Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

A multivariate model of physics problem solving


Gita Taasoobshirazi a,⁎, John Farley b
a
Kennesaw State University, Department of Secondary & Middle Grades Education, United States
b
University of Nevada, Las Vegas Department of Physics, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: A model of expertise in physics problem solving was tested on undergraduate science, physics, and engineer-
Received 12 October 2011 ing majors enrolled in an introductory-level physics course. Structural equation modeling was used to test
Received in revised form 11 March 2012 hypothesized relationships among variables linked to expertise in physics problem solving including motiva-
Accepted 1 May 2012
tion, metacognitive planning, strategy use, categorization skills, and free-body diagrams. Results indicated
that student motivation had a significant influence on metacognitive planning, strategy use, and categoriza-
Keywords:
Physics
tion skills. Categorization skills influenced problem-solution scores directly, and indirectly, through strategy
Problem solving use. Categorization skills also had a significant influence on free-body diagrams. Free-body diagrams and
Expertise metacognitive planning both had a significant influence on strategy use. Finally, strategy use had a significant
Motivation influence on problem-solution scores. The implications of these findings for future research and developing
Statistics/multivariate students' expertise in physics problem solving are discussed.
© 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction A number of critical variables contribute to experts' success in a


domain like physics, and discriminates experts from novices. Within
One major goal in educating college physics students is to foster their domain of expertise, experts use more goal-directed strategies
their expertise in physics problem solving. Carl Wieman, a recipient when solving problems, have greater and more organized knowledge,
of the Noble Prize in physics, explains this goal in the following way: are more metacognitive, have greater motivation, and tend to receive
more social support (e.g., Alexander, 2003; Ericsson, 2006; Hatano &
Expert competence is a primary goal of education and is an area in
Oura, 2003; Zimmerman, 2006). The research on expertise in physics
which research has provided useful insights. An apt metaphor is
problem solving suggests that the following variables are important:
that of the student and the expert separated by the mental equiv-
the way physics problems are solved, including the actual problem
alent of a canyon; the function of teaching is to guide the student
solving strategies used; the way problems are categorized which sug-
along the path that leads safely and effectively across the canyon
gests depth, breadth, and organization of conceptual knowledge; the
to the nirvana of expert like thinking. Educational research, careful
metacognitive strategies used; and whether problems are pictorially
measurement, and new technology make it possible to guide most
represented in the form of a free-body diagram (e.g., Anzai, 1991;
students safely across the path towards a true understanding and
Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009; Chi, 2006). These variables contribute
appreciation of physics (Wieman & Perkins, 2005, pp. 38–40).
to the development of expertise in physics problem solving and dis-
criminate experts from novices in physics.
Unfortunately, research has found that college physics instruction
Although research specifies the variables that lead to expertise in
often does a poor job at helping students acquire the knowledge and
physics problem solving, these variables are usually studied individual-
skills needed for expertise in physics problem solving. Students are
ly. There is a lack of research examining the relationships among these
leaving both their introductory level and more advanced level physics
variables and a lack of research indicating which variables are most im-
courses confused about important concepts in mechanics, optics,
portant for problem-solving success. As a result, physics instructors do
electricity, and magnetism, and using poor problem-solving strategies
not know what should be focused on during instruction and what is of
(e.g., Henderson, 2005; McDermott, 2001). In response, physics edu-
less importance. Little research provides guidance on the best way to
cators are calling for reform in the teaching and learning of physics
move students towards more expert levels of performance, although re-
because they would like all of their students to become, relatively
searchers are suggesting we do just that (e.g. Gerace, 2001; Wieman &
speaking, experts in solving the problems posed in these courses
Perkins, 2005). This is especially important for physics students who
(Wieman & Perkins, 2005).
are majoring in science, physics, and engineering and developing their
expertise in physics problem solving.
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 770 339 8281. Furthermore, within the domain of physics and most sciences, the
E-mail address: gita.tshirazi@gmail.com (G. Taasoobshirazi). research on expertise focuses almost exclusively on cognitive

1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.05.001
54 G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

variables, including differences in problem-solving skills and knowl- complex and additional factors (e.g., forces) begin to play a role in the
edge. There is little examination of the role that motivation plays in problems.
influencing expert problem solving. Given these limitations and the Before solving a problem, experts tend to draw a free-body dia-
need to understand and support students' expertise in physics problem gram. Novices, in contrast, tend to focus solely on setting up and solv-
solving, the goal of the present study was to examine the research on ex- ing equations (Dhillon, 1998; Larkin, McDermott, Simon, & Simon,
pert and novice differences in problem solving in general, and in physics 1980; Larkin & Simon, 1987; Stylianou & Silver, 2004). When novices
in particular, in order to formulate and test a model of expertise in physics do draw free-body diagrams, the diagrams are simpler and less thor-
problem solving. The model identifies the important components of prob- ough than those drawn by experts; experts tend to draw more com-
lem solving, describes how these components influence each other, and plex pictures, including more of the important factors that play a
quantifies the relative contributions of each component. Specifically, the role in a problem (e.g., Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009). It is thought
model examines the influence of motivation, metacognitive planning, that novices often fail to draw adequate free-body diagrams because
strategy use, categorization skills, and free-body diagrams on problem- they do not understand the concepts and principles underlying the
solution scores. The knowledge that results from studying these compo- problems they are solving (Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009). Thus, as
nents, and how they ideally interact, can be used to improve the teaching novices progress towards expertise and gain more conceptual knowl-
and learning of physics problem solving. Because some of the edge, the use of free-body diagrams is likely to increase, as is the qual-
problem-solving components (e.g., strategy use, categorization skills) in ity of those diagrams.
physics are similar to those in chemistry, biology, and other sciences
(e.g., Stains & Talanquer, 2008; Williams & Noyes, 2007), the model will 2.2. Strategy use
be informative to researchers and instructors in other sciences as well.
A second major difference between experts and novices is in the
2. Expertise in physics problem solving strategies they use to solve physics problems. Novices tend to use
the working-backward strategy when solving physics problems
A review of the research on the variables used to develop the (Chi, 2006; Larkin, 1985; Williams & Noyes, 2007). They start with
model (Fig. 1) is presented in detail below. The research examining an equation that contains the goal and other unknowns that cannot
what is important for expertise in physics problem solving typically be directly calculated. The novices then construct additional equa-
does so by comparing experts to novices as they solve or categorize tions to calculate these unknowns. When the unknowns are calculat-
physics problems. ed, the novices reverse, or chain backwards, inserting the values of
the unknowns in the preceding equations, in order to solve the prob-
2.1. Free-body diagrams lem. The process is data driven with a goal of performing calculations
to solve equations to find unknowns. Experts, in contrast, tend to use
One major difference between experts and novices in physics is in the working-forward strategy (Chi, 2006; Davidson, 2003; Williams &
their use of pictorial representations. These pictorial representations, Noyes, 2007). When solving physics problems, experts start with un-
typically called free-body diagrams, are used by a problem solver to de- known quantities that can be directly calculated and construct a set of
pict key variables (e.g., objects, forces) and their relationships in a phys- equations that lead to the goal of the problem. This process is more
ics problem. Drawing a free-body diagram before beginning to work on goal directed and cognitively efficient (i.e., exerting less cognitive
calculations allows the problem solver to determine which approach to load on working memory) than the working-backward strategy
the problem is appropriate, to identify the forces and energies at work, (Davidson, 2003; Sweller, 2003).
and to reduce the amount of information that must be attended to at Although the working-forward and working-backward strategies
one time (Anderson & Leinhardt, 2002; Rosengrant, Van Heuvelen & may result in the same answer, the working-forward strategy is consid-
Etkina, 2009; Friedman & diSessa, 1999; Hewitt, 2005). Drawing a ered to be purposeful problem solving (Snyder, 2000). In contrast, the
free-body diagram is particularly important as problems become more working-backward strategy involves manipulating equations with al-
most no planning and little conceptual understanding of what is being
Motivation done (Williams & Noyes, 2007). It is for these reasons that the
working-forward strategy is more likely to lead to the correct problem
solution (e.g., Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009; Zajchowski & Martin, 1993).
Although research indicates that the working-forward strategy is used
primarily by experts, novices are capable of using this strategy. For exam-
ple, Zajchowski and Martin (1993) examined 10 introductory-level col-
lege physics students solving mechanics problems and thinking aloud as
Metacognitive
Planning they solved the problems. They found that the more novice problem
solvers (as assessed by a pretest), who had been instructed to conceptu-
ally analyze problems prior to solving them, were using the working-
forward strategy to the same extent as the more expert problem solvers.
The authors suggested that these results were due to the novices' practice
conceptually analyzing the mechanics problems. One goal of testing the
Strategy Problem model in the present study was to examine the extent to which the
Categorization working-forward strategy is linked to conceptual knowledge of physics.

