Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 14

How Did It Feel for You?

Emotion, Narrative, and the Limits


of Ethnography
Andrew Beatty

ABSTRACT In this article, I present the case for a narrative approach to emotion, identifying conceptual and pre-
sentational weaknesses in standard ethnographic approaches. First-person and confessional accounts, increasingly
offered as a corrective to the distancing and typifying effects of cultural analysis, are shown to be unreliable; shared
experience turns out to be an illusion. Instead, I suggest we look to literary examples for lessons in how to capture
the full significance of emotion in action. Here, however, we reach the limits of ethnography.

Keywords: emotion, narrative, ethnographic writing, fieldwork, literature

H ow should we write about emotion? The answer, pre-


sumably, will depend in part on our conception of
what emotions are: Basic human functions underlying cul-
amples. For not only do emotions, in a quite obvious way,
belong to stories; they also build on, allude to, and echo
other emotions and events; they refer to interwoven lives.
tural difference or infinitely diverse ways of apprehending As such, their significances are not easily contained within
the world? Portable human attributes or fixtures of the cul- an illustrative and generalizing format. In two senses, then,
tural landscape? If the former, can I rely on personal ex- emotions possess a narrative aspect: they make sense within
perience for ethnographic insights; if the latter, can I ever a narrative sequence, and they “tell a story.” This claim ap-
really connect? To incline one way or the other is to imply plies as much to basic human experiences as to intricately
a particular presentational strategy, an ethnographic genre, plotted affairs of the heart. The most intense, immediate
perhaps even a way of doing fieldwork. It may also be the emotions—joy at the birth of a child, grief at a sudden
case, conversely, that the way we write about emotion limits death—derive their fullest meaning from complex personal
our possibilities of understanding. Without attempting here histories, even if they also possess a paradigmatic signifi-
a singular definition of emotion—a polythetic concept if ever cance, a pattern that can be grasped instantly by unfamiliar
there was one—the purpose of this article is twofold: (1) to others (Carrithers 1992:165–176). Such emotions are not
critique some widely used and long-standing paradigms in the creation of a moment; they participate in manifold rela-
the reporting of emotion and (2) to suggest the advantages tionships formed over periods of time. Our writing, if it is
and challenges of adopting a narrative approach. A linking to have any credibility, must reflect this complexity.1
theme concerns the analytical relevance of the ethnogra-
pher’s own emotions. Does my anger connect me with my ANTHROPOLOGICAL WRITING ABOUT EMOTION:
fieldwork hosts whose anger I seem to share but whose POLAR POSITIONS
culture I will never fully master? Or do my feelings mean The question of how to write about emotion has divided
something only against my own inescapable background, be- the closest of colleagues. Michelle Rosaldo saw emotions as
longing, as such, to a different form of life—indeed, to a “embodied thoughts” whose diversity of form “reflect[s] con-
different life? In writing about emotions in the field—our sistent differences in the organization of social life” (Rosaldo
own and others’—where should we draw the line? as quoted in Levy 1983:133). “Affects,” she wrote, “what-
A narrative approach to the emotions demands a con- ever their similarities, are no more similar than the societies
textual richness that overspills the tight frames of standard in which we live” (Rosaldo 1984:145). Her husband, after
“realist ethnographies” (Stoller 1989; Van Maanen 1988) her tragic death in a fieldwork accident, came to abandon
with their case histories and descriptive summaries. Indeed, this position and to emphasize the insights given by com-
to get a better sense of how emotions are entangled in mon, if not universal, factors in human experience. Renato
personal histories, we may be better served by literary ex- Rosaldo’s (1989) brave and influential essay “Grief and a

AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST, Vol. 112, Issue 3, pp. 430–443, ISSN 0002-7294 online ISSN 1548-1433. 2010
c by the American Anthropological
Association. All rights reserved. DOI: 10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01250.x
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 431

Headhunter’s Rage” brought into focus, almost for the first discourses of emotion. From being nowhere, “emotion”—
time—Jean Briggs (1970) was the pioneer—the interrela- like “the body” before it—is suddenly everywhere (Wilce
tions among emotions, fieldwork, and writing, showing that 2004). But thinking on the more basic process of how we
what we feel, how we think and write about it, and how it write about emotions felt and encountered in the field has
all happened are questions that cannot easily be separated. not kept pace. In the scramble for new territory, too much
Against the profession’s trademark relativism and the sep- has been taken for granted.
aration it implies, Rosaldo argues for the centrality of the If emotion is now ubiquitous in the human sciences and
anthropologist’s emotion and personal experience in mak- beyond, so too, of course, is narrative: this article is only
ing sense of others’ worlds. I shall argue—partly using his a small contribution to a long-established multidisciplinary
case—for the reverse. I suggest that the anthropologist’s endeavor (Bruner 1991; Ochs and Capps 1996; Peacock and
emotions in the field are mostly to do with the trials of Holland 1993; Tedlock 1991; Turner and Bruner 1986; Van
fieldwork: they do not (with rare exceptions) illuminate the Maanen 1988). In bringing together these two concerns, I
predicaments of our hosts. To the extent that our emotions claim no special novelty: I am building on the work of
do enter the swim and make sense within others’ lifeworlds, many scholars. Nevertheless, the question of how emotion
they cannot be captured in conventional ethnographic de- is related to narrative and how that relation bears on the way
scription; indeed, as I shall argue, in writing about emotion we write is less considered. It may be a truism to say that
we come up against the limits of ethnography. emotions are a part of narratives—what isn’t? It is much less
To find your way in the woods, sometimes you need to obvious that emotions tell a story of their own.
retrace your steps. In developing a skeptical position on what
can credibly be said about emotion and on whether our own THE HISTORICAL PARALLEL
feelings tell us anything useful, I shall backtrack to some fa- In advancing the case for a narrative approach to emotion,
miliar positions. How well do they stand up to the evidence? we can learn something from the debates in history about
How strong are their arguments? A standard recourse in whether narrative shape inheres in events or is something
disciplinary rethinkings, anthropological and other, is to re- imposed retrospectively on mere sequence.3 As ethnogra-
turn to the firm ground of personal experience. Following phers, we confront many of the problems tackled by histo-
postmodernist critiques, however, theoretical Square One rians (the perspective of narratologists like Barthes [1977]
was no longer available. The reflexive revolution in anthro- and Genette [1979] is less clearly relevant). But our profes-
pology, epitomized a quarter of a century ago in Writing sional advantage—personal familiarity with what and whom
Culture (Clifford and Marcus 1986), famously undermined we write about—shows that it is possible to steer a middle
the authority of classic ethnographies by drawing attention course between the opposing positions of realism and episte-
to the circumstances of their creation. Titles like Observers mological despair that have framed the history debates. One
Observed (Stocking 1983) and The Headman and I (Dumont can accept Louis Mink’s point that “narrative form in history,
1978) captured the disciplinary double take. Clifford Geertz, as in fiction, is an artifice, the product of individual imagi-
who took some of the blame as well as the credit, diagnosed nation” (Mink 2001:218), without accepting that temporal-
epistemological and moral hypochondria (1988:130–138). causal shape is entirely alien to history-as-lived. (And what
But if the impact of the debate was initially skeptical, form of words, verbal or written, present tense or past
once the critics had seized the stage, doubts were replaced historic, is not artifice? When has reality excluded imagina-
in some quarters by a restored faith in the authority of ex- tion?) Similarly, one can accept Hayden White’s point that
perience.2 Against the intricacies of symbolic interpretation “historical situations do not have built into them intrinsic
and the invisible interpreter, the voice from the field pre- meaning in the way that literary texts do” (White 2001:224)
sented a rhetorical challenge that recalled Dr. Johnson’s without accepting all his reasons and consequences. White’s
stone-kicking reply to the idealist: “I refute it thus!” Here, claim is plausible not because historical situations as they
in the theoretical whirlpool, was something like bedrock. In happen are meaningless but because the meanings of text
the opposite camp, cultural constructionism, often inflected and experience are of a different order and scale.
by an attention to political economy, retained a powerful David Carr’s influential defense of narrative history—I
position in the analysis of emotion (Lutz 1988; Lutz and quote his conclusions, not his reasoning—resonates strongly
Abu-Lughod 1990; White 1992). with ethnography:
However, the debate between those who would focus
Narrative has not merely an epistemological but also an ontological
on cultural difference (if only to expose its political ba- value. That is, it is not only a “cognitive instrument” as Mink
sis) and those who would transcend it ran out of steam. In claimed—a primary way of seeking, organizing and expressing
the last decade, anthropologists working on emotion have our knowledge of a part of reality. It is constitutive of our very
forged ahead, tracing the role of emotion in such fields as being, it is our way of existing, of constituting ourselves. [Carr
2001:198]
political violence, religious piety, ethnic affiliation, and kin
making. A great deal has been learned about the reach of The return of the story has been good news for anthro-
emotions into diverse spheres of life and the mutual en- pology. Indeed, the narrative turn in medical anthropology
gagement of institutions and actors through emotion and and “trauma studies” builds substantially on the historians’
432 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

