Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 68

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/321774740

Affect, Critical Thinking, and Decision Making

Thesis · January 2015

CITATIONS READS
0 129

1 author:

Alexander Hough
Wright State University
8 PUBLICATIONS   6 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Cognitive effort in individual and group tasks View project

Cuttlefish learning View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Alexander Hough on 13 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Affect, Critical Thinking, and Decision Making

Alexander Ryan Hough

Shippensburg University
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      1
 

Abstract

Individuals make decisions every day. Most require little effort (e.g., what to eat), while

others require careful thought and impact the population (e.g., government policy). According to

the decision making literature, there are two different kinds of cognitive information processing

(e.g., System 1 and System 2). System 1 is similar to our perceptual systems, it automatically

creates meaning, emotional reactions, and generates conclusions out of limited information.

System 2 processing is essentially analytical thought activated by conscious deliberation, or

when System 1 has a problem generating a conclusion. The literature has consistently

demonstrated that System 1 processing occurs first and is usually responsible for our decisions,

however, System 2 processing leads to higher quality decisions (Halpern, 2014; Kahneman,

2011). The purpose of this study was to challenge Mikels, Maglio, Reed, and Kaplowitz’s 2011

study, which found that participants were able to choose the best option, out of four possible

choices, more often when using System 1 compared to System 2 processing. They hypothesized

that memory and System 1 processing were responsible for these results. This thesis assessed the

reliability of the Mikels et al. (2011) experiment, then tested an alternative hypothesis with an

additional experiment including a memory manipulation. Results for the replication were similar

to those found by Mikels et al. (2011). However, overall results suggested that memory and

System 2 processing were responsible for Mikels et al.’s (2011) findings. An additional analysis

demonstrated that predispositions to rely on System 1 or System 2 can significantly affect

performance during decision tasks. Overall, the current results challenge Mikels et al. (2011),

support the decision making literature, and also warrant further investigation into individual

differences in thinking strategies.

Keywords: Judgment, feelings, critical thinking, heuristics, choice


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      2
 

Affect, Critical Thinking, and Decision Making.

Decision making is an important process which is involved in everyday activities. An

individual usually chooses when to get out of bed, what to eat for breakfast, and what to wear.

Some decisions are more important, like choosing a career path or purchasing a car. Since most

decisions are simple and require little effort, individuals tend to rely on generalized strategies to

increase efficiency. However, when making complex or important decisions, these general

strategies are not sufficient (e.g., purchasing or adopting a cat without considering money, time

and effort involved). To better understand how and why individuals make certain types of

decisions, the following literature review and research study focused on the influences of mental

processes, intuition, affect, and critical thinking on judgments and decisions. A major concern in

this study was whether individuals made better overall decisions while relying on intuition or

affect compared to critical thinking.

How and Why We Make Decisions

There are few things in life that can be determined with absolute certainty. Everything

else involves varying levels of uncertainty and can only be judged in terms of probabilities.

Mathematically, probability is calculated by determining the number of ways that an event can

occur and dividing this number by the total number of possible events (Gray & Kinnear, 2012).

However, as human’s process information, there are two ways to determine probabilities:

objectively or subjectively.

Objective probability involves using numbers to determine the mathematical likelihood

of an event occurring in the long run (Halpern, 2014). The objective probability of rolling a two

with a normal six sided die is 1/6 or 16.6 percent, because there is only one way to roll a two and

there are six possible outcomes. However, there is an issue with objective probability, since
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      3
 

calculations reflect the average outcome over time. For instance, if an individual was approached

and asked to make a large bet on how many times a two will be rolled in 1000 attempts, this

individual can be relatively confident betting that this will be the case 16.6 percent of the time.

However, confidence would be decreased if an individual is asked to make a large bet on one roll

of the die. The second bet is referring to a chance event, which is more difficult to determine

objectively. This is because it is possible to roll a die ten times in a row and not roll a two. The

point here is that long-run probabilities are not always representative in the short-run, they are

theoretical estimates based on mathematical principles.

In situations where numbers or values are not available, an individual can determine

probability subjectively. If a student is asked to generate the probability of getting an A on an

exam, the student may estimate a subjective probability based on factors such as amount of time

studying, difficulty of the material, or performance on past tests. When using subjective

probabilities, the outcome is determined by the quality of the personal estimates, which may or

may not be accurate.

The literature on judgment and decision making often makes little distinction between a

judgment and a decision, but it is important to understand the difference. Halpern (2014)

provided a definition for decision making, which “always involves making a choice between a

set of possible alternatives.” (p. 399). For example, if an individual is ready to purchase a new

car at a dealership, they are presented with an assortment of cars and must ultimately make a

choice between these possible alternatives. Halpern (2014) also mentions that decision making is

not an “exact science like mathematics” due to differences in personal values (p. 403), which is

evident in the fact that people do not choose the same cars or careers. In contrast to a decision, a

judgment is the process of evaluating one or possibly two pieces of information. In the car
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      4
 

example, this could mean subjectively judging which aspects of each car are preferred (e.g., I

like the interior or I do not like the seats). After judging the pros (i.e., likes) and cons (i.e.,

dislikes) for each car, a decision can be made which considers all the cars at the same time. In

this example, judgments come first and are used to give values for attributes of each car before

an actual decision can be made.

In order to make quality decisions, an individual needs to be able to think critically (e.g.,

the decision example above). Thinking critically can reduce errors and increase the quality of the

judgments and decisions we make (Halpern, 2014). However, research has shown that many

individuals lack the ability to think critically. In a telephone survey of over 2,000 American

adults, more than 25 percent did not know that the Earth revolves around the sun (Asimov,

1989). This is rather frightening, since there is a lot of available information to show otherwise.

Asimov (1989) also mentioned that reports in 1988 and 1989 indicated that American students

were mathematically and scientifically illiterate. According to the National Science Board

(NSB), the level of scientific literacy of the public has not changed significantly in the last two

decades. For instance, in 1992, approximately 60 percent of the public that were surveyed could

correctly answer factual knowledge questions, compared to 64 percent in 2012 (NSB, 2014). One

would hope that a college education would improve knowledge and thinking skills for these

individuals. However, after investigating how education improves critical thinking, Arum and

Roksa (2011) found that approximately 760 of the 2,300 students (i.e., 33 percent) sampled from

24 universities showed no improvement in critical thinking over four years. After thinking about

these statistics, one may conclude that the problem may lie in the education system. However,

Halpern (2014) suggests that attitudes towards critical thinking may be the main problem,
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      5
 

because individuals can always improve critical thinking skills if they are specifically instructed

and are willing to put forth the effort.

To understand the thought processes involved in decision making, researchers have

suggested two systems or processes (Kahneman, 2003; Sjöberg, 1971; Zajonc, 1980), which

were described in Kahneman’s recent book (2011). System 1 is characterized as fast, intuitive,

emotional, automatic, and impulsive. It uses associative memories, emotions, and stereotypes to

construct a coherent picture of the world (Kahneman, 2011). System 1 works unconsciously and

is behind most of the judgments and decisions that individuals make. System 1 thinking provides

an innate framework to understand the world, it never shuts off or stops making conclusions. It is

efficient and right most of the time. However, due to the nature of automatic functioning, errors

can be made without awareness. We tend not to doubt the information and conclusions created

by System 1 thinking. System 2 becomes more active when System 1 encounters a problem or

when an individual consciously deliberates. Critical thinking is considered System 2 processing.

Anytime an individual is aware of their thoughts or knowledge (i.e., metacognition), System 2 is

active. It is conscious, rational, analytical, and effortful. System 2 “calls the shots” and is able to

“reprogram” the way System 1 operates (Kahneman, 2011). However, since System 1 operates

automatically and with a sense of ease, System 2 usually endorses its responses. This cognitive

ease, created by automatic assumptions of System 1, causes a sense of confidence that everything

is under control and reduces the involvement of System 2. Cognitive ease can be thought of as

“if it’s easy, it’s accurate”. In addition to cognitive ease, System 2 thinking can also be reduced

since it can be aversive in the same way physical exercise can be. However, using System 2 to

“run the numbers” will always lead to higher quality decisions compared to “running” with the

first answer that comes to mind (i.e., System 1). It is important to note that memory is involved
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      6
 

in both systems. Memory has been called the mediator of all cognitive processes (Halpern,

2014). Taking this into consideration, anything that affects memory can also affect both

processing systems.

Decision making can be stressful, aversive, and time consuming which can promote

negative attitudes and avoidance. Most people do not have good critical thinking skills,

knowledge of probabilities, large amounts of time, or have the motivation to engage in high

effort strategies. This is why individuals tend to rely more on System 1. For example, even if

individuals do use effortful decision strategies (i.e., System 2), there is no guarantee that

resulting decisions will be optimal. This is partly because it is difficult to determine the quality

of a decision, because the other possible outcomes are never known, and the accuracy of the

information can be ambiguous at times. The only way to evaluate a decision is by evaluating the

processes that went into making the decision (i.e., the amount of effort, the quality of the thought

processes, and the accuracy of information used) (Halpern, 2014). This evaluation requires

critical thinking (i.e., System 2 processing), which can significantly improve the likelihood of

successful outcomes from decision making. Unfortunately, individuals often endorse initial

answers resulting from general strategies that decrease mental effort (i.e., System 1), which

reduces the likelihood of successful outcomes from the decision making process.

Intuitive Strategies

Individuals frequently use heuristics or shortcut strategies as a guide for making

judgments and decisions. A heuristic is defined as an exploratory problem solving aid that

increases performance (Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary, 2014). Kahneman (2011) suggests

that using heuristics is a strategy of substituting an easier question for one that is hard to solve,

which reduces cognitive effort and increases efficiency. After investigating how individuals
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      7
 

made errors in decision making, Tversky and Kahneman (1974) described three intuitive

heuristics that individuals commonly use: representativeness, availability, and anchoring. These

heuristics played significant roles in developing the field of judgment and decision making, but

for the purposes of this thesis, only availability will be discussed.

Availability of Memory

The availability heuristic is characterized by assessing probability or frequency by

relying on the ease that an object or event can be retrieved from memory. The availability

heuristic is mostly explained by System 1 as an automatic operation of associative memory

(Kahneman, 2011), however, an individual can activate System 2 by consciously searching for

information that is less available in memory. The feeling of cognitive ease and confidence is

closely tied to the availability of information in memory. To demonstrate how the availability

heuristic works, consider asking an individual to indicate which pet is most prevalent in the

United States. It is likely that the response will be cat or dog. In fact, freshwater fish are the most

prevalent pet in the U.S., outnumbering cats and dogs by 50 percent (American Pet Products

Association, 2013). This response is given because individuals have more memories with cats

and dogs as pets. When objects or events are easier to recall, individuals assume there is a higher

probability or frequency of occurrence. An early experiment by Tversky and Kahneman (1973)

presented participants with a list of male and female names, which were either famous or less

famous. Results showed that 80 percent reported that there were more names in a list when the

list contained more famous names, that is, those names were easier to retrieve due to their

familiarity and were, therefore, more “available”. In a replication of these findings, McKelvie

(1997) found that 78.9 percent of participants indicated that the list with famous names had a

larger amount of names.


