Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

JSLHR

Article

Changes in Acoustic Characteristics of the Voice


Across the Life Span: Measures From
Individuals 4–93 Years of Age
Elaine T. Stathopoulos,a Jessica E. Huber,b and Joan E. Sussmana

Purpose: The purpose of the present investigation was to examine significant nonlinear trends for F0, but the trends were stronger
acoustic voice changes across the life span. Previous voice for male participants. Female participants demonstrated a
production investigations used small numbers of participants, similar nonlinear trend for SNR, whereas male participants
had limited age ranges, and produced contradictory results. demonstrated linear increases in SNR with age. Variability of
Method: Voice recordings were made from 192 male and female F0, SPL, and SNR followed nonlinear trends, higher at younger
participants 4–93 years of age. Acoustic measures of fundamental and older ages.
frequency (F0), sound pressure level (SPL), and signal-to-noise Conclusions: Changes in voice production occur throughout the
ratio (SNR) were made. Coefficients of variation for F0, SPL, and life span, often in a nonlinear way and differently for male and
SNR served as measures of variability. Variables were analyzed female individuals. Higher variability of acoustic measures of
separately with stepwise regressions, using age and sex as voice in both young and old speakers reflects changes in anatomic
predictors. structure, physiologic mechanisms, and motor control.
Results: Differences in F0 and SNR across the life span were
sex specific. Male and female participants demonstrated Key Words: voice, life span, acoustic

R
esearchers are altering their perspective of the de- several years (Huntley, Hollien, & Shipp, 1987; Linville &
velopmental process and are appropriately discuss- Fisher, 1985). Perceptual indices of voice known to dif-
ing both development and aging as one life-span ferentiate young speakers from old speakers include pitch,
phenomenon (Gallahue & Ozmun, 1995; Shadden & Toner, loudness, breathiness, and—especially for the elderly—
1997). Speech scientists and naBve listeners would agree tremulousness (Hollien, 1987; Linville, 2002). These per-
that the sound of an individual’s voice changes through- ceptual indices have been linked to acoustic measures of
out his/her life span (e.g., Harnsberger, Shrivastav, Brown, frequency, vocal intensity, noise, and acoustic variability.
Rothman, & Hollien, 2008; Linville, 1987; Morris & Brown, Acoustic measurements have been extensively used
1987). Listeners can successfully identify the sex of pre- to make deductions about the underlying speech phys-
pubertal speakers as young as 4 years of age (Bennett & iology. Acoustic output of the vocal tract is closely asso-
Weinberg, 1979; Ingrisano, Weismer, & Schuckers, 1980) ciated with underlying physiological mechanisms, and it
and can ascertain the person’s decade of life; they can forms a bridge to human speech perception (Forrest &
even distinguish a speaker’s chronological age to within Weismer, 2009; Kreiman, Gerratt, & Antoñanzas-Barroso,
2007). For example, acoustic measures have been used
a
University at Buffalo, NY to demonstrate that manipulations of speaking rate and
b
Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN vocal loudness result in changes to underlying articu-
Correspondence to Elaine T. Stathopoulos: stathop@buffalo.edu latory processes in speakers who are both neurologi-
Editor: Robert Schlauch cally disordered/dysarthric and neurologically normal
Associate Editor: Robert Hillman (Tjaden & Wilding, 2004). Acoustic measurements have
Received February 8, 2010 also been used to examine the changes to the physiol-
Revision received August 24, 2010 ogy of voice production across development and typical
Accepted November 22, 2010 aging. However, studies spanning the entire life span
DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0036) are lacking.

