Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 98

V O L U ME 2 , ISS U E 10 OC T O BE R 2 0 1 8

MASS
M ONTHLY A PPL ICATIO N S IN
STRE N G TH SPO R T

E R IC H E LMS | G R E G NUC KOLS | MIC HAEL ZO URDO S


The Reviewers
Eric Helms
Eric Helms is a coach, athlete, author, and educator. He is a coach for drug-free strength and
physique competitors at all levels as a part of team 3D Muscle Journey. Eric regularly publishes
peer-reviewed articles in exercise science and nutrition journals on physique and strength sport, in
addition to writing for commercial fitness publications. He’s taught undergraduate- and graduate-
level nutrition and exercise science and speaks internationally at academic and commercial
conferences. He has a B.S. in fitness and wellness, an M.S. in exercise science, a second Master’s
in sports nutrition, a Ph.D. in strength and conditioning, and is a research fellow for the Sports
Performance Research Institute New Zealand at Auckland University of Technology. Eric earned pro status as a natural
bodybuilder with the PNBA in 2011 and competes in the IPF at international-level events as an unequipped powerlifter.

Greg Nuckols
Greg Nuckols has over a decade of experience under the bar and a B.S. in exercise and sports
science. Greg is currently enrolled in the exercise science M.A. program at the University of North
Carolina at Chapel Hill. He’s held three all-time world records in powerlifting in the 220lb and
242lb classes. He’s trained hundreds of athletes and regular folks, both online and in-person.
He’s written for many of the major magazines and websites in the fitness industry, including Men’s
Health, Men’s Fitness, Muscle & Fitness, Bodybuilding.com, T-Nation, and Schwarzenegger.com.
Furthermore, he’s had the opportunity to work with and learn from numerous record holders,
champion athletes, and collegiate and professional strength and conditioning coaches through his previous job as Chief
Content Director for Juggernaut Training Systems and current full-time work on StrongerByScience.com.

Michael C. Zourdos
Michael (Mike) C. Zourdos, Ph.D, CSCS, is an associate professor in exercise science at Florida
Atlantic University (FAU) in Boca Raton, FL., USA, with a specialization in strength and conditioning
and skeletal muscle physiology.  He earned his Ph.D. in exercise physiology from The Florida State
University (FSU) in 2012 under the guidance of Dr. Jeong-Su Kim. Prior to attending FSU, Mike
received his B.S. in exercise science from Marietta College and M.S. in applied health physiology
from Salisbury University. Mike served as the head powerlifting coach of FSU’s 2011 and 2012
state championship teams. As an associate professor at FAU, Mike is the director of the FAU
Muscle Physiology Research Laboratory. He also competes as a powerlifter in the USAPL, and
among his best competition lifts is a 230kg (507lbs) raw squat at a body weight of 76kg. Mike
owns the company Training Revolution, LLC., where he has coached more than 100 lifters, including a USAPL open
division national champion.

2
Letter from
the Reviewers

W
elcome to Volume 2, Issue 10 of Monthly Applications in Strength Sport. It’s been an
amazing 2018 for MASS, and we intend to keep that trend going for the last few issues
of the year.
Over the past month, a recent study published in Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise (one
of the best journals in our field) that examined different set-volumes on hypertrophy and strength
outcomes has already garnered significant chatter in the online community. Eric has thoroughly bro-
ken down the study’s findings and provided an unbiased analysis of the online chatter and criticisms.
We think this will clarify some points you may have seen. For his other written effort, Eric tackles
the concept of caffeine as an appetite suppressant.
We’ve had our interpreting research guide on the member’s site since Day 1 of MASS; this month,
Greg significantly adds to your research understanding with a concept review titled “When to Trust
the Results of a Study.” This one-of-a-kind article breaks down the potential flaws, biases, and issues
you may see in a study to aid your ability in pinpointing these factors when reading original research.
We wish we could tell you to believe everything on PubMed; however, as this article points out, that
isn’t quite the case.
Mike returns to the concept of daily 1RM training, but this time, a new study has compared max-
ing just a few times per week versus typical periodization training in powerlifters preparing for a
competition. The results were equivocal, but recommendations are provided for how you can use the
data. Further, Mike analyzes a mechanistic study that examines the relationship between how fast
a muscle fiber can transmit an action potential and how quickly a lifter can reach peak rate of force
development.
In the video content, Mike’s video overlaps with his mechanistic article, as it lays out some prac-
tical strategies to take advantage of the findings from that article (or, in other words, improve “neu-
romuscular efficiency”). Eric’s video examines approaches to run specialization cycles for a particular
muscle group. Of great benefit, Eric lays out what exercises he would count toward a muscle group
(i.e. rows count for biceps) when running a specialization.
It continues to be our honor to work on MASS each month. Please touch base in the Facebook
group with any questions or comments, and give the audio roundtables a listen.
We hope you enjoy, and we’re already working away on the November issue.

The MASS Team


Eric, Greg, and Mike

3
Table of Contents

6
BY G R EG NUCKOL S

When To Trust the Results of a Study


It’s becoming clear that a lot of published research is unreplicable and untrustworthy.
How do incorrect findings occur, and how can we predict whether the results of a
particular study are likely to be sound?

24
BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS

Neuromuscular Efficiency and How it Can Help Your Strength


We often think that we know things even when data haven’t shown them to be true yet.
It’s a treat when we review a study that confirms our long-held suspicions. This study
demonstrates exactly what neuromuscular efficiency is.

34
BY E RI C HEL MS

Understanding Volume
A recent study reported that very high volumes were associated with the greatest
hypertrophy when compared to lower volumes. Before you conclude that this
changes everything and double your sets, read this first.

53
BY G R EG NUCKOL S

Log Press is a Grind, Even for the Experts


There haven’t been many studies on overhead pressing exercises. A recent study
compared barbell push press to log push press in competitive strongmen, finding
that log press allows you to use the same loads, but with slower speeds.

4
61
BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS

New Data Makes Cases Both For and Against 1RM-Type Training
Data have shown that maxing out every day or just a few times a week substantially
boosts strength, but does it work as well as typical periodized training? This study
compares just maxing a few times per week to periodized training in powerlifters
preparing for a competition.

73
BY E RI C HEL MS

Is Caffeine an Effective Appetite Suppressant?


It’s common for caffeine to be included in fat-loss supplements as an appetite
suppressant, but does it actually work?

81
BY G R EG NUCKOL S

Can You Build More Size and Strength with Overloaded Eccentrics?
You can produce more force eccentrically than concentrically, so it stands to reason
that you’ll get better results by handling heavier eccentric loads, right? A recent study
puts this idea to the test.

92
BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS

VIDEO: Strategies to Improve Neuromuscular Efficiency


Rate of force development can be a limiting factor of max strength, but specifics of
training this process are rarely explained. This video provides practical strategies to
improve your neuromuscular efficiency.

94
BY E RI C HEL MS

VIDEO: Muscle Group Specialization Cycles


While volume should only be increased if plateaued and if all other variables are
optimal, there may come a time when higher volumes for hypertrophy are necessary.
One way to increase volume without digging too deep of a recovery hole is the use
of muscle group specialization cycles.

5
Concept Review

When To Trust the


Results of a Study
BY G RE G NUC KO LS

It’s becoming clear that a lot of published research is unreplicable and


untrustworthy. How do incorrect findings occur, and how can we predict
whether the results of a particular study are likely to be sound?

6
H
ere at MASS, we’re mostly in
the business of breaking down What’s a concept review?
individual studies. We also do A written concept review is similar
concept reviews, and we attempt to con- to our signature video reviews. The
textualize each study based on the rest of aim of this article type is to review a
the literature, but reviews of individual cornerstone topic in physiology or
studies are our bread and butter. So, it applied science research.
matters to us that our analysis and inter-
pretation of each study is as accurate as
possible. Often, if an experiment doesn’t turn up
However, that presents us with a prob- significant results, a scientist won’t even
lem: Many published studies reach in- bother submitting it for publication, ei-
correct conclusions. By one famous es- ther because they view the experiment
timation (1), most published research as a failure (even though null results are
findings are false, and in projects de- important results too!), or because they
signed to directly replicate landmark know they’ll deal with multiple rounds
studies, replication rates of positive find- of submissions, revisions, reformatting,
ings are often below 50% (2, 3, 4). further submissions, further revisions,
further reformatting, etc., before even-
In this article, I want to explain why tually landing the study in a low-impact
incorrect research findings are so com- journal that probably won’t have much
mon, and then discuss criteria you can of an effect on their career prospects.
use to predict the likelihood that the Trying to get a non-significant result
findings of a paper are accurate. published can just seem like a huge un-
There are several reasons why incorrect dertaking with very little payoff.
results frequently get published. More often, however, publication bias
is driven by the journals rejecting per-
fectly good science that just didn’t hap-
Publication Bias pen to get statistically significant results.
I think publication bias is the primary Publication bias is insidious due to
reason why the literature contains a dis- sheer probability. If all research is per-
proportionate amount of incorrect find- formed in good faith (i.e. there’s no
ings. Journals are much more likely to shady data analysis going on), a cer-
publish “statistically significant” results tain percentage of study results will be
than non-significant results (5), because false positives purely due to chance. If
significant results are often seen as sexier all true positives and all false positives
and more exciting. get published, while most true nega-

7

Journals are much more likely to
publish “statistically significant”
results than non-significant results.

tives and most false negatives wind up factors than is warranted).


in a file drawer, the rate of published false It’s worth noting that publication bias
positive results will be much higher than doesn’t always work in favor of significant
the rate of actual false positive results. I findings. It works in favor of “exciting”
made a spreadsheet that helps illustrate findings, which are usually significant
this point; you can find the spreadsheet findings. However, a null result that runs
and instructions for how to use it here. counter to some well-supported ortho-
As I mentioned, I think publication doxy may also be viewed as exciting, and
bias is the primary reason why the scien- thus be likely to get published. For ex-
tific literature is littered with incorrect, ample, you’d expect lower energy intake
non-replicable research findings. This is to lead to greater weight loss, if all other
not primarily the fault of the scientists variables are controlled. If a metabolic
– it’s a matter of journals and publishers ward study found no significant differ-
responding to market incentives. Posi- ence in weight loss with two dramatical-
tive results are exciting, exciting results ly different levels of energy intake, that
get cited, citations drive impact factors, would be a very exciting null finding
and impact factors drive subscriptions and would have absolutely no problem
and revenue. Thus, publication bias is getting published (assuming the study’s
primarily driven by journals, but is also methodology wasn’t horrible). For the
driven by university press departments most part, though, “exciting” findings
(who hype significant findings more tend to be significant results.
than non-significant findings), lay-press
science writers (who create awareness
for exciting, significant findings), fund- P-hacking
ing agencies (who also like to see excit- A second factor contributing to the
ing, significant findings before opening publication of incorrect research find-
their wallets to fund big projects build- ings is p-hacking. P-hacking is probably
ing on that research), and the scientific at least somewhat motivated by publica-
community as a whole (which still plac- tion bias, but while journals bear most
es way more weight on journal impact of the responsibility for publication bias,

8
scientists bear most of the responsibility
for p-hacking.
P-hacking describes a wide variety of P-HACKING DESCRIBES A
tools and approaches for finding “statis-
tically significant” results in a dataset af- WIDE VARIETY OF TOOLS
ter you fail to find the significant effect
you were actually looking for. This can AND APPROACHES FOR
be accomplished several different ways. FINDING “STATISTICALLY
One common method of p-hacking
(or ensuring you have a dataset you can SIGNIFICANT” RESULTS IN
p-hack) is simply to collect a load of
outcome variables: the more variables A DATASET AFTER YOU FAIL
you have to analyze, the greater your
chances of having at least one false posi-
TO FIND THE SIGNIFICANT
tive. For example, in this famous “sting,”
researchers intentionally set up a study
EFFECT YOU WERE
to demonstrate the ease of p-hacking ACTUALLY LOOKING FOR.
by testing the effects of chocolate con-
sumption on a whole host of measures
associated with health. Sure enough,
purely by chance, they got some “signif-
you only report the significant results
icant” results showing that chocolate led
and don’t use statistical procedures to
to weight loss and improved cholesterol
adjust your false positive risk, you can
levels. Their study got a ton of press (peo-
wind up with a lot of “significant” find-
ple like to hear that chocolate is good for
ings due to chance alone.
them) before the researchers confessed
to the game they were playing. Another method of p-hacking is the
use of sub-analyses. For example, if you
Now, simply collecting a ton of data
compare two training approaches in a
isn’t necessarily insidious. The issue is
large, heterogeneous sample, you may
with the way the data are reported and
not find any significant differences with
analyzed. If you have 30 outcome mea-
the full sample. However, you can then
sures, and you report the results for all
isolate the analysis to just men, or just
variables and use the correct statistical
women, or just older subjects, or just
procedures to adjust your false positive
younger subjects, or just subjects that
risk (making it harder to attain signifi-
trained in the morning, or just subjects
cance for any single variable), collecting
that ate pancakes for breakfast, or just
more data is a good thing. However, if

9
involves statistical shenanigans. There
are often multiple ways you can analyze
SOME P-HACKING IS a dataset. Some approaches are conser-
vative, with low risk of false positives but
CONDUCTED MALICIOUSLY, greater risk of failing to detect an actual
BUT I THINK MOST significant effect. Other approaches are
more liberal, detecting almost all actu-
INSTANCES ARE SIMPLY al significant effects, along with more
false positives. There are situations when
DUE TO PEOPLE NOT more conservative approaches are pref-
erable and situations when more liber-
KNOWING ANY BETTER. al approaches are preferable. However,
that’s a decision that should be made
before data analysis actually starts. If
a researcher decides to use a conserva-
Geminis, etc. If you have a dataset that
tive statistical approach when planning
allows for a lot of sub-analyses, some of
a study, doesn’t find significant results
them are likely to give you significant
once the data are collected, and switches
results due to chance alone, unless you
to a more liberal approach in order to
perform statistical adjustments to con-
find significance, that’s p-hacking.
trol your false positive risk. While sta-
tistical adjustments for multiple out- Finally, one very blunt way to p-hack is
come measures are common, statistical simply to drop the results of one or two
adjustments for multiple sub-analyses participants who are holding you back
are much less common. And again, au- from attaining statistical significance.
thors can also choose whether to report In a training study, the participants in
all sub-analyses they ran, or to just re- group A may have mostly made larger
port the ones that gave them significant strength gains than the participants in
results. The presence of sub-analyses group B, but one or two members of
doesn’t always indicate that p-hacking group B made huge gains, which keep
occurred – the authors may have set the results from being significant. If you
up the experiment with clear hypothe- had a predefined plan for dealing with
ses about the entire cohort, and about a outliers (and if these subjects meet some
particular subsection of the cohort – but objective criterion to qualify as outliers),
running a slew of sub-analyses is a com- then maybe you could legitimately re-
mon tool for squeezing false positives move their results. But if you didn’t have
out of noisy datasets. a predefined plan for dealing with out-
liers, and you make the decision to toss
Another method of p-hacking simply

10

Additional noise from sloppy data
collection can dramatically increase the
odds of finding erroneous significant
effects in an underpowered study.

out their data after all the results are in, more mundane. However, sloppiness
that’s p-hacking. can also contribute to incorrect research
I think that some p-hacking is con- findings.
ducted maliciously, but I think most P-hacking and “creative” data analy-
instances are simply due to people not sis rely on finding false positives. When
knowing any better. When you get a data aren’t collected cleanly, that intro-
dataset, it’s fun to poke around and see duces more noise into the dataset. Espe-
what sort of relationships you can find. cially when statistical power is low, all it
If you don’t have a defined data analysis can take are a few erroneously low data
plan, you may not realize how much you points in one group and a few errone-
actually poked around, and how many ously high data points in another group
of the significant results you found are for a “significant” effect to materialize.
likely due to chance. However, regard- Low power is almost a fact of life in ex-
less of intent, p-hacking increases the ercise science, but low power shouldn’t
number of incorrect research findings, increase false positive risk if the data are
compounding the effects of publication collected cleanly (in fact, low power has
bias. the opposite effect, decreasing your odds
of detecting true positive effects). How-
ever, additional noise from sloppy data
General Sloppiness collection can dramatically increase the
Publication bias and p-hacking are ex- odds of finding erroneous significant ef-
citing (at least to nerds like me) because fects in an underpowered study.
they represent systemic problems with- Another domain where sloppiness can
in the entire system and may even arise rear its head is in the planning phase of a
from malicious intent in some cases. study. Some studies simply use data from
Sloppiness, on the other hand, is much assessments that are inadequate for an-

11
swering their research questions, or use
statistical methods that are improper for
the data collected. Both of those issues
could be avoided by better attention to
IF YOU DON’T HAVE A
detail when designing the experiment. CLEARLY DEFINED ENDPOINT,
In a perfect world, scientists would al-
ways consult with methodologists and AND YOU ANALYZE YOUR
statisticians when designing a research
project to make sure the design of the DATA AS YOU COLLECT IT,
study and the statistical analysis plan are
appropriate, but that rarely happens, at
YOU CAN INFLATE THE ODDS
least in our field. OF FINDING FALSE POSITIVES.
Data Peeking/Lack of
Clearly Defined Endpoint no significant findings.
When you run a study, you should Peeking at your data and analyzing
have a clearly defined endpoint. Typ- as you go can dramatically increase the
ically, that endpoint is defined by the risk of false positives, especially if your
number of subjects recruited. As the study doesn’t have a clearly defined
study rolls along, you may enter the data endpoint. If you planned to recruit 40
as you collect it, but you shouldn’t start subjects, instead of just worrying about
analyzing the data until data collection your risk of false positives once all data
is finished. However, if you don’t have a is collected, you also have to deal with
clearly defined endpoint, and you ana- the risk of false positives after 10 sub-
lyze your data as you collect it, you can jects have been through the study, after
inflate the odds of finding false posi- 11 subjects have been through the study,
tives. During the process of data collec- 12, 13, 14, etc. When you combine data
tion, you may just hit a random run of peeking with p-hacking and a dash of
subjects that all have results leaning in sloppiness, it would be incredibly un-
one direction. If you’re peeking at your likely that you wouldn’t end up with at
data as you collect it, you may notice least one false positive.
that your results have attained signif- This paper explains in more detail
icance, and stop data collection there. why this is a problem (6).
However, if you ran more subjects
through the study, those abnormal re-
sults would wash out, leaving you with HARKing
12
p-hacked your study to smithereens.
However, if you instead identify the sig-
HARKING IS PROBABLY nificant results, then craft a hypothesis to
make it look like the study was designed
THE MOST COMMON to investigate those specific variables,
IN CROSS-SECTIONAL and don’t report results for the measures
that didn’t yield significant results, no
AND EPIDEMIOLOGICAL one would know that you should have
statistically adjusted for the multitude
RESEARCH, WHICH of unreported outcome measures. On
the surface, your paper looks solid, but
OFTEN DEALS WITH in reality, you got away with statistical
murder, and there’s a pretty good chance
HUGE DATASETS AND A your results are false positives.
MULTITUDE OF VARIABLES. HARKing is probably the most com-
mon in cross-sectional and epidemiolog-
ical research, which often deals with huge
datasets and a multitude of variables.
HARKing (7) stands for “hypothesiz- For example, if you download the data
ing after the results are known.” In other from the National Health and Nutri-
words, instead of designing a study to tion Examination Survey (NHANES),
investigate a clear hypothesis, you run an it wouldn’t surprise me if you could find
experiment, see what significant results hundreds (or even thousands) of signif-
you can find, and write your manuscript icant effects. Once you have your result,
as if the study was designed to investi- you just need to come up with a reason
gate the specific variable(s) where the why you expected to find that result, use
outcome was significant. that made-up reason to justify a hypoth-
esis, and ignore the dozen analyses you
HARKing doesn’t exist in a vacuum.
ran that didn’t find significant results.
It’s primarily just a method to make
p-hacking more powerful and convinc-
ing. For example, if you run a study,
collect 30 variables and analyze them
Low Power
all with t-tests (instead of an ANO- The other issues primarily increase risk
VA, which would adjust the false posi- of false positive findings. However, low
tive risk), report all outcomes, and don’t statistical power is a double whammy –
clearly state a hypothesis in your paper, it increases the risk of false positives and
it’ll be clear to most people that you false negatives (8).

13

When there are mechanistic studies, you
can compare theory and outcome to see
if results of a study “make sense.”

