Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 13

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/233330147

Criteria for Qualitative Research in


Psychology

Article in Qualitative Research in Psychology · January 2004


DOI: 10.1191/1478088704qp010oa

CITATIONS READS

85 1,036

1 author:

Ian Parker
University of Leicester
215 PUBLICATIONS 3,965 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Lacanian Discourse Analysis View project

Psicanálise e Análise de Discurso View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ian Parker on 11 February 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

www.QualResearchPsych.com Qualitative Research in Psychology 2004; 1: 1 /12

Criteria for qualitative research in


psychology
Ian Parker
Department of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Elizabeth
Gaskell Campus, Hathersage Road, Manchester M13 0JA, UK

F
This paper, designed for supervisors of qualitative research projects, and

O
addressed to students to enable to think through how they may be
evaluated, reviews some problems in the formulation of criteria for
qualitative research in the discipline and issues that are important for

O
the generation and paradigmatic framing of open flexible criteria. The
guidelines that are presented here are developed as an attempt to explicate

PR
the parameters of criteria, rather than closing down future innovative work.
Ten key points are elaborated ‘under erasure’ (that the study should be
objective, valid, reliable, neutral, confirmed, definitive, established,
coherent, accessible and psychological) and core principles of
‘apprenticeship’, ‘scholarship’ and ‘innovation’ are designed to open the
D
way for good research to go beyond these criteria. Qualitative Research in
Psychology 2004; 1: 1 /12
TE

Key words: criteria; exceptions; innovation; paradigms; rules


EC

Introduction Some of the most innovative studies in


quantitative research have broken the rules
Qualitative research in psychology poses of accepted scientific inquiry, and it has
R

questions about the nature of ‘criteria’ that been through awareness that something
need to be reflexively embedded in any radically different was being undertaken
R

alternative guidelines. and an argument for what was new that


Two problems with fixed ‘criteria’ should progress has been possible (Salmon, 2003).
O

be noted. First, most traditional quantitative It would be reasonable to ask that quantita-
psychological research does not adhere to tive research should also justify itself
C

the criteria it desires, and in many cases it against the issues raised here. Secondly,
would be very difficult to insist that it does. criteria of any kind risks legitimating cer-
N

Correspondence: Ian Parker, Department of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Manchester Metropolitan
U

University, Elizabeth Gaskell Campus, Hathersage Road, Manchester M13 0JA, UK.
E-mail: i.a.parker@mmu.ac.uk

# Arnold 2004 10.1191/1478088704qp010oa


Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

2 I Parker

tain varieties of qualitative research, mar- (and beyond) the psychological community
ginalizing others, and so stifling new meth- and exploring what they may mean is more
odological developments (Elliott et al ., important here than fixing meanings and
1999). That is why I explicate some para- then refusing reinterpretation. Accordingly,
meters for research and emphasize that the there are four key issues that qualitative
key question that the researcher should researchers in psychology do need to ad-
explicate for themselves and perhaps even dress.
indicate to their readers is ‘by what should I
be judged?’ . The first is an awareness of the difference
An appeal to different distinctive criteria between qualitative approaches and tra-
is one way of warranting the range of ditional quantitative research. There are
innovative ways of going about research. historical divisions between the two ap-

F
For students the criteria elaborated in this proaches, and this has led some of the
paper will draw attention to issues that they discussion of supervision of undergradu-

O
might consider as they formulate their ate qualitative research as a sometimes
research and prepare it for evaluation. For marginalized speciality, with recommen-

O
supervisors these guidelines could serve to dations put forward as to which particu-
provide enough common ground between lar methods might be easier to

