Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 23

Working together

www.rcis.ro

Revista de cercetare [i interven]ie social\


ISSN: 1583-3410 (print), ISSN: 1584-5397 (electronic)
Selected by coverage in Social Sciences Citation Index, ISI databases

WHAT DO ROMANIAN PARENTS AND ADOLESCENTS


HAVE CONFLICTS ABOUT?

Maria Nicoleta TURLIUC, Marius MARICI


Revista de cercetare [i interven]ie social\, 2013, vol. 42, pp. 28-49
The online version of this article can be found at:
www.rcis.ro, www.doaj.org and www.scopus.com

Published by:
Expert Projects Publishing House

expert projects
publishing
On behalf of:
„Alexandru Ioan Cuza” University,
Department of Sociology and Social Work
and
Holt Romania Foundation
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA
is indexed by ISI Thomson Reuters - Social Sciences Citation Index
(Sociology and Social Work Domains)

3
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013
Working together
www.rcis.ro

What do Romanian Parents and Adolescents


Have Conflicts About?
Maria Nicoleta TURLIUC1, Marius MARICI2

Abstract

Parent-adolescent (P-A) conflict presents ethno-cultural particularities with


regard to its thematic content. The purpose of the present research was to establish
the main thematic categories of the P-A conflict within the Romanian culture and
understand how various dimensions of conflict relate to the P-A relationship.
Concerning methodology, based on the in-depth, semi-structured interviews, 122
adolescents (Mage = 15,4) provided information about conflicting issues with their
parents, as well as data about the intensity, frequency, justifications, or stimuli of
conflict. The results indicated twelve thematic conflicting categories. “Parental
control of adolescents”, “personal activities” or “school and education” categories
recorded the most frequent conflicts, a situation which is explainable in Romanian
context. What is more, adolescents’ specific preoccupations and age were related
to conflict, whereas the conflict actors’ different attributions with regard to acci-
dents or to parents’ behavior proved to elicit conflict, as well. In addition, ado-
lescents reported frequent conflicts as a result of the parents’ use of psychological
control related strategies. Overall, this research underlines the necessity of cultu-
ral-featured research in deconstructing the P-A conflict.
Keywords: parent-adolescent conflict; themes of conflict; Romanian culture;
conflict frequency and intensity; gender; socio-cognitive domains.

Introduction

Conflict is inevitable in interpersonal relationships and scientists agree that a


moderate level of conflict is generally considered to be beneficial for adolescents.
Parents, though, perceive their children’ transition from childhood to adolescence

1
Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, Department of Medical Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and
Education Sciences, Ia[i, ROMANIA. E-mail: turliuc@uaic.ro
2
Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, Ia[i,
ROMANIA. E-mail: maricimarius@yahoo.com

28
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

as a stressful period (Olson, et al., 1983), in the context in which, on average,


adolescents’ negative affect (Buchanan, Eccles and Becker, 1992), P-A conflict or
adolescents’ autonomy solicitation increase (Lerner et al., 1996), while the quality
of P-A relationship declines (McGue, Elkins, Walden and Iacono, 2005). In the
first place, the conflict was defined in various ways: Montemayor (1982) referred
to the P-A conflict as a negative interaction between one parent and a child
implying a varying degree of disagreement. The Issues Checklist (Robin and
Foster, 1989) defined it as a discussion between the conflict actors, while Sorkhabi
(2010) viewed it as a fight or disagreement.
Furthermore, Stanley Hall, the one who achieved the first doctorate in psy-
chology in the USA, claimed that adolescence is a period of “storm and stress”
(Hall, 1904), which leads to conflict between parents and adolescents, this being
a universal and biological tendency. Moreover, in his view adolescents rather
suffer from mood disruption and get involved in risky behavior, but P-A conflict
is not normative. They often have school troubles and low self-esteem (Arnett,
1999). As Arnett concludes, there is no pure biological determination of “storm
and stress” in adolescence, although there is evidence that adolescence is a special
period involving a higher level of conflict with parents, more frequent variations
of mood disruptions and an intensification of risk behavior as compared to the
other developmental stages (Arnett, 1999). In addition, Ellis-Schwabe and Thorn-
burg (1986) found that children between 10 and 14 had most conflicts about
“home responsibilities” and “spending money” and at least conflicts about “watc-
hing TV”, in a period when virtual reality was not such an available leisure option
as today. Another research (Tesser, Forehand, Brody and Long, 1989) which used
“Issue Checklist” measure, found that children aged between 11 and 14 discussed
the least with parents about “drug use”, “drinking or smoking” and the most about
“chores”, “homework”, “time to go to bed” or “cleaning”.
Smetana (1989) found that P-A conflict focused mainly on ten thematic ca-
tegories such as: parental control, school issues, money, curfew, appearance a. s.
o. She concluded that these conflicts are about daily, mundane issue pertaining to
everyday interactions, they are generally not serious, but they are normative in the
P-A relationship. Conflict helps adolescents to gain more autonomy (Adams and
Laursen, 2001). It is not a wonder that Laursen found evidence that adolescents
(Mage = 16,7) spent in their daily interactions about 62%3 of their time with friends,
in comparison with the time spent with their family (28%) or with the other adults
(11%). Actually, adolescents transfer some of their parents’ functions to the same-
age friends. What is more, just a fraction of conflicts are very intense and they are
often in the moral domain.
In Chinese culture, Yeh (1995, 1997) performed some research in order to find
out the origins of parent-child conflict. The study found 6 sources of conflict.

3
Numbers are rounded

29
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

They are: (1) parental requests conflict child’s needs, (2) children do not have the
ability to meet parents’ requests (3) filial duty conflicts the other responsibilities
the child has (4) parents’ unreasonable behavior (5) immoral parental requests
and (6) conflict between parents. About 60% of the total conflicts belong to the
first category. These results actually reflect the findings in the western culture
(Barber, 1994) which show that P-A conflict is mainly about everyday, mundane
issues. In fact, cultural differences in parenting and especially in the conflict
domain were attributed to individualistic/collectivistic features of a culture. These
two forces shape thoughts, emotions, morality, goals, or attributions of individuals
(Triandis, 2001). This means that we could presumably hypothesis that in a
different culture, such as the Romanian one, the picture of P-A conflict might be
depicted in different nuances.
What is more, other studies show that P-A conflict is normative and moderate
levels of conflict elicit a positive influence on autonomy negotiation and inde-
pendence in comparison with families where no conflict or too much conflict
exists (Adams and Laursen, 2001). Autonomy represents a universal need of
human beings regardless of language or culture. Conflict encourages a rene-
gotiation of adolescents’ personal jurisdiction limits and boundaries, which deter-
mines parents to provide some degree of autonomy in one of more domains of
autonomy support such as: physical closeness, thinking, decision making or other
domains (Soenens and Beyers, 2012). P-A conflict is the most frequent in early
adolescence and drops until late adolescence (Laursen, Coy and Collins, 1998).
Starting with early adolescence, P-A interactions are redefined and they suffer
major transformations from a unilateral to a more mutual relationship (Doorn,
Branje and Meeus, 2011). Actually, a small number of these conflicts are the
result of some serious tense P-A exchanges. Some reported that 5% to 15% of the
total P-A conflicts are serious, (Collins and Laursen, 2004), while others 15% to
20% (Montemayor, 1983). What is more, adolescents’ relationship with their
parents maintains relatively warm during this period (Schaefer, 1965).
Besides this, from the perspective of socio-cognitive domain theory, conflict
arises because of different perspectives of parents’ and children’ upon the issues
which pertain to the personal domain of adolescents. Literature defines the moral
domain with reference to actions which have the potential to negatively affect the
welfare or rights of the others, the conventional domain refers to the issues which
are arbitrary and which were established through social interaction and agreement,
the prudential issues have the potential of harming or negatively influencing the
personal comfort or health, the friendship domain includes issues referring to
relationships, while the personal domain focus on issues which are within the
jurisdiction of personal choice and do not affect negatively the welfare or health
of the others (Smetana, 2011). On the one hand, the research found that P-A
conflict is the most frequent in the personal domain and it is rarely about more
serious issues. Smetana (1989, p. 1052) writes: “conflict rarely occurs over topics

30
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

such as religion, politics, sex, and drugs … but rather occurs over issues of rule
breaking and noncompliance to parental requests.” On the other hand, only a
clinical minority of adolescents have conflicts in the moral domain, especially
those with conduct disorders or oppositional behavior disorders (Smetana, 2005).
The most intense conflicts are in the moral domain (Smetana, 2011).
On top of that, the concept of “protagonist” and “antagonist” of conflict mirrors
the literary writings in which the later opposes in some way the forces of the
former. The isolation of the very first stimulus that triggered conflict seems a
harder task owing to the fact that the tendencies for the actors involved in conflict
is to defend themselves or to react negatively towards each other. Parents tend to
under-appreciate while children are inclined to exaggerate in their self-reports
(Patterson et al., 1992). Yet, adolescents’ reports are closer to reality, and thus
preferred.
Finally, concerning gender, generally, mother-adolescent conflicts are more
frequent than father-adolescent conflicts (Marici and Turliuc, 2011) and mother-
girl conflicts are more frequent than any other combinations (Laursen, 1995).
Generally, mothers are the main caregivers of their children meaning that they
spend significantly more time with their children (Comparable time use statistics,
2005). This means more closeness and thus more conflict. Girls are more sensitive
to conflicts and they are socialized to explore and try to solve conflicts, while
boys are socialized to deny or avoid conflicts (Turliuc, 2004). Other studies found
no gender differences regarding conflict (Dekovic, 1999).

Particularities of Romanian Culture and Family Life

Romania is predominantly a Christian Orthodox country (about 86%) and


formally a collectivistic society, where values such as obedience, relationships, or
authority are more valued than their opposites. In the last two decades, these
values have been often questioned under the more individualistic invasion of the
western culture. After the 1989 revolution, Romanians showed much openness
for the expression of liberty, consumerism, interest for property and the new. Yet,
most of these changes did not transform mentalities overnight, and yesterday’s
teenagers become today’s parents, some of whom are still captive of the old
patterns of thinking and of inefficient practices. In the process of change from the
traditional way of living to the more modern ones, the Romanian families are
being confronted with the instability of norms and values. Today, families increa-
singly need external support in order to adapt to this changes, parents claiming
either an inability to manage the relationship with their own children, or an
attachment to traditional values and an idealization of traditional family (Cojocaru
& Cojocaru, 2011). In comparison with their husbands, Romanian traditional
mothers promote closeness, cohesion, social courtesy, and parental authority.
They exert greater supervision (instrumental control) and relational control

31
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

(constrains of emotional attachment) in relation with their children, especially


with their daughters; sons being often exposed to fewer maternal controls and
encouraged to take risks. In other word, traditionally mothers bring up their
children and fathers are generally responsible for the family economic welfare,
although the paradigm is also being permanently questioned. The less traditional
mothers and fathers expect autonomy and self-actualizations, verbal and physical
assertiveness and independence, and their expectations are not so much related to
the gender of the child (Turliuc, 2004). Even so, mothers spend about twice more
time with their adolescent children, while fathers get involved mainly in re-
creational activities (Comparable time use statistics, 2005). Most adolescents
spend daily six or seven hours at school while the time for the next-day homework
takes other several hours. It is well-known that the Romanian school curriculum
is over-dimensioned and it has been often subject to major modifications made by
the Ministry of Education. High school requirements lead to higher expectations
and control on the part of the parents.

Aims of the Research

P-A conflict is characterized by ethno-cultural particularities, which means


that culture plays a significant role in determining the content of the P-A conflict,
which might elicit significant variations among different cultures. The purpose of
the present research was to address the following questions: (1) “What are the
main thematic categories of P-A conflict in the Romanian culture?”, and (2)
“How other dimensions of conflict such as intensity, frequency, gender, conflict
stimuli or socio-cognitive domains relate to P-A relationship?”

Sample

The participants (Mage = 15,4) were 122 adolescents from four high schools
from Suceava, a city in the Northwestern of Romania of about 91 000 citizens. We
tried to obtain a non-clinical, homogeneous sample of Romanian participants,
whose parents were married, and had lived together in the same dwelling, for at
least 2 months prior to the time of the interview. At the same time, as a condition
for the inclusion in the sample, adolescent participants, as well as their parents
had to suffer no severe mental, physical disability or chronic disease and ado-
lescents had to have both their parents.
The participants were 66 girls and 56 boys. 50% of mothers graduated from
high school and 33% graduated from a college, while in case of fathers 51%
graduated high school and 36% had a college diploma. Most participants were the
only child (45%) and 33% had one more sibling. 56% of adolescents were the first

32
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

child, and 28% were the second child. 77% of children belong to Orthodox
families.

Data Collection Methods

The research on P-A conflict was conducted in the fall of 2012, and it was
based on face to face, in-depth, semi-structured interviews combined with the
questionnaire method. According to the research ethics and country laws parental
written acceptance was obtained for the participation in the research of their
minor children. The participation was optional and only a small minority (8%)
refused to take part in the present study. In addition, school principals facilitated
our access to participants, offered us a room for the interviews, set up a schedule
and appointed people to assist us in our work.
The selection of schools was based mainly on principles’ openness to our
research. Two principles rejected our request to initiate a research on the basis of
lack of time on the part of the students. The research started by verbally providing
students with information and details about the researcher, purpose of the study,
confidentiality of information and explanations about the theme of research. The
introduction informed participants that the purpose of the researcher was to gather
information about every single conflict from the last four weeks, which they can
remember. Besides, adolescents had to indicate with whom the conflict was (with
the mother, the father or with both), to rate the most three intense conflicts, and to
indicate the socio-cognitive domains by answering a question referring to their
justifications for conflicts (“Why do you think it is good/bad to do/not to do
that?”) (Smetana, 1989).
Prior to the interview we asked participants to write in brief all the conflicts
they can remember in the last month, to indicate the day/date and with whom it
was, the mother, the father or with both. After completion, the participants had the
actual interview and they provided details about the P-A conflict, using the notes
which they previously made. They were encouraged to add even more conflicts if
they remembered. Demographics were assessed using a standardized questio-
nnaire after the interview. The adolescents’ answers were audio-recorded or noted
by the researcher during the interview.

Data Analysis Methods

For the present research we used a combination of inductive and deductive


qualitative thematic analysis together with the quantitative method. We investi-
gated the data using QSR NVivo data management program. Nvivo is a computer
software designed for qualitative research which allows users to organize, classify,

33
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

sort, examine relationships, search for information, or quantitatively investigate


and analyze non-numerical data from various fields of knowledge. This tool
supports more formats such as audio, video, picture, web, database formats and
helps researchers comprehend and systematize information (Nvivo 10, n.d.). We
entered transcripts and notes in Nvivo as project documents, manual codes were
treated as nodes and segments of data were selected and labeled as codes.

Qualitative Analysis

The research was based on thematic analysis (Strauss, 1987) which is a scien-
tific process of encoding qualitative information. We combined inductive (Boy-
atzis, 1998) and deductive (Crabtree and Miller, 1999) methods, relying on the
revised and improved procedure written by Feredazy and Muir-Cochrane (2006).
Code Manual Development. “A code manual” refers to all codes which are to
be used in a research. “A template” was defined as a list of codes used for a section
from research or for a broader theoretical construct. “A code” is the minimal unit
used for analysis in a qualitative research and it is defined as a “pattern found in
the information that at a minimum describes and organizes the possible obser-
vations or at a maximum interprets aspects of a phenomenon.” (Boyatzis, 1998, p.
4). At this point, we used a priori codes. Codes were identified by their (1) label,
(2) definition of what the theme refers to, (3) a description of how to know that
the theme occurs (“flagging”) (4) criteria for exclusion of themes, and (5) exam-
ples in order to eliminate potential confusions when searching for the themes
(Boyatzis, 1998, pp. 10-11). A priori codes were established before the data
analysis procedure started. We had such codes (including labels, definitions,
descriptions, and examples) for the socio-cognitive domains, which were the-
orized and tested in international literature and which were established based on
the respondents’ justifications (Smetana and Asquith, 1994, p. 1152). They were:
the “moral domain”, the “conventional domain”, the “prudential domain”, the
“friendship domain” and the “personal domain”. Then, we turned the ten thematic
categories resulted in Smetana’s research (1989) into a priori codes for the purpose
of comparison of the conflict thematic categories from the American culture with
those in the Romanian one. Finally, we used a priori codes to determine who the
conflict stimuli belonged to and for this we had the following codes: “Parent-
protagonist”, “Adolescent-protagonist”, “Parent-antagonist”, “Adolescent-anta-
gonist”. When the very first action/fact reported belonged to one actor of conflict,
he/she was considered the protagonist of conflict, while the other actor was labeled
as the antagonist, and the vice-versa.
Code Reliability Testing. We used double-coding as a means of reliability
testing (Miles and Huberman, 1994) meaning that two research assistants coded
the same field data. As a result we calculated the inter-coder reliability which was
88 for the thematic categories in Romanian culture, 87 for the recoding of

34
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

Romanian themes of conflict according to Smetana’s thematic categories of


conflict (a priori codes), and 85 for the determination of antagonists/protagonists
of conflict. Inter-coder reliability was calculated by the formulae: (the number of
total agreements between coders \ the total number of agreements + The total
number of disagreements).
Data Summary and Identification of Initial Themes. The stage involved rea-
ding, understanding, high-lightening pieces of information and summarizing the
raw data, which, in fact, all these actually, being initially done when the re-
searchers transcribed the audio-taped reports or when we checked the written
notes made during reports. The stage involved two steps. In the first place, we
organized the data according to the research questions from the semi-structured
interview or according to the purpose of the research, in order to facilitate a better
data management. In the second place, the research assistants continued the
process of identification of initial themes, which started with the transcription, in
a more focused and systematic manner. As a result of this phase we obtained
inductive codes and preliminary item-related themes of P-A conflicts. In the
process of coding, the two raters initially agreed to a number of 24 “thematic
categories” (nodes), which they refined and reduced to a number of 14, and then
to 12 categories. The two assistant researchers were teachers of Romanian, aged
31 and respectively 34, with more than 8 years of experience.
Applying Template of Codes (and Additional Coding). In this stage we used the
templates of codes, described above, to disseminate the data in a deductive
manner. At this point, we had no additional coding. The result was a preliminary
coding of individual conflicts according to the codes in the templates.
Connecting the Codes and Identifying the Thematic Categories. The purpose
of this stage was to cluster the resulting coding from the previous stage into
higher-order thematic entities. Individual codes were correlated with each other
and were organized by thematic content. At this point the research assistant had
the task of agreeing on the number and labels of thematic categories. In case of
inter-cultural comparison the whole thematic analysis was started over again and
every single conflict item was recoded in order to fit the thematic categories from
the research of Smetana (1989). From Smetana’s research we selected the data
reported by adolescents in early, middle and late adolescence, excluding the
reports of preadolescents. The sole purpose of this maneuver was to equalize the
age mean within the two samples of adolescents. Broadly, most thematic cate-
gories from our research found a partial equivalent in Smetana’s thematic cate-
gories, though, some did not, such as: the “Parents’ behavior” or the “Accidents”
categories, in which most items were removed or recoded in the other thematic
categories.
Corroborating and Legitimating Coded Thematic Categories. The last step
was meant to relate the data results to the interpretations and theoretical and

35
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

empirical findings in literature about conflict. The assistants attentively scrutinized


the initial codes and the final thematic categories in order to make sure that the re-
ordering process was correct. The whole procedure was a multi-step procedure
and it implied an iterative process of reading, coding and recoding the data.

Quantitative analysis

We used the quantitative method in several instances for example: to determine


the sample characteristics based the standardized questionnaire, to count with
which the conflict was, to find how frequent the themes occurred, to rate the
frequency of the most intense conflicts or to figure out who the stimuli of conflict
belonged to. For this we used the matrices in Nvivo, which perform quantitative
analysis according to various criteria.

Findings

Themes of P-A Conflict

Research assistants’ coding of P-A conflicts indicated a number of 507


conflicts, with an average of 1,27 conflicts per week, which were assigned to 12
categories. They are:
Table 1: The thematic categories of P-A conflict

No. Thematic categories No. of frequencies %


1. Control of personal activities 119 23,47
2. School and education 113 22,29
3. Chores and cleaning 62 12,23
4. Parents’ behavior 50 9,86
5. Adolescent’s social behavior 35 6,90
6. Curfew 26 5,13
7. Money and buying 26 5,13
8. Interpersonal control 24 4,73
9. Adolescent’s personality 17 3,35
10. Health and hygiene 15 2,96
11. Clothing and appearance 12 2,37
12. Accidents 8 1,58
Total 507 100%

In order to test whether there are significant differences between these ca-
tegories of conflict we performed the Chi-Square Goodness-of-Fit test. The results
indicated that Chi-Square test is significant: χ2(10) = 328.327, p < .000. We
conclude that there are significant differences between the rank of the categories
of P-A conflict.

36
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

Description of Categories

Control of personal activities. Adolescents did not agree with parents con-
cerning the following issues: when to go to church, playing games on the mobile
or computer, the selection of TV programs and the time spent watching. For
example: “my mom was in the kitchen and I wanted to finish my game on my
mobile phone and my mom sent me to answer to the phone as nobody was there.
We had a fight because I did not want to leave the game and my mother got upset.”
[male, 16] The boy considered that his activity was a matter of personal interest
and he had the right to decide whether or not, and when to go. Another girl relates:
“the food was served and everybody was in the kitchen and I didn’t show up. I was
buttoning my mobile and watching a movie. However, I wasn’t hungry. Dad said
to leave my phone and come to dinner. I didn’t want to and we quarreled” [female,
17].
School and education: Adolescents had conflicts about not preparing for
school: “I had a fight with my mother about my learning style. She doesn’t allow
me to waste my time playing outside or staying on the computer. She says that I do
not learn well if I do something else first.” [female, 14]. Another girl aged 15 had
a conflict about low grades at school or Olympics: “my mother asked to see my
school report, and I pretended not to hear, so I didn’t get it” or “I failed to make
the grade so my mother scorned me. Well, it’s not a big deal what happened, but
it’s up to me what I do with my grades. I am a grown-up.” In this case, the mother
who was much more responsible about her daughter’s education, had unmet
expectations. Thus, she used her parental authority to communicate in a punishing
voice that she was disappointed. The girl counter argued and reminded her mother
that she was a mature person. Other themes include: skipping classes, not pre-
paring for exams, not doing homework for the next day, refusing to learn or to do
extra-work, playing instead of doing homework, making mistakes in homework,
refusal to take part in national competitions. From this percentage 1,8% reported
that they had conflicts with parents owing to the graduation examination in the
12th grade (baccalaureate) or owing to national examinations (“capacitate”) in the
8th grade.
Chores and cleaning: Conflicts arose because adolescents did not do chores or
did not put their room in order, as expected by parents: “I came from school and
I was tired so I threw my clothes over the room. I had a quarrel with my mom
because it was my room and I decide what I do and when I do.” [male, 14] A
recurrent motif in children’s reports was the urgency of performing the task
demanded by parents. Usually, children fail, delay too much, forget, or simply
refuse to help parents. Other themes of conflict include: putting clothes on han-
gers, messy rooms, thrown clothes, refusal to vacuum, not dumping the garbage,
delaying to feed domestic birds, refusal to clean immediately, refusal to do the
dishes.

37
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

Parents’ behavior: Parents use a series of methods in order to manipulate


children’s thoughts and emotions, and by this they try to psychologically control
their children (Barber and Harmon, 2002, p. 15). The parents initiate requests
using counterproductive methods of conversation which lead to conflict. Thus,
psychological aggression (4,86%) becomes the main theme of conflict: “I am fed
up with my parents’ madness. They are always getting on my nerves because’ I
don’t do things right, and they know all this from my brothers.” [male, 15] or “I
was driving home with my mom in the passenger’s seat when I remained stuck in
the traffic jam and she started to shout and accuse me although I really had no
other option. I don’t like the way she speaks.” [male, 18] In another situation
parents conflicted with adolescents because of their insisting requests (1%): “…my
dad found in the kitchen a beetle and he wanted to give it to me to keep it in my
hand. I didn’t want to but he kept forcing me and I got angry because of that.”
[female, 17] or “she asked me a lot of times if I smoked. I answered her tens of
times that I don’t but she kept asking me. She makes me mad with that.” [male,
18]. Finally, parents invaded their children’s personal life (4%) in different ways:
the parents listened to adolescents’ phone conversations, they checked their dau-
ghter’s purse or searched through children’s personal belongings. The parents
entered their room without knocking, remained too much in their room, opened
their correspondence, the parents accessed their children’s e-mail or checked their
phone messages, searched their pockets or logged on their Facebook account.
Usually, children felt very upset when such things happen, and they used super-
latives (like “incredible” or “shocking”), they expressed amazement (“I couldn’t
believe my eyes.”) or they exclaimed (“How could he dear to do it?”) in order to
describe what they perceived to be a serious wrongdoing.
Adolescent’s social behavior: The social behavior of adolescents refers to
issues such as: teasing a drunkard, quarrel with siblings, neglecting younger
siblings as a result of a transfer of parental responsibility from parents to ado-
lescents, improperly talking to parents, improper tone with parents, contradicting
parents, fight with siblings or school mates. For example: “there on the street was
a drunkard and I teased him, I was with some friends. I told him all kind of bad
things and he got mad. My father found out, I don’t know how, and we had a
fight.” [male, 16] or “my sister told my father about it and my dad said that I beat
her and he punished me. The truth is that it was my fault.” [male, 17] In several
situations, children acknowledged that they were wrong about their deeds but
they related that their friends pushed them to do so, more as a proof of their
bravery. They also felt momentary pleasure doing it, and they didn’t think much
of possible serious consequences. Actually they felt that it was more of fun than
some sort of antisocial behavior.
Curfew: The parents and children had disagreements because of: leaving to
school at a late hour, staying outside with friends too much, sleeping overnight at
a friend’s house and not coming home, disagreeing with parents’ curfew. “I woke

38
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

up a bit later and I was late for school. I ate and I couldn’t decide what clothing
to wear. When my mother saw me she said that I am always late and that’s why I
have school problems.” [female, 14] Those who return late are rebuked and they
receive the promised punishment or they are threatened that that was the last time
when they were not punished. In most cases the adolescents knew well that it was
their fault, they became less talkative to their parents and some answered back
only if their parents insisted with argumentation or threatening.
Money and buying: Adolescents reported issues such as: “I often quarrel
because of money for food at school. They often tease me that I spend much money
on junk food. It is my money and it’s none of their business what I do with it.”
[female, 15] or “One day my dad let me walk to the bus station and I was on high
hills. He didn’t give me enough money to call a taxi. He didn’t want to, he said that
I can deal with it by myself and I was really in a hurry because I was late.”
[female, 17] Other themes of conflict include: children asked for a larger amount
of money, they wasted money on clothing, received too little money from parents,
parents refused to give money for a concert, taxi or for dining out, parents refused
to buy their children different things such as a dress, a dog, shoes or parents
refused to give their children money immediately.
Interpersonal control: Adolescents reported that they had conflicts with parents
about: meeting/choosing their girlfriend/boyfriend, parents’ disagreement on child’s
meeting with a boyfriend/girlfriend, going to a friend’s birthday, sleeping over-
night at a friend’s home, going out with friends, choosing to go with friends
instead of going out with family, playing with friends outside, calling friends to
come at home, inviting friends to come at home when parents are gone. For
example: “He didn’t let me go to my friend. He said he should come to me. Until
now I used to go to him but my dad didn’t want to let me go. We had a fight and
in the end I didn’t go.” [female, 17] “He took my mobile in order not to talk to my
girlfriend. I don’t know why he doesn’t like her, but it’s none of his business what
I do… He said I should not do what I do and if I don’t obey I will stay the whole
summer inside the house.” [male, 18] Some parents are quite strong on their
position when they forbid children to see with their opposite-sex friends. Some
adolescents regretted sharing with their parents or being sincere towards them,
and as a result this led to concealment.
Adolescent’s personality: Adolescents reported issues such as: reading daily
too much, being rude to parents, defending sister in front of parents, being a
negative influence upon his sister, being a teaser with a drunkard on the street, not
being attentive when talking, egocentrism by not letting his sister on computer.
For example: “I was speaking with my father and he accused me that I am not a
good listener and I don’t listen to him, but I told him I don’t know what he is
talking about. He accuses me I’m not interested in what he speaks and that I obey
my mom, but in fact he also doesn’t hear what I say. He doesn’t like how I speak
to him.” [male, 16] or “I use the computer when I want and my mom wants me to

39
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

let my sister on the computer too. She said that I am egocentric that I care only
about me and that I should leave her some time on the computer too.” [male, 14]
The source of these conflict is parents’ negative comments with regard to ado-
lescents’ personality traits.
Health and hygiene: The themes of conflict are: drinking alcohol, smoking or
eating fast food. For example: “She found my cigarettes in my purse and they
rebuked me and I got angry because they also smoke and it’s my lungs and they
had to explain to me because I can understand if they speak nicely.” [female, 18]
We recorded few such cases, as the Romanian laws forbid drinking alcohol or
selling cigarettes to children under 18. Consequently, we speculate that some
children might have avoided sharing with us such conflicts.
Clothing and appearance: Disagreement concerned child’s personal appea-
rance, beard wearing, choosing a dress, too much time spent for dressing up,
wearing inappropriate clothing for the weather, or walking without slippers at
home. For example, “I didn’t shave and my mother said I am too young to wear
beard. My father wore beard when he was like me. He told me. But mom said that
I look nice and I would better shave. I argued but finally I did what she said,
although I didn’t want to.” [male, 17]
Accidents: Conflict between parents and children arose because either parents
or children were involved in some sorts of accidents. For example: “…dad broke
my mobile when he dropped it down.” [female, 16] “…by mistake my mom blea-
ched my blouse when she washed it” [female, 15] or “I had an accident with my
father’s car which made my parents become mad.” [male, 18].

Parents’ Gender and Conflict

Adolescents indicated for about 95% of the total conflicts reported who the
conflict was with. Based on the data we had, quantitative analysis using matrices
in Nvivo showed that adolescents had more conflicts with mothers (51%) than
with fathers (33%) while conflicts with both parents represented 16%. As a matter
of fact, adolescents pronounced in their conversations the word “mother” or
“mom” significantly more than the word “father” or “dad”.

The Intensity and Frequency of P-A Conflict in Socio-Cognitive Domains

As a result of rating the most intense three conflicts adolescents indicated the
following (see Table 2):

40
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

Table 2. Percent regarding the intensity and the frequency of P-A conflicts in socio-
cognitive domains according to adolescents’ justifications.

Moral Conventional Prudential Friendship Personal Total


Intensity
High 86 43 33 38 40 -
Medium 7 36 42 33 33 -
Low 7 21 25 29 27 -
Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% -
Frequency of 3 21 18 4 54 100%
conflicts

P-A conflict is the most intense in the moral domain followed by the con-
ventional one. The computed scores show that the most intense conflicts are in the
socially regulated domain (moral plus conventional), 64,5%, compared to the
personally regulated domain (prudential, friendship and personal domains) repre-
sented by 37% of conflicts. The most frequent conflicts were in the personal
domain followed by those in the conventional, prudential, friendship and finally
in the moral domain (see Table 2).

Protagonists and Antagonists of Conflict

Based on the information available from adolescents’ reports we noticed that


not all conflicts started from a stimulus belonging to adolescents. Parents’ actions
also triggered P-A conflicts. Conflict cannot exist but in the active presence of
two related people. The theme of conflict refers to “What the conflict was about?”
(Laursen and Collins, 1994), whereas the question “Who performed the very first
action which triggered a conflict?” focuses on the protagonist of the conflict. In
addition, interpersonal conflict involves divergent interpretations of events and
their expression. The collected data indicated that 7,51% from all conflicts were
triggered by parents’ actions. For example: “By mistake, my father dropped my
mobile phone and broke it. We had a fight because of that.” [female, 16], “When
my girlfriend phones and my parents are around and hear me speaking; they are
always making fun of me. They do this even if I know I don’t like it.” [male, 17].
In the context in which conflicts were reported by adolescents, 92,49% of all
conflicts were triggered by an action/stimulus belonging to them.

Comparison between the Present Research and the Research of Smetana

The recoding of our conflict items according to Smetana’s research categories


(1989) led to the following results (see Table 3).

41
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

Table 3: Thematic categories of P-A conflict coded according to the categories of


Smetana (1989)

Note: in blue (present research), in red (Smetana, 1989)


1. Control of personal activities4 [REGULATING ACTIVITIES]5
2. School and education [HOMEWORK/ACHIEVEMENT]
3. Chores and cleaning [CHORES]
4. Adolescent's social behavior [INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS]
5. Curfew [BEDTINE/CURFEW]
6. Money and buying [FINANCES/MONEY]
7. Interpersonal control [INTERPERSONAL REGULATION]
8. Adolescent's personality [BEHAVIORAL STYLE]
9. Health and hygiene [HEALTH AND HYGIENE]
10. Clothing and appearance [APPEARANCE]

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the thematic content of P-A conflict in
Romanian culture from the perspective of dimensions such as: intensity, freq-
uency, gender, conflict stimuli or socio-cognitive domains.
Firstly, it was found that adolescents’ conflict is related to the adolescents’
development niche (Harkness and Super, 1992) and their macro-system (Bronfen-
brenner, 1994). On the one hand, conflict can be about anything as long as there
is an interest in that particular issue on the part of the actors of conflict. For
4
Labels of the thematic categories in our research
5
Labels of the thematic categories in the research of Smetana (1989)

42
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

example, adolescents in the 12th grade reported conflicts about the national school
evaluation (baccalaureate) which generally takes place at this age dedicated to
preparing for the exam. This theme is transitory and age-specific meaning that it
is less likely to be reported by some other age-groups, as it actually happened. A
study investigating conflict between adult children (Mage = 64) and older parents
(Mage = 39) showed that the themes of conflict changed with age. Unlike regular
P-A conflicts, these referred to conflict themes such as child-rearing practices and
values, politics, religion, ideology, work, household maintenance, communication
or interaction style, habits or lifestyle (Clarke, Preston, Raksin and Bengtson,
1999). On the other hand, further recoding of categories indicated that the foll-
owing categories scored less on frequency in the present research as compared
with the study of Smetana: “adolescents’ social behavior” [Interpersonal re-
lations6] – 38% less, “curfew” [Bedtime and curfew] – 54% less, and “inter-
personal control” [Regulation of interpersonal activities] – 61% less. “Clothing
and appearance” [Appearance] category was about thrice more frequent in Ame-
rican than in Romanian culture. In addition, “chores and cleaning” [Chores]
category scored 30% less while “adolescent’s personality” [Personality/behavioral
style] category scored 33% less in our research, too. Cultural particularities
account for the variations in the frequency of P-A conflict. For example, the
interviews on Afro-American families indicated that the daily chores or putting
the bedroom in order were the hottest themes of conflict (Smetana, 2011). One
third of conflicts were on these themes, while in case of European-American
families conflict frequency scored 18% (Smetana, Daddis and Chuang, 2003).
Another research (Özmete and Bayoglu, 2009) on young adults (Mage = 21) found
that “putting the room in order” was one of the main themes of conflict, while in
Smetana’s research (1989) this theme generated the most conflicts.
Secondly, conflict can also arise from irrational reasons. The “accidents”
category refers to conflicts which took place owing to one actor’s mistake, for
example the father dropped the child’s phone and broke it. The very essence of
these particular issues is that accidents were not done on purpose or intention on
the part of the actor involved in conflict. Yet, in some cases, they are perceived as
intentional acts or unintentional, yet the author of the action is guilty of clumsi-
ness. Parents and adolescents endorse different beliefs concerning the cause of
accidents and they make entirely different attributions. When the father broke
child’s phone, for example, he explained that it simply happened, it was chance,
but the child who was the owner of the phone, attributed it to her father’s poor
skills in handling it, so it was his fault. This phenomenon can be explained by
attribution biases. The more negative the attributions in P-A dyads become, the
more dyadic conflict there is (Grace, Kelley and McCain, 1993).

5
The category labels in Smetana’s research (1989)

43
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

Thirdly, in the context of this research the most frequent conflicts fell in the
category of “Parental control of personal activities” or “School and education”. A
recoding of our thematic categories and a comparison with the thematic categories
found in Smetana’s research (1989) indicates that these two categories are at least
twice more frequent than their correspondents [id est, “Regulation of activities”
and “Homework and academic achievement”] in American culture. As an ex-
communist country where the regime used to be “omnipresent” and “omniscient”
owing to its controlling system, most parents still manifest dysfunctional ten-
dencies and behaviors reminiscent of those times, which they often project unto
their children. M\n\stireanu writes with reference to Romania under Communism
that people “living in oppressive environments … tend to be very protective of
their loved ones and community members, but this legitimate desire to protect can
easily lead to illegitimate control over the lives of others and, in the end, perpetual
immaturity for those controlled. This actually replicates authoritarian reflexes,
which presume that ordinary members of society cannot think for themselves and
that this is why governing authorities have to make decisions for them.” (M\n\-
stireanu, 2012, p. 54). Parents might show high levels of control towards their
children owing to their deep level of mistrust in relation to the others learned
under the Communist regime. Furthermore, a possible explanation for conflicts
which fall into the “School and education” category is that in the last two school
years The Ministry of Education decided to implement video cameras in order to
prevent exam frauds. Consequently, the exam passing rates decreasing signi-
ficantly to below 50%. This might have led parents to have higher expectations as
compared with the previous years, when most teenagers passed the exam, and to
intensify their efforts to discuss more with their children. Thus, they increased
control pressure and associatively increased P-A conflict. In addition, the over-
dimensioned school curriculum and long school schedules might have put addi-
tional pressure on parents to control their children in order to make sure that their
children will achieve a better school performance.
Fourthly, there is evidence (Sorkhabi, 2010) which shows that conflict can be
triggered by formal (control, aggression…) as well as content (control of socio-
cognitive domains) variables. Yet, with reference to the “parents’ behavior” cate-
gory our research found strong evidence that, within the Romanian cultural
context, formal-type variables such as “psychological aggression” or “insisting
demands of parents upon adolescents” as well as content-type variables such as
“intrusion into the personal jurisdiction of adolescent” lead to P-A conflict. In our
research adolescents often reported cases of the following type: if parents demand
in a high voice “Go and put your room in order!” some children perceive that
parents talked to them badly. Although there is a theme of conflict implied by
parents, their demanding style (formal variable) conveys a different message in
the adolescents’ mind. The actual theme (putting the room in order) subsides and
a new theme emerges from parents’ tone. Poor parental communication skills

44
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

account for this situation, but “Learned techniques may result in an improvement
of communication and in the prevention of conflicts” (Cojocaru and Cojocaru,
2011, p. 217) if parents would attend parenting programs. Moreover, parents’
inappropriate behaviors towards adolescents, through which they violate their
privacy, are perceived by adolescents as disrespectful, as intrusions into their
personal intimacy, and a proof of disregard towards adolescents’ need for a
personal domain. Parental control of the adolescent’s personal domain is asso-
ciated with conflict.
Finally, the most intense conflicts were found in the moral domain, while the
most frequent in the personal domain, results which confirm the findings in the
international literature (see Smetana, 2011). However, the present qualitative
research facilitated observations which indicated that a part of the conflict in the
moral domain does not become manifest P-A conflict. This is because adolescents
avoid confrontation and conversation with parents owing to the seriousness of the
issues or owing to the fear of negative consequences, especially when parents’
style is authoritarian.

Conclusion

To sum up, to our knowledge, this is the first study of this kind whose aim is to
investigate the themes of P-A conflict in Romanian culture. Our research streng-
thened the idea that P-A conflict takes place about daily, mundane issues and it
usually takes place in the family dwelling where children and parents meet most
often. Conflict is inevitable and normal and is serves a certain function in the
family system, and it is basically an expression of some unmet needs on the part
of adolescents or parents. They feel entitled to demand more autonomy and a
moderate conflict is considered by adolescents to be the royal way to claim it.
Moreover, our conflict typology indicates that conflict is a dynamic phenomenon
and the thematic content changes according to a series of variables such as age,
preoccupations or culture.
One limit of our research is that the data was self-reported, which raises the
question of data reliability. In addition, in determining the protagonist or anta-
gonist of conflict, the data should be generalized with caution owing to the fact
that we relied exclusively on adolescents’ indirect reports, and not on a direct
question which could have indicated their belief about who was the first to do
something to elicit conflict.
All in all, we investigated only families with married parents, who lived
together with their children, but future studies could extend research to mono-
parental families or divorced parents. Such studies could indicate variations in P-
A conflict, as parenting resources are much more limited and P-A interaction has

45
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

specific particularities. Further research could also investigate silent conflict which
refers to conflict which is “neither verbal nor physical” (Kielpikowski & Pryor,
2008, p. 217) and which is also part of the total variance of conflict between
parents and children. Our research dealt only with manifest P-A conflict. At the
same time, our study could significantly contribute to the developing of local
parenting programs. The P-A relationship and communication need improving
through an efficient and professional transfer of skills from experts to parents in
order to make the intervention on the child permanent (Cojocaru, 2011).

Acknowledgements

This work was partially supported by the European Social Fund in Romania,
under the responsibility of the Managing Authority for the Sectoral Operational
Programme for Human Resources Development 2007-2013 [Grant POSDRU/
CPP 107/DMI 1.5/S/78342] The grant was awarded to Marici Marius.

References

Adams, R., & Laursen, B. (2001). The organization and dynamics of adolescent conflict
with parents and friends. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 63, 97-110.
Arnett, J. A. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist,
54, 317-326.
Barber, B. K. (1994). Cultural, family, and personal contexts of parent-adolescent conflict.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 375-386.
Barber, B. K., & Harmon, E. L. (2002). Violating the self: Parental psychological control
of children and adolescents. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How
psychological control affects children and adolescents, Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association, 15-52.
Boyatzis, R. (1998). Transforming Qualitative Information: Thematic Analysis and Code
Development. Sage Publications: London and New Delhi.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human development. In International
Encyclopedia of Education, vol. 3, 2nd. Oxford: Elsevier.
Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J., & Becker, J. (1992). Are adolescents the victims of raging
hormones? Evidence for activational effects of hormones on moods and behavior
at adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 62-107.
Clarke, E. J., Preston, M., Raksin, J., & Bengtson, V. L. (1999). Types of Conflicts and
Tensions Between Older Parents and Adult Children. The Gerontologist, 39(3),
261-270.
Cojocaru, D. (2011). Attending parenting education programmes in Romania. The case of
the Holt Romania Iasi programme. Revista de cercetare si interventie sociala, 32,
140-154.
Cojocaru, D., & Cojocaru, S. (2011). The deprivatization of family and its effects on
parenting in Romania. Revista de cercetare si interventie sociala, 33, 209-222.

46
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

Collins, W.A., & Laursen, B. (2004). Parent-adolescent relationships and influences. In:
Lerner, R. M, Steinberg, L., (eds.). (2004) Handbook of adolescent psychology.
2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, 331-362.
Comparable Time Use Statistics. (2005). National Tables from Ten European Countries.
Working Papers and Studies. Luxemburg: European Commission.
Crabtree, B. F. & Miller, W. L. (1999). Doing qualitative research, 2nd edit., Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Dekovic, M. (1999). Parent-Adolescent Conflict: Possible Determinants and Consequen-
ces. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23(4), 977-1000.
Doorn, M. D., Branje, S. J., & Meeus, W. H. (2011). Developmental changes in conflict
resolution styles in parent-adolescent relationships: a four-wave longitudinal study.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 97-107.
Ellis-Schwabe, M. E., & Thornburg, H. D. (1986). Conflict areas between parents and
their adolescents. The Journal of Psychology, 120, 59-68.
Feredazy, J. & Muir-Cochrane, E. (2006). Demonstrating Rigor Using Thematic Analysis:
A Hybrid Approach of Inductive and Deductive Coding and Theme Development.
International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5(1), 80-92.
Grace, N. C., Kelley, M. L., & McCain, A. P. (1993). Attribution processes in mother-
adolescent conflict. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21(2), 199-211.
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its psychology and its relation to physiology, anthro-
pology, sociology, sex, crime, religion, and education (Vols. I & II). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1992). The developmental niche: A theoretical framework
for analyzing the household production of health. Social Science and Medicine,
38(2), 217-226.
Kielpikowski, M. M., & Pryor, J. E. (2008). Silent parental conflict: Parents’ perspective.
Journal of Family Studies, 14(2-3), 217-227.
Laursen, B. (1995). Conflict and Social Interaction in Adolescent Relationships. Journal
of Research on Adolescence (Lawrence Erlbaum), 5(1), 55-70.
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interpersonal conflict during adolescence. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 115, 197-209.
Laursen, B., Coy, K. C. & Collins, W. A. (1998). Reconsidering Changes in Parent-Child
Conflict across Adolescence: A Meta-Analysis. Child Development, 69, 817-832.
Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., von Eye, A., Ostrom, C. W., Nitz, K., Talwar-Soni, R., &
Tubman, J. G. (1996). Continuity and discontinuity across the transition of early
adolescence: A developmental contextual perspective. In J. A. Graber, J. Brooks-
Gunn, & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions through adolescence (pp. 3-22). Mah-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
M\n\stireanu, D. (2012). After Liberation, Then What? Enabling and Protecting Commu-
nities in Post-Authoritarian Contexts. World Vision International.
Marici, M. & Turliuc, M. N. (2011). How much does it matter? Exploring the role of
parental variables in school deviance in Romania. Journal of Psychological and
Educational Research, 19(1), 9-33.
McGue, M., Elkins, I., Walden, B., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Perceptions of the parent–
adolescent relationship: A longitudinal investigation. Developmental Psychology,
41, 971-984.

47
REVISTA DE CERCETARE SI INTERVENTIE SOCIALA - VOLUME 42/2013

Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Montemayor, R. (1982). The Relationship between Parent-Adolescent Conflict and the
Amount of Time Adolescents Spend Alone with Parents and Peers. Child Develop-
ment, 53(6), 1512-1519.
Montemayor, R. (1983). Parents and adolescents in conflict: All families some of the time
and some families most of the time. Journal of Early Adolescence, 3, 83-103.
Nvivo 10, An Overview of Nvivo, (n.d.) http://download.qsrinternational.com/Resource/
NVivo10/nvivo10-overview.pdf, accessed on the 7th of March 2013.
Olson, D., McCubbin, H., Barnes, H., Larson, A., Muxen, M., & Wilson, M. (1983).
Families, what makes them work. Beverly Hills, CA.: Sage.
Özmete, E., & Bayoglu, A. S. (2009). Parent-young adult conflict: a measurement on
frequency and intensity of conflict issues. The Journal of International Social
Research, 2(8), 313-322.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys. A social interactional
approach, vol. 4, Eugene OR: Castalia.
Robin, A. L, & Foster, S. L. (1989). Negotiating Parent-Adolescent Conflict: A Beha-
vioral-Family Systems Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s report of parental behavior: An inventory. Child
Development, 36, 413-424.
Smetana J. G., Daddis, C., & Chuang, S. S. (2003). “Clean your room!” A longitudinal
investigation of adolescent-parent conflict and conflict resolution in middle class
African American families. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 631-650.
Smetana, J. G. (1989). Adolescents’ and Parents’ Reasoning about Actual Family Conflict.
Child Development, 60(5), 1052-1067.
Smetana, J. G. (2005). Adolescent-parent conflict: Resistance and subversion as develop-
mental processes. In L. Nucci (Ed.), Conflict, contradiction, and contrarian ele-
ments in moral development and education (pp. 69-91). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Smetana, J. G. (2011). Adolescents, Families, and Social Development: How Teens
Construct Their Worlds, Wiley-Blackwell.
Smetana, J., & Asquith, P. (1994). Adolescents and parents conceptions of parental
authority and personal autonomy. Child development, 65(4), 1147-1162.
Soenens, B., & Beyers, W. (2012). The Cross-Cultural Significance of Control and
Autonomy in Parent-Adolescent Relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 243-
248.
Sorkhabi, N. (2010). Sources of parent-adolescent conflict: Content and form of parenting.
Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 38(6), 761-761.
Strauss, A. (1987). Qualitative analysis for social scientists. Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Tesser, A., Forehand, R., Brody, G., & Long, N. (1989). Conflict: The role of calm and
angry parent-child discussion in adolescent adjustment. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 8(3), 317-330.
Triandis, H. C. (2001), Individualism-Collectivism and Personality. Journal of Perso-
nality, 69, 907-924.
Turliuc, M. N. (2004). Psychology of Couples and Family. Iasi, Ro.: Performantica.

48
REALITIES IN A KALEIDOSCOPE

Yeh, K. H. (1995). Solution types in lial dilemmas and their correlates [in Chinese].
Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica, 79, 87-118.
Yeh, K. H. (1997). Parent-child conicts and their solution types: Discussion from the
viewpoint of lial piety [in Chinese]. Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia
Sinica, 82, 65-114.

49

Вам также может понравиться