2.3. Conceptual knowledge: problem categorization

Another major difference between experts and novices is in their


conceptual knowledge. Experts have greater domain knowledge,
Problem Free-Body and this knowledge is better organized in comparison to that of nov-
Solution Diagram
ices (Alexander, 2003; Hatano & Oura, 2003; Sabella & Redish, 2007).
In physics, evidence of the difference between experts and novices in
Fig. 1. Theoretical model of expertise in physics problem solving. their conceptual knowledge and how they store, relate, and use this
G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62 55

knowledge can be found in how they categorize problems (Chi, 2.5. Motivation
Feltovich, & Glaser, 1981; Snyder, 2000; Zimmerman & Campillo,
2003). In these studies, conceptual knowledge is operationally defined Although not described in the literature on expertise in physics prob-
as the categorization (sorting) of problems by type (e.g., kinematics). lem solving, the role of motivation in influencing expertise is expected to
Studies of problem categorization indicate that expert problem be critical in any domain (e.g., Ericsson, 1996, 2004, 2006). Students who
solvers tend to view two problems as similar when the same law or are highly motivated engage persistently in the deliberate practice that
principle can be applied to the problems. The reason for this is that leads to expertise (Ericsson, 2006; Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Romer,
experts' knowledge is based on principles critical to a problem's solu- 1993). Deliberate practice involves hard work, which is unlikely to
tion, not on details that are superficial to a solution. Experts are able occur without significant motivation (Ericsson, 2006; Zimmerman &
to focus on meaningful patterns of information, identify relevant Campillo, 2003). Research indicates that the important components that
cues as to what constitutes important information, and make accurate should be taken into account when examining students' motivation in sci-
inferences based on that information (Hambrick & Engle, 2003). ence include intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, task relevancy,
Novice problem solvers, in contrast, tend to view two problems as self-determination, self-efficacy, and assessment anxiety (e.g., Duncan &
similar when the problems share the same surface features such as McKeachie, 2005; Glynn & Koballa, 2006; Glynn, Taasoobshirazi, &
terminology or objects (Chi et al., 1981; Chi & Slotta, 1993; Williams Brickman, 2007; Pintrich & Schunk, 2002).
& Noyes, 2007). The reason for this is that novices often either lack Motivation to perform a task for its own sake is intrinsic, whereas
relevant knowledge, or the knowledge they have is less complete, motivation to perform a task as a means to an end is extrinsic (Ryan &
interconnected, and accurate (Wieman & Perkins, 2005). Further- Deci, 2000). Students who are intrinsically motivated work on a task
more, novices are typically unable to differentiate relevant problem because they find it interesting; students who are extrinsically moti-
information from irrelevant details. And, even when novices can dis- vated work on a task to attain a desirable outcome such as a good
tinguish relevant from irrelevant details, they are usually unable to grade. Both types of motivation, however, are important in contribut-
generate inferences and relations not explicitly stated in the prob- ing to students' success (Ryan & Deci, 2000).
lems (Hambrick & Engle, 2003). Other important components of motivation include task relevancy
Differences in problem categorizations have been linked to strategy and self-determination. How relevant students see a task to their per-
use and problem-solution scores with principle-based categorizations sonal goals has been found to influence time and effort spent on the
supporting the more expert working-forward strategy and accurate task (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). Self-determination refers to students
problem-solving solutions (Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009; Schneider, having some choice in and control over their learning (Reeve, Hamm,
1993). For the present study, problem categorization was expected to & Nix, 2003). For instance, when college science students have the
directly predict strategy use, problem-solution scores, and free-body opportunity to choose what their assignments will be, they are
diagrams. more likely to learn from the assignments (Glynn & Koballa, 2006).
Self-efficacy refers to students' beliefs about their capabilities in a
specific area, which influences choice of activities and achievement
2.4. Metacognitive planning (Bong, 2001). Zusho and Pintrich (2003) found that even after control-
ling for prior achievement, students' self-efficacy was the best predictor
Metacognitive planning, a component of metacognition, involves of grades in an introductory-level college chemistry course. Finally, as-
thinking about what one needs in order to accomplish a goal and think- sessment anxiety is an important component of motivation. A high
ing about how one intends to achieve that goal. In a problem-solving level of assessment anxiety has been found to interfere with a student's
context, this includes thinking about what a problem is asking, possible performance on a task, and students perform best when their level of
strategies for solving a problem, and how to most successfully imple- anxiety is at a low to moderate level (e.g., Cassady & Johnson, 2002).
ment a strategy in order to solve a problem (Schraw & Moshman, The work by Ericsson (2006) discusses the importance of deliber-
1995; Shin, Jonassen, & McGee, 2003). Ideally, planning occurs before ate practice for expertise, but high levels of practice are a symptom of
beginning a problem, and it occurs throughout the problem-solving a number of underlying motivations. For instance, students who en-
process as a student revisits what is needed in order to attain a goal gage in practice both see the domain as relevant and believe that
(Zimmerman, 1989). they will be able to achieve expertise in that domain. For the present
Metacognitive planning, more than any other metacognitive com- study, motivation was expected to be directly linked to metacognitive
ponent, has been found to be important for expertise in problem solv- planning, strategy use, and categorizations.
ing (Schraw, Crippen, & Hartley, 2006; Zimmerman, 2002). Research
indicates that experts are more metacognitive in comparison to 3. Present study
novices primarily because they spend more time planning during
scientific problem solving (Schraw, 1998; Schraw et al., 2006). For ex- The present study refined and tested—by means of structural equa-
ample, experts do more planning before beginning calculations, set- tion modeling—a theoretical model of expertise in physics problem
ting subordinate goals that are hierarchically related to their overall solving. The model—developed by (Taasoobshirazi & Carr, 2009)—was
goal of solving a problem. Furthermore, experts are more motivated refined by the inclusion of metacognitive planning, consistent with
than novices to engage in metacognitive planning (Zimmerman, the perspective on metacognitive planning recently described by
1998, 2002). Lajoie (2008) and Schraw et al. (2006). The goal was to refine, extend
Although research indicates the importance of metacognitive on, and retest the previous model, and examine the interaction among
planning for expertise in physics problem solving (e.g., Neto & the model variables once metacognitive planning was included in the
Valente, 1997; Zimmerman, 2006), little is known about exactly model. The model tested in the present study depicts relationships
how planning influences success in physics problem solving. A critical among key components of students' physics problem solving, including
question addressed in this study is: Does metacognitive planning sup- motivation, metacognitive planning, strategy use, categorization skills,
port the use of an effective strategy, which, in turn, supports success- and free-body diagrams. By means of structural equation modeling, re-
ful problem solving? Also of interest was the extent to which lationships among these components are examined, and the students'
metacognitive planning is linked to and influenced by motivation. success in physics problem solving explained.
For the present study, motivation was expected to directly influence Using the existing research on expert and novice differences in
metacognitive planning, and planning was expected to support the problem solving in general, and in physics in particular, the model
use of the working-forward strategy. shown in Fig. 1 was developed. As can be seen in the figure, students
56 G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

with higher motivation were expected to be more inclined to engage learning (e.g., I enjoy learning physics), extrinsically motivated phys-
in the practice that would support them learning the material at a deeper ics learning (e.g., I like to do better than other students on physics
level, resulting in better categorizations. Similarly, students with higher tests), relevance of learning physics to personal goals (e.g., The phys-
motivation were expected to engage in more problem-solving practice, ics I learn relates to my personal goals.), self-determination for learn-
supporting the more expert working-forward strategy. Furthermore, ing physics (e.g., It is my fault if I do not understand physics.),
motivated students were expected to be more likely to engage in self-efficacy in learning physics (e.g., I believe I can master the knowl-
metacognitive planning when solving physics problems. Metacognitive edge and skills in physics courses.), and anxiety about physics assess-
planning was expected to influence strategy use, specifically the ment (e.g., I become anxious when it is time to take a physics test.).
working-forward strategy. Therefore, it was expected that students Students responded to each of the 30 randomly-ordered items on a
who planned out how they would solve problems before beginning cal- 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always) from the
culations would be more likely to use the more expert working-forward perspective of when learning physics. The anxiety about physics as-
strategy. sessment items were reverse scored when added to the total, so
Conceptual knowledge, as assessed by problem categorizations, was that a higher score on this component meant less anxiety.
expected to influence problem-solution scores both directly, and indi- Previous findings (Glynn & Koballa, 2006) indicate that the PMQ is
rectly, through use of the more expert working-forward strategy. reliable as measured by coefficient alpha (α = .93), and valid in terms
Consistent with the research, better problem categorizations were of positive correlations with college students' physics grades, interest
expected to support better free-body diagrams. With better diagrams, in physics-related careers, and number of physics courses taken. For
the use of the working-forward strategy was expected to increase as a this study, coefficient alpha was found to be .88.
deeper understanding of the concepts and principles and their relation-
ship to one another increased. Finally, the working-forward strategy 4.2.2. Metacognitive planning
was expected to directly predict problem-solution scores. Items from Howard, McGee, Shia, and Hong's (2000) Metacognitive
Because all of the model variables are observed rather than latent Self-Regulation for problem solving were administered to students. The
and because there is one measure of each variable, the structural items were adapted for physics Problem Solving. The adapted items ad-
equation modeling technique of path analysis was used to examine ministered in the present study were:
the hypothesized relationships among the variables (Kline, 2005).
• I try to understand what the physics problem is asking me.
Using path analysis, the specific goals were to estimate the direct
• I think about what information I need to solve the physics problem.
and indirect effects in the model, to control for the correlations
• I make sure I complete each step of the physics problem.
among the hypothesized causal variables, and to decompose the ob-
• I identify all the important parts of the physics problem.
served correlations into their component parts.
• I try to understand the physics problem so I know what to do.
• I pick out the steps I need to solve a physics problem.
4. Method
• I try and break down the physics problem to just the necessary
information.
4.1. Participants
These items were selected and categorized as metacognitive plan-
Subjects included 125 Georgia and Nevada students (90 men and 35 ning because of the similarity of these items and the planning items
women) from a total of approximately 171 students (119 men and 52 on Schraw and Dennison's (1994) metacognitive awareness invento-
women) enrolled in introductory-level physics courses (trigonometry- ry. Students responded to each of these items on a 5-point Likert scale
based or calculus-based physics) for science, physics, and engineering ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Items were found to be reliable
majors. Regarding ethnicity, 58 students were Caucasian (46%), 29 in terms of their internal consistency, as measured by coefficient
were Asian (23%), 27 were Hispanic (22%), 10 were African American alpha (α = .81). Any missing data/responses for the items and for
(8%), and 1 was Native American (0.8%). the PMQ were scored using a response on a similar item.
Overall, a majority of the students participated in the study (73%),
and students volunteered to earn a small amount of extra credit and 4.2.3. Strategy use, free-body diagrams, and problem-solution scores
help study how students solve physics problems. Students' participation Five mechanics problems were administered to the students
was voluntary rather than compulsory, as specified by the guidelines for (Table 1). The students were asked to solve the five quantitative,
research with human participants specified by the institutional review well-defined problems, showing their work, and producing the math-
board. The students participated towards the end of the 15-week ematical solution associated with each. Each of the problems is based
semester: At this point in time, the students had already covered the on one of five major topics in mechanics including Kinematics, Forces
types of mechanics problems that they were asked to solve and catego- and Newton's Laws of Motion, Work and Energy, Impulse and Mo-
rize in this study. mentum, and Simple Harmonic Motion and Elasticity (Cutnell &
Johnson, 2007). The first problem involves Forces and Newton's
4.2. Procedure and materials Laws of Motion; the second problem involves Kinematics; the third
problem involves Impulse and Momentum; the fourth problem in-
Students were administered a packet that included, in order, a moti- volves Work and Energy; the fifth problem involves Simple Harmonic
vation questionnaire, questions assessing metacognitive planning, five Motion and Elasticity.
physics problems designed to assess strategy use, free-body diagrams, The first of the five problems was designed by Priest and Lindsay
and problem-solution scores, and four categorization tasks designed (1992), and used in their own study assessing the working-forward
to assess if students are focusing on conceptual or surface features of and working-backward strategies with intermediate-level physics stu-
physics problems. Students were asked to complete the packet individ- dents. The other four problems were from a physics final examination
ually. The packet can be obtained by contacting the author. used by a professor teaching the course at a university that did not par-
ticipate in the study. This was done to ensure that the problems were dif-
4.2.1. Motivation ferent than those the students had seen in their course. Strategy-scoring
Student motivation in physics was assessed using the Physics Mo- guidelines based on those by Chi (2006) and Priest and Lindsay (1992)
tivation Questionnaire (PMQ) (Glynn & Koballa, 2006). The PMQ in- were used to score the strategies as either working forward or working
cludes 30 items that assess six important components of student backward. For the five problems, the less efficient working-backward
motivation in physics including intrinsically motivated physics strategy was identified as being used by a student when the first
G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62 57

