Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 18

Terminology disclaimer

The terminology used to describe people with disabilities has changed over time. The Minnesota Department of Human
Services ("Department") supports the use of "People First" language. Although outmoded and offensive terms might be
found within documents on the Department's website, the Department does not endorse these terms.

Development and Psychopathology, 9 (1997), 251–268


Copyright  1997 Cambridge University Press
Printed in the United States of America

Psychopathology as an outcome
of development

L. ALAN SROUFE
University of Minnesota

Abstract
When maladaptation is viewed as development rather than as disease, a transformed understanding results and a
fundamentally different research agenda emerges. Within a developmental perspective, maladaptation is viewed as
evolving through the successive adaptations of persons in their environments. It is not something a person “has” or
an ineluctable expression of an endogenous pathogen. It is the complex result of a myriad of risk and protective
factors operating over time. Key research questions within this framework center on discovery of factors that place
individuals on pathways probabilistically leading to later disturbances and factors and processes which maintain
individuals on, or deflect them from, such pathways once enjoined. There is an interest in recognizing patterns of
maladaptation which, while not properly considered disorder themselves, commonly are precursors of disorder and
also in conditions of risk that lie outside of the individual, as well as any endogenous influences. Likewise, there is
a focus on factors and processes that lead individuals away from disorder that has emerged, which goes beyond
interest in management of symptoms. Finally, many topics that currently are capturing attention in the field, such as
“comorbidity” and “resilience,” are seen in new ways from within the perspective of development.

How childhood problems and psychological investigators may treat assumed meanings of
disturbance are conceptualized has a profound observations as factual and may fail to recog­
influence on research that is conducted and nize powerful and compelling alternative in-
the interpretation of research findings. More terpretations. Another consequence is that
than two decades ago Lazare (1973) intro- limitations of the model, for example how it
duced the idea of “hidden conceptual models” constrains research questions, may be ob-
in psychopathology. By conceptual models he scured.
meant the frameworks for understanding psy- Embracing a particular model of disturb­
chological disorder, the set of guiding as- ance is analogous to putting on lenses which
sumptions utilized by clinicians and research- may bring some issues or questions into focus
ers to make sense out of their observations while distorting others in ways that may not
of disturbed behavior, thought, and affect. By be obvious to the observer. The thesis of this
using the term “hidden,” he underscored the paper is that what will be called a develop-
fact that such assumptions often are not made mental model leads to a unique and at times
explicit as assumptions and that people often radically different view than the position La-
are not aware that such models are being em- zare referred to as the “medical model,” a
ployed, that is, that they are viewing the view in which disorders often are seen as dis-
world from a particular perspective. One ma- crete and as arising from singular, endoge­
jor consequence of such hidden models is that nous pathogens. While not always obvious,
this medical model remains a dominant influ-
Preparation of this work and the research described ence in the field, even though in its simple
herein were supported by a grant from the National Insti­ form it is outmoded in much of medicine it­
tute of Mental Health (MH 40864).
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Alan self (Rutter, 1996).
Sroufe, Institute of Child Development, University of Within the classic medical model an anal-
Minnesota, 51 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455. ogy is made between childhood behavioral

251
252 L. A. Sroufe

and emotional problems and organic disease. same principles that govern normal develop­
This principal guiding assumption has sweep­ ment. Just as personality or the emergence of
ing implications. It is reflected in description competence involves a progressive, dynamic
and conceptualization of disorder itself, in the unfolding in which prior adaptation interacts
nature of research questions that are given pri­ with current circumstances in an ongoing
ority (centered on endogenous factors) and in way, so too does maladaptation or disorder.
how research findings are interpreted. One Sroufe and Rutter (1984) presented some
manifestation of this model is the diagnostic of the guiding assumptions of a develop­
classification system of disorders proposed by mental perspective, including holism and di­
the American Psychiatric Association (Ameri­ rectedness (see Santostefano, 1978). Thus,
can Psychiatric Association, 1994). Problems meaning of behavior is inseparable from its
shown by children as well as adults are context and the influence of one factor (an ex­
grouped into disorders, considered to be dis­ perience, a stressor, a genetic variation) is de­
crete and distinctive, and often given names pendent on the other factors. It is the unique
suggesting that they reflect enduring condi­ combination of risk and protective factors that
tions of the individual. For example, the cate­ governs the emergence of maladaptation (see
gory attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994; Gottlieb, 1991;
not only provides the important function of Rutter, 1996; Sameroff & Fiese, 1989). More­
summarizing an array of problem behaviors, over, with development the individual plays
but it also implies (via deficit) an endogenous an increasingly active role in adaptation, in­
problem of the child. This is no mere over­ terpreting and creating experience as well as
sight of terminology. It follows directly from responding to external and internal changes.
using the organic disease analogy for consid­ From a developmental point of view behavior
ering behavioral and emotional disturbance. is not simply the interaction of genes and en­
Environmental factors may be viewed as play­ vironment but genes, environment, and the
ing a role, as they do of course in many medi­ history of adaptation to that point (Sroufe &
cal conditions, but core aspects of etiology are Egeland, 1991). This neglected third factor,
assumed to lie in neurophysiological pathol­ prior adaptation, is of profound importance
ogy, whether due to genetic defect or environ­ and deserves a central place on the research
mental pathogens. Likewise, medical treat­ agenda.
ments are emphasized in research and clinical One may argue that a caricature of the
practice. Environmental manipulations may medical model is being presented and that the
have some role, but they are seen in terms of contrast with the developmental model is
managing “symptoms,” not as efforts to trans­ overdrawn. Indeed, Rutter (1996) has argued
form the child’s adaptation or to alter the that in internal medicine multifactorial causa­
larger child–environment system. All of this tion is seen as the rule and that risk factors
is despite the fact that there is little empirical may be dimensional as well as discrete. Rutter
evidence that these children have an attention provides numerous examples of complex cau­
deficit at all (Taylor, 1994). Without the unac­ sality in physical ailments, with environmen­
knowledged disease assumption, the term def­ tal factors and context playing a powerful
icit would have no place in the description of role, interacting with endogenous factors.
this set of problems. Thus, the classic medical model has been sup­
Within a developmental model, in contrast, planted in much of medical research itself.
organism and context are viewed as insepara­ Moreover, multiple causality is widely em­
ble (see Cohen & Stewart, 1994). There is no braced by researchers of diverse persuasions
attempt to explain behavior as merely an ex­ in the study of psychopathology, and environ­
pression of underlying, endogenous neurophys­ mental context can be considered even when
iological differences. Behavioral and emotional organic factors are emphasized. Develop­
disturbance is viewed as a developmental con­ mental history also can be considered, as in,
struction, reflecting a succession of adapta­ for example, the distinction between good and
tions that evolve over time in accord with the poor premorbid schizophrenia. An emphasis
Psychopathology as development 253

on endogenous factors can be integrated with


a variety of other positions, using the concept
of development (Eisenberg, 1977; Lazare,
1973; Rutter, 1980). It need not be so nar­
rowly construed.
Nonetheless, the position here is that the
medical model (henceforth referred to as the
classic medical model or disease model),
though outmoded even in medicine, still exer­
cises a dominant influence in the field of child
psychopathology. Because the assumptions
underlying this classic model are often unre­
flectively accepted and not explicitly ac­
knowledged, it exercises a pervasive, if often
subtle, influence on the conduct and interpre­
tation of research. Claims for the importance
of a broad causal net, and for an emphasis on
process, may be the mode, but in reality prior­
ity is often still given to the search for particu­
lar endogenous pathogens of a disorder. Envi­ Figure 1. A schematic representation of the devel­
ronmental as well as endogenous influences opmental pathways concept. (A) Continuity of
on child psychopathology may be examined. maladaptation, culminating in disorder. (B) Contin­
But “environment” often refers to prenatal te­ uous positive adaptation. (C) Initial maladaptation
ratogens or lead-painted walls (which are, of followed by positive change (resilience). (D) Initial
positive adaptation followed by negative change
course, surrogates for endogenous influences), toward pathology.
as though these are broadly representative of
exogenous factors. Also, as discussed in the
section on research below, physiological con­ of as a succession of branchings which take
comitants of disturbance are routinely inter­ the child away from pathways leading to com­
preted as causes, rather than simply as corre­ petent functioning. Five major implications of
lates or markers. The disease model takes on this model may be summarized as follows
the status of the description of reality rather (see also Sroufe, 1989; Sroufe & Rutter,
than as one point of view, which may in fact 1984; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990).
often be distorted. Before turning to further 1. Disorder as deviation over time: Pathol­
examples of the still prevailing influence of ogy is viewed in terms of developmental devi­
the classic medical model at the expense of ation. This requires first an understanding of
alternative points of view, an elaboration of normative developmental issues (e.g., secure
a contrasting developmental position will be attachment, modulated impulse control, effec­
presented. tive entry into the peer group) and the various
patterns of positive adaptation with respect to
The Pathways Framework them. A significant deviation in pattern of ad­
Many implications of a developmental model aptation represents an increased probability of
of disturbance can be captured with the con­ problems in negotiating subsequent develop­
cept of developmental pathways, introduced mental issues. Pathology generally reflects re­
by Waddington (1957) and adapted by peated failure of adaptation with respect to
Bowlby (1973). Bowlby’s preferred meta­ these issues. A particular adaptational failure
phoric representation of the pathways concept at any point in time is best viewed as placing
is the continuous branching of tracks in a rail­ an individual on a pathway potentially leading
way train yard (see also Loeber, 1991), but it to disorder or moving the individual toward
may be pictorially represented as a tree as such a pathway. Thus, for example, maladap­
well (see Fig. 1). Pathology may be thought tive patterns of attachment in infancy (anxious
254 L. A. Sroufe

attachment) are not viewed as psychopathol­ think of maladaptation or disturbance as


ogy per se but in terms of developmental risk something a child either “has” or “does not
for disturbance (see below). Pathology in­ have” in the sense of a permanent condition.
volves a succession of deviations away from Within this perspective, extremely stable con­
normative patterns. ditions such as early emerging conduct dis­
2. Multiple pathways to similar manifest turbance call for research on supports for such
outcomes: When development is viewed in problems, centering on positive feedback cy­
terms of a succession of branchings, it follows cles between child maladaptation and envi­
that individuals beginning on different path­ ronmental reaction (Patterson, DeBarysh, &
ways may nonetheless converge toward simi­ Ramsey, 1989; Richters & Cicchetti, 1993).
lar patterns of adaptation. Different influences 5. Change is constrained by prior adapta­
and different courses may be germain for dif­ tion: This final implication somewhat coun­
ferent individuals (see Cicchetti & Rogosch, terbalances the fourth. It suggests that the
1996). A pattern of maladaptation with many longer a maladaptive pathway has been fol­
features in common (e.g., lack of social en­ lowed (especially in the sense of going across
gagement, depressed mood, low self-esteem) phases of development), the less likely it is
may be the result of distinctly different devel­ that the person will reclaim positive adapta­
opmental pathways, one rooted in alienation, tion. (Bowlby implied that adolescence might
and one rooted in anxiety and helplessness mark the end of relative flexibility.) This is
(see also Blatt, 1995). If so, quite different consistent with the “active child” principle.
interventions may prove helpful to members By creating negative experiences in an ongo­
of these two groups, and it may be inappropri­ ing way, failing to engage positive opportuni­
ate to employ the same label to describe them, ties, and interpreting even benign experience
despite similar manifest behavior. Whether as malevolent (which often are core features
such phenotypically similar individuals differ of maladaptation), the child’s adaptation may
in terms of “prognosis,” subsequent outcome, make positive change less likely. Dodge’s
or effective intervention become key research work on attribution no longer is interpreted in
questions (e.g., Moffitt, 1993). terms of inherent cognitive deficit, but does
3. Different outcomes of the same pathway: suggest that negative experiences of some
Similarly, the concept of successive branch­ children lead to interpretive frameworks (and
ings suggests that individuals beginning on a congruent behavior) that further lead to nega­
similar pathway may diverge, ultimately tive experiences and so on (Crick & Dodge,
showing different patterns of pathology (or 1994; Rieder & Cicchetti, 1989; Rogosch,
positive adaptation) (cf. Cicchetti & Rogosch, Cicchetti, & Aber, 1995; Suess, Gross­
1996). Despite the phenotypic dissimilarity of mann, & Sroufe, 1992). This proposition also
such outcomes, it remains possible that they is in accord with abundant empirical data, in­
will represent a coherent family. The study of cluding the finding that children who enjoin
branching pathways over time may suggest early, and consistently pursue, the path from
radically different approaches to classifica­ defiance to aggression to antisocial behavior
tion, based on developmental trajectory rather are highly likely to persist toward criminality
than final manifest behavior alone (Loeber, (Loeber et al., 1993; Moffitt, 1993).
1991; Thelen, 1990). Like any metaphor, this branching pathway
4. Change is possible at many points: De­ model has its limitations, implying for exam­
spite early deviation, changes in develop­ ple that certain sorts of outcomes would be
mental challenges or other aspects of context absolutely impossible for some individuals,
may lead the individual back toward a more whereas at this stage of our knowledge “im­
serviceable pathway. Not only is pathology probable” would seem more likely. Still, it
typically not simply an endogenous given, but does provide a useful framework for summa­
even when a maladaptive pathway is en­ rizing a great deal of information about devel­
joined, return to positive functioning often re­ opment. It also provides a distinctive alterna­
mains possible. It is generally inappropriate to tive for interpreting findings from research on
Psychopathology as development 255

childhood disturbance and suggests an impor­ lescence are not on the same pathway as those
tant research agenda which hitherto has been whose behavior arises in, and persists from,
largely neglected, namely, processes of initia­ early childhood. Only the latter are likely to
tion, continuity, and change in maladaptation. show criminality in adulthood. Thus, despite
Some distinctions between this presenta­ the overlapping conduct problems in adoles­
tion of the developmental pathways concept cence, these two groups should not be treated
and other recent discussions should be pointed as members of one disease entity group. Like­
out. Loeber (1991), for example, has nicely wise, those showing the problem cluster in
summarized a set of ideas somewhat parallel question, plus particular patterns of other
to points 3–5 above. However, for him the problems, may again be distinguished by an­
starting point of a “pathway” is the presence tecedent and subsequent development. They
of disordered behavior patterns. The pathway may not be on the same pathway. (As a matter
is defined by the problem behaviors, and the of internal consistency with regard to the
focus is on persistence and desistance of pathway metaphor, it certainly makes no
these. Other problem behaviors that are pres­ sense to say that individuals may be on two
ent or “emerge” are subsumed within the con­ separate pathways at once, one in common
cept of “comorbidity” (discussed below) and with another group and one distinctive.) Start­
viewed as influences on subsequent develop­ ing with symptoms to define a pathway sim­
ment (p. 107). An important contribution of ply accepts that the medical model-based clas­
the present pathways model is emphasis on sification system is valid, leading inevitably
patterns of adaptation (with respect to norma­ to additional problems being interpreted as
tive developmental issues) that precede the the cooccurrence of second diseases and dif­
emergence of frank disturbance, that include ferential persistence as due to varying time
strengths as well as weaknesses, and that are courses of the disease. The critical research
viewed as an important part of a causal net­ questions regarding developmental process
work of influences. Early patterns of adapta­ that arise from these observations are simply
tion are viewed as prototypes—root forms swept aside. The existing disease-based clas­
with diverse potential—that are linked to later sification system, and the classic medical
outcomes as part of a multidetermined pro­ model of psychopathology in general, need to
cess. Some individuals on a pathway may, in be tested, not simply assumed as the starting
fact, never go on to disorder. The work of point for studying problem behaviors over
Loeber, and other work that he discusses, is time.
important in indicating that early onset, dura­
tion, and number of problems are of great
Conceptualizing Competence and
prognostic significance. However, to provide
Disturbance
a fully viable and distinctive alternative to the
classic medical model, developmental path­ Varying conceptualizations of basic phenom­
ways must be traced from a point prior to the ena in the field reveal the operation of differ­
onset of disturbance. ent models. Two examples are considered
Tracing pathways from a point prior to the here, one from the domain of competence and
emergence of disturbance allows one to dis­ one from the domain of disturbance. The first
cover heterogeneity in disorder. Individuals phenomenon to be discussed has been termed
showing similar “symptoms” may in fact be “resilience” and the second comorbidity. That
on different pathways if examined longitudi­ the medical model is frequently in operation
nally and may have predictably different out­ in discussions of the former, as well as the
comes. From the viewpoint of development, latter, is testimony to its pervasiveness.
they may not be manifesting the same disturb­
ance. This has been illustrated by Moffitt
Resilience
(1993) in work on “adolescent limited” and
early emerging antisocial behavior. Those The concept of resilience can be used to illus­
whose problem behavior emerged first in ado­ trate the distinctiveness of the developmental
256 L. A. Sroufe