PR
psychologists carrying out qualitative or incorporate in teaching in the discipline
quantitative research, but this common (e.g., Gough et al ., 2003). There is also a
ground can only be secured on the under- history of argument over the ‘paradigm’
standing that some flexibility and negotia-
that should govern psychological re-
tion is needed so that both students and
search (Harré, 2004), an issue covered in
supervisors can formulate their own parti-
Box 1
D
cular procedures for describing, explaining
. Secondly, there is a range of resources
TE
and justifying what they have done. So, the
qualitative researchers have drawn upon
criteria for good research are guidelines that
to develop alternative methods. Some of
are closed enough to guide evaluation and
the most valuable of these resources are
open enough to enable transformation of
EC

outside the discipline. Feminist debates


assumptions.
about how we produce knowledge have
These descriptions of ‘criteria’, then,
should be read as flexible guidelines or been crucial to critical reflections on
touchstones and they are made as explicit science, and have huge consequences
R

as possible here as part of the process of for qualitative research (e.g., Hartsock,
making transparent the reasons and ways 1987). Qualitative research outside psy-
R

we go about doing research and the reasons chology, the best of which has been
and ways we judge, and ask others to judge heavily influenced by feminism, has
O

its value. also led to different varieties of ‘construc-


tionist’ criteria that challenge main-
C

stream psychology (e.g., Denzin and


Four key issues in the formulation of Lincoln, 2000).
N

criteria . The third issue is explicit consideration


of the way criteria should be understood
U

The process of making research questions in a particular case. There is no overall


and their evaluation transparent within set of criteria that would work to justify a
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

Criteria for qualitative research in psychology 3

specific study, for a new research ques- uninterested in quantitative research. In


tion calls for a new rationale and combi- fact, the questioning of assumptions that
nation of methodological resources to underpin much quantitative psychology has
explore it, and the terms in which a been going on in the field of social statistics
research question is framed will entail for many years (e.g., Dorling and Simpson,
particular methods. And the best re- 1999) and some of us would like to see good
search entails an innovation not only quantitative research in psychology take
with respect to the topic but also with these debates on board. There are three
respect to the methodology that will be main issues here.
appropriate to address it. The problem of First, it is possible to see ‘objectivity’
how to legitimize existing research can- as something that is constructed, sometimes
not be solved by constructed an iron grid for very good reasons. This construction

F
that will thereby invalidate all the new of objectivity does not mean that some
things that will be developed later on views of the world are not better than

O
(Capdevila, 2003). others, but it does mean that we cannot
. Fourthly, there is a requirement that the take a seemingly objective account for

O
researcher gives an account of the ways granted. The most important issue here is
in which the research relates specifically that with respect to psychological research,

PR
to psychology. The problem that needs to the person carrying out the research always
be grasped here is that psychology as a has a certain stance toward the questions
discipline has historically defined itself that are being explored. Hunches,
with reference to methodology more than intuitions, hopes and assumptions about
by the objects or topics of research (Rose,
the nature of human beings all play a role
1985). Now it is necessary to find a way
D
in the apparently ‘objective’ pursuit of a
to open up new ways of thinking about
psychologist. This position / whether
TE
the domain of the ‘psychological’ /
as empathic involvement or studied detach-
perhaps by refocusing on such things as
ment / is a form of subjectivity (Hollway,
‘experience’, ‘subjectivity’ or ‘interac-
1999). This is why qualitative researchers
tion’ / so that methodologies we develop
EC

often prefer to work with subjectivity


follow from the research question.
rather than against it (which is what we
This paper addresses those four issues in take quantitative researchers often to
relation to how we might think about the be doing when they say they are being
R

‘criteria’ for good research. objective).


Secondly, qualitative researchers see
R

‘validity’ as resting on a mistaken view


that different ways of representing phenom-

O

Paradigmatic framing / options and ena will necessarily be representing


exclusions the same thing. It is all the more the case
C

with psychological research that the


It is worthwhile reminding ourselves about particular standpoint we have towards
N

some of the ways in which assumptions that other people, and what we expect them to
underpin quantitative research are inap- be like, will make every description we
U

propriate to qualitative studies. This is not give open to challenge. The claim that
at all to say that qualitative researchers are qualitative researchers are describing the
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

