Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258622676

Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard-of-hearing


children

Article  in  Educational and Child Psychology · January 2010

CITATIONS READS

8 2,791

2 authors:

Nicholas Wood Julie Dockrell


University of East London University College London
8 PUBLICATIONS   28 CITATIONS    207 PUBLICATIONS   3,851 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Social emotional development of youngsters with specific language impairment View project

Whiteness; Racism View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nicholas Wood on 31 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological assessment procedures for
assessing deaf or hard-of-hearing children
Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

This paper provides an overview of deaf or hard-of-hearing development, with particular relevance to
emerging cognitive skills. A contextual approach is adopted, emphasising linguistic, cultural and ‘non-
verbal’ factors. Some generic assessment considerations are provided for the optimal, evidence-based
assessment of deaf or hard-of-hearing children. Thereafter, a number of potentially useful psychometric tools
are described. The limitation of adopting a purely standardised approach to assessment with this population
is discussed. Finally, a coherent model of assessment pertinent to this population is depicted, that is,
a ‘situated cognition’ model emphasising fluidity and the importance of the wider context when evaluating
performances with this group of children, but suitably adapted to take account of within-child factors.

On the Search for ‘Deaf’ cognition;

T
HE AIM OF THIS PAPER is to optimise
access to performances that are closest The implications of potential cognitive
to the underlying abilities of deaf or differences between D/HOH and typically
hard-of-hearing children (D/HOH) across a developing children hold important ramifi-
range of hearing difficulties and with partic- cations not just for assessment but for the
ular reference to cognitive skills. This development of subsequent remedial or
overview will initially focus on a discussion of intervention strategies as well. Whilst it is
similarities and differences between typically acknowledged that the research history of
developing and deaf and hard-of-hearing deaf children’s abilities has been tainted by
children’s abilities, particularly with regard poorly designed studies suggesting global
to their cognitive and linguistic functioning. ‘inferiorities’, the notion of cognitive differ-
Acknowledging the huge diversity in deaf ences does not necessarily imply deficits or
development, we proceed to look at several deficiencies (Marschark, 2003, 2007;
salient factors requiring consideration Vernon, 2005).
during assessment. We hope this outline of Marschark (2007) argues that it may be
the developing evidence will help inform an more detrimental to the child if such differ-
evidence-based approach to the issues of ences are denied without good reason. Deaf-
clinical-educational psychometric assess- ness in itself does not make D/HOH
ment with D/HOH children. Thereafter, the children less cognitively able than their
issue of appropriate test selection and the hearing counterparts, but the reduced audi-
respective merits of two main theoretical tory stimulation may lead to different func-
assessment modalities is briefly discussed, tional organisation of the brain (Edward &
namely, dynamic versus static assessment Crocker, 2008). Furthermore, it is likely that
models (see Resing & Figg, 2006, special cognitive differences may be accentuated
issue of Educational and Child Psychology on owing to differing developmental and social
intervention-based psychological assess- histories (Conrad & Weiskrantz, 1981; Dock-
ment). Finally, the implications of suggested rell & Grove, 1999; Kusche et al., 1983;
assessment processes are developed into a Marschark & Hauser, 2008; Naglieri, Welch
tentative theoretical model for the optimal & Braden, 1994; Plapinger & Sikora, 1995;
assessment of D/HOH children. Sisco & Anderson, 1980; Wood, 2003; Wood
& Dockrell, 2000; Zwiebel, 1991).

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 11


© The British Psychological Society, 2010
Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

Although D/HOH children may follow 1997). D/HOH children born within an
typical developmental milestones – apart environment with at least one deaf parent
from obvious oral linguistic and literacy diffi- and early language access such as British
culties associated with varying degrees of Sign Language (BSL) often perform signifi-
hearing loss – the path to an accurate and cantly better, but at least at parity with a
representative psychological assessment of variety of measures with hearing children
their abilities is still not an uncomplicated (Conrad & Weiskrantz, 1981; Gregory &
one (Edward & Crocker, 2008; Metz, Miller Hindley, 1996; Marschark, 1994, 2007).
& Thomas-Presswood, 2009). Furthermore, a However, this is not necessarily the case
wide heterogeneity within D/HOH samples with regard to literacy skills, which are
has been noted consistently through assisted by auditory phonetic access (Edward
research findings across the decades & Crocker, 2008). Although sign bilingual
(Vernon, 2005). This should alert the approaches argue for the early foundation of
assessor to considering a variety of potential sign to use as a bridge to English literacy
factors in order to obtain as comprehensive skills, the most effective pathway and timing
an individualised assessment picture as to linking sign and literacy is not yet clearly
possible. Arguably the most salient factors established, as the languages are not equiva-
for consideration are language, context, lent (Harris, 2000; Watson, 1999; Webster,
culture, cognition and possible ‘additional’ 2000). As Webster (1986) comments, ‘Unfor-
neurological considerations. tunately non-speech codes may be at a disad-
vantage when used to transduce printed
Language, context and culture words, since these derive from spoken words
There are obvious discrepancies between in the first place’ (p.178). Consideration
D/HOH and typically developing children’s should thus be made as to the extent of an
performances when tasks are laden with early common accessible language within
verbal language, although these differences the family (Powers, 1998). Closely linked to
may vary according to the extent and nature this is an awareness of possible cultural
of the hearing loss, including factors such as factors relevant to development, such as
aetiology, age of onset and cochlear implants varying degrees of familial identification
(Edward & Crocker, 2008; Wood, 2003). with ‘Deaf Culture’, where deafness is not
More than 90 per cent of D/HOH children perceived as a disability and is embraced or
come from families with hearing back- affirmed as a positive core of identity
grounds with varying degrees of impaired (Freeman, 1989; Lane, 1992; Metz et al.,
access to verbal communication and almost 2009). Optimal language access improves
invariably no or limited access to a fluent communicative opportunities and can also
signing environment (Braden, 2005). This encourage greater opportunities to enhance
communicative-linguistic difficulty is likely to attachment and socio-emotional develop-
have significant effects not just on devel- ment, the bedrock for learning and
oping literacy skills, but potentially affects expressing skills confidently (Austen &
attachment, social cognition and skills, iden- Crocker, 2004).
tity development and other areas (Austen &
Crocker, 2004). Non-verbal cognition
Not surprisingly, evidence consistently Given lesser linguistic loading, non-verbal
confirms that children with restricted access tests are potentially useful methods for
to language do less well on linguistic tasks examining the cognitive processing of
and it is thus crucial to document not only D/HOH children, following the maxim of
the details of the hearing difficulties but the assessing through an intact modality
linguistic opportunities available (or not) (Blennerhassett, 1990, 2000; Braden, 1992,
within the child’s environment (Campbell, 2001, 2005; Culbertson & Gyurke, 1990).