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      8
 

In another demonstration of availability, Kahneman and Tversky (1973) asked

participants if there are more words that start with the letter R, or have R as the third letter. Most

of the participants believed there were more words beginning with R, however, there are actually

more words with R as the third letter (Berger, 1995). It was easier for participants to retrieve

instances where R was the first letter of a word, which caused them to believe that more words

begin with R.

Availability can also affect judgments of the perceived frequency of events. In a series of

experiments, Folkes (1988) found ease of recalling product failure or success was correlated with

judged likelihood of future outcomes. If failure or success could be easily brought to mind, then

participants assumed a greater frequency of this occurrence. Another study found that frequency

of dramatic deaths are overestimated compared to less dramatic deaths, particularly due to

inaccurate frequencies reported by the media or from direct experiences (Lichtenstein, Slovic,

Fischhoff, Layman, & Combs, 1978). This overestimation of dramatic events may be caused by

the common belief that memories of emotional events are accurate, even though research has

shown that these memories are often distorted (Phelps & Sharot, 2008).

The availability heuristic also works when little can be brought to mind, in what

Kahneman (2011) labels the “unexplained unavailability heuristic” (p. 133). Fox (2006) provided

a demonstration of this phenomenon by asking participants to generate ways to improve a

recently taken college class. One group was asked to generate two ways and the second group

was asked to generate ten ways. The second group had difficulty generating ten ways to improve

the class, which caused these participants to indicate that they liked the class more than the first

group. In the earlier discussion of availability, cognitive ease increased confidence, but Fox’s

(2006) experiment showed that lack of cognitive ease can have the reverse effect. These results
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      9
 

suggested that the ease of recalling objects or events were powerful, which reduced the

activation of System 2. However, it is possible to reduce the bias caused by availability, if

participants are made aware of the reason that they experience or do not experience cognitive

ease, which can activate System 2 (Kahneman, 2011). Even if System 2 is activated, the

information provided by System 1 is still available and can be difficult to ignore.

There are, however, instances when information that is more available can increase the

quality of a decision. In these cases, individuals first engage in effortful processing (i.e., System

2 thinking) to organize and categorize information in memory to enhance the availability of

information, quality, and speed of recall. This type of memory organization explains why experts

are able to generate quick and accurate answers, because they acquired large amounts of

information over their career and frequently engage in critical thinking. Although this seems like

intuition, it is often referred to as automatic or fast thinking, because it is based on extensive

knowledge and experience engaging in critical thinking (Halpern, 2014; Kahneman, 2011).

Laypeople (i.e., non-experts) can also use this strategy if they realize that they need to be able to

recall this information at a later time. One of the first researchers to explore this type of

organization was George Miller (1956), who demonstrated that individuals can only hold five to

nine pieces or chunks of information in memory at a given time (i.e., working memory). When

this information is grouped into a category (i.e., chunked), individuals are able to hold more

information in working memory (i.e., greater availability). For example, consider this list of eight

words: monkey, zoo, young, enclosure, employee, banana, jumped, and eat. According to

Miller’s (1956) research, it would be difficult for individuals to recall all eight words after

studying the list. However, individuals could remember all eight words if they were grouped into

four chunks, such as: young monkey, jumped enclosure, zoo employee, and eat banana. By
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      10
 

grouping the words together, we have reduced the pieces of information from eight to four; now

an individual should be able to recall all eight words. In addition to the benefits of chunking

information to reduce mental load, memory is improved when there is more meaning attached to

stimuli. For example, years after leaving college, individuals tended to remember important ideas

and concepts better than less meaningful names and dates (Conway, Cohen, & Stanhope, 1991).

The importance of memory in decision making is well demonstrated and documented (Folkes,

1988; Fox, 2006; Kahneman & Tversky, 1973; McKelvie, 1997; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974),

however, some research (e.g., Johnson & Tversky, 1983) suggests that there is more to the story.

After various experiments and literature reviews, many researchers now agree that affect plays

an important role in the decision making process (Peters, Vāstfjäll, Gärling, & Slovic, 2006;

Slovic, Finucane, Peters, & MacGregor, 2002).

The Influence and Availability of Affect

Zajonc (1968, 1980, 2001) was one of the first researchers to focus on the importance of

affect during decision making. After completing many studies, he concluded that affective

reactions occur first when making decisions, are independent of further cognitive processing, and

are present in all perceptions of stimuli. Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, and Damasio (1997)

conducted a study that supported Zajonc’s theory. Their task involved participants choosing a

card out of four decks of cards, half of which have a greater likelihood of winning and the other

half had a greater likelihood of losing. The participants could not have known when they were

going to win or lose; however, they generated changes in skin conductance and brain activity

prior to choosing a card. These physiological changes correlated with card choice, which showed

that unconscious biases preceded conscious thought. A large amount of research has investigated

the function of an “affect heuristic” while making decisions (Peters et al., 2006; Pfister & Böhm,
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      11
 

2008; Slovic et al., 2002), influencing decision researchers to consider that affect may underlie

some of the more rational heuristics, such as anchoring (Kahneman & Frederick, 2001). When an

individual uses the “affect heuristic”, they rely on immediate emotional reactions (e.g., good or

bad) caused by stimuli to guide their decisions (Slovic et al., 2002). The affect heuristic is similar

to the other heuristics previously mentioned; it can increase the efficiency of judgments and

decisions, but as an operation of System 1 it is prone to error.

Probability and Frequency

In a series of experiments, Johnson and Tversky (1983) investigated the effect of mood

on judgments of risky event frequencies. Results indicated that while participants’ evaluated

frequencies of negative events, positive affect decreased perceived frequency judgments, while

negative affect increased perceived frequency judgments (Johnson & Tversky, 1983). This study

will be discussed in more detail in the following sections. Other studies investigating how affect

influences probability and frequency judgments showed similar types of results (Denes-Raj &

Epstein, 1994; Lowenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch, 2001).

Affect can change how an individual perceives the chances of winning during gambling

tasks, even when the probability of winning is provided. Lowenstein et al. (2001) showed how

individual’s feelings about winning a lottery show little variation whether the probability of

winning is one in 10,000, or one in a million. Denes-Raj and Epstein (1994) found that

individuals prefer the odds of 7 in 100 versus 1 in 10 during a gambling situation. To win during

these gambles, participants had to draw a jelly bean out of a bowl of kidney beans. Participants

were given the opportunity to choose between two bowls. The first bowl consisted of one jelly

bean and ten kidney beans, while the other bowl contained seven jelly beans and 100 kidney

beans. Apparently seeing the larger number of jelly beans in the second bowl induced positive
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      12
 

affect, which caused participants to choose this bowl, even though there was actually less chance

of winning. An interesting finding of this study is that participants realized that these decisions

were irrational, however, the feelings associated with this judgment were too powerful to

overcome (Denes-Raj & Epstein, 1994). Participants in the previous two studies were strongly

influenced by System 1, even after activating System 2. This is partially due to the cognitive ease

of System 1, and also the tendency to assign more weight to strong feelings, which may serve as

an anchor (i.e., reference point). However, individuals can always engage in System 2 thinking

to determine why they experienced cognitive ease and if the weight assigned to feelings was, in

fact, rational. The previous studies also seem to imply that individuals disregard probability

information when they have other information to rely on (e.g., affect or frequencies).

Type and Intensity of Affect

Affective states appear to have different degrees of influence, based on the intensity and

type of emotion that are present during the decision making process. As mentioned earlier, a

study using a card gambling task demonstrated that participants had affective reactions prior to

cognitive deliberation (Bechara et al., 1997). Another study using the same gambling task

demonstrated that positive mood states increased the reliance on these initial affective reactions,

which actually led to better outcomes early in the experiment (de Vries, Holland, & Witteman,

2008). Early in the experiment, there was no way to accurately determine which card would

produce a win or loss, but participants showed a tendency toward the two more advantageous

decks. Later in the experiment, participants were able to consciously recognize which decks were

more advantageous (de Vries et al., 2008). Depending on how the choices are presented, the

intensity of feelings associated with outcomes can vary. When decisions are known to have a

high degree of uncertainty, the intensity of affect is reduced (van Dijk & Zeelenberg, 2006), but
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      13
 

over time, affect associated with outcomes from prior decisions increases in intensity (Ritov,

2006). In addition to the level of affect, different types of affect are likely to result in different

outcomes as well (Johnson & Tversky, 1983; Schwartz & Clore, 1988).

Most of the research that has been presented, specifically how individuals tend to rely on

intuition or affect to make judgments and decisions, implies that humans are not very good at

making decisions. This could be partially due to the trend in psychology to focus on errors and

problems in an attempt to understand where individuals go wrong or deviate from the norm.

There are, however, some positive findings regarding the use of heuristics. Gigerenzer and

Goldstein (2011) have shown that heuristics based on intuition can be quite accurate in many

situations, especially when the questions relate to real life or when factual information is scarce.

When the most accurate or appropriate choice is uncertain, individuals use the information that is

available to make the best possible decision. Gigerenzer also criticized some of the literature on

heuristics, stating that they used trick questions. This emphasizes the importance of how

questions are framed, which is discussed later.

Kahneman (2011) also acknowledged that humans are capable of making good, rational

decisions, by mentioning that biases can sometimes be overcome by being vigilant and aware of

the possible pitfalls of trusting one’s own intuition. In essence, Kahneman was saying that we

should be aware that many of our conclusions come from System 1 and should be checked for

accuracy by engaging in System 2 thinking. This is similar to the conclusion made by Halpern

(2014), who stated that thinking critically (i.e., System 2 processing) is an important process

which can help individuals reduce errors and make higher quality judgments and decisions.

However, sometimes the context of a situation can influence whether an individual uses System

1 or System 2 to make a judgment or decision.


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      14
 

Influence of Context on Decision Strategies

Presentation and Focus

The way information is presented (i.e., framed) or the outcome associated with a decision

(i.e., context) can influence the decision strategy an individual uses while making a judgment.

For instance, individuals appear to rely on affective information when they have to make

decisions in short periods of time and have limited attentional resources. Shiv and Fedorikhin

(1999) found that when individuals have to divide attention between tasks (i.e., multitasking),

there is a greater reliance on System 1 due to reduced availability of mental resources, but when

attentional resources are more available, individuals increase engagement of System 2. Many

individuals multitask frequently (Carrier,  Cheever, Rosen, Benitez & Chang, 2009), because they

tend to be overconfident that they can retain a high level of performance while multitasking;

however, research has shown that those who multitask most often are actually less capable then

those who multitask less often (Sanbonmatsu, Strayer, Medeiros-Ward, & Watson, 2013).

Overconfidence is another reason why individuals make decisions based on System 1 processing.

In a similar fashion, when individuals are under time pressure, they must make decisions in short

periods of time without much opportunity to deliberate. Research has shown that time pressure

triggers affect, arousal, and reduction of cognitive deliberation (Maule & Svenson, 1993), which

causes a greater reliance on System 1 and can improve the efficiency of judgments, since

evaluations of criteria are derived from one source (Finucane, Alhakami, Slovic, & Johnson,

2000). Even though System 1 may increase efficiency under time pressure, it is likely that the

quality of these decisions would be reduced.