Table 1 students received one point for each main factor pictorially repre-
The physics problems solved by the students. sented with scores ranging from 0 to 15.
A block of mass 7 kg starts sliding down a plane of length 5 m, inclined at an angle of In order to obtain problem-solution scores, students' responses on
30° to the horizontal. If the coefficient of friction between the block and the plane is the five physics problems were scored one point for correct and zero
0.2, find the velocity vt of the block when it reaches the bottom of the plane. points for incorrect. However, if a simple calculation error occurred,
An airplane flies horizontally with a speed of 300 m/s at an altitude of 400 m.
full credit was still given for the problem. This occurred in only
Assume that the ground is level. What horizontal distance from a target must
the pilot release a bomb so as to hit the target? about 50 of the 625 possible instances (8%). A simple calculation
A 0.15 kg steel ball is dropped onto a steel plate where its speed just before impact error was computational as opposed to conceptual, and there was ev-
and after impact is 4.5 m/s and 4.2 m/s, respectively. If the ball is in contact with idence in the work shown that indicated that the student was able to
the plate for .03 seconds, what is the magnitude of the average force (in N)
correctly set up the problem.
applied by the plate on the ball?
An escalator is 30.0 meters long and slants 30° relative to the horizontal. If it
moves at 1.00 m/s, at what rate does it do work in lifting a 50.0 kg man from 4.2.4. Problem categorization
the bottom to the top of the escalator? Four problem categorization tasks based on major topics in me-
A 1.0 kg block is released from rest at the top of a frictionless incline that makes an chanics including Kinematics, Forces and Newton's Laws of Motion,
angle of 37° with the horizontal. An unknown distance down the incline from
Work and Energy, and Impulse and Momentum (Cutnell & Johnson,
the point of release, there is a spring with k = 200 N/m. It is observed that the
mass is brought momentarily to rest after compressing the spring 0.20 m. 2007) were administered to the students. Each task included four
What distance does the mass slide from the point of release until it is brought physics problems, two problems from two major subtopics within a
momentarily to rest? major topic. Students were not required to solve the problems, but
were asked to categorize the four problems into pairs, and then de-
scribe in detail why they felt those particular problems went togeth-
equation generated attempted to solve (impossibly) for the sought er. For the first task, concerning the topic of Kinematics, the problems
quantity, and additional equations attempted to solve for unknown involved either kinematics in one dimension or kinematics in two di-
quantities introduced by previous equations. These newly found quanti- mensions. For the second task, Forces and Newton's Laws of Motion,
ties were then inserted into the preceding equations in order to work the problems involved either the equilibrium or nonequilibrium ap-
backwards to solve the problem. The more efficient working-forward plication of Newton's laws of motion. For the third task, Work and En-
strategy was identified as being used by a student when the first equa- ergy, the problems involved either work done by a constant force or
tion contained only a single unknown. Each subsequent equation provid- work done by a varying force. Finally for the fourth task, Impulse
ed the value of a single unknown quantity, resulting in equations that led and Momentum, the problems involved either collisions in one di-
to the sought quantity. mension or collisions in two dimensions. Examples of the physics
Students received zero points for using the working-backward strat- problems categorized by the students can be seen in Table 2. The
egy and one point for using the working-forward strategy. Consistent problems for the tasks came from two intermediate-level college
with the research scoring the working-forward and working-backward physics textbooks (Cutnell & Johnson, 2001; Serway & Faughn,
strategies, the strategies were found to fall clearly into one of the two cat- 2002) not being used at the participating universities.
egories so that strategy use for each problem was scored as either work- Students' categorizations and explanations were used to determine
ing forward or working backward. Points were summed across the if students were focusing on surface features of the problems or on
problems and ranged from zero to five. the underlying conceptual laws and principles. In order to deal with
Each problem was also analyzed for the quality of free-body diagrams. chance levels of correct responses to the pairings, correct explanations
Typically, prior studies have only scored whether or not a picture was cre- were needed to receive full credit for the pairings of problems. Specifi-
ated during problem solving. For this study, the quality of student dia- cally, for each task, students could receive up to three points: one point
grams was examined because, more recently, considerable emphasis in for correctly categorizing the problems, and one point for each correct
physics instruction is placed on the importance of students drawing a di- explanation. Students could receive up to a total of 12 points, where a
agram to accompany the problems they are solving (e.g., Taasoobshirazi higher score suggested a focus on and understanding of laws and prin-
& Carr, 2009; Rosengrant et al., 2009). Physics textbooks, college physics ciples when encountering physics problems.
instructors, and researchers are emphasizing the importance of drawing
free-body diagrams when solving physics problems. Thus, it was 5. Results
expected that students who do not necessarily have a good understand-
ing of the material would attempt to draw free-body diagrams, but the di- The Statistical Program for the Social Sciences, version 14.0 (SPSS
agrams would be more simple and less thorough than those drawn by Inc., 2005) was used to compute descriptive statistics, mean compari-
students who have a good conceptual understanding of the material. sons, and correlations among the model variables. Structural equation
The diagrams were scored by comparing the students' diagrams to a modeling was used to test the model. Specifically, LISREL Version 8.80
target sketch for each problem. The target sketches for the five prob- (Jörkeskog & Sörbom, 2007), with a covariance matrix generated by
lems were established by a college physics professor and included the PRELIS Version 2.80 (Jörkeskog & Sorbom, 2006) was used to test the
necessary factors needed to have a complete and inclusive diagram. theoretical model.
For the first problem, a complete sketch included the representation
of three forces (normal force, frictional force, and force of gravity broken 5.1. Descriptive statistics, mean comparisons, and correlations
into its horizontal and vertical components), and one angle, resulting in
a total of four main factors. In the second problem, a complete sketch in- The results of independent-samples t-tests indicated that there
cluded two important factors, the change in the vertical distance, and was a significant difference between the men (M=104.96, SD=15.75)
the horizontal velocity. In the third problem, a complete sketch in- and women (M= 98.94, SD= 12.33) in their motivation ratings
cluded two important factors, the velocity of the object both before t(123) = −2.028, p =.05. The results of a one-way ANOVA indicated
and after collision. In the fourth problem, a complete sketch included no significant differences between students in the different ethnic
a total of four factors, including two forces (the normal force and the groups on any of the model variables. The data for all students were
force of gravity broken into its horizontal and vertical components), combined for analyses.
length of the slant, and the angle of the slant. In the fifth problem, a All correlations are reported in Table 3. Among the model vari-
complete sketch included the angle, spring, and length of the com- ables, there were significant correlations between motivation and
pression of the spring, for a total of three factors. For each sketch, strategy use, motivation and metacognitive planning, and motivation
58 G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

Table 2 were solved incorrectly using the working-forward strategy (112) than
Examples of the physics problems categorized by the students. the working-backward strategy (288).
Kinematics: Kinematics in one dimension
Starting from rest, a jet is moves forward with a constant acceleration of +31 m/s2 5.2. Model testing
along a straight line and reaches a velocity of +6.2 m/s. Find the displacement of
the jet.
Before empirically testing the model, the data were examined for
Kinematics: Kinematics in two dimensions
You are driving in a convertible with the top down. The car is moving at a normality and homoscedasticity. Based on the data plots (histograms
constant velocity of 25 m/s, due east along flat ground. You throw a tomato of the variables), examination of skewness and kurtosis statistics
straight upward at a speed of 11 m/s. How far has the car moved when you (Table 3), and Mardia's coefficient = 1.01, the data were consistent
get a chance to catch the tomato?
with the assumptions of both univariate and multivariate normality,
Forces and Newton's Laws of Motion: Equilibrium application
A jet plane is flying with a constant speed along a straight line, at an angle of 30°
justifying the use of maximum-likelihood estimation to test the
above the horizon. The plane has weight W whose magnitude is 86,500 N, and model.
its engines provide a forward thrust T of magnitude T= 103,000 N. In addition, The overall fit of the model was very good, as indicated by a number
the lift force L (directed perpendicular to the wings) and the force R of air resis- of fit indices, all of which are described in detail in the paragraphs
tance (directed opposite to the motion) act on the plane. Find L and R.
below. Any given index evaluates only a particular aspect of model fit.
Forces and Newton's Laws of Motion: Non-equilibrium application
A 1300 kg car is moving due east with an initial speed of 27.0 m/s. After 8.00 s Therefore, to evaluate the fit of the model, it is recommended that
the car has slowed down to 17.0 m/s. Find the magnitude and direction of the several fit indices be used (Kline, 2005). First, the chi-square statistic
net force that causes the car to slow down. was used. The chi-square is a fit index that addresses the degree to
Work and Energy: Work done by a constant force
which the variances and covariances implied by the model match the
A student removes a 10.5 kg amplifier from a shelf that is 1.85 m high. The
amplifier is lowered at a constant speed to a height of 0.75 m. What is the
observed variances and covariances. A non-significant chi-square indi-
work done by (a) the person and (b) the gravitational force that acts on the cates that the model is a good representation of the underlying covari-
amplifier? ance matrix. The chi-square, χ2 (6) = 10.96, p = .09, indicated a good fit
Work and Energy: Work done by a varying force because the p-value was greater than .05. Further, the χ2/df ratio was
A force acting on an object is given by Fx = (8x − 16) N, where x is in meters.
1.83, suggesting a good fit based on Kline's (1998) rule that values
Find the net work done by this force as the object moves from x = 0 to x = 3.
Impulse and Momentum: Collisions in one dimension less than three indicate a good fit.
Kevin has a mass of 87 kg and is skating with in-line skates. He sees his 22 kg The Steiger–Lind root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA)
younger brother straight ahead skating straight towards him. Kevin grabs his assesses a lack of fit of the data to the estimated model. It is an index that
brother, holds on, and rolls off at a speed of 2.4 m/s. Ignoring friction, find includes adjustments for model complexity so that evaluation of fit is not
Kevin's speed just before he grabbed his brother.
Impulse and Momentum: Collisions in two dimensions
overly influenced by the number of parameters in the model (Steiger,
A 2000 kg car moving east at 10.0 m/s collides with a 3000 kg car moving north. 1995). Browne and Cudeck (1993) suggest that a RMSEA value of .08 or
The cars stick together and move as a unit after the collision north of east and at less indicates a good model fit. The RMSEA for this model was .07.
a speed of 5.22 m/s. Find the speed of the 3000 kg car before the collision. The standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) is an index
based on the residuals between the observed and estimated covari-
and categorization skills. There was also a significant correlation be- ance matrices (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The advantage of the SRMR is
tween metacognitive planning and strategy use, indicating that en- that it is sensitive to model misspecification (Hu & Bentler, 1998).
gaging in metacognitive planning when solving physics problems is Worthington and Whittaker (2006) suggest that a SRMR value of
related to the use of the more expert working-forward strategy. .10 or less indicates a good model fit. The SRMR for this model was
The correlation between categorization skills and free-body diagrams .08.
was significant, indicating that a focus on the conceptual aspects of phys- The incremental fit index (IFI) is a fit index that is sensitive to
ics problems and material is related to more complex diagrams; diagram model misspecification, but not to sample size (Bentler, 1990; Hu &
use was significantly correlated with strategy use. Furthermore, there Bentler, 1999; Widaman & Thompson, 2003), making it a valuable in-
were significant correlations between categorization skills and strategy dication of fit. The IFI compares the model to a baseline model in
use and categorization skills and problem-solution scores, indicating which all variables are assumed to be uncorrelated. This is the stan-
that a focus on the conceptual aspects of physics problems and material dard null model (independence model) that assumes zero population
is related to more expert strategy use and higher problem-solution covariances among the observed variables. The IFI values range from
scores. Finally, strategy use was significantly and highly correlated with 0 to 1, with larger values indicating a better fit. A value greater than
problem-solution scores. Thus the use of the more expert working- .95 is considered to be a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The value for
forward strategy is related to increased problem-solving accuracy. this model was .98.
More problems were solved correctly using the working-forward strate- Finally, the Bentler comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, 1990) was
gy (199) than the working-backward strategy (26), and fewer problems used to compare the model to a baseline model in which the covari-
ances among all the variables are assumed to be zero. Like the IFI,
Table 3 this index ranges from 0 to 1, with larger values indicating a better
Correlation matrix, means, standard deviations, skewness, and kurtosis for the physics fit. A value greater than .95 is considered to be a good fit (Hu &
students. Bentler, 1999). The value for this model was .98.
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 Path analysis was used to estimate the direct and indirect effects
in the model, control for the correlations among the hypothesized
Motivation –
Planning .56⁎⁎ –
variables, and “decompose” the observed correlations into their com-
Categorization .21⁎ .18⁎ – ponent parts. The standardized path values and their associated
Diagram .24⁎⁎ .27⁎⁎ .27⁎⁎ – t-values for the model are reported in Table 4; the model with stan-
Strategy .34⁎⁎ .37⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .45⁎⁎ – dardized path values can be seen in Fig. 2. A cutoff value of t = 1.96
Solution .27⁎⁎ .32⁎⁎ .52⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .79⁎⁎ –
for a two-tailed test was used to determine if direct and indirect
Mean 103.27 29.64 4.84 6.84 2.49 1.64
SD 15.07 3.67 3.61 3.77 1.65 1.36 paths were statistically significant. In terms of size and influence of
Skewness .08 −.79 .95 −.45 −.04 .52 the standardized path values, Keith's (1993) recommended criteria
Kurtosis −.31 .78 .67 −.77 −1.20 −.61 were used: standardized path values ranging from .05 to .10 are
⁎ p b .05. small, but meaningful influences; path values ranging from .11 to
⁎⁎ p b .01. .25 are moderate in size and influence, and path values above .25
G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62 59