12 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2


Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard of hearing children

For these reasons, performance scales and verbal tests with deaf children (Blennerhas-
non-verbal tests have increasingly been used sett, 1990; Marschark, 1993; Moores & Sweet,
in the assessment of deaf children, such as 1990; Sullivan & Montoya, 1997). With
the performance scales from the Wechsler regard to academic functioning it is impor-
Intelligence Scales for Children (WISC) tant to consider the possible presence of
(Braden, 2001, 2005; Miller, 1991). These additional neurological difficulties (Metz et
have some evidence for reasonable relia- al., 2009).
bility, although validity remains to be
confirmed with deaf samples. The WISC-IV Additional neurological difficulties
version has additional administrative guide- Some of the heterogeneity within the deaf
lines (Flanagan & Kaufman, 2004; Krouse, population may stem from aetiological
2008). factors that have associated neurological
Even here, however, differing develop- concomitants, such as developmental menin-
mental histories may impact on perform- gitis, which may have an impact not just on
ance. Thus, for example, Marschark (2003) auditory processing but even on so-called
suggests that deaf and hearing individuals spatial tasks (Braden, 2005). A part of the
who have acquired American Sign Language assessment process may thus involve evalu-
(ASL) as their first language have been ating this potential factor carefully, via
found to be faster than non-signing equiva- guided interviewing and psychometric
lent peers in generating and manipulating assessment (Edward & Crocker, 2008; Metz
complex mental images. Native deaf signers et al., 2009). Some generic considerations
have also been found to have better facial around the actual assessment process are
recognition skills when the differing facial outlined below.
features are those associated with carrying
grammatical information in ASL General assessment considerations
(Marschark, 2007). There is also evidence Background of assessor
suggesting that supposedly ‘non-verbal’ Ideally all psychologists working with
tasks, such as visuo-spatial memory tasks, can D/HOH children should have at least
be successfully ‘bootstrapped’ by access to adequate training and knowledge with
language generally, sign or verbal (Bloom & regard to the language and culture of deaf
Keil, 2001; Wood, 2003). That is, language children (Andrews, Leigh & Weiner, 2003).
can act as a meta-cognitive resource to aid This is especially important because histori-
non-verbal task success. cally deaf children’s abilities have been
Although non-verbal tests may be useful under-estimated owing to poor methodolo-
to exclude global cognitive difficulties, they gies, inappropriate test usage or inadequate
are hampered by their poor predictive rapport between assessor and child
validity with regard to academic success (Freeman, 1989). The issue of necessary
(Braden, 2005; Freeman, 1989). For refer- competence within a specified work area is
rals where academic difficulties are of an ethical and professional obligation
central concern, they may have limited (Health Professions Council, 2009b).
direct utility with regard to developing inter- However, it is likely that current training,
vention hypotheses. Generally, it has been perhaps particularly for clinical and educa-
noted that verbal sub-tests provide better tional psychologists, may be relatively sparse.
predictive validity than performance sub- There is a need to train not only more deaf
tests for academic performance. This may professionals but to increase the general
stem from the linguistic loading of many training exposure to disabilities that include
educational curricula. Some researchers deafness (Atherton & Dent, 2003; Dent &
have consequently made an argument for Atherton, 2004; The British Psychological
the use of a language test alongside non- Society, 2006).

