Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 22

Economic Stress, Coercive Family Process, and

Developmental Problems of Adolescents

Rand D. Conger and Xiaojia Ge


Iowa State University

Glen H. Elder, Jr.


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Frederick O. Lorenz and Ronald L. Simons


Iowa State University

CONGER, RAND D.; CE, XIAOJIA; ELDER, CLEN H . , JR.; LORENZ, FREDERICK O.; and SIMONS, RON-
ALD L. Economic Stress, Coercive Family Process, and Developmental Problems of Adolescents.
CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1994, 65, 541-561. We propose a model of family conflict and coercion
that links economic stress in family life to adolescent symptoms of internalizing and externalizing
emotions and bebaviors. The 180 boys and 198 girls in the study were living in intact families
in the rural Midwest, an area characterized hy economic decline and uncertainty. Theoretical
constructs in the model were measured using hoth trained observer and family member reports.
These adolescents and their parents were interviewed each year for 3 years during the seventh,
eighth, and ninth grades. Our theoretical model proposes that economic pressure experienced
by parents increases parental dysphoria and marital conflict as well as conflicts between parents
and children over money. High levels of spousal irritability, coupled with coercive exchanges
over money matters, were expected to he associated with greater hostility in general by parents
toward their children. These hostile/coercive exchanges were expected to increase the likelihood
of adolescent emotional and behavioral problems. Overall, results were consistent with the pro-
posed model. Moreover, the hypothesized processes applied equally well to the hehavior of
mothers and iathers, as well as sons and daughters.

During the past decade, the rural Mid- we examine the influence of continuing
west suffered its most dramatic economic economic strain on people living in rural ar-
decline since the Depression years of the eas that are not expected to regain the eco-
1930s. Literally thousands of farmers and nomic prosperity they once enjoyed even a
small town businesses went bankrupt, the decade ago.
rural poverty rate increased beyond that for
metropolitan areas, and the financial pros- Research evidence continues to accu-
pects of many rural residents were placed in mulate that economic hardship, such as that
jeopardy for the foreseeable future (David- experienced in the rural Midwest, has an ad-
son, 1990; Friedberger, 1989; Murdock & verse influence on psychological well-being
Leistritz, 1988). As in urban America, these and on the quality of family relationships
economic problems are likely to be most de- (Conger, McCarty, Yang, Lahey, & Kropp,
structive for a community and for individual 1984; Conger et al., 1990, 1991, 1992, 1993;
family members when they become chronic. Elder, 1974; Elder & Caspi, 1988; Elder,
seemingly without solution (Davidson, 1990; Conger, Foster, 6c Ardelt, 1992; Liker & El-
Wilson, 1987). In the present report involv- der, 1983; McLoyd, 1989; Voydanoff, 1990;
ing rural Iowa families and adolescents, Whitbeck et al., 1991). Moreover, an increas-

This paper is hased on collaborative research involving the Iowa Youth and Families Project
at Iowa State University, Ames, and the Social Change Project at the University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill. The comhined research effort is currently supported hy the National Institute of
Mental Health (MH43270 and MH48165), the National Institute on Dnig Abuse (DA05347), the
John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Program for Successful Adolescent Develop-
ment among Youth in High-Risk Settings, and a Research Scientist Award (MH00567). Requests
for reprints should he addressed to Rand Conger, Department of Sociology, 107 East Hall, Iowa
State University, Ames, IA, 50011; Project No. 2931 of the Iowa Agriculture and Home Econom-
ics Experiinent Station, Ames, Iowa.
[Child Development, 1994, 65, 541-561. © 1994 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/94/6502-000.3$01.0d]
542 Child Development
ing number of studies has shown that the family member. More specifically, he notes
influence of economic stress on adolescents that "the term coercive refers to the use of
is mediated to a significant degree by disrup- an aversive stimulus by one member of a
tions in parent-child relations (Flanagan, dyad that is contingent on the behavior of
1990; Lempers, Clark-Lempers, & Simons, the other person" (Patterson et al., 1992, p.
1989; McLoyd & Wilson, 1990, 1991). The 39).
processes through which economic strains
exacerbate emotional distress or troubled re- Patterson (1982) postulates that specific
lationships in families, however, have not reinforcement contingencies operate to es-
been adequately identified. calate the level of coercive or aversive inter-
changes; however, research to date has
We expect that strong emotional re- failed to demonstrate the impact of such
sponses to hardship, including withdrawal contingencies on problematic adolescent
from intimates as well as hostility in marital development independent of the base rates
and parent-child interactions, are consonant for coercive interactions (Knutson, 1982;
with findings from a rich history of social Patterson, 1982; Patterson et al., 1992;
psychological research that demonstrates Robinson & Jacobson, 1987).^ As Robinson
the negative consequences of aversive and Jacobson (1987) have noted, the central
events and conditions for human health and finding to date from research on coercion
behavior (Berkowitz, 1989). The loss or ab- theory is that families at risk for having chil-
sence of valued resources has been shown dren with developmental problems are con-
both experimentally and in naturalistic stud- siderably more likely than controls to dem-
ies to produce a variety of emotional re- onstrate high levels of conHict or aggression
sponses such as apathy, depression, anger, in their interactions. Thus, we use the con-
or aggression (Berkowitz, 1989; Conger et cept of coercive family process to describe
al., 1984; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992). the degree to which families engage in high
The adverse impact of economic hardship on levels of aggressive or aversive interaction,
individual well-being is further exacerbated presumably as a mode of interpersonal con-
by its indirect effects through the behaviors trol. We propose that economic stress is an
of other family members. Indeed, Coyne and environmental factor that increases the like-
Downey (1991) have recently argued that lihood of such control strategies, particularly
the acute stresses and chronic strains of daily in struggles or disagreements over the use
life may have their most debilitating effect of material resources.
on individual functioning through the con-
flicts and disruptions they may create in
one's closest social relationships. Earlier studies have suggested that such
a process may be involved in the emotional
This study examines the possibility that and relationship difficulties of economically
the aversive experience of economic stress hard-pressed couples. For example, in their
will increase the occurrence of what Pat- research on Depression era families, Liker
terson has called coercive family processes and Elder (1983) found that economic stress
(Patterson, 1982; Patterson et al., 1992). Ac- was associated with spouses' confiicts over
cording to Patterson, a high density of irrita- finances. For a contemporary sample of 76
ble, hostile interchanges between family couples. Conger et al. (1990) also found that
members often occurs when stress is high financial difficulties were associated with
and parents suffer the dysphoria associated higher levels of marital conflict. This result
with such life circumstances. He suggests has been replicated with a second sample as
that these negative interactions often in- well (Conger et al., 1992). These latter two
volve a negative reinforcement mechanism studies, however, were concerned with con-
in which hostile behaviors are used in at- fiicts in general and not those specifically
tempts to control the behavior of another related to finances.

^According to Patterson (1982), the operation of reinforcement contingencies can he in-