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Considering voice fundamental frequency (F0) first, sum, the findings about changes in the control of vocal
many investigations have substantiated the decrease in intensity across the life span are equivocal.
F0 from infancy and/or preschool through puberty (Eguchi Another variable that impacts how voices sound is
& Hirsh, 1969; Huber, Stathopoulos, Curione, Ash, & the amount of additive noise in the voice signal, which is
Johnson, 1999; Kent, 1976; Peterson & Barney, 1952; thought to reflect underlying processes of vocal fold clo-
Stathopoulos & Sapienza, 1997). These changes in F0 sure. It has been long known that perceived breathiness
are generally considered to be a result of increased length is closely linked to increased glottal opening and noise
and mass of the vocal folds as the human body grows. (e.g., Deal & Emanuel, 1978; Fritzell, Hammarberg,
The change is sharper for male adolescents at puberty Gauffin, Karlsson, & Sundberg, 1986; Hollien, 1987;
because of a larger increase in mass as the hormonal Linville, 2002; Södersten, Lindestad, & Hammarberg,
environment changes. Related to aging, Russell, Penny, 1991). Further, acoustic and modeling data support the
and Pemberton (1995) as well as Brown, Morris, Hollien, fact that women produce voice with larger glottal open-
and Howell (1991) found that F0 in women and men ings and open quotients than men (Hanson & Chuang,
generally decreased over a 50-year period. Toward the 1999; Holmberg, Hillman, & Perkell, 1988; Klatt &
end of that period, F0 decreased more in women and Klatt, 1990; Mendoza, Valencia, Muñoz, & Trujillo, 1996;
increased slightly in men. In both men and women, the Stathopoulos & Sapienza, 1997; Titze, 1989). Hanson and
changes in F0 have been related to hormonal changes, Chuang (1999) have provided a detailed analysis of the
leading to thinning of the folds in men and to thickening underlying causes of the increased aspiration noise in
or edematous folds in women. the voicing signal of women. Women’s stronger aspira-
A second important acoustic variable reflecting un- tion noise results in several acoustic measures, including
derlying processes is sound pressure level (SPL). SPL the following: less well-defined formants, steeper spectral
is controlled primarily through an interaction of respi- tilt, and additional high-frequency noise around the third
ratory and laryngeal mechanisms, requiring coordina- formant. A strong consensus is that women produce voice
tion of muscular (Finnegan, Luschei, & Hoffman, 2000; with more of a posterior glottal opening than men. In
Isshiki, 1965) and neural (Hirano, Kurita, & Nakashima, typically aging adults, increased glottal gaps—in par-
1983; Nagai, Ota, Konopacki, & Connor, 2005) control ticular, spindle-shaped gaps—have been substantiated
systems. McAllister, Sederholm, Sundberg, and Gramming by several laryngoscopic studies (Biever & Bless, 1989;
(1994) found that women and older children approaching Honjo & Isshiki, 1980; Linville, 1992; Pontes, Yamasaki,
pubescence are capable of producing a wider dynamic & Behlau, 2006). Along with findings of increased breath-
range than younger normal-speaking 10-year-olds. Dy- iness, findings show that the glottal mechanisms of el-
namic range is not an indication of speaking SPL, so it is derly men and women display some degree of vocal fold
unknown whether children spoke at different SPLs than edema, atrophy, increased membranous glottal gap with
adults. A large cross-sectional study of young children and vocal fold vibration showing greater vibratory aperio-
young adults showed that young children used higher SPL dicity, altered mucosal wave, and reduced amplitude of
than did the young adults when instructed to phonate a vibration. These changes to the vocal folds and glottal
syllable train at comfortable loudness levels (Stathopoulos closure result in larger amounts of noise in the voicing
& Sapienza, 1997); however, there were no speaker data signal. Signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and harmonics-to-
extending into the later years. When examining the noise ratio (HNR) are useful for quantifying the amount
older end of the life-span continuum, some studies have of noise in the voicing signal (Awan & Frenkel, 1994;
found that voices of older adults tend to be associated with Ferrand, 2002; Yumoto, Sasaki, & Okamura, 1984).
decreased vocal intensity (Linville, Skarin, & Fornatto, Ferrand (2002) reported that elderly speakers produced
1989). In fact, researchers believe that low vocal inten- voice with lower HNR (indicating more noise in the voice
sity may be an important indicator of old-sounding voices, signal) than middle-age and young participants, where-
and it is used as a target for treatment of aged speakers as jitter values (period-to-period changes in F0) did not
(L. O. Ramig et al., 2001). However, not all studies have vary across participant groups. Ferrand and Yumoto
reported higher SPL for young children or lower SPL in et al. (1984) concluded that a measure such as HNR is a
older adult speakers. Data from Huber et al. (1999) did more sensitive measure for quantifying the amount of
not show differences in SPL for young children versus additive acoustic noise in the voicing signal than jitter.
adults. Several other authors also reported no differences There is little agreement on whether children pro-
in SPL between younger and older adults (Biever & duce voice with more or less noise characteristics than
Bless, 1989; Huber, 2008; Sapienza & Dutka, 1996). adults, as the data from children with normal voice are
Huber and Spruill (2008) reported that older adults also scarce. McAllister et al. (1994) reported a high incidence
responded to several cues to increase loudness with the of glottal opening and breathiness and/or hoarseness in
same amount of loudness change as young adults. In 60 school children who were 10 years of age and reduced

1012 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
voice range profiles (F0 and SPL). Their stroboscopic 2. SPL will remain steady for most of the life span, but
findings indicate that 10% of the children had vocal nod- it will decline in older adults. There will be no sex
ules, and 23% had glottal openings. In children, an ab- differences.
normal glottal opening can lead to production of glottal 3. SNR will reflect higher noise in voice signals of
noise and breathier sounding voices. However, McAllister younger children and older adults. There will be no
et al. did not report HNR or SNR for these children. Data sex differences in younger children, but there will
from a glottal airflow investigation of normal-speaking be sex differences beginning at 20 years of age.
children (Stathopoulos & Sapienza, 1997) show that chil-
dren produce more glottal airflow than men during the 4. Variability of acoustic measures will demonstrate
closed portion of the phonatory cycle, which could lead U-shaped curves with higher variability for young
to more noise in the voice signal; however, measures of children and older adults. There will be no sex
noise were not made in the study. differences.

