Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 17

THE CORRELATION OF SELF-REGULATION

AND MOTIVATION WITH RETENTION


AND ATTRITION IN DISTANCE EDUCATION

Larry Peck
Old Dominion University

FS
Jill E. Stefaniak
University of Georgia
Smruti J. Shah
Old Dominion University

O O
This study surveyed 113 undergraduate and graduate distance education students at 2 U.S. universities using

R 8
the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). The MSLQ is an 81-item, self-report instru-
ment designed to measure study participants’ motivational orientations and their use of different learning

P 1
strategies. The motivational orientations measured were: intrinsic goal orientation, extrinsic goal orientation,
task value, control beliefs, self-efficacy for learning and performance, and test anxiety. The learning strategies

0
measured were: rehearsal, elaboration, organization, critical thinking, metacognitive self-regulation, time and

P 2
study environment, effort regulation, peer learning, and help seeking. The study revealed that self-efficacy,
effort regulation, and peer-learning correlated with student retention in the distance education programs.

INTRODUCTION
IA ©
To provide greater access to students and to
meet market demands, institutions of higher
education have adopted online delivery of
2009). The focus of this study is to explore the
correlation of self-regulation and motivation
with retention and attrition in distance educa-
tion.
A quantitative study conducted at a univer-
instruction and expanding course and program sity in the southeastern United States found
offerings at a rapid pace (Patterson & McFad- that online students were much more likely to
den, 2009). The growth rate of online enroll- drop out than the campus-based students. An
ments has been substantially greater than MBA program and a communication sciences
overall higher education enrollments (Allen & and disorders (CSD) program were evaluated
Seaman, 2007). The 12.9% growth rate for with the finding that 11% of the campus-based
online enrollment is much greater than the students in the MBA program dropped out as
1.2% growth overall of the higher education compared to 43% of the online students. In the
student population (Patterson & McFadden, CSD program, 4% of the campus students

• Jill Stefaniak, Jill Stefaniak, University of Georgia, 221 Rivers Crossing, 850 College Station Road, Athens, GA 30602.
Telephone: (706) 542-1682. E-mail: jill.stefaniak@uga.edu

The Quarterly Review of Distance Education, Volume 19(3), 2018, pp. 1–15 ISSN 1528-3518
Copyright © 2018 Information Age Publishing, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
2 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

dropped out compared to 23.5% of the online can apply what they have learned, and when
students (Patterson & McFadden, 2009). At a they are supported in the learning process.
community college in Texas, there was an 11% The importance of student-content interac-
to 15% point difference between course com- tion in the learning process has also been high-
pletion rates in the online and on-campus lighted in several research studies (Bernard et
course programs (Carr, 2000). Some of the al., 2009; Morrison & Anglin, 2006; Wittrock,
reasons for this are explored in this study as we 2010). While the face-to-face resident students
look at the defining elements of distance edu- may have the advantage of instructional strate-
cation and the effect those elements have on gies given by the instructor in the classroom,
the motivation and regulation of the learning this advantage does not always translate to the
and teaching process. Motivation and self-reg- online environment. The simple posting of
ulation issues can be significant factors in aca- information (e.g., PowerPoint slides) is not

S
demic success when students are separated teaching. Learners must be actively involved
in the learning process and generate their own

F
from each other and from the instructors.
Distance education offers significant bene- associations between new information and

O
fits to institutions desiring to expand student what they already know. It is through this deep
accessibility as well as significant educational processing of information initiated by the

O
challenges. Both the benefits and the chal- instructional strategy that significant learning
lenges have to do with the nature of distance occurs and students are integrated, challenged,

R 8
education. Keegan’s definition of distance and motived in the learning process. Distance

1
education students do not always have an

P
education states that distance education is
external stimulus to engage in this type of deep

0
characterized by:
processing with the content and may fail to do

P 2
so if the interaction is not part of the instruc-
• The separation of the teacher and learner
tional design.

IA ©
throughout the educational process.
Moore (1989) states that the lack of interac-
• The influence of an educational organiza-
tion between the learner and teacher leaves
tion in the planning and preparation of the
“ultimate responsibility for maintaining moti-
learning materials along with student sup-
vation, for interacting with the presentation,
port.
for analyzing the success of application, and
• The use of technical media to connect for diagnosing difficulties on the learners
teacher and learner and deliver the content themselves requiring a high degree of learner
of the course. autonomy” (p. 3). In distance education, as
• The provision of two-way communication, compared to traditional classroom face-to-face
and the quasi-permanent absence of a education, additional metacognitive, motiva-
learning group so that instruction is usually tional, and behavioral responsibility is placed
to an individual (Keegan, 1996). on the student to plan, organize, self-instruct,
self-monitor, motivate, and self-evaluate their
The work of Moore (1989) has given spe- learning process.
cific meaning and submeanings to the term Many of life’s endeavors require directed
“interaction.” He suggested that distance edu- and sustained effort over long periods of time
cators agree on the distinctions between three with minimal external regulation and direc-
types of interaction, which he labeled learner- tion. In these situations, students must self-reg-
content interaction, learner-instructor interac- ulate and direct their own motivation and
tion, and learner-learner interaction. Students action (Bandura & Schunk, 1981). This con-
transform when they interact intellectually cept of self-regulation is seen by a number of
with the content, when they are stimulated, researchers as a mediator between computer-
directed, and motivated to learn, when they based learning environments and academic
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 3

