Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

Copyright 1996 by This article presents a summary overview of the most distinctive psychological theories of

The Gerontological Society of America


aging since World War II. Theoretical developments are broadly classified into three
The Cerontologist periods, respectively: The Classical period ('40s-'70s), represented by Developmental
Vol. 36, No. 6, 742-748
Tasks/Activity Theory, Psychosocial Theory of Personality Development, Counterpart
Theory, Disengagement/Activity Theory, Personality Theory of Age and Aging, and
Cognitive Theory of Personality and Aging; the Modern period ('70s-'90s), which includes
theories on Life-span Development and Aging, Reduced Processing Resources, Personality
and Aging, Behavioral Genetics and Aging; and the New period ('80s—'90s), represented by
Gerotranscendence and Gerodynamics/Branching Theory. The overview ends with an
outlook on psychogerontological theorizing.
Key Words: Psychological theories of aging, Gerodynamics, Branching theory,
Gerotranscendence

Theoretical Developments

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


in the Psychology of Aging
Johannes J. F. Schroots, PhD1

The scientific history of the psychology of aging is the Modern period (70s-'90s), and the New period
quite brief (Birren, 1961). Aside from pioneer work by ('80s-'90s; Schroots, 1996). From each group the
G. S. Hall (1922), Charlotte Buhler (1933), Carl G. Jung most distinctive psychological theories of aging are
(1933), and Walter Miles (1933), theoretical develop- represented in Table 1. Before summarizing these
ments in psychogerontology started basically after theories, with the emphasis on Modern and New
World War II. Since then, research in the psychology theories, some general issues in gerontological theo-
of aging has been guided by a somewhat diverse rizing need to be discussed.
collection of theories, models, and metaphors, of
which the time-based, gerontological research ap- Some Issues
proach is by far the most important (Schroots &
Birren, 1990). From a calendar time perspective there The first issue concerns the distinction between
are three foci — the aged, age, and aging (see also the terms theory, model, and metaphor. According
Treas & Passuth, 1988). The first, the psychology of to Lewin (1951), there is nothing so practical as a
the aged, focuses on older people and later life. good theory. A theoretical framework helps the sci-
Grounded in a stages-of-life perspective, most stud- entist to accumulate and integrate data into a body of
ies of the aged demonstrate a thematic, descriptive knowledge, as well as to provide directions for new
approach and little coherence (e.g., studies of Alz- research. Early attempts to develop formal, quantita-
heimer's disease, life satisfaction, widowhood, re- tive theories in the behavioral and social sciences
tirement, cognition, death and dying). The second have not been very successful. The attention of re-
approach in psychogerontology falls within the psy- searchers was turned, therefore, to the formalization
chology of age, which studies age differences in of models, which are regarded to be more simple
behavior by comparing groups of different ages in and quantifiable than theories, but also focus on
cross-sectional research. Productive research fo- limited aspects of behavior. Formalization in terms of
cuses on identifying the causes and consequences of quantitative models (or theories, for that matter),
the processes responsible for age-related differ- ignores the constructivist view that science is in an
ences (e.g., processing speed, attentional capacity, essential way metaphorical or characteristically em-
etc.). Finally, the psychology of aging studies behav- ploys metaphors. In fact, models are extended and
ioral patterns of change with age, integrating both systematic metaphors. This explains why the terms
the psychology of age and the aged in longitudinal theory, model, and metaphor are often used inter-
research. changeably in the research literature (Schroots, Bir-
After World War 11 theoretical developments in the ren, & Kenyon, 1991). The relative significance of
subject matter can be broadly classified into three these terms can be summarized as follows: the meta-
groups or periods: the Classical period ('40s-70s), phor drives the theory in the psychology of aging in
the first place, and the model functions as a more
general, extended or systematic kind of metaphor,
1
which connects theory with empirical research. In
Address correspondence to Johannes J. F. Schroots, PhD, ERGO/
European Research Institute on Health and Aging, University of Amster- the following sections, the term "theory" should be
dam, Roetersstraat 15,1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands. understood in the broadest sense of the word.