are large in size and influence. The criterion R 2 (proportion of vari- Motivation
ance explained) by strategy use was .34, by problem categorization
was .04, by metacognitive planning was .32, by free-body diagram
was .07, and by problem-solution score was .65.
The decomposition of effects can be seen in Table 4. All of the direct
.56
paths were statistically significant. Student motivation had a significant
and large influence (.29) on strategy use, indicating that students with .29
Metacognitive
.21
higher motivation are more likely to use the working-forward strategy. Planning
The influence of motivation on problem categorization was significant
and moderate in size (.21), indicating that students with higher motiva-
tion are more likely to focus on the conceptual aspects of physics prob- .18
lems and material. Motivation had a significant and large influence (.56)
on metacognitive planning, indicating that students with higher moti-
vation are more likely to engage in metacognitive planning when .39
Strategy Problem
solving physics problems. Planning had a significant and moderate in- Categorization
fluence (.18) on strategy use, indicating that students who are more .31 .49
likely to think about a problem and how they will solve it are more like-
ly to use the working-forward strategy. .69 .27
Student categorizations had a significant and large influence (.27)
on free-body diagrams, indicating that a focus on the more conceptu-
al aspects of physics problems and material supports more complex Problem Free-Body
Solution Diagram
and expert diagrams. Free-body diagrams had a significant and large
influence (.31) on strategy use; problem categorization also had a sig-
nificant and large influence (.39) on strategy use. Thus more expert Fig. 2. Tested model of expertise in physics problem solving, with standardized path
pictures and a focus on the conceptual aspects of physics problems values.

and material support the use of the more expert working-forward


strategy. The working-forward strategy had a significant and large in- how to solve it supports better strategy use, which, in turn, supports
fluence (.69) on problem-solution scores. Finally, problem categoriza- accurate problem solutions. Problem categorization had a significant
tion had a significant and large influence (.49) on problem-solution influence on problem-solution scores through its influence on strate-
scores. gy use, as the path from Categorization to Strategy to Solution was
Indirect paths and associated t-values can also be seen in Table 4. significant and large in size (.27). The indirect path from Categoriza-
Motivation had a significant influence on problem-solution scores tion to Diagram to Strategy was small in size (.08), but significant, in-
through its influence on strategy use and problem categorization. dicating that a focus on conceptual aspects of physics problems and
This indicates that higher motivation supports the use of the more ex- material supports more expert diagrams, which, in turn, supports
pert working-forward strategy and better conceptual understanding; the use of the more expert working-forward strategy. Finally, the in-
this, in turn, influences problem-solution scores. Motivation had a direct path from Diagram to Strategy to Solution was significant and
significant influence on strategy use through its influence on problem large in size (.21), indicating that more expert pictures are linked to
categorization and metacognitive planning. This indicates that higher better strategy use, which, in turn, is linked to problem-solution
motivation supports better categorizations and increased planning, scores.
which, in turn, influences strategy use.
The path from Planning to Strategy to Solution was significant and 6. Discussion
moderate in size (.12), indicating that thinking about a problem and
The purpose of this study was to develop and test a theoretical model
of expertise in physics problem solving. By means of structural equation
Table 4 modeling, a model of expertise in physics problem solving was tested
Decomposition of effects in the model for the physics students.
and validated. This model is innovative and comprehensive because it in-
Predictor Criterion Effect corporates the influences of motivation, metacognitive planning, strate-
Direct Indirect
gy use, categorization skills, and free-body diagrams on problem-solving
success, allowing an opportunity to determine the contributions of indi-
PC t PC t
vidual variables when other variables are considered.
Motivation Planning .56 7.57 Results indicated that the model effectively explained the relation-
Strategy .29 3.55 .18 2.80
ship among the variables. Motivation had a significant influence on
Diagram .05 1.85
Categorization .21 2.33 the students' strategy use, categorization skills, and metacognitive plan-
Solution .25 3.56 ning. Planning had a significant influence on strategy use. Problem
Planning Strategy .18 1.99 categorization had a significant influence on strategy use, free-body di-
Solution .12 1.96 agrams, and problem-solution scores. Free-body diagrams had a signif-
Categorization Strategy .39 4.91 .08 2.44
Diagram .27 3.06
icant influence on strategy use; strategy use had a significant influence
Solution .49 6.39 .27 4.53 on problem-solution scores.
Strategy Solution .69 11.72 Results also indicated important indirect paths. Motivation had a
Diagram Strategy .31 4.03 significant influence on problem-solution scores as mediated by strat-
Solution .21 3.81
egy use and problem categorization; motivation also had a significant
Note. PC refers to standardized path coefficient. A cutoff value of t = 1.96 for a influence on strategy use as mediated by problem categorization and
two-tailed test was used to determine if paths were statistically significant. In terms metacognitive planning. Planning had a significant influence on
of the relative size and influence of the standardized path coefficients, paths ranging
from .05 to .10 are considered small, but meaningful influences. Paths ranging from
problem-solution scores as mediated by strategy use. Problem cate-
.11 to .25 are moderate in size and influence, and paths above .25 are considered gorization had a significant influence on strategy use as mediated by
large in size and influence (Keith, 1993). free-body diagrams. These diagrams had a significant influence on
60 G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