ferred from sequential chains of ohserved hehavior when predicted conditional prohahilities are
signiflcantly greater than unconditional probabilities or base rates. For example, coercion theory
suggests that, in a coercive family, the conditional probability of the parent being hostile to the
child after the child has first been hostile to the parent will he significantly greater than the
parent's unconditional probability, or base rate, of being hostile to the child. This escalating
process has not yet been clearly demonstrated; however, it is clear that problem families are
generally more aversive or hostile toward one another (i.e., they have higher hase rates) than
controls.
Conger et al. 543
Although it seems reasonable that, for paying its debts, and (c) has had to cut back
parents and older children or adolescents, on everyday expenses in an attempt to live
similar confiicts and control attempts over within available means (Conger et al., 1990,
the use of money might occur, this possibil- 1992).^ Consistent with the research just re-
ity has not been directly tested in previous viewed, we postulated that, when economic
research. To be sure, several studies have conditions worsen to the point that parents
demonstrated that financial pressures may experience this degree of deterioration in
increase parental hostility toward children their financial situation, they will become
(Conger et al., 1984, 1991, 1992, 1993; Elder depressed, demoralized, pessimistic about
& Caspi, 1988; Elder et al., 1992; McLoyd, the future, and generally less stable emo-
1989), but the specific role of confiicts about tionally (Fig. 1).
money has not been evaluated in the analy-
ses. In the following presentation of the the- The proposed model both builds upon
oretical model guiding the present study, we and also extends earlier theory regarding
hypothesize that confiicts over money will family mediation of economic stress infiu-
directly infiuence adolescent development ences on adolescent development. Consis-
and also exacerbate more generally hostile tent with previous work, we hypothesize
behavior by parents. that economic distress is indirectly linked to
adolescent adjustment through parental
mood and marital difficulties (Elder &
The Theoretieal Model Caspi, 1988; McLoyd, 1989). The model
Drawing on past research with finan- adds to previous theory and empirical work
cially distressed families, we proposed a the- by also postulating a direct path from eco-
oretical model that specifies the role that co- nomic pressure to parent/adolescent confiict
ercive family processes might play in linking over finances. This construct describes con-
family economic stress to problematic ado- tingent, aversive interactions (coercive be-
lescent development (Fig. 1). Several stud- haviors in Patterson's terms) involving strug-
ies have demonstrated that adverse eco- gles over money that are expected to occur
nomic conditions (the exogenous construct when parents experience economic difficul-
in the model), such as low income, high debt ties and cannot provide the level of material
relative to assets, job disruptions, or income support desired by their children. We hy-
loss, affect individual distress and family re- pothesized that these circumstances would
lations through the daily strains or pressures be associated with greater risk for coercive
they create in family economic life (e.g.. exchanges involving disagreements in
Conger et al., 1992, 1993; Elder et al., 1992; which parents attempt to reduce demands
Flanagan, 1990; Kessler, Turner, & House, for money and adolescents use aversive
1988; Simons, Lorenz, Conger, & Wu, 1992; techniques in their attempt to meet their ma-
Voydanoff & Donnelly, 1988). Therefore, as terial needs. As one father in the study notes,
indicated by the economic pressure con- "You don't have it (money) and they argue
struct in Figure 1, we postulated that ad- with ya. They want it and you don't have
verse financial circumstances would affect it and you holler at them more." Findings
parents' emotional state and the quality of reported by McLoyd and Wilson (1990)
family interactions primarily through paren- showed that parent-adolescent discussions
tal behaviors, emotions, and cognitions that about financial problems may partially link
refiect parents' awareness of and responses economic stress to adverse developmental
to economic difficulties. outcomes, offering indirect evidence for the
process hypothesized here.
In the conceptual model (Fig. 1), a high
level of economic pressure indicates spousal Regarding the indirect path to adoles-
agreement that the family (a) cannot meet cent adjustment through parental emotions
its material needs, (b) often falls behind in and marital troubles, depressed mood repre-

^ Although economic pressure or strain is often thought to be a subjective measure of eco-


nomic circumstances, in our conceptualization it also has many objective dimensions. For exam-
ple, one indicator involves parents' reports that they cannot pay their bills and another that they
have had to give up what many would consider necessary expenditures (e.g., medical insurance).
These measures assess ohjective experiences that can he ohserved, not simply subjective evalua-
tions from good to bad of one's financial state. From our perspective, it is objective experiences
like having to cut back or heing unable to pay bills that are at the core of the economic pressure
construct. When material resources decline to the point that these experiences occur, then family
members are at psychological and hehavioral risk.
Conger et al. 545
sents the principal mechanism by which fi- (Fig. 1). Harsh, coercive behaviors by par-
nancial problems are hypothesized to pro- ents have repeatedly been shown to ad-
voke or intensify marital conflict. There is versely affect their children's development
considerable empirical evidence to support (Cecas & Seff, 1990; Maccoby & Martin,
the prediction that hardship-related depres- 1983; Patterson et al., 1992; Peterson & Rol-
sion will increase the risk of marital conflict. lins, 1987).
Both experimental and naturalistic studies of
depressed affect have shown that negative In the present report we focus on two
mood is positively related to irritable or hos- generally accepted categories of develop-
tile behavior between intimates (Berkowitz, mental problems during adolescence: inter-
1989; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Gotlib & nalizing and externalizing emotions and be-
McCabe, 1990). According to our conceptual haviors (Cicchetti & Toth, 1991). Theory and
model (Fig. 1), spousal conflict, in turn, was empirical findings suggest that repeatedly
expected to intensify financial conflicts be- hostile confrontations between parents and
tween parents and their children. children should diminish sense-of-self as in-
dicated by child or adolescent internalizing
We reason that parents who are already symptoms such as depression and anxiety
irritable and angry with one another are (Cecas & Seff, 1990). Parents' aversive be-
more likely to respond in a hostile fashion haviors toward their children are also likely
during confrontations with children or ado- to increase the risk of overt aggression or
lescents about troubled finances (Berkowitz, conduct problems, core symptoms of exter-
1989; Conger et al., 1991). This "spillover" nalization. Adolescents who learn from their
effect from marital conflict to aversive be- parents to use coercive techniques for con-
havior toward children was predicted to trolling relations with others are more likely
have both a specific influence on financial to become antisocial and troublesome to
disagreements as well as a more pervasive those around them (Patterson et al., 1992).
influence on parental hostility in general Inasmuch as externalizing and internalizing
(Fig. 1). Following Berkowitz (1989), we ex- symptoms are moderately to highly corre-
pected that the anger reflected by marital lated (Cicchetti & Toth, 1991), we expected
conflict would be more strongly related than that they may well be affected by similar pa-
depressed mood to hostile/aggressive be- rental behaviors.
haviors toward the adolescent at a later point
in time. That is, aggressive and angry acts in An alternative perspective to the chain
the marriage were predicted to directly re- of events described in Figure 1, of course,
late to later conflict and hostility and to serve is that depressed mood leads to perceptions
as a conduit through which parental depres- or behaviors associated with economic pres-
sion would affect interactions with children. sure. That is, the negative perceptual bias
Thus, the model includes direct paths from typical of depression may lead to a judgment
marital conflict at wave 1 (family interviews that one's financial circumstances are worse
in 1989) to financial conflicts and parent hos- than they are, or depressive symptoms may
tility at wave 2 (family interviews in 1990), disrupt work performance and limit financial
but only indirect paths for depressed mood success. Earlier community studies, how-
through conflicts in the marriage. ever, provide no support for the hypothesis
that depressive symptoms or psychological
As noted in the model, conflicts over fi- well-being significantly affect economic cir-
nances were also predicted to affect a par- cumstances (e.g.. Conger et al., 1993; Dun-
ent's overall level of hostility toward a child can & Liker, 1983; Iversen & Sabroe, 1988;
or adolescent. Here, again, we expected a Kessler et al., 1988; Liem & Liem, 1988;
spillover effect akin to Berkowitz's (1989) McLoyd & Wilson, 1991; Warr, Jackson, &
thesis that higher levels of anger and nega- Banks, 1988). Longitudinal studies consis-
tive mood increase the risk for more general- tently show that economic difficulties dis-
ized aggression and hostility. This hypothe- rupt psychological well-being and that emo-
sis is also consistent with a number of recent tional distress does not predict later
studies that have demonstrated increased economic status. Indeed, Kessler, Turner,
risk for parent hostility or parent-child con- and House (1989) found that relatively more
flict in response to economic stress (e.g., depressed unemployed workers were more
Flanagan, 1990; Lempers et al., 1989). Fi- motivated in their search for work and were
nally, we postulated that high levels of fi- reemployed more quickly. Thus, we con-
nancial conflict and parental hostility toward clude that the weight of the research evi-
children lead both to greater internalizing dence from community studies such as this
and externalizing symptoms for adolescents one supports our thesis that current eco-
546 Child Development
nomic pressure leads to emotional distress. 1989; Voydanoff, 1990). In addition, we em-
The following analyses address this ques- ploy a multimethod, multi-informant study
tion more directly. design which improves both the measure-
ment of theoretical constructs and the esti-
In this report, we test the empirical ade- mation of relations among them (Conger et
quacy of this postulated model of economic al., 1990). These procedures help to correct
stress, coercive family process, and adoles- for single respondent biases in the study of
cent developmental problems employing a family process (Bank, Dishion, Skinner, &
sample of two-parent rural families who Patterson, 1990; Lorenz, Conger, Simons,
were interviewed once each year in 1989, Whitbeck, & Elder, 1991).
1990, and 1991. By studying intact families,
we were able to examine the differential im- Method
pact of economic stre. on mothers and fa-
thers. This aspect of the investigation is im- Sample
portant because earlier research suggests When first interviewed in 1989, the sam-
that fathers may be more adversely affected ple for these analyses consisted of 451 fami-
than mothers by family financial difficulties lies. Only white families were studied be-
(McLoyd, 1989). Although more contempo- cause there are too few minority families in
rary studies demonstrate that modern rural Iowa to generate meaningful data for
women may be just as strongly affected by them. In these analyses, listwise deletion of
economic stress as men and that their roles missing data was used across three waves of
in family hardship may affect adolescents as data collection, leaving a total of 378 families
much as paternal response to financial prob- for whom complete data were available.
lems (Conger et al., 1992, 1993; Flanagan & Each family included two parents, a sev-
Eccles, 1993; Romero, Castro, & Cervantes, enth-grade adolescent (198 girls, 180 boys),
1988), we separate the analyses for mothers and a sibling within 4 years of age of the
and fathers to determine whether parental seventh grader. Families lived in one of
gender differences in the stress process will eight adjacent counties in a midwestem
be present for these contemporary, but rela- state in an area heavily dependent on agri-
tively traditional, rural husbands and wives. culture. For the final sample of 378 families,
32% lived on farms, 12% lived in rural areas
In addition, although some researchers but not on a farm, and 56% lived in towns
report that economic hardship differentially or small cities with a population under 6,500
influences boys and girls prior to adoles- (all but one of the towns was under 5,000).
cence and, perhaps, during early adoles- These families were selected for study be-
cence (Elder & Caspi, 1988; Flanagan, cause of the dramatic, adverse changes in
1990), other studies of both early and later the rural economy produced by the past de-
adolescence find no adolescent gender dif- cade's crisis in agriculture (Murdock & Leis-
ferences in response to economic stress tritz, 1988). For example, 51 of the 378 fami-
(Flanagan & Eccles, 1993; Lempers et al., lies had been displaced from farming.
1989). Moreover, there is evidence that sex Participants were interviewed during the
differences in stress vulnerability in general first 3 months of 1989, 1990, and 1991.
may decline by mid-adolescence (Forehand,
Neighbors, & Wierson, 1991). Thus, because Family median income for these 378
of the inconsistent evidence regarding ado- families from all sources for the past year
lescent gender differences in response to (1988) in 1989 was $33,800, about $5,000 per
economic stress and to stress processes more year less than married couples with children
generally by mid-adolescence, we did not in the United States as a whole. A total of
predict adolescent gender differences in the 11.0% of the families (N = 42) had incomes
following analyses. We did test for the possi- below the federal poverty line, about twice
bility of such differences, however. the proportion (5.6%) for married-couple
families nationally in 1988 (Bureau of the
This research adds to other contempo- Census, 1989). Because of the sampling cri-
rary studies of family economic hardship by teria, the average number of family members
(a) considering the behavior of both mothers was 4.95. Table 1 contains additional demo-
and fathers (see also Phares & Compas, graphic information for these families. Per
1992), (b) examining the influence of eco- capita income ranged from a net loss of over
nomic stress across time, and (c) carefully $10,000 (a troubled farm family) to $51,800
tracing specific interactional processes in the last year. The sample clearly con-
through which economic problems should tained sufficient variability to test the impact
affect the developing adolescent (McLoyd, of differing economic circumstances on fam-
Conger et al. 547
TABLE 1
DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SAMPLE {N = 378)