A final factor gaining attention for examining un-


derlying processes of voice production is variability. Nu-
merous investigators support the concept that increased
Method
variability observed in acoustic voice measures reflects a Participants
more variable motor control system. Increased variabil-
The sample included 192 (88 male, 104 female) par-
ity in children’s speech has been found in acoustic mea-
ticipants who were 4–93 years of age. Exact numbers of
sures such as F0 and voice onset time (Eguchi & Hirsh,
individuals, Ms, SDs, and age ranges for the age groups
1969; Ohde, 1985; B. L. Smith, 1994; Sussman & Sapienza,
are presented in Table 1. Participants had normal speech
1994) and in aerodynamic measures (Stathopoulos, 1995).
and voice for their age, as determined by a speech-
Additionally, increased variability of voice measures
language pathologist, and spoke with a general North
has also been associated with typical speakers as they
American dialect. Participants younger than 30 years of
age (Linville, 1988; Linville & Korabic, 1986; Max &
age were required to have hearing within normal limits,
Mueller, 1996; Orlikoff, 1990). For example, increases
whereas those older than 30 years of age were required
in the SDs of F0 and SPL have been found in older men
to have hearing ability adequate to follow instructions.
and women and are associated with tremor (Linville,
All participants had to be able to perform the speech
1988; Linville & Korabic, 1986; Orlikoff, 1990). Dur-
tasks for the voice recordings. Participants reported the
ing the developmental process, more variable acoustic
absence of any major health problems, as determined by
measures have led researchers to conclude that speech
an interview and a health questionnaire, and freedom
motor control must change to accommodate changing
anatomical and neurophysiological systems (Callan,
Kent, Guenther, & Vorperian, 2000). Orlikoff (1990), in
Table 1. Participant age summary descriptive data by sex.
relation to acoustic measures, also has suggested that
increased variability of speech production in very old
Female participants Male participants
participants is not unlike the increased variability of Age
the developing child. (years) n M (SD ) Range n M (SD ) Range
Empirical studies of voice characteristics have been
4 6 4.5 (0.28) 4.0–4.8 6 4.6 (0.23) 4.3–4.8
lacking, as broad age ranges have not been studied, 6 6 6.4 (0.24) 6.1–6.6 6 6.2 (0.10) 6.0–6.3
numbers of participants have been limited, and empir- 8 6 8.5 (0.35) 8.1–8.9 6 8.4 (0.18) 8.2–8.7
ical findings have been contradictory across investiga- 10 6 10.2 (0.06) 10.1–10.3 6 10.4 (0.20) 10.2–10.7
tions (Linville, 2001). A systematic data set utilizing a 12 6 12.4 (0.33) 12.0–12.8 6 12.6 (0.24) 12.1–12.8
large number of participants is needed to clearly charac- 14 6 14.4 (0.28) 14.1–14.8 6 14.3 (0.20) 14.1–14.7
terize the acoustic changes that reflect typical life-span 16 6 16.3 (0.20) 16.2–16.7 6 16.3 (0.18) 16.0–16.5
changes. The present research design allowed assess- 18 6 18.6 (0.29) 18.2–18.8 6 18.4 (0.28) 18.0–18.7
ment of life-span acoustic changes. On the basis of pre- 20 6 22.4 (0.82) 20.9–23.1 6 21.6 (0.55) 20.8–22.4
vious literature, we hypothesized the following: 30 6 31.6 (2.80) 29.6–37.2 6 32.4 (1.97) 30.8–36.1
40 6 46.3 (2.42) 41.8–48.5 4 44.7 (3.73) 40.3–48.7
1. F0 data will be consistent with previously published 50 6 55.0 (3.79) 50.0–58.2 6 54.4 (3.05) 49.8–59.0
data. F0 will be highest for the youngest speakers 60 7 65.0 (3.07) 60.0–68.8 5 65.5 (3.90) 59.9–69.4
and will decrease until puberty for both sexes be- 70 11 75.2 (1.79) 73.0–79.3 6 75.4 (3.43) 69.7–79.3
cause of growth. As speakers approach the fifth de- 80 10 82.3 (1.54) 80.0–84.7 7 83.5 (2.19) 79.6–86.3
cade of life, F0 will decrease for female participants 90 4 92.2 (0.88) 91.3–93.4 0
and will increase for male participants, reflecting Total 104 88
hormonal changes in the body.

Stathopoulos et al.: Acoustic Characteristics of Voice Across the Life Span 1013

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
from any acute conditions such as allergies, colds, or flu microphone signal during the study. The sound-level meter
on the day of testing. Participants were nonsmokers for was set for C-weighting. The gain applied to the micro-
at least the last 5 years, and they had no professional phone signal by the sound-level meter and the ART Tube
voice training. amplifier varied depending on how loudly an individual
spoke and was factored in when calibrating the acoustic
signal for SPL measurements.
Speech Tasks
Participants were asked to sustain an [a] for a “com-
fortable” period of time using their “comfortable every- Measurements
day pitch and loudness levels.” Three trials of the task were
The middle interval of each vowel production (500 ms
performed. Data collection from participants 4–25 years
for the 4–25-year-old participants; 2,000 ms for the 30–
of age and participants 30–93 years of age was com-
93-year-old participants) was extracted for data mea-
pleted at different times, separated by about 4 years.
surement. For most samples, all three measurements
The F0 and SPL data from the participants 4–25 years
listed below were based on the entire interval as one
of age were published previously in Huber et al.’s (1999)
uninterrupted sample using TF32 Jitter/Shimmer/
study.
Voice Aperiodicity SNR Analysis (Milenkovic, 2001).
If a sample contained a voice break, the voice break was
Equipment and Procedure eliminated, and measurements were made from more
than one shorter sample around the voice break and
For children and adults 4–25 years of age, voice then were averaged to obtain the trial value.
recordings were obtained using a free-field microphone
(Quest condenser microphone with sound-level meter, 1. Average F0 in Hz: The average F0 was calculated
Model 1700) and filter/amplifier (Frequency Devices, by averaging the values over the selected mid-vowel
Model 901) using a constant 6-cm mouth-to-microphone sample and then by averaging across the three trials
distance. The microphone signal was directly digitized for each participant.
into an IBM PC (analog-to-digital conversion board Data 2. SPL in dB: SPL provides information reflecting the
Translation, DT2821) using CSpeech at a sampling rate vocal intensity level of the production. SPL values
of 20 KHz (Milenkovic, 2001). The signals were low-pass were calculated by comparing the root-mean-square
filtered for anti-aliasing at 8 kHz by the Frequency De- values of vowel production with calibration tone
vices filter/amplifier. A sound-level meter and the Fre- values. Reported values were averaged across the
quency Devices filter/amplifier, coupled to the microphone, sample.
amplified the microphone signal during the study. The 3. SNR: SNR is the ratio of the periodic energy in the
sound-level meter was set for C-weighting. The gain ap- vowel signal to the energy in the aperiodic compo-
plied to the microphone signal by the sound-level meter nent of the voiced signal.
and the Frequency Devices filter/amplifier varied de-
pending on how loudly an individual spoke and was
factored in when calibrating the acoustic signal for SPL
measurements.
Statistical Analysis
For adults 30–93 years of age, voice recordings were Data were analyzed in two ways. First, to examine
obtained using a free-field, head-mounted microphone mean differences across the broad age range, participant
(AKG C240) and preamplifier (ART Tube Amplifier) means of the three trials were analyzed. Second, to pro-
at a constant 4-cm mouth-to-microphone distance. Be- vide an estimate of variability, coefficients of variation
cause there were two different mouth-to-microphone (COVARs) were computed for F0, SPL, and SNR. To ob-
distance recordings, the SPL data from the 30–95-year- tain the COVAR, Ms and SDs across the three trials for
old group were corrected from a 4-cm to a 6-cm mouth- each participant were computed. COVARs were calcu-
to-microphone distance using the following formula: lated for each participant by dividing the SD across the
three trials by the mean across the three trials and then
½20  Logð4=6Þ ¼ 3:5: ð1Þ multiplying the quotient by 100.
Each dependent variable was analyzed separately
The microphone signal was directly digitized into a Dell using stepwise regressions. Age (linear regression), age-
PC (Sound Card, Model SigmaTel C-Major, 9704) using squared (nonlinear regression), and sex were loaded into
TF32 (Milenkovic, 2001) at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz the stepwise regression as predictors. If sex was a sig-
and was low-pass filtered by the onboard SigmaTel nificant predictor, a second set of stepwise regressions
chip at 17.64 Hz. A sound-level meter and the ART Tube was completed on male and female participants indi-
amplifier, coupled to the microphone, amplified the vidually; age and age-squared were loaded into the

