Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Hand Therapy


journal homepage: www.jhandtherapy.org

Social participation in persons with upper limb amputation


receiving an esthetic prosthesis
Freyja Kristjansdottir OT BSc a, *, Lars B. Dahlin MD a, b, Hans-Eric Rosberg MD, PhD a, b,
Ingela K. Carlsson OT PhD a, b
a
Department of Hand Surgery, Skåne University Hospital, Malmö, Sweden
b
Department of Translational Medicine e Hand Surgery, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Study Design: Qualitative study.


Received 14 March 2018 Introduction: An amputation injury to the hand may lead to not only impaired hand function but also
Received in revised form psychosocial consequences.
18 March 2019
Purpose of the Study: The purpose of the study was to explore personal experiences of social participation
Accepted 20 March 2019
Available online xxx
for persons provided with an esthetic prosthesis after acquired upper limb amputation.
Methods: Thirteen persons with acquired upper limb amputation, who were in need of and had received
an esthetic prosthesis, were interviewed. The transcribed text was subjected to content analysis.
Keywords:
Participation
Results: The emotional reactions to a visibly different hand were linked to a changed appearance and a
Visible difference feeling of being exposed. Recollecting the accident could result in nightmares and sleeping disorders. A
Upper limb amputation change of personality, due to sadness after the amputation was expressed, as well as social insecurity and
Adaptation impact on relations and life roles. Adapting to social challenges comprised hiding or exposing the hand,
Prosthesis using personal internal resources and receiving support from others. The esthetic prosthesis contributed
Social participation to an intact appearance and could serve as a facilitator for initial or long-term social participation. The
time that had passed since the injury made it easier to deal with the consequences or in achieving
acceptance.
Discussion: Coping with emotions and social relations after an acquired amputation can be difficult and
complex.
Conclusions: Individual needs must be considered and questions about appearance and how it may affect
social participation must be asked. An esthetic prosthesis can normalize the appearance and offer the
confidence needed to facilitate social participation in those struggling with appearance-related concerns.
Ó 2019 Hanley & Belfus, an imprint of Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction the disfigurement.6 Adjusting to an acquired amputation, or other


visible differences, is a complex process involving individual vari-
Yearly in Sweden, approximately 4 persons per 100,000 in- ations.7-10 The challenges the patients face includes not only im-
habitants suffer an upper limb amputation.1 Devastation and pairments in body function and activity limitations but also
distress have been described as initial reactions after an upper limb participation restrictions.3,11,12 The International Classification of
amputation.2 Significantly higher levels of anxiety and depression Functioning, Disability and Health defines participation as a per-
compared to a general population as well as esthetic concerns have son’s involvement in life situations.13 Participation includes societal
also been reported.2-5 The psychological impact of a visible disfig- involvement, but a clear definition of the concept is lacking,
urement, or visible difference, may depend more on the self- particularly the distinction between activity and participation.14,15
perceived appearance than on an objectively assessed extent of The additional concept “social participation” has been suggested
as being a more appropriate term to emphasize both the societal
involvement and the subjective experience of participation.15
This study was supported by grants from the Healthcare Academy at Skane Uni- Prostheses in general facilitate a person’s activity, well-being,
versity Hospital. and social interaction.3,5,11,16 An esthetic prostheses, with its life-
* Corresponding author. Department of Hand Surgery, Skåne University Hospital, like appearance, provide the wearer with the opportunity to look
Malmö, Sweden Jan Waldenströms gata 5, S-205 02 Malmö, Sweden. Tel.: þ46 40
33 26 89; fax: þ46 40 33 70 65.
outwardly intact. That, in turn, can facilitate adjustment by mini-
E-mail address: freyja.kristjansdottir@skane.se (F. Kristjansdottir). mizing both the difference between the current and former

0894-1130/$ e see front matter Ó 2019 Hanley & Belfus, an imprint of Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jht.2019.03.010
2 F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7