Statistical power is the likelihood of p-hacking or HARKing probably counts


detecting a true effect if one exists. In as fraud (especially if you know what
our field, we’re supposed to aim for 80% you’re doing and aren’t simply engag-
power (detecting true effects 80% of the ing in those practices because you don’t
time), though actual power is likely much know any better), while wholesale data
lower. On the surface, this means that if manipulation or fabrication is much
you do everything right (you don’t peek more serious fraud. It’s impossible to
at your data, you don’t p-hack, etc.) but know how much research is truly fraud-
underpower your study, you’ll be more ulent, but my hunch is that it’s a fairly
likely to “miss” true, positive effects. small minority. However, it does hap-
However, if you combine low power pen, and it does contribute to the rate of
with unethical research practices, you incorrect research findings.
also inflate the chance of finding false ___________________________
positives. In a scenario where there’s
truly no effect, large sample sizes should
If you made it this far, congratulations!
converge on the “true” effect size of 0.
You now know more about creative ways
However, the observed effect in a sample
to mishandle data (and why they’re prob-
can fluctuate substantially before even-
lematic) than most researchers. While I
tually approaching zero. It’s a lot easier
consider myself an advocate for science,
to wind up with 8 atypical people in an
I think it’s important the people are also
experimental group than 50.
informed about its dark side. Unless
you know about the problems, you don’t
Fraud know how to protect yourself. Also note,
I’m certainly not saying that science is
Outright fraud is the most serious worthless; I recognize that there are se-
transgression on this list, but probably rious flaws in the way it’s often practiced,
the least common. Fraud exists on a but I also think it’s the best process we
continuum, such that aggressive enough have for discovering truths about the

14
world around us. The march of scientific
and technological progress over the past
300 years should be evidence enough
that, in spite of its flaws, the scientific
IF THE RESULTS OF A STUDY
process is ultimately effective. ARE SIMILAR TO THOSE OF
With that in mind, here are some
things to look out for when reading re- PREVIOUS STUDIES, YOU CAN
search that will help you judge the like-
lihood that the findings of an individual
HAVE MORE CONFIDENCE IN
paper are accurate. THE RESULTS. HOWEVER, IF
A STUDY REPORTS RESULTS
Biological Plausibility
One problem in our field is that we of-
THAT ARE MARKEDLY
ten rush to applied research before do- DIFFERENT FROM SIMILAR
ing mechanistic research. In a field like
medicine, the molecular effects of a drug RESEARCH, IT DESERVES
are generally thoroughly researched be-
fore testing the efficacy of that drug in MORE SKEPTICISM.
animals or humans. In exercise science,
we often investigate “does this work” be-
fore investigating “mechanistically, why
should we expect that this would work?” exactly what we see: For short-duration
However, when there are mechanis- and long-duration performance (which
tic studies, you can compare theory and likely aren’t limited by metabolic acido-
outcome to see if results of a study “make sis), beta-alanine doesn’t seem to have
sense.” For example, we know that be- much of an effect, while it does seem
ta-alanine increases muscle carnosine to improve moderate-duration perfor-
levels, and we know that carnosine is an mance (9). If a new study finds that be-
important biological buffer. Thus, before ta-alanine improves 800m run times,
running any experiments, we’d expect we know that’s a plausible and expected
that beta-alanine would boost perfor- outcome, since 800m running is at least
mance in situations when metabolic ac- partially limited by metabolic acidosis.
idosis would limit performance, but not However, if a new study finds that acute
in situations when metabolic acidosis beta-alanine supplementation increases
isn’t likely to limit performance. When 1RM deadlift strength, we’d have every
we compare theory to outcomes, that’s right to be skeptical, as there’s no clear

15
mechanistic reason to expect such a re- you’re really passionate about, it’s worth
sult. conducting your own literature search
If a paper is well-written (and if mech- to see how that study fits into the lit-
anistic research exists on the subject), it erature. Oftentimes, authors will discuss
should discuss possible mechanistic rea- a majority of the research findings that
sons for the observed results. If a paper agree with their results but fail to men-
doesn’t discuss possible mechanisms, or tion several studies that had different
if it notes that the results are the oppo- outcomes. And, of course, older studies
site of what would be expected based on won’t cite more recent research, and the
known mechanisms, then you have ev- weight of the evidence may have shifted
ery right to be a bit more skeptical of the between the time a study was published
results. and the time that you’re reading it.

Comparisons to Past Financial Incentives and


Research Funding Sources
If a study investigates a completely I’m not going to harp on financial in-
novel hypothesis, you won’t have any- centives and funding sources (10) too
thing to compare the results to. How- much, because they seem to be one of
ever, most studies investigate hypothe- the few things most people are already
ses that are similar to those of previous aware of when critically reading re-
research, while perhaps using a slightly search, and discussions of financial in-
different population or experimental centives or conflicts can often venture
setup. If the results of a study are similar into tinfoil-hat territory.
to those of previous studies, you can have However, the mere presence of a finan-
more confidence in the results. However, cial incentive (funding source or affilia-
if a study reports results that are mark- tion of the authors) doesn’t necessarily
edly different from similar research, it mean the results of a study are incorrect,
deserves more skepticism. If this occurs, and it certainly doesn’t mean the re-
a well-written paper will discuss possi- sults are fraudulent. Financial incentives
ble reasons for its differing results. If a probably primarily influence study de-
paper doesn’t discuss possible reasons sign (using a design that maximizes the
for its divergent results, or if the authors odds of a positive result), suppression of
seem to be grasping at straws, the results negative or null results (magnifying the
deserve even more skepticism. effect of publication bias), and interpre-
If a study tackles a research question tation (casting positive results in an even
more laudatory light). So, when reading a

16
Reported P-Values
BLINDING INCREASES I won’t belabor this point, because I
know it would likely make many people’s
THE TRUSTWORTHINESS eyes glaze over. However, most research
in our field determines statistical signif-
OF ANY STUDY, BUT IT’S icance based on p-value thresholds, and
ESPECIALLY IMPORTANT the most common threshold is p<0.05.
The issue with a threshold of p<0.05
FOR STUDIES WHERE THE is that it’s not too hard to p-hack your
way to a p-value of 0.049. However, it
AUTHORS HAVE CLEAR is hard to p-hack your way to a p-value
FINANCIAL INCENTIVES OR of 0.01, and even harder to p-hack your
way to a p-value of 0.001. If you see a lot
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST. of p-values in a paper between 0.04 and
0.05, that doesn’t necessarily mean the
authors p-hacked or HARKed their way
to significant results, but it is certainly
a cause for concern and skepticism. On
study with a financial incentive, pay extra
the other hand, it’s much more likely
close attention to the study protocol (i.e.
that very low p-values are indicative of
was the protocol specifically designed to
true positive results. A very low p-value
show an effect?), and read the interpreta-
doesn’t guarantee that a finding is legit,
tion with a bit more skepticism, but don’t
but it certainly increases the likelihood.
necessarily assume the results are incor-
rect. Additional skepticism is certainly If a paper doesn’t report p-values (i.e.
warranted, however. it just reports p<0.05, but not p=0.021),
but it does report sample sizes and t-val-
Finally, in studies where there’s a clear
ues, r-values, or F-ratios, you can use on-
financial incentive, pay attention to
line calculators to convert those number
blinding. If the person analyzing the data
to p-values.
isn’t blinded to the subjects’ group alloca-
tion, the risk of biased data collection and
biased analysis increases. Blinding in-
creases the trustworthiness of any study,
Effect Sizes
but it’s especially important for studies Effect sizes give you additional infor-
where the authors have clear financial in- mation that helps you determine the
centives or conflicts of interest. importance of a significant result. They
also help you ascertain the likelihood of

17

Statistically significant comparisons or
associations with moderate-to-large
effect sizes are more likely to be true
positives than comparisons or associations
with trivial-to-small effect sizes.

false positives in large datasets. icant comparisons or associations with


If a result is statistically significant but moderate-to-large effect sizes are more
only associated with a small or trivial ef- likely to be true positives than compari-
fect size, it may not actually be very im- sons or associations with trivial-to-small
portant for coaches or athletes, even if effect sizes, simply because greater sam-
the p-value is really low. In exercise sci- pling bias is required to produce erro-
ence, that doesn’t happen too often since neous large effects than erroneous small
most studies don’t use large samples (you effects.
generally need a medium or large effect
to achieve statistical significance if your
sample is small), but it’s at least worth Pre-Registration
keeping in mind. If you pre-register a study (11), you
Furthermore, in large samples, ef- tell the world exactly how you’re going
fect sizes help you determine wheth- to conduct the study and analyze the
er a significant finding is likely to be a data before initiating data collection. If
false positive. If a large study uses true a study is pre-registered, and the pub-
random sampling, then the data should lished methods and statistical analyses
be accurate and unbiased. Howev- of the study match the pre-registered
er, true random sampling is extremely methods and analyses, risk of p-hacking,
rare, meaning that large studies can still HARKing, etc., is dramatically reduced.
have biased samples. With hundreds or You can be much more confident in its
thousands of participants, almost every results.
comparison or association will be signif- However, if a study is pre-registered,
icant, simply because statistical power is and the methods or analyses differ from
so high. When you combine enormous the pre-registered account, you can be
statistical power with non-random sam- quite confident that some sort of she-
pling, you have a high risk of false posi- nanigans are afoot. This is pretty rare,
tive results. However, statistically signif- though, because the researchers know

18
and don’t use labor-intensive measures),
and some studies are colossal undertak-
IF A STUDY REPORTS ings. Specifically, in our field, training
studies are a HUGE pain in the ass, and
MULTIPLE BARELY can easily require 500+ man hours for all
SIGNIFICANT OUTCOME of the training visits.
I can’t pretend like I have data on this,
MEASURES, AND IT’S but I think that there’s a strong relation-
ship between ease of conducting a study
PRETTY OBVIOUS THAT and risk of publication bias.
THE RESEARCHERS HAD If you didn’t have to sink much time
into a study, and the study doesn’t turn
OTHER OUTCOME MEASURES up any significant results, it’s not a huge
loss if the study doesn’t get published.
THAT WEREN’T REPORTED, You may not even bother submitting
it, or you may just cut your losses after
THERE’S A PRETTY one or two rejections. If, on the oth-
GOOD CHANCE THAT THE er hand, you spent months of your life
and hundreds of hours carrying out a
STUDY WAS P-HACKED. study, you’re going to be willing to wade
through hell or high water to make sure
it gets published. If you don’t have any
significant results, it may not get accept-
that they’re leaving a paper trail that will ed in a high-impact journal, but you’re
expose them. going to keep submitting it until it finds
Pre-registration is great, and more a home.
people should do it.

Evidence of P-Hacking
Ease of Conducting a or HARKing
Study P-hacking and HARKing are dan-
This is a factor you wouldn’t consider gerous because, if someone knows what
unless you’ve conducted research. they’re doing, they can completely hide
Some studies don’t take much time or the fact that any chicanery took place.
effort to conduct (primarily cross-sec- However, many authors aren’t that slick.
tional studies that are easy to recruit for For example, if a study reports multi-

19
ple barely significant outcome measures,
and it’s pretty obvious that the research-
ers had other outcome measures that
weren’t reported, there’s a pretty good
SCIENTISTS GENERALLY
chance that the study was p-hacked. HAVE A PRETTY GOOD IDEA
To illustrate, if someone uses bar kinet-
ics for their outcome measures, and they OF WHICH STUDIES WILL
report significant differences in mean
power and peak velocity with p-values
REPLICATE AND WHICH ARE
between ~0.03 and 0.05, but they don’t PROBABLY FALSE POSITIVES
report any other measures of bar kinet-
ics, they probably p-hacked. If they re-
port mean power and peak velocity, you
know they also have data on peak power
and mean velocity. Since they have pow- post-hoc tests, pearson regression, spear-
er data (which has a force component), man regression, chi-squared tests, mul-
they also have data on mean and peak tiple regression, logistic regression, or
force. Since power and velocity both have random effects meta-analysis (the most
time components, they also have data on common procedures in our field), ask
impulse. Thus, we know that they have at a friend who knows about stats about
least seven outcome measures (impulse, whether the statistical procedures used
mean and peak power, mean and peak were valid.
velocity, and mean and peak force), but
they only reported two outcomes, and
both outcomes were barely significant. Prediction
If they reported all outcomes and used One interesting finding in recent years
correct statistical procedures to control is that scientists generally have a pretty
their false positive rate, their mean pow- good idea of which studies will replicate
er and peak velocity findings would not and which are probably false positives
have been significant anymore (12). (4, 13). Researchers set up betting mar-
A method of p-hacking that’s easier to kets, and scientists can place bets on the
spot (if you’re at all conversant in statis- studies they think are most likely to suc-
tics) is the use of non-standard, inappro- cessfully replicate. These betting markets
priate statistical procedures. If you’re not have done a surprisingly good job of
conversant in stats, and you see a study predicting which studies won’t replicate
use something other than t-tests, ANO- and which will.
VAs, ANCOVAs, Bonferroni or Tukey’s Obviously, you won’t have the time to

20

If the research is unblinded, consider
whether the measurements used in the study
allow room for the researchers to either
purposefully or inadvertently “tip the scales.”

set up a betting market for every new even if they’re trying to be as objective
study that’s published, but I think it can and impartial as possible. The choice of
be helpful to ask experts about the re- measurements can make a big difference.
sults of studies that seem fishy to you. For example, if strength is assessed via
If multiple experts aren’t surprised by a squat, the researchers may be slightly
the results, then there’s a decent chance more strict on depth requirements (ei-
the results are accurate. However, if re- ther intentionally or unintentionally) in
searchers in the field are also surprised at the group they don’t expect to do as well;
the result, or if they also think it seems on the other hand, if strength is assessed
fishy, there’s a decent chance that its re- via bench press, there’s little subjectivi-
sults are incorrect (either as a false posi- ty in observing whether the bar touched
tive or a false negative). the chest, and whether the lifter locked
Note: I’m not making an appeal to au- the rep out. When assessing body com-
thority. I’m not saying that expert opin- position, DEXA scans leave little room
ion about a study is inherently correct. for user error (assuming you’re enforcing
I’m saying that we have good evidence the same pre-testing guidelines for all
that experts are able to predict whether a subjects), while there’s more subjectivity
particular research finding will replicate when taking skinfold measurements us-
with a pretty high degree of accuracy, ing calipers.
meaning that if they think a particular If the research is unblinded, consider
finding is legit, that should rationally whether the measurements used in the
increase your perceived odds of it being study allow room for the researchers to
legit. either purposefully or inadvertently “tip
the scales.” If the measurements contain
less room for bias to influence the out-
Measures with Minimal come, you can be a bit more confident in
Room for Subjectivity the results. If the measurements contain
more room for bias to influence the out-
In unblinded research, there’s still come, you need to be a bit more wary.
room for researcher bias to skew results,

21
Conclusion
So, to wrap things up, the findings of
individual studies aren’t nearly as trust-
worthy as most people would like to be-
lieve. There are a variety of ways to wind
up with false positive results, and the
publishing system more heavily rewards
positive results, which means a dispro-
portionate amount of false positive re-
sults wind up in the literature. How-
ever, if you know what to look for, you
can roughly appraise individual studies
to determine the likelihood that their
results are incorrect. The simplest and
most straightforward things to look for
are large effects, small p-values (below
0.01, not 0.05), results that can be ex-
plained by known mechanisms, and re-
sults that jive with prior research. If you
want to get more sophisticated, you can
see whether the study was pre-registered,
look at financial incentives, determine if
the study would be time-consuming to
conduct, look for evidence of p-hacking
or HARKing, see whether the measure-
ments allow substantial room for bias,
and ask experts if they would predict
that the findings would replicate.
Ultimately, you can never be sure
about the accuracy of any single study,
but if you know how people can cheat
the system, and you know the hallmarks
of solid research findings, you’ll be able
to more accurately appraise and inter-
pret research.

22
References
1. Ioannidis JP. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005 Aug;2(8):e124.
2. Open Science Collaboration. PSYCHOLOGY. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological sci-
ence. Science. 2015 Aug 28;349(6251):aac4716.
3. Begley CG, Ellis LM. Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research. Nature.
2012 Mar 28;483(7391):531-3.
4. Camerer CF et al. Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science
between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behavior. 2018 Aug 27; 2:637-44.
5. Mlinarić A, Horvat M, Šupak Smolčić V. Dealing with the positive publication bias: Why you
should really publish your negative results. Biochem Med (Zagreb). 2017 Oct 15;27(3):030201.
6. Wood J, Freemantle N, King M, Nazareth I. Trap of trends to statistical significance: likelihood of
near significant P value becoming more significant with extra data. BMJ. 2014 Mar 31;348:g2215.
7. Kerr NL. HARKing: hypothesizing after the results are known. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 1998;2(3):196-
217.
8. Dumas-Mallet E, Button KS, Boraud T, Gonon F, Munafò MR. Low statistical power in biomedical
science: a review of three human research domains. R Soc Open Sci. 2017 Feb 1;4(2):160254.
9. Saunders B, Elliott-Sale K, Artioli GG, Swinton PA, Dolan E, Roschel H, Sale C, Gualano B.
β-alanine supplementation to improve exercise capacity and performance: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2017 Apr;51(8):658-669.
10. Lexchin J. Sponsorship bias in clinical research. Int J Risk Saf Med. 2012;24(4):233-42.
11. Nosek BA, Ebersole CR, DeHaven AC, Mellor DT. The preregistration revolution. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A. 2018 Mar 13;115(11):2600-2606.
12. Obviously, you need to be reasonable when judging whether it’s likely that variables were examined
but weren’t reported on. Using the example of a linear position transducer, you could also get data on
rate of force development over half a dozen time windows, eccentric as well as concentric measures
for force, power, velocity, and impulse, and some LPTs even tell you about bar path. You could also
subdivide all of these analyses by lift phase (for example, in the squat, you could also look at power,
velocity, and force up to the point of minimum bar velocity, and after the point of minimum bar
velocity). You could come up with 30 different outcome measures pretty easily, but most of them
wouldn’t be variables that most researchers would typically look at. Also keep the aim of the study
in mind. For example, a study designed to examine load-velocity profiles really only needs to report
velocity; the researchers have data on a ton of other variables, but velocity is the only variable that’s
relevant to their research question and is probably the only variable they actually analyzed.
13. Dreber A, Pfeiffer T, Almenberg J, Isaksson S, Wilson B, Chen Y, Nosek BA, Johannesson M. Using
prediction markets to estimate the reproducibility of scientific research. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2015 Dec 15;112(50):15343-7.

23
Study Reviewed: Higher Muscle Fiber Conduction Velocity and Early Rate of Torque
Development in Chronically Strength Trained Individuals. Del Vecchio et al. (2018)

Neuromuscular Efficiency and How it


Can Help Your Strength
BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S

We often think that we know things even when data haven’t shown them
to be true yet. It’s a treat when we review a study that confirms our long-
held suspicions. This study demonstrates exactly what neuromuscular
efficiency is.

24
KEY POINTS
1. This paper found for the first time that rate of torque development during a biceps
curl was related to muscle fiber conduction velocity.
2. In other words, this paper is stating that with an increased training status, individuals
are able to transmit action potentials more quickly. This translates into quicker
recruitment of motor units.
3. Subsequently, improvements in rate of torque (or force) development are a key
factor in maximizing strength. Overall, these data help to explain “neuromuscular
efficiency,” and this article discusses practical uses for these findings.

R
ecently, MASS reviewed a er conduction velocity or transmission
study demonstrating that pow- of an action potential along the mus-
er-type training works for some cle fiber should be related to increased
but not all lifters. Training to improve rate of force development. If an indi-
rate of force development (i.e. faster re- vidual has a higher fiber conduction
cruitment of motor units) maximizes velocity and can recruit motor units
strength gains in some lifters, but not more quickly, it might be possible to
others (2). Theoretically, rate of force improve our “neuromuscular efficien-
development is related to muscle fiber cy,” which is an important element in
conduction velocity, which is the rate at max strength. This study (1) compared
which an action potential is transmit- trained versus untrained individuals
ted. In brief, skeletal muscle is inner- and found that trained individuals had
vated by the somatic nervous system, greater fiber conduction velocity and
and an action potential (i.e. electrical faster motor unit recruitment than the
signal) is originally sent from the ner- untrained individuals. This may suggest
vous system to initiate the muscle con- that improving the rate of motor unit
traction process. Once the action po- recruitment during a heavy contraction
tential from the motor neuron reaches may be a trainable process. While these
the muscle fibers (at the neuromuscular findings are at the neuromuscular lev-
junction), it triggers another action po- el, this article will discuss how to make
tential in the muscle fiber itself, which training adjustments to optimize this
then must travel along the muscle fiber adaptation.
to trigger the processes which initiate
muscle contraction. Therefore, a fast-

25
Table 1 Subject characteristics
Training Age
Subjects Age (years) Body Mass (kg) Height (cm)
(years)
Trained (n=8) 22.2 ± 2.5 85.2 ± 8.3 181.2 ± 9.3 At least 3 years
Untrained (n=8) 23.4 ± 3.1 73.2 ± 7.5 177.3 ± 7.5 Untrained

Data are Mean ± SD


Subject characteristics from Del Vecchio et al. 2018 (1)

Purpose and Research Subjects and Methods


Questions Subjects
Purpose Eight trained and eight untrained
men participated in this study. Typical
The purpose of this study was to deter-
multi-joint exercises weren’t used in this
mine if muscle fiber conduction velocity is
study, so it’s difficult to know the exact
associated with greater rate of force devel-
training status, but the trained subjects
opment in trained versus untrained indi-
had to have been training at least three
viduals during a biceps curl.
times per week for at least three years.
Research Questions Also, seven subjects in the trained group
were athletes who had performed at the
1. Do trained individuals have greater national or international level in volley-
muscle fiber conduction velocity and ball, boxing, karate, or the javelin throw;
rate of force development than un- thus, it stands to reason they were well-
trained individuals? trained. Details of the subjects are in Ta-
2. Is rate of force development related ble 1.
to muscle fiber conduction velocity?
Protocol
Hypotheses Subjects came to the lab twice to com-
The authors hypothesized that great- plete the study. The first day was simply
er rate of force development would occur to familiarize the subjects with the exer-
early in the contraction in trained versus cise, which was contraction of the elbow
untrained individuals. The authors also flexors (i.e. a biceps curl) using only the
predicted that there would be a significant dominant arm on an isokinetic dyna-
relationship between fiber conduction ve- mometer. At this point, we should note
locity and rate of force development. that in the paper, the authors referred
26
Table 2 List of outcome measures

Outcome measure Methods of obtainment

Maximal torque The highest torque of the three maximal contractions


• Torque was determined using the explosive contractions
at increments of milliseconds (ms) as follows: T50, T100,
T150, T200
• Rate of torque development was then calculated at
Rate of torque development
T50, and for all time intervals: T50 – T100, T100 – T150, and
T150 – T200
• The above were calculated from the 5 (of the 8) explosive
contractions which has the greatest torque at T150
• EMG electrodes were on the muscle for the
explosive contractions
• A validated algorithm was used to extrapolate
conduction velocity
• Conduction velocity was calculated using the average of
the 5 best explosive contractions
• Max conduction velocity was calculated, as well as
velocity conduction velocity during the 0-50, 50-100, 100-150,
and 150-200 ms time intervals
• Conduction velocity was also calculated during the
electromechanical delay, which is the time between
the onset of muscle activation and actual muscle
force production.