742 The Gerontologist


Table 1. Schematic Overview of Psychological Theories of Aging

Theory Author(s)
Classical Theories
Developmental Tasks/Activity Theory Havighurst (1948)
Psychosocial Theory of Personality Development Erikson (1950)
Counterpart Theory Birren (1960)
Disengagement/Activity Theory Cumming & Henry (1961)
Personality Theory of Age and Aging Neugarten (1968)
Cognitive Theory of Personality and Aging Thomae (1970)
Modern Theories
Life-span Development and Aging Baltes et al. (1980,1987,1992)
Reduced Processing Resources Salthouse (1985,1988,1991)
Personality and Aging Erikson (1950); Levinson (1978); Costa & McCrae (1988,1992)
Behavioral Genetics and Aging Plomin & McClearn (1990); Pedersen (1996)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


New Theories
Cerotranscendence Tornstam (1989,1992,1994)
Cerodynamics/Branching Theory Schroots (1988,1995a, b)

The second issue concerns the term "aging." In the following overview, the term aging should be
Briefly summarized, the psychology of aging studies understood as primary aging.
the regular changes in behavior after young adult- The fourth and last issue concerns the term "psy-
hood (Birren & Renner, 1977). Thus defined, the chological." Psychology as a science has laborious
psychology of aging is grounded in a two-stages-of- relations with the adjacent biological and social sci-
life perspective, development and aging, which are ences. Sometimes, psychological theories of aging
usually described as two successive processes of are labeled as "psychosocial" (with the emphasis on
change in time, with the transition point or apex at "social"); at other times they are conceived as biobe-
maturity. The classic metaphor for the two stages of havioral, behavioral genetic, or neuropsychological,
age-related change in life is the " h i l l " metaphor with the emphasis on the biological substrate. In
(Hall, 1922), which is based on biological concep- either case, the aging individual falls into the trap of
tions of growth and decline, particularly, the so- biological or social reductionism. But also within the
called mortality curve: mortality rates are high for field of psychogerontology there is the question of
infants, regularly decline to a minimum at about age reductionism, because of the unidirectional empha-
10, and then rise progressively throughout the re- sis on cognition and information processing, to the
mainder of the life span (Gompertz' law, 1825). Psy- neglect of affective and conative processes of change
chological processes of change, however, do not over the life span. Obviously, the psychology of
necessarily parallel biological changes along the life- aging is in a permanent identity crisis. As Birren and
span. For example, fluid abilities like speed of infor- Lanum (1991, p. 114-115) would say,
mation processing reflect genetic-biological deter- There is no major theory or underlying metaphor that
minants and tend to decline with age. Crystallized links the various areas of psychology. Its state is much
abilities, on the other hand, represent social-cultural like physics was in the first decades of this century in
influences on general world knowledge, for exam- which there was little unifying theory that linked the
ple, and may display some growth with age (Horn, topics of optics, sound, levers, heat, and light. Con-
1989). This cognitive phenomenon raises the as yet temporary psychology has similar topical islands of
unsolved problem of to what extent psychological knowledge organized under the headings of sensa-
processes of development and aging are different tion and perception, memory, learning, psycholin-
from each other, as both processes refer to age- guistics, social psychology, motor skills, psychome-
related changes. trics, and developmental psychology. It is not
surprising that the psychology of aging takes on a
The third, also unresolved issue is what changes complexion derived from these subdivisions.
are typical or normal psychological processes of
change in the individual, and what changes are atypi- The following overview aptly illustrates Birren and
cal, abnormal, or pathological patterns. The bounda- Lanum's point.
ries between these phenomena are often indistinct.
To clarify this issue, Busse (1969) made a conceptual Classical Theories
distinction between primary and secondary aging.
Primary aging refers to changes intrinsic to the aging Developmental Tasks/Activity Theory. — In 1948
process that are ultimately irreversible. Secondary Robert J. Havighurst published his often reprinted
aging refers to changes caused by illnesses that are book on the concept of developmental tasks in a life-
correlated with age but may be reversible or prevent- span perspective. A developmental task arises at or
able. Later, a third distinction was added, tertiary about a certain period in the life of the individual,
aging, which refers to changes that occur precipi- successful achievement of which leads to his happi-
tously in old age (Birren & Cunningham, 1985, p. 22). ness and success with later tasks, while failure leads

Vol.36, No. 6,1996 743


to unhappiness in the individual, disapproval by the and (b) an increasing preoccupation with self and
society, and difficulty with later tasks. All of these decreasing emotional involvement with others. Al-
tasks have biological (physical maturation), psycho- though the disengagement theory professes to ex-
logical (aspiration or values), and cultural (expecta- plain general psychological and social processes of
tions of society) bases. Havighurst has described six aging, it offers in fact a one-sided view of the aged,
developmental stages or age periods in total, each given the significant proportion of older people who
with its own developmental task. Later on, the cen- do not lose interest in life and do not withdraw from
tral organizing concept of age-related developmen- society. Disengagement theory encouraged the de-
tal tasks has been named "activity theory," as op- velopment of an opposing theory of the aged, activity
posed to "disengagement theory." theory, which is based on the concept of develop-
mental tasks. According to its main proponent, Ro-
Psychosocial Theory of Personality Development. bert J. Havighurst, activity theory states that in order
— In 1950 Erik Erikson formulated a psychosocial to maintain a positive sense of self, elderly persons
theory of eight stages of personality development, must substitute new roles for those lost in old age. As

