Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

Reading Research Quarterly

Vol. 32, No. 2


April/May/June 1997
©1997 International Reading Association
Judith Rosenhouse (pp. 168–183)

The Technion-Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel

Dina Feitelson
University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

Bracha Kita
A.D. Gordon Teachers Training College, Haifa, Israel

Zahava Goldstein
University of Haifa, Israel

Interactive reading aloud to Israeli


first graders: Its contribution to literacy
development

R
ecent literacy research claims that children who (Cochran-Smith, 1986; Heath, 1982); and (c) parents’ me-
do not grow up in a literate environment in diating between the text and the child (Cochran-Smith,
which they develop their literacy skills have less 1984, 1986; Teale & Sulzby, 1987), since stories typically
chance of success in reading acquisition and ex- use high language registers (Martin, 1983).
traction of meaning from texts than children who grow Studies of the benefits of parents’ reading stories
up in different conditions (Mason, 1990; Morrow, 1989b; to their children led to the transfer of this activity from
Purves & Elley, 1994; Strickland & Taylor, 1989). Apply- the home to formal educational institutions at the earliest
ing an ethnographers’ approach, literacy researchers ob- possible age (Morrow, 1989a; Strickland & Taylor, 1989).
served the homes of literate middle-class families to Reading stories aloud as a part of the teachers’ instruc-
study the ways in which parents help develop their chil- tional schedule was shown to contribute to the students’
dren’s literacy (Baghban, 1984; Heath, 1983; Teale, reading growth. It affected their vocabulary acquisition,
1986). Reading to children was a daily activity in these widened their world knowledge, deepened their reading
homes (Durkin, 1966; Taylor, 1983). comprehension, and changed their attitude toward
Such research identified important elements in the books and leisure reading (Elley, 1989; Graves, 1986;
interaction between parents and their children through Hoffman, Roser, & Battle, 1993). Students who enter
storybook reading, including (a) active roles for parents school with less exposure to the tools of literacy and
and children in which the event is a negotiation, with the who are more at risk relative to their literacy acquisition
parents asking, illustrating, and explaining and the child (Roser, Hoffman, & Farest, 1990) benefited most from
reacting and responding, and vice versa (Cochran-Smith, this reading activity, since one of the characteristics dis-
1986; Heath, 1982); (b) parents’ supporting the child, di- tinguishing them from middle-class students was little
recting and guiding him/her in the use of linguistic cues exposure to books or generally different leisure reading

168
ABSTRACTS

Interactive reading aloud to Israeli first graders: Its contribution to literacy development
THREE ISSUES were examined in this study: (a) how reading to a (worksheets, drawing, pasting, etc.). The teachers of the three ex-
first-grade class of children requiring enhancement (who may be perimental groups were asked to read interactively to their students,
identifiable as socially and culturally challenged) affects their that is, to interact with their students before, during, and after read-
achievement in decoding, reading comprehension, and storytelling; ing in order to help them to understand the story. They were to do
(b) how reading stories out of a series written by one author affects this during the last 20 minutes of the day, five times a week, for 6
their achievement as compared with reading isolated stories; and, (c) months. The findings indicated that classroom story-reading to first-
how reading different types of literature affects the amount of vol- grade students led them to increases in decoding, reading compre-
untary reading. Fifteen Israeli first-grade classes (339 participants) hension, and picture storytelling. Among the various types of treat-
were randomly divided into four treatment groups, three experi- ments, reading by teachers from a series of stories in installments
mental groups and one control group. The first group (four classes) was shown to have the greatest effect on reading achievement on
listened to stories (published in school readers) by different authors. the extent of reading for pleasure and on the quantity of books pur-
The second group (four classes) listened to stories by one author. chased for leisure reading. The findings suggested that exposure to a
The third group (three classes) listened in installments to a multi- series of stories initiates a process (a magic secret), which stimu-
ple-volume series of stories written by the same author as the second lates young readers to reread these books.
group. The control group (4 classes) engaged in regular activities

Lectura interactiva en voz alta a niños israelíes de primer grado: Sus contribuciones al desarrallo de
la alfabetización
EN ESTE estudio se examinaron tres cuestiones: (a) cómo la lectura control (cuatro clases) realizó las actividades regulares (hojas de tra-
a niños de primer grado que requieren apoyo (social y culturalmente bajo, dibujos, etc.) A las maestras de los tres grupos experimentales
en riesgo) afecta su desempeño en decodificación, comprensión lec- se les pidió que leyeran interactivamente a sus alumnos, es decir, que
tora y relato de historias, (b) cómo la lectura de historias tomadas de interactuaran con sus alumnos antes, durante, y después de la lec-
una serie escrita por un autor afecta su desempeño en comparación tura para ayudarlos a comprender la historia. Debían hacer esto los
con la lectura de historias aisladas, y (c) cómo la lectura de difer- últimos 20 minutos del día, cinco veces por semana, durante seis
entes tipos de literatura afecta la cantidad de lectura voluntaria. meses. Los hallazgos muestran que la lectura de historias en el aula
Quince clases israelíes de primer grado (339 participantes) fueron mejoró la decodificación, la comprensión lectora y el relato de histo-
divididas al azar en cuatro grupos de tratamiento: tres grupos exper- rias a partir de un dibujo en los niños de primer grado. Entre los difer-
imentales y un grupo de control. El primer grupo (cuatro clases) es- entes tipos de tratamiento, la lectura en etapas de una colección de
cuchó historias (de libros de lectura) de diferentes autores. El segun- historias tuvo el mayor efecto en el desempeño en lectura y en la can-
do grupo (cuatro clases) escuchó historias de un mismo autor. El tidad de lectura por placer y de libros comprados. Estos hallazgos
tercer grupo (tres clases) escuchó en etapas una colección de histo- sugieren que la exposición a una serie de historias inicia un proceso
rias escritas por el mismo autor que el segundo grupo. El grupo de (un secreto mágico) que estimula a los niños a releer esos libros.

Interaktives Vorlesen bei israelischen Erstklässlern: Beiträge zur Entwicklung der Literarisierung
DREI BEREICHE wurden in dieser Studie untersucht: (a) Wie das zweiten Gruppe. Die Kontrollgruppe (vier Klassen) beschäftigte sich
Vorlesen bei Kindern auf der ersten Schulstufe deren Leistungen im mit lehrplanmäßigen Aktivitäten (Arbeitsblätter, Zeichnungen,
Dekodieren, in der Sinnerfassung von Texten und im Erzählen von Knetmasse, etc.). Die Lehrer der drei Untersuchungsgruppen waren
Geschichten beeinflußt—was eine qualitative Leistungssteigerung er- beauftragt, interaktiv mit ihren Schülern zu arbeiten, d.h. vor,
forderlich macht und welche Kinder als sozial und kulturell Heraus- während und nach der Lektüre zu interagieren, als Hilfestellung zur
geforderte anzusehen sind. (b) Wie das Vorlesen von Serienge- Sinnerfassung der Geschichten. Sie mußten dies jeden Tag in den
schichten eines Autors die Leistungen der Schüler beeinflußt im letzten 20 Minuten des Unterichts tun, fünfmal pro Woche, sechs Mo-
Vergleich mit dem Lesen von in sich abgeschlossenen Geschichten; nate lang. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, daß das Vorlesen im Klassenzim-
(c) Wie das Vorlesen von verschiedenen Textgattungen den Anteil mer bei Erstklässlern zu einer Leistungssteigerung beim Dekodieren,
an selbständiger, freiwilliger Lektüre beeinflußt. 15 israelische erste bei der Texterfassung und bei der Erfassung von Bildgeschichten
Klassen (339 TeilnehmerInnen) wurden nach dem Zufallsprinzip in führte. Unter Anwendung von verschiedenen methodischen Formen
vier differenzierte Leistungsgruppen geteilt, und zwar in drei Unter- zeigte es sich, daß das Vorlesen von Fortsetzungsgeschichten den
suchungsgruppen und in eine Kontrollgruppe. Die erste Gruppe größten Einfluß auf die Leseleistung hatte, und zwar im Bereich des
(vier Klassen) bekam Geschichten verschiedener AutorInnen aus den unterhaltenden Lesens und in der Anzahl der Bücher, die als
approbierten Lesebüchern vorgelesen; die zweite Gruppe (vier Klas- Freizeitlektüre gekauft wurden. Die Ergebnisse legen den Schluß
sen) hörte Geschichten von einem Autor. Die dritte Gruppe (drei nahe, daß die Begegnung mit einer Serie von Geschichten eine
Klassen) hörte Fortsetzungsgeschichten einer umfangreichen Serie Entwicklung (ein magisches Geheimnis) einleitete, die junge
von Geschichten, geschrieben vom gleichen Autor wie die in der LeserInnen dazu anregt, diese Bücher nochmals selbst zu lesen.

