Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Social Science & Medicine


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/socscimed

Genotype–environment interaction and sociology: Contributions


and complexities
Jamie A. Seabrook a, *, William R. Avison a, b, c, d
a
Departments of Sociology and Paediatrics, The University of Western Ontario, 1151 Richmond Street, London, Ontario, Canada, N6A 3K7
b
Childrens Health Research Institute, Childrens Hospital, London Health Sciences Centre, 800 Commissioners Road East, London, Ontario, Canada N6C 2V5
c
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, The University of Western Ontario, 1151 Richmond Street, London, Ontario, Canada, N6A 3K7
d
Lawson Health Research Institute, London Health Sciences Centre, London, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Genotype–environment interaction (GE) refers to situations in which genetic effects connected to
Available online 12 February 2010 a phenotype are dependent upon variability in the environment, or when genes modify an organism’s
sensitivity to particular environmental features. Using a typology suggested in the GE literature, we
Keywords: provide an overview of recent papers that show how social context can trigger a genetic vulnerability,
Genes compensate for a genetic vulnerability, control behaviors for which a genetic vulnerability exists, and
Environment
improve adaptation via proximal causes. We argue that to improve their understanding of social
Social environment
structure, sociologists can take advantage of research in behavior genetics by assessing the impact of
Medical sociology
Review within-group variance of various health outcomes and complex human behaviors that are explainable by
genotype, environment and their interaction. Insights from life course sociology can aid in ensuring that
the dynamic nature of the environment in GE has been accounted for. Identification of an appropriate
entry point for sociologists interested in GE research could begin with the choice of an environmental
feature of interest, a genetic factor of interest, and/or behavior of interest. Optimizing measurement in
order to capture the complexity of GE is critical. Examining the interaction between poorly measured
environmental factors and well measured genetic variables will overestimate the effects of genetic
variables while underestimating the effect of environmental influences, thereby distorting the interac-
tion between genotype and environment. Although the expense of collecting environmental data is very
high, reliable and precise measurement of an environmental pathogen enhances a study’s statistical
power.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Genotype–environment interaction (GE) refers to situations in and environment make to a particular disease or human behaviour
which genetic effects connected to a phenotype are dependent distorts the estimates of how genes, the environment, and GE
upon variability in the environment, or when genes modify an contribute to disease or behavioural outcomes.
organism’s sensitivity to particular environmental features Despite the recognition that most complex human behaviours
(Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). Although both genetic and environ- and diseases evolve through GE, few empirical studies assessing
mental factors play important roles in the causality of human the interaction between genotype and environment have been
disease as well as complex human behaviours (e.g., problem conducted (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). While much sociological
behaviours, health-related behaviours, motivation, psychopa- research has focused on the impact that environment has had on
thology), the combined interaction between genotype and envi- contributing to mental disorders (Schnittker, 2008), gene research
ronmental factors is greater than their independent effects (Botto & into mental disorders by contrast has largely ignored the contri-
Khoury, 2001; Grigorenko, 2005; Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). This is butions that environmental factors have made (Moffitt, Caspi, &
because assessment of only the separate contributions that genes Rutter, 2005). The assumption in much of genetic research that
direct paths will be found from gene to diseases has not been found
for complex psychiatric disorders (Caspi et al., 2003).
* Corresponding author. The discipline of sociology has traditionally been reluctant
E-mail addresses: jamie.seabrook@lhsc.on.ca, jseabroo@uwo.ca (J.A. Seabrook). about embracing other sources of explanation regarding social

0277-9536/$ – see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.016
1278 J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284

behaviour and disease. For the most part, sociologists have felt isolated from one another (Horwitz, 2007). Durkheim called
attempted to discredit and deny biological explanations about these types of suicides egoistic suicides. He also found that too much
human behaviour and disease, which has resulted in the majority of integration increased the likelihood of altruistic suicides because
sociologists knowing little about alternative causes of behaviour individuals felt overwhelmed by social demands (Horwitz, 2007).
(Udry, 1995). As Schnittker (2008: S235) notes, ‘‘Even among those For Karl Marx, widespread alienation of humans from the
interested in genetic influence, most focus on interpreting herita- products they produce, from the natural world, other people, and
bility appropriately, rather than examining the implications of their own human nature, creates misery and has major negative
heritability for theories of environmental influence.’’ psychological consequences on individuals (Horwitz, 2007). This
Only recently has there been an increased interest in sociology alienation will persist, according to Marx, until private property is
about how genetic differences can explain individual outcomes of abolished and communism emancipates the human essence.
interest to social scientists (Freese, 2008). In a recently published Finally, Max Weber focused on explaining social action in its
article, Shanahan, Vaisey, Erickson, and Smolen (2008) argue that historical context, and emphasized social values, culture, and
given the expertise of sociologists in conceptualizing and meaning of human action. These values are fundamentally social
measuring social context, studies of GE can capitalize on the work because they motivate individuals and contribute to the meaning of
of sociologists in this area. Schnittker (2008) believes that one’s existence (Horwitz, 2007). Further, Weber felt that the spread
analyzing genes will reveal environmental complexity in such of bureaucracy and technocratic ideas in modern society would
a way that sociologists can more appropriately argue the impor- negatively impact psychological well-being, although he was not
tance of environment in research on well-being. Freese (2008) also explicit in his writing on this.