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 13


Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

Given that there is a shortage of specialist start with a formal assessment might perhaps
psychological services for D/HOH children inhibit the child’s early performance, owing
(Braden, 2005), consideration may be given to potential similarities with academic
to experiential learning, provided this is demands, with which they may have negative
within an appropriately supervised profes- associations.
sional development arrangement. Such an A full developmental history from rele-
arrangement should always be acknowl- vant care-givers and significant others such
edged and psychologists may need to struc- as teachers is crucial to be obtained initially.
ture this within required continuing This should cover factors such as aetiology
professional development frameworks and severity of hearing loss, medical, familial
(Health Professions Council, 2009a). and developmental details, cultural-
linguistic background, siblings, school and
Use of language behaviour (Edward & Crocker, 2008).
As noted, language is a crucial component in
deaf development, and for an adequate Test variety
assessment to take place there needs to be A few earlier studies have argued for a mild
good rapport, based on the development of global ‘lag’ in development amongst deaf
mutual understanding. It is obvious that this children (see, for example, Bond, 1987;
requires a good enough match between the Bracken & Cato, 1986). These have,
child and the assessor, with the ideal assess- however, been limited in terms of the diver-
ment procedure utilising the child’s primary sity of measures used. Assessment composed
communicative language (Braden, 2005). of a variety of instruments is more likely to
The use of BSL interpreters may be indi- reflect a valid and comprehensive evaluation
cated if necessary, but this should be mutu- of the child than one relying on a single
ally agreed and it is acknowledged that it instrument (Paul, 1998; Plapinger & Sikora,
may bring its own attendant problems, 1995). Different data sources, if carefully
including interpretative quality and confi- selected, can potentially complement one
dentiality issues (Cromwell, 2005; Freeman, another and ‘converge to bring into focus a
1989, Tribe & Lane, 2009; Tribe & Raval, single coherent (bigger) picture’ (Born-
2003). Marschark, Peterson and Winston stein, Haynes & Painter, 1998, p.370).
(2005) offer some research-based guidelines It is apparent that, in terms of the psycho-
for the use of signing interpreters. The issue metric assessment of D/HOH children, care
of language is also of central concern when needs to be exercised in test selection. Given
it comes to test selection and possible adap- the wide range of factors that impinge on
tations (Cromwell, 2005). academic performance, for example, it is
highly unlikely that one test or score will
Test selection adequately capture enough performance
Observe first data to ensure sufficiently detailed pedagog-
It is not necessary to leap into test adminis- ical/remedial prescriptions (Blau, 1991;
tration. As an expert in child development, Bradley-Johnson, 1991; Dockrell & Lindsay,
the assessor can pick up much important 1998; Kyle, 1980). As Ray (1989) has stated,
information from observational material: ‘No one method or instrument is sufficient for
what play material does a child engage with, the assessment of intellectual or cognitive
what do they do with it, do they attempt to abilities of hearing impaired children. What is
engage others; if so, how? The child’s spon- recommended is careful consideration of the
taneous elaboration of skills including instruments chosen, in view of its limitations,
communication is potentially more informa- the limitations of examiner and child, and the
tive than pressurising them early on within a purpose of assessment. The best assessment will
formal assessment schedule. A too quick come from a combination of instruments and

14 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2


Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard of hearing children

methods, interpreted in the light of the child’s population, it obviously raises issues about
age, linguistic competence, and cultural the limited potential for comparisons with
background.’ (p.37) other groups of children (Snidjers, Tellegen
This should not be done with a ‘scatter-gun’ & Laros, 1989, p.18). In addition, owing to
approach, however, but should emerge from the well documented heterogeneity in
a provisional formulation arising from D/HOH samples, the acquisition of broadly
referral and collateral data involving the accurate norms even within this group is
compiling of a suitable and complementary fraught with difficulties (Marschark, 1993;
variety of tools. For example, verbal, non- Meadow, 1980).
verbal, socio-emotional and academic vari- Deaf norms are perhaps more suitable
ables may all be of relevance with academic for verbal tests, given the general equiva-
difficulties (Edward & Crocker, 2008). lence in non-verbal cognitive functioning
(Andrews et al., 2003, p.190). However, even
Test difficulty and setting considering the significant variability within
One of the keys to selecting well is an aware- ‘typical’ development, the relative selection
ness of appropriate test ‘floors’ and ‘ceilings’ of ‘cut off’ points for atypical classification
– that is, choosing a test which is neither too and/or impaired performance remains
easy nor too difficult. This will help ensure controversial too (Dockrell & Lindsay, 1998).
that the child has enough early successes to It may also be that in order to access a test
encourage motivation and perseverance, more fully, the child requires a specific
given they may be prone to ‘giving up’ after ‘adaptation’ of the test.
a history of failures within non-ideal contexts
(Wolraich, 1987). Encouragement and Test adaptations
support within administrative guidelines are The procedure of adjusting a test to
thus important. Furthermore, given many maximise information is referred to as ‘test
D/HOH children’s reliance on visual cues, accommodation’ (Braden, 2001, p.24).
particularly when it involves possible lip- However, these changes need to be explicitly
reading or response to facial and non-verbal recorded and a decision made as to whether
cues, it is important to ensure that adequate these are ‘accommodations’ in order to
and correct lighting is available, and to avoid compensate for a child’s additional difficul-
situations such as the assessor sitting in ties, or actual modifications that may impact
shadow. A position opposite rather than next on the validity of the test and affect the use
to the child may also be helpful (Edward & of norms (Braden, 2001). A supplementary
Crocker, 2008). use of sign along with verbal instructions
(bilingual administration) may be helpful,
Test norms for example (Metz et al., 2009).
The issue of using separately obtained norms Signed test adaptations should always be
for D/HOH children is a contentious one. used cautiously – for example, on the recep-
There are some who feel that standardised tive language vocabulary measure, the British
IQ scores are generally biased against deaf Picture Vocabulary Scale-II (BPVS-II). There is a
children, as they make no allowances for potential for over-estimating ability if one
obvious difficulties with verbal performance embarks on a signed administration, owing
(Braden, 1994; Gregory & Hindley, 1996). to iconic clues within some of the signed
There have been attempts to develop sepa- items. Thus, some signs present a fairly trans-
rate norms for D/HOH children, for parent visual idea of their meaning (for
example, Tellegen (1993) on the Snidjers- example, a drinking action for drink).
Oomen Non-Verbal Test-Revised (SON-R). Others are a little less obvious (‘trans-
While this practice seems to be suitable for lucent’), whereas yet others have no clear
looking at differences within the D/HOH pictorial link with the designated word