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


each with its own characteristic crisis that arises out such, activity theory presents a more realistic view of
of the conflict between two opposite tendencies. older people.
The developmental task of each age period is to
resolve its conflict, which requires the integration of Personality Theory of Age and Aging. — Starting in
personal needs with the demands of society. The the 1950s, Bernice L. Neugarten (1968) and associates
successful resolution of each conflict leads to devel- studied the life cycle with two theoretical emphases.
opmental strength in terms of a new virtue. Failure, The first emphasis is on the timing of transitional
however, to deal adequately with a task during its events in the lives and roles of individuals. Life
period of ascendancy is damaging to personality de- events, such as marriage or parenthood, are norma-
velopment. Erikson's psychosocial stages of devel- tively scheduled: that is, they are expected to occur
opment are not tied closely to specific age periods. within certain ages and in a certain sequence. As
The early stages are defined in much more detail than such, they lead to changes in self-concept and iden-
the later ones: postadolescence, for example, in- tity. However, unexpected events (e.g., accidents) or
cludes about three quarters of the life span, but only age-normative events that occur "off time" (e.g.,
the last three stages. This division reflects the in- early widowhood) may have negative developmental
crease in psychosocial variability with age: the devel- consequences, such as life crises. The second em-
opmental tasks of an infant are relatively universal, phasis is on the study of personality type as predictor
but the tasks in later life are dependent as much on for successful aging. Aging is viewed as a process of
personal experiences as on general principles. adaptation in which personality is the key element.
Eight different patterns of aging have been distin-
Counterpart Theory. — In 1960 James E. Birren guished. The successfully aging individual not only
presented a general theory of aging as a counterpart plays an active role in adapting to the biological and
of development. The term "counterpart" is meant to social changes that occur with the passage of time,
express the idea that there are latent structures of but also in creating patterns of life that will give him
behavior (emotions, cognition, and motivations) car- or her greatest ego involvement and life satisfaction.
ried forward from earlier experience that interact
with present situations. Counterpart theory advo- Cognitive Theory of Personality and Aging. — In
cates indirect selection for positive late-life charac- 1970 Hans Thomae described briefly a cognitive the-
teristics that embrace a wide range of complex bio- ory of the aging personality, one which is intended to
logical (e.g., potential for a long life) and behavioral integrate various biological, sociological, and interac-
(e.g., intelligence) characteristics. For example, al- tionist perspectives while at the same time focusing
though individual differences in longevity do not upon the psychodynamics of aging. Central concepts
appear until long after reproduction has been com- in his theory are those of perception, perceived situa-
pleted, intelligent, long-lived parents are able to tion, and perceived self. Thomae postulates, for ex-
provide an environment (in terms of food and pro- ample, that perceived change rather than objective
tection) favorable for their young to survive. Birren's change is related to behavioral change, and that
counterpart theory expanded the classical " h i l l " met- change is perceived and evaluated in terms of the
aphor of development and aging to include ques- aging person's dominant concerns and expectations.
tions about their relationships and how behavior Successful adaptation to age-related changes, then,
comes to be organized over the adult years of life, if relates to the maintenance and restructuring of the
not over the whole life span. balance between cognitive and motivational systems;
for instance, the balance between acceptance of one-
Disengagement/Activity Theory. — The term "dis- self as old or rejection of this perception, which is one
engagement" refers to the withdrawal of people from of the developmental tasks of aging persons.
previous roles or activities. Starting from the assump-
tion that people turn inward from middle age and Modern Theories
over, Cumming and Henry theorized in 1961 that this
primary mental process produces: (a) a natural and Life-span Development and Aging. — Since the
normal withdrawal from social roles and activities, beginning of the 1980s, Paul B. Baltes and his associ-