notes how attention to genetic differences will enhance under- Given that biological and genetic determinants of behavior were
standing of the social mechanisms by which genetic differences not well understood in the nineteenth century, Durkheim, Marx,
result in various behaviours. and Weber were more focused on how the mental health of indi-
Understanding GE may also improve methods of disease viduals were shaped by their relationship to large-scale structural
prevention, and provide better treatment after a disease has been processes. Contemporary theoretical and empirical work in soci-
diagnosed. Although there have been many recent unprecedented ology continues to emphasize the importance of social stratification
discoveries in the genomics of such diseases as obesity, prostate and its subsequent impact on health disparities and behavioural
cancer, breast cancer, lymphoblastic leukemia, coronary heart outcomes. The social causation hypothesis is based on the notion
disease, and type 2 diabetes mellitus, the important question for that position in the social structure causes variations in health
sociologists is how genetics can advance sociology assuming that outcomes (Turner, Wheaton, & Lloyd, 1995). These social causes of
individuals do in fact differ in their genetic propensities for health disparities, moreover, can be divided into proximate (life-
complex human behaviour (Guo, Tong, & Cai, 2008). style) and fundamental causes, with the former including such
After providing a basis for understanding sociological theories factors as diet, exercise, and hypertension. According to Link and
on the causes and consequences of health disparities, we will Phelan (1995), the problem with epidemiological research
describe various genotype–environment interactions that have emphasizing proximate risk factors is that it assumes that the
been published from the 1990s to the present using a typology individual has the capability to control his or her fate. Instead, Link
which identifies ways in which the environment can moderate and Phelan argue that it is important to contextualize risk factors by
genetic expression. We then conclude by highlighting some of the understanding why some people are more likely to be exposed to
complexities inherent in genotype–environment interaction individually-based risk factors. Central to their argument is the
research. We argue that the life course perspective in sociology can notion that there are more distal ‘‘fundamental causes’’ of disease,
aid in ensuring that the dynamic nature of the environment in such as socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, gender and social
genotype–environment interaction has been accounted for. support, which determine the extent to which people are able to
adopt protective strategies and avoid risks that contribute to
Sociological theories on health and illness differences in morbidity and mortality. Across a wide range of
illnesses and disease, people with higher levels of knowledge,
A core principle in sociology is that social experience affects the money, power, prestige and social connections are less likely to be
psychological state of individuals. This social experience is derived afflicted by disease (Chang & Lauderdale, 2009; Link & Phelan,
from what sociologists refer to as social structure and social inter- 1995; Luftey & Freese, 2005). An important implication of this is
action. The social structure is stratified in such a way that factors that the association between fundamental social causes and disease
such as socioeconomic status, gender, age, and the life course all will persist when health policy focuses its interventions on
have mental health consequences for individuals. Thus, for reducing proximal causes of disease. The reason for this persistent
example, poverty has been a consistent risk factor for such mental association is that resources are important causes of risk factors
disorders as depression, antisocial personality, substance-use and, henceforth, fundamental causes are associated with many
disorders and anxiety disorders (Muntaner, Borrell, & Chung, disease outcomes via multiple risk-factor mechanisms (Link &
2007). Social interaction, on the other hand, involves social Phelan, 1995).
exchanges between individuals that can be either positive or While social causation examines the impact that social structure
negative. Research on social relationships has found that has on causing health disparities, social selection posits that people
unpleasant interactions between individuals perceived by a recip- who have a physical and/or mental illness are more likely to
ient as unsupportive, critical, and demanding increase the likeli- experience downward mobility throughout the life course (Turner
hood of mental illness, and that negative social interactions et al., 1995). In a study assessing the impact of marital transitions
influence mental health more so than do measures of social support and mental health, Wade and Pevalin (2004) found that separated,
(Evans-Campell, Lincoln, & Takeuchi, 2007). divorced and widowed individuals had poorer mental health than
The founders of sociology were also effective in demonstrating married persons, and that the rate of poor mental health among
how individual attributes were a reflection of structural processes people who experienced a marital disruption was higher than
and social arrangements. In his classic study on suicide rates, Emile married persons prior to the transition of the marital disruption,
Durkheim showed how suicide resulted from deficiencies in social thus providing evidence of both social causation and social selec-
integration (familial, religious, and political), in which individuals tion, respectively. The evidence for social selection also appears
J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284 1279

strong, for example, in people with schizophrenia (Link & Phelan, depression was 14.6%. Those individuals with the highest genetic
1995; Turner et al., 1995). risk for the onset of major depression and who experienced
Differences in exposure to stress are also critical in under- a severe stressful life event were therefore 2.4 times more likely to
standing variations in health outcomes. Turner and Avison (2003) have an onset of major depression than were individuals at the
show that there are social status differences in stress exposure lowest genetic risk and who did not experience a severe stressful
based on such factors as gender, race, and social class and that life event. The observation that some triggering interactions may
differences in mental health are at least partially attributable to involve stressful life events suggests that the stress process para-
systematic differences in stress exposure by people differentially digm (Pearlin, 1989; Pearlin, Lieberman, Menaghan, & Mullan,
situated in the social structure. In other words, exposure to stress is 1981; Pearlin, Schieman, Fazio, & Meersman, 2005) may have an
rooted in structural contexts, consistent with the social causation important contribution to play in understanding certain gene–
hypothesis. For example, adolescents tend to experience more environment interactions.
stressful events than older people, and unmarried people report It is also useful to examine the long-term patterns of social risk
higher occurrences of undesirable life events than do married factors and their impact on depression. For example, adult children
persons (Turner et al., 1995). of parental divorce not only have higher risk for depression, but
It seems clear that sociological theory and research has signifi- also have lower levels of socioeconomic status, and are more likely
cantly advanced our understanding of the ways in which social to divorce and remarry (McLeod, 1991; O’Connor, Thorpe, Dunn, &
structure and social experiences affect health. Udry (1995) has Golding, 1999; Ross & Mirowsky, 1999). Two studies further show
argued persuasively that biological processes might also be usefully that the association between parental divorce and depression in
integrated into sociological perspectives on health and illness. adulthood is largely explained by hostile parental conflict during
We argue that to advance their understanding of social struc- childhood as well as poor parent–child relations, particularly
ture, sociologists can take advantage of research in behavior neglect (Harris, Brown, & Bifulco, 1990; O’Connor et al., 1999).
genetics by assessing the impact of within-group variance of Although these studies establish relationships between childhood
various health outcomes and complex human behaviours that are adversities and adult depression, perhaps more important is the
explainable by genotype, environment and their interaction. identification of pathways which contribute to these life course
Furthermore, the sociological study of genetic data can provide adversities that were first experienced in childhood.