169
ABSTRACTS

La lecture à haute voix interactive avec des enfants israéliens de 1° année: sa contribution au
développement de la lecture-écriture
ON A examiné trois questions dans cette recherche: (a) comment la habituelles (fiches de travail, dessin, collage, etc.). On a demandé
lecture à une classe de première année d’enfants ayant besoin de aux enseignants de faire une lecture interactive à leurs élèves, c’est-
soutien (identifiable comme un défi social et culturel) affecte leur à-dire d’interagir avec eux avant, pendant et après la lecture de façon
réussite en décodage, compréhension de lecture, et lecture d’his- à les aider à comprendre l’histoire. Ils devaient procéder ainsi
toires; (b) comment la lecture à haute voix d’une collection écrite par chaque jour pendant les 20 dernières minutes de classe, cinq fois par
un auteur affecte leur réussite par rapport à la lecture d’histoires sé- semaine et pendant six mois. Les résultats indiquent que la lecture
parées; et (c) comment la lecture de différents types d’écrits affecte d’histoires à des élèves de première année les conduit à des pro-
le volume de lecture personnelle. On a réparti au hasard quinze grès en décodage, compréhension de lecture, et lecture d’images.
classes de première année en quatre groupes de traitement, trois Parmi les différents types de traitement, on a pu montrer que la lec-
groupes expérimentaux et un groupe contrôle. Le premier groupe ture par les enseignants d’une collection d’histoires par épisodes a eu
(quatre classes) a écouté des histoires d’auteurs différents (publiées l’effet le plus massif sur la réussite en lecture, le volume de lectures
dans des manuels scolaires). Le second groupe (quatre classes) a pour le plaisir et la quantité de livres achetés pour la lecture de loisir.
écouté des histoires d’un seul auteur. Le troisième groupe (trois Les résultats suggèrent que l’exposition à une collection d’histoires
classes) a écouté des épisodes d’une collection d’histoires en induit un processus (un secret magique), qui stimule la relecture de
plusieurs volumes écrites par le même auteur comme le second ces livres par les jeunes lecteurs.
groupe. Le groupe contrôle (quatre classes) a effectué des activités

170
Interactive reading 171

habits (Feitelson, 1987; Feitelson & Goldstein, 1986; outscored the children in the control group (which in-
Mason, 1990; Morrow, 1988; Roser et al., 1990). cluded two classes) on measures of decoding, reading
It should be questioned, however, whether such comprehension, and active use of language.
intervention is too late, and whether it is possible to The advantage of reading series stories in groups
compensate such populations by intervention in the for- (Richek & McTague, 1988) or in the whole-class setting
mal educational framework. Studies on this issue have (Feitelson et al., 1986; Feitelson & Offir, 1988) may be
pointed out the positive effect of reading programs for defined as “hooking beginners on reading” (Feitelson,
at-risk students (Cohen, 1968; Mason, 1990; Morrow, 1988, p. 166). This is of great importance since it affects
1987, 1988; Roser et al., 1990). beginning readers and stimulates them to read books for
In Israel, this topic was studied for kindergarten pleasure. Subsequently, it helps them to develop their
and elementary school populations in both individual literacy.
and small-group reading sessions. Other studies dealt
Schema theory (although some researchers claim
with reading in a whole-class setting with kindergarten
that there is no single schema theory, but a family of
children and school children. In these studies the chil-
schema theories—cf. Whitney, Budd, Bramucci, & Crane,
dren of the experimental groups scored better than their
1995) may shed light on the phenomenon of the attrac-
peers in control groups in reading, reading comprehen-
sion, and picture storytelling (Feitelson, Goldstein, Iraqi, tion of young readers to series stories. According to this
& Share, 1993; Feitelson, Kita, & Goldstein, 1986). theory, previous world knowledge affects the reader’s
A central issue examined as part of the application comprehension. Another factor is previous knowledge of
of reading programs in educational frameworks is the story grammar (Dimino, Taylor, & Gersten, 1995;
development of children’s attitude toward books and the Pearson & Fielding, 1991; Wilson & Anderson, 1986).
extent of their leisure reading (Elley, 1994; McKenna, The question that follows this theoretical approach is
Kear, & Ellsworth, 1995). It has been suggested that whether the familiarity element of series stories, for ex-
reading books should be placed in the classroom, prefer- ample, topics and structures (Billman, 1984; Chall, 1983)
ably in open-shelf libraries with numerous attractive vol- affects reading comprehension and hence affects the
umes. Correct use of the library invites children to bor- amount of pleasure reading done by readers exposed to
row books and read them at leisure and to share them this kind of text. In other words, will the familiarity ele-
with their friends through reading aloud in class ments (i.e., world knowledge and story grammar famil-
(Morrow, 1989a; Strickland, 1989; Strickland & Morrow, iarity) have an impact on hooking beginning readers on
1990). Studies of such programs point to changes in chil- reading?
dren’s attitudes toward reading and to their growing in- The conditions of the Feitelson et al. (1986) study
terest in pleasure reading (Morrow, 1982; Morrow & on a first-grade class requiring enhancement did not al-
Weinstein, 1986). Similar results were found for children low generalization of the conclusions since it was based
at risk with little previous experience in literacy on one experimental class in which the teacher was
(Morrow, 1987). asked to read aloud installments of series stories to her
An experiment in Israel that examined the effect of students, and two control classes with no systematic
teachers systematically reading stories aloud in class also reading. Moreover, it could not be determined whether
revealed an increase in book borrowing from open-shelf
the achievement of the experimental class was due to
class libraries and interest in voluntary reading by chil-
exposure to reading in general or to exposure to reading
dren in the experimental group (Feitelson & Offir, 1988).
series stories in installments.
This usually occurred after the teacher had read to the
The present study tackled this issue again with
class passages from books, especially series stories (i.e.,
stories written by the same author in which the hero is more subjects and more text manipulations related to the
accompanied by a few permanent secondary characters earlier findings. Specifically, we examined three ques-
who move on from one adventure to another). tions: (a) What is the effect of reading to first graders re-
A similar finding emerged from another study con- quiring enhancement on their achievement in decoding,
ducted with a first-grade class (Feitelson et al., 1986) reading comprehension, and storytelling? (b) What is the
where students requiring enhancement served as the ex- effect on their achievement of reading from series stories
perimental group. Besides the enthusiasm for listening to by one author as compared to reading isolated stories by
series stories read aloud and an increase in leisure read- one author or by different authors? and (c) Is there a
ing by these children, the teacher reported an increase in correlation between the kind of literature read to the
purchases of trade books (sold commercially) for volun- class and the amount of voluntary reading that subse-
tary reading. It was also found that these students quently takes place?
172 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

