Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Body appreciation, interest in cosmetic enhancements, and need for


uniqueness among U.S. college students
Meghan M. Gillen a,∗ , Jamie Dunaev b
a
Division of Social Sciences, 1600 Woodland Road, The Pennsylvania State University, Abington, PA 19001, United States
b
Health Sciences Program, Rutgers University, 311 N. 5th St., Camden, NJ 08102, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The aim of the current study was to examine associations between body appreciation and putative cor-
Received 15 November 2016 relates that focus on self-enhancement and self-expression. Students (N = 261; mean age = 20.16 years,
Received in revised form 23 June 2017 SD = 3.68; 60.9% female) from a non-residential college in the northeastern United States completed a
Accepted 26 June 2017
questionnaire measuring body appreciation, interest in cosmetic enhancements, and need for uniqueness.
Available online 28 July 2017
Individuals with higher body appreciation and African Americans/Blacks reported significantly higher
self-attributed need for uniqueness and significantly higher investment in a distinctive appearance. The
Keywords:
association between body appreciation and interest in cosmetic enhancements (e.g., hair coloring) was
Body appreciation
Positive body image
not significant. Results suggest that body appreciation may be linked to a desire to express one’s own
Cosmetic enhancements unique qualities.
Need for uniqueness © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Appearance investment

1. Introduction 1.1. Body appreciation and interest in cosmetic enhancements

Historically, body image research has focused on negative body Body appreciation is associated with appearance-related atti-
image. Although important for understanding the pervasiveness of tudes and behaviors that are protective of health and well-being
body concerns and potentially associated eating problems, this nar- (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a). Individuals with higher body
row focus failed to address the complexity of thoughts and feelings appreciation value, respect, and feel pride in their bodies more
people may have about their bodies, including positive percep- than those with lower body appreciation, suggesting that they may
tions. Recent research has indicated that positive body image is be better equipped to resist cultural pressure to meet unrealistic
in fact a separate construct from negative body image, with its standards of attractiveness (Chrisler, Marvan, Gorman, & Rossini,
own unique dimensions and correlates (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015; Gillen, 2015). In resisting this pressure, they may be less
2015a). Tylka and Wood-Barcalow (2015a) define positive body accepting of, and engage less frequently in, behaviors that can help
image as body appreciation, loving and feeling comfort with one’s them meet a thin, toned, and tan ideal (Grant, Gillen, & Bernstein,
body, having broad ideas of attractiveness, adaptive self-care for 2014; Murnen, 2011). For example, those with higher body appreci-
appearance, manifesting positive feelings through the body, and ation report significantly lower drive for muscularity and more sun
filtering information about the body in protective ways. Psycho- protective attitudes (Gillen, 2015). Their behaviors are also more
metrically sound measures have been developed to capture these oriented toward health and well-being: they report significantly
constructs including body appreciation, the most widely studied lower dieting behaviors, less disordered eating, higher intuitive eat-
aspect of positive body image (Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow, ing (following their body’s cues for hunger and satiety), less talk
2005; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a). The current study focused about dieting and weight loss, more sun protective behaviors, and
on associations between body appreciation and putative correlates more engagement in exercise (Andrew, Tiggemann, & Clark, 2016;
that relate to the desire to enhance or to express one’s appearance. Gillen, 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Wasylkiw & Butler,
2014).
One appearance-related attitude that has not been frequently
examined is interest in cosmetic enhancements. Cosmetic enhance-
ments (e.g., manicures, genital waxing) are typically lower risk
procedures, take relatively little time, and can be performed out-
∗ Corresponding author. Fax: +1 215 881 7623.
side of a medical context by trained staff. Yet, perhaps contrary to
E-mail addresses: mmg204@psu.edu (M.M. Gillen),
common beliefs, these procedures, as well as some self-performed
Jamie.Dunaev@camden.rutgers.edu (J. Dunaev). cosmetic enhancements (e.g., high-heeled shoes), can come with

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.06.008
1740-1445/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143 137

health risks (Saltzberg & Chrisler, 1995). For example, some women compared to European Americans, which may include spending
have experienced toxic side effects from cosmetics (Saltzberg time and effort on cosmetic enhancements (Gillen & Lefkowitz,
& Chrisler, 1995). Thus, cosmetic enhancements are pursued to 2012). Yet, previous research indicates that Caucasian women
improve appearance, yet come with some level of risk. report greater acceptance of cosmetic surgery than their South
Given that body appreciation is associated with health protec- Asian and African Caribbean female counterparts (Swami et al.,
tive attitudes and behaviors (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a), it is 2012), suggesting that Caucasians may also be more interested in
possible that individuals higher in body appreciation are less inter- cosmetic enhancements than individuals in ethnic minority groups.
ested in cosmetic enhancements. Because individuals with higher Based on this previous work, we might expect that European Amer-
body appreciation may conform less to culturally constructed icans/Whites or African Americans/Blacks may be more interested
appearance ideals (Gillen, 2015), they may be less interested in in cosmetic enhancements.
procedures aimed at achieving these ideals. Prior research pro-
vides support for this contention, although most of this work has 1.2. Body appreciation and need for uniqueness
focused on cosmetic surgery attitudes. These attitudes capture indi-
viduals’ opinions on cosmetic surgery procedures, including their Another variable related to self-enhancement/expression is
personal willingness to undergo these procedures. For example, need for uniqueness. We might expect that individuals with higher
body appreciation was significantly associated with lower accep- body appreciation would also be more interested in striving for and
tance of cosmetic surgery attitudes in university students from the expressing their unique personal qualities. According to the the-
United Kingdom and South Korea (Swami, 2009; Swami, Campana, ory of uniqueness (Snyder & Fromkin, 1977, p. 518), uniqueness is
& Coles, 2012; Swami, Hwang, & Jung, 2012), and in community “a positive striving for differentness relative to other people”, and
samples of adults from Malaysia, Brazil, and the United States (U.S.) should not be taken as a negative concept akin to abnormality and
(Swami, 2010; Swami et al., 2011; Tylka & Iannantuono, 2016). deviance, but rather one related to enhanced self-esteem. There are
However, body appreciation was not significantly associated with individual differences in the need for uniqueness; some individuals
cosmetic surgery attitudes in adolescents in Sweden (Lunde, 2013), want to feel special and distinct more so than others do.
and in middle-aged women in the U.S. (Chrisler, Gorman, Serra, & Tiggemann and Golder (2006) and Tiggemann and Hopkins
Chapman, 2012). (2011) applied the theory of uniqueness to tattooing, a form of
Yet, little is known about associations between body appreci- body modification. Because tattooing is a means by which unique-
ation and attitudes toward cosmetic enhancements. As previous ness can be displayed visibly on the body, they expected, and found,
literature on cosmetic surgery attitudes suggests (e.g., Swami, that those with at least one tattoo had a significantly higher need for
2009), body appreciation may be associated with less interest in uniqueness than non-tattooed individuals. Swami (2012) reported
cosmetic enhancements, as cosmetic enhancements can help indi- similar findings. However, on distinctive appearance investment,
viduals conform to cultural ideals of attractiveness (albeit with a measure of the desire to have a noticeable, distinct appearance,
lower risks than surgery). Another possibility is that cosmetic Swami (2012) found that those who visited a tattoo parlor for their
enhancements can help individuals express their unique quali- first tattoo had significantly higher distinctive appearance invest-
ties, and therefore interest in these procedures may be linked to ment than those who visited a tattoo parlor and did not obtain a
higher body appreciation. For example, individuals may dye their tattoo. Yet, Tiggemann and Golder (2006) found no significant dif-
hair blond or obtain genital waxing to conform to cultural ideals ferences on distinctive appearance investment between those with
of beauty, or they might get a purple streak in their hair and their and without tattoos, and those with easily concealed, moderately
nails painted an unusual pattern to express uniqueness. Thus far, visible, and no tattoos (Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). Other research
these associations have not been empirically tested. suggests that the process of acquiring a tattoo can increase feelings
We examined sex as a moderating factor in the association of uniqueness (Swami, 2011). Specifically, individuals reported sig-
between body appreciation and interest in cosmetic enhancements. nificantly higher need for uniqueness and distinctive appearance
That is, we explored the possibility that the associations between investment several weeks after obtaining a tattoo (Swami, 2011).
body appreciation and cosmetic enhancements might vary by sex. As was done with tattooing, we can also extend the the-
Women are significantly more interested in obtaining cosmetic ory of uniqueness to body appreciation. The core features of
procedures than men (Markey & Markey, 2010; Swami, Chamorro- body appreciation—acceptance, respect, and protection—provide a
Premuzic, Bridges, & Furnham, 2009; Swami et al., 2013). Yet body framework for understanding how one’s body is unique in appear-
appreciation is linked to positive health outcomes for both men ance and function (Tylka, 2011). Feeling as if one’s body is special
and women (Gillen, 2015), suggesting that it might be related to and distinct—whether for how it looks or what it can do—may be
less interest in cosmetic enhancements for both sexes. Because associated with a need for uniqueness. Some individuals have nat-
these associations are not yet clear, in the current study, the mod- ural characteristics that are unusual (e.g., an uncommon name)
ifying effect of sex on body appreciation and interest in cosmetic that in turn predispose them to a higher need for distinctiveness
enhancements was explored. (Lynn & Snyder, 2002). Individuals with higher body appreciation
In addition to body appreciation, it is important to consider are unusual for having positive opinions about their body. Body
the influence of several demographic variables on interest in image concerns are widespread among people of all ages includ-
cosmetic enhancements. In particular, body mass index (BMI) ing children and adults (Gillen & Markey, 2016). Within a cultural
and race/ethnicity may be associated with interest in cosmetic context of bodily discontent, body appreciation may set individuals
enhancements. Because of the pervasive thin ideal in U.S. culture apart from others and therefore predispose them to a dispositional
(Murnen, 2011), individuals who have higher BMIs may be more need for distinctiveness.
interested in cosmetic enhancements as methods for increasing On the other hand, it is also possible that a higher need
perceived attractiveness. Cosmetic enhancements may allow indi- for uniqueness could lead individuals to appreciate their bod-
viduals to feel attractive in ways that do not directly alter weight ies more. For example, individuals who are higher in need for
(e.g., hair dye). Although there is heterogeneity and variability uniqueness are significantly more willing to take risks (Schumpe,
within all racial/ethnic groups, previous research suggests that we Herzberg, & Erb, 2016); more extraverted, less neurotic, and open
might expect some between-group differences. African Americans to new experiences (Schumpe et al., 2016; Swami, Hadji-Michael,
are more satisfied with their appearance and bodies, and they are & Furnham, 2008); more independent in their opinions (Snyder &
more cognitively and behaviorally invested in their appearance as Fromkin, 1977); have higher self-esteem (Gillen, 2015), and; strive
138 M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143

for uniqueness through consumption such as having a stronger body appreciation would be less interested in cosmetic enhance-
desire for scarce, innovative, and customized products (Lynn & ments as compared to individuals with lower body appreciation.
Harris, 1997). Such attitudes and behaviors reflecting a tendency We explored interactions between body appreciation and sex.
toward assertiveness, socialness, and creativity could then promote Second, we hypothesized that individuals with higher body appre-
higher body appreciation. Perhaps these individuals encounter ciation would have a higher need for uniqueness, both general
more people and objects that are different from their own selves uniqueness and uniqueness specific to the appearance domain
and experiences, and this leads them to appreciate body diver- as compared to individuals with lower body appreciation. We
sity, including how their own bodies fit within this framework. Or, explored interactions between body appreciation and sex.
their tendency to be social and noticeable in a positive way may
invite more encouraging feedback from others that enhances their 2. Method
appreciation for their body.
A need for uniqueness among those with higher body appreci- 2.1. Participants
ation may manifest more generally, as well as specifically in the
domain of appearance. People are more interested in striving for Undergraduates enrolled at a regional college of a larger
uniqueness in areas that are salient to their self-concept (Lynn & university in the northeastern United States participated in a
Snyder, 2002). One of these domains is appearance. Appearance study. Participants’ mean age was M = 20.16 years (SD = 3.68), and
is likely an important domain for many people because the unre- 60.9% identified as women. The sample was racially/ethnically
alistic images of men and women in popular culture suggest that diverse with 44.4% identifying as European American/White
there are beauty ideals to which all should aspire (Murnen, 2011). (n = 116), 20.3% as Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islander (n = 53),
Although those with higher body appreciation may actively resist 17.6% as African American/Black (n = 46), 8.0% as Latino/a Amer-
these ideals (Tylka, 2011), resistance nonetheless indicates cogni- ican/Hispanic (n = 21), and 9.6% as other (n = 25). Prior to data
tive effort in the appearance domain. Further, resistance of cultural collection, it was determined that a sample size of at least 250
ideals coupled with love and respect for the body, can develop into was required in order to conduct the planned analyses. This deter-
a defining aspect of the self because of its scarcity (Gillen & Markey, mination was made based on recent literature suggesting that
2016). This reasoning suggests that those with higher body appre- correlations tend to stabilize, in most situations, as sample sizes
ciation may have a high need for uniqueness in the specific realm approach 250 (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013). The sample size for
of appearance. However, because links between body appreciation the current study was N = 261.
and need for uniqueness have not been explored previously, a gen-
eral measure of need for uniqueness as well as one more specific to 2.2. Measures
appearance (i.e., distinctive appearance investment; Tiggemann &
Golder, 2006) were included in the current study. 2.2.1. Body mass index (BMI)
We also examined the modifying effect of sex on body apprecia- Participants reported their height and weight and from these
tion and need for uniqueness to determine if these associations are data, BMI was calculated (see Centers for Disease Control and
significant for both men and women or women only. Although no Prevention, 2016, for formula). Descriptive data on BMI from this
significant sex differences have been found on need for uniqueness sample were also measured and presented in Gillen (2015).
and distinctive appearance investment (Swami, 2011), findings
on body appreciation have been more variable, with some work 2.2.2. Body appreciation
showing significant (albeit small) sex differences and other work The Body Appreciation Scale (BAS; Avalos et al., 2005) was used
showing non-significant sex differences (Gillen, 2015; Swami et al., to assess positive body image. The original 13-item measure was
2008; Tylka, 2013). Thus, previous empirical work does not inform used because the second version of the measure (the Body Appreci-
hypotheses about how sex might act as a moderator in the relations ation Scale-2; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015b) was not available at
between body appreciation and need for uniqueness, suggesting the time of data collection. This measure captures appreciation for
that this question is worth exploring in the current study. the body (sample item: “I feel good about my body”), one aspect of
In addition to body appreciation, we examined associations the multifaceted construct of positive body image (Tylka & Wood-
between several demographic variables and need for uniqueness. Barcalow, 2015a). Although the BAS was initially created for use
BMI may relate to need for uniqueness. Because individuals with with women, scores on the scale were found to be invariant for U.S.
larger BMIs do not meet cultural definitions of attractiveness men and women (Tylka, 2013), with differential item responding
(Murnen, 2011), they may have a stronger need to feel distinct so on one item. Responses were made on a 5-point scale (1 = Never
that they stand out in a positive way. This may be true on global to 5 = Always). Items were averaged to create a total score, with
measures of need for uniqueness, but it may be particularly evi- higher scores representing higher body appreciation. BAS scores
dent on measures specific to appearance. Further, race/ethnicity have demonstrated satisfactory validity and reliability in previous
may be relevant to need for uniqueness. African Americans/Blacks studies of U.S. college students (Avalos et al., 2005; Tylka, 2013).
may have a stronger desire for uniqueness than those in other Internal consistency reliability scores in the current study were
racial/ethnic groups. Parker et al. (1995) found that African Amer- adequate (Cronbach’s ˛: women ˛ = .92, men ˛ = .90). Descriptive
ican girls have more flexible ideas of beauty than White girls. In data on body appreciation from this sample were also measured
particular, African American girls’ ideas of attractiveness include and presented in Gillen (2015).
“looking good,” or being well-kept, confident, and making your
own assets work for you, suggesting that unique personal styles 2.2.3. Interest in cosmetic enhancements
are desirable and something toward which to strive (Parker et al., A subscale from the Interest in Cosmetic Surgery Scale (Markey
1995). & Markey, 2009) was used to assess interest in cosmetic enhance-
ments. This subscale contains 6 items and measures interest in
1.3. Hypotheses obtaining cosmetic procedures that are typically low in cost and
invasiveness, do not require a large time commitment, and do
Drawing on research and theory on interest in cosmetic not need to be performed in a medical context. The items were:
enhancements and need for uniqueness, we proposed two facial, manicure, pedicure, facial waxing (hair removal on face using
hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that individuals with higher wax, e.g., eyebrows), genital waxing (hair removal on genital area
M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143 139

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations for study variables.

BMI BAS ICE SANU DAI

BMI (body mass index) – −.31** .05 .14 .06


Body appreciation (BAS) −.31** – .10 .17* .24**
Interest in cosmetic enhancements (ICE) .08 −.12 – .08 .10
Self-attributed need for uniqueness (SANU) .04 .22* .06 – .55**
Distinctive appearance investment (DAI) .06 .12 .02 .40** –
M (SD) — men 24.52 (4.19) 3.99 (0.65) 0.50 (0.22) 3.14 (0.71) 3.12 (0.71)
M (SD) — women 24.12 (5.38) 3.65 (0.79) 1.33 (0.33) 3.20 (0.72) 3.20 (0.66)
ta 0.63 3.62 22.52 0.77 1.12
p .529 <.001 <.001 .445 .265
Cohen’s d 0.08 0.47 2.96 0.08 0.12

Note: N = 261. Women’s correlations are above the diagonal and men’s are below.
a
Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, critical value .0125 (.05/4).
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

using wax, e.g., bikini wax), and hair coloring/dye. Response options students’ attitudes and behaviors related to body modification.
range from 1 = I would never consider this procedure to 5 = I would Measures were presented in the same order for all participants.
definitely consider this procedure. The average of the items was used Other measures in the study pertain to body image, body modifica-
for the subscale total. Higher scores indicate greater interest in cos- tion (e.g., piercings and tattoos), risk behaviors and attitudes, and
metic enhancements. Internal consistency reliability scores were mental health. The Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the univer-
adequate (women ˛ = .75, men ˛ = .72). sity approved this study.

2.2.4. Distinctive appearance investment 2.4. Statistical analyses


The Distinctive Appearance Investment Scale (Tiggemann &
Golder, 2006) was used to measure desire for uniqueness in the All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS v.24 for Windows.
appearance domain (sample items: “I use my physical appearance All p-values <.05 were considered statistically significant, and two-
to express who I am,” “When it comes to appearance, I am some- tailed p-values are reported for all hypothesis tests. We initially
what of a non-conformist.”). It has 6 items to which participants assessed the data for missing values. Participants with more than
respond on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly 20% of their data missing were excluded from all analyses. Using Lit-
agree). Items were averaged to create a total score, with higher tle’s test (Little, 1988), the remaining missing data were determined
scores indicating stronger desire for a unique appearance. Previous to be missing completely at random (MCAR). We used pooled esti-
research demonstrates the validity and reliability of scores on this mates from multiple imputation, using all relevant study variables
measure in men and women (Swami, 2011; Tiggemann & Golder, in the imputation model and 20 iterations, to replace the remain-
2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). Internal consistency reliabil- ing missing data. Following replacement of the missing values, data
ity scores in the current study were adequate (women ˛ = .76, men were assessed for entry errors and significant outliers (±3 SD). No
˛ = .82). entry errors or significant outliers were found and therefore no
further changes were made to the data.
2.2.5. Need for uniqueness Independent samples t-tests were conducted to assess dif-
The Self-Attributed Need for Uniqueness Scale (Lynn & Harris, ferences in key study variables for men and women, and
1997) was used to measure desire for uniqueness. Unlike the Dis- one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to determine
tinctive Appearance Investment Scale (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006), racial/ethnic differences in these same variables. All ANOVA mod-
it is a more general measure of desire for uniqueness not limited els met the appropriate assumptions (e.g., normality, homogeneity
to the appearance domain. Although it was developed for con- of variance). To control for Type I error, Tukey HSD post hoc pair-
sumer research (Lynn & Harris, 1997), it has been used previously wise comparisons were used. Partial eta-squared (p 2 ) was used
in body image research (Swami, 2011). It has 4 “fill-in the-blank” to estimate effect sizes of the ANOVAs, using guidelines suggested
items to which participants respond on a 5-point scale. Response by Ferguson (2009; small = .04; medium = .25; large = .64). Cohen’s d
options differ across items (sample item: “Being distinctive is was used to estimate effect sizes of the pairwise comparisons, using
important to me.” a = Not at all, b = Slightly, c = Moderately, d = Very, guidelines suggested by Cohen (1988; small = 0.20; medium = 0.50;
e = Extremely). Items were averaged to create a total score, with large = 0.80). Bivariate correlations were used to examine the asso-
higher scores indicating stronger desire for uniqueness. Scores on ciations among key study variables. Stepwise linear regression
this measure have shown satisfactory validity and reliability in men models were used to test the hypotheses. To avoid issues with
and women in previous studies (Lynn & Harris, 1997; Swami, 2011). multicollinearity, all variables used in the regression models were
Internal consistency reliability scores in the current study were mean-centered. All regression models met the appropriate regres-
adequate (women ˛ = .75, men ˛ = .73). sion assumptions (e.g., normality, VIF levels).

2.3. Procedures 3. Results

After giving informed consent, participants completed a paper- Table 1 shows mean scores for all analytic variables separated
and-pencil questionnaire on campus in a group setting, supervised by sex. A series of Bonferroni-corrected t-tests were used to test
by members of the research team. Participants’ responses were for significant sex differences. Men had significantly higher body
not linked to their names. Students were recruited from a partic- appreciation than women (d = 0.47; medium effect), and women
ipant pool at the college, so they elected to participate for course were significantly more interested in cosmetic enhancements than
credit in one of several foundational courses in psychology. The men (d = 2.96; large effect). Men and women did not differ signif-
study was advertised as a research study about body modification icantly on BMI, need for uniqueness, and distinctive appearance
in college students with the purpose being to learn about college investment.
140 M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143

Table 2
Racial/ethnic group comparisons on study variables.

Variablea European African Asian American/ Latino/a Other (n = 25) F p Partial 2


American/ White American/ Black Asian/Pacific American/
(n = 116) (n = 46) Islander (n = 53) Hispanic (n = 21)

BMI 23.83 (4.98)a 26.04 (5.76)a 23.45 (4.23)a 24.29 (4.73)a 24.86 (4.16)a 2.21 .069 .03
BAS 3.68 (0.82)a 3.96 (0.68)a 3.72 (0.65)a 3.99 (0.68)a 3.90 (0.82)a 1.81 .128 .03
ICE 3.04 (1.53)abc 3.31 (1.47)abc 2.52 (1.32)b 3.63 (1.44)c 2.82 (1.54)abc 2.98 .020 .05
SANU 3.04 (0.60)a 3.65 (0.83)b 3.06 (0.66)a 3.14 (0.59)a 3.20 (0.88)ab 6.92 <.001 .10
DAI 3.06 (0.60)a 3.53 (0.76)b 3.11 (0.57)a 2.98 (0.74)a 3.25 (0.84)ab 4.96 .001 .07

Note: BMI = Body mass index, BAS = Body Appreciation Scale, ICE = Interest in cosmetic enhancements, SANU = Self-attributed need for uniqueness, DAI = Distinctive appearance
investment. Same superscripts indicate that group means do not differ.
a
Means (and standard deviations) reported for each variable.

Five one-way ANOVAs were conducted to test for mean dif- enhancement and/or self-expression, including interest in cosmetic
ferences in key study variables across racial/ethnic groups (see enhancements and need for uniqueness. Results generally sug-
Table 2). There were no significant racial/ethnic differences in gested that individuals with higher body appreciation also tended
body appreciation or in BMI (ps > .05). However, overall mod- to show a stronger need for uniqueness. Body appreciation was
els revealed small significant differences between the groups in associated with a greater desire for uniqueness both generally and
need for uniqueness, F(4, 256) = 6.92, p < .001, p 2 = .10, distinc- in the domain of appearance; these findings emerged once the
tive appearance investment, F(4, 256) = 4.96, p = .001, p 2 = .07, effects of sex, BMI, and race/ethnicity had been accounted for. Body
and interest in cosmetic enhancements, F(4, 256) = 2.98, p = .020, appreciation was not significantly associated with interest in cos-
p 2 = .05. Post hoc Tukey HSD pair-wise comparisons revealed that metic enhancements.
African American/Black participants reported significantly higher Body appreciation significantly predicted both uniqueness vari-
need for uniqueness and distinctive appearance investment (all ables: distinctive appearance investment and self-attributed need
ps < .047, ds = 0.71–.84; large effects) when compared to all other for uniqueness. Distinctive appearance investment includes the
racial/ethnic groups with the exception of those identifying as need to stand out in the domain of appearance. People strive to
‘other.’ For interest in cosmetic enhancements, the only significant be unique in domains that are more integral to their sense of self
difference was between Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islanders and (Lynn & Snyder, 2002). Appearance may be particularly important
Latino/a Americans/Hispanics (p = .038, d = 0.81; large effect), in to those with higher body appreciation, but not just for aesthetic
that Latino/a Americans/Hispanics reported greater interest in cos- reasons. Individuals with higher body appreciation may value their
metic enhancements than Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islanders. bodies for what they can do, as well as the special combination of
Because of these significant group differences, race/ethnicity is features they have that comprise their unique selves. Feeling a close
examined as predictor in all subsequent regression analyses. connection with and sense of appreciation for one’s body (Tylka,
Next, correlations were performed to illustrate bivariate asso- 2011) may be related to wanting to publically express its perceived
ciations between variables, and were conducted separately by sex special qualities.
(see Table 1). In men, higher body appreciation was significantly Self-attributed need for uniqueness is a more general measure
associated with both lower BMI and higher self-attributed need of need for uniqueness, and also demonstrated significant asso-
for uniqueness, although the associations were weak. In women, ciations with body appreciation. That is, individuals with higher
higher body appreciation was significantly associated with lower body appreciation also tended to show a higher need to be dis-
BMI, higher self-attributed need for uniqueness, and higher distinc- tinct. The Self-Attributed Need for Uniqueness Scale is a relatively
tive appearance investment, although the associations were again brief measure that does not specify domains in which individu-
weak. als strive for uniqueness. Yet, it appears that individuals’ need for
Next, two linear regressions were performed to illustrate associ- uniqueness extends beyond the domain of appearance into a more
ations between key study variables and to determine the amount of general need to feel different from others. Being different might
variance accounted for in the outcomes by body appreciation (see lead to being challenged by others for one’s unique characteristics,
Table 3). In order to use race/ethnicity as a predictor variable in the and therefore requires a degree of confidence in upholding these
regression models, we created five dummy variables. In the analy- same qualities. As supported by the theory of uniqueness (Snyder &
ses, European American/White was omitted and thus served as the Fromkin, 1977), striving for difference is a positive process related
reference category for each racial/ethnic group. Demographic vari- to higher self-esteem. Previous research also demonstrates that
ables including sex, BMI, and the five dummy coded race/ethnicity body appreciation is significantly associated with characteristics
variables were entered in Step 1, body appreciation was entered that are conducive to self-expression such as higher self-esteem
in Step 2, and body appreciation × sex was entered in Step 3. The (Gillen, 2015), lower neuroticism (Swami et al., 2008), higher
outcomes were self-attributed need for uniqueness and distinctive extraversion (Swami et al., 2008), lower maladaptive perfection-
appearance investment (because body appreciation was not sig- ism (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012), greater sexual liberalism and
nificantly associated with interest in cosmetic enhancements, we more positive attitudes toward unconventional sexual practices
omitted the regression model with interest in cosmetic enhance- (Swami, Weis, Barron, & Furnham, 2017). Thus, body appreciation
ments as the outcome). Step 2 (but not Step 3) added a significant may encourage confidence that allows people to strive for differ-
amount of variance in both regression models (see Table 3). Indi- ence. It is also possible that the mere presence of body appreciation
viduals with higher body appreciation and those who identified is a unique quality. It is normative for people to have body image
as African American/Black reported significantly higher distinc- concerns (Gillen & Markey, 2016), and less common to score high on
tive appearance investment and significantly higher self-attributed measures of positive body image, at least among women (Wood-
need for uniqueness. Barcalow, Tylka, & Augustus-Horvath, 2010). Amidst widespread
4. Discussion body concerns, higher body appreciation may set individuals
apart from others. Naturally unusual qualities (i.e., body appre-
The goal of the current study was to explore associations ciation) may, over time, instill in people a trait level need for
between body appreciation and variables that reflect self- distinctiveness (Lynn & Snyder, 2002). It is important to consider,
M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143 141

however, that the need for uniqueness may be culturally-bound. Fromkin, 1977), and may explain why African Americans/Blacks
Individuals in cultures with independent construals of the self, like tend to have more positive body image than those in other
the U.S., may be more prone to strive for uniqueness than those racial/ethnic groups, particularly European Americans/Whites
in cultures that encourage interdependent perspectives of the self (Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2012; Parker et al., 1995; Swami, Airs, Chouhan,
(Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Future research should focus on asso- Leon, & Towell, 2009). This finding may also reflect Majors and
ciations between body appreciation and need for uniqueness in Billson’s (1992) work on African American men, who adapt a ‘cool
non-Western cultures that are more oriented toward interdepen- pose’ in an effort to distinguish their identities in a culture that
dent selves. limits their opportunities for success. Thus, for some oppressed
Interestingly, body appreciation was not associated with inter- groups, appearance is a controllable domain through which they
est in cosmetic enhancements. Cosmetic enhancements (e.g., can express their unique qualities (Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2006).
genital waxing, hair coloring) are not very time consuming or costly,
and are generally easily accessible and low in risk. Therefore, body
appreciation may not distinguish those who are interested in pur- 4.1. Limitations, strengths, and conclusions
suing these relatively commonplace procedures from those who
are not. Another potential explanation for the lack of association The present study has limitations. It is correlational and there-
between body appreciation and interest in cosmetic enhancements fore does not allow conclusions to be drawn regarding the direction
is the meaning individuals attach to these procedures. Some may of associations. For example, body appreciation may lead individ-
engage in them to achieve cultural ideals of beauty, some may uals to strive for uniqueness, just as need for uniqueness may also
obtain them to achieve a distinct appearance, whereas others may be conducive to appreciating one’s body. These associations may
see them as a form of self-care. These different motives may wash also be confounded by third variables such as personality and self-
out any significant associations between these variables. esteem. The measure of cosmetic enhancements is limited in scope
Results from the regressions also showed that African Amer- (i.e., 6 items) and could be expanded to include other procedures,
icans/Blacks had significantly higher need for uniqueness, both including some that may be more common in men, and some
generally and in the domain of appearance. This finding is consis- that could better reflect need for uniqueness (e.g., adding items
tent with previous research illustrating that African American girls on type/color of hair dye). In addition, our results are not widely
value and strive for utilizing and expressing the unique aspects generalizable. Participants represented a select group of individu-
of one’s appearance (Parker et al., 1995). Striving for uniqueness als: psychology students from one non-residential college located
is a positive process related to improved self-esteem (Snyder & in the northeastern U.S. The study was advertised as one about
body modification, so individuals who have body modifications
Table 3
Regression models predicting need for uniqueness.

Distinctive appearance investment Self-attributed need for uniqueness

B SE ˇ t pr2 B SE ˇ t pr2

Step 1
Sex .08 .09 .06 0.99 .06 .05 .09 .03 0.52 .03
BMI .00 .01 .02 0.26 .02 .01 .01 .06 0.90 .06
Race/ethnicity
African American/Black .46*** .12 .26 3.93 .24 .58*** .12 .31 4.75 .29
Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islander .05 .11 .03 0.46 .03 .02 .12 .01 0.18 .01
Latino/a American/Hispanic -.10 .16 -.04 -0.62 -.04 .08 .16 .03 0.51 .03
Other .19 .15 .08 1.29 .08 .14 .15 .06 0.94 .06

Step 2
Sex .16 .09 .12 1.85 .12 .13 .09 .09 1.39 .09
BMI .01 .01 .09 1.37 .09 .02 .01 .13 1.96 .12
Race/ethnicity
African American/Black .38** .12 .21 3.18 .20 .49*** .12 .26 4.00 .24
Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islander .05 .11 .03 0.44 .03 .02 .11 .01 0.16 .01
Latino/a American/Hispanic -.18 .16 -.07 -1.13 -.07 .00 .16 .00 0.01 .00
Other .13 .15 .06 0.92 .06 .09 .15 .04 0.57 .04
Body appreciation .19** .06 .21 3.11 .19 .19** .06 .20 3.09 .19

Step 3
Sex -.09 .49 -.07 -0.19 -.01 .45 .50 .31 0.90 .06
BMI .01 .01 .09 1.37 .09 .02 .01 .13 1.95 .12
Race/ethnicity
African American/Black .37** .12 .21 3.11 .19 .50*** .12 .27 4.04 .25
Asian American/Asian/Pacific Islander .03 .11 .02 0.31 .02 .04 .12 .02 0.30 .02
Latino/a American/Hispanic -.18 .16 -.07 -1.15 -.07 .01 .16 .00 0.06 .00
Other .13 .15 .06 0.89 .06 .09 .15 .04 0.60 .04
Body appreciation .14 .10 .16 1.39 .09 .25* .11 .26 2.34 .15
Body appreciation × sex .07 .12 .18 0.53 .03 -.08 .13 -.22 -0.66 -.04

Step 1 adjusted R2 .05** .08***


Step 2 adjusted R2 .09*** .11***
Step 3 adjusted R2 .08*** .11***
R2 1–2 .03** .03**
R2 2–3 .00 .00

Note: N = 261. pr2 = partial correlation. Sex is coded as 0 = men, 1 = women, race/ethnicity was dummy coded as five variables with European American/White serving as the
reference group.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
142 M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143

themselves or feel particularly positive or negative toward body Lynn, M., & Snyder, C. R. (2002). Uniqueness seeking. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.),
modifications may have been more likely to participate. Although Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 395–410). New York: Oxford University
Press.
not available at the time of data collection, there is a newer version Majors, R., & Billson, J. M. (1992). Cool pose: The dilemmas of Black manhood in America.
of the Body Appreciation Scale (BAS-2; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, New York: Lexington Books.
2015b) that addresses some of the limitations of the original BAS Markey, C. N., & Markey, P. M. (2009). Correlates of young women’s interest in obtain-
ing cosmetic surgery. Sex Roles, 61, 158–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-
and incorporates new information gleaned from the positive body 009-9625-5
image literature. This newer measure may have better captured Markey, C. N., & Markey, P. M. (2010). A correlational and experimental examination
body appreciation in our sample. of reality television viewing and interest in cosmetic surgery. Body Image, 7,
165–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.10.006
A strength of the current study is its focus on several putative
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition,
correlates of body appreciation that focus on self-enhancement emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. http://dx.doi.org/
and/or expression that have not been frequently examined in prior 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
Murnen, S. K. (2011). Gender and body images. In T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body
research. Results show that individuals with higher body appreci-
image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd ed., pp. 173–179).
ation also had a stronger desire to be different from others which New York: Guilford Press.
may represent an underlying confidence in themselves and their Parker, S., Nichter, M., Nichter, M., Vuckovic, N., Sims, C., & Ritenbaugh, C. (1995).
bodies. Future studies should continue to focus on associations Body image and weight concerns among African American and white adolescent
females: Differences that make a difference. Human Organization, 54, 103–114.
between body appreciation and need for uniqueness. It would http://dx.doi.org/10.17730/humo.54.2.06h6637445q650450
be interesting, for example, to examine if body appreciation is Saltzberg, E. A., & Chrisler, J. C. (1995). Beauty is the beast: Psychological effects
associated with markers that signify a unique appearance such as of the pursuit of the perfect female body. In J. Freeman (Ed.), Women: A femi-
nist perspective (5th ed., pp. 306–315). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing
tattoos, piercings, facial cosmetics, and clothing styles (Tiggemann Company.
& Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). Potential mediators Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabi-
of the links between body appreciation and need for uniqueness lize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 609–612. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2013.05.009
could also be explored including self-esteem and personality traits Schumpe, B. M., Herzberg, P. Y., & Erb, H. (2016). Assessing the need for uniqueness:
(e.g., extraversion). The present findings add to the growing litera- Validation of the German NfU-G scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 90,
ture on positive body image by indicating that body appreciation is 231–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t07564-000
Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1977). Abnormality as a positive characteristic: The
related to indicators of “positive striving for differentness” (Snyder
development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal
& Fromkin, 1977, p. 518) of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 518–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.86.
5.518
Swami, V. (2009). Body appreciation, media influence, and weight status predict
References consideration of cosmetic surgery among female undergraduates. Body Image,
6, 315–317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.07.001
Andrew, R., Tiggemann, M., & Clark, L. (2016). Positive body image and young Swami, V. (2010). Translation and validation of the Malay Acceptance of Cosmetic
women’s health: Implications for sun protection cancer screening, weight loss, Surgery Scale. Body Image, 7, 372–375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.
and alcohol consumption behaviours. Journal of Health Psychology, 21, 28–39. 07.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105314520814 Swami, V. (2011). Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance
Avalos, L., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale: anxiety and dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Body
Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2, 285–297. http://dx. Image, 8, 237–244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.005
doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.06.002 Swami, V. (2012). Written on the body? Individual differences between British adults
Centers for Disease Control, & Prevention. (2016). About BMI for adults.. Retrieved who do and do not obtain a first tattoo. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53,
from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult bmi/index.html 407–412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00960.x
(Accessed 31 October 2016) Swami, V., Airs, N., Chouhan, B., Leon, M. A. P., & Towell, T. (2009). Are there eth-
Chrisler, J. C., Gorman, J. A., Serra, K. E., & Chapman, K. R. (2012). Facing up to aging: nic differences in positive body image among female British undergraduates?
Mid-life women’s attitudes toward cosmetic procedures. Women and Therapy, European Psychologist, 14, 288–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.14.4.
35, 193–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02703149.2012.684540 288
Chrisler, J. C., Marvan, M. L., Gorman, J. A., & Rossini, M. (2015). Body appreciation Swami, V., Campana, A. N., & Coles, R. (2012). Acceptance of cosmetic surgery among
and attitudes toward menstruation. Body Image, 12, 78–81. http://dx.doi.org/10. British female university students: Are there ethnic differences? European Psy-
1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.003 chologist, 17, 55–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000049
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Swami, V., Campana, A. N., Ferreira, L., Barrett, S., Harris, A. S., & Tavares, M. C. (2011).
Routledge Academic. The Acceptance of Cosmetic Surgery Scale: Initial examination of its factor struc-
Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. ture and correlates among Brazilian adults. Body Image, 8, 179–185. http://dx.
Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 40, 532–538. http://dx.doi.org/10. doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.01.001
1037/a0015808 Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Bridges, S., & Furnham, A. (2009). Acceptance of
Gillen, M. M. (2015). Associations between positive body image and indicators of cosmetic surgery: Personality and individual difference predictors. Body Image,
men’s and women’s mental and physical health. Body Image, 13, 67–74. http:// 6, 7–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t21011-000
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.01.002 Swami, V., Hadji-Michael, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). Personality and individual dif-
Gillen, M. M., & Lefkowitz, E. S. (2006). Gender role development and body image ference correlates of positive body image. Body Image, 5, 322–325. http://dx.doi.
among male and female first year college students. Sex Roles, 55, 25–37. http:// org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.03.007
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9057-4 Swami, V., Hwang, C. S., & Jung, J. (2012). Factor structure and correlates of
Gillen, M. M., & Lefkowitz, E. S. (2012). Gender and racial/ethnic differences in body the Acceptance of Cosmetic Surgery Scale among South Korean University
image development among college students. Body Image, 9, 126–130. http://dx. students. Aesthetic Surgery Journal, 32, 220–229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.09.004 1090820X11431577
Gillen, M. M., & Markey, C. H. (2016). Body image and mental health. In H. S. Fried- Swami, V., Pietschnig, J., Stewart, N., Nader, N. W., Stieger, S., Shannon, S., &
man (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health (2nd ed., pp. 187–192). Waltham, MA: Voracek, M. (2013). Blame it on patriarchy: More sexist attitudes are associated
Academic Press. with stronger consideration of cosmetic surgery for oneself and one’s partner.
Grant, R. L., Gillen, M. M., & Bernstein, M. J. (2014). The tan bias: Perceptions of International Journal of Psychology, 48, 1221–1229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
tan and non-tan Caucasian faces. Health Behavior and Policy Review, 1, 484–489. 00207594.2012.740566
http://dx.doi.org/10.14485/HBPR.1.6.6 Swami, V., Weis, L., Barron, D., & Furnham, A. (2017). Associations between posi-
Iannantuono, A. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2012). Interpersonal and intrapersonal links to body tive body image, sexual liberalism, and unconventional sexual practices in U.S.
appreciation in college women: An exploratory model. Body Image, 9, 227–235. adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0924-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01.004 y
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate Tiggemann, M., & Golder, F. (2006). Tattooing: An expression of uniqueness in
data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83, the appearance domain. Body Image, 3, 309–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
1198–1202. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2290157 bodyim.2006.09.002
Lunde, C. (2013). Acceptance of cosmetic surgery, body appreciation, body ideal Tiggemann, M., & Hopkins, L. A. (2011). Tattoos and piercings: Bodily expression of
internalization, and fashion blog reading among late adolescents in Sweden. uniqueness? Body Image, 8, 245–250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.
Body Image, 10, 632–635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.06.007 03.007
Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997). Individual differences in the pursuit of self-uniqueness Tylka, T. L. (2011). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In T. F. Cash &
through consumption. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1861–1883. http:// L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1997.tb01629.x ed., pp. 56–64). New York: Guilford Press.
M.M. Gillen, J. Dunaev / Body Image 22 (2017) 136–143 143

Tylka, T. L. (2013). Evidence for the Body Appreciation Scale’s measurement equiv- Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015b). The Body Appreciation Scale-2: Item
alence/invariance between U.S. College women and men. Body Image, 10, refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12, 53–67. http://dx.doi.
415–418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.02.006 org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006
Tylka, T. L., & Iannantuono, A. C. (2016). Perceiving beauty in all women: Psychome- Wasylkiw, L., & Butler, N. A. (2014). Body talk among undergraduate women: Why
tric evaluation of the Broad Conceptualization of Beauty Scale. Body Image, 17, conversations about exercise and weight loss differentially predict body appre-
67–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.005 ciation. Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 1013–1024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The Intuitive Eating Scale-2: Item refine- 1359105313483155
ment and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. Journal of Wood-Barcalow, N. L., Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. L. (2010). But I like my
Counseling Psychology, 60, 137–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030893 body: Positive body image characteristics and a holistic model for young-adult
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015a). What is and what is not positive women. Body Image, 7, 106–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.01.
body image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition. Body Image, 14, 001
118–129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.001

Вам также может понравиться