Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 18

Letter https://doi.org/10.

1038/s41586-019-1043-4

Complex societies precede moralizing gods


throughout world history
Harvey Whitehouse1,15, Pieter François1,2,15, Patrick E. Savage1,3,15*, Thomas E. Currie4, Kevin C. Feeney5, Enrico Cioni6,
Rosalind Purcell6, Robert M. Ross1,7,8, Jennifer Larson9, John Baines10, Barend ter Haar11, Alan Covey12 & Peter Turchin13,14

The origins of religion and of complex societies represent comparative methods to infer historical changes in Austronesian reli-
evolutionary puzzles1–8. The ‘moralizing gods’ hypothesis offers gions, reported that moralizing gods (BSP but not MHG) preceded the
a solution to both puzzles by proposing that belief in morally evolution of complex societies16. The same conclusion was reached
concerned supernatural agents culturally evolved to facilitate in an analysis of historical and archaeological data from Viking-age
cooperation among strangers in large-scale societies9–13. Although Scandinavia18. By contrast, another study of Eurasian empires has
previous research has suggested an association between the presence reported that moralizing gods followed—rather than preceded—the
of moralizing gods and social complexity3,6,7,9–18, the relationship rise of complex, affluent societies20. However, all of these studies are
between the two is disputed9–13,19–24, and attempts to establish restricted in geographical scope and use proxies for social complexity
causality have been hampered by limitations in the availability of that the authors themselves concede are ‘very crude’20 (for example,
detailed global longitudinal data. To overcome these limitations, the binary classification of societies as of either high or low complexity).
here we systematically coded records from 414 societies that span To overcome these limitations, we used ‘Seshat: Global History
the past 10,000 years from 30 regions around the world, using Databank’29, a repository of standardized data on social structure, reli-
51 measures of social complexity and 4 measures of supernatural gion and other domains for hundreds of societies throughout world his-
enforcement of morality. Our analyses not only confirm the tory. In contrast to other databases that attempt to model history using
association between moralizing gods and social complexity, but contemporary ethnographic data, Seshat directly samples over time as
also reveal that moralizing gods follow—rather than precede— well as space. Seshat also includes estimates of expert disagreement and
large increases in social complexity. Contrary to previous uncertainty, and uses more-detailed variables than many databases.
predictions9,12,16,18, powerful moralizing ‘big gods’ and prosocial To test the moralizing gods hypothesis, we coded data on 55 var-
supernatural punishment tend to appear only after the emergence iables from 414 polities (independent political units) that occupied
of ‘megasocieties’ with populations of more than around one million 30 geographical regions from the beginning of the Neolithic period
people. Moralizing gods are not a prerequisite for the evolution to the beginning of Industrial and/or colonial periods (Fig. 1 and
of social complexity, but they may help to sustain and expand Supplementary Data). We used a recently developed and validated
complex multi-ethnic empires after they have become established. measure of social complexity that condenses 51 social complexity
By contrast, rituals that facilitate the standardization of religious variables (Extended Data Table 5) into a single principal component
traditions across large populations25,26 generally precede the that captures three quarters of the observed variation, which we call
appearance of moralizing gods. This suggests that ritual practices ‘social complexity’8. The remaining four variables were selected to test
were more important than the particular content of religious belief the MHG and BSP subtypes of the moralizing gods hypothesis. The
to the initial rise of social complexity. MHG variable was coded following the MHG variable used as stand-
Supernatural agents that punish direct affronts to themselves (for ard in the literature on this topic11,14–17,30, which requires that a high
example, failure to perform sacrifices or observe taboos) are commonly god who created and/or governs the cosmos actively enforces human
represented in global history, but rarely are such deities believed to morality. Because the concept of morality is complex, multidimensional
punish moral violations in interactions between humans2. Recent mil- and in some respects culturally relative—and because not all moralizing
lennia, however, have seen the rise and spread of several ‘prosocial gods are ‘high gods’—we also coded three different variables related to
religions’, which include either powerful ‘moralizing high gods’ (MHG; BSP that are specifically relevant to prosocial cooperation: reciprocity,
for example, the Abrahamic God) or more general ‘broad supernatu- fairness and in-group loyalty. For analysis, these three variables were
ral punishment’ (BSP) of moral transgressions (for example, karma in combined into a single BSP variable. The Methods, Supplementary
Buddhism)9,12,16–18. Such moralizing gods may have provided a crucial Information and http://seshatdatabank.info/methods/codebook pro-
mechanism for overcoming the classic free-rider problem in large-scale vide further methodological details, definitions and justifications,
societies11. The association between moralizing gods and complex including a discussion of the relationship between MHG, BSP and big
societies has been supported by two forms of evidence: psychological gods.
experiments3,6,27,28 and cross-cultural comparative analyses7,11,14–18,20. Figure 1 and Extended Data Table 1 show the temporal and geo-
The contributions of theistic beliefs to cooperation, as well as the his- graphical distribution of the appearance of moralizing gods in our
torical question of whether moralizing gods precede or follow the estab- sample. Although societies in all 30 regions possessed beliefs about
lishment of large-scale cooperation, have been much debated9,10,12,23,24. appeasing supernatural agents through the performance of rituals, in 10
Three recent studies that explicitly model temporal causality have come out of the 30 regions, there was no evidence for moralizing gods before
to contrasting conclusions. One study, which applied phylogenetic their introduction under colonial powers. The remaining 20 regions

1
Centre for the Study of Social Cohesion, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. 2St Benet’s Hall, Oxford, UK. 3Faculty of Environment and Information Studies, Keio University, Fujisawa, Japan.
4
Human Behaviour & Cultural Evolution Group, Department of Biosciences, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK. 5School of Computer Science and Statistics, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland.
6
Seshat: Global History Databank, Evolution Institute, San Antonio, FL, USA. 7ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders and Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of
London, Egham, UK. 8Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK. 9Department of Modern & Classical Language Studies, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA.
10
Faculty of Oriental Studies, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. 11Department of Chinese Language and Culture, Asia-Africa-Institute, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany. 12Department of
Anthropology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA. 13Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. 14Complexity Science Hub Vienna, Wien,
Austria. 15These authors contributed equally: Harvey Whitehouse, Pieter François, Patrick E. Savage. *e-mail: psavage@sfc.keio.ac.jp

N A t U r e | www.nature.com/nature
RESEARCH Letter

1.0
2.1 1.7
2.5 2.5
3.5
3 1.4
4.2
2.5
4.8
0.2 0.9 1.6 2.3 1.7 0.2
0.3
Earliest precolonial evidence
of moralizing gods (ka) 0.5 1.2
Zoroastrianism
Abrahamic
Other MHG
Buddhism
Other BSP
Absent

Fig. 1 | Locations of the 30 sampled regions on the world map, labelled The three transnational religious systems that represent the first
according to precolonial evidence of moralizing gods. The area of appearance of moralizing gods in more than one region—Zoroastrianism,
each circle is proportional to social complexity of the earliest polity with Abrahamic religions (Judaism, Islam and Christianity) and Buddhism—are
moralizing gods to occupy the region or the latest precolonial polity for coloured red, orange and blue, respectively, whereas other local religious
regions without precolonial moralizing gods. For regions with precolonial systems with beliefs in MHG or BSP are coloured yellow and purple,
moralizing gods, the date of earliest evidence of such beliefs is displayed in respectively. See Extended Data Table 1 for further details.
thousands of years ago (ka), coloured by type of moralizing gods.