1014 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
stepwise regression as predictors. Significance level was stepwise regression, but the nonlinear regression, age-
set at p < .05. squared (F = 0.005, p = .95), was not significant. SPL
linearly increased with age for both female and male
participants (see Figure 2).
Results
F0 SNR
Sex was a significant factor in the stepwise regres- Sex was a significant factor in the stepwise regres-
sion (F = 140.71, p < .001, R2 = .22), so further analysis sion (F = 134.86, p = .01, R2 = .18), so further analysis was
was completed separately for male and female partic- completed separately for male and female participants.
ipants. For female participants, both age (F = 37.60, For female participants, both age (F = 29.41, p < .001,
p < .001, R2 = .48) and age-squared (F = 16.87, p < .001, R2 = .05) and age-squared (F = 24.93, p < .001, R2 = .24)
R2 = .55) were significant factors in the stepwise regres- were significant factors in the stepwise regression, but
sion. For male participants, both age (F = 122.14, p < .001, the nonlinear regression was stronger than the linear one.
R2 = .30) and age-squared (F = 88.85, p < .001, R2 = .66) SNR rose to 50 years of age and then fell after 50 years
were significant factors in the stepwise regression. The of age, indicating more additive noise in the voicing sig-
fit for the nonlinear regression was much stronger for nal at both young and old ages (see Figure 3). For male
both sexes than the linear one, and the relationship be- participants, the linear trend for age (F = 7.43, p = .008,
tween F0 and age was somewhat stronger for male than R2 = .08) was significant in the stepwise regression, but
female participants (see Figure 1). In male participants, the nonlinear regression, age-squared (F = 1.43, p = .23),
F0 declined steadily from 4 to 50 years of age and then was not significant. For male participants, SNR linearly
began to steadily rise. For female participants, F0 de- increased with age, indicating less additive noise as the
clined, although with a shallower slope than for men, to male participants aged (see Figure 3).
60 years of age and then rose slightly.
F0-COVAR
SPL Sex was not a significant factor in the stepwise re-
Sex was not a significant factor in the stepwise re- gression (F = 0.00, p = .99), so the analysis reflects male
gression (F = 0.08, p = .77), so the analysis reflects male and female participants combined. Both age (F = 23.24,
and female participants combined. The linear trend for p = .005, R2 = .23) and age-squared (F = 17.86, p < .001,
age (F = 29.60, p < .001, R2 = .13) was significant in the R2 = .20) were significant factors in the stepwise regres-
sion. For both female and male participants, F0-COVAR

Figure 1. Scatter plot of fundamental frequency (F0) by age: male


participants (open circles) and female participants (filled circles).
Figure 2. Scatter plot of sound pressure level (SPL) by age: male
Linear and nonlinear trends for both male and female participants
participants (open circles) and female participants (filled circles).
fit to the data.
Linear trend fit to the data across sexes.

Stathopoulos et al.: Acoustic Characteristics of Voice Across the Life Span 1015

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 3. Scatter plot of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) by age: male R2 = .03) were significant factors in the stepwise regres-
participants (open circles) and female participants (filled circles). sion. For both female and male participants, SPL-COVAR
Linear and nonlinear trends fit for the female participants. Linear declined to about 20 years of age and then started to rise
trend fit for the male participants. at about 60 years of age, indicating higher SPL variability
for young and older speakers (see Figure 5).

SNR-COVAR
Sex was not a significant factor in the stepwise re-
gression (F = 0.08, p = .77), so the analysis reflects male
and female participants combined. The nonlinear re-
gression, age-squared (F = 6.71, p = .01, R2 = .03), was
significant in the stepwise regression, but the linear trend,
age (F = 3.61, p = .06), did not add significantly to the
model. For both female and male participants, SNR-
COVAR declined slightly to about 20 years of age and
started to rise at 50 years of age, indicating higher SNR
variability for younger and older speakers (see Figure 6).

Discussion
declined steadily from 4 to 30 years of age and then The purpose of the present cross-sectional investiga-
began to steadily rise, indicating higher F0 variability tion was to examine age- and sex-related acoustic voice
for both the young and older ages (see Figure 4). characteristics across the life span, beginning in early
childhood at 4 years of age and ending with the very old
SPL-COVAR at 93 years of age. The present acoustic measures are
discussed in terms of the reflected changes to the under-
Sex was not a significant factor in the stepwise re- lying anatomical and physiological components involved
gression (F = 0.80 p = .37), so the analysis reflects male in voice production.
and female participants combined. Both age (F = 12.26,
F0 was included in this examination, despite the
p < .001, R2 = .09) and age-squared (F = 16.00, p < .001,
large amount of data already available from previous

Figure 5. Scatter plot of sound pressure level coefficients of variation


Figure 4. Scatter plot of fundamental frequency coefficients of
(SPL-COVARs) by age: male participants (open circles) and female
variation (F0-COVARs) by age: male participants (open circles) and
participants (filled circles). Linear and nonlinear trends fit to the data
female participants (filled circles). Linear and nonlinear trends fit
across sexes.
to the data across sexes.