4 I Parker

‘same thing’ is to close down the possible


alternative ways of describing experience,
Conceptual resources
care
/ handled with

even leaving aside for the moment


the difficult question as to whether we Some of the conceptual resources qualita-
should simply respect the accounts that tive researchers have used will be appro-
someone gives of their own experience or priate for future research, but we also need
whether the reasons people do things can be to take into account the particular ways in
made transparent to the researcher (Night- which they may be helpful or unhelpful
ingale and Cromby, 1999). This is why depending on each study. These resources
qualitative researchers often prefer to do not of themselves solve problems in
explore the various different ways of de- research. Rather, qualitative researchers
scribing an issue. take them seriously because of the addi-

F
Thirdly, traditionally ‘reliability’ is seen tional fruitful questions they raise.
First, the claim to be ‘neutral’ in research

O
as taking for granted that our objects
of study remain stable over time rather is one that sustains a particular standpoint
and to prevent the standpoint from being

O
than being liable to change. This is an
assumption underlying conceptions opened to question. In contrast, a qualita-
of method in psychology that is closely tive researcher may address subjectivity,

PR
linked to deeper assumptions about exploring emotional investments in the
the importance of ‘consistency’ and ‘ration- topic, focusing on the position of the
ality’ in western psychology. But some researcher, and making our moral-political
forms of research, action research, make standpoint clear (Wilkinson, 1988). A
reflexive analysis in a report can serve to
that process of change the very topic that
D
help the reader understand something more
is focussed upon, and there is an explicit
about the work. This is sometimes included
TE
attempt to make sure that things do not stay
in the report as one of the subsections of the
the same (Kagan and Burton, 2000).
‘analysis’ and marked as ‘reflexive analysis’
The lesson of community empowerment
or put in the discussion as part of a
work and ‘prefigurative’ action research for
EC

reflection on the process of carrying out


psychology is not so much that change can
the research (or both). This is the place
occur, but that it is happening all the time.
to consider the passionate interests that
This is why good qualitative research often
drive some of the best research (Maso,
R

focuses on change and traces a process, 2003). However, this attempt to question
rather than treating patterns of human the ostensible neutrality of the research can
R

behaviour or thinking as things that are be tackled in what some might see
fixed. as ‘confessional’ mode (in which there is a
O

These perspectives on ‘objectivity’, ‘va- story about the researcher’s journey into the
lidity’ and ‘reliability’, of course, raise research and how they felt about it), but
C

questions for the kind of positivist science it may also be tackled by giving an account
that has been popular with psychologists of the institutional background for the
N

because it makes it seem as if stable facts formulation and representation of what the
about people can be accumulated and then research is about. That is, the ‘position
U

taken for granted for further study by an of the researcher’ is a question of institu-
objective researcher. tional reflexivity that draws attention
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

Criteria for qualitative research in psychology 5

Box 1. Where are we now with science?

Psychologists sometimes argue that only quantitative research is properly ‘scientific’, or


that qualitative research should be evaluated against the kind of criteria that have
usually been employed to assess quantitative research. However, these arguments can
just as well be turned around to put the quantitative researchers on the spot. In a recent
article, the philosopher of science Rom Harré (2004), for example, argues that it is
qualitative research that is properly scientific, and that it is only in relation to
methodological debates in that strand of work that we can start to explore how
quantitative research might measure up to it. The relevant elements of Harré’s argument
are the following.

F
1) Reflexivity / the particular object of study for any science needs to be carefully
specified. Qualitative research takes seriously a crucial aspect of the nature of its

O
object of study, human action and experience. The human being is able to reflect on
its behaviour and to engage in second-level reflection on those reflections. This is

O
why the reflexive work of the researcher is also a crucial part of any genuine
scientific study.