Demographic Mean Standard Minimum Maximum


Variable Score Deviation Value Value
1. Per capita family
income in dollars .. 8,163 5,829 -10,245 51,800
2. Father's occupational
prestige score (on a
scale from 1 to 99) .. 42.75 1L93 22.05 86.05
3. Mother's occupational
prestige score (on a
scale from 1 to 99) .. 33.66 15.67 22.05 66.37
4. Father's education
in years 13 57 2 14 8 00 20 00
5. Mother's education
in years .. 13.31 1.76 9.00 18 00
6. Father's age
in years .... 40 01 4 86 31 00 68 00
7. Mother's age
in years .. 38.01 4.10 29.00 53 00
8. Target child age
in years 12.6 .34 12.10 14.27

ily processes. Occupational prestige scores consonant with those for other longitudinal
(from 1 = least prestigious to 99 = most studies of families and adolescents which
prestigious) for fathers (M = 42.75) and also report greater attrition for less educated
mothers (M = 33.66) indicate that fathers parents (e.g., Flanagan & Eccles, 1993).
were generally skilled laborers or entrepre-
neurs and working mothers averaged less Procedures
skilled occupations typically in the service Families were recruited in 1989 through
sector of the economy. On average, these 34 public and private schools in the eight
families could best be described as middle counties. Names and addresses of seventh-
to lower middle class, ranging from house- grade students and their parents were ob-
hold heads who are unskilled laborers tained from all schools in communities of
(22.05) to those who are professionals (86.05) 6,500 or less. Families were sent a letter ex-
(Nakao & Treas, 1990). plaining the project and were subsequently
contacted by telephone and asked to partici-
The retention rate at each wave of data pate. In 1989, 78.8% of the families agreed
collection was about 95%. Overall retention to be interviewed. Each family member was
rate from 1989 (N = 451) to 1991 (N = 407) compensated at a rate of about $10 per hour
was 90%, although the final sample for these for his or her time in the study.
analyses was reduced to 378 (84% of the
original sample) by listwise deletion for Each year interviewers visited each
missing data. Families not included in the family at home for approximately 2 hours on
analyses did not have significantly lower in- each of two occasions. During the first visit,
comes than those who remained in the sam- each of the four family members completed
ple nor were they likely to demonstrate a set of questionnaires focusing upon indi-
higher levels of emotional distress. How- vidual family member characteristics, the
ever, parents not included in the final sam- quality of family relationships and interac-
ple of 378 families were slightly less well tions, and family economic circumstances.
educated. Fathers not in the analyses aver- During the second visit to the home, which
aged 12.74 years of education compared to occurred within 2 weeks of the first, the fam-
13.57 years for those who remained in the ily members were videotaped as they en-
study, a statistically significant difference gaged in several different structured interac-
(p < .05). For mothers, the mean years of tion tasks. A trained interviewer began the
education were 12.87 (missing subjects) and session by asking each individual to com-
13.31 (in the final sample), which was not plete independently a short questionnaire
a significant difference. These findings are designed to identify issues of concern that
548 Child Development
led to disagreements within the family (e.g., each task. For approximately 12% of all tasks
chores, recreation, money, etc.). The family in 1989, and for 20% of all tasks in subse-
members were then gathered around a table quent years, a second observer was ran-
and given a set of cards with questions for domly assigned to code independently
them to read and discuss. All four family family interactions so that interobserver reli-
members were involved in this first task, ability coefficients (an intraclass correlation)
which lasted 35 min. The cards for the first could be estimated. An observer rating man-
task asked general questions about family ual with complete definitions for all rating
life, such as approaches to parenting, perfor- scales and all task procedures is available
mance in school, household chores, and im- from the first author.
portant family events.
Measures
After explaining the procedures, com- The description of measures moves
pleting a practice card with the family, and across constructs from left to right following
checking the video-recording equipment for the model presented in Figure 1. We first
task 1 (and each subsequent task), the inter- describe measures of family economic status
viewer left the room for another part of the indicative of adverse economic conditions
house where she or he could not hear the (per capita income, debt-to-asset ratio, unsta-
discussion. The family members were asked ble work, income loss). These measures
to discuss among themselves each of the were predicted to affect the first endogenous
items listed on the cards and to continue construct, economic pressure. Initial eco-
talking until the interviewer returned. The nomic conditions and the first three endoge-
video camera recorded the family's interac- nous constructs (economic pressure, parent
tion around the issues raised by the task mood, marital conflict) were measured with
cards. After the first task was completed, the data collected during wave 1 (1989) of the
interviewer returned, stopped the discus- study. The second two constructs (financial
sion, and described the second task. The conflicts and parental hostility) were as-
next tasks proceeded in a similar fashion. sessed during wave 2 (1990), and adolescent
The second interaction task, 15 min in outcomes were measured at wave 3 (1991).
length, also involved all four family mem- To minimize the biases in estimates of
bers. For this task, the interviewer selected path coefficients produced by single sources
three topics based on the questionnaires of information (Bank et al., 1990; Lorenz et
completed at the beginning of the visit. The al., 1991), we follow the advice of Bank and
family members were asked to discuss and his colleagues (1990) and, to the extent pos-
try to resolve the issue that they had identi- sible, vary reporting agents across and
fied as leading to the greatest conflict in their within constructs. Our basic rule was that,
family. If they resolved this problem, they for adjacent constructs, the indicators for the
could go on to the second or even the third two constructs could not be based only on
issue during the task. data from a single reporter. That is, we at-
The third task involved only the siblings tempted to control for the unique disposi-
and was 15 min in length. We did not ana- tional biases of a single reporter that might
lyze data from this task in the present study. produce a spuriously high association be-
The fourth task involved just the married tween two adjacent constructs in the model.
couple and lasted for 30 min. We asked For example, the measures of adolescent
spouses to discuss the history and current externalizing and internalizing symptoms
status of their relationship, areas of agree- were based on adolescent self-report. There-
ment and disagreement (e.g., parenting, fi- fore, parent and observer as well as adoles-
nances), and their plans for the future. Dur- cent reports were used to measure financial
ing the marital task, siblings completed a conflicts and parental hostility. This proce-
questionnaire on important events in their dure assured that significant relations would
lives in a location where they could not hear not be obtained between the predictor and
their parents' discussion. outcome variables simply as a result of
shared method variance in the measures.
Trained observers rated several dimen-
sions of family interaction and family mem- Adverse economic conditions.—Four
ber characteristics from the videotapes. Ob- different constructs were used to describe
servers received 2 months of training and adverse economic conditions. Each con-
had to pass extensive written and viewing struct was based on a single indicator. We
tests before they could code videotapes. A asked the spouse who handles family fi-
separate, independent coder was used for nances to provide information about their
Conger et al. 549
economic situation. Thus, these data came was .66; therefore, the father and mother
from either the husband or wife or, in some measures were combined and averaged to
cases, from both of them if they were equally produce a single indicator for the construct.
involved in the family's economic activities.
Family per capita income includes total in- Each parent also responded to seven
come from all sources (e.g., wages, interest, items which asked if they agreed or dis-
business profits, etc.) divided by the number agreed on a five-point scale from strongly
of family members. This measure correlates agree to strongly disagree that their family
highly (r = .98) with family income divided had the money they needed for a home,
by family need (the poverty line for a family clothing, household items, a car, food, medi-