insight on how the environment can mediate and moderate the Taylor (1995: 67) uses the term ‘‘historical constructionism’’ to
effect of genetic dispositions on stratification-related outcomes connote that ‘‘science in the making is a process of agents building
(Shanahan et al., 2008). It is to genotype–environment interactions by combining a diversity of components’’. He maintains that the
that we now turn. social origins of severe depression among women are the result of
a combination of social class, familial characteristics, psychological
Genotype–environment interactions factors, and possibly genetic vulnerability, all of which coexist
throughout the life course. Specifically, the loss of or prolonged
The study of GE requires information on both genetic vulner- separation from the mother when the woman was a child, a severe
ability as well as environmental factors because although a genetic adverse event in the year prior to depression onset, the lack of
effect can influence the phenotype of an organism, this genetic a caring partner, and persistently difficult living conditions are four
effect can be altered by environmental conditions. Genetic key factors identified in adult women with severe depression. What
vulnerability can be inferred from direct analysis of DNA sequence, Taylor advocates is that social, psychological and biological factors
family history, and phenotype. Environmental factors are typically which contribute to the origins of depression should not be thought
measured using self-report through such methods as interviews, of as separate contributing causes, but rather causal links or path-
questionnaires, direct measurement of participants, or records ways that build on each other. These increase the likelihood of
(Hunter, 2005). depression in later life among some women. Moreover, if depres-
In their review of GE, Shanahan and Hofer (2005) propose sion is the result of a combination of heterogeneous components
a typology identifying four ways by which environment moderates that build on one another, this undercuts the conventional
gene expression. The authors assert that social context can trigger dichotomy of characterizing a given outcome as either purely
a genetic vulnerability, compensate for a genetic vulnerability, genetic or purely environmental.
control behaviours for which a genetic vulnerability exists, and The growing interest in epigenetics is also relevant to any
improve adaptation via proximal causes. consideration of GE research. In their informative review, Rutter,
Moffitt, and Caspi (2006) examine the role that epigenetic effects
Triggering interaction may have in understanding the etiology of psychopathology.
Jaenisch and Bird’s (2003) classic work on the chemical bases of gene
A triggering interaction is a situation in which a genetic expression demonstrated how methylation influenced this process.
vulnerability is only expressed in the presence of a particular This set the stage for a substantial growth in research focusing on the
stressor or trigger (Shanahan & Boardman, 2009). Several exam- ways in which environmental exposures might alter gene expres-
ples exist in the literature demonstrating the triggering interaction sion. Perhaps the most compelling work on epigenetic influences on
of social context to a genetic vulnerability. For example, Kendler psychopathology has been a series of studies by Meaney and his
et al. (1995) examined the etiology of major depression by colleagues (Cameron et al., 2005; Champagne et al., 2004) in which
assessing the interaction between genetic liability and stressful life maternal behaviors of rats – nursing, licking, and grooming – affect
events in a population-based sample of female twins. The authors gene expression among offspring in the brain regions that control
found that for individuals at lowest genetic risk (monozygotic defensive and reproductive behaviors.
twins with unaffected co-twins), the risk of onset of major It remains to be seen whether epigenetics research among
depression in the month of a severe stressful life event was 6.2% human subjects will be as fruitful. There are several methodological
compared to .5% in individuals at lowest genetic risk who did not and ethical obstacles to studying the epigenetics of human
experience a severe stressful life event. However, for those at psychopathology that may make it extremely difficult to generate
highest genetic risk (monozygotic twins with affected co-twins), in convincing results. Nevertheless, it is theoretically important and
the month of a severe stressful life event, the risk of onset of major intriguing to continue to think about the possibilities that
1280 J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284

environmental experiences and exposures might alter the expres- attenuated in both black and white boys by having a teacher who is
sion of genes among humans. also a mentor.
Although sensitivity to stressful life events may be dependent on Other recent evidence has shown how positive social environ-
an individual’s genetic makeup, less is known about whether ments can prevent the expression of a genetic vulnerability. Guo
specific genes intensify or buffer the effects of stressful life events on et al. (2008) found that among white males, the 10R genotype in
depression. In a prospective longitudinal study, Caspi et al. (2003) the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) is associated with higher
found that a functional polymorphism in the promoter region of numbers of sexual partners relative to 9R/9R genotype. However,
the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) was found to alter environmental even with the 10R genotype among males, there was a substantial
life stress on depression. Specifically, those individuals with one or and negative relationship between those who attended church
two copies of the short allele of the 5-HTT promoter polymorphism services on a weekly basis and their total number of sexual partners.
were more likely to be experience depressive symptoms, a diagnosis Specifically, those who attended church on a weekly basis reported
of major depression, and suicide ideation or attempt in relation to 60% fewer sexual partners than those who had never attend church
life stress when compared to individuals with two copies of the long services or those who attended on an infrequent basis only.
allele. What makes this finding unique from a sociological Using data from the Collaborative Study of the Genetics of
perspective is that it found that the apparent link between stressful Alcoholism (COGA), Pescosolido et al. (2008) found evidence for
life events and depression occurred almost exclusively in individuals a modest but significant increase in the likelihood of being diag-
with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HTT promoter nosed with alcohol dependence when having the GABRA2 gene
polymorphism. As a result of this finding, Shanahan et al. (2008: 2) (OR ¼ 1.26, p < .01). Interestingly, however, it was only for men with
argue that ‘‘the challenge then becomes the identification of social a high-risk genotype on GABRA2 that resulted in a higher probability
psychological mechanisms by which the risky variant combines of alcohol dependence. That is, women had the same probability of
with life events to increase depression’’. alcohol dependence whether or nor they had a high or low risk on
Another example demonstrating the triggering interaction of GABRA2, suggesting an important genotype-by-gender interaction.
