Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

clinical obesity doi: 10.1111/cob.

12059

Diffusing obesity myths

X. Ramos Salas1, M. Forhan2 and A. M. Sharma3

What is already known about the subject What this study adds
• Misinformation or myths about obesity can lead to weight bias and • Ten obesity myth–fact messages were developed and validated by
obesity stigma. obesity experts.
• The myth/fact message format method has been used widely in • Obesity experts and health professionals supported these obesity
public health and social marketing strategies as way to reduce myth–fact messages but they did not perceive them to be sufficient
stigma. to change the deeply rooted negative attitudes and beliefs about
obesity.

1
School of Public Health and Canadian Summary
Obesity Network, University of Alberta, Misinformation or myths about obesity can lead to weight bias and obesity
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada; 2
Faculty of stigma. Counteracting myths with facts and evidence has been shown to be
Rehabilitation Medicine, Department of effective educational tools to increase an individuals’ knowledge about a certain
Occupational Therapy, University of Alberta, condition and to reduce stigma.The purpose of this study was to identify common
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada; obesity myths within the healthcare and public domains and to develop evidence-
3
CanadianObesity Network, and Faculty of based counterarguments to diffuse them. An online search of grey literature,
Medicine & Dentistry, Obesity Research & media and public health information sources was conducted to identify common
Management, University of Alberta, obesity myths. A list of 10 obesity myths was developed and reviewed by obesity
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada experts and key opinion leaders. Counterarguments were developed using current
research evidence and validated by obesity experts. A survey of obesity experts
Received 20 March 2014; revised 8 April and health professionals was conducted to determine the usability and potential
2014; accepted 10 April 2014 effectiveness of the myth–fact messages to reduce weight bias. A total of 754
individuals responded to the request to complete the survey. Of those who
Address for correspondence: Ms. X. Ramos responded, 464 (61.5%) completed the survey. All 10 obesity myths were iden-
Salas, Canadian Obesity Network, University tified to be deeply pervasive within Canadian healthcare and public domains.
of Alberta, Li Ka Shing Centre for Health Although the myth–fact messages were endorsed, respondents also indicated that
Research Innovation, Room 2-126, Edmonton they would likely not be sufficient to reduce weight bias. Diffusing deeply perva-
Alberta, T6G 2E1, Canada. E-mail: sive obesity myths will require multilevel approaches.
ramos-salas@obesitynetwork.ca
Keywords: Obesity stigma, myth/fact message format, myths.

weight bias and obesity stigma. A number of myths and


Introduction
misperceptions about obesity exist that associate obesity
Despite a better understanding of the causes and effective with ‘ugliness, sexlessness, undesirability and moral failings
treatment strategies for obesity, this chronic condition is such as lack of self-control, social irresponsibility, inepti-
not effectively managed in the healthcare system (1). This is tude and laziness across cultures and borders (2).’
in part due to the negative views and attitudes about Obesity stigma can affect a person’s mental health, their
obesity, which are disseminated globally (2). These negative interpersonal relationships, educational achievement and
views are manifested through myths and stereotypes about employment opportunities, and ultimately lead to social
obesity and/or prejudice towards people with excess and health inequities (3,4). Many national and interna-
weight. Misinformation or myths about obesity can lead to tional obesity organizations have identified weight bias as a

© 2014 The Authors 189


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196
190 Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al. clinical obesity