of a particular size) and, thvis, appears to ad- cal care, and recreational activities. The
just for economies of scale achieved with summed scales (alpha = .87 for mothers, .88
larger family sizes. Both parents responded for fathers) for each parent were highly cor-
to four items (1 = yes, 0 = no) that assessed related (r = .52) and were averaged to create
unstable work: (a) changing jobs for a worse the material needs indicator for economic
one, (b) trouble at work such as demotions, pressure. The final indicator for the con-
(c) being laid off or fired, and (d) other invol- struct, economic adjustments, consists of re-
untary losses of work. Mother and father re- ports from both parents concerning changes
sponses were summed to create a single in- they have made in response to financial dif-
dex for the family. ficulties during the past year. Each spouse
noted (1 = yes, 0 = no) whether their family
We computed a debt-to-asset ratio score had made any of 17 possible cutbacks in ex-
for each family. The estimated value of all penditures or assets during the past year, for
debts was divided by total family assets. Be- example, giving up medical insurance, re-
cause of skewness in the resulting distribu- ducing utility costs, etc. Parent responses
were combined into a single index. If either
tion of ratios, in the analyses we used a natu- or both parents responded yes to an item,
ral log transformation of the original the index increased by one (range = 0-17).
variable. Each parent also provided an inde- Separate parental reports of adjustments
pendent estimate of whether family income were significantly correlated (r = .64).
increased, decreased, or stayed about the
same during the past year. The nine possible Parents' depressed mood.—Three mea-
responses ranged from "increased more than sures were used as indicators of each par-
30%" to "decreased more than 30%." Scores ent's depressed mood. Both mothers and fa-
for the couple were summed and averaged thers completed the depression subscale
to provide an estimate oi income loss. from the SCL-90-R (Derogatis, 1983) which
Family economic pressure.—Our theo- has demonstrated reliability and validity.
retical model (Fig. 1) postulates that these The alpha was .87 for father's and .86 for
conditions of economic hardship influence mother's self-reported depression. Parents
parents' moods and behaviors through the also answered two questions that were
daily financial pressures they create. For ex- summed to create a spouse report of depres-
ample, a loss in income, according to the sion. They each indicated on a five-point
model, should have adverse consequences scale from strongly disagree to strongly
for family members to the extent that it cre- agree whether they agreed or disagreed that
ates difficulties in meeting material needs. their spouse is a happy person (item 1, re-
verse coded), or whether he or she is always
Three indicators were used to assess sad or depressed (item 2). The correlations
family economic pressure, each of which between these two items were .44 and .57
was based on reports from both parents. Two for mothers and fathers, respectively.
items assessed whether parents felt they
can't make ends meet. Each spouse reported The final measure of depressed mood,
whether they have difficulty paying bills observer-reported depression, came from a
each month (1 = no difficulty at all, 5 = a single rating during task 4 (marital interac-
great deal of difficulty) and whether they tion) by an observer who reported, on a
have money left over at the end of the month five-point scale from low to high, whether
(1 = more than enough money left over, 4 each parent was sad or unhappy during their
= not enough to make ends meet). The two videotaped interactions. Intraclass correla-
items were correlated .61 for mothers, .65 for tion (generalizability) coefficients were used
fathers. Each spouse's responses were stan- to estimate interobserver reliability (.60, fa-
thers; .67, mothers).
dardized and summed. The correlation be-
tween parent reports for the summed scales Marital conflict.—During the marital
550 Child Development
interaction task (task 4), each observer used ent's hostility to target (alpha = .86 for fa-
five-point scales to rate both parents, from thers and .88 for mothers). Interobserver re-
low to high, on their degree of hostility, anti- liabilities (.80 for fathers and .72 for mothers)
social behavior, angry coerciveness, and re- for the summed scale were acceptable.
ciprocal negativism toward one another. The
four rating scales (hostility, angry coercion, Adolescent internalizing symptoms.—
antisocial, transactional conflict) for each Adolescent self-reported depression and
spouse were summed as they were highly anxiety were assessed using these subscales
internally consistent (alpha = .78 for fathers from the SCL-90-R (Derogatis, 1983) and the
and .83 for mothers). The two summed NEO-personality inventory (Costa &
scales were combined to form an observer McCrae, 1985). Each of the four subscales
rating of marital conflict. The overall alpha was used as a separate indicator for the la-
coefficient for the combined scale is .86 (in- tent construct for internalizing symptoms.
terobserver reliability = .83). The internal consistency alphas were .89 for
depression and .87 for anxiety for the SCL-
Using a seven-point scale (1 = never 90-R. They were .85 for the depression and
and 7 = always), mothers and fathers each .71 for the anxiety subscales from the NEO.
independently rated one another on 12 We employed both state (SCL-90-R) and
items describing how often the partner en- trait (NEO) measures for the internalizing
gaged in hostile behaviors such as getting symptoms to determine whether these prob-
angry, shouting or yelling, threats, etc., to- lems would reflect ongoing personality char-
ward the respondent. The internal consis- acteristics, momentary moods, or both.
tency alpha is .93 for father and mother re- Adolescent externalizing symptoms.—
port. Each of these summed scales was used Hostile, aggressive, antisocial behaviors also
as a separate indicator for the marital conflictwere assessed using four different adoles-
construct. cent self-reports. The hostility subscales
Parent-adolescent financial conflict.— from the SCL-90-R (state hostility, alpha =
The latent construct for parent-adolescent .84) and the NEO (trait hostility, alpha =
financial conflict was based on independent .71) were used to measure both momentary
reports by mothers, fathers, and the target and more enduring dimensions of a hostile
adolescent. On a five-point scale (5 = all the personality. Six items were used to assess
time), mothers SiXid fathers were asked to in- the frequency of aggressive behaviors oc-
dicate how often they and their adolescent curring in the community during the past
child disagree or get upset with one another year (beat up someone; purposely damage
about money matters. Likewise, adolescents or destroy other's property; throw objects at
were asked to rate on a five-point scale (5 people; attack someone with a weapon; use
= always) how often they argued with their a weapon or force to get money or things
parents about not having enough money. from someone; set fire to a building; alpha
Each of these items was used as a separate = .67). Nine items selected from the Buss
indicator for the financial conflict construct. and Durkee (1957) hostility scale that most
reflect overt aggression were used to assess
Parent hostility.—There also were adolescent antisocial behaviors. High scores
three indicators for the parent hostility con- on a five-point scale indicate greater antiso-
struct based on separate reports by the par- cial behavior. Responses were internally
ents, adolescents, and observers. Using a consistent (alpha = .86) and were summed
seven-point scale (7 = always), each parent to create an indicator for antisocial behavior.
independently reported how often during These four scales were used as separate in-
the past month he or she got angry at, criti- dicators of externalizing symptoms.
cized, shouted or yelled at, or argued with
the target adolescent (alpha = .83 for moth-
ers and .82 for fathers). The target adolescent Results
answered the same items about his or her Correlational Analyses
parents using the same response categories Table 2 contains the correlations among
to rate hostile behaviors during the past all variables used in testing the theoretical
month (alpha = .84 for fathers and .83 for model for mothers; Table 3 provides the cor-
mothers). Using five-point scales, observers relations for fathers. Intercorrelations among
(task 1) rated each parent's hostility, angry indicators within constructs are underlined.
coerciveness, and antisocial behavior toward Cenerally speaking, correlations between
the target adolescent. The three items were measures within constructs are higher than
summed to create an observer report of par- those across constructs. For example, the in-
<J5^HCD-HIOIO—•a3CC'^H<£iO'^lOOOCJ3"^lCCNCOCOrNi
t-iocMco-H(£>r-cn—«cT><M05fMt^r^a50cococqco:oa5(
-—t-rJH-rJ*O5CSl—• t— C O ' *