744 The Gerontologist


ates (Baltes, 1987; Baltes, Reese, & Lipsitt, 1980; Researchers have advanced several explanations for
Baltes, Smith, & Staudinger, 1992) have conducted a this phenomenon of aging, but so far only the
series of studies on psychological processes of de- resource-reduction view has found wide support. In
velopment and aging from a life-span perspective. In this view, aging leads to a reduction in the quantity of
line with the tradition of life-span developmental one or more processing resources, such as atten-
psychology, development and aging are conceived tional capacity, working memory capacity or speed
as synonyms for behavioral changes across the life of processing. According to Timothy A. Salthouse
span. Starting from these studies, Baltes has devel- (1985, 1988, 1991) — a typical exponent of this view
oped a theoretical framework of seven propositions since the eighties — processing resources are char-
about the nature of human aging from a psychologi- acterized by three properties: (1) they are limited in
cal point of view: (1) there are major differences quantity, with a measurable aspect such as quantity
between normal, pathological, and optimal aging, or effectiveness of allocation increasing up until ma-
the latter defined as aging under development- turity and then decreasing across the adult years; (2)
enhancing and age-friendly environmental condi- they enable or enhance cognitive processing so that

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


tions; (2) the course of aging shows much inter- performance in many cognitive tasks is improved
individual variability (heterogeneity); (3) there is when greater amounts of the resources are available;
much latent reserve capacity in old age; (4) there is and (3) they are not local or specific in the sense that
aging loss in the range of reserve capacity or adaptiv- they are restricted to a small number of highly similar
ity; (5) individual and social knowledge (crystallized cognitive tasks, but instead are relevant to a broad
intelligence) enriches the mind and can compensate range of cognitive processes.
for age-related decline in fluid intelligence (aging The three properties of processing resources have
losses); (6) with age, the balance between gains and generated a number of specific theories. They can be
losses becomes increasingly negative; and finally, (7) classified into three categories, based on the domi-
the self in old age remains a resilient system of nant metaphor used in theorizing on resource-
coping and maintaining integrity. reduction, i.e., metaphors of space, energy, and/or
Based on this framework of propositions, a psy- time. That is, space limitations correspond to restric-
chological model of successful aging has been de- tions on the size of the computational or working
vised, called "selective optimization with compensa- memory region available for processing; energy limi-
tion." The central focus of this model is on the tations correspond to attentional capacity restric-
management of the dynamics between gains and tions; and time limitations refer to restrictions im-
losses, i.e., a general process of adaptation, consist- posed by tradeoffs between the rate at which
ing of three interacting elements. First, there is the information can be processed and the rate at which it
element of selection, which refers to an increasing becomes unavailable through decay, interference or
restriction of one's life to fewer domains of function- some other mechanism.
ing because of an age-related loss in the range of In a series of experimental studies, Salthouse and
adaptive potential. The second element, optimiza- his associates have focused on the time metaphor of
tion, reflects the view that people engage in behav- processing speed as explanatory construct of cogni-
iors to enrich and augment their general reserves tive aging. Their findings indicate that processing
and to maximize their chosen life courses (and asso- speed is a fundamental construct in human cogni-
ciated forms of behavior) with regard to quantity and tion, linked to explicit changes in neural structure
quality. The third element, compensation, results and functioning on the one hand and to higher-order
also (like selection) from restrictions in the range of cognitive processes like reasoning and abstraction
adaptive potential. It becomes operative when spe- on the other. As such, Salthouse hypothesizes that
cific behavioral capacities are lost or are reduced processing speed may well provide the cornerstone
below a standard required for adequate functioning. for integrative theories of cognitive aging. It should
The lifelong process of selective optimization with be noted, however, that the resource-reduction view
compensation allows people to age successfully, i.e., leaves unanswered the fundamental questions of
to engage in life tasks that are important to them why the reduction in resources occurs, and how that
despite a reduction in energy. For instance, the fa- reduction results in lower levels of cognitive perfor-
mous pianist Rubinstein remarked in a television mance (for a review, see Birren & Fisher, 1995).
interview that he conquers the weaknesses of aging
(adaptation) in his piano playing in the following Personality and Aging. — Studies of personality
manner: First, he reduces his repertoire and plays a and aging reflect the concept of personality behind
smaller number of pieces (selection); second, he them, here defined as the set of characteristic dispo-
practices these more often (optimization); and third, sitions that determine emotional, interpersonal, ex-
he slows down his speed of playing prior to fast periential, attitudinal, and motivational styles. Gen-
movements, thereby producing a contrast that en- erally speaking, two theoretical traditions can be
hances the impression of speed in the fast move- distinguished in this field, trait and developmental-
ments (compensation). stage models. In both traditions, the central issue
concerns the extent and nature of personality stabil-
Reduced Processing Resources. — For some time, ity and change over the life span; or, to put it differ-
it has been generally accepted that there is an aver- ently, the extent to which aging processes per se are
age age-related decline in cognitive performance. responsible for personality change.

