Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 93

MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 1

Managing the Aging Workforce

Guido Hertel1 and Hannes Zacher2


1
University of Münster
2
University of Groningen

Chapter to appear in Viswesvaran, C., Anderson, N., Ones, D. S., & Sinangil, H. K. The SAGE

Handbook of Industrial, Work, & Organizational Psychology, 2nd Edition, Volume 3.

Author Contact

Guido Hertel (corresponding author), University of Münster , Department of Psychology,

Fliednerstrasse 21, 48149 Münster, Germany, Phone: +49 251 8334161, e-mail: ghertel@uni-

muenster.de

Hannes Zacher, University of Groningen, Department of Psychology, Grote Kruisstraat

2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands, Phone: +31 50363 6187, e-mail: h.zacher@rug.nl
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 2

Abstract

The aging of the workforces in most developed and many developing countries has significant

implications for employees, human resource management, organizations, and societies.

However, the prevailing perception of these demographic changes is often negative based on

broader stereotypes and misconceptions of age at work. The goal of this chapter is to provide an

integrative account of extant knowledge on work and aging that corrects existing

misconceptions, emphasizes (partly neglected) potentials particularly of older workers, and helps

maximizing the opportunities of an aging workforce. We first review key demographic trends in

the 21st century as well as their reasons and implications. Second, we present an overview of

common age stereotypes and their potential effects on discrimination in organizations. Third, we

outline important theoretical frameworks and methodological considerations in the field of work

and aging. Fourth, we review empirical findings on relationships between age and cognitive

abilities, occupational health and workability, work motivation, self-regulation and socio-

emotional skills, work performance, and occupational well-being. We conclude the chapter by

outlining implications for an age-inclusive human resource management and future research

needs in the growing field of work and aging.

Keywords: Aging workforce; demographic change; lifespan development; age stereotypes; age-

inclusive management
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 3

Introduction

The unprecedented and rapid aging of populations and workforces in most developed and

many developing countries has significant implications for employees, human resource

management, organizations, and societies as a whole (Carstensen, 2009; Chand & Tung, 2014;

Ilmarinen, 2009; Vaupel & Loichinger, 2006). Over the past 15 years, relationships between age

and work outcomes have been discussed in several authoritative books (P. M. Bal, Kooij, &

Rousseau, 2015; Czaja & Sharit, 2009; Field, Burke, & Cooper, 2013; Finkelstein, Truxillo,

Fraccaroli, & Kanfer, 2015; Hedge & Borman, 2012; Shultz & Adams, 2007a), review articles

(Bowen, Noack, & Staudinger, 2011; Farr & Ringseis, 2002; Hertel, Thielgen, et al., 2013;

Schalk et al., 2010; Truxillo, Cadiz, & Hammer, 2015; Warr, 2001), special issues of

organizational psychology journals (e.g., B. B. Baltes & Finkelstein, 2011; Deller & Hertel,

2009; Hertel, Van der Heijden, de Lange, & Deller, 2013; Truxillo & Fraccaroli, 2013), and

meta-analyses (e.g., Kooij, De Lange, Jansen, Kanfer, & Dikkers, 2011; Ng & Feldman, 2008,

2010; Rauschenbach, Krumm, Thielgen, & Hertel, 2013). Moreover, since 2015, the new journal

Work, Aging and Retirement provides a central forum for evidence-based and translational

research on worker aging and retirement (Wang, 2015). While research on work and aging is

clearly gaining momentum, workforce aging is still often viewed negatively by practitioners,

focusing on difficulties and conflicts instead of potentials and opportunities. This negative bias is

partly due to stereotypes and misconceptions of age at work and older workers. The goal of this

chapter is to provide an integrative account of extant knowledge on work and aging that corrects

existing misconceptions, emphasizes (partly neglected) potentials particularly of older workers,

and helps maximizing the opportunities of an aging workforce.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 4

We begin with a review of key demographic trends related to workforce aging in the 21st

century, as well as their reasons and implications. Second, we present an overview of popular

age stereotypes that often determine how demographic changes are currently addressed in

organizations, partly leading to age discrimination. Third, we outline central theoretical

frameworks and methodological considerations in the field of work and aging. Fourth, we review

empirical findings on relationships between age and both workers’ capabilities and work

motivation, as well as work outcomes such as performance and occupational well-being. Based

on these concepts and findings, we derive evidence-based implications for an age-inclusive

human resource management (HRM) in organizations, as well as implications for individual

workers and society. To move research on aging at work forward, we conclude the chapter by

outlining desirable characteristics of future studies in this area.

Demographic Change: Reasons and Implications

The populations in nearly all countries and world regions are aging at a faster rate than

ever before in human history – yet there is great diversity in terms of their current and projected

proportions of older people (Phillips & Siu, 2012). Population aging is driven by low or falling

fertility rates (i.e., number of live births per woman) and continuous increases in life expectancy.

Due to improvements in health care, nutrition, lifestyles, safety, technologies, and workplaces,

the global average life expectancy has increased from 48 years in 1950 to 68 years in 2010 (L.

Roberts, 2011). Over the same period, life expectancies have increased in the United States from

69 to 78 years and in Europe from 66 to 75 years, with even greater proportional increases in

Africa and Asia. Global average life expectancy is predicted to further increase to 76 years in

2050 and 81 years in 2100 (Bloom, 2011). In more developed world regions, life expectancy is

likely to be 83 years in 2050 and 88 years in 2100, whereas in less developed regions it is likely
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 5

to be 75 years in 2050 and 80 years in 2100. Over the next decades, population aging in most

industrialized countries is further intensified by the aging of the large cohort of baby boomers

born between 1946 and 1964 (Alley & Crimmins, 2007).

A widely-used indicator to illustrate population aging is the potential support ratio (PSR),

which is calculated by dividing the number of people of traditional “working age” (i.e., 16-64

years) by the number of “older people” 65 and over. According to Gerland et al. (2014), the PSR

in the United States is expected to decline from currently 4.6 to 1.9 in 2100 (80% prediction

interval [PI]: 1.6-2.2), and in Germany from currently 2.9 to 1.4 in 2100 (80% PI: 1.1-1.7).

Especially in European countries such as Germany, Italy, and Spain, which have historically

different immigration patterns than the United States or Australia (cf. Zacher & Griffin, 2015),

the simultaneous aging and shrinking of populations will lead to shortages of skilled workers, as

well as an increased pressure on individual workers to prepare financially for retirement, as there

will not be enough people of working age to support them when they reach retirement age.

One potential solution to the shrinkages of qualified workers sometimes discussed is to

increase immigration, particularly from developing countries. Indeed, overall the world’s

population has increased exponentially over the last 200 years, more than doubled between 1960

and today (Bloom, 2011), and it is estimated that it will continue to grow for the remainder of the

21st century, increasing from 7.2 billion people today to 9.6 billion in 2050 and 10.9 billion in

2100 (80% PI: 9.6-12.3 billion; Gerland et al., 2014). While the populations of more developed

countries are expected to remain relatively stable or shrink in size (one exception is Australia;

Bloom, 2011; Zacher & Griffin, 2015), the largest share of population growth until 2050 (97%)

is taking place in Africa (which accounts for 49% of growth) as well as in China, India, and other

less developed countries. The ratio of total population size in developing countries compared to
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 6

that of developed countries was 2 to 1 in 1950, and is increasing to 6 to 1 by 2050 (L. Roberts,

2011). However, strikingly similar population aging trends to those in developed countries are

also expected in developing countries such as China and India, which at present are the most

populous countries in the world. For instance, the PSR in China is expected to decline from

currently 7.8 to 1.8 in 2100 (80% PI: 1.4-2.3), and in India from currently 10.9 to 2.3 in 2100

(80% PI: 1.5-3.2; Gerland et al., 2014). Thus, while in absolute terms these countries will

continue to have very large working populations, they are rapidly transforming into older

societies and will also face the challenges of population aging toward the end of the 21st century

(James, 2011; Peng, 2011). In the least developed countries (e.g., Ethiopia, Uganda), the PSRs

will also decrease over the next decades, but will still be above current PSRs in developed

countries (L. Roberts, 2011).

The aging of the workforces in most industrialized countries is not only influenced by

population aging, but also by changes in workforce participation patterns, including delayed

workforce entry as well as early or delayed retirement (Alley & Crimmins, 2007; Beehr &

Bennett, 2015; Martin & Xiang, 2015). Factors that influence workforce participation include

improved education and health, increased participation of women, changes in family structures,

the stability of economic conditions, availability of bridge employment and flexible retirement

options, as well as organizations’ attitudes and policies toward older workers (Alley &

Crimmins, 2007). Moreover, in developed countries, the proportionally fastest growth of the

“oldest old” demographic group (85 years and older) has implications for current members of the

workforce (Phillips & Siu, 2012). On the one hand, younger relatives (mostly daughters) may

decide to leave the workforce to provide care to the oldest old – or decide to combine work and

eldercare, which may negatively impact on their productivity (Zacher, Jimmieson, & Winter,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 7

2012). On the other hand, the significant rise in the numbers of oldest old people can also create

opportunities, such as new jobs in health care and service sectors and the introduction of more

flexible working time models (Phillips & Siu, 2012).

Researchers have consistently emphasized that changes in the age structure of

populations and workforces may not only result in economic and societal challenges, but can

also open up many new opportunities for increased productivity and well-being (Bloom, 2011).

Changes in public policies, such as increases in retirement age, encouragement of workforce

participation of women, and enabling more flexible careers (including longer downtimes for

learning, family, or traveling) can strengthen the positive effects of population aging

(Carstensen, 2009; Staudinger, 2015). Vaupel and Loichinger (2006) argued that population

aging offers excellent possibilities to conduct policy-relevant research, for instance, studies on

how educational and work environments should be designed to improve older workers’

productivity and retention in the workforce, and how work can be better distributed across

people and different periods of the lifespan in order to help individuals’ combine work,

education, leisure, and family. Of course, such changes, together with research on how to reduce

work-related stressors and early exists from the workforce, also benefit younger workers,

therefore contributing to an overall humanization of the workplace.

Phillips and Siu (2012) suggested that organizational psychologists are in a particularly

good position to investigate how economic growth will be possible in the context of workforce

aging, as the abilities, needs, and acceptance of older workers are thought to play a central role in

this regard. Indeed, demographic change is likely to lead to a much stronger emphasis on the

work- and non-work interests (e.g., provision of child care) of the decreasing number of highly-

skilled employees in order to attract, motivate, and retain them. In a similar vein, Kulik, Ryan,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 8

Harper, and George (2014) proposed that the challenges for organizational practice imposed by

population and workforce aging offer excellent opportunities for organizational psychologists

and management researchers who study how to make better use of older workers’ knowledge,

skills, and abilities and to better accommodate their changing needs (e.g., physically less

demanding tasks). In sum, the ongoing demographic changes provide excellent opportunities for

the improvement of both the organization and the distribution of work for more effective and

healthy employees. However, if and how these opportunities are used in organizations is partly

contingent on prevailing assumptions and stereotypes about aging and older workers.

Age Stereotypes and Age Discrimination

In spite of the opportunities of demographic developments described, and despite the

need and potential benefits of retaining older workers, the percentage of older workers in

organizations only slowly concur with the ongoing demographic changes. This slow adaptation

is partly due to stereotypes and myths about aging at work and older workers. Such popular

assumptions not only affect the expectations of managers and HRM professionals, but also how

older workers perceive themselves and act accordingly. Therefore, a thorough understanding and

critical reflection of prevailing age stereotypes is a key aspect of managing an aging workforce.

Activation of Age Stereotypes at Work

The age of a worker is an important aspect of general and often automatic assessments by

others. These assessments are influenced by age stereotypes, reflecting expectations and beliefs

about capabilities, attitudes, and behavior on the basis of chronological or perceived age (Levy,

2009; Posthuma & Campion, 2009). Age stereotypes are qualified by context conditions such as

job type or national culture, and gradually change over time. Empirical research of age

stereotypes at work (A. C. Bal, Reiss, Rudolph, & Baltes, 2011; Posthuma & Campion, 2009)
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 9

has revealed both positive and negative expectations and beliefs about older as well as younger

workers (cf. Table 1).The prevalence of these age stereotypes is usually in favor of younger

workers in industrialized countries, in line with a more general youth centeredness of Western

societies today, while being older is predominantly associated with negative connotations of

performance decreases, physical decline, dependency on others, and costs to society (Staudinger,

2015; Zacher, 2015).

< Insert Table 1 about here >

Interestingly, today only very few of these age stereotypes correspond with empirical

data, most others do not, and for some even the opposite seems to be true (Hertel, Van der

Heijden, et al., 2013; Ng & Feldman, 2012b, cf. Table 1). Therefore, reliance on age stereotypes

is often misleading and unduly reduces the employability of older workers. For instance,

contrary to the general assumption that older workers are more reluctant to change, a recent

study with more than 2,900 workers from different companies in Europe found a slight but

significant negative correlation between age and resistance to change (Kunze, Boehm, & Bruch,

2013; see also Ng & Feldman, 2012b, for meta-analytic evidence). Moreover, meta-analyses do

not support age stereotypes assuming older workers to be less motivated, less trusting, or more

vulnerable to work-family imbalance (Ng & Feldman, 2012b). Finally, neither job performance

nor emotional resilience have been shown to be generally related to age in meta-analyses (e.g.,

Ng & Feldman, 2008; Rauschenbach, Goritz, & Hertel, 2012). Thus, critical reflection and

empirical examination of popular assumptions about older workers and aging at work are crucial,

particularly as aging effects may have changed in the last decades due to improved work

conditions and health management. Indeed, some of the prevailing age stereotypes seem to

originate from work conditions some fifty years ago, with only slow adaptations to the current
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 10

situation. Moreover, observations of very old people (> 80 years of age) might be

overgeneralized to “older” workers. Such misperceptions are further stabilized by confirmation

biases that overstate information consistent and discount information inconsistent with age

stereotypes (Ng & Feldman, 2012b). Finally, self-stereotyping and stereotype threat among older

workers can cause behavior consistent with specific age stereotypes, supporting these stereotypes

as self-fulfilling prophecies (Finkelstein, King, & Voyles, 2015; Staudinger, 2015; Von Hippel,

Kalokerinos, & Henry, 2012).

The prevalence of age stereotypes is qualified by characteristics of the assessor, such as

age (Posthuma & Campion, 2009). Older as compared to younger assessors seem to be more

differentiated and sometimes more tolerant with both older and younger workers (Rauschenbach

et al., 2012), probably because they have been younger workers themselves, and therefore

possess more valid information to adopt their perspective of the other group (Finkelstein, Burke,

& Raju, 1995). A second group of moderators of age stereotypes at work include job

characteristics and job requirements. For instance, being a computer programmer might be seen

as more typical for younger workers, whereas being a doctor may be more typical for older

workers (Perry, Dokko, & Golom, 2012). Thus, jobs can also be “stereotyped” with regard to the

typical or appropriate age of job incumbents. Perry and Finkelstein (1999) suggested that the fit

between perceived job requirements and worker age might be an important predictor of

managers’ performance judgments, support, and rewards. However, empirical research supports

this assumption only for young- but not for old-typed jobs (Finkelstein et al., 1995). Finally,

older and younger workers also have different (and often false) assumptions of how they are

perceived by the respective other group (e.g., Finkelstein, Ryan, & King, 2013).

Age Stereotypes and Discrimination at Work


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 11

Although stereotypes are conceptually different from prejudice and discrimination (Fiske,

2004), negative stereotypes can be the starting point of discriminatory behavior at work, causing

strain (Sánchez Palacios, Torres, & Blanca Mena, 2009) and performance impediments for the

focused persons in addition to potential litigation costs (Ng & Feldman, 2012b). In addition,

managerial decisions about rewards and career opportunities might be distorted, reducing

organizational success. Finally, self-fulfilling prophecies and stereotype threat might hinder older

workers from releasing their full potential at work (Staudinger, 2015; Von Hippel et al., 2012;

Zaniboni, 2015). For instance, when confronted with low expectations from their supervisors,

older workers might expect their learning abilities to be low, which in turn might reduce their

interest in training activities. As a consequence, supervisors might perceive their pre-

assumptions confirmed. Indeed, the negative relation between age and training motivation can be

significantly buffered by supervisors’ favorable beliefs about older workers (Van Vianen,

Dalhoeven, & De Pater, 2011). Moreover, Greller and Stroh (2004) showed that negative

stereotypes about development ability play a central role in predicting motivation, and,

eventually, the retirement of older workers (see also Gaillard & Desmette, 2008). Thus, while a

negative correlation between workers’ age and their training motivation was the only confirmed

age stereotype in the meta-analysis by Ng and Feldman (2012b), this correlation can at least

partly be explained by self-fulfilling prophecies and stereotype threat mechanisms rather than

genuine aging effects. In summary, remedies against age discrimination are an important step for

managing an aging workforce.

Theories on Aging (at Work)

There is so far no single integrative theory of aging in psychology (B. B. Baltes, Rudolph,

& Bal, 2012; V. L. Bengtson, Gans, Putney, & Silverstein, 2009). Instead, different theories
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 12

focus on different phenomena contingent on chronological age (e.g., anticipating, learning,

deciding, and striving). Moreover, these processes are embedded in micro and macro processes

of aging described in models from biology and sociology (see B. B. Baltes et al., 2012; V. L.

Bengtson et al., 2009, for more details). We briefly summarize the main assumptions of the

(currently) most relevant theories in this field, starting with more general scientific approaches

and ending in specific theories from work and organizational psychology.