appearances and the sense of being different from others.5 Patients dream job, but is now working in a different field. The Modified
with upper limb amputations, who desired and were in need of Hand Injury Severity Score, with its 4 levels ranging from minor to
esthetic prostheses due to appearance-related concerns, have been major, was used to classify the severity of the injuries.18,19 Table 1
provided with such an aid since the late 1990s at our clinic. Vari- shows the characteristics of the participants.
ations in how frequent, for how long, and in which situations the
prostheses is used may, however, occur. In what way social
participation is affected for those provided with an esthetic pros- Procedure and ethics
thesis is sparsely described previously. A deepened knowledge
about experiences arising in social situations and the influence an The interview guide was drawn up by the first author and fourth
esthetic prosthesis may have are, therefore, of interest to further author (F.K. and I.K.C.) based on earlier literature on participation
develop therapeutic approaches in accordance with patients’ in- and was also discussed with experienced occupational therapists
dividual needs. and physiotherapists working with hand rehabilitation.13,15,20-22
The interviews started with the participant being introduced to
Purpose of the study the aim of the study, confidentiality was guaranteed, and the
meaning of the social participation concept was explained. To
The aim of the present study was to explore personal experi- reveal the area of social participation, the participants were asked
ences of social participation for persons provided with an esthetic to describe important social situations and the impact the ampu-
prosthesis after acquired upper limb amputation. tation may have had. Furthermore, questions concerning how the
participants dealt with social challenges and the possible influence
Methods of the esthetic prosthesis on social participation were raised.
Follow-up questions were asked to deepen the content of the an-
Design and participants swers, for example: Which emotions were evoked? What were
your thoughts? How did you handle that? Initially, a pilot interview
The study design is qualitative with an inductive approach to was carried out, which was later included in the study. The pilot
capture the participants’ own experiences of how the prosthesis interview did not generate any changes in the interview guide.
has influenced their social participation.17 The inclusion criteria First, 10 participants were contacted by telephone to arrange a
were persons with acquired upper limb amputations and who, for time and place for the interview. As new information was revealed
various reasons, were in need of and had received their first during the last 3 of these 10 interviews, an additional 3 people were
esthetic prostheses within a reasonable time span, estimated to be contacted and interviewed. These interviews added no new infor-
one to 5 years before the interview. The time span was set on the mation indicating that data saturation had been reached.23
basis that the person should be able to remember what it was like The interviews were carried out by the first author (F.K.) over a
before they received the esthetic prosthesis and could, therefore, 10-month period and were held at a place of the participant’s choice,
share their experiences both with and without the prosthesis. The mostly in a quiet room at the clinic. One interview was conducted by
exclusion criteria were the presence of mental, cognitive, linguistic telephone due to long distance. The interviews were audio-recorded
impairment or any other disorders that could overshadow the and transcribed verbatim by the first author (F.K.) after the
experience of the phenomena in focus or the ability to participate in completion of each interview. Nonverbal expressions were noted.
an interview. Fifty-one people received their first esthetic pros- The interviews lasted between 17 and 74 min (median 40 min).
thesis from the Hand Surgery Department, Skane University Hos- Before the interview, the participants completed the acute
pital, in the years 2012 to 2015 and were thus eligible for inclusion. version of the Short Form 36 (SF-36) questionnaire to provide
Fifteen were excluded: 6 had insufficient knowledge of the Swedish further information about their health status. SF-36 is a self-
language, 4 suffered from a current psychiatric disorder, 3 had reported questionnaire comprising aspects of physical and
congenital absence, and 2 could not be reached due to the lack of emotional roles as well as social functioning.24 (Table 2) Funda-
contact details, leaving 36 potential participants, who were cate- mental ethical considerations, based on the Helsinki Declaration,
gorized by age, gender, and level of amputation to create a pur- were taken into account, and approval of the study was obtained
posive sample. In total, 18 were selected, representing the different from the local ethics committee, Lund University, Sweden (Dnr
categories mentioned previously. A letter with information about 2016/550).
the purpose and voluntary nature of the study, and with an invi-
tation for their participation, was sent continuously as the study
proceeded. Two refused to participate through lack of interest, 2 Table 1
stated that their working conditions were an obstacle, and one Characteristics of the participants
thought it was too upsetting to talk about the amputation, leaving a Participant Age Gender MHISS score Current prosthesis usage:
total of 13 persons who ended up taking part in the study. daily/occasionally/never
The group had a median age of 52 years (range 23-74 years) and 1 51-60 M Major Occasionally
had suffered a variety of amputations from partial finger or thumb 2 71-80 M Major Occasionally
amputation to total amputation of all fingers. All were married or 3 61-70 M Major Occasionally
4 21-30 M Major Occasionally
lived with a partner. Six had injured their dominant hand, 5 the 5 71-80 F Moderate Occasionally
nondominant hand, and 2 had bilateral injuries. The median time 6 61-70 F Moderatea Occasionally
since the amputation was 3 years (range 2-7 years), and the median 7 21-30 F Severe Occasionally
time since receiving the first esthetic prosthesis was 3 years (range 8 31-40 F Major Daily
9 21-30 F Major Daily
1-5 years). The participants represented different occupational
10 41-50 M Severe Occasionally
fields: student (n ¼ 1), wage earner (n ¼ 9), and retiree (n ¼ 3). The 11 51-60 F Major Occasionally
types of work were light office work, lighter manual work, and 12 51-60 F Major Never
heavy manual work. All but one of those working returned to their 13 21-30 M Moderate Never
jobs after the amputation injury; the one who did not was forced to MHISS ¼ Modified Hand Injury Severity Score.
a
change professional plans because of the amputation, giving up a Amputation cause: disease. Estimated MHISS score.
F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7 3