insights (e.g., Antze and Lambek 1996; Kleinman 1989; 1989:13). Because the force of emotions is muted in gen-
Mattingly and Garro 2000). However, other than in a few eralizing, culturalist approaches, the symbols-and-meanings
relatively specialized publications—and in psychoanalytical ethnographer lacks empathy and therefore insight. More rad-
accounts that address a quite different agenda (Crapanzano ically, Rosaldo contends that without common experience
1992; Mimica 2007)—ethnographers have generally under- there can be no understanding (1989:7–8). The scales fell
rated the historical emplotment of emotions.4 This is odd from his eyes when, following his wife’s tragic death in a
because if narrative, in Carr’s (2001:198) extended sense, fieldwork accident, he abandoned his search for a cultural
applies to “our very being . . . our way of existing,” it ought explanation of headhunting. Experience had revealed the
to apply especially to our emotional lives and to the emo- truth in Ilongot explanations: they go headhunting to work
tions that we share with others. The ethnographer, unlike off the rage associated with bereavement. Anger was not
the historian, is on the spot and ought to profit from this sit- merely a cultural pretext for violence, as he had assumed; it
uation. But with respect to emotions, his position is tricky. was the aftershock of loss (Rosaldo 1989:8–10).
The transition from life-as-lived to life-as-written poses a Plainly, at a certain level, Rosaldo’s thesis must be true.
difficulty that the historian does not have to face, because Experience shapes perception, the young cannot really know
ethnographers—actually, not just imaginatively—are part what it is to be old, nor the tall the perspective of the short—
of the story. How much a part is a moot point. Once we and so on through all the human types, male and female, gay
acknowledge the deep emplacement of emotions, their en- and straight, black and white, wise and foolish. To under-
tanglement in stories, plots, and pasts involving significant stand another’s emotion we must have experienced some-
others, we cannot rely on our own emotions for insights into thing like it ourselves.5 But the argument is self-limiting. For
the emotions of people living very different lives. Against its if common experience is necessary for ethnographic under-
author’s intention, “Grief and a Headhunter’s Rage” nicely standing, only the reader who has had a life like Rosaldo’s
illustrates the point, so let me interrupt the case for nar- will be capable of accepting his point.
rative with a discussion of the strongest counterexample. The argument from experience is encapsulated in Ros-
I choose this text with some hesitation, given its sensitive aldo’s notion of the “positioned subject,” which seems to have
content. But two considerations—apart from its benchmark two aspects: (1) the subject’s place within a structure and (2)
status and the decent interval that has passed since its first the vantage afforded by the observer’s stock of generic life
publication—permit criticism: first, Rosaldo’s own icono- experiences. Positioning is synchronic: a matter of status,
clasm, his penchant for no-holds-barred deconstruction of structure, and analogy. As Rosaldo explains, “Rather than
the classics; second, his public use of a personal experience to speaking of death in general, one must consider the subject’s
further theoretical argument. My criticisms, like his own— position within a field of social relations in order to grasp
and with due respect for personal feelings—accordingly con- one’s emotional experience” (1989:2); furthermore, “only
cern argument, presentation, and theoretical claims. after being repositioned through a devastating loss of my own
could I better grasp that Ilongot men mean precisely what
EMOTIONS IN CONTEXT—BUT WHOSE CONTEXT? they say when they describe the anger in bereavement as the
Rosaldo’s manifesto-like essay (it doubles as introduction to source of their desire to cut off human heads” (1989:3). What
his polemical book, Culture and Truth, published in 1989) matters is not how one got to a certain point—the details
has been described as “the most celebrated work on affect of the story, all the things that make it personally significant
of the last generation” (Johnson and Michaelsen 2008:60). for the individual involved—but one’s having got there, the
Cited in hundreds of works, it continues to inspire debate structural position and the life stage one has reached. Narra-
(for the latest example, see Ethos 2008). An established clas- tive serves to reveal parallels between author and informants
sic, then; but its first publication in 1984 came at a critical rather than to thicken description and add historical depth.
moment in the human sciences, appearing to turn the tide Rosaldo is disarmingly explicit on this point. “My use of
against culturalist approaches in favor of a more engaged and personal experience serves as a vehicle for making the qual-
personal ethnography. In making his case, Rosaldo contrasts ity and intensity of the rage in Ilongot grief more readily
what he calls the “force” of emotions, accessible only to his accessible to readers than certain more detached modes of
style of testimonial anthropology, with the more impersonal composition” (Rosaldo 1989:11). But if personal experience
“cultural depth” offered by interpretivism, writing: “My ef- is useful in understanding others, its usefulness surely de-
fort to show the force of a simple statement taken literally pends on relevance, closeness of fit; and relevance, in turn,
goes against anthropology’s classic norms, which prefer to depends on the historical particularities—in a word, the
explicate culture through the gradual thickening of webs of story.
meaning” (1989:2). For Rosaldo, the Geertzian approach Far from home, Rosaldo puts his faith in what he knows
cannot capture real emotion because it typologizes, pre- or rather in what he feels. It is shock that compels him to ac-
ferring the collective, anonymous displays of ritual to the cept as simple truth Ilongot statements that they take heads
everyday, particular experiences of the individual. He cites because they are angry over the deaths of their kinsmen.
an example that “masks the emotional force of bereavement And rather than persuade us (a story persuades by revealing
by reducing funerary ritual to orderly routine” (Rosaldo connections and causes), he batters us into submission. You
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 433

thought emotions were cultural constructions? They are all of these emotions in the wedding dispute, takes more than
about force, according to Rosaldo: “the emotional force of empathy and knowledge of cultural context. It requires an
death” (1989:2), “the force of anger in grief” (1989:3), “the attention to character and circumstance that goes well be-
force of the dilemma faced by the Ilongots” (1989:4), “the yond the ethnographic vignette.
overwhelming force of rage possible in such grief” (1989:9), Narrative density is, of course, what we expect from
“the cultural force of rage” (1989:16), and “the cultural a novel since a novelist—at least in the realist tradition—
force of emotions” (1989:19). Behind the battery of as- works by accumulating details of character-in-action. The
sertions, different kinds of experience, different sorts of ethnographer, bound as she is to fit cases to arguments (ar-
feeling, are being equated, particularities erased. Some ex- guments that transcend the context), cannot do this. Nor
amples seem more like sorrow than rage. A man’s con- do her fieldwork predicaments, although real enough, pro-
version to Christianity following the death of his child is vide much insight into the very different predicaments of
cited, implausibly, as “the force of the grieving man’s de- others. Her feelings toward her fieldwork hosts have no
sire to vent his rage” (1989:5). People listening to a taped past, or their past is rooted in relations with other, quite
lament seem to express sorrow, regret, and frustration, different people back home.7 If she wants to write about
not rage; but rage is Rosaldo’s preferred term. Words are emotion in the ethnographic mode, she is therefore faced
used here to obscure rather than illuminate or differentiate with a dilemma: stick to her own case and admit the lim-
experiences. itations of ethnographic reporting or venture dangerously
But there is another perspective, a way into the motiva- outside them. The case for an anthropology of emotion that
tions of headhunters that is less obvious but more plausible depends on a reflexive or confessional stance collapses. As
than recourse to panhuman experience. Why do they do it? Rosaldo’s example unhappily shows, our emotions lose their
The ideal of head taking, the quest for vengeance, the taunts explanatory power away from home—they belong to dif-
of peers, the desire for prestige, and the years of mourning ferent narratives—and the appeal to experience, the “only
prohibitions that Rosaldo describes are among the pressures connect” thesis, must fail.
building the anger that slaughter releases. The fact that these Although the possibilities of mistaken empathy or pro-
pressures are culturally defined rather than rooted in per- jection are diminished in that their experiences more closely
sonal experiences does not make them any less psychologi- resemble those of their hosts, the situation of anthropolo-
cally real; nor does it make them a culturalist’s projection. In gists working “at home” is not in principle different. They
contrast, the fixing of headhunters’ rage in past grief (some- mostly operate outside their habitual circle, among farmers,
times generations old) may, paradoxically, be a cultural fic- factory hands, or stockbrokers—cultural and social others—
tion, an alibi for the immediate provocations of violent rage. not really at home. Or, as their writing focuses inward and
Rosaldo’s personal story linking his own grief with rage does approaches autobiography or family biography, it ceases to
not convincingly translate to the Ilongot because their expe- do the things expected of ethnography, turning into memoir
riences are too different. What his argument reveals is the (see Narayan 2007).
need for a genuinely narrative approach: a building of charac- Yet insights can still be had and the self made relevant.
ter, circumstance, motivation, and action. Yet such a narra- The risk of misleading analogy demonstrated by Rosaldo
tive would fatally expose the differences between the anthro- is neatly sidestepped in a recent set of articles that draw
pologist and the people he studies. If Rosaldo persuades us of on personal experience. In a moving discussion of the life
the weakness of a generalizing interpretivism, his remedy—a history of her mother, a Polish Catholic caught up in the
variant of what Geertz (2000:59) calls “the return of Human Holocaust, Barbara Rylko-Bauer introduces the notion of
Nature”—fails to convince. What his evidence calls out for is “intimate ethnography, where the personal and the emo-
a more precise, circumstantial account that shows how emo- tional suffuse the work at all levels” (2005:12). The an-
tions are justified by experience—narrative, not cultural, thropologist’s own relatives become informants and the
depth.6 mixed feelings called up by efforts to excavate life histo-
By reading between the lines, one sees the alternative ries serve as clues to “how social forces become embod-
case, not so much for the proof of experience but, con- ied as individual experience” (Waterston and Rylko-Bauer
trariwise, for the evocative power of narrative. I want to 2006:409). Linked essays by Philippe Bourgois (2005) and
pursue this case here because my argument is that a con- Alisse Waterston (2005) on similar themes probe the dif-
vincing account of emotions—a proper appraisal of their ficulties attending intimate ethnography: the complex ne-
significance—depends on an attention to narrative context. gotiation of roles, the construction of a “truthful” account,
We witness a dispute at a wedding, displays of anger and and so on. What legitimizes the project in each case—and
jealousy. But only when we are familiar with the background contrasts it with Rosaldo’s trickier notion of empathy based
stories, the dispositions of people and their long interrela- on analogous experience—is the authors’ insider status as
tions, can we really grasp what is going on. (It is in this children of their informants, their necessary involvement in
sense that emotions can be said to “have a story” as well as the reconstitution of personal history. Only when the stories
belonging to stories.) To understand why the bridegroom’s are made to address broader themes do we feel the power of
jealousy provokes his sister’s anger, and to read the effects the original testimonies weakened as memories are distilled
434 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