key barrier to effectively addressing the obesity epidemic erable overlap in subject matter. The experts also suggested
(3). Challenging myths about obesity and reducing obesity key research studies that could be used to counteract each
stigma among health professionals and the public are myth.
important in order to obtain support for effective obesity Counterarguments were developed and sent for review by
prevention and treatment strategies and policies (5). obesity experts. Finally, the updated list of obesity myths
The myth/fact message format (MFMF) is a method used with evidence-based counterarguments was distributed to
widely in public health and social marketing strategies all members of the Canadian Obesity Network (CON) who
(6–8) as a way to reduce stigma and increase knowledge had valid email addresses (n = 7221). The CON is Canada’s
about a particular condition. The MFMF takes an incorrect only association of obesity professionals with a common
viewpoint labelled as a myth and remedies it with a counter goal to reduce the burden of obesity on Canadians. CON’s
view labelled as a fact (9). Counteracting myths with facts membership includes researchers, interdisciplinary health
and evidence has been shown to be effective educational practitioners, policy makers and individuals from govern-
tools to increase individuals’ knowledge about a certain ment, industry and non-profit sectors. The list of myths and
condition (6). To our knowledge, this method has not been counterarguments was provided in the context of a survey.
used as a way to directly confront widely held misconcep- The overall purpose of the survey was to determine the
tions and myths about obesity in Canada. The purpose of pervasiveness of these myths in the healthcare and public
this study was to identify common myths about obesity domains, and to assess the usability of the myth–fact mes-
within the Canadian healthcare and public domains and to sages as a way to address weight bias in the healthcare and
develop evidence-based counterarguments that can be used public domain. Participants were asked to respond to a
in future public health education campaigns. series of three questions designed to measure the profile of
the myth, the validity of the counterargument and the extent
to which the counterargument could influence weight bias.
Materials and methods
The survey was open for 5 days.
An ad hoc group of obesity experts and key opinion leaders
was established to provide advice throughout the project.
Results
An online search of grey literature, media and public health
information sources was conducted to identify common The statement of obesity myths and the matched evidence-
obesity myths. The following search terms were used on based counterarguments (Table 1) were sent to CON
Google’s search engine: ‘obesity’, ‘weight’, ‘overweight’, members (n = 7221) for review in the format of an online
AND ‘causes’, ‘consequences’, ‘solutions’, ‘recommenda- survey created using Survey Monkey. A total of 754 CON
tions’. An obesity myth was defined as a misconception members responded to the request to complete the survey
about obesity causes and consequences. The myths were (10.4% response rate). Of those who responded, 464
organized by source (websites). A team of three reviewers (61.5%) of the members completed the survey. All 10 of the
who coded each statement reviewed statements about the obesity myths were identified to be pervasive within the
causes, consequences and recommendations for addressing general public in Canada. The pervasiveness of the myth
obesity. The statements were coded into the following was determined by calculating the percentage of partici-
themes: (1) link between weight and health; (2) character- pants who rated a high level of agreement that the obesity
istics of individuals with obesity and (3) obesity reduction myth was pervasive among Canadians. A list of the obesity
strategies. Myths about individual weight loss approaches myths in the order of pervasiveness as perceived by survey
(i.e. fad diets and fallacies) were excluded from the search. participants is found in Table 1.
Only myths that were considered to contribute to further Validation of the counterarguments was determined by
weight bias were included in the final list. the response of participants who agreed that the
These statements were tabulated according to pervasive- counterargument was fully valid or mostly valid with
ness and source. Consensus on the themes was reached respect to their knowledge of current evidence. A review of
after the researchers shared their coding results electroni- the comments in response to the validation item on the
cally, and with final agreement reached during a conference survey revealed no additional references to include or
call with all reviewers. The original list of myths included errors in the interpretation of the evidence used to counter-
15 statements that responded directly with the original act the obesity myth identified in the public domain. A
themes identified by the reviewers of the grey, media and summary of the validation results is found in Table 1.
public health information sources. The 15 statements were Participants were also asked to indicate how effective
sent out to experts in the areas of physical activity, child- they thought the counterargument could be to reduce
hood obesity and obesity prevention and management. The weight bias in healthcare and public domains. This proved
experts recommended that the 15 statements should be to be the most difficult question to respond to as noted by
amalgamated into a list of 10 obesity myths due to consid- participant comments indicating that although they

© 2014 The Authors


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196
Table 1 Summary of obesity myths, evidence-based counterarguments, validation and effect of counterarguments listed in order of perceived pervasiveness

Obesity myth Ranked Percentage Evidence-based counterargument Percentage of Percentage of


statement order agreement agreement about agreement for
with myth the validity of the counterargument to
counterargument. reduce weight bias.