Outcome measures assessed in Del Vecchio et al. 2018 (1)

to rate of force development as rate of jects performed maximal contractions


torque development because a biceps and explosive contractions. First, to test
curl uses angular motion. Torque is es- max strength, the maximal voluntary
sentially the rotating force. Thus, we will contractions were performed with one
use rate of torque development for the minute between contractions. The best
remainder of this article. of the three contractions was used in the
To test the research questions, sub- results. Then, subjects began a series of

27
Figure 1 RTD is greater in trained individuals from 0-50 ms

1200

1000
Trained
Untrained
800
RTD (N·m·s-1)

600

400

200

0
0-50 50-100 100-150 150-200

Time windows from torque onset (ms)

RTD = rate of torque development; * = Significantly greater RTD than untrained

eight explosive contractions. Four explo- scope; rather, just understand that both
sive contractions were performed with were obtained using the five best explo-
20 seconds of rest between reps, with five sive contractions.
minutes of rest after the fourth contrac-
tion. Next, four more contractions were
performed with 20 seconds of rest be- Findings
tween reps. The outcome measures ob- So that we don’t get bogged down too
tained during either the maximal or ex- much in the details, I’ve chosen to report
plosive contractions are listed in Table 2. only the findings that are relevant to us.
Please note that I left out a few details of
exactly how rate of torque development Maximal Torque
and conduction velocity were calculated,
As expected, the trained subjects had
as those methods are little outside of our

28
Figure 2 MFCV is greater in trained individuals

5.5

5.0
MFCV (m/s)

4.5

4.0

3.5
EMD 0-50 50-100 100-150 150-200

Time windows from EMG and force onset (ms)

MFCV = muscle fiber conduction velocity; * = significantly greater than untrained

significantly greater (p<0.001) maximal 50-100 millisecond time interval, suggest-


elbow flexion torque than the untrained ing that full motor unit recruitment might
subjects (39.3% greater in trained). have occurred early in the contraction.
Perhaps the most novel finding was that
Rate of Torque Development and Conduc- significant correlations were observed be-
tion Velocity tween rate of torque development and
Rate of torque development was great- muscle fiber conduction velocity in trained
er in the trained individuals during the (R2 = 0.65) and untrained subjects (R2 =
0-50 millisecond time interval (Figure 1), 0.57), but only during the 0-50 millisec-
and muscle fiber conduction velocity was ond time interval. This indicates that there
greater in the trained subjects during all is a positive relationship between muscle
time intervals (Figure 2). Further, conduc- fiber conduction velocity and rate of force
tion velocity reached its peak during the development.

29
Interpretation THESE FINDINGS SUGGEST
Since the findings were a little mech-
anistic, let’s provide a straightforward THAT THE PROCESS
explanation of the results. The trained
group was stronger (of course), the
OF “NEUROMUSCULAR
trained lifters had greater rate of torque
development during the early part of the
EFFICIENCY” – THE ABILITY
explosive contractions, and the trained TO QUICKLY RECRUIT
lifters had greater muscle fiber conduc-
tion velocity throughout the entirety of MOTOR UNITS – IS
an explosive contraction. In other words,
the trained lifters possibly recruited mo-
POSSIBLY TRAINABLE.
tor units more quickly (torque develop-
ment) and that was related to faster ac-
tion potential transmission (conduction
velocity), thus the authors’ hypotheses didn’t have a great citation at the time
were supported. However, it is necessary to make that claim (although I was con-
to state that the measures of motor unit fident in what I was saying); the current
recruitment were indirect, so it’s likely study provides that citation.
that motor units were recruited more
quickly in the trained lifters, but we can- So, how does this information affect
not be entirely sure. training? General training should im-
prove neuromuscular efficiency in the
These findings suggest that the pro- early stages of training. The question
cess of “neuromuscular efficiency” – the then becomes how can we continue to
ability to quickly recruit motor units – is improve neuromuscular efficiency af-
possibly trainable. Various studies have ter we pass the intermediate stage since
shown that lifters who are more trained there’s a point of diminishing returns
perform 1RMs at slower velocities than for training status to produce slower ve-
less-trained lifters (3, 4, 5). Some of locities at high intensities (5). The sim-
these studies have cited neuromuscular plest way to improve quickness of motor
efficiency as the reason for the velocity unit recruitment is by training heavier.
difference, but haven’t provided a clear Assuming that we are all training heavy
explanation of the term. That is indeed enough with normal periodized training,
a knock on those studies, but check the we can add in additional heavy work by
references: I’m knocking myself. I just including semi-heavy singles every once

30
showed some lifters maximize rate of
force development with power training
I WOULDN’T BE AFRAID and some don’t. Essentially, speed work
or explosive training likely isn’t bene-
TO INCLUDE A SINGLE ficial for everyone, but it is for some.
AROUND A 7-8RPE EITHER Therefore, if you’re not great at grind-
ing a 1RM and you’re already including
BEFORE OR AFTER YOUR a bunch of heavy singles, then try some
explosive training between 40-60% of
NORMAL TRAINING DURING 1RM. It’s possible you could do this at
even heavier loads (i.e. 80%), but ei-
A VOLUME BLOCK. ther way, the RPE should remain low
(i.e. 1-4 RPE or >5 RIR). This strate-
gy seems to increase rate of force de-
velopment in those who are “power-re-
in a while in an effort to improve neu-
sponders.” I would recommend heavy
romuscular efficiency. If you notice that
singles as your first option to improve
you have trouble grinding out a 1RM,
rate of force development, but if neuro-
then including heavy-ish singles once
muscular efficiency still isn’t improving,
a week or every other week through-
then give “power-type” training a shot.
out the year (not only when peaking)
For details on how to incorporate this
may be beneficial to practice working
strategy, I would strongly recommend
with heavy loads. Therefore, I wouldn’t
our previous article on the topic and
be afraid to include a single around a
my video from this month.
7-8RPE either before or after your nor-
mal training during a volume block. For Lastly, it should be noted that signif-
example, if you have 4 sets of 8 @70%, a icant relationships have been observed
single @7-8RPE beforehand will allow between muscle fiber conduction veloc-
you to get more experience with heavy ity and both muscle cross-sectional area
loads in an effort to improve the quick- and percentage of type IIx fibers (6). To
ness of motor unit recruitment, but it be clear, this does not mean that great-
shouldn’t be too fatiguing that it actu- er CSA is a causative factor in conduc-
ally affects your volume work. tion velocity; however, this relationship
means that you cannot rule out some
Another practical application is to
degree of hypertrophy being important
incorporate more power training if
to maximize rate of torque or force de-
your strength has plateaued a bit. We
velopment.
examined this concept in MASS re-
cently when reviewing a study (2) that

31
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
1. Trained lifters have a greater rate of torque development in the early part of a
contraction than untrained individuals. This is related to greater muscle fiber
conduction velocity.
2. Therefore, neuromuscular efficiency seems to increase with training status. Whether
it is a directly trainable process or not remains equivocal; however, these results
do indicate that the ability to recruit motor units quickly does improve with training
experience. There is one training study, however, that suggests otherwise (7).
3. Both muscle size and absolute strength seem to be important for maximizing
neuromuscular efficiency. If you don’t have the ability to grind a heavy weight,
your efficiency may be lacking. Incorporating semi-frequent heavy singles or
some explosive power-type training may be warranted to improve neuromuscular
capabilities.

Next Steps
The next question I’d like to see an-
swered is at what point in a training
career is neuromuscular efficiency max-
imized? In other words, at what relative
strength or what training age do mea-
surable changes in neuromuscular effi-
ciency stop occurring? To answer this
question, a similar design to the present
study could be used and subjects of vari-
ous training ages could be included. For
example, a cohort with no training expe-
rience, a cohort with two years of expe-
rience, and other cohorts with four, six,
eight, and ten years of experience. This
proposed design should be able to de-
termine if a point of diminishing returns
occurs for neuromuscular efficiency.

32
References
1. Del Vecchio A, Negro F, Falla D, Bazzucchi I, Farina D, Felici F. Higher muscle fiber conduction
velocity and early rate of torque development in chronically strength trained individuals. Journal of
Applied Physiology. 2018.
2. Peltonen H, Walker S, Hackney AC, Avela J, Häkkinen K. Increased rate of force development
during periodized maximum strength and power training is highly individual. European journal of
applied physiology. 2018 May 1;118(5):1033-42.
3. Torrejón A, Balsalobre-Fernández C, Haff GG, García-Ramos A. The load-velocity profile differs
more between men and women than between individuals with different strength levels. Sports bio-
mechanics. 2018 Mar 21:1-1.
4. Ormsbee MJ, Carzoli JP, Klemp A, Allman BR, Zourdos MC, Kim JS, Panton LB. Efficacy of the
repetitions in reserve-based rating of perceived exertion for the bench press in experienced and nov-
ice benchers. J. Strength Cond. Res. doi. 2017 Mar 13;10.
5. Zourdos MC, Klemp A, Dolan C, Quiles JM, Schau KA, Jo E, Helms E, Esgro B, Duncan S, Me-
rino SG, Blanco R. Novel resistance training–specific rating of perceived exertion scale measuring
repetitions in reserve. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2016 Jan 1;30(1):267-75.
6. Methenitis S, Karandreas N, Spengos K, Zaras N, Stasinaki AN, Terzis G. Muscle Fiber Conduc-
tion Velocity, Muscle Fiber Composition, and Power Performance. Medicine and science in sports
and exercise. 2016 Sep;48(9):1761-71.
7. Matsunaga, S, Sadoyama, T, Miyata H, Katsuta S. The effects of strength training on muscle fi-
ber conduction velocity of surface action potential. Japanese Journal of Physical Fitness and Sports
Medicine. 1990 Apr 1;39(2):99-105.

33
Study Reviewed: Resistance Training Volume Enhances Muscle Hypertrophy.
Schoenfeld et al. (2018)

Understanding Volume
BY E RI C HE LMS

A recent study reported that very high volumes were associated with
the greatest hypertrophy when compared to lower volumes. Before you
conclude that this changes everything and double your sets, read this first.

34
KEY POINTS
1. Trained males were divided into three groups performing one, three, or five sets
per exercise during a three-day-per-week full body training program, resulting in
6-9, 18-27, and 30-45 sets per muscle group (counting isolation and compound
exercises) for both the upper and lower limbs.
2. Subjects performed 8-12 reps per set to failure, increased load when possible
week-to-week, and rested 2 minutes between exercises and 90 seconds between
sets. Similar strength and muscular endurance outcomes were reported between
groups, but hypertrophy tended to increase with volume.
3. Broadly speaking, performing 10-20 sets per muscle group/movement seems to
most consistently result in peak gains in muscle size and strength; however, when
experienced lifters are plateaued and all other factors are optimized, increasing
volume may be an appropriate strategy to make further progress.

T
he authors of the present study respectively. Volume per muscle group
are the first to examine what most was quantified by simply counting every
people would consider very high set performed in which a given muscle
training volumes in trained lifters (1). group contributed to an exercise (isola-
While a prior meta-analysis found that tion or compound). Interestingly, 1RM
performing 10+ sets per muscle group bench press and squat and maximum
per week resulted in greater hypertrophy bench press repetitions at 50% 1RM did
than 1-4 sets or 5-9 sets, “10+” is a very not differ significantly nor meaningfully
broad guideline. Thus, examining volume between groups after eight weeks. How-
levels beyond 10 sets per muscle group ever, muscle growth, measured via mus-
per week can help us better understand cle thickness, was significantly greater in
the point at which the relationship be- the high- versus low-volume group for
tween volume and muscular adaptation the biceps and quadriceps. Additionally,
changes. To explore this topic, the au- while not significant, evidence also tend-
thors had three groups perform a three- ed to favor a dose-response relationship
day-per-week full body program, with between volume and biceps and quad-
each group performing a different num- riceps hypertrophy (according to Bayes
ber of sets per exercise. The groups per- Factors, which I’ll explain later), show-
formed either one, three, or five sets per ing that the medium-volume group in-
exercise, resulting in 6-9, 18-27, and 30- creased muscle thickness more than the
45 weekly sets per muscle group in the low-volume group, and the high-volume
low-, medium-, and high-volume groups, group increased muscle thickness more

35
than the medium-volume group. These males between the ages of 18-35 com-
outcomes have already spurred discussion pleted this study (height = 175.0 ± 7.9
in the practical realm, leading the authors cm; body mass = 82.5 ± 13.8 kg; age
to discuss the findings in further detail. = 23.8 ± 3.8 years; training experience
Therefore, this article will discuss this = 4.4 ± 3.9 years). To qualify for this
study and use the authors’ further com- study, participants had to have at least
mentary to contextualize these results. one year of consistently training at least
three times per week and had to confirm
that they had not used supplements or
Purpose and Research drugs known to affect muscular perfor-
Questions mance for at least the prior year. To give
you another metric partially associated
with training experience, the partici-
Purpose
pants, as a group, had an average bench
The purpose of this study was to exam- press of 93.8kg (~207lb) and an average
ine previously unexplored levels of volume squat of 108.9kg (~240lb) at a body-
in trained lifters and to observe chang- weight of 82.5kg (~182lb). This means
es in strength, muscular endurance, and they benched about 1.14x bodyweight
muscle mass between groups performing and squatted about 1.32x bodyweight.
different levels of training volume. From a strength perspective, this means
the participants were reasonably well-
Hypothesis trained bench pressers, but actually not
The authors hypothesized that there very well-trained squatters. Participants
would be a dose-response relationship were randomized into a low (n=11;
for all outcomes in relation to volume. In one set per exercise per day; 6-9 sets
other words, the more sets performed, the per muscle group per week), medium
greater the predicted increases in strength, (n=12; three sets per exercise, 18-27 sets
hypertrophy, and muscular endurance (of per muscle group), or high (n=11; five
note, a dose-response relationship does sets per exercise, 30-45 sets per muscle
not necessarily mean a linear relation- group) volume group and were instruct-
ship). ed to not perform any resistance train-
ing or high-intensity cardio outside of
the study.
Subjects and Methods
Training
Subjects and Design The participants trained full body, three
Thirty-four resistance trained, healthy days per week, on non-consecutive days.

36
All sets were taken to momentary mus- thickness measurements of the biceps,
cular failure for 8-12 reps on every set. triceps, vastus lateralis, and rectus fem-
All sessions consisted of the back squat, oris at pre- and post-study. The post-
leg press, leg extension, bench press, study measurements were taken 48-72
military press, lat pulldown, and cable hours after the last training session to let
row. There were 90 seconds between swelling subside (2).
sets and two minutes between exercises.
Load was initially set via 10-repetition Quality of Methods
maximum (established during pre-test- I’ve seen the quality of the methods in
ing after 1RM tests) prior to the initi- this study heavily critiqued. While I be-
ation of training, and load was adjusted lieve we should always critically evaluate
intra-session if fewer than 8 reps were studies (scientists can always improve
completed to ensure failure occurred in and develop better methods over time),
the 8-12 rep range. Additionally, load I think the methods in this study were
was progressively increased from week sound, as someone who both conducts
to week as needed. and reviews exercise science research.
Thus, I want to point out specific aspects
Diet of the methods which, in my mind, show
Participants were given a scoop of that the authors did their due diligence
protein by the research staff on training by avoiding many methodological errors
days to better ensure consistent and ad- that would confound the results.
equate protein intakes between groups. For one, the authors reported very good
Subjects were taught how to weigh and reliability statistics for both ultrasound
track foods, and they used MyFitnessPal and strength testing, in terms of both
for five days to track nutrition during the intraclass correlation coefficients (0.93-
first and last weeks of the study. Subjects 0.998) and standard errors of measure-
were instructed to maintain their nor- ment (0.34-1.09 mm and 2.0-2.4 kg).
mal eating habits and not consume any Also, unfortunately, in many exercise
supplements outside of the protein pro- studies, nutrition is not addressed at all.
vided. However, the authors made an effort to
control for nutrition as a potential con-
Outcome Testing founder. The authors also waited 48-
At pre- and post-study, back squat and 72 hours after the last training session
bench press 1RM were tested along with before performing post-training ultra-
muscular endurance on the bench press sound measurements to prevent swell-
(max reps at 50% of pre-test 1RM). ing from confounding the results, which
Ultrasound was used to obtain muscle is often not considered in our field. This

37
was likely adequate to prevent swelling ticipants visited the lab 26 times for this
from muddying the hypertrophy re- study: twice to do pre- and post-testing,
sults (see my citation in the “Outcome and 24 times to be trained. If you blind
Testing” section), especially consider- your ultrasound technician to group as-
ing the body part frequency and trained signment, this means they can’t be pres-
status of the participants (the repeated ent for training, which is over 90% of
bout effect would be in play). Howev- the lab visits. In exercise science, where
er, given how high the volumes were, it personnel is limited, this may create an
might have been even better to have a insurmountable logistical barrier. It’s
taper week where each group performed worth noting that technicians aren’t
½-⅔ their normal volume the week be- blinded in the vast majority of studies
fore testing. This could have let any re- in our field. So, if a lack of blinding did
sidual fatigue subside, which might have create bias, the same bias has occurred
modified the outcomes for strength or in nearly every other study published
muscular endurance, while also ensuring in this area. Personally, when I see this
swelling had subsided and should have brought up, it makes me wonder why it’s
been adequate to maintain the gains they being presented as something out of the
made. With that said, the leg extension norm (not that this shouldn’t change),
volume load improvements (and their when that’s not the case, and why it
relationship with hypertrophy) – which hasn’t been mentioned in relation to the
I’ll discuss in the interpretation – sug- other studies on this topic. Lastly, for
gests hypertrophy was indeed the driver any researchers reading this, it would be
of the muscle thickness changes rather feasible to simply have someone who’s
than swelling. Further contradicting the uninvolved in the study analyze the im-
idea that swelling drove the hypertro- ages, which would help reduce bias. It
phy differences between groups, there is much easier to train someone to use
are studies using ultrasound, which did a digital caliper on a computer program
not specify any extended waiting peri- than it is to actually use an ultrasound.
od, that don’t show a dose response with
hypertrophy and volume (3); if swelling
had caused the outcomes, this would be Findings
a finding in all studies of this design. The authors used two different sta-
Lastly, I’ve seen the critique that the tistical models to analyze and present
ultrasound technician was not blinded their findings. They used a tradition-
to the groups, which could potential- al null-hypothesis testing model called
ly introduce bias. In a perfect world, I an ANCOVA (analysis of covariance),
agree that it’s always ideal to do as much which compares groups while adjust-
blinding as possible. However, the par-