I I C C<1 > * CO QO O CO •
I CO ^ o q —'

CMj (M CD l ^ O l

(MIOCOCCO^-^OO
^H^Hr-HC^^^CD^HO V

cooo(Ma5mcDQO
— ' O O O O O O

I —I —• C<1 - H —• O O •

w
O
PQ ) O O]
o < o o
muo ' O CO
o
C^I(N|CO(MO.-H(M-HC-1(^O
W 3C'—^a>c^cM^co*^rt<icypco
s; o o o
B:
o - H Cvl
o o
u
o I T ^ QC O J ^ ^ t^*- C*^ OO '

I
'•—<-—« C£) O cq - ^ O '
o ) C ^ C 5 ^T^ t * ^ ^H f j c ^ < 1 0 0 38
I I I I
iOTO O O3 CO IO ; D 1
H

W I c q 00 ^H oq ^H • ^ O3 t~- O IO !>• CO ^H
I O O ' - H O O — ^ O O O ooO
o0 '' 3
So
Q
a
EE
Q I I I I I
g ^j
I
coggmioeog305(NO(N'-H
<^P ^ J ^'^^ £^i t^^ T-H t^^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^H
I I I I I I I I I I
i n —I
o o
I I
s;
Z

II
O T)

III
sJ
QE

I i3 4' S e "I
o "
a- D Q ,
O5 I—^ CO ^ ^ 1

cql M^i t ic -H
• ^ 1 CSfl C i O CO "
O5 CO
CO t o

iOosocoQOo
V
a.
o'
t CO o i n

u
O
U
O l|-T-tM«—c—.—.—. — — l O O '
I I

2 =: S 2 S
^ Q3 t^^ oo ^ CO ^ " ^ CO o ^ ."^

5 lCOC<]'—(O
OOOO

tMl col C O O I l ^ c o ^ T O O C ^ O l ^ O ^ ^ l ^ ^ ' I C<I .-H i


> oo <
I I
C O l O C O l ~ ^ C O C O " ^ i — I ' — ^ C O f O f ^ O ^ H ' — ^ 1 — \ <Ji I
. . ^ _ _ O - H O O O O O O — " O O O O O i 00
I I II I I

I I

!o o

II I I I

Q. V

H o
u ^
Q E
^ flj c 2^ ^ ^
•^ " , ^ D. Q4 >< >< I =>
-" - - aj aj c c I '•''

ui 0)

;§?3SSS!3SE5
Conger et al. 553
tercorrelations among the indicators for eco- ness), we first conducted multigroup com-
nomic pressure range from .63 (economic ad- parisons for the structural equations. A
justments correlated with material needs for multigroup comparison of the full model
both mothers and fathers) to .79 (can't make showed no statistically significant adoles-
ends meet by material needs for both cent gender differences in the relations
mothers and fathers). Similarly, father's self- among concepts. Because the multigroup
report of his own hostility (Table 3) corre- test for the full model included too few cases
lates .35 with adolescent report of his hostil- (fewer than five) for the number of parame-
ity; for mothers and adolescents (Table 2) ters estimated, we repeated the analyses us-
the same correlation is .42. ing just financial conflicts and parent hostil-
ity to predict adolescent internalizing and
In addition, the correlations among indi- externalizing symptoms. Again, no signifi-
cators across constructs provide preliminary cant gender difference was found; therefore,
support for the hypothesized family coercion we examined the correspondence between
model. For instance, the indicators of par- the model and the data using the complete
ent-child financial conflict correlate with pa- sample of boys and girls. By estimating the
rental hostility from .10 (father report of fi- combined measurement and causal model
nancial conflicts with adolescent report of using the full sample, we were able to main-
mother's hostility) to .35 (father's report of tain an adequate number of cases (10 or
financial conflicts with self-report of his own more) per parameter estimate as recom-
hostility toward the adolescent). Taken to- mended by Bentler and Chou (1987) and
gether, the correlations suggest some prom- Bollen (1989).
ise for a more formal test of the theoretical
model. To test the full model shown in Figure
1 would have reduced well below 10 the
Structural Equation Models for the number of cases per parameter estimate. For
Complete Sample that reason, we first tested the direct relation
Latent-variable structural equation mod- between the measures of adverse economic
els were used to examine the empirical conditions and economic pressure, as shown
credibility of the proposed theoretical pro- in Figure 2. All of these measures of adverse
cesses (Fig. 1). Maximum likelihood esti- economic conditions were significantly re-
mates of the model coefficients were ob- lated to economic pressure ranging from a
tained using LISREL VII (Joreskog & standardized regression coefficient of .11
Sorbom, 1989). Because analyses of mean (imstable work) to — .39 (per capita income).
differences (see Table 4) between boys and The set of economic conditions accounted
girls showed that girls were significantly for 39% of the variance in economic pres-
higher on all measures of internalization and sure. The modest correlations among the
were significantly lower on two measures of measures of economic conditions support
externalization (antisocial and aggressive- their use as separate constructs. The model

TABLE 4
MEAN DIFFERENCES BETWEEN NINTH-GKADE BOYS AND GIRLS ON
MEASURES OF INTEHNALIZATION AND EXTERNALIZATION

BOYS GIRLS
{N = 180) (N = 198)

MEASURES M SD M SD t TEST

Internalization:
State depression 16.34 4.50 19.46 7.29 -4.99*
Trait depression 19.45 4.48 22.01 6.11 -4.47*
State anxiety 13.27 3.81 14.29 4.76 -2.25*
Trait anxiety 21.07 3.77 23.91 4.70 -6.39*
Externalization;
State hostility 8.94 3.26 8.68 3.20 .49
Trait hostility 22.16 3.81 22.15 4.75 .11
Antisocial 24.65 6.29 20.36 6.27 6.59*
Aggressiveness 6.72 1.09 6.21 .66 5.02*
* p < .05.
** p < .001.
554 Child Development

Adjustments Ends
Meet Needs

X",,, - 14.89
p - .061
GFI - .989
Critical N - 380.75
FIG. 2.—Maximum likelihood estimation of the model predicting economic pressure from eco-
nomic conditions (*p < .05).

fit the data fairly well as indicated by a non- supports the model in Figure 1 showing only
significant chi-square, a goodness-of-Rt in- an indirect association between economic
dex of almost 1.0, and a critical N greater conditions and later constructs in the model.
than 200 (Hoelter, 1983; Joreskog & Sorbom, These findings allowed us to test a less com-
1989). plex model, using economic pressure as
the exogenous construct, in the following
To assure that the exogenous measures analyses.
of adverse economic conditions had only an
indirect association through hypothesized Figure 3 presents the results for a
linking variables with other constructs in the mother and father version ofthe model (stan-
model, we estimated the paths from per cap- dardized coefficients for the mother model
ita income, unstable work, debt-asset ratio, are in parentheses) predicting internalizing
and income loss to each endogenous con- symptoms of the adolescents at wave 3
struct controlling for postulated intervening (1991) when they were in the ninth grade."^
variables (see Fig. 1). For example, each For the mother model, the depression and
economic measure was used to predict mari- parent hostility constructs pertain only to
tal confiict controlling for economic pressure her depressed mood and hostile behavior.
and parent depression. A total of 32 separate The same procedure was followed in the fa-
paths were estimated, only one of which was ther model, estimated separately from the
statistically significant. Because this out- mother model, in which the depression and
come would be expected by chance at the hostility constructs refer only to his moods
.05 level, we conclude that the evidence and behavior. Because unique dispositions