social context to a genetic vulnerability can be found in a recent It is also possible that other factors may help to explain this apparent
study assessing the role of genotype in the cycle of violence in genotype-by-gender interaction. For example, gender differences in
maltreated children. In this study, Caspi et al. (2002) examined the alcohol dependence may be attributable to different messages that
impact of childhood maltreatment in males from birth to adulthood males and females receive during adolescent socialization about
to see why it is that some maltreated children experience antisocial how they are supposed to behave. Research indicates that adoles-
behaviour (conduct disorder, disposition toward violence, convic- cent females tend to receive messages about putting others first to
tions for violent offenses, and antisocial personality disorder the exclusion of the self, while males learn about the importance of
symptoms) in adulthood while others who were also maltreated in putting one’s own interests first (Rosenfield, Lennon, & White,
childhood do not. Using a functional polymorphism in the 2005). As a result of very different self-salient messages received
promoter of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene to charac- via socialization, males tend to respond to distress externally
terize genetic susceptibility to childhood maltreatment, the authors whereas women are more likely to respond to distress by experi-
found that severely maltreated males with low levels of MAOA encing internalizing problems. Specifically, women tend to respond
expression were more likely to develop antisocial problems in to distress by experiencing depressive symptoms and anxiety, while
young adulthood than were severely maltreated males with men are more predisposed to alcohol abuse and antisocial behaviour
a genotype demonstrating high levels of MAOA activity. The (Rosenfield et al., 2005).
authors suggest that high levels of MAOA are able to sufficiently Another interesting finding in Pescosolido et al.’ study (2008)
metabolize high levels of neurotransmitters. Thus, the association was that the predicted probabilities of alcohol dependence were
between childhood maltreatment and antisocial behaviour in much lower when individuals perceived themselves as having high
young adulthood is conditional on the child’s MAOA genotype. levels of social support from family members. This coincides with
The impact of childhood deprivation, as measured by not having other research that has shown the importance of perceived support
enough food to eat because of poverty, has also been shown to on positively affecting an individual’s adjustment, health, and well-
interact with genotype in research on alcoholism. For example, one being (Hartwell & Benson, 2007). It also suggests a possible buff-
study revealed that among men having the high-risk GABRA2 ering mechanism among those individuals at high risk on GABRA2
genotype for alcohol dependence, the predicted probability of for alcohol dependence when they perceive themselves as having
being diagnosed with alcohol abuse was much higher for individ- high levels of family social support. In fact, in a context of strong
uals who experienced childhood deprivation compared to those familial support, the increased probability of alcohol dependence
who did not (Pescosolido, Perry, Long, & Martin, 2008). among men with the GABRA2 gene is virtually eliminated
(Pescosolido et al., 2008).
Social compensation
Social control
It is also possible to thwart the expression of a genetic vulner-
ability through a positive social context. Focusing on DRD2, a gene Whereas social compensation refers to avoiding low levels of
on chromosome 11 (q23.1) which aids in the functioning of neural functioning as a result of an enriched environmental setting or the
circuits in the brain, Shanahan et al. (2008) found that although absence of a stressor, the social control GE negates the genetic
DRD2 risk increases the likelihood of discontinuing education after expression of a diathesis as a result of social norms and structures
secondary school in both white and black boys, high parental that limit people’s behaviour and choices (Shanahan & Boardman,
socioeconomic status, high parental involvement in school, and 2009). Returning again to research on alcohol dependence,
attending a high quality school helps compensate for this rela- Pescosolido et al. (2008) found that there was no difference in alcohol
tionship. Likewise, in a similar study assessing the role of mentors dependence between women at a low or high risk for alcohol abuse.
and the likelihood of continuing education beyond secondary Because of women’s lower alcohol tolerance, these findings can be
school, Shanahan, Erickson, Vaisey, and Smolen (2007) found that interpreted to demonstrate the effectiveness of social regulation in
the negative relationship between DRD2 risk allele and the prob- curbing a genetic vulnerability to alcohol abuse. Similarly, Shanahan
ability of continuing education beyond high school was fully and Hofer (2005) note that the suppressive effect of the ALDH2*2
J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284 1281

genotype in inhibiting alcoholism, coupled with higher prevalence of phenomenon. Although genes can be expressed at specific times
this genotype among Jews, may be one reason why Jews drink less in development, a cross-sectional conceptualization and measure-
than Caucasians. However, the authors suggest that the inhibitory ment of social context does not capture the dynamic properties of
effect of ADH2*2 could be contingent on environment. For instance, environment which determine the meaning of social experiences
the effect of ADH2*2 in suppressing alcohol consumption was much (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). Turner and colleagues (Turner & Avison,
larger among Israeli Ashkenazi and Sephardic Jews than among 2003; Turner et al., 1995) report that less than 10 percent of the
Russian Jews, the latter of whom were exposed to heavy drinking variance in indices of mental health and well-being can be accoun-
prior to their immigration (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). ted for by examination of life events. One reason that the magnitude
Other examples of social control GE can be found. Providing of the relationship between life events and distress is modest is
evidence showing that obesity has risen in the adult U.S. population because stressful life events constitute only one dimension of
from 4% in the 1890s to almost 33% at the turn of the twenty-first stressful experience. It may also be the case, however, that stressors
century, Martin (2008: S68) argues that the assertion from are contingent on prior, contemporaneous, and subsequent indi-
behavioural genetics research that the intergenerational correla- vidual experiences (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). Insights from life
tion in weight is solely the result of genetic vulnerability does not course sociology, therefore, can aid at ensuring that the dynamic
account for the social, technological, and economical changes nature of the environment in GE has been accounted for.
occurring over the last century, as well as the fact that underlying The four types of GE – contextual triggering, social context
genetic vulnerability could not have changed that dramatically in as a compensation, social control, and social enhancement are
such a short timeframe. Moreover, although Martin found that 74% also likely to differ in their meaning depending upon their
of the variation in body mass index could be accounted for by temporal qualities, synchronization across domains, and timing
genetic vulnerability, families’ behavioral and social characteristics in an individual’s life (Shanahan & Boardman, 2009). Childhood
were also important. For example, even when adolescents had and adolescence, for example, are crucial periods of risk for
a vulnerability to be obese, this expression was reduced when mental health and illness. While such childhood traumas as
adolescents ate three meals per day, were physically active, and sexual abuse, physical abuse, parental death, and parental
when their parents’ socioeconomic status was high. divorce can have proximal effects on mental illness, they can
also have distal effects in that they are also significantly related
Social enhancement to mental disorders in middle and later adulthood (George,
2007). These childhood traumas and adversities therefore can
Finally, a GE enhancement interaction is one in which the set trajectories of periods of mental illness throughout the life
environment accentuates a ‘‘positive’’ genetic tendency (Shanahan course.