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 15


Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

(‘encoded’: Miles, 1988, pp.65–76; Miller, states that this finding may derive from the
2008). Whilst it may be an option to try to LIPS perhaps being used more on ‘lower
identify iconic and transparent words (for functioning’ deaf children, that is, a greater
example, ‘hand’), and to withhold these proportion of children with additional diffi-
specific words, this leads to an erratic accom- culties. However, it is also possible that there
modation of the test presentation and may be differences in terms of their
violates norms (Braden, 2001). ‘construct validity’ (McCauley & Swisher,
Braden (2005) lists six potential features 1984) – the efficiency with which they
of the assessment task that can perhaps be measure abilities. Simply stated, some tests
changed to accommodate the difficulties of may be ‘better’ than others with respect to
D/HOH children, but these should all be the constructs they purport to measure. The
carefully documented and their potential revision of the LIPS is significantly improved,
impact on test validity evaluated, with suit- with additional sub-tests for visual memory
able caution around norm usage. The six and attention (LIPS-R: Edward & Crocker,
possible changes are: (a) presentation 2008). Table 1 provides a list of some recom-
format – for example, sign or print instead of mended tests for use with D/HOH children.
oral administration; (b) response format – Additional resources may be found in the
for example, signed rather than oral web-based guidelines for the assessment of
response; (c) timing – testing the limits, D/HOH children provided by Gallaudet
especially if the child is slowed by additional University (http://gri.gallaudet.edu/Assess-
motor problems; (d) test setting – for ment/). For a service-user supportive
example, home-based rather than clinic; (e) perspective on dealing with assessment
portions of test – see BPVS-II example above; issues, Marschark (2007) is recommended.
(f) substitute/alternative assessments – see
example below. Static versus dynamic modes of
It is acknowledged that there is currently assessment
a paucity of suitably standardised tests in Standardised testing, although it may gener-
alternative formats such as sign language. ally provide useful data, has been criticised
One example of a web-based test of BSL on the basis that it is largely a ‘snap-shot’ of
vocabulary being developed at City Univer- learned skills (see Resing & Figg, 2006). It
sity (Haug & Mann, 2008) is found at the has been argued that D/HOH children are
online address: www.dcal.ucl.ac.uk/ prone to a deficit in experiences mediated
Research/bsl_vocabulary_test.html. Addi- by significant others, or ‘mediated learning
tional generic tests that may be useful for the experiences’, particularly if there is a
assessment of D/HOH children are shown language disjunction between hearing
in Table 1. parent and D/HOH child (Blennerhassett,
1990, 2000). Tzuriel and Caspi (1992) have
Some potentially useful tests for suggested that dynamic tests may be most
D/HOH children suitable for D/HOH children because,
Tests are not necessarily equivalent, even if amongst other reasons, the procedure allows
they claim to assess similar areas of func- for decreasing the children’s anxiety by
tioning. Thus, with respect to the deaf popu- familiarisation with the test.
lation, Braden (1992) has shown that the use Tzuriel (2001) also postulated that the
of the Leiter International Performance test-teach-retest format of the assessment
Scale (LIPS) resulted in fairly consistent process can begin to redress the deficit with
lower scores amongst deaf children than the regard to mediated learning experiences
WISC-R Performance Scale, despite both faced by many D/HOH children. That is, by
tests supposedly measuring similar skills assessing how much performance improves
(non-verbal, performance related). Braden with guided support (mediation) across the

16 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2


Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard of hearing children

Table 1: Some recommended tests for the assessment of D/HOH children


(see Braden, 2005; Edward & Crocker, 2008; Metz et al., 2009).

Targeted Ability Recommended Test Deaf


Norms
Non-verbal cognition Snidjers-Oomen Non-Verbal Test (SON-R) Yes
(Leiter International Performance Scale) LIPS-R Yes
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV) No
Performance (Admin
guidance)
British Ability Scales (BAS-II) No
Visual motor Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of No
Visual Motor Integration (VMI)
Visual motor and Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT) No
memory
Receptive vocabulary British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS-II) (see caveats in text) No
BSL Receptive Skills Test (Haug & Mann, 2008) Yes
Dyslexia Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) No
Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (NARA-II) No
Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions (WORD) No
Dyspraxia Movement Assessment Battery for Children (Movement ABC) No
Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities (WRAVMA) No
Executive functioning Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) No
Speech and language Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4) No
WISC-IV (UK) Verbal Scales No
(Admin
Guidance)

retest phase, it is possible to make more hearing peers on this sub-test, controlling
specific academic or remedial suggestions for the higher initial scores of the hearing
than may be the case with standardised sample and ceiling effects (Wood, 2003).
assessments. A suggested test of utility is the Dynamic tests, although not group-normed
Cognitive Modifiability Battery (CMB) – as per standardised tests, may, therefore,
which addresses a broad range of cognitive have additional efficacy in detecting and
abilities and processes (Elliott, Lauchlan & developing strategies that may assist in opti-
Stringer, 1996). mising the performances of D/HOH
The sub-test specifically recommended children via enhancing meta-cognitive strate-
for use with deaf children by Tzuriel gies. It is unlikely that static cognitive
(personal communication) is the Reproduc- performance abstracted from the classroom
tion of Patterns, which has the least language context is sufficient both to account
required to assist with mediation, and also adequately for and provide sufficient guid-
applicability across a broad age range. ance to improving academic performance,
Evidence suggests that D/HOH children unless process issues around learning style
improve significantly more than matched and potential are also assessed.

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 17


Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

Finally, bearing in mind the importance (Haywood, 1997). This appears to be rele-
of contextual factors, assessment is incom- vant with children, where change is central,
plete without an appraisal of how the child and particularly relevant for deaf children,
functions in the classroom (Buck & where negative academic predictions may
Youngman, 1996; Haywood, 1997; Keogh, prevail (Conrad, 1979; Powers, 1998). Thus,
1993; Ray, 1989). Assessments concerned for example, Wood (2003) indicates that
with academic performance should, there- deaf children may have access to additional
fore, routinely include classroom observa- cognitive potential, which can be elicited
tions, thereby enhancing the ‘ecological with facilitative adult scaffolding. Contextual
validity’ of assessments (Bradley-Johnson, information includes informant measures
1991). These need to be ongoing and (treated with due caution) and suitable crite-
appraise the dynamic ‘fit’, that is, the inter- rion measures such as Webster and Webster’s
action between child and classroom context (1990) ‘Profiles of the Hearing Impaired’.
(Galloway, 2001). However, it should also include direct obser-
vations assessing the child’s interaction with
Towards a model of assessment for peers and teachers, both within the teaching
D/HOH children context and within less structured ‘play’ and
To integrate the variety of data required for familial contexts (Galloway, 2001; Gregory,
a fully informative and comprehensive Bishop & Sheldon, 1995).
assessment requires a diversity of assessment It is apparent that obtaining all this infor-
tools (Paul, 1998). The use of standardised mation is expensive and time consuming on
assessment measures has useful predictive limited resources, and thus it is important to
validity in terms of academic performance tailor the assessment to what is suggested by
and correlates with a range of factors associ- the assessment rationale for each individual
ated with educational performance. child (Gipps, 1994). Thus, a ‘comprehen-
However, it is likely that standardised tests sive’ assessment does not imply the
alone cannot account for individual unguided over-inclusive accumulation of
(unique) performances, as they are based on information. Sometimes ‘screening’ infor-
normative comparisons from standardised mation alone will be adequate to the assess-
data (Anastasi, 1976; Dockrell, 2001). ment question (Lindsay, 1988). However, it
Furthermore, they provide ‘static’ samples of does imply that a range of factors needs to be
performance and may be underestimating considered, and this may include ostensibly
the cognitive potential of deaf children. ‘non-cognitive’ factors such as emotional
Therefore, although for many it may be stan- functioning and strategic approach to tasks.
dard practice to use a normative test, on its The assessment model would thus need
own it may lead to reduced expectations to be one based on a transactional process
based on a performance that reflects a lack between child and context, and part of the
of mediated opportunities (Buck, 1998; assessment would involve evaluating the
Keane and Kretschmer, 1987). ‘goodness of fit’ between child and environ-
It is important for an assessment model ment. It is suggested that the ‘situated cogni-
to be able to identify cognitive potential in tion model’ (Brown, Collins & Duguid, 1989;
deaf children’s performance, as well as ways Greeno, 1989) may be particularly pertinent
of accessing this. Dynamic assessment as a model of assessment for deaf children,
models have been posited as useful ‘alterna- as it emphasises contextual data and the
tives’ to standardised tests, given their socio-cultural context of learning. However,
emphasis on process, and these models have the limitation of this model is that it argues
also been supported on the basis that they for the unique particularities of all cognitive
may uncover ‘learning potential’ which may activity, rejecting the notion of ‘within-child’
help defy negative academic predictions attributes (Desforges, 1999; Rogoff, 1990;

18 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2


Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard of hearing children

Saljo, 1996). This would obviously negate to with the professional involvement of well
some extent the validity of standardised trained teachers harnesses a powerful tool for
measures, based as they are on within-child learning’ (p.175). Thus resources will need
attributions of performance, established to be set aside for training and support.
with reference to normative data within a This does not imply the abdication of
normal distribution (Anastasi, 1976). psychological skill and expertise by psycholo-
However, appropriately selected standard- gists, who are potential training resources
ised measures (such as the SON-R or LIPS-R) and would retain the use of those tests
can provide useful information to a cognitive deemed too specialised for ‘release’ to other
assessment (Braden, 2001). It would thus be professionals. However, this assessment
unwise to dispense with their use. Further- model also has important professional rami-
more, Blennerhassett (2000) has cautioned fications for psychologists. Such a model is
that ‘multiple sources of data are needed to not easy to implement without sufficient
provide a comprehensive picture of clients. training, especially as the adequate assess-
The focus of an assessment is the person not ment of D/HOH children is fraught with
the test … The test alone is insufficient’ potential difficulties (Braden, 2001). Thus,
(p.190). training procedures to ensure proper (equi-
Thus, the ‘situated cognition’ model table) assessment procedures are followed,
does provide some helpful guidelines with with regard to the assessment of deaf
regard to developing a model of assessment. children, is essential.
Its emphasis on the uniqueness of potential An assessment model that is suitable for
development in varied contexts suggests that D/HOH children should thus embrace a
additional qualitative and process data are comprehensive appraisal of a variety of
needed to supplement standardised test data factors relevant to the assessment goal,
(Rogoff, 1997). This is especially apt for within an ongoing transactional (or
D/HOH children, who are a heterogeneous systemic) framework (Sameroff, Seifer &
group with a multitude of interacting factors, Bartko, 1997). That is, it should utilise a
which may contribute to a diversity of poten- multi-factorial transactional model. Compo-
tial developments (Marschark, 1993, 2000). nents of the model include static, dynamic
The successful use of the dynamic assess- and observational measures, with qualitative
ment measure by a deaf teaching assistant data where necessary that may assist with
raises the possibility that deaf signing adults providing an individualised account of the
may potentially assist with assessment as well child and their contextual ‘fit’. Such a model
as educative tasks (Stewart & Kluwin, 2001; will thus focus on assessing not just the child,
Webster & Heinemann-Gosschalk, 2000). but their context too, and on practical
They may be especially well placed to ‘decen- suggestions to optimise performance by
tre’, that is, to ‘think about the pupils’ looking at the influence of the child’s envi-
perspectives as well as their subject (test), to ronment.
learn from pupils’ responses, to adjust their
teaching (assessment) reciprocally as they Address for correspondence
interact with children’ (Webster, Beveridge & Dr Nicholas Wood
Reid, 1996, p.31). However, as Gipps (1994) School of Psychology,
maintains, ‘Any assessment policy will only be University of Hertfordshire,
as good as the teachers who use it’. There- Hatfield AL10 9AB.
fore, ‘to embrace educational assessment E-mail: n.1.wood@herts.ac.uk

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 19


Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

References
Anastasi, A. (1976). Psychological testing (4th ed.). British Psychological Society (2006). Clinical
New York: Macmillan. psychology training and disability: Information, guid-
Andrews, J.F., Leigh, IW. & Weiner, MT. (2003). ance and good practice guidelines. Leicester: Author.
Deaf people: Evolving perspectives from psychology, Brown, J.S., Collins, A. & Duguid, S. (1989). Situated
education and sociology. Boston, MA: Allyn & cognition and the culture of learning. Educa-
Bacon (Pearson). tional Researcher, 18(1), 32–42.
Atherton, R. & Dent, A. (2003). Training deaf clinical Buck, D. (1998). The relationship between educa-
psychologists. Clinical Psychologist, 32, 17–20. tional psychologists’ assessments and the alloca-
Austen, S. & Crocker, S. (Eds.) (2004). Deafness in tion of provision to children with special
mind: Working psychologically with deaf people across educational needs. Educational & Child Psychology,
the lifespan. London: Whurr. 15(4), 91–102.
Blau, T.H. (1991). The psychological examination of the Buck, D.N. & Youngman, M. (1996). An investigation
child. New York: John Wiley & Sons. of the relationship between reported results of
Blennerhassett, L. (1990). Intellectual assessment. educational psychologists’ assessments, and
In D.F. Moores & K.P. Meadow-Orlans (Eds.), levels of special needs provision, determined by
Educational and developmental aspects of deafness statutory assessment procedures. European Journal
(pp.255–282). Washington, DC: Gallaudet of Special Needs Education, 11(3), 298–309.
University Press. Campbell, R. (1997). Read the lips: Speculations on
Blennerhassett, L. (2000). Psychological assessments. the nature and role of lip-reading in the
In P. Hindley & N. Kitson (Eds.), Mental health cognitive development of deaf children.
and deafness (pp.185–205). London and Philadel- In M. Marschark, P. Siple, D. Lillo-Martin,
phia: Whurr. R. Campbell, & V.S. Everhart (Eds.), Relations of
Bloom, P. & Keil, F.C. (2001). Thinking through language and thought: The view from sign language
language. Mind and Language, 16(4), 351–367. and deaf children (pp.110–146). Oxford: Oxford
Bond, G.G. (1987). An assessment of cognitive abili- University Press.
ties in hearing and hearing-impaired pre-school Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf schoolchild: Language and
children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, cognitive function. London: Harper & Row.
52, 319–323. Conrad, R. & Weiskrantz, B.C. (1981). On the cogni-
Bornstein, M.H., Haynes, M.O. & Painter, K.M. tive ability of deaf children with deaf parents.
(1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: American Annals of the Deaf, 126, 995–1003.
A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, Cromwell, J. (2005). Deafness and the art of psycho-
25, 367–393. metric testing. The Psychologist, 18(12), 738–740.
Bracken, B.A. & Cato, L.A. (1986). Rate of concep- Culbertson, J.L. &. Gyurke, J. (1990). Assessment of
tual development among deaf pre-school and cognitive and motor development in infancy and
primary children, as compared to a matched childhood. In J.H. Johnson & J. Goldman (Eds.),
group of non-hearing-impaired children. Developmental assessment in clinical child psychology:
Psychology in the Schools, 23, 95–99. A handbook (pp.100–131). New York: Pergamon
Braden, J.P. (1992). Intellectual assessment of deaf Press.
and hard of hearing people: A qualitative and Dent, A. & Atherton, R. (2004). A sign of our times:
quantitative research synthesis. School Psychology Clinical psychologists’ awareness, concerns and
Review, 21(1), 82-94. interest in supervising a deaf trainee. Clinical
Braden, J.P. (1994). Deafness, deprivation, and IQ. Psychologist, 33, 30–34.
New York and London: Plenum Press. Desforges, C. (1999). Classroom learning. Leicester:
Braden, J.P. (2001). The clinical assessment of deaf British Psychological Society.
people’s cognitive abilities. In M.D. Clark, Dockrell, J. & Grove, N. (1999). Growing and
M. Marschark & M. Karchmer (Eds.), Context, learning with sensory impairments. In D. Messer
cognition and deafness (pp.14–37). Washington, & S. Miller (Eds.), Exploring developmental
DC: Gallaudet University Press. psychology (pp.220–242). London: Arnold.
Braden, J.P. (2005). Hard of hearing and deaf clients: Dockrell, J. & Lindsay, G. (1998). The ways in which
Using the WISC-IV with clients who are hard of speech and language difficulties impact on
hearing or deaf. In A. Priftera, D.H. Saklofske & children’s access to the curriculum. Child
L.G. Weiss (Eds.), WISC-IV: Clinical use and Language, Teaching and Therapy, 14(2),117–133.
interpretation. Scientist-practitioner perspectives Dockrell, J. (2001). Assessing language skills in pre-
(pp.352–381). London: Elsevier Academic Press. school children. Child Psychology and Psychiatry
Bradley-Johnson, S. (1991). Psycho-educational assess- Review, 6(2), 74–85.
ment of hearing impaired students: Infancy through
high school. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