performance (Winters, Greene, & Costich, interaction treatments with other distance edu-
2008). Since no environment ensures learning cation instructional treatments. Interaction
and even settings with advanced levels of treatments are the instructional conditions in
interaction do not guarantee learning, students the courses that are intended to facilitate the
must activate, alter, or create schema for learn- three types of interactions. The study sup-
ing to occur. Self-regulated learning theorists ported the importance of the three types of
view students as metacognitively, motivation- interaction treatments and the strength of the
ally, and behaviorally active participants in interaction treatments is found to be associated
their own learning process (Zimmerman, with increased achievement outcomes. A
1986). There is a growing body of laboratory strong association was found between interac-
and field research that highlight the impor- tive treatment strength and achievement in
tance of students’ use of self-regulated learn- asynchronous courses and distance education

S
ing strategies in their academic achievement literature gives overwhelming support to the
(Zimmerman, 1990). importance of interaction for achievement.

F
Circumstantial factors play an increasing Anderson (2003) has polled distance educa-
role as older, nontraditional students enter tion students over the years and has concluded

O
online programs to further their education that there is a wide range of need and prefer-
while maintaining work and family responsi- ence for different combinations of course

O
bilities. In a study at the Indira Gandhi delivery and activity, which has led to the for-

R 8
National Open University in India (Fozdar, mation of an equivalency theorem:
Kumar, & Kannan, 2006), 250 students who

P 1
had not completed the program completed a Deep and meaningful formal learning is sup-

0
21-item questionnaire listing reasons in rela- ported as long as one of the three forms of
interaction (student-teacher; student-student;

P 2
tive importance for dropping out. The main
student-content) is at a high level. The other
reasons students gave for withdrawal were

IA ©
two may be offered at minimal levels, or even
absence of interaction with fellow students eliminated, without degrading the educa-
(47.06%), high cost of attending to laboratory tional experience. (p. 4)
work (38.24%), lack of time due to changing
family circumstances (35.29%), followed by Theories of retention and attrition in dis-
changes in employment status (35.29%). Other tance education have highlighted the complex
factors found to affect withdrawal included and multifaceted personal, circumstantial, and
marriage obligations (8.82%) and poor health institutional variables and their varying cor-
conditions (8.82%). For these older students, relation with success in the online academic
circumstances have a major effect on reten- environment (Bell, 2006). The common factor
tion. These factors would apply to U.S. stu- in all these variables is students and their reac-
dents as well, since the same factors appear in tions and interactions to the personal, circum-
our student culture. stantial, and institutional environments they
are confronted with as they pursue academic
success. Since there is no perfect learner or
The Importance
perfect learning environment, self-regulation
of Interaction in Achievement
of reactions and interactions may be an import-
Moore highlighted the importance of inter- ant key to understanding academic success in
action in education and defined three types of distance education.
interaction: student-content (S-C), student-
teacher (S-T), and student-student (S-S) Self-Regulated Learning
(Moore, 1989). A meta-analysis by Bernard et
al. (2009) of the experimental literature of dis- The research of Bandura and Schunk
tance education compares different types of (1981) on self-motivation, efficacy, and intrin-
4 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

sic interest is a recurring theme in academic regulation of their personal functioning,


success studies especially in distance educa- behavior, or learning environment. Self-effi-
tion where students do not have external fac- cacy is the student’s perception of perfor-
tors to motivate them, help them to believe in mance skill and capabilities. Academic goals
their adequacy, or organize and implement can be grades, social standing, self-esteem,
actions for academic success. As described, employment, obtaining good grades, improv-
the lack of adequate interaction in the educa- ing self-esteem, being equipped for a good job,
tional process requires a high degree of learner avoiding failure and embarrassment, and
autonomy to maintain motivation, interact pleasing teachers or parents.
with the content, diagnose difficulties, and get
the necessary support. The research of Zim-
merman (1989) on active participation in the
Selection and Use of Strategies

S
learning process is especially important for Students’ use of strategies to self-regulate
distance education, which requires students to

F
their behavior, environment, and personal
be able to initiate and self-regulate their own (covert) functions in a distance learning envi-
learning. Active participation would be

O
ronment is dependent on a feedback loop con-
defined as any interaction the student has with sisting of monitoring performance, self-

O
the content, the instructor, the environment, efficacy, and motivation (Zimmerman &
and the other students as part of the learning Martinez-Pons, 1990). Current theory and

R 8
process. research points to the importance of self-regu-

1
By definition, self-regulated students are lation in the distance education environment

P
metacognitively, motivationally, and behav- and the strategies that enhance it (Zimmerman

0
iorally active in their own learning process & Pons, 1986).