38
Table 1 Adjusted and unadjusted differences between groups and significance
Baseline
Pre Mean ± Post Mean ± Unadjusted
Outcome Group Adjusted
SD SD Δ (±SD)
Δ (CI)***
Squat 1RM (kg) 1 104.5 ± 14.2 123.4 ± 12.9 18.9 ± 6.0 18.6 (13.7, 23.4)
3 114.9 ± 26.0 128.5 ± 24.7 13.6 ± 5.4 14.1 (9.5, 18.7)
5 106.6 ± 24.0 126.2 ± 25.0 19.6 ± 10.0 19.4 (14.9, 24.0)
Bench 1RM (kg) 1 93.6 ± 16.1 102.9 ± 15.2 9.3 ± 4.4 9.3 (6.9, 11.9)
3 96.4 ± 21.2 102.1 ± 20.1 5.7 ± 5.8 5.9 (3.4, 8.4)
5 91.1 ± 20.9 97.9 ± 20.0 6.8 ± 2.3 6.6 (4.0, 9.2)
Bench endurance 1 25.1 ± 3.6 28.2 ± 4.6 3.1 ± 3.9 3.1 (0.8, 5.4)
3 23.7 ± 5.2 28.0 ± 5.6 4.3 ± 4.1 4.2 (2.0, 6.4)
5 26.2 ± 4.3 31.0 ± 6.1 4.8 ± 2.9 4.9 (2.6, 7.3)
Biceps thickness (mm) 1 42.6 ± 4.3 43.3 ± 5.1 0.7 ± 2.0 0.7 (-0.4, 1.8)*
3 44.6 ± 5.9 46.7 ± 5.8 2.1 ± 1.6 2.1 (1.1, 3.2)ab
5 41.9 ± 3.6 44.8 ± 4.1 2.9 ± 1.7 2.9 (1.8, 4.0)b
Triceps thickness (mm) 1 47.2 ± 4.5 47.7 ± 4.6 0.6 ± 2.0 0.5 (-1.0, 2.1)
3 48.4 ± 6.2 49.8 ± 6.3 1.4 ± 3.1 1.4 (-0.1, 3.0)
5 47.1 ± 3.5 49.7 ± 4.9 2.6 ± 2.3 2.6 (1.0, 4.1)
Rectus femoris thickness (mm) 1 59.7 ± 6.7 61.7 ± 5.5 2.0 ± 2.6 2.2 (0.3, 4.2)a
3 57.9 ± 8.1 61.0 ± 8.7 3.0 ± 3.1 3.1 (1.0, 5.2)ab
5 54.4 ± 3.4 61.2 ± 4.5 6.8 ± 3.6 6.4 (4.2, 8.6)b
Vastus lateralis thickness (mm) 1 57.5 ± 6.0 60.4 ± 6.3 2.9 ± 1.9 3.1 (1.6, 4.5)a
3 57.9 ± 8.0 62.5 ± 7.0 4.6 ± 2.3 4.9 (3.5, 6.3)ab
5 52.4 ± 6.2 59.6 ± 5.8 7.2 ± 3.0 6.8 (5.2, 8.3)b

*** = When superscript letters are different it indicates a significant (p<0.05) difference between groups. When no
superscripts are present, no significant differences were present, and when superscript letters are the same,
differences between groups are also nonsignificant.

ing for baseline values and also produces than or greater than 0.05) – is an odds
p-values (which MASS readers are likely ratio, whereby the evidence is consid-
familiar with) in addition to a less com- ered stronger if the odds ratio is larger.
mon (but no less valid) Bayesian mod- Specifically, a Bayes factor (abbreviated
el. Briefly, Bayesian statistics provide BF10) of two that favors a higher out-
probabilities of evidential strength that come in group A versus group B sug-
a value is or is not different than another gests the outcome is twice as likely to
value, based on a theorem that mathe- be higher in group A than in group B,
matically accounts for prior knowledge while a Bayes factor of four would mean
on the topic. Its output – rather than be- four times as likely, and so forth. Bayes
ing a binary outcome (e.g. p is either less factors are qualitatively described in cat-

39
Figure 1 Hypertrophy outcomes of individuals within each group

7 7

6
6

Δ Tricep Thickness (mm)


Δ Bicep Thickness (mm)

5
4 4

3
2
a 2 b
1 0
0
-2
-1

-2 -2
-3 1 set 3 sets 5 sets
1 set 3 sets 5 sets
Δ Rectus femoris thickness (mm)

12 14

Δ Vastus lateralis thickness (mm)


10 12

8 10

6 8
c 4
d 6

2 4

0 2

-2 0

-3 -2
1 set 3 sets 5 sets 1 set 3 sets 5 sets

egories much like effect sizes: a BF10 also presented in James Krieger’s (one
of 1-3 is considered “weak” evidence in of the authors) blog in a figure that was
favor of an outcome; a BF10 of 3-20 is submitted but wasn’t included in the fi-
considered “positive” evidence; a BF10 nal, peer-reviewed version of the study.
of 20-150 “strong” evidence; and a BF10 I think this figure is useful, as it shows
of >150 is considered “very strong” evi- each individual’s data point, the baseline,
dence. and the group means in a visually intui-
With that background information tive manner, which I’ve included here as
out of the way, strength, muscular en- Figure 1.
durance, and hypertrophy outcomes are To sum up the results, there were no
presented in Table 1 both as raw and significant nor meaningful differenc-
baseline-adjusted values, with indica- es between groups for either muscular
tions of significance when found. strength or endurance (although I will
The muscle thickness differences were discuss an interesting specific perfor-
mance outcome in the interpretation).

40
However, biceps and both quadriceps a reason that this study is bogus. To be
muscle thickness measures increased sure, 30-45 sets is a lot of volume, but it’s
more in the high-volume group than the also important to point out that this val-
low-volume group. Further, Bayes fac- ue counts each set for each muscle group
tors indicated “weak” evidence in favor on a 1:1 basis, regardless of whether the
of the medium-volume group increas- muscle is directly or indirectly trained.
ing muscle thickness when compared For example, if I told the average lift-
to the low-volume group for the vastus er to do four sets of lat pulldowns, rows,
lateralis (BF10 = 1.42) and biceps (BF10 = cable curls, and DB hammer curls twice
1.30). Likewise, Bayes factors indicated per week, they probably wouldn’t think
weak evidence in favor of the high-vol- that was too crazy (even though, by the
ume group increasing muscle thickness counting method used here, that’s 32
more than the medium-volume group sets for biceps). However, if I told them
in the vastus lateralis (BF10 = 2.25) and to do 32 sets for biceps per week without
rectus femoris (BF10 = 2.34). While the preamble, their initial reaction would
mean changes in triceps thickness scaled probably be a wide-eyed expression until
with volume, which can been seen when they understood the counting method. I
looking at Figure 1, you can also see how do think it’s a fair concern that this study
variable the individual outcomes were, could lead people to overdo it; however,
which prevents any firm conclusions it’s also important to remember that no
from being made for this specific mea- single study tells you how to train. You
surement site. have to put this study into context with
the broader literature, rather than taking
it as the sole study of import in this area,
Interpretation and subsequently taking the conclusions
This study has resulted in a lot of con- right to application.
troversy since publication, and I think Secondary to hypertrophy, some of
that’s understandable. Most people in the most interesting findings in this
health and fitness want others to be fit study are the dissimilar outcomes be-
and healthy (shocking, I know). At the tween strength and muscle thickness.
core of the controversy, I think many of While this may surprise many, it isn’t a
the folks picking apart this study and surprising finding to me given just how
looking for holes in its methods, analy- specific 1RM strength on a compound
sis, or discussion are concerned that ev- movement is. While strength, like hy-
eryone seeking hypertrophy is going to pertrophy, has a positive relationship
start doing 30-45 sets per muscle group with volume (4), the principle of speci-
and get hurt, so they are hoping to find ficity is still highly important. Meaning,

41
when trying to improve 1RM strength, (and I don’t think it was designed to be),
a unit of volume that is not specific to and if anything, it highlights the impor-
the strength-test is going to contribute tance of specificity. I would also venture
less than a unit of volume that is specif- a guess that the subjects probably weren’t
ic. Thus, I’m not that surprised that do- previously squatting and bench pressing
ing 3, 9, or 15 weekly sets of squats or three times per week, and regardless of
bench press in the 8-12 rep range had volume, this alone might have had an ef-
similar effects on 1RM squat or bench fect on strength, washing out differences
press strength, especially when you con- between groups.
sider the participants only rested 90 sec- With that said, one intriguing bit of
onds between sets. With this short of a data was presented in James’ blog on a
rest, each subsequent set after the first less specific measure of performance
set was in a fatigued state, and subse- that shows how hypertrophy can impact
quently, often performed with a lighter strength. Specifically, he analyzed the
load (and, therefore, was increasingly within-group volume load improvement
less specific). Mike covered a study in on the leg extension, comparing the
MASS showing that the top intensity in percentage change from the first week
a series of sets likely has a greater impact to the last week of the study. Interest-
on changes in strength than the average ingly, there was a relationship just short
intensity across sets. Also, consulting of significance (p = 0.057) between sets
other literature using similar repetition performed and the improvements in leg
ranges on the same movements, some- extension volume load. The low-volume
times more sets are associated with sig- group improved their leg extension vol-
nificant improvements in strength (5), ume load by 23.8%, medium-volume by
but sometimes they aren’t (3). Thus, all in 33%, and high-volume by 54%. While
all, these findings aren’t that surprising. this could be related to improvements in
While it’s established that the load on interset recovery in groups doing more
the bar (above a certain very low thresh- sets, he also found a significant correla-
old) is not that important for hypertro- tion between the percentage improve-
phy so long as you train with adequate ment in vastus lateralis thickness and
effort, the same can’t be said for 1RM the percentage improvement in leg ex-
strength, where load is considered a pri- tension volume load (r = 0.40; p = 0.02)
mary variable of importance (6). On the and a correlation that approached signif-
other hand, volume does seem to have icance with the improvement in rectus
an impact on strength development, femoris thickness (r = 0.34; p = 0.075).
but when meta-analyzed, the effects are This makes sense: When you remove
trivial to small (4). Also, this was not an more of the skill and neurological com-
ideal protocol for strength development

42
ponents that contribute to strength and
isolate a single muscle group, the impact
of hypertrophy on force development PROGRESSIVE TENSION OVERLOAD
should be clearer (something Greg has
discussed in MASS previously). IS THE PRIMARY MECHANISM BY
Now, going back to what I said earli- WHICH RESISTANCE-TRAINING
er, a single study doesn’t tell you what to
do in the gym, and we have to put this
INDUCED HYPERTROPHY OCCURS,
study in context. Thus, I’ll discuss it in WHILE VOLUME IS SIMPLY THE
a number of ways including: 1) concep-
tualizing and quantifying volume and AMOUNT OF PROGRESSIVE
2) how the results compare to other re-
search exploring high-volume training.
TENSION OVERLOAD YOU ARE
SUPPLYING WHEN ALL ELSE IS
Volume as a Theoretical Construct and
Quantification HELD CONSTANT. ONE CAN’T
Ever since this study was published, BE OPPOSED TO THE OTHER;
I’ve watched people debate whether
progressive tension overload or volume
RATHER, ONE IS QUANTIFYING
is the primary driver of hypertrophy. THE AMOUNT OF THE OTHER.
Conceptually, I don’t think this debate
makes sense. Progressive tension over-
load is the primary mechanism by which
resistance-training induced hypertrophy While we don’t have all of the answers
occurs (7), while volume is simply the to these questions, we have some. In
amount of progressive tension over- MASS, we’ve already reviewed research
load you are supplying when all else is that established the boundaries of what
held constant. One can’t be opposed to would make a set an “effective” dose of
the other; rather, one is quantifying the volume for hypertrophy. As covered by
amount of the other. Better questions Greg a few issues ago, high-rep sets with
might be: How much volume should one very low loads (< 30% 1RM) when taken
do at different effort levels (proximity to failure can’t produce enough muscular
to failure), loads, when using single- or stimulus to put them on equal footing
multi-joint movements, when plateaued, with sets of heavier loads, likely due to
or at different stages of a lifting career, inadequate tension, as failure might oc-
and how do drop sets and rest-pause sets cur more via global versus local muscu-
figure into these calculations? lar fatigue (8). Likewise, it’s been shown

43
that 3 x 2-4 RM results in less hypertro- much smaller difference in hypertrophy
phy than 3 x 8-12 RM, even though the than you might expect and often doesn’t
number of sets is matched (9). Presum- reach the point of statistical significance
ably, this is because high-load, low-rep (12, 13), but sometimes does (14). Addi-
sets (< 6) simply don’t last long enough tionally, there is a study comparing bicep
to give enough of a training stimulus to curls to lat pulldowns head-to-head on
the fibers being recruited. Collectively, a set-to-set basis, finding they both pro-
this body of research culminated in a sys- duce similar biceps hypertrophy – albeit
tematic review that Mike dove into last in untrained subjects (15).
month, in which the authors reviewed I’ve heard many practitioners who I
research supporting the notion that any respect take different approaches to an-
set of adequate effort in the 6-20 (I’d swering the question of what contrib-
venture 6-30) rep range results in a sim- utes to volume of a specific muscle group.
ilar hypertrophy stimulus as any other Some have suggested only counting sets
reasonably hard set in that rep range. toward the prime movers (although you
Subsequently, the authors concluded can debate what defines a prime mover),
that to count hypertrophy-specific vol- while others suggest counting second-
ume, you can just count the number of ary muscle groups as getting half or one
hard sets in this rep range (10). third of a set, etc. Greg suggested that
Another issue that this definition of how a movement might “count” toward
volume doesn’t clarify is how we quan- a muscle group’s volume is likely based
tify muscle group-specific volume given on the range of motion of the movement
the full spectrum of exercises we can use for a given joint and its associated mus-
in training. In research, in order to code cles. I think this makes sense, and this
data for analysis, we have a precedent supposition is at least partially support-
of counting every set for every muscle ed by the lat pulldown versus biceps curl
group that contributes to the movement. study I mentioned in the last paragraph
Meaning, a row, a pulldown, and a bi- (15), as a full range of motion pulldown
ceps curl are all counted toward biceps takes you through a full range of elbow
volume on a 1:1 basis. This was how vol- flexion. Perhaps dips, a full range of
ume was counted in the present study motion overhead press, and close-grip
and in a prior meta-analysis (11). How bench press can be counted fully toward
does it actually work though? That’s triceps volume just like a triceps push-
hard to say. There are data showing that down, while a powerlifting-style bench
adding single-joint exercises to multi- press cannot? Perhaps lat pulldowns and
joint exercises targeting the same mus- chins can be counted fully toward biceps
cle group improves arm size, but it’s a volume just like a curl, while a bent-

44
Table 2 Greatest gains associated with number of weekly sets in high volume studies
Population and
Study Low volume Med volume High volume Outcome
length

45 men, 4.4 ± 3.9 of greater hypertrophy with higher volume,


Schoenfeld - 6-9 sets per 18-27 sets per 30-45 sets per
years experience,
present study (1) muscle group# muscle group muscle group*^
8 weeks comparing high to low. Leg extension

quadriceps hypertrophy.
51 men aged
1923 total reps 2481 total reps 3030 total reps
16.6 ± 1.5 years,
Gonzalez-Badillo of snatch, C&J, of snatch, C&J, of snatch, C&J,
3.7 ± 0.7 years
(20) squats, and squats, and squats, and snatch 1RM in med vs low, and approached
experience,
derivatives derivatives# derivatives
10 weeks

27 men, 1-4 years 6-14 sets per 12-28 sets per Triceps CSA % change greatest in med
3-7 sets per
Ostrowski (3) experience, 10 muscle group* muscle group^ and high (2.3%, 4.7%, 4.8%). Quad CSA
muscle group#
weeks (14 for triceps) (12 for quads) % change was greatest in high (6.7%, 5%,
13.1%).

18 men, untrained, 3-6 sets per 9-18 sets per


Paulsen (17) NA
6 weeks muscle group muscle group#
high vs low.
Upper body 5RM mostly scaled with
volume, similar lower body 5RM among
48 military men, 3-6 sets per 9-18 sets per 15-30 sets per
groups. Lower and upper 20RM scaled with
Radaelli (16) only callisthenics muscle group muscle group muscle group
volume. Biceps muscle thickness scaled with
history, 6 months (9 for triceps) (27 for triceps) (45 for triceps)*^
volume only.
49 men, min 1
Heaselgrave (21) year experience, 9 sets for biceps 18 sets for biceps* 27 sets for biceps# high vs baseline. Trend for greatest muscle
6 weeks thickness increase in med.
12 men, min 1
9-18 sets per 14-28 sets per
Hackett (23) year experience, NA
muscle group *^# muscle group
12 weeks lean mass vs baseline in higher.
19 men, min 1
9-18 sets per 14-28 sets per
Amirthalingam (22) year experience, NA
muscle group*^# muscle group and greater trunk lean mass in lower volume.
6 weeks
16 men, min 1
9-12 sets per 27-36 sets per
Baker (19) year experience, NA
muscle group# muscle group between groups.
8 weeks

strength in lower body, but not upper in high


21 men, untrained, 3-6 sets per 9-18 sets per
Rønnestad (18) NA
11 weeks muscle group muscle group*^#
high vs low, trend for all other muscle groups
being greater as well.

For each study, the lowest volume group where a meaningful outcome occured is marked:
* = Best for upper body hypertrophy; ^ = Best for lower body hypertrophy; # = Best for muscular performance

over row cannot? Perhaps full range of my best crack in my book’s second edi-
motion squats, leg presses, and leg ex- tion). However, we mustn't think that
tensions all count similarly to quad hy- just because a theoretical model for
pertrophy? You could certainly make a counting the volume of different move-
system informed by our understanding ments exists and is convenient that it
of biomechanics in an effort to quantify therefore must be correct. We have to
muscle-group volume based on exercise accept that we simply don't know and
selection (hell, I will probably give it that not everything can be quantified at

45
Figure 2 Inverted U relationship with volume and gains

End of dose-response, End of plateau, beginning


beginning of plateau of decreased progress rate
Gains

End of decreased progress,


beginning of regression

Volume

this stage. leagues found that 5-12+ sets per week


seemed to optimize strength gains (3),
Research Exploring High Volumes while Schoenfeld and colleagues found
One study never tells you everything. that 10+ sets per week seemed to opti-
Developing science-based conclusions, mize hypertrophy (11). Both reported
especially in exercise science where we a graded dose response (that does not
never have huge studies with a ton of mean a linear dose-response, mind you)
funding, requires the consideration of the between volume and gains. However, in
existing literature on a given topic. Thus, both meta-analyses, the authors pointed
the present study must be considered as out the limitation of not having enough
a data point alongside other studies that data to make truly conclusive recom-
explore “higher volume” training. But mendations, leaving the door open that
what defines “higher volume”? For one, perhaps even higher volumes could con-
the goal. MASS readers are interested in tinue the trend.
optimizing strength and/or hypertrophy. Thus, in Table 2, I’ve listed the out-
Fortunately, two recent meta-analyses comes of all the studies I’m aware of
have helped us quantify the upper lim- that assessed strength and hypertrophy
its of what we currently know regarding where one or more groups utilized week-
volume’s impact on strength and hyper- ly volumes of training beyond the upper
trophy. An analysis by Ralston and col- limits of what has been observed in the

46
creased past a certain point (22), or an
actual regression in gains relative to
I THINK ~10-20 SETS PER baseline when volume is increased past
a certain point (23). While these might
MUSCLE GROUP/MOVEMENT seem like different outcomes, they all
paint different parts of the same pic-
IS PROBABLY WHERE THE ture, just from the perspective of differ-
“OPTIMAL VOLUME” IS ent populations (e.g. training ages) and
different points along the volume spec-
FOR MOST PEOPLE (E.G. trum. In aggregate, we can see that in-
creasing volume initially results in faster
WHAT YOU WOULD START gains. Then, you get to a point where no
WITH IF YOU WERE TO additional progress is made as you add
sets. If you continue to add sets, prog-
WRITE A PROGRAM WITH ress slows. And eventually, if you keep
adding sets, you may plateau or actually
NO PRIOR KNOWLEDGE regress. This “inverted U” (Figure 2) is
ABOUT SOMEONE). pretty much the way all adaptations to
stress occur in the body, and you could
argue it is a core principle of biology.
To give the semblance of guidelines,
aforementioned meta-analyses. For each we can do apples-to-apples compar-
study, I’ve noted the lowest volume at isons by counting the number of sets
which the highest meaningful (not nec- per muscle group the same way as was
essarily statistically significant) increas- done in the present study to see where
es in hypertrophy or strength occurred. the volume thresholds often lie. For ex-
When assessing these studies, you’ll see ample, Ostrowski observed non-signifi-
a few different outcomes: greater gains cant differences between groups, but 2.3,
associated with greater volumes (1, 16, 4.7, and 4.8% increases in triceps muscle
17, 18), similar rates of gains in lower thickness when comparing low, medium,
and higher volume even though more and high volumes, with 14 sets per week
sets are performed in a higher volume causing about as much growth as 28 sets
group (5, 19), a slower rate of muscle or per week. In the quadriceps, however,
strength gain (but a gain nonetheless) as 6.7, 5.0, and 13.1% increases in muscle
volume is increased past a certain point thickness occurred in the low, medium,
(20, 21), no meaningful gains occurring and high groups, respectively, but the
relative to baseline when volume is in- high-volume group only performed 12

47
sets (3). Paulsen found that 9-18 sets broad range) is probably where the “op-
outperformed 3-6 sets for strength (17), timal volume” is for most people (e.g.
and Baker found that 27-36 sets was what you would start with if you were to
no better than 9-12 (19). Heaselgrave write a program with no prior knowledge
reported the most meaningful biceps about someone). In the two studies that
hypertrophy in a group performing 18 demonstrated a higher optimal volume –
versus 27 and 9 weekly sets of combined the present study (1) and Radaelli (16) –
biceps work – rows, curls, and lat pull- it’s important to point out that they have
downs (21). Rønnestad reported great- some unique facets. In Radaelli, while
er strength and hypertrophy in a group the subjects were “untrained,” they were
performing 9-18 sets compared to 3-6 also young, fit, Navy men with a calis-
(18). As covered previously by Mike in thenics background, confined to a ship
his article on the first “German Volume with regular sleeping and eating sched-
Training” study, and in his brief review ules. Probably more importantly, it was a
of the follow-up, 14-28 sets per muscle 6-month study, while every other study
group resulted in poorer hypertrophy was 6-12 weeks; meaning, the subjects
and strength compared to 9-18 (22, 23). had more time to adapt to the higher
In my assessment, this paints a picture volumes (16). In the present study by
that there is a wide range of individual Schoenfeld (1), the subjects were among
responses to volume. These differences, the more trained out of all the studies
since they are always observed in the I’ve assessed. Comparing them to the
short term (studies typically last 6-12 first German Volume Training study
weeks), could be driven by the previ- (22), where high volume led to smaller
ous program someone was doing and gains than lower volume, Schoenfeld’s
also by their prior training experience. subjects were on average 6.5kg (~14lb)
When you have a reasonably low min- heavier despite being 5cm (~2in) short-
imum training experience and strength er and could bench press 19kg (~42lb)
cut off in a study, like bench pressing more. Thus, it may be that factors such as
at least bodyweight and at least 1 year the recovery environment and training
of experience (common metrics), you experience should be considered before
will get participants from broad train- very high-volume training is undertak-
ing backgrounds (notice that the stan- en.
dard deviation is nearly the same as the
mean for the training age in the present
study). Given that fact and based on the Next Steps
best data we have, I think ~10-20 sets At its core, I didn’t find the outcomes of
per muscle group/movement (which is a this study that surprising, simply based