PR
2) Meaning / the nature of the material that is studied by a science needs to be
understood. Qualitative research focuses on the way in which meaningful qualities
of human ‘experience’ or ‘subjectivity’ are represented to others. The accounts that
people give for what they do may or may not correspond to what they actually
think about those things. But the ‘discovery’ or ‘production’ of meaning is a
D
necessary aspect of the scientific study of human psychology.
3) Specificity / the level of analysis and the claims that are made from work in a
TE
particular domain needs to be stated. Qualitative research often engages in
intensive case studies that are not directly extrapolated to populations, or in
studies of collective activity that are not directly extrapolated to individual
members. The scientific task in this work is to account for specific nature and
EC

limits of the account, and for what may be learnt from it.

The old laboratory-experimental ‘paradigm’ of research in psychology was, Harré


argues, ‘pre-scientific’, and the task that quantitative researchers face now is how to
R

account for the reflexive capacity of human beings, the meaningful nature of the data
they produce and the way that claims are made about individuals from aggregated
R

descriptions of behaviour from particular populations. There are some possibilities of


very good innovative research in this tradition that addresses itself to these issues and
O

to the guidelines outlined in this paper.


C

to how this research is being carried out and they are treated as mere objects by tradi-
N

in whose interests. tional psychological research. The key ques-


Secondly, qualitative research has opened tion here is how the different positions that
U

up a series of questions about the participa- are brought to bear on the research throw
tion of those who are studied and whether new light on key issues. We might ask our
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

6 I Parker

coresearchers (our ‘subjects’) to respond to These questions about ‘neutrality’, ‘con-


the analysis, and this may be with the aim firmation’ of findings and the idea that there
of securing respondent validation (to con- is a ‘definitive’ account draw attention to
firm certain interpretations) or to encourage the contested nature of qualitative research.
disagreement (and to raise alternative inter- What this ‘quality’ amounts to is a question
pretations). These options, however morally of debate, as we note in Box 2.
compelling, cannot apply to all kinds of
qualitative research, especially where the
analysis of meaning does not aim to
privilege the immediate perspective of the
speaker (Wilkinson and Kitzinger, 1997).
Guidelines
exceptions
 / the rules and the
Most forms of discourse analysis, for exam-

F
ple, need to clarify why it would not be
Three overarching criteria for good research
appropriate to add further accounts to the

O
can be identified, but we need these to be
layers of interpretation they have con-
able to operate in ways that are not fixed in
structed in their readings of the texts (Bur-

O
stone (that is, they may be reflexively
man, 2003). employed or challenged depending on the
Thirdly, however tempting it is to claim kind of study). In each case, then, we

PR
that the research has provided a clearer or should value each criteria and each excep-
more superior view of what is going on, tion.
we need to hesitate a moment and explore First, with respect to the grounding of the
what grounds we have for that claim. We work in existing research, this means
may use a variety of different methods
that help us to ‘triangulate’ our inquiry,
D
that the work should identify existing lines
of research around the issue and
and qualitative researchers may do this to
TE
locate itself, but there may be cases when
show the different ways in which an absences in the research literature are
issue might be understood. This may either important and so the research will have
be with the aim of arriving at a to address these (e.g., Phoenix, 1994). If it
EC

common view or alternatively to illustrate were not possible to identify an absence


the intrinsic complexity of the issue (Tin- and explore why that were so, no new
dall, 1994). If there was really a definite objects of psychological research would
explanation then the assessment of research ever appear.
R

would be quite simple, and would revolve Secondly, with respect to coherence in
around the brutal question as to whether the the argument of the study, this means that
R

researcher has got it right or wrong there could be a cumulative linear narrative
(in which case they pass or fail the evalua- which moves clearly from point to point to
O

tion). Processes of ‘triangulation’ of arrive at conclusions, but there may be


research, however, are better used in such a cases where a more deliberately open frag-
C

way that alternative explanations may mented narrative will be more appropriate
coexist, and then the question to be (e.g., Curt, 1994). The standard format of a
N

addressed by the researcher is how research report is a secure framework for


their own particular interpretations can be many writers, but it is itself a particular
U

justified and what the consequences would genre of writing that can turn into a con-
be of taking them seriously. straint and inhibit innovative work.
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:9

Criteria for qualitative research in psychology 7

Box 2. Open questions about quality

It is crucial to the enterprise of scientific work generally, and qualitative research in


particular, that the way we go about it is open to debate. Here are some questions for
which there are no clear answers and much disagreement.

1) What counts as good? (a) It corresponds to the norms of established scientific study.
(b) It will improve the lives of those who participated. (c) It is intrinsically
interesting and will provoke and satisfy those who are curious about the questions
posed.
2) Who should it be for? (a) It should be directly accessible to ordinary people outside
psychology. (b) It should contribute to the accumulating body of knowledge for the

F
use of other researchers. (c) Those who participated should gain something from it
in exchange for their time.

O
3) What counts as analysis? (a) A careful redescription using some categories from a
particular framework. (b) The discovery of something that can be empirically

O
confirmed as true or refused as false. (c) The emergence of a new meaning that was
entirely unexpected.

PR
4) What is the role of theory? (a) Mystification by those versed in jargon at the expense
of those who participated. (b) A necessary antidote to the commonsense and often
mistaken explanations for human behaviour. (c) The space for thinking afresh about
something.
D
This is not a multiple-choice test (which, of course, would be a most inappropriate
assessment for qualitative research). These open questions are puzzles for us and for our
TE
colleagues, and good research does also puzzle about them a bit further and position
itself in relation to them.
EC

Thirdly, with respect to the accessibility This is an issue that connects with
R

of presentation, clearly accounting for the the commonsense views of the self
conceptual background, research process that circulate in psychology, views that
R

and new perspectives (which may include may only be opened up for study by
accounts, knowledge, interpretations) refusing to speak those descriptions
O

are important. We may also want the work of ourselves that everyone takes for
to be accessible to those outside the granted (Terre Blanche and Durrheim,
C

research community, but there may be times 1999).


when difficult arguments make difficult The questions in Box 3, which explicates
N

reading. With respect to each of these, the ‘parameters of criteria’, are designed to
the study should make clear by what kinds draw attention to assumptions about
U

of rules it should be evaluated, and warrant research that usually govern quantitative
the following or breaking of these rules. research in psychology, but these main-
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:10

8 I Parker

Box 3. Explicating the parameters of criteria

These points summarize key questions that should be considered in the process of
carrying out qualitative research in psychology.

1) Objective? / Have you described what theoretical resources you draw upon to make
your subjectivity into a useful device and how those resources impact on the
research?
2) Valid? / Have you made clear the ways in which the account you give is distinctive
and paradigmatically different from other things that might be categorized along
with it?
3) Reliable? / Have you traced a process of change in your understanding and other

F
people’s understanding of the topic and explored how views of it may continue to
change?

O
4) Neutral? / Is there a reflexive analysis which steps back from the account you have
given and allows the reader to see something of the institutional vantage point from

O
which the story is told?
5) Confirmed? / Is there an attempt to bring research participants’ responses to the

PR
analysis into the study, and an attempt both to clarify the ways in which they agree
and disagree with what you say and to analyse why and how these different
responses may have come about?
6) Definitive? / Is there an attempt to ‘triangulate’ views of the topic and a decision
about whether this triangulation should be taken as arriving at a clearer view or an
D
explication of what is apparent from different vantage points?
7) Established? / If you did not study and refer to an established line of research, did
TE
you discuss the reasons why this may not appear in the research literature?
8) Coherent? / If you did not organize your material in a coherent way, did you say
why you chose a different kind of narrative to display your research and thus
persuade the reader that this work is worthwhile?
EC

9) Accessible? / If you did not arrive at something that could be easily accessible to
someone in the discipline or outside it, did you say why your work needed to be
more complex?
10) Psychological? /Have you made clear that the theoretical or methodological frame-
R

work you have used is from within the domain of psychology, or made clear how the
topic is usually understood by psychology, or examined what the implications
R

might be for psychology of what you have done?


O


C

stream assumptions are placed ‘under Psychological questions / including


erasure’. They are marked in this way questioning psychology
N

precisely to remind you that any checklist


always need to be challenged as such if the Qualitative research is sometimes opposed
U

guidelines are to be flexible enough to leave by quantitatively inclined assessors in the


space for innovations in research practice. discipline on the grounds that it is not
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:10

Criteria for qualitative research in psychology 9

‘really’ psychology. This objection to quali- Three core principles are worth bearing in
tative research is sometimes raised along- mind as we orientate ourselves toward good
side the argument that psychology should research in psychology that is able to
be a ‘science’ (a topic addressed in Box 1). encompass work that is critical of the
This opposition between science and non- underlying assumptions that psychologists
science that is used by some quantitative usually make about what counts as good
psychologists tends to reinforce an unne- research. These principles, outlined by qua-
cessarily firm distinction between quantita- litative researchers in the discipline who
tive and qualitative approaches, a have helped to push the limits of what was
distinction that has itself been challenged acceptable under the old ‘laboratory-experi-
by the most innovative research in psychol- mental’ paradigm, help set out a way of
ogy over the years. We need to remember thinking about how supervisors might facil-

F
that many kinds of research in psychology itate and evaluate student work.
have developed through interdisciplinary The first is that of ‘apprenticeship’. This

O
work which has disturbed the boundaries notion draws attention to the way that
between the discipline of psychology and students learn to speak the language of the

O
other disciplines, and that research in other particular discipline or craft they are learn-
countries which is carried out in psychol- ing, and how they set out any innovations

PR
ogy departments often draws on a range of against the background of those existing
different theoretical frameworks and meth- practices. We are keen to emphasize that
odological approaches. Broadly speaking, qualitative work does not at all mean dis-
though, qualitative research may defend its regarding or throwing away the knowledge
place in the discipline on one or more of the that has been accumulated by psychology.
following grounds.
D
The question we are concerned with is how
First, the theory used or challenged in the the researcher positions themselves against
TE
study may be from within psychology. Here that knowledge. To put the point at its most
we would expect that the theories con- extreme, and for those who really have
cerned are outlined and referenced so that concluded that this knowledge is useless,
EC

a judgement can be made of the pertinence we still expect that the sceptic is able to
of the critique to the study. Secondly, the weave an impression of excellence. Un-
topic being explored or reframed may thinkingly reproducing the language of the
usually be included within the domain of discipline regardless of the research para-
R

psychology. Here we would expect an out- digm that has been adopted is an error
line of the relevant literature on the topic (Burman, 1998).
R

and some questions to be raised about that The second principle is that of ‘scholar-
literature that pave the way for the particu- ship’. We encourage the student to be
O

lar research questions in the study. Thirdly, immersed in the relevant debates so that
that there are psychological implications of they are able to make an argument for what
C

the research clearly stated at the outset. they find to be valuable or unsatisfactory
Here we may expect that the relevant issues about them. A degree of rhetorical, some-
N

concerning ‘psychology’, ‘experience’ or times polemical, skill is required in order to


‘subjectivity’ are outlined and the conse- construct an argument that both marshals
U

quences of adopting one or other of these evidence and steers a course through de-
descriptive terms would be explored. bates in order to persuade the reader. To put
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:10

10 I Parker

this point at its most extreme, and for those the case that a particular methodology
who feel passionately that what psychology becomes the topic of the research, but
says is wrong, we still expect that this even in this case there is a research question
opponent of mainstream psychology is (which focuses on the way something is
able to find some grounds from which investigated). To put this point in the most
they can reason with their audience. All extreme way, for those who do want to break
good scientific research is driven by a all the rules about methodology that they
passion to explore particular questions and have learned in the discipline, we still
to persuade others of a point of view (Billig, expect that this anarchic position is able to
1988). show that it knows very well what it is
The third principle concerns ‘innova- pitting itself against so that it does not let
tion’. There is a corresponding danger here those who simply repeat the tried and tested

F
of ‘methodolatry’, which is that the way in rules of research off the hook. Those who
which the research is designed and carried have simply refused the rules of method

O
out takes precedence and the actual re- without careful argument for the position
search question gets lost. It may indeed be they take and why are all the more likely to

O
PR
Box 4. Don’t stop there! Beyond criteria

The best research goes beyond even these criteria to open up something new, so that the
work is innovative not only with respect to the content of the study (what has been
studied and what has been discovered) but also with respect to its form (how the
D
research questions were explored and how they were interpreted). Three watchwords for
opening the way to the best research are:
TE
1) Apprenticeship / the ability to use existing resources and position oneself within or
in relation to a certain tradition of work. A good competent research report (for a
lower second class grade) is one that displays an understanding of the key issues
EC

outlined in this paper and which has thought through the specific questions that the
guidelines raise.
2) Scholarship / showing that some underlying premises and assumptions in existing
relevant studies have been grasped. A very good research report (for an upper
R

second class grade) is one that brings that understanding to bear in order to
construct an argument, perhaps polemical, against the limits of methodological
R

procedures that may inhibit new ways of doing research.


3) Innovation / producing work that may transform the coordinates by which a
O

problem is usually understood. An excellent research report (for a first class grade)
is one that ‘discovers’ or ‘produces’ something new and which is able to reflexively
C

embed its account of what has happened within and against usual taken-for-granted
practices in research.
N

Use the guidelines insofar as they help, but if necessary challenge the ground-rules
U

that we have used to formulate them!.


Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:10

Criteria for qualitative research in psychology 11

fall foul of ‘methodolatry’ (e.g., Reicher, Burman, E. 1998: Disciplinary apprentices: ‘qua-
2000). litative methods’ in student psychological
These three principles are summed up in research. International Journal of Social
Box 4. Research Methodology 1, 25 /45.
This paper covers the terrain over which Burman, E. 2003: Discourse analysis means
qualitative researchers in psychology may analysing discourse: some comments on
have to travel, and its aim is not so much to Antaki, Billig, Edwards and Potter ‘Dis-
be prescriptive as to clearly outline the course analysis means doing analysis: a
range of issues that researchers (whether critique of six analytic shortcomings’, Dis-
course Analysis Online .
qualitative and quantitative) need to tackle.
The paper focuses on psychology, but also Capdevila, R. 2003: Marginality and methodol-
has strong affinity with debates in qualita- ogy: negotiating legitimacy, ISTP, June.

F
tive evaluation produced by the National Curt, B.C. 1994: Textuality and tectonics: trou-
Health Service Research and Development bling social and psychological science .

O
Programme (Murphy et al ., 1998) and for Buckingham: Open University Press.
the Cabinet Office (Spencer et al ., 2003). It Denzin, N. and Lincoln, Y., editors, 2000: Hand-

O
is important for researchers to bear these book of qualitative research , second edition.
issues in mind when, for example, they do London: Sage.

PR
or do not formulate ‘hypotheses’ before they Dorling, D. and Simpson, S., editors, 1999:
carry out their study, when they do or do Statistics in society: the arithmetic of poli-
not follow a series of methodological steps, tics . London: Arnold.
or when they do or do not separate analysis Elliott, R., Fischer, C.T. and Rennie, D.L. 1999:
from discussion in the written report. Quan- Evolving guidelines for publication of qua-
D
titative researchers too-often follow well litative research studies in psychology and
established procedures without reflecting related fields, British Journal of Clinical
TE
upon what they are doing and why. We Psychology 38, 215 /29.
should not encourage qualitative research- Gough, B., Lawton, R., Madill, A. and Stratton, P.
ers to make the same mistake. Qualitative 2003: Guidelines for the supervision of
research often seems more difficult because undergraduate qualitative research in psy-
EC

it does require a higher level of reflection chology. http://www.psychology.ltsn.ac.uk/


and accountability (to oneself, to colleagues reports.html. Accessed 27 October 2003.
and to others), and this is why an appar- Hartsock, N. 1987: The feminist standpoint:
ently simple issue like ‘criteria’ involves developing the ground for a specifically
R

some sustained conceptual work that feminist historical materialism. In Harding,


should then find its way into the writing S., editor, Feminism and methodology: so-
R

of the report. cial science issues . Bloomington, IN: Indi-


ana University Press.
O

Harré, R. 2004: Staking our claim for qualitative


psychology as science, Qualitative Research
References
C

in Psychology 1, *.
Billig, M. 1988: Methodology and scholarship in Hollway, W. 1989: Subjectivity and method in
N

understanding ideological explanation. In psychology: gender, meaning and science .