Method month after the beginning of the treatment, it became ev-


ident that a teacher of a class in the third group was not
acting precisely according to the researchers’ require-
Participants ments, namely, reading daily and systematically, and
Sixteen first-grade classes requiring enhancement therefore this class was excluded from the study. Hence,
(defined in Hebrew te’unei-tipuah) from eight elemen- the third group now consisted of three classes, while the
tary schools in Haifa and its suburbs participated in the first, second, and the fourth groups had four classes.
study. The nearest North American term for te’unei- Each group was exposed to a different treatment,
tipuah is possibly socially and culturally challenged. as follows. The first group, the multiple-author group,
However, due to unique social/historical circumstances listened to isolated stories written by different writers, se-
in the Middle East, the conventional Hebrew term lected by the teachers from various readers used in ele-
te’unei-tipuah is retained here. It literally means “requir- mentary schools in Israel. The second group, the single-
ing enhancement.” This term was coined in the 1970s author group, listened to isolated stories written by one
following studies that diagnosed differences in students’ Israeli writer. The third group, the series group, used a
scholarly achievement on the basis of countries of origin series of stories written by the same author whose single
(Asia-Africa vs. Europe-America). The Ministry of stories were read to the second group. The fourth, the
Education viewed such differences with concern because control group, engaged in the regular learning activities
major accepted values in Israeli society included equality of first-grade classes—worksheets, drawing, and pasting.
of social opportunities, social integration, and the merg- In most Israeli first-grade classes, libraries are main-
ing of groups from different diasporas. Various enhance- tained and stories are often read aloud on Fridays at the
ment and compensatory strategies were necessary, and traditional weekly Sabbath Eve ceremony. However, dai-
the Ministry of Education sought specific means of iden- ly systematic read-aloud sessions did not take place in
tifying the population that required enhancement. The the classes selected for the study. Thus, the difference
definition of students as te’unei-tipuah was established between the groups in this respect was that the control
in 1974 (Algrable, 1974) on the basis of the analysis of group children were not exposed to increased and con-
the 1972 annual all-country achievement test results (this trolled read-aloud sessions in class.
test assembles the results of 8 years of elementary
schooling). Schools that were defined as requiring en- Materials
hancement were entitled to governmental help and fi-
nancial support according to the proportion of the stu- Pretest
dents requiring enhancement. The Hebrew version of the Vocabulary Sub-test
In all, 339 students, Israeli-born Hebrew speakers, taken from the Revised Wechsler Intelligence Child test
were sampled by a two-stage cluster sampling method. (Lieblich, 1979) served as a pretest. Since the Vocabulary
Children who were new immigrants in these classes, and Sub-test results were connected by Nevo (1972) with
thus not native speakers of Hebrew, were excluded from general learning achievement (presumably including
the statistical calculations though they took part in all the reading achievement) and it was used by other re-
activities of the experiment to prevent feelings of alien- searchers (cf. Feitelson et al., 1993) who applied the
ation among class members. Age range was 5 years, 8 same method, we employed it in the study as a control
months to 6 years, 11 months, with 6 years, 4 months covariate. This was intended to neutralize possible pre-
being the average age at the beginning of the experi- liminary inequalities among the groups.
ment. All the teachers who participated in the study had The pretest was conducted individually outside the
the same professional background: They were graduates classroom before the treatments began. Each child’s
of a teachers’ training college and had taught at least 5 score was expressed as a standard score based on the
years in elementary schools. raw score, which was the total, accumulated from the 23
The total of 16 classes were randomly assigned to words of the test, in accordance with the Vocabulary
four treatment groups as follows: Six randomly chosen Sub-test for children (Lieblich, 1979). Raw score range
pairs of classes from six schools served as the experimen- was 0–39 and the standardized score range was 0–19.
tal groups. These 12 classes were randomly divided into
three experimental groups of four classes each. The last Posttests
two pairs of classes from the last two schools formed a At the end of the treatment period subjects were
fourth group, the control group. Both classes in the pair assessed using three tests: decoding, reading compre-
from the same school were exposed to the same treat- hension, and picture storytelling. All the tests were con-
ment to avoid possible contamination by the study. A ducted in the presence of at least two examiners. The
Interactive reading 173

reading comprehension test was given in the classroom the written version was corrected according to the
in the teacher’s presence. Reading and storytelling tests recordings.
were conducted consecutively during one individual ses- To assess the children’s stories we chose six mea-
sion per child outside the classroom. sures recognized in the literature as analyzing the devel-
Decoding test. A 46-word text taken from a Hebrew opment of literacy, that is, the level of the concept of a
reading diagnosis booklet was used for diagnosing de- story (Bamberg, 1987). We decided to introduce only
coding reading skill (Illan, n.d.). Some changes were those measures that could best reveal and distinguish ma-
made in the text so that it contained most of the conso- jor literacy features acquired by the children during the
nants, vowels, and final letters (graphemes) of modern year of the experiment under the different treatments.
Hebrew. (The validity of these changes had been exam- Since literacy begins to emerge in the first grade, we
ined and confirmed in a pilot study to be described lat- chose to highlight the noteworthy features among our
er.) Each child was asked to read the text aloud while first graders’ achievements. The measures dealt with (a)
the examiner marked the child’s errors without him or story length, (b) story structure, and (c) story cohesion.
her noticing it. The score was defined by the number of (a) Story length. Story length was defined by the
correctly read words in the range 0–46. number of clauses per story. Unlike some researchers
Reading comprehension test. A multiple-choice test who use the proposition (based on Fillmore, 1968) as
with 16 items was selected from various versions of the basic linguistic unit for story analysis (Kintsch, 1977;
reading comprehension tests in Hebrew (Ortar, 1976; Poulsen et al., 1979; Roth & Spekman, 1986; Stein &
Ortar & Sar’el, 1986). We chose texts with questions re- Glenn, 1979), we used the clause (Berman, 1988; Martin,
ferring only to the information provided in the text. The 1983). According to Berman and Slobin (1994, p. 26) a
order of the items was based on the results of the pilot clause refers to “any unit containing a unified predica-
study. Different scores were allotted to the different tion,” for example (in Hebrew) hine yeled (here’s a boy),
items, taking into account varying degrees of difficulty ve po hayeled al ofanayim (and here the boy is on a
(from 1 for easiest items, e.g., “Circle the bird [in the bike), and ve hu nafal (and he has fallen down).
above figures] that is not raising its head,” to 5 for the (b) Story structure. This was analyzed by the fol-
most difficult, e.g., “‘When I was a boy in kindergarten lowing measures: story grammar, beginnings, and codas.
we all went out on Tree Day to plant a tree. Now after Story grammar was analyzed on the basis of the story
30 years I went back and found a forest.’ Who wrote grammar model. This model is a hierarchical system of
this? A young fellow, a little boy, an old man, an adult.”). laws by which we can characterize the categories in the
Each child had to read silently the 16-item multiple- story, describe the relations among the categories, fix
choice test and mark with a circle the answer s/he con- their place in the story, and determine the conditions
sidered to be correct (one of four options). The reading dictating their transfer or elision (Mandler, 1982;
comprehension test score (range 0–48) expressed the Rumelhart, 1975; Stein & Glenn, 1979). Following
number of correct answers. Whaley (1981) and Kozminsky (1986), we decided to
Picture storytelling test. The task of telling a story use Mandler and Johnson’s (1977) model.
elicited by a sequence of pictures served in studies on To analyze the stories, an ideal story was defined
the development of the ability to tell a story (Berman, (which, according to Stein & Glenn, 1982, can also be de-
1988) as well as in studies on the development of the fined as a well-formed story) based on 25 adult stories.
story concept, that is, story schema (Poulsen, Kintsch, The ideal story thus included 15 categories. A child’s sto-
Kintsch, & Premack, 1979; Stein & Glenn, 1982). The test ry was defined as ideal if it included all 15 categories.
consisted of four colored pictures depicting an eventful Story beginnings and codas were examined accord-
story (Hechtkopff, 1970): (a) A boy is riding his bicycle; ing to the children’s ability to introduce and end stories
(b) hearing an aeroplane, he looks up at the sky, fails to effectively (Bamberg, 1987; Labov, 1972). The occur-
notice a tree, and bumps into it; (c) another boy who rence of a story beginning and coda, according to narra-
watches the scene comes to his aid and takes him home; tive conventions, would be considered good (Galda,
and (d) his mother puts a bandage over his wounded 1984; Garnett, 1986; Poulsen et al., 1979; Rumelhart,
knee. These pictures were used successfully elsewhere 1975). A 5-point assessment scale was designed.
(Feitelson et al., 1993; Feitelson et al., 1986) and were Regarding beginnings, the scale ran from 1 point for the
therefore applied in this study. Each child was asked to poorest beginning related to the context, such as “I see a
tell the story of the pictures to the examiner in an indi- boy here” (see Zahava’s story, Appendix A), to 5 points
vidual session. The child’s speech was simultaneously for the best beginning according to narrative conven-
transcribed and tape-recorded. Later, story analyses were tions, such as “Once there was a boy who liked to ride a
based on the written protocols, and, in case of doubt, bike” (see Danny’s story, Appendix A).
174 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