problem-solution scores as mediated by strategy use, and problem the indirect path Diagram to Strategy to Solution. This is in contrast to
categorization had a significant influence on problem-solution scores findings by Taasoobshirazi and Carr (2009) which indicated that the
as mediated by strategy use. use of free-body diagrams among college physics students did not im-
Strategy use played a central role in the model, influencing pact problem-solving strategy and achievement.
problem-solving success more than any other model component. Stu- As a result of instructors' and textbook authors' encouragement to
dents who used a working-forward strategy tended to answer the use free-body diagrams, students are drawing free-body diagrams
problems correctly, whereas those who used a working-backward more frequently, but these diagrams are often simple, incomplete and,
strategy tended to answer the problems incorrectly. While there is consequently, incorrect (Rosengrant et al., 2009). Research suggests
evidence that physics students can be taught the working-forward that a major reason why students draw incomplete free-body diagrams
strategy (Zajchowski & Martin, 1993), results of this study indicate is because they have difficulty representing the various forces that play
that strategy use is linked to students' understanding of the principles a role in physics problems (e.g., Henderson, 2007; Rosengrant et al.,
that underlie the solution to that problem. When teaching students 2009). To help students represent forces well, resources such as Internet
the working-forward strategy, instructors should concurrently attend tutorials, downloadable software programs, and textbook CDs have
to students' conceptual knowledge. The working-forward strategy is a been developed which show students how forces should be included
tool that enables students to efficiently apply their knowledge in in free-body diagrams (e.g., Henderson, 2007). These tutorials, pro-
order to engage in purposeful and goal directed problem solving grams, and CDs teach students about the nature of forces such as the
(Snyder, 2000). Thus instructors should ensure that students have force of gravity, normal force, frictional force, applied force, and tension,
the conceptual knowledge needed to solve physics problems when and are designed to improve the quality of students' free-body diagrams
teaching the working-forward strategy, and should ensure that stu- and, consequently, increase the likelihood that students will successful-
dents activate their conceptual knowledge when solving problems. ly solve problems.
Results of this study indicate that conceptual knowledge, as There is a need for more research on how students are using
assessed by problem categorization, has a significant impact on strat- free-body diagrams to solve physics problems. This research should
egy use, free-body diagrams, and problem-solution scores. The cur- be connected to what is known about students' metacognitive knowl-
rent findings support the small number of studies pointing to the edge of problem representations: This sort of thinking is described as
need for instruction supporting conceptual understanding in physics meta-representational (diSessa, 2004; diSessa & Sherin, 2000; Kohl &
(e.g., Taconis, Ferguson-Hessler, & Broekkamp, 2001). One way to Finkelstein, 2006). The goals of meta-representation research are to
help students develop a good conceptual understanding is to ask understand what students typically know about various types of rep-
them to compare and contrast physics problems, identify valid simi- resentations, and how students can best use this knowledge to repre-
larities and differences, and then categorize the problems through in- sent and solve problems.
structional activities such as categorization tasks or concept mapping
(Ruiz-Primo, Schultz, Li, & Shavelson, 2001). The problems that stu-
dents categorize or map should not be limited to those they are cur- 7. Directions for future research
rently learning, but should also include those they learned in the
past. Helping students to make a connection between concepts they In future research, the model should be expanded to provide a
learned in the past to those they are presently learning will increase more comprehensive explanation of expertise in physics problem
the cohesion of the students' knowledge and help improve the con- solving. For example, the impact of social support is expected to be
ceptual understanding that supports successful problem solving. critical for expertise in any domain (Bloom, 1985; Ericsson, 2006),
An important finding of the present study was that students' and should also be assessed and included in future models of exper-
metacognitive planning influenced the use of the working-forward strat- tise in physics problem solving. Social support has not been studied
egy, which, in turn, influenced problem-solution scores. Physics instruc- in the research on expertise in physics problem solving, but could
tors can help students to engage in planning during physics problem influence successful physics problem solving and uniquely contribute
solving by defining metacognitive planning, explaining what it entails, to the model's potential for explaining the variance in students'
modeling the process for them, and encouraging them to engage in problem-solving success. Furthermore, additional cognitive (e.g., mis-
metacognitive planning when solving problems (Duggan & Gott, 2002). conceptions), motivational (e.g., goal orientation), and metacognitive
Schraw (1998) created a metacognitive planning checklist that students variables (e.g., debugging, evaluation) could be included in the model
can use when solving problems. The checklist includes four questions to determine how they influence and are influenced by other vari-
students ask themselves throughout the problem-solving process in- ables. The model tested in the present study, and variations of the
cluding: (1) What is the nature of the task/problem? (2) What is my model, can be tested and compared among students across varying
goal? (3) What kind of information and strategies do I need? and levels of expertise, such as students in introductory, intermediate,
(4) How much time and resources will I need? The use of this and sim- and advanced-level physics courses. Doing so would allow re-
ilar checklists have been found to improve problem-solving success in searchers to determine which variables are important and at what
science (Schraw, 1998). In addition, explaining to students the differ- time, as different variables may be more critical depending on stu-
ence between the working-forward and working-backward strategies, dents' level of expertise.
and how these strategies are connected to conceptual knowledge can For the present study, patterns of correlations were used to exam-
help promote successful metacognitive planning. ine the interaction among the model variables. Follow up studies
The results of this study indicate that motivation is important for using randomized controlled trials with experimental and control
expertise in physics problem solving. Researchers examining exper- groups, however, would allow the opportunity to go a step further
tise in physics problem solving should study the impact of motivation and obtain causal evidence about the impact of one variable on an-
on problem-solving success. Results of the present study indicate that other. In addition, such studies would allow for comparisons of the
efforts made to increase student motivation will be helpful for im- impact of various interventions on problem-solving success.
proving metacognitive planning, strategy use, conceptual under- Comprehensive, open-ended, individual interviews should be
standing, and ultimately problem-solution scores. conducted in future research to gain additional information about the
Free-body diagrams had a significant influence on strategy use, in- variables in the model (Patton, 2002). Using qualitative procedures, in
dicating that students who drew more complex pictures were more conjunction with quantitative procedures, can help provide a compre-
likely to use the working-forward strategy; this more expert strategy hensive understanding of the nature of expertise in physics problem
use, in turn, influenced problem-solution scores. This is evidenced by solving (Sabella & Redish, 2007).
G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62 61