P 2
(Zimmerman, 1989). In self-regulated learn- A study of college-age students involved in
ing, the acquisition of knowledge is initiated

IA ©
an asynchronous web-based course was con-
and directed by the students themselves, and ducted by Bell (2006) to explore whether fac-
they are less dependent on others for direction tors related to self-regulated learning and
and motivation. Metacognitively, the self-reg- epistemological beliefs could predict learning
ulated student plans, organizes, self-instructs, achievement. The study included 201 under-
self-monitors, and self-evaluates at various graduate students enrolled in a variety of asyn-
stages during the learning process. Motivation- chronous web-based courses at a university in
ally, self-regulated students view themselves the southeastern United States. Data were col-
as competent, self-efficacious, autonomous, lected via a web-based questionnaire and sub-
and perceive their efforts and outcomes as jected to a factor analysis of self-regulated
valuable and worthwhile. Behaviorally, the learning using 24 questions from the Moti-
self-regulated students select strategies, struc- vated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire
ture their learning, and create a learning envi- (MSLQ). This study’s findings suggest that
ronment that optimizes their learning. individuals with the greatest expectancy for
According to Zimmerman (1986), the self-reg- learning were the most successful asynchro-
ulated students are constantly aware of the out- nous learners.
comes of their thinking patterns and their Two key components of the learning and
actions. teaching process are motivation and attitude
Three important elements in self-regulated (Morrison, Ross, Kalman, & Kemp, 2013).
learning are learning strategies, self-efficacy, Morrison et al. (2013) state:
and commitment to academic goals (Zimmer-
man, 1989). Learning strategies are actions For many instructors, learner motivation is
and processes that are used to acquire informa- actually considered to be the most important
tion or skill and to improve students’ self- determinant of success. Learners who “just
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 5

don’t care” or, worse, are actively resistant to 1. To what extent is there a correlation
the instruction are not likely to respond in the between the student’s score on the moti-
same way to the learning activities as would vation scales and retention?
highly motivated students. (p. 55) 2. To what extent is there a correlation
between the student’s score on the learn-
Learner attitude, while it can affect the
ing strategies scales and retention?
learning process, is different than motivation.
3. What are the expressed reasons given by
A student may be motivated to learn a subject
students for either dropping a course
but may feel inadequate to do so. This lack of
beyond the allowable drop date, dropping
confidence will result in less than optimum
out of the program completely, or for con-
learning even though the motivation may be
tinuing with a course until completion?
present. As we look at self-regulation in the

S
learning process, both motivation and attitude
play a large role in the successful regulation of METHODS

F
the process. While both students and teachers
bring their levels of motivation and attitude Research Design

O
into the learning context, the design and con-
duct of the instructional process can have a In this study, two groups of students were

O
direct affect on both motivation and attitude. administered the MSLQ survey. The first

8
(Anderman & Dawson, 2011; Driscoll, 2005;

R
group consisted of students who had dropped
Mayer, 2011; Schunk & Zimmerman, 2006). out of a distance education class or a particular

P 1
distance education program. The second group

0
consisted of students who were still enrolled in
Purpose of Study

P 2
the distance program and have not dropped
Based on the literature, there seems to be a any of their classes. An 81-item MSLQ was

IA ©
paucity of research on how motivation and used to determine the level of student motiva-
self-regulation influence student retention in a tion and use of learning strategies. The results
higher education setting. This study extends were analyzed for determining differences in
the current research of student retention by average responses between the two groups. In
examining the role of motivation and self-reg- addition to the 81 MSLQ questions, partici-
ulation in online course attrition and retention pants were asked to provide information
and is unique in that it measures levels of moti- regarding their enrollment status in the pro-
vation and self-regulation using the MSLQ gram (i.e., dropped or not dropped) by select-
with students who are or have been enrolled in ing from the list a statement that best describes
a distance education program at two different their status in the program. An open-ended
schools of higher education. While motivation question asked participants about their felt rea-
and self-regulation are known to be important sons for attrition or retention in the program.
factors in education success, this study mea-
sures those two factors with students who are Setting
currently enrolled in a distance education pro-
gram or have recently dropped out of one. The This study targeted distance education stu-
primary purpose of the study was to attempt to dents at two universities in the Midwest region
correlate levels of motivation and regulation of the United States. Both universities have a
with attrition or retention in two higher educa- residential face-to-face program and an equiv-
tion distance education programs and two dif- alent distance education program. Both
ferent student groups. schools are accredited private liberal arts uni-
This study was guided by the following versities offering 4-year degrees and a gradu-
research questions: ate program. One university has an enrollment
6 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

of 1,300 undergraduate and 230 graduate stu- task value), expectancy components (i.e., con-
dents. The other university has 2,200 under- trol beliefs and self-efficacy for learning and
graduate and 600 graduate students. Approval performance), and affective components (i.e.
to administer the survey was granted by the test anxiety).
individual school’s institutional review boards. Learning strategies are used by students to
self-regulate their personal processes, their
Participants environment, and their behavior. The learning
strategies scales consists of 31 items regarding
The study focused on students in the under- students’ use of different cognitive and meta-
graduate and graduate distance education pro- cognitive strategies (i.e., rehearsal, elabora-
grams at the selected schools and two groups tion, organization, critical thinking, and

S
of students were evaluated. A total of 347 stu- metacognitive self-regulation) and 19 items
dents in the enrolled category were contacted concerning student management of different

F
and a total of 162 in the dropped category were resources such as time and study environment,
contacted. Of the 509 students contacted, 22 effort regulation, peer learning, and help seek-

O
from the dropped category and 91 from the not ing. The questions use a Likert type scale rang-
dropped category participated in the study by ing from 1 (not at all true of me) to 7 (very true

O
completing the survey. Using a database of of me). The MSLQ was used in this study to

R 8
students provided by the schools, students determine levels of motivation and learning
were contacted via e-mail and regular mail

1
strategy use. The MSLQ questionnaire’s sub-

P
inviting them to participate in the study. The scales have high internal consistency. The

0
first group of students consisted of those who Cronbach’s alpha range from .52 to .93. Help

P 2
dropped a class beyond the allowable drop seeking scale had the lowest Cronbach’s alpha
date while enrolled in the program or had ( = .52) and self-efficacy had the highest

IA ©
dropped out of the program entirely. The sec- Cronbach’s alpha ( = .93) (Pintrich et al.,
ond group consisted of those who had not 1991).
dropped courses beyond the allowable drop
date and continue to be enrolled in the pro-
gram. RESULTS

A total of 347 students in the enrolled category


Instrument
were contacted and a total of 162 in the
The MSLQ is an 81 item, self-report instru- dropped category were contacted. Of the 509
ment (Appendix A) designed to measure col- students contacted, 22 from the dropped cate-
lege students’ motivational orientations and gory and 91 from the not dropped category
their use of different learning strategies (Pin- participated in the study by completing the sur-
trich, Smith, Garcia, & Mckeachie, 1993). The vey. Overall, there was a low rate of participa-
MSLQ uses a social-cognitive view of motiva- tion from both dropped and enrolled groups.
tion and self-regulation and directly links stu- However, the enrolled group had higher partic-
dents’ motivation to their ability to self- ipation rate than the dropped group. The data
regulate their learning activities. The MSLQ were examined to check if there were any
items are divided into two broad categories of missing scores. The results indicate that there
motivation scales and learning strategies were no missing scores in the data. Means and
scales. The motivation scales consist of 31 standard deviations for each the MSLQ sub-
items that assess value components (i.e., scales across retention groups are listed in
intrinsic and extrinsic goal orientations, and Table 1.
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 7

TABLE 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Each MSLQ Subscale Across Retention Groups

Dropped Not Dropped

MSLQ Subscale M SD M SD

Motivation
Intrinsic goal 5.534 0.907 5.635 0.882
Extrinsic goal 4.886 1.115 4.942 1.261
Task value 5.709 0.809 5.972 0.970
Control belief 5.000 0.883 5.412 0.993
Self-efficacy for learning and performance 5.426 0.859 5.906 0.749

S
Test anxiety 3.827 1.084 3.620 1.573

F
Learning Strategy
Rehearsal 4.341 1.538 4.264 1.393

O
Elaboration 4.977 1.099 5.249 0.924

O
Organization 4.114 1.447 4.646 1.221
Critical thinking 4.855 1.532 4.969 1.043

R 8
Metacognitive self-regulation 4.508 0.938 4.805 0.752

P 1
Time and study environment 5.173 1.336 5.460 1.074

0
Effort regulation 5.489 1.248 5.981 0.901

P 2
Peer learning 2.788 1.121 3.403 1.340
Help seeking 3.409 1.278 3.668 1.270

Motivation and Retention


IA ©
The Pearson r correlation was conducted to
assess the motivation variables that correlate
with retention in the distance education pro-
.036) and between peer learning and retention
(r = .185, p = .049). The results suggest that as
effort regulation and peer learning increases,
students’ retention in the distance program
also increases. There were no other significant
gram. The analysis indicated that there was a correlations found between the learning
significant positive correlation between self- strategy subscales and student retention (see
efficacy for learning and performance and Table 3).
retention (r = .241, p = .010), such that as the A Mann-Whitney U test, a nonparametric
self-efficacy scores increases, the retention in test was conducted to assess if there is a differ-
the distance education program also increases. ence between the dropped and enrolled groups.
There were no other significant correlations This test was chosen as an alternative to the t
found between the motivation subscales and test because of the two groups had unequal
student retention (see Table 2). sample size and were not normally distributed.
The Mann-Whitney U test is similar to the t
Learning Strategies and Retention test but it is more appropriate to be conducted
when the data do not meet the parametric
The correlation analysis indicated that there assumptions of the t test (McKnight & Najab,
was a significant positive correlation between 2010). The results indicated that the self-effi-
effort regulation and retention (r = .198, p = cacy score of the enrolled group (M = 5.906,
8 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

TABLE 2
Correlation Between Motivation Subscales and Retention

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Retention 1
2. Intrinsic goal 0.045 1
3. Extrinsic goal 0.018 0.135 1
4. Task value 0.111 0.632*** 0.242** 1
5. Control belief 0.167 0.337*** 0.014 0.283** 1
6. Self-efficacy for learning 0.241** 0.436*** 0.239* 0.410*** 0.435*** 1
and performance

S
7. Test anxiety –0.055*** -0.073 0.313** -0.05 -0.244 -0.337*** 1
Note: *p < .05, two-tailed; **p ≤ .010, two-tailed; ***p < .001, two-tailed.

O F TABLE 3
Correlation Between Learning Strategies and Retention

O
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

R 8
1. Retention 1

P 1
2. Rehearsal –0.022* 1

0
3. Elaboration 0.112 .440*** 1

P 2
4. Organization 0.165 .566*** .596*** 1
5. Critical think- 0.04 0.168*** .567*** .391*** 1

IA ©
ing
6. Metacognitive 0.149 .512*** .724*** .661*** .443*** 1
self-regulation
7. Time and study 0.101 .420*** .461*** .473*** 0.178 .604*** 1
environment
8. Effort regula- .198* .273** .479*** .380*** 0.091 .599*** .704*** 1
tion
9. Peer learning .185* .334*** .410*** .395*** .223* .372*** 0.152 0.111 1
10. Help seeking 0.081 .418*** .311*** .317** 0.062 .359*** .343*** .225* .566*** 1
Note: *p < .05, two-tailed; **p ≤ .010, two-tailed; ***p ≤ .001, two-tailed.

SD = .748) is significantly different from that such situation, conducting a binary logistic
of the dropped group (M = 5.426, SD = .859), regression analysis is more appropriate option
U = 634.00, p = .008). than conducting simple regression analysis. A
The goal of this study was to be able to pre- binary logistic regression using the forward
dict which students will drop out. The Mann- likelihood ratio (LR) method was conducted.
Whitney U test revealed that the two groups The chi-square test was significant (2(1) =
differed on the self-efficacy scores. However, 6.061, p = .014) which indicates that the new
a regression analysis is necessary to create a model is significantly better at predicting stu-
model that predicts student retention. The dent retention compared to the baseline model.
dependent variable (i.e., retention) has only The Hosmer and Lemeshow test had a signifi-
two outcomes (i.e., enrolled or dropped). In cance value greater than p =.05, which sug-
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 9

gests that the model is a good-fit with the data effort regulation and peer learning, subscales
(2(8) = 12.589, p = .127). The results indi- of learning strategy, were also found to be sig-
cated that self-efficacy was the only significant nificantly correlated with student retention.
predictor of student retention in the program ( This study poses significance from an instruc-
= .703, p = .015). The Exp(B) value associated tional design perspective since the motiva-
with self-efficacy is 2.021. This means that tional and self-regulation aspects can be built
with ever one unit increase in self-efficacy into the courses. For instance, good instruc-
score, the students are 2.021 times more likely tional design can build courses that are per-
to stay enrolled in the program. ceived as having a high task value, encourage
and motivate the students in their progress, and
build learning strategies into the course and
Reasons for Dropping and Continuing study work so that students can improve their

S
The stated reasons for dropping or not drop- performance as they work through the mate-
rial. In short, good instructional design can

F
ping give interesting insight into the motiva-
tions of the students. Of the 97 surveyed boost motivation and self-regulation (Ander-
man & Dawson, 2011).

O
participants who provided information about
their motivations for not dropping the pro- The two retention groups significantly dif-

O
gram, 67% (n = 65) stated that extrinsic goal fered from each other on the self-efficacy for
orientation was the reason they did not drop learning and performance score. The self-

R 8
the program. This shows that the main concern efficacy scale comprises two aspects—namely

1
is not the learning process itself (i.e., taking the expectancy for success and self-efficacy.

P
class and doing the work), but the means to an Expectancy for success alludes to performance

0
end, be it career, improvement, and grades, expectations. Self-efficacy refers to self-

P 2
among others. Of the 15 surveyed participants appraisal of one’s ability to master a given
who provided information about their motiva- task. It also refers to one’s ability to accom-

IA ©
tions for dropping the program, 33% (n = 5) plish a given task and to one’s confidence in
gave low task value as their reason to drop out. one’s skills to perform the task (Pintrich et al.,
Low task value refers to the student’s percep- 1991). Moore (1989) suggested that in a set-
tions of the program in terms of interest, ting where students and teachers are separated,
importance, and utility. According to the additional responsibility is placed on students
Binary logistic regression, self-efficacy was to evaluate and motivate their learning pro-
found to be a good predictor of student reten- cess. As the students progress through the
tion, whereas, according to student report, task learning process, they engage in what Bandura
value and extrinsic goal orientation were found (1976) and Zimmerman (1989) referred to as a
to be the reason for student retention and attri- reciprocal causation among three influence
tion in the program. Self-efficacy, task value, processes: personal, environmental, and
and extrinsic goal orientation fall under the behavioral. This process can lead to interpreta-
motivation scale category of the MSLQ. tions and conclusions by the students that have
a direct influence on their self-efficacy for
learning and performance. As Zimmerman and
DISCUSSION Martinez-Pons (1990) indicated, self-efficacy
beliefs influence achievement behaviors. Also,
This study revealed a statistically significant achievement has a reciprocal influence on self-
correlation between self-efficacy, a motivation efficacy. This study depicts significance of
subscale, and student retention. The felt rea- self-efficacy beliefs on retention in the online
sons given for attrition or retention (i.e., extrin- environment. Motivation and attitude are two
sic goal orientation and task value) were also key components of the learning and teaching
from the motivation category. Additionally, process. A negative reciprocal causation can
10 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

directly impact self-efficacy, a key component smaller (n = 22). It is possible that a larger
in distance education success (Morrison et al., sample size for both the dropped and not
2013). dropped could have yielded different results.
Effort regulation, a resource management Some limitations of surveying students who
strategy, refers to the students’ ability to con- have already dropped from a particular pro-
trol their effort and attention when faced with gram are the difficulty of contacting them,
distractions and uninteresting tasks (Pintrich et their unwillingness to participate in a survey
al., 1991). Bandura and Schunk (1981) indi- regarding a program they are no longer part of,
cated the importance of self-motivation in the and receiving survey results that may not accu-
distance education environment where the stu- rately reflect the reality of when they were
dents do not have external factors to motivate enrolled in the program and deciding to with-
them. The results of the present study indicate draw.

S
that students who are not able to self-regulate

F
their efforts are at a higher risk of dropping Implications for Future Research
out.

O
Peer learning, a resource management strat- Studies seeking to correlate motivated strat-
egy, involves collaborating and communicat- egies with retention and attrition may have a

O
ing with peers to clarify and to gain insights on better return of student feedback if they are
course materials (Pintrich et al., 1991). Moore surveyed at the beginning of their program and

R 8
(1989) listed learner-learner interaction as one near the end of the first semester while they are

1
still enrolled. The results could then be cor-

P
of the three significant interactions in the
related with future retention or attrition. One of

0
learning process. In this study, the correlation
analysis indicated that peer learning (i.e., stu- the difficulties of distance education attrition

P 2
dent-student interaction) is significantly cor- studies is getting quality feedback from those
who are no longer enrolled in the academic

IA ©
related with retention in online program. The
result therefore supports the findings reported program. Future research is needed to explore
by Moore (1989). the impact of the instructional design of a
This study, unlike the studies in the litera- course that intentionally seeks to show the
value of what was being learned, that encour-
ture review, uses a very specific definition of
ages and strengthens the students’ perception
academic success. It defines academic success
of their ability to do the course, and that rec-
as the improper dropping of courses or attri-
ommends several learning strategies such as
tion, and retention. This study directly
peer learning and time management as part of
addresses the issue of high attrition rate in dis-
the course.
tance education. This is one of the biggest
problems of distance education. The signifi-
cant correlation between retention and self- REFERENCES
efficacy for learning and performance, effort
regulation, and peer learning informs the Allen, I. E., & Seaman, J. (2007). Online nation five
instructional design process to minimize the ears of growth in online learning. Needham,
impact of the separation of the students from MA: Sloan-C.
the teacher and their fellow students. Anderman, E. M., & Dawson, H. (2011). Learning
with motivation. In R. E. Mayer & P. A. Alexan-
der (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning
Limitations of the Study and instruction (pp. 219–241). New York, NY:
Routledge.
The overall sample size of this study is Anderson, T. (2003). Getting the mix right again:
small (n = 113) and the number of dropped stu- An updated and theoretical rationale for interac-
dents who participated in the study was even tion. International Review of Research in Open
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 11

and Distance Learning, 4(2). doi:10.19173/ Patterson, B., & McFadden, C. (2009). Attrition in
irrodl.v4i2.149 online and campus degree programs. Online
Bandura, A. (1976). Social learning theory. Engle- Journal of Distance Learning Administration,
wood Cliffs, NJ: Erlbaum. 12(2).
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Gracia, T., &
competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic Interest McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A manual for the use
through proximal self-motivation. Journal of of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Ques-
Personality and Social Psychology, 41(3), 586– tionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbor, MI. National
598. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.3.586 Center for Research to Improve Post-secondary
Bell, P. D. (2006). Can factors related to self-regu- Teaching and Learning.
lated learning and epistemological beliefs pre- Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., &
dict learning achievement in undergraduate
McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predic-
synchronous web-based courses? Online
tive validity of the Motivated Strategies for

S
Research Journal Perspectives in Health Infor-
Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational
mation Management, 3(7).

F
and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801–
Bernard, R. M., Abrami, P. C., Borokhovski, E.,
813. doi:10.1177/0013164493053003024
Wade, C. A., Tamim, R. M., Surkes, M. A., &

O
Bethel, E. C. (2009). A meta-analysis of three Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Com-
types of interaction treatments in distance educa- petence and control beliefs: Distinguishing the

O
tion. Review of Educational Research, 79(3), means and the ends. In P. A. Alexander & P. H.
1243–1289. doi: 10.3102/0034654309333844 Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psy-

R 8
Carr, S. (2000, February 11). As distance education chology (2nd ed., pp. 349–368). Mahwah, NJ:

1
comes of age, the challenge is keeping the stu- Erlbaum.

P
dents. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. 7. Winters, F. I., Greene, J. A., & Costich, C. M.

0
Retrieved from http://Chronicle.com (2008). Self-regulation of learning within com-

P 2
Driscoll, M. P. (2005). Psychology of learning from puter based learning environments. Education
instruction (3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Psychology Review, 20, 429–444.

IA ©
Allyn & Bacon. Wittrock, M. C. (2010). Learning as a generative
Fozdar, B. I., Kumar, L. S., & Kannan, S. (2006). A process. Educational Psychologist, 45(1), 40–
survey of a study on the reasons responsible for 45. doi: 10.1080/00461520903433554
student dropout from the bachelor of science Zimmerman, B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regu-
programme at Indira Gandhi National Open Uni- lated learner: Which are the key subprocesses.
versity. International Review of Research in Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11(4),
Open & Distance Learning, 7(3), 1–15. 307–313.
Keegan, D. (1996). Foundations of distance educa- Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view
tion (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
of self-regulated academic learning. Journal of
Mayer, R. E. (2011). Towards a science of moti- Educational Psychology, 81(3).
vated learning in technology-supported environ-
Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning
ments. Educational Technology Research and
and academic achievement: An overview. Edu-
Development, 59(2), 301–308.
cational Psychologist, 25(1), 3.
Moore, M. G. (1989). Three types of interaction.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1990).
The American Journal of Distance Education,
3(2), 1–6. Student differences in self-regulated learning—
Morrison, G. R., & Anglin, G. J. (2006). An instruc- Relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-effi-
tional design approach for effective shovelware cacy and strategy use. Journal of Educational
modifying materials for distance education. Psychology, 82(1), 51–59. doi:10.1037/0022-
Quarterly Review of Distance Education, 7(1), 0663.82.1.51
63–74. Zimmerman, B. J., & Pons, M. M. (1986). Develop-
Morrison, G. R., Ross, S. M., Kalman, H. K., & ment of a structured interview for assessing stu-
Kemp, J. E. (2013). Designing effective instruc- dent use of self-regulated learning strategies.
tion (7th ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & American Educational Research Journal, 23(4),
Sons. 614–628.
12 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

APPENDIX A

Part 1: Motivation

Very
Not True
at All of Me

1. In a class like this, I prefer course material that really challenges me so I can learn 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
new things.
2. If I study in appropriate ways, then I will be able to learn the material in this 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
course.
3. When I take a test I think about how poorly I am doing compared with other 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

S
students.

F
4. I think I will be able to use what I learn in this course in other courses. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. I believe I will receive an excellent grade in this class. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

O
6. I’m certain I can understand the most difficult material presented in the readings 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
for this course.

O
7. Getting a good grade in this class is the most satisfying thing for me right now. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8
8. When I take a test I think about items on other parts of the test I can’t answer. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

R
9. It is my own fault if I don’t learn the material in this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

P 1
10. It is important for me to learn the course material in this class. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

0
11. The most important thing for me right now is improving my overall grade point 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

P 2
average, so my main concern in this class is getting a good grade.
12. I’m confident I can learn the basic concepts taught in this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

IA ©
13. If I can, I want to get better grades in this class than most of the other students. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
14. When I take tests I think of the consequences of failing. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
15. I’m confident I can understand the most complex material presented by the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
instructor in this course.
16. In a class like this, I prefer course material that arouses my curiosity, even if it is 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
difficult to learn.
17. I am very interested in the content area of this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
18. If I try hard enough, then I will understand the course material. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
19. I have an uneasy, upset feeling when I take an exam. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
20. I’m confident I can do an excellent job on the assignments and tests in this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
21. I expect to do well in this class. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
22. The most satisfying thing for me in this course is trying to understand the content 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
as thoroughly as possible.
23. I think the course material in this class is useful for me to learn. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
24. When I have the opportunity in this class, I choose course assignments that I can 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
learn from even if they don’t guarantee a good grade.
25. If I don’t understand the course material, it is because I didn’t try hard enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
26. I like the subject matter of this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
27. Understanding the subject matter of this course is very important to me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
28. I feel my heart beating fast when I take an exam. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
29. I’m certain I can master the skills being taught in this class. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 13

30. I want to do well in this class because it is important to show my ability to my 1 2 3 4 5 6 7


family, friends, employer, or others.
31. Considering the difficulty of this course, the teacher, and my skills, I think I will 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
do well in this class.

Part 2: Learning Strategies

32. When I study the readings for this course, I outline the material to help me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
organize my thoughts.
33. During class time I often miss important points because I’m thinking of other 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
things.
34. When studying for this course, I often try to explain the material to a classmate 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

S
or friend.

F
35. I usually study in a place where I can concentrate on my course work. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
36. When reading for this course, I make up questions to help focus my reading. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

O
37. I often feel so lazy or bored when I study for this class that I quit before I finish 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
what I planned to do.

O
38. I often find myself questioning things I hear or read in this course to decide if I 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
find them convincing.

R 8
39. When I study for this class, I practice saying the material to myself over and over. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

P 1
40. Even if I have trouble learning the material in this class, I try to do the work on 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

0
my own, without help from anyone.
41. When I become confused about something I’m reading for this class, I go back 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

P 2
and try to figure it out.

IA ©
42. When I study for this course, I go through the readings and my class notes and try 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
to find the most important ideas.
43. I make good use of my study time for this course. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
44. If course readings are difficult to understand, I change the way I read the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
material.
45. I try to work with other students from this class to complete the course 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
assignments.
46. When studying for this course, I read my class notes and the course readings over 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
and over again.
47. When a theory, interpretation, or conclusion is presented in class or in the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
readings, I try to decide if there is good supporting evidence.
48. I work hard to do well in this class even if I don’t like what we are doing. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
49. I make simple charts, diagrams, or tables to help me organize course material. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
50. When studying for this course, I often set aside time to discuss course material 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
with a group of students from the class.
51. I treat the course material as a starting point and try to develop my own ideas 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
about it.
52. I find it hard to stick to a study schedule. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
53. When I study for this class, I pull together information from different sources, 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
such as lectures, readings, and discussions.
54. Before I study new course material thoroughly, I often skim it to see how it is 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
organized.
14 The Quarterly Review of Distance Education Vol. 19, No. 3, 2018

55. I ask myself questions to make sure I understand the material I have been 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
studying in this class.
56. I try to change the way I study in order to fit the course requirements and the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
instructor’s teaching style.
57. I often find that I have been reading for this class but don’t know what it was all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
about.
58. I ask the instructor to clarify concepts I don’t understand well. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
59. I memorize key words to remind me of important concepts in this class. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
60. When course work is difficult, I either give up or only study the easy parts. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
61. I try to think through a topic and decide what I am supposed to learn from it 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
rather than just reading it over when studying for this course.

S
62. I try to relate ideas in this subject to those in other courses whenever possible. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
63. When I study for this course, I go over my class notes and make an outline of 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

F
important concepts.
64. When reading for this class, I try to relate the material to what I already know. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

O
65. I have a regular place set aside for studying. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

O
66. I try to play around with ideas of my own related to what I am learning in this 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
course.

R 8
67. When I study for this course, I write brief summaries of the main ideas from the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1
readings and my class notes.

P
68. When I can’t understand the material in this course, I ask another student in this 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

0
class for help.

P 2
69. I try to understand the material in this class by making connections between the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
readings and the concepts from the lectures.

IA ©
70. I make sure that I keep up with the weekly readings and assignments for this 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
course.
71. Whenever I read or hear an assertion or conclusion in this class, I think about 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
possible alternatives.
72. I make lists of important items for this course and memorize the lists. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
73. I attend this class regularly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
74. Even when course materials are dull and uninteresting, I manage to keep 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
working until I finish.
75. I try to identify students in this class whom I can ask for help if necessary. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
76. When studying for this course I try to determine which concepts I don’t 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
understand well.
77. I often find that I don’t spend very much time on this course because of other 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
activities.
78. When I study for this class, I set goals for myself in order to direct my activities 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
in each study period.
79. If I get confused taking notes in class, I make sure I sort it out afterwards. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
80. I rarely find time to review my notes or readings before an exam. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
81. I try to apply ideas from course readings in other class activities such as lecture 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
and discussion.
The Correlation of Self-Regulation and Motivation With Retention and Attrition in Distance Education 15

Please check which statement best describes your student status.


82. Which statement describes your current status as a student:

I have dropped courses beyond the allowable drop date or I have withdrawn (even for a brief period of time) from
the distance program.
I have not dropped courses beyond the allowable drop date and I continue to be enrolled in the distance program.
83. Please give a short answer describing your primary reasons either for continuing in the program (what keeps you
going), for dropping courses beyond the allowable drop date, or for withdrawing from the program.

FS
O O
P R 18
P 2 0
IA ©
Copyright of Quarterly Review of Distance Education is the property of Information Age
Publishing and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

Вам также может понравиться