48
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
1. Remember that this was an eight-week, test-of-concept study, not a dictate of how
to train. My anecdotal experience as a coach and the majority of research would
suggest that ~10-20 sets per muscle group/movement is a much more appropriate
beginning range to program for hypertrophy and strength. Sometimes, higher
volumes actually result in worse outcomes.
2. Of note, if you look at Figure 1, you can see the number of negative or non-responders
decreased with increasing volume – also a finding in endurance training, by the way
(24). This indicates that perhaps for those who really struggle to make gains, more
volume might be a consideration. However, volume should be increased only if a
plateau has truly occurred, not simply because of a lack of satisfaction with a rate
of progress you perceive as too slow.
3. With that said, increasing volume should be your final option when addressing a
plateau after ensuring nutrition, sleep, life stress, proximity to failure, technique,
frequency, and adherence are all taken care of. If all of those boxes are ticked,
and the only stone left unturned is an increase in volume, then you can consider a
higher volume approach. For hypertrophy, this may be best delivered via muscle-
group specialization, which I discuss in this month’s video.

on what we know about the drivers of hy- phy to 1RM strength. I’d love to know
pertrophy and strength. Rather than the what would happen if you tacked on a
outcomes themselves, the magnitude of low- to moderate-volume strength pro-
volumes performed was what surprised gram with longer rest periods, less ex-
me. Thus, I think interesting future re- ercises, and higher load, lower rep work
search might take the angle of trying to on bench press and squat to the end of
determine what individual factors might this protocol. Would the highest volume
predict an appropriate volume. I think group that grew the most also end up
training age is a big player, but perhaps transferring that hypertrophy into great-
a test of set-to-set strength-endurance er 1RM strength compared to the low-
(how many reps do you “lose” at a fixed er volume groups? I don’t know, but I’d
load across multiple sets when training love to find out. Lastly, I think a study
to failure in a standardized fashion) or specifically examining joint pain, subjec-
some other biological or performance tive ratings of motivation, desire to train,
marker could be developed as a screen- number of dropouts, injuries, and other
ing tool to assess appropriate volumes? potential over-use variables in relation to
Another aspect of this research I’m in- volume is an absolute necessity before we
terested in is the conversion of hypertro- even consider very high-volume training

49
(such as the protocol used in this study)
in the real world. It’s very possible that
high volumes could prove superior over
time periods common among studies. In
the real world, though, you may end up
with a tortoise versus the hare scenario
in which a more conservative approach
that keeps you from being injured even-
tually proves superior when compared to
a higher volume approach.

50
References
1. Schoenfeld, B.J., et al., Resistance Training Volume Enhances Muscle Hypertrophy. Med Sci Sports
Exerc, 2018.
2. Ogasawara, R., et al., Time course for arm and chest muscle thickness changes following bench press
training. Interventional Medicine & Applied Science, 2012. 4(4): p. 217-220.
3. Ostrowski, K.J., et al., The Effect of Weight Training Volume on Hormonal Output and Muscular
Size and Function. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research, 1997. 11(3): p. 148-154.
4. Ralston, G.W., et al., The Effect of Weekly Set Volume on Strength Gain: A Meta-Analysis. Sports
Med, 2017. 47(12): p. 2585-2601.
5. Robbins, D.W., P.W. Marshall, and M. McEwen, The effect of training volume on lower-body
strength. J Strength Cond Res, 2012. 26(1): p. 34-9.
6. Schoenfeld, B.J., et al., Strength and hypertrophy adaptations between low- versus high-load resis-
tance training: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Strength Cond Res, 2017.
7. Goldberg, A.L., et al., Mechanism of work-induced hypertrophy of skeletal muscle. Medicine and
Science in Sports, 1975. 7(3): p. 185-98.
8. Lasevicius, T., et al., Effects of different intensities of resistance training with equated volume load
on muscle strength and hypertrophy. Eur J Sport Sci, 2018. 18(6): p. 772-780.
9. Schoenfeld, B.J., et al., Differential Effects of Heavy Versus Moderate Loads on Measures of Strength
and Hypertrophy in Resistance-Trained Men. J Sports Sci Med, 2016. 15(4): p. 715-722.
10. Baz-Valle, E., M. Fontes-Villalba, and J. Santos-Concejero, Total Number of Sets as a Training Vol-
ume Quantification Method for Muscle Hypertrophy: A Systematic Review. J Strength Cond Res,
2018.
11. Schoenfeld, B.J., D. Ogborn, and J.W. Krieger, Dose-response relationship between weekly resis-
tance training volume and increases in muscle mass: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sports
Sci, 2017. 35(11): p. 1073-1082.
12. Gentil, P., et al., Effect of adding single-joint exercises to a multi-joint exercise resistance-training
program on strength and hypertrophy in untrained subjects. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab, 2013. 38(3):
p. 341-4.
13. de Franca, H.S., et al., The effects of adding single-joint exercises to a multi-joint exercise resistance
training program on upper body muscle strength and size in trained men. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab,
2015. 40(8): p. 822-6.
14. Barbalho, M., et al., Influence of Adding Single-Joint Exercise to a Multijoint Resistance Training
Program in Untrained Young Women. J Strength Cond Res, 2018.
15. Gentil, P., S. Soares, and M. Bottaro, Single vs. Multi-Joint Resistance Exercises: Effects on Muscle
Strength and Hypertrophy. Asian J Sports Med, 2015. 6(2): p. e24057.
16. Radaelli, R., et al., Dose-response of 1, 3, and 5 sets of resistance exercise on strength, local muscular
endurance, and hypertrophy. J Strength Cond Res, 2015. 29(5): p. 1349-58.
17. Paulsen, G., D. Myklestad, and T. Raastad, The influence of volume of exercise on early adaptations
to strength training. J Strength Cond Res, 2003. 17(1): p. 115-20.

51
18. Ronnestad, B.R., et al., Dissimilar effects of one- and three-set strength training on strength and
muscle mass gains in upper and lower body in untrained subjects. J Strength Cond Res, 2007. 21(1):
p. 157-63.
19. Baker, J.S., et al., Strength and Body Composition Changes in Recreationally Strength-Trained
Individuals: Comparison of One versus Three Sets Resistance-Training Programmes. BioMed Re-
search International, 2013. 2013: p. 615901.
20. Gonzalez-Badillo, J.J., et al., Moderate resistance training volume produces more favorable strength
gains than high or low volumes during a short-term training cycle. J Strength Cond Res, 2005. 19(3):
p. 689-97.
21. Heaselgrave, S.R., et al., Dose-Response of Weekly Resistance Training Volume and Frequency on
Muscular Adaptations in Trained Males. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Perfor-
mance, 2018: p. 1-28.
22. Amirthalingam, T., et al., Effects of a Modified German Volume Training Program on Muscular
Hypertrophy and Strength. J Strength Cond Res, 2017. 31(11): p. 3109-3119.
23. Hackett, D., et al., Effects of a 12-Week Modified German Volume Training Program on Muscle
Strength and Hypertrophy—A Pilot Study. Sports, 2018. 6(1): p. 7.
24. Montero, D. and C. Lundby, Refuting the myth of non-response to exercise training: ‘non-respond-
ers’ do respond to higher dose of training. The Journal of Physiology, 2017. 595(11): p. 3377-3387.

52
Study Reviewed: Strongman Log Push Press: The Effect Log Diameter has on
Force-Time Characteristics. Renals et al. (2018)

Log Press is a Grind,


Even for the Experts
BY G RE G NUC KO LS

There haven’t been many studies on overhead pressing exercises.


A recent study compared barbell push press to log push press in
competitive strongmen, finding that log press allows you to use the
same loads, but with slower speeds.

53
KEY POINTS
1. In this sample of competitive strongmen, the athletes could log press as much as
they could push press with a barbell.
2. Even with the same load, however, force, velocity, and power were lower with log
push press than barbell push press.
3. If you aim to compete in strongman, you should practice log press until you
can log press about as much as you push press. However, if you don’t aim to
compete in strongman, barbell push press seems to be the better option.

A Purpose and Research


mong the compound exercises that
most lifters care about, overhead
pressing gets the least love in the Questions
research. So, when the presently reviewed
study was published, I knew I had to tackle Purpose
it this month, since it compares the over- The purpose of this study was to com-
head pressing exercise that I personally pare the force-time characteristics of bar-
think is best (barbell push press) to the bell push press, push press with a small log,
overhead pressing exercise I personally en- and push press with a large log.
joy the most (log push press).
The athletes in this study were competi- Research Questions
tive strongmen who could, on average, push Would choice of pressing implement af-
press the same amount of weight with both fect push press impulse, force, power, dis-
a log and a barbell. The athletes completed placement, and phase length?
three reps with 65% of their 1RM using
a barbell, a small log, and a large log. The Hypotheses
researchers examined average force output,
It was hypothesized that impulse, mean
velocity, power, and impulse during the dip
force, and mean power would be greater
phase and the propulsive phase of the push
with barbell push press than log push press,
press with all three implements. Average
but that there would be no differences be-
force, power, velocity, and impulse were all
tween different sized logs. It was also hy-
higher with barbell push press than with
pothesized that displacement and lift du-
either of the logs, and force, power, velocity,
ration would be the same with all pressing
and impulse tended to be (non-significant-
implements.
ly) higher with the small log than with the
large log.

54
Table 1 Mean (±SD) subject physical characteristics and training experience

Demographic and training experience Mean ± SD

Age (years) 29.80 ± 3.68


Height (cm) 183.52 ± 6.31
Mass (kg) 116.04 ± 16.88
Weight (N) 1138.35 ± 165.55
Resistance training experience (years) 9.70 ± 5.70
Strongman training experience (years) 4.45 ± 2.61
Number of sessions (per week) 4.30 ± 0.95
Average training time per session (minutes) 99 ± 20.25
Log push press 1RM (kg) 145.45 ± 10.29
Barbell push press 1RM (kg) 145.70 ± 9.63
Barbell push press 65% 1RM (kg) 94.71 ± 6.26
Barbell push press 1RM relative to body mass 1.28 ± 0.22
Squat 1RM (kg) 244.50 ± 20.20
Squat 1RM relative to body mass 2.10 ± 0.30

Subjects and Methods force plates to measure force-time vari-


ables. The load used was 65% of their
max barbell push press. They performed
Subjects 3 sets of 1 rep with a barbell, small log
The subjects were 10 strongman com- (250mm diameter; 9.8 inches), and
petitors. They had an average of 9.7 large log (620mm diameter; 24.4 inch-
years of lifting experience and 4.45 es) in a randomized order. The athletes
years of strongman training experience. were not allowed to use a jerk tech-
Their average push press was approxi- nique. In a push press, you use your legs
mately 145kg (~320lb) with both a bar- to assist you in driving the bar upward
bell and a log. Further details about the at the start of the press and finish the
participants can be seen in Table 1. lift solely with your upper body, but you
don’t re-bend your legs after the initial
Experimental Methods dip. In a jerk, on the other hand, you
The subjects performed push press use your legs to assist you in driving the
from the rack (i.e. they didn’t have to bar upward, and you also drop under
clean the implements before the press) the bar (in either a split jerk or squat
with all three implements, standing on jerk position), catching it with your

55
elbows locked out. The athletes in this
study were only allowed to use the push
press technique. THE SHORTENED RANGE OF
Measures MOTION OF A LOG PRESS
The researchers measured impulse HELPS TO COMPENSATE
(force multiplied by time), average ve-
locity, average force, and average pow- FOR A SLOWER, GRIND-
er during each phase of the push press.
The lift was split into three phases: the IER PRESSING MOTION.
dip (from the initiation of the lift un-
til the bar started moving upward), the
braking phase (from the point of max- the logs, and there weren’t significant
imum downward velocity to the point differences between the two logs for
when the bar started moving upward), any variable measured. However, there
and the propulsive phase (when the bar were non-significant trends for many
was moving upward during the actual of these variables to be greater with
press). the small log than the large log, so
mean differences tended to show the
largest values when using a barbell,
Findings the smallest values when using the
Braking impulse, average braking large log, and middle-of-the-road val-
force, average braking velocity, average ues when using the small log (with the
braking power, propulsive impulse, av- small log values being slightly closer
erage propulsive force, average propul- to the large log values than the barbell
sive velocity, average propulsive power, values). Propulsion phase duration, dip
dip displacement, and propulsion dis- phase duration, and total lift duration
placement were all significantly great- didn’t significantly differ between im-
er with the barbell than with either of plements.

Table 2 Mean (±SD) push press braking parameters

Barbell Small log Big log

Braking impulse (N·s) 130.50 ± 27.29*† 116.05 ± 28.73 106.04 ± 27.83


Braking force (N) 775.38 ± 316.85 † 680.13 ± 262.43 625.12 ± 251.90
Braking velocity (m·s-1) -0.40 ± 0.05* † -0.36 ± 0.06 -0.33 ± 0.06
Braking power (W) -1087.34 ± 283.43* † -946.26 ± 280.97 -854.09 ± 275.88

* = Significant difference between barbell and small log (p<0.05)


† = Significant difference between barbell and big log (p<0.05)

56
This was a simple study with simple
findings, but I thought it was worth re-
BASED ON THE RESULTS viewing simply because there’s so little
research on overhead pressing. There’s
OF THIS STUDY, I THINK reams of research about the squat and
bench press and a reasonable amount of
BARBELL PUSH PRESS research about the deadlift, but overhead
pressing tends to get the short end of
WOULD BE A BETTER the stick, at least among the core com-
OPTION FOR MOST LIFTERS pound exercises most lifters care about.
If you’re interested in the research that
WHO AREN’T TRAINING TO we do have on overhead pressing, check
the reference list of this article; all of the
COMPETE IN STRONGMAN. major studies will be linked.
The finding that bar displacement dif-
fered between implements isn’t too sur-
prising. Since the handles of a log are
located in the middle of the log, range
Interpretation of motion should decrease by approxi-
The results of this study were pretty mately the radius of the bar. If a log has
straightforward once you strip away the an 18-inch diameter, you’d expect range
jargon. Lift duration didn’t differ, but of motion to be approximately 9 inch-
displacement, power, force, impulse, and es shorter than it would be for a barbell
velocity did. Thus, the lifters used a larg- push press.
er dip and had a longer press when using However, it is interesting that push
a barbell, but accomplished the lift just press and log press 1RMs were similar,
as fast, meaning they applied more force in spite of lower average force, power, ve-
to the barbell and moved it faster than locity, and impulse during the log press.
they moved the logs. This tells us that the shortened range of

Table 3 Mean (±SD) push press propulsion parameters

Barbell Small log Big log

Propulsive impulse (N·s) 293.08 ± 40.04*† 254.89 ± 38.79 241.19 ± 28.67


Propulsive force (N) 3399.02 ± 492.02*† 3232.74 ± 356.73 3131.29 ± 362.65
Propulsive velocity (m·s-1) 0.74 ± 0.07*† 0.64 ± 0.07 0.62 ± 0.06
Propulsive power (W) 2469.00 ± 481.85*† 2040.56 ± 376.70 1896.64 ± 295.12

* = Significant difference between barbell and small log (p<0.05)


† = Significant difference between barbell and big log (p<0.05)

57
motion of a log press helps to compen-
sate for a slower, grind-ier pressing mo-
tion. The athletes in this study didn’t dip YOUR LOG PRESS STRENGTH
quite as far before initiating the press
when using a log, which may contribute
CAN (AND PROBABLY
to the lower velocity and power output.
If you’ve ever done heavy log presses and
SHOULD) BE COMPARABLE
heavy barbell push presses, this shouldn’t TO YOUR BARBELL PUSH
come as a surprise. With a push press,
the bar can rest in a fully supported rack PRESS STRENGTH.
position when you dip, which allows you
to dip a bit lower and generate a lot of
power as you drive upward. With a log
are a bit safer as well, since the move-
press, on the other hand, the rack posi-
ment is a bit more controlled.
tion is pretty awkward, and you have to
dip a bit more gingerly so that the log On the flip side, this study also pro-
doesn’t start to fall forward or crumple vides us with good information for
you backward. strongman competitors. The kinetic dif-
ferences show us that barbell push press
As strongman gets more mainstream,
and log press are meaningfully different
log pressing is getting more popular.
movements and that log press proba-
However, based on the results of this
bly becomes increasingly difficult as log
study, I think barbell push press would
size increases. We also see that, in these
be a better option for most lifters who
highly trained strongmen, they could log
aren’t training to compete in strongman.
press as much as they could push press
Barbell push presses allow for a slightly
(which would be highly uncommon for
longer range of motion and higher force
non-strongmen). If you aim to compete
and power output with the same load;
in strongman, it would behoove you to
though this study didn’t quantify injury
include the log press in your training
risk, I’d assume that barbell push presses

Table 4 Displacement and time parameters of push press

Barbell Small log Big log

Dip displacement (m) -0.17 ± 0.04*† -0.14 ± 0.03 -0.13 ± 0.02


Propulsion displacement (m) 0.17 ± 0.03*† 0.14 ± 0.02 0.14 ± 0.02
Propulsion duration (s) 0.22 ± 0.03*† 0.22 ± 0.02 0.22 ± 0.02
Total duration (s) 0.54 ± 0.47*† 0.67 ± 0.06 0.64 ± 0.07

* = Significant difference between barbell and small log (p<0.05)


† = Significant difference between barbell and big log (p<0.05)

58
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
If you aim to compete in strongman, you absolutely need to practice log press, and
you should expect the reps to move a bit slower than barbell push press. However, if
you don’t plan on competing in strongman, barbell push press seems to be the better
option.

(and not just hope that your log press


strength goes up as your overall pressing
strength goes up), especially if you hav-
en’t yet mastered the movement. Your log
press strength can (and probably should)
be comparable to your barbell push press
strength; if it’s not, you should spend
some time hammering log press tech-
nique until your log press numbers catch
up to your barbell push press numbers,
since your performance is probably bot-
tlenecked moreso by technique than
strength.

Next Steps
Several studies have now investigated
the kinetics (force, power, etc.) of over-
head pressing, but I haven’t yet seen a
good kinematic (joint angles, bar path,
etc.) analysis of overhead pressing. Just
for the sake of thoroughly describing
and characterizing the lift, that would be
an important contribution to the litera-
ture. A study comparing overhead press
kinematics of expert and novice press-
ers would also help coaches objectively
critique their athletes’ overhead pressing
technique.

59
References
1. Renals L, Lake J, Keogh J, Austin K. Strongman Log Push Press: The Effect Log Diameter has on
Force-Time Characteristics. J Strength Cond Res. 2018 Jul 26.
2. Shinkle J, Nesser TW, Demchak TJ, McMannus DM. Effect of core strength on the measure of
power in the extremities. J Strength Cond Res. 2012 Feb;26(2):373-80.
3. Dicus JR, Holmstrup ME, Shuler KT, Rice TT, Raybuck SD, Siddons CA. Stability of Resistance
Training Implement alters EMG Activity during the Overhead Press. Int J Exerc Sci. 2018 Jun
1;11(1):708-716.
4. Williams MR Jr, Hendricks DS, Dannen MJ, Arnold AM, Lawrence MA. Activity of Shoulder Sta-
bilizers and Prime Movers During an Unstable Overhead Press. J Strength Cond Res. 2018 Jun 8.
5. Saeterbakken AH, Fimland MS. Effects of body position and loading modality on muscle activity
and strength in shoulder presses. J Strength Cond Res. 2013 Jul;27(7):1824-31.
6. Kalb JS, Hunter GR. Weight training economy as a function of intensity of the squat and overhead
press exercise. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 1991 Jun;31(2):154-60.
7. Uribe BP, Coburn JW, Brown LE, Judelson DA, Khamoui AV, Nguyen D. Muscle activation when
performing the chest press and shoulder press on a stable bench vs. a Swiss ball. J Strength Cond
Res. 2010 Apr;24(4):1028-33.
8. Ichihashi N, Ibuki S, Otsuka N, Takashima S, Matsumura A. Kinematic characteristics of the scap-
ula and clavicle during military press exercise and shoulder flexion. J Shoulder Elbow Surg. 2014
May;23(5):649-57.
9. Büll ML, Vitti M, Freitas V, Rosa GJ. Electromyographic validation of the trapezius and serratus
anterior muscles in military press exercises with middle grip. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol. 2001
Jul-Aug;41(5):263-8.
10. Lake JP, Mundy PD, Comfort P. Power and impulse applied during push press exercise. J Strength
Cond Res. 2014 Sep;28(9):2552-9.
11. Grabe SA, Widule CJ. Comparative Biomechanics of the Jerk in Olympic Weightlifting. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1988; 59:1, 1-8.
12. McGill SM, McDermott A, Fenwick CM. Comparison of different strongman events: trunk muscle
activation and lumbar spine motion, load, and stiffness. J Strength Cond Res. 2009 Jul;23(4):1148-
61.
13. Paoli A, Marcolin G, Petrone N. Influence of different ranges of motion on selective recruitment
of shoulder muscles in the sitting military press: an electromyographic study. J Strength Cond Res.
2010 Jun;24(6):1578-83.
14. Winwood PW, Cronin JB, Posthumus LR, Finlayson SJ, Gill ND, Keogh JW. Strongman vs. tradi-
tional resistance training effects on muscular function and performance. J Strength Cond Res. 2015
Feb;29(2):429-39.
15. Winwood PW, Cronin JB, Brown S, Keogh JW. A biomechanical analysis of the strongman log lift
and comparison with weightlifting’s clean and jerk. Dissertation, Bond University. 2016.