© 2014 The Authors


Obese people are less 1 (93%) Research does not support the notion that overweight or obese individuals are substantially less active 93% 61%
physically active than normal weight Canadians. The 2007–2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey shows that only 5%
of Canadian children and adults meet the weekly recommendations for moderate to vigorous physical
clinical obesity

activity. However, the report also shows that overweight and obese children and adults with obesity are
not significantly less active their normal weight counterparts. Thus, for example, overweight and obese
girls aged 6 to 19 years have exactly the same minutes of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity
(MVPA) as girls who are neither overweight nor obese (between 44 and 48 minutes per day) (10–12).
Although overweight men and women may accumulate fewer minutes of MVPA per day, it is important
to consider that for the same amount of physical activity, overweight and obese individuals do more
physical work and expend more energy due to moving a larger body mass than their normal weight
counterparts. In addition, overweight and obese individuals may experience greater environmental,
medical and emotional barriers to being more physically active.
Body weight is a 2 (87%) Although the commonly used term ‘healthy weight’ suggests that health can be measured by simply 93% 67%
measure of good stepping on a scale, the relationship between body weight or body mass index (BMI) and health is

Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196


health more complex. While at a population level increased BMI is associated with a greater risk for a wide
range of health problems, individuals can maintain good health over a wide range of body weights.
BMI or weight should be used in conjunction with other health measures to obtain an accurate indicator
of health. Individuals with excess weight, who are physically fit, report a better quality of life and have a
lower risk of morbidity and mortality than normal weight individuals, who are unfit (13–18). Whether or
not excess weight affects health is also related to fat distribution or ectopic fat. The health impact of
excess weight is also influenced by societal norms and discrimination, which can promote body
dissatisfaction and maladaptive weight-loss behaviours.
All obese people eat 3 (86%) Although an increase in the availability and consumption of calorie-dense foods and beverages is 89% 57%
unhealthy diets associated with increased rates of obesity in populations, the nutritional quality of diets consumed by
overweight or obese persons is not substantially different from that of normal weight individuals (19).
Many individuals with excess weight consume diets that are as healthy or unhealthy as those of their
normal weight peers. Therefore, it is important not to make assumptions about dietary habits of an
obese individual.
Anyone can control their 4 (84%) While diet and exercise can promote weight loss in the short term, long-term results of behavioural weight 84% 58%
weight with diet and management even in the rather ideal confines of a clinical trial are disappointing. Thus, behavioural
exercise intervention programmes focusing only on improving diet and activity levels generally do not result in
more than 3–5% sustainable weight loss (1,20) Although, this degree of weight loss can have
substantial health benefits, maintenance of even this modest weight loss requires continuous (life-long)
effort. The US National Weight Control Registry shows that maintenance of significantly more weight
loss requires a substantial reduction in daily caloric intake often paired with a rigorous daily exercise
regimen that would not be realistic for most Canadians.
Obese people lack 5 (81%) Studies comparing restraint and dis-inhibition between normal weight and obese individuals do not 80% 55%
motivation and support the notion that most obese individuals lack will-power or self-control. On the other hand, there
self-control are specific mental health conditions such as binge eating disorder, atypical depression or attention
deficit disorder that are characterized by lack of control. Given the high recidivism of weight-loss
efforts, many overweight and obese individuals lack self-efficacy and motivation to re-engage in
weight-loss efforts. This problem is further perpetuated by the lack of competencies and self-efficacy of
healthcare professionals in promoting and managing behaviour change in their patients. Together,
Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al.

these factors may lead to the misconception that individuals with excess weight are less motivated or
unable to muster the will-power to better manage their weight (21,22).
191
Table 1 Continued

Obesity myth Ranked Percentage Evidence-based counterargument Percentage of Percentage of


statement order agreement agreement about agreement for
with myth the validity of the counterargument to
counterargument. reduce weight bias.