Table 2
Health-related quality of life (SF-36) in persons with an amputation to the hand (n ¼ 13) and in a general Swedish population (n ¼ 8930)

SF-36 scalea Participants, median (range) Participants, mean (25%-75%) General Swedish population, mean (25%-75%)
Physical functioning (PF) 85 (50-100) 83 [75-90] 88 [85-100]
Role physical (RP)b 100 (0-100) 75 [75-100] 83 [75-100]
Bodily pain (BP) 70 (43-100) 74 [57,5-90] 75 [52-100]
General health (GH) 80 (15-100) 72 [62,5-95] 76 [62,5-92]
Vitality (VT) 63 (25-100) 65 [45-90] 69 [55-85]
Social functioning (SF) 75 (13-100) 75 [50-100] 89 [87,5-100]
Role emotional (RE)b 67 (33,5-100) 69 [33,5-100] 86 [100-100]
Mental health (MH) 78 (28-100) 73 [56-92] 81 [72-96]

SF-36 ¼ Short Form 36.


a
Higher score indicates better health state.
b
Subscales measuring participation.25

Analyses Appearance-related concerns


Feelings of dislike, of being abnormal and ugly, truncated,
The text was analyzed using conventional content analysis.26,27 alienated, or even feeling disgusted with the hand were emotional
The first author and fourth author (F.K. and I.K.C.) read and reread reactions linked to appearance-related concerns. This could result
the transcripts independently to obtain an overall understanding of in only looking at the hand when necessary, for instance, when
the content. The next step was to find meaning units by identifying cutting the nails. Feeling truncated was emotionally hard and made
words or sentences related to the aim of the study. The meaning it difficult with self-confidence. Alienation toward the changed
units were then coded, carefully retaining their meaning. The hand was also experienced and could be expressed as in the
codes, in turn, were compared and discussed, and similarities and following: “I think it’s disgusting because I haven’t accepted the hand
differences were identified, leading to the emergence of sub- yet. So when I see it in the mirror I get such a shock, I don’t get that it is
categories. Those subcategories that shared commonalities were my hand, I think it is someone else’s.” Ambivalent emotions, such as
finally abstracted into main categories (Table 3). Each category was thinking it is all right to show the hand but still preferring to put it
critically analyzed and compared with each other until a reasonable in a pocket, existed. At the same time they both wanted and did not
interpretation was reached. The process so far was performed by want to tell people about the situation. “You don’t want people to see
the first author and fourth author (F.K. and I.K.C.). The categories the thumb because it’s not my thumb, I still feel that. But at the same
were thereafter discussed, processed, and adjusted with the help of time it is who I am.”
the second author (L.B.D.) who read 3 interviews of dissimilar
character and the third author (H.E.R.) who read all 13 interviews, A feeling of exposure
to ensure that all aspects present in the text were covered. Con- The mental strain of having the hand constantly exposed, or at
cerning the authors’ preunderstanding, the first author and fourth risk of being exposed, was experienced in several situations: when
author (F.K. and I.K.C.) are occupational therapists with long shaking hands, eating, drinking, or standing in front of a group
experience of hand therapy, and the second author and third author presenting something. A general feeling that everybody was look-
(L.B.D. and H.E.R.) are experienced hand surgeons. Some of the ing at the hand and worries about how others might react to the
participants had previously been treated by the interviewer, and all hand raised many questions: What would people think, or say?
were aware of or were informed of the interviewers’ clinical Would they associate them only with the amputated hand? Will
experience. All authors are educated or experienced in qualitative they consider it unpleasant or disgusting? Are people going to talk
research and work in a specialized unit. about them behind their backs? There were feelings of shame
about its appearance when the hand was exposed. Further stress-
related emotions such as nervousness and anguish were experi-
Results enced when the hand was exposed, or at risk of being exposed. This
could give rise to physical symptoms, such as tingling and a feeling
The results are presented as 3 main categories: emotional re- of heatstroke.
actions to a visibly different hand, consequences for social inter-
action, and adapting to social challenges (Table 4). Recollecting the accident
People often asked questions about the injury, which resulted in
memories and flashbacks of the accident. These memories were
Emotional reactions to a visibly different hand also experienced at night, resulting in nightmares and sleeping
disorders that could occur even many years after the amputation.
Dissatisfaction with the appearance and other people’s atten- Some needed psychological support to overcome their difficulties.
tion to the visible difference was emotionally stressful. Memories of Even if they did not need psychological support, feelings of sadness
the accident could be painful, sometimes requiring professional about what had occurred were engendered when people noticed
treatment. The visibly different hand was also experienced as and commented on the different appearance of the hand. Just the
affecting personality. fact that they saw the hand all the time could serve as a daily