1016 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 6. Scatter plot of signal-to-noise ratio coefficients of variation examination of the data shows that F0 decreased in both
(SNR-COVARs) by age: male participants (open circles) and female women and men, with most of the F0 decline occurring
participants (filled circles). Nonlinear trend fit to the data across sexes. by 50 years of age. Older men demonstrated a relatively
large increase in F0 after 50 years of age, potentially
reflecting a decrease in the mass of the folds because of
changes to the hormonal environment. It is clear that la-
ryngeal cartilages and muscle function change as a func-
tion of age. Although length characteristics do not seem
to change in the aging larynx, there are other multiple
changes to the cover, lamina propria, and body (muscle)
of the vocal folds that may account for the changes in F0
(Hirano et al., 1983). A more recent investigation ex-
plored the reasons for some of the vocal parameter changes
in the elderly male voice (Gugatschka et al., 2010). Men
who displayed increased F0 were also found to have
lower estrogen levels than age-matched controls.
Unlike F0 data, there were very little previously
published data on SPL and SNR across the life span. The
prediction for SPL on the basis of the limited literature
was that SPL would remain steady for most of the life
span but would decline in older adults. It was, therefore,
somewhat surprising that data from the present inves-
studies, because of the need for systematic replication tigation indicated that SPL increased as speakers aged.
of F0 data across a broad age range. Our predictions Huber et al. (1999) found no differences in SPL between
were generally confirmed. In male participants, F0 de- children and young adults. Several other authors also
clined steadily from 4 to 50 years of age and then began found no differences in SPL between younger and older
to steadily rise. In female participants, F0 declined adult speakers (Biever & Bless, 1989; Huber, 2008; Huber
steadily from 4 to 60 years of age but with a shallower & Spruill, 2008; Sapienza & Dutka, 1996). Overall, on
slope than that of male participants. Women’s F0 begin the basis of previous literature and the data from the
to rise slightly but not until 80 years of age. An obvious present study, it seems that average SPL does not reflect
explanation for young children’s steadily lowering F0 until the declining laryngeal system. However, variability of
puberty is increased length and mass of the vocal folds SPL shows a different result and may be a more sen-
(e.g., Stathopoulos, 2000). In middle-age women, the low- sitive measure of aging effects.
ering of F0 has always been explained by menopausal hor- The third prediction was that SNR would demon-
monal changes causing thickened or edematous folds. In strate a U-shaped curve, with higher noise in voices of
middle-age men, the vocal folds thin (Brown et al., 1991; younger children and older adults. This prediction was
Eguchi & Hirsh, 1969; Huber et al., 1999; Kent, 1976; partially supported by the data from the present study.
Peterson & Barney, 1952; Stathopoulos & Sapienza, Female participants followed the predicted trend. A lower
1997). Abitbol, Abitbol, and Abitbol (1999) studied 100 SNR for the young and older female participants reflects
postmenopausal women who were professional voice more additive noise in the voiced signal (see Figure 3). As
users. A small number of the women (17) showed clin- men aged, SNR steadily increased, indicating less ad-
ical level voice symptoms, and another 83 women showed ditive noise in the voicing signal. The fact that women
slight changes to the voice, including lower vocal inten- displayed higher noise characteristics than men begin-
sity, increased vocal fatigue, decreased range of high ning in their 50s fits with what we know about female
frequencies, and loss of timbre in their spoken and sing- hormonal changes. The average age for menopause is
ing voices. The changes in women’s voices were asso- 51 years, and it is well-known that the hormonal changes
ciated with a change in their hormonal environment, associated with menopause affect bone, skin, and mus-
including increased production of progesterone, which in cle, as well as other body systems such as the cardio-
turn was hypothesized to result in atrophy of the vo- vascular system (National Institute on Aging, 2008).
cal fold mucosa and thyroarytenoid muscles of the vocal There is a plausible explanation for why women produce
folds. voice with more additive noise than men. Along with the
The data from Raj, Gupta, Chowdhury, and Chadha acoustic studies mentioned in the previous section of this
(2010) are in agreement with those of Abitbol et al. (1999); article (Hanson & Chuang, 1999; Mendoza et al., 1996),
as estrogen decreased and progesterone increased, sub- videostroboscopic investigations of both young and older
stantial changes occurred in the women’s voices. Close women with normal voices showed that both groups