& Hofer, 2005). Using data from the white male DNA sample in the If substantial progress is to be made in GE research, it may be
National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, Guo et al. (2008) important for scientists to develop fine-grained measures of social
show that the protective factor of the 9R/9R genotype relative to the and environmental experiences that can be precisely mapped. One
Any10 R genotype depends on the percentage of adolescents in such promising approach is dynamic systems theory (DS). DS,
school that have already had sex by the age of 16. For instance, a mathematical language used to study how a system changes over
although adolescents with the 9R/9R genotype report only about time, has been used to examine process-level accounts of system
63% as many sexual partners as adolescents with Any10 R, this behavior organization and structure (Granic & Hollenstein, 2003;
protective factor is virtually lost once 50% of students have had sex Granic & Lamey, 2002; Granic, Hollenstein, Dishion, & Patterson,
by age 16. If only 25% of students have had sex by age 16, those 2003). Four principles central to the DS framework are the
adolescents with the 9R/9R genotype only report about one sexual discontinuous nature of change in developmental systems, the
partner, while those with Any10 R have on average four sexual emergence of novelty through self-organization processes,
partners. Thus we see the importance of school culture and its role changing patterns in real-time behavior and their relation to
in interacting with genotype. Similarly, the protective effect of 9R/ changes in developmental patterns, and how increases in vari-
9R is also conditional on cognitive ability. Interestingly, while ability are representative of a less stable system and one that is
cognitive ability was not significantly associated with total number poised to change (Granic & Hollenstein, 2003). A relatively new
of sexual partners, the protective effect of 9R/9R with respect to approach in DS studies under development in psychology is state
lowers numbers of sexual partners was conditional on having space grids. A state space is a topographical map that represents
average to higher levels of cognitive ability. many behavioral possibilities for a given system, and is a hypo-
In a study comparing twins and non-twin siblings, Rowe, thetical landscape of behaviors that have become stabilized over
Jacobson, and Van den Oord (1999) report that the genetic poten- development (Granic et al., 2003). Granic and Lamey (2002) used
tial for verbal intelligence is much greater among high-education state space grids to compare the diversity of mother–child inter-
households than among low-education households. When the actions in aggressive children exhibiting ‘‘pure’’ externalizing
average educational level of both parents exceeded high school, the behavior and those with ‘‘mixed’’ externalizing and internalizing
heritability of verbal intelligence was .74 among offspring problems. The results revealed that although both of the ‘‘pure’’ and
compared to only .26 when parents had less than a high school ‘‘mixed’’ dyads engaged in a permissive pattern (child hostile–
education. The authors argue that heritability is much higher parent neutral/positive) during a problem-solving session, only the
among offspring of higher educated parents because the latter are mixed dyads became mutually hostile after a perturbation was
able to provide better proximal causes. These might include such implemented. This suggests that changing interactional contexts
factors as greater parental involvement in their children’s educa- can reveal variability in behavioral outcomes. Furthermore,
tion or the opportunity for their children to attend a high quality although genetic influences likely play a role in the development of
school (Shanahan et al., 2008). subtypes of aggressive children, state space grids offer a strategy to
study heterogeneous, interactional processes between parents and
Complexities of genotype–environment interactions children, which in turn are associated with child psychopathology
development (Granic & Lamey, 2002). Thus, fine-grained
One important limitation in GE research has been the concep- measurement of social interactions can be combined with genetic
tualization and measurement of social context as a cross-sectional information to estimate potential GE interactions more precisely.
1282 J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284

Another limitation in much GE research has been the study of context should consider the dynamic properties of the environ-
just one dimension of social context and a single indicator of ment, that longitudinal studies on the same subjects are essential,
genetic risk (Grigorenko, 2005; Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). The that measures should be age-appropriate, and that social context
problem with such an approach is that it artificially minimizes the should be categorized as distal or proximal (Shanahan & Boardman,
effect of both contextual factors and genetic influence, resulting in 2009).
an oversimplification of interactions. It is likely that the majority of Distal environmental factors include such influences as socio-
mental illnesses and complex human behaviors depend on economic status, race/ethnicity and gender. These distal factors
a variety of combinations of genes, as well as multiple and alternate determine the extent to which people are able to adopt protective
interactions between environmental influences and genetic factors strategies and avoid risks that contribute to differences in
(Grigorenko, 2005; Schooler, 2007; Shanahan & Hofer, 2005). morbidity and mortality (Link & Phelan, 1995). Distal effects of
Indeed, one of the current controversies surrounding the value of social disadvantage have profound consequences on children’s
genome-wide association studies is that although more than 250 mental health and disorder and evidence suggests that these distal
genetic loci have been identified between 2007 and 2009 in which effects function through proximal parent–child relationships
common genetic variants occur that are reproducibly related to (Avison, 1999; Moffitt et al., 2005). Avison (1999) argues that
polygenic traits (Hirschhorn, 2009), only modest effect sizes of studies of intergenerational transmission of mental illness that do
common variants have been found, thus contributing to only not consider distal social factors are more apt to overestimate the
a small fraction of heritability (Goldstein, 2009; Hirschhorn, 2009; effects of parental illness on the mental health of their children. He
Kraft & Hunter, 2009). Moreover, Goldstein (2009) argues that also highlights the importance of careful measurement of such
genome-wide association studies will likely yield too many loci environmental variables as social status, family processes, social
resulting in the possibility of every gene in the genome be impli- stressors, and psychosocial resources so that sociologists and
cated, with the end result being limited biological insights. Using geneticists can more accurately estimate how much variance in the
the term gene–environment correlation (rGE), which is the intergenerational transmission of mental illness can be attributed
tendency for people to experience particular social contexts that to genetic influences. According to Moffitt et al. (2005), proximal
are correlated with their genotype, Shanahan and Hofer (2005) environmental influences, which directly impact the individual
maintain that interpreting bivariate correlations between one through social and physical exposures, are more ideal than distal
genotype and one contextual factor can be misleading given the risk factors for studying GE interaction because they are more apt
strong correlations that are often found between contextual vari- to be eligible for pathogen status and to be included in biological
ables. These correlated environmental variables co-occur and work hypotheses which test their influence on neurobiological systems
interactively and are more apt to provide a realistic view of context that act as mediators of psychiatric symptoms.
given the complex nature through which environmental factors Although the expense of collecting environmental data is very
moderate genetic expression. Thus, while rGE is often estimated high, reliable and precise measurement of an environmental
with a Pearson correlation coefficient to assess the strength and pathogen enhances a study’s statistical power. As sample size
direction of a linear relationship between two variables, in reality depends on allele frequency, the magnitude of the interaction term,
the bivariate correlation does not address the dynamic processes and the strength of the relationship between an environmental
through which causal associations extend from genotype to context exposure and outcome, simulations have revealed 20-fold differ-
to behaviour (Shanahan & Boardman, 2009). ences in sample size between unreliable versus reliable measure-
Although the foregoing highlights some of the challenges and ments (Moffitt et al., 2005).
limitations in previously conducted GE research, Shanahan and Finally, the study design of GE research is also very important.
Boardman (2009) have suggested ways to proceed in terms of Shanahan and Boardman (2009) insist that more effective retro-
empirical research. Emphasizing the importance of studying social spective measurement strategies are necessary. They argue that the
context from a life course perspective, the authors recommend multifaceted nature of social context should be captured before the
ways to identify an entry point for purposes of analysis, and to commencement of data collection since recall bias can be particu-
optimize measurement to capture complexity. larly challenging the farther back one is expected to remember.
Beginning with identification of an entry point, Shanahan and Additionally, selection bias can be problematic when the use of
Boardman (2009) suggest the value of choosing a feature of envi- controls in case-control studies does not represent the population
ronmental interest, a genetic factor of interest, and/or behaviour of from which the cases were drawn. For instance, if substantial
interest. In evaluating environmental factors, it is important to difference exists in the ethnicity between the cases and the
ensure that an environmental factor has a causal relationship with controls, differences in ethnicity will produce spurious relation-
the predicted behaviour, that there is considerable variability in ships with gene variants (Hunter, 2005).
how people respond to the specific environmental factor and that
there is a plausible link between human biology and the environ- Genetics and social change
ment. Likewise, if first identifying a genetic candidate or behaviour
of interest, life course sociologists would follow the same pattern as Less than two decades ago, Lippman (1991, 1992) used the term
those beginning with a feature of environmental interest – namely, ‘geneticization’ to refer to the interplay among genetics, medicine,
causality, variance, and plausibility. and culture. According to Lippman and others (Rothman, 1998;
Optimizing measurement in order to capture the complexity of Nelkin & Lindee, 1995; Shakespeare, 2006), as the public became
GE is also critical. For example, examining the interaction more aware of genetic research, relatively uncritical acceptance of
between poorly measured environmental factors and well the promise of genetics and molecular biology to address problems
measured genetic variables will overestimate the effects of genetic of disease, health, and ‘deviant’ behaviour resulted in changes in
variables while underestimating the effect of environmental cultural belief systems that stressed genetic determinism and
influences, thereby distorting the interaction between genotype redefined individual identity in terms of DNA and genomics.
and environment. As Schooler (2007: 62) correctly notes, ‘‘Good Although there has certainly been widespread public accep-
measurement requires good understanding of the processes being tance of the promise of the genetics revolution, other authors have
measured and good understanding of such processes depends on been somewhat more critical in their assessment of the concept of
good basic research’’. This means that measurement of social geneticization. Hedgecoe (1998) has argued that there is little
J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284 1283

evidence of the wholesale geneticization of society and suggests, tendency. These four types of genotype–environment interaction
instead, that the assertion that genetics permeates society is largely are likely to differ in their meaning depending upon their temporal
polemical. Instead, Hedgecoe (2001) suggests that a more nuanced qualities, synchronization across domains, and timing in an indi-
or ‘enlightened geneticization’ has emerged in which scientists vidual’s life.
subtly give priority to genetic explanations of diseases such as Although there is likely a genetically heritable component to
schizophrenia. most outcomes of interest to social scientists, sociologists can
Others have drawn attention to the ways in which the concept of uniquely show how genomic causation is a product of social
geneticization may have considerable similarity to the process of conditions (Freese, 2008). Unlike geneticists who primarily study
medicalization (Shakespeare, 2006; ten Have, 2001). In ten Have’s the main effects of genes, sociologists can benefit from genetic
view, geneticization may be a valuable cultural tool for focusing information by investigating how social context and genotype
public debate on the ethical and moral issues surrounding the use depend on one another. By doing so, we believe that sociologists
of genetic knowledge. Shakespeare (2006) maintains that will develop a more thorough understanding of social context by
commercial interests may see business potential in genetic diag- knowledge of its mediation and moderation effects of genetic
nosis, gene therapy, and pharmacogenetics by providing an vulnerability on various health outcomes. Isolating genetically
incentive to promote the biological basis of human behaviors and distinct groups will also aid in understanding which social contexts
socially stigmatized diseases. Although the promotion of organic matter for those at high risk (Shanahan et al., 2008). Finally,
and genetic causes of conditions may undermine the importance of insights from life course sociology can aid at ensuring that the
complex social causes of health and illness, it may work to remove dynamic nature of the environment in GE has been accounted for.
some of the blame that is often attributed to individuals with
various health conditions (Shakespeare, 2006).
References
More recently, Freese and Shostak (2009) have written exten-
sively on the interplay between genetics and social research. They Avison, W. R. (1999). The impact of mental illness on the family. In C. S. Aneshensel,
have made some perceptive comments with respect to genetici- & J. C. Phelan (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of mental health (pp. 495–515).
zation and the public belief in the importance of genetic causes. New York: Plenum Press.
Botto, L. D., & Khoury, M. J. (2001). Commentary: facing the challenge of gene–
They point out that a distinction can be made between genetici- environment interaction: the two-by-four table and beyond. American Journal
zation and genetic determinism and suggest that advances in the of Epidemiology, 153(10), 1016–1020.
study of gene–environment interactions and epigenetics may lead Cameron, N. M., Champagne, F. A., Parent, C., Fish, E. W., Ozaki-Kuroda, K., &
Meaney, M. J. (2005). The programming of individual differences in defensive
social scientists to reconsider the concept of geneticization.
responses and reproductive strategies in the rat through variations in maternal
Freese and Shostak also argue that the public’s understanding of care. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 843–865.
genetics is somewhat more sophisticated than some observers have Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of
suggested. Indeed, Shostak, Conrad, and Horwitz (2008) have genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582),
851–854.
argued that the public opinion importantly affects the way in which Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003).
findings about genetic influences are interpreted. In a similar vein, Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the
Phelan (2005) has shown that there is a complicated relationship 5-htt gene. Science, 301(5631), 386–389.
Champagne, F., Chretien, P., Stevenson, C. W., Zhang, T. Y., Gratton, A., &
between genetic attributions of mental illness and stigma that is Meaney, M. J. (2004). Variations in nucleus accumbens dopamine associated
difficult to disentangle. with individual differences in maternal behavior in the rat. Journal of Neuro-
All of these considerations suggest that the social and cultural science, 24, 4113–4123.
Chang, V. W., & Lauderdale, D. S. (2009). Fundamental cause theory, technological
consequences of genetic research are likely to produce social and innovation, and health disparities: the case of cholesterol in the era of Statins.
cultural changes, especially in public beliefs and in discourse about Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 50, 245–260.
social and public health policy. Thus, advances in genetic research Evans-Campell, T., Lincoln, K. D., & Takeuchi, D. T. (2007). Race and mental health: past
debates, new opportunities. In W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido
seem destined to provide social scientists with a rich source of
(Eds.), Mental health, social mirror (pp. 169–189). New York: Springer.
research topics that range from epigenetics and gene–environment Freese, J. (2008). Genetics and the social science explanation of individual
interactions to the nature of geneticization and its consequences for outcomes. American Journal of Sociology, 114, S1–S35.
Freese, J., & Shostak, S. (2009). Genetics and social inquiry. Annual Review of Soci-
cultural belief systems.
ology, 35, 107–128.
George, L. K. (2007). Life course perspectives on social factors and mental illness. In
Summary and conclusions W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido (Eds.), Mental health, social mirror
(pp. 191–218). New York: Springer.
Goldstein, D. B. (2009). Common genetic variation and human traits. New England
Genotype–environment interaction (GE) refers to situations in Journal of Medicine, 360, 1696–1698.
which genetic effects connected to a phenotype are dependent Granic, I., & Hollenstein, T. (2003). Dynamic systems method for models of devel-
upon variability in the environment, or when genes modify an opmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 641–669.
Granic, I., Hollenstein, T., Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (2003). Longitudinal
organism’s sensitivity to particular environmental features. The analysis of flexibility and reorganization in early adolescence: a dynamic
study of GE requires information on both genetic vulnerability as systems study of family interactions. Developmental Psychology, 39, 606–617.
well as environmental factors. Genetic vulnerability can be inferred Granic, I., & Lamey, A. V. (2002). Combining dynamic systems and multivariate
analyses to compare the mother–child interactions of externalizing subtypes.
from direct analysis of DNA sequence, family history, and pheno- Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 265–283.
type. Environmental factors are typically measured using self- Grigorenko, E. L. (2005). The inherent complexities of gene–environment interac-
report through such methods as interviews, questionnaires, direct tions. Journals of Gerontology Series B – Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,
60, 53–64.
measurement of participants, or records. Guo, G., Tong, Y., & Cai, T. (2008). Gene by social context interactions for number of
A triggering interaction occurs when a genetic vulnerability is sexual partners among white male youths: genetics-informed sociology.
expressed in the presence of a stressor or trigger. Whereas social American Journal of Sociology, 114, S36–S66.
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1990). Loss of parent in childhood and adult
compensation refers to avoiding low levels of functioning as
psychiatric disorder: a tentative overall model. Development and Psychopa-
a result of an enriched environmental setting or the absence of thology, 2, 311–328.
a stressor, the social control GE negates the genetic expression of Hartwell, S. W., & Benson, P. R. (2007). Social integration: a conceptual overview
a diathesis as a result of social norms and structures that limit and two case studies. In W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido (Eds.),
Mental health, social mirror (pp. 329–353). New York: Springer.
people’s behaviour and choices. An enhancement interaction is one ten Have, H. A. M. J. (2001). Genetics and culture: the geneticization thesis. Medi-
in which the environment accentuates a ‘‘positive’’ genetic cine, Health Care and Philosophy, 4, 295–304.
1284 J.A. Seabrook, W.R. Avison / Social Science & Medicine 70 (2010) 1277–1284

Hedgecoe, A. (1998). Geneticization, medicalisation and polemics. Medicine, Health Phelan, J. C. (2005). Geneticization of deviant behavior and consequences for
Care and Philosophy, 1, 235–243. stigma: the case of mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46,
Hedgecoe, A. (2001). Schizophrenia and the narrative of enlightened geneticization. 307–322.
Social Studies of Science, 31, 875–911. Rosenfield, S., Lennon, M. C., & White, H. R. (2005). The self and mental health: self-
Hirschhorn, J. N. (2009). Genomewide association studies – illuminating biological salience and the emergence of internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal
pathways. New England Journal of Medicine, 360, 1699–1701. of Health and Social Behavior, 46(2), 323–340.