Forcing people to take 6 63% Tactics designed to force a person living with obesity to feel bad about the way they look, eat, move and 92% 71%
responsibility for their live their life are nothing short of weight-related bullying. Shaming or bullying stems from a belief that a
obesity is the best person with obesity is oblivious to their weight and shape and would benefit from societal pressures to
way to facilitate change their ways. However, research shows that shame does not catalyze health behaviour change; it
behaviour change. paralyzes it (4,23). The personal responsibility approach to addressing obesity has been used in North
America for decades and it has not worked. A shift in focus from personal blame to a focus on
empowering health starts by acknowledging the various biological, psychological, social and
environmental factors that contribute to obesity. This shift is more meaningful and provides an
opportunity to look at the shared personal and societal responsibility to promote health and well-being.
Personal responsibility is just one piece of a much more complex problem. Simply focusing on one
192 Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al.

piece of this problem issue will not be effective.


Suggesting weight 7 52% While indicators for the health of a population are necessary to assess the impact of interventions for 77% 46%
targets for a health promotion and to identify areas in which changes need to be made, setting population weight
population is targets may neither be helpful nor feasible. According to current estimates, even just a 5% decrease in
important. obesity prevalence would require a 75% reduction in the current rate of weight gain in the population
(24). Thus, although public health initiatives to promote healthy eating and active living have the
potential to improve the overall health and wellness of the population, available evidence on the
efficacy of lifestyle changes in weight management do not predict that there would be detectable
changes in the average weight of Canadians. Therefore, a focus on promoting ‘healthy’ weights, rather
than on improving overall health of Canadians may undermine the perceived benefits of health
promotion strategies.
Food bans and taxes 8 16% There is little evidence to support higher tax levies on foods deemed to be contributing to the increase in 75% 35%
are effective methods obesity rates. Although increasing the price of foods deemed to be of high energy content but low in
to reduce obesity. nutritional value may affect the purchasing behaviour of populations, how this translates into overall
caloric consumption and outcomes such as body weight and waist circumference of a population is not
known. Outright bans of certain foods in schools, recreation centers or other public or private venues
can influence the types of food consumed while on the premises but may merely shift the purchase or
consumption of the banned items off-site (25,26).
Efforts to reduce 9 13% Although traditionally efforts to tackle conditions that impact health and well-being are targeted towards 91% 64%
childhood obesity the person living with that condition, in the case of childhood obesity, it is critical that the system in
should focus only on which the child lives is included in these efforts. This includes parents, educators and care providers.
the children. Recent studies show that a parent-only approach to dealing with childhood obesity is at least as, if not
more, effective than parent-and-child strategies. Similarly, studies have found that targeting parents’
diets has a greater effect on the weight of their overweight children than trying to getting the children to
be more active. There is also accumulating data demonstrating a significant ‘halo’ effect on childhood
obesity from effectively managing adult obesity (25,27,28).
Obese people are less 10 11% There is no evidence that overweight or obese adults and children are less intelligent than their normal 85% 58%
intelligent weight peers. Although obese children may have more difficulty in performing visual-spatial tasks and
memorization of details, such findings do not indicate that they are not capable of performing such
tasks. When adjusted for educational and socio-economic status, associations between intelligence
scores and obesity or the development of obesity cease to exist. Weight-based teasing and bullying in
the schools by peers together with reduced expectations of educators can lead to academic
disengagement resulting in a reduction of post-secondary education, particularly among adolescent
females with obesity (29–35).

© 2014 The Authors


clinical obesity

Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196


clinical obesity Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al. 193

endorsed the validity of the counterargument(s), they were to explore associative factors between obesity myths and
unsure that evidence statements would be enough to weight bias in the healthcare and public domains in
change the negative attitudes and beliefs that Canadians are Canada. Therefore, conclusions regarding the relationship
perceived to have towards persons with obesity. Eight of between obesity professionals’ perceptions and the Cana-
the 10 counterarguments were identified as having the dian population as a whole cannot be made. Another study
potential to reduce weight bias by 50% or more of partici- limitation is the low response rate to our web-based survey.
pants. Less than 50% of respondents believed that myths This could be attributed, in part, to technical and meth-
ranked number eight (food bans and taxes are effective odological issues related to conducting web-based surveys.
methods to reduce obesity) and number seven (suggesting The sensitivity of weight bias as a topic may have also
weight targets for a population is important) could be influenced the overall response rate.
positively impacted by the matched evidence-based However, survey respondents included obesity profes-
counterargument. A review of the participant comments sionals working in diverse disciplines and sectors in Canada.
found that it was not the quality of the counterargument Their responses provide us with a broad view of the social,
that lead to a negative response but rather the complexity cultural and institutional contexts in which obesity profes-
of the topic or the participants’ perceptions – that no sionals live and practice. The majority of respondents
matter what evidence existed, those who have negative believed that obesity stereotypes and myths are pervasive
attitudes and beliefs about obesity will continue to support and deeply rooted in social and cultural attitudes and would
taxation of higher fat, higher sugar foods and to promote be challenging to confront. Although, many participants
weight targets as indicative of health. None of the validated and endorsed the proposed counterarguments,
counterarguments were thought to have the potential to they questioned whether dissemination of myths and
increase or endorse the obesity myth statement. The degree counterarguments could actually reduce obesity stigma.
of agreement associated with each counterargument is This is an important finding to consider when developing
listed in Table 1. future obesity stigma reduction initiatives.
Theories on stereotype processing stipulate that stereo-
types are learned associations that are automatically acti-
Discussion
vated when a person comes into contact with a stereotyped
This study produced a list of 10 obesity myths considered group. In addition, non-prejudiced behaviour requires
to be pervasive within the Canadian healthcare and public intentional inhibition of automatically retrieved stereo-
domains. To counteract these myths, 10 relevant evidence- types, which can only be achieved if individuals are moti-
based statements were developed. Although the aim of vated to do so (38). There are internal (e.g. self-image
these counterarguments was to reduce weight bias, many preservation) and external (e.g. social desirability) drivers
obesity experts and professionals surveyed in this study that can motivate individuals to inhibit their automatically
believed the myth–fact messages would not be sufficient to activated stereotypes (39). Intervention studies to influence
change deeply rooted misconceptions about the link automatic stereotype activation have shown mixed results.
between weight and health, characteristics of individuals However, suppression interventions that use some type of
with obesity, and non-evidence-based obesity reduction ‘stereotype negation’ training as well as interventions that
strategies. This indicates that weight bias is deeply perva- promote counter-stereotypes and challenge dominant ste-
sive in Canada, not unlike other countries such as the reotypes show some promise. This demonstrates that
United States (4). counterarguments alone may not be sufficient to motivate
Research stemming as far back as the 1960s has demon- individuals to inhibit their automatic stereotypes. Multi-
strated that stigma has adverse effects on health (36). It is level (intrapersonal, interpersonal and structural)
recognized that policy makers and public health institu- approaches are needed to reduce the effects of stigma
tions should combat stigma because of its adverse health across system levels (40,41).
effects and consequences for social and health inequities Based on a belief that repeating a stereotype, even though
(37). However, despite its prevalence, weight bias and it is counteracted with evidence, may actually strengthen
obesity stigma are rarely challenged in our society, leaving the learned stereotype by both strengthening the associa-
persons who are classified as overweight or obese vulner- tion and weakening the evidence Yeh et al. (2013) argue
able to social injustice, unfair treatment and impaired that it may be better to present just the evidence in order to
quality of life as a result of substantial disadvantages. This create a new learned association and change attitudes (40).
study was a first step towards confronting widely held In their study, Yeh et al. used a 2 × 2 between-subjects
misconceptions and myths about obesity in an effort to factorial study design in which participants (109 under-
ultimately address obesity stigma in Canada. graduate students enrolled in an introductory marketing
Our study is not without limitations that should be class) randomly received either the MFMF or the fact-only
acknowledged. We surveyed obesity professionals in order message format. Since personal relevance was considered to

© 2014 The Authors


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196
194 Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al. clinical obesity

be a potential, motivating factor that can consciously that obesity is self-inflicted and individuals should be held
inhibit the automatically activated stereotype and inten- personally responsible for their weight and health.
tionally activate non-prejudicial beliefs, it was also meas- In 2013, a group of international obesity experts identi-
ured using a three-item scale. Personal relevance was fied weight loss and control-related myths, presumptions
defined as having personal experience with mental illness and facts (42). The authors argue that researchers and
either by directly having a mental illness or having had a obesity professionals must address distortions of research
mental illness or having a close loved one who has or had outcomes that can happen in advocacy work because these
a mental illness. Outcome measures included attitudes distortions can contribute to obesity myths. For example,
towards people with mental illness and perceived learning many evidence-based treatments may be beneficial at the
about mental illness. Familiarity with the content of the individual level but may not be appropriate for obesity
message and age were considered covariates. The results prevention public health approaches. We must be careful to
demonstrated that using fact-only messages is an effective balance public health messages with existing evidence so
way to create positive attitudes towards mental illness. that we do not contribute to unintended consequences for
However, the impact of the fact-only messages is mod- individuals living with obesity, including weight bias and
erated by personal relevance. Although not statistically sig- obesity stigma.
nificant, the study found that perceived learning about In summary, this study produced obesity myth–fact mes-
mental illness was higher among those participants without sages that could be used in future obesity stigma reduction
personal relevance and who received myth/fact messages. interventions. The impact of these counterarguments to
The authors explain that this may be why social marketing reduce weight bias was not tested in this study; however,
campaigns use the myth/fact message since the expected none of the counterarguments were identified as having the
outcome is increased learning about a particular health potential to endorse or enhance weight bias.
issue. Thus, if the goal is to reduce stigma, this may not
be an appropriate method. Considering that over 60% of
Conflict of interest statement
Canadians have overweight and/or obesity, chances are
that most Canadians will have a personal relevance factor. This study was funded through an unrestricted educational
It is therefore important to consider the moderating grant from the Public Health Agency of Canada to the
effect of personal relevance in future weight bias reduction Canadian Obesity Network. The views and opinions
strategies. expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not
A campaign to reduce weight bias may benefit from a necessarily reflect the official policy or position of the
mix of programmes including training interventions such as Public Health Agency of Canada.
‘stereotype negation’ training and training programmes
that promote counter-stereotypes and challenge dominant
stereotypes (39). Social marketing strategies aimed at
Acknowledgements
reducing weight bias may need to be based on fact-only MF and XRS managed this study and oversaw the consul-
messages in order to create new positive views about tations with obesity experts and health professionals. MF,
obesity. Moreover, social marketing strategies that create XRS and AMS conceived the survey and analyzed the data.
empathy for people with obesity by making the issue more All authors have reviewed and approved the complete
personally relevant may also be necessary. Yeh et al. also manuscript (including tables, figures and ancillary
noted that some of the most effective strategies to educate materials).
people without personal relevance are those that create
opportunities for contact with people living or affected by
the stigmatized condition. References
Interestingly, in our study, participants commented on 1. Kirk SFL, Penney TL, McHugh TF, Sharma AM. Effective
the fact that no matter what evidence is presented about the weight management practice: a review of the lifestyle intervention
complexity of obesity, those who have negative attitudes evidence. Int J Obes 2012; 36: 178–185.
and beliefs about obesity will continue to support simplistic 2. Brewis AA, Wutich A, Falleta-Cowden A, Rodriguez-Soto I.
Body norms and fat stigma in global perspective (report). Curr
messages such as ‘eat less, move more, taxations of higher Anthropol 2011; 52: 269–276.
fat, higher sugar foods and the promotion of weight targets 3. Forhan M, Ramos Salas X. Inequities in healthcare: a review of
as indicative of health’. These comments indicate that bias and discrimination in obesity treatment. Can J Diabetes 2013;
obesity stigma also exists among obesity experts and pro- 37: 205–209.
fessionals. More research is needed to explore how the 4. Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and
update. Obesity (19307381) 2009; 17: 941–964.
obesity label has been normalized historically through 5. Sikorski C, Luppa M, Kaiser M et al. The stigma of obesity in
dominant cultural and medical ideologies – mainly the the general public and its implications for public health – a sys-
personal responsibility of health ideology that stipulates tematic review. BMC Public Health 2011; 11: 661–661.

© 2014 The Authors


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196
clinical obesity Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al. 195

6. Cameron KA, Roloff ME, Friesema EM et al. Patient knowl- 25. Thow AM, Jan S, Leeder S, Swinburn B. The effect of fiscal
edge and recall of health information following exposure to ‘facts policy on diet, obesity and chronic disease: a systematic review.
and myths’ message format variations. Patient Educ Couns 2013; Bull World Health Organ 2010; 88: 609–614.
92: 381–387. 26. Shrewsbury VA, Steinbeck KS, Torvaldsen S, Baur LA. The
7. Canadian Foundation for Healthcare Improvement. (2013). role of parents in pre-adolescent and adolescent overweight and
Mythbusters. [WWW document]. URL http://www.cfhi-fcass.ca/ obesity treatment: a systematic review of clinical recommenda-
PublicationsAndResources/Mythbusters.aspx (accessed October tions. Obes Rev 2011; 12: 759–769.
25, 2013). 27. Boutelle KN, Cafri G, Crow SJ. Parent-only treatment for
8. Canadian Mental Health Association. (2013). Myths about childhood obesity: a randomized controlled trial. Obesity
mental illness. [WWW document]. URL http://www.cmha.ca/ (19307381) 2011; 19: 574–580.
mental_health/myths-about-mental-illness/#.UqIEUmRDtVo 28. Collins CE, Okely AD, Morgan PJ et al. Parent diet modifi-
(accessed October 25, 2013). cation, child activity, or both in obese children: an RCT.(clinical
9. McGuire WJ. The effectiveness of supportive and refutational report). Pediatrics 2011; 4: 619.
defenses in immunizing and restoring beliefs against persuasion. 29. Li Y, Dai Q, Jackson JC, Zhang J. Overweight is associated
Sociometry 1961; 24: 184–197. with decreased cognitive functioning among school-age children
10. Colley RC, Garriguet D, Janssen I et al. Physical activity of and adolescents. Obesity 2008; 16: 1809–1815.
Canadian adults: accelerometer results from the 2007 to 2009 30. Karnehed N, Rasmussen F, Hemmingsson T, Tynelius P.
Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Rep 2011; 22: 7– Obesity and attained education: cohort study of more than 700 000
14. Swedish men. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006; 14: 1421–1428.
11. Colley RC, Garriguet D, Janssen I et al. Physical activity of 31. Yu ZB, Han SP, Cao XG, Guo XR. Intelligence in relation to
Canadian children and youth: accelerometer results from the 2007 obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 2010;
to 2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Rep 2011; 22: 11: 656–670.
15–23. 32. Lawlor DA, Clark H, Smith GD, Leon DA. Childhood intel-
12. Mauro M, Taylor V, Wharton S, Sharma AM. Barriers to ligence, educational attainment and adult body mass index: find-
obesity treatment. Eur J Intern Med 2008; 19: 173–180. ings from a prospective cohort and within sibling-pairs analysis.
13. Katzmarzyk PT, Church TS, Janssen I, Ross R, Blair SN. Int J Obes 2006; 30: 1758–1765.
Metabolic syndrome, obesity, and mortality: impact of cardiores- 33. Puhl RM, Luedicke J. Weight-based victimization among ado-
piratory fitness. Diabetes Care 2005; 28: 391–397. lescents in the school setting: emotional reactions and coping
14. Kuk JL, Katzmarzyk PT, Nichaman MZ et al. Visceral fat is behaviors. J Youth Adolesc 2012; 41: 27–40.
an independent predictor of all-cause mortality in men. Obesity 34. Puhl RM. Weight stigmatization toward youth: a significant
2006; 14: 336–341. problem in need of societal solutions. Childhood Obesity 2011; 7:
15. Farrell SW, Braun L, Barlow CE, Cheng YJ, Blair SN. 359.
The relation of body mass index, cardiorespiratory fitness, 35. Crosnoe R. Gender, obesity, and education. Sociol Educ 2007;