Stathopoulos et al.: Acoustic Characteristics of Voice Across the Life Span 1017

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
displayed glottal gap configurations during phonation. the increased variability could be linked to physical
Younger women tended to display a posterior glottal gap substrates such as reduced differentiation of the vocal
configuration, whereas older women displayed more an- fold layer structure/vocal ligament.
terior gap or spindle-shaped configurations (Linville, Higher SPL and SNR variability at young and older
1992; Pontes, Kyrillos, Behlau, De Biase, & Pontes, 2002; ages can be explained by changes to the laryngeal his-
Pontes et al., 2006). Hirano, Kiyokawa, and Kurita (1988) tologic structure for both young children and aging adults.
provided data linking the larger posterior opening in Laryngeal cartilages display different levels of ossifica-
women to the morphological structure of the posterior tion with additional age-dependent structural changes
glottis. The vocal fold angle is larger in women than to the cover and layer structure of the vocal folds (Gray,
in men. As women age and go through the soft tissue/ Hirano, & Sato, 1993; Hirano et al., 1983; Kahane, 1983;
cartilage changes that are inevitable, these changes are Sato, Hirano, & Nakashima, 2002). It is likely that the
coupled with the incomplete glottal closure pattern. The changes to laryngeal cartilages, soft tissue, and muscle
plausible result is an even noisier voice for older women affect the control of adductory forces that are so impor-
than for older men. The present large cross-sectional tant to the overall control of voice production. SPL and
study presents new data showing that aging affects men SNR show higher variability for the younger and older
and women differently—in particular, relative to noise speakers, reflecting changes to the physical substrates
characteristics of the voice. at both ends of the life-span continuum.
Children also demonstrated increased SNR, in both The variability findings in the present study concur
male and female participants. There are several laryn- with findings in previous studies, both in acoustic mea-
geal differences in children that may explain this find- sures and kinematic measures. Numerous investigators
ing. These differences include the following: an immature have supported the concept that increased acoustic vari-
vocal ligament until 20 years of age, differences in the ability of voice measures reflects a more variable motor
texture of the laryngeal cartilage and the shape of ar- control system (Eguchi & Hirsh, 1969; Linville, 1988;
ticular surfaces, and less dense ligamentous fibers such Linville & Korabic, 1986; Max & Mueller, 1996; Ohde,
as the macula flava (Hirano et al., 1983; Kahane, 1978). 1985; Orlikoff, 1990; B. L. Smith, 1994; Sussman &
These anatomic and physiologic changes may cause chil- Sapienza, 1994). Increased variability of nonacoustic mea-
dren to have reduced vocal fold closure and are likely to sures such as the spatiotemporal index in both young
result in increased spectral noise for the children. The children and older adults, as compared with that of young
present data establish that young children produce voice adults, substantiates the acoustic findings of increased
with more additive noise than young and middle-age variability at the ends of the life span (A. Smith &
adults. Zelaznik, 2004; Walsh & Smith, 2002; Wohlert & Smith,
1998). A. Smith and colleagues as well as other research-
COVARs were calculated as indicators of variabil-
ers have hypothesized that increased spatiotemporal
ity for each of the measures. There were important life-
index occurs in both children and older adults because
span effects for variability, but none were sex specific.
of changes in motor control and neuroanatomical sys-
All three variability measures showed significant U-shaped
tems (Callan et al., 2000; L. A. Ramig & Ringel, 1983;
curves, indicating more variability at younger and older
A. Smith & Zelaznik, 2004; Wohlert & Smith, 1998).
ages.
As mentioned earlier, Orlikoff (1990) has likened in-
F0-COVAR declined to 30 years of age and then rose
creased variability of speech production in very old par-
again. These data showed that both children and older
ticipants to increased variability of the developing child.
adults demonstrated higher F0 variability. Higher vari-
This hypothesis was supported in the data from the pres-
ability in older adults has been associated with tremor in
ent study in which all three variability measures dem-
the voice (Linville, 1988). Vocal tremor may be caused by
onstrated significant U-shaped curves. Without a life-span
both laryngeal and respiratory tremor (Farinella, Hixon,
study such as the present one, it would be difficult to
Hoit, Story, & Jones, 2006) and may reflect instability
discern these kinds of nonlinear trends across the life
in the underlying neurological substrates in older adults
span. Both child and adult voices change because of
(Aronson & Hartman, 1981; Duffy, 2005). Young children
their changing anatomy and physiology. Children are
have been shown to have less control over their vocal undergoing maturation, whereas older adults are un-
fold tension, possibly because of immature motor be- dergoing a lessening and weakening of their anatomical
havior or because of shorter vocal folds. For example, in and physiological systems (Gallahue & Ozmun, 1995).
Stathopoulos and Sapienza’s (1997) study, F0 increased
with increased SPL, and subglottal pressure was much
larger for the 4- and 6-year-old children than for the adults. References
The reduced control of vocal fold tension could help ex- Abitbol, J., Abitbol, P., & Abitbol, B. (1999). Sex hormones
plain the present findings for F0 variability. Alternatively, and the female voice. Journal of Voice, 13, 424–446.

1018 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Aronson, A. E., & Hartman, D. E. (1981). Adductor spastic Hanson, H. M., & Chuang, E. S. (1999). Glottal character-
dysphonia as a sign of essential (voice) tremor. Journal of istics of male speakers: Acoustic correlates and comparison
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 52–58. with female data. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
Awan, S. N., & Frenkel, M. L. (1994). Improvements in esti- America, 106, 1064–1077.
mating the harmonics-to-noise ratio of the voice. Journal Harnsberger, J. D., Shrivastav, R., Brown, S., Rothman,
of Voice, 8, 255–262. H., & Hollien, H. (2008). Speaking rate and fundamental
Bennett, S., & Weinberg, B. (1979). Sexual characteristics of frequency as speech cues to perceived age. Journal of Voice,
preadolescent children’s voices. The Journal of the Acoustical 22, 58–69.
Society of America, 65, 179–189. Hirano, M., Kiyokawa, K., & Kurita, S. (1988). Laryngeal
Biever, D. M., & Bless, D. M. (1989). Vibratory characteris- muscles and glottal shaping. In O. Fujimura (Ed.), Vocal
tics of the vocal folds in young adult and geriatric women. physiology voice production, mechanisms, and functions
Journal of Voice, 3, 120–131. (pp. 249–264). New York, NY: Raven Press.

