Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

3.

14 Underwater Hearing
A N Popper, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
D R Ketten, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, Woods Hole, MA, USA
ª 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

3.14.1 The Origins of Vertebrate Underwater Hearing 227


3.14.2 The Evolution of Vertebrate Hearing in Water 227
3.14.3 The Inner Ear of Fishes 228
3.14.4 Hearing Capabilities of Fishes 228
3.14.5 Marine Mammals 230
3.14.5.1 Marine Mammal Auditory Systems 230
3.14.5.2 Marine Mammal Audiometrics 232
References 235

Glossary
audiogram A graph of hearing ability conventionally intensity would have a 100 dB re 20 mPa in air but a
displayed as frequency in hertz or kilohertz (abscissa) decibel value in water of approximately 160 dB re
versus sensitivity expressed as sound pressure level 1 mPa.
or intensity measured in decibels (ordinate). echolocation The ability to image features of the
bony fishes All fishes with skeletons made of environment including size, shape, and location of
bone. This is the largest of all vertebrate groups and an object by analyzing echoes from a self-
includes most of the species commonly thought of generated sound. Bats and toothed whales have
as fishes such as goldfish, tuna, etc. the best known animal echolocation or biosonar.
cartilaginous fishes Sharks, skates, and rays. end organ The sensory regions of the inner ear,
The skeleton in these species is made of very particularly the otolithic receptors and the recep-
dense cartilage rather than bone. tors of the semicircular canals.
Cetacea The name of the taxonomic order hertz A unit of sound frequency equal to 1 cycle
encompassing all whales, dolphins, and porpoises s 1 (see also kHz).
derived from the Latin word for whale, cetus. There impedance A complex ratio of power versus flow
are two suborders: Mysticeti (baleen whales) and properties, describing effectively the resistance or
Odontoceti (toothed whales) (see below). efficiency of transfer. Acoustic impedance can be
cochlea The snail-shaped canals and sensory thought of as the impact of the medium on sound
organs of the inner ear that provide hearing by transmission.
transforming sound energy into neural impulses. inner ear The combined organs of hearing and
decibel (dB) A unit of measurement based upon balance, the cochlea and vestibular system.
the logarithm of the ratio of pressure versus an infrasonic Frequencies below 20 Hz, the lower
arbitrary reference pressure. For sound measure- extreme of human hearing.
ments, the decibel scale usually represents sound kilohertz (kHz) Equals 1000 Hz (or 1000 cycles s 1).
pressure level (SPL), but it may also be used as a lagena One of the end organs of the ear in fishes. It
measure of intensity (pressure squared). The refer- is also present in all nonmammalian terrestrial
ence pressures used are different for air (20 mPa) vertebrates.
and underwater (1 mPa) sound measurements; lamprey A group of primitive vertebrates that do
therefore, the decibel level of a sound is properly not have a lower jaw. Lampreys represent the most
stated in the form of n dB re n mPa. Two sounds that primitive group of vertebrates.
are perceived as equally loud may have different mandible Lower jaw bone.
decibel values based on the reference pressures masking The impact of one sound on the ability to
used in the measurements. Two sounds with equal detect another sound.

225
226 Underwater Hearing

middle ear The chamber and structures that pinna The outer ear flap in most mammals,
amplify sound and connect the outer ear and inner including humans. The pinnae assist with focusing
ear. A form of middle ear is found in all terrestrial sound and localization, the ability to detect the
vertebrates. direction of a sound.
Mysticetis The suborder of baleen-bearing or Pinnipedia The marine mammal order comprised
moustached whales comprising all great whales of seals, sea lions, and walruses.
such as blue, finback, right, and humpback whales. rorquals A type of baleen whale that feeds on
Baleen is a series of horny overlapping plates with small fish and zooplankton by lunge-feeding.
feathered edges that mysticetes use in lieu of teeth saccule One of the regions of the ear. In fishes the
for straining food. Mysticetes have not been shown saccule is generally considered to be the major
to echolocate. auditory end organ but it may also be used to
octave An octave is broadly defined as a doubling determine head position relative to gravity. In ter-
of frequency. Thus, a one octave shift from 500 Hz restrial vertebrates and all mammals, the saccule,
is 1000 Hz; from 3000 Hz it is 6000 Hz. Adult along with the utricle, provides information about
humans have on typically an eight-octave func- head position relative to gravity and helps the ani-
tional hearing range of 32 Hz to 16 kHz. mal maintain eye position as head position
octavolateralis system The term that refers to the changes.
combined ear and lateral line system of fishes. semicircular canals Fluid filled canals of the res-
Odontoceti The suborder of cetaceans compris- tibular system of the inner ear. Motion of these
ing the toothed whales, such as sperm whales, fluids excite cells that detect angular acceleration
dolphins, and porpoises. All odontocetes are of the head. Feedback from the canals affects eye
believed to echolocate (to use sound for imaging movements.
the environment). sensory hair cells The fundamental cell of the
ossicles The bones that act as levers in the middle inner ear and lateral line of vertebrates. These cells
ear to amplify and transduce sound energy detect mechanical motion and convert that motion
impinging on the tympanum or eardrum into to a signal that can stimulate the nerves that carry
mechanical forces driving the inner ear responses. information to the brain.
otoconia Calcium carbonate crystals found in the sound localization Determination of the position
otolithic organs of terrestrial vertebrates, cartilagi- of a sound source compared to the position of an
nous fishes, and primitive bony fishes. The crystals animal based on differences in its intensity and
are embedded in a gelatinous matrix. arrival times between ears or receptors.
otolith Calcium carbonate structures found in the swim bladder An air chamber in the abdominal
saccule, lagena, and utricle of most bony fishes. cavity of most bony fishes that is used to adjust
The otolith is several times denser than the rest of buoyancy so that the animal can maintain its posi-
the fish body. Relative motion between the otolith tion in the water column without expenditure of
and the sensory epithelium results in stimulation of energy. The swim bladder in some fishes is used in
the sensory hair cells. The saccular otolith has sound production and in sound detection.
growth rings that can be visualized to determine ultrasonic Sounds above 20 kHz, which is the
fish age. upper extreme of human hearing.
otolithic organs A general term for the saccule, utricle One of the regions of the inner ear. In fishes
lagena, and utricle of the inner ear. Each has a the utricle is involved in both hearing and determi-
sensory epithelium and an overlying calcium car- nation of head position relative to gravity. In
bonate mass (otoconia or otolith, depending on the terrestrial vertebrates and all mammals, the utricle
species). Relative motion between the mass and is primarily a receptor for determination of head
the sensory epithelium causes excitation of the position relative to gravity and helps the animal
sensory hair cells. maintain eye position as the head changes
Otophysi A group of fishes that includes goldfish, position.
catfish, and carp. All members of this mostly vestibular system The part of the inner ear, that
freshwater group of fishes have a series of bones, determines the position and acceleration of the
the Weberian ossicles, which connect the swim head relative to gravity, commonly thought of as the
bladder to the inner ear. balance system. The vestibular system includes the
Underwater Hearing 227

sensory organs of the saccule and utricule plus the as an acoustic coupling device between the
semicircular canals. pressure-receiving swim bladder and the saccule
Weberian ossicles A series of bones between of the inner ear in a way that is functionally ana-
the swim bladder and inner ear of Otophysan logous to the middle ear of terrestrial
fishes that appears to enhance hearing by serving vertebrates.