Vol. 36, No. 6,1996 745


Theoretical models of adult personality develop- netic differences among individuals; the rest of the
ment represent the first and oldest tradition in the variation, the nongenetic portion, is called environ-
personality-and-aging field. Two theories, devel- mental. Thus, change in heritability over the life span
oped by Erik Erikson (1950) and Daniel Levinson indicates that the relative roles of genetic and envi-
(1978), respectively, offer developmental stages be- ronmental influences can change with age in terms
yond the period of early adulthood. Erikson's eight of their effects on biological and behavioral differ-
stages, extending from infancy to old age, were for- ences among individuals in the population.
mulated more than 45 years ago. From this perspec- Robert Plomin and Gerald E. McClearn (1990), two
tive, it is surprising that there has been collected only leading researchers since the eighties, have convinc-
limited empirical evidence for the maturity and old- ingly shown that behavioral genetics provides a the-
age stages, i.e., generativity vs stagnation and integ- ory and methods that can go beyond simple nature-
rity vs despair. There are no longitudinal studies, for nurture comparisons to consider age differences,
example, that ask whether the achievement of age changes, shared and nonshared environments,
generativity in midlife is a necessary precursor for and multivariate analyses. Nevertheless, theory for-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


the achievement of integrity in the later years. In mation in gerontological behavioral genetics is still in
Levinson's theory of personality development, based its infancy. On the basis of recent analyses of mostly
on a series of in-depth interviews with 40 men, each twin studies of aging, the third leading researcher in
man's life structure goes through an orderly se- this field, Nancy L. Pedersen (1996), comes to the
quence of three periods: early adulthood, middle following tentative conclusion: (a) the relative im-
adulthood, and late adulthood. The timing and portance of genetic and environmental effects on
length of each period and the development that individual differences in the elderly is phenotype-
takes place within it vary from man to man depend- specific. Heritability is low to moderate for personal-
ing on the biological, psychological, and social con- ity traits and measures of well-being, moderate for
ditions of his life. Nevertheless, a close linkage of health-related phenotypes, and greater for cognitive
periods with age intervals is suggested. Levinson's abilities, whereas heritability for memory is lower
theory can be severely criticized on many grounds, than for verbal and spatial abilities or perceptual
of which the impossibility of replicating the in-depth speed; (b) there are age differences in heritability,
interviews poses the most serious problem in aging the pattern of which is phenotype-dependent. For
research. some measures, particularly health-related charac-
Trait models represent the second tradition. Over- teristics, the relative importance of genetic effects
all, longitudinal studies of personality traits have con- appears to decrease across age groups. For others,
sistently found structural invariance of personality heritability is stable, increases, or reflects an inverted
over time, i.e., a marked pattern of similarity in factor L-shaped function. Variance changes may reflect ei-
structure across instruments, cohorts, types and ther an increase in environmental or genetic influ-
times of measurement. According to Paul Costa and ences, depending on the phenotype. More often,
Robert McCrae (1988,1992) — typical proponents of environmental effects account for the increase in
the trait model—the same five major factors (neuroti- variability in health-related phenotypes; (c) across
cism, extroversion, openness to experience, agree- short spans of time, genetic effects are more stable
ableness, and conscientiousness) have emerged from than environmental effects for personality and cogni-
longitudinal studies using somewhat different ap- tion. Environmental effects of importance for indi-
proaches. In conclusion of the evidence, they state vidual differences late in life are changing. Neverthe-
that people stay much the same in their basic disposi- less, environmental influences are at least as
tions and show a high degree of stability of personal- important for phenotypic stability across short (3-6-
ity, particularly during the latter half of their life year) spans of time.
course. More recent extensions of the trait model
pertain to personality-linked constructs like locus of New Theories
control and self-concept. Generalizations about sta-
bility and change are limited by the relatively small Gerotranscendence. — In 1989 Lars Tornstam sug-
number of studies available, the large majority of gested that human aging, the very process of living
which are cross-sectional in design. However, there is into old age, encompasses a general potential to-
a growing consensus that personality traits tend to be wards gerotranscendence; that is, a shift in meta-
stable with age whereas key aspects of self such as
perspective from a materialistic and rational vision to
goals, values, coping styles and control beliefs are
a more cosmic and transcendent one, normally fol-
more amenable to change (for a review, see Ruth &
lowed by an increase in life satisfaction. On the basis
Coleman, 1996).
of qualitative and quantitative studies, Tornstam
(1992, 1994) developed the theoretical concept of
Behavioral Genetics and Aging. — Behavioral gerotranscendence at three levels of age-related on-
geneticists of aging are concerned with the extent tological change: (1) cosmic level — changes in the
to which hereditary factors influence age-related perception of time, space and objects, increase of
changes over the life span of the individual. Here, affinity with past and coming generations, changes in
heritability is defined as a descriptive statistic refer- the perception of life, disappearing fear of death,
ring to the portion of observed, phenotypic variation acceptance of the mystery dimension in life, and
in the population that can be accounted for by ge- increase of cosmic communion with the spirit of the