Aging from a Biological and Neurological Perspective

Biology and neurology describe aging processes at a micro-level, focusing on physical

body changes of individuals as a function of time or chronological age. Most of these theories

conceptualize aging as cumulative damage due to effects of environmental factors on

individuals’ organisms. Whereas stochastic theories consider these environmental damages to be

random, development-genetic theories assume that aging effects are genetically controlled by

biological mechanisms (e.g., secretion of hormones, declines of the immune system, effects of

free radicals; see B. B. Baltes et al., 2012, for a review). Nevertheless, redundancy in human

body systems can compensate for many of such environmental damages, for instance, by

replacing brain cells with cells from other areas, and thus contribute to functionality and higher

life expectancy (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). Biological theories have direct implications for

the estimation of workers’ physical strength, health, and resilience. Moreover, the experience of

biological changes affects the subjective experience of aging and outcomes such as well-being

and self-efficacy (Staudinger, 2015). Therefore, biological theories are important building blocks

for an understanding of psychological changes when workers age. Indeed, these theories are

often used to predict declines of work-related capabilities with higher age (Kanfer & Ackerman,

2004; Warr, 2001).


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 13

Age Effects from a Sociology and Economics Perspective

Sociologists and economists examine aging processes at a macro-level, considering the

embedment of aging processes in society and economic contexts. For instance, the specific

historical context with both negative (e.g., war, economic depression) and positive events and

developments (e.g., new education systems, technological discoveries, economic growth), affect

workers’ experiences and development in addition to mere bodily changes (P. B. Baltes, 1987;

Vl L. Bengtson, Putney, & Johnson, 2005). These differences in historical or economical context

can help explain differences in the societal perception (and stereotyping) of older workers,

varying considerably across different time periods (e.g., 1900 as compared to 2000) or cultures

(e.g., United States as compared to Japan). Interestingly, these societal expectations are not

always fitting to the existing structures and requirements of organizations. The current misfit

between age stereotypes and the need to retain older workers in many countries is a good

example. Moreover, stability and wealth of economic systems together with political decisions

(e.g., on retirement age) determine work time perspectives and retirement decisions of workers.

Sociology and economics describe such context conditions as important complements for a more

holistic and complete picture of aging processes at work (Mayer, 2009).

Together, biological theories and theories from sociology and economics describe

important framework conditions in which aging at work takes place. Consequently, both

biological processes (mostly decreasing resources) and context conditions in work organizations

(often detrimental for older workers) influence aging at work. However, it should be noted that

workers not only react to biological aging and social context conditions but also influence

context conditions in their work organizations, for instance by changing organizational policies

and age-related climate, by job crafting and requests for age-inclusive job design (Heckhausen,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 14

Wrosch, & Schulz, 2010). While biological aging processes cannot be stopped or reversed

entirely, individual workers do have considerable control how fast biological functions relevant

for work ability wear out, for instance, by specific risk and health management at work, but also

by maintaining a healthy and active lifestyle in general. These activities should be supported by

employers and organizations as part of management an aging workforce.

Aging from a Psychological Perspective

Psychological aging theories focus on individuals’ experience of aging, both in reaction

to as well as in anticipation of time-related changes. In doing so, they consider both bodily

changes described in biological approaches of aging as well as the social (and societal) context in

which aging takes place, as described in sociological and economics perspectives. Moreover, a

genuine psychological approach towards aging includes the individual anticipation of and

reactions to these bodily changes and context conditions. Thus, psychological models address the

perception and coping with the fact that time (a limited resource) is passing. Next, we describe

the main ideas of psychological theories of aging, and link them to work-related processes.

Lifespan Development Theories. The general idea of lifespan development theories is

that humans face different challenges across their individual lifespan that require learning and

adaptation as well as decisions about changing resources (P. B. Baltes, 1987). These challenges

occur due to the ongoing interplay of both individuals’ abilities and needs and environmental

requirements and resources. However, in contrast to early conceptualizations of developmental

stages in theories on child development (e.g., Piaget, 1947), theories of development during

adulthood – which are particularly relevant for aging at work – are still rare. One early prominent

approach covering the whole lifespan was introduced by Erikson (1950), suggesting eight

specific stages of psychosocial development during which individuals have to negotiate between
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 15

biological forces and sociocultural requirements. Four of these stages are placed in the usual

lifespan at work, and include central themes and developmental tasks such as developing a work-

related identity (late adolescence), committing to long-term relationships (young adulthood),

taking responsibility for others both at work and in private life (“generativity”; middle

adulthood), and becoming a retired person (late adulthood). While the age range for each stage

can vary, for instance, when it takes more time to achieve skills needed for a specific stage (e.g.,

longer education times in industrialized societies), the different developmental tasks provide

potential for high satisfaction at different points in life. Indeed, recent research has revealed

generativity as an important work value particularly of older workers (Hertel, Thielgen, et al.,

2013; Kooij et al., 2011).

Instead of discrete developmental stages, more recent lifespan development approaches

describe continuous processes that vary in their relative importance. The lifespan theory of

control (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995) conceptualizes age differences in the relative importance

of two basic mechanisms of self-control. Primary control strategies are directed at the external

environment in order to fulfill individual needs. Secondary control strategies refer to

intrapersonal changes of thinking and feeling in order to compensate, maintain, or expand

existing levels of primary control (see also Brandtstädter & Renner, 1990, for a similar

conceptualization of assimilation of and accommodation to the environment). Age-contingent

primary control strategies are assumed to follow an inverted U-shape pattern, with growing

primary control during adolescence and young adulthood, a plateau during adulthood, and a

decline during late adulthood when both internal resources and external resources diminish.

Secondary control, in contrast, is assumed to increase constantly during the lifespan, reflecting

growing self-management skills. The lifespan theory of control and the more recent motivational
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 16

theory of lifespan development (Heckhausen et al., 2010) provide important building blocks for

understanding age-related changes of motivation and self-regulation at work (e.g., Hertel,

Rauschenbach, Thielgen, & Krumm, 2015; Scheibe & Zacher, 2013).

A second group of lifespan development theories emphasize goal orientations and

behavior coordination as central age-contingent processes. The general idea is again that

changing biological resources and sociocultural opportunities require individuals to change their

strategies for successful development. Among the most prominent examples is the selection,

optimization, and compensation (SOC) model developed by P. B. Baltes and Baltes (1990).

According to this model, behavioral strategies of selection (choosing alternative contexts,

outcomes, or goals), optimization (allocation or refinement of resources to achieve goals), and

compensation (replacing activities that are no longer achievable) should be used more frequently

when people age in order to cope with declining resources and capabilities. Illustrating the SOC

strategies, P. B. Baltes and Baltes (1990, p. 26) refer to a television interview with the famous

concert pianist Arthur Rubinstein, who maintained high levels of artistic performance even at a

high age. Being asked how he compensated for age-related declines of motor speed and

flexibility, Rubinstein answered that he now plays fewer pieces (selection) and practices these

pieces more often (optimization). Moreover, prior to fast parts that are difficult to play, he now

slows down his speed so that the following part appears faster, using contrast effects as

compensation strategy.

While the SOC model has been initially validated in research across the entire lifespan,

recent studies have also shown their relevance in work contexts (Bajor & Baltes, 2003; B. B.

Baltes & Heydens-Gahir, 2003; Schmitt, Zacher, & Frese, 2012; Weigl, Müller, Hornung,

Zacher, & Angerer, 2013; Wiese, Freund, & Baltes, 2000; Yeung & Fung, 2009; Zacher & Frese,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 17

2011). For instance, Weigl et al. (2013) demonstrated that older nurses in hospitals report

significantly fewer health problems when using SOC strategies. While age was generally

negatively related to work ability, older nurses who used SOC strategies had about the same

level of work ability as their younger colleagues. These results are promising also with respect to

potential applications of the SOC model in training activities.

Similar to the SOC model, socio-emotional selectivity theory (SST; e.g., Carstensen,

2006) considers behavioral consequences of aging in terms of changes in goal priorities.

However, SST describes aging predominantly in terms of subjective perceptions of (remaining)

time rather than changing biological or contextual resources. According to SST, individuals

select goals and priorities as a function of the future time they perceive. When future time is

perceived as unlimited, information acquisition and expanding social networks are more strongly

prioritized. However, when future time is increasingly perceived as constrained, feelings and

emotional well-being are more strongly emphasized, and people focus on the present rather than

the future (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999). This idea is similar to rational choice

models, assuming that persons (consciously or unconsciously) calculate potential returns of their

investments. When future time is unlimited, it is beneficial to invest in learning activities that

might pay off in the future. However, when future time and potential benefits from learned skills

are constrained, investing in costly learning activities might not be reasonable.

Notably, the focus of SST on psychological perceptions instead of biological resources or

societal opportunities provides interesting opportunities for the prevention of some negative

aging effects. For instance, the prospect of being able to work as a coach or trainer after

retirement might increase learning motivation of older workers. Moreover, while other lifespan

models usually refer to chronological age, SST can be adopted to different domains
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 18

irrespectively of chronological age. For instance, when applied to remaining time in a specific

job (e.g., Zacher & Frese, 2009), perceptions of limited future time and related behavioral

implications may apply also to young persons, for instance when working on a contract base.

Recently, SST has often been applied in work and organizational psychology research, for

instance to predict age-related changes in work motivation (Hertel, Thielgen, et al., 2013;

Truxillo, Cadiz, Rineer, Zaniboni, & Fraccaroli, 2012), job attitudes (Ng & Feldman, 2010;

Thielgen, Krumm, Rauschenbach, & Hertel, 2015), or reactions to performance feedback (Wang,

Burlacu, Truxillo, James, & Yao, 2015). For instance, SST explains why the fit between

workers’ motives and characteristics of their job (need-supply fit) is more important for older

than for younger workers (Krumm, Grube, & Hertel, 2013b).

Partly building on and extending the SST, Charles (2010) introduced the strength and

vulnerability integration model (SAVI) that considers age-contingent strengths and weaknesses

in emotion regulation in addition to predictions of SST. An important strength of older persons is

the accumulated knowledge from life experience about how to regulate emotions and social

contacts (Blanchard-Fields, 2007). SAVI postulates that these skills should enable older persons

to regulate relatively low levels of stressors better than younger persons, and to better avoid

negative experiences. However, a decreasing flexibility of older persons’ biological systems

should make regulation of high physiological arousal more difficult. Therefore, older persons are

assumed to have more difficulties maintaining their well-being under high levels of stressors,

particularly if these high stressors persist over time. Initial empirical evidence shows promising

results for these different predictions with respect to general life satisfaction (Charles, 2010;

Charles & Carstensen, 2010); however, research applying SAVI in the field of work and

organizational psychology is still lacking (for an initial example, see Scheibe & Zacher, 2013).
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 19

Theories on Cognitive Development. Perhaps the most popular research findings on

aging effects relevant for work refer to changes in cognitive capabilities. Based on dual-

component theories of human intelligence (Cattell, 1943), early cross-sectional research

suggested that fluid intelligence, which involves working memory, abstract reasoning, and

solving of novel problems, increases until early adulthood (mid-twenties) and declines slowly

thereafter. In contrast, crystallized intelligence, that is, general knowledge, vocabulary, and

verbal comprehension, has been shown to increase constantly until high ages (Salthouse, 2012).

The early decline of fluid intelligence was attributed to production deficiencies (i.e., older

persons being less able to show their capabilities) or processing deficiencies due to biological

processes (i.e., older persons loosing processing speed), with the latter account receiving more

empirical evidence (Salthouse, 2013). However, these results have been criticized on

methodological grounds, for instance, a high reliance on cross-sectional research designs that

might suffer from cohort effects due to different learning environments. Indeed, longitudinal

research (e.g., Schaie, 2013) suggests that decreases in fluid intelligence generally occur much

later in life, and might increasingly fan out with higher age. Moreover, the measures of fluid

intelligence might disadvantage older persons because the tasks are more similar to tasks trained

in school than usual tasks in regular jobs (Ackerman, 1996). In sum, the distinction between fluid

and crystallized intelligence has been helpful to integrate different age-contingent trajectories in

cognitive capabilities; however, recent research seems to suggest that age-effects are not as

imperative as is often assumed.

Theories on Personality Development. While temporal stability was often considered as

a defining feature of personality traits (e.g., P. T. Costa & McCrae, 1994), this has been

criticized for various reasons (e.g., Mischel, 1969, for a prominent early critique). More recent
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 20

approaches now discuss temporal variability and development of personality characteristics (B.

W. Roberts, 2009). Driving forces of such development can be both biological processes (e.g.,

changes in the serotonergic and dopaminergic systems; DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2002) as

well as environmental factors (e.g., enduring stress at work affecting cognition, emotion, and

behavior; B. W. Roberts & Wood, 2006). Moreover, systematic age effects can be predicted

based on lifespan theories. For instance, the general shift of older persons toward emotion

regulation strategies to reduce negative affect, as predicted by SAVI (Charles, 2010), might

potentially buffer life stressors, leading to lower levels of neuroticism. In a similar way,

establishing more satisfying and supportive relationships with higher age, as predicted by SST

(Carstensen, 2006), might also reduce overall levels of neuroticism (Soto & John, 2012) as well

as openness to new experience (Staudinger, 2015). Indeed, empirical studies have shown that

during the usual work-life of adults (i.e., ages between 20 and 65), neuroticism and openness

decrease slightly whereas conscientiousness (particularly self-discipline and reliability, but not

orderliness) as well as agreeableness increase slightly. However, there is also evidence for

considerable between-person variability as well as for narrow mechanisms acting at the level of

personality facets rather than at the level of higher order factors (B. W. Roberts & Mroczek,

2008; B. W. Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006; Soto & John, 2012; Staudinger, 2015).

Age in Work and Organizational Psychology Theories

Theoretical approaches in work and organizational psychology usually considered

chronological age as a control variable rather than as a substantive construct. Although age

differences at work have been addressed in earlier research, for instance, on stereotyping (Rosen

& Jerdee, 1976), work values (Super, 1980), work attitudes (Rhodes, 1983), expertise (Gordon &

Johnson, 1982), or job performance (Waldman & Avolio, 1986), this initial research was rather
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 21

atheoretical (Kacmar & Ferris, 1989). One reason might be a more general neglect of time and

time-related changes in work and organizational psychology theories (Roe, 2008; Sonnentag,

2012). Moreover, addressing changes over time empirically requires laborious longitudinal

studies. Finally, neglecting workers’ age is in line with a general youth centeredness and

disrespect of older workers in industrialized societies. However, neglecting age and time renders

work and organizational psychology theories unspecific and imprecise. The frequent reliance on

younger adults (e.g., students) in empirical research might even lead to wrong conclusions.

In the last few years, the consequences of demographic changes in many organizations

(increasing average age of workers, increasing age diversity, increasing need for qualified

professionals) as well as industrialized societies (challenged pension systems) have triggered

many research programs on age at work. Moreover, established theories from work and

organizational psychology have been fruitfully connected with lifespan models of age-related

changes, providing more differentiated theoretical approaches for this evolving research. As one

of the first examples, Warr (1993) predicted age differences in job performance integrating

theories on work characteristics and age-related changes in cognitive capabilities. Negative age

effects were predicted in jobs with continuous and paced data-processing and rapid learning, and

positive age effects in jobs with knowledge-based judgments with no time pressure. Moreover,

Warr (2001) pointed out that age-related changes also affect self-efficacy perceptions of workers,

leading to (mostly negative) implications of aging for work motivation, such as habituation to

incentives (“hedonic treadmill”), development of routines and habits that might impede

innovation, and social comparison with younger colleagues.

Extending this approach, Kanfer and Ackerman (2004) proposed moderating effects of

worker age on perceived contingencies between effort, utilities, and performance. For instance,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 22

age is expected to decrease the perceived contingency between effort and performance outcomes

in jobs that require high levels of fluid intelligence (e.g., air traffic controller), but increase this

contingency in jobs that require high levels of crystallized intelligence (e.g., elementary school

teachers; cf. Kanfer & Ackerman, 2004, p. 449). Moreover, age is assumed to decrease the

perceived contingency between effort and subjective utility at work because career perspectives

and related achievement motives decline with age. Kanfer, Beier, and Ackerman (2013) further

differentiated these thoughts for different types of motivation (i.e., motivation to work,

motivation at work, and motivation to retire), considering intrapersonal factors (e.g., self-

efficacy, attitudes, personality), context variables (e.g., finances, health), work conditions (e.g.,

autonomy, age climate), and societal/economic context variables (e.g., retirement policies) (see

also Ng & Feldman, 2013b). In contrast to the common perspective that fluid and crystallized

intelligence are independent resources that might compensate each other, McDaniel, Pesta, and

Banks (2012) pointed out that fluid and crystallized intelligence are positively related at work

because fluid intelligence is conducive to accumulating knowledge and skills over time.

In addition to age-related differences in motivation and performance, researchers have

started to integrate age-related changes in models of occupational health and well-being. For

instance, Scheibe and Zacher (2013) integrated models of stress and affective experiences at

work with lifespan models of emotion regulation. According to their model, age and age-related

emotional competencies should not only affect the initial appraisal of work events and the

management of stress and emotions during these events, but also the types and frequency of

work events encountered. As a consequence, older as compared to younger workers should be

less affected by social stressors due to better coping strategies. However, older workers are also

expected to be more vulnerable to extreme job stressors, such as highly intense and/or prevailing
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 23

stressors. Zacher, Feldman, and Schulz (2014) extended these predictions by a person-

environment fit perspective, integrating changing job demands (e.g., increasing learning

requirements) with age-related person changes, resulting in implications for age-inclusive job

design and staffing. In general, congruency between workers’ age-related strengths and task

requirements seems to be not only conducive for high job performance (Kanfer & Ackerman,

2004; Warr, 1993), but also for health and well-being at work (Krumm et al., 2013b; Scheibe,

Stamov-Roßnagel, & Zacher, 2015; Truxillo et al., 2012).

Methodological Issues in Research on Work and Aging

In this section, we discuss two categories of important methodological issues in research

on work and aging. First, we focus on the measurement of age and age-related constructs in

organizational research. Second, we outline considerations for research design in the field of

work and aging (see also Baird, Pitzer, Russell, & Bergeman, 2012; Shultz & Adams, 2007b).

Measurement

Most research on work and aging has defined and operationalized age as chronological

age, that is, the time individuals have lived since birth (Schwall, 2012). Reasons for using

chronological age include that it is easily measured, is based on the same underlying metric as

change over time (i.e., months or years), and is widely used and accepted by organizational

practitioners and policy makers. Alternative conceptualizations of age, including functional,

organizational, subjective/psychological, social, and relative age, have been discussed, and initial

studies have shown that these variables exhibit incremental validity above and beyond

chronological age in predicting work outcomes (Barnes-Farrell, Rumery, & Swody, 2002;

Cleveland, Shore, & Murphy, 1997; Kooij, De Lange, Jansen, & Dikkers, 2008; Schwall, 2012).

However, it appears that many of these alternative age definitions overlap to a great extend with
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 24

established age-related constructs such as job and organizational tenure, experience, physical

health, cognitive functioning, positive affectivity, and age differences/diversity. In any case, it is

now generally considered good practice in research on work and aging, including meta-analyses

(e.g., Ng & Feldman, 2013c; Sturman, 2003), to statistically control for other time-related

variables such as job tenure. Related to this issue, researchers have argued that work and aging

research is not advanced by only examining employee age as a moderator variable; studies,

therefore, should also examine mediators of the moderating effects of age (for recent examples,

see P. M. Bal, de Lange, Zacher, & van der Heijden, 2013; Wang et al., 2015; Zacher, 2013).

A common question refers to the definition of “older workers” (Ashbaugh & Fay, 1987).

While some studies use arbitrary cut-offs specified by legislation or advocacy groups to define

older workers (e.g., 40 or 60 years; United Nations, 2007; United States Equal Employment

Opportunity Commission, 2014), most current research on work and aging follows the lifespan

psychology approach (P. B. Baltes, 1987) and recommendations by methodologists (MacCallum,

Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002) and investigates employee age as a continuous variable,

typically ranging from 18 to 65 years and older. In this research, labels such as “younger” and

“older workers” are used for descriptive purposes only. Importantly, even within the group of

older workers (e.g., defined as those 50 years and older), age defined as a continuous variable

can explain meaningful amounts of variance in work outcomes (Zacher, 2013).

While most previous research examines linear relationships between age and work

outcomes, it is important to note that the relationships between age and work outcomes may also

be curvilinear, with mid-career workers differing from or being more similar to either young or

older workers. For instance, research has shown that occupational well-being has a U-shaped

relationship with age (Clark, Oswald, & Warr, 1996; Zacher, Jimmieson, & Bordia, 2014), and
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 25

that task performance has an inverted U-shaped relationship with age in less complex jobs

(McDaniel et al., 2012; Sturman, 2003). Thus, when examining age as predictor or moderator

variable, considering potential curvilinear effects is recommended.

Developmental researchers have argued for a long time that age per se is not an

explanatory variable. Therefore, researchers are well-advised to consider the age-related

mechanisms that predict work outcomes, such as physical, cognitive, motivational, and affective

factors, instead of treating age as a causal variable; particularly as variance in most of

individuals’ experiences and behaviors steadily increases after adolescence and as employees

grow older, rendering the age variable less and less informative over time (Bowen et al., 2011;

Staudinger, 2015). Importantly, relevant age-related mechanisms include not only person-related

factors but also contextual factors on different conceptual levels (e.g., job, team, organization)

and non-work factors (e.g., family, community) which may change with age over time (Zacher,

Feldman, et al., 2014). Psychological research on work and aging has so far mainly focused on

age-related person mechanisms and largely neglected contextual factors as explanations, with the

exception of age discrimination (Shultz & Adams, 2007b; Zacher, Feldman, et al., 2014).

Finally, reliability and validity of measures should be the same for younger and older

employees in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies on work and age. When longitudinal data is

collected, researchers need to ensure that the psychometric properties of their measures do not

change over time and contaminate potential aging effects. This can be demonstrated by testing

for measurement invariance of latent factor loadings and relationships among factors across time

(Hertzog & Nesselroade, 2003; Horn & McArdle, 1992; Krumm et al., 2013b). Moreover, it is

important to avoid test-experience effects in longitudinal studies, which can occur when

participants are tested repeatedly with the same measure (Salthouse, 2014).
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 26

Research Design

Most research on work and aging to date (including meta-analyses) has relied on cross-

sectional research designs, which compare employees of different ages at a single point in time

(Shultz & Adams, 2007b). Cross-sectional studies are not suited to investigate the process of

aging at work, which involves intraindividual changes in employees’ experiences and behaviors

over time (Lindenberger, Von Oertzen, Ghisletta, & Hertzog, 2011). Moreover, cross-sectional

designs do not allow researchers to disentangle aging effects from alternative explanations such

as cohort, measurement period, and selection (or “healthy worker”) effects (Hofer & Piccinin,

2010). Implicitly or explicitly, however, most cross-sectional research on work and age

conducted to date is based on the assumption that age differences observed at one point in time

are the result of intraindividual age-related changes across the working lifespan (Ng & Feldman,

2013b). While cross-sectional findings on age differences in work outcomes can be theoretically

consistent with the notion of intraindividual age-related change over time, they cannot provide

definite empirical evidence for such change. For cross-sectional age differences to be consistent

with intraindividual changes, researchers have to assume that cohort, period, and selection

effects do not exist and that short-term intraindividual variability and measurement error did not

bias the results (Li & Schmiedek, 2002; Zacher, 2015). However, it is important to note that

findings based on cross-sectional designs can be practically important for organizations that have

to manage age differences regardless of the causes for these differences.

Longitudinal research designs, in which the same participants are assessed repeatedly

over time, are necessary to examine intraindividual age-related changes as well as interindividual

differences in change patterns (P. B. Baltes & Nesselroade, 1979; Ferrer & McArdle, 2010).

These studies can range from several months (e.g., across critical career transitions) to decades;
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 27

however, longitudinal studies covering employees’ partial or entire working lifespans are rare

(Judge & Hurst, 2008; Ng & Feldman, 2008). Research on successful aging at work involves

investigating the person-related and contextual resources (and the absence of respective

constraints) that explain increased heterogeneity in work experiences and behaviors with age and

over time, and help employees maintain or improve positive work outcomes (Hansson,

DeKoekkoek, Neece, & Patterson, 1997; Zacher, 2015). Longitudinal data on successful aging at

work can be analyzed using sophisticated approaches such as multi-level (or random coefficient)

modeling, time series and survival analyses, as well as latent growth curve and latent change

score models (Ferrer & McArdle, 2010; Hertzog & Nesselroade, 2003; McArdle, 2009).

However, longitudinal designs also suffer from some limitations, including their inability to rule

out alternative explanations due to cohort effects, selective sampling and drop-out, as well as

retest effects; these limitations can be largely overcome by conducting cohort-sequential designs

with continuous resampling (Baird et al., 2012). Unfortunately, longitudinal and cohort-

sequential research on work and aging is time consuming and expensive (Ng & Feldman, 2008).

A possible alternative is the use of archival data sets such as the Hourehold Income and Labour

Dynamics in Australia Study, the Health and Retirement Study in the United States, and the

Socioeconomic Panel Study in Germany (e.g., Biemann, Zacher, & Feldman, 2012; Fisher et al.,

2014; Heybroek, Haynes, & Baxter, 2015; Warren, 2015).

Only controlled randomized experimental designs can yield strong conclusions about

causal effects on successful aging outcomes. An ideal study would involve random assignment

of participants to an experimental and a control group, strict control of the treatment, and

monitoring of the outcome variable across (parts of) the working lifespan (Salthouse, 2006).

However, such a research design would not only be highly impractical but also unethical. Thus,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 28

developmental researchers recommend applying approximations of this ideal study design as

well as using data gathered with multiple different methodological approaches, including cross-

sectional, longitudinal, intervention, and quasi-experimental designs (Hertzog, Kramer, Wilson,

& Lindenberger, 2009; Salthouse, 2006). Finally, a relatively recent methodological innovation

in research on aging (and work and aging) is the experience sampling approach, which allows

capturing short-term intraindividual fluctuations and change indicative of older people’s

plasticity in functioning (Beal, 2012; Ram & Gerstorf, 2009). In combination with longitudinal

data collection across several years, this approach has also been called “measurement burst

design” (Ram, Gerstorf, Lindenberger, & Smith, 2011). For instance, Ram et al. (2011) showed

that higher levels of cognitive plasticity, lower cardiovascular variability, and emotional

diversity across two weeks were positively related to successful cognitive aging over 13 years.

Empirical Results on Age Differences in Employee and Work Outcomes

After describing the theoretical and methodological basics of research on aging at work,

we now turn to a review of empirically established age differences in the work context (see Table

2 for an overview).

< Insert Table 2 about here >

Cognitive Abilities

Cognitive abilities have received a great deal of attention in the field of work and aging,

because age seems to be reliably associated with different cognitive abilities which, in turn, are

important predictors of task performance at work (Kanfer & Ackerman, 2004; Müller et al.,

2015; Salthouse, 2012). Most research in this area is based on the distinction between fluid and

crystallized intelligence introduced earlier. Studies show that crystallized intelligence is

relatively stable or increasing across the working lifespan. In contrast, cross-sectional studies
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 29

typically show that fluid intellectual abilities peak in the mid-20s and then decrease with age,

with more accelerated decline after age 50 (Verhaeghen & Salthouse, 1997). For instance, Klein,

Dilchert, Ones, and Dages (2015) showed that older executives scored higher on verbal ability

(an indicator of crystallized intelligence) and lower figural and inductive reasoning (indicators of

fluid intelligence) than younger executives. However, longitudinal studies found a much later

onset (around age 60) and more gradual declines in fluid abilities than cross-sectional studies,

with more pronounced declines in later adulthood (Schaie, 2013).

Based on the longitudinal findings on fluid intelligence trajectories, and the compensatory

role of crystallized intelligence, McDaniel et al. (2012) advised researchers to be cautious when

applying results from the cognitive aging literature to older employees because cognitive

declines are unlikely to have a noticeable impact on functioning at work in this age range.

Moreover, improvements in cognitive functioning of up to 1.5 standard deviations across

consecutive birth cohorts within 50 years (i.e., the “Flynn effect”; e.g., Staudinger, 2015) suggest

that today’s older employees have, on average, higher levels of cognitive functioning than older

employees a few decades ago, and these cohort effects are likely to continue in the future due to

sociocultural changes (Gerstorf, Ram, Hoppmann, Willis, & Schaie, 2011; Salthouse, 2015;

Skirbekk, Stonawski, Bonsang, & Staudinger, 2013).

There is also evidence from both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies demonstrating

considerable interindividual variation in older adults’ level of cognitive functioning and

cognitive aging trajectories. Reviews on successful cognitive aging suggest that maintaining an

intellectually, physically, and socially active lifestyle as well as cognitive and physical exercise

interventions can have meaningful short-term effects on older adults’ cognitive functioning, and

slow down the rate of cognitive decline (Au et al., 2014; Hertzog et al., 2009). Research further
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 30

suggests that older adults’ cognitive performance is more consistent and varies less across days,

despite the different average levels of cognitive performance compared to younger adults

(Schmiedek, Lövdén, & Lindenberger, 2013). These findings question the notion of a general

cognitive decline with age, highlighting considerable interindividual differences between aging

adults as well as intraindividual plasticity in functioning up until old age (Staudinger, 2015).

Research on cognitive aging at work has focused on two major issues. In the first stream,

researchers have examined the interplay between age-related differences in cognitive functioning

and work demands and resources on outcomes such as work motivation and performance (Beier

& Kanfer, 2013; Kanfer & Ackerman, 2004; Salthouse, 2012). Most studies in this area so far

were conducted using actual or simulated air traffic control tasks. In this occupation, the

demands on fluid intellectual abilities such as working memory, speed and interference control,

as well as divided and selective attention are very high and therefore age is typically negatively

related to the task performance (e.g., accuracy; Taylor, O'Hara, Mumenthaler, Rosen, &

Yesavage, 2005). However, studies have shown that this negative relationship can be buffered by

high levels of crystallized intelligence and reliance on environmental supports. For instance,

experimental research using air traffic control tasks has shown that the effects of age-related

declines in fluid intelligence on complex task performance can be mitigated by task-specific

experience and compensatory means such as note taking (Morrow et al., 2003; Nunes & Kramer,

2009). Recent field research suggested that age-related changes in cognitive ability only impact

on older workers’ performance when job demands are high and job control is low (Müller et al.,

2015). In sum, the findings is this research stream suggest that older employees can maintain

high levels of performance in complex jobs if they possess relevant experience and the work

environment is designed to buffer age-related declines in fluid intelligence.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 31

In the second line of research on work and cognitive aging, lifespan psychologists and

gerontologists have examined effects of job and occupational characteristics on individuals’ level

of cognitive functioning and age-related trajectories of cognitive functioning before and after

retirement, and well into old age. Based on the “use it or lose it” hypothesis (Salthouse, 2006),

this research investigates whether the mental challenges employees encounter at work and across

the working lifespan (e.g., cognitively stimulating job characteristics) buffer against age-related

cognitive decline and dementia in later adulthood. Then et al. (2014) conducted a review of 17

high-quality studies examining the influence of psychosocial work conditions on the

development of cognitive functioning and dementia over time. They concluded that there is

substantial evidence for protective effects of high levels of job complexity, cognitively

demanding work conditions, and job control on the risk of cognitive decline and dementia in

later adulthood. Thus, this research suggests that work design may have more than short-term

effects as it can help maintain individuals’ cognitive abilities in later phases of the lifespan.

An example for research in this area is the seminal work by Schooler and colleagues.

Building on research initiated in the 1970s, Schooler, Mulatu, and Oates (2004) showed that the

level of “occupational self-directedness” (i.e., job complexity, routinization, and closeness of

supervision) had a positive lagged effect on intellectual functioning and self-directed personality

orientation 20 years later. A more recent study by Fisher et al. (2014) came to similar

conclusions. Using 18-year longitudinal data from the Health and Retirement Study, the

researchers examined the development of cognitive functioning before and after retirement.

Their findings showed that working in an occupation characterized by high mental job demands

was positively related to employees’ cognitive functioning before retirement, and had a small but

reliable protective effect on individuals’ rate of cognitive decline after retirement.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 32

In summary, research on work and cognitive aging is burgeoning. Important tasks for

future research in this area are to provide empirical evidence for the proposed interactive effects

of employee age, cognitive functioning, and different job demands on job performance, and to

conceptually integrate the hitherto disconnected streams in the fields of organizational and

lifespan psychology. With respect to risks, jobs low in complexity appear to be disadvantageous

to aging employees, as they do not allow for compensation through acquired job knowledge and

may contribute to age-related cognitive decline later in life. In contrast, the design of complex,

knowledge-based jobs seems to present an opportunity to maintain older employees’ effective

functioning at work and even contributes to cognitive functioning in old age.

Occupational Health and Workability

A common stereotype suggests that older employees have lower levels of occupational

health than younger employees (Jex, Wang, & Zarubin, 2007; Maertens, Putter, Chen, Diehl, &

Huang, 2012). However, according to a recent meta-analysis by Ng and Feldman (2013a), this

stereotype was only partially supported. Employee age was weakly to moderately related to

objective measures of physical ill-health such as blood pressure (ρ = .34), cholesterol level (ρ =

.20), and body mass index (ρ = .21), as well as to self-reported measures of insomnia (ρ = .12),

and muscle pain (ρ = .14). In contrast, age was unrelated to overall subjective physical health (ρ

= .00), somatic and psychosomatic complaints (ρs = .02 and .03, respectively), depression (ρ = -

.03), and anxiety (ρ = -.01), and weakly negatively related to mental health (ρ = -.05), fatigue (ρ

= -.10), negative mood (ρ = -.10), anger (ρ = -.15), and irritation (ρ = -.09). The findings of this

meta-analysis, however, have to be interpreted in light of the primary studies’ cross-sectional

designs (which do not allow ruling out cohort and selection or “healthy worker” effects) as well
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 33

as restriction in the range of employee ages included (18-58 years, with only 6 of 198 studies

including employees who had an average age of over 50 years).

Generally, the aging process goes along with declines in different physiological and

physical abilities (e.g., sensory, muscle, cardiovascular, respiratory, neurological, and immune

system functions), and with great interindividual variation in aging trajectories. These changes

may impact on employees’ occupational health and well-being, including safety outcomes such

as injuries and accidents (for a comprehensive review, see Maertens et al., 2012). Compensatory

employee behaviors as well as organizational interventions such as job redesign, health

promotion, and improvements in safety climate can help weaken the association between age-

related physiological and physical changes and occupational health outcomes (Jex et al., 2007).

Moreover, research in the field of cognitive epidemiology suggests that age-related physical and

cognitive changes are related; for instance, employees’ GMA has been shown to predict older

employees’ physical and mental health outcomes as well as mortality (McDaniel et al., 2012;

Salthouse, 2012). Overall, research suggests that organizational interventions to maintain

cognitive functioning and occupational health among older employees (e.g., designing mentally

stimulating and ergonomic jobs) or to train compensatory behaviors (e.g., physical exercise) may

help protect employees from age-related decline in physical, mental, and social well-being.

The concept of work ability captures employees’ ability to carry out their work with

regard to physical and psychological job demands and resources, and its proponents consider it

to be a holistic occupational health construct that is crucial in relation to the aging workforce

(Ilmarinen, 2009). Interestingly, work ability is conceptually closely related to the well-

established notion of demands-abilities fit in the person-environment fit literature, which has so

far largely neglected the role of age (Feldman & Vogel, 2009; Perry et al., 2012; Zacher,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 34

Feldman, et al., 2014). Initial studies on work ability were conducted with older municipal

employees in Finland, and demonstrated increased interindividual variation in work ability with

age and identified several occupational characteristics that predicted work ability across an 11-

year period (Tuomi, Ilmarinen, Martikainen, Aalto, & Klockars, 1997). Since the 1990s, work

ability has been primarily investigated in European countries (which are currently aging at a

faster rate than, for instance, the United States), and it has become a popular concept among

organizational practitioners in these countries. Based on this concept, the Work Ability Index

(Tuomi et al., 1997) assesses two independent dimensions: people’s reports of more objective

health indicators (i.e., diagnosed diseases, functional impairments), and people’s subjective

perceptions of their work ability (Martus, Jakob, Rose, Seibt, & Freude, 2010).

An example for more recent research on work ability is a study by Costa and Sartori

(2007), which found that the negative relationship between Italian workers’ age and self-

perceived work ability was weaker in jobs with high mental involvement, high job autonomy,

and low physical demands. Another study with German employees showed that the generally

negative relationship between age and supervisor ratings of work ability was buffered by job

autonomy and action regulation strategies (Weigl et al., 2013). Finally, a paper with three studies

including employees in the United States did not find consistent support for a negative

relationship between age and perceived work ability, possibly due to restriction in age range

(McGonagle, Fisher, Barnes‐Farrell, & Grosch, 2015). Interestingly, however, perceived work

ability had consistent lagged effects on absenteeism, retirement, and disability leave in the

studies.

Work ability appears to be a promising new concept in the area of occupational health

and aging. Further research is needed that examines the conceptual overlap of work ability with
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 35

established person-job fit constructs, and further probes the association between age and work

ability, with a particular focus on predictors of interindividual differences in age-related

trajectories of work ability over time. This research should also include job tenure as a

potentially important mediating variable, as work ability may not necessarily decrease with age,

but with time spent working under certain job conditions.

Work Motivation

In addition to cognitive and physical capabilities, work motivation and related job

attitudes (e.g., commitment, identification; cf. Ng & Feldman, 2010) are further determinants of

job performance that are potentially affected by age differences. Indeed, workers rarely work at

their maximum strength, and lacking skills can often be compensated by high work motivation

and organizational commitment (Warr, 2001). Thus, age-related differences and changes in work

motivation and attitudes are additional building blocks for age-inclusive HRM.

Systematic research on age differences in work motivation is surprisingly new. While

earlier studies on work motivation often considered worker age as one of the conventional

control variables, only few studies primarily focused on age effects on work motivation so far

(Rhodes, 1983; see Rudolph, Baltes, & Zabel, 2013, for a review). This situation has changed in

the last years, partly triggered by seminal work of Warr (2001) and Kanfer and Ackerman

(2004), so that today not only numerous primary studies exist but also meta-analyses (Kooij et

al., 2011; Ng & Feldman, 2010, 2012b). In general, empirical results do not confirm the

pessimistic perspective predicting a decline of work motivation over the years (Stamov-

Roßnagel & Hertel, 2010). If at all, work motivation seem to be rather positively correlated with

workers’ age (e.g., ρ = .11; Ng & Feldman, 2012). Meta-analyses on job attitudes related to work

motivation yield similar results. For instance, older as compared to younger workers report
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 36

higher job involvement (ρ = .25), higher interpersonal and organizational trust (ρs = .17 and .09),

higher affective commitment (ρ = .24), and higher loyalty and organizational identification (ρs =

.21 and .20; Ng & Feldman, 2010). Although these differences are not very large and subject to

various moderating conditions, the fact that they are observed across different samples and work

conditions underlines the robustness of these age differences. Moreover, the findings

theoretically correspond to lifespan theories as well as theories of organizational socialization

(e.g., Schneider, Goldstein, & Smith, 1995), predicting that time at work not only leads to higher

job expertise but also to more general strengths due to life-long learning and selection processes

during which individuals maximize their social and emotional well-being. As a consequence,

older workers seem to be more likely in jobs they enjoy, cooperate with colleagues they like, and

work in organizations they experience as supportive and comfortable (Ng & Feldman, 2010).

In addition to these age-related trends in average work motivation and job attitudes,

significant differences occur with respect to more specific domains of work motivation, as

reflected in work values, needs, and priorities, providing interesting opportunities to attract and

motivate workers of different ages. Empirical results are again mostly consistent with lifespan

theories. For instance, age correlated negatively with career development motivation as well as

with learning motivation, learning self-efficacy, and training motivation in a meta-analysis by Ng

and Feldman (2012; ρs = -.14, -.14, -.17, and -.05, respectively; see also Kooij et al., 2011),

suggesting that priorities shift from information-related towards emotion-related goals across age

(Carstensen, 2009).

In contrast, older workers appear to prioritize goals related to emotion regulation and

positive experiences at work, which is also in line with lifespan theories. For instance, age has

been found to be positively related to use of existing skills (ρ = .10; Kooij et al., 2011) and
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 37

intrinsic motivation (ρ = .21; Ng & Feldman, 2010). Moreover, one of the most robust age

difference relates to autonomy at work (ρ = .27; Kooij et al., 2011). Apart from providing

additional flexibility to handle potential age-related capacity declines, high autonomy at work

can also signal trust and recognition of achievements at work. Thus, autonomy at work is

sometimes part of the role expectations for older workers.

Another way to experience recognition and meaningfulness at work is by helping others

based on expertise and job experience. Interestingly, such generativity goals were neglected in

established work value taxonomies but can be important strengths of older workers (Krumm,

Grube, & Hertel, 2013a). Indeed, age has been shown to be positively related to prosocial

motives (ρ = .09, Kooij et al., 2011), and with a cluster of generativity values at work including

helping others and legacy (r = .08; Krumm et al., 2013a). Finally, age was slightly negatively

related to motivation based on compensation and benefits (ρ = -.10; Kooij et al., 2011).

The observed age differences in work values can provide interesting guidelines for HRM

interventions, such as age-differentiated task design or allocation (Zacher, Feldman, et al., 2014).

The relative fit between persons’ work values and corresponding task characteristics (“need-

supply fit”; Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, & Johnson, 2005) has been shown to be positively

related to both work motivation and job satisfaction . Krumm et al. (2013a) demonstrated that

this effect is particularly strong for older workers for a broad range of work values, including

extrinsic growth values (e.g., career, monetary benefits), generativity values (e.g., helping,

legacy), emotion-related values (e.g., appreciation, interesting work), and context-related values

(e.g., health, security). Thus, older workers seem to be particularly sensitive to misfits between

their personal needs and the current job supplies, consistent with lifespan theories predicting
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 38

increasing importance of maintaining positive affect when remaining time becomes restricted

(Carstensen, 2006).

Recent research has also compared workers’ explicit motives (usually assessed with self-

report questionnaires) with their implicit motives, reflecting rather unconscious dispositional

preferences (assessed with projective or semi-projective tests) that are nevertheless important

additional drivers of human behavior (McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989). Indeed,

incongruence between explicit and implicit motives (e.g., a person conceiving himself as a

hardliner at work but having a high affiliation motive) has been considered a hidden stressor due

to permanent motive conflicts. In this respect, Thielgen, Krumm, Rauschenbach, and Hertel

(2015) found higher congruency between implicit and explicit motives for older as compared to

younger workers, probably due to learning processes and adaptations of explicit motives to their

dispositional implicit motive profile. Moreover, the researchers expected and found that older

workers reacted more negatively to incongruence between implicit and explicit motives, which is

again in line with the general assumption of lifespan theories that maintaining positive affect is

more important for older than for younger people (Carstensen, 2006; Charles, 2010).

Summarizing the empirical results on age and work motivation, older workers seem to

have considerable strengths as compared to younger workers: Older workers seem to have higher

overall work motivation, particularly with respect to intrinsic motivation but also in terms of

commitment and job involvement. Moreover, older workers particularly value high autonomy at

work, positive work experiences (including meaningful work), opportunities to help others and

pass on own experiences, and work conditions that fit to their work values. Finally, older

workers seem to experience less intrapersonal motive conflicts than younger workers, reducing

the need for self-regulation (Thielgen, Krumm, Rauschenbach, & Hertel, 2015). Younger
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 39

workers, in contrast, seem to be particularly motivated by learning and career related goals. Of

course, these general trends are further qualified by moderators such as job type, gender,

personality, or cultural variables (Stamov-Roßnagel & Biemann, 2012). Moreover, curvilinear

effects are often still to be examined. Finally, cohort effects reflecting different historical

conditions have to be considered as alternative or additional explanations, for instance, of work

value differences (Twenge, Campbell, Hoffman, & Lance, 2010).

Self-Regulation and Socio-Emotional Skills

Related to work motivation, self-regulation and socio-emotional skills are another area

where substantial age differences occur. Both self-regulation and socio-emotional skills

increasingly gain importance in modern organizations, for instance, because service industries

are replacing production industries in many industrialized countries, and because the increasing

decentralization of work processes, for instance in de-located teams or virtual organizations,

requires high self-management skills of workers. In general, self-regulation and socio-emotional

skills have been shown to increase with age (Blanchard-Fields, 2007). For instance, older

persons process positive emotional information more deeply than negative emotional information

(Löckenhoff & Carstensen, 2007). Moreover, older persons are less affected by tense social

situations, more forgiving and loyal, and seem to consider a broader range of emotion-regulation

strategies, which together yield better self-regulation and better skills to manage social

relationships (e.g., Birditt, Fingerman, & Almeida, 2005; Johnson, Holdsworth, Hoel, & Zapf,

2013; Scheibe & Blanchard-Fields, 2009; see Scheibe & Zacher, 2013, for a review).

Theoretically, these skills of older persons are not only consistent with more general learning

processes and accumulation of experience across time, but should be additionally amplified by

motivational changes predicted by lifespan theories (Carstensen, 2006; Charles, 2010), assuming
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 40

that maintaining positive and nurturing social relationships is more important when remaining

time becomes limited.

Empirical examinations of age differences in self-regulation and socio-emotional skills at

work are still rare. Among the initial examples, Thielgen, Krumm, and Hertel (2015) found

higher self-regulation skills for older as compared to younger workers, which seem to buffer

negative effects of intrapersonal motive conflicts on work motivation. Moreover, in a

longitudinal study on stress coping strategies, worker age was positively related to active coping

strategies requiring high self-regulation skills, and negatively related to passive avoidance coping

particularly in jobs with low job control (see also Hertel et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2013, for

evidence on more effective stress management of older workers). These initial findings are

promising and suggest that the general advantages of older persons in self-regulation and socio-

emotional skills can also be found in work settings. However, it should also be noted that higher

self-regulation skills of older workers cannot be taken for granted. For instance, the use of SOC

strategies (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990) as another form of self-regulation was not correlated with

age in work settings (Weigl et al., 2013; Zacher & Frese, 2011). More research is certainly

desirable here.

Work Performance

Work performance is arguably one of the most important employee outcomes in

organizations and is defined as measurable behaviors under the employee’s control that

contribute to organizational goals (Campbell, McCloy, Oppler, & Sager, 1993). The pervasive

stereotype that older employees are poorer performers than younger employees appears to have

been refuted by four meta-analyses conducted over the past 30 years, which all found near zero

relationships between age and task performance (McEvoy & Cascio, 1989; Ng & Feldman,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 41

2008; Sturman, 2003; Waldman & Avolio, 1986). In the most recent meta-analysis, Ng and

Feldman (2008) showed that age did not have meaningful relationships with supervisor-rated

task performance (ρ = .02), creativity (ρ = .01), and performance in training programs (ρ = -.04).

Type of performance measure (i.e., self- and supervisor ratings, objective measure) did not

moderate the age-task performance relationship, suggesting that age stereotypes did not play a

role (Ng & Feldman, 2008). Contradicting popular stereotypes, supervisors rated older as

compared to younger workers higher on task-directed organizational citizenship behavior (ρ =

.21). Moreover, older workers complied more with safety rules (ρ = .10), had fewer work-related

injuries (ρ = -.08), and showed less counterproductive work behaviors (ρ = -.09), workplace

aggression (ρ = -.08), substance use (ρ = -.07), tardiness (ρ = -.26), and voluntary absenteeism (ρ

= -.10) than younger employees. Important limitations of meta-analyses on age and work

performance to date are that they include mainly primary studies that sampled only very few

workers 50 years and over, thus, generalizations to older workers (including examination of

curvilinear patterns) have to be made with care (McDaniel et al., 2012).

The results of meta-analyses showing weak or close to zero relationships between age

and work performance do not preclude that age-related mechanisms impact on employees’

behavior at work. In fact, Ng and Feldman (2013b) argued that there is evidence suggesting that

the relationships between age and different forms of job performance are mediated by within-

person changes in cognitive functioning, personality characteristics, goal orientation, socio-

emotional experience, and health. Importantly, some of these mediators are likely to have

countervailing effects on work performance, leading to weak overall relationships; for instance,

age-related increase in conscientiousness may compensate for age-related decreases in fluid

intelligence. A number of recent studies have explored indirect effects of age on work
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 42

performance. For instance, Zacher, Heusner, Schmitz, Zwierzanska, and Frese (2010) showed

that the relationship between age and work performance was mediated by employees’

perceptions of future occupational opportunities. Kunze et al. (2013) investigated resistance to

change as a mediator in the association between age and self-reported goal accomplishment.

Contrary to stereotypes, they found that age was negatively related to resistance to change, and

had an indirect effect on goal accomplishment through resistance to change.

Besides task, citizenship, and counterproductive performance, the job performance

domain has been extended over the past decade to also include employees’ proactive, team,

adaptive, and innovative performance (Griffin, Neal, & Parker, 2007). Of these dimensions, the

relationship between age and innovative performance has received most attention to date. In a

meta-analysis, Ng and Feldman (2013c) found that age and job tenure were weakly positively

related to self-rated innovative performance and unrelated to more objective measures of

innovative performance. Thus, this research does not support the widespread stereotype that

older workers are less innovative than younger workers. A number of primary studies have

further explored the boundary conditions of the relationship between age and innovative

performance. Binnewies, Ohly, and Niessen (2008) found that age interacted with job control

and support for creativity from coworkers and supervisors in predicting innovative performance,

such that the relationship was positive when job control was high and negative when job control

and support for creativity were low. Ng and Feldman (2012a) reported a positive relationship

between age and innovative performance among employees experiencing low supervisor

undermining as well as among employees with both high supervisor undermining and a proactive

personality. In contrast, the age-innovative performance relationship was negative when

supervisor undermining was high and employees’ proactive personality was low.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 43

Overall, the relationship between age and job performance continues to mesmerize

organizational researchers (Beier & Kanfer, 2013; McDaniel et al., 2012). An interesting

possibility for future research is to examine age-related changes in work performance across

longer periods of the working lifespan, and mediating mechanisms of these changes (Cleveland

& Lim, 2007). In addition, more research is needed on age differences and age-related changes in

performance dimensions that are becoming increasingly important in the changing workplace,

such as proactive, team (including informal mentoring activities), and adaptive performance

(Griffin et al., 2007). Finally, a possible avenue for future research is how action regulation

strategies moderate the age-performance relationship. For instance, initial research by Yeung and

Fung (2009) showed that the use of goal selection and compensatory strategies was more

strongly positively related to job performance among older compared to younger employees.

The greatest risks in terms of work performance appear to exist for older employees in

jobs that require fast information processing without compensation opportunities by accumulated

knowledge or job experience (McDaniel et al., 2012). In these cases, older workers might change

to different work roles (e.g., trainer or supervisor). The finding that older workers tend to be

better organizational citizens suggests that they are in a good position to represent their company

well to organizational newcomers and outsiders, for instance in mentoring roles and as

organizational representatives (Calo, 2005).

Occupational Well-Being

In addition to job performance, occupational well-being involves another group of

important outcome variables in work and organizational psychology. In the context of an aging

workforce, well-being indicators receive additional importance as they are crucial for various

management goals, such as retention of (older) workers, maintaining high work ability at older
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 44

ages, or maintaining high work motivation and commitment across time. Thus, we briefly review

age differences in job satisfaction and strain/stress experience as main well-being indicators.

Job satisfaction was among the very first outcome variables investigated in relation to

worker age (Birdi, Warr, & Oswald, 1995; Rhodes, 1983). The resulting patterns yielded

sometimes linear (age correlates positively with job satisfaction; e.g., Ng & Feldman, 2010) and

sometimes curvilinear patterns, such that middle-aged workers report lower job satisfaction than

older and younger workers (Clark et al., 1996; Hochwarter, Ferris, Perrewe, Witt, & Kiewitz,

2001; Zacher, Jimmieson, et al., 2014). However, in nearly all studies (also across countries),

older workers consistently report higher satisfaction as compared to middle-aged workers. The

reason for this consistently higher job satisfaction of older workers is multifactorial, including

objective conditions such as (on average) higher pay, higher status, and more autonomy due to

seniority principles (at least for male workers), but also subjective processes such as lower

aspirations and emotion-regulation in light of a restricted remaining time at work. Moreover,

motivational processes described above (e.g., better person-environment fit and a higher

congruency between implicit and explicit motives for older persons) further contribute to a

higher job satisfaction of older workers. On the other hand, older as compared to younger

workers’ react more strongly to unfairness at work (Tenhiälä et al., 2013) and to breaches of

psychological contracts (P. M. Bal, De Lange, Jansen, & Van Der Velde, 2008). Again, limited

remaining time at work might explain this higher sensitivity of older workers’ job satisfaction to

the “here and now” at work.

In terms of age differences in the judgment of job satisfaction, recent research has

revealed that the correlation between attitudinal measures of job satisfaction (e.g., “All in all,

how satisfied are you with your current job?”) and experience-based measures “in situ” (based
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 45

on experience sampling, reconstruction, or diary methods; Rauschenbach & Hertel, 2011)

correspond more strongly for older than for younger workers (Hertel, Thielgen, et al., 2013).

This result suggests that older workers do not simply retrieve pre-prepared job satisfaction

ratings from their memory (which might be predicted by a more pessimistic perception of older

persons; e.g., Schwarz & Knäuper, 1999) but conduct their overall satisfaction judgment in light

of their current experiences at work. Younger workers, in turn, seem to consider additional

aspects (e.g., future opportunities) in their overall job satisfaction ratings, leading to a lower

correlation between general attitudinal ratings and experience-based judgments.

The relation between age and occupational strain has become the focus of empirical

research much more recently. While stereotypic beliefs often suggest that age is negatively

related to stress resistance due to (assumed) higher vulnerability of older persons, theoretical

predictions consider multiple processes that partly compensate each other (Rauschenbach et al.,

2013). While decreasing physical and/or cognitive capacities might indeed lead to higher

vulnerability of older workers (Charles, 2010), increasing socio-emotional skills and self-

regulation skills should at least partly compensate these processes and reduce strain at work

which, in turn, might facilitate more active coping strategies that further reduce strain (see Hertel

et al., 2015, for initial longitudinal evidence for such resource gain spirals). Moreover, context

conditions change over the lifespan, often placing particularly high demands on middle-aged

workers due to a sandwich position between different role expectations from occupational and

private life, and due to particularly high responsibilities in this life stage (Heckhausen et al.,

2010; Rauschenbach & Hertel, 2011; Zacher, Jimmieson, et al., 2014). Older workers, in

contrast, often have less stressful jobs in more strategic positions with higher autonomy enabling

more efficient stress coping strategies. Moreover, work-family conflicts tend to be higher for
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 46

middle-aged workers, who often have small children and/or family care duties (Huffman,

Culbertson, Henning, & Goh, 2013), whereas older workers may be more independent and thus

more open to flexible work arrangements, including traveling and longer working times.

As a consequence of these different and partly countervailing processes, the overall

correlation between age and strain might be rather low. Indeed, recent meta-analyses found no

overall correlation (neither linear nor curvilinear) between age and strain, neither for proximal

strain indicators such as irritation (Rauschenbach et al., 2013) nor for more distal indicators such

as burnout (Ng & Feldman, 2010). For emotional exhaustion as one facet of the burnout

construct (Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001), meta-analytic data even revealed a (slight)

negative correlation with age (ρ = -.09; Ng & Feldman, 2013a). Thus, empirical evidence again

speaks against an overall pessimistic perception of older workers in terms of stress resilience (Ng

& Feldman, 2012b). Instead, more differentiated research is desirable that considers potential

moderating conditions. For instance, strain does slightly increase with age in jobs with high

physical demands (e.g., production or construction work), and shows a reversed U-shaped

relation with age in social jobs (e.g., nurses or teachers), with middle-aged workers reporting the

highest level of strain (Rauschenbach et al., 2013). Further research is needed on different job

types but also on gender, education level, and other potential moderator variables.

Future empirical research might not only focus on a few selected components but rather

try to cover the complete stress process, considering different age-related processes

simultaneously (Zacher, Feldman, et al., 2014). More research is needed on potential mediating

mechanisms and moderators of these age differences in order to enable more focused

interventions. For instance, a recent study by Zacher, Jimmieson, et al. (2014) found that lower

job satisfaction and higher emotional exhaustion of middle-aged employees in the construction
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 47

industry as compared to both younger and older workers was mediated by perceived time

pressure and coworker support (but not work-family conflict in this study with predominantly

male participants). Of course, it may be possible that these findings are at least partly affected by

cohort effects or earlier dropouts of highly stressed workers (“healthy worker effect”). While

these questions require future research with longitudinal and cohort-sequential designs, a recent

study of general well-being showed that U-shaped patterns with age remain stable even when

cohort effects are considered (Blanchflower & Oswald, 2008).

Summarizing the empirical findings with respect to age and well-being at work, results

speak against popular age stereotypes and for a higher probability of older workers to experience

high well-being at work as compared to middle-aged workers. Remarkably, these differences are

not only due to less stressful jobs among older workers (Rauschenbach & Hertel, 2011), but also

seem to be a consequence of better self-regulation and more active coping strategies (Hertel et

al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2013). In consequence, age-inclusive management of well-being should

particularly address middle-aged workers, trying to reduce their burdens due to multiple role

expectations, and to provide additional resources such as support and more flexible working

conditions.

Practical Implications

In this section, we outline suggestions for evidence-based HRM strategies to address

demographic changes, in order to profit from evolving opportunities and prevent potential risks.

In addition to strategies at the level of organization management, we also discuss strategies that

can be used by individual workers, and broader societal decision processes (see Table 3).

< Insert Table 3 about here >

Age-Inclusive HRM in Organizations


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 48

To motivate and retain workers from different age groups, and to maintain or increase

their work ability with age, organizations might want to focus on employees’ age-related

strengths and potentials, assist them in compensating for challenges, and avoid age

discrimination during all phases of the employment process. In the following, we discuss

possible interventions during (1) personnel attraction and selection, (2) work (re-)design, (3)

implementation of personnel development practices such as training and career development, and

health and performance management, (4) establishment and maintenance of positive

organizational cultures and climates, and (5) the retirement transition.

First, non-discriminatory selection procedures are key aspects of an age-inclusive HRM

(Böhm, Schröder, & Kunze, 2013; Naegele & Walker, 2011). Lievens, Van Hoye, and Zacher

(2012) summarized steps organizations might take to attract and recruit an age-diverse

workforce. For instance, organizations can conduct thorough work analyses to understand the

specific demands on workers, and which age-differentiated abilities are necessary to meet these

demands now and in the upcoming years (cf. Sanchez & Levine, 2012). Moreover, work

analyses can help organizations understand how they can meet the needs and preferences of

employees from different age groups. Organizations also need to choose recruitment messages

(e.g., which strengths of older workers are valued in this company) and recruitment sources (e.g.,

specialized online portals) that are consistent with the preferences and search behaviors of job

seekers from different age groups. In addition, it is recommended that organizations ensure

unbiased selection procedures that, for instance, do not only take the quantity but also the quality

of employment experiences into account, and that base hiring decisions on knowledge, skills, and

abilities rather than on age (Naegele & Walker, 2011). Once employees are on the job,

organizational performance measurement and reward systems should be non-age discriminatory;


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 49

this involves increasing the awareness of supervisors for age-related changes, younger and older

workers’ strengths and weaknesses, and implementing formal procedures to reduce the influence

of age bias (e.g., objective performance indicators). Initial tests of interventions showed that

training effectively reduced age stereotypes and team conflicts (Wegge et al., 2012).

Second, organizations can design and re-design jobs and broader work roles to motivate,

retain, and manage the performance of employees from different age groups (Griffiths, 1999).

This could involve, for instance, the implementation of support systems (e.g., robotics or

computer systems) that compensate for age-related declines in physical strength or working

memory. With regard to motivational work characteristics, research showed that younger

employees benefit more from high task variety and feedback, whereas older employees prefer

autonomous work that allows them to demonstrate and pass on their knowledge and skills

(Truxillo et al., 2012; Zaniboni, Truxillo, & Fraccaroli, 2013). Research at the team level further

suggests that work design and leadership are also important in the context of age-heterogeneous

teams. Specifically, age diverse teams show higher performance when working on complex

tasks, whereas they develop more physical health problem when working on routine tasks

(Wegge, Roth, Neubach, Schmidt, & Kanfer, 2008). Transformational leadership has been

shown to buffer potential negative effects of age diversity on team performance (Kearney &

Gebert, 2009), whereas negative stereotypes about older employees can intensify the negative

effects of age diversity on younger and older employees’ health (Liebermann, Wegge,

Jungmann, & Schmidt, 2013). It has further been recommend that companies introduce flexible

working time arrangements, to avoid demotivation and early exit from the workforce (Naegele &

Walker, 2011). These arrangements include paid and unpaid leaves for continuing education and
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 50

family caregiving responsibilities, as well as the adjustment of shift schedules, reduction of

working hours, job sharing, and relief from overtime.

Third, research suggests that younger and older workers differ in their reactions to

various HRM practices (Boehm & Dwertmann, 2015). Consistent with theory and findings on

age differences in work motivation, Kooij, Jansen, Dikkers, and De Lange (2010) found that the

relationships between perceived “maintenance HRM practices” (i.e., practices that help maintain

performance and well-being, such as job security, health and performance management, and

work-life policies) and job attitudes are stronger among older workers, whereas the relationships

between perceived “development HRM practices” (i.e., practices that further enhance

functioning, such as training and development, promotion and career development) and job

attitudes are stronger among younger workers. This research may suggest that, if the goal is to

enhance job attitudes such as job satisfaction and organizational commitment, companies should

apply maintenance HRM practices to older employees and development HRM practices to

younger employees. However, to fully tap the potentials of both younger and older workers,

companies should adopt differentiated maintenance and development practices for both younger

and older workers. For instance, research showed that younger employees react more positively

to performance feedback that is highly informative, whereas older employees react more

positively to feedback that is favorable and considerate (Wang et al., 2015). Moreover,

companies should ensure that they offer employees, regardless of their age, equal opportunities

for continuous (“life-long”) learning, training, and development, as well as for promotions,

transferals, and career progression (Naegele & Walker, 2011). Possibilities for career

development at higher ages include not only hierarchical career moves, but also horizontal
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 51

moves (e.g., into mentoring and organizational ambassador roles), project work assignments, and

downshifting.

Another maintenance HRM strategy that appears to be important to employees from

different age groups is the management of physical, mental, and social health (Böhm et al., 2013;

Naegele & Walker, 2011). Indeed, research shows that particularly employees in mid-career may

benefit from workplace interventions targeting mental health and well-being, such as decreasing

time pressure and increasing coworker support (Zacher, Jimmieson, et al., 2014). Generally,

holistic approaches that foster employees’ health and work ability are more beneficial than single

intervention (Ilmarinen, 2009). Possibilities for health initiatives include voluntary preventive

health checks, exercise opportunities, and improvement of ergonomic working conditions to

prevent accidents, injuries, and chronic diseases (Böhm et al., 2013). Notably, these initiatives do

not only benefit older workers, but also – and perhaps even more – their younger counterparts,

helping them to stay healthy and maintain work motivation for a longer work life.

Fourth, companies should ensure that negative age stereotypes are not engrained in their

organizational culture and climate (Staudinger, 2015). Organizational culture refers to the shared

and deeply-rooted values, norms, and beliefs within a company, whereas organizational climate

involves employees’ shared perceptions of more concrete organizational policies, practices, and

procedures; both constructs have been shown to impact employee attitudes and behaviors as well

as firm performance (Schneider, Ehrhart, & Macey, 2013). Research on age cultures suggests

that organizations differ in the extent to which the groups of younger and older employees are

generally perceived favorably, and that these differences are influenced by attitudes of top

management toward different age groups (Staudinger, 2015; Zacher & Gielnik, 2014). As

organizational age cultures are likely to have trickle-down effects on individual supervisor and
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 52

employee age stereotypes, attitudes, and work outcomes, it is important that top managers are

“age- and aging-sensitive” and their actions are not biased by age stereotypes. Research on age-

inclusive leadership is in its infancy, but the evidence so far suggests that supervisor

encouragement and recognition can help maintain and increase older employees’ work

performance and occupational well-being (Ilmarinen, 2009; Wegge et al., 2012; Zacher, Clark,

Anderson, & Ayoko, 2015). Research on the organizational level further showed that companies’

age discrimination and age diversity climates can negatively impact on firm and team

performance, and on employee retention via employees’ affective commitment and social

exchange perceptions (Böhm, Kunze, & Bruch, 2014; Kunze, Böhm, & Bruch, 2011). Overall,

research on age culture and climate suggests that organizations need to ensure that shared values

and beliefs, as well as policies, practices, and procedures are not age discriminatory but

recognize the strengths and potentials of employees from different age groups (see Posthuma &

Guerrero, 2013, for specific strategies also considering different cultural contexts).

Finally, organizations might want to support a smooth transition of older employees to

retirement (cf. Wang & Shi, 2014). This may include helping employees prepare for retirement

(e.g., through workshops and counseling facilities) and offering opportunities for keeping in

touch after retirement (e.g., as senior advisors or relief staff). Moreover, companies could offer a

choice of retirement models to match different needs (e.g., partial, phased, trial, and bridge

employment; Beehr & Bennett, 2015). Further important elements for the organization are a

constant monitoring of the demographic profile, and a structured and proactive knowledge

transfer to ensure that needed expertise remains in the company (Leibold & Voelpel, 2006).

Altogether, the outlined interventions at different phases in the HRM process illustrate

that much can be done by organizations to support an aging and age-diverse workforce. Notably,
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 53

most if not all of these changes benefit at least indirectly younger and middle-aged workers as

well, for instance, by increasing flexibility and adaptability at work as well as long-term support

of good health and high motivation. While reliable tests of economic benefits of an age-inclusive

management are still lacking, the advantages for humane work conditions and associated benefits

for attracting and retaining skilled workers should be obvious. Moreover, while developing

trainings of age-inclusive management might be easier for large organizations due to a better

ratio of investments to number of affected workers, screening and development of organizational

culture and age-climate might be easier for small and mid-size companies involving fewer

people.

Individuals

In addition to the responsibilities of organizations, the empirical findings described

suggest also various opportunities for the individual worker. In general, the perspective of a

longer work span highlights the individual’s responsibilities to maintain health and work ability.

For instance, participation in and planning of life-long learning activities becomes more

important when workers face a longer working life with more job changes in a career (Biemann

et al., 2012). In addition, the empirical findings suggest that maintenance of cognitive abilities

with increased age (including fluid intelligence) might be at least to some degree influenced by

individuals themselves, for instance, by changing daily tasks and seeking new challenges. Of

course, a realistic estimation of the potential effect size is still pending, but workers should

realize that cognitive capabilities, for instance, for learning new work techniques, are not

dramatically decreased for older workers.

In addition to maintaining task-related skills, pro-active and/or preventive health

management becomes an increasingly important task for workers, including regular physical

exercise activities, a balanced diet, and non-excessive alcohol use. While such activities can be
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 54

supported by the employer, the responsibilities of the workers themselves are increasing given a

growing general trend towards time-restricted job contracts (Eby, Butts, & Lockwood, 2003). In

general, a longer work-life requires considerable self-management skills for workers of all ages.

Such self-management skills can be addressed by specific training activities, for instance, using

the SOC principles of successful aging (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990) as guidelines that have been

shown to increase performance and health at work (Bajor & Baltes, 2003; Weigl et al., 2013).

Moreover, some workers may benefit from engagement in job crafting activities and making

idiosyncratic deals with their organization to improve the fit between their personal needs and

their work situation (P. M. Bal, De Jong, Jansen, & Bakker, 2012; Kooij, Tims, & Kanfer, 2015).

Workers should be aware that the ongoing demographic changes increase the pressure on

organizations to support such self-managing activities of their workers, for instance by offering

financial support, development opportunities, or more flexibility at work.

Society

The suggested changes and interventions to address demographic changes are not limited

to individual workers or organizations. In fact, these suggestions have to be considered in their

societal context, not only because this context can determine what is doable (e.g., by formal

legislation), but also because society is directly affected by how the emerging challenges are

solved. For instance, distributional justice of work and related resources is a main factor for

social peace in a community. As the growing numbers of retirees depend on a shrinking work

population in many countries, population aging challenges economic security and retirement

plans. Over the last years, many governments have reacted with initial campaigns, for instance,

increasing the official retirement age (e.g., from 65 to 67 in Germany), promoting healthy aging

and fighting age discrimination at work, or launching life-long learning programs.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 55

However, more can be done to further support an aging and increasingly age-diverse

workforce. For instance, rigid legalization is often a main hindrance for more flexible shifts from

work to retirement at the end of a career. Governmental and legislative support for more flexible

work arrangements (e.g., bridge employment, postretirement activities; cf. Wang & Shultz,

2010) are certainly helpful to retain those workers with changing priorities (e.g., lower strain,

fewer work hours, more time for family or other non-work activities). At the same time, careful

measures are needed to prevent that flexible work arrangements are misused as a simple cost

reduction strategy. Moreover, laws against age discrimination such as the Age Discrimination in

Employment Act in the United States are important means and signals how minorities are treated

in a society, and are today established in many industrialized countries. However, many of these

laws have been implemented only recently and still have to be shown to effectively prevent age

discrimination both in hiring as well as in termination decisions (Hedge & Borman, 2012)

Given that changes in legislation require broad consensus, governments and other societal

forces (e.g., labor unions, employer associations, etc.) need to further support an open discussion

about the opportunities of an aging workforce in order to provide leeway for innovative solutions

and experiments, and to explore policy options for an extended working life (Phillipson, 2013).

In particular, the frequent execution of early retirement options in the past decades (introduced as

a mean to cope with structural unemployment in those days) have created the expectation that

early retirement is a vested right, and any changes to this prospect are sometimes being perceived

as a betrayal. These normative expectations are difficult to change. Scientific research plays an

important role in this respect, providing evidence-based and potential new perspectives on

developmental opportunities. Therefore, higher presence of work and organizational

psychologists in governmental and societal decision boards and committees is certainly


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 56

desirable. In general, long-term campaigns are more advisable than mere singular actions. One

main challenge of such campaigns is to avoid focusing on the interests of selected groups only

(e.g., older well-educated males) in order to avoid reactance from other parties involved.

Future Research Needs

Based on our review of the work and aging literature, we now offer a number of

recommendations for future research (see Table 4 for a summary). First, we encourage

researchers in this field to conceptualize and examine mediating mechanisms of assumed age

differences and effects, including mediators of age as a moderator (Wang et al., 2015), instead of

treating age as a causal variable. Moreover, it is important to examine increased variability (i.e.,

fanning out) in work outcomes with increasing age in addition to average age trends, and to

conceptualize moderating factors that may help explain this increased heterogeneity among older

workers (Zacher, 2015). Importantly, age-related mediators and moderators may not only be

person-related but also contextual factors. Second, researchers should consider alternatives to

cross-sectional designs, such as (short-term) longitudinal studies and diary studies, as well as

training interventions both at the level of individual skills (Toril, Reales, & Ballesteros, 2014)

and at the level of HRM interventions (e.g., age-differentiated leadership training; Ries et al.,

2013). To better understand the role of age at work, it is important to consider insights from

studies using multiple different methodological approaches (cf. Hertzog et al., 2009).

< Insert Table 4 about here >

Third, to preserve statistical power, it is important to treat age as a continuous variable

instead of using arbitrary cut-offs and groups for younger, middle-aged, and older workers.

Similarly, it is desirable that workers of different ages are equally represented in studies;

unfortunately, previous studies and meta-analyses have largely neglected actual older workers.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 57

At least, sampling limitations and proportions of workers in different age groups should be

acknowledged. Researchers should also routinely examine curvilinear age effects (even if not

hypothesized), and report their findings. Fourth, other time-related constructs such as job and

organizational tenure should be considered as control variables to rule out alternative

explanations of age effects. Fifth, we encourage researchers to collect data from multiple

sources, including subjective and objective work outcomes. Last but not least, we strongly

recommend studying specific subgroups of older workers (cf. Marcus & Fritzsche, 2015),

especially those that are less privileged (e.g., women, less educated workers, unemployed) and

might show significantly different age trajectories. This is not only important for theory

development, but also for more efficient strategies to support these groups across the whole

work-life.

Conclusion

In this chapter we discussed the implications of demographic change and workforce aging

for individual employees, human resource management, organizations, and societies. We

reviewed common stereotypes of older workers and their potential effects on age discrimination

in the workplace and we attempted to correct existing misconceptions by comparing stereotypes

with empirical evidence. We outlined several currently important theoretical frameworks and

methodological consideration in the rapidly growing field of work and aging. In our review of

research on age differences and age-related changes in cognitive, physical, and self-regulatory

capabilities, as well as work motivation and work outcomes, we focused particularly on how the

effects of older workers’ strengths and potentials can be maximized, for instance through

workplace interventions. Based on the empirical evidence, practitioners can implement age-
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 58

inclusive HRM practices, and researchers can design more sophisticated studies to move

research on work and aging forward.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 59

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality,

interest, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22(2), 227-257. doi: 10.1016/S0160-

2896(96)90016-1

Alley, D., & Crimmins, E. (2007). The demography of aging and work. In K. S. Shultz & G. A.

Adams (Eds.), Aging and work in the 21st century (pp. 7-23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Ashbaugh, D. L., & Fay, C. H. (1987). The threshold for aging in the workplace. Research on

Aging, 9(3), 417-427. doi: 10.1177/0164027587093005

Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2014). Improving

fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x

Baird, B. M., Pitzer, L., Russell, A., & Bergeman, C. S. (2012). Studying the aging worker:

Research designs and methodologies. In J. W. Hedge & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The

Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp. 151-168). New York: Oxford University

Press.

Bajor, J. K., & Baltes, B. B. (2003). The relationship between selection optimization with

compensation, conscientiousness, motivation, and performance. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 63, 347-367. doi: 10.1016/S0001-8791(02)00035-0

Bal, A. C., Reiss, A. E. B., Rudolph, C. W., & Baltes, B. B. (2011). Examining positive and

negative perceptions of older workers: A meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology

Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66(6), 687-698. doi:

10.1093/geronb/gbr056
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 60

Bal, P. M., De Jong, S. B., Jansen, P. G. W., & Bakker, A. B. (2012). Motivating employees to

work beyond retirement: A multi-level study of the role of I-deals and unit climate.

Journal of Management Studies, 49(2), 306-331. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2011.01026.x

Bal, P. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., & Van Der Velde, M. E. G. (2008). Psychological

contract breach and job attitudes: A meta-analysis of age as a moderator. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 72(1), 143-158. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2007.10.005

Bal, P. M., de Lange, A. H., Zacher, H., & van der Heijden, B. I. J. M. (2013). A lifespan

perspective on psychological contracts and their relations with organizational

commitment. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(3), 279-

292. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.741595

Bal, P. M., Kooij, D. T. A. M., & Rousseau, D. (Eds.). (2015). Aging workers and the employee-

employer relationship. New York: Springer.

Baltes, B. B., & Finkelstein, L. M. (2011). Contemporary empirical advancements in the study of

aging in the workplace. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(2), 151-154. doi:

10.1002/job.727

Baltes, B. B., & Heydens-Gahir, H. A. (2003). Reduction of work-family conflict through the use

of selection, optimization, and compensation behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology,

88(6), 1005-1018. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.1005

Baltes, B. B., Rudolph, C. W., & Bal, A. C. (2012). A review of aging theories and modern work

perspectives. In J. W. Hedge & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and

Aging (pp. 117-136). New York: Oxford University Press.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 61

Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the

dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 611-626. doi:

10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611

Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The

model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes

(Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1-34). New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1979). History and rationale of longitudinal research. In J. R.

Nesselroade & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and

development (pp. 1-39). New York: Academic Press.

Barnes-Farrell, J. L., Rumery, S. M., & Swody, C. A. (2002). How do concepts of age relate to

work and off-the-job stresses and strains? A field study of health care workers in five

nations. Experimental Aging Research, 28(1), 87-98. doi: 10.1080/036107302753365577

Beal, D. J. (2012). Industrial/organizational psychology. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.),

Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 601-619). New York: Guilford

Press.

Beehr, T. A., & Bennett, M. M. (2015). Working after retirement: Features of bridge

employment and research directions. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(1), 112-128. doi:

10.1093/workar/wau007

Beier, M. E., & Kanfer, R. (2013). Work performance and the older worker. In J. Field, R. J.

Burke & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of aging, work and society (pp. 97-

118). London: Sage.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 62

Bengtson, V. L., Gans, D., Putney, N. M., & Silverstein, M. (2009). Theories about age and

aging. In M. Silverstein, V. L. Bengtson, N. M. Putney & D. Gans (Eds.), Handbook of

theories of aging (pp. 3-23). New York: Springer.

Bengtson, V. L., Putney, N. M., & Johnson, M. L. (2005). The problem of theory in gerontology

today. In M. L. Johnson (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of age and ageing (pp. 3-21).

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Biemann, T., Zacher, H., & Feldman, D. C. (2012). Career patterns: A twenty-year panel study.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81(2), 159-170. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2012.06.003

Binnewies, C., Ohly, S., & Niessen, C. (2008). Age and creativity at work: The interplay

between job resources, age and idea creativity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 23(4),

438-457. doi: 10.1108/02683940810869042

Birdi, K., Warr, P. B., & Oswald, A. (1995). Age differences in three components of employee

well-being. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 44(4), 345-373. doi:

10.1111/j.1464-0597.1995.tb01085.x

Birditt, K. S., Fingerman, K. L., & Almeida, D. M. (2005). Age differences in exposure and

reactions to interpersonal tensions: A daily diary study. Psychology and Aging, 20(2),

330-340. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.330

Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental

perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(1), 26-31. doi:

10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x

Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Social

Science & Medicine, 66(8), 1733-1749. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.030


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 63

Bloom, D. E. (2011). 7 billion and counting. Science, 333(6042), 562-569. doi:

10.1126/science.1209290

Boehm, S. A., & Dwertmann, D. J. G. (2015). Forging a single-edged sword: Facilitating

positive age and disability diversity effects in the workplace through leadership, positive

climates, and HR practices. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(1), 41-63. doi:

10.1093/workar/wau008

Böhm, S. A., Kunze, F., & Bruch, H. (2014). Spotlight on age-diversity climate: The impact of

age-inclusive HR practices on firm-level outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 67(3), 667-

704. doi: 10.1111/peps.12047

Böhm, S. A., Schröder, H. S., & Kunze, F. (2013). Comparative age management: Theoretical

perspective and practical implications The SAGE handbook on work, aging, and society

(pp. 211-237). Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage.

Bowen, C. E., Noack, C. M. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2011). Aging in the work context. In K.

W. Schaie & S. L. Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (7th ed., pp. 263-

277). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment:

Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accomodative strategies of

coping. Psychology and Aging, 5(1), 58-67. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.1.58

Calo, T. J. (2005). The generativity track: A transitional approach to retirement. Public

Personnel Management, 34(4), 301-312. doi: 10.1177/009102600503400402

Campbell, J. P., McCloy, R. A., Oppler, S. H., & Sager, C. E. (1993). A theory of performance.

In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 35-79).

San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 64

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science,

312(5782), 1913-1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488

Carstensen, L. L. (2009). A long bright future: An action plan for a lifetime of happiness, health,

and financial security. New York: Broadway Books.

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of

socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165-181. doi: 10.1037/0003-

066X.54.3.165

Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 40(3), 153-

193. doi: 10.1037/h0059973

Chand, M., & Tung, R. L. (2014). The aging of the world's population and its effects on global

business. Academy of Management Perspectives, 28(4), 409-429. doi:

10.5465/amp.2012.0070

Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of emotional well-being

across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1068-1091. doi: 10.1037/a0021232

Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of

Psychology, 61, 383-409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448

Clark, A., Oswald, A., & Warr, P. (1996). Is job satisfaction U-shaped in age? Journal of

Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69, 57-81. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-

8325.1996.tb00600.x

Cleveland, J. N., & Lim, A. S. (2007). Employee age and performance in organizations. In K. S.

Shultz & G. A. Adams (Eds.), Aging and work in the 21st century (pp. 109-137).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 65

Cleveland, J. N., Shore, L. M., & Murphy, K. R. (1997). Person- and context-oriented perceptual

age measures: Additional evidence of distinctiveness and usefulness. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 18(3), 239-251. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-

1379(199705)18:3<239::AID-JOB794>3.0.CO;2-A

Costa, G., & Sartori, S. (2007). Ageing, working hours and work ability. Ergonomics, 50(11),

1914-1930. doi: 10.1080/00140130701676054

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult

personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp.

21-40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Czaja, S. J., & Sharit, J. (2009). Aging and work: Issues and implications in a changing

landscape. Maryland, BA: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Deller, J., & Hertel, G. (2009). Demographic change in work organisations: Special issue of

Zeitschrift für Personalpsychologie [Journal of Personnel Psychology]. Zeitschrift für

Personalpsychologie, 8(2), 45-46. doi: 10.1026/1617-6391.8.2.45

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five

predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences,

33(4), 533-552. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00171-4

Eby, L. T., Butts, M., & Lockwood, A. (2003). Predictors of success in the era of the

boundaryless career. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24(6), 689-708. doi:

10.1002/job.214

Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 66

Farr, J. L., & Ringseis, E. L. (2002). The older worker in organizational context: Beyond the

individual. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International Review of Industrial

and Organizational Psychology (pp. 31-75). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Feldman, D. C., & Vogel, R. M. (2009). The aging process and person-environment fit. In S. G.

Baugh & S. E. Sullivan (Eds.), Research in careers (pp. 1-25). Charlotte, NC:

Information Age Press.

Ferrer, E., & McArdle, J. J. (2010). Longitudinal modeling of developmental changes in

psychological research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 149-154.

doi: 10.1177/0963721410370300

Field, J., Burke, R. J., & Cooper, C. L. (Eds.). (2013). The SAGE Handbook of Aging, Work and

Society. London: Sage.

Finkelstein, L. M., Burke, M. J., & Raju, M. S. (1995). Age discrimination in simulated

employment contexts: An integrative analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(6),

652-663. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.80.6.652

Finkelstein, L. M., King, E. B., & Voyles, E. C. (2015). Age metastereotyping and cross-age

workplace interactions: A meta view of age stereotypes at work. Work, Aging and

Retirement, 1(1), 26-40. doi: 10.1093/workar/wau002

Finkelstein, L. M., Ryan, K. M., & King, E. B. (2013). What do the young (old) people think of

me? Content and accuracy of age-based metastereotypes. European Journal of Work and

Organizational Psychology, 22(6), 633-657. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.673279

Finkelstein, L. M., Truxillo, D., Fraccaroli, F., & Kanfer, R. (Eds.). (2015). Facing the

challenges of a multi-age workforce: A use-inspired approach: Taylor &

Francis/Routledge.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 67

Fisher, G. G., Stachowski, A., Infurna, F., Faul, J. D., Grosch, J., & Tetrick, L. E. (2014). Mental

work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive decline. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 19(2), 231-242. doi: 10.1037/a0035724

Fiske, S. T. (2004). Intent and ordinary bias: Unintended thought and social motivation create

casual prejudice. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 117-127. doi:

10.1023/B:SORE.0000027405.94966.23

Gaillard, M., & Desmette, D. (2008). Intergroup predictors of older workers' attitudes towards

work and early exit. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 17(4),

450-481. doi: 10.1080/13594320801908564

Gerland, P., Raftery, A. E., Ševčíková, H., Li, N., Gu, D., Spoorenberg, T., . . . Wilmoth, J.

(2014). World population stabilization unlikely this century. Science, 346(6206), 234-

237. doi: 10.1126/science.1257469

Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Hoppmann, C., Willis, S. L., & Schaie, K. W. (2011). Cohort differences

in cognitive aging and terminal decline in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Developmental

Psychology, 47(4), 1026-1041. doi: 10.1037/a0023426

Greller, M. M., & Stroh, L. K. (2004). Making the most of “late-career” for employers and

workers themselves:: Becoming elders not relics. Organizational Dynamics, 33(2), 202-

214. doi: 10.1016/j.orgdyn.2004.01.007

Griffin, M. A., Neal, A., & Parker, S. K. (2007). A new model of work role performance:

Positive behavior in uncertain and interdependent contexts. Academy of Management

Journal, 50, 327-347. doi: 10.5465/AMJ.2007.24634438

Griffiths, A. (1999). Work design and management: The older worker. Experimental Aging

Research, 25, 411-420.


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 68

Hansson, R. O., DeKoekkoek, P. D., Neece, W. M., & Patterson, D. W. (1997). Successful aging

at work: Annual review, 1992-1996: The older workers and transitions to retirement.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51, 202-233. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1997.1605

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102,

284-304. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.284

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span

development. Psychological Review, 117(1), 32-60. doi: 10.1037/a0017668

Hedge, J. W., & Borman, W. C. (2012). The Oxford handbook of work and aging. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Hertel, G., Rauschenbach, C., Thielgen, M., & Krumm, S. (2015). Are older workers more active

copers? Longitudinal effects of age-contingent coping on strain at work. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 36(4), 514-537. doi: 10.1002/job.1995

Hertel, G., Thielgen, M., Rauschenbach, C., Grube, A., Stamov-Roßnagel, C., & Krumm, S.

(2013). Age differences in motivation and stress at work. In C. Schlick, E. Frieling & J.

Wegge (Eds.), Age-differentiated work systems (pp. 119-147). Berlin, Germany:

Springer.

Hertel, G., Van der Heijden, B. I. J. M., de Lange, A. H., & Deller, J. (2013). Facilitating age

diversity in organizations–part I: Challenging popular misbeliefs. Journal of Managerial

Psychology, 28(7/8), 729-740. doi: 10.1108/JMP-07-2013-0233

Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Enrichment effects on

adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and

enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9(1), 1-65. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-

6053.2009.01034.x
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 69

Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). Assessing psychological change in adulthood: An

overview of methodological issues. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 639-657. doi:

10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.639

Heybroek, L., Haynes, M., & Baxter, J. (2015). Life satisfaction and retirement in Australia: A

longitudinal approach. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(2), 166-180. doi:

10.1093/workar/wav006

Hochwarter, W. A., Ferris, G. R., Perrewe, P. L., Witt, L. A., & Kiewitz, C. (2001). A note on

the nonlinearity of the age-job-satisfaction relationship. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology, 31(6), 1223-1237. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02671.x

Hofer, S. M., & Piccinin, A. M. (2010). Toward an integrative science of life-span development

and aging. Journal of Grontology: Psychological Sciences, 65B(3), 269-278. doi:

10.1093/geronb/gbq017

Horn, J. L., & McArdle, J. J. (1992). A practical and theoretical guide to measurement invariance

in aging research. Experimental Aging Research, 18(3), 117-144. doi:

10.1080/03610739208253916

Huffman, A., Culbertson, S. S., Henning, J. B., & Goh, A. (2013). Work-family conflict across

the lifespan. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 28(7/8), 761-780. doi: 10.1108/JMP-07-

2013-0220

Ilmarinen, J. (2009). Aging and work: An international perspective. In S. J. Czaja & J. Sharit

(Eds.), Aging and work: Issues and implications in a changing landscape (pp. 51-73).

Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

James, K. S. (2011). India’s demographic change: Opportunities and challenges. Science,

333(6042), 576-580. doi: 10.1126/science.1207969


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 70

Jex, S. M., Wang, M., & Zarubin, A. (2007). Aging and occupational health. In K. S. Shultz &

G. A. Adams (Eds.), Aging and work in the 21st century (pp. 199-223). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Johnson, S. J., Holdsworth, L., Hoel, H., & Zapf, D. (2013). Customer stressors in service

organizations: The impact of age on stress management and burnout. European Journal

of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(3), 318-330. doi:

10.1080/1359432X.2013.772581

Judge, T. A., & Hurst, C. (2008). How the rich (and happy) get richer (and happier):

Relationship of core self-evaluations to trajectories in attaining work success. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 93(4), 849-863. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.849

Kacmar, K. M., & Ferris, G. R. (1989). Theoretical and methodological considerations in the

age-job satisfaction relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(2), 201-207. doi:

10.1037/0021-9010.74.2.201

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult development, and work motivation.

Academy of Management Review, 29(3), 440-458. doi: 10.5465/AMR.2004.13670969

Kanfer, R., Beier, M. E., & Ackerman, P. L. (2013). Goals and motivation related to work in

later adulthood: An organizing framework. European Journal of Work and

Organizational Psychology, 22(3), 253-264. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.734298

Kearney, E., & Gebert, D. (2009). Managing diversity and enhancing team outcomes: The

promise of transformational leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(1), 77-89. doi:

10.1037/a0013077
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 71

Klein, R. M., Dilchert, S., Ones, D. S., & Dages, K. D. (2015). Cognitive predictors and age-

based adverse impact among business executives. Journal of Applied Psychology. doi:

10.1037/a0038991

Kooij, D. T. A. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., & Dikkers, J. S. E. (2008). Older

workers' motivation to continue to work: Five meanings of age. Journal of Managerial

Psychology, 23(4), 364-394. doi: 10.1108/02683940810869015

Kooij, D. T. A. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., Kanfer, R., & Dikkers, J. S. E. (2011).

Age and work-related motives: Results of a meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 32(2), 197-225. doi: 10.1002/job.665

Kooij, D. T. A. M., Jansen, P. G. W., Dikkers, J. S. E., & De Lange, A. H. (2010). The influence

of age on the associations between HR practices and both affective commitment and job

satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31, 1111-1136. doi:

10.1002/job.666

Kooij, D. T. A. M., Tims, M., & Kanfer, R. (2015). Successful aging at work: The role of job

crafting. In P. M. Bal, D. T. A. M. Kooij & D. M. Rousseau (Eds.), Aging workers and

the employee-employer relationship (pp. 145-161). New York: Springer.

Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). Consequences of individuals'

fit at work: A meta-analysis of person-job, person-organization, person-group, and

person-supervisor fit. Personnel Psychology, 58(2), 281-342. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-

6570.2005.00672.x

Krumm, S., Grube, A., & Hertel, G. (2013a). The Munster Work Value Measure. Journal of

Managerial Psychology, 28(5), 532-560. doi: 10.1108/JMP-07-2011-0023


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 72

Krumm, S., Grube, A., & Hertel, G. (2013b). No time for compromises: Age as a moderator of

the relation between needs–supply fit and job satisfaction. European Journal of Work and

Organizational Psychology, 22(5), 547-562. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.676248

Kulik, C. T., Ryan, S., Harper, S., & George, G. (2014). From the editors: Aging populations and

management. Academy of Management Journal, 57(4), 929-935. doi:

10.5465/amj.2014.4004

Kunze, F., Boehm, S., & Bruch, H. (2013). Age, resistance to change, and job performance.

Journal of Managerial Psychology, 28(7/8), 741-760. doi: 10.1108/JMP-06-2013-0194

Kunze, F., Böhm, S. A., & Bruch, H. (2011). Age diversity, age discrimination climate and

performance consequences: A cross organizational study. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 32(2), 264-290. doi: 10.1002/job.698

Leibold, M., & Voelpel, S. (2006). Managing the aging workforce: Challenges and solutions.

Erlangen, Germany: Publicis.

Levy, B. R. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A psychosocial approach to aging. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 332-336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8721.2009.01662.x

Li, S.-C., & Schmiedek, F. (2002). Age is not necessarily aging: Another step towards

understanding the ‘clocks’ that time aging. Gerontology & Geriatrics Education, 48(1),

5-12. doi: 10.1159/000048917

Liebermann, S. C., Wegge, J., Jungmann, F., & Schmidt, K. H. (2013). Age diversity and

individual team member health: The moderating role of age and age stereotypes. Journal

of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 86(2), 184-202. doi:

10.1111/joop.12016
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 73

Lievens, F., Van Hoye, G., & Zacher, H. (2012). Recruiting/hiring of older workers. In J. W.

Hedge & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp. 380-391).

New York: Oxford University Press.

Lindenberger, U., Von Oertzen, T., Ghisletta, P., & Hertzog, C. (2011). Cross-sectional age

variance extraction: What's change got to do with it? Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 34-

47. doi: 10.1037/a0020525

Löckenhoff, C. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2007). Aging, emotion, and health-related decision

strategies: Motivational manipulations can reduce age differences. Psychology and

Aging, 22(1), 134-146. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.134

MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of

dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19-40. doi:

10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.19

Maertens, J. A., Putter, S. E., Chen, P. Y., Diehl, M., & Huang, Y.-H. (2012). Physical

capabilities and occupational health of older workers. In J. W. Hedge & W. C. Borman

(Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging. New York: Oxford University Press.

Marcus, J., & Fritzsche, B. A. (2015). One size doesn’t fit all. Toward a theory on the

intersectional salience of ageism at work. Organizational Psychology Review, 5(2), 168-

188. doi: 10.1177/2041386614556015

Martin, B., & Xiang, N. (2015). The Australian retirement income system: Structure, effects and

future. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(2), 133-143. doi: 10.1093/workar/wav003

Martus, P., Jakob, O., Rose, U., Seibt, R., & Freude, G. (2010). A comparative analysis of the

Work Ability Index. Occupational Medicine, 60(7), 517-524. doi:

10.1093/occmed/kqq093
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 74

Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of

Psychology, 52(1), 397-422. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397

Mayer, K. U. (2009). New directions in life course research. Annual review of sociology, 35,

423-424. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.34.040507.134619

McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal

data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577-605. doi:

10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612

McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit

motives differ? Psychological Review, 96(4), 690-702. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690

McDaniel, M. A., Pesta, B. J., & Banks, G. C. (2012). Job performance and the aging worker. In

J. W. Hedge & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp. 280-

297). New York: Oxford University Press.

McEvoy, G. M., & Cascio, W. F. (1989). Cumulative evidence of the relationship between

employee age and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(1), 11-17. doi:

10.1037/0021-9010.74.1.11

McGonagle, A., Fisher, G. G., Barnes‐Farrell, J. L., & Grosch, J. (2015). Individual and work

factors related to perceived work ability and labor force outcomes. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 100(2), 376-398. doi: 10.1037/a0037974

Mischel, W. (1969). Continuity and change in personality. American Psychologist, 24(11), 1012-

1018. doi: 10.1037/h0028886

Morrow, D. G., Ridolfo, H. E., Menard, W. E., Sanborn, A., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Magnor,

C., . . . Bryant, D. (2003). Environmental support promotes expertise-based mitigation of


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 75

age differences on pilot communication tasks. Psychology and Aging, 18(2), 268-284.

doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.268

Müller, A., De Lange, A., Weigl, M., Van der Heijden, B., Ackermans, J., & Wilkenloh, J.

(2015). Task performance among employees above age 65: The role of cognitive

functioning and job demand-control. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(3). doi:

10.1093/workar/wav001

Naegele, G., & Walker, A. (2011). Age management in organisations in the European Union. In

M. Malloch, B. N. O'Connor, K. Evans & L. Cairns (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of

workplace learning (pp. 251-267).

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2008). The relationship of age to ten dimensions of job

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 392-423. doi: 10.1037/0021-

9010.93.2.392

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2010). The relationship of age with job attitudes: A meta-

analysis. Personnel Psychology, 63(3), 667-718. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01184.x

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2012a). Age and innovation-related behavior: The joint

moderating effects of supervisor undermining and proactive personality. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 34(5), 583-606. doi: 10.1002/job.1802

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2012b). Evaluating six common stereotypes about older

workers with meta-analytical data. Personnel Psychology, 65(4), 821-858. doi:

10.1111/peps.12003

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2013a). Employee age and health. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 83(3), 336-345. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2013.06.004


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 76

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2013b). How do within-person changes due to aging affect job

performance? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 83(3), 500-513. doi:

10.1016/j.jvb.2013.07.007

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2013c). A meta-analysis of the relationships of age and tenure

with innovation-related behaviour. Journal of Occupational and Organizational

Psychology. doi: 10.1111/joop.12031

Nunes, A., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Experience-based mitigation of age-related performance

declines: Evidence from air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,

15(1), 12-24. doi: 10.1037/a0014947

Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive

scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173-196. doi:

10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

Peng, X. (2011). China’s demographic history and future challenges. Science, 333(6042), 581-

587. doi: 10.1126/science.1209396

Perry, E. L., Dokko, G., & Golom, F. D. (2012). The aging worker and person–environment fit.

In J. W. Hedge & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp.

187-212). New York: Oxford University Press.

Perry, E. L., & Finkelstein, L. M. (1999). Toward a broader view of age discrimination in

employment-related decisions: A joint consideration of organizational factors and

cognitive processes. Human Resource Management Review, 9(1), 21-49. doi:

10.1016/S1053-4822(99)00010-8
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 77

Phillips, D. R., & Siu, O. (2012). Global aging and aging workers. In J. W. Hedge & W. C.

Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp. 11-32). New York:

Oxford University Press.

Piaget, J. (1947). Le Jugement et le raisonnement chez l'enfant Neuchatel, Switzerland:

Delachaux & Niestl.

Posthuma, R. A., & Campion, M. A. (2009). Age stereotypes in the workplace: Common

stereotypes, moderators, and future research directions. Journal of Management, 35(1),

158-188. doi: 10.1177/0149206308318617

Posthuma, R. A., & Guerrero, L. (2013). Age stereotypes in the workplace: Multidimensionality,

cross-cultural applications, and directions for future research. In J. Field, R. J. Burke &

C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of aging, work and society (pp. 250-265).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2009). Time-structured and net intraindividual variability: Tools for

examining the development of dynamic characteristics and processes. Psychology and

Aging, 24(4), 778-791. doi: 10.1037/a0017915

Ram, N., Gerstorf, D., Lindenberger, U., & Smith, J. (2011). Developmental change and

intraindividual variability: Relating cognitive aging to cognitive plasticity, cardiovascular

lability, and emotional diversity. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 363-371. doi:

10.1037/a0021500

Rauschenbach, C., Goritz, A. S., & Hertel, G. (2012). Age stereotypes about emotional resilience

at work. Educational Gerontology, 38, 511-519. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2011.567187

Rauschenbach, C., & Hertel, G. (2011). Age differences in strain and emotional reactivity to

stressors in professional careers. Stress and Health, 27, e48-e60. doi: 10.1002/smi.1335
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 78

Rauschenbach, C., Krumm, S., Thielgen, M. M., & Hertel, G. (2013). Age and work-related

stress: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 28(7/8), 781-804.

doi: 10.1108/JMP-07-2013-0251

Rhodes, S. R. (1983). Age-related differences in work attitudes and behavior: A review and

conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 93(2), 328-367. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.93.2.328

Ries, B. C., Diestel, S., Shemla, M., Liebermann, S. C., Jungmann, F., Wegge, J., & Schmidt, K.

H. (2013). Age diversity and team effectiveness. In C. M. Schlick (Ed.), Age-

differentiated work systems (pp. 89-118). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

Roberts, B. W. (2009). Back to the future: Personality and Assessment and personality

development. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(2), 137-145. doi:

10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.015

Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. K. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 31-35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8721.2008.00543.x

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in

personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies.

Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 1-25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1

Roberts, B. W., & Wood, D. (2006). Personality development in the context of the Neo-

Socioanalytic Model of Personality. In D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of

personality development (pp. 11-39). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.

Roberts, L. (2011). 9 Billion? Science, 333(6042), 540-543. doi: 10.1126/science.333.6042.540


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 79

Roe, R. A. (2008). Time in applied psychology: The study of "what happens" rather than "what

is". European Psychologist, 13(1), 37-52. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.13.1.37

Rosen, B., & Jerdee, T. H. (1976). The influence on age stereotypes on managerial decisions.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(4), 428-432. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.61.4.428

Rudolph, C., Baltes, B. B., & Zabel, K. L. (2013). Age and work motives. In J. Field, R. J. Burke

& C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of aging, work and society (pp. 118-140).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it

or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68–87. doi:

10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x

Salthouse, T. A. (2012). Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annual Review of

Psychology, 63, 201-226. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328

Salthouse, T. A. (2013). Within-cohort age-related differences in cognitive functioning.

Psychological science, 0956797612450893. Psychological Science, 24(2), 123-130. doi:

10.1177/0956797612450893

Salthouse, T. A. (2014). Why are there different age relations in cross-sectional and longitudinal

comparisons of cognitive functioning? Current Directions in Psychological Science,

23(4), 252-256. doi: 10.1177/0963721414535212

Salthouse, T. A. (2015). Implications of the Flynn effect for age–cognition relations.

Intelligence, 48(1), 51-57. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.007

Sanchez, J. I., & Levine, E. L. (2012). The rise and fall of job analysis and the future of work

analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 397-425. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-

120710-100401
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 80

Sánchez Palacios, C., Torres, T., & Blanca Mena, M. J. (2009). Negative aging stereotypes and

their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Archives of

Gerontology and Geriatrics, 48(3), 385-390. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2008.03.007

Schaie, K. W. (2013). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal

Study (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Schalk, R., van Veldhoven, M., de Lange, A. H., De Witte, H., Kraus, K., Roßnagel, C., . . .

Zacher, H. (2010). Moving European research on work and ageing forward: Overview

and agenda. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 19(1), 76-101.

doi: 10.1080/13594320802674629

Scheibe, S., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (2009). Effects of regulating emotions on cognitive

performance: What is costly for young adults is not so costly for older adults. Psychology

and Aging, 24(1), 217-223. doi: 10.1037/a0013807

Scheibe, S., Stamov-Roßnagel, C., & Zacher, H. (2015). Links between emotional job demands

and occupational well-being: Age differences depend on type of demand. Work, Aging

and Retirement, 1(3). doi: 10.1093/workar/wav007

Scheibe, S., & Zacher, H. (2013). A lifespan perspective on emotion regulation, stress, and well-

being in the workplace. In P. L. Perrewé, J. Halbesleben & C. C. Rosen (Eds.), Research

in occupational stress and well-being (Vol. 11, pp. 167-197). Bingley, UK: Emerald.

Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2013). Keeping it steady: Older adults perform

more consistently on cognitive tasks than younger adults. Psychological Science, 24(9),

1747-1754. doi: 10.1177/0956797613479611

Schmitt, A., Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2012). The buffering effect of selection, optimization, and

compensation strategy use on the relationship between problem solving demands and
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 81

occupational well-being: A daily diary study. Journal of Occupational Health

Psychology, 17, 139-149. doi: 10.1037/a0027054

Schneider, B., Ehrhart, M. G., & Macey, W. H. (2013). Organizational climate and culture.

Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 361-388. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143809

Schneider, B., Goldstein, H. W., & Smith, D. B. (1995). The ASA framework: An update.

Personnel Psychology, 48(4), 747-773. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1995.tb01780.x

Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (2004). Occupational self-direction, intellectual

functioning, and self-directed orientation in older workers: Findings and implications for

individuals and societies. American Journal of Sociology, 110(1), 161-197. doi:

10.1086/385430

Schwall, A. R. (2012). Defining age and using age-relevant constructs. In J. W. Hedge & W. C.

Borman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Work and Aging (pp. 169-186). New York:

Oxford University Press.

Shultz, K. S., & Adams, G. A. (2007a). Aging and work in the 21st century. Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Shultz, K. S., & Adams, G. A. (2007b). In search of a unifying paradigm for understanding aging

and work in the 21st century. In K. S. Shultz & G. A. Adams (Eds.), Aging and work in

the 21st century (pp. 303-319). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Skirbekk, V., Stonawski, M., Bonsang, E., & Staudinger, U. M. (2013). The Flynn effect and

population aging. Intelligence, 41(3), 169-177. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.02.001

Sonnentag, S. (2012). Time in organizational research: Catching up on a long neglected topic in

order to improve theory. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(4), 361-368. doi:

10.1177/2041386612442079
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 82

Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2012). Development of Big Five domains and facets in adulthood:

Mean‐level age trends and broadly versus narrowly acting mechanisms. Journal of

Personality, 80(4), 881-914. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00752.x

Stamov-Roßnagel, C., & Biemann, T. (2012). Ageing and work motivation: A task-level

perspective. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 27(5), 459-478. doi:

10.1108/02683941211235382

Stamov-Roßnagel, C., & Hertel, G. (2010). Older workers' motivation: Against the myth of

general decline. Management Decision, 48(6), 894-906. doi:

10.1108/00251741011053451

Staudinger, U. M. (2015). Images of aging: Outside and inside perspectives. Annual Review of

Gerontology and Geriatrics, 35(1), 187-209. doi: 10.1891/0198-8794.35.187

Sturman, M. C. (2003). Searching for the inverted U-shaped relationship between time and

performance: Meta-analyses of the experience/performance, tenure/performance, and

age/performance relationships. Journal of Management, 29(5), 609-640. doi:

10.1016/S0149-2063(03)00028-X

Super, D. E. (1980). A life-span, life-space approach to career development. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 16(3), 282-298. doi: 10.1016/0001-8791(80)90056-1

Taylor, J. L., O'Hara, R., Mumenthaler, M. S., Rosen, A. C., & Yesavage, J. A. (2005).

Cognitive ability, expertise, and age differences in following air-traffic control

instructions. Psychology and Aging, 20(1), 117-133. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.117

Tenhiälä, A., Linna, A., von Bonsdorff, M., Pentti, J., Vahtera, J., Kivimäki, M., & Elovainio, M.

(2013). Organizational justice, sickness absence and employee age. Journal of

Managerial Psychology, 28(7/8), 805-825. doi: 10.1108/JMP-07-2013-0246


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 83

Then, F. S., Luck, T., Luppa, M., Thinschmidt, M., Deckert, S., Nieuwenhuijsen, K., . . . Riedel-

Heller, S. G. (2014). Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working

environment on cognition and dementia. Occupational and Environmental Medicine,

71(5), 358-365. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2013-101760

Thielgen, M. M., Krumm, S., & Hertel, G. (2015). When being old pays off: Age mitigates

adverse effects of low implicit–explicit motive congruency on work motivation. Journal

of Career Assessment. doi: 10.1177/1069072714547613

Thielgen, M.M., Krumm, S., Rauschenbach, C., & Hertel, G. (2015). Older but wiser: Age

moderates congruency effects between implicit and explicit motives on job satisfaction.

Motivation and Emotion, 39(2), 182-200. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9448-8

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., & Ballesteros, S. (2014). Video game training enhances cognition of

older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 706-716. doi:

10.1037/a0037507

Truxillo, D. M., Cadiz, D. M., & Hammer, L. B. (2015). Supporting the aging workforce: A

research review and recommendations for workplace intervention research. Annual

Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2, 351-381. doi:

10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111435

Truxillo, D. M., Cadiz, D. M., Rineer, J. R., Zaniboni, S., & Fraccaroli, F. (2012). A lifespan

perspective on job design: Fitting the job and the worker to promote job satisfaction,

engagement, and performance. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(4), 340-360. doi:

10.1177/2041386612454043
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 84

Truxillo, D. M., & Fraccaroli, F. (2013). Research themes on age and work: Introduction to the

special issue. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(3), 249-252.

doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2013.786604

Tuomi, K., Ilmarinen, J., Martikainen, R., Aalto, L., & Klockars, M. (1997). Aging, work, life-

style and work ability among Finnish municipal workers in 1981–1992. Scandinavian

Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 23(Suppl 1), 58-65. doi:

http://www.jstor.org/stable/40966692

Twenge, J. M., Campbell, S. M., Hoffman, B. J., & Lance, C. E. (2010). Generational

differences in work values: Leisure and extrinsic values increasing, social and intrinsic

values decreasing. Journal of Management, 36(5), 1117-1142. doi:

10.1177/0149206309352246

United Nations. (2007). World economic and social survey: Development in an ageing world.

New York: United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs.

United States Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2014). The Age Discrimination in

Employment Act of 1967. Retrieved November 5, 2014, from

http://www.eeoc.gov/laws/statutes/adea.cfm

Van Vianen, A. E., Dalhoeven, B. A., & De Pater, I. E. (2011). Aging and training and

development willingness: Employee and supervisor mindsets. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 32(2), 226-247. doi: 10.1002/job.685

Vaupel, J. W., & Loichinger, E. (2006). Redistributing work in aging Europe. Science, 312,

1911-1913. doi: 10.1126/science.1127487


MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 85

Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in

adulthood: Estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models.

Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), 231-249. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.3.231

Von Hippel, C., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Henry, J. D. (2012). Stereotype threat among older

employees: Relationships with job attitudes and turnover intentions. Psychology and

Aging, 28(1), 17-27. doi: 10.1037/a0029825

Waldman, D. A., & Avolio, B. J. (1986). A meta-analysis of age differences in job performance.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 71(1), 33-38. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.71.1.33

Wang, M. (2015). Inaugural editorial. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(1), 1-3. doi:

10.1093/workar/wau010

Wang, M., Burlacu, G., Truxillo, D., James, K. S., & Yao, X. (2015). Age differences in

feedback reactions: The roles of employee feedback orientation on social awareness and

utility. Journal of Applied Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0038334

Wang, M., & Shi, J. (2014). Psychological research on retirement. Annual Review of Psychology,

65, 1.1-1.25. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115131

Wang, M., & Shultz, K. S. (2010). Employee retirement: A review and recommendations for

future investigation. Journal of Management, 36(1), 172-206.

Warr, P. B. (1993). In what circumstances does job performance vary with age? European Work

and Organizational Psychologist, 3(3), 237-249. doi: 10.1080/09602009308408593

Warr, P. B. (2001). Age and work behaviour: Physical attributes, cognitive abilities, knowledge,

personality traits and motives. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International

Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (Vol. 16, pp. 1-36). Chichester, UK:

Wiley.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 86

Warren, D. A. (2015). Pathways to retirement in Australia: Evidence from the HILDA survey.

Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(2), 144-165. doi: 10.1093/workar/wau013

Wegge, J., Jungmann, F., Liebermann, S., Shemla, M., Ries, B. C., Diestel, S., & Schmidt, K. H.

(2012). What makes age diverse teams effective? Results from a six-year research

program. Work: A Journal of Prevention, Assessment and Rehabilitation, 41, 5145-5151.

doi: 10.3233/WOR-2012-0084-5145

Wegge, J., Roth, C., Neubach, B., Schmidt, K. H., & Kanfer, R. (2008). Age and gender

diversity as determinants of performance and health in a public organization: The role of

task complexity and group size. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1301-1313. doi:

10.1037/a0012680

Weigl, M., Müller, A., Hornung, S., Zacher, H., & Angerer, P. (2013). The moderating effects of

job control and selection, optimization, and compensation strategies on the age-work

ability relationship. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34, 607-628. doi:

10.1002/job.1810

Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Selection, optimization, and compensation:

An action-related approach to work and partnership. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 57,

273-300. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.2000.1752

Yeung, D. Y., & Fung, H. H. (2009). Aging and work: How do SOC strategies contribute to job

performance across adulthood? Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 927-940. doi:

10.1037/a0017531

Zacher, H. (2013). Older job seekers' job search intensity: The interplay of proactive personality,

age, and occupational future time perspective. Ageing & Society, 33(7), 1139-1166. doi:

10.1017/S0144686X12000451
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 87

Zacher, H. (2015). Successful aging at work. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(1), 4-25. doi:

10.1093/workar/wau006

Zacher, H., Clark, M., Anderson, E. C., & Ayoko, O. B. (2015). A lifespan perspective on

leadership. In P. M. Bal, D. T. A. M. Kooij & D. M. Rousseau (Eds.), Aging workers and

the employee-employer relationship (pp. 87-104): Springer.

Zacher, H., Feldman, D. C., & Schulz, H. (2014). Age, occupational strain, and well-being: A

person-environment fit perspective. In P. L. Perrewé, J. Halbesleben & C. C. Rosen

(Eds.), Research in occupational stress and well-being (Vol. 12, pp. 83-111). Bingley,

UK: Emerald.

Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2009). Remaining time and opportunities at work: Relationships

between age, work characteristics, and occupational future time perspective. Psychology

and Aging, 24(2), 487-493. doi: 10.1037/a0015425

Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2011). Maintaining a focus on opportunities at work: The interplay

between age, job complexity, and the use of selection, optimization, and compensation

strategies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 291-318. doi: 10.1002/job.683

Zacher, H., & Gielnik, M. (2014). Organizational age cultures: The interplay of chief executive

officers’ age and attitudes toward younger and older employees. International Small

Business Journal, 32(3), 327-349. doi: 10.1177/0266242612463025

Zacher, H., & Griffin, B. (2015). Work, aging, and retirement in Australia: Introduction to the

special issue. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(2), 129-132. doi: 10.1093/workar/wau011

Zacher, H., Heusner, S., Schmitz, M., Zwierzanska, M. M., & Frese, M. (2010). Focus on

opportunities as a mediator of the relationships between age, job complexity, and work
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 88

performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 374-386. doi:

10.1016/j.jvb.2009.09.001

Zacher, H., Jimmieson, N. L., & Bordia, P. (2014). Time pressure and coworker support mediate

the curvilinear relationship between age and occupational well-being. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 19(4), 462-475. doi: 10.1037/a0036995

Zacher, H., Jimmieson, N. L., & Winter, G. (2012). Eldercare demands, mental health, and work

performance: The moderating role of satisfaction with eldercare tasks. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 17(1), 52-64. doi: 10.1037/a0025154

Zaniboni, S. (2015). The interaction between older workers’ personal resources and perceived

age discrimination affects the desired retirement age and the expected adjustment. Work,

Aging and Retirement, 1(3). doi: 10.1093/workar/wav010

Zaniboni, S., Truxillo, D. M., & Fraccaroli, F. (2013). Differential effects of task variety and

skill variety on burnout and turnover intentions for older and younger workers. European

Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(3), 306-317. doi:

10.1080/1359432X.2013.782288
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 89

Table 1

Age Stereotypes at Work (e.g., Finkelstein et al., 1995; Gordon & Arvey, 2004; Posthuma &

Campion, 2009) and Related Empirical Evidence

Age Stereotypes Favoring Younger Workers

Productivity
 Older workers perform not as well as younger workers (not confirmed).
 Older workers are less motivated than younger workers (not confirmed).
 Older workers have lower work abilities than younger workers (only confirmed for physical
skills and fluid intelligence, and with high inter-individual variability).
 Older workers are less creative and innovative (not confirmed).

Learning, Development, and Return of Training Investments


 Older workers have lower learning motivation than younger workers (confirmed, but this may
be due to stereotype threat and self-fulfilling prophecies rather than aging per se).
 Older workers have lower learning abilities than younger workers (not confirmed).
 Older workers are more resistant to change and less flexible than younger workers (not
confirmed).

Health and Stress Resilience


 Older workers have lower health (only confirmed for some physical indicators).
 Older workers take more sick leaves than younger workers (only partly confirmed).
 Older workers are more vulnerable to stress at work (not confirmed).

Age Stereotypes Favoring Older Workers

Reliability
 Older workers are more dependable, careful, loyal, and trustworthy than younger workers
(confirmed).
 Older workers have more experience and are thus more stable and cool-headed than younger
workers (partly confirmed).
 Older workers are more committed to the job than younger workers (confirmed).
 Older workers are less likely to miss work (confirmed for voluntary absenteeism).
 Older workers are less likely to turnover quickly (confirmed).
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 90

Table 2

Empirically Established Age Differences in Work-Related Capabilities, Motivation, and Work

Outcomes

Work-Related Capabilities
 Lower physiological and physical abilities (e.g., sensory, muscle, cardiovascular, respiratory,
neurological, and immune system functions) among older workers, but also larger inter-
individual variation.
 Slightly higher values for blood pressure, cholesterol level, body mass index, and self-
reported insomnia among older workers.
 Higher fluid intelligence (abstract reasoning, selective attention, novel problem solving, and
rapid information processing) among younger workers, but also larger inter-individual
variation in fluid intelligence between older workers.
 Higher crystallized intelligence (experience-based and domain-specific knowledge,
vocabulary, and verbal comprehension) among older workers.
 Higher stability and less fluctuation of cognitive performance among older workers.
 Higher emotional and self-regulation skills among older workers.
 More active coping strategies among older workers.
 Slightly higher consciousness and agreeableness, and slightly lower neuroticism among older
workers.

Work Motivation
 Higher learning motivation among younger workers.
 Higher autonomy needs and pro-social values (generativity) among older workers.
 Higher sensitivity for person-environment fit for among workers.
 Higher congruency between explicit and implicit motives (i.e., lower motive conflicts) among
older workers.
 Lower resistance to change among older workers.

Work Performance and Occupational Well-being


 No age differences in core task performance.
 Higher contextual performance and organizational citizenship behavior among older workers.
 Higher job satisfaction among older workers.
 More positive job attitudes among older workers (e.g., loyalty, trust, commitment).
 Higher strain and exhaustion among middle-aged workers.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 91

Table 3

Implications for Age-Inclusive Human Resource Management

Organizations
 Personnel attraction and selection: Conduct age-inclusive job analyses and implement non-
discriminatory selection procedures.
 Work (re-)design: Consider different age-related abilities and needs (particularly in team
contexts and as a leader) and implement relevant support systems.
 Personnel development: Ensure adequate training and career development opportunities, as
well as health and performance management for workers irrespective of their age.
 Positive organizational age culture and climate: Eliminate negative age stereotypes in shared
beliefs and values, and eliminate age discrimination in policies, practices, and procedures.
 Facilitating the retirement transition: Help older workers prepare for retirement, offer
different retirement options, and provide opportunities to keep in touch.

Individuals
 Maintaining physical and mental health: Take advantage of prevention opportunities and
health check-ups; exercise, sleep, eat well, and be socially active.
 Sustain work ability, employability, and career adaptability: Participate in continuous
learning and career development activities in order to adapt to work and career changes and
challenges.
 Stay cognitively active: A mentally, physically, and socially active lifestyle can help
maintain cognitive functioning and prevent age-related cognitive decline.
 Proactively manage yourself and your work: Use selection, optimization, and compensation
strategies to deal with (potential) age-related resource losses, and job crafting and
idiosyncratic deals to adapt your job to your age-related abilities and needs.

Society
 Distribute work and associated resources fairly across individuals and different life phases.
 Increases in retirement age should be complemented with adequate work opportunities that
promote healthy and productive aging.
 Support flexible work arrangements such as bridge employment and post-retirement work.
 Introduce and rigorously enforce laws against age discrimination in employment.
 Support an open discussion of potentials and opportunities of an aging workforce to change
outdated beliefs about aging.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 92

Table 4

Desirable Characteristics of Future Studies on Aging at Work

 Age is not a causal variable but a correlate: Conceptualize and examine person and
contextual mediating mechanisms of assumed age differences and aging effects (e.g., future
time perspective, subject matter expertise), and also include mediators when age is
conceptualized as a moderator variable.
 Workers become more heterogeneous with age: Consider age-related differences in
variability (i.e.., fanning out) in addition to main effects of age, and the person and
contextual factors that may explain increased variability among older workers.
 Don’t forget the context: Examine age-related changes in both individual and context
characteristics (e.g., children, eldercare demands).
 Consider alternatives to cross-section designs: Conduct (short-term) longitudinal studies
(e.g., across critical career periods), training interventions, and daily diary/experience
sampling studies.
 No “age bracketing”: Operationalize age as a continuous variable instead of creating
arbitrary age groups, to avoid loss of statistical power.
 Pay attention to generalizability: Aim for equal proportions of participants across the
focused age range; at least report proportion of workers in different age groups.
 Consider higher complexity: Examine potential curvilinear in addition to linear age effects.
 Examine alternative explanations: Control for other time-related constructs, such as job and
organizational tenure.
 Assess both subjective and objective outcomes of aging and their interrelationships over
time (e.g., not only performance, but also perceived meaningfulness of work).
 Study also subgroups of older workers, particularly those that are less privileged (e.g.,
women, less educated workers, the unemployed) and how they can be supported.
MANAGING THE AGING WORKFORCE 93

Author Biographies

Guido Hertel (ghertel@uni-muenster.de) holds a chair in organizational and business


psychology at the University of Münster, Germany (since 2008). His research includes a wide
range of basic and applied topics within organizational and business psychology. In addition to
aging at work and demographic changes in organizations, his current research activities address
process gains in teams, distributed (virtual) collaboration, electronic human resource
management, and negotiation and conflict management. His research results have been published
in Psychological Bulletin, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of Applied
Psychology, Journal of Management, Research Policy, Human Resource Management Review,
Journal of Managerial Psychology, and European Journal of Work and Organizational
Psychology (among others). Guido Hertel currently serves as associate editor of Group and
Organization Management, and is member of several editorial boards of journals, among them
the founding editorial board of Work, Aging and Retirement.

Hannes Zacher (h.zacher@rug.nl) is Associate Professor of organizational psychology in the


Department of Psychology at the University of Groningen. He received his Ph.D. from the
University of Giessen (2009). Subsequently, he was postdoctoral fellow at Jacobs University and
lecturer at the University of Queensland. His research program investigates sustainability in and
of organizations, with foci on successful aging at work, career development, adaptability, and
occupational well-being; proactivity, innovation, leadership, and entrepreneurship; and pro-
environmental employee behavior. His research has been published in journals such as Journal
of Organizational Behavior, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, Journal of Vocational
Behavior, Journal of Management, Leadership Quarterly, Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, and Psychology and Aging. He is research fellow at the Sloan
Center on Aging & Work at Boston College and member of the founding editorial board of
Work, Aging and Retirement.

Вам также может понравиться