^ Because we assume that the acute economic decline of the middle 1980s had led to rela-
tively chronic differences in economic well-being for these families by the time they were
interviewed in 1989, the proposed model describes processes hypothesized to be in a state of
equilibrium rather than continuing change. In this situation, including controls at time 1 for
endogenous constructs assessed at time 2 and time 3 would misspecify the model (see Dwyer,
1983; Kessler, 1987; Mulaik, 1987). For that reason, lagged variables were not used in the
analyses.
Conger et al. 555
Wave 1 (1989) W.VC 2 (1990) Wave 3 (1991)

Adjustments Ends Meet Material Needs Father Mother Adolescent

Spouse

State State Trait Trait


Depression Anidety Depression Aniiety

Parent Obsen

FIG. 3.—Maximum likelihood estimation ofthe model for internalizing symptoms (*p < .05, *p <
.10). Residuals for indicators involving the same reporters were allowed to correlate across constructs
(not shown). For the mothers' model (numbers in parentheses), x"(13''') = 268.2, GFI = .931, and
critical N = 230.2. For the fathers' model, x^(137) = 221.5, GFI = .943, and critical N = 277.2. (R^'s
are reported in the circles.)

of individual reporters may systematically pressure which, in turn, would increase the
influence their measures for different con- risk of marital conflict. This alternative or-
structs (e.g., father's report of own depres- dering of the variables significantly reduced
sion and his report of confiict in the mar- the fit ofthe data with the model, providing
riage), error terms for indicators based on the further support for the originally hypothe-
same reporter for different constructs were sized theoretical processes.
allowed to covary in these analyses. This
procedure reduces the impact of method As expected, economic pressure at wave
variance error on the findings (Bank et al., 1 is significantly related to parent/adoles-
1990). To simplify the presentation, these cent financial conflict at wave 2 (beta = .28,
correlations are not shown in the path mod- mothers; beta = .29, fathers). Marital con-
els (Figs. 3, 4). Path coefficients predicted to flict is even more strongly related to finan-
be nonsignificant (e.g., economic pressure to cial conflicts (beta = .48, fathers; beta = .50,
parent hostility) were fixed to zero, and other mothers). Consistent with the postulated
parameters were allowed to be estimated. processes, allowing a direct path from de-
pressed mood to any of the wave 2 or wave
With the exception ofthe nonsignificant 3 constructs did not significantly improve
direct path from financial conflicts to prob- the fit of the data with the model, that is,
lems of internalization, the results were con- none of these path coefficients was statisti-
sistent with the proposed model (Fig. 3). At cally significant. The numbers within the
wave 1 the standardized path coefficients circles representing endogenous constructs
from economic pressure to depressed mood are squared multiple correlation coefficients
are statistically significant for both mothers (R^). They show that the wave 1 variables
(beta = .63, t = 6.51) and fathers (beta = account for from 40% (fathers) to 44% (moth-
.58, t = 5.37). For both parents, depressed ers) of the variance in wave 2 financial con-
mood is significantly related to marital con- flicts between parents and adolescents. Mar-
flict. Because this portion of the model is ital confiict is also predictive of the global
estimated at a single point in time, we exam- measure of parental hostility toward adoles-
ined the earlier discussed possibility that cents, although for mothers the path coeffi-
depressed mood would lead to economic cient of .18 is only marginally significant (t
556 Child Development
Wave 1 (1989) W«ve 2 (1990) Wive 3 (1991)

SpOU!

Observer

Father Obsei

FIG. 4.—Maximum likelihood estimation ofthe model for externalizing symptoms {'p < .05, ^p <
.10). Residuals for indicators involving the same reporters were allowed to correlate across constructs
(not shown). For the mothers' model (numbers in parentheses), x^(138) = 261.5, GFI = .932, and
critical N = 238.4. For the fathers' model, x^(138) = 227.6, GFI = .941, and critical N = 273.4. (R^'s
are reported in the circles.)