746 The Gerontologist


universe; (2) self — discovery of hidden (both good structures can be translated in terms of mortality
and bad) aspects of the self, decrease of self- (probability of dying, life expectancy), morbidity
centeredness, self-transcendence from egoism to al- (disease, disorder, disability or dysfunction) and
truism, rediscovery of the child within, and ego- quality of life (well-being, life satisfaction). For exam-
integrity; (3) social and individual relations — less ple, traumatic life events and a healthy life style may
interest in superficial relations, increasing need for result in lower and higher order structures, respec-
solitude, more understanding of the difference be- tively, and consequently in higher and lower proba-
tween self and role, decreasing interest in material bilities of dying. It should be noted, however, that
things, and increase of reflection. lower order bifurcations at the biological or psycho-
The new theory of gerotranscendence reminds logical level of functioning (e.g., illness or divorce)
one of the classical concepts of disengagement do not always result in lower order branching behav-
(Cumming & Henry, 1961) and integrity (Erikson, ior. Some people, for instance, are strengthened by
1950), but differs qualitatively in some opposite as- illness, and divorce may have a positive rather than a
pects. For example, gerotranscendence implies a negative effect on mental health in terms of life

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


"redefinition of reality," while disengagement is re- expectancy and quality of life.
stricted to "turning inwards"; also, gerotranscen- Briefly summarized, branching theory studies the
dence is connected with "social activity" and a need determinants and patterns of branching behavior
for solitary "philosophizing" at the same time, while across the life span. As yet, this innovative theory of
disengagement encompasses social withdrawal only. aging is not based on empirical evidence. It remains
Cerotranscendence refers to offensive, multi-coping to be seen how empirical research in progress lends
strategies, while disengagement implies defensive support to its theoretical claims.
coping strategies and social breakdown. Finally,
Erikson's integrity refers primarily to the integration Next Ten Years
of elements in a life that have passed, while gero- Owing to circumstances, a schematic overview of
transcendence implies more of a forward or outward postwar psychological theories of aging was lacking
direction, including the redefinition of reality. Sum- in Emergent Theories of Aging (Birren & Bengtson,
marizing, even though Tornstam's theory of gero- 1988). Hopefully, the foregoing supplies a need,
transcendence is based on limited empirical evi- even though this overview illustrates painfully Bir-
dence, it nevertheless makes a promising attempt to ren's classic dictum that the field of psychogeronto-
integrate and further develop some classical and logy is data-rich but theory-poor. The question arises
modern psychosocial theories of aging. how psychogerontological theorizing will look ten
years from now. Extrapolation of the section on New
Gerodynamicsi'Branching Theory. — Recently, the Theories teaches that two types of theoretical devel-
author (Schroots, 1995a, 1995b) presented a brief out- opments can be expected (see also Birren &
line of a dynamic systems theory of aging, called Schroots, 1996).
gerodynamics. This theory at its inception, which The first development is taking place at the inter-
elaborates his 1988 essay, is based on general systems section of science and the humanities, and relates to
theory, notably the 2nd law of thermodynamics, and interesting combinations of qualitative and quantita-
dynamic systems theory (chaos theory). The 2nd law tive research (Denzin & Lincoln, 1994). The roots of
states that there is an increase of entropy or disorder qualitative research lie in the traditional soil of idio-
with age in living systems, resulting in the system's graphic methods and techniques for the study of
death. Chaos theory postulates that internal or exter- individual lives. In its modern form, qualitative re-
nal fluctuations of dynamic, far-from-equilibrium sys- search on aging is partly a reaction to what has been
tems can pass a critical point — the transformation omitted in quantitative research, i.e., the experi-
point — and create order out of disorder through a ences of growing old and being old. In this respect,
process of self-organization, that is, a process by personal life narratives are rediscovered as a rich
which a structure or pattern of change emerges with source of new insights into the experience of human
the passage of time. From this metatheoretical view- aging. Increasingly, life stories (e.g., letters, diaries,
point, the aging of living systems can be conceived as autobiographies, interviews) are being used for a
a nonlinear series of transformations into higher and/ wide range of research studies on topics of psycho-
or lower order structures or processes, showing a logical importance (Birren, Kenyon, Ruth, Schroots,
progressive trend toward more disorder than order & Svensson, 1996). This development will no doubt
over the life span, and resulting in the system's death. be stimulated by the recent publication of sophisti-
Cerodynamics lies at the root of a new aspect cated software packages for the qualitative and quan-
theory of aging, called branching theory. The basic titative analysis of non-numerical, unstructured, bio-
principle of this theory is the bifurcation or branch- graphical data (e.g., QSR NUD*IST [Richards &
ing behavior of the individual at the biological, psy- Richards, 1995] and WINMAX Pro [Kuckartz, 1996]).
chological or social level of functioning. Metaphori- The second development is concerned with the
cally speaking, bifurcation means that the fluctuating theoretical expansion of what has been called gero-
individual (organism) passes a critical point — the dynamics and the derived branching theory of aging.
bifurcation, branching or transformation point — Increasingly, the study of aging depends on longi-
and can branch off into higher and/or lower order tudinal and time-series data to gain insights into
structures or processes. Higher and lower order the branching patterns and processes of change.

Vol. 36, No. 6,1996 747


Findings suggest that there is a wide range of individ- Charness, N. (1995). Psychological models of aging: How, who, and what? A
comment. Canadian journal on Aging, 14, 67-73.
ual differences in the rate and manner of aging at all Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year
levels of analysis — biological, psychological, and longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Per-
sonality Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
social (Schroots, 1993). From a methodological per- 853-863.
spective, considerable progress has been made in Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Trait psychology comes of age. In T. B.
characterizing the time-varying distributions associ- Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Psychology
and aging. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
ated with aging (e.g., longitudinal factor analysis, Cumming, E., & Henry, W. E. (1961). Crowing old. New York: Basic Books.
latent growth curve analysis, state-space modeling). Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (Eds.). (1994). Handbook of qualitative
Methodological sophistication, however, is in research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W. W. Norton.
marked contrast with the verbal form of most psy- Gompertz, B. (1825). On the nature of the function expressive of the law of
chological theories of aging (Charness, 1995; Mole- human mortality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
naar, 1993). For this reason, the primary task of the London, 1, 513-585.
Hall, G. S. (1922). Senescence: The last half of life. New York: Appleton.
next ten years is to search for ways to integrate Havighurst, R. J. (1948). Developmental tasks and education. New York:
theoretical thought and empirical research. A prom- McKay.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article-abstract/36/6/742/567063 by guest on 19 October 2019