Antaki, C., editor, Analysing everyday ex- London: Sage.
U

planation: a casebook of methods . London: Kagan, C. and Burton, M. 2000: Prefigurative


Sage. action research: an alternative basis for
Y:/Arnold/QP/articles/qp010oa/qp010oa.3d[x] Wednesday, 25th February 2004 9:44:10

12 I Parker

critical psychology? Annual Review of Cri- from a feminist perspective . London: Taylor
tical Psychology 2, 73 /87. & Francis.
Kitzinger, C. and Wilkinson, S. 1997: Validating Reicher, S. 2000: Against methodolatry: some
women’s experience? Dilemmas in feminist comments on Elliott, Fischer and Rennie.
research. Feminism and Psychology 7, 566 / British Journal of Clinical Psychology 39, 1 /
74. 6.
Maso, I. 2003: Necessary subjectivity: exploiting Rose, N. 1985: The psychological complex: psy-
researchers’ motives, passions and preju- chology, politics and society in England
dices in pursuit of answering ‘true’ ques- 1869 /1939 . London: Routledge & Kegan
tions. In Finlay, L. and Gough, B., editors, Paul.
Reflexivity: a practical guide for researchers Salmon, P. 2003: How do we recognise good
in health and social sciences . Oxford: Black- research? The Psychologist 16, 24 /27.

F
well. Seale, C. 1999: The quality of qualitative re-
Murphy, E., Dingwall, R. Greatbach, D., Parker, S. search . London: Sage.

O
and Watson, P. 1998: Qualitative research Spencer, L., Ritchie, J., Lewis, J. and Dillon, L.
methods in health technology assessment: a 2003: Quality in qualitative evaluation: a

O
review of the literature. Health Technology framework for assessing research evidence .
Assessment 2(16). Retrieved 18 November, Retrieved 24 November, 2003, from:
2003, from: www.nechta.org www.policyhub.gov.uk

PR
Nightingale, D.J. and Cromby, J., editors, 1999: Terre Blanche, M. and Durrheim, K., editors,
Social constructionist psychology: a critical 1999: Research in practice: applied methods
analysis of theory and practice . Bucking- for the social sciences . Cape Town: UCT
ham: Open University Press. Press. D
Parker, I. 1999: Qualitative data and the subjec- Tindall, C. 1994: Issues of evaluation. In Banister,
tivity of ‘objective’ facts. In Dorling, D. and P., Burman, E., Parker, I., Taylor, M. and
Simpson, L., editors, Statistics in society: the
TE
Tindall, C. 1994: Qualitative methods in
arithmetic of politics. London: Arnold. psychology: a research guide . Buckingham:
Phoenix, A. 1994: Practising feminist research: Open University Press.
the intersection of gender and ‘race’ in the Wilkinson, S. 1988: The role of reflexivity in
EC

research process. In Maynard, M. and Pur- feminist psychology, Women’s Studies Inter-
vis, J., editors, Researching women’s lives national Forum 11:493 /502.

About the author


R

IAN PARKER is Professor of Psychology at Manchester Metropolitan University. He has been


teaching qualitative methods for over 20 years and is author of numerous books on
R

innovative research and radical theory, including Qualitative Methods in Psychology (with
Banister, Burman, Taylor and Tindall, published by Open University Press in 1994) and
O

Critical Discursive Psychology (Palgrave, 2002). His next method book is Qualitative
Psychology: Introducing Radical Research (Open University Press, 2005).
C
N
U

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться