Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Journal of Apicultural Research

ISSN: 0021-8839 (Print) 2078-6913 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tjar20

Multilevel assessment of grooming behavior


against Varroa destructor in Italian and
Africanized honey bees

Ciro Invernizzi, Ignacio Zefferino, Estela Santos, Lucía Sánchez & Yamandú
Mendoza

To cite this article: Ciro Invernizzi, Ignacio Zefferino, Estela Santos, Lucía Sánchez &
Yamandú Mendoza (2016): Multilevel assessment of grooming behavior against Varroa
destructor in Italian and Africanized honey bees, Journal of Apicultural Research, DOI:
10.1080/00218839.2016.1159055

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00218839.2016.1159055

Published online: 11 May 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tjar20

Download by: [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] Date: 12 May 2016, At: 09:47
Journal of Apicultural Research, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00218839.2016.1159055

ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE


Multilevel assessment of grooming behavior against Varroa destructor in Italian and
Africanized honey bees
Ciro Invernizzia*, Ignacio Zefferinoa, Estela Santosa, Lucı́a Sáncheza and Yamandú Mendozab
a
Sección Etologı´a, Facultad de Ciencias, Iguá 4225, CP 11400, Montevideo, Uruguay.; bLaboratorio de Apicultura. Instituto Nacional de
Investigación Agropecuaria.(INIA), Ruta 50 km11, CP 70000, Colonia, Uruguay
(Received 9 July 2013; accepted 15 June 2014)

The ectoparasitic mite Varroa destructor is one of the main plagues of honey bees Apis mellifera. Grooming behavior is a
resistance mechanism through which parasitized bees can dislodge mites by themselves (autogrooming) or by the
action of other bees (allogrooming). The objective of this study was to evaluate grooming behavior in Italian (A. m. ligus-
tica) and Africanized (hybrids of A. m. scutellata) bees at the individual, group, and colony levels. Firstly, five behaviors
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

were recorded observing bees individually placed on a Petri dish and after placing a mite on their thorax. Secondly, 30
bees of each colony were placed in a Petri dish along with 20 mites and 24 h later fallen mites were counted. Lastly,
the proportion of injured mites collected in the hive floor was determined. At the individual level, Africanized bees
showed a higher total number of reaction behaviors to V. destructor than did Italian bees (U = 182.5; p = 0.02). Groups
of Italian bees could dislodge 60.8 ± 20.0% of mites and Africanized bees dislodged 65.9 ± 15.6% of mites, without
showing significant differences (t = 0.735; p = 0.47). Colonies of Africanized bees showed a higher proportion of injured
mites (29.0 ± 8.6%) than colonies of Italian bees did (17.7 ± 9.8%) (t = 2.92; p = 0.009). Africanized bees are character-
ized by presenting higher resistance to V. destructor than European bees. This study shows that such difference can be,
partly due to grooming behavior. The importance of auto and allogrooming regarding resistance to V. destructor is dis-
cussed.

Evaluación a varios niveles del comportamiento de limpieza contra Varroa destructor en abejas de la
miel africanizadas e italianas

El ectoparásito Varroa destructor es una de las principales plagas de las abejas melı́feras Apis mellifera. El comportamiento
de grooming es un mecanismo de resistencia con el que las abejas parasitadas pueden desprenderse de los ácaros por
sı́ mismas (autogrooming) o por la acción de otras abejas (allogrooming). El objetivo de este estudio fue evaluar el
comportamiento de grooming en abejas italianas (A. m. ligustica) y africanizadas (hı́bridos de A. m. scutellata) a nivel indi-
vidual, grupal y colonial. Primeramente, en abejas ubicadas individualmente en placas de Petri se registró la expresión
de cinco comportamientos de reacción luego de colocarles un ácaro en el tórax. Posteriormente se colocaron en una
caja de Petri 30 abejas de cada colonia con 20 ácaros y 24 horas después se contaron los ácaros caı́dos. Por último, en
cada colonia se determinó la proporción de ácaros mutilados colectados del piso de la colmena. En la evaluación a nivel
individual las abejas africanizadas mostraron un mayor número total de comportamientos de reacción frente a V.
destructor que las italianas (U = 182,5; P = 0,02). Los grupos de abejas italianas consiguieron desprenderse del 60,8 ±
20,0% de los ácaros y los de las abejas africanizadas del 65,9 ± 15,6% de los ácaros, no mostrando diferencias significa-
tivas (t = 0,735; P = 0,47). Las colonias de abejas africanizadas presentaron una mayor proporción de ácaros mutilados
(29,0 ± 8,6%) que las colonias de abejas italianas (17,7 ± 9,8%) (t = 2.92; P = 0,009). Las abejas africanizadas se carac-
terizan por presentar mayor resistencia a V. destructor que las abejas de razas europeas. Este estudio muestra que esta
diferencia puede deberse en parte al comportamiento de grooming. La importancia del auto y el allogrooming en la
resistencia a V. destructor son discutidos.
Keywords: varroosis; resistance; mites; honey bees subspecies; selection

Introduction of new specific chemicals (Elzen, Baxter, Spivak, &


The ectoparasitic mite Varroa destructor colonized the Wilson, 2000; Milani, 1999; Spreafico, Eördegh, Bernar-
honey bees (Apis mellifera) and distributed worldwide dinelli, & Colombo, 2001).
during the second half of the twentieth century. Since In the search for new tools to control V. destructor,
then, it has become the main sanitary threat for the much research has been done aiming to identify the
beekeeping industry (Dietemann et al., 2012; Dietemann resistance mechanisms that honey bees poses (reviewed
et al., 2013; Rosenkranz, Aumeier, & Ziegelmann, 2010). in Boecking & Spivak, 1999; Büchler, Berg, & Le Conte,
Controlling V. destructor using synthetic acaricides is 2010; Rinderer et al., 2010; Rosenkranz et al., 2010).
becoming more difficult due to the mite’s acquisition of One of the behavioral resistance mechanisms found is
resistance against the most used products and the lack grooming, by which parasite bees can dislodge mites by

*Corresponding author. Email: ciro@fcien.edu.uy

© 2016 International Bee Research Association


2 C. Invernizzi et al.

themselves (autogrooming) or receiving help from other (with mutilated legs and occasionally cuts in the idio-
bees (allogrooming) (Boecking & Spivak, 1999). In the soma) collected in the hive floor (Bienefeld, Zautke,
latter, a parasitized bee, by means of a specific dance, Pronin, & Mazeed, 1999; Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012;
incites its mates to remove mites from its body (Land & Mondragón et al., 2005; Rosenkranz, Fries, Boecking, &
Seeley, 2004). This behavioral resistance mechanism is Sturner, 1997 ;Ruttner & Hänel, 1992; Vandame et al.,
readily expressed in the Asian bee Apis cerana, the origi- 2002). Nevertheless, a proportion of injured mites can
nal host of Varroa spp., and contributes to the control be product of the cleaning of cells with infested brood
of mite populations (but see Büchler, Drescher, & Tor- (hygienic behavior) (Rosenkranz et al., 1997) or caused
nier1992; Fries, Huazhen, Jin, & Wei, 1996; Peng, Fang, by predators (Bienefeld et al., 1999), or by the moth
Xu, & Ge, 1987). Galeria mellonella (Szabo & Walker, 1995). Marks on the
The importance of grooming behavior for controlling idiosoma of mites can be the result of malformation
V. destructor is a controversial issue. On the one hand, (Davis, 2009), so they should not be necessarily inter-
grooming behavior is negatively associated to the amount preted as a consequence of bee grooming behavior.
of mites in the colonies (Andino & Hunt, 2011; Arechava- The expression of grooming behavior is also affected
leta-Velasco, & Guzman-Novoa, 2001; Boecking & Ritter, by environmental factors. Currie & Tahmasbi, 2008
1993; Büchler, 1993; Delfinado-Baker, Rath, & Boecking, found that bees selected for high and low expression of
1992; Guzmán-Novoa, Vandame, & Arechavaleta-Velasco, grooming behavior kept their differences at 25 and
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

2012; Mondragón, Spivak, & Vandame, 2005). On the 34 ˚C, but not at 10 ˚C. Büchler, 1993 and Moosbeck-
other hand, it is doubted whether this behavior has a rele- hofer, 1997 found that the proportion of injured mites
vant role in controlling mite population (Harbo & Harris, presented seasonal variation.
1999; Locke & Fries, 2011; Lodesani, Crailsheim, & Mor- Grooming behavior has been assessed under con-
itz, 2002; Rosenkranz & Engels, 1994). trolled conditions in small groups of bees infested by V.
Moretto, Gonçalves, & De Jong, 1993 determined destructor confined in boxes (Andino & Hunt, 2011; Are-
that the heritability (h2) of grooming behavior is high chavaleta-Velasco & Guzman-Novoa, 2001; Currie &
(0.71), which would enable improving such resistance Tahmasbi, 2008) or observing the behavior of a bee
mechanism through artificial selection. However, Stan- placed in a Petri dish after placing a mite on its body
imirovic, Jevrosima, & Nevenka, 2010 assessed grooming (Aumeier, 2001; Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012).
behavior under field conditions along three generations In Uruguay, most bees are hybrids, crosses between
of queens found moderate to low heritability values European A. m. mellifera and African A. m. scutellata bees,
(0.49; 0.18 and 0.16), whereas Ehrhardt, Reinsch, Büch- and almost 100% of African haplotypes can be found in
ler, Garrido, & Bienefeld, 2007 found even lower values the borderline with Brazil following a southward
(<0.15) under similar conditions. Büchler, 2000success- decreasing gradient (Burgett, Shorney, Cordara, Gardiol,
fully selected colonies showing this resistance behavior & Sheppard, 1995; Collet, Ferreira, Arias, Soares, & Del
and, after some generations, found more injured mites Lama, 2006; Diniz, Soares, Sheppard, & Del Lama,
as well as lower rates of infection than those found in 2003). Recently, Branchiccela et al., 2014 showed that
colonies under no selection. Africanized bees are completely established in Uruguay,
The expression of grooming behavior varies among representing 80% of the analyzed colonies, distributed
subspecies of bees. Comparative studies have noticed that throughout the whole country. However, in the last
Africanized bees (hybrids of A. m. scutellata) show a more years, beekeepers have introduced a great amount of
efficient grooming than European bees (Aumeier, 2001; queens of Italian bees A. m. ligustica, mainly in the west-
Guzman-Novoa et al., 1999; Guzmán-Novoa, Emsen, ern littoral provinces, taking advantage of the gentleness
Unger, Espinosa-Montaño, & Petukhova, 2012; Moretto of this bee subspecies (Branchiccella et al., 2014).
et al., 1993). Such differences could partially explain the Most Uruguayan beekeepers control varroa using
well-known resistance to V. destructor of Africanized bees conventional synthetic acaricides in autumn (Invernizzi
in many South and Central American regions (Martin & et al., 2011). Nevertheless, there are still some areas in
Medina, 2004). In the USA, Rinderer et al., 2001 found the East and North of the country where bees are
that bees brought from the region of Primorsky (Russia), much Africanized and there is little beekeeping activity,
which presented good resistance to V. destructor, showed and where V. destructor does not cause severe damage
more grooming behavior than bees from Louisiana. (a loss of untreated colonies less than 30%; Yamandú
Assessing grooming behavior in bees is problematic. Mendoza, unpublished data).
Both direct observation or video recording of infested The aim of this study was to evaluate grooming
bees in observation hives is difficult, as both methods behavior in Africanized and Italian bees at the individual,
are time consuming and do not allow obtaining continu- group, and colony levels.
ous records (Moretto et al., 1993; Peng et al., 1987;
Thakur, Bienefeld, & Keller, 1997; Vandame, Morand,
Colin, & Belzunces, 2002). Thus, evaluating grooming Materials and methods
behavior in bees of a given colony is usually done indi- Studies were conducted in February and March in 2011
rectly by considering the proportion of injured mites (summer in the southern hemisphere) at the experimen-
Grooming behavior against Varroa destructor 3

tal station “La Estanzuela” of the Instituto Nacional de mites. Bees were incubated for 24 h at 28 ˚C and then
Investigación Agropecuaria (Colonia Province, Uruguay). the number of dead bees was recorded. Fallen mites
Grooming behavior was evaluated in Africanized and were observed using a binocular microscope (40x) to
Italian bees at the individual, group, and colony levels. record whether they had mutilated legs. When more
Africanized bees were collected at Treinta y Tres Pro- than 10% of bees were dead, evaluations were not
vince (east region of the country), where there is an taken into account and the experiment was repeated.
important Africanization of bees and good tolerance to
V. destructor. Italian bees were provided by a beekeeper
Experiment 3. Evaluation of grooming behavior at
from Colonia Province who imported queens of this
the colony level
subspecies from the Buenos Aires Province (Argentina).
Adult female mites were obtained from the brood of Grooming at the colony level was evaluated in 10 colonies
heavily infested colonies, and kept in Eppendorf tubes, of Italian bees and 10 colonies of Africanized bees. The
3–5 h before starting the evaluations. bees filled completely the brood chamber of Langstroth
hives. A 3-mm mesh screen was used as the hive floor, and
this had a tray under it that had a paper coated with petro-
Experiment 1. Evaluation of grooming behavior at leum jelly to retain fallen mites (Guzmán-Novoa et al.,
the individual level 2012). Fallen mites were collected for 10 days, and 30
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

In order to observe the behavioral response of a bee mites were observed using a binocular microscope (40x)
when a mite contacts it, 24 Italian and 24 Africanized to determine the proportion of mites with mutilated legs.
bees were put each in a separate Petri dish (10-cm
diameter). They were obtained from six colonies of
Statistical analyses
each subspecies (four bees of each colony, collected
from brood combs). A mite was placed on the dorsal All data were analyzed to check for normal distribution
part of the thorax of a bee using a small brush, and then and homogeneity of variances. If both conditions were
the bee was observed for two minutes. We recorded met, then parametric tests were performed. In Experi-
the occurrence of five reactions which compose the ment 1, grooming behavior was quantified as the num-
grooming behavior, similar to those described by: ber of Italian and Africanized bees which reacted to V.
Aumeier, 2001 destructor. A Chi-square test was used to analyze
Cleaning: cleaning antennae and body with legs. whether there were differences between subspecies,
Shaking: shaking the abdomen and between the number of bees which reacted to the
Biting: trying to remove the mite using the mandibles mite in the first and second minute. Grooming was also
Rolling: doing a somersault while hunting the mite measured as the average number of times a bee mani-
and using all pairs of legs. fested each behavioral reaction toward the mite. Mann–
Attempting to fly Whitney test was used to check for differences between
Our study did not consider the classification done subspecies and also between the number of bees which
by Aumeier, 2001 in which Cleaning and Biting was weak reacted to the mite in the first and second minute.
or intense depending on whether they lasted less or In Experiment 2, grooming behavior in colonies was
more than 5 s, respectively. quantified as the number of fallen mites over the total
We recorded whether reactions occurred in the number of mites. A Student’s t-test was used to check
first or second minute, and if the mite was removed in for differences of mean values between groups of Italian
the observation time. and Africanized bees. In Experiment 3, grooming behav-
ior of the colonies was quantified as the percentage of
injured mites. Differences between subspecies were ana-
Experiment 2. Evaluation of grooming behavior at lyzed using the Student test for mean values. Finally, a
the group level Spearman linear correlation was done between the per-
Grooming at the group level was evaluated in 13 colo- centage of dislodged mites in the laboratory test (Exper-
nies of Italian bees and 13 of Africanized bees. Thirty iment 2) and the percentage of mutilated mites in the
bees were taken from brood combs and placed in a field test (Experiment 3) using only the results obtained
Petri dish, covered by a 3-mm mesh screen. Bees were in colonies included in both experiments (8 Italian and
exposed to low temperatures (10–15 min at 4 ˚C) in 10 Africanized). All statistical analyses were considered
order to manipulate them easier. A 1-cm diameter ball as significant when P was < 0.05.
of candy (honey and powdered sugar) was put in each
Petri dish to feed the bees. Once all the bees were in
Results
the dish, 20 mites were added through the screen using
a small brush. When all mites were on the bees, the Experiment 1. Evaluation of grooming behavior at
dish was inverted and put on the lid which served as the individual level
the floor of the device. The lid was covered with a Cleaning, Shaking, Biting, Rolling, and Attempting to fly
paper spread with petroleum jelly to pick up the fallen were observed with low frequency in the 48 Italian and
4 C. Invernizzi et al.

Africanized bees after placing a mite on their thorax. Experiment 2. Evaluation of grooming behavior at
Notably, four Africanized bees were observed beating the group level
their wings throughout the two minutes of observation. Groups of Italian bees dislodged 60.8 ± 20.0% of the
Sixteen Italian and 19 Africanized bees presented at mites while groups of Africanized bees did 65.9
least one of the reactions to V. destructor; these frequen- ± 15.60%, but this difference is not significant (t = 0.735;
cies did not differ between subspecies (Chi p = 0.47). No mutilated legs were observed in any of
square = 0.95; df = 1; p = 0.33). The number of bees the 312 fallen mites.
that reacted to the mite in the first and second minute
of the observation time was 10 and 9, respectively, for
Italian bees, and 15 and 12 for Africanized bees. Within Experiment 3. Evaluation of grooming behavior at
each subspecies, no significant difference was found in the colony level
the number of bees that reacted in each minute of The analysis of fallen mites showed that Africanized bee
observation (Chi square = 0.09; df = 1; p = 0.77, for colonies contained a higher proportion of mites with
Italian bees; Chi square = 0.76; p = 0.38, for Africanized mutilated legs (29.0 ± 8.6%) than Italian bee colonies
bees). (17.7 ± 9.8%) (t = 2.92; p = 0.009).
There were no significant differences between sub- Finally, there was no association between the num-
species of bees in the number of times each of the five ber of mites dislodged in the laboratory tests (Experi-
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

reactions was observed (Cleaning: U = 239; p = 0.26; ment 2) and percentage of injured mites collected in the
Shaking: U = 243; p = 0.26; Biting: U = 288; p = 0.98; artificial hive floor (Experiment 3), taking into account
Rolling: U = 251; p = 0.22; Attempting to fly: U = 286; the 18 colonies used for both experiments (8 of Italian
p = 0.96; Figure 1). However, Africanized bees showed bees and 10 of Africanized bees) (r = 0.24; p = 0.34).
an overall higher number of reactions to V. destructor However, a significant correlation was found (r = 0.48;
than Italian bees (U = 182.5; p = 0.02) (Figure 1). p = 0.05) when one colony was removed from the anal-
Overall, bees that manifested any response to V. ysis (Figure 2). Such colony showed a response outlying
destructor did it between one and six times in an obser- the tendency of the remaining colonies of the group,
vation period, and repeated it up to four times. In bees i.e., it had the largest percentage of the mites dislodged
of both subspecies, Cleaning and Shaking were the most in Experiment 2 and the least injured mites in Experi-
frequent responses, i.e., 60% of the total reactions ment 3.
observed (Figure 1). Only two Italian and two African-
ized bees (8% out of the total) bit mites, and only two
Italian and one Africanized bees (6.3% out of the total) Discussion
dislodged the mite successfully. Africanized bees have been reported to have higher
There were no significant differences in the average resistance to V. destructor than European (Camazine,
number of reactions to V. destructor between the first 1986; De Jong, 1997; Moretto, Gonçalves, De Jong, &
and second minute of observation for Italian and Bichuette, 1991; Rosenkranz et al., 2010). Grooming
Africanized bees (U = 271.5; p = 0.70 and U = 249; could be one trait which contributes to resistance of
p = 0.40, respectively). Africanized bees. In this study, grooming was assessed in
Italian bees (A. m. ligustica) and Africanized bees (cross-
ings of European bees, mainly A. m. mellifera, with Afri-
can bees A. m. scutellata) at the individual, group, and
Italians bees Africanized bees colony levels.
4 P = 0,025
Reactions per bee

2 n.s.

n.s. n.s.
1 n.s.
n.s.

0
Cleaning Shaking Biting Rolling Attempting Sum of
to fly reactions

Figure 1. Frequency of the reactions Cleaning, Shaking, Biting,


Rolling, and Attempting to fly in Italian and Africanized bees, in
two minutes of observation after placing a mite on their tho- Figure 2. Spearman linear correlation between the percent-
rax. age of dislodged mites in the laboratory test (Experiment 2)
Notes: ns: not significant differences for Mann–Whitney test and the percentage of mutilated mites in the field test
(p > 0.05). (Experiment 3) (r = 0.48; n = 17; p = 0.05).
Grooming behavior against Varroa destructor 5

In the first experiment, the five reaction behaviors tner & Hänel, 1992; Experiment 3 of this study). In
reported by, Aumeier, 2001 Cleaning, Shaking, Biting, colonies under natural conditions, it is likely that most
Rolling, and Attempting to fly, were recorded while mites dislodged by bees are not injured and fall on
observing solitary bees infested by females V. destructor. other bees without any consequences regarding mite
The low frequency with which these behaviors occurred removal of the colony. If mites fall on the hive floor,
coincides with that reported by Aumeier, 2001 and can these can probably walk up and get in touch with bees
be what prevented detecting differences between Italian again. In this sense, one of the authors of this study
and Africanized bees. Nevertheless, when all behaviors (Estela Santos) has observed that, under laboratory
were grouped, we found that Africanized bees reacted conditions, females V. destructor can walk vertically
more to V. destructor than Italian bees did. Aumeier, enough so as to climb the hive floor up to the nest
2001 analyzed the data similarly and found that African- without any difficulty, and get in contact with the bees
ized bees reacted more frequently than European bees again. Nonetheless, those mites with mutilated appen-
(A. m. carnica) when V. destructor females were placed on dixes will probably have difficulties in grabbing to the
their thorax. Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012 used an analo- bee body, have more exposure to being dislodged and
gous methodology to the one used in our study to com- end up dying as a result of injuries. Thus, when fallen
pare the intensity of grooming in bees with genotypes mites are collected from artificial hive floors, the
resistant and susceptible to V. destructor. They found observed proportion of injured/not injured mites is
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

that the behavior was expressed with higher intensity in likely to be higher than that expected for all mites
the former ones, and that such intense grooming was removed by bees.
more efficient to dislodge mites than a mild one. In the Evaluation of grooming using little groups of bees
same study it was verified that Africanized bees have a artificially infested with V. destructor allows discarding
higher ability to dislodge a mite than Italian bees do. environmental factors not controlled under field condi-
In this study, four Africanized bees beat their wings tions (nectar flow, infestation level, environmental tem-
constantly during the observation period, a behavior not perature, colony size, etc.). Using this technique, Currie
reported by Aumeier, 2001. Beating wings is one of the & Tahmasbi, 2008 got, through a bidirectional selection,
responses of bees toward ants (Fell, 1997). It is possible bees that express high and low efficiency for grooming
that bees infested by V. destructor beat their wings as a behavior.
similar reaction to that recorded toward ants. In the last experiment, grooming behavior in colo-
Out of the 48 bees observed, only 3 (6.3%) (2 Italian nies of Italian and Africanized bees was evaluated using
and 1 Africanized) could dislodge the mite, which is less a conventional methodology, i.e., observing damage in
than found by Aumeier, 2001 and Guzmán-Novoa et al., fallen mites. Africanized bee colonies presented almost
2012 using the same technique (though with an addi- twice as many injured mites as Italian bee colonies.
tional minute of observation). Aumeier, 2001 found that These results match those reported by Moretto et al.,
18.3% of Africanized bees and 9.1% of Carniolan bees 1993; Aumeier, 2001; Vandame et al., 2002; and Guz-
could remove the mite. Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012 mán-Novoa et al., 2012 and those found in Experiment
found that between 15.0 and 34.9% of the bees bearing 1 of this study, in which grooming of Africanized and
resistant genotypes could dislodge the mite (29.4% in European bees was assessed using a variety of method-
Africanized bees), while 4.3 (Italian bees) to 16.2% of ologies.
bees bearing susceptible genotypes could do it. Marked Differential resistance to varroosis by Africanized
differences in the proportion of bees that could dislodge and European bees is often explained through factors
the mite between our study and those of Aumeier, such as hygienic behavior, grooming behavior, brood
2001 and Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012 could be due to period, duration of the brood cycle, cell size, swarming
little methodological differences which greatly affect the frequency, brood attractiveness (De Jong, 1997; Rosenk-
ability either of bees to dislodge the mite or of the mite ranz et al., 2010). Results obtained herein, along with
to avoid bees’ attempts of being removed. those of Moretto et al., 1993; Aumeier, 2001; Vandame
In the second experiment, groups of Italian and et al., 2002 and Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012 show that
Africanized bees artificially infested with mites were put grooming can be a factor of main importance to explain
in Petri dishes. It was found that the number of higher resistance of Africanized bees to V. destructor.
removed mites was somewhat superior to 60%, without Our results show that bees in Uruguay, which have
detecting differences between subspecies groups. This a significant degree of Africanization, express grooming
result contrasts the one obtained in the first experi- behavior against V. destructor in a more efficient way
ment, in which Africanized bees showed more reaction than Italian bees do. Since varroosis is the main sanitary
behaviors to V. destructor than Italian bees. problem in honey bees of the country (Invernizzi et al.,
The analysis of fallen mites did not show any indi- 2011) caution should be taken when entering and repro-
viduals with mutilations, as expected for mites col- ducing Italian bees. On doing so, there is risk of weak-
lected from hive floors (typically between 10 and 30%) ening the behavioral resistance in local populations of
(Bienefeld et al., 1999; Guzmán-Novoa et al., 2012; Africanized bees due to the impossibility of preventing
Mondragón et al., 2005; Rosenkranz et al., 1997; Rut- crossings among different bee subspecies.
6 C. Invernizzi et al.

Grooming is a behavioral resistance mechanism References


honey bees present against V. destructor mites, likely to Andino, G. K., & Hunt, G. J. (2011). A scientific note on a new
be selected in order to gain tolerance against the para- assay to measure honey bee mite-grooming behavior. Api-
site (Büchler, 2000). When implementing a methodology dologie, 42, 481–484. doi:10.1007/s13592-011-0004-1
Arechavaleta-Velasco, M., & Guzman-Novoa, E. (2001).
to evaluate a valuable heritable characteristic, the influ-
Relative effect of four characteristics that restrain the
ence of environmental factors should be taken into population growth of the mite Varroa destructor in honey
account as well as its practicality and economy. Either bee (Apis mellifera) colonies. Apidologie, 32, 157–174.
observing the behavior of isolated bees after getting in doi:10.1051/apido:2001121
touch with V. destructor under laboratory conditions, or Aumeier, P. (2001). Bioassay for grooming effectiveness
towards Varroa destructor mites in Africanized and Carnio-
quantifying the number of mites a confined group of
lan honey bees. Apidologie, 32, 81–90. doi:10.1051/
artificially infested bees can dislodge, stand out as apido:2001113
promising techniques to indirectly assess grooming Bienefeld, K., Zautke, F., Pronin, D., & Mazeed, A. (1999).
behavior of colonies. Still, these evaluations are time Recording the proportion of damaged Varroa jacobsoni
consuming and require obtaining female V. destructor Oud. in the debris of honey bee colonies (Apis mellifera).
Apidologie, 30, 249–256. doi:10.1051/apido:19990401
mites to infect bees, so they would be restricted to
Boecking, O., & Ritter, W. (1993). Grooming and removal
research groups and professional technicians who have behavior of Apis mellifera intermissa in Tunisia against Varroa
the necessary resources. However, Andino & Hunt, jacobsoni. Journal of Apicultural Research, 32, 127–134.
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

2011 have recently put forth a more accessible tech- doi:10.1080/00218839.1993.11101297


nique to assess grooming behavior at the laboratory. Boecking, O., & Spivak, M. (1999). Behavioral defenses of
honey bees against Varroa jacobsoni Oud. Apidologie, 30,
This technique consists in introducing a frame with bees
141–158. doi:10.1051/apido:19990205
from the colony in a cage (52.5 27 7.2 cm) with a Branchiccela, B., Aguirre, C., Parra, G., Estay, P., Zunino, P.,
metal screen bottom. After 72 h, the fallen mites and & Antúnez, K. (2014). Genetic changes in Apis mellifera
the mites remaining on the bees are counted. The pro- after 40 years of Africanization. Apidologie, 45, 752-756.
portion of dropped mites is positively correlated with doi:10.1007/s13592-014-0293-2
Burgett, M., Shorney, S., Cordara, J., Gardiol, G., & Sheppard,
the number of mutilated mites in the colony.
W. S. (1995). The present status of Africanized honey
A problem that has not been approached yet is the bees in Uruguay. American Bee Journal, 135, 328–330.
estimation of the relative importance auto and Büchler, R. (1993). Rate of damaged mites in natural mite fall
allogrooming have in dislodging V. destructor from the with regard to seasonal effects and infestation develop-
body of the bees. Through different behavioral compo- ment. Apidologie, 24, 492–493.
Büchler, R. (2000). Design and success of a German breeding
nents of autogrooming (in which the main are cleaning
program for Varroa tolerance. American Bee Journal, 140,
using the legs and shaking), bees may be able to remove 662–665.
the mite from its body without damaging it. Instead, if a Büchler, R., Berg, S., & Le Conte, Y. (2010). Breeding for
bee used its mandibles to dislodge the mite from resistance to Varroa destructor in Europe. Apidologie, 41,
another bee, it could end up in lethal mutilations for the 393–408. doi:10.1051/apido/2010011
Büchler, R., Drescher, W., & Tornier, I. (1992). Grooming
mite. Hence, a standard evaluation of grooming behavior
behaviour of Apis cerana, Apis mellifera and Apis dorsata and
of a colony, in which the proportion of injured mites is its effect on the parasitic mites Varroa jacobsoni and
quantified, may only indicate the magnitude of Tropilaelaps clareae. Experimental and Applied Acarology, 16,
allogrooming in the colony. Future studies should be 313–319.
addressed to elucidate the weight of individual (auto- Camazine, S. (1986). Differential reproduction of the Mite, Var-
roa jacobsoni (Mesostigmata: Varroidae), on Africanized and
grooming) and social (allogrooming) responses, as well
European honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Annals of
as the relationship between both of them, in the behav- the Entomological Society of America, 79, 801–803.
ioral resistance against V. destructor. Collet, T., Ferreira, K. M., Arias, M. C., Soares A. E. E., &
Del Lama, M. A. (2006). Genetic structure of Africanized
honey bee populations (Apis mellifera L.) from Brazil
and Uruguay viewed through mitochondrial DNA COI–
Acknowledgments COII patterns. Heredity, 97, 329–335. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.
We thank Matı́as Maggi for helpful comments on the manu- 6800875
script. Currie, R. W., & Tahmasbi, G. H. (2008). The ability of high-
and low-grooming lines of honey bees to remove the para-
sitic mite Varroa destructor is affected by environmental
conditions. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 86, 1059–1067.
Disclosure statement doi:10.1139/Z08-083
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Davis, A. R. (2009). Regular dorsal dimples on Varroa destructor
– damage symptoms or developmental origin? Apidologie,
40, 151–162. doi:10.1051/apido/2009001
De Jong, D. (1997). Mites: varroa and other parasites of brood.
Funding
In R. A. Morse & K. Flottum (Eds.), Honey bee pests, preda-
This research was supported by the Instituto Nacional de tors, & diseases (pp. 279–327). Ohio, USA: A. I. Root Com-
Investigación Agropecuaria. pany; Medina.
Grooming behavior against Varroa destructor 7

Delfinado-Baker, M., Rath, W., & Boecking, O. (1992). Martin, S. J., & Medina, L. M. (2004). Africanized honey bees
Phoretic bee mites and honey bee grooming behavior. have unique tolerance to Varroa mites. Trends in Parasitol-
International Journal of Acarology, 18, 315–322. ogy, 20, 112–114. doi:10.1016/j.pt.2004.01.001
Dietemann, V., Pflugfelder, J., Anderson, D., Charrière, J.-D., Milani, N. (1999). The resistance of Varroa jacobsoni Oud. to
Chejanovsky, N., Dainat, B., … Neumann, P. (2012). Varroa acaricides. Apidologie, 30, 229–234. doi:10.1051/
destructor: research avenues towards sustainable control. apido:19990211
Journal of Apicultural Research, 51, 125–132. doi:10.3896/ Mondragón, L., Spivak, M., & Vandame, R. (2005). A multifacto-
IBRA.1.51.1.15 rial study of the resistance of honey bees Apis mellifera to
Dietemann, V., Nazzi, F., Martin, S. J., Anderson, D., Locke, B., the mite Varroa destructor over one year in Mexico. Apidolo-
Delaplane, K. S., … Ellis, J. D. (2013). Standard methods gie, 36, 345–358. doi:10.1051/apido:2005022
for varroa research. In V. Dietemann, J. D. Ellis & P. Moosbeckhofer, R. (1997). Observations on reproduction rate
Neumann (Eds.), The COLOSS BEEBOOK, Volume II: standard of Varroa jacobsoni and the occurrence of mutilated mites
methods for Apis mellifera pest and pathogen research. Journal in Apis mellifera carnica colonies. Apidologie, 28, 193–195.
of Apicultural Research, 52(1). doi: 10.3896/IBRA.1.52.1.09 Moretto, G., Gonçalves L. S., & De Jong, D. (1993). Heritability
Diniz, N. M., Soares, A. E. G., Sheppard, W. S., & Del Lama, of Africanized and European honey bee defense behavior
M. A. (2003). Genetic structure of honey bee populations against the mite Varroa jacobsoni. Brazilian Journal of Genet-
from southern Brazil and Uruguay. Genetics and Molecular ics, 16, 71–77.
Research, 26, 47–52. Moretto, G., Gonçalves, L. S., De Jong, D., & Bichuette, M. Z.
Ehrhardt, K., Reinsch, N., Büchler, R., Garrido, C., & Bienefeld, (1991). The effects of climate and bee race on Varroa
K. (2007, September). Genetic parameters for Varroa jacobsoni Oud. infestations in Brazil. Apidologie, 22, 197–
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 09:47 12 May 2016

resistance in the honey bee. In Proceedings of the 40th 203. doi:10.1051/apido:19910303


International Apicultural Congress, Melbourne, Australia. Peng, Y. S., Fang, Y., Xu, S., & Ge, L. (1987). The resistance
(pp. 145-146). mechanism of the Asian honey bee Apis cerana Fabr. to an
Elzen, P. J., Baxter, J. R., Spivak, M., & Wilson, W. T. (2000). ectoparasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni Oudemans. Journal of
Control of Varroa jacobsoni Oud resistant to fluvalinate and Invertebrate Pathology, 49, 54–60. doi:10.1016/0022-2011
amitraz using coumaphos. Apidologie, 31, 437–441. (87)90125-x
doi:10.1051/apido:2000134 Rinderer, T. E., De Guzman, L. I., Delatte, G. T., Stelzer, J. A.,
Fell, R. D. (1997). Insects: Hymenoptera (ants, wasps, and Lancaster, V. A., Kuznetsov, V., … Harris, J. W. (2001).
bees). In R. A. Morse & K. Flottum (Eds.), Honey bee pests, Resistance to the parasitic mite Varroa destructor in honey
predators, & diseases (pp. 163–202). Ohio, USA: A. I. Root bees from far-eastern Russia. Apidologie, 32, 381–394.
Company; Medina. doi:10.1051/apido:2001138
Fries, I., Huazhen, W., Jin C. S., & Wei, S. (1996). Grooming Rinderer, T. E., Harris, J. W., Hunt, G. J., & Guzman De, L. I.
behavior and damaged mites (Varroa jacobsoni) in Apis cer- (2010). Breeding for resistance to Varroa destructor in North
ana cerana and Apis mellifera ligustica. Apidologie, 27, 3–11. America. Apidologie, 41, 409–424. doi:10.1051/apido/2010015
doi:10.1051/apido:19960101 Rosenkranz, P., Aumeier, P., & Ziegelmann, B. (2010). Biology
Guzmán-Novoa, E., Emsen, B., Unger, P., Espinosa-Montaño, L. and control of Varroa destructor. Journal of Invertebrate
G., & Petukhova, T. (2012). Genotypic variability and rela- Pathology, 103, S96–S119. doi:10.1016/j.jip.2009.07.016
tionships between mite infestation levels, mite damage, Rosenkranz, P., & Engels, W. (1994). Infertility of Varroa jacob-
grooming intensity, and removal of Varroa destructor mites soni females after invasion into Apis mellifera worker brood
in selected strains of worker honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). as a tolerance factor against varroatosis. Apidologie, 25,
Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 110, 314–320. doi:10.1016/ 402–411. doi:10.1051/apido:19940407
j.jip.2012.03.020 Rosenkranz, P., Fries, I., Boecking, O., & Sturner, M. (1997).
Guzman-Novoa, E., Vandame, R., & Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. Damaged Varroa mites in the debris of honey bee (Apis
E. (1999). Susceptibility of European and Africanized honey mellifera L) colonies with and without hatching brood. Api-
bees (Apis mellifera L.) to Varroa jacobsoni Oud. in Mexico. dologie, 28, 427–437. doi:10.1051/apido:19970609
Apidologie, 30, 173–182. doi:10.1051/apido:19990207 Ruttner, F., & Hänel, H. (1992). Active defense against Varroa
Harbo, J. R., & Harris, J. W. (1999). Heritability in honey bees mites in a Carniolan strain of honey bee (Apis mellifera
(Hymenoptera: Apidae) of characteristics associated with carnica Pollmann). Apidologie, 23, 173–187. doi:10.1051/
resistance to Varroa jacobsoni (Mesostigmata: Varroidae). apido:19920210
Journal of Economic Entomology, 92, 261–265. Spreafico, M., Eördegh, F. R., Bernardinelli, I., & Colombo, M.
Invernizzi, C., Antúnez, K., Campa, J. P., Harriet, J., Mendoza, (2001). First detection of strains of Varroa destructor resis-
Y., Santos, E.., & Zunino, P. (2011). Situación sanitaria de tant to coumaphos. Results of laboratory tests and field
las abejas melı́feras en Uruguay (Health status of honey trials. Apidologie, 32, 49–55. doi:10.1051/apido:2001110
bees in Uruguay). Veterinaria, 42, 9–13. Stanimirovic, Z., Jevrosima, S., & Nevenka, A. (2010). Heritability
Land, B. B., & Seeley, T. D. (2004). The grooming invitation of grooming behaviour in grey honey bees (Apis mellifera Car-
dance of the honey bee. Ethology, 110, 1–10. doi:10.1046/ nica). Acta Veterinaria, 60, 313–323. doi:10.2298/AVB1003313S
j.1439-0310.2003.00947.x Szabo, T. I., & Walker, C. R. T. (1995). Damages to dead Var-
Locke, B., & Fries, I. (2011). Characteristics of honey bee colo- roa jacobsoni caused by the larvae of Galleria mellonella.
nies (Apis mellifera) in Sweden surviving Varroa destructor American Bee Journal, 135, 421–422.
infestation. Apidologie, 42, 533–542. doi:10.1007/s13592- Thakur, R. K., Bienefeld, K., & Keller, R. (1997). Varroa defense
011-0029-5 behavior in A. mellifera carnica. American Bee Journal, 137,
Lodesani, M., Crailsheim, K., & Moritz, R. F. A. (2002). Effect 143–148.
of some characters on the population growth of mite Vandame, R., Morand, S., Colin, M. E., & Belzunces, L. P. (2002).
Varroa jacobsoni in Apis mellifera L. colonies and results of a Parasitism in the social bee Apis mellifera: quantifying costs
bi-directional selection. Journal of Applied Entomology, 126, and benefits of behavioral resistance to Varroa destructor
130–137. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0418.2002.00615.x mites. Apidologie, 33, 433–445. doi:10.1051/apido:2002025

Вам также может понравиться