8. Conclusion Glynn, S. M., & Koballa, T. R. (2006). Motivation to learn college science. In J. Mintzes, &
W. H. Leonard (Eds.), Handbook of college science teaching. Arlington, VA: National
Science Teachers Association Press.
Although developing expertise takes years, students can develop Glynn, S. M., Taasoobshirazi, G., & Brickman, P. (2007). Nonscience majors learning sci-
surprising amounts of expertise quickly through effective instruction ence: A theoretical model of motivation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
44(8), 1088–1107.
(Ericsson, 1996; Schraw et al., 2006). Results of this study have im- Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2003). The role of working memory in problem solving.
portant implications for physics instruction and for interventions In R. J. Sternberg, & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of problem solving. New York:
aimed at developing problem-solving expertise in physics students. Cambridge.
Hatano, G., & Oura, Y. (2003). Commentary: Reconceptualizing school learning using
Results indicate that physics instructors should spend more time fo- insight from expertise research. Educational Researcher, 32, 26–29.
cusing students on the conceptual aspects of the material they are Henderson, C. (2005). The challenges of instructional change under the best of
learning, and help foster the motivation that supports conceptual un- circumstances: A case study of one college physics instructor. American Journal of
Physics, 73, 778–786.
derstanding. Fostering student motivation and encouraging students
Henderson, T. (2007). Drawing free-body diagrams. Available at: http://www.
engage in metacognitive planning when solving physics problems physicsclassroom.com/Class/newtlaws/
should increase the use of the more expert working-forward strategy. Hewitt, P. G. (2005). Conceptual physics. New York, NY: Addison Wesley.
Instructors should also show students the difference between the Howard, B. C., McGee, S., Shia, R., & Hong, N. (2000). Metacognitive self-regulation and
problem-solving: Expanding the theory base through factor analysis. New Orleans: Paper
working-forward and working-backward strategies, the connection presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association.
between the two strategies and conceptual understanding, and en- Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity
courage students to work-forward when solving problems. Finally, to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424–453.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Internet tutorials, downloadable software programs, and textbook Conventional fit criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
CDs can be used to improve students' free-body diagrams, which Jörkeskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (2006). PRELIS 2: User's reference guide. A program for
should, in turn, improve strategy use and problem-solution scores. multivariate data screening and data summarization: A preprocessor for LISREL.
Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
Jörkeskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2007). LISREL 8.80. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software Inter-
References national, Inc.
Keith, T. Z. (1993). Causal influences on school learning. In H. J. Walberg (Ed.), Analytic
Alexander, P. A. (2003). Can we get there from here? Educational Researcher, 32, 3–4. methods for educational productivity (pp. 21–47). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Anderson, K. C., & Leinhardt, G. (2002). Maps as representations: Expert novice com- Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:
parison of projectile understanding. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 283–321. Guilford Press.
Anzai, Y. (1991). Learning and use of representations for physics expertise. In K. A. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:
Anders, & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise (pp. 64–92). New Guilford Press.
York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Kohl, P. B., & Finkelstein, N. D. (2006). Effects of representation on students solving
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, physics problems: A fine-grained characterization. Physical Review Special Topics:
107, 238–246. Physics Education Research, 2, 1–12.
Bloom, B. S. (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballantine Books. Lajoie, S. P. (2008). Metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated learning: A rose
Bong, M. (2001). Role of self-efficacy and task value in predicting college students' by any other name? Educational Psychology Review, 20, 469–475.
course performance and future enrollment intentions. Contemporary Educational Larkin, J. H. (1985). Understanding problem representations and skill in physics. In S. F.
Psychology, 26, 553–570. Chipman, J. W. Segal, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills: Vol. 2. Research
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. and open questions. (pp. 141–159) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Larkin, J. H., McDermott, J., Simon, H., & Simon, D. (1980). Expert and novice perfor-
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. mance in solving physics problems. Science, 208, 1335–1342.
Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic perfor- Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand
mance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270–295. words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.
Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts' characteristics. In N. McDermott, L. C. (2001). Oersted Medal Lecture 2001: Physics education research—The
Charness, P. J. Feltovich, R. R. Hoffman, & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook key to student learning. American Journal of Physics, 69, 1127–1137.
of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21–30). New York, NY: Cambridge University Neto, A. J., & Valente, M. O. (1997). Problem solving in physics: Towards a synergetic
Press. metacognitively developed approach. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of National Association for Research in Science Teaching.
physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152. Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Chi, M. T. H., & Slotta, J. D. (1993). The ontological coherence of intuitive physics. Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and
Cognition and Instruction, 10, 249–260. applications. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall.
Cutnell, J. D., & Johson, K. W. (2001). Physics. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Priest, A. G., & Lindsay, R. O. (1992). New light on novice-expert differences in physics
Cutnell, J. D., & Johson, K. W. (2007). Physics. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. problem solving. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 389–405.
Davidson, J. E. (2003). Insights about insightful problem solving. In J. E. Davidson, & R. J. Reeve, J., Hamm, D., & Nix, G. (2003). Testing models of the experience of self determi-
Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 149–175). New York, NY: nation in intrinsic motivation and the conundrum of choice. Journal of Educational
Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 95, 375–392.
Dhillon, A. S. (1998). Individual differences within problem-solving strategies used in Rosengrant, D., Van Heuvelen, A. V., & Etkina, E. (2009). Do students use and under-
physics. Science Education, 82, 379–405. stand free-body diagrams? Physical Review Special Topics: Physics Education
diSessa, A. A. (2004). Metarepresentation: Native competence and targets for instruc- Research, 5, 1–13.
tion. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 293–331. Ruiz-Primo, M. A., Schultz, S. E., Li, M., & Shavelson, R. J. (2001). Comparison of the re-
diSessa, A. A., & Sherin, B. (2000). Meta-representation: An introduction. The Journal of liability and validity of scores from two concept-mapping techniques. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 19, 385–398. Research in Science Teaching, 38, 260–278.
Duggan, S., & Gott, R. (2002). What sort of science education do we really need? International Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions
Journal of Science Education, 24, 661–679. and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67.
Duncan, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (2005). The making of the Motivated Strategies for Sabella, M. S., & Redish, E. F. (2007). Knowledge organization and activation in physics
Learning Questionnaire. Educational Psychologist, 40, 117–128. problem solving. American Journal of Physics, 75, 1017–1029.
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The Schneider, W. (1993). Acquiring expertise: Determinants of exceptional performance. In
road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts, sciences, sports, K. A. Heller, F. J. Monks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), International handbook of research and
and games (pp. 1–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. development of giftedness and talent (pp. 311–324). United Kingdom: Pergamon Press.
Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of ex- Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instructional Science,
pert performance in medicine and related domains. Academic Medicine, 79, 70–81. 26, 113–125.
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the devel- Schraw, G., Crippen, K. J., & Hartley, K. D. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science
opment of superior expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, education: Metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Research
& R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance in Science Education, 36, 111–139.
(pp. 685–706). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the Educational Psychology, 19, 460–475.
acquisition of expert performance. American Psychological Association, 100, 363–406. Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology
Friedman, J. S., & diSessa, A. A. (1999). What students should know about technology: Review, 7, 351–373.
The case of scientific visualization. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 8, Serway, R. A., & Faughn, J. S. (2002). College physics. Belmont, CA: Thomson Press.
175–195. Shin, N., Jonassen, D. H., & McGee, S. (2003). Predictors of well-structured and
Gerace, W. (2001). Problem solving and conceptual understanding. Paper presented at ill-structured problem solving in an astronomy simulation. Journal of Research in
the 2001 Physics Education Research Conference, Rochester, New York. Science Teaching, 40, 6–33.
62 G. Taasoobshirazi, J. Farley / Learning and Individual Differences 24 (2013) 53–62

Snyder, J. L. (2000). An investigation of the knowledge structures of experts, interme- Zajchowski, R., & Martin, J. (1993). Differences in the problem solving of stronger and
diates and novices in physics. International Journal of Science Education, 22, weaker novices in physics: Knowledge strategies, or knowledge structure. Journal
979–992. of Research in Science Teaching, 30, 459–470.
SPSS Inc. (2005). SPSS base 14.0 for windows. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc. Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning.
Stains, M., & Talanquer, V. (2008). Classification of chemical reactions: Stages of exper- Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 329–339.
tise. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 45, 771–793. Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Developing self-fulfilling cycles of academic regulation: An
Steiger, J. H. (1995). SEPATH. Statistica 5.0. Tulsa, LI: StatSoft. analysis of exemplary instructional models. In D. H. Schunk, & B. J. Zimmerman
Stylianou, D. A., & Silver, E. A. (2004). The role of visual representations in advanced (Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective practice (pp. 1–19).
mathematical problem solving: An examination of expert-novice similarities and New York: The Guilford Press.
differences. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6, 353–387. Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory Into
Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Practice, 41, 64–70.
psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 215–266). San Diego: Academic Press. Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Development and adaptation of expertise: The role of
Taasoobshirazi, G., & Carr, M. (2009). A structural equation model of expertise in col- self-regulatory processes and beliefs. In N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, R. R. Hoffman,
lege physics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 630–643. & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance
Taconis, R., Ferguson-Hessler, M. G. M., & Broekkamp, H. (2001). Teaching science (pp. 705–722). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
problem solving: An overview of experimental work. Journal of Research in Science Zimmerman, B. J., & Campillo, M. (2003). Motivating self-regulated problem solvers. In J. E.
Teaching, 38, 442–468. Davidson, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 233–262).
Widaman, K. F., & Thompson, J. S. (2003). On specifying the null model for incremental New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
fit indices in structural equation modeling. Psychological Methods, 8, 16–37. Zusho, A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). Skill and will: The role of motivation and cognition in
Wieman, C., & Perkins, K. (2005). Transforming physics education. Physics Today, 58, the learning of college chemistry. International Journal of Science Education, 25,
36–41. 1081–1094.
Williams, D. J., & Noyes, J. M. (2007). Effect of experience and mode of presentation on
problem solving. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 258–274.
Worthington, R. L., & Whittaker, T. A. (2006). Scale development research: A content anal-
ysis and recommendations for best practice. The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 806–838.

Вам также может понравиться