= L59). The path coefficients from financial critical N greater than 200 (Hoelter, 1983)
conflicts to hostility are both statistically sig- and a goodness-of-fit index greater than .90
nificant. Interestingly, while economic pres- (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989). Releasing the
sure explains a greater portion of the vari- paths from economic pressure, parental
ance in mother's (40%) than father's (33%) mood, and marital conflict to additional en-
depression, and mother's mood is more pre- dogenous variables (i.e., testing the fully re-
dictive of marital conflicts than father's de- cursive model) did not significantly improve
pression, the marital and financial conflict the fit of either the father or mother model;
variables explained a greater portion of the thus, the theoretical model was accepted as
variance in father's (45%) than mother's the more parsimonious representation of re-
(31%) hostility. These differences between lations among the constructs (see Bollen,
spouses must be cautiously interpreted, 1989). In general, the coefficients in Figure
however, because none of them attains sta- 3 also demonstrate a reasonable degree of
tistical significance. association between indicators (all were sta-
tistically significant) and constructs. Only in
Only one of the final two paths in the the case of adolescent reports of financial
model is supportive of the predicted set of conflicts did a factor weight drop below .40.
relations. Financial conflict is not signifi-
cantly related to internalizing symptoms Figure 4 provides the results ofthe anal-
{t = -1.35, mothers; t = -.68, fathers), but yses predicting externalizing problems. As
it does make a difference indirectly through would be expected, the findings up to paren-
the more general measure of parental hostil- tal hostility are quite similar to those in Fig-
ity. Mother's hostile behavior accounts for ure 3. There is even less evidence, however,
over twice the variance (22%) in internaliz- of a direct connection between marital
ing symptoms as father's (10%) hostility. conflict and mother's hostility toward the
Again, this difference is not statistically sig- adolescent. For mothers, marital conflict
nificant. appears to affect generalized hostility
primarily indirectly through financial con-
Both the mother and father models fit flicts. Rather unexpectedly, the associations
the data reasonably well as indicated by a between externalizing symptoms, financial
Conger et al. 557
conflicts, and parent hostility are of about both marital conflict and financial conflict
the same magnitude as those for internaliza- were significantly related to parent hostility
tion. Although not actually predicted, we as- toward the child, although the effect of mari-
sumed that aversive family interactions tal conflict was primarily indirect for moth-
would be more likely to generate similar be- ers through parent-adolescent financial con-
haviors in adolescents. These findings sug- flict. Moreover, financial conflict affected
gest that coercive or hostile family interac- problems in adolescent development only
tions associated with economic stress indirectly through parent hostility which, in
increase in a similar fashion the risk for both turn, directly increased the risk of adoles-
internalization and externalization problems. cent symptoms of internalization and exter-
nalization.
As with internalizing symptoms, finan-
cial conflicts are only indirectly related to Although several path coefficients were
externalizing problems through their associ- suggestive of differences in the model for
ation with overall parental hostility. More- mothers and fathers, none ofthe differences
over, mother's hostility explains twice the in these parameter estimates was statisti-
variance in externalizing symptoms as fa- cally significant. Consistent with other re-
ther's hostility. This difference, however, cent studies (e.g.. Conger et al., 1991), there
was not statistically significant. The model was little support in these findings for the
presented in Figure 4 fits the data reason- notion that fathers will be more adversely
ably well using the criteria reviewed earlier. affected by family financial problems than
A fully recursive estimate ofthe model did mothers. It may be that men and women in
not significantly improve the fit. Moreover, contemporary families experience economic
the indicators for constructs are all statisti- stress in a more similar fashion than those
cally significant and load on the latent vari- in earlier studies because of the changing
ables as expected. role of women in the workforce. Moreover,
there were no gender differences in the sep-
arate estimates of path coefficients for boys
Discussion and girls even though predictable differ-
Farlier research suggests that economic ences in symptomatology were observed
stress may exacerbate conflict and irritability (i.e., greater internalization for girls and ex-
in family interactions (McLoyd, 1989; Voy- ternalization for boys). This finding is also
danoff, 1990). An important aspect of this consistent with other recent reports (e.g.,
process involves arguments and disagree- Flanagan & Eccles, 1993; Lempers et al.,
ments about Tnoney, although only one pre- 1989). This difference from Depression era
vious study has directly addressed this pos- findings also may reflect a secular trend to-
sibility, and the findings pertained only to ward greater similarity in expectations re-
interactions between husbands and wives garding males and females.
(Liker & Elder, 1983). In this report we ex-
tended this earlier work by investigating the The results presented here suggest
role of financial conflicts between parents other promising extensions ofthe family co-
and adolescents in the economic stress pro- ercion model. Patterson et al. (1992) frame
cess. Such financial conflicts are likely to in- the coercion process within interaction se-
volve coercive interactions of the type de- quences composed of aversive attacks and
scribed by Patterson (1982) in which family counterattacks by family members. These
members rely on aversive behaviors as a tac- conflicts may be very general or they may
tic in attempts to control one another's ac- be quite specific involving, for example, the
tions. We also proposed that this coercion financial disagreements between parents
process would be associated with more gen- and adolescents reported in the present
eralized hostility toward children and ado- study. Although Patterson's work has been
lescents by economically stressed parents. based on analyses of microsocial process in-
volving thousands of family interactive
The findings were generally supportive events, the present findings suggest that
of the theoretical model (Fig. 1). Adverse questionnaire responses, as well as sequen-
economic conditions were indirectly related tial analysis of moment-by-moment ex-
through economic pressure to other con- changes, can shed understanding on coer-
structs in the model. Economic pressure was cive family behaviors. Indeed, these data
associated directly with parent-adolescent indicate that coercive family processes can
financial conflicts as well as indirectly profitably be studied around specific dis-
through parent depressed mood and hostile putes involving such areas of disagreement
interactions in the marriage. As expected. as money, chores, schoolwork, etc. Future
558 Child Development
analyses should examine how specific family greater economic pressure than those with a
stressors (e.g., time pressures in dual-earner higher income, as suggested by the present
households) lead to arguments and disagree- findings, the psychological impact of such
ments of particular types (e.g., concerning objective markers of economic conditions
child and adolescent household responsibil- will vary depending on other constraints or
ities) and how these coercive family ex- demands in family life. The economic pres-
changes affect child and adolescent devel- sure construct indirectly takes into account
opment. these other contextual influences.
Equally important will be research on Finally, we question exactly how objec-
family dynamics that decrease the risk for tive many of the "hard" measures of eco-
these destructive processes under condi- nomic circumstances really are. Given the
tions of family stress. For example, returning limited time available for collecting eco-
to Figure 1, one might ask what emotional nomic data in most behavioral studies, the
or instrumental competencies in families re- information on economic conditions is typi-
duce the impact of economic pressure on cally incomplete. For example, we already
parent-adolescent financial conflict? Be- noted that ongoing and necessary expendi-
cause economic problems cause distress for tures (e.g., for chronic medical problems)
a significant proportion of American families may vary dramatically across families and
(Voydanoff, 1990), an especially important significantly affect the demands created by
product of research on families and eco- a particular income level. Yet, this informa-
nomic hardship will be the identification of tion on family expenditures is rarely consid-
processes through which families and indi- ered in behavioral research, including our
viduals reduce the negative impact of such own. Moreover, we find only moderate
experiences (Rutter, 1990). agreement between spouse reports on fam-
ily income, income loss, etc. We suspect that
Another issue raised by the present parents often do not have exact information
findings concerns subjective versus objec- on their economic circumstances or, if they
tive experience of economic difficulties. do, may be reticent to report it. For example,
One might assert, for example, that the con- many of the parents in our study appear to
struct "economic pressure" represents a be involved in barter economies or sideline
subjective reality only partially connected to "cash only" businesses that are not reflected
more objective indicators of economic hard- in the income figures they report. Thus, we
ship such as income loss or high debt. Al- expect that there are significant biases in re-
though this is a plausible perspective, for ports of economic conditions, biases that
several reasons we take an opposite view of may be systematic and subjective in nature.
the findings.
First, we believe that economic pres- To summarize, although our primary ex-
sures such as cutting back on expenditures ogenous construct (economic pressure) has
and having difficulty paying bills are real, a subjective dimension, we propose that it
everyday experiences, not simply subjective also reflects objective features of a family's
impressions of reality. Second, from our per- daily economic reality. We suspect that more
spective, the financial realities and adjust- comprehensive and accurate measurement
ments represented by the economic pres- of family economic conditions would in-
sure construct provide the means by which crease the association between putatively
economic conditions are translated into psy- objective economic measures and the pres-
chologically meaningful life events for fam- sure construct. From a policy perspective,
ily members. That is, a seemingly objective economic pressure represents a key psycho-
measure of hardship, such as low income, logical link between economic hard times
should affect the individual only to the ex- and daily family experience. The present
tent that it creates demands for oftentimes findings suggest that the most fundamental
painful behavioral adjustments, like cutting means for reducing economic pressure and
back on expenditures. its adverse influences on adolescents and
parents is to increase family economic well-
Especially important, when income is being.
low, parents with high resource demands
such as ongoing medical costs for chronic Although the findings presented here
illnesses will need to make more difficult are consonant with the hypothesized theo-
economic adjustments than those without retical model, several limitations in the re-
such demands. Thus, although people with search should be noted. First, despite our
a lower income will, in general, experience consideration of family coercive processes
Conger et al. 559
across time, a passive longitudinal study of (1993). Family economic stress and adjust-
this type does not rival an experiment in ment of early adolescent girls. Developmental
terms of causal inference (Dwyer, 1983). Psychology, 29, 206-219.
Nevertheless, a longitudinal design pro- Gonger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., Lorenz, F. O.,
vides greater confidence in the temporal or- Gonger, K. J., Simons, R. L., Whitbeck, L. B.,
dering of constructs than does a cross- Huck, S., & Melby, J. N. (1990). Linking eco-
sectional research strategy. Second, the nomic hardship to marital quality and insta-
generalizability of the findings should be bility. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
tested in urban as well as rural settings, with 52, 643-656.
different family structures (e.g., step- and Gonger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O., Elder, G. H., Jr.,
single-parent), with children of different Melby, J. N., Simons, R. L., & Gonger, K. J.
ages, and with other than white families. If (1991). A process model of family economic
the present findings prove robust upon fur- pressure and early adolescent alcohol use.
ther examination, they provide a basis for Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, 430-449.
understanding and minimizing the coercive Gonger, R. D., McGarty, J. A., Yang, R. K., Lahey,
processes through which economic stress B. B., & Kropp, J. P. (1984). Perception of
has adverse developmental consequences. child, childrearing values, and emotional dis-
The prevention of such stress-related emo- tress as mediating links between environ-
tional and relationship problems should re- mental stressors and observed maternal be-
duce long-term difficulties along these di- havior. Child Development, 55, 2234-2247.
mensions and also assist families in focusing Gosta, P. T., & McGrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO
their energies more effectively on the man- personality inventory manual. Orlando, FL:
agement of hard times. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Goyne, J. G., & Downey, G. (1991). Social factors
and psychopathology: Stress, social support,
References and coping processes. In M. R. Rosenzweig
Bank, L., Dishion, T., Skinner, M., & Patterson, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psy-
G. R. (1990). Method variance in structural chology (Vol. 42, pp. 401-425). Palo Alto, GA:
equation modeling; Living with "glop." In Annual Reviews.
G. R. Patterson (Ed.), Ag^re.ssion and depres- Davidson, O. G. (1990). Broken heartland: The
sion in family interactions (pp. 247-279). rise of America's rural ghetto. New York:
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Free Press.
Bentler, P. M., & Ghou, G. P. (1987). Practical is- Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R administration,
sues in structural modeling. Sociological scoring, and procedures manual-ll. Towson,
Methods and Research, 16, 78-117. MD.: Glinical Psychometric Research.
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hy- Downey, G., & Goyne, J. G. (1990). Ghildren of
pothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psy- depressed parents: An integrative review.
chological Bulletin, 106, 59-73. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 50-76.
Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with la- Duncan, G. J., & Liker, J. (1983). Disentangling
tent variables. New York: Wiley. the efficacy-earnings relationship among
Bureau ofthe Gensus. (1989). Money income and white men. In G. J. Duncan & J. N. Morgan
poverty .status in the United States: 1988 (Se- (Eds.), Five thousand American families: Pat-
ries P-60, No. 166, October). Washington, terns of economic progress (Vol. 10, pp. 218-
DG: U.S. Department of Gommerce. 248). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Re-
Buss, A., & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for search.
assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal Dwyer, J. H. (1983). Statistical models for the so-
of Consulting Psychology, 21, 343-349. cial and behavioral sciences. New York: Ox-
Gicchetti, D., 6f Toth, S. L. (1991). A develop- ford University Press.
mental perspective on internalizing and ex- Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children ofthe Great De-
ternalizing disorders. In D. Gicchetti & S. L. pression: Social change in life experience.
Toth (Eds.), Internalizing and externalizing Ghicago: University of Ghicago Press.
expressions of dysfunction (pp. 1-19). New Elder, G. H., Jr., & Gaspi, A. (1988). Economic
York: Erlbaum. stress in lives: Developmental perspectives.
Gonger, R. D., Gonger, K. J., Elder, G. H., Jr., Journal of Social Issues, 44, 25—45.
Lorenz, F. O., Simons, R. L., & Whitbeck, L. B. Elder, G. H., Jr., Gonger, R. D., Foster, E. M., &
(1992). A family process model of economic Ardelt, M. (1992). Families under economic
hardship and adjustment of early adolescent pressure. Journa/ of Family Issues, 13, 5-37.
boys. Child Development, 63, 526-541. Flanagan, G. A. (1990). Ghange in family work sta-
Gonger, R. D., Gonger, K. J., Elder, G. H., Jr., tus: Effects on parent-adolescent decision
Lorenz, F. O., Simons, R. L., & Whitbeck, L. B. making. Child Development, 61, 163—177.
560 Child Development
Flanagan, G. A., & Eccles, J. S. (1993). Ghanges Lorenz, F. O., Gonger, R. D., Simons, R. L., Whit-
in parents' work status and adolescents' ad- beck, L. B., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1991). Eco-
justment at school. Child Development, 64, nomic pressure and marital quality: An illus-
246-257. tration ofthe method variance problem in the
Forehand, R., Neighbors, B., & Wierson, M. causal modeling of family processes. Journal
(1991). Tbe transition to adolescence: The of Marriage and the Family, 53, 375-388.
role of gender and stress in problem behavior Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socializa-
and competence. JoMrnaZ of Child Psychology tion in the context of the family: Parent-child
and Psychiatry, 32, 929-937. interaction. In E. M. Hetberington (Ed.),
Friedberger, M. (1989). Shake-out: Iowa farm P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child
families in the 1980s. Lexington: University psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personal-
Press of Kentucky. ity, and social development (pp. 1-101). New
Gecas, V., & SefF, M. A. (1990). Families and ado- York: Wiley.
lescents: A review of the 1980s. Journal of McLoyd, V. G. (1989). Socialization and develop-
Marriage and the Family, 52, 941-958. ment in a changing economy. American Psy-
Gotlib, I. H., & McGabe, S. B. (1990). Marriage chologist, 44, 293-302.
and psychopathology. In F. D. Fincham & McLoyd, V. G., & Wilson, L. (1990). Maternal be-
T. N. Bradbury (Eds.), The psychology of havior, social support, and economic condi-
marriage: Basic issues and applications (pp. tions as predictors of distress in children. In
226-257). New York: Guilford. V. G. McLoyd & G. A. Flanagan (Eds.), Eco-
Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance nomic stress: Effects on family life and child
structures: Goodness-of-fit indices. Sociologi- development (pp. 49-69) (New Directions for
cal Methods and Research, 11, 325-344. Ghild Development, 46). San Francisco:
Iversen, L., & Sabroe, S. (1988). Psychological Jossey-Bass.
well-being among unemployed and em- McLoyd, V. G., & Wilson, L. (1991). The strain
ployed people after a company closedown: A of living poor: Parenting, social support, and
longitudinal study. Journal of Social Issues, child mental health. In A. G. Huston (Ed.),
44, 141-152. Children in poverty: Child development and
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1989). LISREL public policy (pp. 105-135). New York: Gam-
VII: User's reference guide (1st ed.). Moores- bridge University Press.
ville, IN: Scientific Software. Mulaik, S. A. (1987). Toward a conception of cau-
Kessler, R. G. (1987). The interplay of research sality applicable to experimentation and cau-
design strategies and data analysis proce- sal modeling. Child Development, 58, 18—32.
dures in evaluating the effects of stress on Murdock, S. H., & Leistritz, F. L. (Eds.). (1988).
health. In S. V. Kasl & G. L. Gooper (Eds.), The farm financial crisis: Socioeconomic di-
Stress and health: Issues in research method- mensions and implications for producers and
ology (pp. 113-137). New York: Wiley. rural areas. Boulder, GO: Westvicw.
Kessler, R. G., Turner, J. B., & House, J. S. (1988). Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1990). Computing 1989 oc-
Effects of unemployment on health in a com- cupational prestige scores (General Social
munity survey: Main, modifying, and mediat- Survey Methodological Report No. 70). Ghi-
ing effects. Journal of Social Issues, 44, 69- cago: National Opinion Research Genter.
85. Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process.
Kessler, R. G., Turner, J. B., & House, J. S. (1989). Eugene, OR: Gastalia.
Unemployment, reemployment and emo- Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. G., & Dishion, T. J.
tional functioning in a community sample. (1992). Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Gas-
American Sociological Review, 54, 648-657. talia.
Knutson, J. F. (1982). Perspective on chapter 7. In Peterson, G. W., & Rollins, B. G. (1987). Parent-
G. R. Patterson (Ed.), A social learning ap- child socialization: A review of research and
proach: Vol. 3. Coercive family process. Eu- applications of symbolic interaction concepts.
gene, OR: Gastalia. In M. B. Sussman & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.),
Lempers, J. D., Glark-Lempers, D., & Simons, Handbook of marriage and the family (pp.
R. L. (1989). Economic hardship, parenting, 471-507). New York: Plenum.
and distress in adolescence. Child Develop- Phares, V., & Gompas, B. (1992). The role of fa-
ment, 60, 25-39. thers in child and adolescent psychopatbol-
Liem, R., & Liem, J. H. (1988). Psychological ef- ogy: Make room for daddy. Psychological Bul-
fects of unemployment on workers and their letin, 111, 387-412.
families. Journa/ of Social Issues, 44, 87—105. Robinson, E. A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1987). Social
Liker, J. K., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1983). Economic learning theory and family psychopathology:
hardship and marital relations in the 1930s. A Kantian model in behaviorism. In T. Jacob
American Sociological Review, 48, 343—359. (Ed.), Family interaction and psychopathol-
Conger et al. 561
ogy: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. Voydanoff, P. (1990). Economic distress and fam-
117-162). New York: Plenum. ily relations: A review ofthe eighties. Joitrna/
Romero, G. J., Gastro, F. G., & Gervantes, R. G. of Marriage and the Family, 52, 1099-1115.
(1988). Latinas without work: Family, occupa- Voydanoff, P., & Donnelly, B. W. (1988). Eco-
tional, and economic stress following unem- nomic distress, family coping, and quality of
ployment. Psychology of Women Quarterly, family life. In P. Voydanoff & L. G. Majka
12, 281-297. (Eds.), Families and economic distress: Cop-
Rutter, M. (1990). Psycbosocial resilience and pro- ing strategies and social policy (pp. 97-116).
tective mechanisms. In J. Rolf, A. S. Masten, Beverly Hills, GA: Sage.
D. Gicchetti, K. H. Nuecbterlein, & S. Wein- Warr, P., Jackson, P., & Banks, M. (1988). Unem-
traub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in ployment and mental health: Some British
the development of psychopathology (pp. studies. Journal of Social Issues, 44, 47-68.
181—214). New York: Gambridge University Whitbeck, L. B., Simons, R. L., Gonger, R. D., Lo-
Press. renz, F. O., Huck, S., & Elder, G. H., Jr.
Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Gonger, R. D., & (1991). Family economic hardship, parental
Wu, G. (1992). Support from spouse as media- support, and adolescent self-esteem. Social
tor and moderator of the disruptive influence Psychology Quarterly, 54, 353-363.
of economic strain on parenting. Child Devel- Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged.
opment, 63, 1282-1301. Ghicago: University of Ghicago Press.

Вам также может понравиться