Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

University of Nebraska at Omaha

DigitalCommons@UNO
Communication Faculty Publications School of Communication

Summer 1987

The Nonreader Problem: A Closer Look at


Avoiding the Newspaper
Jeremy Harris Lipschultz
University of Nebraska at Omaha, jeremy.lipschultz@gmail.com

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/commfacpub


Part of the Communication Commons

Recommended Citation
Lipschultz, Jeremy Harris, "The Nonreader Problem: A Closer Look at Avoiding the Newspaper" (1987). Communication Faculty
Publications. 78.
https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/commfacpub/78

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the School of
Communication at DigitalCommons@UNO. It has been accepted for
inclusion in Communication Faculty Publications by an authorized
administrator of DigitalCommons@UNO. For more information, please
contact unodigitalcommons@unomaha.edu.
by Jeremy Harris Lipschultz

The Nonreader
Problem: .A Closer
Look at Avoiding the
Newspaper
Why do some people avoid reading ~j,apers? Are their reasons different
from those of regular readers? Findings rom a survey suggest responses to a
sample of avoidance statements by readers are more clearly_ defined than those
of nonreaders. This study also raises ~uestions about distinguishing between
'regular" and "casual" readers, asfindmgs provide only limited supportfor the
use of_ "casual" readers in future research. Further development of avoidance
gratifu:ation theory migJit help newspapers convert nonreaders to future
recufers.

Understanding why some J:JeOple qo The focus in uses and gratification


not read newspapers or use other media literature in recent years has broadened,
has both practical and theoretical im- turning toward an expectancy-value
portance. approach,3 the use of dependency
Emphasis in the 1970s on the uses theory,4 or evolutionary empirical
and gratifications approach I to media models.5 These models attempt to
studies failed to produce much data on extend previous efforts by adapting the
so-called avoidance items. Unlike uses theory to a more cognitive perspective.
of mass media, avoidance questions In this connection Fishbein's6 work
ask for responses on why one might on attitudes and beliefs and Vroom's7
nQ.t use mass media. on motivation are important in theory
Becker2 provided the most concep- development. As SwansonS noted
tual analysis to date. His factor "commitment to addressing conceptual
analysis yielded a cluster of what was issues may thus be read, without
termed "avoidance motivations of overstating the case too strongly, as
various sorts." In this framework
avoidances measured were not mirror- Lipschultz is a graduate student in the
opposites of gratifications; they are School of Journalism at Southern
Illinois University. Data contained in
quite distinct from positive gratifi- this article were collected by graduate
cations. students in a research methods course.

Newspaper Research Journal, Vol. 8, No.4, Summer, 1987/59


60/Newspaper Research Journal

one sign of the field's maturation." "atypical" nonreaders, those not fitting
Refining a useful model, however, has the categorical demographic defini-
sometimes been hampered by an tions.
ambiguity of concepts. Further, a Much of the avoidance research has
more complete picture of the uses and centered on the nonreader without
gratifications paradigm may be diffi- relating data to that collected about
cult, if not impossible, without a readers. Analyses sometimes assume
stronger conceptualization of what regular users of mass media do not
avoiding mass media means to the have their own special avoidances,
potential user. The present study whether that be toward type of news
focuses on the act of using or avoiding stories, sections of the newspaper, or
use of newspapers. more generally toward types of mass
Early work on avoidances was pri- media which may use color, pictures,
marily limited to a discussion of the the written or the spoken word.
demographic patterns of the newspaper Studying avoidances may also relate
nonreader. Westley and Severin9 to newspaper business questions.
found that nonreaders were generally Research on the presumed poor health
very young or old, living in rural areas, of newspapers suggests it may be the
and of lower income, educational or psychological health rather than
occupational status. business health that is poor. Skylar12
A similar study a decade laterlO reports that the $18 billion in annual
found that the number of nonreaders revenues are more than radio and
appeared to be increasing, but their television combined. Yet newspaper
demographic characteristics were es- readership has declined while magazine
sentially the same. This replication and paperback book sales soar.
study showed income and education One answer to this may be found in
were important variables in describing work done on readability. Fowler and
the newspaper nonreader. Smith13 found that over time magazines
Poindexterll went beyond simple appear to be easier to read than news-
demographic data analysis of the news- papers. There may be a connection
paper nonreader to answer: "Why between the ability to read comfortably,
don't nonreaders read newspapers?" socialization, motivation and the
Lack of time, use of another medium, significance of education as a predictor
cost and lack of interest apparently all of nonreading.
were important in the decisions. A list The decision to read or not read a
of 15 avoidance items was reduced to newspaper is just one decision of many
five factors: 1) newspaper content; 2) about mass media use. We need to
use of other media; 3) poor eyesight; 4) know more about whether avoiding
bias; and 5) lack of time. one media leads to use of another.
Poindexter's study identified two Previous avoidance research fails to
groups of nonreaders: typical (young explain adequately the forces behind
or old, poor and under-educated) and reading or not reading the newspaper.
Jeremy H. Lipschultz/Cioser Look at Avoiding/61

Exploratory work on the more general one on the day questioned.


issue of mass media avoidance would 2) Casual readers said they usually
be helpful, especially since other media read a newspaper but had not read one
have been shown to be significant in the day questioned.
the avoidance of the newspaper. 3) Regular readers said they usually
While Becker found avoidances are read a newspaper and had read one the
different from positive gratifications, it day they were questioned.
is hypothesized here that within a list of Twelve avoidance statements were
avoidances there will be clusters of rated by the group in the sample. A
differing motivations. In particular, three-point scale of agreement was
these differences may be related to used to determine whether the reason
levels of interest. For instance, applied (3 = a lot, 2 = somewhat, or 1
Edelstein and Larsen14 noted variations =not at all). The questions were read:
in reader intensity. Some readers were "Here are some reasons people have
grouped and called "fans," regular,
given for not reading newspapers or
moderate or casual in a typology; for not reading very often. Do they
This study examines 12 avoidance
apply a lot, somewhat, or not at all?
related characteristics in the decision to
One reason people have given is... "
not read a newspaper. The focus is on
attitll.dinal rather than demographic The reasons were then listed.
data. It is hoped some perspective can - It takes too much time to read a
be brought to the avoidance gratifica- newspaper ·regularly.
tion paradigm that will suggest sophis- - Newspapers don't print much of
ticated questions in future research. interest to me.
- Newspapers cost more than they
Methodology
are worth to me.
Data for the study were collected as - Newspaper stories are too one-
part ·of a more general newspaper sided and biased.
readership survey in Carbondale, Ill., -You cannot trust what you read in
in 1985. A total of 408 persons were most n~wspapers.
questioned by phone about their news- - Newspapers have too much
paper usage. IS advertising.
Almost 15% of the sample identi- - Newspapers generally are poorly
fied themselves as not usually reading a written.
newspaper. When asked if they had - Most stories in newspapers are
read a newspaper today, nonreaders difficult to read
jumped to 43%.16 Three groups were - There is not much in newspapers
developed from the two sets of res- that is useful to me in my daily life.
ponses (nonreaders, casual readers and - There is too much detail in most
regular readers). newspaper stories.
1) Nonreaders said they usually do - By the time I see a story in a
not read a newspaper and had not read newspaper, I've already heard about it
62/Newspaper Research Journal

TABLE 1: Mean Scores for Nonreaders and Readers

Nonreader Casual Usual Total p


(n=55) (n=ll9) (n=229)
Item
Radio-TV 2.44 1.90 1.99 2.02 .001
Cost 2.09 1.47 1.49 1.56 .001
Useful 2.00 1.34 1.35 1.44 .001
Time 1.84 1.61 1.32 1.48 .001
Interest 1.84 1.38 1.34 1.42 .001
Trust 1.83 1.47 1.49 1.53 .001
Ads 1.76 1.55 1.61 1.61 n.s.
Attractive* 1.76 1.50 1.51 1.54 .03
Bias 1.72 1.62 1.65 1.65 n.s.
Details 1.50 1.29 1.22 1.28 .01
Writing 1.45 1.50 1.42 1.44 n.s.
Reading 1.31 1.19 1.18 1.20 n.s.

*In all cases but the variable "atlractive," a significant difference indicates there were
two signfiicant paris of differences. The Scheffe procedure in ANOVA was used.

from radio or television. toward strong agreement by nonreaders


- Most newspapers are not very as a strongly held attitude.
attractive or easy to look at On six of the avoidance items,
Analysis in this report is segmented nonreaders were significantly different
into three stages. First, mean score from both groups of readers.
ratings are compared between readers Nonreaders were more likely to cite use
and nonreaders on the avoidance of radio-TV, cost, usefulness, interest,
statements. Second, demographics of trust and the amount of detail in the
the subgroups are considered. Third, newspaper as reasons for not reading a
factor structures are developed. newspaper.
On only one item, time ("It takes
Results too much time to read a newspaper
Analysis of variance was used to regularly"), casual readers were
test for significant differences between significantly different from the usual
readers and nonreaders on the avoid- reader. The casual reader rated the
ance items. Nonreaders gave signifi- avoidance statement with a significantly
cantly higher levels of agreement, higher level of agreement
indicative of a theoretically distinct On only one item, attractive ("Most
group. Yet only the response "already newspapers are not very attractive or
heard on radio or television" leaned easy to look at.", nonreaders were
Jeremy H. Lipschultz/Closer Look at Avoiding/63

significantly different from regular reading that newspaper. Fowler17


readers, but not from the casual reader. found that the newspaper reading habit
So there is another aspect to the may adjust over time to such a change.
avoidance question. Avoidance, in Overall, interest in the content of the
some cases, may exist for readers, newspaper was a major response in
whether they are casual or regular. this phase of the questioning. How-
In this study if a respondent ever, it is important to note ,that these
reported not reading one of the two results may be specific to the role of a
local newspapers (the Southern campus paper in a college town. Per-
Illinoisan or the Daily Egyptian), then a haps the responses to the open-ended
open-ended question was asked: "Is questions would be most useful in
there any special reason you do not designing future avoidance statements.
read the ?" Nonreader
Demographics
answers were quite different for each
of the two papers. More than 200 Past research has dealt extensively
responses were obtained, with multiple with the demographic differences be-
responses included in the descriptive tween readers and nonreaders of
analysis, (see Table 2). newspapers. If has been reported that
Local factors may play a role in nonreaders tend to be very young or
"avoiding the newspaper." In the case old, of lower income levels, from rural
of the student-operated newspaper, for areas and with less formal education.
example, distribution free of charge In the present study gender, urban/
eliminates cost as a factor. But, limited rural status, employment status and
off-campus distribution makes avail- marital status were DQt significant in
ability most important. describing differences.
In the case of the Southern Illi- Education was a significant consid-
noisan a recent change from afternoon eration and age provided limited expla-
to morning publication was mentioned natory power. Nonreaders tended to
by some people as a reason for not have a high school education or less,

TABLE 2: Reasons for Not Reading Local Newspapers

Southern Illinoisan Daily Egyptian


(n=128) (n=l33)

Content 24.2% Availability 37.6%


Cost 22.7 Interest 34.6
Read other 18.7 Chance only 13.5
Morning change 3.9 Time 10.5
Availability 3.1 Other response 3.8
Other response 55
64/Newspaper Research Journal

TABLE 3: Years Formal Education for Readers and Nonreaders

0-12 13-Plus
Years Years
(n=l35) (n=268)
Groupsa

Nonreaders (n=55) 655% 34.5%

Casual Readers (n=l19)b 26.1 73.9

Readers (n=229) 29.7 70.3

ax2 =30.65; df =5; p < .01


bCasual readers are not significantly different from usual readers.

and were less likely to have a higher group less likely to be in the older
education of any kind. break. Casual readers tended to be
The data on age are helpful in younger.
making some descriptive distinctions In this study, age differences might
between readers and nonreaders of be explained by relocation of younger
newspapers. Unlike data reported in people to a relatively isolated college
the past, the nonreader was not very town. Stamm18 considered the rela-
young or old, although the sum of tionship between community ties and
nonreaders and casual readers yields a readership. And since other studies

TABLE 4: Respondents in Age Classifications

Oto25 26-56 57-plus


(n=l27) (n=l72) (n=l04)
Groups'l
Nonreaders (n=55) 30.9% 38.2% 30.9%

Casual Readers (n=119) b 40.3 47.9 11.8

Readers (n=229) 27.1 41.0 31.9

ax2 = 18.46; 8 df; p <.05


bAge differences are the strongest support in the present study for
distinguishing "casual" readers.
Jeremy H. LipschulbiCioser Look at Avoiding/65

have found length of residence and study, a factor analysis of the variables
anticipated length of stay in a locale are used should help direct future resear-
significant readership predictors, age chers design avoidance statements
might be an artifact of the Carbondale along theoretical dimensions. This
market. should aid in hypothesis testing.
Yet the results of this investigation Factor analysisl9 was used to con-
suggest further clarification of the "non- struct cognitive patterns among avoid-
reader" and "casual reader" is needed. ance items for the three readership
groups. In the Poindexter study pre-
Factor Structures
viously mentioned, a five-factor solu-
In line with the goals of the present tion was developed for nonreaders (con-

TABLE 5: Factor Analysis of Avoidance Statements by Groups

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3


Groups:
Nonreaders*
Interest .65 .06 .35
Cost .55 .26 .21
Bias .30 .00 .75
Trust .51 .34 .79
Writing .29 .22 .62
Reading .25 .95 .21
Useful .89 .30 .45
Detail .19 .59 .19
Radio-TV .58 .10 .31
Attractive .34 .31 .48

Readers**
Interest .61 .34 .39
Cost .55 .27 .26
Bias .36 .61 .31
Trust .34 .75 .33
Writing .30 .42 .52
Reading .37 .31 .68
Useful .75 .27 .41
Detail .47 .17 .51
Radio-TV .33 .16 .22
Attractive .45 .21 .48

*The nonreaders solution accounts for 64% of the total variance. Within the solution,
factor 1 accounts for 61%,;factor 2, 22%; and factor 3, 17%.
••The readers solution accounts for 55% of the total variance. Within the solution,
factor 1 accounts for 73%; factor 2, 16% and factor 3, 11%.
66/Newspaper Research Journal

tent, media, eyesight, bias and time).


Discussion
The present study's avoidance items
were analyzed for structure. Two Newspaper readers and nonreaders
items, time and advertising, did not share many perceptions about why they
correlate with any other item and were avoid reading. Yet we can also see that
not included in the secondary analysis. they are theoretically distinct.
Casual and regular readers were com- There may be subtle differences
bined for the factor analysis.20 between the regular and casual reader,
Analysis of casual readers was not although a different methodological
helpful and suggests that a more clearly design would be required to explain
defined concept is needed. 21 The such subtle differences. A more
theoretical "casual reader" simply did sensitive instrument might suggest
not produce distinct data for purposes some of those in the casual reader
of interpretation. However, the state- group properly belong with readers,
ments used in the study may not have while others may properly group with
been sensitive to the potential group nonreaders.
differences. Beyond the significant differences
Nonreaders, similar to readers, dis- in amounts of formal education and
played a clear utility factor which in- age, demographics were not very use-
cluded interest in stories, cost of the ful in highlighting the differences
publication, usefulness of the infor- between readers and nonreaders.
mation and timeliness relative to radio It is possible that some of the signi-
and television reports. ficant differences described in past
A second clustering for the non- work may be impacted by demographic
reader developed around difficulty of changes over time. Lifestyles have
news stories and detail of information. also changed during the last three
The credibility questions of bias and decades. Television news viewing,
trust were lumped with poor news- fewer afternoon newspapers and
paper writing and attractiveness of the changes to morning publication may be
paper. important in understanding nonreaders.
However, for readers the divisions In the context of Stamm's obser-
were clear. A factor clustered about vations on community and com-
credibility, with bias of news stories munication, mobility and ties to place
and trust of information included. may be important. Length of stay in a
Another factor for readers was community and anticipated longevity
evaluative in nature including difficulty can be used as variables to predict
of news stories to read, poor writing, media use.
detail of stories and attractiveness. Attitude studies, such as the present
This suggests readers as a group effort, operate at the individual level of
exhibit a more consistent pattern of analysis. Yet many of the answers
responses to the sample of avoidance being sought are ultimately societal in
statements in this study. nature. And the lack of data over long
Jeremy H. Lipschultz/Closer Look at Avoiding/67

periods of time limit the ability to draw involvement, accounted for 16% of the
causal inferences. sample. "Partial readers," average on
Factor structures suggest that exposure and involvement, accounted
different patterns of cognition exist for for 33% of the sample. This group
readers and nonreaders. Both groups was distinguished from regular readers
share common but not identical (40%) by high levels of exposure, yet
structures on utility items. Psycho- only average involvement. And "fans"
logical data might help explain the (11%) were high on both involvement
motivations behind reading or not and exposure. This typology should
reading the newspaper. The factor be used to test a list of avoidance
structures here suggest perceptions of statements.
newspapers by nonreaders are not as The conceptualization of a "casual"
clear as those by readers. (partial?) reader poses other problems.
Past conceptualization of the One is a measurement problem of
"atypical" nonreader is not clear, and determining how to quantify news-
more work needs to be done to define paper readership. Number of days per
and refine the group. As Becker has week of reading, number of papers
said, avoidance gratifications appear to read, number of stories read, type of
be neither mirror-opposites nor separ- stories read, recall ability and long-term
ate factors. memory of content all may be useful in
When respondents agree, "It takes developing stronger measurement
too much time to read a newspaper," tools.
are they indicating a function of their There might also be a group of
activity or are they really saying there is "heavy" readers (fans?) displaying
nothing in the paper worth their time different patterns from regular readers.
relative to other activities? The answer It is clear that simple lise of "reader"
to this question is important for under- compared to "nonreader" is not ade-
standing differences between casual quate for future research on avoidance.
and regular readers, if there are any c1arification of the meaning of
differences. "avoidances" would also be helpful.
Admittedly, one of the weaknesses Factors, such as utility, credibility and
in this type of research has been the physical description of newspapers
problem of clearly defining the should be applied to design research to
concepts of "readers" and "nonread- probe more deeply.
ers." It may be more a case of levels of It is difficult to imagine uses and
media use which could be discovered gratifications research progressing
through more intensive questioning. substantially without a more developed
J anowitz,22 for example, as early as conceptualization of avoidance grati-
the 1950s developed a typology using fication.
levels of exposure and involvement to It seems clear that the area of
· develop a readership index. Non- avoidance is worth more attention by
readers, low on both exposure and scholars than has been given pre-
68/Newspaper Research Journal

viously. More sophisticated rese!lfch use in general." This should provide


might well yield a better understanding greater insight into why people read or
of the motivations - both reward and do not read newspapers.
punishment - involved in mass media

NOTES

1. Jay G. Blumler and Elihu Katz Quarterly, (1964), 41:45-50, 156.


(eds.), The Uses of Mass Com- 10. Jeanne Penrose, David H. Weaver,
munications: Current Perspectives on Richard R. Cole and Donald L. Shaw,
Gratifications Research, (Beverly Hills: "The Newspaper Non-Reader 10 Years
SAGE, 1974). Later: A Partial Replication ofWestley-
2. Lee Becker, "Measurement of Severin," Journalism Quarterly,
Gratifications," Communication Re- (1974), 51:631-38.
search, (1979), 6:54-73. 11. Paula Poindexter, "Daily News-
3. Philip Palmgreen and J.D. Rayburn paper Non-Readers: Why they don't
II, "Merging Uses and Gratifications read?" Jow·nalism Quarterly, (1979),
and Expectancy-Value Theory," Com- 54:764-70.
munication Research, (1984 ), 11:537- 12. David Skylar, "Why Newspapers
62. Die," The Quill, (July-August 1984),
4. Lawrence Wenner, "Gratifications 12-16.
Sought and Obtained in Program 13. Gilbert L. Fowler and Edward J.
Dependency: A Study of Network Smith, "Readability of Newspapers
Evening News Programs and 60 and Magazines Over Time, Newspaper
Minutes," Communication Research, Research Journal, (1979), 1:3-8.
(1983), 9:539-60. 14. Alex Edelstein and Otto Larsen,
5. Jack M. McLeod, Carl R. Bybee "The Weekly Press' Contribution To a
and Jean A. Durall, "Evaluating Media Sense of Urban Community," Jour-
Performance by Gratifications Sought nalism Quarterly, (1960), 37:489-98.
and Received," Journalism Quarterly, 15. Data were collected by a group of
(1982), 59:3-12, 59. graduate students in a research methods
6. M. Fishbein, "An investigation of course at Southern Illinois University
the relationships between beliefs about under the direction of Dr. L. Erwin
an object and the attitude toward that Atwood. The data on avoidances in
object," Human Relations, (1963), this study are a small part of a broader
16:233-40. survey on newspaper uses in the
7. V~H. Vroom, Work and Motivation, Carbondale area.
(New York: John Wiley, 1964). 16. All calling was done during the
8. David L. Swanson, "The Continu- after dinner hours. Use of "today" was
ing Evolution of the Uses and not expected to cause a problem since
Gratifications Approach," Commun- both local newspapers are morning
icationResearch, (1979), 6:3-7. publications.
9. Bruce H. Westley and Werner J. 17. Gilbert L. Fowler, "An Exam-
Severin, "A Profile of the Daily ination of Readership Changes: Does
Newspaper Non-Reader," Journalism Altering Publication Time Affect the
Jeremy H. Lipschultz/Cioser Look at Avoiding/69

Reading Habit?" Newspaper Research with the larger group of readers could a
Journal, (1985), 7:37-44. Unfortun- comparative analysis be conducted.
ately, no data were collected on the The results do not provide insight into
change in Carbondale. whether there are differences between
18. Keith R. Stamm, Newspaper Use readers with different habits of reading.
and Community Ties, (Norwood, NJ: 21. Analysis of casual readers yielded a
Ablex, 1985). one-factor solution based upon an
19. A rotated oblique solution was Eigenvalue of 1.00 or greater. The
obtained using SPSS. Correlations variables of usefulness, interest, details
between factors one and two were - and cost explained 57% of the var-
.47, one and three .48, and two and iance.
three .42. The results were not statis- 22. Morris Janowitz, The Community
tically significant. The solution Press in an Urban Setting, (Chicago:
provided is orthogonal in nature. University of Chicago Press, 1952,
20. Only by combining casual readers 1967).

Вам также может понравиться