Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 18

European Journal of Social Psychology, Vol.

2 4 , 4 5 6 2 ( 1 994)

Negative affect and social judgment: the


differential impact of anger and sadness

GALEN V BODENHAUSEN
Michigan State University
LORI A. SHEPPARD
Michigan Stare University
and
GEOFFREY P KRAMER
Indiana University - Kokomo

Abstract
The overwhelming majority of research on affect and social information processing
has focused on the judgments and memories of people in good or bad moods rather
than examining more specific kinds of emotional experience within the broad categories
ofpositive and negative affect. Are all varieties of negative affect alike in their impact
on social perception? Three experiments were conducted to examine the possibility
that different kin& of negative affect (in this case, anger and sadness) can have very
different kinds of effects on social information processing. Experiment I showed that
angry subjects rendered more stereotypic judgments in a social perception task than
did sad subjects, who did not difler from neutral mood subjects. E.uperiments 2 and
3 similarly revealed a greater reliance upon heuristic cues in a persuasion situation
among angry subjects. Specrfically. their level of agreement with unpopular positions
was guided more by the credibility of the person advocating the position. TheseJindings
are discussed in terms of the impact of emotional experience on social information-
processing strategies.

INTRODUCTION

Recent research addressing the impact of affect on social perception and memory
has tended to focus on the effects of global mood rather than more specific kinds
Addressee for correspondence: Galen V Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Michigan State Univer-
sity, 129 Psychology Research Building, East Lansing, MI 48824-1 117, U.S.A.
This article was wntten while the first author was a Visiting Professor at the University of Heidelberg
and at ZUMA, Mannheim, Germany; the support of these institutions is gratefully acknowledged. Thanks
are extended to Herbert Bless, Gerd Bohner, Michael Conway, Norbert Schwarz, Bob Sinclair, Fritz
Strack, and Michaela Wanke for helpful suggestions concerning this research.

00462772/94/010045-18$14.00 Received 27 August 1992


0 1994 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 1 March 1993
46 G. V Bodenhausen et al.

of emotional experiences. The assumption inherent in this approach is that the overall
valence of affective experience is the variable of overriding importance in determining
the effects of affect on cognition. Presumably, bad moods are expected to have simiiar
effects, regardless of whether they are characterized primarily by sadness, anger,
anxiety, guilt, or some other negative emotion. The term ‘mood’ is used by some
theorists to refer to diffuse affective states often having antecedents or referents
that are unclear to the person experiencing the mood. Emotion terms such as ‘anger’
or ‘sadness’ have more focus and presumably arise from appraisals of specific actual
or contemplated states of the world (e.g. Ortony, Clore and Collins, 1988; Smith
and Lazarus, 1990). To obtain a richer understanding of the impact of affect on
cognition, it may ultimately prove to be necessary for researchers to look beyond
the global mood and examine the effects of more discrete kinds of emotional exper-
ience (cf Eagly and Chaiken, 1993). The research to be reported examined this
possibility in the realm of negative affect by comparing the effects of anger and
sadness on social perception.
There are several reasons to suspect that anger and sadness may have different
kinds of effects on cognitive processes. The two types of negative affect show marked
differences in their physiological manifestations, and they appear to be mediated
by different elements of the limbic system. Anger, which primarily involves activity
in the amygdala, is associated with an increase in pulse, blood pressure, and secretion
of epinephrine; sadness, which primarily involves activation in the hippocampus,
does not produce comparable physiological effects (see Henry, 1986). These differ-
ences are reflected in subjective experience of bodily states, in that angry people
report experiencing symptoms of arousal (e.g. perceptions of greater cardiac activity,
a sense of restlessness, etc.) while sad people do not (Shields, 1984). To the extent
that the physiological concomitants of emotional expenence play any direct or indir-
ect role in producing the effects of affect on cognition, anger and sadness may have
very different kinds of impact (for evidence linking physiological states to patterns
of social judgment and memory, see Bodenhausen (1990) Clark (1982) and Wilder
(1993)).
The literature on the effects of affect on social judgments has emphasized cognitive
and motivational theoretical accounts rather than ones rooted more directly in phy-
siological mechanisms such as arousal or other neuroendocrine phenomena. It is
interesting to consider whether the logic of these accounts would imply any differences
in the effects of anger versus sadness on social perception. Two theoretical issues
will be considered. First we will consider accounts for the mood congruency effect
in judgment, then we will turn to research linking affective states to differences
in information-processing strategies.

MOOD CONGRUENCY PROCESSES

The mood congruency effect refers to the tendency for people to render judgments
that are biased in the direction of a prevailing mood state. Theoretical accounts
of this effect emphasize the role of affect-laden concepts within the structure of
associative memory (e.g. Bower, 1991, Isen, 1987). According to these models, affect
‘primes’ or activates concepts in memory that are associated (semantically or episodi-
cally) with the affective state. These concepts may cue the retrieval from memory
Negative afecl and socialjudgment 41

of specific information relevant to the judgment, or they may be used in the interpre-
tation of ambiguous evidence relevant to the judgment. The net impact of these
processes is the generation of ajudgment that is biased in the direction of the valence
of the prevailing mood (see Bower, 1991, Forgas and Moyian, 1987). If one assumes
that negative concepts are interassociated in semantic memory, it becomes difficult
to generate a compelling theoretical rationale for systematic differences between
the effects of anger and sadness within these models of the mood congruency effect.
Both anger and sadness would be expected to activate negative concepts, and both
should therefore produce a tendency toward more negative judgments, relative to
the judgments of those experiencing neutral or positive emotional states.
A different explanation for the mood congruency effect has been offered by Schwarz
and Clore (1983, 1988). They propose that such effects may have little to do with
memory processes, but instead may arise more directly from the use of mood as
informational input in the judgment process. When asked to make an evaluative
Judgment, people may ask themselves, ‘How do I feel about it?’ and use their mood
as one obvious gauge, provided that they interpret their momentary feelings as being
at least partly a reaction to the object to be evaluated. Thus, according to this
view, a misattribution process occurs in which people mistake their current mood
(whatever its actual origins) for a reaction to a to-be-judged stimulus. When subtly
reminded that their current mood is in fact.based on something other than a reaction
to the object ofjudgment, people no longer show a mood congruency effect (Schwarz
and Clore, 1983). Thus, this mood-as-information effect may be limited to more
diffuse kinds of affective experience. Even if the anger or sadness being experienced
is sufficiently diffuse, the subjective reaction is negative in either case and should
translate into a more negative appraisal of the target to be judged.

AFFECT AND COGNITIVE STRATEGIES

Another theoretical approach to the affect-cognition interface has emphasized the


role of mood or emotion in the generation of cognitive strategies for processing
evidence relevant to a social judgment (Forgas, 1989; Isen and Means, 1983; Kuhl,
1983; Schwarz, 1990). Although there are undoubtedly many different ways of classi-
fying or identifying the information-processing strategies of the social perceiver (Q
Forgas, 1992a,b), most attention has focused on the distinction between thorough,
systematic, detail-oriented processing on one hand and cursory, heuristic, and global
processing on the other (see Chaiken, Liberman and Eagly, 1989; Fiske and Neuberg,
1990; Petty and Cacioppo, 1986). Within this theoretical approach, it has been
hypothesized that different emotional states may predispose social perceivers to be
more or less systematic in their information-processing strategies. Much of the
research in this vein has focused on happiness, which has been associated with more
heuristic styles of thinking. Happy people appear to rely more on source cues and
less on systematic assessment of message quality in persuasion situations (Mackie
and Worth, 1989, 1991, Schwarz, Bless and Bohner, 1991, Worth and Mackie, 1987),
to rely on stereotypes to a greater extent in determining the validity of allegations
of guilt (Bodenhausen, 1993; Bodenhausen, Kramer and Siisser, in press), and to
48 G. V. Bodenhausen et al.

approach problem-solving tasks in a generally more heuristic fashion (Schwarz and


Bless, 1991; for a comprehensive review, see Sinclair and Mark, 1992). These findings
have been interpreted as showing that happy people have less motivation for systema-
tic thought (in the absence of personal consequences) or that they have less cognitive
capacity for such thought (see Schwarz et at., 1991). But what about negative
affect? A growing body of research has addressed the question of how sadness IS
related to information processing. In contrast to happy people, sad or mildly
depressed people seem to use more systematic, detail-oriented strategies in social
perception. For instance, they are more sensitive to covariation information (Weary,
1990), they show less halo bias in performance appraisals (Sinclair, 1988), and they
appear to engage in a more thoughtful cost-benefit analysis when deciding whether
to help someone in need (Schaller and Cialdini, 1990). Schwarz (1990) has provided
an intriguing interpretation of this phenomenon. He argued that sadness, because
it is associated with problematic life circumstances, is likely to trigger cognitive strate-
gies that are most likely to afford effective problem solving. By thinking more systema-
tically and thoroughly about the social environment, sad people stand the best chance
of finding solutions for their life problems. It may also be that sad people prefer
to become cognitively absorbed in information-processing tasks as a means of dis-
tracting themselves from thoughts about the source of their unhappiness (cJ Wenzlaff,
Wegner and Roper, 1988).
While the extant empirical literature clearly suggests marked differences in the
processing strategies of sad and happy people, it is largely silent on the issue of
anger and information-processing strategies. Intuitively, it seems unlikely that being
very angry will be associated with thoughtful, systematic information processing.
On the contrary, angry people seem prone to impulsive, ill-considered judgment
and action (Kuhl, 1983). In certain legal contexts, we say a person was ‘blind’ with
rage and therefore unlikely to have engaged in thoughtful (albeit malicious) premedi-
tation of a crime. If adaptational or evolutionary pressures have produced a tendency
for sad people to think more systematically in the interest of remediating their prob-
lems, perhaps people have developed a tendency to react quickly and heuristically
when angry for similarly adaptive reasons. Anger typically arises in agonistic contexts,
which often require a quick response that can be invoked without extensive contem-
plation of the various alternative courses of action (cf. Scott, 1980). Unlike sadness,
anger is likely to be associated with more immediate threat or insult. I t may also
be that when one is angry, it is difficult to concentrate on other matters. This, too,
may have its adaptive advantages. Like fear (which bears many neuroendocrine
similarities to anger, but not to sadness), anger seems to be disruptive to coordinated
cerebral activity (Hebb, 1946). If so, angry people may have a reduced capacity
for systematic thinking. These considerations lead to a predicted difference in the
Judgments of sad versus angry people: whereas sad people seem to process social
information more systematically, angry people may react more impulsively and less
deliberately
In sum, two different predictions seem reasonable on Q prrori grounds concerning
the comparative effects of anger and sadness on social judgments. From the stand-
point of research and theory on mood congruency effects, there is little basis for
Negative aflect and socral judgment 49

expecting any differences between the two affective states’. From the standpoint
of research and theory on emotion and social information-processing strategies, how-
ever, it was expected that angry and sad subjects may differ in their tendency to
rely on global, heuristic strategies for generating a quick response requiring less
effort and fewer cognitive resources. Specifically,angry people may show this prefer-
ence for heuristic strategies, while sad people may be prone to be detail-oriented
and more thorough in their processing. To test these issues empirically, three exper-
iments were conducted in which sad, angry, and neutral mood subjects were asked
to render evaluative judgments based on a set of presented information. In addition
to the specific, relatively detailed information that was relevant to the judgment,
subjects were also provided with simple heuristic cues that could provide a basis
for responding to the judgment task. Of focal concern were two questions: (1) will
the judgments of both angry and sad people tend to be evaluatively more negative
than those of neutral mood subjects? and (2) will the judgments of angry subjects
reflect greater use of simple response heuristics, relative to both sad and neutral
mood subjects?

EXPERIMENT 1

Stereotypes can be viewed as judgmental heuristics that are relied upon by social
perceivers whenever they lack the ability or the inclination to think more extensively
about the unique personal qualities of outgroup members (Bodenhausen, 1988; 1990;
Bodenhausen and Lichtenstein, 1987; Bodenhausen and Wyer, 1985; Chaiken et
al., 1989). Therefore, if a given emotional state is likely to engender heuristic styles
of thinking, this should include stereotypic thinking. In a series of four expenments,
Bodenhausen et al. (in press) found that happiness had precisely this effect: people
who had been made happy prior to engaging in a social judgment task rendered
more stereotypic judgments. Thus, the paradigm they employed seemed especially
useful in examining possible differences in processing strategies between angry and
sad people.
In the first experiment, subjects were induced to feel either angry, sad, or a neutral
mood. Then, as part of an ostensibly unrelated experiment, they were asked to read
about a case of alleged misconduct on the part of a fellow student and to make
some Judgments about the case. For approximately half of the cases, the accused
student was identified as a member of a social group that is stereotypically associated
with the type of offence alleged in the case. Otherwise, the case evidence was identical.
It was expected that angry subjects (but not sad ones) would show greater use of
the stereotype in making judgments about the case.

’ It should be noted that Bower (1980) did propose that discrete emotional states may each be associated
with different subsets of information in memory. That is, there IS no reason to assume that all negatively
valenced infomation is interassociated. However, even though different negative emotions may be associ-
ated with different cognitive content, such content is still likely to be generally negative in tone. Hence,
It would be difficult for ‘pnming’ modefs of mood effects, especially as they have been developed in
the social cognition literature, to account for differential impact of different negative emotions without
adding new assumptions to these frameworks.
50 G. V Bodenhausenetal.

Method

Subjecfs and design


One hundred and thirty-five subjects participated in groups of approximately eight.
All subjects were introductory psychology students who participated in fulfilment
of a course requirement. Each subject was randomly assigned to one of six conditions
defined by a 3 (affect: angry, sad, or neutral) X 2 (stereotype: present versus absent)
between-subjects factorial design.

Procedure and materials


Mood induction Subjects were recruited for a study ostensibly concerned with mood
and memory Upon arrival at the laboratory, they were greeted by an experimenter
who explained that, because the mood study was so short, they would also be doing
an unrelated experiment ‘if the experimenter shows up’ The first experimenter then
explained the basic nature of the mood study, which involved reminiscing and writing
about life events. In angry conditions, subjects were asked to vividly recall an episode
that had made them feel very angry, and to describe in detail how the event occurred.
In the sad conditions, the same instructions were used, except that the to-be-recalled
event was sad rather than angry These affect induction procedures were adapted
from those used successfully by Strack, Schwarz and Gschneidinger (1985). In the
neutral mood condition, subjects were simply asked to recall and describe in detail
the mundane events of the previous day’. In each case, subjects were given 12
minutes to complete the task, which they worked on in private cubicles. Upon com-
pletion of the memory task, the experimenter collected their forms and left them
in the charge of a second experimenter who had arrived in the meantime.
Social perception task The second experimenter (who distributed a second consent
form) described his or her study as being concerned with the issue of legal socializa-
tion, particularly the issue of how college students react to the misbehaviour of
their peers. The actual social perception task was the same one used previously
by Bodenhausen (1990, Experiment 2). In this task, subjects are asked to take the
role of a member of a peer judicial review board, to read a case of alleged misconduct,
and to render judgments about the case. Each case consisted of approximately five
to six sentences of specific evidence, mixed in its overall implications. Two cases
were constructed, one involving assault and one involving cheating on an examin-
ation. For half of the assault cases, the student defendant was given an obviously
Hispanic name (‘Juan Garcia’), and for the remainder he was given a non-Hispanic
name (‘John Gamer’). For half of the cheating cases, the student defendant’s name
was followed by the phrase, ‘a well known track-and-field athlete on campus’, whereas
in the remainder, this phrase was omitted. Previous research within the same subject
population demonstrated that male Hispanics were stereotypically viewed as aggress-
ive, while student athletes were seen as being prone to cheating in their academic
work (Bodenhausen, 1990).
Dependent measures Immediately prior to reading the case, subjects were asked

*The neutral/control group Subjects in this experiment were the same as those in Bodenhausen ef a/.
(in press, Experiment 1). Data from these subjects were collected during the same experimental sessions
(and from the same Subject population) as the angry and sad conditions.
Negative affect and socialjudgment 51

to fill out a short demographic/psychological profile ostensibly being used to docu-


ment sample characteristics. In addition to standard demographic items, the question-
naire contained a series of self-ratings in which respondents were asked to rate how
well each of several adjectives described them at the moment. Each rating was made
by circling a number on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). Embedded
within numerous fillers were affect terms (‘sad’ and ‘irritated’) intended to gauge
the effectiveness of the mood induction. The term ‘irritated’ was chosen instead
of ‘angry’ because in pilot work, ‘angry’ proved to be too reactive a term, producing
floor effects in self-ratings.
Immediately after reading the case they had been given, subjects were asked to
fill out a brief questionnaire outlining their reactions to the case. Of foremost interest
was their rating of the likelihood of the student defendant’s guilt. This rating was
made on an 1 l-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all likely) to 10 (extremely likely).
After completion of a probe for suspicions, subjects were debriefed and dismissed’

Results and discussion

Manipulation check
Effectiveness of the mood manipulation was assessed by a series of planned compari-
sons in which self-ratings of sadness and irritation were compared across the angry,
sad and neutral conditions. As expected, subjects in the angry condition rated them-
selves as being more irritated than did subjects in the neutral control condition
(Ms = 1.98 versus 1.33, p < 0.05), but sad subjects did not differ from the control
condition on this rating. Conversely, subjects In the sad condition rated themselves
as significantly more sad than did subjects in the neutral control condition (Ms
= 2.48 versus 1.62, p < 0.005), but angry subjects did not differ from the neutral
condition on this rating. Thus, the manipulations were successful in inducing the
intended, differentially negative affective states.

Perceived guilt
Because the results for the two cases (i.e. assault and cheating) were comparable
(case did not interact with any of the independent variables), the data were collapsed
across this replication factor. Mean ratings of defendant guilt are depicted in Figure
1 as a function of subjects’ affective state and whether or not a stereotype had
been activated. It is clear from an inspection of the figure that stereotype activation
had no discernible impact on subjects’ guilt judgments when they were in either
a sad or a neutral mood (Fs < l), but there was a marked increase in the perceived
guilt of stereotyped targets on the part of angry subjects, F(1,38) = 9 . 2 1 , ~< 0.005.
The interaction of stereotype activation and affect condition was reliable, 42,129)
= 3.83,~ < 0.025.
This pattern of guilt judgments is noteworthy in several respects. First, it provides
empirical corroboration for the idea that anger is associated with more heuristic
information processing. Angry subjects clearly made greater use of their stereotypic
None of the Subjects demonstrated any insight into expenmental hypotheses during the probe for suspi-
cions, although one subject did chide the researchers for failing to consider the possible contaminating
effects of the ‘first’ experiment on the ‘second’ one.
52 G. V. Bodenhausen et al.

Stereotype
7s 1
Present
Absent
b~ 6.5 -
.fi
.r

2 6-

'i
4.5
ANGRY SAD NEUTRAL
-

Affect Condition
Figure 1. Mean judgments of guilt as a function of affect condition and stereotype activation

beliefs than did sad subjects. Thus, it is evident that different kinds of negative
affect can indeed have different kinds of effects on social information processing.
Interestingly, the angry subjects in the first experiment displayed exactly the same
pattern of judgments as those of happy subjects who considered identical cases in
a previous set of experiments (Bodenhausen et al., in press). That happiness and
anger can have similar effects on social judgment while sadness and anger have
quite different effects underscores the importance of looking beyond affect valence
per se to the particular effects that are likely to accompany specific kinds of emotional
experience.
These findings are also interesting in light of traditional views linking prejudice
and stereotyping to emotional experience. While negative affect has often been postu-
lated to be associated with increases in prejudice and stereotyping (e.g. Dollard,
Miller, Doob, Mowrer and Sears, 1939; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Rosen-
blatt, Veeder, Kirkland and Lyon, 1990; Stephan and Stephan, 1985), the available
empirical evidence has focused almost exclusively on anger/frustration and anxiety
The data from Experiment 1 show that anger is associated with greater use of heuristic
cues in judgment, while Baron, Inman, Kao and Logan (1992) have shown that
anxious people use more heuristic strategies for social information processing. Based
on the literature reviewed earlier, it appears that sad people, in contrast, seem to
prefer more systematic strategies for social judgment. Correspondingly, we found
no evidence of increased use of stereotypes in social judgment on the part of sad
people, nor have previous studies of which we are aware.
On the other hand, Esses, Haddock and Zanna (in press) have reported a study
in which people were made sad (via the Velton procedure or via sad music) and
then asked to list characteristics they consider to be descriptive of typical members
of various social groups. These characteristics were then rated in terms of their
Negative aflect and social Judgment 53

valence (positive or negative) and the proportion of people in the group possessing
the Characteristic. A composite stereotype score was computed for each group for
each subject by multiplying the proportion and valence ratings for each Characteristic,
summing this product across all characteristics listed by the subject for the group,
and then dividing this sum by the number of characteristics listed for the group.
For some of the social groups being described, it was the case that those in a negative
mood had more negative stereotype composite scores than those in a neutral mood
condition. This pattern was interpreted as reflecting an increased use of negative
categories in the interpretation of the evaluative meaning of specific characteristics.
Such a process would be directly in line with theoretical accounts for the mood
congruency effect outlined earlier. Note, however, that subjects were engaging in
a free association task in which they listed and interpreted the attributes of social
groups. Under these conditions we mght expect the sort of ‘mood priming’ described
by mood congruency researchers to be most evident, as the task involves a fairly
unconstrained search of associative memory (for similar conclusions, see Fiedler
(1991) and Forgas (1992~)).In the present study, however, we found no evidence
that sad people show a mood congruency effect in judgments of specific members
of outgroups. In our task, negative affect may primarily have its effects via its impact
on processing strategies, whereas in the task used by Esses et ~ l . its, effects may
be mediated by mood-based priming of negative concepts in memory (for a systematic
review of the different processes through which mood affects social judgment, see
Forgas, 1992a,b)).

EXPERIMENT 2

If anger leads to an increased reliance on heuristic cues in social judgment, but


sadness leads to more systematic processing, then differences in the effects of anger
and sadness should be evident not only in the use of stereotypes in perceptions
of guilt but also in other social perception situations and with other kinds of heuristic
cues. Experiments 2 and 3 were undertaken to determine whether the findings of
the first experimentwould generalize to persuasion situations. Reactions to persuasive
communications represent a particularly apt arena for investigating the impact of
affect on social information processing because the dominant theoretical perspectives
in contemporary persuasion research strongly emphasize the distinction between
responses based on simple, relatively superficial cues and responses based on a
thoughtful analysis of the content of a persuasive appeal (Chaiken, 1980, 1987; Petty
and Cacioppo, 1986). According to these theoretical approaches, reliance on superli-
cia1cues will be greater when motivation or ability to engage in more effortful process-
ing of the message is constrained. To the extent that affective states affect processing
motivation or capacity as implied by recent theories of affect and cognition, the
tendency to use heuristic cues in a persuasive situation should be related to one’s
momentary affective state. Previous research has already documented a tendency
for happy people to be less affected by variations in argument quality and more
affected by simple cues indicating the probable validity of the communication (Mackie
and Worth, 1989, 1991, Worth and Mackie, 1987) and sad people have been shown
to be more attuned to the quality of presented arguments (Bless, Bohner, Schwarz
and Strack, 1990), in line with our theorizing. However, it is still unclear what kind
54 G. V. Bodenhausen et al.

of use sad people make of heuristic cues, and the effects of anger on reactions to
persuasive appeals have been largely unexplored. The present research was intended
to provide a direct comparison of the effects of heuristic cues on angry versus sad
recipients of persuasive messages.
Since the pioneering research of Hovland and Weiss (1951), it has been repeatedly
documented that under many circumstances, people agree more with a source who
is reputedly high in credibility, compared to a less credible source who advances
identical arguments (for reviews, see Eagly and Chaiken, 1993; Hass, 1981, McGuire,
1985). The two principal elements of source credibility are expertise (e.g. Kelman
and Hovland, 1953) and trustworthiness (e.g. Walster, Aronson and Abrahams,
1966). In Experiment 2, we examined the impact of variations in the expertise of
a communicator on the reactions of sad, angry, and neutral mood subjects to a
persuasive appeal, while in Experiment 3, we examined the impact of variations
in communicator trustworthiness.

Method

Subjects and design


Eighty-three subjects participated in groups of approximately eight. All subjects
were introductory psychology student? who participated in fulfilment of a course
requirement. Each subject was randomly assigned to one of six conditions defined
by a 3 (affect: angry, sad, or neutral) x 2 (source expertise: high versus low) between-
subjects factorial design.

Procedure and materials


Affect induction The same basic procedure successfully used to induce affective
states in Experiment 1 was used again for Experiment 2. Under the guise of two
short, unrelated studies, Subjects first completed a 12-minute reminiscence task
designed to create feelings of anger, sadness, or no mood in particular, after which
the persuasion task commenced.
Persuasion task The ‘second’ experiment was described to participants as a study
of college students’ opinions on a range of issues important to them. Specifically,
they were told that the researchers were interested in getting their reactions to various
essays outlining possible new social policies. In reality, everyone was given an essay
on the same topic, namely raising the legal driving age from 16 to 18. The message
consisted of six arguments for the advocated position, presented on the same page
in the form of a coherent essay The essay was attributed to one of two possible
sources. Under high expertise conditions, the message was said to have been produced
by ‘a group of transportation policy experts at Princeton University’ Under low
expertise conditions, the essay was attributed to ‘a group of students at Sinclair
Community College in New Jersey’. These two sources had been pretested and found
to differ substantially in their perceived expertise in this topic domain. After perusing
the essay, subjects were asked to report the extent of their agreement with the advo-
cated position on an ll-point scale (0 = strongly disagree, 10 = strongly agree).
Then they were asked to complete a thought-listing measure in which they were
asked to retrospectively report any and all thoughts that occurred to them as they
Negative affect andsocialjudgment 55

read the persuasive appeal. After listing their thoughts, participants then coded them
in terms of whether they were favourable, unfavourable, or neutral with respect
to the advocated position. Finally, Subjects were probed for suspicions and provided
with an educational debriefing.

Results and discussion


Based on the results of our stereotyping study, we expected that the reactions of
angry subjects to a persuasive appeal would be strongly influenced by the presence
of a simple source cue, in this case expertise, while the reactions of sad people
would not be appreciably affected by this cue. Data relevant to this prediction are
provided in Table 1, which shows subjects’ mean agreement with the advocated
position as a function of their emotional state and the communicator’s level of exper-
tise. In an analysis of variance (ANOVA) on agreement scores, the overall interaction
of source expertise and affective state was statistically reliable, F(2,77) = 4.09,
p < 0.025. The simple effects of the expertise cue within each affect condition are
of particular interest. While neutral mood subjects showed a trend to agree more
with the expert source, this difference did not prove to be statistically significant.
Angry subjects, however, showed a strong and significant tendency to agree with
the high-expertise source to a greater extent. Interestingly, sad subjects actually
showed a trend toward agreeing more with the low expertise source, although this
reversal did not prove to be statistically significant.

Table 1. Mean agreement with advocated position as a function of affect condition and source
expertise (Expenment 1)
Source expertise
Affect condition Low High Difference P
Angry 2.00 4.67 2.67 0.01
Sad 3.86 2.69 -- 1 . 1 7 0.15
Neutral 2.36 3.79 I .43 0.20

Overall, the data in Table 1 indicate that subjects showed little inclination to
agree with the advocated position, and thought-listing data revealed a preponderance
of counterarguing. At least two-thirds of all listed thoughts were negative in all
conditions except for the angry-expert condition, in which only 54 per cent of the
thoughts were negative, on average. However, there were no significant effects in
an ANOVA on thought-listing measures.
The results obtained in the second experiment nicely corroborate the implications
of the first study using a completely different social information-processing task
and a very different kind of heuristic cue. Anger appears to have very different
effects from sadness, in this case promoting the greater use of a simple heuristic
cue. Certainly there is no general mood congruency pattern evident among the judg-
ments of angry or sad subjects. One intriguing aspect of the data was the apparent
trend for sad subjects to use the expertise cue in a reversed fashion. That is, they
appeared to agree more with the low expertise source. However, this finding did
not reach conventional levels of significance. In order to determine whether this
56 G. V Bodenhausen et al.

is a phenomenon worth speculating about, a third study was undertaken to examine


the generality of the findings reported in Experiment 2.

EXPERIMENT 3

In the final experiment, we again examined the use of source cues in a persuasion
situation among angry, sad, and neural mood subjects. However, this time we manipu-
lated the source’s trustworthiness. When communicators advocate a policy that serves
their own interests and agendas, they are perceived as less trustworthy and often
have less persuasive impact than when they advocate a position that is unexpected
or at odds with their own interests (e.g. Eagly, Wood, and Chaiken, 1978; Walster
et al. 1966). The apparent trustworthiness of the communicator provides another
basis for a quick, heuristic response to a persuasive appeal that does not necessitate
thoughtful processing of message content. In the third experiment we once again
induced angry, sad, or neutral moods among recipients of persuasive appeals, and
systematically manipulated the apparent trustworthiness of the communicator by
varying the extent to which the advocated position served the communicator’svested
interests.

Method

Subjects and design


Ninety-one subjects participated in groups of approximately eight. All subjects were
introductory psychology students who participated in fulfilment of a course require-
ment. Each subject was randomly assigned to one of six conditions defined by a
3 (affect: angry, sad, or neutral) x 2 (source trustworthiness: high versus low)
between-subjectsfactorial design.

Procedure and materials


Aflect induction The same method used to induce affective states in the previous
experiments was used again.
Persuasion task The ‘second’ experiment was again described to participants as
a study of college students’ opinions on a range of issues important to them. In
this study, everyone was given an essay on the banning of meat in University residence
hall dining rooms at breakfast and lunch. The message consisted of five arguments
for the advocated position, presented on the same page in the form of a coherent
essay. The essay was attributed to one of two possible sources. Under high trust-
worthiness conditions, the message was said to have been produced by ‘the Student
Government League, which actively promotes the interests and welfare of all college
students’. Under low trustworthiness conditions, the essay was attributed to ‘the
Student Vegetarian League, which actively promotes vegetarianism and animal
rights’ In pretesting, these two sources were found to differ substantially in their
perceived trustworthiness for this topic. After reading the essays, subjects were asked
to report the extent of their agreement with the advocated position on an 11-point
scale (0 = strongly disagree, 10 = strongly agree). Then they were asked to complete
Negative affect and socrafJudgment 57

a thought-listing measure in which they were asked to report any and all thoughts
that occurred to them as they read the persuasive appeal. After listing their thoughts,
participants then coded them in terms of whether they were favourable, unfavourable,
or neutral with respect to the advocated position. Finally, subjects were probed
for suspicions and provided with an educational debriefing.

Results and discussion


Mean ratings of agreement with the advocated position are presented in Table 2
as a function of subjects’ affective state and the communicator’s level of trustworthi-
ness. As in Experiment 2, neutral mood subjects showed only a nonsignificant trend
toward greater agreement with the high credibility source. Angry subjects, however,
showed a marked and statistically reliable pattern of agreeing with the trustworthy
source to a greater extent than the source low in trustworthiness. And once again,
sad subjects showed a tendency toward the reversed pattern. Although the difference
between sad subjects’ levels of agreement with the high versus low trustworthy sources
was not significant, it is clear that those in a sad mood did disagree significantly
more with the trustworthy source compared to those in an angry or neutral mood.
Overall, the interaction of affect condition and source trustworthiness was nearly
significant, F(2,85) = 2.82, p < 0.06.

Table 2. Mean agreement with advocated position as a function of affect condition and source
trustworthiness(Experiment 3)
Source trustworthiness
Affect condition Low High Difference P
Angry 1.72 3.94 2.22 0.04
Sad 2.42 1.47 -0.95 0.25
Neutral 2.40 3.93 1.53 0.13

As in Experiment 2, thought-listing measures were not significantly affected by


the experimental manipulations, and they revealed a high proportion of negative
thoughts in all conditions (M = 0.75). Clearly, subjects did not find the presented
arguments for banning meat from residence hall dining rooms particularly compell-
ing. But when these arguments were presented by an apparently trustworthy source,
it was only the sad subjects who failed to show elevations in agreement levels. Instead,
they showed the very lowest levels of agreement under this condition.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Researchers have recently made great strides toward increasing our understanding
of the role of affect in social cognition. The present studies contribute to this literature
in several respects, each of which will be discussed in turn.

Going beyond valence in research on affect and cognition


While it has been natural, and indeed fundamental, for researchers to make compari-
sons between the effects of good and bad moods, the present research highlights
58 G. V Rodenhausenetal.

the importance of looking beyond the valence of the social perceiver’s affective state
per se and beginning to explore the effects of discrete kinds of emotional experience
on information processing. In all three studies, angry and sad subjects showed clear
differences in the use they made of simple heuristic cues in the process of generating
evaluations of social information. Anger, sadness, anxiety, guilt, and many other
negative emotional states may each produce their own behavioural and judgmental
tendencies, and the same may be true for various positive states such as JOY, hope,
and pride. Consequently, it will be necessary for theoretical developments occurring
at the interface of affect and cognition to begin to explicitly account for the specific
effects of discrete emotions (cf;Bodenhausen, 1993).
With this in mind, it becomes clear that a straightforward mood congruency model
of affect and cognition will be inadequate to capture the full nchness of the impact
of affect on social thought and judgment. Because mood congruency perspectives,
at least as they are currently articulated, cannot easily account for variations in
patterns of judgment across different varieties of negative affect, they cannot account
for the overall pattern evident in these three studies. Recent theoretical developments
have indeed begun to incorporate multiple processes through which affect impinges
on social cognition (e.g. Forgas, 1992b), and it will become an important issue for
further exploration to uncover the conditions under which simple mood congruency
effects do and do not obtain, versus conditions in which more emotion specific
effects may be evident. The present data fit well within an adaptationist framework
arguing that emotions are associated with patterns of thought and action that serve
the needs of the types of situations in which the motions typically anse (e.g. Schwarz,
1990; Smith and Lazarus, 1990).
In order to make systematic progress in understanding the influence of emotional
experience on social information processing, it will be increasingly important for
researchers to look beyond mood valence. This will require the development of emo-
tion typologies that explicitly identify the consequential dimensions on which differ-
ent emotional states differ. As suggested elsewhere (Bodenhausen, 1993), emotions
may differ in their autonomic manifestations, the extent to which they produce rumi-
nation or distraction, their evolutionary connections to mental and behavioural pro-
clivities, and their tendencies to endure across time, as well as in many other ways.
It remains for future research to delineate the relative importance of these differences
in accounting for the differential effects of emotional states on perception, judgment,
and action.

Anger and social information processing


Centuries ago, Virgil wrote that ‘anger carries the mind away’, yet there has been
relatively little empirical documentation of the thought processes of angry minds.
It is certainly an issue of considerable practical importance to understand the effects
of being angry on one’s response to persuasive appeals or to members of stigmatized
social groups. The present research indicates that angry people are significantly more
likely to rely on simple cues in reacting to social stimuli. Their judgements of accused
mscreants were more affected by social stereotypes, and their level of agreement
with unpopular positions was guided more by the apparent credibility of the person
advocating the position. Why should this be SO? It was argued that anger arises
in situations involving physical and/or psychological harm, and as such, it may be
Negative afect and soc~aljudgmeni 59

most adaptive to respond relatively quickly, If an apparently diagnostic cue provides


a basis for a quick response, angry people appear to opt for this strategy to a greater
extent than those in a neutral mood. This may be true regardless of whether the
task requiring a response is related to the source of the anger (termed integral uflect
by Bodenhausen, 1993) or irrelevant to it (termed incidental a f e c t ) . The present
studies investigated the case of incidental affect, in that the social perception tasks
employed in the experiments were unrelated to the sources of subjects’ anger.
It is unclear whether angry subjects’ greater reliance on simple heuristic cues is
due to reduced motivation for thoughtful analysis of judgment-relevant information,
reduced capacity for such analysis, or something else, and it may prove difficult
to disentangle the various possibilities (cJ Schwarz el al. 1991). Nevertheless, it
will be useful for future research on the effects of anger on social judgment to begin
to document the specific mechanisms producing the greater reliance on heuristic
cue evident among angry social perceivers.

Sadness and the use of heuristic cues


Recent evidence has suggested that, unlike our angry subjects, sad people are more
prone to thoughtful, detail-oriented analysis of social information (e.g. Bless et al.,
1990; Weary, 1990). However, there has been little in the way of evidence concerning
the way(s) sad people make use of heuristic cues in social judgment. We initially
expected, in line with earlier research, that sad people may actually make very little
use of such global cues, preferring instead to base their reactions on a consideration
of the implications of the specific details contained in relevant information. Our
data, particularly that of Experiment 3, suggests that something a bit more complex
may be going on. It appears that sad people may not disregard source cues entirely
in persuasive situations. At least in the cases we examned, they actually appeared
to use them in a manner opposrte to the way they are used by angry subjects. At
the very least, we can say with confidence that sad subjects disagreed more with
a highly credible source then did angry or neutral mood Subjects.
Additional research will be necessary to fully understand when and how sad people
make use of global cues in reacting to social stimuli, but the present data suggest
that, whatever they are doing with these cues, it is something very different from
what angry people are doing with them. All negative moods are not alike. The
three studies reported here lend credence to the claim that future research on affect
and social cognition will need to develop a theoretical richness sufficient to account
for the rich variety of influences of different kinds of emotional expenences on human
thought and action.

REFERENCES

Baron, R. S., Inman, M. L., Kao, C. F and Logan, H. (1992).‘Negativeemotionand superficial


social processing’ Motivation and Emotion, 16: 323-346.
Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N. and Strack, F (1990). ‘Mood and persuasion: A cognitive
response analysis’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16: 33 1-345.
Bodenhausen, G. V (1988). ‘Stereotypic biases in social decision making and memory: Testing
process models of stereotype use’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55: 726-137
60 G. V Bodenhausenet al.

Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). ‘Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian varia-


tions in discrimination’, Psychological Science. 1: 3 19-322.
Bodenhausen, G. V (1993). ‘Emotion, arousal, and stereotypic judgment: A heuristic model
of affect and stereotyping’ In: Mackie, D. and Hamilton, D. (Eds) Aflect, Cognition and
Stereotyping: Interactive Processes in Intergroup Perception, Academic Press, San Diego,
CA, pp. 13-37
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P and Siisser, K. (in press). ‘Happiness and stereotypic
thinking in social judgment’ Journal of Personalify and Social Psychology.
Bodenhausen, G. V. and Lichtenstein, M. (1987). ‘Social stereotypes and information process-
ing strategies: The impact of task complexity’ Journal of Personality and Soczaf Psychology,
5 2 87 1-880.
Bodenhausen, G. V. and Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1985). ‘Effects of stereotypes on decision making
and information-processing strateges’, Jourtial of Personality and Social Psychology, 48:
262-282.
Bower, G . H. (1991). ‘Mood congruity of social Judgment’ In: Forgas, J. (Ed.) Emotion
and Social Judgment, Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp. 3 1-54.
Chaiken, S. (1980). ‘Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source
versus message cues in persuasion’, Journal of Personality andSoccal Psychology, 39: 752-766.
Chaiken, S. (1987). ‘The heuristic model of persuasion’. In: Zanna, M. P., Olson, J. M. and
Herman, C. P (Eds) Socral Influence: The Ontario Symposrwn, Vol. 5, Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
NJ, pp. 3-39.
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A. and Eagly, A. H. (1989). ‘Heuristic and systematic information
processing within and beyond the persuasion context’ In: Uleman, J. and Bargh, J. (Eds)
Unintended Thought, Guilford, New York, pp. 212-252.
Clark, M. S. (1982). ‘A role for arousal in the link between feeling states, judgments, and
behavior’ In: Clark, M. S. and Fiske, S. T. (Eds, Aflect and Cognition: The Seventeenth
Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 263-289.
Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, 0. H. and Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration
and Aggression, Yale University Press, New Haven, CN.
Eagly, A. H. and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes, Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich,
Ft. Worth, TX.
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W. and Chaiken, S. (1978). ‘Causal inferences about communicators
and their effects on opinion change’, Journal of Personality and Socrol Psychology, 36:
424-435.
Esses, V M., Haddock, G. and Zanna, M. P (in press). ‘The role of mood in the expression
of intergroup stereotypes’ In: Zanna, M. P and Olson, J. M. (Eds) The Psychoiogy of
Prejudice: the Ontario Symposium Vol. 7, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Fiedler, K. (1991). ‘On the task, the measures, and the mood m research on affect and cogni-
tion’ In: Forgas, J. P. (Ed.) Emotion and Social Judgments, Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp.
83-104.
Fiske, S. T and Neuberg, S. L. (1990). ‘A continuum of impression formation from category-
based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention
and interpretation’. In: Zanna, M. P (Ed.) Advances in Experimental socud Psychology,
Vol. 23, Academc Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 1-74.
Forgas, J. P (1989). ‘Mood effects on decision-making strateges’, Australian Journal of Psy-
chology, 41: 197-214.
Forgas, J. P (1992a). ‘Affect and social perception: Research evidence and an integrative
theory’ In: Stroebe, W and Hewstone, M. (Eds) European Review of Soccal Psychology,
Vol3, Wiley, Chichester. England.
Forgas, J. P (1992b). ‘Affect in social judgments and decisions: A multi-process model’
In: Zanna, M. P (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 25, Academic
Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 227-275.
Forgas, J. P (1992~).‘On mood and peculiar people: Affect and person typicality in mpression
formation’, Journal of Personality and Socral Psychology, 62: 863-875.
Forgas, J. P and Moylan, S. J. (1987). ‘After the movies: The effects of transient mood
states on social judgments’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13: 478489
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S. and
Negative affect and sociafjudgment 61

Lyon, D. (1990). ‘Evidence for terror management theory 11: The effects of mortality salience
on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview’, Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 58: 308-3 18.
Hass, R. G. (1981). ‘Effects of source characteristics on cognitive responses and persuasion’
In: Petty, R., Ostrom, T and Brock, T (Eds) Cognitive Responses m Persuasion, Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 141-172.
Hebb, D. 0. (1946). ‘On the nature of fear’, Psychological Review, 53: 259-276.
Henry, J. P (1986). ‘Neuroendocrine patterns of emotional response’ In: Plutchik, R. and
Kellerman, H (Eds) Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience Vol. 3, Academic Press,
Orlando, FI, pp. 37-60.
Hovland, C. I. and Weiss, W (1951). ‘The influence of source credibility on communication
effectiveness’, Public Opinion Quarterly, 15: 635-650.
Isen, A. M. (1987). ‘Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior’ In: Berkowitz,
L. (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 20, Academic Press, Orlando,
FL, pp. 203-253.
Isen, A. M. and Means, B. (1983). ‘The influence of positive affect on decision-makingstrategy’,
Social Cognition, 2: 18-3 1.
Kelman, H. C. and Hovland, C. I. (1953). “‘Reinstatement” of the communicator in delayed
measurement of opinion change’, Journal olAbnorma1 and Social Psychology, 48: 327-335.
Kuhl, J. (1983). ‘Emotion, Kognition, und Motivation 11: Die funktionale Bedeutung der
Emotionen fur das problemlosende Denke und fur das konkrete Handeln’, Sprache & Kogni-
tion, 4: 228-253,
Mackie, D. M. and Worth, L. (1989). ‘Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect
in persuasion’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57: 2 7 4 0 .
Mackie, D. M. and Worth, L. (1991). ‘Feeling good, but not thinking straight: The unpact
of positive mood on persuasion’ In: Forgas, J. P (Ed.) Emotion and Social Judgment,
Pergamon, Oxford; pp. 201-220.
McGuire, W J. (1985). ‘Attitudes and attitude change’ In: Lindzey, G. and Aronson, E.
(Eds) Handbook ofSocial Psychology, 3rd edn, Vol. 2, Random House, New York, pp. 233-
346.
Ortony, A,, Clore, G. L. and Collins, A. (1988). Cognitive Structure of Emotions, Cambndge
University Press, Cambndge.
Petty, R. E. and Cacioppo, J. T (1986). ‘The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion’
In: Berkowitz L. (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 19, Academic
Press, New York, pp. 124-203.
Schaller, M. and Cialdini, R. B. (1990). ‘Happiness, sadness, and helping: A motivational
integration’ In: Higgins, E. T and Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds) Handbook of Motivation and
Cognition, Vol. 2, Guilford, New York, pp. 265-296.
Schwarz, N. ( I 990). ‘Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of
affective states’ In: Higgins, E. T and Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds) Handbook of Motivation
and Cognition, Vol. 2 , Guilford, New York, pp. 527-561.
Schwarz, N. and Bless, H. (1991). ‘Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact
of affective states on analytic reasoning’ In: Forgas, J. P (Ed.) Emotion and Social Judgment,
Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp. 55-7 1.
Schwarz, N., Bless, H. and Bohner, G. (1991). ‘Mood and persuasion: Affective states influence
the processing of persuasive communications’ In: Zanna, M. P (Ed.) Advances in Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, Voi. 24, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 161-199
Schwarz, N. and Clore, G. L. (1983). ‘Mood, muattribution, and judgments of well-being:
Informative and directive functions of affective states’, Journal of Personahy and Social
Psychology, 45: 5 13-523.
Schwarz, N. and Clore, G. L. (1988). ‘How do I feel about it?. The informative function
of affective states’ In: Fiedler, K. and Forgas, J. (Eds) Affect. Cognition, andSocialBehavior,
Hogrefe, Toronto, pp. 44-62.
Scott, J. P (1980). ‘The function of emotions in behavioral systems: A systems theory analysis.
In: Plutchik, R. and Kellerman, H. (Eds) Emotion: Theory, Research, Experience, Vol. I ,
Academic Press, New York, pp. 35-56.
62 G. V Bodenhausen et al.

Shields, S. A. (1984). ‘Reports of bodily change in anxiety, sadness, and anger’, Motivation
and Emotion, 8: 1-21.
Sinclair, R. C. (1988). ‘Mood, categorization breadth, and performance appraisal: The effects
of order of information acquisition and affective state on halo, accuracy, information retrie-
val, and evaluations’, Organizational Behavior and Human Declsion Processes, 42: 2 2 4 6 .
Sinclair, R. C. and Mark, M. M. (1992). ‘The influence of mood state on judgment and
action: Effects on persuasion, categorization, socialjustice, person perception, andjudgmen-
tal accuracy’ In: Martin, L. L. and Tesser, A. (Eds) The Construction of Social Judgments,
Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 165-193.
Smith, C. A. and Lazarus, R. S. (1990). ‘Emotion and adaption’ In: Pervin, L. (Ed.) Handbook
of Personality. Theory and Research, Guilford, New York, pp. 609-637
Stephan, W C. and Stephan, C. W (1985). ‘Intergroup anxiety’, Journal of Social Issues,
41(3): 157-175.
Strack, F., Schwarz, N. and Gschneidinger, E. (1985). ‘Happiness and reminiscing: The role
of time perspective, mood, and mode of thinking, ‘ Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 49. 1460- 1469
Walster, E., Aronson, E. and Abraham, D. (1966). ‘On increasing the persuasiveness of
a low prestige communicator’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2: 325-342.
Weary, G. (1990). ‘Depression and sensitivity to social information’ In: Moore, B. S. and
Isen, A. M. (Eds) Aflecf andSocial Behavior, Cambridge University Cambridge, pp. 207-230.
Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M. and Roper, D. W (1988). ‘Depression and mental control:
The resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
55: 882-892.
Wilder, D. (1993). ‘Arousal and intergroup bias: Facilitative effects of arousal on stereotyping’
In: Mackie, D. and Hamilton, D. (Eds) Aflect, Cognition, and Stereotyping: Interactive
Processes in Group Perception, Academic press, San Diego, CA.
Worth, L. T and Mackie, D. M. (1987). ‘Cognitive mediation of positive affect in persuasion’,
Social Cognition, 5: 76-94.

Вам также может понравиться