to familiar lessons about dehumanization and structural social currents. It would be wrong to look here for detailed
violence. eyewitness reports of emotional episodes. The Ethnos con-
tributions, for example, are concerned with “cultural log-
ETHNOGRAPHIC APPLICATIONS: WHAT CAN BE ics,” discourses, and “performative definitions”: emotions
DONE on a bigger stage. Since their aims are theoretical, report-
As these examples of “intimate ethnography” demonstrate, ing leads away from personal experience and emotions-in-
to address the limitations of ethnographic writing is not, action to discussions of political economy and “emotional
by any means, to reject all anthropological discussion of landscapes.”
emotion (still less, of course, is it to damn ethnography
generally). Indeed, some of the best recent work attests
to the theoretical advantages of considering emotion in a EMOTIONS IN FOCUS
broader analytical frame. Nancy Scheper-Hughes (2007), But what about those full-length studies that prioritize emo-
for example, writes of a “political economy of emotions” tion, such as “person-centered ethnographies”? With the
in South Africa. Focusing on the painful transition from scope to depict and describe, surely they can keep a steadier
apartheid, she shows how specific emotions were deliber- eye on the actuality, on the people inhabiting the emotional
ately engaged—even mandated—in the process of national landscapes? Surprisingly, no. In two notable and in other
healing staged by the Truth and Reconciliation Commission. respects excellent specimens of the genre, long sections
At the hearings, the free flow of appropriate emotions was devoted to emotion contain no examples of actual occur-
encouraged as a step toward redemption and the search for rences witnessed by the authors (Hollan and Wellenkamp
a shared experiential truth. 1994:107–123; Parish 1994:190–230). The ethnographic
Emotion and memory are implicated in other recent material consists solely of responses to questions such as
studies of political subjectivity. In a powerful ethnography, follows: “What is the relationship of hopelessness with de-
Nicolas Argenti (2008) shows how the “unspeakable vio- sire?” (Parish 1994:229); “What happens inside you when
lence” of West Africa’s slave-trading past finds ambiguous you have pastae?” (Parish 1994:224); or “Can you remem-
expression in youth masquerades that mingle terror with ber a time when you were really angry?” (Hollan and
hilarity, at once acknowledging and attacking the age hierar- Wellenkamp 1994:116). Replies take the form of remi-
chies that perpetuate the old inequities. On a related theme, niscences, explanations, descriptions of “hypothetical sce-
Marus̆ka Svas̆ek, distinguishing between emotions remem- nario[s]” (Parish 1994:206), or verbal “recreations” of events
bered and reexperienced, shows how collective trauma (Hollan and Wellenkamp 1994:110). Parish’s respondents
lives on in the commemorations of Sudeten Germans who are mostly anonymized and generic (e.g., “one high-caste
were expelled from wartime Czechoslovakia. The “chosen Newar said . . .” [1994:208]). In both books, the authors’
trauma” of postwar generations does not equate to the suffer- summary statements take the form of context-free, present-
ing of their parents but is made similar by their “identification tense generalizations: “Toraja believe that if one avoids get-
as co-victims” (Svasek 2005:202). Narratives of loss, terror, ting angry . . .” (Hollan and Wellenkamp 1994:119); “In
and nostalgia for the homeland generate feelings of distress the Toraja view of emotions . . .” (Hollan and Wellenkamp
that unite young with old and serve the political ends of the 1994:110); “For Newars, a key moral emotion is lajya”
imagined community in the quest for restitution. (Parish 1994:199); “What Newars know, as moral beings,
In a special issue of Ethnos that explores the positioning of is mediated by this pain and fear” (Parish 1994:215). The
emotion in the globalizing political economies of Southeast lack of concrete examples can give this kind of ethnography
Asia, Tom Boellstorff (2004) shows how malu (roughly, a secondhand, uncertain feel: “a spouse’s unfaithfulness may
shame) is triggered among Indonesian Islamists by the claims evoke feelings of intense shame and anger and possibly lead
of gays to a public voice in civil society. Malu leads to violent to violence . . . However, our sense is that fantasies and talk
rage, as Islamist youths feel their masculinity to be under about violence in connection with adultery are much more
threat. The acts of puritan violence that follow mark a “sea common than actual occurrences” (Hollan and Wellenkamp
change” from the tolerant (if heterosexist) Suharto era to a 1994:116).
visceral politicized homophobia. In the same Ethnos issue, Although person-centered ethnography can, with some
Johan Lindquist shows how prostitutes and migrant workers justice, claim to give us the “native point of view”—or at
in the industrial zone of Batam overcome an inhibiting malu least the elicited native point of view—it violates the other
by the apparently opposite means of recourse to drugs or Malinowskian axiom of testing word against practice. Parish
wearing the veil. By veiling or taking ecstasy, the pious and seems aware of the objection and asserts that “we should not
the not so pious are able to manage malu and thereby “engage ignore the ways emotion is embedded in action and practice
with, rather than withdraw from, a new kind of world and in ways that go beyond what cultural theories of emotion say”
its contradictions” (Lindquist 2004:503). (1994:215). Indeed, in his conclusion (Parish 1994:278), he
These diverse studies escape my criticism because they affirms that “culture is lived—it is embodied in experience,
are not primarily concerned with the predicaments of indi- action, and life. It emerges in lives”—a conception of culture
viduals or the nature of emotion but, rather, with broader to which interview-based ethnography seems poorly suited.
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 435