displayed a diverse range of 15 different systems of belief in moraliz- Table 1 and Extended Data Fig. 1). Notably, average rates of increase
ing gods: in some, the first evidence of moralizing gods came in the of social complexity were over five times greater before—not after—
form of MHG and in others it came in the form of BSP (Extended the appearance of moralizing gods (paired t-test, t = −6.6, d.f. = 199,
Data Table 1). The first appearance of moralizing gods in our sample P < 1 × 10−9; Fig. 2). This trend was significant both globally and
was in Egypt, where the concept of supernatural enforcement of individually for 10 out of the 12 regional time-series analyses (Extended
Maat (order) is attested by the Second Dynasty, around 2800 bc. This Data Table 1 and Extended Data Fig. 1). None of these 12 regions dis-
was followed by sporadic appearances in local religions throughout played a significantly greater rate of increase in social complexity after
Eurasia (Mesopotamia (around 2200 bc), Anatolia (around 1500 bc) the appearance of moralizing gods than before. Robustness analyses
and China (around 1000 bc)) before the wider spread of transnational showed that our primary finding of higher rates of increasing social
religions began during the first millennium bc with Zoroastrianism complexity before the appearance of moralizing gods was present
and Buddhism, followed later by Christianity and Islam. Although regardless of the type of moralizing gods (MHG or BSP), the choice of
Christianity and Islam would eventually become the most widespread variables used to estimate social complexity, uncertainty in the timing
religions, local forms of moralizing gods were present well before they of appearance of moralizing gods, or the time windows used to estimate
arrived in most regions (for example, Roman gods were believed to rates of change in social complexity (Extended Data Table 4).
punish oath-breaking from as early as 500 bc, almost a millennium In summary, although our analyses are consistent with previous stud-
before Christianity was adopted as the official Roman religion). The ies that show an association between moralizing gods and complex
diverse range of religious systems represented in our global sample societies7,11,14–18,30, we find that moralizing gods usually follow—rather
makes it possible to draw more general conclusions about religion than than precede—the rise of social complexity. Notably, most societies that
have previously been possible. exceeded a certain social complexity threshold developed a conception
Although our sampling scheme reduces non-independence, our of moralizing gods. Specifically, in 10 out of the 12 regions analysed,
polities still cannot be considered statistically independent because the transition to moralizing gods came within 100 years of exceeding a
of the historical relationships among them. We controlled for these social complexity value of 0.6 (which we call a megasociety, as it corre-
using a logistic regression model to account for temporal, geographi- sponds roughly to a population in the order of one million; Extended
cal and cultural dependencies in the global distribution of moralizing Data Fig. 1). This megasociety threshold does not seem to correspond
gods (see Methods). This analysis revealed that social complexity was to the point at which societies develop writing, which might have sug-
a stronger predictor of moralizing gods than temporal, geographical or gested that moralizing gods were present earlier but were not preserved
linguistic relationships, and remained highly significant even after con- archaeologically. Although we cannot rule out this possibility, the fact
trolling for these relationships (z = 6.8, degrees of freedom (d.f.) = 800, that written records preceded the development of moralizing gods in
P < 1 × 10−11; Extended Data Table 2), conceptually replicating pre- 9 out of the 12 regions analysed (by an average period of 400 years;
vious studies7,11,14,15. Supplementary Table 2)—combined with the fact that evidence for
The moralizing gods hypothesis posits a ‘statistical causal relation- moralizing gods is lacking in the majority of non-literate societies2—
ship’10 in which moralizing gods facilitate the evolution of complex suggests that such beliefs were not widespread before the invention
societies9,12,16–18. This indicates that, on average, social complexity of writing. The few small-scale societies that did display precolonial
should increase more rapidly following the appearance of moralizing evidence of moralizing gods came from regions that had previously
gods. To test this prediction, we conducted time-series analyses of the been used to support the claim that moralizing gods contributed to the
12 regions for which social complexity data were available both before rise of social complexity (Austronesia16 and Iceland18), which suggests
and after the appearance of moralizing gods (Fig. 2, Extended Data that such regions are the exception rather than the rule.

N A t U r e | www.nature.com/nature
Letter RESEARCH

a 1.0 b
80

0.8
Social complexity 60

0.6

Frequency
40
0.4

Moralizing gods
20
0.2

Doctrinal rituals
0 0
–2,000 –1,000 0 1,000 2,000 –15 –10 –5 0 5

Time Rates of change in


(years before/after moralizing gods) social complexity (SC per kyr)
Fig. 2 | Social complexity before and after the appearance of polity in which moralizing gods appeared (because times are normalized
moralizing gods. a, Time series showing mean social complexity over to the time of first evidence of moralizing gods, and there is thus no
time for 2,000 years before and after the appearance of moralizing gods. variance in this parameter). b, Histogram of the differences in rates of
n = 12 regions with social complexity data for before and after moralizing change in social complexity (SC) after minus before the appearance
gods. Social complexity has been scaled so that the society with the highest of moralizing gods. n = 200 time windows from the 12 regions. kyr,
social complexity (Qing Dynasty, China, around ad 1900) has a value thousand years. The y axis represents the number of time windows out
of 1 and the society with the lowest social complexity (Early Woodland, of 200. See Extended Data Fig. 1 for data for each of the 12 regions and
Illinois, USA, around 400 bc) has a value of 0. Vertical bands represent Extended Data Fig. 2 for a version extending beyond 2,000 years before
the period in which moralizing gods and doctrinal rituals first appeared. and after moralizing gods. The analyses in this figure treat the presence of
All errors represent 95% confidence intervals, with the exception of the either MHG or BSP as ‘moralizing gods’—see Extended Data Fig. 3 for an
vertical bar for moralizing gods, which represents the mean duration of the alternative analysis restricted only to the presence of MHG.