Horn, J. L. (1989). Models of intelligence. In R. Linn (Ed.), Intelligence:
ising approach consists of dynamic systems model- Measurement, theory and public policy. Urbana, IL: University of
ing — that is, on the basis of time-series data, an Illinois.
individual is characterized by a set of parameter Jung, C. G. (1933). Modern man in search of a soul. New York: Harvest.
Kuckartz, U. (1996). WINMAX Pro. Berlin: BSS/Freie Universitat.
values and a history of events that shape his or her Levinson, D. J. (1978). The seasons of a man's life. New York: Knopf
particular aging (growth) trajectory. Paraphrasing Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.
Van Geert (1994, p. 277), who advocates a so-called Miles, W. R. (1933). Age and human ability. Psychological Review, 40,
99-123.
experimental theoretical developmental psychology, Molenaar, P. C. M. (1993). Some innovatory methodological aspects of
mathematical model-building is necessary to peel longitudinal studies of health and aging. In J. J. F. Schroots (Ed.), Aging,
the psychological theories of aging out of the verbal health and competence: The next generation of longitudinal research.
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
form in which they have been caught. Once the Neugarten, B. L. (Ed.). (1968). Middle age and aging. Chicago: University of
dynamic theory or model is expressed in a set of Chicago Press.
equations, it is possible to try out the model under as Pedersen, N. L. (1996). Gerontological behavior genetics. In J. E. Birren & K.
W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed.). San
many conditions as possible. The Latin origin of the Diego, CA: Academic Press.
word "experiment" is "the act of trying," and it is in Plomin, R., & McClearn, G. E. (1990). Human behavioral genetics of aging.
this sense that the study of the behavior of dynamic In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
aging (3rd. ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
models is a form of the experimental theoretical Richards, T., & Richards, L. (1995). QSR NUD*IST. London: Sage
psychology of aging that promotes the integration of Publications.
data and theory in the future. Ruth, J. E., & Coleman, P. (1996). Personality and aging: Coping and
management of the self in later life. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.),
Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed.). San Diego: Academic
Press.
References Salthouse, T. A. (1985). A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North-
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental Holland.
psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Develop- Salthouse, T. A. (1988). Resource-reduction interpretations of cognitive
mental Psychology, 23, 611-626. aging. Developmental Review, 8, 238-272.
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hills-
psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 65-110. dale, N): Erlbaum.
Baltes, P. B., Smith, J., & Staudinger, U. M. (1992). Wisdom and successful Schroots, J. J. F. (1988). On growing, formative change, and aging. In J. E.
aging. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Birren & V. L. Bengtson (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging. New York:
Psychology and aging. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Springer.
Birren, J. E. (1960). Behavioral theories of aging. In N. W. Shock (Ed.), Aging: Schroots, J. J. F. (Ed.). (1993). Aging, health and competence: The next
Some social and biological aspects. Washington, DC: American Associ- generation of longitudinal research. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science
ation for the Advancement of Science. Publishers.
Birren, J. E. (1961). A brief history of the psychology of aging. The Cerontol- Schroots, J. J. F. (1995a). Psychological models of aging. Canadian Journal
ogist, 7,69-77,127-134. on Aging, 14, 44-66.
Birren, J. E.,& Bengtson, V. L. (1988). Emergent theories of aging. New York: Schroots, J. J. F. (1995b). Gerodynamics: Toward a branching theory of
Springer. aging. Canadian Journal on Aging, 14, 74-81.
Birren, J. E., & Cunningham, W. R. (1985). Research on the psychology of Schroots, J. J. F. (1996). Theories of aging: Psychological. In J. E. Birren (Ed.),
aging: Principles, concepts and theory. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie Encyclopedia of gerontology (Vol. 2). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed.). New York: Van Schroots, J. J. F., & Birren, J. E. (1990). Concepts of time and aging in
Nostrand Reinhold. science. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychol-
Birren,). E., & Fisher, L. M. (1995). Aging and speed of behavior: Possible ogy of aging (3rd. ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
consequences for psychological functioning. Annual Review of Psy- Schroots, J. J. F., Birren, J. E., & Kenyon, G. M. (1991). Metaphors and aging:
chology, 46, 329-353. An overview. In G. M. Kenyon, J. E. Birren, & J. J. F. Schroots (Eds.),
Birren, J. E., Kenyon, C. M., Ruth, J. E., Schroots, J. J. F., & Svensson, T. Metaphors of aging in science and the humanities. New York: Springer.
(Eds.). (1996). Aging and biography: Explorations in adult development. Thomae, H. (1970). Theory of aging and cognitive theory of personality.
New York: Springer. Human Development, 13, 1-16.
Birren, J. E., & Lanum, J. C. (1991). Metaphors of psychology and aging. In Tornstam, L. (1989). Gero-transcendence; A reformulation of the disen-
C. M. Kenyon, J. E. Birren, & J. J. F. Schroots (Eds.), Metaphors of aging gagement theory. Aging, 1, 55-63.
in science and the humanities. New York: Springer. Tornstam, L. (1992). The quo vadis of gerontology: On the scientific para-
digm of gerontology. The Cerontologist, 32, 318-326.
Birren, ). E., & Renner, V. J. (1977). Research on the psychology of aging:
Tornstam, L. (1994). Gero-transcendence: A theoretical and empirical ex-
Principles and experimentation. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.),
ploration. In L. E. Thomas & S. A. Eisenhandler (Eds.), Aging and the
Handbook of the psychology of aging. New York: Van Nostrand
religious dimension. Westport, CT: Auburn House.
Reinhold.
Treas, J., & Passuth, P. M. (1988). The three sociologies: Age, aging and the
Birren, J. E., & Schroots, J. J. F. (1996). History, concepts, and theory in the
aged. In F. Borgatta & K. Cook (Eds.), The future of sociology. Beverly
psychology of aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of
Hills, CA: Sage.
the psychology of aging (4th ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.
Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development — Change between
Buhler, C. (1933). Der menschliche Lebenslauf als psychologisches Prob- complexity and chaos. New York: Harvester/Wheatsheaf.
lem. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel.
Busse, E. W. (1969). Theories of aging. In E. W. Busse & E. Pfeiffer (Eds.), Received July 3, 1996
Behavior and adaptation in late life. Boston, MA: Little, Brown. Accepted August 3, 1996

748 The Gerontologist

Вам также может понравиться