Table 3
Example of meaning units, codes, subcategories, and categories

Meaning unit Code Subcategory Category


“Lots of people are watching, it’s kind of unpleasant.” Discomfort A feeling of exposure Emotional reactions to a visibly different hand
“With the prosthesis I don’t stand out, nobody is looking at me.” Blend in with others Utilizing esthetic prosthesis Adapting to social challenges
4 F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7

Table 4 have become a lone wolf and I have no friends anymore.” There were,
Main categories and subcategories however, examples of friendships and other relations being
Main categories Subcategories enhanced; for example, a formerly complicated relationship with a
Emotional reactions to a visibly Appearance-related concerns parent matured and deepened after the accident. Some participants
different hand A feeling of exposure experienced not being invited to events or asked to go out, friends
Recollecting the accident distancing themselves as if they did not know how to treat them
A feeling of a changed personality
after the amputation, or as for acquaintances choosing to go down a
Consequences for social interaction Social insecurity
Impact on relations different aisle in the grocery store to avoid a meeting. The ampu-
Adapting to social challenges Hiding or exposing the hand tation also affected the families to varying extents, for instance, the
Using personal internal resources children were sad and worried or a family member suffered from a
Receiving support from others depression because of the injury. Interaction between family
Utilizing esthetic prosthesis
Acceptance
members was also affected, as when a participant found it difficult
to approach a child who needed help with buttons or zippers.
Although the child did not care, such a move meant that the hand,
which was always kept hidden, was exposed making both the vi-
reminder. Some got trapped in dwelling on the incident and
sual difference and the impaired function apparent. Fear of
blaming themselves for what had happened. One example of
touching a partner with the amputated hand, believing the partner
recollection at a later stage, after the esthetic prosthesis had been
thought it was ugly, was presented. The participants did not believe
received, was that handling the prosthesis, putting it on and taking
their partners when they stressed that they had no such thoughts.
it off, led to thoughts of the accident resurfacing. Although the
Although the partner did not care, there was a feeling of being
esthetic prosthesis fulfilled its purpose by improving the appear-
unattractive and not feminine or sexy. Others described an inability
ance of the hand, the mental strain of handling it led to it being
to feel relaxed in intimate situations; “You don’t feel as relaxed
abandoned.
because you feel that you are always . (pause) He doesn’t think about
it but I do, and it affects how you are a lot. So I have become more
A feeling of a changed personality
careful and it doesn’t feel as natural.”
Awareness that the amputation had changed their personality
was described. “I think I have changed as a person. I have become
Adapting to social challenges
more insecure, I think more because the injury affects my personality
so much.” There was a former leader who had withdrawn after the
Different adaptation strategies to overcome challenges in social
accident feeling that the energy was lacking, and a social type who
interaction were described. Support from others was appreciated
became a loner, unable to feel the same joy as before and not even
both from an emotional and practical aspect. The esthetic pros-
feeling like the same person anymore - “Friends, family and ac-
thesis was considered being a part of the adaptation process. The
quaintances they still see you as you, but you don’t see you as yourself
time that had passed since the injury made it easier to deal with the
anymore.” Being constantly reminded of the injury in their daily life
consequences or in achieving acceptance.
evoked emotions such as sadness and bitterness, described as
affecting their personality. A sense of being less worthy because Hiding or exposing the hand
they were unable to use both hands or carry out ordinary tasks was
Strategies used to conceal the hand included hiding it by putting
described. it in a pocket, hiding it under the other hand, holding the fingers at
a favorable angle, or using various kinds of coverings. The initial
Consequences for social interaction phase, when it was still covered with dressings, was sometimes
experienced as easier since the hand was not exposed. This could
Social participation was restricted by insecurity experienced in lead to the dressings being used longer than was necessary.
company of others. Friendship, family relations, and intimate sit- Another strategy was to constantly keep the hand in a fist position
uations with a partner were affected to a varying extent. to hide it from oneself and thus avoid upsetting emotions. “I hide
the hand. I hide the hand all the time - from myself.” Although many
Social insecurity years had passed, and participants were used to, and felt
Social insecurity could be experienced in both small and large comfortable about, showing the hand, they chose to keep it in their
groups and was often connected with exposure or the fear of being pocket. In contrast, another approach was exposing the hand and
exposed. There was a constant worry about whether or not some- being open about its condition, sometimes by consciously seeking
one had discovered the injury together with a fear of being seen as attention. It was described as healing to talk about it and it was
incomplete. A fear of being belittled by others when the amputa- even used consciously as a topic of conversation to focus attention
tion was discovered was also expressed, that people would see you or as a warning example to others. To tell people about the
as being of less worth. Another example of social insecurity was the amputation could also be experienced as easier than waiting for it
inability of being relaxed among others which could be intensified to be discovered. Instead of the distress of not knowing what other
by others believing that after a certain time you should have learnt people were thinking when they saw the amputated hand, they
to live with it. Yet another example was a feeling of being pitied; chose to tell them about it and how it had happened.
that is, that friends invited you or came over for dinner because
they felt sorry for you not because they wanted to see you. Using personal internal resources
Various personal resources were identified, which influenced
Impact on relations the approach the participants used and their ability to adapt.
The amputation could have an impact on relations, thereby Determination not to let the amputation affect their lives, not
affecting life roles as a friend, parent, or spouse. One response to the allowing themselves to dwell on the situation, and demanding that
social insecurity described previously was to withdraw or to others treat them normally were mentioned. An ability to plan
decline invitations and stay at home. This withdrawal behavior ahead how to solve certain practical tasks directly after the
could lead to friends finally giving up on them. “After the injury I amputation while still in hospital was also described. Both humor
F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7 5