As to codas, the scale ran from 1 point for the etry and the Bible). For our first graders, who had only
poorest coda, which suspended the narrative at a high just learned how to read, only vowelled books could be
point without concluding the event, such as “and he applied in the study.
took him to the nurse” (see Allon’s story, Appendix A), Thus, the multiple-author group was given readers
to 5 points for the best coda, which included a resolu- intended for the second and third grades used in various
tion of the problem introduced into the story, such as elementary schools. The books included stories and po-
“and the doctor bandaged his leg, and he went home ems by several writers and poets, representing different
safely and told mother everything that happened to him” styles and story schemas. The average length of the sto-
(see Linoor’s story, Appendix A). ries was three to five pages. Each class of the multiple-
(c) Story cohesion. Story cohesion was examined to author group received six readers for its open-shelf li-
test the children’s ability to tell coherent stories with suf- brary, from which the teachers read the stories aloud.
ficient cohesion components by the following measures: For the single-author group we purchased a num-
deictic expressions and reference system. Deictic expres- ber of anthologies comprising short stories by the same
sions of time and location were counted in each child’s Israeli author who wrote the series acquired for the third
story. Following Karmiloff-Smith (1979), deictic expres- group. Following Radday (1969) and Radday et al. (1985),
sions were defined as the use of verbal expressions with we assumed that since the short stories were written by
nonverbal expressions (i.e., pointing gestures) for loca- the same author as the series stories, the language and
tion (e.g., “I see a boy here”, see Zahava’s story, style would be similar. In Radday (1969) three linguistic
Appendix A) or for time (e.g., “Now I see an aero- levels (phonology, morphology, and syntax) of the book
plane”). The proportion of these expressions to the total of Isaiah were examined to determine whether it was
number of clauses was then calculated, yielding the ratio written by one, two, or more authors. The underlying as-
between the amount of deictics and the total length of sumption on which Radday based his study was that
the story. Note that high scores in this measure actually one’s language takes shape as the outcome of one’s lin-
mean low achievement because much use of deictics in- guistic experiences, which evolve in the course of life.
dicates a child’s high dependence on the immediate con- Hence, we can assume that the style of the stories given
text of the pictures and lack of abstraction as well as to the single-author group and the series group were
relatively poor vocabulary. similar and that differences between results may be at-
The reference system examines children’s ability to tributed to the different genres.
present new (undefined) participants and later refer to Surveys of Israeli children’s reading preferences
them as known (with the definite article) (Clark & found the Kofiko series to be popular among fourth-,
Haviland, 1977; Haviland & Clark, 1974; Martin, 1983); fifth-, and sixth-grade Israeli students (Bergson & Levy,
for example, “Once there was a boy who liked to ride 1984a, 1984b). Based on this and the results of a previ-
bicycles. One day an aeroplane flew by and the boy ous study (Feitelson et al., 1986), six stories from this se-
looked at it” (see Danny’s story, Appendix A). For scor- ries were acquired for the series group. The hero of the
ing purposes, all such references were first counted, and series is a naughty monkey, who is accompanied by a
a second count included only the correct occurrences of few permanent secondary characters who experience
references in the story. The ratio between these two different adventures in typical Israeli environments in
counts constituted the child’s score for this measure. each volume. The Curious George series is similar
(though intended for younger readers). We may com-
Allocation of books for the treatment groups pare Kofiko also to The Baby-sitters Club, Goosebumps,
Two fundamental factors in the decision about Henry and Mudge, or Noddy series. Out of the numer-
which books to acquire for the experiment were (a) the ous published versions of this popular series, we chose a
books’ use of vowels and (b) their types of literature nonchildish format. Each book (22.5cm x 15cm) contains
(i.e., isolated stories written by various authors with dif- chapters, is about 100 pages long, and includes tiny
ferent styles vs. series stories written by one author using black-and-white illustrations.
a uniform style). Hebrew orthography is basically conso- Thus, a different kind or genre of literature was
nantal; that is, it lacks vowel letters (English a,e,i,o,u). chosen for each of the three experimental groups with
Instead, diacritical points and some other marks within, the goal being to compare the different effects of these
above, and below the letters are used to indicate vowels. listening sessions. As noted, the control group was given
These marks are used for teaching children to read and no extra books for the class library. However, at our re-
are printed in readers for first to third grade. Books for quest the control group classes kept open-shelf libraries,
fourth grade on are printed without these diacritical each with about 30–40 books, like the experimental
marks as are all other modern Hebrew texts (except po- groups (for a full list of the books, see Kita, 1992).
Interactive reading 175

Procedure utes, 5 days a week, at the end of the school day. After
reading aloud, they placed the books on open shelves
Pilot study so that children could read them during the school day
A pilot study was conducted a year prior to the or borrow them for leisure reading at home.
main study to check the research design, materials, etc. These sessions proceeded with enormous enthusi-
A school of students requiring enhancement in Haifa asm, not the least on the part of the children themselves.
agreed to participate in the pilot study, which lasted 4 The sessions were temporarily suspended only before
months and included four classes: two parallel first-grade holidays and because of formal ceremony preparations
and two parallel second-grade classes. The conclusions or extracurricular activities such as a visit to the zoo. All
drawn from this study were applied in planning and ex- the classes retained their regular seating for the read-
ecuting the main study as described earlier (e.g., focus- aloud sessions except for one class who sat together on
ing on first graders only and adding stories by the same the rug in the middle of the classroom. At the end of the
author to compare with stories written by different treatments, posttests were administered during the last
authors). month of the school year.

Teachers’ preparation Observations and interviews


At the beginning of the school year, a meeting was The active read-aloud treatment was accompanied
held with the principals and teachers at each of the eight by an observation study. One of the authors (Kita) sat in
schools involved in the study to describe the study and the class as an observer once a month, on average, for
explain the researchers’ requirements. The teachers were about half an hour each observation. The passive partici-
instructed to read each story only once, applying an in- pant observation technique, generally used by ethnogra-
teractive reading model (Mason, Peterman, & Kerr, phers, was applied during these observations. The ob-
1989). This model requires activities before, during, and server, engaged in passive participation, “is present at the
after the reading in order to help students to understand scene of action but does not participate or interact with
the story. Thus, before reading, the teachers were asked other people to any great extent. About all you need to
to show the book cover to the children, read the title do is find an ‘observation post’ from which to observe
and ask the children to explain it, and clarify their ex- and record what goes on” (Spradley, 1980, p. 59).
pectations from the story. Then the teachers were re- Accordingly, the observer in this study, whose presence
quested to read the story literally as written, but to insert was evident to both teachers and children, watched the
explanations of difficult lexical items, idioms, and pas- scene and took notes without interfering in the work.
sages during the reading. They were asked to activate After class observation the observer held collective
the children’s prior knowledge during the session and talks with each class and individual interviews with
foster the process of deepening their comprehension of teachers and children. The interviews were intended to
the story. They were requested to encourage the chil- assess the teachers’ impressions of the experiment and
dren to ask questions, think about the content, and re- to discuss problems as they arose. The interviews with
late the story to their own experience. After the reading the children were meant to assess the amount of their
the teachers were asked to summarize the gist of the sto- leisure reading and book acquisitions.
ry with the children and repeat its main ideas. An exam- The children were asked about the leisure books
ple of such activity, taken from Bracha Kita’s field notes, they had read, either school books or trade books.
is found in Appendix B. Reference books such as dictionaries or atlases were not
Although each teacher approached the reading in counted. School books were defined as books in com-
an individual manner—that is, used different intonation, mon use for teaching at school, such as readers, math
mimicry, etc.—they did their utmost to satisfy the re- books, spelling books, and geography books. Trade
searchers’ instructions. The interactive reading activity books were nonschool reading books, written for plea-
was monitored by accompanying observation studies. sure by various authors and published in Hebrew by dif-
ferent publishers (e.g., Pinocchio, The Ugly Duckling).
Treatments Immediately following this question, children were
After the pretest, the allocated books were distrib- asked about purchasing books for pleasure reading. In
uted to the class teachers, and the treatments began in this category only trade books were considered. Three
December. The experiment continued until June, nearly interviews were held with each child during the study,
to the end of the school year. the first about 2 months after it began. The observations,
As mentioned, the teachers in the three experi- the class discussions, and the individual interviews were
mental groups read from the selected books for 20 min- all systematically documented.
176 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

Table 1 Posttest scores by treatment groups

Criterion Groupa Unadjusted Adjusted Fc Effect Posthoc


variables mean (SD) meanb sized group
(range) differencese

Decoding A 41.60 (4.04) 41.51 .176 B C D


(0–46) B 39.15 (6.75) 39.05 F = 9.11 –.261 A * * ns
C 43.20 (2.89) 43.32 .497 B — * ns
D 40.43 (5.63) 40.52 C — — *
Reading A 31.00 (9.21) 30.66 .553 B C D
comprehension B 27.50 (9.13) 23.37 F = 8.98 .226 A * ns *
(0–48) C 30.85 (7.62) 31.32 .618 B — * ns
D 24.72 (10.08) 25.09 C — — *

Picture storytelling
Story length A 10.31 (3.80) 10.19 .706 B C D
(unlimited) B 8.92 (3.87) 8.78 F = 10.13 .230 A * ns *
C 10.89 (4.64) 11.06 1.000 B — * ns
D 7.97 (2.96) 8.10 C — — *
Story grammar A 7.23 (1.56) 7.17 .714 B C D
(0–15) B 6.67 (1.83) 6.59 F = 18.75 .383 A * * *
C 7.82 (1.98) 7.91 1.137 B — * *
D 5.85 (1.75) 5.92 C — — *
Story beginnings A 2.83 (1.37) 2.82 .275 B C D
(1–5) B 2.98 (1.34) 2.97 F = 5.87 .373 A n.s * *
C 3.34 (1.53) 3.35 .621 B — ns *
D 2.38 (1.53) 2.40 C — — *
Story codas A 3.07 (.54) 3.06 .773 B C D
(1–5) B 2.86 (.68) 2.85 F = 24.39 .534 A * ns *
C 3.20 (.50) 3.22 .955 B — * *
D 2.37 (.88) 2.38 C — — *
Deictic A 7.0 (17) 6.9 –.402 B C D
expressions B 8.0 (22) 7.9 F = 10.22 –.381 A ns ns *
percentage C 3.8 (11) 4.0 –.461 B — ns *
(0–100) D 26.5 (49) 26.6 C — — *
References A 74 (18) 74 .428 B C D
percentage B 79 (16) 78 F = 10.69 .619 A ns * *
(0–100) C 79 (15) 79 .666 B — ns *
D 64 (21) 65 C — — *
a Sample sizes were as follows: Experimental A (multiple-author group), n = 106;
Experimental B (single-author group), n = 76; Experimental C (series group), n = 67; Control D, n =90.
b Adjusted for pretest WISC–R vocabulary.
c All F ratios are statistically significant beyond the .001 level according to MANCOVA test.
d adj. M of experimental group – adj. M of control group
Effect size = _______________________________________________
SD of control group
e Tukey’s posthoc test (1949) for comparing the adjusted means of the treatment groups. * p < .05.

Results ed score range: minimum = 0; maximum = 19). As a re-


sult of this, posttest differences were tested with multi-
variate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), using pretest
Pretest scores as the control covariate.
A statistically significant difference was found, F =
4.60, p < .01, on the pre-test WISC-R vocabulary measure. Posttests
Means and standard deviations were as follows: multiple- Table 1 presents the experimental and control
author group: M = 8.99, SD = 2.47; single-author group: group means both adjusted and unadjusted for pretest
M = 9.03, SD = 2.56; series group: M = 7.98, SD = 2.09; differences. The findings indicate statistically significant
control group: M = 8.11, SD = 2.25 (standardized expect- differences among the four treatment groups in decoding,
Interactive reading 177

Table 2 Mean numbers of books read and purchased by treatment groups based on three interviews per child

Criterion Groupa M (SD) Fb Effect Coefficient Posthoc


variables sizec of variationd group
differencesc

Reading A 6.19 (3.48) .175 56.21 B C D


trade B 6.40 (4.16) F = 36.76 .334 65.00 A ns * ns
books C 11.68 (5.30) 1.830 45.37 B — * ns
D 5.22 (3.53) 67.62 C — — *

Reading A 2.32 (1.98) –.045 85.34 B C D


school B 2.18 (1.74) F = 19.66 –.124 79.81 A ns * ns
books C .54 (.86) –1.051 159.25 B — * ns
D 2.40 (1.77) 73.75 C — — *

Purchasing A 2.39 (2.55) .458 106.69 B C D


trade B 1.72 (1.92) F = 11.55 .105 111.62 A ns * ns
books C 4.32 (5.67) 1.474 131.25 B — * ns
D 1.52 (1.90) 125.00 C — — *
a Sample sizes were as follows: Experimental A (multiple-author group), n = 106;
Experimental B (single-author group), n = 76; Experimental C (series group), n = 67; Control D (control group), n = 90.
b All F ratios are statistically significant beyond the .001 level according to analysis of variance.
c M of experimental group – M of control group
Effect size = _______________________________________________
SD of control group
d SD
CV = ___ 3 100
M
e Duncan’s multiple range test (1955).

reading comprehension, and picture storytelling. The reported number of books read by the child during the
posthoc group differences in decoding show that the se- experiment. Each child’s score was equivalent to the num-
ries group significantly outscored the other groups (see ber of books bought for him/her. Group differences were
Table 1). Thus it seems that the treatment applied to the tested by multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).
group had the strongest effect on accuracy in reading. Table 2 shows the following results. First, in read-
As to reading comprehension, the posthoc differ- ing trade books for pleasure, the group difference in
ences (see Table 1) indicate that among the three experi- number of such books read was statistically significant, F
mental groups, both the multiple-author group and se- = 36.76, p = .0001 (see Table 2). This finding was attrib-
ries group significantly outperformed counterparts in the uted to the fact that the series group children read a sig-
control group. Thus, it seems that these two types of ex- nificantly larger number of books than did children in
posure to reading significantly affect comprehension. any of the other groups. The series group’s achievement
Posthoc group differences in picture storytelling was especially great when compared with that of the
show that control group scores were statistically signifi- control group (almost 2 standard deviations above the
cantly the lowest in nearly all the measures (the high standardised score of the control group, see Table 2).
scores of the control group in deictic expressions actual- Second, in voluntary reading of school books, the
ly indicate low achievement). These story analysis mea- difference among the groups was statistically significant,
sures, we believe, are valuable in displaying children’s F = 19.66, p = .0001 (see Table 2). The number of school
progress. They reflect an internalised awareness and books read was significantly higher in children of the
growth by the experimental groups of the story concept. multiple-author group, the single-author group, and the
control group than in the series group. There was more
Book purchasing and leisure reading than one standard deviation difference between the se-
Assessment of leisure reading and book purchasing ries group and the control group. These findings com-
were based on the three interviews with the children, in plemented those given previously and reflect the differ-
which children were asked about any leisure books they ent reading preferences among the treatment groups.
had read, either school books or trade books. Each book While the series group preferred trade books including
scored one point, and the total score represented the self- series they heard in class, the other groups preferred the
178 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

familiar school books (especially the class readers), even found to yield different effects in decoding. The statisti-
for leisure reading. cally significant differences between the scores of the se-
We noted too that though the amount of reading ries group and the scores of the other groups suggest
by the series group had a high standard deviation, the that listening to series stories was the most effective
relative dispersion of this group in regard to the amount method among those tested here.
of leisure reading of school books was rather small (see It is possible, however, that the series group scores
Table 2). This meant that for this latter measure most of were high not only because of the reading sessions and
the children of the series group were clustered around the specific material. It is plausible to assume that the
the mean. presence of an additional factor such as extensive leisure
Finally, in purchasing trade books for leisure read- reading caused this result.
ing, statistically significant differences existed among the Extensive voluntary reading has a unique signifi-
groups, F = 11.55, p = .0001 (see Table 2). Calculation of cance for decoding: “If we want children to learn to read
the coefficient of variation (i.e., the ratio between the well, we must find a way to induce them to read lots”
standard deviation and the mean) showed that all the (Adams, 1990, p. 5). It seems that children who listened
groups were heterogeneous in this measure and their to series stories were motivated or induced to “read
standard deviations were higher than the mean. The se- lots.” This helped them to practice their reading and
ries group revealed the highest rate of heterogeneity, move on from the first reading stage to the second.
and its standard deviation was 131.25% from the mean. According to Chall (1987), this stage involves the transfer
Comparing the mean of book acquisition by each group from “letter-sound correspondences to knowledge of the
showed that on the average children of the series group alphabetic principle and skill in its use” and it “integrates
bought significantly more books than children from the knowledge and skills acquired in stage 0 and 1, reliance
other groups (see Table 2). Altogether, during the period on context and meaning as well as on decoding (phon-
of the experiment, children of the series group acquired ics), for identifying new words and reading with greater
twice the number of reading books than children of the fluency” (p. 69). In our view, we instilled in the series
other groups. The effect size showed a difference of stories children the capacity and disposition to read
more than one standard deviation in favour of the series freely and fluently.
group, as compared with the other groups. The lowest From the reading comprehension test results, we
average number of acquired books was found for the may conclude that any methodological exposure to story
control group (see Table 2). reading, as in our study, enriches children’s vocabulary.
Although we did not test vocabulary acquisition, it can
be assumed that listening to oral story reading promotes
Discussion vocabulary growth (Elley, 1989). This, in turn, affects the
One of the aims of this study was to examine the reading comprehension achievement of the listeners
effects of systematic daily exposure to listening to stories since “researchers concluded that vocabulary markedly
in the classroom on reading achievement of Israeli first- influences reading comprehension” (Graves, 1986,
grade students requiring enhancement. The skills tested p. 59). In addition, read-aloud sessions presumably affect
at the end of the experiment were decoding, reading students’ knowledge of story structure—that is, of story
comprehension, and picture storytelling. Based on the grammar. Although there was no direct instruction of
differences between the three experimental groups and story grammar in our study, we assume that such expo-
the control group, we can assume that such treatment af- sure promotes reading comprehension (Dimino, Taylor,
fects reading achievement. In addition to a pairwise & Gersten, 1995).
posthoc comparison of the treatment groups, we carried Likewise, read-aloud sessions probably affect stu-
out a comparison of the three combined experimental dents’ world knowledge—their knowledge schemata.
groups versus the control group for all the eight mea- This world knowledge, in turn, positively affects chil-
sures used in this research. The combined experimental dren’s reading comprehension. Interactive reading ex-
group markedly outscored the control group in all the pressed by the teacher’s support before, during, and
measures. This important outcome means that exposure after the read-aloud sessions (Mason et al., 1989) proved
to any kind of reading enhances literacy; moreover, it conducive to the children’s progress. Our findings also
means that such intervention in the development of chil- corroborate the model of reading aloud in classrooms
dren requiring enhancement may not be too late even at described by Hoffman et al. (1993). The mediation be-
the age they enter school, and it is possible (at least part- tween the texts and the listeners in the read-aloud ses-
ly) to compensate these children in the formal educa- sion helped to bridge the gap between the colloquial
tional framework. Furthermore, different treatments were and literary registers that exists in Hebrew (Berman,
Interactive reading 179

1988; Berman & Neeman, 1994), as well as in many oth- nalise the necessary elements of the story’s structure.
er languages, and contributed to improving the chil- These elements are later used in children’s own story
dren’s scores in this measure. productions.
Comparing the three experimental groups’ achieve- The ability of a narrator to tell a coherent story de-
ment in reading comprehension with those of the con- pends on, among other factors, the cohesive elements
trol group shows that only the multiple-author group used in the story. A narrator has to deliver the story so
and the series group children statistically significantly that it may be comprehended without the immediate
outscored the control group. In general, we may con- context. Deictics and reference markers are elements of
clude that exposure to reading by teachers in class, and immediate context that take part in a story (Berman,
particularly exposure to special sorts of literature, affect 1988; Berman & Slobin, 1994; Halliday & Hasan, 1976;
reading comprehension. Karmiloff-Smith, 1979; Martin, 1983). Our experiment
The picture storytelling results, examined in light of shows that the control group was greatly deficient in
measures accepted in the literature (Bamberg, 1987; these respects compared with the experimental groups.
Galda, 1984; Garnett, 1986; Mandler, 1982; Rumelhart, It was profuse mainly in the use of deictic expressions in
1975; Stein & Glenn, 1979), reveal progress in produc- its stories—on average four times more than in the ex-
tion of stories by children in the experimental groups re- perimental groups. From a developmental point of view,
garding such aspects as story length, story grammar, sto- the control group’s stories resembled those typical of a
ry beginning, story coda, deictic expressions, and 3-year-old native speaker of Hebrew, who anchors the
reference expressions. ongoing discourse in deictic picture description, for ex-
Story length shows that systematic exposure to sto- ample, “by the spatial term here” (Berman & Neeman,
ries affects the ability of listeners to produce longer sto- 1994, p. 137). On this account the control group’s stories
ries. Research on children’s stories shows a steady rise in were less cohesive.
story length with age (Berman, 1988; Hudson & Shapiro, Turning to voluntary reading of trade books we
1991); in our opinion, the experimental groups greatly see that exposure to daily read-aloud sessions affected
improved in this aspect. This finding is to be considered this measure (it was statistically significant, however,
with caution, however, for on its own it cannot predict only in the series group). As for school books, it seems
story quality (Berman, 1988). that only the series group drew away from them.
The story grammar results allow the conclusion Increased book purchasing, which was an unex-
that exposure to stories affected the story structure pro- pected finding in a previous study (Feitelson et al.,
duced by the listeners. Stories narrated by the experi- 1986), was repeated here in the experimental groups
mental groups included significantly more categories (again, statistically significant only in the series group).
outlined in our ideal story than those produced by the This result may be associated with the differential impor-
control group. The control group perhaps had a less tance placed on reading and books by these children.
elaborate concept of a story. We reiterate the findings concerning the series
In general, the beginnings and codas of the experi- group—an increased amount of leisure reading as well
mental groups’ stories were better than those of the con- as purchasing of trade books for leisure reading. The
trol group. It should be noted that the story beginnings children of this group read about 12 books on average
by the experimental groups mainly referred to the time during the study period while those of the other experi-
of the event (e.g., “Once upon a time” or “One day a mental groups read about half that number, and the con-
boy got out on his bike”) rather than to its location. The trol group children read even less. On the other hand,
codas generally resolved the problem in the story (e.g., the series group children read more trade books and
“Finally the doctor bandaged his leg”) . A few of them fewer school books than the other children. Moreover,
included a moral (e.g., “He promised his mother to be the series group children reported their preference for
careful and watch the road when he rode his bike”). By the series stories. Altogether, 246 books from this series
contrast, the control group’s beginnings revealed less fa- were counted for these children. By their self-report,
miliarity with the conventional story setting, and their each of them read at least one book from the series. In
stories remained in a sense unended, suspending the contrast, only eight children of the other three groups re-
narrative without a coda at a high point. Such story be- ported having read books as difficult as, for example,
ginnings and codas give us a notion of the development the Kofiko series. Furthermore, the children of the other
of the story concept in children (Bamberg, 1987; Galda, groups mostly remained at a fairly low level of reading
1984; Garnett, 1986; Rumelhart, 1975). It seems that both short stories found in readers and thin trade books in-
exposure to and discussion of numerous stories in class tended for the reading acquisition stage (about 10–20
teach children story schemata and enable them to inter- pages each). At the same time the series group skipped
180 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

to reading at a higher level, namely, adventure books more they read additional volumes of the series. This is
(about 100 pages each) such as the Kofiko series, as- the magic secret.
sessed by a reading preference survey as suitable for The findings of this research are limited to Israeli
children of Grade 2 and above (Bergson & Levy, 1984a, first graders. More work is needed to further examine the
1984b). effect of interactive read-aloud stories (either isolated or
In our opinion, extensive leisure reading by the se- from series) on reading achievements and voluntary
ries group developed partly as a result of availability of reading gains. It would be worthwhile to conduct a study
purchased books at home. The children could read and of additional age groups. Furthermore, it is important to
reread the books as they liked. Following studies of re- keep in mind that as a classroom intervention, this study
peated reading (Fresch, 1995; Martinez & Roser, 1985; provides some information on basic literacy acquisition
Martinez & Teale, 1988; Morrow, 1988; Yaden, 1988), we measures but little information on school success.
assume that rereading the books helps children gain Although we have stressed the growth of leisure
control of their reading strategies. It also enables the reading as well as the purchase of books for voluntary
readers to internalise the language patterns characteristic reading among children in the series group, we must
of written language, thus producing deeper interpreta- modify our conclusions with a degree of caution since
tion of the contents, better prediction of plot develop- they were based on self-report measures. Moreover, in
ment, and linking of the story with personal experience. our experiment we used only one Israeli series of sto-
Later, they “become confident enough to attempt some- ries. Hence, the effect of series in general on the mea-
thing new after they have read and reread a familiar sures mentioned above should be reexamined using oth-
text” (Fresch, 1995, p. 222). er series (although Feitelson & Offir, 1988, indicate
The achievement of the children of the series similar results with more than one series). Furthermore,
group is conspicuous compared with those of the single- the different readability and linguistic difficulty levels of
author group, who listened to stories written by the this series of stories were not compared to the other
same author. That is, even with similar stylistic-linguistic books in the various treatments. Thus, the impact of text
factors (phonemic, morphemic, syntactic, and lexical type used in read-aloud sessions should be reexamined
components) (Radday, 1969; Radday et al., 1985), the re- and needs further control. Further work can be focused
sults in terms of leisure reading were different. on a causal linking of the potential of the read-aloud
The amount of leisure reading and the purchase of sessions with enhancing reading skills and reading for
books for voluntary reading found in the series group, as pleasure.
described previously, seems to indicate a magic secret. It Some questions, for example, could not be an-
starts with listening to series stories and later reading swered in this framework: Is exposure to reading aloud,
them. Explanations of this magic secret encountered in general, and series stories read-aloud sessions, in par-
elsewhere (e.g., Feitelson et al., 1986; Feitelson & Offir, ticular, effective in the long term? Is it necessary to con-
1988; Richek & McTague, 1988) suggest that this kind of tinue such intervention in upper grades to maintain read-
literature is just right for children at the first stage of ing achievement? Would there be similar findings if the
reading, that is, the decoding stage (Chall, 1983, 1987). intervention were applied to older children? Are the re-
This genre is appropriate because (among other reasons) sults of exposure to reading carried over to school suc-
at its base is a familiar element that facilitates the begin- cess, or are they limited to the reading skills examined in
ning readers’ contention with reading: The topics, struc- the present study? Would we obtain similar results if the
tures, and language patterns are familiar (Billman, 1984; intervention were applied in foreign language instruc-
Chall, 1983; Fresch, 1995; Martinez & Teale, 1988). tion, such as ESL (English as a Second Language)?
In terms of schema theory, familiar schemata trans- To answer these questions, further controlled stud-
ferred from one volume to another equip readers with ies are necessary. They should be undertaken experi-
world knowledge and story structure knowledge, which mentally in order to advance our understanding of litera-
are essential for reading comprehension (Pearson & cy in general and to develop reading literacy among
Fielding, 1991; Wilson & Anderson, 1986). As a result a populations requiring enhancement in particular.
chain reaction starts: The more the beginning readers are To conclude, the study has examined a hitherto
exposed to the series books, the more background infor- uninvestigated issue—the distinction between effects of
mation they accumulate about the world of the series three text types on beginning readers requiring enhance-
and the story structures; the more background informa- ment. We found (as in other studies) that, generally, ex-
tion they get, the better they understand the series sto- posing beginning readers to read-aloud sessions in an
ries; the better they understand the series stories, the early stage of reading acquisition improves their reading
more they enjoy them; the more they enjoy them, the skills and enhances them to become leisure readers.
Interactive reading 181

Comparison of the three text types has yielded clear in- ELLEY, W.B. (1994). Voluntary reading activities. In W.B. Elley
dications of the advantage of reading series stories over (Ed.), The IEA study of reading: Achievement and instruction in thirty-
two school systems (pp. 65–87). Oxford, England: Pergamon.
isolated stories on certain reading skills, and mainly on FEITELSON, D. (1987). Reconsidering the effects of school and
voluntary reading. The findings allow us to recommend home for literacy in a multicultural cross-language context: The case
the use of read-aloud series stories as a tool to motivate of Israel. In D. Wagner (Ed.), The future of literacy in a changing
voluntary reading, at least for first graders. Likewise, world (pp. 174–185). Oxford, England: Pergamon.
there is a basis to assume that voluntary reading has a FEITELSON, D. (1988). Facts and fads in beginning reading: A
cross-language perspective. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
unique impact on reading decoding, reading compre- FEITELSON, D., & GOLDSTEIN, Z. (1986). Patterns of book owner-
hension, and storytelling abilities. ship and reading to young children in Israeli school-oriented and
nonschool-oriented families. The Reading Teacher, 39, 924–930.
REFERENCES FEITELSON, D., GOLDSTEIN, Z., IRAQI, J., & SHARE, D. (1993).
ADAMS, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning Effects of listening to story reading on aspects of literacy acquisition in
about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. a diglossic situation. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 71–79.
ALGRABLE, M. (1974). Measures for characterizing the social struc- FEITELSON, D., KITA, B., & GOLDSTEIN, Z. (1986). Effects of lis-
ture of school and a method for distributing the enhancement budget tening to series-stories on first graders’ comprehension and use of lan-
among schools. Megamot, 21, 219–227. guage. Research in the Teaching of English, 20, 339–356.
BAGHBAN, M. (1984). Our daughter learns to read and write: A case FEITELSON, D., & OFFIR, Y. (1988). Promoting independent read-
study from birth to three. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. ing. Haifa, Israel: University of Haifa.
BAMBERG, M. (1987). The acquisition of narrative: Learning to use FILLMORE, C.J. (1968). The case for case. In E. Bach & R. Harms
language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. (Eds.), Universals in linguistic theory (pp. 1–90). New York: Academic
BERGSON, G., & LEVY, A. (1984a). Study of leisure reading of Press.
children in the fourth, sixth and eighth grades (part 1). Safrut Yeladim FRESCH, M.J. (1995). Self-selection of early literacy learners. The
Va-Noar, 10, 7–14. Reading Teacher, 49, 220–227.
BERGSON, G., & LEVY, A. (1984b). Study of leisure reading of GALDA, L. (1984). Narrative competence: Play, storytelling and sto-
children in the fourth, sixth and eighth grades (part 2). Safrut Yeladim ry comprehension. In A.D. Pellegrini & T.D. Yawkey (Eds.), The devel-
Va-Noar, 11, 3–11. opment of oral and written language in social context (pp. 105–117).
BERMAN, R.A. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Discourse Processes, 11, 469–497. GARNETT, K. (1986). Discourse skills: The social-academic inter-
BERMAN, R.A., & NEEMAN, Y. (1994). Development of linguistic connection. Topics in Language Disorders, 6, 44–56.
forms: Hebrew. In R.A. Berman & D.I. Slobin (Eds.), Relating events in GRAVES, M. (1986). Vocabulary learning and instruction. In E.Z.
narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study (pp. 285–328). Hillsdale, Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of research in education (pp. 49–89).
NJ: Erlbaum. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.
BERMAN, R.A., & SLOBIN, D.I. (Eds.) (1994). Relating events in HALLIDAY, M.A.K., & HASAN, R. (1976). Cohesion in English.
narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. London: Longman.
BILLMAN, C. (1984). The child as a sleuth. Children’s Literature in HAVILAND, S.E., & CLARK, H.H. (1974). What’s new? Acquiring
Education, 25, 30–41. new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal
CHALL, J.S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512–521.
McGraw-Hill. HEATH, S.B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills
CHALL, J.S. (1987). Developing literacy...in children and adults. In at home and school. Language in Society, 11, 49–76.
D.A. Wagner (Ed.), The future of literacy in a changing world (pp. HEATH, S.B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in
65–80). Oxford, England: Pergamon. communities and classrooms. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
CLARK, H.H., & HAVILAND, S.E. (1977). Comprehension and the Press.
“given-new contract.” In R.O. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and HECHTKOPFF, H. (1970). Stories in pictures. Hed Ha-Ggan, 4.
comprehension (pp. 1–40). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. HOFFMAN, J.V., ROSER, N.L., & BATTLE, J. (1993). Reading aloud
COCHRAN-SMITH, M. (1984). The making of a reader. Norwood, in classrooms: From a modal to a “model.” The Reading Teacher, 46,
NJ: Ablex. 496–503.
COCHRAN-SMITH, M. (1986). Reading to children: A model of un- HUDSON, J.A., & SHAPIRO, L.R. (1991). From knowing to telling:
derstanding texts. In B.B. Schieffelin & P. Gilmore (Eds.), The acquisi- The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives.
tion of literacy: Ethnographic perspectives (pp. 35–54). Norwood, NJ: In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure
Ablex. (pp. 89–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
COHEN, D. (1968). The effect of literature on vocabulary and read- ILLAN, Z. (n.d.). Reading diagnostics’ texts. Ramla, Israel: Wellfare
ing achievement. Elementary English, 45, 209–213, 217. Project.
DIMINO, J.A., TAYLOR, R.M., & GERSTEN, R.M. (1995). Synthesis KARMILOFF-SMITH, A. (1979). A functional approach to child lan-
of the research on story grammar as a means to increase comprehen- guage. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
sion. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 11, 53–72. KINTSCH, W. (1977). On comprehending stories. In A.M. Just & P.A.
DUNCAN, D.B. (1955). Multiple range and multiple F-tests. Carpenter (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension (pp. 33–62).
Biometrics, 11, 1–42. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
DURKIN, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York: Teachers KITA, B. (1992). Reading series stories to first graders compared to
College Press. other texts as a means to improve reading skills and habits of leisure
ELLEY, W.B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to sto- reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Haifa, Israel.
ries. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 174–186. KOZMINSKY, E. (1986). “Once there was a nice girl”; Story com-
182 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY April/May/June 1997 32/2

prehension by children. Iyunim Be-Hinuh, 45, 83–104. RADDAY, J.T. (1969). Studies in statistical linguistics in Isaiah.
LABOV, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the black Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem,
English vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Israel.
LIEBLICH, A. (1979). WPPSI: A Hebrew guide to the Wechsler test of RADDAY, J.T., & SHORE, H., with WICKMAN, D., POLLATSCHEK,
intelligence at the kindergarten age. Jerusalem, Israel: The Hebrew M.A., RABIN, CH., TALMON, SH., and Preface by FREEDMAN, D.N.
University. (1985). Genesis: An authorship study in computer-assisted statistical
MANDLER, J.M. (1982). Recent research on story grammars. In L.F. linguistics. Rome: Biblical Institute.
LeNy & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Language and comprehension (pp. RICHEK, M.A., & MCTAGUE, B.K. (1988). “The Curious George”
207–218). Amsterdam: North Holland. strategy for students with reading problems. The Reading Teacher, 42,
MANDLER, J.M., & JOHNSON, N.S. (1977). Remembrance of things 220–225.
parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151. ROSER, N.L., HOFFMAN, J.V., & FAREST, C. (1990). Language, lit-
MARTIN, J.R. (1983). The development of register. In J. Fine & erature, and at-risk children. The Reading Teacher, 43, 554–559.
R.O. Freedle (Eds.), Developmental issues in discourse (pp. 1–39). ROTH, F., & SPEKMAN, N.J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spon-
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. taneously generated stories of learning-disabled and normally achiev-
MARTINEZ, M., & ROSER, N. (1985). Read it again: The value of ing students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 8–23.
repeated readings during storytime. The Reading Teacher, 38, RUMELHART, D.E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D.G.
782–786. Brown & A.M. Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding:
MARTINEZ, M., & TEALE, W.H. (1988). Reading in a kindergarten Studies in cognitive science (pp. 211–236). New York: Academic Press.
classroom library. The Reading Teacher, 41, 568–572. SPRADLEY, J.P. (1980). Participant observation. New York: Holt,
MASON, J.M. (1990). Reading stories to preliterate children: A pro- Rinehart & Winston.
posed connection to reading (Tech. Rep. No. 510). Champaign: STEIN, N.L., & GLENN, C.G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehen-
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. sion in elementary school children. In R.O. Freedle (Ed.), New direction
MASON, J.M., PETERMAN, C.L., & KERR, B.M. (1989). Reading to in discourse processing (Vol. 2, pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
kindergarten children. In D.S. Strickland & L.M. Morrow (Eds.), STEIN, N.L., & GLENN, C.G. (1982). Children’s concept of the de-
Emerging literacy: Young children learn to read and write (pp. 52–79). velopment of a story schema. In W.J. Friedman (Ed.), The develop-
Newark, DE: International Reading Association. mental psychology of time (pp. 255–282). New York: Academic Press.
MCKENNA, M.C., KEAR, D.J., & ELLSWORTH, R.A. (1995). STRICKLAND, D.S. (1989). A model for change: Framework for an
Children’s attitude toward reading: A national survey. Reading emergent literacy curriculum. In D.S. Strickland & L.M. Morrow (Eds.),
Research Quarterly, 30, 934–956. Emerging literacy: Young children learn to read and write (pp.
MORROW, L.M. (1982). Relationships between literature programs, 135–146). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
library corners designs, and children’s use of literature. Journal of STRICKLAND, D.S., & MORROW, L.M. (1990). Sharing big-books.
Educational Research, 75, 339–344. The Reading Teacher, 43, 342–343.
MORROW, L.M. (1987). Promoting innercity children’s recreational STRICKLAND, D.S., & TAYLOR, D. (1989). Family storybook read-
reading. The Reading Teacher, 41, 266–274. ing: Implications for children, families and curriculum. In D.S.
MORROW, L.M. (1988). Young children’s responses to one to one Strickland & L.M. Morrow (Eds.), Emerging literacy: Young children
story readings in school settings. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, learn to read and write (pp. 27–34). Newark, DE: International
89–107. Reading Association.
MORROW, L.M. (1989a). Designing classrooms to promote literacy TAYLOR, D. (1983). Family literacy: Young children learning to
development. In D.S. Strickland & L.M. Morrow (Eds.), Emerging liter- read and write. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
acy: Young children learn to read and write (pp. 121–134). Newark, TEALE, W.H. (1986). Home background and young children’s liter-
DE: International Reading Association. acy development. In W.H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy:
MORROW, L.M. (1989b). Literacy development in the early years: Writing and reading (pp. 173–206). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Helping children read and write. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. TEALE, W.H., & SULZBY, E. (1987). Literacy acquisition in early
MORROW, L.M., & WEINSTEIN, C.S. (1986). Encouraging voluntary childhood: The roles of access and mediation in storybook reading. In
reading: The impact of a literature program on children’s use of library D.A. Wagner (Ed.), The future of literacy in a changing world (pp.
centers. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 330–346. 111–130). Oxford, England: Pergamon.
NEVO, B. (1972). Class and community differences in a number of TUKEY, I.W. (1949). Comparing individual means in the analysis
intelligence measures of youth in Israel. Unpublished doctoral disserta- of variance. Biometrics, 5, 99–114.
tion, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel. WHALEY, J.F. (1981). Story grammars and reading instruction. The
ORTAR, G. (1976). Reading comprehension test. Jerusalem, Israel: Reading Teacher, 34, 762–771.
Department of Education, The Bureau of Educational Measurement. WHITNEY, P., BUDD, D., BRAMUCCI, R.S., & CRANE, R.S. (1995).
ORTAR, G., & SAR’EL, T. (1986). Reading comprehension test. On babies, bath water, and schemata: A reconsideration of top-down
Jerusalem, Israel: The Hebrew University. processes in comprehension. Discourse Processes, 20, 135–166.
PEARSON, P.D., & FIELDING, L. (1991). Comprehension instruction. WILSON, P.T., & ANDERSON, R.C. (1986). What they don’t know
In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook will hurt them: The role of prior knowledge in comprehension. In J.
of reading research: Vol. II (pp. 815–860). New York: Longman. Orasanu (Ed.), Reading comprehension (pp. 31–48). Hillsdale, NJ:
POULSEN, D., KINTSCH, E., KINTSCH, W., & PREMACK, D. Erlbaum.
(1979). Children’s comprehension and memory for stories. Journal of YADEN, D. (1988). Understanding stories through repeated read-
Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 379–403. alouds: How many does it take? The Reading Teacher, 41, 556–560.
PURVES, A.C., & ELLEY, W.B. (1994). The role of the home and
students’ differences in reading performance. In W.B. Elley (Ed.), The Received March 27, 1996
IEA study of reading: Achievement and instruction in thirty-two school Final revision received May 23, 1996
systems (pp. 89–121). Oxford, England: Pergamon. Accepted May 28, 1996
Interactive reading 183

APPENDIX A
Stories produced by children from different groups

Control group “Don’t cry, it will pass,” and he said to him, “But what
Allon (a boy). The boy sees an aeroplane. And about the bike?” He said to him, “Never mind, soon I
here he has fallen. And a boy is running. And he took shall come and fetch it.” And in the end the boy came
him to the nurse. and took him to the doctor. And the doctor gave him a
Zahava (a girl). I see a boy here and the bike blood test, and he got well.
looking at the aeroplane. And a boy fell off the bike.
And a boy ran to help him. And a boy is crying. And the The single-author group
second boy comes and takes him home. Linoor (a girl). One fine day Ron went for a walk
with the bike. In the blue of the sky he saw an aero-
Experimental groups plane. He looked at the aeroplane and looked, and sud-
The series group denly bumped into a tree. A boy ran quickly because he
Danny (a boy). Once there was a boy who liked to saw him. It must have been a friend of his. He picked
ride the bike. One day an aeroplane flew and the boy him up and did a good deed. He helped him and
looked at it and the bike went wherever it wanted be- brought him to the doctor and the doctor bandaged his
cause he did not look. And suddenly he bumped into a leg and he went home safely and told mother everything
tree and blood came down his leg and it hurt him. A that had happened to him.
friend of his came and helped him and said to him

APPENDIX B
ÒThe Fisherman and the KingfisherÓ: An example of a teacherÕs work at class (from Bracha KitaÕs field notes)

It’s 11:30 a.m. Teacher D. tells the children, “Okay, or idiom was not clear she would stop reading and ex-
everybody collect your things, we’re going to read a sto- plain in detail. For example, when she read the sentence
ry. Everybody move on to the rug.” Noisily and with evi- “The women of the village used to buy fish from the
dent joy, urging each other to hurry up, the children as- fisherman or trade them for barter” she stopped and
semble quickly in order to hear the whole story before asked “What is barter?” Getting no correct answer she
the bell rings. D. opens a book and declares, “Today explained, “To barter is to exchange one item of mer-
we’re going to read the story ‘The Fisherman and the chandise for another. When you go to buy something,
Kingfisher.’ Does anyone know what a kingfisher is? what do you give the seller? Right, you pay him money.
Right, it’s a bird that eats fish. It takes up fish with its But in our story some of the village women did not pay
beak. What is to take up? Right, catches, takes up is to with money, but with what? Good, with potatoes or
catch fish with the beak. [The Hebrew name for the bird eggs. Fine, let’s continue.”
is a compound comprising the root of the verb take up At the end of the story D. asked the class, “So why
and the noun fish.] Now let’s listen to the story and we’ll was the bird really called kingfisher?” Together with the
see how the kingfisher helped his friend the fisherman.” children she summarized the sequence of events: A
D. begins to read the story, pausing occasionally to good fairy helped the kind-hearted fisherman by turning
explain a word. For example, when reading the sen- the beak of the bird in the story into a beak to take fish
tence, “the old man crouched and placed it in his large out of the water. Thus the kingfisher saved the sick fish-
warm palm,” she asked, “What is crouched? Bent,” and erman from hunger, in return for the fisherman’s kind
continued reading. Whenever she felt that an expression treatment of the bird’s injury.

Вам также может понравиться