In Söllner, Bröder, and Hillbig’s (2013) experiment, individuals tended to use intuitive

strategies (i.e., System 1) when information was easily accessed, but used a more deliberate
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      15
 

strategy (i.e., System 2) when situations required a more active search for information relevant to

a decision. However, when information relevant to a decision is not available, even by effortful

searching, individuals tend to rely on affect for discriminating between alternatives (Pachur,

Hertwig, & Steinmann, 2012). For example, Hsee (1996, 1998) investigated how context affects

mental processing in single and dual evaluations. In Hsee’s (1996) study, participants had the

choice between two dictionaries (i.e., dual evaluation). One was new and included 10,000

entries, the other one was also new, but had a torn cover and contained 20,000 entries.

Participants were willing to pay more for the dictionary that had more entries. However, in a

single evaluation condition, participants were presented with either the new dictionary or the one

with a torn cover. Both groups were instructed to indicate how much they would pay for the

dictionary. The willingness to pay for those presented with the new dictionary was higher than

the willingness to pay for the dictionary with the torn cover. Hsee (1996) explains that

individuals based their decisions solely on the attractiveness of the dictionary, which is an

operation of System 1 and is not a very important indication of the usefulness of a dictionary.

The participants in the single evaluation condition were not presented with information that

could be used to compare the number of entries with another dictionary, which could have

activated System 2. Due to the lack of information for comparison, participants used the most

available resource to guide their decision (e.g., affect). An additional experiment supported this

conclusion by showing that participants were willing to pay more money for an ice cream

container that was overflowing, rather than a larger non overflowing container containing more

ice cream (Hsee, 1998).

Affective information also appears to be more available when decision outcomes are

known to directly impact individuals. For instance, Sjöberg (2003) presented participants with 28
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      16
 

decision situations and asked them to indicate which decisions should be made with an intuitive

or analytical approach. The 28 decision situations involved hypothetical outcomes that would or

would not directly affect each participant. Results indicated that participants tended to make

more analytical decisions (i.e., System 2 processing) when the outcome did not affect them

personally or when there was little ability to influence outcomes, compared to more affective

decisions (i.e., System 1 processing) when the outcome had personal significance or when there

was a greater ability to influence outcomes. Individuals may be better able to imagine which

feelings will accompany an outcome when they have some control, which could induce feelings

of empathy that may guide their decision strategy. In contrast, when outcomes are not personal

and are not able to be influenced, individuals take a colder analytical approach (i.e., System 2

thinking).

There are benefits of relying on System 1 while making decisions, but these benefits are

typically only found with experts, not laypeople. For example, after becoming an expert at a task,

such as chess, individuals tend to make automatic decisions of high quality (Kahneman &

Frederick, 2001). When experts, like the chess player, focus on deliberation, automatic decisions

can be interfered with, which may cause a drop in performance. This effect has been

characterized as “choking under pressure” (Beilock & Carr, 2001). It is important to note that

this drop in performance is not found with all types of experts, especially those that frequently

engage in System 2 thinking (e.g., cognitive psychologists, physicists, or philosophers).

Individual Differences

Individuals may be more or less prone to System 1 or System 2 thinking while making

judgments or decisions. To test individual differences in decision strategies, researchers created

the Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI; Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier, 1996). These
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      17
 

researchers used a modified Need for Cognition Scale (NFC; Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) for

analytical or rational styles, and created a new scale to measure intuitive or affective decisions,

called Faith in Intuition (i.e., FI). In essence, these scales measure an individual’s predisposition

to rely more heavily on System 1 or System 2. Epstein et al. (1996) found that participants

scoring higher on the FI accepted intuition as being rational and logical, which reduced

flexibility in being able to shift between strategies. Based on these findings and an additional

experiment, these researchers concluded that quality decisions require the ability to use both

decision strategies, depending on the context of the situation (Epstein et al., 1996). Individuals

who score high on the FI scale will rely more heavily on System 1, have greater difficulty

engaging in System 2 thinking, and will therefore make lower quality decisions. To further

investigate these individual differences, Mata, Ferreira, and Sherman (2012) used the Cognitive

Reflection Test (CRT; Frederick, 2005) to differentiate between deliberate and intuitive

responders before asking participants to rate their own performance and the performance of

others. The deliberate responders consistently gave the correct response to every problem, and

the intuitive responders consistently gave an intuitive answer for every problem. Results

indicated that deliberate responders accurately rated their own performance and the performance

of others. In contrast, intuitive responders overestimated their own performance, as well as the

performance of others. Further implications of the study by Mata et al. (2012) will be discussed

in the next section.

Integration of Processing Systems

Typically, System 1 and System 2 are discussed as separate processing systems, however,

some recent studies have suggested that there is a mediating mechanism between them. For

instance, Thompson, Prowse Turner, and Pennycook (2011) conducted an experiment to


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      18
 

determine if metacognition, in the form of a feeling of rightness (i.e., FOR), guides the decision

whether to endorse a first initial judgment (i.e., System 1) or to engage in further processing (i.e.,

System 2). Findings indicated that judgments made with ease, which is an indication of System 1

activation, were highly correlated with high FOR scores, while lower FOR scores had a robust

relationship with rethinking and answer changes (i.e., activation of System 2). These researchers

concluded that a first initial judgment is made, then metacognitive processes determine if further

deliberation is needed, in the form of analytical thinking. Results from Mata et al.’s (2012)

previously mentioned study, which discussed differences between deliberate and intuitive

thinkers, elaborated on Epstein et al.’s (1996) findings. The intuitive responders could only think

of one solution to the problem, however, the deliberate responders were aware of both the

intuitive (i.e., wrong) answer and the more rational (i.e., right) answer. The researchers

concluded that the deliberate responders had greater metacognitive awareness, which explains

why they were able to accurately judge their own performance, as well as others. To further

investigate this conclusion, Mata et al. (2012) included a condition where participants completed

the CRT, and then were given clues to focus attention (e.g., underlined words) on the alternative

answer to the problem. During this condition, the participants who initially responded intuitively

changed their responses to the correct answer. This is particularly interesting, because it showed

that participants predisposed to relying solely on intuition were easily influenced to think more

deliberately when contextual cues caused them to refocus their attention, which improved their

metacognitive awareness and response accuracy.

Pachur et al. (2012) conducted an interesting experiment on risk perception by comparing

and contrasting the availability heuristic against the affect heuristic, both of which are guided by

memory. During the experiment, types of cancers were presented to participants in pairs.
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      19
 

Participants were then instructed to list the number of instances that could be recalled (i.e.,

availability), rate their feeling of dread (i.e., affect), and indicate how much money should be

spent to help reduce each type of cancer (i.e., value of a statistical life, or VSL). All three

assessments were made separately for both cancer types. The type of cancer that had more

recalled instances was the answer provided by the availability heuristic, and the cancer type that

had a higher rating of dread was the answer made by the affect heuristic. Occasionally,

participants recalled or rated both types of cancer equally, in these cases the heuristics failed to

discriminate. Results demonstrated that participants were better able to discriminate between the

two types of cancer by using the affect heuristic (63.8 percent), compared to the availability

heuristic (25.2 percent) (Pachur et al., 2012). It is important to note that discrimination does not

mean quality. When participants were able to recall instances of individuals developing or dying

from cancer, the use of the availability heuristic produced more accurate judgments of frequency.

However, the influence of affect was more prominent when deciding how much money should

be spent to reduce each type of cancer (i.e., VSL). Although affect was more prominent, there

was not a significant difference between affect and availability by recall for the VSL decisions. It

is possible that availability by recall induced affect when participants had direct experience with

a type of cancer. However, if individuals could not recall such instances (i.e., low availability),

then participants may have imagined what it would be like to suffer from a certain type of

cancer, which induced affect to guide their decision. In either case, availability clearly influenced

decisions and feelings of affect, but results indicated that affect contributed more than or equal to

availability by recall while making this decision. In summary, this experiment suggested that

individuals can use the availability heuristic and affect heuristic in combination or in a serial

fashion. During the experiment, participants used the availability heuristic before the affect
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      20
 

heuristic (i.e., serial use) when asked to indicate the frequency of occurrence or death regarding

types of cancer, but used both availability and affect (i.e., combined use) to indicate how much

money should be allotted to reducing types of cancer (i.e., VSL judgment). When deciding how

much money should be allotted to researching cancer types, individuals considered the amount of

instances that could be recalled (i.e., availability), as well as evaluating the degree of suffering

(i.e., affect) that the type of cancer could produce. In a second experiment, Pachur et al. (2012)

further experimentally investigated the availability heuristic by differentiating between direct

(i.e., social environment) and indirect (i.e., media) experience. Results for this second

experiment showed that direct experience plays a key role in individuals risk judgments, but

when taking affect and direct experience into account, indirect experience does not. However, it

may be possible that indirect experience is more tied to affect than availability. Consider the

topic of nuclear power. Most people do not possess the real frequency of accidents that have

occurred, however it is probable that they have heard of at least one or two accidents (e.g.,

Chernobyl or Three Mile Island). Due to the news reports of these rare and emotion inducing

accidents, individuals have likely associated (i.e., tagged) nuclear facilities with negative

feelings. News reports usually focus on the negative impacts of events and do not typically

include the actual frequency of occurrence. It seems that the news corporations are aware of the

power of negative, rare, and dramatic events to get one’s attention, which encourages people to

tune in or read articles. As mentioned earlier, individuals are overconfident about their emotional

memories which are often distorted (Phelps & Sharot, 2008). It is likely that individual’s feelings

about these events could integrate into their knowledge, which may influence how they think

about it (i.e., as an exemplar), and how they assess the frequency of accidents.
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      21
 

Overall, the study by Pachur et al. (2012) demonstrates that the use of information is

better for making quality decisions, but when knowledge is low and the risks become personal,

affect gains the upper hand. Depending on the context, the affect heuristic can substitute for the

availability heuristic (e.g., while deciding the VSL) and vice versa (e.g., judging the frequency of

death from a type of cancer). Even though affect may gain the upper hand in certain situations, it

does not increase the quality of a decision, it merely allows an individual to make a choice when

there is no relevant information to depend on (e.g., discriminating which type of cancer has the

greatest risk of death).

The literature, particularly with risks and benefits, has indicated that affect can be quite

influential when making judgments and decisions, even if the affect does not provide information

to increase the quality of conclusions. Affect is so influential, because it is the first psychological

reaction to a stimulus and may help determine the amount of deliberation. However, affect and

rationality are intimately linked and both exert influence on judgments. Using critical thinking to

consider all the evidence is the best option for most decisions, but the quality of the available

information, knowledge, and other factors influencing the judgment are important. Depending on

the quality of information, both strategies can fail or succeed. For instance, a fast intuitive

judgment can be very accurate when based on sound information (e.g., experts automatic

thinking), and a more deliberate time consuming approach can fail when information is faulty or

biased (e.g., the Challenger Disaster, overconfidence, and the confirmation bias). It seems that

the main contributor for how risks and benefits are evaluated is the context of a situation, which

determines the resources that are available to make conclusions, however, availability of

information, quality of information, and associated affect also influence initial judgments and

later decisions.
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      22
 

The literature on judgment and decision making sought to understand how and why

individuals made errors. Kahneman and Tversky (1974) convincingly showed how individuals

made errors in judgment when using heuristics based on “rational intuition”. More recently,

researchers emphasized the influence of feelings in decision making, which became the “affect

heuristic” (Finucane et al., 2000). Researchers typically used affect to explain how individuals

perceive risks and benefits to be negatively correlated and how feelings can guide or influence

judgments and decisions (Alhakami & Slovic, 1994; Denes-Raj & Epstein, 1994; Finucane et al.,

2000; Fischhoff et al., 1978; Johnson & Tversky, 1983; Savadori et al., 2004). These researchers

generally do not claim that reliance on affect (i.e., System 1) increases the quality of decisions,

compared to deliberation (i.e., System 2). However, they do suggest that affect can serve as an

anchor (i.e., reference point), increase cognitive ease, or can tag information (e.g., learning about

a nuclear power plant meltdown could cause an individual to associate nuclear power plants with

negative affect), which can increase an individual’s confidence and reliance on such information

while making judgments and decisions. In some cases, it may appear that reliance on feelings,

intuition, or automatic processing results in higher quality decisions, but this is typically found

only when relevant information is scarce (Pachur et al., 2012) or when individuals have a high

degree of knowledge and experience (e.g., experts) in that specific area (Kahneman & Frederick,

2001; Savadori et al., 2004; Slovic, 1999). However, a recent study (Mikels, Maglio, Reed, &

Kaplowitz, 2011) concluded that “affective strategies are indeed an effective means to making

good decisions…when the going gets tough, go with your gut—but with the qualification that

one should not overthink their decision” (Mikels et al., 2011, p. 10). These researchers based

their methodology and theory on Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, and Van Baaren’s (2006)

research, which focused on unconscious thought theory (i.e., UTT). According to UTT, effortless
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      23
 

unconscious thought can result in better outcomes for complex matters compared to effortful

deliberate thought. The conclusions made by Mikels et al. (2011) and UTT seem to be

inconsistent with the literature on judgment and decision making, particularly with the affect

heuristic, which implied that most errors in decision making occur because individuals tend to

rely on their feelings and intuition (i.e., System 1 processes), instead of engaging in critical

thinking (i.e., System 2). To better understand how Mikels et al. (2011) came to this conclusion,

all four experiments from the study will discussed in detail.

During the first experiment, participants in four conditions (i.e., feeling focused simple,

feeling focused complex, detail focused simple, and detail focused complex) were instructed to

choose the best option out of a group of four cars (e.g., Car A, Car B, Car C, and Car D). The

simple conditions exposed participants to four attributes per car, and the complex conditions

presented 12 attributes per car. These attributes were either framed as good or bad (e.g., this car

gets poor/good gas mileage) to ensure participants perceived them as positive or negative.

Participants in the feeling focused conditions were instructed to follow their feelings, then rate

how they felt about each attribute on a seven point scale (e.g., 1 = very negative and 7 = very

positive). Participants in the detail focused conditions were instructed to attempt to remember all

the attributes and to indicate how well they were remembering the previous attributes on a seven

point scale (e.g., 1 = not at all and 7 = very well). All attributes were presented on a computer

screen one at a time, for four seconds each, then participants made a rating before the

presentation of the next attribute. After all attributes (i.e., four for simple trial and 12 for

complex trial) were presented for each car option, participants were instructed to choose the best

option. Out of the four cars, one car had 75 percent positive attributes, two had 50 percent

positive attributes, and one car had 25 percent positive attributes. After making the decision,
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      24
 

participants were asked to write down all the attributes that could be recalled. Results for the

simple decision indicated that 47.4 percent of participants in the detail focused condition chose

the best car option, compared to 31.6 percent in the feeling focused condition. However, a Chi

square test indicated that this difference was not significant (p > .05). During the complex

decision, those in the feeling focused condition chose the best car option significantly more often

(68.4 percent) than those in the detail focused condition (26.3 percent). There were no significant

differences in recalled attributes due to focus, however, there was a significant difference

between the simple and complex conditions. The researchers concluded that focusing on feelings

can result in higher quality objective decisions than focusing on details. However, it is possible

that this experiment was confounded with memory. Participants may have experienced difficulty

attempting to hold all 48 attributes (e.g., during the complex trial) in working memory in the

detail focused condition, but were able to simplify the information by grouping the attributes into

categories (e.g., good or bad) in the feeling focused condition. This would allow participants to

remember how many attributes were good or bad instead of attempting to remember entire

attributes. This would also help explain why there were no differences in amount of attributes

recalled between the feeling focused and detail focused conditions, even though there was a

difference in performance. The interpretation of the results will be discussed in more detail later

in this section.

In the second experiment, the researchers investigated subjective choice quality. The

methods were similar to experiment one, except participants were assigned to either a feeling

focused condition or a detail focused condition and made only the complex decision. Also, there

was not a dominating alternative like the first experiment (e.g., a car with 75 percent positive

attributes), each car had equal amounts of positive and negative attributes (i.e., 50 percent). After
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      25
 

participants made car choices, they rated their confidence, satisfaction, and attempted to recall all

attributes for each car. Results indicated that participants in the feeling focused condition were

significantly more confident and satisfied with their choice, but were not able to recall more

attributes than the detail focused condition. These results are consistent with research

investigating choices based on feelings (i.e., System 1). Thompson et al. (2011) showed that

higher levels of confidence and satisfaction (i.e., the feeling of rightness) are highly correlated

with System 1 thinking, while low levels are correlated with System 2 thinking.

In the third experiment, the relative framework from experiment one was used, except

participants made only complex decisions and either deliberated for three minutes or were

distracted by a working memory task before deciding the best car option. Results indicated that

participants in the detail focused condition performed equally well whether they deliberated for

three minutes or were distracted before making a decision. However, in the feeling focused

condition, participants who deliberated performed significantly worse than participants who were

distracted by the working memory task. To ensure that these results were not due to the

performance on the working memory task, the researchers examined scores for both conditions

and found no difference in performance on the task. The researchers concluded that additional

deliberation was beneficial for participants that were focusing on details, but impaired choice

quality for participants focusing on their feelings. These results are consistent with previous

literature, which has indicated that affective reactions occur automatically, but are reduced with

conscious deliberation, since they are typically not rational.

In the fourth experiment, the researchers utilized a within subjects design. All participants

made a decision in four different domains (e.g., apartments, vacations, physicians, and medical

treatments) for each of three strategies (e.g., feeling focused, reason focused, and no focus), for a
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      26
 

total of 12 decisions. For each decision, participants had four choices (e.g., Apartment A,

Apartment B, Apartment C, and Apartment D) and 12 attributes that were either positive or

negative for each choice. As in the first experiment, choices had varying degrees of positive

attributes (e.g., 75 percent, 50 percent, and 25 percent), with one choice being the best option

(i.e., 75 percent positive attributes). Results indicated that the feeling focused decisions were

higher in choice quality, compared to decisions based on reason or without a focus. However,

affective decisions were only found to be significantly better than reasoned decisions in the

domain of apartments. The researchers concluded that affective decisions were of better quality

overall, compared to decisions without a focus, or those focusing on reason.

All of these experiments implied that reliance on affect (i.e., System 1) increased the

probability of choosing the best option under complex choice, compared to reliance on details

and deliberation (i.e., System 2). However, it appears that Mikels et al.’s (2011) conclusions are

inconsistent with the decision making literature, especially concerning the affect heuristic and

how affect influences judgments and decisions (e.g., serving as an anchor, reducing activation of

System 2). Their conclusions are somewhat consistent with the idea of tagging (i.e., associating)

information with affect (e.g., tagging nuclear power plants with negative affect after learning

about a meltdown). However, individuals typically tag objects or information with affect based

on their own personal attitudes or feelings (Alhakami & Slovic, 1994). However, in the Mikels et

al. (2011) experiment, participants were explicitly given information that basically informs them

which car is the best choice. It seems that in this experiment, attributes were tagged subjectively,

but were based on objective information. Affective information is typically only used to

discriminate between options when there is no other guiding information (e.g., memory
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      27
 

availability, objective information, or provided information), but when information is available,

individuals reduce their reliance on affect (Pachur et al., 2012).

There may be other explanations for the results in Mikels et al. studies. One possible

reason Mikels et al. (2011) found greater involvement of System 1, in the complex condition,

could be that participants had to divide attention by keeping the attributes for each car in working

memory while also paying attention to the next attributes that were presented every four seconds.

When individuals divide attention or are under time pressure, they tend to make decisions based

on System 1 processing (Maule & Svenson, 1993; Finucane et al., 2000). However, in this

experiment, individuals were only under time pressure when given the attributes, but could spend

time deliberating options before making a decision. Mikels et al. (2011) explained that

individuals may have encoded information in memory in conjunction with affect, which

facilitated subsequent recall. This may be true, however, it is more likely that individuals

grouped or categorized information (i.e., chunked), which reduced the attributes down to a single

value or category (i.e., good or bad). This process could have reduced the mental workload and

amount of effort needed by System 2, which could have allowed participants to utilize the

information without having to remember whole attributes. If this is the case, then it may be

misleading to imply that the increased quality of decisions was due to the affective tagging of

information.

Based on these interpretations, it is possible that the feeling focused participants in

Mikels et al.’s (2011) study chunked information into two categories (i.e., good or bad), which

decreased mental workload, compared to those in the detail focused condition who were

encouraged to remember entire attributes. Therefore, Mikels et al.’s (2011) findings might to be

due to memory load and not affect. To test this alternate explanation, the present investigation
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      28
 

was designed to test whether the individuals in the feeling focused condition based their

decisions on their feelings or by simplifying the details of the attributes and making a decision

based on critical thinking. To test this interpretation, the first experiment by Mikels et al. (2011)

was first replicated and then extended with two additional conditions. An overview of the entire

study is presented below.

In order to replicate the first experiment, two trials were conducted using only the feeling

focused and detail focused conditions. Participants in both of these conditions made a simple and

complex decision based on the methodology from Mikels et al. (2011). The replication was

conducted as a 2 (complexity) x 2 (focus) mixed factorial design, with complexity as a within

subjects variable and focus as a between subjects variable. It was believed that feeling focused

participants would make significantly better decisions than the detail focused participants, for the

complex decisions, which would replicate the findings from Mikels et al. (2011) and allow for

further comparison with the two additional conditions.

After the replication experiment, all participants completed the main experiment, which

consisted of three simple (i.e., four attributes per option) and three complex decisions (i.e., 12

attributes per option). Two additional conditions were added as a manipulation: a feeling focused

note taking condition and a detail focused note taking condition. In total, there were four

conditions for the main experiment: feeling focused no notes, feeling focused notes, detail

focused no notes, and detail focused notes. The addition of the memory manipulation changed

the design to a mixed 2 (complexity) x 2 (focus) x 2 (notes). Participants did not make ratings in

the main experiment, as it would have been counterproductive for participants to make ratings

and take notes. Also, it was believed that the ratings for the feeling focused participants were

more useful than the ratings made by the detail focused participants, however, note taking was
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      29
 

expected to be equally useful for both focus conditions. The note taking manipulation was added

to challenge the overall conclusion by Mikels et al. (2011) (i.e., focusing on feelings results in

better decisions than thinking critically) by testing whether individuals actually focused on their

feelings when they decided which option was the best choice or if they were merely grouping the

information into a category (i.e., good or bad), which increased cognitive ease, efficiency, and

the quality of decisions.