and an underlying optimism influenced their approach. “I play golf, Although some participants became almost addicted to their
so I have a double handicap” a participant joked, holding up the prosthesis, others used theirs as an accessory simply to enhance
hand that had no fingers. The ability to joke about the situation was their appearance in certain social situations, perhaps not only when
described as healing. Stubbornness could be triggered when others meeting unknown people but also when together with people they
doubted their capacity to perform certain activities. “People are knew well. When going to a party or celebration, with people who
saying you can’t do this or that. Well, then instead I become kind of: I were used to see them without the prosthesis, they nevertheless
will show them I can.” put it on to look extra good and to improve their appearance. “I
would have managed without, but at the same time I like to put it on if
Receiving support from others I’m going out. It is somehow a part of me.” It was compared to putting
Old friends turned up and showed their support after hearing on make-up, spending time choosing the right shoes to suit their
about the accident. Family, friends, and neighbors offered much outfit, or putting on an extra necklace or ring. Actually, they could
appreciated emotional and practical support and provided a safe have gone without the prosthesis, but they chose not to because
environment where one did not have to explain what had they felt nicer with it on. “I think I feel a little, you can call it vanity if
happened because everyone already knew. Although the support you want, you feel a little more . nice, or what to say. It’s kind of, yes,
given was appreciated, it could result in a sense of lost indepen- like an accessory.” After initially having been worn more frequently
dence, for instance, in the initial phase when one had to have one’s this is how the prosthesis could eventually end up being used.
food cut up by one’s partner.
Acceptance
Utilizing esthetic prosthesis Achieving acceptance could be linked to the time that had passed
The esthetic prosthesis itself can be seen as a part of the adap- since the injury occurred as it became easier to deal with the conse-
tation process. Among the expectations regarding the prosthesis, quences over time. The participants and those around them got used
the most common was a wish to improve or normalize one’s to their new appearance, which was described as helping them to
appearance. The prosthesis actually met this expectation by making achieve acceptance. However, there could also be a kind of determi-
the hand appear intact; while worn, it concealed the injury. The nation in an early phase that the situation must be accepted. A change
person wearing it looked like everyone else and blended in with over time was noted even among those who had not accepted the
others. As one participant put it: “I would not want to be without it, situation, and although they continued to have major difficulties, it
I’m really grateful for it. You know that it is there, you are safe. If you became easier over the years to handle the consequences.
want to be normal you put it on.” Wearing the prosthesis offered a
relief from people staring at the hand and asking questions. This Discussion
applied particularly to those who constantly met new people, for
instance, in their jobs. They had experienced people being very The emotional reactions to a visibly different hand were linked
curious, almost forcing them to talk about the injury and the ac- to a changed appearance and a feeling of being exposed. Recol-
cident, and wearing the prosthesis allowed them to avoid this. The lecting the accident could result in nightmares and sleeping dis-
normalizing effect also applied not only to their own perception of orders. A change of personality, due to insecurity, sadness, and a
the hand but also to that of the people around them. The prosthesis sense of being less worthy after the amputation, was also
made them feel as they did before the accident, and they forgot for expressed. The consequences for social interaction included social
a while that they had undergone an amputation. They felt whole insecurity and an impact on relations and life roles. Adapting to
and could even wonder if the amputation had really happened. social challenges comprised hiding or exposing the hand, using
The esthetic prosthesis offered social security and enabled social personal internal resources, and receiving support from others. For
interaction with others, privately and at work. It could be vital for a those who were dissatisfied with their appearance or had not come
return to work as it provided the power and security needed to be to terms with the disfigurement, the esthetic prosthesis was seen as
able to meet people. “It was only when I got the prosthesis that I felt normalizing and offering social security. For some the esthetic
safe.” Another description was that it made it possible to relax, for prosthesis was crucial for social participation. It could also be a tool
instance, at a party. Even among those who did not use their in the emotional process, functioning as a bridge between the
prosthesis daily, there were times when they felt extra vulnerable former and the current appearance.
and when the prosthesis instilled a sense of confidence. The The SF-36 survey instrument was used to give additional in-
esthetic prosthesis could be indispensable and crucial in all social formation about the participants’ self-perceived health status,
contacts; they needed it even simply to be able to step outside the providing a background description of the group. Two subscales of
door. These participants were still suffering from their loss and the SF-36 have been categorized as measuring participation: role
were not able to accept their new appearance and its consequences. physical and role emotional.25 It is notable that the participants’
For them it was unthinkable to be without their esthetic prosthesis result on the role emotional subscale is low compared to that of a
in any social situation, or even with their partners. “It’s a wall to me general Swedish population. The mean value for the subscale social
sometimes, you want to take it off, but you can’t. It’s like the end of the functioning, which measures role limitations due to emotional
world. I’m nervous and worried he will realize it is awful.” problems, is also lower than normal.24 This reinforces the view that
Sometimes the prosthesis was used only for a transitory the amputation affected the participants’ social participation.
period, and after a couple of years, it was used much less or not at The appearance-related concerns, for instance, feeling abnormal
all, as the person got used to their new appearance and those and ugly or being hypervigilant regarding the reactions of others,
around them no longer paid any attention. “So I was at a party, it can be likened to the concept of body image. Body image consti-
was a company party, and I did not wear . everybody knows . tutes the feelings and attitudes a person has toward their own body
Nobody looks at me, nobody notices it and everybody knows me and has been defined as “the combination of an individual’s psy-
without fingers, well so that is no problem!” Some realized, after chological experiences, feelings, and attitudes that relate to the
having used the esthetic prosthesis for a while, that they managed form, function, appearance, and desirability of one’s own body
better without it. Having been able to try an artificial limb, and which is influenced by individual and environmental factors.”28 In
realizing its limitations, may have helped them to adapt to the line with previous studies, our findings highlight the complexity
new situation. surrounding body image.11 There were participants who had
6 F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7

suffered a major amputation injury and yet signalized satisfaction light of the participants being a group who, for some reason, had
with both appearance and function, as well as the opposite; minor received an esthetic prosthesis.
amputations generate extensive psychological struggles.
The impact of the amputation on relations and life roles varied. Clinical implications
Loss of friends and changed interaction with a child or spouse were
given as examples. This reflects the results from a study on changes Providing esthetic prostheses is a time-consuming and costly
in life roles after hand injury which found alterations in the daily intervention and should preferably be given to them who need it
patterns and routines after injury and advocates a “top-down” the most. A deepened knowledge concerning social participation
approach to treatment.29 In a “top-down” approach, the person’s following amputation and the impact of an esthetic prosthesis may
roles, habits, and activities are identified and used as a starting contribute to targeted rehabilitative interventions for this group of
point for appropriate treatment interventions to minimize the patients. Its contribution in facilitating social participation can
impact on life roles.30 Motivating occupations and relationships justify the prosthesis being used only for a limited period of time.
that are important to the individual should be maintained and, Therapists, working with hand rehabilitation, may risk
when needed, facilitated in recovery after a hand injury.8 becoming accustomed to hands that look different, where only the
Various strategies were used to cope with the situation and its extremes stand out. The consequences for individuals with minor
possible stigmatizing consequences.31 One of the major benefits of amputations are thus at risk of being underestimated or overlooked
the esthetic prosthesis, described in this study, was that it offered by the therapist. There is also a risk of the person never being
the possibility of concealing the visible difference. This can be referred to the rehabilitation unit by the doctor for the same reason.
related to the term “passing,” meaning that the individual, by Individual circumstances and needs must always be taken into
concealing their condition, becomes a part of the “normal” group.32 account and questions about the appearance and how it may affect
Although concealment may enable social participation, it might social participation must be asked.
also exacerbate the underlying psychological distress.33 Thus, the
esthetic prosthesis can be a facilitator for social interaction and, at Conclusion
the same time, perpetuate unsolved psychological struggles. This
raises the question of whether some would have benefited more In conclusion, the present findings show the complexity of
from appropriate psychological support than from receiving an handling emotions and social relations after an acquired amputa-
esthetic prosthesis or at least from the 2 in parallel. Over time, tion. In those struggling with appearance-related concerns, an
desensitization to the reactions of others can occur, but to reach esthetic prosthesis can play a normalizing role and offer social se-
that level of adaptation, the person must be exposed to others curity which facilitate social participation during the adaptation
rather than preserving their negative feelings by isolating them- process or in a long term.
selves and hiding the injury.5,34 Some said that once everyone had
found out about their situation, they no longer used their pros-
Acknowledgments
thesis. People no longer paid any attention to the amputation,
indicating that a certain amount of exposure could facilitate
The authors thank the participants for sharing their experiences.
adaptation. It has been proposed that those who are forced to
expose their difference (e.g., facial burn scars) and confront the
References
reactions of others learn to adapt to their altered appearance.34
1. Asplund M, Nilsson M, Jacobsson A, von Holst H. Incidence of traumatic pe-
Methodological considerations ripheral nerve injuries and amputations in Sweden between 1998 and 2006.
