Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Electronic Commerce Research and Applications


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ecra

Which is more important in Internet shopping, perceived price or trust?


Hee-Woong Kim a,⇑, Yunjie Xu b,1, Sumeet Gupta c,2
a
Graduate School of Information, Yonsei University, 134 Sinchon, Seodaemun, Seoul 120-749, Republic of Korea
b
School of Management, Fudan University, 670 Guoshun Road, Shanghai 200433, China
c
Department of Business Administration, Shri Shankaracharya Institute of Technology and Management, Junwani, Bhilai 490 020, India

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Price and trust are considered to be two important factors that influence customer purchasing decisions
Received 12 August 2010 in Internet shopping. This paper examines the relative influence they have on online purchasing decisions
Received in revised form 3 June 2011 for both potential and repeat customers. The knowledge of their relative impacts and changes in their rel-
Accepted 3 June 2011
ative roles over customer transaction experience is useful in developing customized sales strategies to
Available online 24 June 2011
target different groups of customers. The results of this study revealed that perceived trust exerted a
stronger effect than perceived price on purchase intentions for both potential and repeat customers of
Keywords:
an online store. The results also revealed that perceived price exerted a stronger influence on purchase
Internet shopping
Perceived trust
decisions of repeat customers as compared to that of potential customers. Perceived trust exerted a stron-
Perceived price ger influence on purchase decisions of potential customers as compared to that of repeat customers.
Mental accounting theory Ó 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Online purchase decision making

1. Introduction customers to easily compare prices across vendors and find the
cheapest possible alternative.
Product price has long been considered a key predictor of cus- Conversely, because of the physical and temporal distance be-
tomer choice. Price has been regarded as either a monetary sacri- tween buyer and seller in the electronic marketplace, Internet
fice for obtaining a product or a quality signal of a product shopping incurs uncertainty and embodies risk, which arises from
(Lichtenstein et al. 1993, Zeithaml 1988). Most leading product cat- the time lapse between the purchase and the delivery of the prod-
egories in the context of Internet shopping (e.g., tickets, books, and ucts. In the presence of such risk and uncertainty, lack of trust in an
music CDs) involve ‘low touch’ products and ‘no touch’ services Internet vendor has been identified as one of the greatest barriers
(EIAA 2006, Lynch et al. 2001). When products are of a low touch inhibiting Internet transactions with the vendor (Hoffman et al.
nature3 (i.e., search products), product quality remains constant 1999, Pavlou et al. 2007). Many studies (e.g., Gefen et al. 2003,
across vendors (Klein 1998), allowing customers to focus primarily Grazioli and Jarvenpaa 2000, McKnight et al. 2002, Pavlou and
on price minimization (Dodds et al. 1991, Garbarino and Maxwell Gefen 2004) have argued that trust in an online store has become
2010, Von Neumann and Morgenstern 1953, Zeithaml et al. 1988). a key predictor of customer decisions in Internet shopping. Indeed,
The efforts of customers to seek out the vendors offering the best trust is often regarded as one of the most important prerequisites
prices are facilitated in part by Internet shopbots or comparison for the success of e-commerce (Hoffman et al. 1999, Pavlou et al.
websites (e.g., BizRate.com). Electronic markets thus allow 2007) as well as mobile commerce (Luo et al. 2010, Siau and Shen
2003). A popular press article (Vintone 2001) mentioned that trust
is the top factor affecting the success of e-commerce.
Although previous research has delineated the importance of
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +82 10 8976 4410. both price and trust in an online store, the synergy effect of these
E-mail addresses: kimhw@yonsei.ac.kr (H.-W. Kim), yunjiexu@fudan.edu.cn two factors on online purchase decision needs more attention. For
(Y. Xu), sumeetgupta@ssitm.ac.in (S. Gupta).
1 Internet vendors, this is a critical strategy issue, because they need
Tel.: +86 21 2501 1198.
2
Tel.: +91 788 2291621.
to decide whether to compete with others on the basis of price or
3
There are two types of products: search product (i.e., low touch product) and trust. Based on the facts that price has long been considered a key
experience product (i.e., high touch product) (Klein 1998). Experience products are predictor of customer choice and that customers can easily com-
those where there is great variation is product quality such as flowers, jewelry, pare prices among Internet vendors and select cheaper alterna-
luxurious items, and apparels. Search products are those products whose product
tives, Internet vendors could consider developing price-oriented
quality does not vary across stores. For example, books and CDs are of standard
quality and therefore their quality is standard do not vary from one store to another competition strategies. Conversely, based on the argument
store. advocating trust proponents, Internet vendors could consider

1567-4223/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.elerap.2011.06.003
242 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

developing trust-oriented strategies. However, trust building re- derived from a particular commodity or choice alternative. It has
quires additional investments, such as reputation building, service been argued that this approach is of limited explanatory power
quality improvement, and customer satisfaction enhancement (Jar- when applied to customer choice under conditions of uncertainty
venpaa et al. 2000, Kim and Benbasat 2006, Kim et al. 2004, Singh (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). As a result, the prospect theory
and Sirdeshmukh 2000). Compromising between these two ex- (Kahneman and Tversky 1979) was proposed as an alternative ap-
tremes, Internet vendors could try to achieve a balance between proach for explaining choices made by customers under conditions
price and trust by putting different weights on the two factors in of uncertainty.
their strategies. In order to do this, Internet vendors need to know Prospect theory explains human decisions under conditions of
the relative importance of trust and price in the online purchase risk and uncertainty from a value maximization perspective (Kahn-
decision of customers. It would also be useful for Internet vendors eman and Tversky 1979). This theory suggests that people put
to know how trust and price may influence initial sales for new more weight on positive outcomes that are considered certain than
customers and repeat sales for returning customers differently so positive outcomes that are deemed probable. It is this certainty
as to appropriately market to these two customer groups. For effect that causes people to be risk-averse when making decisions
example, (Reibstein 2002) found that price is the most important involving gains, and this effect explains why people tend to prefer
factor for initial online sales, but the least important factor for on- an option with certain but lower benefit over an option with uncer-
line repeat sales. tain but higher benefit. Prospect theory essentially highlights the
Based on the research needs outlined above, this study aims to fact that the certainty effect brings about risk-aversion in choices
examine the relative and differing importance of perceived trust involving certain gains. Indeed, risk aversion is considered one of
(i.e., a set of beliefs about the trustworthiness of an Internet ven- the best-known generalizations about risky choices involving gains
dor) and perceived price (i.e., subjective perception of price at an (Kahneman and Tversky 1979).
Internet vendor in comparison with prices at other vendors) in cus- Using prospect theory as a basis, Thaler (1985) proposed the
tomer decisions in the context of Internet shopping. Specifically, mental accounting theory. Total utility, which is the sum of acqui-
we seek to answer two research questions: (1) How would per- sition utility and transaction utility, represents the perceived total
ceived price and perceived trust affect online purchase intention value derived from purchasing a product. Total utility plays a key
differently for potential and repeat customers at an Internet store? role in predicting customer choice and decision making. High total
(2) Of the two factors, perceived trust and perceived price, which utility leads to a high possibility of customer purchase. Acquisition
factor would exert a stronger effect on online purchase intention? utility is perceived based on a comparison between the equivalent
In this study, potential customers are those who have browsed the value of a product and its objective price. Equivalent value is a
website of an Internet vendor, but have yet to purchase from the measure of the benefit of having a product, as perceived by the cus-
vendor. They may have transaction experience with other Internet tomer. The objective price is the total amount that a customer has
vendors, but not at the focal Internet vendor. Repeat customers are to pay to get the product. Intuitively, acquisition utility is the per-
those who have purchased from the target Internet vendor at least ception of whether the product being purchased is worth its price.
once previously. Prior research has suggested that product quality enhances the
This study contributes to our knowledge on customer decisions equivalent value of a product (e.g., Chang and Wildt 1994, Dodds
in a number of ways. First, it examines the relative importance of et al. 1991, Sweeney et al. 1999, Zeithaml 1988).
perceived trust and perceived price when customers make pur- Transaction utility is perceived based on the difference between
chase decisions with an Internet vendor. Second, it examines the the objective price and the reference price of the product being
changes in the effect of perceived trust and perceived price on cus- purchased. Unlike objective price, the reference price is the price
tomer purchase decisions over customer type (from being a poten- that customers expect to pay for a product (Thaler 1985). Custom-
tial customer to being a repeat customer) with an Internet vendor. ers derive reference prices from previous experience or sales
Third, it offers practical insights for Internet vendors, explaining messages (Puto 1987). Internet shopbots that facilitate price com-
how potential and repeat customers weigh perceived trust and parisons among Internet vendors also help customers derive refer-
perceived price differently when making their purchase decisions. ence prices. Intuitively, transaction utility represents the price
The rest of the paper is organized as follows. In the next section advantage or disadvantage of a deal relative to the reference price.
we present the theoretical background, followed by the research While prior research (e.g., Chang and Wildt 1994, Dodds et al.
model and hypotheses. We then describe the research methodol- 1991, Sweeney et al. 1999, Urbany et al. 1997, Zeithaml 1988)
ogy. After interpreting the empirical results, we discuss the theo- has focused on the monetary aspects of price, the non-monetary
retical and practical implications and conclude with a summary. aspects may be critical (Zeithaml 1988). Examples of non-mone-
tary aspects of price are time and effort (Downs 1961). For Internet
shopping, risk is another important non-monetary aspect (Grewal
2. Theoretical background et al. 2003) because customer deception by Internet vendors is
becoming increasingly common. Risk is conceptualized as the
Internet shopping is characterized by risk and uncertainty for probability of a future loss that reduces the equivalent value of a
customers. Therefore, theories that explain human behavior under product at the time of purchase (Sweeney et al. 1999) .
conditions of risk and uncertainty can shed light on customer
behavior in the context of Internet shopping. Two such theories 2.2. Perceived price and trust
are the prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky 1979) and the
mental accounting theory (Thaler 1985). The impact of the two key factors (i.e., price and trust) on cus-
tomer decisions in the context of Internet shopping can be ac-
2.1. Theoretical foundation counted for using mental accounting theory. Jacobby and Olson
(1997) distinguished between the objective price of a product
Based on the assumption of rational choice and probabilistic (which may include shipping costs in the context of Internet shop-
consideration between options, the expected utility theory (Von ping) and the price encoded by customers. Zeithaml (1982) posited
Neumann and Morgenstern 1953) was developed for decision anal- that customers usually do not remember the objective price of a
ysis. The term, ‘‘utility,’’ in the theory was originally used to de- product. Instead, customers encode prices in ways that are mean-
scribe the overall wealth or consumption, or net satisfaction ingful to them. For example, they compare objective prices
H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252 243