In the Mikels et al. (2011) experiment, attributes were shown to participants for four

seconds and then participants were given time to make ratings about feelings or how well they

remembered previous attributes. This could have been a potential problem for note taking

conditions in the current study, since participants could have written down the entire set of

attributes. To account for this possible problem, participants were presented with attributes in

blocks of four. Attributes were still presented one at a time, for four seconds each, but

participants were only allowed to take notes after four attributes had been presented. Following

the presentation of an attribute block, participants were given a 16 second delay to take notes

before moving on to the next option (e.g., simple trials) or next four attribute block (e.g.,

complex trials). To allow for equal treatment, participants in the no note taking conditions also

had a 16 second delay; however, they were instructed to think about the last four attributes while

focusing on the details (i.e., detail focused) or their own feelings (i.e., feeling focused).

Participants in the detail focused condition were expected to make more accurate choices,

compared to those in the feeling focused condition, based on the assumption that participants in

the feeling focused conditions actually focused on their feelings.

To summarize, this thesis project attempted to challenge the idea that participants in the

Mikels et al. (2011) experiment made better decisions by focusing on their feelings, compared to
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      30
 

thinking critically, when making decisions. It was believed that these findings could be explained

by memory, not the affect heuristic or UTT. Participants could have reduced the information into

groups (i.e., good or bad), which may have reduced the amount of cognitive effort required to

make the best choice, yielding a result that looked like affect or unconscious thought led to better

decision making than critical thinking.

This study had three hypotheses:


 
1) Participants in the feeling focused no notes condition were expected to make better

complex decisions than those in the detail focused no notes condition.

2) Participants in the detail focused notes condition were expected to make better complex

decisions, compared to those in the feeling focused notes condition.

3) Participants in the detail focused notes condition were expected to outperform those in

the detail focused no notes condition.

Method

Participants

One hundred and ninety two participants (61% female; mean age = 18.6) were recruited

from general psychology classes at Shippensburg University of Pennsylvania between July 29th

and October 15th of 2014. The majority of the participants were freshman (152), with some

sophomores (31), juniors (8), and seniors (1). The frequency of academic majors from greatest to

least were: Undeclared (46), Education (28), Biology/Health (25), Psychology (20), Criminology

(17), Business (17), Communications (9), Accounting (7), Social Work (7), History (3), Art (3),

English (2), Marketing (2), Economics (2), Computer Science (2), Political Science (2),

Engineering (2), Sociology (1), Chemistry (1), Journalism (1), Public Administration (1). Mean
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      31
 

GPA could not be calculated due to the high number of participants (N = 133) who did not know

their GPA.

Materials and Procedure

Participants (N = 192) were recruited in small groups (M = 4.5) and were randomly

assigned to one of four conditions: feeling focused no notes (n = 49), feeling focused notes (n =

47), detail focused no notes (n = 50), and detail focused notes (n = 46), and one of four orders for

counterbalancing. All participants in the study completed a replication of the Mikels et al. (2011)

experiment, followed by the main experiment with an added memory manipulation. Both of the

current experiments had the same basic elements. Participants made choices based on provided

information. Two trials (i.e., a simple and complex) were completed for each category (e.g., car,

job, apartment, and computer). There were four options in each category (e.g., Car A, Car B, Car

C, and Car D). Information given for each option was in the form of positive or negative

attributes (e.g., “this car gets above average or below average gas mileage”, “this job has above

or below average wages”, “this apartment includes or does not include utilities”, or “this

computer has below or above average chance of overheating”; See Appendix A). There were

four attributes given for each option during simple trials and 12 given during complex trials. All

attributes for each option were presented before moving on to the next option. Each option

possessed a different amount of positive and negative attributes. One option had 75 percent

positive attributes, two had 50 percent positive attributes, and one had 25 percent positive

attributes. The option with 75 percent positive attributes was defined as the correct choice. These

methods were adapted from Mikels et al. (2011), except participants made both simple and

complex decisions, and all data were recorded on paper instead of using a computer program.
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      32
 

Prior to starting the study, participants filled out a consent form (See Appendix B) and

then were given the basic instructions for the entire study verbally. Participants were given the

following instructions: 1) “you will be making choices based on information presented on

PowerPoint slides”, 2) “an object or category (e.g., car) will be presented”, 3) “then you will be

given information about four different options (e.g., Car A, Car B, Car C, and Car D), which will

be presented one at a time”, 4) “the information will give value to each option”, 5) “after all

information for each option is presented you will choose which option (e.g., Car A – D) you

believe is the best choice by circling an option on the provided choice sheet” (See Appendix C).

The choice sheet was used to record all choices throughout the experiment. Specific instructions

for focus and note taking were then given before the start of the replication and the main

experiment.

Replication. The design for the replication experiment was a 2 (Complexity) x 2 (Focus)

mixed design. There were only two different conditions (i.e., feeling focused and detail focused)

and participants completed both a simple and complex trial. As previously mentioned,

participants were given a consent form and basic instructions. Prior to starting the replication,

rating sheets were passed out (See Appendix D) and participants were given further instructions.

Participants were instructed to focus on their feelings (i.e., feeling focused) or on the details of

the provided information (i.e., detail focused), depending on which condition they were

randomly assigned to. Next, participants were informed that they “would be given an eight

second delay between each attribute presentation to make ratings”. The feeling focused

participants were instructed to rate how they felt about the attributes for each option on a 7-point

scale, where 1 = very negative and 7 = very positive. Participants in the detail focused condition

were instructed to rate how well they were remembering the attributes for each option on a
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      33
 

similar 7-point scale, where 1 = not at all and 7 = very well. Before starting the PowerPoint,

participants were informed that “the current option (e.g., Car A, Car B, etc.) would always be

shown at the top of the screen” and that they would be shown four pieces of information for each

option (i.e., simple trial). After making sure participants were clear on how to perform, the

PowerPoint presentation began. The first PowerPoint slide indicated the category (i.e., car for the

replication experiment), before moving on to the first option (i.e., Car A). All four pieces of

information were presented for an option before moving on to the next option, which were

presented in the following order: A, B, C, and D. Pieces of information were presented one at a

time, for four seconds each, followed by an eight second rating period. After the information for

all options was presented, the choice sheet was passed out, and participants were instructed to

circle which option they believed to be the optimal choice. After participants made their choice,

the rating sheet for the complex trial was passed out and participants were informed that the next

trial would follow the same procedure, except there would be 12 pieces of information for each

option. After participants finished the complex trial, the replication experiment was complete.

Main Experiment. After participants completed the replication experiment, they were

given further instructions for the remainder of the experiment. Participants were informed that

the remaining trials had the same procedure, except 1) “four pieces of information would be

presented before the delay period, which is now extended to 16 seconds”, and 2) “instead of

making ratings during the delay, there will be a different task”. At this time, note taking sheets

(See Appendix E) were passed out to the participants in the note taking conditions. Note taking

participants were instructed to take short notes about information based on the details (i.e., detail

focused) or how they felt about it (i.e., feeling focused). The participants not taking notes were

instructed to think about the details (i.e., detail focused), or how they felt about the information
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      34
 

for the current option (i.e., feeling focused). After verifying that participants were clear on the

instructions, the experiment began using the same procedure as the replication experiment with

the addition of the note taking manipulation and four attribute blocks. The addition of the note

taking conditions for the main experiment changed the design to a mixed 2 (Complexity) x 2

(Focus) x 2 (Notes). Participants completed two trials (i.e., a simple and complex trial) for job,

apartment, and computer categories. In total, there were six trials: three simple and three

complex. After completion of all six trials, a demographic form (See Appendix F) and the

Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI-revised; Pacini & Epstein, 1999; See Appendix G) were

passed out to each participant. Participants were instructed to read and fill out both the

demographic form and questionnaire. After completing both forms, participants were informed

that the experiment was over and were given an experimental debriefing (See Appendix H).

Results

Replication Trials

Although all participants completed the replication trials, only the data from 110

participants (63% female; mean age = 18.8) could be used, as the rest of the data had been

compromised due to a repeating attribute which resulted in two correct answers for the simple

trial. This error was corrected for the 110 participants used in the analysis. The data were

analyzed by comparing the number of correct choices between the two conditions using chi-

square tests, which was the same method used by Mikels et al. (2011). Refer to Table 1 for

comparisons between the focus conditions.

Table 1
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      35
 

Percent of Participants in the Feeling Focused and Detail Focused Conditions with Correct

Choices, Correct Ratings, and Rating Based Decisions.

Correct Choice Correct Rating Rating Based Decisions


Trials n Simple Complex Simple Complex Simple Complex
Feeling Focused 55 69% 95% 70% 96% 73% 98%
Detail Focused 55 51% 62% 22% 31% 31% 31%
Mean 110 60% 78% 46% 64% 52% 65%

Participants in the feeling focused condition (69%) significantly outperformed those in

the detail focused condition (51%) for the simple trial, χ2 (1, N = 110) = 3.76, p < .05. For the

complex trial, the feeling focused participants (95%) also outperformed the detail focused

participants (62%), χ2 (1, N = 110) = 17.268, p < .001. Differences between simple and complex

trials were also analyzed using chi-square tests. Participants in the feeling focused condition

performed significantly better for the complex trial (95%) compared to the simple trial (69%), χ2

(1, N = 55) = 11.96, p < .001. Participants in the detail focused condition also performed better

for the complex trial (62%), compared to the simple trial (51%), however, this difference was not

significant, χ2 (1, N = 55) = 1.33, p > .10.

Participant’s ratings were analyzed using chi-square tests to compare how often the

highest rated option corresponded to the correct choice and how often the decision was based on

the highest rated option. For the simple trial, the feeling focused ratings corresponded with the

correct choice more often (70%) than detail focused ratings (22%), χ2 (1, N = 110) = 26.65, p <

.001. The feeling focused participants also based decisions on the highest rated option more often

(73%) than the detail focused participants (31%), χ2 (1, N = 110) = 19.26, p < .001. For the

complex trial, the same pattern was found. The feeling focused participant ratings corresponded

with the correct choice more often (96%), compared to the detail focused participant ratings
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      36
 

(31%), χ2 (1, N = 110) = 50.91, p < .001. The feeling focused participants also based decisions

on the highest rated option (98%) more often than the detail focused participants (31%), χ2 (1, N

= 110) = 54.38, p < .001.

Main Experiment

The data for all 192 participants were analyzed using a 2 (Complexity) x 2 (Focus) x 2

(Notes) mixed factorial design with proportion correct as the dependent variable, complexity as a

within subjects variable, and focus and notes as between subjects variables. Refer to Table 2 for

differences in proportion correct and standard deviations across conditions.