3.14.1 The Origins of Vertebrate sensory cells and other structures similar to those of
Underwater Hearing modern vertebrates. However, because the ear com-
prises both the vestibular and auditory senses, it is not
The vertebrate ear and hearing evolved in aquatic clear whether the earliest ears were primarily for hear-
animals. Although there is no evidence as to when the ing or for detecting acceleration and head position.
first vertebrates were able to hear, it is likely this Even if ears did not originally arise for hearing,
occurred several hundred million years ago. they likely subsumed this role very early in the evolu-
Once vertebrates came onto land, they had to tion of vertebrates because there were considerable
develop an array of new mechanisms to enable use of selective pressures favoring sound detection (van
their fluid-filled and water-adapted inner ears for Bergeijk, W. A., 1967; Wever, E. G., 1974; Fay, R. R.
detection of airborne sounds. The problem that ter- and Popper, A. N., 2000). In effect, hearing is likely to
restrial vertebrates had to overcome was the difference have arisen because other senses available to the ear-
in impedance between the fluids of the inner ear that liest vertebrates, including vision, touch, and olfaction,
had evolved to detect sound in water and the impe- function effectively only over short ranges. For exam-
dance of sound in air. Without special structures for ple, vision can only function when there is sufficient
capture, transduction, and amplification, the sound light to excite photoreceptors in the eye (Fay, R. R.
energy in air would not be efficiently transferred to and Popper, A. N., 2000) and light does not penetrate
the fluids of the inner ear. As a consequence, airborne very far into the ocean. In contrast to light, sound
sound would be largely undetectable. This need to travels well in water, and the ocean is filled with
accommodate the impedance mismatch of air and sounds (Wenz, G. M., 1964) that can provide animals,
water led to the evolution of the middle ear present through hearing, with a broader range of information
in virtually all terrestrial vertebrates. about the environment at greater distances from the
Conversely, there was also a readaptation when air- receiver than virtually any other sense.
adapted vertebrates once again entered the water. Air- Animals just detect presence of sound to use it
adapted ears of land animals (including human divers) effectively. Particularly for survival, the animal needs
function poorly in water because they are not adapted to be able to determine the direction and, ideally, the
to the higher pressures and faster sound speeds in water relative distance of sounds and to determine which are
(approximately 1550 m s 1 in water versus 330 m s 1 in biologically relevant (e.g., predator, prey, mate, or
air) that are critical elements for discriminating competitor) in the presence of other sounds. Thus, it
between sounds and determining the position of a is possible to hypothesize that very early in the evolu-
sound source in the space. Consequently, aquatic ver- tion of hearing, vertebrates developed mechanisms
tebrates have two levels of adaptation: the original that would enabled them to localize sound sources,
water-derived ear of fishes and a range of partial to discriminate among signals, and detect signals in the
full remodifications of an air-to-water ear in animals presence of masking noise. This implies that in addi-
that have returned to the marine environment. tion to evolving an ear, they also had to evolve the
mechanisms for analyzing multiple features of sounds
(e.g., Fay, R. R. and Popper, A. N., 2000).
3.14.2 The Evolution of Vertebrate It is likely also that use of sound for communica-
Hearing in Water tion among animals of the same species, such as for
mating, evolved later than hearing per se. Although
The earliest vertebrates were jawless species resem- data are limited, it is known that sharks and rays use
bling the modern-day lamprey. Lampreys, like their sound to detect possible prey, but there is no evi-
vertebrate ancestors, have a very recognizable ear with dence that any member of this very successful
228 Underwater Hearing

vertebrate group uses sound for communication several otolithic end organs, two of which, the saccule
(Myrberg, A. A., 2001). Similarly, although goldfish and utricle, are found in all vertebrates. The third end
have excellent hearing (Fay, R. R., 1988), no one has organ, the lagena, is found in all vertebrates except
yet detected that goldfish producing sounds. Marine mammals but may have given rise to the cochlea
mammals by contrast employ complex signals not (Wever, E. G., 1974). The function of the otolithic
only for communication but, in some cases, for end organs in terrestrial animals is to determine head
sophisticated acoustic imaging called echolocation motion relative to gravity. For fishes, however, these
or biosonar as discussed below. organs provide vestibular functions as well as hearing
(Platt, C., 1983; Popper, A. N. et al., 2003).
Fish hearing involves the use of sensory hair cells
that are virtually the same as those found in terrestrial
3.14.3 The Inner Ear of Fishes vertebrates (Coffin, A. et al., 2004). In cartilaginous
fishes (sharks and rays), terrestrial vertebrates, and
The ears of cartilaginous fishes (sharks and rays) and of more primitive bony fishes (e.g., sturgeon and lung-
bony fishes resemble those of terrestrial vertebrates fish), the sensory epithelium containing the sensory
(Figure 1). All vertebrate ears (other than jawless fishes) cells is overlain by a mass of calcium carbonate crystals
have three semicircular canals that are used for deter- (otoconia) embedded in a gelatinous matrix. In the
mination of angular acceleration. They also have majority of bony fishes, however, the calcium carbo-
nate fuses into a single dense structure, the otolith.
Hearing by fishes results when a sound field impinges
(a) on the body and moves tissues more readily than the
denser otoliths. Because the ciliary bundles on the hair
cells are mechanically coupled to the otoliths, the
relative motion between the fish’s body and the otolith
results in bending of the ciliary bundles and stimula-
tion of the hair cells, leading to detection of sound (or
body motion in water).
The precise role of each of the end organs in fish
hearing is not known, and it is likely that the specific
m
o roles vary among the more then 25 000 extant species
l
of fish (Popper, A. N. et al., 2003). For example, the
saccule is thought to be the major auditory end organ
o in goldfish and related species (e.g., most catfish),
s
(b) o whereas in a few species the utricle has a major role
in hearing, as discussed below.

3.14.4 Hearing Capabilities of Fishes

Hearing has been measured in cartilaginous and bony


fishes. These measures include both the range of
l
frequencies detectable and the lowest sound levels
m that are detected at each frequency. While limited,
o these data show that cartilaginous fishes are able to
o
s detect sounds up to about 1000 Hz (Figure 2).
o s
si Although there are hearing data on fewer than 100
o
of the 25 000 species of bony fish, it is clear that there
Figure 1 Illustrations of the right inner ears from the perch is greater variation in the abilities of bony fish to
Stizostedion lucioperca (formerly Lucioperca sandra) (a) and
detect in sound (Figure 2). Some fishes detect sounds
goldfish Carassius auratus (b). Both from Retzius G. (1881).
l, lagena; m, utricular macula; o, otolith; s, saccule; si, sinus only up to 500–1000 Hz, whereas others can detect
impar (to connect left and right ears in goldfish). Anterior is sounds with better sensitivity and at frequencies up
to the left and dorsal to the top. to 3000 or 4000 Hz. A few species are able to detect
Underwater Hearing 229

180 communication evolved the ability to detect higher


frequencies. The most likely reason, at least for fresh-
160
Threshold (dB re 1uPa)

water species, is that the fish evolved in shallow water


140 where there is substantial and rapid attenuation of
120
low frequencies (Rogers, P. H. and Cox, M., 1988). In
Goldfish
order for these species to glean any information
100 Atlantic salmon about their environment using sound, they would
Tuna
80
Atlantic cod
Plaice
need adaptations to detect the higher frequencies
Bull shark that propagate over longer distances in such waters.
American shad
60 Silver perch Specializations for enhancement of hearing gen-
Atlantic croaker
40 erally involve mechanisms that acoustically couple
100 1000 10000 100000 an air bubble, such as the swim bladder, with the ear.
Frequency (Hz) It is likely that the swim bladder evolved to enable
Figure 2 Auditory thresholds for a variety of different fish fishes to maintain neutral buoyancy, thus cutting
and shark species. Note that the audiograms were all down on energy used to maintain a particular depth
determined using different methods. Moreover, while in the water column. However, mechanisms have
thresholds are most likely correctly represented using
pressure values for hearing specialists such as goldfish and
evolved that enable many fish species to use this
silver perch, it is not clear whether pressure or particle organ for both sound production (Zelick, R. et al.,
motion (not shown here) would be more suitable for the 1999) and hearing (Ladich, F. and Popper, A. N.,
nonspecialists. All data from Fay, R. R. (1988) other than 2004).
silver perch (Ramcharitar, J. U. et al., 2004) and Atlantic The nature of the coupling between the swim
croaker (Ramcharitar, J. U. and Popper, A. N., 2004).
bladder and inner ear varies among hearing specia-
lists. The best-known coupling is found in the fishes
of the superorder Otophysi, a group of primarily
sounds well into the infrasonic and ultrasonic ranges
freshwater fishes that includes goldfish and catfish.
(reviewed in Popper, A. N. et al., 2003; Ladich, F. and
These species have a series of bones, the Weberian
Popper, A. N., 2004).
ossicles, connecting the swim bladder directly to the
Fishes that detect sounds above about 1500 Hz are
inner ear. It has been hypothesized that motion of the
often referred to as hearing specialists, whereas those
walls of the swim bladder during sound stimulation
that do not possess such capabilities are called non-
results in motion of the Weberian ossicles that, in
specialists (or generalists). It has been suggested that in
turn, moves the fluids of the inner ear (Alexander, R.
the hearing specialists, one or more of the otolith
McN., 1962) in a manner analogous to the function of
organs may respond to sound pressure as well as to the mammalian middle ear bones.
acoustic particle motion (e.g., Popper, A. N. et al., 2003). Beyond the Otophysi, there are diverse connec-
The response to sound pressure is thought to be tions between the swim bladder or other air bubbles
mediated by mechanical coupling between the swim and the inner ear among taxonomically distinct spe-
bladder (a gas-filled chamber in the abdominal cavity cies. These may include anterior projections of the
that enables a fish to maintain neutral buoyancy) or swim bladder that terminate in contact with the
other gas bubbles and the inner ear. With this coupling, ear and/or newly evolved air bubbles located near
the expansion and contraction of the gas-filled struc- the ear.
ture in a pressure field is translated to the ear. In One significant point about all of these structures is
nonspecialists, however, the lack of a swim bladder that multiple mechanisms evolved to enhance hearing
or its lack of coupling to the ear probably results in that are spread throughout the fish taxa. Indeed, there
any signal from the swim bladder attenuating before it are instances of closely related species with very dif-
gets to the ear. As a consequence, these fishes detect ferent hearing capabilities and air bubble connections
little or no pressure component of the sound (Popper, to the ear. For example, one species of the squirrel fish
A. N. et al., 2003). Hearing generalists are most sensi- family (Holocentridae) only detects sounds to about
tive to sounds produced by relatively near sources. 1000 Hz, whereas another species that lives in the
While some hearing specialists, such as catfish, use same caves detects sounds to over 3000 Hz (Coombs,
sound for communication (as do some hearing gen- S. and Popper, A. N., 1979). The two species have
eralists), others do not. Then here has been some substantial differences in ear structure and in the
question about why fishes that do not use sound for relationship between the swim bladder and ear.
230 Underwater Hearing

Fishes of the commercially important family cetaceans (dolphins and whales) and the pinnipeds
Sciaenidae (croakers and drums) also have great (seals, sea lions, and walruses). These will form the
diversity in ear structure, in the relationship between focus of this discussion because there are sufficient
the ear and swim bladder, and in hearing capabilities data on hearing in some of their component species
(Ramcharitar, J. U. et al., 2001). to provide insights into how aquatic ears re-evolved
Although the saccule and lagena are most often from air-adapted ancestors. They also include species
implicated in fish hearing, there are a number of that range from totally aquatic to amphibious to pri-
cases in which the utricle appears to have taken on a marily land (or ice), air-adapted ears.
special role to enhance detection of low- or high- Cetaceans are divided into two distinct groups. The
frequency sounds. There is evidence that some marine Odontoceti (or toothed whales) are predatory high-
catfish use low-frequency echolocation to glean infor- frequency echolocators typified by the well-studied
mation about their environment (Tavolga, W. N., bottlenose dolphins. The Mysticeti (or baleen whales)
1976), much as dolphins use high-frequency sounds include the largest species ever recorded, the blue
for biosonar. Echolocating catfish have hypertrophied whale, as well as some smaller rorqual whale species.
utricles that are hypothesized to function like a low- All cetaceans live entirely in water and may spend as
frequency accelerometer to detect echoes of their own much as 80% of their time submerged. Pinnipeds, by
sounds in order to navigate through the environment contrast, are truly amphibious and all species spend
(Popper, A. N. and Tavolga, W. N., 1981). some portion of their time and do key activities (e.g.,
At the other end of the frequency range, the breeding and rearing young) on land. The sirenians
clupeiform fishes (e.g., herring, anchovies, and sar- (manatees and dugongs) are, like cetaceans, obligate
dines) have utricles unlike those found in any other aquatics, but to date few manatees have been studied
vertebrate. Recent evidence has implicated this spe- and there are no hearing data on dugongs. There are no
cialization for the detection of sounds up to 180 kHz hearing or ear data on any member of the two remain-
in a number of clupeids including the American shad ing and most recently evolved marine mammal groups,
(Mann, D. A. et al., 2001). The adaptive significance of the sea otters and polar bears.
this capability appears to be for the detection of
sounds produced by the echolocating dolphins
3.14.5.1 Marine Mammal Auditory Systems
(Plachta, D. T. T. and Popper, A. N., 2003) that
often prey upon schools of clupeids. Significant structural differences between marine and
The bottom line is perhaps the exceptional diver- land mammal ears are primarily found in the outer
sity of structures that fishes have evolved that enable and inner ears. All marine mammals have special
them to use sound to obtain environmental informa- adaptations of the external ear (valvular canal closure
tion. Although we are at the earliest stages of mechanisms, semioccluded canals, and wax plugs)
understanding this diversity, an example of the critical and middle ear (heavily vascularized and innervated
use of sound includes evidence that deep-sea fishes middle ear mucosa, diminished or absent mastoids,
evolved specializations to enhance hearing at depths and broad-bore Eustachian tubes) consistent with
where there is little or no light (e.g., Buran, B. N. et al., deep, rapid diving and long-term submersion.
2005). More recently, it has become apparent that some However, recent data suggest that marine mammals
larval fishes use sounds produced by reefs to find places retain air-filled middle ears and the same fundamen-
to settle (Simpson, S. D. et al., 2005). Data is currently tal inner ear components and hearing mechanisms as
available on only a few extant species. It is likely that terrestrial species.
further study will continue to demonstrate that hearing Cetaceans have the most derived ear of any mam-
not only evolved underwater but also that the broadest mal, marine or otherwise. As cetaceans evolved into
adaptations are in this environment as well. obligate aquatic mammals, their head tissues were
pushed, inflated, and telescoped (Barnes, L. G. et al.,
1985) to form a novel cranium with elongated jaws
3.14.5 Marine Mammals (rostrum), scooped frontal bones, and nares trans-
posed upward and rearward, dorsal to the eyes.
Marine mammals, like fishes, encompass diverse Telescoping may have been driven head reshaping
groups of species, although it is arguable that they primarily to allow minimal head exposure at the
are less diverse than fishes. Marine mammals comprise surface, but the ear was effectively dragged along
five vertebrate groups. The two best known are the with other skull elements to new positions in the
Underwater Hearing 231

head, and profound structural changes for sound hearing. First, the ears have a three- to fivefold
transduction took place in virtually all parts of the greater separation than if they retained the intracra-
peripheral auditory system. nial position common in land mammals, which means
In cetaceans, sound most likely arrives at the ear they are separated to match the increased speed of
via specialized fatty tissues that are coupled to the sound in water. Second, both the middle and inner
middle ear and that act as both pinnae and external ears are acoustically isolated from most head tissues
canals (Figure 3). Norris K. S. (1969) speculated that that could conduct sound to the ear which improves
a fatty core in the posterior third of the mandible acts sound localization which is critical for echolocation.
as a preferential path for underwater sound. Recent Odontocetes and mysticetes have middle ears
studies have determined that these perimandibular adapted for radically different frequencies. Odonto-
fats have complex shapes that effectively form inter- cetes follow the ultrasonic land mammal pattern but
nal pinnae (Ketten, D. R. et al., 2004). They also are carry it to extremes with multiple stiffening agents for
composed of very unusual lipids that have physical the middle ear ossicles that include both well-devel-
properties consistent with acting as a specialized oped stapedial ligaments and incudal struts (Ketten,
channel matched to the acoustic impedance of under- D. R., 2000). Further, the tympanic membrane is sub-
water sound (Koopman, H. N. et al., 2006). stantially reduced and relatively rigid in all
Cetacean middle and inner ears in general were odontocetes. Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
reshaped also to meet the demands of underwater have recently been shown to have middle ear stiff-
physics of sound. They became encased in distinc- nesses that are sixfold greater than those of land
tive, shell-shaped bony capsules (tympanic and mammals (Miller, B. S. et al., 2006). All of this is
periotic bullae) that moved outside the brain case consistent with audiometric data for odontocetes (dis-
and, at least in most modern odontocetes, lost all cussed below) that shows broad hearing ranges from
firm bony connections to the skull. Instead, the tym- about 150 Hz to well over 100 kHz, with peak hearing
panoperiotic bullae, and thus the middle and inner over 40 kHz for most dolphins species.
ears of dolphins, are suspended by ligaments or struts Mysticetes, by contrast, have low-frequency,
in a spacious, soft tissue-filled extracranial sinus. This mass-dominated middle ears. Unlike the small, stiff
accomplishes two critical things for underwater middle ear and eardrum of dolphins, the mysticetes

Inner ear

Inner mandibular fats

Posterior
fats

Outer mandibular
fats

Figure 3 A three-dimensional reconstruction based on computerized tomography data of a bottlenose dolphin. The skull,
including the upper and lower jaws or rostrum, are transparent to show the brain (pink) inside the skull and the ear bones (red).
The ear bones, housing middle and inner ear structures, are outside the skull but inside the lower jaw (mandible) and are
connected to the three fat bodies (orange) (Ketten, D. R. et al., 2004, reprinted with permission, ª Ketten, D. R., 2003). These
fats form rabbit ears that are oriented anteriorly and slightly ventral and provide a low-impedance path to the middle ear
compared to other head tissues. Reprinted by permission of Ketten, D. R.
232 Underwater Hearing

have a thick, multilayered eardrum that is a unique range from 25 mm at the base to 5 mm at the apex or
elongated pouch called the glove finger (Reysenbach double those of most other species. Basilar membrane
de Haan, F. W., 1956). This unusual structure not widths vary in the opposite direction; basal widths
only reduces the probability of tympanic membrane are 30–50 mm at the base and increase to 300–500 mm
perforation in diving but also increases middle apically. Thus the odontocete basilar membrane is
ear volume and is coupled to a disproportionately relatively thick and narrow. It therefore resembles a
large middle ear cavity, all of which are consistent stiff beam throughout its length.
with the expectation of good infrasonic to low-fre- Mysticete membranes are thin rectangles
quency hearing in most mysticetes (see Watkins, W. throughout, with base-to-apex membrane ratios of
A. and Wartzok, D., 1985 for vocalizations and 0.1 : 0.001 based on thicknesses from 7 mm at the
Ketten, D. R., 2000 for anatomical correlates). base to 2 mm at the apex and widths ranging from
There is no evidence to date of echolocation by 100 mm at the base (similar to the base in humans) to
mysticetes, and whether they preceded, succeeded, 2200 mm at the apex or threefold that of humans and
or coevolved with odontocetes is not yet clear 1.2-fold that of elephants (Ketten, D. R., 2000). These
(Thewissen, J. G. M., 1998). ratios are directly correlated with the resonant fre-
Thus, as a group, mysticete and odontocete mid- quencies along the membrane, whereas the gradients
dle ears span infra- to ultrasonic hearing with reflect the octave range, which for all cetaceans
adaptations that clearly couple these ears so effec- implies hearing covering 10–12 octaves.
tively to underwater sound that they may no longer Pinniped ears are more conventional than those of
be able to sense airborne sound at anything except cetaceans. External pinnae are reduced or absent in
very high levels. some species, but conventional external ear canals
There is no clear consensus on how whale and are present and functional at least for in-air hearing.
dolphin middle ears function underwater. Both con- One critical amphibious adaptation in seals and sea
ventional ossicular motion and translational bone lions is a spiral cartilage–muscle sphincter that seals
conduction have been proposed for dolphins (see the meatus (Møhl, B., 1968), implying that the canal
review in Ketten, D. R., 2000), and the role of the can be voluntarily closed. Whether the external canal
middle ear in cetaceans therefore remains an open remains air-filled, collapses, or floods when under-
question. water remains unclear; neither is it known if the canal
Cetacean inner ears have a well-developed spiral is a functional sound path to the ear in water.
cochlea but substantially reduced vestibular systems. The tympanic and periotic bones of pinnipeds
In some ways the cetacean cochlea is in fact hyper- resemble those of terrestrial mammals. The ear
trophied in having more cells and more complex bones are not acoustically isolated as with those of
membrane structures than most land mammals. All cetaceans, and the middle ears only have a moderate
whales and dolphins typically have more than 2000 layer of cavernous tissue with a wide range of ossi-
ganglion cells mm 1 of basilar membrane, which is cular chain configurations ranging from very massive
double to triple the ganglion cell densities of other (elephant seal) to moderately stiff (harbor seal;
mammals (Ketten, D. R., 2000). By contrast, the ves- Møhl, B., 1968; Marsh, S. E. et al., 2001), implying a
tibular and optic nerves in cetaceans, in comparison to wide variation in peak frequencies among species.
those of most land mammals, are depauperate. Most pinniped inner ears resemble those of terres-
Cetacean vestibular systems have both far smaller trial high-frequency generalists with evidence for
canal radii than other mammals (the blue whale semi- some ultrasonic, but not well-developed, high ultra-
circular canals are comparable to those of mice) and sonic hearing (Solntseva, G. N., 1990). At least one
very few nerve fibers. Vestibular fibers comprise less species, the elephant seal, has a membrane structure
than 5% of the acousticovestibular nerve cetaceans in consistent with better low-frequency hearing than
contrast to 40–50% in most terrestrial mammals. most other pinnipeds (Wartzok, D. and Ketten, D. R.,
In odontocetes, acute ultrasonic hearing is 1999)
achieved by multiple stiffening elements in the
inner ear, including doubled bony membrane suspen-
3.14.5.2 Marine Mammal Audiometrics
sions and exceptional thickness-to-width basilar
membrane ratios (1.0 : 0.01) that exceed even those Audiograms are available for 11 of the nearly 80
of bats (Ketten, D. R. and Wartzok, D., 1990). cetacean species (Au, W. W. L., 2000), but the data
Odontocete basilar membrane thicknesses typically are only from smaller odontocetes. Consistent with
Underwater Hearing 233

2D Graph 4 200 kHz, burst-pulse click trains, and constant-


160 frequency (CF) or frequency-modulated (FM) whis-
Amazon River dolphin
Beluga dolphin tles with peak spectra from 4 to 16 kHz (Tyack, P.,
140 Harbor porpoise
Threshold (dB re 1 uPa)

False killer whale


Bottlenose dolphin
1999) that appear to be used for intraspecific
120 communication.
100 Functional significance for species variations in
echolocation and communication signals has not
80
been fully investigated, but habitat and peak spectra
60 of echolocation signals are strongly correlated
(Ketten, D. R. and Wartzok, D., 1990). Odontocetes
40
that employ signals above 100 kHz tend to be inshore,
20 estuarine, and freshwater dolphins that hunt in rela-
1 10 100
tively shallow water environments such as harbors or
Frequency (kHz)
rivers. These species locate small fish amidst tangled
Figure 4 Hearing thresholds for a variety of odontocete roots and stems in heavily silted waters. They have
cetaceans. Data for the Amazon River dolphin and the
auditory systems adapted primarily to receive and
beluga dolphin (Popov, V. and Supin, A., 1990) were
measured using auditory brainstem response methods. analyze ultrahigh frequencies which have short
Data from the harbor porpoise (Kastelein, R. et al., 2002), wavelengths consistent with echolocating small,
false killer whale (Thomas, J. A. et al., 1988), and bottlenose nearby objects.
dolphin (Johnson, C. S., 1967) were all obtained with Off-shore animals are generally larger, travel in
behavioral measures.
highly social pods, and forage in deeper, clearer
waters. These species have broader frequency-adapted
ears consistent with processing ultrasonic but lower-
the anatomically derived basilar membrane esti- frequency peak spectra and longer ranging echoloca-
mates, electrophysiological and behavioral measures tion signals as well as even lower mid-sonic frequency
show functional hearing ranges that span 10 octaves whistles for communication with podmates.
or more and excellent ultrasonic capacities for the Bottlenose dolphins, the most commonly studied
species tested (Figure 4). Best thresholds measured to dolphin species, perform 6–8 dB better than an ideal
date typically range from 40 dB re 1 mPa to 60 dB re receiver. Most species are capable of 1–4 azimuthal
1 mPa, with peak sensitivities ranging from 12 kHz in resolution, although as little as 0.5 has been
the killer whale (Orcinus orca) to well over 100 kHz in reported for harbor porpoises. Minimal intensity dis-
several species including the freshwater Amazonian crimination in bottlenose dolphins (1 dB) equals
boto (Inia geoffrensis) and the harbor porpoise human values, and temporal discrimination (8%
(Phocoena phocoena; Nachtigall, P. E. et al., 2000; of signal duration) is superior to that of humans.
Kastelein, R. et al., 2002). Frequency discrimination in dolphins is significantly
Ultrasonic echolocation signals produced by dol- better than in most mammals and is best, unsurpris-
phins have been recorded from 21 species and are ingly, between 5 kHz and 60 kHz in the most
highly species specific, although active echolocation common species which are mostly offshore (Popper,
has been demonstrated unequivocally in only the 11 A. N., 1980). Intensity discrimination thresholds
species tested to date (Nachtigall, P. E. et al., 2000). (ITD) for odontocetes equal those of any land mam-
Virtually all parameters echolocation signals can be mal and decline with increasing sound level.
modified by the animal, including pulse repetition Measured values range from 1–2 dB for bottlenose
rate, interval, intensity, and spectra, given sufficient dolphins up to 3–5 dB for harbor porpoises and the
background interference (Au, W. W. L., 2000). Amazonian boto.
However, the dominant normal click spectra for Detection of a source position, known as localiza-
each species is relatively constant and ranges tion, is a third feature at which odontocetes excel.
between 12 and 120 kHz, with source levels typically Two cues are critical for mammalian auditory system
near 120–140 dB re 1 mPa but reportedly as high localization: differences in arrival time (interaural
as 230 dB re 1 mPa (summary table in Wartzok, D. time) and sound level (interaural intensity). On
and Ketten, D. R., 1999). Species recorded to date land, the normal sound path is around the head,
produce species-stereotypic broadband echolocation pinna to pinna, and thus the interaural time differ-
clicks, with peak energy between 10 kHz and ences (ITDs) are calculated based on the distance
234 Underwater Hearing

between receiver points in the head. In most mam- and Ketten, D. R., 1999). Mysticete sounds are char-
mals, this is the distance measured around the head acterized as low-frequency moans (fundamental
from the center of each pinna divided by the speed of frequency below 200 Hz); simple calls (impulsive, nar-
sound in air. In aquatic animals, however, sound rowband signals below 1 kHz); complex calls
could travel through the head via tissues with den- (broadband pulsatile AM or FM signals); and complex,
sities close to sea water. seasonally varied songs (Watkins, W. A. and Wartzok,
As discussed above, anatomical evidence suggests D., 1985). Infrasonic signals, typically in the 10–16 Hz
that specialized jaw fats act as preferential, low-impe- range, are well documented in at least two mysticetes,
dance channels that are able to carry sound to the blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) and fin whales
middle ear in odontocetes. Behavioral experiments (Balaenoptera physalus ; Watkins, W. A., 1981). It has
(Brill, R. L. et al., 1988) found that echolocation by been speculated that mysticetes use infrasonic signals
bottlenose dolphins is impaired if the jaws are blocked for long-distance communication or topographic scale
from receiving sounds by neoprene shields. These echolocation, but there is not as yet any clear evidence
results suggest that interjaw distances are the appro- or field tests that support the idea.
priate measure for ITD values in odontocetes. The Pinnipeds are divided into three groups: Phocidae:
interjaw or interear distances of dolphins seem large true or non-eared seals; Otariidae: eared seals; and
but because of the speed of sound in water, they equate Odobenidae: walruses. Audiometric data show they
to the ear separations of larger bats, which is consistent vary from partially to fully adapted for underwater
with equally good angular resolution in both groups. hearing (Figure 5). However, to date it is not certain
In addition to localization, detection of signals in how most species accomplish hearing in both media.
noise is also critical for echolocation in natural envi- One possibility is that pinnipeds have dual systems
ronments. Critical bands and critical ratios are two operating independently for aquatic and airborne sti-
measures of the ability of an ear to overcome masking, muli and in different frequency ranges related to
the phenomenon in which competing sounds elimi- behaviors in each medium (e.g., feeding in water versus
nate or degrade perception of another (Yost, W. A., pup location on land). A second possibility is that
1994). Odontocetes have more critical bands than most pinnipeds are adapted primarily for one environment
mammals and smaller critical ratios than humans or or the other or have one system for hearing in both
even many bats, making them better than most mam- media but not optimized for either. The current data
mals at detecting signals in noise. The best critical suggest that among tested species both of these possible
ratios to date (8–40 dB) are for the false killer whale strategies have evolved.
(Pseudorca crassidens) which is also the species that has The 11 pinniped species tested to date have peak
performed best in echolocation discrimination tasks sensitivities between 10 and 30 kHz, with a functional
(Nachtigall, P. E. et al., 2000; Supin, A. Y. et al., 2003). high-frequency limits up to 75 kHz (Richardson, W. J.
Recent studies using auditory evoked potentials
(AEP) or auditory brainstem responses (ABR) employ-
ing amplitude-modulated stimuli (AMS) in bottlenose 180
Northern elephant seal
dolphins and false killer whales have demonstrated that California sea lion
160 Harbor seal
Threshold (dB re 1 uPa)

odontocetes extract envelope features at higher mod- Harp seal

ulation frequencies than other mammals, including 140


humans where they are important for speech percep-
120
tion, particularly at low frequencies (Dolphin, W. F.,
1995; Supin, A. Y. et al., 2003). These data suggest that 100
auditory central nervous system adaptations in toothed
80
whales may include regions specialized for both low
and high frequencies. 60
There are no direct behavioral or electrophysiolo-
0.1 1 10 100
gic hearing data for mysticetes, but deep ocean Frequency (kHz)
stationary arrays and deployable recording tags have
Figure 5 Underwater behavioral response auditory
substantially increased knowledge of mysticete
thresholds for four species of pinniped. Northern elephant
sounds. In general, mysticete vocalizations are signifi- seal and California sea lion from Kastak D. and
cantly lower in frequency than those of odontocetes, as Schusterman R. (1998); Harbor seal from Møhl B. (1964);
hypothesized from anatomical evidence (Wartzok, D. Harp seal from Terhune J. M. and Ronald K. (1972).
Underwater Hearing 235

et al., 1995, Kastelein, R. et al., 2002; Southall, B. L. et al., other mammals, are consistent with conclusions from
2004). Low-frequency functional limits are not yet ear models and with the general characteristics of
well established for most pinnipeds, but parallel stu- audiograms in tested species.
dies of harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) versus elephant
seals (Mirounga angustirostris) suggest that elephant
seals have significantly better underwater low-fre-
References
quency hearing sensitivity than other pinnipeds
tested to date, with functional hearing down to 75 Hz
Alexander, R. McN. 1962. The structure of the Weberian
(Kastak, D. and Schusterman, R., 1998). apparatus in the Cyprini. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond.
In air pinniped audiograms have peak sensitivities 139, 451–473.
at slightly lower frequencies than in water (3–10 kHz; Au, W. W. L. 2000. Echolocation in Dolphins. In: Hearing by
Whales and Dolphins (eds. W. W. L. Au, R. R. Fay, and
Kastak, D. and Schusterman, R., 1998). Thresholds for A. N. Popper), pp. 364–408. Springer.
airborne sounds are poorer, on average, in phocids, the Barnes, L. G., Domning, D. P., and Ray, C. E. 1985. Status of
seals, which have no overt pinna, but there is little studies on fossil marine mammals. Mar. Mamm. Sci.
1, 15–53.
difference in aerial versus underwater audiograms for Brill, R. L., Sevenich, M. L., Sullivan, T., Sustman, J. D., and
otariids, the eared seals and sea lions. This implies that Witt, R. E. 1988. Behavioral evidence for hearing through the
phocids may be better adapted for underwater hear- lower jaw by an echolocating dolphin, Tursiops truncatus.
Mar. Mamm. Sci. 4, 223–230.
ing, whereas otariids evolved a single system that Buran, B. N., Deng, X., and Popper, A. N. 2005. Structural
operates adequately but not optimally in air or water. variation in the inner ears of four deep-sea elopomorph
In frequency discrimination and localization tasks, fishes. J. Morphol. 265, 215–225.
Coffin, A., Kelley, M., Manley, G. A., and Popper, A. N. 2004.
pinnipeds perform more poorly than odontocetes. Evolution of Sensory Hair Cells. In: Evolution of the
Angular resolution ranges from 1.5 to 9.0 , but Vertebrate Auditory System (eds. G. A. Manley,
most have 4.0 to 6.0 of resolution (Richardson, W. A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay), pp. 55–94. Springer.
Coombs, S. and Popper, A. N. 1979. Hearing differences
J. et al., 1995). Minimal intensity discrimination (3 dB) among Hawaiian squirrelfishes (family Holocentridae) related
in California sea lions is poorer than that of dolphins to differences in the peripheral auditory system. J. Comp.
or humans, but frequency discrimination in phocids Physiol. 132, 203–207.
Dolphin, W. F. 1995. Steady-State Auditory-Evoked Potentials
and sea lions is similar to that of bottlenose dolphins. in Three Cetacean Species Elicited Using Amplitude-
Critical ratios for pinnipeds are generally equal to Modulated Stimuli. In: Sensory Systems of Aquatic
those of dolphins and humans. Like odontocetes, Mammals (eds. R. A. Kastelein, J. A. Thomas, and
P. E. Nachtigall), pp. 25–47. DeSpil Publishers.
pinnipeds in water could have small-to-large func- Fay, R. R. 1988. Hearing in Vertebrates, a Psychophysics
tional inter-ear distances depending upon whether Databook. Hill-Fay Associates.
they use the external canal, specialized tissues, or Fay, R. R. and Popper, A. N. 2000. Evolution of hearing in
vertebrates: the inner ears and processing. Hear. Res.
the inner ear as the sound reception point under- 149, 1–10.
water. However, to date, there are no clear data on Johnson, C. S. 1967. Sound Detection Thresholds in Marine
the configuration of any pinniped underwater sound Mammals. In: Marine Bio-Acoustics (ed. W. N. Tavolga),
pp. 247–260. Pergamon.
reception path. Kastak, D. and Schusterman, R. 1998. Low-frequency
Most pinniped sounds recorded to date are in the amphibious hearing in pinnipeds: methods, measurements,
sonic range, and the repertoires are complex, varying noise, and ecology. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103, 2216–2228.
Kastelein, R., Buskoek, P., Hageoom, M., Au, W., and
by context, sex, and season (Watkins, W. A. and de Haan, D. 2002. Audiogram of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena
Wartzok, D., 1985; Richardson, W. J. et al., 1995). phocoena) measured with narrow-band frequency modulated
Call descriptions run the gamut from grunts to signals. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 335–346.
Ketten, D. R. 2000. Cetacean Ears. In: Hearing by Whales and
rasps, rattles, and warbles in addition to whistles, Dolphins (eds. W. Au, R. R. Fay, and A. N. Popper),
clicks, and pulses ranging from 100 Hz to 15 kHz, pp. 43–108. Springer.
with peak spectra below 5 kHz. Like cetaceans, Ketten, D. R. and Wartzok, D. 1990. Three-Dimensional
Reconstructions of the Dolphin Cochlea. In: Sensory Abilities of
source levels in water are commonly near 130 dB re Cetaceans: Laboratory and Field Evidence (eds. J. A. Thomas
1 mPa although levels as high as 193 dB re 1 mPa have and R. A. Kastelein), pp. 81–105. Plenum.
been reported (Richardson, W. J. et al., 1995). Despite Ketten, D. R., Simmons, J., Hubbard, A. E., and Mountain, D. A.
2004. Dolphin and bat sonar: convergence, divergence, or
producing click-like sounds, there is no evidence that parallelism. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 2517.
pinnipeds are able to echolocate. Infrasonic to seismic Koopman, H. N., Budge, S. M., Ketten, D. R., and Iverson, S. J.
sounds are produced by northern elephant seals in 2006. The topographical distribution of lipids inside the
mandibular fat bodies of odontocetes: remarkable
air. Thus, both air and underwater vocalizations for complexity and consistency. IEEE J. Ocean. Eng.
these amphibious species, like those of cetaceans and 31, 95–106.
236 Underwater Hearing

Ladich, F. and Popper, A. N. 2004. Parallel Evolution in Fish Rogers, P. H. and Cox, M. 1988. Underwater Sound as a
Hearing Organs. In: Evolution of the Vertebrate Auditory Biological Stimulus. In: Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals
System (eds. G. A. Manley, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay), (eds. J. Atema, R. R. Fay, A. N. Popper, and W. N. Tavolga),
pp. 95–127. Springer. pp. 131–149. Springer.
Mann, D. A., Higgs, D. M., Tavolga, W. N., Souza, M. J., and Simpson, S. D., Meekan, M., Montgomery, J., McCauley, R.,
Popper, A. N. 2001. Ultrasound detection by clupeiform and Jeffs, A. 2005. Homeward sound. Science 308, 221.
fishes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 3048–3054. Solntseva, G. N. 1990. Formation of an Adaptive Structure of the
Marsh, S. E., Fischer, I., Arruda, J., and Ketten, D. R. 2001. Peripheral Part of the Auditory Analyzer in Aquatic, Echo-
Morphometric analyses of hearing in two families of Locating Mammals during Ontogenesis. In: Sensory Abilities of
pinnipeds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110, 2749. Cetaceans: Laboratory and Field Evidence (eds. J. A. Thomas
Miller, B. S., Zosuls, A. L., Ketten, D. R., and Mountain, D. A. and R. A. Kastelein), pp. 363–384. Plenum.
2006. Middle ear stiffness of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops Southall, B. L., Schusterman, R. J., Kastak, D., and
truncatus). IEEE J. Ocean. Eng. 31, 87–94. Kastak, C. R. 2004. Underwater hearing thresholds in
Møhl, B. 1964. Preliminary studies on hearing in seals. pinnipeds measured over a 6-year period. J. Acoust. Soc.
Videnskabelige Meddelelser Fra Dansk Naturhistorisk Am. 116, 2504.
Forening I Kjobenhaven 127, 283–294. Supin, A. Y., Nachtigall, P. E., Pawloski, J. L., and Au, W. W. L.
Møhl, B. 1968. Hearing in Seals. In: The Behavior and 2003. Evoked potential recording during echolocation in a
Physiology of Pinnipeds (eds. R. Harrison, R. Hubbard, false killer whale Pseudorca crassidens (L). J. Acoust. Soc.
R. Peterson, C. Rice, and R. Schusterman), pp. 172–195. Am. 113, 2408–2411.
Appleton-Century. Tavolga, W. N. 1976. Acoustic Obstacle Detection in the Sea
Myrberg, A. A., Jr. 2001. The acoustic biology of Catfish (Arius felis). In: Sound Reception in Fish
elasmobranches. Environ. Biol. Fish 60, 31–45. (eds. A. Schuijf and A. D. Hawkins), pp. 185–204. Elsevier.
Nachtigall, P. E., Lemonds, D. W., and Roitblat, H. L. 2000. Terhune, J. M. and Ronald, K. 1972. The harp seal (Pagophilus
Psychoacoustic Studies of Dolphin and Whale Hearing. groenlandicus (Erxleben, 1777). III. The underwater
In: Hearing by Whales and Dolphins (eds. W. W. L. Au, audiogram Can. J. Zool. 50, 465–469.
R. R. Fay, and A. Popper), pp. 330–361. Springer. Thewissen, J. G. M. 1998. The Emergence of Whales,
Norris, K. S. 1969. The Echolocation of Marine Mammals. Evolutionary Patterns in the Origin of Cetacea. Plenum.
In: The Biology of Marine Mammals (ed. H. J. Andersen), Thomas, J. A., Chun, N., and Au, W. W. L. 1988. Underwater
pp. 391–423. Academic Press. audiogram of a false killer whale (Pseudorca crassidens). J.
Plachta, D. T. T. and Popper, A. N. 2003. Evasive responses of Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 936–940.
American shad (Alosa sapidissima) to ultrasonic stimuli. Tyack, P. 1999. Communication and Cognition. In: Biology of
Acoust. Res. Lett. Online 4, 25–30. Marine Mammals (eds. J. Reynolds and S. Rommel),
Platt, C. 1983. The Peripheral Vestibular System in Fishes. pp. 287–323. Smithsonian Institution Press.
In: Fish Neurobiology (eds. R. G. Northcutt and R. E. Davis), van Bergeijk, W. A. 1967. The Evolution of Vertebrate Hearing.
pp. 89–124. University of Michigan Press. In: Contributions to Sensory Physiology (ed. W. D. Neff),
Popov, V. and Supin, A. 1990. Electrophysiological Studies of pp. 1–49. Academic Press.
Hearing in Some Cetaceans and a Manatee. In: Sensory Wartzok, D. and Ketten, D. R. 1999. Marine Mammal Sensory
Abilities of Cetaceans: Laboratory and Field Evidence Systems. In: Biology of Marine Mammals (eds. J. Reynolds
(eds. J. A. Thomas and R. A. Kasterlein), pp. 405–415. Plenum. and S. Rommel), pp. 117–175. Smithsonian Institution Press.
Popper, A. N. 1980. Sound Emission and Detection by Watkins, W. A. 1981. The activities and underwater sounds of
Delphinids. In: Cetacean Behavior: Mechanisms and fin whales. Sci. Rep. Whales Res. Inst. 33, 83–117.
Functions (ed. L. M. Herman), pp. 1–52. Wiley. Watkins, W. A. and Wartzok, D. 1985. Sensory biophysics of
Popper, A. N. and Tavolga, W. N. 1981. Structure and function marine mammals. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 1, 219–260.
of the ear of the marine catfish, Arius felis. J. Comp. Physiol. Wenz, G. M. 1964. Curious Noises and the Sonic Environment in
144, 27–34. the Ocean. In: Marine Bio-Acoustics (ed. W. N. Tavolga),
Popper, A. N., Fay, R. R., Platt, C., and Sand, O. 2003. Sound pp. 101–119. Pergamon.
Detection Mechanisms and Capabilities of Teleost Fishes. Wever, E. G. 1974. The Evolution of Vertebrate Hearing.
In: Sensory Processing in Aquatic Environments In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vol. V/1 Auditory
(eds. S. P. Collin and N. J. Marshall), pp. 3–38. Springer. System. (eds. W. D. Keidel and W. D. Neff), pp. 423–454.
Ramcharitar, J. U., Deng, X., Ketten, D., and Popper, A. N. Springer.
2004. Form and function in the unique inner ear of a teleost Yost, W. A. 1994. Fundamentals of Hearing: An Introduction,
fish: the silver perch (Bairdiella chrysoura). J. Comp. Neurol. 3rd edn. Academic Press.
475, 531–539. Zelick, R., Mann, D., and Popper, A. N. 1999. Acoustic
Ramcharitar, J., Higgs, D. M., and Popper, A. N. 2001. Sciaenid Communication in Fishes and Frogs. In: Comparative
inner ears: a study in diversity. Brain Behav. Evol. 58, 152–162. Hearing: Fish and Amphibians (eds. R. R. Fay and
Ramcharitar, J. U. and Popper, A. N. 2004. Masked auditory A. N. Popper), pp. 363–411. Springer.
thresholds in sciaenid fishes: a comparative study. J.
Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 1687–1691.
Retzius, G. 1881. Das Gehörorgan der Wirbelthiere, Vol. I.
Samson and Wallin.
Reysenbach de Haan, F. W. 1956. Hearing in whales. Acta Further Reading
Otolaryngol. Suppl. 134, 1–114.
Richardson, W. J., Greene, C. R., Jr., Malme, C. I., and Popper, A. N., Ramcharitar, J., and Campana, S. E. 2005. Why
Thomson, D. H. 1995. Marine Mammals and Noise. otoliths? Insights from inner ear physiology and fisheries
Academic Press. biology. Mar. Freshwater Res. 56, 497–504.

Вам также может понравиться