Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 17

Child Development, September/October 2009, Volume 80, Number 5, Pages 1514–1530

The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate on


Socioemotional and Academic Adjustment: A Comparison of Chinese
and American Adolescents
Yueming Jia Niobe Way
Southeast University New York University

Guangming Ling Hirokazu Yoshikawa


Educational Testing Service (ETS) Harvard University

Xinyin Chen Diane Hughes


University of Western Ontario New York University

Xiaoyan Ke Zuhong Lu
Nanjing Brain Hospital Southeast University

This study explored students’ perceptions of 3 dimensions of school climate (teacher support, student–student
support, and opportunities for autonomy in the classroom) and the associations between these dimensions
and adolescent psychological and academic adjustment in China and the United States. Data were drawn
from 2 studies involving 706 middle school students (M = 12.26) from Nanjing, China, and 709 middle school
students (M = 12.36) from New York City. Findings revealed that students in China perceived higher levels
of teacher support, student–student support, and opportunities for autonomy in the classroom than students
in the United States. Furthermore, students’ perceptions of teacher support and student–student support were
positively associated with adolescents’ self-esteem and grade point average but negatively associated with
depressive symptoms for both Chinese and American adolescents.

Over the past 10 years, there has been a growing (e.g., interpersonal support), affect not only their
body of research on the impact of students’ percep- academic adjustment but also their social and
tions of school climate on their psychological, emotional well-being (Kuperminc, Leadbeater,
social, and academic adjustment. Grounded in eco- Emmons, & Blatt, 1997; Roeser, Eccles, & Sameroff,
logical theories of development, scholars have 2000). Empirical studies focused on such questions
argued that students’ experiences of their school have repeatedly confirmed that students’ percep-
environment, particularly those experiences that tions of various dimensions of school climate (e.g.,
meet the developmental needs of adolescence teacher support) have significant concurrent and
prospective associations with multiple psychologi-
This study was funded by the Cultivation Fund of the Key Sci- cal and academic indicators such as anxiety (Rigby,
entific and Technical Innovation Project (704025); the Program of 2000), depressive symptoms (Loukas, Suzuki, &
Introducing Talents of Discipline to Universities (B08024) of the Horton, 2006), self-esteem (Reddy, Rhodes, & Mul-
Ministry of Education, P. R. China; The Office of Global Pro- hall, 2003; Way, Reddy, & Rhodes, 2007), academic
grams (2005–2006) and the Institute for Human Development
motivation, and grade point average (GPA; Roeser,
and Social Change (2006–2007) at New York University; The Spe-
cial Opportunity Fund (2009) at the Harvard Graduate School of Eccles, & Sameroff, 1998).
Education and the Harvard University China Fund grant (2008– Despite the focus on the influences of context on
2009); and The National Science Foundation Grant to Niobe psychological and academic outcomes, these studies
Way, Hirokazu Yoshikawa, Diane Hughes, and Catherine Tamis- examine primarily one context—schools in the
LeMonda.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Yueming Jia, Southeast University, No. 2 Sipailou, Nanjing,  2009, Copyright the Author(s)
JS 210096, China. Electronic mail may be sent to yueming. Journal Compilation  2009, Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
jia@gmail.com. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2009/8005-0015
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1515

United States (Roeser et al., 1998). Few studies have student support (Colarossi & Eccles, 2003; Pianta,
investigated school climate and its association with Nimetz, & Bennett, 1997; Roeser et al., 1998), stu-
adolescent adjustment in contexts such as China dent–student support (Bachman & O’Malley, 1986;
even though Chinese people represent a signifi- Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Loukas et al., 2006), and
cantly larger proportion of the world’s population opportunities for autonomy in the classroom (Con-
(20%) than do people from the United States (4%). nell & Ilardi, 1987; Kasen, Johnson, & Cohen, 1990;
Arnett (2008) recently called attention to the fact that Way et al., 2007). Such research has focused on ado-
although the United States represents only a small lescents’ perceptions of school climate in particular
proportion of the world’s population, over 98% of with the belief that adolescents’ own experiences of
developmental research is on children from the Uni- school climate will be particularly important in their
ted States. Thus, more research is needed in coun- trajectories of adjustment (Connell & Wellborn,
tries outside of the United States to have a better 1991; Eccles, 1993). Drawing from the school climate
grasp on developmental processes across contexts. literature, student–student support refers to a per-
Even fewer studies have compared the patterns of ceived emotional support among students (i.e., car-
effects between school climate and adjustment ing about and trusting each other; Bachman &
across cultures and nations. The few existing studies O’Malley, 1986; Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Loukas
that do compare across contexts have found varia- et al., 2006), teacher support is typically a combina-
tions across cultures and contexts in the experiences tion of emotional and academic support with a focus
of school climate (Kuperminc et al., 1997; Wentzel, on social support from teachers (Colarossi & Eccles,
1994) and in the links between perceptions of school 2003; Pianta et al., 1997; Reddy et al., 2003; Roeser
climate and adolescent adjustment within the Uni- et al., 1998), and opportunities for autonomy in the
ted States (Crosnoe, Johnson, & Elder, 2004) and classroom refer to the opportunities for choice and
across nations (Samdal, Wold, Klepf, & Kannas, decision making with regard to learning and class-
1998, 2000). room life.
The purpose of this current study is to explore Findings from research with American adoles-
the similarities and differences in students’ percep- cents are consistent with theory, showing perceived
tions of school climate and in the links between school climate (i.e., quality of relations with teachers
these perceptions and self-esteem, depressive and students and opportunities of autonomy) as sig-
symptoms, or GPA across two distinct cultural con- nificantly associated with self-esteem, depressive
texts—urban China and urban United States. The symptoms (e.g., Loukas et al., 2006; Reddy et al.,
analysis draws from two large-scale studies of 2003; Roeser et al., 1998; Way et al., 2007), and aca-
urban middle school students in Nanjing, China, demic adjustment (e.g., Colarossi & Eccles, 2003;
and New York City that aim to understand the Crosnoe et al., 2004; Demaray & Malecki, 2002). For
social, emotional, and cognitive trajectories of example, a study conducted by Crosnoe et al. (2004)
middle school students and the ways in which the of ethnically diverse adolescents found that a caring
family and school contexts shape these trajectories. and warm teacher–student relationship was posi-
tively and significantly associated with a higher GPA
across ethnicities. Colarossi and Eccles’s (2003) study
Perceived School Climate and Adolescent Adjustment
suggested that emotional support from both teachers
Developmental theorists have repeatedly empha- and peers was negatively associated with adoles-
sized that successful development during adoles- cents’ depressive symptoms and positively associ-
cence requires ‘‘trusting and caring relationships ated with self-esteem. Furthermore, Way et al. (2007)
and autonomous self-expression, choice, and deci- found in their longitudinal study that a decline in
sion making’’ (Roeser et al., 2000, p. 459). Students adolescents’ perceived opportunities for autonomy,
who attend schools in which they are not offered teacher support, and student–student support was
opportunities for such relationships or autonomy significantly associated with corresponding increases
will experience a ‘‘mismatch’’ between their devel- in depressive symptoms and decreases in self-esteem
opmental needs and the context in which they are over the course of middle school.
embedded and thus will suffer in terms of psycho- Although the school climate literature has consis-
logical and academic adjustment (Connell & Well- tently indicated that various dimensions of school
born, 1991; Eccles, 1993; Roeser et al., 2000). climate are linked to adjustment, few researchers
Drawing from such theories of development, have examined the relative influence of these
researchers of school climate have focused primarily school climate dimensions on adjustment when
on three components of school climate: teacher– comparing it with one of the most commonly found
1516 Jia et al.

correlate of adolescent adjustment, namely, paren- of an impact on academic and psychological well-
tal emotional support. Researchers have repeatedly being than in the United States, where contact
found that parental emotional support is an impor- between teachers and students is less extensive.
tant predictor of both psychological well-being Therefore, the influence of teacher support relative
(Shek, 1998; Steinberg, Blatt-Eisengart, & Cauffman, to parental support may be more important for
2006) and positive academic outcomes (Chao, 2001; academic and psychological adjustment in China
Fuligni, 1997). To understand the contribution of than in the United States.
experiences of school to students’ adjustment, it is Student–student support as well as opportunities
important to explore how dimensions such as tea- for autonomy in school and the correlates of such
cher support might influence academic or psycho- support may also vary by nationality. Research has
logical adjustment in comparison to parental found that student–student support or friendships
support. among Chinese as well as Chinese American ado-
lescents often consist of doing homework together
(Chen, Chang, & He, 2003; Jia, Way, & Chen, 2008;
Cultural Differences in the Association Between School
Stevenson et al., 1990; Way & Chen, 2000), whereas
Climate and Adjustment
such activities form a smaller part of friendships
Scholars have also argued that teacher support, among non-Chinese American adolescents. This
student–student support, and opportunities for pattern, by itself, suggests that the links between
autonomy in the classroom and the association student–student support and academic adjustment
among these dimensions and adjustment may may be stronger in China than in the United States.
vary by sex and nationality. Perceptions of teach- However, given the stronger emphasis in the Uni-
ers and students and the norms, values, and ted States on nonfamilial friendships during adoles-
expectations related to the construct of autonomy cence than in China (see Way & Chen, 2000),
vary within and across cultures (Noguera, 2003; student–student support may be more important
Wentzel, 1994), and thus the associations between for psychological well-being in the United States
such dimensions of school climate and adjustment than in China. Furthermore, the historically stron-
may also vary by sex and nationality. Girls from ger emphases in the United States on autonomy
the United States, for example, often report receiv- than in China (see Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2007)
ing more teacher and peer support than boys may lead to associations between support for
(Hughes, Cavell, & Willson, 2001; Kuperminc autonomy in the classroom and academic and psy-
et al., 1997). Similarly, European American and chological adjustment being significantly stronger
Asian American students often report receiving for the American than for the Chinese adolescents.
more teacher support than their Black or Latino As many developmental psychologists have sug-
peers (Noguera, 2003; Wentzel, 1994). Researchers gested (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Eccles, 1993;
have also found that the associations between tea- Roeser et al., 2000), autonomy is one of the major
cher support and internalizing problems (e.g., developmental needs during adolescence in the
depression and social withdrawal) are stronger for United States and thus meeting this need may
boys than girls (Kuperminc et al., 1997) and stron- enhance adolescents’ academic and psychological
ger for Black adolescents than White adolescents well-being.
(Ferguson, 2003). In China, teachers are highly Cross-national studies on school climate and
valued by both students and parents and viewed adolescent development are rare. Exceptions
as role models to students (Biggs, 1996; Bond, include two studies conducted by Samdal et al.
1991; Chan & Chan, 2005). Teachers in China also (1998, 2000) comparing the association between
spend a lot of time with their students, as the teacher support and student support and adoles-
school day in middle schools typically extends cents’ school satisfaction in Finland, Norway,
from 8:00 a.m. until 5:00 p.m. with Saturday clas- Latvia, and Slovakia. Their findings suggested that
ses occurring on a regular basis (e.g., Li, 2007; Yu, teacher support had a stronger relation with school
2007). Teachers, furthermore, usually have the satisfaction in Latvia and Slovakia than in Finland
same students from Grades 7 to 9 (Chen, Chen, and Norway; the association between student–stu-
Kaspar, & Noh, 2000). Given such intensive and dent support and school satisfaction, however, was
ongoing contact between teachers and students, stronger in Norway than in Finland. This study
Chinese students may not only report higher lev- underscores the importance of examining the
els of teacher support than American students, impact of perceptions of school climate on adoles-
but also the support they receive may have more cent outcomes across national contexts. Such stud-
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1517

ies further enhance our understanding of the ways Method


in which context makes a difference in child devel-
Participants
opment.
Participants in the China study were 706 stu-
dents (50.5% girls and 49.5% boys) in Grade 7
Current Study
(mean age = 12.26) from three public middle
In response to gaps in the research, we sought schools that were purposively sampled in Nanjing
to examine the following: (a) How do students’ China with the goal of having high-, middle-, and
perceptions of school climate (i.e., teacher sup- low-achieving middle schools in the study. Nanjing
port, student–student support, and opportunities is a medium size city in China with a population of
for autonomy in the classroom) vary by national- approximately 6 million people. Sixty-seven percent
ity (China vs. the United States) and sex? (b) of the mothers and 57.8% of the fathers had an edu-
What are the relative influences of students’ cational level of high school or below. Thirty-three
perceptions of school climate (i.e., teacher sup- percent of the mothers and 42.2% of the fathers had
port, student–student support, opportunities for an educational level of college or above. Data were
autonomy in the classroom) and of parent collected in Nanjing during the fall of 2006. Partici-
support (i.e., mother and father support) on psy- pants in the U.S. study were 709 (51.8% girl and
chological (i.e., self-esteem and depressive symp- 48.2% boy) students in Grade 7 drawn from five
toms) and academic adjustment (i.e., GPA)? (c) public middle schools that were purposively cho-
How do nationality (China vs. the United States) sen in New York City based on their ethnic compo-
and sex moderate the associations between per- sition and variation in aggregate math–language
ceptions of school climate and psychological and achievement test score data to represent diverse
academic adjustment? Before addressing these ethnic groups and different levels of academic
questions, we conducted a measurement equiva- achievement in the study. New York City is a simi-
lence analysis using structural equation modeling lar size city as Nanjing with a population of
(SEM) to determine if the measures we selected approximately 7 million people. Students were 21%
had similar psychometric properties across African American, 29.9% Latino, 18.5% Chinese
national contexts. American, and 30.6% White. The mean age of chil-
Based on previous research, we hypothesized dren was 12.36 years (SD = .56). Sixteen percent of
that girls would report more positive perceptions of the mothers and 16.8% of the fathers had an educa-
teacher support and student–student support than tional level of high school or below high school,
boys and Chinese adolescents would report higher 8.4% of the mothers and 6.0% of the fathers went to
levels of teacher support than American adoles- a vocational school or took some college courses,
cents. We also hypothesized that positive percep- and 49.8% of the mothers and 41.6 of the fathers
tions of each dimension of school climate would be had an educational level of college or above college.
positively associated with self-esteem and GPA, Twenty-six percent of the mothers’ and 36% of the
and negatively associated with depressive symp- fathers’ educational levels were unknown. Data
toms. We expected, however, that teacher support were collected in New York City from two cohorts
would have a stronger association with self-esteem, of seventh graders: One was collected in the spring
depressive symptoms and GPA for Chinese adoles- of 2005 and one was collected in the spring of 2006.
cents when compared to American adolescents. We The U.S sample consisted of students surveyed
also expected that student–student support would each year in sixth, seventh, and eighth grades. For
have a stronger association with the GPA of this study, we used only the seventh-grade data to
Chinese adolescents than of American adolescents. match the Nanjing sample. The sampling of this
Student–student support, however, was expected to study aimed to capture the existing variation
have a stronger association with self-esteem and among the local community, reflecting relevant
depressive symptoms of American adolescents than dimensions within each of the two cities.
of Chinese adolescents. Finally, we hypothesized
that teacher support in China and the United States
Procedure
would have a stronger association with GPA than
parental support, whereas parental support in For both studies, we administered a 40-min
China and the United States would have a stronger survey to the students during classroom time.
association with self-esteem and depressive In addition, data concerning students’ GPA
symptoms. were obtained from school records. Measures were
1518 Jia et al.

translated and back-translated to ensure compara- intimacy, affection, and satisfaction were also com-
bility with the original English versions. The puted so they could be used as indicators of father
administration of all measures was carried out by a support or mother support in SEM analyses. The
group of graduate students at the Southeast Uni- NRI has been used repeatedly with ethnically
versity in Nanjing, China, and the New York Uni- diverse populations, in Chinese and American
versity in New York City. contexts, and has yielded good reliability (e.g.,
Buhrmester, 1990; Chen et al., 2000; Way & Chen,
2000). In the current sample, the measure demon-
Measures
strated good reliability: a = .90 for father support
Perceived school climate. The school climate mea- and a = .90 for mother support in the Chinese
sure used in this study is a revised 25-item version sample; a = .89 for father support and a = .88 for
of two school climate measures (Brand, Felner, mother support in the U.S. sample.
Shim, Seitsinger, & Dumas, 2003; Emmons, Haynes, Depressive symptoms. Depressive symptoms were
& Comer, 2002). The combined measure assesses indexed with a shortened version of the Children’s
three dimensions of school climate–teacher support Depressive Inventory (Kovacs, 1980). The 10-item
(e.g., ‘‘teachers believe I can do well’’) with a focus self-report scale measures cognitive, affective, and
on emotional support and one item related to aca- behavioral symptoms of depression. Students were
demic support, student–student support (e.g., ‘‘stu- asked to rate each item on a 3-point scale ranging
dents care about one another’’), and opportunities from 0 (never) to 2 (always), indicating how often
for autonomy in the classroom (e.g., ‘‘students are she or he had the certain symptom. The 10 items
given the chance to help make decisions’’). All were averaged to yield a single aggregated score.
items have a 4-point response scale (1 = never, Lower scores indicated lower levels of reported
4 = always). The negative keyed items were reverse- depressive symptoms. The Children’s Depression
coded, and the mean of each subscale was taken, Inventory has been used with both Chinese and
with higher values indicating higher levels of sup- American populations and has yielded good reli-
port or opportunities for autonomy in the class- ability (Chen & Li, 2000; Dong, Wang, & Ollendick,
room. Good reliability and validity have been 2002; Kovacs, 1980). Reliability for the current
reported for both measures in the United States sample was a = .79 for the Chinese sample and
(Brand et al., 2003; Kuperminc et al., 1997; Way & a = .86 for the U.S. sample.
Robinson, 2003). For the current study, the measure Self-esteem. The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem
demonstrated acceptable reliability for each sub- Inventory (Rosenberg, 1979) was used to measure
scale. Teacher support (seven items): a = .81 for the students’ self-esteem. Students were asked to rate
Chinese sample and a = .84 for the U.S. sample; each item on a 4-point scale ranging from 1
peer support (13 items): a = .86 for the Chinese (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), indicating
sample and a = .82 for the U.S. sample; opportuni- how well each of these items described himself or
ties for autonomy in the classroom (five items): herself. One item was deleted as its item–total
a = .69 for the Chinese sample and a = .70 for the correlation was only .18, and the item-deleted reli-
U.S. sample. ability of the scale was greater with this item
Parental support. The Network of Relationships deleted. The negatively keyed items were reverse-
Inventory (NRI; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985) was coded, and a mean score was calculated. Higher
used to assess students’ perception of the quality of scores indicated a higher level of self-esteem. The
support from their mothers and fathers separately. Rosenberg scale has been used with Chinese and
The 12 items in this measure assess four positive American populations and has yielded good reli-
dimensions of parental emotional support (i.e., ability (Cheung, 1986; Cheung & Tam, 1984; Shek,
companionship, intimacy, affection, satisfaction). 1992). Reliability for the current sample was a = .82
There are 3 items tapping each dimension. Students for the Chinese sample and a = .86 for the U.S.
were asked to rate each item on a 5-point scale sample.
(1 = never, 5 = always), indicating the frequency of GPA. Information concerning GPA was
occurence between father or mother and child (e.g., obtained from school records in both cohorts.
‘‘how much do you play around or have fun with Two subjects—Chinese–English and Mathematics
your mom’’). The four dimensions were combined in a total of four exams’ scores (two midterms
to form a mean score representing father or mother and two finals)—were obtained from the schools
support, with higher values indicating high levels in China and the United States. These scores
of support. Separate means for companionship, were all on a 0–100 scale, with 60 indicating a
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1519

pass. An average score over these four scores


was obtained for each individual. The means of
this averaged score were 77.03 (SD = 11.20) for
the Chinese sample and 85.06 (SD = 10.63) for
the U.S. sample. The averaged scores were later
coded, with 1 = 0–59, 2 = 60–69, 3 = 70–79,
4 = 80–89, 5 = 90–100, and used as the indicator
of a student’s school grades. The GPA scores in
the China sample were based on city or
districtwide standardized tests, which are com- Figure 1. Conceptual path model to adolescent outcomes
monly used in midterm and final exams in (models involving depressive symptoms, self-esteem, and
academic achievement will be assessed separately).
China. The scores in the U.S. sample were based
on tests designed and rated by local teachers,
which are commonly used in midterm and final
values were estimated using the expectation-maxi-
exams in American schools. The data on GPA
mization algorithm (MaLachlan & Krishnan, 1997).
were collected mainly to compare how multiple
Approximately 10%–16% of each measure had
dimensions of school climate are associated with
missing values; the values were imputed in the two
adolescents’ psychological and academic out-
samples separately.
comes within each group instead of comparing
students’ GPA across the two national groups.

Results
Analytic Strategy
Measurement Equivalence
The goal of this study was to compare percep-
tions of school climate by nationality and sex and To test the factorial invariance of measures
the association of perceptions of school climate and across the two countries, a two-group model in
parental support with psychological and academic which the factor loadings of each construct were
adjustment in Chinese and American adolescents. constrained to be the same between the Chinese
To address these questions, the following analyses and the U.S. samples was compared to a fully
were conducted. First, measurement invariance estimated model in which all factor loadings were
across samples was examined with each measure allowed to vary across samples. A nonsignificant
using SEM to evaluate whether the same measures change of the fit across the constrained and
(in English or Chinese) tap the central constructs in unconstrained models indicated full measurement
comparable ways for adolescents from the two invariance. If the initial model of full measure-
countries (Knight & Hill, 1998). Second, descriptive ment invariance was rejected, further analysis
statistics (mean and standard deviations), and anal- were conducted to examine whether partial facto-
yses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted and rial invariance of measures for at least half the
compared for the perceived school climate mea- items per construct ⁄ factor (Byrne, Shavelson, &
sure, by nationality and sex. Finally, several sets of Muthén, 1989) across the two countries might
two-group structural models (see Figure 1) and exist, allowing for cross-group comparisons. The
their nested models were analyzed and compared partial measurement invariance test consisted of a
to examine the association of school climate and two-step analysis. First, the factor loading of each
parental supports with adolescents’ adjustment item or indicator was constrained to be the same
across the two groups. across groups one by one and was compared to
All analyses were performed with LISREL 8.70 the unconstrained model by which we would
(Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004). The SEM models were identify the items or indicators that were respon-
evaluated based on four criteria: the chi-square test, sible for factor variance. Then, we compared the
the root mean square error of approximation partially constrained model (partial metric model)
(RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the in which the factorial constraints on the identified
non-normed fit index (NNFI, also know as the items or indicators were lifted from the fully con-
Tucker–Lewis index). For the comparison between strained model to the unconstrained model. A
the hypothetical model and a more constrained nonsignificant change of the fit across the partially
model, the chi-square difference was used as the constrained model and unconstrained model indi-
criterion (Raykov & Marcoulides, 2000). Missing cated partial measurement invariance.
1520 Jia et al.

Five sets of two-group confirmatory factor analy- support, in which the loading parameters of all
sis (CFA) models were examined to separately test indicators except two were constrained to be the
the factorial invariance for measures of school cli- same across the two groups, did not signifi-
mate, depressive symptoms, self-esteem, mother cantly differ from the unconstrained model, Dv2(1,
support, and father support. For the measure of NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 0.77, p > .05,
school climate, including teacher support, student– again suggesting partial measurement invariance
student support, and opportunities for autonomy, (Table 1).
both unconstrained and fully constrained models
had an acceptable fit to the data, CFIs = .94,
Perceived Teacher Support, Student–Student Support,
NNFIs = .93, RMSEAs < .08, but the fit of the con-
and Opportunities for Autonomy in the Classroom:
strained model significantly differed from that of
Variation Across Nationality and Sex
the unconstrained model. The hypothesis of full
measurement invariance for these measures was The previous tests indicated the appropriateness
rejected. Data were further analyzed and four items of comparison across our Chinese and American
(one in teacher support measure; three in student– samples at the relational level. To compare latent
student support measure) were identified to be means, however, requires partial scalar invariance,
responsible for the factorial variance. Then, the fac- that is, that the vector of intercepts for at least two
torial constraints on the four items were lifted from of the items in each construct is invariant across
the fully constrained model. The fit of this partially groups (Steenkamp & Baumgartner, 1998; Vanden-
constrained model (partial metric model) did not berg & Lance, 2000). Since conducting comparison
significantly differ from the unconstrained model, of latent mean scores on perceptions of school cli-
Dv2(18, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 25.81, mate across nationality was one of the major goals
p > .05, indicating partial measurement invariance. in this study, partial scalar invariance test was fur-
The same analytic process was used for the facto- ther examined with the measures of school climate.
rial invariance tests of other measures. The hypothe- Additional restrictions were added to the partially
sis of full measurement invariance was rejected for constrained model (partial metric model) in which
all the rest of measures, but partial measurement the intercepts of 11 items (3 for teacher support, 3
invariance was retained. For depressive symptoms, for opportunities for autonomy, and 5 for student–
the fit of the partially constrained model, in which student support) were constrained to be equal
the loading parameters of all items except four were across the two samples. The partial scalar
constrained to be the same across the two groups, did invariance model did not significantly differ
not significantly differ from the unconstrained from partial metric invariance model, Dv2(6,
model, Dv2(5, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 11.12, p > .05,
= 9.02, p > .05, suggesting partial measurement suggesting partial scalar invariance. This suggested
invariance existed. For the measure of self-esteem, that the comparison of means for school climate
the fit of the partially constrained model, in which measures would be meaningful.
the loading parameters of all items except one were ANOVA was conducted to examine adolescents’
constrained to be the same across the two groups, did perceptions of teacher support, peer support and
not significantly differ from that of the unconstrained opportunities for autonomy in the classroom
model, Dv2(7, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = among Chinese and American middle school
14.04, p > .05, and thus indicated partial measure- students. The results (Table 2) revealed that the
ment invariance. Chinese students reported significantly higher lev-
As for the factorial invariance tests of the mother els of teacher support, student–student support,
support and father support measures, the four sub- and opportunities for autonomy in the classroom
scale scores (i.e., the mean of the items in the sub- than the American students. The American
scale) were used in the structural analyses. The fit students reported significantly higher mother sup-
of the partially constrained model for measure of port and self-esteem and lower levels of depressive
mother support, in which the loading parameters symptoms than the Chinese students; however, the
of all indicators except two were constrained to be magnitudes of differences in self-esteem and
the same across the two countries, did not signifi- depressive symptoms were small between the two
cantly differ from the unconstrained model, Dv2(1, groups. There were no significant sex differences
NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 0.17, p > .05, on perceptions of all the three components of
and thus indicated partial measurement invariance. school climate among the Chinese adolescents,
The fit of the partially constrained model for father whereas among the U.S. sample girls reported
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1521

Table 1
Summary of Partial Factorial Invariance CFA Models

China United States

Unstandardized Unstandardized
Measure and variable factor loading SE factor loading SE

Teacher support
I can talk to my teachers about my problems 1.00a — 1.00a —
My teachers care about me 1.66 0.10 1.66 0.10
Teachers help students with problems 1.44 0.09 1.44 0.09
Teachers help students with school problems 1.47 0.10 1.47 0.09
Teachers believe I can do well 1.61 0.11 1.20 0.11
Teachers work hard to get me to do well on tests 1.31 0.09 1.31 0.09
My teachers make me feel good about myself 1.56 0.10 1.56 0.10
Opportunities for autonomy
Students get to help decide some of the rules 1.00a — 1.00a —
Students help decide how class time is spent 0.94 0.07 0.94 0.07
Students are given the chance to help make decisions 1.08 0.07 1.08 0.07
Teachers ask students what they want to learn about 0.89 0.07 0.89 0.07
Students have a say in how things work 0.96 0.07 0.96 0.07
Student–student support
Students call each other bad namesb 1.00 — 1.00 —
Students are mean to each otherb 1.11 0.08 1.11 0.08
Students respect one another 1.91 0.16 1.91 0.16
Students help one another 1.65 0.14 1.65 0.14
Someone has beaten me up or hurt meb 0.54 0.08 0.54 0.08
Students like one another 1.96 0.17 1.36 0.14
There are students who pick on other studentsb 1.07 0.11 1.07 0.11
Students are caring people 1.86 0.16 1.86 0.16
Students have trouble getting along with each other b 0.88 0.09 0.88 0.09
I have been afraid that someone 0.54 0.08 0.54 0.08
will hurt or bother me in school b
Students behave well 1.62 0.14 1.62 0.14
Students fight a lot 1.00 0.11 1.46 0.16
Students trust one another 1.95 0.17 1.63 0.15
Depressive symptoms
I am sad once in a whileb 1.00a — 1.00a —
Nothing ever works out for me 1.05 0.06 1.05 0.06
I do most things O.K. 1.00 0.08 0.72 0.05
I hate myselfb 1.19 0.06 1.19 0.06
I feel like crying everyday 0.97 0.08 1.15 0.05
I look O.K.b 0.85 0.06 0.85 0.06
I do not feel alone 1.28 0.09 1.04 0.06
I have plenty of friends 0.67 0.05 0.67 0.05
Nobody really loves me 0.89 0.06 0.89 0.06
Things bother me all the time 1.52 0.10 1.29 0.08
Self-esteem
I am satisfied with myself 1.00a — 1.00a —
At times, I think I am no good at allb 0.95 0.06 0.95 0.06
I feel I have many good qualities 1.29 0.08 0.95 0.05
I am able to do things as well as most other people 0.99 0.05 0.99 0.05
I feel I do not have much to be proud of 1.02 0.06 1.02 0.06
I certainly feel useless at timesb 0.89 0.06 0.89 0.06
I feel that I am as good as others 1.15 0.06 1.15 0.06
I feel that I am a failureb 0.96 0.05 0.96 0.05
I take a positive attitude towards myself 1.03 0.05 1.03 0.05
1522 Jia et al.

Table 1
(Continued)

China United States

Unstandardized Unstandardized
Measure and variable factor loading SE factor loading SE

Mother support
Companionshipb 1.00a — 1.00a —
Intimacyb 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04
Satisfaction 1.54 0.09 1.08 0.06
Affection 1.12 0.07 0.67 0.04
Father support
Companionshipb 1.00a — 1.00a —
Intimacyb 0.63 0.04 0.63 0.04
Satisfactionb 1.45 0.09 1.19 0.06
Affection 1.08 0.06 0.70 0.04

Note: CFA = confirmatory factor analysis.


a
For each construct, factor loading of one indicator was conventionally fixed to unity for model identification (see Raykov &
Marcoulides, 2000). All factor loadings significantly at p < .05. bErrors of this indicator were allowed to correlate with the errors of
other indicators according to modification indexes.

significant higher student–student support (F = only. Both teacher support and student–student
6.67, p < .05) than boys. Within the American support were positively associated with GPA, but
sample, White students reported higher teacher opportunities for autonomy in the classroom were
and student–student support than Chinese Ameri- negatively associated with GPA for the American
can and African American students. adolescents. For the Chinese adolescents, only stu-
dent–student support among the three dimensions of
school climate was significantly and positively asso-
Associations Among Student Perceptions of School
ciated with GPA. Both father and mother support
Climate, Parental Support, and Adjustment
were significantly and positively associated with
Correlation analysis of all study variables (Table 3) self-esteem and negatively associated with depres-
found that teacher support and student–student sup- sive symptoms for the Chinese and the American
port were significantly associated with lower depres- adolescents; father support and mother support were
sive symptoms and higher levels of self-esteem in also significantly associated with GPA for the Ameri-
both cohorts. Opportunities for autonomy in the can adolescents, but the correlations were small.
classroom were significantly and positively associ- Only father support had a significant and positive
ated with self-esteem and negatively associated with association with GPA for the Chinese adolescents
depressive symptoms for the Chinese adolescents but, once again, the correlation was small.

Table 2
Mean Difference Between Chinese (N = 706) and the United States (N = 709) on All Variables

Chinese United States

Source M SD M SD F

Teacher support 2.98 0.59 2.86 0.62 13.91**


Student–student support 3.03 0.53 2.68 0.43 192.40**
Opportunities for autonomy 2.38 0.65 1.89 0.53 245.61**
Father support 3.52 0.73 3.59 0.72 2.81
Mother support 3.62 0.73 3.87 0.67 44.34**
Depressive symptoms 0.31 0.31 0.28 0.35 4.5*
Self-esteem 3.15 0.54 3.22 0.55 5.44*

*p < .05. **p < .01.


The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1523

Table 3
Correlations Among All Studied Variables for Adolescents in China (N = 706) and in the United States (N = 709)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Sex — ).01 ).01 .06 .08* ).03 ).04 .03 ).12**


2. Teacher support ).08* — .48** .56** .27** .28** ).34** .33** .06
3. Student–student support ).11** .37** — .36** .22** .22** ).41** .26** .12**
4. Opportunities for autonomy .04 .32** .06 — .22** .17** ).23** .20** ).07
5. Father support .07 .29** .23** .03 — .47** ).30** .28** .08*
6. Mother support ).12** .37** .24** .09* .48** — ).34** .27** .04
7. Depression symptom ).04 ).25** ).28** ).07 ).28** ).28** — ).61** ).10**
8. Rosenberg self-esteem ).02 .33** .27** .036 .27** .29** ).67** — .18**
9. Grade point average ).13** .11** .19** ).16** .07* .09* ).05 .13** —

Note. Results for the Chinese adolescents are presented above the diagonal and for the American adolescents are below the diagonal.
*p < .05 (two-tailed). **p < .01 (two-tailed).

Pathway to depressive symptoms. The baseline To compare the relation of student–student sup-
model (Figure 1) with paths from all dimensions of port, teacher support, mother support, and father
school climate and parental support to depressive support to depressive symptoms, additional sets of
symptoms was tested with Chinese and American structural model analysis were conducted. Equality
adolescents together without any cross-group con- restrictions on the paths from teacher support,
straints, and then equal cross-group path con- father support, and mother support to depressive
straints were introduced to the baseline model. symptoms were introduced to the structural model
Both the unconstrained and constrained models fit tested earlier. Adding these restrictions did not
the data well, CFIs = 0.91, NNFIs = 0.90, significantly worsen the fit, Dv2(2) = 2.76, p > .05,
RMSEAs = 0.06, and the fit of the equal constrained indicating teacher support, father support, and
model did not significantly differ from the baseline mother support did not have significantly different
model Dv2(5, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) association with depressive symptoms. Then,
= 0.82, p > .05, indicating that the model fit the data another equality restriction on the path from
equally across the two groups. Trimming the non- student–student support to depressive symptoms
significant path (opportunity for autonomy to was introduced to this model. Adding this
depressive symptoms) in equal constrained model path restriction significantly worsened the fit,
also did not worsen the fit, Dv2(1, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ Dv2(1) = 27.31, p < .05, suggesting that student–stu-
NNew York City = 709) = 0.89, p > .05. This analysis dent support did not have an equal association
showed that for Chinese and American adolescents, with depressive symptoms relative to the other fac-
the path model (Figure 2) on depressive symptoms tors. This analysis indicated that student–student
was the same. Student–student support, teacher support (b = ).24) was the strongest correlate of
support, father support, and mother support were depressive symptoms.
significantly and negatively associated with depres- Pathway to self-esteem. The baseline model
sive symptoms, whereas opportunities for auton- (Figure 1) with paths from all dimensions of school
omy in the classroom had no unique effect on climate and parental support to self-esteem was
depressive symptoms. A set of two-group analyses tested with the Chinese and American adolescents
were also conducted with boys and girls. The groups together without any cross-group con-
results showed that this model fit the data equally straints, and then equal path constraints were
across sex. introduced to the baseline model. Both the uncon-
strained and constrained models fit the data
well, CFIs = 0.93, NNFIs = 0.93, RMSEAs = 0.06,
and the fit of the equal constrained model did not
significantly differ from the baseline model Dv2(5,
NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 3.30, p > .05,
indicating that the model fit the data equally across
Figure 2. Path model to depressive symptoms among Chinese and
the two groups. One nonsignificant path (opportu-
American adolescents; standardized coefficients in parentheses; nities for autonomy to self-esteem) was then
unstandardized coefficients outside parentheses. trimmed. Trimming the nonsignificant path did not
1524 Jia et al.

worsen the fit, Dv2(2, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = nificant paths (father support and mother support
709) = 1.18, p > .05. This analysis showed that for to GPA) were trimmed one by one. Trimming these
both the Chinese and the American adolescents, the paths also did not worsen the fit, Dv2(2, NNanj-
path model to self-esteem (Figure 3) was the same. ing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709) = 2.05, p > .05. This
Teacher support, student–student support, mother analysis showed that for the Chinese and the Ameri-
support, and father support each had a positive can adolescents, the path model to GPA (Figure 4)
association with self-esteem whereas opportunities was the same. Student–student support and teacher
for autonomy had no unique effect. A set of two- support had a positive and significant association
group analyses were also conducted with boys and with GPA; opportunities for autonomy in the class-
girls. The results showed that this model fit the room had a negative association with GPA whereas
data equally across sex. neither mother support nor father support were
Additional sets of path model analysis were con- uniquely related to GPA. A set of two-group analyses
ducted to compare the effects of student–student were also conducted with boys and girls. The results
support, teacher support, mother support and showed that this model fit the data equally across
father support on self-esteem. Equality restrictions sex.
on the paths leaving student–student support, Additional sets of structural model analyses
father support, and mother support and ending at were conducted to compare the relation of student–
self-esteem were introduced to the model tested student support and teacher support to GPA. The
earlier. Adding these restrictions did not signifi- results indicated that the association between
cantly worsen the fit, Dv2(2) = 1.32, p > .05, indicat- student–student support and GPA did not signifi-
ing that student–student support, father support, cantly differ from that of teacher support and GPA,
and mother support did not have significantly dif- Dv2(1) = 2.71, p > .05.
ferent association with self-esteem. Then another
equality restriction on the path from student–
student support to self-esteem was introduced.
Discussion
Adding this path restriction significantly worsen
the fit, Dv2(1) = 31.49, p < .05, suggesting that tea- The goal of our project was to explore students’
cher support did not have an equal association with perceptions of three dimensions of school climate
depression symptoms relative to the other factors. and the associations between these dimensions and
This analysis indicated that teacher support self-esteem, depressive symptoms, and GPA across
(b = .28) was the strongest correlate of self-esteem. two distinct cultural contexts. Our findings indicate
Pathways to GPA. The baseline model (Figure 1) striking similarities and differences across Chinese
with paths from all dimensions of school climate and American urban contexts. First, students’
and parental support to GPA was tested with Chi- perceptions of teacher support, student–student
nese and American adolescents together without support, and support for autonomy in the class-
any cross-group constraints, and then equal path room were higher in the Chinese sample than in
constraints were introduced to the baseline model. the American sample. The higher level of perceived
Both the unconstrained and constrained model teacher support among the Chinese students is
fit the data well, CFIs = 0.92, NNFIs = 0.91, likely due to the high status of teachers as research
RMSEAs = 0.07, and the fit of the equal constrained has suggested that those in higher status are usu-
model did not significantly differ from the baseline ally perceived more positively by others (Way, San-
model Dv2(5, NNanjing = 706 ⁄ NNew York City = 709)
= 6.03, p > .05, indicating that the model fit the data
equally across the two nationality groups. Nonsig-

Teacher Mother
Support Support
.34 .11
(.28) Self (.15)
Esteem
.06
Student-Student .11 (.10) Father
Support (.07) Support

Figure 3. Path model to self-esteem among Chinese and Figure 4. Path model to grade point average among Chinese and
American adolescents standardized coefficients in parentheses; American adolescents; standardized coefficients in parentheses;
unstandardized coefficients outside parentheses. unstandardized coefficients outside parentheses.
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1525

tos, Niwa, & Kim-Gervey, 2008). Therefore, the 2004) suggest that there are numerous opportuni-
high status of teachers in China might lead Chinese ties for students to participate in the decision-mak-
students to be less critical of teachers as well as the ing process in Chinese classrooms. For example, in
teachers to be more supportive of their students. In middle schools in Nanjing as well as in middle
addition, Chinese students spend significantly schools across China, each class has a committee of
longer periods of time with their teachers than do student leaders elected by the students themselves.
the American students in school and often visit This committee is responsible for assisting the
their teachers at home for additional tutoring dur- teachers with classroom management and activities
ing the weekends and holidays. Furthermore, and taking charge of classwide affairs, such as man-
unlike the U.S. schools, Chinese schools often aging school tasks and learning groups, organizing
arrange overnight trips for the students and their various sport matches, or contests across classes.
teachers during the holidays throughout the school They are also responsible for monitoring students’
year. The additional time spent with teachers may behaviors in the classroom. Furthermore, there are
enhance Chinese students’ perceptions of the sup- weekly or biweekly class meetings in which stu-
port that their teachers provide. dents present and discuss their ideas and opinions
The Chinese adolescents also perceived more about school or class regulations or arrangements
student–student support than the American adoles- and make decision together regarding class activi-
cents. Given the extensive amount of time Chinese ties. American middle school students, especially
students spend with their teachers and with other those attending public middle schools, rarely have
students, the higher levels of both teacher–student such opportunities in the classroom.
and student–student support perceived by the Chi- Our data also suggested that the American
nese students were not surprising, as research has students perceived more emotional support from
shown that more time spent with peers and friends their mothers than the Chinese students. This result
increases feelings of support (Hartup, 1992). In is consistent with previous studies finding that Chi-
addition, Chinese students have many opportuni- nese mothers show less warmth in their parenting
ties to interact with their classmates during the practice in comparison to their Western counter-
school day. For example, after each 40-min class parts (Leung, Lau, & Lam, 1998; Wu et al., 2002). It
during the day, middle school students have a may be, however, that our measure of ‘‘mother
10-min break to play on the school playground or support’’ was a good assessment of the ways in
talk with their classmates in the classroom. They which the American students experience support
also have one or two periods during the week from their mothers but not the Chinese students.
called ‘‘free-play class’’ during which students Future studies needs to use qualitative methods to
choose which activities they want to do with their investigate how Chinese adolescents experience
classmates without the teachers’ presence in the support from their mothers to determine whether
classroom (the classroom monitor, who is an our finding reflects a limitation of our measure of
elected student from the class, oversees the class- support or a difference in levels of support from
room). American middle school students are typi- mothers across cultural contexts. The American
cally expected to move quickly from class to class students also reported slightly higher self-esteem
during the day and rarely, if ever, get a chance to and lower depressive symptoms than Chinese stu-
socialize with their peers during the day with the dents, which parallels other researchers’ findings
exception of lunch periods. American middle (Chan, 1995; Weine, Phillips, & Achenbach, 1995).
school students, furthermore, rarely have opportu- Given that similar findings have been noted in
nities to go on overnight trips with their classmates numerous studies of Chinese and Chinese Ameri-
during the school year. can students using different measures and using
The Chinese students also reported higher levels qualitative methods (Way et al., 2008), it is likely
of opportunities for autonomy in the classroom that this finding is reflective of a genuine difference
than the American students. Given the extensive across cultural contexts rather than a bias in the
cross-cultural work suggesting that the American measurement of self-esteem or depressive symp-
context emphasizes autonomy more than the Chi- toms. Finally, data from the American sample
nese context (Bond, 1996; Chao & Tseng, 2002), this showing that White students reported higher level
finding was surprising. However, our qualitative of teacher and student–student support than
observation of classrooms in middle schools in Chinese American and African American students
Nanjing as well as informal interviews with stu- also replicated findings from previous research
dents and teachers from other Chinese cities (Fong, (Wentzel, 1994) indicating that, as predicted, White
1526 Jia et al.

students feel more supported in schools in the When compared with parental support and tea-
United States than students of color. cher support, student–student support was the
Regarding the associations between dimensions strongest correlate of adolescents’ depressive symp-
of school climate and student psychological adjust- toms for girls and boys and for Chinese and
ment, our data indicated that teacher support, stu- American adolescents. Research has consistently
dent–student support, and parental support were suggested that peer rejection is a particular strong
each significantly associated with adolescents’ correlate of depressive symptoms during adoles-
depressive symptoms and self-esteem for girls and cence (Morrow, Hubbard, McAuliffe, Rubin, &
boys and for Chinese and American students. Dearing, 2006; Pedersen, Vitaro, Barker, & Borge,
These findings confirm developmental theories that 2007; Prinstein & Aikins, 2004). The striking aspect
suggest that emotional support from both teachers of this finding is the lack of nationality or sex dif-
and students are critical for the psychological well- ferences in the association between peer support
being of adolescents (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; and depressive symptoms, drawing attention to the
Eccles, 1993; Roeser et al., 2000). Although we have importance of peer support (or the negative influ-
known about the significance of such associations ence of peer rejection) in both the American and
for students in the United States for over a decade Chinese contexts and for both boys and girls.
(Henry & Slater, 2007; Kasen et al., 1990; Loukas When we examined the association between per-
et al., 2006; Pianta & Allen, 2008; Roeser et al., 1998; ceptions of school climate and parent support and
Way et al., 2007), we have had little empirical GPA, we found that father support and mother
evidence that such patterns exist outside of the support were not significantly associated with
American context. The current study is the first to GPA. All three dimensions of school climate, on
suggest that such patterns exist in both the U.S. the other hand, were linked to GPA though not
and Chinese context. Examining the processes that necessarily in the expected direction. Teacher sup-
explain the links among teacher, student, and par- port was linked to higher GPA for girls and boys
ent support and psychological adjustment across and for Chinese and American students. This find-
contexts as well as exploring such effects longitudi- ing was not surprising by itself given the extensive
nally is an important next step toward enhancing research on teacher support that has consistently
our understanding of the ways in which contexts found such a pattern among adolescents (Brand
shape trajectories of adjustment. et al., 2003; Eccles, 1993; Kasen et al., 1990; Kuper-
Our data also indicate that when compared to minc et al., 1997; Way et al., 2007). However, peer
parental and peer support, teacher support was a support had the same magnitude of association as
stronger predictor of self-esteem for boys and girls teacher support with GPA in both the Chinese and
and Chinese and American students. This finding American samples. For Chinese adolescents, the
was surprising given the heavy emphasis on the salience of peer support on GPA is consistent with
importance of peer support among the American qualitative data that suggest that Chinese and Chi-
adolescents, particularly female adolescents. We nese American students spend a lot of their time
expected that teacher support would have a greater with friends doing homework (Chen et al., 2000,
influence on self-esteem among the Chinese and 2003). Thus, ‘‘peer support’’ in China may reflect
male students than among the American and the extent to which they receive help from their
female students. These findings suggest that the peers on their homework which could enhance
meaning of teacher support, or its correlates, is not their GPA. Furthermore, those who are high aca-
necessarily dependent on the quantity of time spent demic achievers may receive a lot of peer support
with teachers. Although the Chinese students for their academic accomplishments. In other
spend more time each day with their teachers and words, the association between peer support and
report higher levels of teacher support than the academic success for Chinese students may be
American students, high levels of teacher support bidirectional—high achievers receive a lot of peer
are associated with high levels of self-esteem for support and high levels of peer support fosters
both Chinese and American students. This finding high academic achievement. The processes that
points, once again, to the concept of ‘‘universalism explain the link between peer support and GPA
without uniformity’’ (Shweder & Sullivan, 1993, p. for non-Chinese American students in the United
514) or the possibility of strong cultural similarities States are less clear. It may be that, similar to
in the ways in which support gets interpreted (or China, high academic achievers in the United
self-esteem gets interpreted) but not cultural simi- States feel strongly supported by their peers in
larities in the levels of support across contexts. school. Longitudinal analysis is necessary to
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1527

determine the direction of effect and the ways in to the larger American or Chinese population.
which peer support may be associated with aca- Both studies were conducted in urban locations. It
demic achievement. may be that the associations between dimensions
Unexpectedly, opportunities for autonomy in the of school climate and adjustment may vary by
classroom were associated with lower GPA among region or social class in ways that our study was
the Chinese and the American students. Lower not able to detect. Third, the measures that we
achieving classrooms in our China and American used to assess school climate were standardized
samples have fewer numbers of students in the on an American population and thus we may have
classroom and thus may provide more opportuni- missed elements of the school climate that are
ties for autonomy for the students than higher uniquely important for Chinese students. Fourth,
achieving classrooms that are typically larger in survey items on the school climate measure
size. Our post hoc analysis suggested that the lower included few items related to academic support.
achieving school in China reported numerically Future studies should investigate whether there
higher opportunities for autonomy in the classroom are differences in the association between aca-
than the higher achieving schools, although the dif- demic support, in particular, and academic adjust-
ference between them is not statistically significant. ment across cultural contexts. Finally, GPA for
Alternatively, it may be that if opportunities for each country is based on different standards and
autonomy reduce instruction time, academic measurements, and thus the meaning of academic
achievement may suffer. Additional observational achievement may vary by context. Future studies
research is needed to unpack the processes that should explore such possibilities.
explain this association.
With regard to gender difference, girls reported
Conclusion
significantly higher student–student support than
boys in the American sample, which is consistent This study examined students’ perceptions of
with previous studies (Demaray & Malecki, 2002; school climate and the associations among three
Kuperminc et al., 1997). However, no gender differ- dimensions of school climate and psychological
ence on the three dimensions of school climate was and academic adjustment across two cultural con-
found in the Chinese sample. This result corre- texts—urban United States and urban China. The
sponds to Tsui and Rich’s (2002) findings that girls cross-cultural comparison in the study was
have similar school and education experiences as enhanced by employing measures that were lar-
boys in urban China. There were also no gender dif- gely factor invariant across the two cultures.
ferences in the association of perceptions of school Although adolescents’ perceptions of school sup-
climate with psychological and academic outcomes port varied across cultural contexts, the associa-
in either the Chinese or the American sample, which tions among the three dimensions of school
is inconsistent with the results of Kuperminc et al.’s climate and psychological and academic adjust-
(1997) study that suggested a stronger effect of tea- ment were similar across Chinese and American
cher support on depressive symptoms for boys than adolescents. Furthermore, school-related factors,
for girls in a sample of American adolescents. particularly student–student support, were more
Exploring when, how, and with whom different strongly associated with adjustment than family
dimensions of school climate effect psychological support for the both the Chinese and American
adjustment is necessary for a better understanding adolescents. The findings draw attention to the
of how the context influences adolescent adjust- importance of examining the cultural context in
ment. studies of adolescent adjustment as such compari-
In sum, this study contributed to our under- sons allow us to understand what developmental
standing of how adolescents’ perceptions of school processes are unique to a particular context and
climate are associated with academic and psycho- what processes are not. Strikingly all of the associ-
logical well-being and how these associations vary ations evident in our American study were also
across cultural contexts. Limitations of the student, evident in our Chinese study—two studies that are
however, should be noted. First, this study was situated in distinct contexts but appear, in our
concurrent leaving the direction of effect unclear. data, distinct only in the levels of support but not
Future longitudinal research is needed to investi- in the ways in which support is linked to levels of
gate the direction of effect as well as the ways in adjustment. The need for American psychology,
which the direction of effect may vary by cultural particularly developmental psychology, to become
context. Second, we cannot generalize the findings less American (Arnett, 2008) is underscored in our
1528 Jia et al.

study. It is only when developmental studies Chen, X., & Li, B. (2000). Depressed mood in Chinese
include non-American children will we more fully children: Developmental significance for social and
comprehend the ecological context of child devel- school adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral
opment. Development, 24, 472–479.
Cheung, S. (1986). Reliability and factor structure of
the Chinese version of the depression self-rating
scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 56,
References 142–154.
Cheung, T., & Tam, S. (1984). Analysis of the self-esteem of
Arnett, J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psy- adolescents in Hong Kong: Configuration and determinants.
chology needs to become less American. American Psy- Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.
chologist, 63, 602–614. Colarossi, L. G., & Eccles, J. S. (2003). Differential effects
Bachman, J. G., & O’Malley, P. M. (1986). Self-concept, of support providers on adolescents’ mental health.
self-esteem, and educational experiences: The frog Social Work Research, 27, 19–30.
pond revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Connell, J. P., & Ilardi, B. (1987). Self-system concomitants
ogy, 50, 35–46. of discrepancies between children’s and teachers’ eval-
Biggs, J. B. (1996). Learning, schooling, and socialization: uations of academic competence. Child Development, 58,
A Chinese solution to a Western problem. In S. lau 1297–1307.
(Ed.), Growing up the Chinese way: Chinese child and ado- Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
lescent development (pp. 147–167). Hong Kong: Chinese autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
University Press. self-esteem processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Scroute
Bond, M. H. (1991). Beyond the Chinese face: Insights from (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology: Vol. 23.
psychology. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press. Self-process in development (pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ:
Bond, M. H. (1996). Handbook of Chinese psychology. New Erlbaum.
York: Oxford University Press. Crosnoe, R., Johnson, M. K., & Elder, G. H. (2004). Inter-
Brand, S., Felner, R., Shim, M., Seitsinger, A., & Dumas, generational bonding in school: The behavioral and
N. (2003). Middle school improvement and reform: contextual correlates of student-teacher relationships.
Development and validation of a school-level assess- Sociology of Education, 77, 60–81.
ment of climate, culture pluralism, and school safety. Demaray, M. K., & Malecki, C. K. (2002). Critical
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 570–588. levels of perceived social support associated with
Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interper- student adjustment. School Psychology Quarterly, 17,
sonal competence, and adjustment during preadoles- 213–241.
cence and adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1101–1111. Dong, D., Wang, Y., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). Conse-
Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthén, B. (1989). Test- quences of divorce on the adjustment of children in
ing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean China. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology,
structures: The issue of partial measurement invari- 31, 101–110.
ance. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 456–466. Eccles, J. S. (1993). School and family effects on the
Chan, D. W. (1995). Depressive symptoms and coping ontogeny of children’s interests, self-perceptions, and
strategies among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. activity choices. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska sympo-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 267–279. sium on motivation: Vol. 38. Developmental perspectives on
Chan, K. L., & Chan, C. L. (2005). Chinese culture, social motivation (pp. 145–208). Lincoln: University of
work education and research. International Social Work, Nebraska.
48, 381–389. Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit:
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the conse- Developmentally appropriate in education: Goals and cogni-
quences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and tions. (pp. 13–44). New York: Academic Press.
European Americans. Child Development, 72, 1832– Emmons, C. L., Haynes, N. M., & Comer, J. P. (2002). The
1843. School Climate Survey Revised Edition–Elementary and
Chao, R. K., & Tseng, V. (2002). Asian and Asian Ameri- middle school version. New Haven, CT: Yale University
can parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Child Study Center.
parenting (pp. 59–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Ferguson, R. F. (2003). We care, therefore they learn. Jour-
Chen, X., Chang, L., & He, Y. (2003). The peer group nal of Staff Development, 24, 42–47.
as a context: Mediating and moderating effects on Fong, V. (2004). Only hope: Coming of age under China’s
relations between academic achievement and social one-child policy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
functioning in Chinese children. Child Development, Press.
74, 710–727. Fuligni, A. J. (1997). The academic achievement of adoles-
Chen, X., Chen, H., Kaspar, V., & Noh, S. (2000). Adoles- cents from immigrant families: The roles of family
cent social, emotional and school adjustment in Main- background, attitudes, and behavior. Child Development,
land China. International Journal of Group Tensions, 29, 68, 351–363.
51–78.
The Influence of Student Perceptions of School Climate 1529

Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s percep- friendship: Linking early behavior to early-adolescent
tions of the personal relationships in their social net- adjustment. Child Development, 78, 1037–1051.
works. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1022. Pianta, R. C., & Allen, J. P. (2008). Building capacity for
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Friendships and their developmental positive youth development in secondary school class-
significance. In H. McGurk (Ed.), Childhood social develop- rooms: Changing teachers’ interactions with students. In
ment (pp. 175–201). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. M. Shinn & H. Yoshikawa (Eds.), Towards positive youth
Henry, K. L., & Slater, M. D. (2007). Attachment on development: Transforming schools and community programs
young adolescents alcohol use. Journal of School Health, (pp. 21–39). New York: Oxford University Press.
77, 67–74. Pianta, R. C., Nimetz, S. L., & Bennett, E. (1997). Mother-
Hughes, J. N., Cavell, T. A., & Willson, V. (2001). Further child relationships, teacher-child relationships, and
evidence for the developmental significance of teacher- school outcomes in preschool and kindergarten. Early
student relationships: Peers’ perceptions of support Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 263–280.
and conflict in teacher-student relationships. Journal of Prinstein, M. J., & Aikins, J. W. (2004). Cognitive modera-
School Psychology, 39, 289–301. tors of the longitudinal association between peer rejec-
Jia, Y., Way, N., & Chen, X. (2008, March). Friendships tion and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of
among urban Chinese adolescents. Paper presented at the Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 147–158.
Society for Research on Adolescence, Chicago. Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2000). A first course in
Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (2004). LISREL 8.70. Chicago: structural equation modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Elrlbaum.
Scientific Software. Reddy, R., Rhodes, J. E., & Mulhall, P. (2003). The influ-
Kasen, S. N., Johnson, P. N., & Cohen, P. N. (1990). The ence of teacher support on student adjustment in the
impact of social emotional climate on student psycho- middle school years: A latent growth curve study.
pathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 165– Development and Psychopathology, 15, 119–138.
177. Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools
Knight, G. P., & Hill, N. E. (1998). Measurement equiva- and perceived social support on adolescent well-being.
lence in research involving minority adolescents. In V. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57–68.
C. McLoyd & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Research on minority Roeser, R., Eccles, J., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Academic
adolescents: Conceptual, methodological, and theoretical and emotional functioning in early adolescence:
issues (pp. 183–210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Longitudinal relations, patterns, and prediction by
Kovacs, M. (1980). Rating scales to assess depression in experience in middle school. Developmental and Psycho-
preschool children. Acta Paedopsychiatry, 46, 305–315. pathology, 10, 321–352.
Kuperminc, G., Leadbeater, B., Emmons, C., & Blatt, S. Roeser, R., Eccles, J., & Sameroff, A. (2000). School as a
(1997). Perceived school climate and difficulties in ado- context of early adolescents’ academic and social-emo-
lescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. tional development: A summary of research findings.
Applied Developmental Science, 1, 76–88. The Elementary School Journal, 100, 443–471.
Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W. (1998). Parenting styles Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic
and academic achievement: A cross-culture study. Mer- Books.
rill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 157–172. Samdal, O., Wold, B., Klepf, K. I., & Kannas, L. (1998).
Li, N. (2007). Achieving health and educational goals through
[Elementary and middle schools are ignoring new schools- a study of the importance of the school climate
regulation about school time]. Retrieved September 17, and the students’ satisfaction with school. Health Educa-
2007, from http://www.dqdaj.gov.cn/show.aspx?id= tion Research, 13, 383–397.
1872&cid=42 Samdal, O., Wold, B., Klepf, K. I., & Kannas, L. (2000).
Loukas, A., Suzuki, R., & Horton, K. D. (2006). Examining Students’ perception of school and their smoking and
school connectedness as a mediator of school climate alcohol use: A cross-national study. Addiction Research
effects. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 491–502. & Theory, 8, 141–167.
MaLachlan, G., & Krishnan, T. (1997). The EM algorithm Shek, D. T. (1992). Meaning in life and psychological
and extensions. New York: Wiley. well-being: An empirical study using the Chinese ver-
Morrow, M., Hubbard, J., McAuliffe, M., Rubin, R. M., & sion of the Purpose in Life Questionnaire in a Chinese
Dearing, K. F. (2006). Childhood aggression, depressive context. Journal of Psychology, 12, 77–83.
symptoms, and peer rejection: The mediational model Shek, D. L. (1998). A longitudinal study of the relations
revisited. International Journal of Behavioral Development, between parent-adolescent conflict and adolescent psy-
30, 240–248. chological wellbeing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159,
Noguera, P. (2003). The trouble with Black boys: The role 53–67.
and influence of environmental and cultural factors on Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. (1993). Cultural psy-
the academic performance of African American males. chology: Who needs it? Annual Review of Psychology, 44,
Urban Education, 38, 431–459. 497–523.
Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E., & Borge, A. (2007). Steenkamp, J. E., & Baumgartner, H. (1998). Asse-
The timing of middle-childhood peer rejection and ssing measurement invariance in cross national
1530 Jia et al.

consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, years: Associations with trajectories of psychological
78–90. and behavioral adjustment. American Journal of Commu-
Steinberg, L., Blatt-Eisengart, I., & Cauffman, E. (2006). Pat- nity Psychology, 40, 194–213.
terns of competence and adjustment among adolescents Way, N., & Robinson, M. (2003). The influence of family
from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglect- and friends on the psychological adjustment of ethnic
ful homes: A replication in a sample of serious juvenile minority, low-income adolescents. Journal of Adolescent
offenders. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 47–58. Research, 18, 324–347.
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., Chen, C., Lummis, M., Stigler, Way, N., Santos, C., Niwa, E. Y., & Kim-Gervey, C.
J., Fan, L., et al. (1990). Mathematics achievement of (2008). To be or not to be: An exploration of ethnic
children in China and the U.S. Child Development, 61, identity development in context. The Intersections of
1053–1066. Personal and Social Identities, 120, 61–79.
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, Weine, A. M., Phillips, J. S., & Achenbach, T. M. (1995).
H., Kalman, R., & Niwa, E. (2007). Parents’ goals for Behavioral and emotional problems among Chinese
children: The dynamic co-existence of collectivism and and American children: Parent and teacher reports for
individualism in cultures and individuals. Social Devel- ages 6 to 13. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23,
opment, 17, 183–209. 619–639.
Tsui, M., & Rich, L. (2002). The only child and educa- Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relations of social goal pursuit to
tional opportunity for girls in urban China. Gender & social acceptance, classroom behavior, and perceived
Society, 16, 74–92. social support. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86,
Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and 173–182.
synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Wu, P., Robinson, C. C., Yang, C., Hart, C. H., Olsen, S.
Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for orga- F., Porter, C. L., et al. (2002). Similarities and differ-
nizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3, ences in mothers’ parenting of preschoolers in China
4–70. and the U.S. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). Friendship patterns among ment, 26, 481–491.
African American, Latino, and Asian American Yu, G. (2007). [Report on school
adolescents. The Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 274– time schedule of elementary and middle schools].
301. Retrieved August 10, 2007, from http://www.61.
Way, N., Reddy, R., & Rhodes, J. (2007). Students’ per- gov.cn/dyysk/200708/t20070810_573442.htm
ceptions of school climate during the middle school

Вам также может понравиться