Robert Levy’s Tahitians (1973), the original person- status, or role” (Jackson 1998:33). I would only add that the
centered ethnography, is, happily, packed with people; yet complexity of practice emerges more clearly in narratives
his most influential essay on the subject contains not a single of action than in locally made stories serving quite different
account of an emotion episode. Characteristically of an- ends. In a later book, In Sierra Leone (2004), Jackson com-
thropological theorizing of emotion, it offers only a brief, bines personal memoir with the biography of a politician and
hypothetical case in which a man is abandoned by his wife friend. The deep familiarity, the glancing reflections among
(Levy 1984:219). Equally influential (and admirable in its parallel lives, give an unusual resonance to the text that calls
own way), Catherine Lutz’s constructionist study Unnatu- to mind the meandering, hypnotic prose of W. G. Sebald,
ral Emotions (1988) contains, as far as I can discover, only whose novels Jackson took with him to the field. It’s no de-
one emotion episode witnessed by the author that extends traction of Jackson’s fine book to say that his subject’s story
over more than a couple of lines (Lutz 1988:125–127). The is several steps away from the living moment, shaped, as it
episode concerns a child’s death—not the sort of incident is, by the passage of time and the conventional framing of a
that requires much narrative context or attention to the in- big man’s public career.
flections of interpersonal history. We learn a great deal about Ruth Behar’s Translated Woman (2003) is another bi-
how Ifaluk talk about emotion or think about emotion during ography in progress, a retelling of the informant’s stories
interviews but not how their emotions interlock in reality, against the changing feelings and perspectives of intersect-
how emotions shape and are shaped by personal histories. ing lives. Again, the long familiarity between ethnographer
Unnatural, indeed. Unless we assume that talk about emo- and informant—in this case a Mexican peddler—leads to
tion is emotion itself—and the discourse approach comes an understanding of how narrative shapes a life. It does not
close to this—we are missing something here. lead to—does not claim—any special insight into the ways
What these diverse approaches—person centered, ex- in which emotion is linked to action or character. The action
periential, constructionist, and let’s add, for historical is all across the table of Behar’s kitchen; character is such
completeness, functionalist, structuralist, and Culture and as emerges in words and stories; emotions are remembered
Personality—have in common is a limited temporal context rather than recaptured in their original complexity. Field-
or, rather, a conception of context that pans swiftly outward work memoirs do not, as a rule, attempt to portray complex
to general factors of ideology, power, cultural framing, and chains of living emotion, unless the emotion be that of the
generic experience rather than deeper into individual stories: ethnographer.
wide angle rather than long focus. In most cases, the con- Alma Gottlieb and Philip Graham’s entrancing dou-
text is immediate and reducible to generalities, the approach ble narrative, Parallel Worlds (1993), presents a kind of bil-
broadly synchronic—as if the actors, like the ethnographer, dungsroman detailing the growth of an anthropologist’s and
were fresh to the scene. What we are getting is half the a writer’s mind. Immersion, enquiry, professional guilt,
picture, less than half the story. understanding, and withdrawal: these are the processes il-
luminated in their dialectical tale. In one striking passage
ETHNOGRAPHIC LIVES: THE SEARCH FOR (1993:303–304), Graham intuits a correspondence that calls
COMMON GROUND to mind the book’s title: the maker of fiction gives voice to
The complaint that ethnography diminishes its object is a creatures of his imagination just as the Beng diviner bodies
familiar one. “Under his gaze,” writes Michael Jackson of forth invisible spirits. But this is an insight into the creative
his fictional fieldwork persona, “the vital, elusive, and uni- process and the possibility of intellectual connection rather
fying qualities of existence shriveled and died” (1986:122). than a stroke of empathy. In the memoir’s double vision
Perhaps emotion, that most vital and elusive of qualities, of counterparts, husbands and wives, anthropologists and
is the most easily betrayed. Yet the answer is not simply writers, emotion is vividly present throughout, but it is the
better writing. As Jackson demonstrates, the irreducibility fieldworkers’ emotions and dilemmas that most compel the
of experience poses a challenge that is epistemological and reader.
moral as well as aesthetic. His solution is to focus on the These studies are significant achievements. We should
ingredients of experience that link ethnographer and infor- not blame authors for something they are not attempting.
mant, and much of his work interweaves personal reflection But the contrast between what can be done by the opposed
with a vivid rendition of the ethnographic moment. In this methods of ethnography and literary narrative is perhaps best
perspective, the minutiae of fieldwork take on a different captured by another example. Who could deny that Scheper-
dimension, opening out toward broader human concerns Hughes’s (2001) ethnography of Irish madness powerfully
rather than sideways toward comparative theory building. illuminates the peculiar bad faith of the Irish farming fam-
Working among African storytellers, Jackson draws on local ily, the constricted emotions, oppressive pieties, and half-
tellings to make his points about the performative recasting understood economic forces that once shaped and deformed
of experience in words, the open-endedness and potential- rural families? But to find out what it feels like to live in such
ities of narrative. He argues—and I agree with him—that a family, to apprehend the living emotion, we must turn
“praxis is always a vital and indeterminate relationship with to the fiction writer and memoirist John McGahern (2005,
givenness, a going beyond, a surpassing of one’s situation, 2006), whose prickly rural predicaments, suitably enough,
436 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

resist summary. To say this is to accept that different goals to the ethnographer’s entrapment between experience and
and perspectives lead to distinctive achievements. Ethnog- expression. In Primo Levi’s memoir of his imprisonment
raphy and literary narrative have complementary strengths and liberation from Auschwitz, the narrator emerges from
and limitations. So what can one learn from the other? the liberated camp with feelings of relief, sadness, dismay,
hope, exhilaration, and exhaustion (Levi 2000:218–223). If
A NARRATIVE APPROACH TO EMOTION: we have managed to read this far—the pain of the narrative
LESSONS OF THE MASTERS mitigated by the brilliance of the prose—we know what he
Writers have explored these problems for centuries and means; we know that these theoretically distinct sentiments
ought to have something to teach us. They are not ethnog- are all powerfully present at the same time. Indeed, the
raphers; they don’t have to explain; but like ethnographers cumulative force of the account makes them seem inevitable.
they need to convince, and one of the ways they do this is Levi is fond of strings of emotion words, usually placed
through the evocation and portrayal of emotion. So much at points of transition, and one comes to recognize them as
could be said here, but perhaps the most obvious thing to a kind of signature: they always have about them an aptness,
note is that, in fiction, the balance of narrative and emotional a conclusive flourish. On his long way back to Italy, this is
payoff is quite different from that of even the most personal how Levi and a fellow traveler part company:
ethnography.
We left each other without many words: but at the moment of
In Les Misérables (Hugo 1933[1862]), at the climactic farewell, in a fleeting but distinct manner, I felt a solitary wave of
moment when Jean Valjean unmasks himself in court and friendliness towards my Greek, streaked with tenuous gratitude,
takes the place of a condemned man accused in his name, contempt, respect, animosity, curiosity and regret that I should
we are told nothing of his emotions. Instead, simply this: not see him again. [Levi 2000:268]

He was very pale, and trembled slightly. His hair, already grey Having followed the narrator, having seen what it takes to
when he came to Arras, was now perfectly white. It had become survive and even thrive in the chaotic aftermath of liberation,
so during the hour that he had been there. [Hugo 1933:267]
we can take this listing of six emotions as both exact and true.
Valjean’s agonized examination of conscience; his desper- Levi has so vividly portrayed his relation to “the Greek” that
ate, reluctant, 40-page journey to Arras; his terror of being the evidence of the reader’s own mixed feelings confirms
revealed; the moral imperative of owning up; his despair the parting judgment. Each named emotion scores a hit and
at the prospect of returning to the galleys; and not least produces a subtle moral shift, encoding a series of complex
the reader’s growing anxiety all make this miraculous, ter- evaluations. In the world of the camps, where language has
rible transformation—within the conventions of Romantic been debased and mere words have become hollow and
fiction—entirely credible. Described impersonally from the imprecise, emotions still retain an interpretative fit, a hold
outside, the effect is all the greater. In the crucible of the on reality.
courtroom, we are not told what Valjean is feeling: we But if words lose their grip, so, pushed to the limit,
know it. The story has brought us there. Yet—it hardly can emotions. When, at the long-awaited approach of the
needs stressing—the circumstances are entirely outside our Russians, a guard calls for the camp to be evacuated, Levi
experience. writes:
Or consider Gabriel Conroy at the end of James Joyce’s
The news excited no direct emotion in me. Already for many
great story, “The Dead,” as he contemplates the sleeping months I had no longer felt any pain, joy or fear, except in
figure of his suddenly estranged wife: that detached and distant manner characteristic of the Lager,
which might be described as conditional: if I still had my former
He wondered at his riot of emotions of an hour before. From what sensitivity, I thought, this would be an extremely moving moment.
had it proceeded? From his aunt’s supper, from his own foolish [Levi 2000:182]
speech, from the wine and the dancing, the merry-making when
saying good night in the hall, the pleasure of the walk along the Again, the deadening, oppressive detail makes the condi-
river in the snow. [1944:254]
tionality of emotion here at once a terrible discovery and
In this paragraph, Joyce explains his method. The intense something entirely comprehensible. Without ever leaving
pathos of the scene, its painful irony, derives from the accu- the resources of ordinary language, including ordinary emo-
mulation of happy but trivial incidents preceding the stun- tion words, Levi has shown us something about the human
ning revelation by Gabriel’s wife of an earlier lost love. The condition that we did not know or perhaps had only dimly
psychological plausibility of Gabriel’s baffled desire and his suspected. And, amazingly, he has done so from the depths
feeling of annihilation depends on the slow build up of inci- of circumstances utterly remote from the experience of most
dent and character. Again, the story has amply justified the readers.
emotions. As an ethnographer, one of many things that impress
Without the artifice of a contrived plot, the author me about Levi’s writing is the work that he makes emotions
of nonfiction may still rely on the cumulative power of do in a narrative: they define and shape experiences, sum
narrative to evoke emotion—and on emotion, in turn, to up situations, characterize persons, and conclude sections
color and shape narrative. Consider an example rather closer of narrative, showing us where we are, existentially, in a
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 437