Table 2

Mean Proportion Correct and Standard Deviations in Simple and Complex Trials for Feeling

Focused No Notes, Feeling Focused Notes, Detail Focused No Notes, and Detail Focused Notes.

Feeling Focused Detail Focused


Notes Without With Without With All
n = 49 n = 47 n = 50 n = 46 N = 192
.83 .86 .74 .83 .81
Simple
(.205) (.206) (.318) (.26) (.254)
.79 .84 .79 .87 .82
Complex
(.233) (.229) (.3) (.192) (.243)
.81 .85 .77 .85 .82
Total
(.17) (.188) (.259) (.189) (.206)

Results showed a main effect for notes, F (1, 188) = 4.181, p = .042, η2p = .022, which

indicated that participants performed better, regardless of focus, when they were able to take

notes (Refer to Figure 1).


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      37
 

Main Effect for Notes

0.9
Mean Proportion Correct
0.85

0.8

0.75 No Notes
Notes
0.7
Feeling focused Detail focused
 
Figure. 1. Mean proportion correct for all trials and conditions.

An interaction between complexity and focus nearly reached significance, F(1, 188) =

3.797, p = .053, η2p = .02. This interaction was significant when notes and no notes conditions

were collapsed and analyzed using a 2 (complexity) x 2 (focus) mixed ANOVA, F(1, 190) =

3.885, p = .05, η2p = .02. (Refer to Figure 2).

Interaction between Complexity and Focus

0.86
Mean Proportion Correct

0.84
0.82
0.8
0.78
Feeling focus
0.76 Detail focus
0.74
Simple Complex
Complexity

Figure 2. Mean proportion correct for complexity and focus.

T-tests revealed that this interaction was driven by the difference between feeling focus

(M = .84) and detail focus (M = .78) conditions for simple trials, t(95) = -1.75, p = .041. The
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      38
 

difference between feeling focus (M = .82) and detail focus (M = .83) conditions for the complex

trials was not significant, p > .05.

Although the main argument about memory was supported by the main effect for notes,

there was no evidence to support the three main hypotheses (e.g., predicted interactions were not

significant). However, t-tests were used to explore differences in means between conditions.

First, participants in the feeling focused no notes condition (M = .81) did not significantly

outperform those in the detail focused no notes condition (M = .77), p > .05. Second, participants

in the detail focused notes condition (M = .85) did not significantly outperform those in the

feeling focused notes condition (M = .85), p > .05. Third, participants in the detail focused notes

condition (M = .85) did not significantly outperform those in the detail focused no notes

condition (M = .77), p > .05. However, the trend was in the right direction (see Figure 1). Fourth,

participants in the feeling focus notes condition (M = .85) did not significantly outperform those

in the feeling focus no notes condition (M = .81), p > .05.

Chi-square tests were used to investigate if there were any differences in note taking

between the two focus conditions. Four variables were used to investigate these differences, they

were: large amount of notes, large amount of detail, including emotional content (e.g., good, bad,

smiley faces), and use of symbols (e.g., arrows, pluses and minuses). Although all the tests

revealed results that were not significant (i.e., p > .05), there were some notable trends. More

participants in the detail focused condition wrote a large amount of notes (87%), included a large

amount of detail (93.5%), and used symbols (65.2%), compared to those in the feeling focused

condition (e.g., 72.3% for large amount of notes, 83% for large amount of detail , and 51% for

use of symbols). More participants in the feeling focused condition (21.3%) included emotional

content, compared to those in the detail focused condition (19.5%).


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      39
 

Individual Differences

To investigate individual differences, participants filled out the Rational-Experiential

Inventory (REI-revised; Pacini & Epstein, 1999). As mentioned previously, this inventory

contains two scales to measure the predisposition for thinking with System 1 (e.g., FI or Faith in

Intuition) or System 2 (e.g., NFC or Need for Cognition). Participants were split up into three

groups (e.g., High NFC, Medium NFC, and Low NFC) depending the difference between NFC

and FI scores (e.g., NFC – FI). A 2 (complexity) x 3 (NFC group) mixed factorial design was

then used to determine if there were any differences between the three NFC groups for

proportion correct on simple and complex trials.

Table 4.

Mean Proportion Correct and Standard Deviations in Simple (N = 3) and Complex (N = 3)

Trials for High, Medium, and Low NFC Groups.

High NFC Medium NFC Low NFC All


n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 N = 192
.83 .85 .76 .81
Simple
(.222) (.205) (.316) (.254)
.84 .86 .77 .82
Complex
(.229) (.22) (.27) (.243)
.84 .85 .76 .82
Total
(.19) (.175) (.239) (.206)

The results showed a significant main effect for NFC group, F(2, 189) = 3.805, p = .024,

η2p = .039. Post hoc tests (i.e., LSD) indicated that participants in the High NFC group (M = .84)

performed significantly better than those in the Low NFC group (M =.76), p = .033. Participants

in the Medium NFC group (M =.85) also significantly outperformed those in the Low NFC

group (M =.76), p = .011. To ensure that this effect is indeed due to thinking strategy and not

condition, a mixed factorial ANCOVA was completed with notes and focus as covariates. This
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      40
 

test also revealed a main effect for NFC group, F(2, 187) = 3.134, p = .046, η2p = .032. Post hoc

tests (i.e., LSD) only revealed a significant difference between the Medium NFC group (M =.85)

and the Low NFC group (M = .76), p = .018. These results suggest that predispositions for

thinking styles can affect performance on decision tasks.

Discussion

The main purpose of this thesis was to investigate and challenge the findings from Mikels

et al. (2011), who concluded: “when the going gets tough, go with your gut—but with the

qualification that one should not overthink their decision” (Mikels et al., 2011, p. 10). Based on

the results of the replication and experiment, some evidence was obtained to challenge this

conclusion.

Replication Trials and Ratings

In support of Mikels et al. (2011), participants in the feeling focused condition performed

better than those in the detail focused condition for the complex trial. Also, feeling focused

participants performed better for the complex trial compared to the simple trial. However, the

current study found that feeling focused participants performed better than detail focused

participants for both the simple and complex trial. Since there were more participants in the

current study and participants completed both the simple and complex trial, it may be possible

that individual differences played a role in Mikels et al. (2011).

The analysis for ratings in the replication experiment indicated that when feeling focused

participants made choices, 85.5 percent relied on their highest rated option, which was correct 83

percent of the time. This seems to indicate that ratings for the feeling focused participants were

used to make relatively accurate decisions. Participants may have judged each attribute

subjectively, but this judgment was guided by the objective nature of the attributes, which were
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      41
 

essentially framed as pros and cons (e.g., “this car gets above average gas mileage” or “this car

gets below average gas mileage”). Most people would agree that a fuel efficient car is good;

however, the exact value or intensity of affect may vary slightly from person to person (e.g.,

good to very good). It is interesting that feeling focused participants performed better for the

complex trial (95%) than the simple trial (69%). It is possible that this slight variation in

judgment could have been reduced when participants made more subjective judgments during

the complex trial. For instance, the quality of an average (e.g., average student test score)

increases when there is more data (e.g., more students). However, since all participants

completed the simple trial prior to the complex trial, this finding could have been due to a

practice effect.

In the Mikels et al. (2011) experiment, participants did not have a rating sheet to assist

them in making a choice, they were only able to rely on memory. In an attempt to show that

participants in the feeling focused group performed better due to focus, Mikels et al. (2011)

compared the amount of recalled attributes for the feeling focused and detail focused conditions,

which was not significant. This seems to strengthen the point that participants in the feeling

focused condition only had to remember how many attributes were good (i.e., pros) or bad (i.e.,

cons) instead of having to remember the attributes themselves.

Main Experiment

This experiment was designed to investigate an alternate explanation of the results found

by Mikels et al. (2011) by adding a memory manipulation. Since it was conceivable that ratings

were partially responsible for the increased performance of the feeling focused condition,

participants did not make any kind of ratings during the experiment. In contrast to Mikels et al.

(2011) and the replication, there were no differences between the focus conditions. That is, no
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      42
 

differences in performance were noted between feeling focused and detail focused conditions.

The only significant difference in choice accuracy was due to the memory manipulation, which

was demonstrated by the main effect for notes.

Based on the replication and experiment, it appears that the findings from the Mikels et

al. (2011) study were confounded with memory as hypothesized. For instance, participants in the

feeling focused condition only had to remember how many attributes were rated as good (i.e.,

pros) or bad (i.e., cons), which gave them an advantage since there was less strain on memory.

When participants did not make ratings or take notes, differences between focus conditions were

nonexistent. Also, there were no differences in the amount of notes, amount of detail, emotional

content, or use of symbols between the feeling focused and detail focused participants who took

notes. It seems clear that participants in the feeling focused conditions performed better due to

the nature of the ratings and reduced strain on memory instead of focus.

In addition to being confounded with memory, the results seem to be inconsistent with

the affect heuristic and UTT. According to the affect heuristic, individuals use immediate

emotional reactions to guide decisions (Slovic et al., 2002), not multiple subjective judgments.

UTT states that individuals make better decisions when relying on System 1 processing,

compared to System 2 processing. Although it is possible for an individual to make relatively

accurate decisions based on numerous subjective judgments that may or may not be made with

ease, this depends on the quality of the subjective estimates, the context in which they are used,

and the amount of deliberation (Halpern, 2014). It is also difficult to determine the quality of

these judgments, because of their subjective nature. However, during the Mikels et al. (2011) and

replication experiment, participants made subjective judgments based on objective information,

which may have decreased the impact of personal attitudes. With this in mind, it is possible that
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      43
 

feeling focused participants in both experiments were making higher quality decisions because

they had less strain on memory, were utilizing multiple judgments, and were thinking critically

about each option before making a decision (i.e., engaging in System 2 over System 1).

Experiments using both feeling focus and detail focus conditions may be able to test these ideas

by 1) creating an attribute chunking condition for half of the participants, 2) by having half of

participants only rate the value of each four attribute block, or 3) by putting half of the

participants under time pressure when making the decision, thereby reducing time to deliberate.

Individual Differences

Research in decision making typically does not look at individual differences, however,

some studies have found that individual differences play a significant role (Fleischhauer et al.,

2010; Mata et al., 2012; Peters et al. 2006; Thompson et. al., 2011). The analysis of High,

Medium, and Low NFC groups in the current study suggests that differences in thinking

strategies are good indicators of performance in decision making tasks. These findings suggest

that people who prefer to engage in System 2 thinking over System 1 thinking make higher

quality decisions, which is in agreement with the majority of decision making literature (e.g.,

Kahneman, 2011; Halpern, 2014). Results also suggest that those who equally engage in both

System 1 and System 2 thinking could potentially make the best decisions overall, which agrees

with the individual difference literature previously discussed (Mata et al., 2012; Thompson et al.,

2011). Other researchers investigating individual differences in decision making have found

similar results. For instance, Peters et al. (2006) conducted an interesting study to investigate

individual differences in numerical ability (i.e., the ability to understand numerical concepts and

formats) and found that individuals low in numeracy (i.e., lower critical thinking skills) were

more likely to succumb to pitfalls of decision making, such as: framing effects, and reliance on
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      44
 

affect. Another study (Fleischhauer et al., 2010), investigated the relationship between the need

for cognition and various personality traits using confirmatory factor analysis, correlations, and

F-tests. Results indicated that those with a higher need for cognition (i.e., predisposition to

engage in System 2 processing) have more cognitive motivation to be correct, are more

cognitively active, have stronger goal orientation, and persistence.