perspective. Resilience simply refers to the longitudinal data (e.g., Egeland, Carlson, &
fact that some children facing adversity none­ Sroufe, 1993) reveal that manifestation of re­
theless do well (or return to positive function­ silience is associated either with a history of
ing following a period of maladaptation; e.g., positive experience and positive adaptation
Masten & Coatsworth, 1995). Such an obser­ (prior to the period of stress or maladaptation)
vation is open to a variety of interpretations. and/or positive experience between the period
This observation often is explained in terms of stress and recovery. For example, groups
of endogenous traits (an inherent robustness of malfunctioning 4-year-olds who later were
or other such characteristic of the child). As free of behavior problems at school more of­
is often done in discussing disorders, the term ten had histories of early secure attachment
“resilience” is therefore made the explanation and stable emotional support in the toddler
for the observed phenomenon. Why do some period than did 4-year-olds who showed con­
children do well in the face of adversity? Be­ tinuity of malfunctioning (Sroufe et al., 1990).
cause they are resilient. (Why do some chil­ Had the research started at age 4, the resil­
dren manifest attention problems? Because ience would have appeared mysterious and
they have Attention Deficit Hyperactivity may have been attributed to some children
Disorder, ADHD.) Thus, in this perspective, simply having the “right stuff.” From within
resilience is treated as a trait rather than as a the classic medical model the search for ante­
process. So powerful is such a preconception cedents of resilience (with the exception of IQ
that ambiguous data is often interpreted as im­ or temperamental traits) has had low priority.
plying such an endogenous trait. The well Other research from our project shows that
known work of Werner and Smith (1992) is changes in parental stress and social support
frequently cited as demonstrating that positive also account for differential improvement in
“temperament” is a determinant of resilience. children’s functioning over time (e.g., Ege­
The significant variable underlying this inter­ land et al., 1993). This graphically illustrates
pretation, which shows up only late in in­ that resilience resides more in the develop­
fancy, actually is a parental report of the de­ mental system (which, of course, includes the
gree to which the child is “lovable.” Not only child’s history of adaptation) than in the child
could this variable readily be interpreted as alone.
a caregiving variable (caregiver perception of In a completely parallel manner, under­
loveableness), but also the idea that loveable­ standing desistance of problem behavior will
ness itself is a developmental product is not be enhanced employing a developmental
considered. Only when this finding is uncriti­ model. Both adaptational history and current
cally interpreted within a classic medical supports and challenges are needed to explain
model framework would a trait interpretation those whose disordered behavior improves.
automatically follow from this finding. “Spontaneous recovery” is just a term for cur­
Within a developmental perspective, in rent ignorance, left unexamined because of
contrast, resilience is not something some weddedness to a particular disease model of
children simply “have a lot of.” It develops. disorder.
A capacity to rebound following periods of
maladaptation (or to do well in the face of
Comorbidity
stress) evolves over time within the total con­
text of developmental influences. The capaci­ The phenomenon to which comorbidity refers
ties for staying organized in the face of chal­ is the simple fact that children (especially) of­
lenge, for active coping and for maintaining ten show behaviors that fit two or more of
positive expectations during periods of stress the currently designated diagnostic categories
are evolved by the person in interaction with (see Caron & Rutter, 1991, for a discussion).
the environment across successive periods of Saying that such joint occurrences are a re­
adaptation. And even as an acquired capacity flection of comorbidity is no explanation.
it is not static but is continually influenced Nothing in this observed fact suggests that
by ongoing changes in context. Prospective, children therefore often have concurrently
Psychopathology as development 257

two or more discrete disorders (read, dis­ categories would have led to a questioning of
eases). But the term comorbidity, based in the the entire system, not to a new medical term
hidden assumptive network of the disease in our reference books. Because of the power
model, suggests just that. If a child manifests of the medical models, the literature contains
problems that fit two current categories, it is almost no discussion of concerns about basic
concluded (assumed) that he or she has two tenets underlying the DSM classification sys­
conditions, rather than even considering the tem itself (for recent exceptions, see Rich­
alternative that there is a failure of syndromic ters & Cicchetti, 1993; Wakefield, 1992a,
integrity for one or both categories. The 1992b). Rather, discussion focuses on cate­
power of the hidden assumptions leads inves­ gory changes alone (Rutter et al., 1994). If
tigators to skip right over the challenge the taken seriously, the data on comorbidity could
basic observation poses to the classic medical lead to revolutionary changes in classification
model. of childhood psychological problems and per­
The disease model requires syndromic in­ haps quite distinctive views of disturbance it­
tegrity. If the disease model is apt for chil­ self. The way would be cleared for evolving
dren’s behavioral and emotional problems, classification schemes centered on patterns of
children generally should manifest tight clus­ adaptation and developmental trajectories.
ters of symptoms, with unique indicators of
other syndromes being absent. But in reality
Designing and Interpreting Research
children commonly manifest problems that
cut across established categories. To be sure, The pervasiveness of the medical model not
one disorder may potentiate another in medi­ only has implications for conceptualization
cine as well (Rutter, 1996), but not nearly to (and treatment) of childhood problems but
the extent implied by the prevalence of co- also has a major impact on research. It power­
morbidity of childhood disturbances. Descrip­ fully guides the questions that are asked as
tions of disorders in the literature frequently well as how obtained findings are interpreted.
begin by noting large percentages of overlap Much current research is focused on finding
with other conditions (e.g., Rutter, Taylor, & the pathogen for a given problem—the gene
Hersov, 1994). For example, Harrington or the particular neuropathology assumed to
(1994) reports that most children who meet underly all instances of a disturbance. It fol­
criteria for depression also have been given lows that this search commonly is localized in
another primary diagnosis. Citing other work the person, and the assumption is made that
(Anderson, Williams, McGee, & Silva, 1987), the pathogen by itself accounts for the origin,
he also reports that of 14 11-year-olds with onset, and course of the problem (“disorder”).
depression, 11 qualified for at least one other Nothing, of course, is wrong with neurophysi­
diagnosis, 8 of the 14 qualifying for anxiety ologically oriented research. At this stage of
disorder, attention deficit disorder, and con­ our knowledge of maladaptation, research on
duct disorder. Conduct problems and activity/ numerous fronts is vital. However, singular
attention problems have been found to corre­ devotion to the disease model, with its hidden
late quite highly (e.g., .77; August, MacDon­ assumptions, has the unfortunate consequence
ald, Realmuto, & Skare, 1996). Comorbidity of limiting and narrowing the research en­
is the rule, not the exception. Moreover, broad deavor. When it is taken as a given that disor­
classes of problems such as externalizing be­ der derives from pathogens that are endoge­
haviors are predictive of a myriad of later nous to the individual there will be limited
conditions, including depression and other efforts to discover etiological factors lying
conditions not typified by aggression or other outside of the child (and to understand how
hallmarks of externalizing (Robins & Price, these interact with endogenous factors) or,
1991). Number of problems rivals clustering perhaps especially, to understand what factors
of problems in predicting later dysfunction. may bring the child back toward normal adap­
One might think the discovery that chil­ tation away from a disturbed pattern. Examin­
dren’s problems often cut across the working ing factors that lead a child into or away from
258 L. A. Sroufe

maladaptation is not even a very meaningful gitudinal study of development from birth
issue if disorder is considered something an through adolescence (e.g., Egeland et al.,
individual either has or does not have. 1993; Carlson, Jacobvitz, & Sroufe, 1995;
Within a developmental perspective the re­ Sroufe, Carlson, & Shulman, 1993) can illus­
search agenda changes dramatically from that trate the heuristic value of this viewpoint and
inspired by the disease model, and existing its distinctiveness from the disease model.
data frequently are seen in different light. One The first comes from a prospective, longitudi­
moves away from the search for single patho­ nal investigation of children’s attention and
gens, conceptualized as linear causes ineluc­ activity problems, using criteria of ADHD in
tably producing their outcome, toward the DSM III–R. The second is based on an ado­
search for a complex of influences that initiate lescent outcome study of infant–caregiver at­
a developmental pathway which only probabi­ tachment problems.
listically is associated with disturbance (Cic­
chetti & Tucker, 1994; Sameroff & Fiese,
A developmental view of attention and
1989; Sroufe, 1989). The etiology of disturb­
activity problems
ance is conceptualized in terms of a combina­
tion of risk factors and protective factors of The starting point for a developmental ap­
diverse sorts. Moreover, the possibility or proach to psychopathology is always a con­
even probability of later disturbance may be sideration of normal development. Thus, we
seen in early patterns of maladaptation that in began our investigation of attention/activity
and of themselves are not pathological and in problems by considering factors that normally
aspects of the developmental context even support the development of the capacities to
prior to the appearance of child maladapta­ modulate arousal, regulate affect, control im­
tion. Second, and equally important from this pulses, and direct attention. Basically, a pro­
perspective, is research on factors influencing cess unfolds wherein what begins as care-
continuity and change, that is, processes and giver-orchestrated regulation becomes dyadic
mechanisms that maintain individuals on regulation, with increasingly active participa­
pathways once enjoined or deflect them to­ tion by the infant. Then, progressively, trans­
ward others. This includes the search for fac­ fer of the regulatory responsibility to the child
tors that lead individuals away from disturb­ occurs over the course of early childhood
ance following its manifestation. Disturbance through a series of phases. At each phase, be­
is not a given; it is supported. Pathology is ginning in the early months of life, patterns of
not something a child “has”; it is a pattern of affective, attentional, and behavioral regula­
adaptation reflecting the totality of the devel­ tion are constructed within the caregiving sys­
opmental context to that point. tem. Such developing patterns or prototypes
When disturbance is viewed as develop­ are carried forward and interact with subse­
ment one asks numerous questions. How do quent challenges to regulation as development
individual children get off track? When going continues (see Sroufe, 1989, for more detail).
off track, what deviating track is a particular Given this understanding, we then asked what
child likely to take? What influences (in their factors would be liabilities with respect to
pattern of adaptation and in the total develop­ pursuing the normative pathway toward effec­
mental context) tend to maintain them on the tive self-regulation. What might lead some
track they are on, and what would be required children to get off track?
to bring them back to a more serviceable de­ The data set was comprehensive, and a
velopmental pathway? These are very differ­ range of factors were considered. We exam­
ent than questions about which gene causes, ined a number of early “child” variables, that
or what are the physiological correlates of, a is, variables commonly thought of as residing
particular disorder, which are inspired by the in the child. These included premature birth,
classic medical model and shed limited light nonoptimal newborn neurological status,
on most childhood problems. nurses ratings of fussiness, soothability, and
Two illustrations from the Minnesota lon­ other behaviors in the newborn nursery; ob­
Psychopathology as development 259

servational measures of infant activity level side of the child. Moreover, the single best
and irritability; and parent-based temperament predictor of attention problems was mother’s
questionnaire data in infancy and at age 21⁄2 relationship status at birth; children later
years. While we believe that each of these showing attention and activity problems had
variables is best thought of as reflecting a de­ single mothers. Such a contextual feature can­
velopmental process, it is the case that most not be attributed to the child and shows the
of them (the exception being the parental re­ importance of casting a broad net in defining
ports) can be defined as child characteristics. factors that place children on pathways to dis­
They are manifest in child behavior, observ­ order. With regard to the prediction of atten­
able even when the child is apart from the tional and hyperactivity problems in kinder­
caregiver. garten, we found that there was almost no
But in addition to these child characteris­ overlap between those few cases that were
tics, which along with environmental toxins predictable from newborn motor immaturity
often would exhaust variables in a study and the others that were predicted from the
guided by a disease model of disorder, we parenting and other contextual variables (Ja­
also examined aspects of the developmental cobvitz & Sroufe, 1987). Thus, multiple path­
context. This included the immediate context ways to the same disturbed behavior are sug­
of parenting behaviors (patterns of stimulating gested.
and regulating the child), the broader context While nothing observable in the child dur­
in which parenting was nested (the stress, sup­ ing the infancy period was found to predict
port, and general degree of stability in the par­ later attentional and hyperactivity problems,
ent’s life), and more distal contextual factors, by age 31⁄2 this no longer was so. Consistent
such as marital status at birth. No prior study with other literature (e.g., Campbell, 1990),
had explored the origins of attention and ac­ our observation-based rating of distractibility
tivity problems in this way, though from a de­ was modestly related to ADHD criteria be­
velopmental perspective it is obvious to do so. haviors in early elementary school (account­
The results of this research, based on fol­ ing for about 6% of the variance). By the pre­
lowing some 180 children from birth through school period, then, one might say that some
sixth grade and using teacher Behavior Prob­ children are on the attentional problem/hyper­
lem Checklist data as the outcome, strongly activity pathway, even though enjoining this
supported the heuristic value of a develop­ pathway (the 31⁄2 year distractibility measure)
mental perspective (Jacobvitz & Sroufe, is predictable from contextual variables well
1987; Carlson, Jacobvitz, & Sroufe, 1995). before this time, as is later criterial ADHD
The more than 40 early child variables were behavior itself. Moreover, a combination of
consistently weak in terms of predictive distractibility and early and later contextual
power. One variable from the Brazelton Neo­ variables predicted elementary attention prob­
natal Exam (Motor Immaturity) showed mod­ lems far more strongly than early distractibil­
est prediction of ADHD criteria in kindergar­ ity alone (up to 28% of the variance in Grade
ten, but not thereafter. Observed or parent- 1–3 problem behavior).
reported activity level or other dimensions of In the second phase of the research we
infant temperament were never significantly showed that contextual variables accounted
related to subsequent attention or activity for change in ADHD criterial behaviors over
problems. In contrast, measures of parental in­ time. Changing support for caregivers and
trusiveness and overstimulation, including the changing caregiver relationship status were
single measure of such parenting obtained the most consistent predictors of change in
when the infant was 6 months old, were more child problem behaviors. As the primary care­
predictive, with some consistency across ages. giver’s relationship stabilized or destabilized,
It is important to note that parental intrusive­ the child’s manifestation of attentional and
ness at 6 months was not predicted by any hyperactivity problems changed. Thus, in the
antecedent or concurrent child variable. Thus, current developmental terminology, some
we view this influence as initially lying out­ children who were on the ADHD pathway at
260 L. A. Sroufe

ages 31⁄2, 5, or 6 were apparently not on this further attention, and citations were provided.
pathway at a later age, whereas others not However, “environmental factors” often re­
manifesting such problems early had enjoined ferred to toxins such as lead. Within a medical
this pathway at a later age. More detailed, model, of course, these are the kind of envi­
process data will be required to determine ronmental factors that command attention,
whether such change is mediated primarily by rather than psychosocial stressors and other
change in the caregiver’s behavior toward the aspects of developmental context which might
child, as we would hypothesize. also be considered. Moreover, “family” vari­
A final result is relevant to the pathways ables, it was argued, had been shown to be
model. When cumulative attentional and hy­ irrelevant or to be effects, not causes. A cited
peractivity problems up through third grade example of the former was Goodman and Ste­
are considered, very little change can be ac­ venson’s (1989) twin study. But their family
counted for thereafter. This suggests that, at data were based on contemporaneous parent
least for these types of externalizing prob­ interviews which not surprisingly yielded no
lems, change becomes increasingly difficult predictability; there was no observation of
the longer the pathway is followed. This also caregiver behavior, antecedent or contempo­
seems to be true for aggression (Gottesman, rary. Weak measures are quickly accepted
1995; Loeber et al. 1993; Moffitt, 1993). when the null hypothesis follows from tacit
Reactions to this work during conference assumptions about factors that are irrelevant.
discussions and in editorial review were inter­ Another example is a study by Schachar and
esting with respect to the role of models in Wachsmuth (1990), which could be cited as
research evaluation. The first reaction typi­ showing a lack of family influence on ADHD
cally has been to ask how many of our sub­ (Taylor, 1994). Schachar and Wachsmuth
jects “really had” ADHD? This question, simply examined DSM diagnoses of parents,
steeped in the disease model, presumes the finding no increment in disorders among par­
distinct entity, organically based nature of ents of ADHD cases compared to parents of
such problems. Taken to extreme this would controls (though there was an increase for
preclude scientific investigation. If an organic conduct disorder cases). Such a family vari­
variable is not predictive (or if medication is able follows from a medical model, given a
ineffective long term), then this is taken as preoccupation with genetic causality. How­
evidence that the children in question did not ever, it is not parental psychiatric diagnosis,
have ADHD. The alternative of a continuum but patterns of stimulation, control, and dy­
of problem behaviors is simply not taken as adic regulation that are critical within a devel­
a viable position. We found no evidence for opmental perspective. These were not as­
discontinuity in the distribution (univariate or sessed. A study by Hinshaw and McHale
bivariate) of our variables, nor is there a body (1991) was cited as an example of research
of evidence suggesting the 8 (DSM III–R) or showing that parenting differences are effects
6 (DSM–IV) “symptoms” represent a qualita­ and not causes. These authors reported that
tive break point (Jacobvitz, Sroufe, Stew­ parent controllingness decreased when chil­
art, & Leffert, 1990). We obtained results par­ dren with attention problems were given stim­
allel to those above looking at extreme cases ulant medication, which, as we will discuss
and at the 12 children placed on stimulant below, is not relevant to the question of etiol­
medication (which itself bore little relation to ogy. Finally, many reviewers and discussants
our objective assessment of behavior across of this work, and researchers in general, have
time—a sad commentary on at least some argued that it has already been proven that at­
clinical practice). tentional problems are “largely the result of
Other reactions suggested that the ques­ neurological dysfunction” (e.g., Frick & La-
tions addressed by our work did not make hey, 1991). Such a conclusion has been based
sense or dealt with resolved or superfluous is­ on descriptions of the syndrome, the pre­
sues. Environmental factors, and even family sumed “lack” of evidence for parenting
factors in particular, were said to require no and other contextual influences, short-term re­
Psychopathology as development 261

sponsiveness of the disorder to stimulant med­ behavior and in CNS functioning is no sur­
ication, and occasional neurophysiological prise, given the integrated nature of human
correlates. One highly acclaimed example of functioning. However, this in no way allows
the latter is Zametkin’s (1993) report of fron­ the conclusion of innate damage or even “dys­
tal lobe blood flow differences in adults pre­ function” in the sense of aberrant behavior–
sumed to have been ADHD as children, com­ brain linkages. Such measures thus have the
pared to control subjects. As will be discussed status of markers but not necessarily causes.
below, such a correlation cannot be interpre­ The leap to equate correlation with cause is a
ted as causal. All of this reflects interpretation reflection of a commitment to the medical
of information within a medical model, with­ model. Moreover, even were dysfunction in
out consideration of compelling alternatives. brain functioning shown to be antecedent to
When the medical model lens is removed the emergence of attention and activity prob­
and the literature on etiological factors in at­ lems, which certainly has not been done yet,
tention problems is considered from within a this still would be best interpreted within a
developmental perspective, very different in­ broader causal framework. The recent out­
terpretations result. With respect to parent be­ pouring of evidence concerning experience
havior, for example, cause is not looked at in dependent brain development (Cicchetti &
simple, linear terms. Of course, managing a Tucker, 1994; Greenough, Black, & Wallace,
child with attention problems is extraordi­ 1987; Kraemer, 1992; Schore, 1994) makes it
narily difficult, regardless of what is ulti­ clear that there are massive experiential influ­
mately understood regarding etiology. It also ences on the development of the central ner­
would seem natural for parents to be control­ vous system, including the tuning of systems
ling and even critical of a child having such concerned with activation and regulation of
problems, and the literature contains such affect and behavior. Eclectic investigators
findings (Taylor, 1994). If with intervention will look for ongoing parenting influences on
child attention problems decrease, one would endogenous factors as well as endogenous in­
expect controllingness to decline. Ongoing, fluences on parenting.
mutual influence is the basic expectation Changes in behavior in response to stimu­
within a developmental process model. How­ lant medication likewise do not allow etiolog­
ever, such a finding in no way suggests that ical interpretations. Models of etiology and
caregiving factors are irrelevant to etiology. It models of treatment bear no necessary rela­
would not be hypothesized that parental over- tion to one another. Frankly retarded young­
control would lead to hyperactivity, so the sters may be trained to perform certain cogni­
fact that overcontrol declines with diminished tive tasks, but no one currently argues that
child problems is not germain to this issue. their retardation was the result of insufficient
Our prospective, longitudinal assessments re­ reinforcement. Those who argue that drug
vealed no infant predictors of parental intru­ studies have etiological significance overlook
siveness or overstimulation. At the same time, the fact that stimulants also enhance the per­
these parenting patterns predicted later atten­ formance of normal children and adults and
tion and activity problems. How child factors show little evidence of improving the func­
interact with such parenting variables has as tioning of attention disordered children in the
yet been little explored. long term (see Jacobvitz et al., 1990). Even
Central nervous system correlates also were such results ever demonstrated, and even
would be expected within an integrative de­ if the effective drugs were those with more
velopmental framework. First of all, many of specific neurotransmitter actions (in contrast
the data utilizing brain physiology or blood to broadly acting methylphenidate), this still
flow, such as the findings of Zametkin (1993), would not prove inherent deficit, due to the
are gathered during attentional tasks. The complex, systemic nature of development.
measures therefore simply corroborate the at­ Central nervous system dysfunction also is
tention problem. That lifelong attention prob­ best viewed as developing within a complex
lems would in adulthood be manifest both in causal framework.
262 L. A. Sroufe

A developmental view of infant attachment expect to master challenges and to have


problems and later disturbance power in the world. They believe in them­
selves. Likewise, they value relating and have
A second illustration of developmental re­ an internalized template for empathy and reci­
search on maladaptation concerns the relation procity in relationships.
of early anxious attachment relationships to Patterns evolved in the attachment rela­
later psychiatric problems. Attachment re­ tionship are taken forward at the behavioral
search provides an interesting case for devel­ level as well. The child has been entrained
opmental psychopathology, because attach­ into particular patterns of reciprocity and af­
ment is a relationship construct, not an fective sharing, as well as having evolved a
individual trait construct. Established assess­ sense of curiosity and a skill in exploration,
ments of infant caregiver attachment (e.g., the supported by the secure attachment.
Strange Situation Procedure; Ainsworth, Supporting the behavioral level are pat­
Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) are assess­ terns of arousal regulation, which allow the
ments of relationships, not individuals. This full range of emotional expression with suffi­
has been supported by ample research, includ­ cient modulation, such that organized behav­
ing the findings that attachment pattern with ior can be maintained. Such patterns are
each parent often is different, with concor­ readily established in the context of respon­
dance barely significant (Fox, Kimmerly, & sive care, because responsiveness entails ap­
Schafer, 1991) and that attachment security propriate affective stimulation and interven­
with a given parent changes as a function of tions to keep arousal within reasonable
that parent’s changing life stress (e.g., bounds. Moreover, recent evidence (e.g.,
Vaughn, Egeland, Waters, & Sroufe, 1979). Schore, 1994) suggests that a history of pat­
Clearly, attachment security is not an endoge­ terned, responsive care actually is central in
nous infant trait. tuning and balancing excitatory and inhibitory
Still, patterns of anxious attachment in in­ systems in the central nervous system itself,
fancy are proposed to be risk factors for psy­ which would support emotional regulation
chopathology. The quality of a particular at­ and behavioral flexibility.
tachment relationship, whether secure or In addition to those attachment relation­
anxious, is based on the history of interaction ships judged to be secure (the clear majority
within the pair. When the caregiver is rou­ in most samples), there are three patterns of
tinely responsive to the infant’s signals, the anxious attachment, each of which would
infant develops a confidence that reassurance, compromise the developing capacities for
tending, assistance, and other care will be self-regulation and social behavior (Ains­
available when needed. Such confidence in worth et al., 1978; Main & Hesse, 1990;
support is precisely what is meant by secure Sroufe, 1988). Anxious/resistant attachment is
attachment. In contrast, routinely unrespon­ characterized by difficulty settling with the
sive or inconsistent care undermines security. caregiver when distressed, often tinged with
The patterning of the early primary attach­ anger. Such a pattern is associated with a his­
ment relationship is a prototype for subse­ tory of inconsistent care and/or neglect, leav­
quent development, operating on numerous ing infants hyperaroused, hypervigilant, and
levels (Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, in press). uncertain regarding caregiver availability and
In the secure attachment case, having experi­ their own effectiveness. Anxious/avoidant at­
enced responsive care, the child generalizes tachment involves explicitly failing to seek
the expectation that others will be responsive contact with the caregiver under conditions of
and available; that is, the child develops gen­ stress (e.g., following brief laboratory separa­
erally positive and trusting attitudes toward tions). This pattern is associated with a history
others. Along with this, the child takes for­ of chronic rebuff, especially when the infant
ward a sense of his or her own effectance and sought physical contact with the caregiver.
personal worth. Being able to effectively elicit Such infants learn to cut off or truncate emo­
responsiveness and care from the parent, they tional responses, especially when tender needs
Psychopathology as development 263

are aroused. Finally, disorganized/disoriented parent and show stability with each parent
attachment reflects confusion about or even (Carlson, submitted; Main & Hesse, 1990).
fear of caregivers, who themselves have be­ Thus, anxious attachment in infancy is better
haved in confused, alarming, or dissociated viewed as an initiating condition than as a
ways. Such infants face an unresolveable par­ characteristic of the infant. Still, as a mal­
adox of having caregiver be both the source adaptive relationships pattern it is probablisti­
of alarm and the (biologically) expected cally linked to later psychological disorder.
source or reassurance. Lapses in orientation Nor is anxious attachment viewed as
and failures of integration of emotions, cogni­ causal of later disturbance in a simple sense.
tions, and behavior result. After all, as is true of most singular risk fac­
We recently tested the hypothesis that pat­ tors, the majority of individuals showing early
terns of anxious attachment represent risk fac­ anxious attachment do not show serious dis­
tors for psychopathology across childhood turbance later. Whether disturbance results
and adolescence. For example, at age 171⁄2 we depends on the successive combination of lia­
created an overall index of pathology, based bilities and supports that maintain the individ­
on the number, duration, and seriousness of ual on a pathway to pathology or bring them
diagnoses derived from the Schedule of Af­ back toward positive adaptation. Of course,
fective Disorders and Schizophrenia (Child one of the liabilities (or supports in the case
Form) clinical interview, which was con­ of secure attachment) is the prior adaptation,
ducted and coded completely independent of including prototypical pattern of coping and
attachment history or other knowledge of the affect regulation and expectations concerning
child. The simple correlation of avoidant at­ self, other, and relationships, within which the
tachment at 12–18 months and the pathology person negotiates subsequence developmental
index was .24. This is significant with 170 phases.
subjects though small in absolute terms. The The manner in which attachment theory
combination of “disorganized” attachment and research have been utilized within the
(Main & Hesse, 1990) and avoidant attach­ dominant medical model was predictable.
ment raised the correlation to .41, modest but “Attachment disorders” were added to the
impressive over these many phases of devel­ DSM (APA, 1987, 1994). While the criterion
opment and given the challenges of assessing of pathological care as the source of these
such constructs. The correlation increased still problems marks a break from the traditional
further (into the high .50s) when we also medical model approach, and while the cases
added measures of parenting and adaptation so designated may indeed have attachment
from the preschool and early adolescent peri­ problems (Zeenah, Mammen. & Lieberman,
ods (see Carlson, submitted). The disorga­ 1994), the circumscribing of attachment prob­
nized attachment pattern was specifically re­ lems to specific disorders reveals a failure to
lated to dissociative symptoms in childhood grasp the developmental significance of at­
and adolescence (.40), as predicted by theory. tachment history and the potential power of a
In accord with the developmental model, developmental approach to psychopathology
avoidant and disorganized patterns of attach­ in general. What could become a model for
ment may be thought of as initial develop­ approaching childhood disturbance of all
mental variations, probabilistically associated kinds is sequestered into a circumscribed set
with later disturbance. Such patterns of anx­ of categories.
ious attachment are not thought of as psychi­ Various attachment problems seem to have
atric disorders themselves (Sroufe, 1988). implications for a range of disturbances, cer­
Again, they are viewed as assessments of rela­ tainly not all phenotypically similar to dyadic
tionship qualities with a particular caregiver. behavioral patterns shown in infancy (Blatt,
Avoidance and disorganized/disoriented at­ 1995). For example, given the tendency of
tachment, which show little concordance those in avoidant attachment relationships to
across parenting partners, both are predictable turn from their caregivers when in need, later
from earlier patterns of care by the particular social withdrawal and superficial relation­
264 L. A. Sroufe

ships might be all that were expected based tention deficit hyperactivity disorder,” “anxi­
on linear predictions. But the lack of empathic ety disorder,” and so forth and children not
connection and the alienation inherent in these fitting any DSM category are far more similar
prototypic avoidant attachments also has been to each other than they are to children diag­
viewed as the basis for aggressiveness, bully­ nosed as autistic. The manifestation of most
ing, and conduct disorders, predictions which childhood disturbances, but not autism, is pro­
have been confirmed (Renken et al., 1989; foundly influenced by context. For example,
Troy & Sroufe, 1987). On the other hand, re­ in our emotionally supportive, well-staffed
sistant attachment often is manifest in angry (one adult to four children), activity-oriented
rejection of the caregiver when comfort is of­ summer camps, which included a good mix
fered to the distressed infant, a pattern which of competent 10-year-olds and children with
may be shown in batting away toys, pushing serious conduct problems, aggression was al­
away from contact, squirming, and/or tan­ most nonexistent (e.g., Elicker, Englund, &
truming. Yet this pattern is not associated Sroufe, 1992). This is despite the fact that in
with later Oppositional Defiant Disorder or school settings the troubled children were reli­
other externalizing problems. It is uniquely ably reported by both teachers and observers
related to Anxiety Disorder classifications, as to engage in frequent bullying and other ag­
predicted from the chronic vigilance required gressive behaviors. Such contextual variation,
to monitor an inconsistent caregiver (Warren across time as well as situations, is a hallmark
et al., submitted). of most childhood problems but not most
medical conditions.
It is sometimes difficult to recognize that
Conclusion: Future Directions
the medical model with its assumptive base is
The classic medical model as a framework for being applied broadly to problems of children
approaching behavioral and emotional prob­ and youth. Its wide use partly derives from
lems in childhood has inherent limitations. successes of the model with certain adult dis­
Childhood problems generally are not like orders and with occasional childhood distur­
diseases. They show little evidence of a bances. More general validity of the model is
bounded, discrete, syndromic nature. Often then simply taken for granted and not exam­
children qualifying for diagnosis are quantita­ ined. Moreover, the classification of child­
tively, not qualitatively, different from other hood problems currently in use has served
children. Research often shows number of certain purposes in research. Categories such
problems rather than tight, syndromic coher­ as ADHD promote communication to a de­
ence, to be predictive of later disorder. Fur­ gree; they summarize a set of behaviors in
thermore, most childhood problems are con­ shorthand fashion and provide a starting point
text malleable to a degree that surpasses for research on etiology and treatment. How­
typical medical conditions (especially during ever, the fact that the DSM system is being
the years of onset). All of this is much more used cannot be taken as support for its valid­
consistent with the idea of development than ity. There are children who are impulsive, ag­
the idea of disease. gressive, anxious, and so forth, with frequen­
Exceptions to this general case, such as cies of behavioral manifestation showing
childhood autism, actually further underscore notable stability in childhood. But this is not
the importance of a developmental viewpoint. evidence of syndromic integrity and not evi­
Autism is now classed as a “Pervasive Devel­ dence of endogenous pathogens as primary
opmental Disorder,” and properly so. Such causes.
children are profoundly disturbed in all arenas More research is needed that examines the
of functioning—cognitive, affective, and so­ integrity of existing diagnostic categories and
cial (Hobson & Patrick, 1995). They are qual­ that seeks to uncover new, coherent groupings
itatively different from other children, in­ of problems. Especially important will be re­
cluding those with behavior and emotional search that begins by defining early patterns
problems. Children assigned diagnoses of “at­ of adaptation and then follows individuals
Psychopathology as development 265

showing such patterns to observed families of ited” (p. 224). This statement can be extended
outcomes. This contrasts sharply with the cur­ to most childhood disorders if one broadens
rent dominant approach of simply assuming environment to include experiential factors,
the validity of existing categories and then rather than simply demographic variables and
seeking antecedents. When these two ap­ aspects of the physical environment. Espe­
proaches converge, one would, of course, cially important is longitudinal research be­
have considerable confidence in the meaning­ ginning prior to the onset of disorder. Such
fulness of the taxa in question. At the same research not only is necessary for untangling
time, further efforts to group developmental causal mechanisms and processes, it is the key
trajectories meaningfully should be given to resolving the classification problems dis­
high priority by both researchers and funding cussed above.
agencies. The distinction between adolescent Currently, for example, much discussion
limited and developmentally persistent con­ centers around ADHD with and without con­
duct problems (Moffitt, 1993) is an excellent duct disorders (e.g., Rutter, 1996). Outcomes
example. The problems of these groups of for children so diagnosed are very different,
children are more appropriately distinguished, but it is not clear what the implications are
rather than being lumped into the same cate­ for classification. Such discussion would be
gory. It seems likely that developmental anal­ enlightened enormously by antecedent data.
ysis will reveal similar distinctions among Are there distinctive origins of the comorbid
those who at some time show depression and pattern, or is it simply a combination of the
other problems as well. In general, what is precursors of ADHD and CD? Are the ante­
needed is a fresh examination of the whole cedents of CD alone (or ADHD alone) dis­
issue of classification in child psychopathol­ tinctive from antecedents of those showing
ogy, based on developmental research. the combined pattern (see Loeber, Brin­
A serious consequence of the current dom­ thaupt, & Green, 1990)? Is the comorbid pat­
inance of the medical model has been its con­ tern itself in fact heterogeneous? Such ques­
straining effect on the conduct of research and tions must be approached developmentally. It
interpretation of findings. This is highlighted is not enough to examine selective correlates
when it is contrasted to a developmental of already manifest disorder. In addition to
model. Under the aegis of the medical model, having a broad net of theoretically derived,
environment is defined narrowly (as toxins), potentially differentiating variables, it is nec­
precursors are seen as pathogens or simply essary to examine the relationships between
early forms of the disorder, and course is predictors and problem behaviors over time
viewed as linear. Too often problems are con­ and across ages. Some variables may have
sidered conditions that children have. Thus, stronger differential links with onset of prob­
the role of experience is relatively neglected lems, while others may be more tied to persis­
in research on childhood problems and there tence or desistance (August et al., 1996). Only
is a preoccupation with finding the “gene that longitudinal research can resolve these issues.
causes” disorder or the locus of neuropathol­ Lastly, much more research is needed on
ogy. Little research is conducted on experien­ processes of continuity and change, again
tial risk factors, early adaptation, or processes with renewed emphasis on experiential factors
of change. “Treatment” is viewed narrowly as (e.g., changing support and guidance of the
symptom management and few guides for child). With regard to desistance, both early
early intervention or primary prevention are experiential antecedents (which may provide
uncovered. a foundation for resiliency) and contemporary
More extensive and encompassing research supports command study. In general, there has
is needed on risk factors for disorder. As Rut­ been far too little investigation of the interac­
ter (1996) has recently stated “the understand­ tion between prior adaptation and current
ing of environmental risk factors in both de­ risks or changing support.
pressive and (even more so) anxiety disorders In conclusion, within a developmental ap­
in children and adolescents is decidedly lim­ proach problems are viewed as adaptations.
266 L. A. Sroufe

They may be compromising of development tervention. As with resilience, coping capac­


to be sure, but as adaptations they are subject ity, and personality in general, maladaptation
to change as well as forces for continuity. and, ultimately, disorder may also be pre­
This is especially true as challenges to adapta­ sumed to develop. The same laws that govern
tion are changed. Understanding pathways of normal development govern the pathological
adaptation has promise for both effective pre­ as well (Cicchetti, 1984; Cicchetti & Sroufe,
vention and broadened approaches to later in- 1976; Loevinger, 1976).

References
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resil­
(1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl­ ience as process. Development and Psychopathology,
baum. 5, 517–528.
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Eisenberg, L. (1977). Development as a unifying concept
statistical manual of mental disorders. (3rd ed. rev.). in psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 131,
Washington, DC: Author. 225–237.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Elicker, J., Englund, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Predict­
statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.). ing peer competence and peer relationships in child­
Washington, DC: Author. hood from early parent–child relationships. In R.
Anderson, J., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. Parke & G. Ladd (Eds.), Family–peer relationships:
(1987). DSM III disorders in preadolescent children: Modes of linkage (pp. 77–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl­
Prevalence in a large sample from the general popula­ baum.
tion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 69–76. Fox, N., Kimmerly, N., & Schafer, W. (1991). Attach­
August, G., MacDonald, A., Realmuto, G., & Skare, S. ment to mother/attachment to father: A meta-analysis.
(1996). Hyperactive and aggressive pathways: Effects Child Development, 62, 210–225.
of demographic, family, and child characteristics on Frick, P., & Lahey, B. (1991). The nature and characteris­
children’s adaptive functioning. Journal of Clinical tics of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. School
Child Psychology, 25, 341–351. Psychology Review, 20, 163–173.
Blatt, S. (1995). Representational structures in psycho­ Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989). A twin study of
pathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Roches­ hyperactivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy­
ter symposium on developmental psychopathology: chiatry, 30, 691–709.
Vol. 6. Emotion, cognition, and representation (pp. Gottesman, N. (1995). Environmental and familial ante­
1–34). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. cedents of sixth grade aggression. Unpublished doc­
Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation. New York: Basic Books. toral dissertation. University of Minnesota.
Campbell, S. (1990). The socialization and social devel­ Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behav­
opment of hyperactive children. In M. Lewis & S. ioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychol­
Miller (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psycho­ ogy, 27, 4–13.
pathology (pp. 77–92). New York: Plenum. Greenough, W., Black, J., & Wallace, C. (1987). Experi­
Carlson, E. A longitudinal study of attachment disorgani­ ence and brain development. Child Development, 58,
zation. Manuscript submitted for publication. 539–559.
Carlson, E., Jacobvitz, & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). A devel­ Harrington, R. (1994). Affective disorders. In M. Rutter,
opmental study of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent
Child Development, 66, 37–54. psychiatry (pp. 330–350). London: Blackwell.
Caron, C., & Rutter, M., (1991). Comorbidity in child Hinshaw, S., & McHale, J. (1991). Stimulant medications
psychopathology: Concepts, issues, and research and the social interactions of hyperactive children. In
strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia­ D. Gilbert & J. Connolly (Eds.), Personality, social
try, 32, 1063–1079. skills and psychopathology (pp. 229–253). New York:
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental Plenum.
psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1–7. Hobson, R. P., & Patrick, M. (1995). Emotion, cognition,
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. (Eds.) (1996). Developmental and representation: The interpersonal domain. In D.
pathways: Diversity in process and outcome [Special Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on
Issue]. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600. developmental psychopathology: Vol. 6. Emotion,
Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1976). The relationship cognition, and representation (pp. 167–190). Roches­
between affective and cognitive development in ter, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Down’s syndrome infants. Child Development, 47, Jacobvitz, D., Sroufe, L. A., Stewart, M., & Leffert, N.
920–929. (1990). Treatment of attentional and hyperactivity
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and problems in children with sympathomimetic drugs: A
self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development comprehensive review. Journal of the American
and Psychopathology, 6, 533–549. Academy of Child Psychiatry, 29, 677–688.
Cohen, J., & Stewart, I. (1994). The collapse of chaos. Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1987). The early care-
New York: Penguin Books. giver–child relationship and attention deficit disorder
Crick, N., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformula­ with hyperactivity in kindergarten. Child Develop­
tion of social information-processing mechanisms in ment, 58, 1488–1495.
children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, Kraemer, G. (1992). A psychobiological theory of attach­
115, 74–101. ment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 493–511.
Psychopathology as development 267

Lazare, A. (1973). Hidden conceptual models in clinical Sameroff, A., & Fiese, B. (1989). Transactional regula­
psychiatry. New England Journal of Medicine, 288, tion and early intervention. In S. J. Meisels & J.
345–350. Shonkoff (Eds.), Early intervention: A handbook of
Loeber, R. (1991). Questions and advances in the study theory, practice, and analysis. New York: Cambridge
of developmental pathways. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth University Press.
(Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psy­ Santostefano, S. (1978). A biodevelopmental approach to
chopathology (Vol. 3, pp. 97–115). Rochester, NY: clinical child psychology. New York: Wiley.
Rochester University Press. Schachar, R., & Wachsmuth, R. (1990). Hyperactivity
Loeber, R., Brinthaupt, V., & Green, S. (1990). Attention and parental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psy­
deficits, impulsivity, and hyperactivity with or with­ chology and Psychiatry, 31, 381–392.
out conduct problems: Relationships to delinquency Schore, A. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the
and unique contextual factors. In R. McMahon & R. self. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Peters (Eds.), Behavior disorders of adolescence: Re­ Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant–caregiver attach­
search, intervention, and policy in clinical and school ment in development. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski
settings (pp. 39–61). New York: Plenum. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 18–
Loeber, R., Wang, P., Keenan, K., Giroux, B., Stou- 38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
thamer–Loeber, M., VanKammen, W., & Maughan, Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and malad­
B. (1993). Developmental pathways in disruptive aptation: Psychopathology as developmental devia­
child behaviors. Development and Psychopathology, tion. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Rochester symposium on
5, 103–134. development psychopathology (Vol. 1, pp. 13–40).
Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jossey–Bass. Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents’ unresolved trau­ the self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Transi­
matic experiences are related to infant disorganized tions from infancy to childhood: The self (pp. 281–
attachment status. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & 307). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E., & Shulman, S. (1993). Individ­
years (pp. 161–182). Chicago: University of Chicago uals in relationships: Development from infancy
Press. through adolescence. In D. Funder, R. Parke, C. Tom-
Masten, A., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1995). Competence, re­ linson–Keasey, & K. Widman (Eds.), Studying lives
silience, and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. through time: Personality and development (pp. 315–
Cohen (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology (Vol. 342). Washington, DC: American Psychological As­
2, pp. 715–752). New York: Wiley. sociation.
Moffitt, T. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (in press). One
persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental tax­ social world. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.),
onomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701. Relationships as developmental contexts: The Minne­
Patterson, G., DeBaryshe, B., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A sota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 29). Hills­
developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
American Psychologist, 44, 329–335. Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1991). Illustrations of per­
Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Sroufe, L. A., & son and environment interaction from a longitudinal
Mangelsdorf, S. (1989). Early childhood antecedents study. In T. Wachs & R. Plomin (Eds.), Conceptual­
of aggression and passive withdrawal in early elemen­ ization and measurement of organism–environment
tary school. Journal of Personality, 57, 257–281. interaction (pp. 68–84). Washington, DC: American
Richters, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Mark Twain meets Psychological Association.
DSM-III–R: Conduct disorder development, and the Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The
concept of harmful dysfunction. Development and fate of early experience following developmental
Psychopathology, 5, 5–29. change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adap­
Rieder, C., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Organizational per­ tation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363–
spective on cognitive control functioning and cogni­ 1373.
tive-affective balance in maltreated children. Devel­ Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of devel­
opmental Psychology, 25, 382–393. opmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55,
Robins, L., & Price, R. (1991). Adult disorders predicted 17–29.
by childhood conduct problems. Psychiatry, 54, 116– Suess, G. J., Grossmann, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992).
132. Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on
Rogosch, F., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, L. (1995). The role quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to in­
of child maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive dividual organization of self. International Journal of
and affective processing abilities and later peer rela­ Behavioral Development, 15, 43–66.
tionships. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 591– Taylor, E. (1994). Syndromes of attention deficit and
610. overactivity. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov
Rutter, M. (1980). Introduction. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Sci­ (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 285–
entific foundations of developmental psychiatry. Lon­ 307). London: Blackwell.
don: Heineman. Thelen, E. (1990). Dynamical systems and the generation
Rutter, M. (1996). Developmental psychopathology: of individual differences. In J. Colombo & J. Fagan
Concepts and prospects. In M. Lenzenweger & J. (Eds.), Individual differences in infancy (pp. 19–43).
Havgaard (Eds.), Frontiers of developmental psycho­ Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
pathology (pp. 209–237). New York: Oxford Univer­ Troy, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1987). Victimization among
sity Press. preschoolers: The role of attachment relationship his­
Rutter, M., Taylor, E., & Hersov, L. (1994). Child and tory. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
adolescent psychiatry. London: Blackwell. Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 166–172.
268 L. A. Sroufe

Vaughn, B., Egeland, B., Waters, E., & Sroufe, L. A. Warren, S., Egeland, B., Huston, L., & Sroufe, L. A. (in
(1979). Individual differences in infant–mother at­ press). Child and adolescent anxiety disorders and
tachment at 12 and 18 months: Stability and change early attachment. Journal of the American Academy
in families under stress. Child Development, 50, 971– of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
975. Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1992). Overcoming the odds.
Waddington, C. (1957). The strategy of genes. London: Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Allen & Unwin. Zametkin, A., Nordahl, T., Gross, M., King, A., Semple,
Wakefield, J. C. (1992a). The concept of mental disorder: W., Ramsey, J., Hamburger, S., & Cohen, R. (1990).
On the boundary between biological facts and social Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperac­
values. American Psychologist, 47, 373–388. tivity of childhood onset. New England Journal of
Wakefield, J. C. (1992b). Disorder as harmful dysfunc­ Medicine, 323, 1361–1366.
tion: A conceptual critique of DSM III–R’s definition Zeenah, C., Mammen, O., & Lieberman, A. (1994). Dis­
of mental disorder. Psychological Review, 99, 232– orders of attachment. In Handbook of infant mental
247. health (pp. 332–349). New York: Guilford Press.

Вам также может понравиться