA and their
2011). Endurance was considered, for each hand, as the ability to interaction as independent variables. In these analyses, we centered
maintain at least 50% of one’s maximal voluntary contraction for as threat (low vs. high, coded as -0.5 and 0.5, respectively). SPA was also
long as one could. A force equivalent to 50% of this maximal force was mean-centered before being multiplied to create the interaction term.
then shown on 15.6 inch computer screen by a red line (using the data Residuals of half of the physical performance variables were not nor-
software bought with the dynamometer), and the subjects were in- mally distributed. Results for these variables (b, SE, and 95% con-
structed to press the handle until they reached the line and to hold the fidence intervals) are therefore based on 5000 bootstrapped resamples.
handle at this minimum level for as long as possible. We used the mean Significance for regression analyses was established when the percentile
time of the two endurance measures. confidence intervals did not include zero.

184
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

Table 1 Table 2
Baseline characteristics and performance from the larger longitudinal study as a Sample characteristics as a function of stereotype threat condition.
function of stereotype threat condition.
Low-threat condition High-threat condition
Low-threat High-threat Mean (SD)/n Mean (SD)/n
condition condition
Mean (SD)/n Mean (SD)/n Chronological age 72.03 (5.45) 72.41 (5.37)
Baseline mental subjective age −0.15 (0.15) −0.19 (0.19)
Number of concomittant diseases 4.38 (2.57) 5.06 (2.65) Baseline physical subjective age −0.10 (0.13) −0.10 (0.16)
Number of drugs 3.72 (2.25) 3.50 (2.34) Years of education 13.41 (3.20) 13.25 (2.98)
Anthropometric data Osteoarthritis 0.94 (0.91) 0.66 (0.97)
Height (cm) 166.14 (9.52) 166.04 (7.97) Mental health 6.63 (1.34) 7.34 (1.26)
Weight (kg) 75.76 (14.51) 73.12 (14.37) Physical health 5.91 (1.23) 6.34 (1.50)
Body mass index 27.26 (3.44) 26.38 (3.99) Depressed mood 1.85 (2.34) 1.57 (2.17)
Cognitive function – MMSE score (0–30) 29.31 (0.78) 29.28 (0.73) SPA 30.31 (5.40) 32.16 (4.56)
Depression level – GDS score (0–15) 2.19 (2.81) 2.25 (2.78) Suggestibility 34.84 (8.26) 35.47 (8.00)
Katz total score (6–24) 8.19 (0.40) 8.09 (0.30) Physical activity 9.00 (1.21) 8.66 (1.17)
Lawton score Gender 15 women 14 women
Men total (1–5) 5.00 (0.00) 4.89 (0.47) 17 men 18 men
Women total (1–8) 8.00 (0.00) 8.00 (0.00) Marital status 5 single 2 single
Frailty - Fried score 20 married 18 married
Nonfrail 24 23 4 separated 4 separated
Prefrail 8 8 3 widowed 8 widowed
Frail 0 1
Mini-Nutritional Assessment All differences are non-significant (p > 0.12), except for mental health
Well-nourished 32 32 (p = 0.03).
Quality of Life – SF-36 total score (%) 71.28 (15.90) 70.96 (17.69) SD = Standard deviation. SPA = Self-perceptions of aging.
Tiredness – Mobility test (6 points) 0.78 (1.56) 0.59 (1.27)
Grip strength (kg) 33.98 (13.34) 34.55 (12.31)
Grip work (kg × s) 501.77 (369.84) 510.36 (306.10) 8 mm higher among participants in the high-threat condition compared
Risk of falls to the low-threat condition (b = 7.79, 95% CI [0.2968; 15.1798],
Tinetti gait (16 points) 15.88 (0.42) 15.91 (0.39) p = 0.04).
Tinetti walk (12 points) 11.84 (0.51) 11.84 (0.51)
Tinetti total (28 points) 27.72 (0.89) 27.75 (0.80)
TUG (s) 9.4 (2.02) 9.17 (2.11) 3.3. Subjective age
Physical performance – SPPB
Total score (12 points) 10.84 (1.30) 11.06 (1.13)
Table 3 also shows that threat predicted mental (b = 0.11,
Walking speed (m/s) 1.19 (0.25) 1.24 (0.27)
Time elapsed (days) 44.28 (23.17) 46.5 (20.62)
p = 0.02) and physical subjective age (b = 0.11, p = 0.01), and that
people in the high-threat condition felt 11% older than people in the
All differences are non-significant (p > 0.28). low-threat condition (Fig. 1A). It means, for example, that controlling
SD = Standard deviation; BMI = Body Mass Index; MMSE = Mini-Mental State for initial differences in subjective age, a 70-year-old individual in the
Examination; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; TUG = Timed Up and Go test; high-threat condition felt around 8 years older than a 70-year-old in-
SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery test. dividual in the low-threat condition after the experimental manipula-
tion. Moreover, the prediction of physical subjective age by the
threat × SPA interaction was under the threshold of alpha = 0.05
(b = −0.02, 95% CI [−0.0347; −0.0002], p = 0.048) (Fig. 1B).
3. Results
Simple slopes analyses regarding the effect of threat on physical
subjective age moderated by SPA showed that for people with more
3.1. Participants’ characteristics
positive SPA (1 SD above the mean: b = 0.0192, 95% CI [−0.1003;
0.1387], p = 0.75), there was no difference in physical subjective age
Preliminary analyses revealed that participants in the low- and high-
between people in the low- and high-threat condition. However, for
threat condition did not differ (all p > 0.05) regarding clinical char-
people with more negative SPA, people in the high-threat condition felt
acteristics and baseline physical performance collected during their last
physically older than people in the low-threat condition (1 SD below
meeting as part of the larger longitudinal study (Beaudart et al., 2015)
the mean: b = 0.20, 95% CI [0.0712; 0.3191], p = 0.003).
(Table 1). It can hence be concluded that the random assignment of
Further examination revealed that physical subjective age increased
participants to the experimental condition was successful for these
with negative SPA in the high-threat condition (b = −0.02, 95% CI
baseline characteristics.
[−0.0286; −0.0013], p = 0.03), as shown in Fig. 1B; this relationship
Other characteristics measured during the current study did not
was in the opposite direction but was not significant in the low-threat
vary significantly by condition, except for self-perceived mental health
condition (b = 0.003, 95% CI [−0.0089; 0.0138], p = 0.67).
(Table 2). Because computing the regression analyses with including
self-perceived mental health as a covariate did not change the results,
we did not control for it in regression analyses. 4. Discussion

Older people are usually stereotyped as being mentally and physi-


3.2. Physical performance cally incompetent (Cuddy et al., 2008). While the effects of age ste-
reotypes on cognitive functioning have been widely investigated in
Regression analyses conducted on physical performance are shown older adults (for a review, see Lamont et al., 2015), the literature has
in Table 3. Except for tandem walking (i.e., mean lateral distance be- produced mixed results regarding the impact of negative age-related
tween feet), we did not find any significant effect of stereotype threat information on their physical performance (e.g., Horton et al., 2010;
condition, SPA, or the interaction between these two variables, which Swift et al., 2012). The aim of the present research was, therefore, to
indicated no significant effect. The only significant effect that emerged investigate the effects of stereotype threat on older adults’ physical
showed an effect of the threat condition when participants walked in performance as well as on their subjective age. We focused on physical
tandem. More precisely, the mean lateral distance between feet was performance tasks that can be used to predict balance disorders among

185
M. Marquet et al.

Table 3
Prediction of physical performance and subjective age by stereotype threat condition and SPA.
Intercept Threat SPA Threat x SPA

b SE 95% CI b SE 95% CI b SE 95% CI b SE 95% CI

Physical performance

TUG Time (s) 11.37 0.29 10.83; 11.93 0.14 0.56 −0.97; 1.26 −0.08 0.06 −0.19; 0.03 −0.07 0.12 −0.29 ; 0.16
Comfortable walking speed *Speed (m/s) 1.14 0.02 1.09; 1.19 −0.03 0.05 −0.12; 0.06 0.01 0.00 −0.003; 0.01 0.01 0.01 −0.01 ; 0.03
Double support (s) 0.15 0.00 0.14; 0.16 0.00 0.01 −0.01; 0.01 0.00 0.00 −0.003; 0.0002 0.00 0.00 −0.003; 0.003
Cadence (strides/s) 0.89 0.01 0.88; 0.91 −0.01 0.02 −0.04; 0.02 0.00 0.00 −0.001; 0.01 0.00 0.00 −0.01; 0.004
*Toe clearance (mm) 20.04 1.23 17.22; 22.55 0.79 2.46 −4.03; 5.52 −0.12 0.24 −0.60; 0.35 −0.16 0.49 −1.16; 0.77
Width (mm) 70.45 3.72 63.00; 77.90 10.91 7.45 −3.98; 25.81 −0.99 0.75 −2.50; 0.51 −1.03 1.51 −4.05; 1.99
Rapid walking speed *Speed (m/s) 1.58 0.03 1.51; 1.64 −0.06 0.07 −0.19; 0.08 0.01 0.01 −0.01; 0.02 0.02 0.01 −0.01; 0.05
Double support (s) 0.10 0.00 0.10; 0.11 0.00 0.01 −0.01; 0.02 0.00 0.00 −0.002; 0.0004 0.00 0.00 −0.004; 0.002
Cadence (strides/s) 1.06 0.01 1.04; 1.08 0.00 0.02 −0.05; 0.05 0.00 0.00 −0.001; 0.01 0.00 0.00 −0.01; 0.01
*Toe clearance (mm) 22.69 1.42 19.98; 25.59 1.11 2.83 −4.36; 6.75 −0.13 0.29 −0.71; 0.44 −0.19 0.58 −1.40; 0.90
*Width (mm) 74.97 3.90 67.63; 82.79 10.34 7.82 −5.11; 25.37 −0.95 0.80 −2.53; 0.61 0.54 1.59 −2.70; 3.59
Tandem walkinga *Mean distance (mm) 30.53 3.26 24.54; 37.28 3.57 6.43 −8.83; 16.38 0.61 0.63 −0.59; 1.90 0.01 1.28 −2.59; 2.40

186
*Mean lateral distance (mm) 25.05 1.91 21.33; 29.01 7.79 3.77 0.30; 15.18 0.05 0.47 −0.87; 0.97 0.98 0.95 −0.85; 2.90
Obstacles *Obstacle 1 - Time (s) 2.12 0.03 2.05; 2.19 0.07 0.07 −0.06; 0.20 −0.01 0.01 −0.03; 0.00 0.00 0.01 −0.03; 0.03
Obstacle 2 - Time (s) 2.28 0.04 2.20; 2.37 0.16 0.08 −0.01; 0.33 −0.02 0.01 −0.03; 0.0002 −0.03 0.02 −0.06; 0.01
One leg-standing *Time (s) 12.82 1.32 10.25; 15.43 −0.06 2.64 −5.24; 5.05 0.40 0.32 −0.21; 1.06 0.49 0.62 −0.72; 1.72
Grip Grip strength (kg) 35.64 1.54 32.56; 38.72 1.39 3.08 −4.77; 7.55 −0.24 0.31 −0.86; 0.39 0.54 0.62 −0.70; 1.79
Grip persistence (s) 36.71 2.40 31.90; 41.51 −0.30 4.80 −9.91; 9.31 0.52 0.49 −0.46; 1.49 −0.34 0.97 −2.29; 1.61

Subjective age

Mental subjective agea 0.00 0.03 −0.07; 0.07 0.11 0.05 0.02; 0.21 −0.01 0.00 −0.02; 0.002 −0.01 0.01 −0.03; 0.01
Physical subjective ageb −0.02 0.03 −0.07; 0.04 0.11 0.05 0.02; 0.19 −0.01 0.00 −0.02; 0.003 −0.02 0.01 −0.03;
-0.0002

TUG = Timed Up and Go test. SPA = Self-perceptions of aging. SE = Standard errors. 95% CI = 95% confidence intervals. an = 63. *Bootstrapped coefficients, standard errors, and confidence intervals. aBaseline mental
subjective age in covariate. bBaseline physical subjective age in covariate. Significant hypothesized effects are in bold.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

Fig. 1. Subjective age according to stereotype threat condition and self-perceptions of aging. SPA = Self-perceptions of aging. On the y-axis, value of zero represents
subjective age equivalent to actual age, negative values represent younger subjective age and positive values represent older subjective age.

older adults and we measured physical performance parameters as explain the results we obtained. Confidence intervals indicate that the
precisely and objectively as possible. We also considered participants’ non-significant effects were small or nonexistent. Therefore, non-sig-
perceptions of their own physical functioning as a moderator. nificant effects could not easily be rectified by collecting a larger
sample size.

4.1. Physical performance


4.2. Subjective age
The effects of threat on physical performance were mostly not sig-
nificant. Only one significant effect of threat emerged and it accorded While the effects of threat on physical performance were incon-
with our expectations. During tandem walking we observed that people clusive, we found an influence of our manipulation on psychological
in the high-threat condition exhibited a higher mean lateral distance variables (i.e., subjective age). Moreover, as expected, the effects were
between their feet, indicating that they had more difficulty aligning stronger for physical subjective age than mental subjective age
their feet when they walked heel to toe. (Kornadt & Rothermund, 2015). People in the high-threat condition felt
Overall, these non-significant effects of threat on physical perfor- mentally and physically older than participants in the low-threat con-
mance do not allow better understanding of the mixed findings from dition. However, while participants in the high-threat condition felt
previous studies (Horton et al., 2010; Swift et al., 2012). We suggested their actual age more than people in the low-threat condition when we
that Horton et al. (2010) may not have found effects because there was examined mental subjective age, they even felt older than their
a delay between the manipulation and the physical tasks in their study. chronological age when we examined physical subjective age. In par-
To avoid this potential bias, we repeated our manipulation before every ticular, among those who perceived their physical functioning more
task. However, we did not find effects on strength and persistence, negatively (i.e., more negative SPA), participants in the low-threat
which is contrary to the findings of Swift et al. (2012). We suggest that condition felt 13% younger than their actual age, while participants in
the measures in the current study were not impaired because the ma- the high-threat condition felt 7% older.
nipulation used did not sufficiently target strength but rather balance. The consequences of our manipulation on subjective age are wor-
This explanation accords with studies showing that the influence of rying. Several longitudinal studies have shown that feeling older can be
negative stereotypes on memory and balance performance is stronger damaging for older people’s mental and physical health. For example,
when the stereotype content corresponds to the outcome domain (Levy results of a recent study revealed that people aged 50 years and older
& Leifheit-Limson, 2009). with an older subjective age at baseline presented greater memory
More generally, the threat may not have impaired most physical decline over a 4-year follow-up period, in part because they experi-
performance measures because we used a manipulation including facts enced more depressive symptoms (Stephan et al., 2016). Regarding
about aging. It has been shown that stereotype threat effects are greater physical health, the results of two large longitudinal surveys of adults
when older adults think performance may be judged in relation to age aged 65 years and older showed that an older subjective age was as-
stereotypes, rather than when they are told ‘facts’ about age-related sociated with lower walking speed over a 2 and 4-year period (Stephan,
decline (Lamont et al., 2015). However, since we found expected results Sutin, & Terracciano, 2015a). It has also been demonstrated that people
for subjective age, the reason for an absence of effect may be better aged 70 years and older who were less satisfied with their aging and felt
explained by the physical tasks we used rather than by the stereotype older at baseline presented an increased-mortality risk over a period of
threat manipulation in itself. We suggest that the more proceduralized 12–16 years (Kotter-Grühn, Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Gerstorf, & Smith,
performances measured in our study (i.e., The Timed Up and Go time, 2009). Therefore, even if threatening instructions do not seem to have a
comfortable, and rapid walking speed tests) were unaffected overall great direct influence on physical performance, we suggest that re-
because these measures did not assess high-level proceduralized skills peated exposure to age stereotypes (by making older people feel closer
(e.g., golf putting). Past studies have indeed shown that prompting too to their chronological age or older) could have an effect on physical
much attention to execution processes best run outside conscious con- performance over time rather than directly. Such a hypothesis finds
trol (Beilock et al., 2006) is maladapted to high-level well-learned skills some support in a study conducted in the framework of the stereotype
(i.e., expert golf putting). Moreover, being cautious under high-threat embodiment theory. In this study (Levy, Pilver, Chung, & Slade, 2014),
has been shown to be inappropriate in cognitive tasks structured to older adults were primed subliminally with positive age stereotypes
reward the number of gains (Barber & Mather, 2013a). The physical across multiple sessions. Interestingly, over time, the subliminal acti-
tasks included in this study may not have been sufficiently gain-or- vation of positive stereotypes first produced a positive impact on age
iented to create the regulatory mismatch that has been proven to impair stereotypes and then on SPA. Physical function was improved only after
cognitive performances. Finally, even if it could be argued that the positive SPA had been strengthened. Since these results suggest that the
reason for an absence of significant results regarding physical perfor- increase in physical performance can only be observed after an im-
mance was due to a lack of power, we believe this argument cannot provement of SPA, we could make a similar assumption regarding the