Brown, W. S., Jr., Morris, R. J., Hollien, H., & Howell, E. Hirano, M., Kurita, S., & Nakashima, T. (1983). Growth,
(1991). Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics as a development, and aging of the human vocal folds. In D. B. J.
function of age and professional singing. Journal of Voice, Abbs (Ed.), Vocal fold physiology: Contemporary research
5, 310–315. and clinical issues (pp. 22–43). San Diego, CA: College-Hill
Press.
Callan, D. E., Kent, R. D., Guenther, F. H., & Vorperian,
H. K. (2000). An auditory-feedback-based neural network Hollien, H. (1987). “Old voices”: What do we really know
model of speech production that is robust to developmental about them? Journal of Voice, 1, 2–17.
changes in the size and shape of the articulatory system. Holmberg, E. B., Hillman, R. E., & Perkell, J. S. (1988).
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, Glottal airflow and transglottal air pressure measurements
721–736. for male and female speakers in soft, normal, and loud
Deal, R. E., & Emanuel, F. W. (1978). Some waveform and voice. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84,
spectral features of vowel roughness. Journal of Speech and 511–529.
Hearing Research, 21, 250–264. Honjo, I., & Isshiki, N. (1980). Laryngoscopic and voice
Duffy, J. R. (2005). Hyperkinetic dysarthrias. In J. R. Duffy characteristics of aged persons. Archives of Otolaryngology,
(Ed.), Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diag- 106, 149–150.
nosis, and management (2nd ed., pp. 275–289). St. Louis, Huber, J. E. (2008). Effects of utterance length and vocal
MO: Elsevier Mosby. loudness on speech breathing in older adults. Respiratory
Eguchi, S., & Hirsh, I. J. (1969). Development of speech Physiology & Neurobiology, 164, 323–330.
sounds in children. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 257(Suppl.), 1–51. Huber, J. E., & Spruill, J. (2008). Age-related changes to
Farinella, K. A., Hixon, T. J., Hoit, J. D., Story, B. H., & speech breathing with increased vocal loudness. Journal of
Jones, P. A. (2006). Listener perception of respiratory- Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 651–668.
induced voice tremor. American Journal of Speech-Language Huber, J. E., Stathopoulos, E. T., Curione, G. M., Ash,
Pathology, 15, 72–84. T. A., & Johnson, K. (1999). Formants of children, women,
Ferrand, C. T. (2002). Harmonics-to-noise ratio: An index and men: The effects of vocal intensity variation. The Journal
of vocal aging. Journal of Voice, 16, 480–487. of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 1532–1542.
Finnegan, E. M., Luschei, E. S., & Hoffman, H. T. (2000). Huntley, R., Hollien, H., & Shipp, T. (1987). Influences
Modulations in respiratory and laryngeal activity associated of listener characteristics on perceived age estimations.
with changes in vocal intensity during speech. Journal of Journal of Voice, 1, 49–52.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 934–950.
Ingrisano, D., Weismer, G., & Schuckers, G. H. (1980). Sex
Forrest, K., & Weismer, G. (2009). Acoustic analysis of motor identification of preschool children’s voices. Folia Phoniatrica,
speech disorders. In M. R. McNeil (Ed.), Clinical manage- 32, 61.
ment of sensorimotor speech disorders (2nd ed., pp. 46–63).
New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers. Isshiki, N. (1965). Vocal intensity and air flow rate. Folia
Phoniatrica, 17, 93–104.
Fritzell, B., Hammarberg, B., Gauffin, J., Karlsson, I., &
Sundberg, J. (1986). Breathiness and insufficient vocal Kahane, J. C. (1978). A morphological study of the human
fold closure. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 549–553. prepubertal and pubertal larynx. American Journal of
Anatomy, 151, 11–19.
Gallahue, D. L., & Ozmun, J. C. (1995). Understanding motor
development: An overview. In S. Spoolman (Ed.), Understand- Kahane, J. C. (1983). Postnatal development and aging of
ing motor development: Infants, children, adolescents, adults the human larynx. Seminars in Speech and Language, 4,
(3rd ed., pp. 2–25). Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark. 189–203.
Gray, S., Hirano, M., & Sato, K. (1993). Molecular and Kent, R. D. (1976). Anatomical and neuromuscular matu-
cellular structure of vocal fold tissue. In I. R. Titze (Ed.), ration of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic
Vocal fold physiology: Frontiers in basic science (pp. 1–24). studies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19,
San Diego, CA: Singular. 421–447.
Gugatschka, M., Kiesler, K., Obermayer-Pietsch, B., Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L. C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and
Schoekler, B., Schmid, C., Groselj-Strele, A., & perception of voice quality variations among female and
Friedrich, G. (2010). Sex hormones and the elderly male male talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
voice. Journal of Voice, 24, 369–373. America, 87, 820–857.