20 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2


Psychological assessment procedures for assessing deaf or hard of hearing children

Edward, L. & Crocker, S. (2008). Psychological processes Kusche, C.A., Greenberg, M.T. & Garfield, T.S.
in deaf children with complex needs: An evidence-based (1983). Non-verbal intelligence and verbal
practical guide. London: Jessica Kingsley. achievement in deaf adolescents: An examina-
Elliott, J., Lauchlan, F. & Stringer, P. (1996). Dynamic tion of heredity and environment. American
assessment and its potential for educational Annals of the Deaf, 128, 458–466.
psychologists. Educational Psychology in Practice, Kyle, J.G. (1980). Measuring the intelligence of deaf
12(3), 152–160. children. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society,
Flanagan, D.P. & Kaufman, A.S. (2004). Essentials of 33, 54–57.
WISC-IV assessment. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons. Lane, H.L. (1992). The mask of benevolence: Disabling
Freeman, S.T. (1989). Cultural and linguistic bias in the deaf community. New York: Knopf.
mental health evaluations of deaf people. Rehabil- Lindsay, G.A. (1988). The identification of special
tation Psychology, 34(1), 51–63. educational needs: Screening and beyond. School
Galloway, D. (2001). Jack Tizard memorial lecture: Psychology International, 9, 61–68.
Educational reform and the mental health of Marschark, M. & Hauser, P.C. (2008). Deaf cognition:
vulnerable children and young people. Child Foundations and outcomes. Oxford: Oxford Univer-
Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 6(4), 150–158. sity Press.
Gipps, C.V. (1994). Beyond testing: Towards a theory of Marschark, M. (1993). Psychological development of deaf
educational assessment. London: Falmer. children. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Greeno, J.G. (1989) A perspective on thinking. Marschark, M. (1994). Origins and interactions in
American Psychologist, 44(2), 134–141. social, cognitive and language development of
Gregory, S. & Hindley, P. (1996). Communication deaf children. In M. Marschark & M.D. Clark
strategies for deaf children. Journal of Child (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness
Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(8), 895–906. (pp.7–26). Hove and London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gregory, S., Bishop, S. & Sheldon, J. (1995). Deaf Marschark, M. (2003). Cognitive functioning in deaf
young people and their families. Cambridge: adults and children. In M. Marschark & P.E.
Cambridge University Press. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies,
Harris, M. (2000). Social interaction and early language and education (pp.464–477). Oxford:
language development in deaf children. Deafness Oxford University Press.
and Education International, 2(1), 1–11. Marschark, M. (2007). Raising and educating a deaf
Haug, T. & Mann, W. (2008). Adapting tests of sign child: A comprehensive guide to the choices, controver-
language assessment for other sign languages: sies and decisions faced by parents and educators
A review of linguistic, cultural, and psychometric (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
problems. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Educa- Marschark, M., Peterson, R. & Winston, E.A. (2005).
tion, 13(1), 138–147. Sign language interpreting and interpreter education:
Haywood, H.C. (1997). Interactive assessment. In R. Directions for research and practice. New York:
Taylor (Ed.), Assessment of individuals with mental Oxford University Press.
retardation (pp.103–129). San Diego: Singular McCauley, R.J. & Swisher, L. (1984). Psychometric
Publishing Group. review of language and articulation tests for pre-
Health Professions Council (2009a). Standards of school children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
proficiency: Psychologist practitioners. London: HPC. Disorders, 49, 34–42.
Health Professions Council (2009b). Continuing Meadow, K.P. (1980). Deafness and child development.
professional development and your registration. London: Edward Arnold.
London: HPC. Metz, K., Miller, M. & Thomas-Presswood, T.N.
Keane, K.J. &. Kretschmer, R.E. (1987). Effect of (2009). Assessing children who are deaf or hard
mediated learning experience on cognitive task of hearing. In D.C. Miller (Ed.), Best practices in
performance with a deaf population. Journal of school neuropsychology: Guidelines for effective practice
Educational Psychology, 79(1), 49–53. (pp.419–464). London: Wiley.
Keogh, B.K. (1993). A matrix of decision points in Miles, D. (1998). British Sign Language: A beginner’s
the measurement of learning disabilities. In G.R. guide. London: BBC Books.
Lyon (Ed.), For the assessment of learning disabilities: Miller, M. (2008). Sign iconicity and receptive vocab-
New views on measurement issues (pp.15–26). ulary testing. American Annals of the Deaf, 152(5),
Baltimore: Paul H Brookes. 441–449.
Krouse, H.E. (2008). The reliability and validity of the Miller, M.S. (1991). Experimental use of signed
WISC-IV with deaf and hard-of-hearing children. presentations of the verbal scale of the WISC-R
Raleigh: North Carolina State University. with profoundly deaf children: A preliminary
report. In D.S. Martin (Ed.), Cognition, education
and deafness: Directions for research and instruction.
Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2 21


Nicholas Wood & Julie Dockrell

Moores, D.F. & Sweet, C. (1990). Factors predictive of Tellegen, P. (1993). A non-verbal alternative to the
school achievement. In D.F. Moores & K.P. Wechsler scales:The Snidjers-Oomen Non-verbal Intelli-
Meadow-Orlans (Eds.), Educational and develop- gence Tests. First annual South Padre Island conference
mental aspects of deafness (pp.154–231). Wash- on cognitive assessment of children and youth in school
ington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. and clinical settings. Fort Worth, TX: Cyberspace
Naglieri, J.A., Welch, J.A. & Braden, P. (1994). Publishing Corporation.
Performance of hearing-impaired students on Tribe, R. & Raval, H. (Eds.) (2003). Working with inter-
planning, attention, simultaneous and successive preters in mental health. Hove: Brunner-Routledge.
(PASS) cognitive processing tasks. Journal of Tribe, R. & Lane, P. (2009). Working with inter-
School Psychology, 32(4), 271–283. preters across language and culture in mental
Paul, P.V. (1998). Literacy and deafness: The development health. Journal of Mental Health, 3, 233–241.
of reading, writing and literate thought. Boston: Tzuriel, D. & Caspi, N. (1992). Cognitive modifia-
Allyn & Bacon. bility and cognitive performance of deaf and
Plapinger, D.S. & Sikora, D.M. (1995). The use of hearing pre-school children. Journal of Special
standardised test batteries in assessing the skill Education, 26(3), 235–252.
development of children with mild to moderate Tzuriel, D. (2001). Dynamic assessment of young
sensori-neural hearing loss. Language, Speech and children. New York: Plenum.
Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 39–44. Vernon, M. (2005). Fifty years of research on the
Powers, S. (1998). The educational attainments of intelligence of deaf and hard of hearing
deaf children. In S. Gregory, P. Knight, W. children: A review of literature and discussion of
McCracken, S. Powers & L. Watson (Eds.), Issues implications. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
in deaf education (pp.225–234). London: David Education, 10(3), 225–231.
Fulton. Watson, L.M. (1999). Literacy and deafness:
Ray, S. (1989). Context and the psycho-educational The challenge continues. Deafness and Education
assessment of hearing impaired children. Topics International, 1(2), 96–107.
in Language Disorder, 9(4), 33–44. Webster, A. & Heinemann-Gosschalk, R. (2000). Deaf
Resing, W. & Figg, J. (2006). Intervention-based children’s encounters with written texts:
psychological assessment: New approaches and Contrasts between hearing teachers and deaf
perspectives. Educational and Child Psychology, adults in supporting reading. Deafness and Educa-
23(3). Whole issue. tion International, 2(1), 26–44.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive Webster, A. & Webster, V. (1990). Profiles of the hearing-
development in social context. New York: Oxford impaired. Bristol: Avec Designs.
University Press. Webster, A. (1986). Deafness, development and literacy.
Saljo, R. (1996). Learning and discourse: A sociocultural London: Methuen.
perspective. Leicester: British Psychological Webster, A. (2000). An international research review
Society. of literacy intervention strategies for children
Sameroff, A.J., Seifer, R., & Bartko, W.T. (1997). Envi- with severe to profound deafness. Deafness and
ronmental perspectives on adaptation during Education International, 2(3), 128–141.
childhood and adolescence. In S.S. Luthar, J.A. Webster, A., Beveridge, M. & Reed, M. (1996).
Burack, D. Cicchetti & J.R. Weisz (Eds.), Develop- Managing the literacy curriculum: How schools can
mental psychopathology: Perspectives on adjustment, become communities of readers and writers. London
risk and disorder (pp.507–526). Cambridge: and New York: Routledge.
Cambridge University Press. Wolraich, M.L. (1987). The practical assessment and
Sisco, F.H. & Anderson, R.J. (1980). Deaf children’s management of children with disorders of development
performance on the WISC-R, relative to hearing and learning. Chicago: Year Book Medical
status of parents and child rearing experiences. Publishers.
American Annals of the Deaf, 125, 923–930. Wood, N. & Dockrell, J. (2000). Cognitive development
Snidjers, J.T., Tellegen, P.J. & Laros, J.A. (1989). in deaf children. Paper presented at the Proceed-
Snidjers-Oomen Non-verbal Intelligence Test: SON-R ings of the Child Language Seminar, London.
51/2 – 17: Manual and research report. Groningen: Wood, N. (2003). Cognitive and linguistic profiles of
Wolters-Noordhoff. hearing impaired children. Unpublished Doctoral
Stewart, D.A. &. Kluwin, T.N. (2001). Teaching deaf Thesis, University of London.
and hard of hearing students. Boston: Allyn & Zwiebel, A. (1991). Intellectual structure of hearing
Bacon. impaired children and adolescents. In D.S.
Sullivan, P.M. & Montoya, L.A. (1997). Factor analysis Martin (Ed.), Advances in cognition, education, and
of the WISC-III with deaf and hard-of-hearing deafness (pp.210–215). Washington DC:
children. Psychological Assessment, 9(3), 317–321. Gallaudet University Press.

22 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 27 No. 2

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться