Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

Psychiatry

Interpersonal and Biological Processes

ISSN: 0033-2747 (Print) 1943-281X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/upsy20

Mood Disorders and Patterns of Creativity in


British Writers and Artists

Kay Redfield Jamison

To cite this article: Kay Redfield Jamison (1989) Mood Disorders and Patterns of Creativity in
British Writers and Artists, Psychiatry, 52:2, 125-134, DOI: 10.1080/00332747.1989.11024436

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1989.11024436

Published online: 06 Oct 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 5

View related articles

Citing articles: 22 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=upsy20

Download by: [University of Auckland Library] Date: 23 March 2017, At: 17:24
Mood Disorders and Patterns of Creativity in
British Writers and Artists
Kay Redfield Jamison

EXTREMES in mood, thought and behavior-including psychosis-have been


linked with artistic creativity for as long as man has observed and written
about those who write, paint, sculpt or compose. The history of this long and
fascinating association, as well as speculations about its reasons for being,
have been discussed by several modern authors and investigators, including
Koestler (1975), Storr (1976), Andreasen (1978), Becker (1978), Rothenberg
(1979), Richards (1981), Jamison (in press) and Prentky (in press). The associa-
tion between extreme states of emotion and mind and creativity not only is
fascinating but also has significant theoretical, clinical, literary and societal-
ethical implications. These issues, more thoroughly reviewed elsewhere (J ami-
son et al. 1980; Richards 1981; Jamison, in press), include the understanding of
cognitive, perceptual, mood and behavioral changes common to manic, depres-
sive and creative states; the potential ability to lessen the stigma of mental
illness; effects of psychiatric treatment (for example, lithium) on creativity;
and concerns raised about genetic research on mood disorders.
The current study was designed to ascertain rates of treatment for affective
illness in a sample of eminent British writers and artists; to study differences
in subgroups (poets. novelists, playwrights, biographers, artists); to examine
seasonal patterns of moods and productivity; and to inquire into the perceived
role of very intense moods in the writers' and artists' work. One of the major
purposes of this investigation was to look at possible similarities and dissimi-
larities between periods of intense creative activity and hypomania. Hypothe-
sized similarities were based on the overlapping nature of mood, cognitive and
behavioral changes associated with both; the episodic nature of both; and
possible links between the durational, frequency and seasonal patterns of both
experiences.

Several research strategies exist for ex- on life study investigations of prominent
amining the relationship between affec- individuals (e.g., Lombroso 1891; Ellis
tive illness and creativity. The first, his- 1904; Nisbet 1912; Lange-Eichbaum
torical. and biographical studies, focuses 1932; Juda 1949; Tsanoff 1949; Wittko-

Kay Redfield Jamison, PhD, is Associate Professor in the Department of Psychiatry, The Johns
Hopkins University School of Medicine.
The research was supported by the Institute for Scientific Information (Philadelphia) and Frances Lear.
The author is indebted to Professor Eugene Paykel (Cambridge University) and James Henderson
(Oxford University) for providing research facilities, to Dr. Richard Jed Wyatt for particularly helpful
suggestions concerning the manuscript, and to the superb writers and artists who gave valuable time to
participate in this study.

PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989 125


KAY REDFIELD JAMISON

wer and Wittkower 1963; Goertzel et al. or awards in their respective fields. All
1978; Jamison, in press). These biographi- painters and sculptors, for example, were
cal studies have provided strong, sugges- either Royal Academicians or Associates
tive but as yet anecdotal clues to signifi- of the Royal Academy. Literary prizes
cantly increased rates of mood disorders used as selection criteria included the
and suicide in eminent writers and Queen's Gold Medal for Poetry and the
artists. Hawthornden, Booker, and James Tait
The second strategy, studies of creative Black Memorial Prizes. In addition, 9 of
ability in affectively ill patients, provides the 18 poets in the study sample were al-
a different perspective but corroborating ready represented in The Oxford Book of
evidence. DeLong and Aldershof (1983) Twentieth Century English Verse. Of the
found an unusually high incidence of spe- 8 playwrights, 6 were winners of the New
cial abilities (for example, outstanding ar- York Drama Critics Award or the Evening
tistic and mathematical talent) in a sam- Standard (London) Drama Award; several
ple of children with manic-depressive had won both, had won one of these
illness. Richards et al. (1988) found signif- awards more than once, and/or had re-
icantly increased creativity in manic-de- ceived Tony Awards.
pressive and cyclothymic patients, as well Participants in the study were either
as their normal first-degree relatives, British subjects (87 %) or citizens of the
when compared with control subjects. British Commonwealth or the Republic of
The third major strategy involves sys- Ireland (13%). Most were men (87%), and
tematic diagnostic and psychological the majority were Protestant (77%); 15%
studies of living writers and artists (e.g., were Catholic, and 7 % were agnostic or
Andreasen and Canter 1974; Andreasen had no religious affiliation. The mean age
and Powers 1974; Andreasen 1987). This of the sample was 53.2 years (SD=12.9).
paper concerns research of the latter type. Demographic characteristics for the sub-
Andreasen and her colleagues, using mod- groups - poets, playwrights, novelists, bi-
ern diagnostic techniques, were the first ographers and artists - are summarized in
to systematically study the relationship Table 1. There were no significant differ-
between creativity and psychopathology. ences between subgroups, except that the
They found an exceptionally high rate of poets were disproportionately Protestant
affective illness, especially bipolar, in (94 %) and the novelists disproportionate-
their sample of writers from the Universi- ly Catholic or agnostic (50%).
ty of Iowa Writers' Workshop. Fully 80 %
of the 30 writers studied met Research Di-
agnostic Criteria (Spitzer et al. 1978) for Procedures
an episode of affective illness at some All subjects (N=47) were asked detailed
time during their lives; 43 % met criteria (open-ended and scaled) questions about
for bipolar illness (Andreasen 1987). First- history and type of treatment, if any, for
degree relatives of the writers also demon- affective illness; observed, if any, diurnal
strated a disproportionate rate of affec- and seasonal patterns in their moods and
tive illness, especially major depressive productivity; behavioral, cognitive and
disorder. mood correlates of their periods of crea-
tive work; and the perceived role of very
intense moods in their work. Specific di-
METHOD agnostic criteria were not used in this
study as the primary aim was to ascertain
Subjects actual rates of treatment, a more strin-
The poets, playwrights, novelists, biog- gent criterion for severity of affective ill-
raphers and artists in the study were se- ness. Extensive personality and attitudi-
lected on the basis of having won at least nal data, to be reported in a future paper,
oue of t:ieveral t:ipecifieu prestigiuus prizes were also collected. As partial compensa-

126 PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989


--- - - - - -- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - ----- ------------------------ ---

MOOD DISORDERS AND CREATIVITY


Table 1
DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF SAMPLE
AND SUBGROUPS
Group N Mean Age % Male % Protestant
Poets 18 50.8 89 94
Playwrights 8 48.5 88 75
Novelists 8 55.0 88 50
Biographers 5 61.6 80 80
Artists 8 56.5 88 63
Total Sample 47 53.2 87 77

tion for the considerable amount of time manic episodes. As Table 3 shows, howev-
involved in participating, subjects were er, about one-third of the writers and art-
promised copies of all published reports ists reported histories of severe mood
resulting from the study. swings, essentially cyclothymic in nature,
and one-fourth reported histories of ex-
tended, elated mood states. Novelists and
RESULTS poets more frequently reported the pro-
longed, elated states; playwrights and art-
History of Treatment for Affective Illness ists, on the other hand, were more likely to
report severe mood swings. The relatively
The artists and writers were asked low rate of treatment for affective illness
whether or not they had received treat- in those who are creative in predominant-
ment, and the nature of that treatment, ly nonverbal fields (painting and sculp-
for a mood disorder. The results are shown ture) is interesting and may be due to the
in Table 2. A very high percentage of the fact that artists are less inclined than
total sample, 38 %, had been treated for an writers to seek psychiatric help (especial-
affective illness; three-fourths of those ly if psychiatric treatment is perceived of
treated had been given antidepressants or as primarily verbal in nature). It is as like-
lithium, or had been hospitalized. Poets ly, however, that mood disorders may not
were most likely to have required medica- convey to visual artists the same experi-
tion for their depression (33%) and were ential and cognitive advantages that are
the only ones to have required medical in- useful to writers, and that neuropsycho-
tervention (hospitalization, electrocon- logical differences between those with and
vulsive therapy, lithium) for mania (17 %). without affective illness (i.e., greater diffi-
Fully one-half of the poets had been culties with right hemispheric function-
treated with drugs, psychotherapy, and/ ing; see Sackheim and Steif 1988; Good-
or hospitalization for mood disorders. The win and Jamison, in press) may make
playwrights had the highest total rate of visual artists a lower risk group. Interest-
treatment for affective illness (63%), but a ingly, the biographers-who provided a
relatively large percentage of those comparison group by being outstanding
treated (60%) had been treated with psy- but perhaps less creative writers - report-
chotherapy alone. It is unclear whether ed no history of mood swings or elated
this was due to a difference in severity of states.
illness or in treatment preference.
With the exception of the poets, the
subjects reported being treated for de- Similarities Between Hypomanic and
pression, not mania or hypomania; the de- Creative States
sign of the study did not allow systematic Virtually all subjects (89%) reported
diagnostic inquiry into hypomanic or having experienced intense, highly pro-

PSYCHIATRY, VoL 52, May 1989 127


KAY REDFIELD JAMISON
Table 2
HISTORY OF TREATMENT FOR AFFECTIVE ILLNESS IN
ToTAL SAMPLE AND SUBGROUPS
% Treated for
Bipolar Illness % Treated with Total %
(Hospifalization % Treated with Psychotherapy Treated for
lithium, Antidepressants Alone for an Affective
ECT, etc.) for Depression Depression Illness
Poets 16.7 33.0 5.5 55.2
Playwrights 0.0 25.0 37.5 62.5
Novelists 0.0 25.0 0.0 25.0
Biographers 0.0 20.0 0.0 20.0
Artists 0.0 12.5 0.0 12.5

Total Sample 6.4 23.4 8.5 38.3

ductive and creative episodes (100 % of the talkativeness, spending of money) were re-
poets, novelists, and artists; and 88 % of ported by only a minority of subjects.
the playwrights; but, consistent with the Subjects were asked about changes in
results reported earlier, only 20 % of the sleep and mood occurring just prior to
biographers). The modal duration of these these intense creative episodes. Almost
episodes was two weeks (35%); 55% of the all of the writers and artists (89%) report-
episodes lasted 1-4 weeks and 25 % con- ed a decrease in the need for sleep; 28 %
tinued for longer than a month. One-fifth spontaneously reported waking abruptly
of the episodes lasted 24 hours or less. at 3 or 4 A.M. and being unable to return
The episodes were characterized by in- to sleep. Changes in mood were profound.
creases in enthusiasm, energy, self-confi- One-half of the subjects reported a sharp
dence, speed of mental association, fluen- increase in mood just prior to the begin-
cy of thoughts, elevated mood and a ning of an intensely creative period: for
strong sense of well-being (see Figure 1). example, "excited, anticipatory, energet-
A comparison with DSM-III criteria for ic"; "I have a fever to write, and throw my-
hypomania reveals that mood and cogni- self energetically into new projects";
tive symptoms showed the greatest de- "elated:' "euphoric"; "ecstatic:' Dysphoria
gree of overlap between intensely creative preceded enhanced creativity in 28 % of
and hypomanic episodes. Several of the the subjects: for example, "more anxious";
more behavioral changes typically asso- "near suicide"; "fearfulness, general mood
ciated with hypomania (hypersexuality, of distress and slight paranoia:' Finally,
approximately one-fourth (22%) of the
sample reported mixed mood changes and
Table 3 psychomotor restlessness: "mixture ofela-
HISTORY OF SEVERE MOOD SWINGS AND tion together with some gloominess, feel-
EXTENDED, ELATED MOOD STATES ing of isolation, sexual pressure, fast emo-
Severe Extended, Elated tional responses"; "restlessness"; "low ebb
Mood Swings Mood States bordering on despair often precedes good
phase when work will flow almost as
Poets 28% 33%
Playwrights 50% 25%
though one is a medium, rather than an
Novelists 13% 38% originator"; "restless, dissatisified:'
Biographers 0% 0% When the subjects were asked specifi-
Artists 38% 13% cally about the importance of very intense
feelings and moods in the development
Total Sample 30% 26% and execution of their work, 90 % stated

128 PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989


MOOD DISORDERS AND CREATIVITY
Percent of Total Sample Periods of Intense Creative Activity:
Changes Rated as Pronounced or
Very Pronounced
40 60 80 90
.~. ·::[%i!iiiei ~:W!C::ii !i.:ii'ii~i:::i I .. I
l' enthusiasm
l' energy
l' self-confidence
l' speed of mental associations
l' fluency of thoughts
l' mood, euphoria
l' ability to concentrate
l' emotional intensity
l' sense of well- being
l' rapid thinking
l' expansiveness ( ideas / feelings)
-!- need for sleep
l' sensory awareness
l' restlessness
l' impulsivity
liwlKillllMlil! l' irritability
',,:ifie!!!II!;],,!";1 l' sexuality

..-.
!i;iil<i i i· . ' i+i·. iiiiim t talkativeness
l' drinking
l' religious thoughts / feelings

_
!!It,f14YiI l' spending of money
1,1111 l' anxiety
!!ii!Pieiql l' suspiciousness
t argumentativeness
11m l' sociability

Figure 1.
Mood, cognitive and behavioral changes reported during intense creative episodes.

that such moods and feelings were either the writers and artists relied upon exten-
integral and necessary (60%), or very im- sive notes and journals to assist them in
portant (30%). Consistent with their rate making their ratings. Figure 2 presents
of treatment for affective illness, more po- the mood and productivity curves for
ets than any other group regarded these those subjects reporting no history of
moods as essential to what they did and treatment for affective illness; Figure 3
how they did it. shows the curves for subjects with a histo-
ry of treatment for affective illness. Very
different seasonal patterns emerged.
Seasonal Patterns of Moods and Those in the history of treatment group
Productivity demonstrated inversely related curves for
Subjects were asked to rate their moods summer productivity and moods, while
and productivity for 86 months. Many of those in the no history of t.reat.ment. group

PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989 129


KAY REDFIELD JAMISON

3.0

en
Cl
c
15
cr: 2.5

->-
.:;
;: , I
I
U
:::J
"t:J
0
~
2.0 " '0- .../
a.
"t:J
c
0
"t:J 1.5 • • Mood scores
0
0 0- - -0 Productivity scores
~

1.0 L.--'-----''---'-_L.-.....L.._'---'-_'---'-_L.--'-----'
J FMAMJ JASOND
Figure 2.
Mean mood and productivity ratings (36 months) in writers and artists with no history of treatment for
affective illness (N =32).

3.0
en
Cl
c
:0:
o
cr: 2.5
~

e-
:~
u
:::J
2.0
a.
"t:J
c
o
"t:J 1.5 • • Mood scores
o
o 0- - -0 Productivity scores
~

J FMAMJ JASON 0
Figure 3.
Mean mood and productivity ratings (36 months) in writers and artists with a history of treatment for
affective illness (N = 15).

130 PSYCHIATRY, VoL 52, May 1989


,-----~-~--- - - - - - - - - ---------- --------------_._---- --~~-------,

MOOD DISORDERS AND CREATIVITY

showed mood and productivity curves ers and artists was predominantly male
more directly co-varying. In the treat- (87%).
ment group the peaks for productivity These surprisingly high rates of treat-
preceded and followed the mood peak by ment for affective illness are, however,
three to four months. comparable to those reported by An-
dreasen and her colleagues. In an update
of earlier work done with the well-known
DISCUSSION University of Iowa Writers' Workshop,
Andreasen (1987) found that 80% of her
sample of 30 writers (90 % males) had
Rates of Treatment for Affective Illness experienced an episode of affective ill-
The rate of treatment for affective ill-
ness; 43 % had had manic or hypomanic
ness (38%) was strikingly high in this episodes. Possible explanations for the
sample of outstanding British writers and elevated rates of mood disorders in suc-
artists. Lifetime prevalence rates for bipo-
cessful writers are discussed in detail else-
lar and unipolar disorders in the general where (Andreasen and Powers 1974,1975;
population are 1 % and 5 %, respectively. Richards 1981; Prentky, in press; Jamison
The proportion of individuals who actual- et al. 1980; Jamison, in press) and can be
ly seek or receive treatment, even though only briefly reviewed here. Profound
they meet the formal diagnostic criteria changes in mood, cognition, personality,
sleep, energy and behavior characterize
for affective illness, is far smaller. There-
fore, rates in this study represent a con-both altered mood and creative states.
servative estimate of the actual preva- Cognitive changes occurring during hypo-
lence of affective illness in the sample. manic states-e.g., the increase in speed,
Weissman et al. (1981), for example, foundassociational fluency and flexibility of
that only 20 % of persons with a current thought-are likewise critical to creativi-
psychiatric disorder had seen a mental ty. For writers and artists, who draw so
health professional in the previous year, deeply from their lives and emotions for
and Shapiro et al. (1984) concluded that their work, the wide range, intensity, fluc-
tuation and variability of emotional expe-
only one-third of affectively ill patients ac-
tually make a mental health visit. Like- rience brought about by mood disorders
wise, although lithium is the treatment ofcan work to the advantage, as well as dis-
choice for a minimal 1 % of the general advantage, of original composition. Too,
population, utilization rates determined what hypomania generates in enthusiasm
by McCreadie and Morrison (1985) were and excess, the more critical and obses-
only 0.77 per 1000, and by Escobar et al. sive eye of depression often effectively
(1987), 0.15%; both studies clearly indi- judges and edits.
cated a gross underutilization of lithium
relative to the established prevalence of Similarities Between Hypomanic and
mania. Antidepressants, more frequently
prescribed, were used by 2.5 % of the Epi- Creative States
demiological Catchment Area community The study revealed many overlapping
sample (N=14,998) as reported by Esco- mood, cognitive and behavioral (especial-
bar et al. These drug treatment figures are ly sleep) changes between hypomania and
in marked contrast to the current sample intense creative states, despite the fact
of British writers and artists, 6.4 % of that questions about both states were
whom reported the use of lithium (16.7% asked independently of one another and in
of the poets) and 23.4 % the use of antide- a manner designed to minimize possible
pressants. The contrast in rates is even effects of suggestion. Cognitive and mood
more pronounced if one considers that an- changes shared far more overlap than be-
tidepressant use is far more common in havioral ones, indicating, perhaps, that
women than men, yet the srunple of writ- t.he milder formA of hypomania may mpm-

PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989 131


KAY REDFIELD JAMISON

sent the more productive phases of affec- Implications


tive illness. The continuum that ranges Writers and artists frequently express
from normal state to mildly (or, controlla- concerns about the effects of psychiatric
bly) "hypomanic;' to clinical hypomania treatment on their ability to create and
and mania is an enormously important produce; these concerns are particularly
but poorly understood one. It remains un- pronounced around issues of taking medi-
clear whether the overlap in cognitive and cation. Clearly, not all of these fears are
mood changes represents etiologically re- realistic, but some may be. Early re-
lated syndromes or phenomenologically searchers were well aware of problems
similar but causally unrelated patterns of created by lithium's effects on certain pro-
expression. It also remains unclear the ex- ductive and enjoyable qualities of manic-
tent to which writers and artists are sim- depressive illness (Schou 1968; Polatin
ply more sensitive than the general popu- and Fieve 1971; Van Putten 1975); more
lation to their own mood states and recent studies demonstrate that missing
therefore more able to articulate and re- certain positive features of hypomania is
port them. an important reason for lithium noncom-
pliance (Jamison et al. 1979; Jamison and
Akiskal1983).
Seasonal Patterns of Mood and The short and long-term effects of lithi-
Productivity um, carbamazepine, and the antidepres-
There were significant differences in sants on productivity and creativity re-
seasonal mood and productivity patterns main unclear. Marshall et al. (1970) and
between those writers and artists with a Schou (1979) studied a total of 30 artists,
history of treatment for affective illness writers and businessmen taking lithium.
and those without; in the former group Three-quarters of the subjects reported no
the peaks for productivity preceded, and change, or an increase in their productivi-
followed, the mood peaks by 3 to 4 ty while on lithium. One-quarter reported
months; in the no-treatment group, the a decrease, and 17% refused to continue
peaks for mood and productivity co-var- lithium because of its effect on their work
ied. Several explanations are possible. and lives. Studies of lithium's effect on
First, high productivity associated with cognitive processing, which conflict in
elevated mood is less likely to lead to their findings, are reviewed elsewhere
treatment-seeking behavior than low pro- (Goodwin and Jamison, in press). Two
ductivi'ty associated with high mood. Sec- studies of particular relevance for artistic
ond, the elevated mood of the treatment creativity also conflict in their results.
group probably reflects more "true" hypo- Judd et al. (1977) found no effects of
mania-Le., greater distractibility and short-term lithium treatment on creativi-
dysphoria, and increased stimulus-seek- ty in normal subjects. A recent study us-
ing behavior-which might well lead to ing bipolar patients as their own controls,
less productivity in the acute phase. In however, found substantial, detrimental
the no-treatment group, the periods of in- effects of lithium on associational process-
creased mood and productivity may repre- ing (Shaw et al. 1986). Differences in re-
sent a milder spectrum form of hypoma- sults may be in part due to the fact that
nia, or intensified normal functioning, lithium's effect on cognition is probably
resulting in more simultaneous peaks for quite different in manic-depressive pa-
mood and productivity. For both groups tients and normal volunteers (Pons et al.
the summer peak in moods is consistent 1985). Individual differences in the severi-
with what is known about seasonal pat- ty, frequency and type of affective illness;
terns for hypomania, mania and depres- sensitivity to cognitive side-effects; se-
sion (Rosenthal et al. 1984; Goodwin and rum lithium levels; and clinical state also
J amison, in press). clearly affect the degree to which an indi-

132 PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989


MOOD DISORDERS AND CREATIVITY

vidual will experience impairment in intel- due course, arise about amniocentesis and
lectual functioning, creativity and pro- early identification and treatment of indi-
ductivity. Artists, writers and the many viduals at high risk for affective, especial-
others who rely upon their initiative, in- ly bipolar, illness. It becomes particularly
tellect, emotional intensity and energy for important under these circumstances,
their life's work underscore the need for a and especially when dealing with treata-
reexamination of this problem. ble disorders, to have at least some broad
Artists and writers represent a group at notion of the individual and societal costs
high risk for affective illness and should and benefits of making such decisions.
be assessed and counseled accordingly. Meyerson and Boyle (1941), in a study of
Ideal treatment requires: a sensitive un- manic-depressive psychosis in socially
derstanding of the possible benefits of prominent American families, discussed
mood disorders to creativity, as well as at length possible social consequences of
the severe liabilities, including the risk of sterilization, an extreme procedure but
suicide and of untreated depression and relevant in many of the issues it raises.
mania; use of available medications with In analyzing one prominent family they
awareness of side-effects potentially dam- noted:
aging to the creative process; minimiza-
tion, whenever possible, of drug (especial- If sterilization had been done ... two psychot-
ly lithium) levels; the recognition and ic individuals would have been eliminated
sophisticated use of seasonal patterns in from the American scene, patients with man-
moods and productivity (for example, ic-depressive psychosis, but there would have
through self-charting of moods with a vis- gone with them a man internationally known,
ual analogue scale); and sensitivity to the whose writings still remain as a source of in-
possible role of alcohol and drugs in induc- spiration and life orientation for many people,
ing, maintaining, or exacerbating mood whose school of thought is still to be reckoned
with and who is frequently cited as a figure
states. unique in America and uniquely American.
Yet another implication stemming from The group who clustered around this man left
the close relationship between mood dis- their influence on the whole of America. His
orders and creativity in the arts is a soci- descendants are still extremely eminent and
etal one. Genetic research is progressing also still send patients to hospitals for mental
to the stage where ethical issues will, in disease. [po 18]
I

REFERENCES

ANDREASEN, N. C. Creativity and psychiatric ill- DELONG, G. R., and ALDERSHOF, A. Associations of
ness. Psychiatric Annals (1978) 8:113-19. special abilities with juvenile manic-depressive ill-
ANDREASEN, N. C. Creativity and mental illness: ness. Annals of Neurology (1983) 14:362.
Prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree ELLIS, H. A Study of British Genius. London: Hurst
relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry (1987) and Blackett, 1904.
144:1288-92. ESCOBAR, J. I., ANTHONY, J. C., CANINO, G., et al. Use
ANDREASEN, N. C., and CANTER, A. The creative of neuroleptics, antidepressants, and lithium by
writer: Psychiatric symptoms and family history. U.S. community populations.Psychopharmacolo-
Comprehensive Psychiatry (1974) 15:123-31. gy Bulletin (1987) 23:196-200.
ANDREASEN, N. C., and POWERS, P. Overinclusive GOERTZEL, M. G., GOERTZEL, V., and GOERTZEL, T.
thinking in mania and schizophrenia. British Jour- G. Three Hundred Eminent Personalities. Jossey-
nal of Psychiatry (1974) 125:452-56.. Bass, 1978.
ANDREASEN, N. C., and POWERS, P. Creativity and GOODWIN, F. K., and JAMISON, K. R. Manic-Depres-
psychosis: An examination of conceptual style. sive Illness. Oxford University Press, in press.
Archives of General Psychiatry (1975) 32:70-73. JAMISON, K. R. Manic-depressive illness, creativity,
BECKER, G. The Mad Genius Controversy. Sage, and leadership. In Goodwin and Jamison (in
1978. press).

PSYCHIATRY, Vol 52, May 1989 133


KAY REDFIELD JAMISON
JAMISON, K. R., and AKISKAL, H. S. Medication tation of the evidence. Genetic Psychology Mono-
compliance in patients with bipolar disorder. Pr;y- graphs (1981) 103:261-324.
chiatric Clinics of North America (1983) 6:75-192. RICHARDS, R. L., KINNEY, D. K., LUNDE, I., and
JAMISON, K. R., GERNER, R. H., and GOODWIN, F. K. BENET, M. Creativity in manic-depressives, cy-
Patient and physician attitudes toward lithium. clothymes, and their normal first-degree relatives:
Archives of General Psychiatry (1979) 36:866-69. A preliminary report. Journfl of Abnormal Psy-
JAMISON, K. R., GERNER, R. H., HAMMEN, C., and chology (1988) 97:281-88.
PADESKY, C. Clouds and silver linings: Positive ex- RoBINS, L. N., HELZER, J. E., WEISSMAN, M. M., et
periences associated with primary affective disor· al. Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric dis-
ders. American Journal of Psychiatry (1980) orders in three sites. Archives of General Psychia-
137:198-202. try (1984) 41:949-58.
JUDA, A. The relationship between high mental ca- ROSENTHAL, N. E., SACK, D. A., GILLIN, J. C., et al.
pacity and psychic abnormalities. American Jour- Seasonal affective disorder: A description of the
nal of Psychiatry (1949) 106:296-307. syndrome and preliminary findings with light
JUDD, L. L., HUBBARD, R. B., JANOWSKY, D. S., et al. therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry (1984)
The effect of lithium carbonate on the cognitive 41:72-80.
functions of normal subjects. Archives of General RoTHENBERG, A. The Emerging Goddess: The Crea-
Psychiatry (1977) 34:355-57. tive Process in Art, Science, and Other Fields.
KOESTLER, A. The Act of Creation. London: Pan, University of Chicago Press, 1979.
1975. SACKHEIM, H. A., and STEIF, B. L. Neuropsychology
LANGE-EICHBAUM, W. The Problem of Genius. Mac- of depression and mania. In A. Georgotas and R.
millan, 1932. Cancro, eds., Depression and Mania. Elsevier,
LOMBROSO, C. The Man of Genius. London: Walter 1988.
Scott, 1891. SCHOU, M. Special review of lithium in psychiatric
MCCREADIE, R. G. and MORRISON, D. P. The impact therapy and prophylaxis. Journal of Psychiatric
of lithium in Southwest Scotland. I. Demographic Research (1968) 6:67-95.
and clinical findings. British Journal of Psychia- ScHOU, M. Artistic productivity and lithium prophy-
try (1985) 146:70-74. laxis in manic-depressive illness. British Journal
MARSHALL, M. H., NEUMANN, C. P., and RoBINSON, of Psychiatry (1979) 135:97-103.
M. Lithium, creativity, and manic-depressive ill- SHAPIRO, S., SKINNER, E. A., KESSLER, L. G., et al.
ness. Psychosomatics (1970) 11:406-08. Utilization of health and mental health services:
MEYERSON, A., and BOYLE, R. D. Incidence of man- Three epidemiologic catchment area sites. Ar-
ic-depressive psychosis in certain socially impor- chives of General Psychiatry (1984) 4:971-78.
tant families. American Journal of Psychiatry SHAW, E. D., MANN, J. J., STOKES, P. E., and
(1941) 98:11-21. MANEVITZ, A. Z. A. Effects of lithium carbonate
NICOLSON, H. The health of authors. Lancet (1974) on associative productivity and idiosyncrasy in bi-
ii:709-14. polar outpatients. American Journal of Psychia-
NISBET, J. F. The Insanity of Genius. London: Stan- try (1986) 143:1166-69.
ley Paul, 1912. SPITZER, R. L., ENDICOTT,J., and RoBINS, E. Re-
POLATIN, P., and FIEVE, R. R. Patient rejection of search Diagnostic Criteria: Rationale and reliabili-
lithium carbonate prophylaxis. Journal of the ty. Archives of General Psychiatry (1978) 35:773-
American Medical Association (1971) 218:864- 82.
66. STORR, A. The Dynamics of Creation. Penguin, 1976.
PONS, L., NURNBERGER, J. I., and MURPHY, D. Mood- TSANOFF, R. A. The Ways of Genius. Harper, 1949.
independent aberrancies in associative processes VAN PUTTEN, T. Why do patients with manic-depres-
in bipolar affective disorder: An apparent stabiliz- sive illness stop their lithium? Comprehensive
ing effect of lithium. Psychiatry Research (1985) Psychiatry (1975) 16:179-82.
14:315-22. WEISSMAN, M. M., MYERS, J. K. and THOMPSON, W.
PRENTKY, R. Creativity and Psychopathology. D. Depression and treatment in a U.S. urban com·
Praeger, 1980. munity -1975-1976. Archives of General Psychia-
PRENTKY, R. Creativity and psychopathology: Gam- try (1981) 38:417-21.
bolling at the seat of madness. In J. A. Glover, R. WITTKOWER, R., and WITTKOWER, M. Born Under
R. Ronning, and C. R. Reynolds, eds, Handbook of Saturn: The Character and Conduct of Artists.
Creativity: Assessment, Research and Theory. Random House, 1963.
Plenum Press, in press. WOODY, E., and CLARIDGE, G. Psychoticism and cre-
RICHARDS, R. L. Relationships between creativity ativity. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psy-
and psychopathology: An evaluation and interpre- chology (1977) 16:241-48.

134 PSYCHIATRY, Vol. 52, May 1989

Вам также может понравиться