60
Study Reviewed: Reduced Volume “Daily Max” Training Compared to Higher
Volume Periodized Training in Powerlifters Preparing for Competition – A Pilot Study.
Androulakis-Korakakis et al. (2018)

New Data Makes Cases Both For


and Against 1RM-Type Training
BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S

Data have shown that maxing out every day or just a few times a week
substantially boosts strength, but does it work as well as typical periodized
training? This study compares just maxing a few times per week to
periodized training in powerlifters preparing for a competition.

61
KEY POINTS
1. This paper examined the influence of typical multi-set periodized training and 1RM
training (i.e. maxing 1-3 times per week) in competitive powerlifters preparing for
a meet. The study lasted 11 weeks, and the researchers measured squat, bench
press, and deadlift strength.
2. Both protocols had varying effects, with some lifters improving performance on
competition day and some lifters underperforming compared to pre-testing.
3. Frequent maxing or 1RM-type training can be effective; however, research is still in
its infancy. Thus, at this point in time, it would be inappropriate to recommend it as
a standalone training strategy. Further, this study only had eight subjects, making
it difficult to make between-group comparisons.

D
aily one-repetition maximum in untrained individuals. Both groups in
(1RM) training is the stuff of this study experienced similar strength
legend. Does it really work? Did gains. However, these data in untrained
lifters really survive this? The first study individuals using machine-based training
to truly examine this concept was pub- is not as applicable as the original dai-
lished just two years ago (2), and while it ly 1RM training study on the squat in
showed promise, it was only a case series competitive lifters. The current study (1)
with three participants. As I discussed in compared 1RM training a few times per
a previous MASS video – which I would week (1-3 times per week depending on
recommend watching before moving for- the specific lift) on all three power lifts
ward with this article – daily 1RM train- against traditional periodized training
ing is clearly effective; however, many over 10 weeks in Greek powerlifters. The
questions remain, and one should be sample size (1RM group, n=5; periodized
cautious before embarking on that jour- group, n=3) was quite low, and of the
ney. Among the remaining questions are: three subjects in the periodized group,
1) Does 1RM training have to be every two of them increased their powerlifting
single day? and 2) Does it actually out- total, while the other lifter maintained his
perform typical periodized training with total. Out of the five subjects in the 1RM
higher volumes? group, four of them increased their total
Last year, Buckner et al (3) conducted from pre-testing to the peak point for
an eight-week study comparing twice- each lift; however, from pre- to post-test-
per-week 1RM training to a program ing, three subjects in the 1RM group ac-
involving 4 sets of 8-12 reps twice per tually decreased their powerlifting total.
week on the chest press and leg extension I believe the decreases in total were not

62
Table 1 Subject characteristics
Body Mass Training Age Squat 1RM Bench 1RM Deadlift 1RM
Age (years) Height (cm)
(kg) (years) (kg) (kg) (kg)
27 ± 6 90.5 ± 16.7 178.9 ± 6.2 2-4 177.5 ± 28.29 123.13 ± 18.84 192.5 ± 36.65

Data are Mean ± SD


Subject characteristics from Androulakis-Korakakis et al. 2018 (1)

due to lost strength, but rather a func- lifters who followed a normal periodized
tion of the post-testing being conducted program in the 11 weeks leading up to a
at an actual competition (more on that competition.
later). In general, I’m not sure how much
new knowledge this study contributes to Research Questions
the literature; however, it does illustrate 1. How effective is performing 1RM
that in the short term, trained lifters can training a few times per week on
maintain strength by simply performing a squat, bench, and deadlift when
near max a few times per week. However, preparing for a competition?
does maxing a few times per week work
2. How does 1RM training compare
as well as maxing every day? How would
to normal periodized training when
this play out in the long term compared
preparing for a powerlifting meet?
to typical periodized training? Lastly, if
you were to perform 1RM training every
Hypothesis
day, could you really do it for all three lifts
at the same time? In addition to inter- A formal hypothesis was not given, but
preting the present data, this article will it seems clear that the authors expected
also take a stab at those questions. the 1RM group to get stronger. How-
ever, it’s not clear which group they ex-
pected to perform better. For the record,
Purpose and Research I would have predicted similar adaptations
between the groups, but a large variability
Questions in responses between individuals.

Purpose
The purpose of this study was to exam- Subjects and Methods
ine changes in squat, bench press, and
deadlift strength in powerlifters who Subjects
maxed a few times per week and power- Ten men with a powerlifting experi-

63
Table 2A Training protocol every week for the daily 1RM group

Lift Monday Wednesday Friday

Squat Single @ 9-9.5 RPE Single @ 9-9.5 RPE


Bench Single @ 9-9.5 RPE Single @ 9-9.5 RPE Single @ 9-9.5 RPE
Deadlift Single @ 9-9.5 RPE

1RM = One-repetition maximum. RPE = rating of perceived exertion

ence of 2 ± 1 years and a general training on the other, an aspect that improves
experience of 5 ± 2 years participated. real-world application. Changes in
However, two subjects in the periodized strength from pre- to peak (the highest
group did not complete the study. The 1RM performed in training) were also
paper reported anthropometrics on all calculated in the 1RM group.
ten, and only reported strength levels on
the eight who finished. So, in Table 1, Training Protocol
the anthropometric characteristics are Both groups trained three times per
from ten subjects, while the strength week. Squat was performed twice per
levels are from eight subjects. week, bench press three times per week,
and deadlift once a week. The 1RM
Overall Design group simply worked up to one set of
All subjects trained for 10 weeks (the one rep at a 9-9.5RPE each time a lift
study lasted 11 weeks total with pre- and was performed. The periodized group
post-testing) in either the periodized performed a four-week prep phase, fol-
(n=3) or 1RM group (n=5). Pre-train- lowed by four weeks of transition, and
ing 1RMs for the squat, bench press, then a two-week peaking block. In the
and deadlift were tested in the laborato- periodized group, subjects were instruct-
ry, while post-training 1RM was tested ed not to take any set beyond a 9RPE.
at the Hellenic Powerlifting Federation The training protocols are in Tables 2AB.
(HPF – Greece’s International Pow-
erlifting Federation affiliate) national
championships. We’ll discuss that last Findings
detail more later, but post-testing occur- With only five subjects in one group
ring in a different setting than pre-test- (daily 1RM) and three in the other (pe-
ing (i.e. at a meet versus in the lab) is, on riodized), it wasn’t appropriate to present
one hand, a significant limitation, and typical statistics (i.e. ANOVA) to make

64
Table 2B Program in the periodized group

Lift Week 1 Week 2 Week 3 Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Week 7 Week 8 Week 9 Week 10

Squat
Number of sets 4-6 7-8 6-7 7-8 5-6 5-6 4-5 6-7 4-5 3-5
Repetition range 2-3 2-3 2-3 2-5 2-3 2-3 2-3 1-3 2-3 1-2
%1RM range 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 75-85% 70-85% 70-80% 70-85% 70-90% 70-90% 70-93%
Sessions 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2
Bench press
Number of sets 6-15 7-8 7-9 7-8 6-7 7-11 8-10 7-9 3-5 3-5
Repetition range 3-4 3-4 1-4 2-3 2-3 1-7 2-3 2-3 2-3 1-3
%1RM range 70-85% 70-85% 70-90% 70-85% 70-85% 55-90% 70-85% 70-85% 70-90% 70-93%
Sessions 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3
Deadlift
Number of sets 6 8 9 8 6 5 6 6 5 3
Repetition range 2-3 2-3 1-3 2-3 3 2-3 2-3 3 1-2 1
%1RM range 70-85% 70-80% 70-85% 70-85% 70-80% 70-80% 70-90% 70-80% 70-90% 70%
Sessions 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1

This table shows the specific ranges for number of sets, reps, and intensities that were performed each week for the squat, bench press, and deadlift. In each week the
squat was performed twice, the bench press three times, and the deadlift once.

between-group comparisons. Rather, provided a clear benefit over the other.


a sample size such as this allows us to Finally, the average RPE was 8.9 ± 0.4
present individual responses. Thus, Table in the 1RM group and 8.4 ± 0.1 in the
3AB presents the individual response periodized group. The periodized group
for all subjects. performed substantially more volume
Two of the three subjects in the peri- (as expected) than the 1RM group.
odized group increased their total, and
one lifter had no change in total. In the
1RM group, four out of five lifters im- Interpretation
proved their powerlifting total from pre Overall, it’s awesome that this study was
to peak 1RM, with one subject experi- done. It is one of the first in what I pre-
encing a decrease. However, three sub- dict will be a trend of researchers investi-
jects actually decreased their total from gating some form of frequent maxing (or
pre- to post-testing, while one subject near-maxing) versus more “traditional”
had the same total, and the other had a protocols. The totality of results suggests
small increase in total. We’ll elaborate that both training styles have merit in the
on this later; however, it’s unlikely that short-term; however, the significant lim-
those three subjects in the 1RM group itation of post-testing occuring at a com-
truly got weaker. Rather, it’s more like- petition makes it difficult to infer a lot re-
ly a function of post-testing being con- garding true strength changes. Therefore,
ducted at an actual competition. With let’s examine the impact that limitation
such a limited sample size and limita- may have had on the results, then infer
tions of the post-testing measurement, I what we can from this study.
think it’s fair to say that neither program

65
Table 3A Individual strength changes in the periodized group

Measure Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3

Squat
Pre 1RM (kg) 200 175 175
Post 1RM (kg) 205 175 180
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 2.5 0 2.8
Bench press
Pre 1RM (kg) 145 120 140
Post 1RM (kg) 147.5 120 140
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 1.7 0 0
Deadlift
Pre 1RM (kg) 230 165 200
Post 1RM (kg) 235 165 230
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 2.1 0 14
Total
Pre 1RM (kg) 575 460 515
Post 1RM (kg) 587 460 550
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 2 0 6.5
1RM = One-repetition maximum

Limitations of the Present Study post-test than at pre-test. This lifter’s


Post-testing occurring at an actual bench press also decreased by 15kg at
competition is a limitation because dif- the competition; however, I would be
ferent conditions were present at pre- willing to bet that such a large decrease
and post-testing. Importantly, the point in 11 weeks was more a function of poor
of an actual competition is not always attempt selection on meet day than ac-
to perform a 1RM. Each individual has tual strength loss. That isn’t to say that
different goals: some lifters may be per- true strength may not have decreased in
forming a true 1RM, some are aiming some individuals in a specific lift; how-
to go 9 for 9, while others may be con- ever, we can’t know for sure, due to how
cerned with placing. In the 1RM group, post-testing was conducted. On the oth-
only two subjects increased their total at er hand, one could make the case that
the competition; however, four of them carrying out post-testing at a competi-
increased their total at its “peak” point tion could increase ecological validity;
during the study. For example, partici- however, it still makes it tough to com-
pant four in the 1RM group (Table 3B) pare true strength changes. If there were
increased his total by 11kg from pre to velocities or RPEs on the post-testing
peak; however, he lifted 20kg less at numbers, then we could predict what
the true 1RM change was. Without

66
Table 3B Individual strength changes in the periodized group

Measure Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4 Participant 5

Squat
Pre 1RM (kg) 145 210 145 215 155
Peak 1RM (kg) 155 217.5 145 217.5 155
Post 1RM (kg) 155 210 150 215 155
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 6.6 0 3.3 0 0
Pre-Peak 1RM Δ (%) 6.6 3.5 0 1.1 0
Week of Peak 1RM 8 9 7 6 5
Bench press
Pre 1RM (kg) 100 135 120 132.5 92.5
Peak 1RM (kg) 100 142.5 120 135 92.5
Post 1RM (kg) 100 140 112.5 117.5 82.5
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 0 3.6 -6.4 -12 -11.4
Pre-Peak 1RM Δ (%) 0 5.4 0 1.8 0
Week of Peak 1RM 1 4 4 3 3
Deadlift
Pre 1RM (kg) 160 230 160 240 155
Peak 1RM (kg) 175 240 180 246 150
Post 1RM (kg) 170 250 160 235 145
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 6 8.3 0 -2.1 -6.6
Pre-Peak 1RM Δ (%) 9 4.2 11.7 2.4 -3.2
Week of Peak 1RM 9 6 4 5 7
Total
Pre 1RM (kg) 405 575 425 587.5 402.5
Peak 1RM (kg) 420 600 445 598.5 397.5
Post 1RM (kg) 425 600 422.5 567.5 382.5
Pre-Post 1RM Δ (%) 4.8 4.2 -0.5 -3.4 -5
Pre-Peak 1RM Δ (%) 3.6 4.2 4.5 1.8 -1.2

1RM = One-repetition maximum

that, though, I think the limitations of ed plates were used at the meet versus
competition data override the ecolog- non-calibrated plates in training; and, of
ical validity argument. To the authors’ course, the competition atmosphere can
credit, they mention this limitation. A also affect performance. Finally, judging
secondary limitation, but still important, standards at the meet could have been
is that equipment was likely different at different than in a laboratory setting,
the competition. Specifically, since this which could also skew the results. The
federation is an IPF affiliate, it’s possible criteria for a “good” lift is objective, but
that a stiffer barbell was used at compe- the judging is subjective, thus we must
tition than was used in training; calibrat- add this to the list of limitations.

67
A sample size of only eight subjects is
obviously another limitation. It is true
that it’s difficult to recruit subjects for a IN THE SHORT-TERM,
daily 1RM training study, and it’s dif-
ficult for the investigators to conduct BOTH TRAINING
1RM testing every single day (Chad
Dolan and I know this all too well); STYLES CAN WORK.
however, this study only involved three
near-max sessions a week. Further, the
periodization protocol was pretty typi-
cal. This means that the data collection for some of the “decreases” in strength,
would not have been that arduous. It’s but we should also explore other pos-
more likely that it was difficult to re- sibilities. It is possible that volume was
cruit lifters because the authors needed too low for too long in the 1RM group,
powerlifters who were competing at this and the novelty of the frequent maxing
specific competition and needed the lift- wasn’t effective anymore as competition
ers to start the study exactly 11 weeks approached – hence the increase in peak
beforehand. Furthermore, it’s tough to 1RM, but decrease at the competition.
convince serious lifters to let you put For the periodized group, the same lim-
them on a cookie-cutter training pro- itations exist for post-testing, which may
gram for a study when there’s nothing have caused one lifter to not gain any
on the line; it’s bound to be even harder strength compared to pre-testing (Ta-
to convince a serious lifter to turn their ble 3A). However, the other two lifters
meet prep over to a research study. In in the periodized group gained 12.5 and
that case, the small sample size makes 35kg on their total, which is quite good
sense. for 11 weeks. Therefore, if I was forced
to choose if one protocol was more ef-
Present Results fective than the other in this study, I
My main conclusion is that, in the would choose the periodized program,
short-term, both training styles can work. as it seemed to prepare the subjects
My hunch is that the lifters weren’t used better for competition. At least the pe-
to maxing frequently; thus, maxing a riodized protocol is a “safer” approach
couple of times per week boosted neuro- when peaking, as 1RM-type training is
muscular adaptations, which led to some an unknown for most. In fact, the lifters
impressive increases from pre to peak seemed to agree, as they were asked on
(Table 3A). I do believe that post-test- a 5-point Likert scale (higher responses
ing at the meet is the primary reason are positive) how effective the protocol

68
per week, they might not see much – if
any – progress. In short, different train-
IT IS ALSO UNLIKELY THAT ing styles are appropriate at different
times, and not everyone will respond the
SOMEONE WOULD BE ABLE same way.
TO MAX ON ALL THREE LIFTS When comparing the results of the
1RM group in this study versus the pre-
EVERY DAY AND SUSTAIN vious daily 1RM training study, the pre-
vious study shows slightly greater and
IT FOR A SUBSTANTIAL more consistent strength improvement
(2). Specifically, the previous study saw
AMOUNT OF TIME. well-trained lifters increase their squats
by 5.8, 9.5, and 10.8% over 37 days.
However, the previous study was quite
different from this one, in that subjects
was for optimizing performance at the maxed every day (36 out of 37 days) and
competition, and the average response also performed 5 volume sets at 85%
in the periodized group was 4.3, versus (for 3 reps) or 90% (for 2 reps) of 1RM
the average response of 2.8 in the daily after the daily max. Further, the previ-
1RM group. Obviously, the sample size ous study only had subjects max in the
is small, but the competition results do squat, as opposed to all three lifts. These
seem to match up with the subjects’ per- discrepancies highlight that there is a
ceptions. lot to learn regarding optimizing 1RM
We also must note that responses training. We don’t know if maxing just
are highly individual. To illustrate this, a couple of times per week can increase
two subjects in the daily 1RM group strength to the same degree as max-
increased their totals by 20 and 25kg, ing every day. We also don’t know how
which included an increase from 230- great of a role the volume in the origi-
250kg in the deadlift for one lifter. We nal study played in improving strength.
frequently see a high degree of individual It is also unclear how sustainable 1RM
variability in the training response. This training is over the long term. If truly
is due, in part, to the fact that the style of pressed, I would say that in a trained
training a lifter does before a study im- lifter who has the sole goal of improving
pacts their results during the study, yet their 1RM, the best way to increase that
this is rarely discussed by authors. For in one month is to max every day (or at
example, if someone was maxing every least frequently); however, the best way
day and then started maxing three times to increase 1RM over the long term is

69
to follow a periodized approach, during lifters in the 1RM group (squat = +3.7%,
which daily 1RM training would sim- bench press = +3.6%, deadlift = +4.8%,
ply comprise one mesocycle every now total = +2.58%). This is interesting be-
and then. Admittedly, this conclusion cause the squat, bench, and deadlift were
is somewhat speculative and is built on trained with different frequencies. This
what both science and experience have either means that a single 1RM ses-
taught me not only about adaptation, sion per week is sufficient (used on the
but also feasibility. Ultimately, we need deadlift), or the optimal frequency dif-
more long-term data on 1RM training fers between the squat, bench press, and
to truly decide if 1RM training for an deadlift. I suspect it’s the latter, but not
extended period of time (i.e. >6 months) only for the obvious reason of different
is a good idea, and practicality must be recovery times between the three lifts. I
considered. think that an individual’s biomechanics
Speaking of feasibility, it is also unlike- play a role in the optimal frequency of
ly that someone would be able to max 1RM training. For example, if some-
on all three lifts every day and sustain it one’s biomechanics cause a fair amount
for a substantial amount of time. It’s one of unavoidable forward lean in the squat,
thing to perform daily 1RM training on then back squatting to a max may not be
one lift daily, but performing it on all advisable on a frequent basis, and could
three lifts is a different story. It would actually be a detriment to performance.
be more manageable to max on all three The point is: Individual factors must be
lifts without performing any volume or considered if embarking on max-type
“back off sets” afterward, but I do think training.
some volume is beneficial. Therefore, if Ultimately, my position on daily 1RM
performing true daily 1RM training, it or even frequent 1RM training as a sole
probably makes the most sense to focus method of training is largely unchanged
on one lift for a month and just aim to from the daily 1RM video in MASS.
maintain the other two. Frequent maxing should not be used as
A positive aspect of this study was that a standalone strategy over the long term.
it examined pre to peak changes in 1RM We simply don’t have enough long-term
between the different lifts in the 1RM data to make this claim. I will acknowl-
group. When looking at Tables 3A and edge, though, that my position could
3B, I don’t think there is a clear trend change in the future. For now, though,
that shows that results on one lift were I think it would be inappropriate to rec-
any better than another; however, it does ommend only 1RM-type training based
seem that all three lifts responded simi- on the totality of available data. Finally,
larly when examining the average of all I always feel compelled to recommend

70
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
1. Maxing on a lift a few times per week may causes similar strength gains to performing
typical multi-set volume training over a mesocycle.
2. Post-testing being conducted at a competition is a major limitation; however, it is
worth noting that a majority of participants in the 1RM group underperformed at the
competition. This could have simply been due to poor attempt selection, but the
athletes indicated that they thought the periodized program prepared them better
than the 1RM protocol.
3. Ultimately, I believe sustainability and feasibility must play a role when choosing
whether to implement 1RM-type training. For now, it should be used sparingly and
certainly isn’t a standalone strategy. Further, it remains to be seen if maxing a few
times per week is as effective as maxing every day in the short term. I suspect that
maxing every day is more beneficial, but also comes at a potentially greater cost.

extreme caution before deciding to per- works, but we need a direct comparison
form a daily lRM-type program. I elab- with standardized post-testing. Further,
orate more on this in the linked video. only one study (an older study from
1981) has compared different week-
ly frequencies of 1RM daily maxing: 1,
Next Steps 2, 3, 4, or 5 days per week performing
We need a longer study with more 1RM training on the bench press, ob-
subjects. I’d like to see three groups serving a response scaling with frequen-
training for a long-term training study: cy (4). However, that certainly needs up-
1) A typical periodized group, 2) Daily dating, and the above proposal would be
1RM group, and 3) A group performing the first update to that concept in almost
a 1RM 2-3 times per week. I also think 40 years. Following that proposed study,
that both 1RM groups should perform many research questions remain: Does
back-off sets for volume. Further, the the volume of back-off sets following a
1RM groups should only max on the 1RM really matter? How long is 1RM
squat, as we currently have the most ev- training sustainable? What are the psy-
idence on that lift. While future stud- chological responses to performing pro-
ies on all three lifts will be beneficial, it longed 1RM training? And are there
is prudent in research to go from point differences in how frequently the squat,
A to point B. We know that daily 1RM bench, and deadlift should be performed
training with volume sets works, and we in a 1RM-type program? We’ll get to all
know that normal periodized training of them … eventually.

71
References
1. Androulakis-Korakakis P, Fisher J, Kolokotronis P, Gentil P, Steele J. Reduced Volume ‘Daily Max’
Training Compared to Higher Volume Periodized Training in Powerlifters Preparing for Competi-
tion—A Pilot Study. Sports. 2018 Aug 29;6(3):86.
2. Zourdos MC, Dolan C, Quiles JM, Klemp A, Jo E, Loenneke JP, Blanco R, Whitehurst M. Efficacy
of daily one-repetition maximum training in well-trained powerlifters and weightlifters: a case se-
ries. Nutrición Hospitalaria. 2016;33(2).
3. Mattocks KT, Buckner SL, Jessee MB, Dankel SJ, Mouser JG, Loenneke JP. Practicing the Test Pro-
duces Strength Equivalent to Higher Volume Training. Medicine and science in sports and exercise.
2017 Sep;49(9):1945-54.
4. McKenzie GG. Effects of frequency of weight training on muscle strength enhancement. The Jour-
nal of sports medicine and physical fitness. 1981 Dec;21(4):432-6.

72
Study Reviewed: Caffeine Transiently Affects Food Intake at Breakfast.
Panek-Shirley et al. (2018)

Is Caffeine an Effective
Appetite Suppressant?
BY E RI C HE LMS

It’s common for caffeine to be included in fat-loss supplements as an appetite


suppressant, but does it actually work?

73
KEY POINTS
1. In the present randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover trial, 0,
1, and 3 mg/kg of caffeine were given to participants. Following administration,
energy intake at a buffet breakfast in the lab was assessed. Researchers also
assessed sensations of hunger and energy intake outside of the lab for the rest of
the day.
2. The participants consumed about 10% fewer calories in the lab-provided buffet
breakfast when they were in the 1mg/kg caffeine condition, compared to the 0
and 3 mg/kg caffeine conditions. However, total energy intake for the entire day
was not different between conditions, suggesting that appetite suppression was
transient and that reduced energy intake at breakfast was compensated for later
in the day.
3. The overall evidence for caffeine as an appetite suppressant is not promising, and
even in studies where an effect is shown, the absolute reduction in energy is either
too small to be meaningful or transient in nature. However, caffeine does have
other benefits related to suppressing tiredness and enhancing lifting performance,
which may be desirable while dieting (when fatigue is common).

C
affeine is frequently marketed energy at breakfast than the other con-
as an appetite suppressant or ditions (by about 10%), total 24-hour
fat burner and included in fat- energy intake was not significantly dif-
loss supplements for this reason. While ferent between conditions, indicating
caffeine is one of the most well-studied that the appetite suppression is transient
supplements, its impact on appetite sup- and compensated for in other meals. In
pression is actually one of its least-stud- this review, I’ll discuss caffeine as a po-
ied proposed effects. In the present study tential aid for dieters, what it does and
(1), 50 adult men and women consumed doesn’t do, and how this study sits with
either 0 (placebo), 1, or 3 mg/kg of caf- the rest of the literature on this topic.
feine as a part of a flavored beverage 30
minutes prior to consuming a buffet
breakfast in the lab. Energy intake at Purpose and Research
breakfast was measured, as well as sub-
jective hunger sensations. Then, the par-
Questions
ticipants recorded how much food they
Purpose
consumed for the rest of the day outside
of the lab. While the 1 mg/kg caffeine The purpose of this study was to exam-
condition consumed significantly less ine the impact of acute caffeine adminis-

74
Table 1 Baseline characteristics

BMI < 25 BMI 25-35

Characteristic Mean ± SD N (%) Mean ± SD N (%) P value


Gender (% male) 32 (34) 19 (53)
Age (y) 24.9 ± 1.2 25.7 ± 1.9 0.60
BMI 21.8 ± 1.9 29.1 ± 3.7 <0.001
Weight (kg) 63.8 ± 11.8 84.8 ± 15.7 <0.001
Waist (cm) 75.1 ± 8.5 88.8 ± 12.6 <0.001
Waist-to-hip ratio 0.79 ± 0.06 0.84 ± 0.06 0.01
200.4 ± 205.2 163.2 ± 134.8 0.48

0 N/A N/A
1 47-104 62-129
3 140-311 185-388
Three factor eating questionnaire
Restraint 8.8 ± 5.7 12.2 ± 4.9 0.04
Disinhibition 4.9 ± 3.4 6.0 ± 2.9 0.24
Hunger 4.4 ± 3.2 5.7 ± 3.2 0.17
Binge eating scale 7.2 ± 5.4 9.6 ± 6.1 0.16

Baseline characteristics of 18- to 50-year old men and women (n=51) participating in a clinical trial of dose response to acute caffeine
administration on ad libitum laboratory and free-living energy intake and appetite.

tration on energy intake at breakfast and Fifty adult men and women (21 males,
throughout the remainder of the day in 29 females) between the ages of 18-50
adults. completed this placebo-controlled, ran-
domized, crossover trial. Each partici-
Hypothesis pant visited the laboratory for breakfast
The authors made three hypotheses: on three different occasions. This oc-
(1) caffeine reduces ad libitum labora- curred at the same time of day, on the
tory and free-living energy intake in a same day of the week, for three consec-
dose-dependent manner; (2) caffeine utive weeks to complete each condition.
exerts its effects on energy intake by re- Participant characteristics are shown in
ducing hunger and desire to eat; and (3) Table 1, organized by participants over
the effects of caffeine on appetite and and under a 25 body mass index (BMI),
energy intake are affected by the level of as the authors hypothesized that adipos-
adiposity of the individual. ity would impact the tested outcomes.
Aside from baseline characteristics re-
lated to adiposity (weight, waist to hip
Subjects and Methods ratio, etc.), there were no differences in
demographic characteristics that could
Subjects and Study Design plausibly affect eating behavior such as

75
Table 2 Energy intake organized by body mass index

BMIb < 25 (n=32) BMI 25-35 (n=19)

Intake 0 mg/kg 1 mg/kg 3 mg/kg 0 mg/kg 1 mg/kg 3 mg/kg P valuec d e

Total (kcal) 675.6 ± 438.3 538.2 ± 270.1 715.0 ± 629.5 832.3 ± 461.8 846.3 ± 446.9 739.5 ± 448.4 0.05 0.23 0.49
Carbohydrates (g) 100.0 ± 61.2 83.8 ± 41.4 106.3 ± 79.8 110.7 ± 55.0 111.4 ± 57.7 100.1 ± 51.7 0.42 0.16 0.07
Fats (g) 20.9 ± 20.7 14.4 ± 12.7 20.2 ± 20.5 31.5 ± 24.9 31.4 ± 21.8 26.3 ± 23.1 0.09 0.30 0.08
Proteins (g) 25.7 ± 18.0 21.9 ± 11.4 26.6 ± 25.5 29.7 ± 17.7 32.7 ± 22.8 28.4 ± 22.2 0.46 0.17 0.03
Total intake for entire day
Total (kcal) 2161.8 ± 935.1 1921.5 ± 869.7 2067.7 ± 1092.5 2438.2 ± 900.0 2546.4 ± 1050.0 2225.9 ± 910.1 0.237 0.21 0.05
Carbohydrates (g) 287.0 ± 120.1 256.3 ± 136.0 278.3 ± 133.7 300.3 ± 103.7 292.8 ± 105.3 264.4 ± 112.2 0.457 0.13 0.07
Fats (g) 75.1 ± 44.6 63.0 ± 47.5 71.0 ± 65.0 96.0 ± 49.2 99.9 ± 49.9 85.1 ± 39.3 0.074 0.27 0.05
Proteins (g) 85.2 ± 46.9 84.7 ± 33.1 80.5 ± 34.0 91.3 ± 35.1 111.5 ± 46.4 93.6 ± 43.1 0.075 0.25 0.11

Data are unadjusted mean ± standard deviation for total calories and macronutrients (grams) consumed.
b = BMI = body mass index (calculated as kg/m2); c = p-values from analysis of covariance with BMI group, caffeine treatment, and usual caffeine intake as the independent variables;
d = effect size calculated from unadjusted mean and standard deviation of total calories and macronutrients by BMI and caffeine treatment; e = effect size calculated from unadjusted
mean and standard deviation of total calories and macronutrients by caffeine treatment only

income, ethnicity, menstrual cycle phase, Thirty minutes after beverage con-
or education. Eating behavior question- sumption, the participants were pro-
naires at baseline did show a significant vided a buffet style breakfast and ate as
difference in dietary restraint between much as they wanted. Energy intake was
those with a BMI > 25 compared to recorded by the researchers, and subjec-
those under, which is a common find- tive scores for appetite and hunger were
ing; overweight individuals often have a recorded by participants via a Likert
more restrained attitude toward food, as scale. Participants had their perceptions
they are frequently trying to lose weight of hunger and appetite anchored (points
or prevent further weight gain. on the scale intentionally associated with
subjective feelings of hunger) prior to
Controls the study start, and participants also re-
Participants were required to not con- corded scores using a subset of the Pro-
sume caffeine in the 24 hours prior to file of Mood States questionnaire before
each testing session, and participants and after beverage and breakfast con-
were informed that caffeine abstinence sumption. Participants then left the lab
would be confirmed via salivary testing. and continued their normal day, eating
Saliva was collected (simply as a deter- freely; however, they were required to
rent) but not analyzed. All conditions complete an entry in their dietary habit
received a cold, flavor-of-choice bever- books (which they were trained to use)
age with either a caffeine additive or a at every eating and drinking occasion
bitter flavor-matched placebo additive for the remainder of the day, recording
to control for the effect of palatability on foods, portions, and subjective scores for
subsequent food intake. appetite and hunger.

Outcome Testing

76
Findings
Table 2 displays the energy and mac-
THIS SHOWS THAT EVEN
ronutrient intakes organized by BMI, as IF A TRANSIENT APPETITE
the authors hypothesized BMI would
moderate the effect of caffeine on energy SUPPRESSION OCCURS, IT IS
intake. However, contrary to the authors’
hypothesis, BMI did not alter caffeine’s
COMPENSATED FOR LATER IN
impact on energy intake. When grouped THE DAY, IN MOST CASES.
together, the 1 mg/kg caffeine treatment
had a moderate effect on mean energy
intake at breakfast with their consump-
tion of 650.4 ± 52.2 kcal being signifi- lation effects fade with caffeine toler-
cantly lower (p = 0.046) than the 0 mg/ ance, suppression of tiredness and the
kg (721.2 ± 63.2) or 3 mg/kg (714.7 ± primary mechanisms by which caffeine
79.0) conditions. However, there were enhances lifting performance (pain sup-
no significant differences between con- pression, reduction of perceived effort,
ditions in subjective ratings of hunger and improved calcium handling) largely
and appetite or in total energy intake for remain, even in habitual caffeine con-
the remaining portion of the day. sumers. Most recently, in Volume 1 Is-
sue 9, Mike further clarified caffeine’s
effect on lifting performance, noting
Interpretation that the most consistent performance
Caffeine has many notable effects that improvements occur when consuming 6
we’ve previously covered in MASS. As a mg/kg one hour before training. Mike
recap, Greg, Mike, and I have all written cautioned that you should consume the
about caffeine and its various effects and minimum effective dose, though, as caf-
their nuances. Specifically, in Volume 1 feine can negatively affect sleep (if con-
Issue 3, Greg described the effect of caf- sumed too late in the day) and can cause
feine on strength, noting that at a dose of discomfort and jittery sensations at high
~3-6 mg/kg, increases in 1RM strength doses.
and repetitions to failure at submaximal In contrast, in this article, we examine
loads occur, but that these outcomes are a more tenuous claim: that caffeine sup-
most reliable in lower body exercises. In presses appetite and could subsequently
Volume 1 Issue 6, I discussed the effects result in greater fat loss due to a reduc-
of caffeine tolerance on performance, tion in energy intake. Simply put, the
noting that while the cognitive stimu- current study doesn’t support that no-

77
It’s worth noting that caffeine and cof-
fee are not synonymous. We could ar-
WHILE CAFFEINE MAY NOT gue that coffee is somewhat of a distinct
substance containing other bioactive
HELP WITH THE HUNGER phytonutrients, even if the active ingre-
PANGS, IT CAN CERTAINLY TAKE dient (caffeine) is the cause of its prima-
ry effects. However, despite speculation
THE EDGE OFF LETHARGY, that some other component of coffee (or
maybe simply its flavor or temperature)
IMPROVE FOCUS, AND could have appetite-suppressing effects
RESCUE SOME GYM SESSIONS not captured in the present study, other
research (4) specifically comparing caf-
WHEN YOU FEEL LIKE JUST feinated and decaffeinated coffee and a
placebo reported no significant differ-
WALKING TO THE RACK IS ences in energy intake, subjective hun-
ger ratings, or gastric emptying rates (a
A SIGNIFICANT EFFORT. mechanistic measure related to satiety)
between groups.
As a final note, there is a single study
from the ’80s showing that caffeine re-
tion. It’s also not a one-of-a-kind find- duces energy intake in men but not
ing that caffeine isn’t an effective appe- women (5). If there are differences be-
tite suppressant. In a review published tween men and women, this could have
last year (2), the authors noted an acute impacted the results of the present study
reduction in energy intake during a meal since the participants were 58% female.
in studies when caffeine was consumed This also could have impacted the results
0.5-4 hours prior. However, they noted of the study I mentioned above on cof-
that long-term data were lacking, and fee, as the participants there were 75%
reductions in 24-hour energy consump- female. However, there are two issues
tion were inconsistent; only one of two with the study that reported sex differ-
studies showed a significant decrease in ences: 1) It is a short-term study only
24-hour energy intake, and only in the assessing the meal following caffeine
overweight participants – not those who consumption, so it may be that 24-hour
were lean (3). Again, this shows that energy intake is not changed; and 2) a
even if a transient appetite suppression 2011 study specifically on men did not
occurs, it is compensated for later in the find any effect of caffeine on energy in-
day, in most cases. take, subjective hunger and appetite, or

78
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
1. Caffeine does a great job of suppressing tiredness and improving performance,
both of which are great in and out of diet phases. However, if you are expecting
additional fat loss or lasting appetite suppression, caffeine likely won’t deliver.

hormonal markers related to hunger and that caffeine didn’t result in additional
appetite (6). Thus, on balance, I think it’s fat loss over time, perhaps by some other
safe to conclude that if caffeine has any mechanism than appetite suppression –
appetite-suppressing properties, they are like improving mood and subsequently
short lived and typically compensated reducing emotional eating or via some
for over the course of 24 hours. other mechanism I’m unaware of. How-
With that said, in my experience as a ever, in this case, we already have data
bodybuilding coach, I can tell you that showing that caffeine doesn’t seem to
performance, desire to train, energy lev- aid fat loss longitudinally (7), so I think
els, and focus all become much more in- it’s safe to put the fat loss and appetite
consistent at a certain stage when diet- suppression claims to rest.
ing. While caffeine may not help with
the hunger pangs, it can certainly take
the edge off lethargy, improve focus, and
rescue some gym sessions when you feel
like just walking to the rack is a signifi-
cant effort.

Next Steps
When acute resistance training in-
terventions show an impact on repeti-
tions performed, performance recov-
ery, muscle damage, or a biomarker in
the short term, we always recommend
a long-term intervention to truly see if
the acute effects translate into improved
performance or hypertrophy chronically.
Likewise, I would typically recommend
a long-term intervention just to be sure

79
References
1. Panek-Shirley, L.M., et al., Caffeine Transiently Affects Food Intake at Breakfast. J Acad Nutr Diet,
2018.
2. Schubert, M.M., et al., Caffeine, coffee, and appetite control: a review. Int J Food Sci Nutr, 2017.
68(8): p. 901-912.
3. Gavrieli, A., et al., Effect of different amounts of coffee on dietary intake and appetite of nor-
mal-weight and overweight/obese individuals. Obesity (Silver Spring), 2013. 21(6): p. 1127-32.
4. Schubert, M.M., et al., Coffee for morning hunger pangs. An examination of coffee and caffeine on
appetite, gastric emptying, and energy intake. Appetite, 2014. 83: p. 317-26.
5. Tremblay, A., et al., Caffeine reduces spontaneous energy intake in men but not in women. Nutrition
Research, 1988. 8(5): p. 553-558.
6. Gavrieli, A., et al., Caffeinated coffee does not acutely affect energy intake, appetite, or inflammation
but prevents serum cortisol concentrations from falling in healthy men. J Nutr, 2011. 141(4): p. 703-
7.
7. Astrup, A., et al., The effect and safety of an ephedrine/caffeine compound compared to ephedrine,
caffeine and placebo in obese subjects on an energy restricted diet. A double blind trial. Int J Obes
Relat Metab Disord, 1992. 16(4): p. 269-77.

80
Study Reviewed: Effects of Accentuated Eccentric Loading on Muscle Properties,
Strength, Power, and Speed in Resistance-Trained Rugby Players. Douglas et al. (2018)

Can You Build More Size and


Strength with Overloaded
Eccentrics?
BY G RE G NUC KO LS

You can produce more force eccentrically than concentrically, so it


stands to reason that you’ll get better results by handling heavier
eccentric loads, right? A recent study puts this idea to the test.

81
KEY POINTS
1. This study tested the effects of accentuated eccentrics (using heavier loads for
the eccentric part of the movement than the concentric) compared to traditional
training (using the same load for the eccentric and concentric phases).
2. During the first four-week block of training, training with accentuated eccentrics
led to larger increases in squat strength and a larger increase in maximum sprint
velocity than traditional training.
3. During the second four-week block of training, accentuated eccentrics didn’t seem
to further improve strength or performance when compared to traditional training.

A
ccentuated eccentrics seem like larger strength gains, though that’s far
a slam dunk training idea. On from a unanimous finding, and there’s
average, people can produce no clear evidence that accentuated ec-
about 20-40% more force eccentrically centrics increase hypertrophy beyond
than concentrically. So, if you use the what you’d expect from normal train-
same weight for both the eccentric and ing. However, the body of research on
concentric phases of a lift, it seems that accentuated eccentric training is small,
you wouldn’t be sufficiently taxing your and many of the previous studies were
muscles during the eccentric phase. If conducted on single-joint exercises.
you stop a set when you’re two reps shy In the presently reviewed study, trained
of concentric failure, you may still be a rugby players were split into two groups
dozen reps shy of eccentric failure. With and undertook a training program split
accentuated eccentrics, you use heavier into two four-week blocks. One group
loads for the eccentric phase than the did all of their squat training with accen-
concentric phase, which should help tuated eccentrics, while the other group
ensure that your muscles are adequately performed traditional squats. The first
challenged through both phases of the block of the training program involved
movement. That should lead to larger lighter loads and slow (three-second)
strength gains and more muscle growth, eccentrics, while the second block in-
right? volved heavier loads and faster (one-sec-
Well … the research is hazy, to say the ond) eccentrics. During the first block
least. As previously covered in a guest of training, the accentuated eccentric
article by Caleb Bazyler, the research on group improved their squat and maximal
accentuated eccentric training has been sprint velocity more than the tradition-
underwhelming. There’s some evidence al group. However, during the second
that accentuated eccentrics may lead to block of training, strength gains didn’t
82
differ between groups. Neither group normal eccentric loading. They also hy-
experienced significant or meaningful pothesized that slow eccentric tempos
quad hypertrophy. Overall, the results would lead to larger changes in muscle
of this study are similar to those of pre- morphology, while faster eccentric tem-
vious research – possibly larger strength pos would lead to larger improvements
gains with accentuated eccentrics, with in measures of power, reactive strength,
no difference in hypertrophy. and sprint speed.

Purpose and Research Subjects and Methods


Questions Subjects
Purpose The subjects were 14 young (mean age:
19.4 years), male rugby players. They all
The purpose of this study was to in-
had at least one year of resistance train-
vestigate the effects of accentuated ec-
ing experience, and their average 1RM
centric loading and eccentric tempo on
squat was 1.71 times body mass. The
muscle morphology and a variety of per-
subjects were randomized to two groups;
formance outcomes in academy rugby
both groups carried out the same train-
players.
ing program over 12 weeks, with one
Research Questions difference. One group performed their
squats and lower body plyometrics with
1. Would accentuated eccentric load- additional eccentric load, while the oth-
ing lead to larger performance in- er group performed normal squats (i.e.
creases and changes in muscle the same load was used for both phases
morphology than normal eccentric of the lift) and plyometrics without ad-
loading? ditional eccentric load.
2. Would slow eccentric tempos lead
to larger performance increases Training Program
and changes in muscle morphology Both groups lifted three times per
than faster eccentric tempos? week, performing two strength sessions
and one power session per week. They
Hypotheses also performed one conditioning session
The authors hypothesized that accen- per week, two skill sessions per week,
tuated eccentric loading would lead to and played in one rugby match per week.
larger performance increases (in mea- Everything was the same in both groups
sures of strength, power, and speed) than except for the lifting sessions.

83
Table 1 Weekly training schedule

Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday Saturday Sunday

Gym Session Gym Session Gym Session Rugby Game


AM
(Strength) (Power) (Strength)
Field Session Field Session
PM Conditioning
(Skills) (Skills)

Table 2 Squat program performed during strength sessions:


Accentuated Training
Traditional Training Group
Group
Tempo Sets x Reps %1RM Relative %1RM Relative
Volume Load Volume Load
Test Week 1
Week 2 3-0-1 3x8 92/68 19 74 18
Slow Week 3 3x7 95/70 17 76 16
Week 4 4x6 98/72 20 79 19
Week 5 2x6 98/72 10 79 10
Test Week 6
Week 7
Week 8 1-0-1 3x5 106/77 14 81 12
Fast Week 9 4x4 108/79 15 83 13
Week 10 5x4 110/81 19 85 17
Week 11 2x4 110/81 8 85 7
Test Week 12

The training program lasted for 12 ume load (sets x reps x %1RM) would
weeks. The first week, weeks 6 and 7, and be roughly the same in both groups
week 12 were used for testing. During (though I’m not entirely sure I see how
weeks 2-5, the subjects performed sets relative volume load would be similar; it
of 6-8 reps with an increasing percent- looks like it would still be higher in the
age of their 1RM and did three-second accentuated eccentric group, but I may
eccentrics for all of their squat training. be misunderstanding how the authors
During weeks 8-11, they performed sets calculated it).
of 4-5 reps with an increasing percentage More details about the training pro-
of their 1RM and did one-second eccen- gram can be seen in Table 1.
trics for all of their squat training. Since
To accomplish the accentuated ec-
eccentric load was 18-25% higher in the
centric loading, the researchers used a
accentuated eccentric group, concentric
Smith machine that began applying as-
load was 4-7% lower so that relative vol-

84
Figure 1 Study Design

Weeks 2-5 Weeks 8-11


Training Phase 1 Training Phase 2

Slow accentuated eccentric Fast accentuated eccentric


training (Intervention) training (Intervention)
Week 1 Week 6 Week 7 Week 12
Pre-Testing Mid-Testing Off Post-Testing
Slow traditional training Fast traditional training
(Control) (Control)
Day 1: Day 1: Day 1:

40m sprint profiling 40m sprint profiling 40m sprint profiling

Cycling test Cycling test Cycling test

Day 2: Day 2: Day 2:

Muscle architecture Muscle architecture Muscle architecture

0.50m drop jump 0.50m drop jump 0.50m drop jump

Back squat 1RM Back squat 1RM Back squat 1RM

sistance once a predetermined depth was 40m sprint times, and maximal velocity,
reached. So, for example, if one session contact time, flight time, step rate, verti-
called for the use of 500lb for eccentrics cal stiffness, and leg stiffness during the
and 400lb for concentrics, the bar would 40m sprint.
be loaded to 500lb. When the athlete The authors reported effect sizes
reached a predetermined depth, the ma- and confidence intervals from pre- to
chine would begin applying 100lb of mid-testing and from mid- to post-test-
assistance, so that the concentric load ing and provided qualitative assessments
would be 400lb. The control group per- of the observed changes, but didn’t do
formed regular squats with free weights. any formal significance testing.

Measures You can see a graphical representation


of the study in Figure 1.
For our purposes, the most important
measure taken was back squat 1RM. The
squat 1RMs were performed with free Findings
weights and not in the custom Smith
machine. The researchers also measured The group performing accentuated
vastus lateralis (a quad muscle) thick- eccentrics gained more strength in the
ness, fascicle length, and fascicle angle squat during the first four weeks (lower
using ultrasound. loads and slow eccentrics) than the tra-
ditional group, but both groups failed to
Other measures included power out- increase their squats during the second
put and cadence during a maximal cy- four weeks (higher loads and faster ec-
cling task, contact time, flight time, re- centrics). Maximum sprinting velocity,
active strength, and leg stiffness during ground contact time during sprints, step
a drop jump task, 10m, 20m, 30m, and rate, and vertical stiffness during sprints

85
Figure 2 The standardized Cohen difference for subjects completing slow accentuated eccentrics (n=7) vs.
subjects completing slow traditional training (n=7)

Slow traditional Slow accentuated


training eccentrics Qualitative description
(Control) (Intervention) of differences

Squat 1RM Likely positive


Strength and
Cycling peak power Unclear
power variables
Cycling optimal cadence Likely negative

Reactive Strength Index Unclear


0.50m drop jump
Contact time Unclear
variables
Flight time Possibly negative

10m Unclear
40m sprint 20m Unclear
variables
40m Possibly positive

Max sprint speed Likely positive

Maximum velocity Contact time Likely positive


sprint variables Step rate Likely positive

Vertical stiffness Likely positive

-2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0

Standardized Cohen differences (Effect Size)

Differences are for the change in selected performance variables. Negative values indicate a larger effect with traditional resistance training (TRT), and
positive values indicate a larger effect with accentuated eccentric loading (AEL). Qualitative inferences indicate a positive or negative effect of AEL vs. TRT.
Error bars indicate uncertainty in the true mean changes with 90% confidence intervals. The shaded area represents the smallest worthwhile change.

also tended to improve more in the ac-


centuated eccentric group than the tradi- Interpretation
tional group during the first four weeks. There was a lot going on in this study
During the second four weeks, peak since it was essentially two studies in
power during the cycling test tended to one (since the first half and the second
increase more in the traditional group, half included different eccentric tempos)
while ground contact time during drop and since it included so many outcome
jumps tended to decrease more for the measures. However, the outcomes ended
accentuated eccentrics group. up being pretty straightforward, if a bit
Muscle thickness didn’t meaningfully counterintuitive.
change in either group. However, fascicle To start with, the accentuated eccen-
length may have increased from pre- to tric group had pretty sizeable squat in-
mid-testing in the accentuated eccentric creases during the first block of training
group (ES = 0.61) and decreased back (slow eccentrics and lower loads), while
toward its initial length from mid- to the traditional group actually regressed
post-testing (ES = -0.49). slightly. The authors reported strength as

86
Figure 3 The standardized Cohen difference for subjects completing fast accentuated eccentrics (n=6) vs.
subjects completing fast traditional training (n=7)

Slow traditional Slow accentuated Qualitative description


training eccentrics of differences
(Control) (Intervention)

Squat 1RM Unclear


Strength and
Cycling peak power Likely negative
power variables
Cycling optimal cadence Unclear

Reactive Strength Index Unclear


0.50m drop jump
Contact time Likely positive
variables
Flight time Unclear

10m Unclear
40m sprint 20m Unclear
variables
40m Unclear

Max sprint speed Unclear

Maximum velocity Contact time Unclear


sprint variables Step rate Unclear

Vertical stiffness Unclear

-2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0

Standardized Cohen differences (Effect Size)

Differences are for the change in selected performance variables. Negative values indicate a larger effect with traditional resistance training (TRT),
and positive values indicate a larger effect with accentuated eccentric loading (AEL). Qualitative inferences indicate a positive or negative effect of
AEL vs. TRT. Error bars indicate uncertainty in the true mean changes with 90% confidence intervals. The shaded area represents the smallest
worthwhile change.

a multiple of body mass so we don’t know four weeks of training when loads were
exactly how many kilos the accentuated higher and eccentric duration decreased
eccentric group put on their squats, but from three seconds to one second.
the average increase was 0.09 times body I think there’s a pretty straightfor-
mass (1.77 to 1.86 times body mass), ward explanation for the larger strength
and the average participant was 97kg, gains in the accentuated eccentric group
so the average increase was probably during the first block of training. They
somewhere around 9kg (~19lb). That’s were handling 92-98% of their 1RM for
not too shabby for a month of training eccentrics, while the traditional group
when you already squat almost double didn’t touch any weights exceeding 79%
bodyweight. On the flip side, the aver- of their 1RM, so intensity was consider-
age squat in the traditional group actu- ably higher in the accentuated eccentric
ally regressed by about 0.03 times body group. However, I do think it’s odd that
mass (probably ~3kg). Furthermore, neither group made progress during the
neither group progressed in the second second four-week block. In theory, you’d

87
expect larger strength gains in both
groups during the second block since
the intensity was higher. For the accen- ACCENTUATED ECCENTRICS
tuated eccentric group, progress may
have stalled due to overreaching – han- PROBABLY DON’T INCREASE
dling 100%+ of your max for reps twice
per week isn’t a walk in the park – but HYPERTROPHY BEYOND
I was still surprised that the traditional
group failed to progress. The addition-
WHAT YOU’RE EXPECT FROM
al conditioning work and rugby games
may have impeded strength gains in
TRADITIONAL TRAINING.
both groups.
It’s a bit interesting to me that the imal shortening velocity). However,
accentuated eccentric group improved I’m unaware of a mechanistic reason
their squat at all. The 1RM testing was why the specific combination of slow
performed with free weights, while all eccentrics and overloaded eccentrics
of the squat training for the accentuat- would increase fascicle length in the
ed eccentric group was performed in a first place. Simply performing slow ec-
custom Smith machine. It wouldn’t have centrics didn’t seem to be sufficient (as
surprised me if isolated measures of leg the traditional group didn’t experience
strength improved more (perhaps peak an increase in fascicle length when
knee extension torque, if that would performing slow eccentrics), and sim-
have been measured in this study), but ply overloading the eccentrics doesn’t
I am surprised that free weight squat seem to be sufficient (as the accentuat-
strength improved, given that these ed eccentric group actually had a slight
were trained lifters. This may indicate decrease in fascicle length during the
that specificity doesn’t matter quite as second four weeks of training). Maybe
much as many people believe for lifters there’s a clear mechanistic reason why
who’ve already learned a motor pattern, slow, overloaded eccentrics would in-
at least in the short run. crease fascicle length, and thus sprint
I’m also trying to make sense of the speed, but I’m not aware of it. Fur-
increase in maximal sprint speed in thermore, eccentric training has been
the accentuated eccentric group when shown to increase tendon stiffness (2),
using slow eccentrics. Narrowly, this which may also partially explain the in-
makes sense mechanistically due to the crease in sprint velocity in the accentu-
increase in fascicle length (increased ated eccentric group. However, that still
fascicle length should increase max- doesn’t explain why an increase in sprint

88
warrants further investigation, as a cou-
ple of aspects of this study left a bit to
ACCENTUATED ECCENTRICS be desired. For example, if the accentu-
ated eccentric group trained on a Smith
MAY ENHANCE STRENGTH machine, the traditional group should
have also trained on a Smith machine,
GAINS, BUT THAT THE and testing should have been per-
LITERATURE IS STILL TOO formed using a Smith machine. That
may have revealed an even larger differ-
MURKY TO MAKE A CLEAR ence in strength gains that was masked
by group differences in specificity. Fur-
RECOMMENDATION. thermore, both groups were also doing
a lot of conditioning work and playing
in rugby matches, which likely blunted
strength gains in both groups.
velocity was observed when using slow
However, at this point, the biggest
eccentrics but not faster eccentrics.
problem with accentuated eccentric
For more reading on accentuated ec- training is feasibility. For many exercis-
centrics, you should check out the con- es, accentuated eccentrics either require
cept review Caleb Bazyler wrote a few specialized equipment, or a spotter you
months ago. The main takeaways for really trust. If there was clear evidence
hypertrophy and strength were that ac- that accentuated eccentrics led to larger
centuated eccentrics probably don’t in- gains, it may be worth getting a special-
crease hypertrophy beyond what you’re ized Smith machine or training a spot-
expect from traditional training, and ter to assist you in just the right way.
that accentuated eccentrics may enhance However, as it is, it doesn’t seem that
strength gains, but that the literature is the additional money or effort would
still too murky to make a clear recom- net you much of a payoff. There are ex-
mendation. This study largely supports ercises where accentuated eccentrics are
those takeaways, as there were no hy- easy and feasible (i.e. for machine leg
pertrophy differences between groups curls or leg presses, you can lower the
in this study, and strength gains were weight with one leg, and use your oth-
only larger in the accentuated eccentric er leg to assist a bit on the concentric),
group during the first half of this study but they’re generally exercises primarily
when loads were relatively low. performed for hypertrophy (for which
Overall, I still think accentuated ec- accentuated eccentrics don’t seem to be
centrics are an interesting concept that beneficial), not for strength.

89
APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS
1. For the time being, I still don’t feel confident recommending accentuated eccentrics,
both because the research is pretty hazy and because accentuated eccentrics
are generally just less practical than normal reps. However, if you want to give
them a shot, they currently seem to be more promising (tentatively) for strength
development than for hypertrophy.

Next Steps last two reps. I’d hypothesize that hy-


pertrophy would be similar in all three
This may not be the most scientific groups, but if going to failure or past
research topic, but the most common failure actually led to greater hypertro-
application of accentuated eccentrics in phy, that could have large implications
the “real world” is the “bro spot” – af- for day-to-day training.
ter you reach the point of failure, you
keep lowering the weight yourself, and
your spotter assists you in grinding out
an additional rep or two. I also think a
study designed to test the effects of “bro
spotting” could help flesh out the effects
of training to failure. When you think
about it, concentric failure is a some-
what arbitrary endpoint; if you’re us-
ing 80% of your max, concentric failure
means your capacity to produce concen-
tric force has decreased by 20%, whereas
concentric failure when training at 60%
of your max implies that capacity to pro-
duce force has decreased by 40%. In both
cases, you’re “training to failure,” but the
intraset fatigue implied by that endpoint
differs. I’d be interested in seeing a study
where one group stops all sets two reps
shy of failure, one group trains to con-
centric failure, and one group trains two
reps past failure with a “bro spot” on the

90
References
1. Douglas J, Pearson S, Ross A, McGuigan M. Effects of Accentuated Eccentric Loading on Muscle
Properties, Strength, Power, and Speed in Resistance-Trained Rugby Players. J Strength Cond Res.
2018 Aug 15. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000002772.
2. Malliaras P, Kamal B, Nowell A, Farley T, Dhamu H, Simpson V, Morrissey D, Langberg H, Maf-
fulli N, Reeves ND. Patellar tendon adaptation in relation to load-intensity and contraction type. J
Biomech. 2013 Jul 26;46(11):1893-9. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.04.022.
3. The rest of the studies investigating the effects of accentuated eccentric training on strength gains
(one, two, three, four, five, six) are discussed individually in Caleb’s concept review.

91
VIDEO: Strategies to Improve
Neuromuscular Efficiency
BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S

Rate of force development can be a limiting factor of max strength, but


specifics of training this process are rarely explained. This video provides
practical strategies to improve your neuromuscular efficiency.
Click to watch Michael's presentation.

92
References
1. Zourdos MC, Klemp A, Dolan C, Quiles JM, Schau KA, Jo E, Helms E, Esgro B, Duncan S, Me-
rino SG, Blanco R. Novel resistance training–specific rating of perceived exertion scale measuring
repetitions in reserve. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2016 Jan 1;30(1):267-75.
2. Zourdos MC, Dolan C, Quiles JM, Klemp A, Jo E, Loenneke JP, Blanco R, Whitehurst M. Efficacy
of daily one-repetition maximum training in well-trained powerlifters and weightlifters: a case se-
ries. Nutrición Hospitalaria. 2016;33(2).
3. Peltonen H, Walker S, Hackney AC, Avela J, Häkkinen K. Increased rate of force development
during periodized maximum strength and power training is highly individual. European journal of
applied physiology. 2018 May 1;118(5):1033-42.
4. Del Vecchio A, Negro F, Falla D, Bazzucchi I, Farina D, Felici F. Higher muscle fiber conduction
velocity and early rate of torque development in chronically strength trained individuals. Journal of
Applied Physiology. 2018.
5. Methenitis S, Karandreas N, Spengos K, Zaras N, Stasinaki AN, Terzis G. Muscle Fiber Conduc-
tion Velocity, Muscle Fiber Composition, and Power Performance. Medicine and science in sports
and exercise. 2016 Sep;48(9):1761-71.

93
VIDEO: Muscle Group
Specialization Cycles
BY E RI C HE LMS

While volume should only be increased if plateaued and if all other variables
are optimal, there may come a time when higher volumes for hypertrophy
are necessary. One way to increase volume without digging too deep of a
recovery hole is the use of muscle group specialization cycles.
Click to watch Eric's presentation.

94
References
1. Ralston, G.W., et al., The Effect of Weekly Set Volume on Strength Gain: A Meta-Analysis. Sports
Med, 2017. 47(12): p. 2585-2601.
2. Schoenfeld, B.J., et al., Strength and hypertrophy adaptations between low- versus high-load resis-
tance training: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Strength Cond Res, 2017.
3. Schoenfeld, B.J., D. Ogborn, and J.W. Krieger, Effects of Resistance Training Frequency on Mea-
sures of Muscle Hypertrophy: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Med, 2016. 46(11):
p. 1689-1697.
4. Schoenfeld, B.J., D. Ogborn, and J.W. Krieger, Dose-response relationship between weekly resis-
tance training volume and increases in muscle mass: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sports
Sci, 2017. 35(11): p. 1073-1082.
5. Schoenfeld, B.J., et al., Resistance Training Volume Enhances Muscle Hypertrophy. Med Sci Sports
Exerc, 2018.
6. Montero, D. and C. Lundby, Refuting the myth of non-response to exercise training: ‘non-respond-
ers’ do respond to higher dose of training. The Journal of Physiology, 2017. 595(11): p. 3377-3387.
7. Baz-Valle, E., M. Fontes-Villalba, and J. Santos-Concejero, Total Number of Sets as a Training Vol-
ume Quantification Method for Muscle Hypertrophy: A Systematic Review. J Strength Cond Res,
2018.
8. Lasevicius, T., et al., Effects of different intensities of resistance training with equated volume load
on muscle strength and hypertrophy. Eur J Sport Sci, 2018. 18(6): p. 772-780.

95
Just Missed the Cut
Every month, we consider hundreds of new papers, and they can’t all be
included in MASS. Therefore, we’re happy to share a few pieces of research
that just missed the cut. It’s our hope that with the knowledge gained from
reading MASS, along with our interpreting research guide, you’ll be able to
tackle these on your own.

• Ranchordas et al. Antioxidants for preventing and reducing muscle


soreness after exercise: a Cochrane systematic review
• Bermon et al. Serum androgen levels are positively correlated with athletic
performance and competition results in elite female athletes
• Cavarretta et al. The acute effects of resistance exercise on affect,
anxiety, and mood – practical implications for designing resistance
training programs
• Lundberg et al. Analgesic and anti‐inflammatory drugs in sports:
Implications for exercise performance and training adaptations
• Soares et al. Different Load Distributions Affect Acute Neuromuscular
Responses and Muscle Thickness in Resistance-Trained Men
• Beals et al. Altered anabolic signaling and reduced stimulation of
myofibrillar protein synthesis after feeding and resistance exercise in
people with obesity
• Mazzolari. Exercise dose and individual response of healthy adults: is it
time to re‐evaluate exercise responsiveness and training
• Lee et al. A cellular mechanism of muscle memory facilitates mitochondrial
remodelling following resistance training
• Comfort et al. An Investigation Into the Effects of Excluding the Catch
Phase of the Power Clean on Force-Time Characteristics During Isometric
and Dynamic Tasks: An Intervention Study
• Sharp et al. The Effects of Beef, Chicken, or Whey Protein After Workout
on Body Composition and Muscle Performance
• Aguiar et al. Acute metabolic responses following different resistance
exercise protocols

96
• Eckard et al. The Relationship Between Training Load and Injury in Athletes: A
Systematic Review
• Sumiaki et al. Neuromuscular Adaptations to Work-matched Maximal Eccentric
versus Concentric Training
• Teixeira et al. Leucine Metabolites Do Not Enhance Training-induced
Performance or Muscle Thickness
• Tinsley et al. Fat-free Mass Characteristics of Muscular Physique Athletes

97
Thanks for
reading MASS.
The next issue will be released to
subscribers on November 1.

Graphics by Katherine Whitfield, and layout design by Lyndsey Nuckols.

98

Вам также может понравиться