Stathopoulos et al.: Acoustic Characteristics of Voice Across the Life Span 1019

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Kreiman, J., Gerratt, B. R., & Antoñanzas-Barroso, N. Peterson, G. E., & Barney, H. L. (1952). Control methods
(2007). Measures of the glottal source spectrum. Journal used in a study of the vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 595–610. Society of America, 24, 175–184.
Linville, S. E. (1987). Acoustic-perceptual studies of aging Pontes, P., Kyrillos, L., Behlau, M., De Biase, N., &
voice in women. Journal of Voice, 1, 44–48. Pontes, A. (2002). Vocal nodules and laryngeal morphology.
Linville, S. E. (1988). Intraspeaker variability in fundamen- Journal of Voice, 16, 408–414.
tal frequency stability: An age-related phenomenon? The Pontes, P., Yamasaki, R., & Behlau, M. (2006). Morpho-
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83, 741–745. logical and functional aspects of the senile larynx. Folia
Linville, S. E. (1992). Glottal gap configurations in two age Phoniatrica, 58, 151–158.
groups of women. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Raj, A., Gupta, B., Chowdhury, A., & Chadha, S. (2010).
35, 1209–1215. A study of voice changes in various phases of menstrual
Linville, S. E. (2001). Acoustic aspects of aging voice. In S. E. cycle and in postmenopausal women. Journal of Voice, 24,
Linville (Ed.), Vocal aging (pp. 169–184). San Diego, CA: 363–368.
Singular. Ramig, L. A., & Ringel, R. L. (1983). Effects of physiological
Linville, S. E. (2002). Source characteristics of aged voice aging on selected acoustic characteristics of voice. Journal
assessed from long-term average spectra. Journal of Voice, of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 22–30.
16, 472–479. Ramig, L. O., Gray, S., Baker, K., Corbin-Lewis, K.,
Linville, S. E., & Fisher, H. B. (1985). Acoustic character- Buder, E., Luschei, E., . . . Smith, M. (2001). The aging
istics of perceived versus actual vocal age in controlled voice: A review, treatment data and familial and genetic
phonation by adult females. The Journal of the Acoustical perspectives. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica: Official
Society of America, 78, 40–48. Organ of the International Association of Logopedics and
Phoniatrics (IALP), 53, 252–265.
Linville, S. E., & Korabic, E. W. (1986). Elderly listeners’
estimates of vocal age in adult females. The Journal of the Russell, A., Penny, L., & Pemberton, C. (1995). Speaking
Acoustical Society of America, 80, 692–694. fundamental frequency changes over time in women: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
Linville, S. E., Skarin, B. D., & Fornatto, E. (1989). The 38, 101–109.
interrelationship of measures related to vocal function,
speech rate, and laryngeal appearance in elderly women. Sapienza, C. M., & Dutka, J. (1996). Glottal airflow char-
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 323–330. acteristics of women’s voice production along an aging
continuum. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39,
Max, L., & Mueller, P. B. (1996). Speaking F0 and cepstral 322–328.
periodicity analysis of conversational speech in a 105-year-
old woman: Variability of aging effects. Journal of Voice, Sato, K., Hirano, M., & Nakashima, T. (2002). Age-related
10, 245–251. changes of collagenous fibers in the human vocal fold mucosa.
The Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 111,
McAllister, A., Sederholm, E., Sundberg, J., & Gramming, 15–20.
P. (1994). Relations between voice range profiles and phys-
iological and perceptual voice characteristics in ten-year- Shadden, B. B., & Toner, M. A. (1997). Introduction: The
old children. Journal of Voice, 8, 230–239. continuum of life functions. In B. B. Shadden & M. A. Toner
(Eds.), Aging and communication: For clinicians by clinicians
Mendoza, E., Valencia, N., Muñoz, J., & Trujillo, H. (1996). (pp. 3–18). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Differences in voice quality between men and women: Use
of the long-term average spectrum (LTAS). Journal of Voice, Smith, A., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2004). Development of func-
10, 59–66. tional synergies for speech motor coordination in child-
hood and adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 45,
Milenkovic, P. H. (2001). TF32 user’s manual [Computer 22–33.
software]. Retrieved from http://userpages.chorus.net/cspeech /.
Smith, B. L. (1994). Effects of experimental manipulations
Morris, R. J., & Brown, W. S., Jr. (1987). Age-related voice and intrinsic contrast on relationships between duration
measures among adult women. Journal of Voice, 1, 38–43. and temporal variability in children’s and adult’s speech.
Nagai, H., Ota, F., Konopacki, R., & Connor, N. P. (2005). Journal of Phonetics, 22, 155–175.
Discoordination of laryngeal and respiratory movements Södersten, M., Lindestad, P. A., & Hammarberg, B. (1991).
in aged rats. American Journal of Otolaryngology, 26, Vocal fold closure, perceived breathiness, and acoustic
377–382. characteristics in normal adult speakers. In J. Gauffin &
National Institute on Aging. (2008). Menopause: Time for a B. Hammarberg (Eds.), Vocal fold physiology: Acoustic,
change. Retrieved from www.nia.nih.gov/healthinformation/ perceptual, and physiological aspects of voice mechanisms
publications/menopause/introduction.htm. (pp. 217–224). San Diego, CA: Singular.
Ohde, R. N. (1985). Fundamental frequency correlates of Stathopoulos, E. T. (1995). Variability revisited: An acoustic,
stop consonant voicing and vowel quality in the speech of aerodynamic, and respiratory kinematic comparison of
preadolescent children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society children and adults during speech. Journal of Phonetics,
of America, 78, 1554–1561. 23, 67–80.
Orlikoff, R. F. (1990). The relationship of age and cardio- Stathopoulos, E. T. (2000). A review of the development of
vascular health to certain acoustic characteristics of male the child voice: An anatomical and functional perspective.
voices. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, In P. White (Ed.), Child voice (pp. 1–12). Stockholm, Sweden:
450–457. KTH Voice Research Centre.

1020 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 1011–1021 • August 2011

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Stathopoulos, E. T., & Sapienza, C. M. (1997). Develop- Walsh, B., & Smith, A. (2002). Articulatory movements in
mental changes in laryngeal and respiratory function with adolescents: Evidence for protracted development of speech
variations in sound pressure level. Journal of Speech, motor control processes. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 595–614. Hearing Research, 45, 1119–1133.
Sussman, J. E., & Sapienza, C. (1994). Articulatory, devel- Wohlert, A. B., & Smith, A. (1998). Spatiotemporal stability
opmental, and gender effects on measures of fundamental of lip movements in older adult speakers. Journal of Speech,
frequency and jitter. Journal of Voice, 8, 145–156. Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 41–50.
Titze, I. R. (1989). Physiologic and acoustic differences be- Yumoto, E., Sasaki, Y., & Okamura, H. (1984). Harmonics-
tween male and female voices. The Journal of the Acoustical to-noise ratio and psychophysical measurement of the
Society of America, 85, 1699–1707. degree of hoarseness. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Tjaden, K., & Wilding, G. E. (2004). Rate and loudness Research, 27, 2–6.
manipulations in dysarthria: Acoustic and perceptual find-
ings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
47, 766–783.

Stathopoulos et al.: Acoustic Characteristics of Voice Across the Life Span 1021

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Iberoamericana- Instituto Universitaria User on 05/16/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

Вам также может понравиться