Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 2

Current Biology Vol 22 No 3

R84

3. Sterflinger, K. (2000). Fungi as geologic agents. Antarctica. Rev. Environ. Sci. Biotechnol. 6, 15. Fomina, M., Burford, E.P., Hillier, S.,
Geomicrobiol. J. 17, 97–124. 127–141. Kierans, M., and Gadd, G.M. (2010).
4. Rhee, Y.J., Hillier, S., and Gadd, G.M. (2012). 10. Rooney, D., Hutchens, E., Clipson, N., Rock-Building Fungi. Geomicrobiol. J. 27,
Lead transformation to pyromorphite by fungi. Baldini, J., and McDermott, F. (2010). Microbial 624–629.
Curr. Biol. 22, 237–241. community diversity of moonmilk deposits at 16. Papanikolaou, N.C., Hatzidaki, E.G.,
5. Hawksworth, D.L. (2001). The magnitude of Ballynamintra cave, Co. Waterford, Ireland. Belivanis, S., Tzanakakis, G.N., and
fungal diversity: the 1.5 million species Microb. Ecol. 60, 753–761. Tsatsakis, M. (2005). Lead toxicity update.
estimate revisited. Mycol. Res. 105, 1422–1432. 11. Burford, E.P., Fomina, M., and Gadd, G.M. A brief review. Med. Sci. Mon. 11, 329–336.
6. Kis-Papo, T., Grishkan, I., Oren, A., (2003). Fungal involvement in bioweathering 17. Gadd, G.M. (2010). Metals, minerals and
Wasser, S.P., and Nevo, E. (2001). and biotransformation of rocks and minerals. microbes: geomicrobiology and
Spatiotemporal diversity of filamentous fungi in Mineralogical Mag. 67, 1127–1155. bioremediation. Microbiology 156, 609–643.
the hypersaline Dead Sea. Mycol. Res. 105, 12. van Schöll, L., Kuyper, T.W., Smits, M.M.,
749–756. Landeweert, R., Hoffland, E., and van 1Environmental Microbiology Group, School
7. Gleeson, D.B., Clipson, N., Melville, K., Breemen, N. (2008). Rock-eating mycorrhizas:
Gadd, G.M., and McDermott, F. (2005). their role in plant nutrition and biogeochemical of Biology and Environmental Science and
Characterization of fungal community structure cycles. Plant. Soil 303, 35–47. Earth Institute, University College Dublin,
on a weathered pegmatitic granite. Microbial 13. Burford, E.P., Kierans, M., and Gadd, G.M. Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland. 2School of Earth
Ecol. 50, 360–368. (2003). Geomycology: fungi in mineral and Environment, The University of Western
8. Gadd, G.M. (2007). Geomycology: substrata. Mycologist 17, 98–107.
biogeochemical transformations of rocks, 14. Smits, M.M., Herrmann, A.M., Duane, M.,
Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley,
minerals, metals and radiounuclides by fungi, Duckworth, O.W., Bonneville, S., Benning, L.G., WA 6009, Australia.
bioweathering and bioremediation. Mycol. Res. and Lundstrom, U. (2009). The fungal–mineral *E-mail: nicholas.clipson@ucd.ie
111, 3–49. interface: challenges and considerations of
9. Ruisi, S., Barreca, D., Selbmann, L., micro-analytical developments. Fung. Biol.
Zucconi, L., and Onofri, S. (2007). Fungi in Rev. 23, 122–131. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.037

Social Neuroscience: More Friends, in an individual brain’s gray matter


More Problems.More Gray Matter? density relative to a group-averaged
brain by measuring where, and
how much, the imaged brain needed
to be expanded or compressed
The social brain hypothesis generically posits that increasing social group size
in order to match the average brain.
relates is associated with an increase in neocortex size. A new study identifies,
The ‘determinant’ of the resulting
within a species, the specific neural circuit that may confer the primate ability to
matrix is a scalar value that can be
manage social relationships as they increase in number.
used as a dependent variable and
regions which showed a >5 mm3
Maia ten Brink and Asif A. Ghazanfar positive correlation between neocortex difference from the average brain
size and social group size across were identified as significant.
The oil industrialist, John D. primate species [1], this is a rather Remarkably, the authors found that
Rockefeller, once said that ‘‘The ability coarse measure and not particularly specific regions of the neocortex
to deal with people is as purchasable illuminating — there are a number of varied in the gray matter density
a commodity as sugar or coffee and I other behavioral variables that also according to social group size. The
will pay more for that ability than for any correlate with the size of the neocortex. regions with increased gray
other under the sun’’. It is unclear how A recent study by Sallet and colleagues matter are known to be important
many people Rockefeller actually [2] gives us greater insights into what for processing social signals (facial
bought in his lifetime, but at the very specific neural circuits may mediate expressions, eye gaze, vocalizations)
least he recognizes that the ability the cognitive balancing act required in monkeys [2]: the superior temporal
to interact effectively with them would for increasing the size of one’s social sulcus, the superior and inferior
be more valuable than anything else group. temporal gyri, the amygdala and
in the world. Who could argue with Using magnetic resonance imaging the rostral prefrontal cortex (and we
that? Consider the many different (MRI) scans, Sallet et al. [2] investigated know that most are directly
relationships in your life — your whether group size is related to connected with each other [3]).
parents, siblings, extended family, brain differences in rhesus monkeys For every additional member of
friends, colleagues, competitors, and (Macaca mulatta). According to a social group, density increased
so on. As for all primates, our lives veterinary considerations, 23 monkeys by about 5%.
are an intricate web of relationships; were assigned to groups ranging in This pattern of results is consistent
every relationship is unique and it size from one to seven individuals. with three recent human studies
seems for each we are performing an The authors capitalized on this relating social network size (as
energetically costly balancing act. restructuring of their institution’s measured by Facebook or
Naturally, adding more individuals to monkey colony by scanning each questionnaires) to the superior
one’s web increases the effort required monkey’s brain 15 months after the temporal sulcus [4], the amygdala [5]
to maintain all. How do we manage group assignments and then asking: and the prefrontal cortex [6].
this? There is, of course, the banal is there a correlation between an Nevertheless, to lend credence to the
suggestion that it is our bigger brains individual’s social group size and the idea that these are indeed regions
that confer this ability. While there is a neocortex? They compared differences involved in the task of balancing social
Dispatch
R85

relationships, Sallet et al. [2] measured study of mice trained to perform one within a social group obviates
whether social rank influenced gray of two spatial tasks showed task- and the need for high level processing
matter density independent of group region-specific gray matter density involving mental representations.
size. They found that monkeys with changes (using the same measure Individuals can use this on-going
a higher rank had greater gray as Sallet et al. [2]) and that these structure as an accurate and always
matter density in the inferior temporal changes were associated with the up-to-date model (for example [12]),
gyrus and the rostral prefrontal remodeling of neuronal processes allowing for more efficient action
cortex. What they did not find is as measured by a stain for a selection and execution. With the
also intriguing. Despite looking for growth-associated protein (GAP-43) findings of Sallet et al. [2] we are
such an effect, the authors did not [9]. Like task-induced gray matter now poised to directly investigate
find any relationship between the changes in mice, it should be the case this and other pressing issues
so-called ‘mirror neuron system’ that if the group size in primates is the regarding the mechanisms of social
and social group size. Some cause of the gray matter increases cognition.
hypothesize that the mirror neuron in regions processing social signals,
system plays a critical role in social then there should be evidence that an
References
cognition, possibly by inferring the individual’s ability to interact within 1. Dunbar, R.I.M., and Schultz, S. (2007).
actions of others through simulation the group changes (gets better) with Evolution in the social brain. Science 317,
1344–1347.
[8]. Thus, it is surprising that there time along with a corresponding 2. Sallet, J., Mars, R.B., Noonan, M.P.,
were no changes in gray matter density increase in gray matter density. Sallet Andersson, J.L., O’Reilly, J.X., Jbabdi, S.,
in any of the core regions comprising et al.’s [2] study provides both the Croxson, P.L., Jenkinson, M., Miller, K.L., and
Rushworth, M.F.S. (2011). Social network size
this system. methodological and scientific affects neural circuits in macaques. Science
Overall, the main finding of Sallet framework for such a longitudinal 334, 697–700.
3. Ghazanfar, A.A., and Santos, L.R. (2004).
et al. [2] is that there is a relationship study. It also begs the question of Primate brains in the wild: The sensory bases
between specific neocortical circuits, whether or not the modification of for social interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5,
their size and the number of individuals these circuits requires interactions 603–616.
4. Petrides, M., and Pandya, D.N. (2007).
in a group. What the data do not tell with only conspecifics or if any social Efferent association pathways from the rostral
us is whether the differences in gray agent would do. Do humans, for prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey.
Neurosci. 27, 11573–11586.
matter density are caused by instance, get the same gain of gray 5. Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Roylance, R., and
differences in social group size. matter density increases from Rees, G. (2011). Online social network size is
That is, did the assignment to the interacting with other species, reflected in human brain structure. Proc. R.
Soc. B, in press.
bigger groups lead to increases in such as dogs? Conversely, in dogs, 6. Bickart, K., Wright, C.I., Dautoff, R.J.,
gray matter density? Or did the we know that human contact does Dickerson, B.C., and Barrett, L.F. (2011).
Amygdala volume and social network size in
monkeys with bigger social circuits not fully replace the need for humans. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 163–164.
end up in the bigger groups because socialization with other dogs, but 7. Lewis, P.A., Rezaie, R., Brown, R., Roberts, N.,
they could handle the complexities it does reduce the effects of early and Dunbar, R.I.M. (2011). Ventromedial
prefrontal volume predicts understanding
better? Although the authors deprivation [10]. Does such differential of others and social network size. Neuroimage
seem certain that monkeys were early experience impact this very 57, 1624–1629.
8. Gallese, V., Keyser, C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2004).
assigned to groups according to same ‘group size-related’ neocortical A unifying view of the basis of social cognition.
criteria orthogonal to an individual circuit? Trends in Cognitive Sci. 8, 396–403.
monkey’s sociality [7], all veterinary Identifying the neural circuits related 9. Lerch, J.P., Yiu, A.P., Martinez-Canabal, A.,
Pekar, T., Bohbot, V.D., Frankland, P.W.,
assignments to different groups to social group size also lays the Henkelman, R.M., Josselyn, S.A., and Sled, J.G.
must take sociality into account foundation for studying a deeper (2011). Maze training in mice induces
MRI-detectable brain shape changes specific
as fighting between macaque issue: the content of social brain to the type of learning. Neuroimage 54,
monkeys can be quite vicious networks. The typical interpretation 2086–2095.
(both in the wild and captivity). of the social brain hypothesis presents 10. Fox, M.W., and Stelzner, D. (1967). The effects
of early experience on the development of inter
Furthermore, the reasons some a view of primates as biologically and intraspecies social relationships in the dog.
monkeys get along (or do not prepared for social signals such as Anim. Behav. 15, 377–386.
11. Barrett, L., Henzi, P., and Rendall, D. (2007).
get along) with others are not easily faces and voices as well as for forms Social brains, simple minds: does
related to their personality measures of social engagements that require social complexity really require cognitive
and are often impossible to discern. mental representations of abstract complexity? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362,
561–575.
Thus, the lack of random assignment concepts like family relations and 12. Turesson, H.K., and Ghazanfar, A.A. (2011).
of monkeys to different group alliances in order to negotiate the Statistical learning of social signals
and its implications for the social
sizes, and the lack of MRI data social landscape. An alternative brain hypothesis. Interaction Studies 12,
before the group assignments hypothesis suggests that individuals 397–417.
were made, preclude us from do not need to hold abstract
knowing whether increasing group concepts of family relations and
Neuroscience Institute, Departments of
size causes the increase in gray alliances ‘in mind’ because they can
Psychology and Ecology & Evolutionary
matter density. assess circumstances by directly Biology, Princeton University, Princeton,
That said, it is well-established that monitoring what is happening around NJ 08540, USA.
the adult brain is quite plastic and that them [11]. According to this view, E-mail: asifg@princeton.edu
this plasticity is reflected in changes in the active perception of on-going
specific brain regions. For example, a spatial and temporal structure — the
recent combined MRI–histochemical statistics — of interacting primates DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.042

Вам также может понравиться