Conversely, of the societies in the ten regions that did not develop archaeological data may allow for a more nuanced understanding of
precolonial moralizing gods, only one exceeded the megasociety the timing and possible coevolution of the rise of doctrinal rituals and
threshold (the short-lived Inca Empire, social complexity = 0.61). moralizing gods. Such data appear unlikely to affect our primary claim
This suggests that, even if moralizing gods do not cause the evolution that complex societies preceded moralizing gods, but this is an empiri-
of complex societies, they may represent a cultural adaptation that is cal question open to future testing.
necessary to maintain cooperation in such societies once they have We demonstrate how quantifying cultural characteristics of past
exceeded a certain size, perhaps owing to the need to subject diverse societies can contribute to longstanding debates about the evolution
populations in multi-ethnic empires to a common higher-level power9. of social complexity. Our results suggest that belief in moralizing gods
This may explain why moralizing gods spread when large empires con- was not the only or even the main factor that enabled the expansion
quer smaller—but still complex—societies (for example, the Spanish of human societies, but may have occurred along with other features
conquest of the Incas). In some cases, moralizing doctrines may have of ritual practices and religion to facilitate cooperation in increasingly
helped to stabilize empires, while also limiting further expansion; for complex social systems. In particular, an increase in ritual frequency
example, when emperor Ashoka adopted Buddhism and renounced and doctrinal control may have facilitated the establishment of large-
war following his final conquest of the Kalinga Kingdom that estab- scale collective identities before the spread of beliefs in moralizing
lished the maximum extent of the Mauryan empire. gods. Thus, when it comes to the initial rise of social complexity, how
Although our results do not support the view that moralizing gods you worship may ultimately have been more important than who you
were necessary for the rise of complex societies, they also do not worship.
support a leading alternative hypothesis that moralizing gods only
emerged as a byproduct of a sudden increase in affluence during a Online content
first millennium bc ‘Axial Age’19–22. Instead, in three of our regions Any methods, additional references, Nature Research reporting summaries, source
(Egypt, Mesopotamia and Anatolia), moralizing gods appeared before data, statements of data availability and associated accession codes are available at
1500 bc. We propose that the standardization of beliefs and practices https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1043-4.
via high-frequency repetition and enforcement by religious authorities Received: 29 January 2018; Accepted: 19 February 2019;
enabled the unification of large populations for the first time, establish- Published online xx xx xxxx.
ing common identities across states and empires25,26. Our data show
that doctrinal rituals standardized by routinization (that is, those per-
1. Darwin, C. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (John Murray,
formed weekly or daily) or institutionalized policing (religions with London, 1871).
multiple hierarchical levels) significantly predate moralizing gods, by an 2. Bellah, R. N. Religion in Human Evolution: From the Paleolithic to the Axial Age
average of 1,100 years (t = 2.8, d.f. = 11, P = 0.018; Fig. 2a). Doctrinal (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, 2011).
3. Purzycki, B. G. et al. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the
rituals precede moralizing gods in 9 out of the 12 regions analysed, expansion of human sociality. Nature 530, 327–330 (2016).
and even precede written records in 6 of these cases (by as much as 4. Watts, J., Sheehan, O., Atkinson, Q. D., Bulbulia, J. & Gray, R. D. Ritual human
4,000 years in the case of Çatalhöyük in Anatolia; see Supplementary sacrifice promoted and sustained the evolution of stratified societies. Nature
532, 228–231 (2016).
Table 2). Although analyses of rates of change of social complex- 5. Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T. & Mace, R. Rise and fall of
ity before and after the appearance of doctrinal rituals do not offer political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature 467,
conclusive support for the hypothesis that doctrinal rituals facilitate 801–804 (2010).
increasing social complexity (Extended Data Table 3), these data do at 6. Henrich, J. et al. Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness
and punishment. Science 327, 1480–1484 (2010).
least suggest that doctrinal rituals led to the establishment of large-scale 7. Botero, C. A. et al. The ecology of religious beliefs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111,
religious identities. In the future, higher-quality and higher-resolution 16784–16789 (2014).

N A t U r e | www.nature.com/nature
RESEARCH Letter

8. Turchin, P. et al. Quantitative historical analysis uncovers a single dimension 29. Turchin, P. et al. Seshat: The Global History Databank. Cliodynamics 6, 77–107
of complexity that structures global variation in human social organization. (2015).
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, E144–E151 (2018). 30. Murdock, G. P. Ethnographic atlas: a summary. Ethnology 6, 109–236 (1967).
9. Norenzayan, A. et al. The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behav. Brain
Sci. 39, e1 (2016). Acknowledgements We thank Q. Atkinson and A. Willard for feedback on
10. Norenzayan, A. Big questions about big gods: response and discussion. Religion an earlier version of the manuscript and E. Postma for discussions on parts
Brain Behav. 5, 327–342 (2015). of the statistical analyses. We acknowledge the contributions of our team of
11. Johnson, D. D. P. God’s punishment and public goods: a test of the research assistants, post-doctoral researchers, consultants and experts. See
supernatural punishment hypothesis in 186 world cultures. Hum. Nat. 16, http://www.seshatdatabank.info for a comprehensive list of private donors,
410–446 (2005). partners, experts and consultants. This work was supported by an ESRC Large
12. Johnson, D. D. P. The wrath of the academics: criticisms, applications, and Grant entitled ‘Ritual, Community, and Conflict’ (REF RES-060-25-0085), a
extensions of the supernatural punishment hypothesis. Religion Brain Behav. 8, John Templeton Foundation grant to the Evolution Institute entitled ‘Axial-Age
320–350 (2018). Religions and the Z-Curve of Human Egalitarianism’, a Tricoastal Foundation
13. Schloss, J. P. & Murray, M. J. Evolutionary accounts of belief in supernatural grant to the Evolution Institute entitled ‘The Deep Roots of the Modern World:
punishment: a critical review. Religion Brain Behav. 1, 46–99 (2011). The Cultural Evolution of Economic Growth and Political Stability’, an Advanced
14. Roes, F. L. & Raymond, M. Belief in moralizing gods. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, Grant (‘Ritual Modes: Divergent modes of ritual, social cohesion, prosociality,
126–135 (2003). and conflict’, grant agreement no. 694986) and a Starter Grant (‘The Cultural
15. Peoples, H. C. & Marlowe, F. W. Subsistence and the evolution of religion. Evolution & Ecology of Institutions’, grant agreement no. 716212) from the
Hum. Nat. 23, 253–269 (2012). European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020
16. Watts, J. et al. Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods Research and Innovation Programme, an award from the Templeton World
precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, Charity Foundation entitled ‘Cognitive and Cultural Foundations of Religion
20142556 (2015). and Morality’ (TWCF0164), a Keio Research Institute at SFC Startup Grant, a
17. Gray, R. D. & Watts, J. Cultural macroevolution matters. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA Keio Gijuku Academic Development Fund Individual Grant and a grant from
114, 7846–7852 (2017). the European Union Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme
18. Raffield, B., Price, N. & Collard, M. Religious belief and cooperation: a view from (grant agreement no. 644055 (ALIGNED, www.aligned-project.eu)).
Viking-age Scandinavia. Religion Brain Behav. 9, 2–22 (2019).
19. Baumard, N. & Boyer, P. Explaining moral religions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17,
272–280 (2013). Reviewer information Nature thanks Carol Ember, Peter J. Richerson and the
20. Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I. & Boyer, P. Increased affluence explains the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this
emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Curr. Biol. 25, 10–15 work.
(2015).
21. Mullins, D. A. et al. A systematic assessment of ‘Axial Age’ proposals Author contributions H.W., P.F. and P.E.S. designed the study, with input from
using global comparative historical evidence. Am. Sociol. Rev. 83, 596–626 P.T., T.E.C., K.C.F. and R.M.R.; E.C. and R.P. coded the religion and ritual data, with
(2018). additional input from J.L., J.B., B.t.H and A.C.; P.E.S. analysed the data, with input
22. Purzycki, B. G. et al. Material security, life history, and moralistic religions: a from H.W., P.F., P.T., T.E.C. and R.M.R.; P.E.S., H.W. and P.F. drafted the manuscript.
cross-cultural examination. PLoS ONE 13, e0193856 (2018).
23. Stausberg, M. Big gods in review: introducing Ara Norenzayan and his critics. Competing interests The authors declare no competing interests.
Religion 44, 592–608 (2014).
24. McKay, R. & Whitehouse, H. Religion and morality. Psychol. Bull. 141, 447–473 Additional information
(2015). Extended data is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-
25. Whitehouse, H. Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission 019-1043-4.
(AltaMira, Walnut Creek, 2004). Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/
26. Whitehouse, H., François, P. & Turchin, P. The role of ritual in the evolution of 10.1038/s41586-019-1043-4.
social complexity: five predictions and a drum roll. Cliodynamics 6, 199–216 Reprints and permissions information is available at http://www.nature.com/
(2015). reprints.
27. Norenzayan, A. & Shariff, A. F. The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to P.E.S.
Science 322, 58–62 (2008). Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional
28. Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T. & Norenzayan, A. Religious priming: a claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20, 27–48
(2016). © The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2019

N A t U r e | www.nature.com/nature
Letter RESEARCH

Methods guidance on how to delineate the temporal and geographical boundaries of the
Seshat: Global History Databank overview. Seshat (http://seshatdatabank. polity, assembled an initial reading list and—where necessary—helped to interpret
info/) is a vast database of information about global history from the end of the some of the key historiographical debates associated with the variables. Research
Palaeolithic period up to the Industrial Revolution. Eventually, it is intended that assistants then populated the variables with data and presented this to the Seshat
Seshat will cover the history of all past human societies; however, initially the goal experts for review. The comments and suggestions made by the experts were then
has been to capture as much diversity in global history as possible. We therefore implemented by the research assistants. The next stage required a second team of
created a stratified sample of past societies by identifying ten world regions dis- fully trained research assistants to go over the gathered data and to conduct a series
tributed as widely as possible across the Earth’s surface and within each of those of quality checks, including vetting of the footnotes and the use of correct syntax
regions designated three ‘natural geographical areas’ (NGAs) with discrete eco- for the machine-readable part of the data. Finally, this checked dataset was given
logical boundaries, on average about 10,000 km2 in size, thus creating an initial to the Seshat experts for review. The coding of religion and ritual data required
sampling scheme of 30 such areas around the world. To maximize diversity in the input of experts every step of the way, given the frequent need for complex
the sample, for each world region we chose one NGA in which social complexity and nuanced interpretation of the evidence. By contrast, the data required for the
emerged early (for example, Egypt), one in which it arose relatively recently (for social complexity variables frequently consisted of facts that research assistants
example, Iceland) and one in which it emerged somewhere in the middle of the could procure with less supervision, allowing expert input and review to occur at
range (for example, Japan)29. The 30 regions and their selection rationale have a later stage of the process.
previously been published29, before the start of data collection. Our aim was to Data coding. Social complexity. The 51 variables used to construct the overall social
maximize variability in our global sample while minimizing historical relationships complexity measure are shown in Extended Data Table 5. These variables were
between cultures. chosen because they reflect common features associated with social complexity
Data on political systems (polities) that emerged and persisted in each of the and were grouped into nine complexity characteristics (polity population size,
NGAs were then gathered and entered into Seshat in a continuous time series at capital population size, polity territory size, hierarchy, infrastructure, government,
100-year intervals, going back as far into the history of that area as scholarly liter- information systems, texts and money). Details of coding definitions for these
ature would allow (up to a maximum of roughly 10,000 years before present). In variables have previously been published8,29.
the case of NGAs that contained clusters of very small-scale polities that share a MHG. For consistency with previous studies that have generally used the MHG
similar culture but are not under a single system of jurisdictional control, we refer variable from the Ethnographic Atlas30, the presence of MHG was coded as a binary
to these as ‘quasi-polities’ and code information on all of them generically, unless variable on the basis of the original definition of a high god as “a spiritual being
information is available that would allow us to differentiate between these polities. who is believed to have created all reality and/or to be its ultimate governor, even
All variables for which data have been gathered and entered into Seshat are though his sole act was to create other spirits who, in turn, created or control the
derived from a Seshat Codebook that can be accessed and downloaded (http:// natural world”31. The following categories were used: (1) absent or not reported,
seshatdatabank.info/methods/codebook). The Codebook was designed by, and (2) present but not active in human affairs, (3) present and active in human affairs
is continually updated and extended in consultation with, a large network of pro- but not supportive of human morality and (4) present, active and specifically sup-
fessional historians, archaeologists, anthropologists and other specialists whom portive of human morality11,32.Thus, a coding of high gods as present, active and
we refer to as ‘Seshat experts’. Most variables in Seshat require the data to take the specifically supportive of human morality was coded as a MHG being present,
form of a number or numerical range or they specify a feature that can be coded whereas all other types were coded as absent.
as absent, present or unknown (additionally coding items as ‘inferred present’ or BSP. The terms big gods and MHG are sometimes used interchangeably17, but
‘inferred absent’, where the evidence permits). The first step in data entry was for can have different connotations. The term MHG was previously developed and
trained research assistants to gather and input easily acquired data, and at the same defined, and it was proposed that high gods were associated with social complex-
time to compile lists of data that are more difficult to interpret and that require ity (regardless of their moral concern)31. This definition of the MHG variable
input from Seshat experts. Especially during the early phases of data entry, varia- was incorporated into the Ethnographic Atlas30, resulting in it being widely used
bles in the codebook were revised and improved through continuous discussions in cross-cultural research. These ideas were subsequently extended in the super-
between research assistants and Seshat experts. All data are linked to scholarly natural punishment hypothesis11,12,33, in which the focus lay on the mechanism
sources, including peer-reviewed publications and personal communications from of morality enforcement rather than high gods; however, the MHG variable was
established authorities. On occasions when Seshat experts disagree on a particular used for testing because of the availability of previous research using this defini-
coding, we kept a record of disagreements so that analyses could be run taking into tion. The ideas have been further developed9,10,27,34 to include various additional
account contrasting interpretations. Once used for the purposes of data analysis mechanisms, most notably including cultural group selection to explain the rapid
and publication, that version of the dataset was ‘frozen’ so that it could be inspected spread of moralizing gods without accompanying genetic changes.
by others and used for the purposes of replication. Nevertheless, the data in Seshat The term big gods (defined as “powerful, omniscient, interventionist, morally
continually evolves, as new sources are discovered and as new Seshat experts con- concerned gods”34) was originally the title of a monograph describing this theory.
tribute additional layers of interpretation. Later, however, this definition was relaxed and it was emphasized that the term
The data analysed in the present paper focused on those sectors of the Seshat big gods was a rhetorical device intended to include a broad range of morally
Codebook concerned with social complexity, religion and ritual. A full account concerned supernatural agents, not only MHGs: “…powerful, all-knowing and
of the social complexity variables has previously been published8, using 51 varia- morally concerned supernatural agents who are believed to monitor social inter-
bles (Extended Data Table 5) associated with population size, hierarchy, territory, actions and to reward and sanction behaviours in ways that contribute to the cul-
governance, bureaucracy, infrastructure, record keeping, economic development tural success of the group, including practices that effectively transmit the faith.
and other domains that were previously identified as potentially relevant meas- Rhetorically, we call these ‘Big Gods’; however, we alert readers that we are referring
ures of social complexity. This required engagement with a wide-ranging body to a multidimensional continuum of supernatural agents in which big gods occupy
of literature on social complexity. Because previous researchers disagreed about a particular corner of the space”9.
which dimensions of social complexity were the most important to emphasize (for Subsequently an additional variable was developed that was called broad super-
example, number of jurisdictional levels versus more-horizontal forms of com- natural punishment16, which arguably more closely matches this relaxed definition
plexity; autocracy versus democracy; diversity of specialist roles versus centralized of big gods than does the traditional MHG variable. BSP was defined as follows:
coordination), we included proxies for all potentially relevant measures of social “For BSP to be coded as present in a culture there must be the concept of a super-
complexity that had been identified in the literature. This inclusive strategy was natural agent or process that reliably monitors and punishes selfish actions, and
designed to allow us to investigate whether these different characteristics exhibited this concept must (i) be widely advocated within the community, (ii) involve pun-
strong relationships with each other and whether a single principal component cap- ishment of a broad range of selfish behaviours and (iii) apply to a wide range of
tured most of the observed variation. Our analyses confirmed that both are indeed community members.”16.
the case. Furthermore, we found that different characteristics of social complexity Because selfish actions can occur in a variety of domains, Seshat subdivides the
were highly predictable across different world regions8. types of supernatural enforcement of morality based on nine proposed categories
Whereas previous research has proposed an association between the rise of mor- of morality35,36. For this study, we focused on three domains that are relevant to the
alizing gods and the evolution of social complexity, measures used in the past to establishment of large-scale cooperation: (1) fairness (sharing of resources, such as
capture the latter have been comparatively crude. Variable selection and inclusion dividing disputed resources, bargaining or redistribution of wealth); (2) reciproc-
for moralizing gods was informed by existing literature on so-called big gods, MHG ity (for example, fulfilling contracts, returning gifts, repaying debts or upholding
and BSP, as well as psychological and cross-cultural comparative research on the trust); and (3) in-group loyalty (the need to remain loyal to unrelated members of
hypothesized link between belief in moralizing gods and large-scale cooperation. the same group; for example, helping coreligionists or going to war for one’s group).
Data collection for the religion and ritual variables involved matching each fully BSP was coded as present if at least one of these three sub-types of selfish actions
trained research assistant with one or more Seshat experts. Seshat experts provided was supernaturally enforced.
RESEARCH Letter

Our robustness analyses, which evaluated BSP and MHG separately (Extended Analyses. To ensure consistency and comparability in our analyses, we sampled
Data Table 4) suggest that—rather than moralizing gods following a general pattern polities at 100-year intervals, sampling whichever polity happened to occupy
of evolution from small (BSP) to big (MHG)—the presence or absence of high a given region at ad 100, ad 200, ad 300 and so on, and not including polities
gods independent of their moralizing status has little functional relationship with that existed only between century boundaries8 (see Supplementary Information
social complexity, and instead appears largely contingent on history and geog- for details and examples regarding the temporal sampling procedure). All
raphy. In regions such as southern and eastern Asia, BSP in the form of karmic analyses were performed in R v.3.4.142. All P values reported are from two-tailed
religions (Buddhism and Hinduism) remains the dominant form of moralizing analyses.
gods, whereas in regions such as Europe and Africa moralizing Abrahamic MHGs Quantifying social complexity. To create an overall measure of social complexity,
were commonly adopted or imposed without any intermediate evolution through we took a previously published approach based on principal component analy-
a BSP stage. sis (PCA)8 and applied it to the latest available data from Seshat. This method
Doctrinal rituals. The modes of religiosity hypothesis focuses on two factors that aggregates the 51 social complexity variables (Extended Data Table 5) into nine
facilitate standardization of a body of beliefs and practices. First, high frequency complexity characteristics and then analyses them using PCA.
(for example, daily or weekly) collective rituals facilitate easy detection of devia- PCA is a commonly used tool for dimension reduction—in this case we have
tions from the orthodox canon. Second, religious hierarchy enables enforcement nine different aggregated variables that we want to reduce to a single variable that
of authorized belief and practice. Seshat codes five different types of rituals: the best captures social complexity. However, we obtain the same conclusions even
most frequent, most widespread, largest scale, most euphoric and most dysphoric without using PCA, regardless of which of the nine complexity characteristics we
rituals. For each ritual, frequency is coded as daily, weekly, monthly, seasonally, choose as a proxy for social complexity (Extended Data Table 4).
yearly, generationally or once-in-a-lifetime. Seshat also encodes levels of religious As previously shown, these different complexity characteristics turn out to be
hierarchy. One represents no levels of religious hierarchy beyond the local priest highly correlated and all load heavily onto a single principal component that cap-
or shaman, whereas higher numbers represent multiple levels of hierarchy (for tures 76% of the variance in the individual complexity characteristic variables.
example, senior priests or high druids). Our approach uses multiple imputation43 to account for missing data, uncertainty
Making inferences about prehistoric rituals requires using various measurable and expert disagreement by imputing data based on a range of possible values
archaeological proxies. Previous research has established that both frequent rituals and averaging the results over the course of 20 imputations. The results of this
and multi-level religious hierarchies tend to co-occur with other features of doctri- approach have proven highly robust to a number of different modelling assump-
nal rituals (for example, low arousal)25,37–39. Not all of these features can always be tions8 (Supplementary Information). Full details of this approach and justifica-
found in the archaeological record, so in this paper we use the appearance of either tions for selecting the social complexity variables can be found in a previously
religious hierarchy or frequent rituals as proxies for the appearance of doctrinal published paper8. We previously carried out a number of robustness checks8,
rituals. Doctrinal rituals were thus coded as present if the most frequent ritual including cross-validation analysis and bootstrap resampling to assess whether
occurred weekly or daily, or if there was evidence of multiple levels of religious our PCA methods were robust to spatio-temporal autocorrelation. Specifically,
hierarchy. k-fold cross-validation showed that our multiple imputation methods accurately
Separate re-analyses were also conducted in which doctrinal rituals were defined predicted complexity characteristic values when each geographical region was sys-
based only on ritual frequency and only on religious hierarchy. In both cases, tematically removed from the analysis, and bootstrapping showed that removing
doctrinal rituals still preceded moralizing gods by an average of over 200 years, different geographical regions and time periods did not affect our PCA results8
although this difference only remained significant when using religious hierarchy (see Supplementary Information for full details).
as a proxy for doctrinal ritual practices (religious hierarchy: mean = 991 years, Extensions of PCA (for example, generalized low-rank models44, spatio-tempo-
t = 2.4, d.f. = 11, P = 0.035; ritual frequency: mean = 210 years, t = 1.1, d.f. = 11, ral PCA45 and singular spectrum analysis46) may be worth considering in future
P = 0.30). analyses as alternative methods of accommodating binary variables and spatio-
Note that we coded only aspects of ritual practices and religion associated with temporal autocorrelation. Note, however, that the subsequent regression analyses
the official cult, and so the rituals of interest were not necessarily polity-wide but performed in this paper explicitly control for spatial, temporal and phylogenetic
could be largely or wholly restricted to elite groups. autocorrelation. More importantly, our current results consistently failed to support
Data collation. The process of data collection for the MHG, BSP and doctrinal the temporal sequence of the moralizing gods hypothesis across all geographical
rituals variables involved matching each fully trained research assistant with one regions (Extended Data Table 1 and Extended Data Fig. 1), and robustness analyses
or more experts (recognized authorities on the polity in question, typically holding using each of the nine complexity characteristics independently without perform-
a relevant doctorate and occupying a faculty position in a university). Experts ing PCA also confirmed our main findings (Extended Data Table 4). This confirms
provided guidance on how to delineate the temporal and geographical boundaries that our primary finding that complex societies precede moralizing gods cannot
of the polity, assembled an initial reading list and—where necessary—helped to be an artefact of autocorrelation in our PCA methods.
interpret some of the key historiographical debates associated with the variables. Logistic regression. To examine the association between moralizing gods and social
Research assistants then populated the variables with data and presented these complexity while controlling for non-independence in our data owing to spatio-
data to the experts for review. The comments and suggestions made by the experts temporal autocorrelation and historical connections between cultures47, we fitted
were then implemented by the research assistants. The next stage required a sec- a logistic regression model to the data. A detailed description of this model
ond team of fully trained research assistants to conduct a series of quality checks, has previously been published along with extensive validation of its robustness
including vetting of the footnotes (which currently reference over 2,000 unique when applied to Seshat data48. This approach stems from the field of nonlinear
sources) and the use of correct syntax for the machine-readable part of the data. dynamical systems, and is similar to the concept of Granger causality49,50 (which
Finally, this checked dataset was offered to the experts for review. By contrast, the is commonly used in economics), in that both use linear models with time-lagged
data required for the social complexity variables frequently consisted of facts that variables.
research assistants could procure with less supervision, allowing expert input to Our approach is similar to a previously published study7, except that we use
occur at a later stage of the process. Data vetting in Seshat is a continuously ongo- more fine-grained measures of geographical diffusion and linguistic similarity,
ing dynamic process that includes incorporation of disagreement among experts and also incorporate temporal information, as follows:
within the project and input from external experts via our open-access interface.
There is room for reasonable disagreement about the most effective way of  δi, j 
Yi, t = a + ∑ bτ Yi, t − τ + c ∑ exp −  Yj, t − 1 + h ∑ wi, jYj, t − 1
gathering data about world history, particularly regarding the role of expert con-  d 
τ i≠ j   i≠ j
tributors40,41. An alternative approach would be to have every single data point
signed off by a single recognized expert, perhaps even without requiring further + ∑ gkXk, i, t − 1 + εi, t
k
citations. We trialed such an expert-driven approach to data entry during initial
phases of our project but found it took too long to source experts and have them in which Yi,t is the binary variable encoding the presence or absence of moralizing
enter the data required. Instead we found that faster progress could be made using gods in location i at time t. The time step Δt = 100 years. Starting from the first
the approach described above and having multiple points at which the data were term on the right-hand side, a is the regression constant (intercept). The next
examined and vetted. To this end, we have made not only all our data but also all term captures the influences of past history (autoregressive terms), with τ = 1,
of the metadata and references supporting these data available for everyone to 2, … indexing time-lagged values of Y (as time is measured in centuries, Yi,t − 1
examine and comment on. Rather than relying on the authority of a single expert refers to the presence or absence of moralizing gods 100 years before t). The third
for each entry, Seshat involves regional experts to help to guide data collection term represents potential effects resulting from geographical diffusion51,52. We use
and assess the quality of our data and metadata as one of several complementary a negative-exponential form to relate the distance between society i and society
components in our quality control approach, which also includes incorporating j (δi,j) to the influence of j on i because—in contrast to a linear kernel—a nega-
disagreement among multiple experts. tive exponential does not become negative at very long δi,j; it instead approaches
Letter RESEARCH

0 smoothly. We avoid the problem of endogeneity by using time-lagged Yj,t − 1. To examine whether our results were affected by the definition of moralizing
Thus, the third term is a weighted average of the occurrence of moralizing gods gods, we reran the analyses limiting the definition of moralizing gods exclusively
in the vicinity of society i at the previous time step, with weights falling off to 0 to MHG, rather than the more inclusive definition of BSP used in the primary
as distance from i increases. Parameter d measures how steeply the influence falls analysis.
with distance, and was set to d = 1,000 km after optimizing the Akaike information Our primary analysis treated moralizing gods as being present from the begin-
criterion value using 200-km increments from 200 to 2,000 km (d = 200, 400, 600, ning of the polity in which they appeared. To ensure that our analyses were not
…, 2,000 km). Parameter c is a regression coefficient measuring the importance affected by dating uncertainty, we reran the analyses randomly resampling to treat
of geographical diffusion. Detecting autocorrelations owing to shared cultural moralizing gods as appearing at some point from within the full date range of this
history (next term) is done analogously, except w now represents the weight due polity (for example, 2900–2700 bc for Egypt).
to linguistic similarity (set to 1 if societies i and j share the same language, 0.5 if Our primary analysis used time windows of up to 2,000 years before and
they are in the same linguistic genus, 0.25 if they are in the same linguistic family, after the appearance of moralizing gods, because 2,000 years was intermediate
and 0 if they are in different linguistic families; linguistic genera and families were between the maximum time window for the region with the shallowest time depth
taken from Glottolog53 and the World Atlas of Language Structures54). The rest of (±700 years for Mali) and the deepest time depth (±3,900 years for Iran). To
the right-hand side represents effects of predictor variables Xk,i,t − 1 (time-lagged); examine whether our results were affected by the depth of the time window used,
gk are regression coefficients and εi,t is the error term. This approach allows us to we reran analyses using consistent time windows for each region of up to 700 years
investigate the effects of the predictor variable (social complexity, calculated above before/after moralizing gods (because ± 700 was the maximum time window pos-
via PCA), while controlling for serial autocorrelations, spatial diffusion and auto- sible for Mali), and also using the full time window available for each region (that
correlations due to the shared cultural history. The regression results are detailed is, as wide as 3,900 years for Iran).
in Extended Data Table 2. To ensure that our results were not affected by possible autocorrelation in our
Comparison of before and after moralizing gods. To more directly examine the use of PCA to extract a measurement of social complexity, we reran the analysis
direction of causality predicted by the moralizing gods hypothesis, we created a nine times using each of the nine individual complexity characteristics as a measure
time series of social complexity over time for all 12 regions for which social com- of social complexity without performing any PCA.
plexity data were available both before and after the appearance of moralizing gods All of these robustness analyses (16 in total) produced qualitatively identical
(Extended Data Fig. 1). We then compared rates of change in social complexity results in which the rate of increase of social complexity was significantly greater
over time before and after moralizing gods using sliding time windows. First, we before the appearance of moralizing gods than afterwards (more than double in all
compared rates of change using a 100-year window (that is, comparing the rate cases; Extended Data Table 4), which confirms that our primary conclusion that
for the 100 years before the appearance of moralizing gods with the rate for the complex societies precede moralizing gods is highly robust.
100 years after), then repeated this using a 200-year window, 300-year window Reporting summary. Further information on research design is available in
and so on, up to a maximum of between 700- and 3,900-year windows depending the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this paper.
on the region (different regions have different time depths of data available for Code availability. Source code is available online at http://github.com/pesavage/
making these comparisons). moralizing-gods.
The region with the shallowest time depth was in Mali (±700 years before/after
the appearance of Islam around 1100 ad (400 ad–1800 ad)), whereas the region Data availability
with the deepest time depth was in Iran (±3,900 years before/after evidence of the The full machine-readable dataset is available as Supplementary Data 1, and at
moralizing Mesopotamian sun god Shamash around 2200 bc (6100 bc–1700 ad)). http://seshatdatabank.info/datasets. Full coding data with detailed explanations
If we used all available data (up to ±3,900 years), we risked weighting the analyses and references are available at http://seshatdatabank.info/data, and are summarized
too heavily towards regions such as Iran with deep time depths, whereas using in Supplementary Table 2. The data include the coded levels of uncertainty and
only a consistent upper limit of a maximum of ±700 years risks throwing away disagreement, the textual explanations and the references for each of the variables
too much data. As a compromise, we conducted analyses using an intermediate for all polities used in our analysis. These webpages also make it possible to com-
upper limit of a maximum of ±2,000 years (Fig. 2b), but also repeated the analy- ment on each of our data points and suggest additions or corrections and thus
ses using extreme upper limits of ±700 years and ±3,900 years (see ‘Robustness provide an up-to-date and dynamic dataset that undergoes continual improvement
analyses’). All of these choices produced qualitatively identical results (Extended by members of the Seshat team and external scholars. To maximize transparency,
Data Table 4). we have tied each cluster of variables to the names of the research assistants who
Note that these analyses do not attempt to construct a single average rate of gathered the data, and to the names of the experts who reviewed the data.
change before moralizing gods, a single average rate of change after moralizing
gods, and compare these average rates. We cannot assume such a constant rate of 31. Swanson, G. E. The Birth of the Gods: The Origin of Primitive Beliefs (Michigan
change—and, indeed, Fig. 2a clearly shows that rates of change are not constant. Univ. Press, Ann Arbor, 1960).
Instead, these analyses calculate a difference value for each time window (for exam- 32. Divale, W. Pre-Coded Variables for the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample Vols 1 and
ple, subtracting the rate of change for the 100-year period before moralizing gods 2 (York College, New York, 2000).
33. Johnson, D. God is Watching You: How the Fear of God Makes Us Human (Oxford
from the rate of change for the 100-year period after, and then doing the same Univ. Press, New York, 2016).
for a ±200-year period, and so on). The key prediction of the moralizing gods 34. Norenzayan, A. Big Gods: How Religion Transformed Cooperation and Conflict
hypothesis is that these difference values should tend to be positive (that is, for a (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, 2013).
given time window, the rate of change after moralizing gods should be greater than 35. Haidt, J. & Graham, J. When morality opposes justice: conservatives have
the rate before). However, Fig. 2b demonstrates that—in fact—the distribution of moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Soc. Justice Res. 20, 98–116
(2007).
difference values was significantly negative (paired t-tests, P < 10−9).
36. Curry, O. S. in The Evolution of Morality (eds Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D.)
Robustness analyses. To explore the robustness of our results to modelling assump- 27–51 (Springer, Cham, 2016).
tions, we ran the following robustness analyses. 37. Whitehouse, H. & Lanman, J. The ties that bind us: ritual, fusion, and
To ensure that the analyses are not affected by the fact that religious hierarchy identification. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 674–695 (2014).
is included as one of the social complexity variables in addition to being one of 38. Whitehouse, H. & Martin, L. H. Theorizing Religions Past: Archaeology, History, and
the variables used to define doctrinal mode, we reran the analyses after removing Cognition (AltaMira, Blue Ridge Summit, 2004).
39. Atkinson, Q. D. & Whitehouse, H. The cultural morphospace of ritual form:
the religious hierarchy variable from the social complexity variables. We chose to examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 50–62
do this for robustness analyses rather than the primary analysis in order to use the (2011).
same 51 social complexity variables used in our previously published studies8,48 40. Slingerland, E. & Sullivan, B. Durkheim with data: the Database of Religious
for consistency. History. J. Am. Acad. Relig. 85, 312–347 (2017).
To ensure that the observed plateauing of social complexity was not simply an 41. Wade, L. Birth of the moralizing gods. Science 349, 918–922 (2015).
42. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing.
artefact of a ceiling effect wherein polities ‘max out’ certain variables, we reran https://www.r-project.org/ (2015).
the analyses twice after splitting the social complexity variables in two subsets. 43. Rubin, D. B. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys (Wiley, New York,
The scale subset contained only the subset of seven social complexity variables 1987).
for which there was no theoretical maximum value (from the categories polity 44. Udell, M., Horn, C., Zadeh, R. & Boyd, S. Generalized low rank models. Found.
population, polity territory, capital population and hierarchy). The non-scale sub- Trends Mach. Learn. 9, 1–118 (2016).
45. Stahlschmidt, S., Härdle, W. K. & Thome, H. An application of principal
set contained the remaining 44 social complexity variables for which there was a
component analysis on multivariate time-stationary spatio-temporal data. Spat.
theoretical maximum that could be attained once all our variables were present Econ. Anal. 10, 160–180 (2015).
in a society (from the categories government, money, infrastructure, information 46. Hassani, H. Singular spectrum analysis: methodology and comparison. J. Data
systems and texts). Sci. 5, 239–257 (2007).
RESEARCH Letter

47. Mace, R. & Pagel, M. The comparative method in anthropology. Curr. Anthropol. 51. Eff, E. A. & Dow, M. M. How to deal with missing data and Galton’s problem in
35, 549–564 (1994). cross-cultural survey research: a primer for R. Struct. Dyn. 3, 1–29 (2009).
48. Turchin, P. Fitting dynamical regression models to Seshat data. Cliodynamics 9, 52. Eff, E. A. & Routon, P. W. Farming and fighting: an empirical analysis of the
25–58 (2018). ecological-evolutionary theory of the incidence of warfare. Struct. Dyn. 5, 1–33
49. Granger, C. W. J. Investigating causal relations by econometric (2012).
models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica 37, 424–438 53. Hammarström, H., Forkel, R., Haspelmath, M. & Nordhoff, S. Glottolog version
(1969). 2.3 http://glottolog.org (2014).
50. Kleinberg, S. Causality, Probability, and Time (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 54. Dryer, M. S. & Haspelmath, M. The World Atlas of Language Structures online
2013). http://wals.info (Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 2013).
Letter RESEARCH

Extended Data Fig. 1 | Social complexity time series for individual bands represent the period in which the first evidence of moralizing gods
regions. The 12 regions for which social complexity data are available both (red) and doctrinal rituals (blue) appeared. Grey shading represents 95%
before and after the appearance of moralizing gods are shown. Vertical confidence intervals based on a PCA using multiple imputation8.
RESEARCH Letter

Extended Data Fig. 2 | Full time series showing mean social complexity All errors represent 95% confidence intervals, with the exception of the
over time before and after the appearance of moralizing gods. n = 12 vertical bar for moralizing gods, which represents the mean duration
regions with data before and after the appearance of moralizing gods. of the polity in which moralizing gods appeared (because times are
Social complexity has been scaled so that the society with the highest normalized to the time of first evidence of moralizing gods, and there is
social complexity (Qing Dynasty, China, around ad 1900) has a value thus no variance in this parameter). Lack of confidence intervals indicates
of 1 and the society with the lowest social complexity (Early Woodland, data from only a single region. This figure is identical to Fig. 2a, except
Illinois, USA, around 400 bc) has a value of 0. Vertical bands represent that it also includes all available data before and after moralizing gods,
the period in which moralizing gods and doctrinal rituals first appeared. rather than being restricted to a window of 2,000 years before and after.
Letter RESEARCH

Extended Data Fig. 3 | Social complexity before and after the USA, around 400 bc) has a value of 0. Vertical bands represent the period
appearance of MHG. This is a version of Fig. 2 in which analyses are in which MHG and doctrinal rituals first appeared. All errors represent
restricted to only MHG, rather than the broader definition of moralizing 95% confidence intervals, with the exception of the vertical bar for MHG,
gods used in Fig. 2 and elsewhere (which includes BSP as well as MHG). which represents the mean duration of the polity in which MHG appeared
a, Time series showing the mean social complexity over time for (because times are normalized to the time of first evidence of MHG
2,000 years before and after the appearance of MHG. n = 10 regions with and there is therefore no variance in this parameter). b, Histogram of
social complexity data for before and after moralizing high gods. Social differences in rates of change in social complexity after minus before
complexity has been scaled so that the society with the highest social the appearance of MHG (n = 158 time windows from the 10 regions).
complexity (Qing Dynasty, China, around ad 1900) has a value of 1 and The y axis represents the number of time windows out of 158.
the society with the lowest social complexity (Early Woodland, Illinois,
RESEARCH Letter

Extended Data Table 1 | Timing and rates of change in social complexity before and after the earliest precolonial evidence of moralizing
gods

For locations without precolonial concepts of moralizing gods, the polity represents the latest polity analysed. See Supplementary Table 2 and http://seshatdatabank.info/data for details and
references. Rates of change before and after moralizing gods were compared using paired t-tests on up to 20 time windows (100–2,000 years before and after the appearance of moralizing gods)
for all 12 regions with social complexity data available both before and after the appearance of moralizing gods. Negative t-values represent higher rates of change before moralizing gods. *P < 0.05;
**P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
Letter RESEARCH

Extended Data Table 2 | Logistic regression results predicting moralizing gods

The model includes parameters for social complexity and for geographical, temporal and cultural relationships, ordered by absolute z-value (see Methods for details). SE, standard error. Pr, probability.
RESEARCH Letter

Extended Data Table 3 | Analyses with doctrinal rituals instead of moralizing gods as the dependent variable

See Supplementary Tables 3–5 for full regression results.


Letter RESEARCH

Extended Data Table 4 | Robustness analyses modifying modelling assumptions of the analyses

See Methods for details, and see Extended Data Table 2 and Supplementary Tables 6–18 for full regression results. NA, not applicable.
RESEARCH Letter

Extended Data Table 5 | List of the 51 social complexity variables analysed

See a previous study8 and http://seshatdatabank.info/methods/codebook/ for full definitions and selection rationale.
nature research | reporting summary
Corresponding author(s): Patrick E. Savage
Last updated by author(s): Feb 5, 2019

Reporting Summary
Nature Research wishes to improve the reproducibility of the work that we publish. This form provides structure for consistency and transparency
in reporting. For further information on Nature Research policies, see Authors & Referees and the Editorial Policy Checklist.

Statistics
For all statistical analyses, confirm that the following items are present in the figure legend, table legend, main text, or Methods section.
n/a Confirmed
The exact sample size (n) for each experimental group/condition, given as a discrete number and unit of measurement
A statement on whether measurements were taken from distinct samples or whether the same sample was measured repeatedly
The statistical test(s) used AND whether they are one- or two-sided
Only common tests should be described solely by name; describe more complex techniques in the Methods section.

A description of all covariates tested


A description of any assumptions or corrections, such as tests of normality and adjustment for multiple comparisons
A full description of the statistical parameters including central tendency (e.g. means) or other basic estimates (e.g. regression coefficient)
AND variation (e.g. standard deviation) or associated estimates of uncertainty (e.g. confidence intervals)

For null hypothesis testing, the test statistic (e.g. F, t, r) with confidence intervals, effect sizes, degrees of freedom and P value noted
Give P values as exact values whenever suitable.

For Bayesian analysis, information on the choice of priors and Markov chain Monte Carlo settings
For hierarchical and complex designs, identification of the appropriate level for tests and full reporting of outcomes
Estimates of effect sizes (e.g. Cohen's d, Pearson's r), indicating how they were calculated
Our web collection on statistics for biologists contains articles on many of the points above.

Software and code


Policy information about availability of computer code
Data collection All data was collected via, and is available open-access at: http://seshatdatabank.info/data/

Data analysis All analyses were performed in R V3.4.2 and R Studio V 1.1.383. Source code is available online at http://github.com/pesavage/
moralizing-gods
For manuscripts utilizing custom algorithms or software that are central to the research but not yet described in published literature, software must be made available to editors/reviewers.
We strongly encourage code deposition in a community repository (e.g. GitHub). See the Nature Research guidelines for submitting code & software for further information.

Data
Policy information about availability of data
All manuscripts must include a data availability statement. This statement should provide the following information, where applicable:
- Accession codes, unique identifiers, or web links for publicly available datasets
- A list of figures that have associated raw data
- A description of any restrictions on data availability

The full machine-readable dataset is available as Supplementary Dataset 1, and at http://seshatdatabank.info/datasets. Full codings with detailed explanations and
October 2018

references are available at http://seshatdatabank.info/data, and are summarized in Extended Data Table 2. The data include the coded levels of uncertainty and
disagreement, the textual explanations, and the references for each of the variables for all polities used in our analysis.

1
nature research | reporting summary
Field-specific reporting
Please select the one below that is the best fit for your research. If you are not sure, read the appropriate sections before making your selection.
Life sciences Behavioural & social sciences Ecological, evolutionary & environmental sciences
For a reference copy of the document with all sections, see nature.com/documents/nr-reporting-summary-flat.pdf

Life sciences study design


All studies must disclose on these points even when the disclosure is negative.
Sample size The global sample of 30 regions was selected and published (Turchin et al., 2015) before beginning data collection. The number of polities per
region depended on the availability of historical and archaeological data, and was determined by the relevant expert historian/
archaeologist(s) for each region.

Data exclusions The time period of interest was limited to between the beginning of the Neolithic to the beginning of Colonial/Industrial periods. No data
falling within these periods was excluded.

Replication No experiments were performed. Our primary results were replicated in all robustness analyses (see Methods).

Randomization No randomization was performed.

Blinding Historical data were coded by research assistants and regional experts who could not be blinded to the society they were coding. However,
they were blind to the specific hypotheses being tested.

Reporting for specific materials, systems and methods


We require information from authors about some types of materials, experimental systems and methods used in many studies. Here, indicate whether each material,
system or method listed is relevant to your study. If you are not sure if a list item applies to your research, read the appropriate section before selecting a response.

Materials & experimental systems Methods


n/a Involved in the study n/a Involved in the study
Antibodies ChIP-seq
Eukaryotic cell lines Flow cytometry
Palaeontology MRI-based neuroimaging
Animals and other organisms
Human research participants
Clinical data

October 2018

Вам также может понравиться