Horwitz, A. V. (2007). Classical sociology theory, evolutionary psychology, and Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1999). Parental divorce, life course disruption, and adult
mental health. In W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido (Eds.), Mental depression. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 1034–1045.
health, social mirror (pp. 67–93). New York: Springer. Rothman, B. K. (1998). The book of life: A personal and ethical guide to race, normality,
Hunter, D. J. (2005). Gene–environment interactions in human diseases. Nature and the implications of the human genome project. Boston: Beacon Press.
Reviews Genetics, 6(4), 287–298. Rowe, D. C., Jacobson, K. C., & Van den Oord, E. J. C. G. (1999). Genetic and envi-
Jaenisch, R., & Bird, A. (2003). Epigenetic regulation of gene expression: how the ronmental influences on vocabulary IQ: parental education level as moderator.
genome integrates intrinsic and environmental signals. Nature Genetics Child Development, 70(5), 1151–1162.
Supplement, 33, 245–254. Rutter, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2006). Gene–environment interplay and
Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., MacLean, C., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., et al. psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology
(1995). Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major and Psychiatry, 47, 226–261.
depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 833–842. Schnittker, J. (2008). Happiness and success: genes, families, and the psychological
Kraft, P., & Hunter, D. J. (2009). Genetic risk prediction – are we there yet? New effects of socioeconomic position and social support. American Journal of Soci-
England Journal of Medicine, 360, 1701–1703. ology, 114, S233–S259.
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Schooler, C. (2007). The changing role(s) of sociology (and psychology) in the
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Extra Issue, 80–94. national institute of mental health intramural research program. In
Lippman, A. (1991). Prenatal genetic testing and screening: constructing needs and W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido (Eds.), Mental health, social mirror
reinforcing inequalities. American Journal of Law and Medicine, 17, 15–50. (pp. 55–63). New York: Springer.
Lippman, A. (1992). Led (astray) by genetic maps: the cartography of the human Shakespeare, T. (2006). The dilemma of predictable disablement: a challenge for
genome and health care. Social Science & Medicine, 35, 1469–1476. families and society. In G. T. H. Ellison, & A. H. Goodman (Eds.), The nature of
Luftey, K., & Freese, J. (2005). Toward some fundamentals of fundamental causality: difference: Science, society and human biology (pp. 119–134). Florida: Taylor &
socioeconomic status and health in the routine clinic visit for diabetes. Amer- Francis Group.
ican Journal of Sociology, 110, 1326–1372. Shanahan, M. J., & Boardman, J. D. (2009). Gene–environment interplay across the
Martin, M. A. (2008). The intergenerational correlation in weight: how genetic life course: overview and problematics at a new frontier. In J. Z. Giele, &
resemblance reveals the social role of families. American Journal of Sociology, G. H. Elder, Jr. (Eds.), Methods of life course research: Qualitative and quantitative
114, S67–S105. approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McLeod, J. D. (1991). Childhood parental loss and adult depression. Journal of Health Shanahan, M. J., Erickson, L. D., Vaisey, S., & Smolen, A. (2007). Helping relationships
and Social Behavior, 32, 205–220. and genetic propensities: a combinatoric study of drd2, mentoring, and
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2005). Strategy for investigating interactions educational continuation. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10(2), 285–292.
between measured genes and measured environments. Archives of General Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment inter-
Psychiatry, 62(5), 473–481. actions: retrospect and prospect. Journals of Gerontology Series B – Psychological
Muntaner, C., Borrell, C., & Chung, H. (2007). Class relations, economic inequality Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, 65–76.
and mental health: why social class matters to the sociology of mental health. Shanahan, M. J., Vaisey, S., Erickson, L. D., & Smolen, A. (2008). Environmental
In W. R. Avison, J. D. McLeod, & B. A. Pescosolido (Eds.), Mental health, social contingencies and genetic propensities: social capital, educational continuation,
mirror (pp. 127–141). New York: Springer. and dopamine receptor gene drd2. American Journal of Sociology, 114, S260–S286.
Nelkin, D., & Lindee, M. S. (1995). The DNA mystique: The gene as a cultural icon. New Shostak, S., Conrad, P., & Horwitz, A. V. (2008). Sequencing and its consequences:
York: Freeman. path dependence and the relationships between genetics and medicalization.
O’Connor, T. G., Thorpe, K., Dunn, J., & Golding, J. (1999). Parental divorce and American Journal of Sociology, 114, S287–S316.
adjustment in adulthood: findings from a community sample. Journal of Child Taylor, P. J. (1995). Building on construction: an exploration of heterogeneous
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 777–789. constructionism, using an analogy from psychology and a sketch from socio-
Pearlin, L. I. (1989). The sociological study of stress. Journal of Health and Social economic modeling. Perspectives on Science, 3, 66–98.
Behavior, 30, 241–256. Turner, R. J., & Avison, W. R. (2003). Status variations in stress exposure among
Pearlin, L. I., Lieberman, M. A., Menaghan, E. G., & Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress young adults: implications for the interpretation of prior research. Journal of
process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337–356. Health and Social Behavior, 44, 488–505.
Pearlin, L. I., Schieman, S., Fazio, E. M., & Meersman, S. C. (2005). Stress, health, and Turner, R. J., Wheaton, B., & Lloyd, D. A. (1995). The epidemiology of social stress.
the life course: some conceptual perspectives. Journal of Health and Social American Sociological Review, 60(1), 104–125.
Behavior, 46(2), 205–219. Udry, J. R. (1995). Sociology and biology: what biology do sociologists need to
Pescosolido, B. A., Perry, B. L., Long, J. S., & Martin, J. K. (2008). Under the influence know? Social Forces, 73(4), 1267–1278.
of genetics: how transdisciplinarity leads us to rethink social pathways to Wade, T. J., & Pevalin, D. J. (2004). Marital transitions and mental health. Journal of
illness. American Journal of Sociology, 114, S171–S201. Health and Social Behavior, 45(2), 155–170.

Вам также может понравиться