187
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

results of our study. Exposure to negative age stereotypes could impact negative age stereotypes and lead them to feel older. If the case, it
physical performance over time only if these stereotypes influence older would be necessary to consider effective ways to communicate in-
people to feel older. formation about balance disorders and risk of falls during aging without
inducing negative age stereotypes.
4.3. Limits and future directions
Conflicts of interest
Contribution to the literature notwithstanding, the current study has
several limitations that should be considered in future research. Firstly, All authors of this article have no competing interests.
the question remains whether instructions in clinical settings may lead
to an underestimation of the physical abilities of older people. A first Acknowledgement
possibility for the absence of effects of threat on physical performance
could be that, given the procedure used, the experimenter was not blind This work was supported by the Belgian Fund for Scientific Research
to conditions, which might have influenced participants’ behaviors (F. R. S. – FNRS) to Manon Marquet (Research Fellow).
during the physical tasks. However, the instructions remained stan-
dardized across participants and conditions, and physical performance References
was objectively assessed, which leads us to consider this explanation
insufficient. Moreover, we actually found expected results for subjective Ambrose, A. F., Paul, G., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2013). Risk factors for falls among older
age, which suggests that the physical functions measured relied too adults: A review of the literature. Maturitas, 75(1), 51–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.maturitas.2013.02.009.
heavily on proceduralized skills and may not be highly impaired in a Avlund, K., Kreiner, S., & Schultz-Larsen, K. (1993). Construct validation and the Rasch
stereotype threat situation, especially among healthy older people. model: Functional ability of healthy elderly people. Scandinavian Journal of Social
Future research should assess the effects of stereotype threat on more Medicine, 21(4), 233–244.
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013a). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older
difficult physical tasks that rely less on proceduralized skills (e.g., adults’ memory. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2522–2529. http://dx.doi.org/10.
balance control in responses to unexpected perturbations). 1177/0956797613497023.
Secondly, given the number of dependent variables included in our Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013b). Stereotype threat can reduce older adults’ memory
errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(10), 1888–1895. http://
analyses, we can not exclude the possibility that significant effects of dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.840656.
threat on subjective age were false positive results. In addition to fo- Barrett, R., Mills, P., & Begg, R. (2010). A systematic review of the effect of ageing and
cusing on more difficult physical tasks, future research should also re- falls history on minimum foot clearance characteristics during level walking. Gait &
Posture, 32(4), 429–435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.07.010.
plicate the effects of threat on subjective age.
Beaudart, C., Reginster, J.-Y., Petermans, J., Gillain, S., Quabron, A., Locquet, M., ...
Thirdly, because the number of strides used in our analyses was too Bruyère, O. (2015). Quality of life and physical components linked to sarcopenia: The
small to consider gait variability, future studies should also investigate SarcoPhAge study. Experimental Gerontology, 69, 103–110. http://dx.doi.org/10.
the effects of stereotype threat on stride-to-stride variability more 1016/j.exger.2015.05.003.
Beilock, S. L., Jellison, W. A., Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Carr, T. H. (2006). On the
deeply. Indeed, it has been shown that variability of gait parameters causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working
may best predict balance disorders and risk of falls in older adults (e.g., memory still be threatened? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8),
Ambrose, Paul, & Hausdorff, 2013; Barrett, Mills, & Begg, 2010). 1059–1071. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288489.
Boutaayamou, M., Schwartz, C., Denoël, V., Forthomme, B., Croisier, J.-L., Garraux, G., ...
Fourthly, the sample size as well as the heterogeneity and large Brüls, O. (2014). Development and validation of a 3D kinematic-based method for
amount of dependent variables collected to assess physical performance determining gait events during overground walking. Paper presented at the
prevented us from using more detailed analyses (e.g., structural equa- International Conference on 3D Imaging (IC3D).
Boutaayamou, M., Schwartz, C., Stamatakis, J., Denoël, V., Maquet, D., Forthomme, B., ...
tion modelling). In particular, we were not able to examine whether the Garraux, G. (2015). Development and validation of an accelerometer-based method
change in physical subjective age was attributable to the instructions for quantifying gait events. Medical Engineering & Physics, 37(2), 226–232. http://dx.
given (high- vs. low-threat) or to the general (or even specific) per- doi.org/10.1016/j.medengphy.2015.01.001.
Chiba, H., Ebihara, S., Tomita, N., Sasaki, H., & Butler, J. P. (2005). Differential gait
formance of older adults in the different physical tasks. Similarly, ac- kinematics between fallers and non-fallers in community-dwelling elderly people.
cording to previous studies, it is also possible that beliefs about one’s Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 5(2), 127–134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.
performance, rather actual performance, have influenced subjective 1447-0594.2005.00281.x.
Coudin, G., & Alexopoulos, T. (2010). ’Help me! I’m old!’ How negative aging stereotypes
age. It has been shown in experimental studies that experiencing sen-
create dependency among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 14(5), 516–523.
sory impairment contributes to a subjective feeling of being older http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607861003713182.
(Eibach et al., 2010). Moreover, Stephan et al. (2013) found that older Cruz-Jentoft, A. J., Baeyens, J. P., Bauer, J. M., Boirie, Y., Cederholm, T., Landi, F., ...
adults feel younger than their age when they are told that they are European Working Group on Sarcopenia in Older People (2010). Sarcopenia:
European consensus on definition and diagnosis: Report of the European Working
stronger than their same-aged peers in a grip strength task. Therefore, Group on Sarcopenia in Older People. Age and Ageing, 39(4), 412–423. http://dx.doi.
relationships between actual and perceived physical performance as org/10.1093/ageing/afq034.
well as subjective age should be investigated in further studies. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal
dimensions of social perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map. In
P. Z. Mark (Vol. Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: Vol. 40, (pp. 61–
5. Conclusion 149). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Eibach, R. P., Mock, S. E., & Courtney, E. A. (2010). Having a “senior moment”: Induced
aging phenomenology, subjective age, and susceptibility to ageist stereotypes.
The present study extended previous research by examining the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 643–649. http://dx.doi.org/10.
effects of stereotype threat on older adults’ physical performance and 1016/j.jesp.2010.03.002.
subjective age. The effects of threat on the different tests and physical Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
performance parameters we used, which can be associated with balance
Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146.
disorders among older adults, were mostly not significant. However, we Fernández-Ballesteros, R., Bustillos, A., & Huici, C. (2015). Positive perception of and
found a stronger effect of threat on subjective age. Participants exposed performance in a memory task: Compensating for stereotype threat? Experimental
Aging Research, 41(4), 410–425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2015.
to negative-age-related information regarding their physical aging felt
1053757.
mentally and physically older compared to participants in a control Fried, L. P., Tangen, C. M., Walston, J., Newman, A. B., Hirsch, C., Gottdiener, J., ...
condition. Since feeling older is associated with different negative Burke, G. (2001). Frailty in older adults evidence for a phenotype. The Journals of
health-related outcomes over time, it is important to consider the im- Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(3), 146–157. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146.
pact of age stereotypes on subjective age in experimental and clinical Geraci, L., De Forrest, R., Hughes, M., Saenz, G., & Tirso, R. (2018). The effect of cognitive
settings. These results also raise the question whether preventative testing and feedback on older adults’ subjective age. Aging, Neuropsychology, and
campaigns aimed at preventing older adults from falling could induce Cognition, 25(3), 335–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1299853.

188
M. Marquet et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 78 (2018) 181–189

Guigoz, Y., Vellas, B., & Garry, P. J. (1996). Assessing the nutritional status of the elderly: Psychology and Aging, 4(1), 73–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.1.73.
The Mini Nutritional Assessment as part of the geriatric evaluation. Nutrition Reviews, Pan, H.-F., Hsu, H.-C., Chang, W.-N., Renn, J.-H., & Wu, H.-W. (2016). Strategies for
54(1), 59–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.1996.tb03793.x. obstacle crossing in older adults with high and low risk of falling. Journal of Physical
Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Pieper, C. F., Leveille, S. G., Markides, K. S., Ostir, G. V., ... Therapy Science, 28(5), 1614–1620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1589/jpts.28.1614.
Wallace, R. B. (2000). Lower extremity function and subsequent disability con- Podsiadlo, D., & Richardson, S. (1991). The timed “Up & Go”: A test of basic functional
sistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared mobility for frail elderly persons. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 39(2),
with the short physical performance battery. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: 142–148.
Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 55(4), 221–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ Popham, L. M., & Hess, T. M. (2015). Age differences in the underlying mechanisms of
gerona/55.4.M221. stereotype threat effects. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences
Hausdorff, J. M., Levy, B. R., & Wei, J. Y. (1999). The power of ageism on physical and Social Sciences, 70(2), 225–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt093.
function of older persons: Reversibility of age-related gait changes. Journal of the Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., & Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age
American Geriatrics Society, 47(11), 1346–1349. differences in memory: Now you see them, now you don’t. Psychology and Aging,
Hollman, J. H., McDade, E. M., & Petersen, R. C. (2011). Normative spatiotemporal gait 16(4), 697–706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.16.4.697.
parameters in older adults. Gait & Posture, 34(1), 111–118. http://dx.doi.org/10. Roberts, H. C., Denison, H. J., Martin, H. J., Patel, H. P., Syddall, H., Cooper, C., ... Sayer,
1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.024. A. A. (2011). A review of the measurement of grip strength in clinical and epide-
Horton, S., Baker, J., Pearce, W., & Deakin, J. M. (2010). Immunity to popular stereotypes miological studies: Towards a standardised approach. Age and Ageing, 40(4),
of aging? Seniors and stereotype threat. Educational Gerontology, 36(5), 353–371. 423–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afr051.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03601270903323976. Rubin, D. C., & Berntsen, D. (2006). People over forty feel 20% younger than their age:
Hughes, M. L., Geraci, L., & De Forrest, R. L. (2013). Aging 5 years in 5 minutes: The effect Subjective age across the lifespan. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 776–780.
of taking a memory test on older adults’ subjective age. Psychological Science, 24(12), Sargent-Cox, K. A., Anstey, K. J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2012). The relationship between change
2481–2488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613494853. in self-perceptions of aging and physical functioning in older adults. Psychology and
Hummert, M. L. (2011). Age stereotypes and aging. In K. W. Schaie, & S. L. Willis (Eds.). Aging, 27(3), 750–760. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027578.
Handbook of the Psychology of Aging (pp. 249–262). (7th edition). San Diego, CA: Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test perfor-
Academic Press. mance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5),
Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., & Jaffe, M. W. (1963). Studies of 797–811. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797.
illness in the aged. The index of ADL: A standardized measure of biological and Stephan, Y., Chalabaev, A., Kotter-Grühn, D., & Jaconelli, A. (2013). Feeling younger,
psychosocial function. JAMA, 185(12), 914–919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama. being stronger”: An experimental study of subjective age and physical functioning
1963.03060120024016. among older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and
Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2015). Views on aging: Domain-specific approaches Social Sciences, 68(1), 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs037.
and implications for developmental regulation. Annual Review of Gerontology and Stephan, Y., Sutin, A. R., Caudroit, J., & Terracciano, A. (2016). Subjective age and
Geriatrics, 35(1), 121–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0198-8794.35.121. changes in memory in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological
Kotov, R., Bellman, S., & Watson, D. (2004). Multidimensional Iowa Suggestibility Scale Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(4), 675–683. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/
(MISS). Retrieved fromhttp://www.hsc.stonybrook.edu/som/psychiatry/docs/ gbv010.
MISSBriefManual.pdf. Stephan, Y., Sutin, A. R., & Terracciano, A. (2015a). “Feeling younger, walking faster”:
Kotter-Grühn, D., Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, A., Gerstorf, D., & Smith, J. (2009). Self-per- Subjective age and walking speed in older adults. Age, 37(5), 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/
ceptions of aging predict mortality and change with approaching death: 16-year 10.1007/s11357-015-9830-9.
longitudinal results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), Stephan, Y., Sutin, A. R., & Terracciano, A. (2015b). How old do you feel? The role of age
654–667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016510. discrimination and biological aging in subjective age. PLoS One, 10(3), e0119293.
Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119293.
stereotype threat: Negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Sturnieks, D., St George, R., & Lord, S. (2008). Balance disorders in the elderly. Clinical
Aging, 30(1), 180–193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038586. Neurophysiology, 38(6), 467–478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2008.09.001.
Lawton, M., & Brody, E. M. (1970). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and Swift, H. J., Lamont, R. A., & Abrams, D. (2012). Are they half as strong as they used to
instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3), 179–186. http://dx.doi. be? An experiment testing whether age-related social comparisons impair older
org/10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179. people’s hand grip strength and persistence. BMJ Open, 2(3), 2. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Levy, B. R. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. 1136/bmjopen-2012-001064 e001064.
The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(4), Tinetti, M. E. (1986). Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly
203–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.4.P203. patients. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 34(2), 119–126.
Levy, B. R. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to aging. Current Tombaugh, T. N., & McIntyre, N. J. (1992). The mini-mental state examination: A com-
Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 332–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j. prehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(9), 922–935. http://
1467-8721.2009.01662.x. dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.x.
Levy, B. R., & Leifheit-Limson, E. (2009). The stereotype-matching effect: Greater influ- Uotinen, V., Rantanen, T., & Suutama, T. (2005). Perceived age as a predictor of old age
ence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes. Psychology and mortality: A 13-year prospective study. Age and Ageing, 34(4), 368–372. http://dx.
Aging, 24(1), 230–233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014563. doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afi091.
Levy, B. R., Pilver, C., Chung, P. H., & Slade, M. D. (2014). Subliminal strengthening Verghese, J., Holtzer, R., Lipton, R. B., & Wang, C. (2009). Quantitative gait markers and
improving older individuals’ physical function over time with an implicit-agester- incident fall risk in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological
eotype intervention. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2127–2135. http://dx.doi.org/10. Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64(8), 896–901. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/gerona/
1177/0956797614551970. glp033.
Marquet, M., Missotten, P., Dardenne, B., & Adam, S. (2017). Interactions between ste- Vol, S., Bedouet, M., Gusto, G., Leglu, C., Beslin, E., Decou, P., ... Lantieri, O. (2011).
reotype threat, subjective aging, and memory in older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, Evaluating physical activity: The AQAP questionnaire and its interpretation software.
and Cognition, 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1413166. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 54(8), 478–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Marquet, M., Missotten, P., Schroyen, S., van Sambeek, I., van den Akker, M., Van Den 1016/j.rehab.2011.09.001.
Broeke, C., ... Adam, S. (2016). A validation of the French version of the Attitudes to Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-
Aging Questionnaire (AAQ): Factor Structure, reliability and validity. Psychologica 36): Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.
Belgica, 56(2), 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.5334/pb.301. Westerhof, G. J., Miche, M., Brothers, A. F., Barrett, A. E., Diehl, M., Montepare, J. M., ...
Michikawa, T., Nishiwaki, Y., Takebayashi, T., & Toyama, Y. (2009). One-leg standing Wurm, S. (2014). The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: A meta-
test for elderly populations. Journal of Orthopaedic Science, 14(5), 675–685. http://dx. analysis of longitudinal data. Psychology and Aging, 29(4), 793–802. http://dx.doi.
doi.org/10.1007/s00776-009-1371-6. org/10.1037/a0038016.
Montepare, J. M. (2009). Subjective age: Toward a guiding lifespan framework. Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., ... Leirer, V. O.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(1), 42–46. http://dx.doi.org/10. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A
1177/0165025408095551. preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–49. http://dx.doi.org/
Montepare, J. M., & Lachman, M. E. (1989). "You’re only as old as you feel": Self-per- 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4.
ceptions of age, fears of aging, and life satisfaction from adolescence to old age.

189

Вам также может понравиться