Limitations and Future Research

In the current study, there were some limitations that should be considered. First, the

sample (61% female; mean age = 18.6; 79% freshman; 24% undeclared) was not very

representative of the general population. Second, participants made ratings in the replication

trials on physical rating sheets compared to the computer software used by Mikels et al. (2011).

This could have created a stronger reliance on ratings, since participants were able to see all of

their ratings, and were able to use them to make decisions. Third, the order of simple and

complex trials could have been counterbalanced. However, in the current experiment, there were

four conditions and four different orderings of options for a total of 16 separate conditions.

Adding these additional orderings would bring the total of separate conditions to 32, which

would have created difficultly when attempting to distribute participants equally throughout all

conditions. Fourth, the methodology to measure and focus on feelings in Mikels et al. (2011)

experiment and the replication experiment may have been tied to deliberate thought. It could be

argued that numerical values still represented feelings better than deliberation during both the

Mikels et al. (2011) study and the replication experiment. However, it is possible that

participants could have increased reliance on feelings if they were put under time pressure,

instead of having unlimited time, when making decisions. As mentioned earlier, research has

demonstrated that when individuals make decisions under time pressure, they rely more heavily
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      45
 

on affect (Finucane et al., 2000) and reduce cognitive deliberation (Maule & Svenson, 1993). It

is also possible that participants could have focused more on feelings if the attributes were more

ambiguous and open to interpretation, instead of being framed as either pros or cons. For

instance, instead of saying “this car gets above average gas mileage”, the attribute could say “this

car gets about 30 miles per gallon”. 5) It would have been more beneficial to have participants

fill out the REI prior to the start of the experiment to ensure an equal distribution for System 1

and System 2 thinkers. This had been considered, but was rejected based on time concerns and

the possibility that it would inform participants of the purposes of the study. Based on evidence

from the current study and others (Peters et al. 2006), individual differences in thinking styles

should be considered in future studies involving judgment and decision making.

References

Alhakami, A. S., & Slovic, P. (1994). A psychological study of the inverse relationship between

perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis, 14, 1085-1096. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-

6924.1994.tb00080.x

American Pet Products Association (2013). National pet owner’s survey. Retrieved from

http://www.americanpetproducts.org/press_industrytrends.asp.

Arum, R., & Roksa, J. (2011). Academically adrift. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

isbn: 9780226028569

Asimov, I. (1989, 31 March). Combatting US scientific literacy. Los Angeles Times, p. 8.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. (1997). Deciding advantageously before

knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293-1295.

doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      46
 

Beilock, S. L. & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs

choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701-725.

doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.701

Berger, D. (1995). Errors in judgments and decisions: Understanding our cognitive fallibilities.

In P. Foster (Ed.), Critical thinking: Views and values in college teaching (pp. 48-68).

Riverside, CA: La Sierra University Press.

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of personality and social

psychology, 42(1), 116. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116

Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Benitez, S., & Chang, J. (2009). Multitasking

across generations: multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of

Americans. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 483-489. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.012

Conway, M. A., Cohen, G., & Stanhope, N. (1991). On the very long-term retention of

knowledge acquired through formal education: twelve years of cognitive psychology.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 395-409.

doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.4.395

de Vries, M., Holland, R. W., & Witteman, C. L. M. (2008). In the winning mood: affect in the

Iowa gambling task. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(1), 42-50.

doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.4.395

Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: when

people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 66, 819-829. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.819

Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M., Nordgren, L., & Van Baaren, R. (2006). On making the right choice:

the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311(5763), 1005-1007.


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      47
 

doi:  10.1126/science.1121629

Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-

experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 71, 390-405. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390

Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in

judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(1), 1-17.

doi:  10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S

Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S., & Combs, B. (1978). How safe is safe

enough? a psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits.

Policy Sciences, 9(2), 127-152. doi: 10.1007/BF00143739

Fleischhauer, M., Enge, S., Brocke, B., Ullrich, J., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2010). Same or

different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(1), 82-96.

doi: 10.1177/0146167209351886

Folkes, V. S. (1988). The availability heuristic and perceived risk. Journal of Consumer

Research, 15, 13-23. url: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489168

Fox, C. R. (2006). The availability heuristic in the classroom: How soliciting more criticism can

boost your course ratings. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 86-90. url:

http://journal.sjdm.org/06020/jdm06020.htm

Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (2011). The recognition heuristic: a decade of research.

Judgment and Decision Making, 6(1), 100-121.

url: http://journal.sjdm.org/11/rh15/rh15.html
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      48
 

Gray, C. D., & Kinnear, P. R. (2012). IBM SPSS statistics 19 made simple. New York:

Psychology Press. isbn: 978-1-84872-069-5

Halpern, D. F. (2014). Thought and knowledge: an introduction to critical thinking. New York:

Psychology Press. isbn: 10: 1848726295

Hsee, C. K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: an explanation for preference reversals between

joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 67, 242-257. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0077

Hsee, C. K. (1998). Less is better: when low-value options are valued more highly than high-

value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(2), 107-121.

doi: /10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199806)11:2<107::AID-BDM292>3.0.CO;2-Y

Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 20-31.

doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.20

Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in

intuitive judgment. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, 49-81.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness.

Cognitive psychology, 3, 430-454.

doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological review,

80, 237. doi:  10.1037/h0034747

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

isbn: 10: 0374275637


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      49
 

Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M., & Combs, B. (1978). Judged frequency

of lethal events. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory, 4,

551. doi:  10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.551

Lowenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings.

Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267-286. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267

Mata, A., Ferreira, M. B., & Sherman, S. J. (2013). The metacognitive advantage of deliberative

thinkers: A dual-process perspective on overconfidence. Journal of personality and

social psychology, 105, 353. doi:  10.1037/a0033640

Maule, A. J., & Svenson, O. (1993). Theoretical and empirical approaches to behavioral decision

making and their relation to time constraints. In Time pressure and stress in human

judgment and decision making (pp. 3-25). New York: Plenum Press.

McKelvie, S. J. (1997). The availability heuristic: effects of fame and gender on the estimated

frequency of male and female names. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137(1), 63-78.

doi: 10.1080/00224549709595414

Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary. (2014). Definition for heuristic. Retrieved from

http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/heuristic

Mikels, J. A., Maglio, S. J., Reed, A. E., & Kaplowitz, L. J. (2011). Should I go with my gut?

investigating the benefits of emotion-focused decision making. Emotion, 11, 743. doi:

10.1037/a0023986

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity

for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.

doi:  10.1037/h0043158
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      50
 

National Science Board. (2014). Science and Engineering Indicators 2014:  Chapter 7. Science

and Technology: Public Attitudes and Understanding. Arlington VA: National Science

Foundation (NSB 14-01). Retrieved from

http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind14/index.cfm/chapter-7/c7h.htm

Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing

styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 76, 972-987. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972.

Pachur, T., Hertwig, R. & Steinmann, F. (2012). How do people judge risks: availability

heuristic, affect heuristic or both? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 314-330. doi:  

10.1037/a0028279

Peters, E., Vāstfjäll, D., Gärling, T., & Slovic, P. (2006). Affect and decision making: a “hot”

topic. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 79-85.

doi:  0.1002/bdm.528

Peters, E., Vāstfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy

and decision making. Psychological Science, 17, 407- 413.

doi:  10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01720.x

Pfister, H., & Böhm, G. (2008). The multiplicity of emotions: a framework of emotional

functions in decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(1), 5-17.

url: http://journal.sjdm.org/bb1/bb1.html

Phelps, E. A., & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of

recollection. Current directions in Psychological Science, 17, 147-152.

doi:  10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00565.x
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      51
 

Ritov, I. (2006). The effect of time on pleasure with chosen outcomes. Journal of Behavioral

Decision Making, 19, 177-190. doi: 10.1002/bdm.526

Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Strayer, D. L., Medeiros-Ward, N., & Watson J. M. (2013). Who multi-

tasks and why? multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and

sensation seeking. PLoS ONE, 8(1): e54402. doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0054402

Savadori, L., Savio, S., Nicotra, E., Rumiati, R., Finucane, M., & Slovic, P. (2004). Expert and

public perception of risk from biotechnology. Risk analysis, 24, 1289-1299. doi:  

10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00526.x

Schwartz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? The informative function of

affective states. In K. Fiedler and J. Forgas (Eds), Affect, cognition, and social behavior,

(44-62). Toronto: Hogrefe International. isbn: 978-0-88937-016-6

Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: the interplay of affect and

cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278-292.

doi: 10.1086/209563

Sjöberg, L. (1971). The new functionalism. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 12(1), 29-52.

doi:  10.1111/j.1467-9450.1971.tb00602.x

Sjöberg, L. (2003). Intuitive vs. analytical decision making: which is preferred? Scandinavian

Journal of Management, 19, 17-29. doi:10.1016/S0956-5221(01)00041-0

Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-­‐assessment

battlefield. Risk analysis, 19, 689-701. doi:  10.1023/A:1007041821623

Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic.

European Journal of Operational Research, 177, 1333-1352.

doi:10.1016/j.ejor.2005.04.006
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      52
 

Söllner, A., Bröder, A., & Hillbig, B. E. (2013). Deliberation versus automaticity in decision

making: which present format features facilitate automatic decision making? Judgment

and Decision Making, 8, 278-298.

url:  http://journal.sjdm.org/13/13426/jdm13426.html

Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J. A., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and

metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63(3), 107-140.

doi:  10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and

probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-302. doi:  10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgments under uncertainty: heuristics and biases.

Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131.

url:  http://www.jstor.org/stable/1738360?origin=JSTOR-pdf

van Dijk, E., & Zeelenberg, M. (2006). The dampening effect of uncertainty on positive and

negative emotions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 171-176.

doi:  10.1002/bdm.504

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 9(2), 1-27. doi:  10.1037/h0025848

Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. American

Psychologist, 35, 151-175. doi:  10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151

Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current directions in

psychological science, 10, 224-228. doi:  10.1111/1467-8721.00154


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      53
 

Appendix A

Attribute Lists for All Four Trial Sets

Table B1

The 12 Negative and Positive Attributes Used for Car in Complex Trials, Attributes Used

in Simple Trials are marked with an Asterisk.

Negative Positive
This car has poor gas mileage* This car has good gas mileage*
This car is not good for the environment* This car is relatively good for the environment*
This car has a poor sound system* This car has a good sound system*
This car is not very new* This car is very new*
This car has poor handling This car has good handling
This car has a small trunk This car has a large trunk
This car is not available in many colors This car is available in many different colors
This car has poor service This car has excellent service
This car has poor legroom This car has a lot of leg room
This car is difficult to drive This car is easy to drive
This car has no cup holders This car has cup holders
This car does not have a sunroof This car has a sunroof
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      54
 

Table B2

The 12 Negative and Positive Attributes Used for Job in Complex Trials, Attributes

Used in Simple Trials are marked with an Asterisk.