story. And of course, they are powerful markers of authorial never tried to evoke it. His method, dogmatically structural,
presence, even when they are only, as in the last example, would not allow it. Real people and actual emotions would
“conditional.” For Levi, then, emotions are not added onto only complicate things. And this has been the pattern ever
experience, hovering over the real thing: they are the sub- since. For most of the history of anthropology, what one
stance and possibility of experience. As such, they are as observed in the field was to be seen through, like a veil, to an
hard edged, palpable, and concrete as the physical world; underlying reality—whether of form, function, structure,
perhaps, in the grey limbo of the camps, even more so. or text. You went to the field, sweated and suffered, and
Emotions are signs of life but also instruments of analysis, thought of higher things. “Being there” was really “being
lenses through which we perceive the world. somewhere else.” Participant-observation, or at least the
Can we learn from this conjunction of emotion and cir- writing up, entailed a curious renunciation of the life around
cumstance? Or is the contextual use of emotion to evoke you, a kind of methodological asceticism.
and evaluate off-limits to ethnography? I suggest that it Recent attempts to put people and their emotions back
is not possible to render the emotional dimension of ex- into ethnography have not been entirely successful. The vi-
perience convincingly without giving emotion its proper gnettes and capsule dialogues that feature in most modern
narrative due—something conventional ethnographies have ethnographies (including my own) as often as not fall a little
rarely done. Instead, they are apt to misrepresent emotions flat. Once again, it is the method rather than the skill of the
by highlighting only one or other aspect—of language, feel- author that constrains credibility. Wheeled on to illustrate an
ing, tactics, or cultural meaning—and therefore risk turning argument, counter someone else’s generalization, or, worst
people into caricatures, bearers of difference, social con- of all, add color, the people can seem two dimensional, “flat”
structions. This is not a mere effect of writing; it implies a rather than “round” (in E. M. Forster’s [1988:73] terminol-
misrecognition of emotion in the field. As Anthony Cohen ogy), emphatically making the point for their author or just
has remarked—and his comments apply as much to emo- too odd for the unprepared reader.8 We neither know nor
tion as to the notion of “selfhood” that he is defending—“by care who they are, so we cannot be moved by their stories.
failing to extend to the ‘others’ we study a recognition of Having complained about too little reality, we now find
the personal complexity which we perceive in ourselves, we ourselves wanting less of it and skip straight to the analysis.
are generalizing them into a synthetic fiction which is both Invidious to cite examples (let Beatty 1992 serve the pur-
discredited and discreditable” (Ingold 1996:29). pose); but contrast, for their evocative power and precision,
John Berger’s Pig Earth (1979), Amitav Ghosh’s In an Antique
EMOTION IN ETHNOGRAPHY: PURGING OF THE Land (1992), Carlo Levi’s Christ Stopped at Eboli (1989), and
REAL Paul Stoller and Cheryl Olkes’s novelistic In Sorcery’s Shadow
How did this simplification happen? It began with the pruning (1987).
of what Bronislaw Malinowski (1922) calls “the imponder- Now, the recourse to vignettes might be said to signal
abilia of everyday life” in quest of functions and models. (The an enrichment of the genre, an advance on the streamlined
word was Malinowski’s coinage; but the OED definition of classics. But in its own way, it is more misleading. The
“imponderable” indicates the problem: “a factor that is diffi- illustrative, rather than analytical, style gives emotions a
cult or impossible to estimate or assess.”) Despite our ten- paradigmatic force and objectivity they do not possess. A
dency to contrast the products of field and library, it is a fact market transaction or a wedding speech can be sampled to
that the most highly prized ethnographies have been the least offer insights into economy or gender. But emotions are
“imponderable”—and, in that sense, the least ethnographic. more elusive. Unlike prices and wedding etiquette, they are
Naven (Bateson 1936) and Political Systems of Highland Burma personal and biographical as well as shared; they are of the
(Leach 1954), for example, are interesting solely for their moment but reference the past; they are “in here” as well
theoretical daring, not for their ethnographic exuberance. as “out there.” Mostly they fall outside the ethnographic
Even such classics as E. E. Evans-Pritchard’s The Nuer (1940), frame.
offered to generations of students as a model of ethnogra-
phy, are valued mainly for their expository dazzle—but at WHY EMOTIONS ELUDE ETHNOGRAPHY
the expense of ethnographic detail, of light and shade, of The problem is that emotions have a particularity that is
contrary evidence. It is as if Evans-Pritchard, like a sculptor, uncongenial to ethnography. In writing about the field, we
arrived at his elegant form by a technique of taking away, choose examples for their broader significance within some
a process that is the very opposite of the slow, haphazard theoretical perspective. The people in our case histories are
accumulation of detail that goes into the production of field in unwitting dialogue with the people in other ethnographies;
notes. Only the famous introduction gives us a glimpse of they are never merely themselves. However individuated,
the rough cut. they are types, figures in a larger story that is not their
Part of what gets taken away, of course, is the emotional own. Otherwise, why write about them? James Clifford
quality of social life: the imponderabilia. In Naven, Gregory (1986:104) has commented on this “insistent tug toward the
Bateson had attempted to schematize the patterning of Iatmul general,” and anyone writing today cannot but be aware of
emotions. But although he gave us the concept of “ethos,” he the problem.
438 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

With emotions we reach an irreducible core: the tug is Nor can the antidote be Rosaldo’s brute emotivism, the ap-
resisted, the link snaps. Embedded as they are in biography, peal to one’s own sentimental education. Both approaches
circumstantial but historical, emotions resist ethnographic rest on a failure to grasp the personal-historical complex-
formulation; their particularity defies abstraction. Not that ity of emotions, their grounding in interwoven stories and
we haven’t tried. Claude Lévi-Strauss’s (1968) atom of kin- characters.
ship, with its positive and negative signs denoting contrast- Briggs is highly unusual in bringing off the difficult feat
ing attitudes among kin types, may be the ultimate in the of marrying a clear-eyed analytical perspective on emotion
emotional reductionism: call it Abstract Expressionism. But with the contextual and narrative detail usually found only in
Lévi-Strauss built his atoms from equally schematic accounts realist fiction. And anyone who returns to her ethnography
of hostile brothers and indulgent uncles in the classic ethno- cannot but marvel at the careful plotting of motivations and
graphies. These stereotypes have long passed into anthropo- moods; the minute attention to shades of implication; the
logical folklore, but accounts of generic relationships remain scrupulous distinguishing of observed, attributed, named,
a staple of ethnography, linking, for example, contemporary and hypothetical emotions; and the narrative linking of emo-
person-centered studies with the older, less avowedly hu- tional episodes (how one incident recalls and builds on oth-
manistic approaches of functionalism and structuralism. We ers) and of these with character and cultural expectation.
learn to recognize such behavioral patterns in the field, but But Briggs’s frigid field—“twenty-one people in an expanse
they provide only a rough orientation, and they are not of thirty-five thousand or more square miles, their near-
enough. Even supposing relations between a father and son est neighbours several days’ travel distant” (1970:16)—is
in a certain society are typically cool, the reaction of one to equally exceptional. It resembles less the crowded, porous
the death of the other will be unpredictable, nonstandard in field sites of most ethnographies than the spot-lit scenery of
whatever makes that particular relation humanly interesting. the imagination, its eight households, and fluctuating pop-
The warmth concealed in the reserve, the gestures of rec- ulation of “twenty to thirty-five” approximating the cast of
ompense, the relief that accompanies sorrow: these tell the the average novel.
fuller story. And it is this diversity of human types, circum- A parallel case—as wonderfully achieved, as different as
stances, and histories that gives emotions their distinctive sand from snow—is Lila Abu-Lughod’s Bedouin ethnogra-
hue, their interpretative precision, their social and subjec- phy (1986, 1993). Once again, a shortlist of names affords a
tive significance. Jean Briggs, long ago, showed the way in depth of characterization and cumulative incident that makes
her portrait of an Eskimo family (1970). Of her complex reported emotions both credible and communicable to the
relation to her adoptive father, she writes: reader. In her first book, Veiled Sentiments (1986), Abu-
Lughod hit on a means of connecting shared cultural forms
But though I was convinced that the behaviour that, in Inuttiaq,
I defined as “manipulative” and “autocratic” was both real and
with individual histories and the ordinarily inaccessible realm
Eskimo in its general shape, I am still unsure whether in some of feeling by focusing on the use of oral poetry in everyday
elusive manner Inuttiaq infused the Eskimo patterns with his own discourse. “Ghinnawa,” she writes, “can be considered the
assertive spirit, whether in some subtle ways he went too far in poetry of personal life: individuals recite such poetry in spe-
his dominance or exerted on me more pressure than others did to cific social contexts, for the most part private, articulating in
be submissive. [1970:67]
it sentiments about their personal situations and closest re-
And in a long chapter devoted to Inuttiaq, as well as in lationships” (Abu-Lughod 1986:31). And in Writing Women’s
countless telling details, she provides us with enough to Worlds (1993), Abu-Lughod uses a sequence of stories that
make a judgment for ourselves. subvert generalization (“writing against culture” [1993:25])
A theatrical analogy. To respond to a Hamlet solilo- to achieve an emotional conviction and realism not normally
quy, you need a knowledge of English—a familiarity with available to ethnography.
the conventions of Elizabethan theater, with the world What Briggs, Abu-Lughod, and Stoller (1989) show is
of castles and courts and so on. But the meaning of the that a narrative approach allows us to grasp the humane sig-
speech lies not in its exemplary nature, as an illustration of nificances that define the experience of emotion. An aware-
courtly intrigue, stagecraft, or the Oedipus complex, but ness of emotion in narrative context brings to light the con-
in the revelation of character in action, the motivations, tradictions and conflicts that people experience in their social
and ruminations of Hamlet at that moment in the plot. All life, their not fitting, their resistance or unwilling capitula-
the rest is background. In considering emotion, we have tion to social pressures, their abrasions with reality, their
got background and foreground reversed—trained, as we struggles for meaning. For the same reason, narrative works
are, to read significance in general forms, in the paradig- against a relativism that would encompass emotion within
matic instead of in particulars. But the remedy, the an- culture—depersonalizing it—as if nothing escapes the cul-
tidote to theoretical abstraction or functionalist thinness, tural embrace (Abu-Lughod 1993). A narrative approach
cannot be ever-thicker description, which merely takes leaves opaque what resists social analysis; it acknowledges
us deeper into hidden social factors, cultural frames, and the irreducible; it does not force an answer.
symbolism—the “cultural depth” that Rosaldo questions— If this sounds like the remedy we are seeking, let me be
but tells us nothing about the specific content of emotion. clear: in these very respects (and as the exceptions show),
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 439

narrative departs from and is opposed to most styles of to bumbling announcements of village news when a preacher
ethnography as commonly practiced. took the stage. He was a soldier, and he had a new, forthright
message, promising hell to backsliders and compromisers.
THE NARRATOR AND THE ETHNOGRAPHER: TWO He denounced local leaders for their laxity, youths for aping
OF A KIND? Westerners, and even schoolgirls for wearing trousers. The
These reflections are prompted by the experience of writing, harangue provoked unwonted cheers from the pious, frowns
for a general readership, a fieldwork account several years among the rest: here was a division unseen since the political-
after publishing an ethnography covering the same people religious massacres of the 1960s. At the back, the headman,
and events (Beatty 1992, n.d.). One book, written with a dark with emotion (fury, shame, frustration?), was scribbling
theoretical aim, was based on field notes; the other, written notes. Should he confront his opponent (and be devoured,
to tell a story and to relive the personal encounter, is based as I thought, pleading against intervention) or submit quietly
on diaries and tapes. One offers an explanation; the other in the Javanese way and fix things later behind the scenes?
an evocation. Although they correspond in externals, they When the preacher ordered us to stand and the congregation
could have been written by different people—as, in a sense, rose, the VIPs remained deep in their sofas; but at a signal
they were. I don’t disavow my hard-won ethnography nor from a new haji (Mekka pilgrim), an agitator for reform,
could I have attempted the second book without it. But I they too stood. The headman, protesting and the last to
wonder about the balance sheet: whether a loss of theoretical move, got weakly to his feet, muttering that he was dizzy.
impulse and ethnographic focus isn’t at the same time a gain And then, in the aftermath, he collapsed, subsiding into a
in realism—a gain, to be more specific, in emotional realism. long, unexplained illness that lasted through two attempts
The writer of a narrative, whether fictional or not, to oust him as leader and saw the unraveling of the cultural
confronts certain technical, strictly literary, problems: how compromise that he personified.
much space to give the narrator and her changing understand- To explain—even to identify—the headman’s puzzling
ing; how to reveal character through plot; how to render emotions is, necessarily, to tell the story. But behind the
the emotional feel of a situation. The author abroad has, scenario sketched above lies the much longer story of his
additionally, to worry about the greater burden of explana- battle with those who would sweep away mysticism, spirit
tion required, the need to keep a proper sense of alienness cults, and the easy relations between the sexes; the contest
without alienating the reader, and the desire to present the with his cousin (the haji) who had brought political Islam to
culture through the people and the action (a variation on the the village; and his own checkered career as an easygoing and
character-and-plot equation). Taken together, these techni- not terribly effective leader. A summary gives an impression;
cal problems are key to the credible construction of emotions a case-study analysis (exemplifying rival cultural paradigms,
in narrative. syncretism vs. puritanism, changing styles of leadership, or
For the ethnographer, in contrast, questions of charac- Javanese ethnopsychology) would give us the stage furniture;
ter, pace, and emotional tone are secondary or irrelevant. but it would take a proper narrative to do the story and the
At any rate, in writing about the Indonesian island of Nias emotions justice (for an attempt, see Beatty 2009).
the first time around I was not conscious of these issues; A similar conclusion was forced on me as I began a
I was only aware that in pursuing general problems I had narrative account of my earlier fieldwork in Nias and set
failed to convey what mattered to people as individuals. The about trying to restore what, in the published ethnography
ethnography had gotten in the way. (Beatty 1992), I had systematically excluded: the emotional
This is not a repetition of the old complaint that ethno- life that connected the characters with one another and me
graphies don’t tell the story of the field: many now do. It’s a with them. As I began After the Ancestors (Beatty n.d.), I had
more radical criticism that ethnographic writing, by design, no remedy in mind, but what I fastened on was a sense
gets emotion wrong. The methods—analytical, linguistic, of predicament: the predicaments of individual characters.
experimental, or illustrative—systematically filter out what In a place of stark poverty and profligate feasting, paupers,
for actors is of principal significance: namely, history, char- and overreachers, what concerned people most was not the
acter, implication, strategy, and plot. Let me make the point technicalities of kinship or swidden cultivation—these are
with an example from a later fieldwork in a different part mostly settled matters—but the great questions of life and
of Indonesia. (Since I have come out against capsule exam- death, injustice and suffering, morality and salvation in a
ples, I am in the unusual position of hoping mine will fail to world no longer tribal but not yet modern, a world “after
convince.) the ancestors.”
In 1997, in a rice-growing village in East Java, about 200 These moral questions crystallized in particular dramas:
people—a mix of nominal Muslims, orthodox pious, and the loss of an heir, a murder, rivalry for leadership, an op-
syncretists devoted to a Hinduized mysticism—had gathered portunity to swindle, a rejected marriage proposal, a funeral
in the mosque to celebrate the Prophet’s birthday. Behind the hijacked by missionaries. Unlike structure, habitus, mem-
rows of kneeling faithful, at the back among the VIPs, I sat on ory, agency, or the native point of view, these predicaments
a sofa next to the headman. Gentle exhortations to piety and are observable, dramatic, evolving, and highly revealing.
sermons sounding the tolerant Javanese note had given way And they are the stuff of emotion. When the new account
440 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

began to take shape, it was precisely the emotional qual- we can’t write truthfully about the field until we can leave
ity of the predicaments that came through. In animating those feelings behind.
the characters and reviewing the events, it was my own
half-forgotten feelings of anger, guilt, and amusement that CONCLUSION
guided me in how to construct certain scenes and that illu- Ethnographers have trouble with emotions: with recognizing
minated their personal meaning. Yet the story belongs to the them in strange places ( judging what they are, where they
people: the author’s feelings are largely irrelevant. You sit begin and end) but especially with how to write about them.
nervously through your host’s family quarrel, but unless you There is no priority in this difficulty: the writing problem
are involved, your own feelings are immaterial. You wait contributes to the recognition problem. As my examples
for the dying chief to expire, but your feelings are not those have shown, when anthropologists have explicitly focused
of his kinsmen; nor do you need to feel sad in the same way on emotion, they have tended to avoid detailed description
to recognize their sadness. Although your feelings provide and particularly time depth: the interwoven past relations
some small insight, they are not the story. Only when field- that give feelings their precise circumstantial fit. (Vague
work itself is the focus do the narrator’s emotions become anxieties reflect a vague apprehension of past relations.)
of pressing relevance. Ethnography (in all the schools considered) moves swiftly
But there is another reason for sounding this skeptical outward to general factors, robbing emotions of the personal
note, and it has less to do with how we report than how significance that is—as most authors would agree—their
we feel. In the field, one of the last things we come to essence.
understand is other people’s emotions, and it takes a good The simplest remedy for this failing, recourse to the
deal of experience to begin to relate to our hosts in an ethnographer’s own experience, remains deeply problem-
emotionally sophisticated way. This is not just because it atic. More technical approaches through discourse, proto-
takes time to learn the idiom of emotion but also because types, case histories, and potted biographies can only be a
our knowledge of characters and interwoven histories is preparation, serving to return us to the greater complex-
very slowly constructed. More than this, we ourselves have ity of the natural setting. As the few exceptions indicate,
undergone a transformation. only a narrative approach—because it locates emotion in
Fieldwork is less like two years of a life than a life in practice; in the indivisible flow of action, character, and
two years. And if, as the cliché goes, the early stages reprise history—can reveal the dimensions of emotion hidden by
childhood, then we also grow up in the field: we become other methods. There is nothing very new in this claim.
competent adults, and our feelings about doing fieldwork— Novelists have known it for centuries. But as ethnographers
about being there—mature. By the end, we hardly credit we have still—most of us—to learn the lesson.
what once bothered us; we see things in the round; we have
come through. To write about the past, to recreate the trials
Andrew Beatty Department of Anthropology, School of Social
of fieldwork, is therefore not to reexperience those field-
Sciences, Brunel University, Uxbridge, Middlesex UB8 3PH, U.K.;
work emotions, except in a trivial sense. Our feelings about
Andrew.Beatty@brunel.ac.uk
what happened are informed by everything that happened
later in the field, by everything we now know, and by the
different person that we have become. The evocation of feel-
ings is an exercise in imaginative recovery: a fiction but one NOTES
based in fact. Depending on how diligently and candidly we Acknowledgments. My thanks to Mercedes Garcia de Oteyza
kept our diaries, the facts may be in some sense recoverable. for her suggestions. I am indebted to four AA reviewers for their
Depending on our imagination and memory, we may be able painstaking comments and to AA editor-in-chief Tom Boellstorff for
to project ourselves back into that frame. But the narrator, his detailed criticisms. Their suggestions have helped greatly in the
the “I,” is the instrument of fiction, neither me-now nor shaping of this article.
me-then but a creation of the text, a bridge to the reader, 1. Goodwin and Goodwin (2001) argue for an understanding of
not a real person. He is there to lend credibility, to show emotions within the tight frames of “situated interaction,” but
where the story came from, to show the limits of what could their examples—a hopscotch game and dialogue in the family
be observed. of a man who can say only four words—are exceptionally
To say this much is to suggest that, however we choose constrained. Their article draws on a conception of emotion
to write about the field, our remembered feelings don’t as an internal process that is “made visible” (2001:253) in
matter for the written account in the way they mattered at interaction. For a criticism of this idea, see Beatty 2005.
the time. They had practical consequences for the alliances 2. Renato Rosaldo’s (1989) work best illustrates this pendulum
and enmities we made and for their part in forming us as swing; see also Wikan 1992.
fieldworkers: they were undoubtedly formative. But they 3. For key scholarly statements, see Roberts 2001. Thinking on
were often irrelevant to what we wanted to know. And they these issues goes back a long way. Morson reminds us that
are colored by our later, more mature judgments. Indeed, “in War and Peace Tolstoy constantly contrasts the neatness
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 441

of narrative accounts with the messiness of lived events” 2009 A Shadow Falls: In the Heart of Java. London: Faber and
(2002:213). Faber.
4. The one-to-one clinical encounter and the ethnographic- N.d. After the Ancestors. Unpublished MS, Department of An-
psychoanalytic interview are remote from the emplotment of thropology, School of Social Sciences, Brunel University.
emotion in action. As such, they systematically misrepresent Behar, Ruth
naturally occurring emotion. Because they sample one story 2003 Translated Woman: Crossing the Border with Esperanza’s
to tell another, always finding significance elsewhere, they are Story. Boston: Beacon.
poor models for a narrative approach. By “historical emplot- Berger, John
ment of emotions,” I mean the way in which emotions refer 1979 Pig Earth. London: Writers and Readers Publishing Co-
back to interwoven personal histories. I do not mean broader operative.
changes in structures of feeling or emotional discourses, which Boellstorff, Tom
require a very different kind of historical treatment. 2004 The Emergence of Political Homophobia in Indonesia: Mas-
5. Goldie (2000:33) distinguishes two senses of “knowing what culinity and National Belonging. Ethnos 69(4):465–486.
someone is feeling”: (1) recognizing the “paradigmatic narra- Bourgois, Philippe
tive structure” of an emotion—its typical action context and 2005 Bringing the Past into the Present: Family Narratives of
expression (this is close to Carrithers’s [1992] argument); and Holocaust, Exile, and Diaspora. Anthropological Quarterly
(2) knowing what it is like to have that feeling. Rosaldo conflates 78(1):89–123.
the two. Briggs, Jean L.
6. Rosaldo’s interest in narrative is directed less to particular 1970 Never in Anger. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
stories and sequences than to “other people’s narrative analyses” Bruner, Jerome
(1989:147) and alternative modes of story construction—for 1991 The Narrative Construction of Reality. Critical Inquiry
example, in Ilongot hunting stories. 18(1):1–21.
7. “Transference” is, again, a problem having to do with the prac- Carr, David
tice of fieldwork, part of our baggage; the insights it provides, 2001 Getting the Story Straight: Narrative and Historical Knowl-
doubly unreliable. It does not help us understand the jealous edge. In The History and Narrative Reader. Geoffrey Roberts,
sister at the wedding, even if it gives us that illusion. Some phe- ed. Pp. 197–208. London: Routledge.
nomenologically and psychoanalytically inclined work (e.g., Carrithers, Michael
Devereux 1967; Jackson 1998) analyzes this predicament but 1992 Why Humans Have Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University
does not offer an escape from my general criticism. Press.
8. “The test of a round character is whether it is capable of sur- Clifford, James
prising in a convincing way. . . . It has the incalculability of life 1986 On Ethnographic Allegory. In Writing Culture: The Po-
about it—life within the pages of a book” (Forster 1988:81). etics and Politics of Ethnography. James Clifford and George
Marcus, eds. Pp. 98–121. Berkeley: University of California
REFERENCES CITED Press.
Abu-Lughod, Lila Clifford, James, and George Marcus, eds.
1986 Veiled Sentiments: Honor and Sentiment in a Bedouin So- 1986 Writing Culture: The Poetics and Politics of Ethnography.
ciety. Berkeley: University of California Press. Berkeley: University of California Press.
1993 Writing Women’s Worlds: Bedouin Stories. Berkeley: Uni- Crapanzano, Vincent
versity of California Press. 1992 Hermes’ Dilemma and Hamlet’s Desire. Cambridge, MA:
Antze, Paul, and Michael Lambek, eds. Harvard University Press.
1996 Tense Past: Cultural Essays in Trauma and Memory. Devereux, George
London: Routledge. 1967 From Anxiety to Method in the Behavioral Sciences. The
Argenti, Nicolas Hague: Mouton.
2008 The Intestines of the State: Youth, Violence, and Belated Dumont, Jean-Paul
Histories in the Cameroon Grassfields. Chicago: Chicago Uni- 1978 The Headman and I. Austin: University of Texas Press.
versity Press. Ethos
Barthes, Roland 2008 Special issue, “Whatever Happened to Empathy?” C. Jason
1977 Introduction to the Structural Analysis of Narratives. In Im- Throop and Douglas Hollan, eds. Ethos 36(4).
age, Music, Text. Stephen Heath, trans. Pp. 79–124. London: Evans-Pritchard, E. E.
Fontana. 1940 The Nuer. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Bateson, Gregory Forster, E. M.
1936 Naven. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1988[1927] Aspects of the Novel. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Beatty, Andrew Geertz, Clifford
1992 Society and Exchange in Nias. Oxford: Clarendon Press. 1988 Works and Lives: The Anthropologist as Author. Stanford:
2005 Emotions in the Field: What Are We Talking About? Journal Stanford University Press.
of the Royal Anthropological Institute 11:17–37. 2000 Available Light. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
442 American Anthropologist • Vol. 112, No. 3 • September 2010

Genette, Gérard Lindquist, Johan


1979 Narrative Discourse: An Essay in Method. Ithaca, NY: Cor- 2004 Veils and Ecstasy: Negotiating Shame in the Indonesian
nell University Press. Borderlands. Ethnos 69(4):487–508.
Ghosh, Amitav Lutz, Catherine A.
1992 In an Antique Land. New York: Vintage. 1988 Unnatural Emotions: Everyday Sentiments on a Microne-
Goldie, Peter sian Atoll and Their Challenge to Western Theory. Chicago:
2000 The Emotions: A Philosophical Exploration. Oxford: University of Chicago Press.
Clarendon Press. Lutz, Catherine A., and Lila Abu-Lughod, eds.
Goodwin, Marjorie H., and Charles Goodwin 1990 Language and the Politics of Emotion. Cambridge: Cam-
2001 Emotion within Situated Activity. In Linguistic Anthropol- bridge University Press.
ogy: A Reader. Alessandro Duranti, ed. Pp. 239–257. Oxford: Malinowski, Bronislaw
Blackwell. 1922 Argonauts of the Western Pacific. London: Routledge and
Gottlieb, Alma, and Philip Graham Kegan Paul.
1993 Parallel Worlds: An Anthropologist and a Writer Encounter Mattingly, Cheryl, and Linda Garro
Africa. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 2000 Narrative and the Cultural Construction of Illness and Heal-
Hollan, Douglas W., and Jane C. Wellenkamp ing. Berkeley: University of California Press.
1994 Contentment and Suffering: Culture and Experience in McGahern, John
Toraja. New York: Columbia University Press. 2005 Memoir. London: Faber and Faber.
Hugo, Victor 2006 Creatures of the Earth: Selected Stories. London: Faber and
1933[1862] Les Misérables. Charles Wilbour, trans. London: Faber.
Dent. Mimica, Jadran, ed.
Ingold, Tim, ed. 2007 Explorations in Psychoanalytic Ethnography. Oxford:
1996 Key Debates in Anthropology. London: Routledge. Berghahn.
Jackson, Michael Mink, Louis O.
1986 Barawa and the Way Birds Fly in the Sky. Washington: 2001 Narrative Form as Cognitive Instrument. In The History
Smithsonian Institution Press. and Narrative Reader. Geoffrey Roberts, ed. Pp. 211–220.
1998 Paths toward a Clearing. Bloomington: Indiana University London: Routledge.
Press. Morson, Gary
2004 In Sierra Leone. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. 2002 Conclusion: Reading Dostoevskii. In The Cambridge Com-
Johnson, David E., and Scott Michaelsen panion to Dostoevskii. W. J. Leatherbarrow, ed. Pp. 212–234.
2008 Anthropology’s Wake: Attending to the End of Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bronx, NY: Fordham University Press. Narayan, Kirin
Joyce, James 2007 My Family and Other Saints. Chicago: University of Chicago
1944[1914] The Dead. In Dubliners. James Joyce, author. Press.
Pp. 199–256. London: Jonathan Cape. Ochs, Elinor, and Lisa Capps
Kleinman, Arthur 1996 Narrating the Self. Annual Review of Anthropology 25:19–
1989 The Illness Narratives: Suffering, Healing, and the Human 43.
Condition. New York: Basic Books. Parish, Steven M.
Leach, E. R. 1994 Moral Knowing in a Hindu Sacred City: An Exploration
1954 Political Systems of Highland Burma. London: Athlone. of Mind, Emotion, and Self. New York: Columbia University
Levi, Carlo Press.
1989[1947] Christ Stopped at Eboli. New York: Noonday. Peacock, James L., and Dorothy C. Holland
Levi, Primo 1993 The Narrative Self: Life Stories in Process. Ethos 21(4):367–
2000 If This Is a Man and The Truce. Stuart Woolf, trans. 383.
London: Everyman. Roberts, Geoffrey, ed.
Lévi-Strauss, Claude 2001 The History and Narrative Reader. London: Routledge.
1968 Structural Analysis in Linguistics and in Anthropology. Rosaldo, Michelle
Structural Anthropology, vol. 1. Pp. 31–54. London: Allen 1984 Toward an Anthropology of Self and Feeling. In Culture
Lane. Theory. Richard A. Shweder and Robert A. Levine, eds. Pp.
Levy, Robert I. 137–157. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1973 Tahitians: Mind, and Experience in the Society Islands. Rosaldo, Renato
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1989 Grief and a Headhunter’s Rage: On the Cultural Force of
1983 Introduction: Self and Emotion. Ethos 11(3):128–134. Emotions. In Culture and Truth. Pp. 1–21. Boston: Beacon.
1984 Emotion, Knowing, and Culture. In Culture Theory. Rylko-Bauer, Barbara
Richard A. Shweder and Robert A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 214– 2005 Lessons about Humanity and Survival from My Mother and
237. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. from the Holocaust. Anthropological Quarterly 78(1):11–41.
Beatty • Emotion, Narrative, and Ethnography 443

Scheper-Hughes, Nancy White, Geoffrey M.


2001 Saints, Scholars, and Schizophrenics: Mental Illness in Rural 1992 Ethnopsychology. In New Directions in Psychological An-
Ireland. Berkeley: University of California Press. thropology. Theodore Schwartz, Geoffrey M. White, and
2007 Violence and the Politics of Remorse: Lessons from South Catherine A. Lutz, eds. Pp. 21–46. Cambridge: Cambridge
Africa. In Subjectivity: Ethnographic Investigations. J. Biehl, University Press.
B. Good and A. Kleinman, eds. Pp. 179–233. Berkeley: Uni- White, Hayden
versity of California Press. 2001 The Historical Text as Literary Artifact. In The History
Stocking, George W., Jr. and Narrative Reader. Geoffrey Roberts, ed. Pp. 221–236.
1983 Observers Observed: Essays on Ethnographic Fieldwork. London: Routledge.
History of Anthropology, vol. 1. Madison: University of Wikan, Unni
Wisconsin Press. 1992 Beyond the Words: The Power of Resonance. American
Stoller, Paul Ethnologist 19(3):460–482.
1989 The Taste of Ethnographic Things. Philadelphia: University Wilce, J. M.
of Pennsylvania Press. 2004 Passionate Scholarship: Recent Anthropologies of Emotion.
Stoller, Paul, and Cheryl Olkes Reviews in Anthropology 33:1–17.
1987 In Sorcery’s Shadow. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Svas̆ek, Marus̆ka FOR FURTHER READING
2005 The Politics of Chosen Trauma. In Mixed Emotions. Kay (These selections were made by the American Anthropologist editorial
Milton and Marus̆ka Svas̆ek, eds. Pp. 195–214. Oxford: Berg. interns as examples of research related in some way to this article. They do
Tedlock, Barbara not necessarily reflect the views of the author.)
1991 From Participant Observation to the Observation of Par-
ticipant: The Emergence of Narrative Ethnography. Journal of Pasqualino, Caterina
Anthropological Research 41(1):69–94. 2007 Filming Emotion: The Place of Video in Anthropology.
Turner, Victor, and Edward Bruner Visual Anthropology Review 23(1):84–91.
1986 The Anthropology of Experience. Urbana: University of Sharman, Russell Leigh
Illinois Press. 2007 Style Matters: Ethnography as Method and Genre. Anthro-
Van Maanen, John pology and Humanism 32(2):117–129.
1988 Tales of the Field: On Writing Ethnography. Chicago: Uni- Simon, Gregory M.
versity of Chicago Press. 2008 Shame, Knowing, and Anthropology: On Robert I. Levy
Waterston, Alisse and the Study of Emotion. Ethos 33(4):493–498.
2005 The Story of My Story: An Anthropology of Vio- Simonelli, Jeanne
lence, Dispossession, and Diaspora. Anthropological Quar- 2007 The Active Voice: Narrative in Applied and Activist
terly 78(1):43–61. Anthropology. Anthropology and Humanism 32(2):156–
Waterston, Alisse, and Barbara Rylko-Bauer 170.
2006 Out of the Shadows of History and Memory: Personal Wierzbicka, Anna
Family Narratives in Ethnographies of Rediscovery. American 2003 Emotion and Culture: Arguing with Martha Nussbaum.
Ethnologist 33(3):397–412. Ethos 31(4):577–600.

Вам также может понравиться