(e.g., price offered by the current vendor) with reference prices utility. Perceived trust may then lead customers to purchasing
(e.g., prices offered by the other vendors) (Dodds et al. 1991, Kahn- behavior through total utility.
eman and Tversky 1979) while shopping on the Internet, and then
encode the outcomes as either higher or lower than their refer- 2.3. Potential customers and repeat customers
ences. Such outcomes drive the price perceptions of customers,
which in turn influence their decisions (Jacoby and Olson 1997). The differences between potential customers and repeat cus-
In this study, perceived price is considered to be the perceived level tomers of a same Internet vendor can be accounted for using pros-
of monetary price for one vendor in comparison with prices of pect theory. Compared to potential customers, repeat customers
other vendors. Thus, based on a comparison between the objective usually perceive a higher level of certainty in a transaction with
and reference price, perceived price has a direct impact on the a vendor because of direct transaction experience with the vendor.
monetary aspect of transaction utility in mental accounting theory. According to prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky 1979), cer-
Perceived price may then affect customer purchase behavior tainty in a transaction with a vendor increases the aversion of
through total utility. losses and the desirability of gains from the transaction. Thus, re-
Trust as a social phenomenon has been studied in various disci- peat customers attempt to achieve more gains (i.e., monetary sav-
plines. In marketing, trust has been defined as a psychological state ing) from a transaction with the same vendor than do potential
comprising intention to accept vulnerability based on positive customers.
expectations of the intention of others (Singh and Sirdeshmukh In contrast, perceptions of uncertainty and risk in Internet
2000), or a willingness to rely on exchange partners (Ganesan transactions with an online store are higher for potential custom-
1994). McKnight et al. (2002) further differentiated trust beliefs ers than for repeat customers. Lambert (1972) reported that cus-
and trust intentions. Trust beliefs are the customer’s perceptions tomers tend to go for high price options when they experience
of a specific Internet vendor’s attributes such as competence, uncertainty in a transaction with a vendor. In particular, when cus-
benevolence, and integrity. Trust intention is the intention to en- tomers do not have enough product quality information, they may
gage in trust-related behaviors with a specific Internet vendor. select high price options by interpreting price as a quality signal
Trust beliefs thus indicate the perception that a specific vendor is (Lichtenstein et al. 1993). In addition, potential customers who
trustworthy. Building on this platform, research in e-commerce perceive high uncertainty and risk may place more importance
has conceptualized and operationalized trust beliefs as a set of be- on gaining control in the transaction, allowing prospects of control
liefs about the credibility, competence, benevolence, and integrity rather than of gains (i.e., monetary saving) to determine their
of Internet vendors (e.g., Bhattacherjee 2002, Cho 2006, Dinev and behavior (Koller 1988), which confirms the risk aversion behavior
Hart 2006, Gefen et al. 2003, Kim and Benbasat 2006, Kim et al. highlighted by prospect theory.
2004, McKnight et al. 2002, Pavlou and Fygenson 2006).
Based on the call for further studies on trust in new IT phenom- 3. Hypotheses
ena, Luo et al. (2010) examined three dimensions of trust based on
the trust topology proposed by McKnight and Chervany (2002), The research model for this study is shown in Fig. 1. The
namely, disposition to trust, structural assurance and trust beliefs. hypotheses are discussed in detail below.
Disposition to trust is a general inclination in which people display With a focus on transactions, value has been conceptualized as
a trusting stance towards and show belief or faith in humanity an assessment of benefit against cost when shopping with an Inter-
(McKnight et al. 2002). Structural assurance is the trust perception net vendor (Sweeney and Soutar 2001, Thaler 1985, Zeithaml
about the institutional environment (McKnight et al. 2002). In the 1988). In accordance with the concept of total utility from the
context of this study, it would refer to the availability of legal and mental accounting theory, we define perceived value from the cus-
technical infrastructure to carry out transaction over the online tomer’s perspective as the net benefit (perceived benefit relative to
store. Trust belief is the perception of the trustworthiness of the perceived cost) from a transaction with an Internet vendor.
vendor and consists of a set of specific beliefs about integrity, Past studies on consumer decisions (e.g., Zeithaml 1988) have
benevolence, and competence (Gefen et al. 2003, McKnight et al. assumed that consumers seek value maximization, based on the
2002). Disposition of trust is important for the formation of initial foundations of prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky 1979)
trust and becomes less important for established trust relation- and mental accounting theory (Thaler 1985). Consumers arguably
ships. Since, last decade the volume of online transaction has prefer conducting transactions with vendors whose products offer
grown and thus increased customers’ trust on online transactions maximal value. This view of consumers is consistent with the ra-
and the platform provided for conducting these online transac- tional perspective. Indeed, empirical results have supported such
tions. Therefore, disposition to trust and structural assurance are a perspective by demonstrating that perceived value leads to pur-
not as important dimensions of trust in the context of this study. chase intention (e.g., Chang and Wildt 1994, Dodds et al. 1991).
Trust beliefs is important dimensions because there are number These results are likely to apply to the context of Internet shopping
of online vendors and therefore unless customer has not conducted for both potential and repeat customers. Hence, we hypothesize:
any transaction with the online vendor, there is an element of per-
ceived risk involved in conducting the transaction. Therefore,
building on previous research in e-commerce, this study adopts Perceived
the same definition of perceived trust as a set of beliefs about Trust
the trustworthiness of an Internet vendor. H3a,b
H2a,b
If an online vendor is perceived as trustworthy (e.g., it is ex-
pected to process online transactions honestly rather than deceiv- Perceived H1a,b Purchase
ing customers), such perceived trust reduces risk perception in the Value Intention
context of Internet shopping (Jarvenpaa et al. 2000, Kim et al. H4a,b
2010). In addition, perceived trust can also lower the non-mone- H5a,b
tary transaction price, such as the time and effort needed by cus- Perceived
tomers to choose Internet vendors (Chiles and McMackin 1996). Price
By reducing the non-monetary price, perceived trust may enhance
the acquisition utility and non-monetary aspects of transaction Fig. 1. Research model.
244 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

H1 (a, b). Perceived value has a positive effect on purchase or external control factors (e.g., cooperation of others) (Ajzen
intention for potential customersa and repeat customersb. 2002). Pavlou and Fygenson (2006) regarded trust belief as a per-
ception of control over the exchange environment in which trans-
As discussed in the previous section, perceived trust may lower
actions occur. Gefen (2004) also posited that trust perception
non-monetary transaction price, which includes factors such as the
caters to the human desire to control the exchange environment.
time and effort needed for customers to select an Internet vendor
Since trust belief affects customer’s perceptions of their ability to
(Chiles and McMackin 1996) and the risk of shopping with the ven-
control the exchange environment, perceived trust is likely to
dor (Jarvenpaa et al. 2000, Kim et al. 2010). For example, when cus-
strongly impact the purchase intention of potential customers.
tomers perceive trust in an Internet vendor, they tend to expend
Conversely, repeat customers of the same Internet vendor have
less effort searching for information about the vendor and less cog-
enough information about the vendor because of their prior direct
nitive effort when carrying out transactions with the vendor. By
transaction experience with the vendor. This reduces their level of
reducing such non-monetary price components, perceived trust
perceived risk in carrying out transactions with the Internet vendor.
in an Internet vendor may enhance acquisition utility and the
Through a learning effect (Mitchell and Prince 1993), such experi-
non-monetary aspect of transaction utility, which in turn raise
ence allows customers to build up a perception that they have some
the perceived value as total utility when shopping with the Inter-
control over the transaction environment. When customers per-
net vendor. Perceived trust may also have a direct effect on pur-
ceive the importance of control less and desire less of the control
chase intention, as previous research (e.g., Chiu et al. 2010, Gefen
in their choice, perceived trust may have a weaker impact on pur-
et al. 2003, e.g., Grazioli and Jarvenpaa 2000, Jarvenpaa et al.
chase intention. Previous research (Gefen et al. 2003, Kim et al.
2000, Lu et al. 2010, Pavlou and Gefen 2004) has reported. In the
2009, McKnight et al. 1998) posited that trust belief is especially crit-
context of Internet shopping, these relationships are likely to apply
ical in determining behavioral intention before actual transactions
to both potential and repeat customers. Hence, we hypothesize:
take place because of the perceived risk of Internet transactions.
Hence, in the context of Internet shopping, we hypothesize:
H2 (a, b). Perceived trust in an Internet vendor has a positive
effect on perceived value for potential customersa and repeat
H6. Perceived trust in an Internet vendor has a stronger positive
customersb.
effect on the purchase intention of potential customers than that of
H3 (a, b). Perceived trust in an Internet vendor has a positive
repeat customers.
effect on purchase intention for potential customersa and repeat
customersb. The magnitude of the impact that perceived price has on pur-
chase intention (H5) may differ between potential and repeat cus-
Price can be seen as a monetary sacrifice for obtaining a product
tomers for the same Internet vendor. With direct transaction
or a signal of product quality (Lichtenstein et al. 1993, Zeithaml
experience, repeat customers tend to perceive a lower level of risk
1988). For Internet shopping, product quality tends to be compara-
and a corresponding higher level of certainty when conducting
ble across vendors, and customers are generally familiar with
transactions with the Internet vendor. According to prospect theory,
product attributes; therefore, in this context, price is often consid-
such transaction certainty may cause customers to be more sensitive
ered to be a monetary sacrifice (Reibstein 2002). As a monetary
to monetary gains from transactions. Given that perceived price
sacrifice, an increase in price for the current vendor in comparison
(determined when customers compare the objective price with
with prices of other vendors lowers acquisition utility when the
the reference price) is a reflection of gains in transactions (Dodds
equivalent value of the product remains constant. Furthermore,
et al. 1991), perceived price should strongly affect the behavior of re-
as was previously discussed, perceived price has a direct impact
peat customers. Therefore, perceived price is likely to strongly im-
on the monetary aspect of transaction utility; an increase in per-
pact the purchase intention of repeat customers.
ceived price lowers transaction utility. Thus, perceived price
Conversely, due to a lack of direct transaction experience, poten-
should negatively affect perceived value through acquisition utility
tial customers of the same Internet vendor may perceive higher lev-
and transaction utility. Perceived price may also have a direct ef-
els of risk and uncertainty when conducting transactions with the
fect on purchase intention. For the same product, a high perceived
vendor. Such uncertainty in transactions may cause customers to
price creates a monetary loss for customers, which should deter
be less sensitive to monetary gains from the transactions. Since per-
customers from wanting to purchase the product (Dodds et al.
ceived price is a reflection of gains in transactions (Dodds et al.
1991, Von Neumann and Morgenstern 1953). In the context of
1991), perceived price may affect the behavior of potential custom-
Internet shopping, these relationships are likely to apply to both
ers to a lesser extent. Price advantage becomes a salient decision
potential and repeat customers. Hence, we hypothesize:
component when the level of certainty in a transaction is high.
Therefore, perceived price may have a weaker impact on the pur-
H4 (a, b). Perceived price has a negative effect on perceived value
chase intention of potential customers compared to repeat custom-
for potential customersa and repeat customersb.
ers of the same online vendor. Hence, in the context of Internet
H5 (a, b). Perceived price has a negative effect on purchase inten- shopping, we hypothesize:
tion for potential customersa and repeat customersb.
H7. Perceived price has a stronger negative effect on the purchase
The magnitude of the impact of perceived trust in an Internet
intention of repeat customers than that of potential customers.
vendor on purchase intention (H3) may differ between potential
and repeat customers for the same vendor. Potential customers The magnitude of the impact of perceived trust on purchase
usually lack information about the target Internet vendor, because intention (H3) may differ from that of perceived price on purchase
they have no direct transaction experience with the vendor. Hence, intention (H5). Given that customers may purchase products only
they perceive a higher risk in carrying out a transaction with the when they expect to gain more benefit than sacrifice, prospect the-
vendor. In this more risky transaction environment, the ability to ory suggests that customers are likely to be risk averse and there-
control becomes important in determining customer behavior fore opt for certain, but smaller, gains rather than probable, but
(Koller 1988). According to the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen larger, gains. Hence, in the context of Internet shopping, customers
2002), the behavioral intentions of people are affected by their per- are likely to focus on maximizing certainty by alleviating risk rather
ceived behavioral control. Perceived behavioral control refers to than maximizing monetary benefit. When making purchases on the
beliefs about the presence of internal (e.g., personal knowledge) Internet, the elements of risk include disappointing purchases
H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252 245

(Ehrlich and Fisher 1982) and future possible losses (e.g., loss of pri- Sirdeshmukh et al. (2002) to fit the online context. Items for per-
vacy and other security threats) (Grewal et al. 2003). With trust ceived trust were adapted from Grazioli and Jarvenpaa (2000) be-
perception, such elements of risk are minimized (Jarvenpaa et al. cause of their suitability to the context of this study. Two
2000) because there is more certainty in the transaction environ- additional items were also included to make the measure more
ment (Gefen 2004, Koller 1988). Conversely, a lower perceived price complete in terms of face validity. Items for perceived price were
may only serve to maximize monetary benefit (Dodds et al. 1991), a adapted from Gefen and Devine (2001). Two more items were
factor that is not the main focus of customers who perceive uncer- added to measure customers’ comparisons of prices at the current
tainty and risk. Thus, perceived trust may be more salient than per- online vendor with the prices of other vendors. All items were an-
ceived price for customers making purchase decisions on the chored on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
Internet. Given that the perceived risk of Internet shopping is likely agree) to 7 (strongly agree).
to be higher for potential customers than for repeat customers, the Three information systems scholars and one marketing scholar
aforementioned arguments are likely to be particularly important reviewed the instrument for face validity. A focus group of ten peo-
for potential customers. ple also reviewed the instrument and provided feedback pertaining
We can also explain the relative importance of perceived trust to the length of the instrument, the clarity of the questions, and the
and perceived price on purchase intention in terms of shopping completeness of coverage of the questions. Some of these members
convenience with consumption costs, especially for repeat custom- had previous Internet shopping experience, while others did not.
ers. Shopping convenience means less time and effort are spent on Table 1 shows the final measurement instrument used for data
shopping (Berry et al. 2002). Trust beliefs enhance shopping conve- collection.
nience by allowing customers to spend less time and effort on
Internet shopping with the vendor (Chiles and McMackin 1996). 4.2. Data collection
In addition, most customers regard time and effort costs as more
important than monetary cost when low-cost standard products Since most of the products sold on the Internet are low touch
(e.g., pencils) are concerned, and monetary cost as more important products or search products (such as books and CDs), an online
than time and effort costs when high-cost products (e.g., a house) bookstore (‘‘I-BOOKS’’) in Korea was chosen to test the hypotheses
are concerned (Downs 1961). Most leading products in Internet presented in this study. This store has approximately 120,000 visits
shopping are low-cost, standard products such as books and CDs to its website every day and sells approximately 1500 books daily.
(Lynch et al. 2001). Furthermore, most online customers are more An online survey was developed and posted on the I-BOOKS web-
interested in shopping convenience and are willing to pay more for site, and anyone who visited the website could respond to the sur-
it (Reichheld and Schefter 2000). Hence, in the context of Internet vey. Such a sample best represents the potential and repeat
shopping, we hypothesize: customers of a real-life Internet bookstore.
A banner was placed on the home page of the I-BOOKS website
H8 (a, b). Perceived trust in an Internet vendor has a stronger linking the respondents to the survey site. The survey ran for
effect than perceived price on the purchase intention of potential 1 week. Customers who visited the website participated in the sur-
customersa and repeat customersb. vey voluntarily by clicking on the banner on the home page. Volun-
tary participation in a survey introduces a self-selection bias
4. Research methodology whereby some distortion of statistics may occur because certain
characteristics that correlate to the respondents’ willingness to par-
Survey methodology was used to test the research hypotheses. ticipate are over-represented. To reduce this tendency, respondents
This methodology was chosen because it enhances the generaliz- were offered five dollars as an incentive to participate in the sur-
ability of the results (Dooley 2001, McGrath 1982). vey. Moreover, the self-selection tendency may only exert a
marginal influence because a willingness to participate does not
4.1. Instrument development influence perceived price or perceived trust.
To further prevent biased answers, the survey page assured the
Measurement items for purchase intention were taken from respondents that there was no right or wrong answer and that
Dodds et al. (1991). Items for perceived value were adapted from their response would be kept confidential. For the purpose of

Table 1
Measurement instrument.

Construct Item Wording Reference


Purchase intention INT1 The probability that I would consider buying a book from this store is high Dodds et al. (1991)
INT2 If I were to buy a book, I would consider buying it from this store
INT3 The likelihood of my purchasing a book from this store is high
INT4 My willingness to buy a book from this store is high
Perceived value VAL1 Considering the money I pay for buying books at this store, Internet shopping here is a good deal Sirdeshmukh et al. (2002)
VAL2 Considering the effort I make in shopping at this store, Internet shopping here is worthwhile
VAL3 Considering the risk involved in shopping at this store, Internet shopping here is of value
VAL4 Overall, Internet shopping at this store delivers me good value
Perceived trust TRS1 This store is capable of doing its job Grazioli and Jarvenpaa (2000)
TRS2 This store keeps its promises and commitments
TRS3 This store cares about its customers
TRS4 This store fulfills its job
TRS5 This store is trustworthy
Perceived price PRC1 Buying books at this store may be more expensive than at another store Gefen and Devine (2001)
PRC2 I will probably save more money buying books at another store than at this store
PRC3 It may be possible to get a better discount from another store than from this store
PRC4 It may be cheaper to buy books at this store than at another store (reverse)
246 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

Table 2 Table 3
Respondent characteristics. Results of principal component analysis.

Demographic variable Potential Repeat Item Potential customers Repeat customers


customers customers
INT1 0.28 0.84 0.22 0.07 0.14 0.15 0.80 0.16
Age (years) Mean (std. dev.) 26.9 (7.8) 28.4 (7.6) INT2 0.33 0.82 0.21 0.01 0.26 0.23 0.84 0.17
Internet experience (years) Mean (std. dev.) 5.8 (2.4) 5.5 (2.3) INT3 0.29 0.88 0.23 0.02 0.25 0.20 0.87 0.15
Purchase experience with Mean (std. dev.) 0 (0) 11.6 (13.8) INT4 0.36 0.82 0.23 0.07 0.28 0.20 0.85 0.15
the bookstore (times)
VAL1 0.23 0.11 0.80 0.23 0.18 0.77 0.23 0.37
Internet shopping Yes 86.3% 100.0% VAL2 0.21 0.21 0.84 0.16 0.23 0.84 0.23 0.15
experience VAL3 0.09 0.24 0.84 0.20 0.25 0.85 0.16 0.09
No 13.7% 0.0% VAL4 0.15 0.23 0.83 0.11 0.24 0.83 0.19 0.21
Gender Female 62.7% 66.6% TRS1 0.86 0.21 0.09 0.06 0.80 0.23 0.27 0.04
Male 37.3% 33.4% TRS2 0.91 0.25 0.15 0.06 0.86 0.18 0.16 0.11
TRS3 0.84 0.30 0.15 0.03 0.86 0.17 0.16 0.10
Number of responses 161 352
TRS4 0.90 0.22 0.19 0.07 0.86 0.21 0.17 0.12
TRS5 0.85 0.31 0.20 0.02 0.85 0.16 0.25 0.11
PRC1 0.03 0.12 0.16 0.70 0.04 0.32 0.04 0.62
classifying a respondent as a potential or a repeat customer, the PRC2 0.03 0.07 0.16 0.88 0.11 0.10 0.21 0.87
survey page asked each respondent to indicate the number of PRC3 0.06 0.08 0.11 0.90 0.12 0.06 0.17 0.86
PRC4 0.12 0.33 0.33 0.47 0.17 0.50 0.19 0.61
transactions he or she had conducted with I-BOOKS. To ensure that
respondents actually had some knowledge of the I-BOOKS website,
the survey page asked them to enter the name and price of a book
they were considering before allowing them to respond to the sur- Table 4
vey questions. Results of Convergent Validity Tests.
Of the total 513 responses (see Table 2), 161 were from poten- Item Potential customers Repeat customers
tial customers and 352 were from repeat customers. The t-test re-
Std. path AVE CR a Std. path AVE CR a
sults revealed no significant difference between potential and
loading loading
repeat customers in terms of age or Internet experience. A
INT1 0.88 0.82 0.95 0.95 0.73 0.77 0.94 0.93
Mann–Whitney test revealed that the gender ratio did not differ
INT2 0.88 0.92
significantly between potential and repeat customers. Thus, the INT3 0.94 0.92
samples of the potential and repeat customers were comparable INT4 0.92 0.93
in terms of basic demographics. VAL1 0.82 0.72 0.88 0.91 0.85 0.76 0.91 0.93
VAL2 0.89 0.89
5. Data analysis and results VAL4 0.83 0.88
TRS1 0.84 0.81 0.95 0.95 0.83 0.75 0.94 0.93
5.1. Instrument validation TRS2 0.94 0.86
TRS3 0.88 0.87
TRS4 0.93 0.89
The data for potential and repeat customers were separately TRS5 0.91 0.88
subjected to Principal Component Analysis with Varimax rotation PRC1 0.60 0.67 0.86 0.82 0.62 0.63 0.83 0.79
(see Table 3). For each dataset, four stable factors (with eigenvalue PRC2 0.91 0.89
greater than 1) emerged. These factors explained 79.3% and 79.0% PRC3 0.91 0.84
of the variance in the data for potential and repeat customers,
respectively. All question loadings on the intended factor exceeded
0.5, except PRC4. Because PRC4 had low loading for its intended
factor for potential customers, and PPC4 loaded on two factors statistically significant for both datasets. The composite reliability
for repeat customers, this question was dropped from further and the Cronbach’s a for all constructs exceeded 0.7 for both data-
analysis. sets. Furthermore, the average variance extracted for all constructs
The constructs were assessed for convergent and discriminant exceeded 0.5 for both datasets. Hence, the convergent validity for
validity via confirmatory factory analysis (CFA) (Anderson and the constructs was established.
Gerbing 1988) using linear structural relations (LISREL). We first Discriminant validity is established if the square root of a con-
checked the unidimensionality of each construct. Following the struct’s AVE is larger than its correlation with any other construct
recommended methodological procedures (Anderson and Gerbing (Fornell and Larcker 1981). As shown in Table 5, the square root of
1988, Gefen et al. 2000), we revised the measurement model by AVE for each construct exceeded the correlation between that con-
dropping, one at a time, measurement items that shared a high de- struct and other constructs. Hence, discriminant validity was
gree of residual variance with other items. One item measuring established.
perceived value, VAL3, was dropped because this question shared
a high degree of residual variance with VAL1 and VAL2 for both 5.2. Common method variance
datasets. After dropping this item, the CFA showed an acceptable
model fit. For potential customers, the results were as follows: Method variance refers to the variance that is attributable to the
the goodness-of-fit index (GFI) = 0.91, the normed fit index measurement method rather than the construct of interest
(NFI) = 0.97, the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) = 0.87, the (Podsakoff et al. 2003). It can result from various sources such as
comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.99 and the root mean square of a common rater, a common measurement context, a common item
approximation (RMSEA) = 0.055. For repeat customers, the results context, or from the characteristics of the items themselves.
were: GFI = 0.92, NFI = 0.97, AGFI = 0.89, CFI = 0.99 and RMSEA = Because the responses for this study were self-reported, it is
0.065. important to test for common method variance. Harman’s single
We then checked the convergent validity. As shown in Table 4, factor test (Podsakoff 1986) and the Bentler and Bonnet test (Ben-
the standardized path loadings for all of the questions were tler and Bonett 1980) are widely used tests in IS research (e.g., Song
H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252 247

Table 5
Correlations between Latent Variables.

Construct Potential customers Repeat customers


Mean (std. dev.) INT VAL TRS PRC Mean (std. dev.) INT VAL TRS PRC
Purchase intention (INT) 4.86 0.91 5.60 0.88
(1.23) (1.12)
Perceived value (VAL) 4.66 0.49 0.85 5.38 0.50 0.87
(1.00) (1.03)
Perceived trust (TRS) 5.47 0.61 0.42 0.90 5.86 0.52 0.48 0.87
(1.01) (1.02)
Perceived price (PRC) 4.12 0.10 0.35 0.03 0.80 3.53 0.39 0.475 0.31 0.77
(1.20) (1.31)

Leading diagonal shows the squared root of AVE of each construct.

Table 6
LISREL v2 Comparisons.

Model Potential customers Repeat customers Reference


v2 df % Improvement in variance v2 df % Improvement in variance Straub et al. (1995) Song and Zahedi (2005)
MM0 4471.33 136 – 12010.84 153 – – –
MM1 1771.96 119 60.3% 4600.38 135 61.6% 48.4% 48.8%
MM2 194.06 113 95.6% 467.74 129 96.1% 67.1% 95.4%

and Zahedi 2005), and thus were used in this study for detecting v2 = 2.17, GFI = 0.94, NFI = 0.98, AGFI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.056 and
common method variance. RMR = 0.04). With good fit indices, the standardized path coeffi-
In Harman’s single factor test, an un-rotated solution of factor cients (see Fig. 2) can be used to test the hypotheses.
analysis was examined to determine the number of factors neces- For potential customers, perceived trust (H2a) and perceived
sary to account for the variance of the variables. The results re- price (H4a) had a significant effect on perceived value, explaining
vealed four factors each, for both potential and repeat customers, 33% of the variance. Perceived trust (H3a) and perceived value
with the largest factor accounting for approximately 45% of the (H1a) had a significant effect on purchase intention, explaining
variance between the independent and criterion variables. 48% of the variance. However, perceived price had an insignificant
Although 45% is on the higher side, it is difficult to conclusively effect on purchase intention. Thus, H5a was not supported. For re-
ascertain that a single factor that accounted for a large percentage peat customers, perceived trust (H2b) and perceived price (H4b)
of variance in the data. Therefore, we use Bentler and Bonnet test had a significant effect on perceived value, explaining 40% of the
(Bentler and Bonett 1980) for further confirmation. In the Bentler variance. Perceived value (H1b), perceived trust (H3b), and per-
and Bonnet test, the v2 values of three estimations were obtained, ceived price (H5b) all had a significant effect on purchase inten-
namely: the null model (MM0) that has no underlying factors; a tion, explaining 43% of the variance.
common-factor measurement model (MM1), in which all items To compare the corresponding path coefficients across both
have one underlying factor; and the measurement model for po- structural models (i.e., to test H6 of trust comparison and H7 of
tential and repeat customers (MM2). ‘‘If, for example, the v2 of an- perceived price comparison), this study used a two-step constraint
other competing model, MM1, is 20% of the v2 of MM0, we can approach (Byrne 1998). First, we used LISREL to create a base mod-
conclude that MM1 explains 80% of the total variation’’ (Straub el with the two hypothesized paths, H3 (the effect of perceived
et al. 1995). The findings of the three measurement models for trust on purchase intention) and H5 (the effect of perceived price
the potential and repeat customers are summarized in Table 6. on purchase intention). With this base model, we jointly estimated
The results show that the variance explanation improved to the two sub-models (one for potential customers and one for re-
60.3% in the case of potential customers and 61.6% for repeat cus- peat customers) using the respective datasets. Second, we imposed
tomers when a single variable was introduced. The explanation an equality constraint for the path being studied in both sub-mod-
was 95.6% for potential customers and 95.4% for repeat customers els. For example, when testing H6, the path from perceived trust to
when the measurement model was tested. Following Straub et al. purchase intention was constrained to have the same value for
(1995), a test of significance for the difference between the Chi- both potential and repeat customers. Likewise, when testing H7,
square values of MM1 and MM2 showed that the fit of MM2 was the path from perceived price to purchase intention was con-
statistically superior to the fit of MM1 (p < 0.001) for both the po- strained to have the same coefficient for both potential and repeat
tential and repeat customers. The results are also comparable to customers. Using this constrained model, two sub-models (one for
those found in Straub et al. (1995) and Song and Zahedi (2005). potential customers and one for repeat customers) were estimated
Thus, the measurement model is better than a single factor model jointly with the respective datasets. If the constrained model had a
or null model and does not support the existence of a common significantly different fit (in terms of 2) than the base model, the
method variance in the data. coefficient of the constrained path would be significantly different
across the two sub-models.
5.3. Hypotheses testing Table 7 shows the results of applying the constrained approach.
When testing H6, the change in the model fit was significant
The hypotheses were tested using the LISREL structural model. (Dv2 = 56.14, p < 0.01). Thus, the coefficient of the path from per-
The structural models showed good indices for both the potential ceived trust to purchase intention was significantly higher for po-
customers (v2 to degree of freedom (Normed v2) = 1.37, tential customers than for repeat customers; H6 was supported.
GFI = 0.91, NFI = 0.97, AGFI = 0.87, RMSEA = 0.05, and Root Mean- When testing H7, the change in the model fit was also significant
square Residual (RMR) = 0.04) and the repeat customers (Normed (Dv2 = 4.86, p < 0.03). Therefore, the coefficient of the path from
248 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

0.63***(P)
Perceived 0.41***(R)
trust

0.42***(P)
0.59***(R)
Perceived 0.39***(P) Purchase
value 0.24*** (R) intention
-0.46***(P)
-0.49***(R) R2 =0.33(P) R2 =0.48(P)
R2 =0.40(R) R2 =0.43(R)
Perceived
price ns(P)
-0.22** (R)

*:p<0.05, **:p<0.01, ***:p<0.001,


ns: insignificant at the 0.05 level, (P) :potential customer group, (R): repeat customer group

Fig. 2. Structural model testing results.

Table 7 price to purchase intention; therefore, H8(a) and H8(b) were


Results of constrained tests across sub-models. supported.
Equality constraint Base model Constrained Change in model fit
imposed model 6. Discussion and implications
v2 df v2 df Dv2 Ddf p-Value
Perceived trust to 334.23 170 390.37 171 56.14 1 p < 0.01
The primary objective of this study was to examine the relative
purchase intention impact of perceived price and perceived trust on online purchase
Relative price to 334.23 170 339.09 171 4.86 1 p < 0.03 decision making. The results of this study show that customers
purchase intention in general (including both potential and repeat customers) tend
to value perceived trust over perceived price when making shop-
ping decisions at an online vendor. This finding can also be com-
pared with previous study (Reibstein 2002). Reibstein (2002)
Table 8
found that price was the most important factor that could attract
Results of constrained test within sub-models.
potential customers to transact with Internet vendors while some
Customer type Base model Constrained Change in model fit trust beliefs building factors were ranked low (e.g., customer ser-
model
vice ranked seventh and privacy policies ranked ninth out of 10
v2 df v2 df Dv2 Ddf p-Value factors). Our study shows that perceived trust is an even more
Potential customers 124.03 84 137.40 85 13.37 1 p < 0.01 important factor than perceived price in affecting customer shop-
Repeat customers 181.91 84 228.32 85 46.40 1 p < 0.01 ping decisions.

6.1. Implications for research

perceived price to purchase intention was significantly higher for The results of this study also revealed that perceived trust influ-
repeat customers than for potential customers; H7 was also ences purchase intention more strongly in case of potential cus-
supported. tomers than in case of repeat customers. This result is consistent
To compare the coefficients of two paths within the same struc- with the certainty effect concept proposed by mental accounting
tural model (i.e., to test H8 of perceived trust and perceived price theory. Because of risk aversion, customers are willing to forgo
comparison), we adopted a two-step constraint approach (Byrne monetary gains for certainty in the transaction. Because potential
1998) for each dataset (i.e., we analyzed the data for potential cus- customers have not previously conducted transactions with the
tomers and that for repeat customers separately). First, we created online store, uncertainty about the success of the transaction pre-
a base model using LISREL with the two hypothesized paths, H3 vails; therefore, customers prefer to conduct transactions with on-
(the effect of perceived trust on purchase intention) and H5 (the ef- line stores that are considered trustworthy (even if these online
fect of perceived price on purchase intention), and we estimated stores charge premium prices). Koller (1988) also argues that in
the two paths using the relevant dataset. Second, we imposed an uncertain environment, customers want to have some control
equality constraint for the two paths to be compared (i.e., the path over a transaction. The trust in an online store enables potential
from perceived trust to purchase intention and the path from per- customers to perceive some amount of control over their transac-
ceived price to purchase intention for both customer types). If the tions. Conversely, repeat customers having conducted transactions
constrained model had a significantly different fit (in terms of v2) with the online store, feel more in control of the environment.
than the base model, then the coefficients of the two paths would Therefore, the influence of trust on the purchase intention of repeat
be significantly different. For this comparison, the values for per- customers is not as strong as on the purchase intention of potential
ceived price were reversed because perceived price had a negative customers.
relationship with purchase intention, whereas trust had a positive Regarding the relative impact of perceived price, our results re-
relationship with purchase intention. vealed that its influence on purchase intention is stronger for re-
Table 8 shows the results. For potential customers, the change peat customers than for potential customers. These results are
in model fit was significant (Dv2 = 13.37, p < 0.01). For repeat cus- intuitive and conform to the certainty effect of mental accounting
tomers, the change in model fit was also significant (Dv2 = 46.40, theory. As a customer gains control over the transaction, his desire
p < 0.01). Thus, for both potential and repeat customers, the coeffi- for monetary gains in a transaction increases. However, a number
cient of the path from perceived trust to purchase intention was of studies (e.g., Reibstein 2002, Reichheld and Schefter 2000)
significantly higher than the coefficient of the path from perceived report that repeat customers become less price-sensitive when
H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252 249

purchasing products from an Internet vendor as the number of they are concerned about undesirable consequences arising from
transactions made with that vendor increases. To test this further, the purchase of unsatisfactory products. In our study, potential
a post hoc analysis was conducted taking customers’ transaction customers might have been certain about product quality (i.e.,
experiences (i.e., the number of transactions conducted with the books), but they would have been uncertain about vendor quality
same online vendor) as a moderator of the relationship between because they did not have prior transaction experience with the
perceived price and purchase intention for repeat customers. Internet vendor. For this reason, perceived price might have no sig-
Fig. 3 shows the model for examining moderating effect of transac- nificant effect on purchase intention for the potential customers in
tion experience. our study. Had it been a flower shop or a shop selling luxurious
The results revealed that transaction experience significantly goods, the results might have been different because the quality
moderates the relationship (DR2 = 0.013, F = 5.92, p < 0.05). The of flowers or luxurious goods varies from store to store. We also
coefficients are: perceived price (coefficient = 0.51, p < 0.000), find support for this finding in previous studies.
perceived price  transaction experience (coefficient = 0.28, p < 0
.01), and transaction experience (coefficient = 0.03, p > 0.1). These 6.2. Implications for practice
results imply that price sensitivity in a transaction decreases as the
number of transactions conducted by repeat customers with the The results of this study are of importance to online vendors, as
same vendor increases. they affirm the earlier suggestion that trust is one of the most
The hypothesis testing results, however, showed that repeat important drivers of Internet transactions (Hoffman et al. 1999).
customers are overall more sensitive to perceived price in their Although the lower search costs in the electronic marketplace en-
purchase decision making than potential customers. The certainty able customers to compare prices across vendors so as to find the
effect of mental accounting theory can partially explain these re- cheapest possible alternative, the role of perceptions of trust in
sults; the certainty in a transaction with a vendor increases the facilitating Internet transactions cannot be underestimated. There-
desirability of gains from the transaction. This perception of cer- fore, it is definitely worthwhile for Internet vendors to invest in
tainty should cause repeat customers to value gains in transac- enhancing their trustworthiness, as perceived by their potential
tions. Such gains are often derived in the form of lower perceived and repeat customers. Amazon.com uses recommender agents
price (Dodds et al. 1991). The certainty effect thus increases cus- and customer ratings to improve customer trust in the transac-
tomer sensitivity to monetary savings in the case of repeat custom- tions, in addition to the products being purchased from its store.
ers (particularly those who have conducted less number of The results of this study are also important because they sug-
transactions with the vendor). gest that online stores can charge premium prices from their cus-
Contrary to the expected results, our results show that the influ- tomers if they improve their trustworthiness (Kim and Xu 2007);
ence of perceived price on purchase intention is not significant in this explains how Amazon.com is successful in charging premium
the case of potential customers. Although mental accounting the- prices. Trustworthiness depends on how an online vendor estab-
ory favors trust over price in the case of potential customers, it is lishes itself as a brand amongst customers in general, in addition
unlikely that the relationship between perceived price and pur- to its customer service (including delivery, returns and handing
chase intention would not be significant. There could be a number complaints). For example, Barnes&Noble already had an estab-
of reasons for this. The online bookstore examined in this study lished brand image before it started selling through Internet. In
was not a very well known vendor as compared to Amazon.com other words, it is worthwhile to establish one’s brand image for
or Barnes&Noble.com. In such a case, customers rely on perceived increasing one’s trustworthiness. To establish brand image, online
trust and worry less about price. Even if the vendor is well known, vendors can open small retail outlets in various parts of the coun-
price may not be important because of the nature of the product try. However, this could be an expensive strategy. Another way
examined (books, in this case) is such that there is little variation brand image can be improved is by advertising on standard chan-
in price. Therefore, trust becomes very important. The lack of sig- nels like Television and Newspaper. Online vendors can also tie up
nificant direct effect of perceived price on purchase intention that with other established companies and provide discount coupons as
was previously noted is similar to what was discovered in a prior a free gift on purchase of products of these established companies.
study (Urbany et al. 1997). Urbany et al. (1997) found that transac- While enhancing trustworthiness is a generally useful strategy,
tion utility (akin to perceived price in this study) has no significant Internet vendors may also want to adjust their strategy according
effect on purchase intention when customers are uncertain about to the number of transactions that a customer has conducted (po-
product quality. When customers do not have enough information tential customers versus repeat customers). This is also important
about product quality, they interpret price as a quality signal for improving the retention of repeat customers. Online vendors
(Lichtenstein et al. 1993, Zeithaml 1988). Lambert (1972) also should, for these cases, group practice customization strategies.
reported that customers tend to go for high-priced options when Currently, the general practice is to give freebies or services for
points accumulated by customers through previous transactions.
This is a standard strategy used by most airlines, whereby they dif-
Perceived ferentiate services provided to their customers depending upon the
Trust
number of points that a customer has accumulated through previ-
ous travels. As repeat customers become less price-sensitive with
an increased number of transactions, they can be offered premium
Perceived Purchase
Value Intention services such as free delivery for larger or more expensive pur-
chases. Because it is difficult to differentiate between less experi-
enced and more experienced repeat customers based on price,
additional accessories like gift wrapping or book wrapping services
Perceived
Price can be bundled and targeted to more experienced customers. This
0.51*** would not only enhance trustworthiness, but also improve the loy-
alty of the customers towards the online store.
Transaction
Experience Perceived trust influences the purchase decisions of potential
customers more strongly than those of repeat customers. There-
Fig. 3. Moderating effect of transaction experience. fore, to generate initial sales with potential customers Internet
250 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

vendors may focus on efforts to raise their trustworthiness rather transactional and relational customers. The relationship marketing
than engaging in cut-throat competition with their competitors. literature (e.g., Reichheld and Schefter 2000) suggests that rela-
Generally, online vendors tend to attract sales based on lower tional customers may be less sensitive to price because they are
prices. However, for relatively new online stores, this could be a willing to pay more to conduct their business with familiar ven-
death knell because of the risk involved in online transactions. dors; however, this may not be the case with transactional custom-
Therefore, online vendors should put effort on building their brand ers. It would be interesting to examine how the results for the
by tying-up with established vendors and offering discount coupon repeat customers in this study change when transactional and rela-
for their store as free gifts. They may also tie-up with schools tional repeat customers are examined separately.
which provide textbooks to the students. With such tie-ups schools The post hoc analysis conducted in this study to find the differ-
can offer discount coupon as free gifts for specific purchases that ence in customers’ purchase decisions based on the number of
students can benefit from. Perceived price influences the decisions transactions conducted with the online vendor was based on
of less experienced repeat customers more strongly than it does for cross-sectional data. Given the inherent difficulties in creating a
potential customers and more experienced repeat customers. panel and collecting data for unknown online customers, this study
Therefore, to generate continuous sales, especially with less expe- relied on cross-sectional data. Survey research is better for estab-
rienced repeat customers, Internet vendors may offer lower prices lishing generalizability and the researcher has to forgo some
(in the form of bundled products or freebies because directly lower amount of realism, as argued by Dennis and Valacich (2001). To
prices may signal discrimination between more experienced and further establish the validity of these results and obtain accuracy
less experienced repeat customers) for their products and improve (Dennis and Valacich 2001), a panel can be developed and data col-
their trustworthiness. lected using a longitudinal design. Future studies could adopt dif-
For example, Amazon.com emphasizes its reliable product ferent research methodology, such as experiments, depending on
delivery system (where customers can track their product orders the research objectives because each method has its pros and cons
online), and its product return policy attracts potential customers. (McGrath 1982). For example, a survey approach is advantageous
It also offers product price discounts to repeat customers, giving in generalizing the findings, although it has several methodological
them incentives to continue to do business with Amazon.com. limitations such as a self-selection bias and common method bias.
Given the general importance of perceived trust over perceived In contrast, the experiment approach is advantageous in imparting
price in the context of Internet commerce, successful and trustwor- control of the experimental context to the experimenter, although
thy Internet vendors may charge premium prices, especially during it is disadvantageous in reflecting real situations and generalizing
initial sales with potential customers. Smith and Brynjolfsson’s the findings. A combination of different research methods could
(2001) reported that the three most reputable Internet bookstores, provide interesting implications.
Amazon.com, Borders.com, and Barnes&Noble.com, command a This study focused on the relative impact of perceived price and
$1.72 price premium over other Internet bookstores. Leveraging perceived trust; therefore, other factors were not considered. Other
trustworthiness to charge a higher price is a possible strategy that factors, such as convenience and pleasure, also influence repeat
can help Internet vendors improve their profitability (Ba and Pav- customers’ purchase decisions (Gupta and Kim 2010). Convenience
lou 2002, Kim and Xu 2007). This is an option with a potentially and pleasure are difficult to compare between potential and repeat
multifold impact. McKinsey & Company found that, keeping the customers because potential customers do not have any previous
sales volume constant, a 1% increase in price produces an average purchase experience with the online vendor. An interesting future
increase in profitability of 7.4%. study could examine how convenience and pleasure, along with
price and trust, vary as the number of transactions increases.
6.3. Limitations and implications for future research This study considered trust and not risk. Risk and trust are com-
plementary in the sense that if risk is high, then trust is low and if
The results of this study must be interpreted in the context of its trust is high, then risk is low. Even if risk is not present in a trans-
limitations. First, the data for this study was collected from the po- action (such as in the case of repeat customers), trust is still re-
tential and repeat customers of an online bookstore whose prod- quired to conduct transactions.
ucts belong to a ‘low touch’ or search product category. Since the As this study was a cross-sectional study, it did not examine the
quality of books and their prices vary little across vendors, per- actual purchases of potential customers because behavior comes
ceived price does not emerge as a very important factor for poten- after perception and intention to behave in temporal dimension.
tial customers. EIAA (2006), however, reports that seven of the top The potential customers could return to the website and make pur-
ten products sold online belong to a search product category. Out chases. Future studies could examine the actual purchases made by
of these seven products, four – cinema tickets, CDs, DVDs, and the potential customers and repeat customers using a longitudinal
books – have similar characteristics (i.e., low/medium price, stan- study and examine the effects of price and trust over their pur-
dard product, and no touch services required). Therefore, the find- chase decision.
ings in this study can be applied to other popular Internet shopping Since the data was collected from Korean customers, culture
contexts. However, the results may vary if the product chosen is a may influence the results of this study. In collectivist cultures that
‘high touch’ or experience product, such as luxurious items, jew- Korea people are more risk averse than individualistic cultures as
elry, flowers and so on where product quality is highly variable. well as more adventurous, such as that in America. Future research
It would be interesting to test the proposed hypotheses and com- may address the influence of culture on the results of this study.
pare them across search product categories and experience prod- Finally, a study could be conducted for a service-oriented store,
uct categories, as well as across various online vendors. such as sites that sell online tickets. Online tickets can be
Furthermore, the data in this study was collected through self- purchased through an agent (also an online website) or directly
selection, which may introduce bias. To reduce this bias, respon- through the online store of the firm. Generally, these agents offer
dents were provided with incentives to participate in the survey. some price rebates but charge higher transaction fees thus
Moreover, the self-selection tendency may not have affected this exceeding the normal ticket cost. A study could compare how price
study, because a willingness to participate in the survey would and trust dynamics work when a person has the option of purchas-
not have any effect on perceived price or perceived trust. ing from an online vendor as well as an online agent of the same
Third, this study classified customers into either potential or re- vendor. For example, flight tickets can be purchased both from
peat customers. Repeat customers could be further classified into the website of the flight company and also from an online agent.
H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252 251

The dynamics of price and trust could be interesting in this case be- Garbarino, E., and Maxwell, S. Consumer response to norm-breaking pricing events
in e-commerce. Journal of Business Research, 69, 9–10, 2010, 1066–1072.
cause product quality does not play a role.
Gefen, D. What makes an ERP implementation relationship worthwhile: linking
trust mechanisms and ERP usefulness. Journal of Management Information
Systems, 21, 1, 2004, 263–288.
7. Conclusion
Gefen, D., and Devine, P. Customer loyalty to an online store: The meaning of online
service quality. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Second Annual International
This study investigated the independent and combined impact Conference on Information Systems, New Orleans, LA, USA, 613-5, 2001.
Gefen, D., Karahanna, E., and Straub, D. W. Trust and TAM in online shopping: an
of perceived trust and perceived price on the purchase intention
integrated model. MIS Quarterly, 27, 1, 2003, 51–90.
of online customers. Building on previous research, it determined Gefen, D., Straub, D., and Boudreau, M. Structural equation modeling techniques and
the significance of perceived trust over perceived price as a key fac- regression: guidelines for research practice. Communications of AIS, 4, 7, 2000,
tor affecting customer decisions in the context of Internet shop- 2–76.
Grazioli, S., and Jarvenpaa, S. L. Perils of internet fraud: an empirical investigation of
ping, a finding contrary to what has been reported for traditional deception and trust with experienced internet consumers. IEEE Transactions on
shopping (Von Neumann and Morgenstern 1953) and Internet Systems, Man and Cybernatics-Part A: Systems and Humans, 30, 4, 2000, 395–410.
commerce (Reibstein 2002). Furthermore, this study illustrates Grewal, D., Iyer, G. R., Krishnan, R., and Sharman, A. The internet and the price-
value-loyalty chain. Journal of Business Research, 56, 5, 2003, 391–398.
the differential importance of perceived trust and perceived price Gupta, S., and Kim, H. W. Value-driven internet shopping: the mental accounting
in affecting the decisions of potential and repeat customers at an theory perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 27, 1, 2010, 13–35.
Internet vendor. Based on the prospect and mental accounting the- Hoffman, D. L., Novak, T. P., and Peralta, M. Building consumer trust online.
Communications of the ACM, 42, 4, 1999, 80–85.
ories, the results of this study help to advance our knowledge of Jacoby, J., and Olson, J. C. Consumer response to price: An attitudinal, information
customer decisions in the context of Internet shopping. In particu- processing perspective. In Y. Wind and M. Greenberg (eds.), Moving Ahead with
lar, the findings of this study should equip Internet vendors with Attitude Research, American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, 1997.
Jarvenpaa, S. L., Tractinsky, N., and Vitale, M. Consumer trust in an internet store.
an evidential basis for developing effective and customized strate-
Information Technology and Management, 1, 12, 2000, 45–71.
gies to make initial and repeat sales. Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk.
As the value of Internet commerce continues to grow (Pavlou Econometrica, 47, 2, 1979, 263–292.
Kim, C., Tao, W., Shin, N., and Kim, K.-S. An empirical study of customers’
et al. 2007, Siau and Shen 2003) competition among Internet ven-
perceptions of security and trust in e-payment systems. Electronic Commerce
dors also becomes intense. As Internet vendors strive to attract Research and Applications, 9, 1, 2010, 84–95.
increasingly sophisticated customers in their efforts to increase Kim, D., and Benbasat, I. The effects of trust-assuring arguments on consumer trust
(or at least maintain) their market share, it is imperative that these in internet stores: application of Toulmin’s model of argumentation. Information
Systems Research, 17, 3, 2006, 286–300.
vendors understand the decision calculus of customers. Research Kim, D. J., Ferrin, D. L., and Rao, H. R. Trust and satisfaction, two stepping stones for
along the direction of this study serves to further such a cause. successful e-commerce relationships: a longitudinal exploration. Information
Systems Research, 20, 2, 2009, 237–257.
Kim, H. W., and Xu, Y. Drivers of price premium in electronic market.
References Communications of the ACM, 50, 11, 2007, 91–95.
Kim, H. W., Xu, Y., and Koh, J. A comparison of online trust building factors between
Ajzen, I. Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory potential customers and repeat customers. Journal of the Association for
of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1, 2002, 1–20. Information Systems, 5, 10, 2004, 392–420.
Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. Structural equation modeling in practice: a Klein, G. A. Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA,
review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 3, 1998.
1988, 411–423 (Fall). Koller, M. Risk as a determinant of trust. Basic-and-Applied-Social-Psychology, 9, 4,
Ba, S. L., and Pavlou, P. A. Evidence of the effect of trust building technology in 1988, 265–276.
electronic markets: price premiums and buyer behavior. MIS Quarterly, 26, 3, Lambert, Z. V. Price and choice behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 1, 1972,
2002, 243–268. 35–40.
Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis Lichtenstein, D. R., Ridgway, N. M., and Netemeyer, R. G. Price perceptions and
of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 3, 1980, 588–606. consumer shopping behavior: a field study. Journal of Marketing Research, 30, 2,
Berry, L. L., Seiders, K., and Grewal, D. Understanding service convenience. Journal of 1993, 234–245.
Marketing, 66, 3, 2002, 1–17. Lu, Y., Zhao, L., and Wang, B. From virtual community members to C2C e-commerce
Bhattacherjee, A. Individual trust in online firms: scale development and initial test. buyers: trust in virtual communities and its effect on consumers’ purchase
Journal of Management Information Systems, 19, 3, 2002, 211–241. intention. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 9, 4, 2010, 346–
Byrne, M. B. Structural Equation Modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic 360.
Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Luo, X., Li, H., Zhang, J., and Shim, J. P. Examining multi-dimensional trust and multi-
Mahwah, NJ, 1998. faceted risk in initial acceptance of emerging technologies: an empirical study
Chang, T. Z., and Wildt, A. R. Price, product information and purchase intention: an of mobile banking services. Decision Support Systems, 49, 2, 2010, 222–234.
empirical study. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 22, 1, 1994, 6–27. Lynch, P. D., Kent, R. J., and Srinivasan, S. S. The global internet shopper: evidence
Chiles, T. H., and McMackin, J. F. Integrating variable risk preferences, trust, and from shopping tasks in twelve countries. Journal of Advertising Research, 41, 3,
transaction cost economics. Academy of Management Review, 21, 1, 1996, 73–99. 2001, 15–23.
Chiu, C. M., Huang, H. Y., and Hui, Y. C. Antecedents of trust in online auctions. McGrath, J. E. Dilemmatics: The study of research choices and dilemmas. In J. E.
Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 9, 2, 2010, 148–159. McGrath (ed.), Judgment Calls in Research, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, 1982, 69–80.
Cho, J. The mechanism of trust and distrust formation and their relational outcomes. McKnight, D. H., and Chervany, N. L. What trust means in e-commerce customer
Journal of Retailing, 82, 1, 2006, 25–35. relationships: an interdisciplinary conceptual typology. International Journal of
Dennis, A. R., and Valacich, J. S. Conducting experimental research in information Electronic Commerce, 6, 2, 2002, 35–59.
systems. Communications of the AIS, 7, 5, 2001, 1–41. McKnight, D. H., Choudhury, V., and Kacmar, C. Developing and validating trust
Dinev, T., and Hart, P. An extended privacy calculus model for e-commerce measures for e-commerce: an integrative typology. Information Systems
transactions. Information Systems Research, 17, 1, 2006, 61–80. Research, 13, 3, 2002, 334–359.
Dodds, W. B., Monroe, K. B., and Grewal, D. The effects of price, brand and store McKnight, D. H., Cummings, L. L., and Chervany, N. L. Initial trust formation in new
information on buyers’ product evaluations. Journal of Marketing Research, 28, 3, organizational relationships. Academy of Management Review, 23, 3, 1998, 472–
1991, 307–319. 490.
Dooley, D. Social Research Methods. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. Mitchell, V. W., and Prince, G. S. Retailing to experienced and inexperienced
Downs, A. A theory of consumer efficiency. Journal of Retailing, 37, 1, 1961, consumers. Journal of Retail and Distribution Management, 21, 5, 1993, 10–22.
6–12. Pavlou, P. A., and Fygenson, M. Understanding and predicting electronic commerce
Ehrlich, I., and Fisher, L. The demand for advertising: a theoretical and empirical adoption: an extension of the theory of planned behavior. MIS Quarterly, 30, 1,
investigation. American Economic Review, 72, 3, 1982, 366–388. 2006, 115–143.
EIAA. UK leads the way in online shopping. European Interactive Advertising Pavlou, P. A., and Gefen, D. Building effective online marketplaces with institution-
Association 2006. Available at http://www.eiaa.net/news/eiaa-articles- based trust. Information Systems Research, 15, 1, 2004, 37–59.
details.asp?lang=1&id=85, Accessed On December 20, 2010. Pavlou, P. A., Liang, H. G., and Xue, Y. J. Understanding and mitigating uncertainty in
Fornell, C. R., and Larcker, D. F. Evaluating structural equation models with online exchange relationships: a principal-agent perspective. MIS Quarterly, 31,
unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 1, 2007, 105–136.
18, 1, 1981, 39–50. Podsakoff, M. P., MacKenzie, B. S., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. Common method
Ganesan, S. Determinants of long-term orientation in buyer seller relationships. biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and
Journal of Marketing, 58, 2, 1994, 1–19. recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 5, 2003, 879–890.
252 H.-W. Kim et al. / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 11 (2012) 241–252

Podsakoff, P. M. Self-reports in organizational research: problems and prospects. Sweeney, J. C., and Soutar, G. N. Consumer perceived value: the development of a
Journal of Management, 12, 4, 1986, 531–544. multiple item scale. Journal of Retailing, 77, 2, 2001, 203–220.
Puto, C. P. The framing of buying decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 3, 1987, Sweeney, J. C., Soutar, G. N., and Johnson, L. W. The role of perceived risk in the
301–315. quality–value relationship: a study in a retail environment. Journal of Retailing,
Reibstein, D. J. What attracts customers to online stores and what keeps them 75, 1, 1999, 77–105.
coming back? Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 4, 2002, 465–473. Thaler, R. Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4, 3, 1985,
Reichheld, F. F., and Schefter, P. E-loyalty: your secret weapon on the web. Harvard 199–214.
Business Review, 78, 4, 2000, 105–113. Urbany, J. E., Bearden, W. O., Kaicker, A., and Borrero, M. S. Transaction utility effects
Siau, K., and Shen, Z. X. Building customer trust in mobile commerce. when quality is uncertain. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 24, 1,
Communications of the ACM, 46, 4, 2003, 91–94. 1997, 45–55.
Singh, J., and Sirdeshmukh, D. Agency and trust mechanisms in consumer Vintone, M. Trust and quality are key to thriving on the web today. Marketing, 14,
satisfaction and loyalty judgments. Journal of the Academy of Marketing 2001, 12.
Science, 28, 1, 2000, 150–167. Von Neumann, J., and Morgenstern, O. Theory of Games and Economic Behavior, 3rd
Sirdeshmukh, D., Singh, S., and Sabol, B. Consumer trust, value, and loyalty in edition. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA, 1953.
relational exchange. Journal of Marketing, 66, 1, 2002, 15–37. Zeithaml, V. A. Consumer response to in-store price information environments.
Smith, M., and Brynjolfsson, E. Consumer decision-making at an internet shopbot: Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 4, 1982, 357–369.
brand still maters. Journal of Industrial Economics, 49, 4, 2001, 541–558. Zeithaml, V. A. Consumer perceptions of price, quality, and value: a means-end
Song, J., and Zahedi, M. F. A theoretical approach to web design in e-commerce: a model and synthesis of evidence. Journal of Marketing, 52, 3, 1988, 2–22.
belief reinforcement model. Management Science, 51, 8, 2005, 1219–1235. Zeithaml, V. A., Varadarajan, P., and Zeithaml, C. P. The contingency approach: its
Straub, D., Limayem, M., and Karahanna-Evaristo, E. Measuring system usage: foundations and relevance to theory building and research in marketing.
implications for IS theory testing. Management Science, 41, 8, 1995, 1328–1342. European Journal of Marketing, 22, 7, 1988, 37–64.

Вам также может понравиться