and all-cause mortality in women. Obes Res 2002; 10: 417– 80: 241–260.
423. 36. Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled
16. Stevens J, Cai J, Evenson KR, Thomas R. Fitness and fatness Identity/Erving Goffman. Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ,
as predictors of mortality from all causes and from cardiovascular 1963, p. 1963.
disease in men and women in the lipid research clinics study. Am 37. Weiss M, Ramakrishna J, Somma D. Health-related stigma:
J Epidemiol 2002; 156: 832–841. rethinking concepts and interventions 1. Psychol Health Med
17. Despre’s JP, Lemieux I. Abdominal obesity and metabolic 2006; 11: 277–287.
syndrome [electronic resource]. Nature 2006; 444: 881–887. 38. Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and
18. Puhl RM, Brownell KD. Confronting and coping with weight controlled components. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989; 56: 5–18.
stigma: an investigation of overweight and obese adults. Obesity 39. Blair IV. The malleability of automatic stereotypes and preju-
2006; 14: 1802–1815. dice. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2002; 6: 242–
19. Rosenheck R. Fast food consumption and increased caloric 261.
intake: a systematic review of a trajectory towards weight gain and 40. Yeh MA, Jewell RD, Hu MY. Stereotype processing’s effect on
obesity risk. Obes Rev 2008; 9: 535–547. the impact of the myth/fact message format and the role of per-
20. Lau DCW, Obesity Canada Clinical Practice Guidelines Steer- sonal relevance. Psychol Mark 2013; 30: 36–45.
ing Committee and Expert Panel. Synopsis of the 2006 canadian 41. Cook JE, Purdie-Vaughns V, Meyer IH, Busch JTA. Interven-
clinical practice guidelines on the management and prevention of ing within and across levels: a multilevel approach to stigma and
obesity in adults and children. Can Med Assoc J 2007; 176: public health. Soc Sci Med 2014; 103: 101–109.
1103–1108. 42. Casazza K, Fontaine KR, Astrup A et al. Myths, presump-
21. Taylor VH, McIntyre RS, Remington G et al. Beyond phar- tions, and facts about obesity. N Engl J Med 2013; 368: 446–454.
macotherapy: understanding the links between obesity and chronic
mental illness. Can J Psychiatry 2012; 57: 5–12.
22. Foster GD, Wadden TA, Makris AP et al. Primary care phy- Ad hoc group of obesity expert and key
sicians’ attitudes about obesity and its treatment. Obes Res 2003;
opinion leaders
11: 1168–1177.
23. Schvey NA, Puhl RM, Brownell KD. The impact of weight • Dr. Arya M. Sharma, Chair of Obesity Research and
stigma on caloric consumption. Obesity (19307381) 2011; 19: Management, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, Univer-
1957–1962.
24. Backholer K, Walls HL, Magliano DJ, Peeters A. Setting popu-
sity of Alberta
lation targets for measuring successful obesity prevention. Am J • Dr. Rebecca Puhl, Rudd Centre for Food Policy and
Public Health 2010; 100: 2033–2037. Obesity, Yale University

© 2014 The Authors


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196
196 Obesity myths X. Ramos Salas et al. clinical obesity

• Albert Kwan, Policy Analyst, Strategic Initiatives and • Dr. Michael T. Vallis, Associate Professor, Depart-
Innovations Directorate, Public Health Agency of Canada ments of Psychiatry and Adjunct Professor, Department of
• Dr. Mary Forhan, Assistant Professor, Occupational Psychology, Dalhousie University.
Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Medicine, University of
Alberta

© 2014 The Authors


Clinical Obesity © 2014 World Obesity. clinical obesity 4, 189–196

Вам также может понравиться