Neuroepidemiology. 2009;32:217e228.
2. McKechnie PS, John A. Anxiety and depression following traumatic limb
The various aspects of trustworthiness were taken into account amputation: a systematic review. Injury. 2014;45:1859e1866.
in the study. Credibility was addressed by using purposive sampling 3. Murray CD. The social meanings of prosthesis use. J Health Psychol. 2005;10:425e441.
to create variation in age, gender, and level of amputation. To 4. Murray CD. Being like everybody else: the personal meaning of being av
prosthesis user. Disabil Rehabil. 2009;31:573e581.
strengthen dependability, the interviews were read independently
5. Saradjian A, Thompson AR, Datta D. The experience of men using an upper limb
by the authors and thereafter coded and discussed in depth, so- prosthesis following amputation: positive coping and minimizing feeling
called analyst triangulation.35,36 The authors, through their different. Disabil Rehabil. 2008;30:871e883.
respective professional backgrounds, were also able to contribute 6. Harris D. Types, causes and physical treatments of visible differences. In:
Lansdown R, Rumsey N, Bradbury E, Carr T, Partridge J, eds. Visibly Different:
different views. A constant focus on the text throughout the whole Coping with Disfigurement. Oxford; Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann; 1997.
process of analysis limited the risk of predetermined interpretation. 7. Gallagher P, MacLachlan M. Adjustment to an artificial limb: a qualitative
This is important since some of the participants had been treated by perspective. J Health Psychol. 2001;6:85e100.
8. Chan J, Spencer J. Adaptation to hand injury: an evolving experience. Am J
the interviewer which may have influenced the interview situation. Occup Ther. 2004;58:128e139.
The interplay between the interviewer and the person being 9. Bates E, Mason R. Coping strategies used by people with a major hand injury: a
interviewed may affect the responses. In this case, it may even have review of the literature. Brit J Occup Ther. 2014;77:289e295.
10. Carlsson IK, Chemnitz A, Dahlin LB. Hand injuries and disorders during the life
deepened the conversation and the narratives being shared. Ex- cycle; consequences, adaptation and therapeutic approach. In: Hayre CM,
amples of the abstraction and interpretation process are provided Muller D, eds. Enhancing Healthcare and Rehabilitation: The Impact of Qualitative
in Table 3 to demonstrate the interpretation of the text, and Research. New York: Taylor and Francis: CRC Press; 2019:167e184.
11. Wijk U, Carlsson I. Forearm amputees’ views of prosthesis use and sensory
representative quotations from the transcribed texts are given to feedback. J Hand Ther. 2015;28:269e277. quiz 278.
make the interpretation visible to the reader. Confirmability was 12. Gallagher P, O’Donovan MA, Doyle A, Desmond D. Environmental barriers,
ensured by continuously clarifying and confirming information activity limitations and participation restrictions experienced by people with
major limb amputations. Prosthet Orthot Int. 2011;35:278e284.
during the interviews. A possible limitation may be the time span of
13. World Health Organization. International Classification of Functioning, Disability
2 to 5 years after the amputation. These time frames were chosen to and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001.
capture a wide range of participants’ perspectives and experiences. 14. Badley E. Enhancing the conceptual clarity of the activity and participation
Whether or not the transferability of the results is applicable also to components of the international classification of functioning, disability and
health. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66:2335e2345.
other groups of hand injured patients having a visibly different 15. Piskur B, Daniels R, Jongmans MJ, et al. Participation and social participation:
hand is yet to be confirmed. The results should be considered in the are they distinct concepts? Clin Rehabil. 2014;28:211e220.
F. Kristjansdottir et al. / Journal of Hand Therapy xxx (2019) 1e7 7

16. Pillet J, Didierjean-Pillet A. Aesthetic hand prosthesis: gadget or therapy? 26. Graneheim UH, Lundman B. Qualitative content analysis in nursing research:
Presentation of a new classification. J Hand Surg Br. 2001;26:523e528. concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurs Educ
17. Berg B. Qualitative Research Methods for the Social Sciences. 8th ed. Upper Saddle Today. 2004;24:105e112.
River, NY: Pearson Education; 2012. 27. Hsieh HF, Shannon SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual
18. Urso-Baiarda F, Lyons RA, Laing JH, Brophy S, Wareham K, Camp D. Health Res. 2005;15:1277e1288.
A prospective evaluation of the Modified Hand Injury Severity Score in pre- 28. Rumsey N, Harcourt D. Body image and disfigurement: issues and in-
dicting return to work. Int J Surg. 2008;6:45e50. terventions. Body Image. 2004;1:83e97.
19. Campbell DA, Kay SP. The hand injury severity scoring system. J Hand Surg Br. 29. Schier JS, Chan J. Changes in life roles after hand injury. J Hand Ther. 2007;20:
1996;21:295e298. 57e68. quiz 69.
20. Jette AM, Haley SM, Kooyoomjian JT. Are the ICF activity and participation 30. Weinstock-Zlotnick G, Hinojosa J. Bottom-up or top-down evaluation: is one
dimensions distinct? J Rehabil Med. 2003;35:145e149. better than the other? Am J Occup Ther. 2004;58:594e599.
21. Law M. Participation in the occupations of everyday life. Am J Occup Ther. 31. Joachim G, Acorn S. Stigma of visible and invisible chronic conditions. J Adv
2002;56:640e649. Nurs. 2000;32:243e248.
22. Hammel J, Magasi S, Heinemann A, Whiteneck G, Bogner J, Rodriguez E. What 32. Goffman E. Stigma. Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. NJ: Prentice-
does participation mean? An insider perspective from people with disabilities. Hall, Englewood Cliffs; 1963.
Disabil Rehabil. 2008;30:1445e1460. 33. Thompson A, Kent G. Adjusting to disfigurement: processes involved in dealing
23. Saunders B, Sim J, Kingstone T, et al. Saturation in qualitative research: exploring with being visibly different. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001;21:663e682.
its conceptualization and operationalization. Qual Quant. 2018;52:1893e1907. 34. Cahners SS. Young women with breast burns: a self-help "group by mail. J Burn
24. Sullivan MKJ, Taft C. SF-36 Hälsoenkät: Svensk Manual Och Tolkningsguide, 2:a Care Rehabil. 1992;13:44e47.
Upplagan. Gothenburg: Sahlgrenska University Hospital; 2002. 35. Patton MC. Enhancing the Quality and Credibility of Qualitative Analysis. Health
25. Perenboom RJ, Chorus AM. Measuring participation according to the interna- Serv Res. 1999;34:1189e1208.
tional classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). Disabil Rehabil. 36. Carter N, Bryant-Lukosius D, DiCenso A, Blythe J, Neville AJ. The use of trian-
2003;25:577e587. gulation in qualitative research. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2014;41:545e547.

Вам также может понравиться