Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

MENTAL RETARDATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

RESEARCH REVIEWS 11: 61–67 (2005)

PRECHTL’S ASSESSMENT OF GENERAL


MOVEMENTS: A DIAGNOSTIC TOOL FOR THE
FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT OF THE YOUNG
NERVOUS SYSTEM
Christa Einspieler* and Heinz F. R. Prechtl
Institute of Physiology, Center for Physiological Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria

General movements (GMs) are part of the spontaneous movement Sherrington, it becomes clear that decerebrated animals and
repertoire and are present from early fetal life onwards until the end of the spinal preparations formed the basis of detailed studies on re-
first half a year of life. GMs are complex, occur frequently, and last long
enough to be observed properly. They involve the whole body in a variable flexes and responses to all kinds of sensory stimulation. With this
sequence of arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements. They wax and wane in ingenious trick, it was possible to get rid of the annoying
intensity, force and speed, and they have a gradual beginning and end. interference stemming from the spontaneous activity of the
Rotations along the axis of the limbs and slight changes in the direction of
movements make them fluent and elegant and create the impression of
nervous system. Only under decerebration did it become pos-
complexity and variability. If the nervous system is impaired, GMs loose their sible to study in detail a stable, quantitative relationship between
complex and variable character and become monotonous and poor. Two sensory input and reflexive motor output [Sherrington, 1906].
specific abnormal GM patterns reliably predict later cerebral palsy: 1) a Having accepted this fact, we should not be too surprised if, in
persistent pattern of cramped-synchronized GMs. The movements appear
rigid and lack the normal smooth and fluent character. Limb and trunk general, reflexes are poor indicators of brain function and dys-
muscles contract and relax almost simultaneously. 2) The absence of GMs of function. However, as a logical consequence of all the experi-
fidgety character. So-called fidgety movements are small movements of ments in the realm of classical neurophysiology, spontaneous
moderate speed with variable acceleration of neck, trunk, and limbs in all
directions. Normally, they are the predominant movement pattern in an
motility, as the expression of spontaneous neural activity, is an
awake infant at 3 to 5 months. Beside a sensitivity and specificity of 95% excellent marker of neural dysfunction caused by brain impair-
each, the assessment of GMs is quick, noninvasive, even nonintrusive, and ment. Nevertheless, it may still be surprising that this fact has
cost-effective compared with other techniques, e.g., magnetic resonance been overlooked for such a long time. The enormous success of
imaging, brain ultrasound, and traditional neurological examination.
© 2005 Wiley-Liss, Inc. the classical stimulus–response investigations has created a tre-
MRDD Research Reviews 2005;11:61– 67. mendous bias in our thinking about the function of the nervous
system. Nowadays, neurophysiology has convincingly docu-
mented that the central nervous system is not only a passive
Key Words: CP; infant; minor neurological deficits; prediction; preterm;
quality; spontaneous movements; fidgety movements
organ. Direct experimental evidence for the central origin of
complex and coordinated movement patterns emanates, of
course, only from studies on developing animals in which iso-
lated brainstem and spinal cord preparations are studied in vitro.

W
e present a new approach to functionally assess the Not only do these preparations allow direct extracellular or
young nervous system. To this end, we observed and intracellular recordings from various neurons of the central
assessed the quality of a certain type of spontaneous
pattern generator (CPG) network, but they also permit the study
movements, the so-called general movements (GMs), because it
of transmitters and the use of receptor blockers. In combination
has been demonstrated that these movements in particular are an
excellent marker for early brain impairment and dysfunction. with morphological studies, such an approach led to detailed
knowledge of the neural mechanism of CPGs and provided an
explanation of spontaneous activity [see reviews by Grillner,
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND
The change in paradigm from the traditional testing of 1999; Forssberg; 1999, Prechtl, 2001a; Einspieler et al., 2004].
reflexes and responses to an assessment technique that system-
atizes the observation of the quality of spontaneous movements *Correspondence to: Christa Einspieler, Institute of Physiology, Center for Physio-
was indeed a breakthrough in the functional assessment of the logical Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Harrachgasse 21, A-8010 Graz, Austria.
young nervous system. There is a simple explanation why re- E-mail: christa.einspieler@meduni-graz.at
flexes and responses are less sensitive in the indication of brain Received 15 November 2004; Accepted 16 November 2004
Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).
lesions. If we consider the work of classical neurophysiology, DOI: 10.1002/mrdd.20051
which is connected with the name of the eminent Sir Charles
© 2005 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
SPONTANEOUS MOVEMENTS
IN FETUSES AND YOUNG
INFANTS
The young human nervous system
generates endogenously, i.e., without be-
ing constantly triggered by specific sensory
input, a variety of motor patterns. In the
human fetus, a large variety of specific
movement patterns, such as startles, GMs,
isolated limb movements, twitches,
stretches, yawning, and breathing move-
ments emerge at 9 to 12 weeks’ postmen-
strual age. There is no period of amorphic
and random movements from which these
patterns differentiate. Instead, they appear
as distinct patterns right from their onset.
These endogenously generated movement
patterns continue after birth, irrespective of
when birth occurs. Remarkably, these
Fig. 1. Developmental course of general movements.
movements do not change their form after
birth [for a recent review, see Prechtl,
2001b].
From the many distinct movement
patterns appearing during the course of that neither the increase of the force of as 3-D-motion analyses [Coluccini et al.,
development from fetus to young infant, gravity after birth nor maturation has an 2002] revealed that movement velocity
the one most effective for the functional influence on the appearance of GMs. and amplitude as well as tonic back-
assessment of the young nervous system The GMs of a preterm infant may occa- ground activity decreased during the
had to be selected. The choice of GMs sionally have large amplitudes and are transformation from writhing to fidgety
worked out very well and has met with often of fast speed [Cioni and Prechtl, movements.
substantial verification. GMs are com- 1990; Prechtl et al., 1997a]. Various other movements may oc-
plex, occur frequently, and last long cur together with fidgety movements,
enough to be observed properly. They Writhing Movements such as wiggling-oscillating and saccadic
involve the whole body in a variable During term age and during the arm movements, swipes, mutual manip-
sequence of arm, leg, neck, and trunk first 2 months post-term, GMs com- ulation of fingers, manipulation (fiddling)
movements. They wax and wane in in- monly are referred to as writhing move- of clothing, reaching and touching, legs
tensity, force, and speed, and they have a ments. They are characterized by small- lift with or without hand-knee contact,
gradual beginning and end. Rotations to-moderate amplitude and by slow- to- trunk rotation, and axial rolling [Hopkins
along the axis of the limbs and slight moderate speed. Typically, they are and Prechtl, 1984; Einspieler et al.,
changes in the direction of movements elliptical in form, which creates the im- 2004].
make them fluent and elegant and create pression of a writhing quality [Hopkins Because GMs include activity of all
the impression of complexity and vari- and Prechtl, 1984; Prechtl and Hopkins, segments from cervical to lumbar spinal
ability [Prechtl, 1990]. Although before 1986; Cioni et al., 1989; Prechtl et al., cord, it is likely that the generating neu-
term we refer to them as fetal or preterm 1997a, 1997b]. EMG recordings revealed ronal structure is located supraspinally
GMs [Einspieler et al., 2004], at term age that the burst duration was significantly [Prechtl, 1997]. Because they emerge at 9
until approximately 6 to 9 weeks post- longer during preterm GMs than during to 10 weeks postmenstrual age, it is un-
term age they are called writhing move- writhing movements. However, burst likely that higher structures than the
ments [Hopkins and Prechtl, 1984]. Even amplitude values and tonic background brainstem are involved. It also can be
if age-related minor differences exist, data do not change from preterm GMs to assumed that GMs of writhing quality
GMs have, by and large, a similar appear- writhing GMs [Hadders-Algra and Pre- and those of fidgety quality are generated
ance from early fetal life until the end of chtl, 1993]. by different CPGs. Their temporal over-
the second month post-term (Fig. 1). At lap at the transformation from the one
6 to 9 weeks’ post-term age, GMs with a Fidgety Movements type into the other makes this most plau-
writhing character gradually disappear At 6 to 9 weeks post-term age, sible [Prechtl, 1997]. That writhing
whereas fidgety GMs gradually emerge writhing movements gradually disappear movements do not disappear during sleep
[Hopkins and Prechtl, 1984; Prechtl et whereas fidgety GMs gradually emerge even at 6 months of life is indicative of
al., 1997b]. Fidgety movements are [Hopkins and Prechtl, 1984; Cioni and the prolonged preservation of their CPG
present up to the end of the first half a Prechtl, 1990; Hadders-Algra and during fidgety age and thereafter [Ein-
year of life when intentional and anti- Prechtl, 1992; Prechtl et al., 1997b]. spieler et al., 1994].
gravity movements start to dominate Fidgety movements are of small ampli-
(Fig. 1). A video [Prechtl et al., 1997a] tude, moderate speed, and variable accel- GENERAL MOVEMENTS
illustrates the ontongeny of GMs. eration of neck, trunk, and limbs in all CHANGE THEIR QUALITY IF
directions continually in the awake infant THE NERVOUS SYSTEM IS
Preterm GMs except during fussing and crying [Prechtl IMPAIRED
No difference can be observed be- et al., 1997a, 1997b]. EMG recordings Already 20 years ago, we knew that
tween fetal and preterm GMs, indicating [Hadders-Algra et al., 1992, 1997] as well GMs in low-risk and high-risk infants or
62 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL.
Fig. 2. A longitudinal study on 130 infants with various ultrasound findings: preterm and writhing quality (left) preceding the quality of fidgety
movements (middle), which is predictive for the neurological outcome at three years [Prechtl et al., 1997a].

brain-damaged infants were not different normal GMs [Ferrari, Gioni, and Prechtl, be observed during preterm, term, and
with respect to the rate of their occur- 1990; Prechtl et al., 1997a], are rather early post-term age but are rather rare.
rence, i.e., their quantity, but that they frequent in infants with brain ultrasound Infants with chaotic GMs often develop
did differ with respect to their quality abnormalities and can be followed by cramped-synchronized GMs a few weeks
[Prechtl and Nolte, 1984; Ferrari, Gioni, normal, abnormal, or absent fidgety later [Einspieler et al., 2004].
and Prechtl, 1990]. The quality of GMs is movements. Hence, the predictive value
probably modulated by corticospinal or of poor repertoire GMs is rather low Abnormal Fidgety Movements
reticulospinal pathways and, hence, can [Prechtl et al., 1997a; Einspieler et al., These movements look like nor-
be affected by impairments of these struc- 2004] (Fig. 2). mal fidgety movements, but their ampli-
tures. A disruption of the corticospinal tude, speed, and jerkiness are moderately
projections by periventricular lesions of Cramped-Synchronized GMs or greatly exaggerated [Prechtl et al.,
the corona radiata or internal capsule due These abnormal GMs appear rigid 1997a, 1997b]. Abnormal fidgety move-
to hemorrhages or hypoxic-ischemic le- and lack the normal smooth and fluent ments are rare. Their predictive value is
sions (leukomalacia) leads to abnormal character, all limb and trunk muscles low [Einspieler et al., 2004] (Fig. 2).
GMs [Prechtl, 1997]. GMs loose their contract and relax almost simultaneously
complex and variable character and have [Ferrari, Gioni, and Prechtl, 1990; Absence of Fidgety Movements
either a poor repertoire, are cramped- Prechtl et al., 1997a]. If this abnormal If fidgety movements are never ob-
synchronized, or chaotic. This definition pattern is observed consistently during a served from 9 to 20 weeks post-term, we
holds true for the preterm and writhing number of weeks, it is of high predictive call this abnormality “absence of fidgety
GMs. Fidgety movements can be either value for the development of spastic CP movements.” However, other move-
abnormal or absent. All normal and ab- [Ferrari, Gioni, and Prechtl, 1990; ments can be commonly observed
normal patterns of GMs are demon- Prechtl et al., 1997b; Ferrari et al., 2002] [Prechtl et al., 1997a, 1997b]. The ab-
strated by a video [Prechtl et al., 1997a] (Figs. 2 and 3). sence of fidgety movements is highly
and a DVD [Einspieler et al., 2004]. predictive for later neurological impair-
Chaotic GMs ments; particularly for CP [Prechtl et al.,
Poor-Repertoire GMs Movements of all limbs are of large 1997b; Einspieler et al., 2002] (Fig. 2).
Poor-repertoire GMs, in which amplitude and occur in a chaotic order
the sequence of the successive movement without any fluency or smoothness. ASSESSMENT PROCEDURE
components is monotonous and move- They consistently appear to be abrupt To provide a reliable assessment of
ments of the different body parts do not [Bos et al., 1997a; Ferrari et al., 1997; GMs the recording procedure has to be
occur in the complex way as seen in Prechtl et al., 1997a]. Chaotic GMs can standardized [Einspieler et al., 1997,
MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL. 63
Fig. 3. Individual developmental trajectory of case A born at 31 weeks’ postmenstrual age. Consistent cramped-synchronized GMs during the preterm,
term, and early post-term period are followed by an absence of fidgety movements. Outcome: spastic CP. F-, absence of fidgety movements; AF,
abnormal fidgety movements; CS, cramped–synchronized GMs; Ch, chaotic GMs; PR, poor repertoire GMs; H, hypokinesis; N, normal GMs; wk, weeks.
The age period for obligatory fidgety movements is marked in gray.

2004]. The comfortably dressed infant, post-term age or both, and at least one fants assessed by 90 observers revealed an
preferably with bare arms and legs, is recording between 9 and 15 weeks’ post- agreement between 89% and 93%
videoed in supine position. The duration term. An individual developmental tra- [Prechtl, 1990; Geerdink and Hopkins,
of the recording depends on the age of jectory indicates the consistency or in- 1993; Albers and Jorch, 1994; Bos et al.
the infant. To collect approximately consistency of normal or abnormal 1997b, 1998a, 2000; Cioni et al., 1997a,
three GMs for reliable judgment, we findings. The prediction of the individual 1997c; Einspieler et al., 1997, 2002;
usually record preterm infants for 30 to neurological development is based on Guzzetta et al., 2003]. The average kappa
60 minutes, independent of whether the such a trajectory. [Cohen, 1960] in another four studies on
infant is asleep or awake. This recording 108 infants assessed by 11 observers was
does not require the observer’s presence TRAINING REQUIRED 0.88 [van Kranen-Mastenbroek et al.,
during the recording or the later assess- Standardized basic and advanced 1992; Bos et al., 1997a, 1998b; Cioni et
ment of the whole recording. Later, this training courses, lasting 4 to 5 days, are al., 2000]. The analysis of 20 GM record-
recording is reviewed and approximately provided by the General Movements ings repeated after a time-interval of 2
three GM sequences are copied onto the Trust (http://www.general-movements- years obtained a 100% test–retest reliabil-
assessment tape. From term age onwards, trust.info) Since 1997, more than 1,000 ity for global judgment and an 85% reli-
5 to 10 minutes of optimal recording doctors and therapist have been trained ability for a detailed analysis [Einspieler,
usually is sufficient. It is most helpful if around the world. A manual, including a 1994].
the sequential recordings taken at differ- CD-ROM [Einspieler et al., 2005] and a
ent ages of the infant are stored on such demonstration video, is available in En- THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE
an assessment tape for the documentation glish; Japanese and Italian [Prechtl et al., ASSESSMENT RESULTS
of the individual developmental course 1997a]. An overall sensitivity obtained
[Einspieler et al., 1997]. Recordings of a A recent study indicated that stan- from several studies [Ferrari, Gioni, and
fussing or crying infant cannot be ana- dardized training courses enable profes- Prechtl, 1990, 1993; Prechtl et al.,
lyzed. sionals in the field of infant and child 1997b; Cioni et al., 1997a, 1997c; Ferrari
The assessment is based on global neurology to apply Prechtl’s GM assess- et al., 2002] of 94% indicates that only
visual Gestalt perception, which is a ment accurately and that it improves with 6% were false negatives. Outcome was
powerful-but-vulnerable instrument in additional training. The evaluation of al- measured at least at the end of the second
the analysis of complex phenomena most 9,000 assessments performed by year and CP or developmental retarda-
[Lorenz, 1971]. Attention to detail must some 800 observers showed that 83% of tion (developmental scores below two
be avoided by all means. As environmen- the assessments were correct (i.e., agree- standard deviations) or both were taken
tal interference may impair the observer’s ment with the gold standard) after com- as “disease positives.” The specificity of
Gestalt perception; the assessment should pleting a basic training course. Additional the same studies was lower during the
be done without acoustic signal. Care- training in an advanced course led to a preterm and writhing movement period
givers, siblings, or twins present on the significant increase of 88% correct assess- (46% to 93%). This was attributable to
video; mirror images of the infant; a bed ments. In addition, correct discrimina- the number of infants with abnormal
crowded with toys; or an irritating col- tion between abnormal and normal gen- GMs (mainly poor repertoire) at this
ored blanket should be avoided [Ein- eral movements was significantly higher early age who normalized before or at the
spieler et al., 2004]. after completing an advanced training fidgety movement period (Fig. 2) and
course (94%) compared with a basic who had a normal outcome. With in-
DURATION OF ASSESSMENT training course (92%) [Einspieler et al., creasing age, specificity increased reveal-
The experienced observer does not 2005]. ing values between 82% and 100% dur-
need more than 1 to 3 minutes to judge ing the third month when normal fidgety
a GM recording. However, a snapshot INTEROBSERVER RELIABILITY movements predict a normal neurologi-
assessment of a single recording must be Because the qualitative GM assess- cal outcome [Einspieler et al., 2004] (Fig.
avoided. An individual developmental ment is based on pattern recognition, it is 2). Normal GMs at any age (likelihood
trajectory [Prechtl, 1990] preferably doc- of utmost importance that the interob- ratio [LR]⫺ ⫽ 0.04; 95% confidence
uments two to three recordings of the server agreement is sufficiently high and, interval: 0.005 to 0.27) as well as
preterm period (three GM sequences thus, the method can be considered as cramped-synchronized GMs (LR⫹ ⫽
each), one recording at term or early objective. So far, 11 studies on 358 in- 45; 95% confidence interval: 6.4 to 321)
64 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL.
Fig. 4. Individual developmental trajectory of case B born at 32 weeks’ postmenstrual age. Poor repertoire of preterm and writhing GMs but transient
cramped-synchronized GMs at 37 weeks are followed by normal fidgety movements. Outcome: normal. Abbreviations as in Fig. 3.

and the absence of fidgety movements CP (Fig. 2). The absence of fidgety the hypothesis of a silent period of later
(LR ⬎51) have excellent likelihood ra- movements can be preceded by cram- hemiplegia. Two studies have shown that
tios [Einspieler et al., 2004]. ped–synchronized GMs (Fig. 3) or, ex- infants with subsequent hemiplegia had
ceptionally, by poor repertoire GMs. an absence of fidgety movements after
SPECIFIC SIGNS PREDICTING The validity of fidgety movements is bilateral cramped–synchronized or poor-
CEREBRAL PALSY clearly demonstrated if we deal with an repertoire GMs [Cioni et al., 2000;
Perlman [1998] stated that no early individual developmental trajectory con- Guzzetta et al., 2003]. The first asymmet-
markers for the later development of CP sisting of transient cramped–synchro- ric sign, independent of the head posi-
are recognizable in the neonatal period, nized GMs. Transient cramped–synchro- tion, was segmental movements, which
but he was referring to the traditional nized GMs led to CP if fidgety were reduced or absent contralateral to
neurological examination. With the movements were absent. If transient the side of the lesion. This asymmetry
qualitative assessment of GMs, it is now cramped–synchronized GMs were fol- occurred in preterm born infants from
possible to detect specific neurological lowed by normal fidgety movements the third month post-term age onwards
signs, prenatally in the fetus [for a review, (Fig. 4) the neurological outcome was [Cioni et al., 2000]. In term-born infants
see Prechtl and Einspieler, 1997], or normal [Ferrari et al., 2002]. with neonatal infarction, the asymmetry
postnatally in the preterm or term infant, Thus, specific early signs of later of segmental movements was already
which are highly predictive for the later spastic CP are consistently cramped–syn- present during the second month
development of CP. In this respect, GM chronized GMs and/or absence of fidg- [Guzzetta et al., 2003].
assessment can be considered as a diag- ety movements (Figs. 2 and 3). Both
nostic breakthrough. abnormal qualities of GMs can be seen at The Prediction of Dyskinetic CP
an age at which no evidence on a stan- Until the second month post-term,
Consistent Cramped–Synchronized dard neurological examination of CP is the infant that later will become dyski-
General Movements and the present yet, namely as early as from fetal netic displays a poor repertoire of GMs,
Absence of Fidgety Movements life onwards or from preterm or term “arm movements in circles,” and finger
Predict Spastic CP birth until the third month post-term spreading. Characteristically, these ab-
The first longitudinal study on the [Prechtl et al., 1997b]. normal arm and finger movements re-
predictive value of the various abnormal main until at least 5 months post-term.
GM patterns revealed cramped–synchro- Is It Possible to Differentiate The abnormal unilateral or bilateral “arm
nized GMs as highly predictive for a se- Between Later Spastic Diplegia and movements in circles” are monotonous,
vere neurological outcome [Ferrari, Tetraplegia? slow forward rotations from the shoul-
Gioni, and Prechtl, 1990]. The largest Consistent cramped–synchronized der. Particularly the monotony in speed
longitudinal study of 130 infants consist- GMs predict both spastic diplegia and and amplitude is the most characteristic
ing of the whole spectrum from normal- tetraplegia. Cases with later diplegia had a quality of “arm movements in circles.”
to-abnormal brain ultrasound findings at- later onset and shorter duration of cram- Usually these abnormal arm movements
tributable to hypoxic–ischemic lesions or ped–synchronized GMs than cases with are accompanied by finger spreading
hemorrhages confirmed the importance later tetraplegia [Ferrari et al., 2002]. If in [Einspieler et al., 2002].
of cramped–synchronized GMs. All 40 addition to the cramped–synchronized From 3 months onwards, a lack of
children who at repeated assessments GMs so-called segmental movements movements towards the midline, partic-
showed consistently cramped–synchro- were frequently present in the upper ularly foot–foot contact, was an addi-
nized GMs, later developed a severe spas- limbs, the child most probably developed tional specific sign of later dyskinetic
tic CP [Prechtl et al., 1997b] (Fig. 2). In diplegia [Cioni et al., 1997b]. Segmental cases. In addition, the majority of cases
a recent study of 84 preterm infants with movements are distinct movements of neither displayed hand– hand contact nor
brain lesion indicated by ultrasound scan, hand and feet, fingers and toes occurring hand–mouth contact [Einspieler et al.,
Ferrari and co-workers [2002] reported either isolated or as part of GMs. In the 2002].
that the earlier consistent cramped–syn- latter case they are not part of limb flex- Common to both spastic and dys-
chronized GMs occurred the worse was ion or extension [van der Heide et al., kinetic cases was the absence of fidgety
the later motor impairment. 1999]. movements and the absence of antigrav-
There is yet another early marker ity movements, i.e., legs lifting, during
for later development of CP. Ninety- Early Signs of Hemiplegia the third to fifth month [Einspieler et al.,
eight percent of infants who never The observation of abnormal 2002]. The absence of fidgety move-
showed fidgety movements developed movements from birth onwards refutes ments is of particular interest. Prechtl
MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL. 65
[1997] suggested a specific CPG for fidg- without taking a break [Einspieler et al., consistently accompanied by epileptic
ety movements located, most likely, in 1997]. If many recordings of abnormal discharges in the EEG.
the brainstem. The absence of fidgety GMs are observed in a series, one is ad-
movements in both forms of CP caused vised to watch a gold standard normal STRENGTHS AND BENEFITS
by different brain lesions indicates that recording [Prechtl et al., 1997a; Ein- The methodological breakthrough
intact corticospinal fibers as well as the spieler et al., 2004] from time to time. of our assessment technique lies in the
output from the basal ganglia and cere- This is necessary for re-calibrating the fact that it predicts the later development
bellum are necessary to generate normal Gestalt perception. of CP at a much earlier age than was
fidgety movements [Einspieler et al., A serious concern that has been previously possible. In addition, the qual-
2002]. raised is whether a systemic disease, such itative assessment of GMs is totally non-
as infection without brain involvement intrusive, easily learned, and cost-effec-
GMS AND LATER MINOR (Candida species, coagulase-negative tive. The great advantage of being able to
NEUROLOGICAL DEFICITS Staphylococcus, Staphylococcus aureus), detect the risk of later development of
Abnormal fidgety movements might mimic an impairment of GM qual- CP so early is the possibility to install
were less predictive for the neurological ity similar to those in brain dysfunction. interventions long before pathological
outcome than the absence of fidgety Bos and co-workers [1997b] demon- features of CP develop. It is most un-
movements [Prechtl et al., 1997b] but strated that septicemia has a limited in- likely that these interventions will pre-
have been discussed in the context of the fluence on the GM quality. At first no- vent the development of CP, but they
development of mild neurological defi- tice, the GMs in infants with septicemia can help prevent secondary defects such
cits [Bos et al., 1997a, 1999, 2002]. could be mistaken for GMs with poor as contractures and other forms of immo-
A study on some 50 children indi- repertoire. However, the richness in bility. The psychological support for par-
cated that “mildly abnormal GMs” dur- complexity and variability, in particular ents and the maximal functional deploy-
ing 3 to 4 months (period of fidgety of the sequence of the moving body ment and early adaptation of the
movements) had a prognostic signifi- parts, including superimposed rotations, impaired child are of crucial importance.
cance, pointing to an increased risk for was strictly different from truly poor rep- In addition, it is of similar significance to
the development of minor neurological ertoire GMs. GMs of preterm infants select those infants with normal GMs
deficits, attention deficit hyperactivity with severe infection have just a sluggish who, despite being at-risk because their
disorder, and boisterous, disobedient be- character with a slow speed. Hence, it is history, will have a normal neurological
havior of 4- to 9-year-old children [Had- possible to discriminate between abnor- outcome. f
ders-Algra and Groothuis, 1999]. Ac- mal GMs due to brain lesion and sluggish
cording to Hadders-Algra and co- GMs due to severe systemic infection, REFERENCES
Albers S, Jorch G. 1994. Prognostic significance of
workers [1997], “mildly abnormal GMs” when the complexity of the GMs is con- spontaneous motility in very immature pre-
lack the fluency but still show some com- sidered as the main characteristic for nor- term infants under intensive care treatment.
plexity and variation. However, the clas- mal GMs [Bos, 1993]. Biol Neonat 66:182–187.
sification “mildly abnormal GMs” does For the less-experienced observer, Baldi I. 2002. The preterm infant with prolonged
periventricular hyperechogenicity: prognostic
not exist in Prechtl’s GM assessment. it might be sometimes difficult to distin- role of neurological assessment and the effects
A 15-year follow-up study on 33 guish between abnormal GMs and sei- of neonatal care (in Italian). Medical Doctor
girls and boys revealed that children with zures. GMs with a poor repertoire show Thesis, University of Pisa.
a history of abnormal fidgety movements a monotonous sequence of the successive Bos AF. 1993. Differential effects of brain lesions
and systemic disease on the quality of general
had lower Griffiths Scores [Brandt, 1983] movement components that recalls ste- movements a preliminary report. Early Hum
at 2 years and lower scores in the Bruin- reotyped movements of subtle seizures. Dev 34:39 – 45.
inks Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Similarly, some cramped-synchronized Bos AF, Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, et al. 1999.
[Bruininks, 1978], particularly in fine GMs may resemble tonic posturing of The quality of spontaneous motor activity in
motor performance during midpuberty focal or generalized tonic seizures. What preterm infants as early predictive signs for
minor neurological abnormalities at two
[Einspieler et al., 2004]. is peculiar to seizure movements but cer- years. Newslett Neonat Neurol 8:4 –5.
tainly not to abnormal GMs are the ex- Bos AF, Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR. 2002. Motor
CAUTIONS AND LIMITATIONS treme stereotypy and the ictal character repertoire at early age for prediction of neu-
Several studies on either preterm of the clinical phenomena. The sequence rological deficits at 2 y. Pediatr Res 52:796.
Bos AF, Martijn A, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. 1998a.
infants during individualized develop- of movements does not change during Quality of general movements in preterm in-
mental care [Constantinou et al., 1999; the single seizure or even in successive fants with transient periventricular echoden-
Baldi, 2002] or on 3-month-old infants seizures. Abnormal GMs are monoto- sities. Acta Paediatr 87:328 –335.
during various kinds of sensory stimula- nous in the sequence but are never ste- Bos AF, Martijn A, van Asperen RM, et al. 1998b.
tion [Dibiasi and Einspieler, 2002, 2004] reotyped to such a degree as seizures are. Qualitative assessment of general movements
in high risk preterm infants with chronic lung
indicated that GMs, being endogenously Other ictal motor phenomena, such as disease requiring dexamethasone therapy.
generated and a robust output of the abnormal eye movements or oral– Disabil Rehabil 132:300 –306.
young nervous system, were hardly in- buccal–lingual movements or autonomic Bos AF, van Asperen RM, de Leeuw DM, Prechtl
fluenced by environmental stimulation. phenomena during limb movements, are HFR. 1997a. The influence of septicaemia on
spontaneous motility in preterm infants. Early
For the assessments of GMs, however, never seen during poor repertoire or Hum Dev 50:61–70.
any kind of environmental stimulation cramped–synchronized GMs [Einspieler Bos AF, van Loon AJ, Hadders-Algra M, et al.
may interfere with the observer’s Gestalt et al., 2004]. If the clinical observation 1997b. Spontaneous motility in preterm,
perception and should be avoided. does not provide a differentiation be- small for gestational age infants. II. Qualitative
Because tiredness certainly inter- tween seizure and abnormal GMs, an aspects. Early Hum Dev 50:131–147.
Bos AF, Venema IMJ, Bergervoet M, et al. 2000.
feres with the visual Gestalt perception, EEG-recording can be helpful despite Spontaneous motility in preterm infants
the observer should never assess for not all motor automatisms and particu- treated with indomethacin. Biol Neonat 78:
longer episodes than about 45 minutes larly tonic generalized seizures are not 174 –180.

66 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL.


Brandt I. 1983. Griffiths Entwicklungsskalen (GES) review of the methodology. Early Hum Dev Lorenz K. 1971. Gestalt perception as a source of
zur Beurteilung der Entwicklung in den er- 50:47– 60. scientific knowledge. English translation from
sten beiden Lebensjahren. Weinheim, Basel: Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, van Eykern L, et al. a German paper in 1959. In: Lorenz K, editor.
Beltz. 1994. Observation of movements during Studies in animal and human behaviour. Vol
Bruininks RH. 1978. Bruininks Oseretsky test of sleep in ALTE and apnoeic infants. Early II. London: Methuen. p 281–322.
motor proficiency: examiner’s manual. Circle Hum Dev 40:39 –50. Perlman JM. 1998. White matter injury in the
Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Ferrari F, Cioni G, Einspieler C, et al. 2002. preterm infant: an important determination of
Cioni G, Bos AF, Einspieler C, et al. 2000. Early Cramped synchronised general movements in abnormal neurodevelopmental outcome.
neurological signs in preterm infants with preterm infants as an early marker for CP. Early Hum Dev 53:99 –120.
unilateral intraparenchymal echodensity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 156:460 – 467. Prechtl HFR. 1990. Qualitative changes of spon-
Neuropediatrics 31:240 –251. Ferrari F, Gioni, C, Prechtl HFR. 1990. Qualita- taneous movements in fetus and preterm in-
Cioni G, Ferrari F, Einspieler C, et al. 1997a. tive changes of general movements in preterm fant are a marker of neurological dysfunction.
Comparison between observation of sponta- infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev Early Hum Dev 23:151–158.
neous movements and neurological examina- 23:193–233. Prechtl HFR. 1997. State of the art of a new
tion in preterm infants. Disabil Rehabil 130: Ferrari F, Prechtl HFR, Cioni G, et al. 1997. functional assessment of the young nervous
704 –711. Behavioural states, posture and spontaneous system. An early predictor of CP. Early Hum
Cioni G, Ferrari F, Prechtl HFR. 1989. Posture movements in infants affected by brain mal- Dev 50:1–11.
and spontaneous motility in fullterm infants. formation. Early Hum Dev 50:87–113. Prechtl HFR. 2001a. General movement assess-
Early Hum Dev 7:247–262. Forssberg H. 1999. Neural control of human motor ment as a method of developmental neurol-
Cioni G, Paolicelli PB, Rapisardi G, et al. 1997b. Development. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:676 – ogy: new paradigms and their consequences.
Early natural history of spastic diplegia and 682. The 1999 Ronnie MacKeith Lecture. Dev
tetraplegia. Eur Disabil Rehabil Neurol 1:33. Geerdink JJ, Hopkins B. 1993. Qualitative changes Med Child Neurol 43:836 – 842.
Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. 1990. Preterm and early in general movements and their prognostic Prechtl HFR. 2001b. Prenatal and early postnatal
postterm motor behaviour in low-risk prema- values in preterm infants. Eur J Paediatr 152: development of human motor behaviour. In:
ture infants. Early Hum Dev 23:159 –193. 362–367. Kalverboer AF, Gramsbergen A, editors.
Cioni G, Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, et al. 1997c. Grillner S. 1999. Bridging the gap—from ion chan- Handbook of brain and behaviour in hu-
Which better predicts later outcome in full- nels to networks and behaviour. Curr Opin man development. Amsterdam: Kluver. p
term infants: quality of general movements or Neurobiol 9:663– 669. 415– 427.
neurological examination? Early Hum Dev Guzzetta A, Mercuri E, Rapisardi G, et al. 2003. Prechtl HFR, Bos AF, Cioni G, et al. 1997a. Spon-
50:71– 85. General movements detect early signs of taneous motor activity as a diagnostic tool.
Cohen J. 1960. A coefficient of agreement for hemiplegia in term infants with neonatal ce- Demonstration video. London, Graz: The
nominal scales. Ed Psychol Meas 20:37– 46. rebral infarction. Neuropediatrics 34:61– 66. GM Trust.
Coluccini M, Maini S, Sabatini A, et al. 2002. Hadders-Algra M, Groothuis AMC. 1999. Quality Prechtl HFR, Einspieler C. 1997. Is neurological
Kinematic analysis of general movements in of general movements in infancy is related to assessment of the fetus possible? Eur J Obstetr
early infancy. Dev Med Child Neurol 44:14. neurological dysfunction, attention deficit/ Gynecol Repr Biol 75:81– 84.
Constantinou JC, Adamson-Macedo EN, Steven- hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and aggres- Prechtl HFR, Einspieler C, Cioni G, et al. 1997b. An
son DK, et al. 1999. Effects of skin-to-skin sive behaviour. Dev Med Child Neurol 41: early marker for neurological deficits after peri-
holding on general movements of preterm 381–391. natal brain lesions. Lancet 349:1361–1363.
infants. Clin Pediatr 38:467– 471. Hadders-Algra M, Klip-van den Nieuwendijk AW, Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, Cioni G. 1993. Predictive
Dibiasi J, Einspieler C. 2002. Can spontaneous Martijn A, et al. 1997. Assessment of general value of general movements in asphyxiated
movements be modulated by visual and movements: towards a better understanding fullterm infants. Early Hum Dev 35:91–120.
acoustic stimulation in 3-month-old infants? of a sensitive method to evaluate brain func- Prechtl HFR, Hopkins B. 1986. Developmental
Early Hum Dev 68:27–37. tion in young infants. Dev Med Child Neurol transformations of spontaneous movements in
Dibiasi J, Einspieler C. 2004. Load perturbation 39:88 –98. early infancy. Early Hum Dev 14:233–238.
does not influence spontaneous movements Hadders-Algra M, Prechtl HFR. 1992. Develop- Prechtl HFR, Nolte R. 1984. Motor behaviour of
in 3-month-old infants. Early Hum Dev 77: mental course of general movements in early preterm infants. In: Prechtl HFR, editor.
37– 46. infancy. I. Descriptive analysis of change in Continuity of neural functions from prenatal
Einspieler C. 1994. Abnormal spontaneous move- form. Early Hum Dev 28:201–213. to postnatal life. Clin Dev Med 94. Oxford:
ments in infants with repeated sleep apnoeas. Hadders-Algra M, Prechtl HFR. 1993. EMG cor- Blackwell. p 79 –92.
Early Hum Dev 36:31– 48. relates of general movements in healthy pre- Sherrington C. 1906. The integrative action of the
Einspieler C, Cioni G, Paolicelli PB, et al. 2002. term infants. J Physiol 459:330. CNS. New York: Scribners (reprinted: 1961,
The early markers for later dyskinetic CP are Hadders-Algra M, van Eykern LA, Klip-van den New Haven: Yale University Press).
different from those for spastic CP. Neurope- Nieuwendijk AW, et al. 1992. Developmen- van der Heide JC, Paolicelli PB, Boldrini A, et al.
diatrics 33:73–78. tal course of general movements in early in- 1999. Kinematic and qualitative analysis of
Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, Bos AF, et al. 2004. fancy. II. EMG correlates. Early Hum Dev lower-extremity movements in preterm in-
Prechtl’s method of qualitative assessment of 28:231–251. fants with brain lesions. Phys Ther 79:546 –
general movements in preterm, term and Hopkins B, Prechtl HFR. 1984. A qualitative ap- 557.
young infants (incl. DVD). London: Mac- proach to the development of movements van Kranen-Mastenbroek V, van Oostenbrugge R,
Keith Press. during early infancy. In: Prechtl HFR, editor. Palmans L, et al. 1992. Inter- and intra-ob-
Einspieler C, Prechtl HFR, Ferrari F, et al. 1997. Continuity of neural functions from prenatal server agreement in the assessment of the
The qualitative assessment of general move- to postnatal life. Clin Dev Med 94. Oxford: quality of spontaneous movements in the
ments in preterm, term and young infants- Blackwell. p 179 –197. newborn. Brain Dev 14:289 –293.

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● GENERAL MOVEMENT ASSESSMENT ● EINSPIELER ET AL. 67

Вам также может понравиться