Negative Positive
This Job has lower than average wages* This Job has higher than average wages*
This Job has no opportunities for advancement* This Job has opportunities for advancement*
This Job has a high stress level* This Job has a low stress level*
This Job is not very flexible with work schedules* This Job is very flexible with work schedules*
The workplace temperature is not well controlled The workplace temperature is well controlled
The other employees argue and complain a lot The other employees do not argue or complain a lot
This Job does not have paid breaks This Job has paid breaks
The managers and supervisors are rude to employees The managers and supervisors are polite to employees
This work environment contains health hazards This work environment contains no health hazards
This work environment is small and cramped This work environment is large and open
This Job does not provide health insurance or benefits This Job provides health insurance or benefits
This Job has a strict dress policy This Job does not have a strict dress policy

Table B3
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      55
 

The 12 Negative and Positive Attributes Used for Apartment in Complex Trials,

Attributes Used in Simple Trials are marked with an Asterisk.

Negative Positive
This apartment has smaller than average bedrooms* This apartment has larger than average bedrooms*
This apartment has higher than average rent* This apartment has lower than average rent*
This apartment does not have air conditioning* This apartment has air conditioning*
This apartment has not been renovated recently* This apartment has been renovated recently*
This apartment does not have a washer and dryer This apartment has a washer and dryer
This apartment is not walking distance from campus This apartment is walking distance from campus
This apartment has low quality maintenance service This apartment has high quality maintenance service
This apartment does not have closets or storage space This apartment has closets or storage space
This apartment does not have a patio or balcony This apartment has a patio or balcony
This apartment has a small living room This apartment has a large living room
This apartment does not have a parking lot This apartment has a parking lot
This apartment does not have kitchen appliances This apartment has kitchen appliances

Table B4

The 12 Negative and Positive Attributes Used for Computer in Complex Trials,

Attributes Used in Simple Trials are marked with an Asterisk.

Negative Positive
This Computer does not come with a word This Computer comes with a word
processor* processor*
This Computer has no wireless internet This Computer has wireless internet
capabilities* capabilities*
This Computer has a lower than average This Computer has a higher than average
lifespan* lifespan*
This Computer has difficulty running multiple This Computer has no difficulty running
programs* multiple programs*
This Computer does not come with virus
This Computer comes with virus protection
protection
This Computer has lower than average This Computer has higher than average
processing speed processing speed
This Computer is above average in chance of This Computer is below average in chance of
overheating overheating
This Computer has no built in speaker system This Computer has a built in speaker system
This Computer does not come with a warranty This Computer comes with a warranty
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      56
 

This Computer is smaller and lighter than


This Computer is larger and heavier than average
average
This Computer has below average memory This Computer has above average memory
This Computer has lower than average battery This Computer has higher than average
life battery life
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      57
 

Appendix B

Consent Form Given to Participants Prior to Experiment

Human Subjects Consent Form


Making Decisions When Relevant Information is Available
Alexander Hough
Shippensburg University
Department of Psychology

During this experiment, you will be provided with information about hypothetical objects
and will be asked to choose which hypothetical object you believe is the best choice. You will
also be asked to complete a demographics form and a questionnaire concerning your attitudes
towards making choices. Overall, this experiment will take approximately one hour.

Participant’s rights
I understand that this study may contribute to the understanding of how people make
decisions when information is available to them. I understand that there are no risks involved in
this study that differ from everyday decision processes. I understand that my responses will be
kept confidential along with my personal information. This consent form will be kept separate
from the collected data. Names will not appear in the final report; participants will be labeled by
number. I understand that I may skip any questions that I do not feel comfortable answering and
may decide to withdraw from this study at any time. After completion, I will be a given a full
explanation of the purposes of this study. If any part of this study makes me uncomfortable, I
may contact Dr. Lea Adams by telephone (717) 477-1115 or by email at ltadam@ship.edu, or I
may contact Dr. Jennifer Clements by telephone (717) 477-1633 or by email at
jaclem@ship.edu.

Consent to participate
I acknowledge that I am at least eighteen years of age and understand my rights as a
research participant as described above. I also acknowledge that my participation in this
study is completely voluntary.

Print name:______________________________

Signature:_______________________________

Date:___________________________________
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      58
 

Appendix C

Choice Sheet for Entire Experiment

Choice Sheet
First Set (Cars)

1. First Trial (Four pieces of information)

Car A Car B Car C Car D

2. Second Trial (Twelve pieces of information)

Car A Car B Car C Car D

Second Set (Jobs)

1. First Trial (Four pieces of information)

Job A Job B Job C Job D

2. Second Trial (Twelve pieces of information)

Job A Job B Job C Job D

Third Set (Apartments)

1. First Trial (Four pieces of information)

Apartment A Apartment B Apartment C Apartment D

2. Second Trial (Twelve pieces of information)

Apartment A Apartment B Apartment C Apartment D

Fourth Set (Computers)

1. First Trial (Four pieces of information)

Computer A Computer B Computer C Computer D

2. Second Trial (Twelve pieces of information)

Computer A Computer B Computer C Computer D


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      59
 

Appendix D
Rating Sheet used during Simple Trials for the Feeling Focused Condition

First trial set


Please indicate your feelings about the current option by circling a number after each
attribute. 1 (very negative) 2 (negative) 3 (somewhat negative) 4 (neutral) 5 (somewhat positive)
6 (positive) 7 (very positive)
Car A
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Car B
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Car C
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Car D
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      60
 

Rating Sheet used during Complex Trials for the Detail Focused Condition

First trial set


Please indicate how well you are remembering the current option by circling a number after
each attribute. 1 (not at all) 2 (not much) 3 (very little) 4 (neutral) 5 (somewhat) 6 (pretty well) 7
(very well).
Car A
5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
13. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
14. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
15. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
16. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Car B
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      61
 

Car C
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Car D
1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      62
 

Appendix E
Note Sheet used for Note Taking Conditions

A B C D
Trial 1

Trial 2

Trial 3

Trial 4

Trial 5

Trial 6
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      63
 

Appendix F
Demographic Questionnaire

Demographics

Age: ____________________

Gender: Male Female

Major: ____________________

Current GPA: ____________________

Class Standing: Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Graduate Student


AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      64
 

Appendix G

The Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI-revised; Pacini & Epstein, 1999)

The items are rated using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (definitely not true of myself) to

5 (definitely true of myself).

Rationality scale (Modified Need for Cognition Scale)


I try to avoid situations that require thinking in depth about something. (re‒)
I’m not that good at figuring out complicated problems. (ra‒)
I enjoy intellectual challenges. (re)
I am not very good at solving problems that require careful logical analysis. (ra‒)
I don’t like to have to do a lot of thinking. (re‒)
I enjoy solving problems that require hard thinking. (re)
Thinking is not my idea of an enjoyable activity. (re‒)
I am not a very analytical thinker. (ra‒)
Reasoning things out carefully is not one of my strong points. (ra‒)
I prefer complex problems to simple problems. (re)
Thinking hard and for a long time about something gives me little satisfaction. (re‒)
I don’t reason well under pressure. (ra‒)
I am much better at figuring things out logically than most people. (ra) I have a logical mind. (ra)
I enjoy thinking in abstract term. (re)
I have no problem thinking things through carefully. (ra)
Using logic usually works well for me in figuring out problems in my life. (ra)
Knowing the answer without having to understand the reasoning behind it is good enough for
me. (re‒)
I usually have clear, explainable reasons for my decisions. (ra)
Learning new ways to think would be very appealing to me. (re)

Experientiality scale (Modified Faith in Intuition Scale)


I like to rely on my intuitive impressions. (ee)
I don’t have a very good sense of intuition. (ea‒)
Using my gut feelings usually works well for me in figuring out problems in my life. (ea)
I believe in trusting my hunches. (ea)
Intuition can be a very useful way to solve problems. (ee)
I often go by my instincts when deciding on a course of action. (ee)
I trust my initial feelings about people. (ea)
When it comes to trusting people, I can usually rely on my gut feelings. (ea)
If I were to rely on my gut feelings, I would often make mistakes. (ea‒)
I don’t like situations in which I have to rely on intuition. (ee‒)
I think there are times when one should rely on one’s intuition. (ee)
I think it is foolish to make important decisions based on feelings. (ee‒)
I don’t think it is a good idea to rely on one’s intuition for important decisions. (ee‒)
I generally don’t depend on my feelings to help me make decisions. (ee‒)
I hardly ever go wrong when I listen to my deepest gut feelings to find an answer. (ea)
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      65
 

I would not want to depend on anyone who described himself or herself as intuitive. (ee‒)
My snap judgments are probably not as good as most people’s. (ea‒)
I tend to use my heart as a guide for my actions. (ee)
I can usually feel when a person is right or wrong, even if I can’t explain how I know. (ea)
I suspect my hunches are inaccurate as often as they are accurate. (ea‒)

Note . The name of the subscale to which each item belongs appears in parentheses, ee =
Experiential Engagement; ea = Experiential Ability; re = Rational Engagement; ra = Rational
Ability. A minus sign (—) with a scale name denotes reverse scoring.
AFFECT, CRITICAL THINKING, AND DECISIONS                                                                                                                      66
 

Appendix H

Experimental Debriefing

Thank you for participating in my study. I know it may have been a little mentally
exhausting. In this debriefing, I will tell you a little bit about my study, and then I will briefly
discuss the field of decision making and why it is important.
We are required to make decisions every day, whether we decide what to eat for breakfast
or what to wear. When it comes to making choices, there are two ways of thinking that help us
make decisions. These ways of thinking are usually called system one and system two. For
instance, an individual could use their feelings and intuitions to make a decision (i.e., system
one) or an individual could use critical thinking or analytical thought, which requires effortful
thinking (i.e., system two). The main goal of this experiment is to determine whether
feelings/intuitions or critical thinking/analytical thought can help an individual remember
information and use it to make a decision. A second goal of this experiment is to determine
which type of thinking can lead to better decisions. By studying these types of issues within the
field of decision making, we could help individuals make better decisions. This is particularly
useful for large corporations, the military, recovering drug addicts, college students, etc.
Most studies in decision making have demonstrated that individuals tend to make higher
quality decisions when they engage in critical thinking and consider all possible alternatives.
Typically, people make lower quality decisions when they use their feelings or intuitions to make
a decision, unless they are an expert with the subject matter (e.g., a chess expert). There are times
when a non-expert’s feelings or intuitions can lead to decisions better than chance (e.g., 50/50),
but this is only found when there is no relevant information, or when individuals have no
knowledge about the subject matter. It is common for individuals to make decisions based on
feelings and intuitions, even when it would be more beneficial to engage in more effortful
thinking. People tend to do this because they do not like making decisions, and they perceive
critical thinking to be time consuming and aversive. Although most decisions are not too
important, remember that when you face an important decision, you are more likely to make a
good decision if you think hard about it and consider all the possible outcomes before deciding
which option to take.

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться