Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 23

Family Systems Theory, Attachment Theory,

and Culture*
FRED ROTHBAUM, Ph.D.†
KAREN ROSEN, Ph.D.‡
TATSUO UJIIE, Ph.D.§
NOBUKO UCHIDA, Ph.D.

Family systems theory and attachment theory evidence, and more extensively re-
theory have important similarities and view attachment theory evidence, pertain-
complementarities. Here we consider two ing to these points of convergence. We also
areas in which the theories converge: (a) in review cross-cultural research, which
family system theorists’ description of an leads us to conclude that the dynamics
overly close, or “enmeshed,” mother-child described in both theories reflect, in part,
dyad, which attachment theorists concep- Western ways of thinking and Western
tualize as the interaction of children’s am- patterns of relatedness. Evidence from Ja-
bivalent attachment and mothers’ preoc- pan suggests that extremely close ties be-
cupied attachment; (b) in family system tween mother and child are perceived as
theorists’ description of the “pursuer-dis- adaptive, and are more common, and that
tance cycle” of marital conflict, which at- children experience less adverse effects
tachment theorists conceptualize as the in- from such relationships than do children
teraction of preoccupied and dismissive in the West. Moreover, in Japan there is
partners. We briefly review family systems less emphasis on the importance of the
exclusive spousal relationship, and less
* We thank Ken Gergen for encouraging us to need for the mother and father to find time
extend our earlier work on culture and attachment alone to rekindle romantic, intimate feel-
to the realm of family systems theory; Toshio Yam- ings and to resolve conflicts by openly com-
agishi for introducing the Japanese and American
co-authors to one another; and Kazuko Behrens, municating their differences. Thus, the
Patricia Minuchin, and Betsy Wood for their thought- “maladaptive” pattern frequently cited by
ful comments about many issues raised in this article. Western theorists of an extremely close
† Professor, Eliot-Pearson Department of Child mother-child relationship, an unroman-
Development; Tufts University, 105 College Avenue,
Medford MA 02155; e-mail: fred.rothbaum@tufts.
tic, conflictual marriage characterized by
edu. little verbal communication and a periph-
‡ Professor, Department of Psychology, Boston eral, distant father, may function very dif-
College, Chestnut Hill MA; e-mail: rosenk@bc.edu. ferently in other cultures. While we believe
§ Professor, Center for Developmental Clinical
that both theories will be greatly enriched
Psychology and Psychiatry, Nagoya University,
Nagoya, Japan; e-mail: p47302a@nucc.cc.nagoya.u. by their integration, we caution against
ac.jp. the application of either theory outside the
 Professor, Department of Psychology, Ochano- cultures in which they were developed.
mizu University, Tokyo, Japan; e-mail: uchida@li.
ocha.ac.jp. Fam Proc 41:328 –350, 2002
328
Family Process, Vol. 41, No. 3, 2002 © FPI, Inc.
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 329

T HERE are notable similarities be-


tween attachment theory and fam-
ily systems theory (Byng-Hall, 1999; Mar-
moves toward consideration of myriad ex-
ternal forces beyond the dyad that influ-
ence the attachment system (Belsky,
vin & Stewart, 1990; Minuchin, 1985; 1999b). Both the similarities in and dif-
Stevenson-Hinde, 1990). Some similari- ferences between the theories are seen as
ties are at a broad, conceptual level, such contributing to their compatibility and,
as the theories’ grounding in systems ultimately, their integration (Byng-Hall,
thinking and their concerns about inti- 1999; Marvin & Stewart, 1990; Minuchin,
mate human relationships (e.g., what 1985; Stevenson-Hinde, 1990).
draws people together, what drives them Our focus in this article is on cultural
apart, how they deal with conflicts, and factors that contribute to the rather re-
intergenerational transmission). Other markable synchrony between these theo-
similarities are at a more specific level, ries. We highlight ways in which both the-
such as the correspondence between at- ories are grounded in Western ideas and
tachment classifications of secure, ambiv- experiences and how these contribute to
alent, and avoidant relationships, on one their compatibility. The term “Western” is
hand, and family systems categories of used here because of the variety of North
adaptive, enmeshed, and disengaged rela- American and European cultures in
tionships on the other (Byng-Hall, 1999; which the theories have been tested.
Stevenson-Hinde, 1990). While there is a venerable tradition of
There are also impressive differences testing attachment theory in other cul-
between the theories: (a) attachment the- tures (van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999), the
ory is focused on dynamics involving pro- overwhelming majority of studies were
tection, care, and felt security, whereas conducted in the West, particularly in the
family systems theory is concerned with US. It is our claim that the synchrony
family dynamics, involving structures, between the theories reflects, in part,
roles, communication patterns, bound- Western investigators’ assumptions and
aries, and power relations; (b) attachment interpretations (i.e., the lens through
theory is focused on the dyad, with much which behavior is viewed) as well as West-
of the action occurring within individuals ern ways of functioning (i.e., the incidence
(e.g., “internal working models”), whereas and pattern of behavior per se). While a
family systems theory is focused on the cross-fertilization of the theories is
triad, with much of the action occurring likely to enrich both of them, it is un-
within groups; (c) attachment theory is wise to assume that the theories are
relatively more concerned with children relevant to other cultures in which the
and development, whereas family sys- concepts have been infrequently tested.
tems theory is relatively more concerned Several other reviews have highlighted
with adults and current functioning; and the Western bias evident in prevailing
(d) attachment theory has historically re- theories (e.g., Markus & Kitayama,
lied primarily on empirical research with 1991; Spence, 1985), including attach-
normal populations, whereas family sys- ment theory (Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, &
tems theory relies primarily on case stud- Miyaki, 2000) and family systems the-
ies involving clinic populations. Interest- ory (Tamura & Lau, 1992), but this is
ingly, theorists in both camps emphasize the first review to focus on topics central
the ways in which these differences in to both theories.
orientation complement one another. The Our goal is to consider the convergence
differences are seen as mutually enrich- between attachment theorists and family
ing, especially as attachment theory systems theorists with regard to two main

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


330 / FAMILY PROCESS

topics: (a) adaptive vs. dysfunctional early POINTS OF CONVERGENCE:


mother-child relationships, which builds CASE EXAMPLES
on the family systems notion of enmeshed/ In this section we summarize points of
undifferentiated families (Minuchin, 1974) convergence between attachment theory
and the attachment theory notion of sep- and family systems theory that we later
aration disorder; and (b) adaptive vs. review in greater detail, and we provide
dysfunctional marital relationships and anecdotal evidence, largely from the fam-
their role in mother-child relationships, ily systems theory perspective, regarding
which builds on the family systems no- these points. In the next two sections we
tion of pursuer-distance relationships provide evidence from attachment theory
(Fogarty, 1976; Minuchin & Nichols, bearing on these points, and we consider
1993; Scarf, 1995) and the attachment cultural evidence suggesting that the
theory notion of insecure mate relation- links between the theories reflect West-
ships. Our goal is to point to some op- ern ideas and practices.
portunities and pitfalls involved in a We highlight two main points of conver-
cross-fertilization around these attach- gence between the theories:
ment theory and family systems theory First, they both highlight the adaptive
notions. The particular issues we ad- risks involved in too-close mother-child
dress are not intended to represent all of relationships. Among the pejorative la-
attachment theory nor all of family sys- bels assigned to this kind of relationship
tems theory, but rather convergences are overinvolved, enmeshed, undifferenti-
between the theories that also help illu- ated, overly dependent, and symbiotic.
minate cultural variation. Second, both theories consider the qual-
In the first section, case examples ity of the marital relationship to be key to
from the family systems perspective il- overall family functioning. According to
lustrate similarities between the theo- Marvin & Stewart, “Family therapists
ries. In the next two sections— on the tend to view the spouse dyad as the ‘core’
parent-child dyad and the marital dy- of the family” (1990, p. 81), and attach-
ad—we review evidence from Western ment theorists’ consideration of family re-
research on attachment that sheds light lations is largely focused on the marital
on dynamics highlighted by both theo- dyad. What determines marital quality in
ries. We focus on attachment theory be- these theories is multifaceted, but centers
cause it is relatively less familiar to the on romantic characteristics (e.g., Byng-
readership of this journal. We also re- Hall, 1999; Cassidy & Shaver, 1999;
view evidence from studies comparing Christensen & Jacobson, 2000; Gottman,
Japan and the US. We attempt to show 1999; Marvin & Stewart, 1990). The qual-
that the similarities between attach- ity of the marital relationship is seen as a
ment theory and family systems theory major determinant of the caregiving pro-
are due, in part, to the fact that both vided to the child, and thus of child func-
theories are grounded in Western as- tioning. Attachment theorists provide
sumptions about relationships. At the abundant evidence to support this con-
end of the article, we summarize the nection (Belsky, 1999b; Goldberg & East-
evidence, point to qualifications involv- erbrooks, 1984; Isabella, 1994).
ing intracultural differences and inter- Family systems theorists often rely on
cultural similarities, and examine im- case histories to illustrate these dynam-
plications of the findings for culturally ics. For example, Jay Haley (1976) de-
sensitive approaches to family therapy. scribes a phobic boy who was suffering
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 331
because of unresolved and unexpressed tural differences in what is considered
conflicts between his parents. The par- normative and desirable with regard to
ents’ failure to preserve the boundary these relationships. The evidence sug-
around the marital dyad is depicted as a gests that patterns involving extremely
contributor to the boy’s fear. Haley (1976, high levels of caregiving by wives, dis-
p. 227) states: “The goal of treatment can tancing by husbands, and lack of intimacy
be put simply. It is to shift the relation- in the marital relationship are more com-
ships in the family so that mother and mon, and are less likely to be seen as
father are more intimately involved with maladaptive, in Japan than in the West.
each other and the boy is disengaged from
them . . .” “OVERINVOLVED” MOTHER-CHILD
Similarly, Byng-Hall (1999), an attach- RELATIONSHIPS
ment-oriented family systems theorist,
Evidence from Attachment Theory
highlights ways in which unresolved mar-
ital problems disrupt attachment rela- In the following quote, Marvin and
tionships. He describes a case in which a Stewart (1990) describe “one of the most
mother’s pursuing leads to a father’s dis- common maladapted family structures”
tancing, and vice versa, causing their son that have been identified by family sys-
to become entrapped in their maladaptive tems theorists:
interaction. Specifically, the son’s attach-
ment behavior is encouraged by the . . . mother and (at least one) child are
mother because of her own unresolved at- overinvolved, or enmeshed (Minuchin, 1974).
tachment needs. As the son seeks to use This enmeshed relationship is usually char-
the mother as a secure base from which to acterized by a number of the following: re-
explore, he is thwarted by her dependency ciprocally intrusive, controlling behavior on
needs. the part of mother and child; much insecu-
Family systems theorists believe that rity and distress on the part of both over real
or threatened separation; treatment of the
the “enmeshed mother/disengaged father
child as if he or she were younger than is
syndrome” is common in dysfunctional actually the case; a strong tendency for one
families (Nichols & Schwartz, 1998, p. or both to speak for the other and assume
249). Minuchin and Nichols (1993, p. 121) knowledge of what the other is thinking and
are explicit about the centrality of this feeling without “checking it out” (a really
syndrome: “The signature arrangement of palpable lack of psychological boundaries);
the troubled middle class family [is when] role reversals in attachment and caregiving
a mother’s closeness to her children sub- behaviors; an inability to resolve con-
stitut[es] for closeness in the marriage.” flict . . . (and a) degree of intimacy with one
In the next two sections we elaborate on another inappropriate to their relative ages
these dynamics and argue that important and positions in the family. [p. 79].
elements of the preceding analysis are
grounded in Western thought. First, we Marvin and Stewart suggest that this
review theory and evidence regarding pattern of interaction—a primary concern
“overinvolved” mother-child attachment of family systems theorists—is related to
relationships in the West, as well as evi- the insecure-resistant (also know as am-
dence that such relationships are more bivalent)1 classification identified by at-
common, and are less likely to be seen as tachment theorists. Marvin and Stewart
dysfunctional, in Japan. Next, we review (1990, p. 80) speculate that “these two
theory and evidence regarding marital at- traditions (i.e., family systems and at-
tachments in the West and describe cul- tachment theory) are speaking of the

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


332 / FAMILY PROCESS

‘same’ child.” We agree with their specu- likely to maintain constant contact”
lation and we point to several strands of (Byng-Hall, 1999, p. 628). Below we re-
attachment research that dovetail neatly view evidence—regarding mothers of am-
with family systems theorists description bivalent children, mothers rated as pre-
of mother-child overinvolved/enmeshed occupied (on the Adult Attachment In-
relationships. terview) or ambivalent (based on self
According to attachment theory, ambiv- reports),2 and ambivalent children—that
alent children are unable to use their further supports Marvin and Stewart’s
caregivers as a secure base to explore the speculation that these attachment styles
environment, are distressed upon separa- relate to overinvolved/enmeshed patterns
tion from their caregivers, and, when re- identified by family systems therapists.
united, are likely to seek proximity or con- When interviewed about their caregiv-
tact but cannot be calmed by their care- ing, mothers of ambivalently attached ba-
givers. (These behaviors are assessed bies mentioned several behaviors that
with the Strange Situation paradigm—an were seen as contributing to their chil-
unfamiliar situation in which the child is dren’s insecurity. These mothers “de-
repeatedly separated from and reunited scribed strategies to keep their children
with the caregiver.) The corresponding close, . . . promoted dependency, . . . tend-
behavior in adulthood, referred to as pre- ed to overemphasize caregiving and to
occupied, is assessed via the Adult At- overinterpret their children’s attachment
tachment Interview, which asks about
cues” (George & Solomon, 1999, pp. 661–
past and present relationships with par-
662). They emphasized their children’s
ents. Preoccupied adults display excessive
needs over their own to an extent that
and confused preoccupation with past at-
was seen as unhealthy for both mother
tachment relationships or experiences,
and child.
wander from topic to topic, or become em-
Preoccupied mothers of toddlers have
broiled in excessively lengthy descrip-
difficulty separating from their toddlers
tions of relational problems. The inter-
viewee appears angry, passive, or fearful. and separate in ways that are likely to
The memories aroused by the interview, foster child anxiety while discouraging in-
rather than the questions themselves, ap- dependence (evidence reviewed in Belsky,
pear to guide the interviewee’s attention 1999a). Preoccupied mothers of adoles-
and speech (Hesse, 1999). cents about to leave home convey doubts
There is substantial evidence in the about their children’s ability to function
West of the consistency of these attach- autonomously (Kobak, Ferenz-Gilles, Ever-
ment styles over time and circumstances, hart, & Seabrook, 1994). Belsky (1999a)
and of the relationship of the child and speculates that, in addition to undermin-
adult attachment styles to one another. ing autonomy, such behavior makes the
Meta-analytic findings indicate that pre- child susceptible to parental manipula-
occupied adults are likely to have a child tions. Preoccupied mothers are inappro-
with an ambivalent attachment (van priately responsive and overly attentive
IJzendoorn, 1995; van IJzendoorn & to their children (Goldberg, Blokland,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1997). The child Cayetano, & Benoit, 1998; Haft & Slade,
in these relationships is prone “. . . to re- 1989), are especially responsive to expres-
verse roles and provide the parent with sions of fear (Belsky, 1999a; Haft & Slade,
care, thus becoming indispensable [so as 1989), and in other “. . . ways interfere
to] assure the continuing presence of the with their children’s autonomy or explo-
attachment figure. These strategies are ration” (Cassidy and Berlin, 1994, p. 981).
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 333
Several qualities of ambivalent chil- the adult’s availability while not inter-
dren contribute to their enmeshed rela- fering with the child’s growing autonomy
tionships with their caregivers. These or jeopardizing individuation from the
children are especially demanding and/or mother. By contrast, in Japan, an impor-
preoccupied about gaining and sustaining tant criterion of sensitive caregiving is
attention from their caregivers (Belsky & that it foster a symbiotic relationship be-
Cassidy, 1994; Kunce & Shaver, 1994); tween mother and child in which the
they have trouble maintaining a bound- boundary between them is blurred. Japa-
ary between someone else’s distress and nese mothers may appear overinvolved
their own (Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & and intrusive by US standards because
Carlson, 1999); and they do everything they are much more likely than US moth-
possible to prevent separation (Fraley & ers to anticipate infants’ needs and to
Shaver, 1999). It is easy to understand take proactive measures to minimize in-
how caregivers become enmeshed with fants’ distress rather than to delay their
these children. Even in infancy ambiva- response until the child gives a signal
lent children are very fearful and their (Clancy, 1986; Doi, 1973; Rothbaum,
fearfulness increases as they enter early Weisz, et al., 2000; Vogel, 1991).
childhood—in contrast to other children Affective attunement in Japan, which
whose fearfulness wanes (Kochanska, is much higher than in the US (Behrens,
2001). 1999), takes the form of mothers engaging
These findings regarding ambivalent- in simultaneous vocalizations with their
preoccupied attachment dynamics have children, completing their children’ sen-
important implications for family func- tences for them, and otherwise communi-
tioning. We emphasize that these find- cating that they know what the child is
ings have been obtained primarily in thinking (Behrens, 1999; Clancy, 1986;
Western cultures. Below we provide evi- Fernald & Morikawa, 1993). This behav-
dence suggesting that behaviors Western ior is likely to be regarded as intrusive
investigators typically associate with am- and overly enmeshed in the US and, as
bivalent and preoccupied attachment are noted earlier, is likely to lead to insecure
more common and have different mean- (ambivalent) attachment. Other caregiv-
ings (i.e., there are differences in the eval- ing behaviors shown to foster ambivalent
uation of overt behavior as well as in the attachment in the West that are more
interpretation of its underlying affect and accepted and practiced by Japanese moth-
motivation) in Japan than in the US. ers are prolonged skin-to-skin contact
(“skinship”), co-sleeping, indulgence of de-
Cultural Differences pendency, arranging work to become fully
Here we summarize research on par- devoted to the child, and encouraging the
ent-child relations in Japan and the US child to focus on themselves (the mothers)
that points to differences in the types of as opposed to the environment (Azuma,
caregiving likely to foster security, and in Kashiwagi, & Hess, 1981; Lebra, 1994;
the consequences of security. Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, et al., 2000).
Sensitive caregiving: What constitutes Maternal sensitivity in the US is
sensitive, responsive caregiving is likely largely intended to provide a secure base
to reflect indigenous values and goals that promotes the child’s exploration, as
(Carlson & Harwood, in press). In the well as encouraging the child’s assertion
West, important criteria of sensitive care- of personal desires and autonomous ef-
giving are that it is responsive to the forts to satisfy one’s own needs. By con-
child’s signals and that it communicate trast, in Japan, sensitivity is largely in-

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


334 / FAMILY PROCESS

tended to avoid distress and to promote ued: “From [an East Asian] perspective,
infants’ interdependence and emotional an assertive, autonomous . . . person is
closeness with their mothers (Keller, immature and uncultivated” (Fiske et al.,
Voelker, & Zach, 1997; Rothbaum, Pott, 1998, p. 923).
et al., 2000; Rothbaum, Weisz, et al., Other qualities associated with security
2000; Vogel, 1991). of attachment in US children are emo-
The consequences of security: The dis- tional openness and direct communica-
similarity between Western and Japanese tion, especially in times of conflict (Bowlby,
ideas about consequences of security is 1979; Bretherton, 1995; Cassidy, 1988;
clearest with regard to independence. Ac- Main, Kaplan & Cassidy, 1985). Explicit,
cording to Weinfield et al. (1999, p. 77): verbal sharing of emotions is less likely to
“Children with secure histories seem to be seen as a desirable quality in Japan,
believe that, as was true in infancy, they where parents preserve social harmony
can get their needs met through their own by keeping hostile feelings to themselves,
efforts and bids. In contrast, children by expressing those feelings indirectly,
with anxious histories seem to believe and by encouraging their children to
that . . . they must rely extensively on do likewise (Lebra, 1994; Miyake &
others who may or may not meet their Yamazaki, 1995; Rothbaum, Pott, et al.,
needs.” There is substantial evidence in 2000; Vogel, 1991). In Japan, people are
the West that insecure as compared to expected to respectfully and empatheti-
secure adults tend to be more needy of cally preserve harmony by avoiding any
others, as evidenced by greater clinginess expression of discord or direct expression
and dependence, anxiety about gaining of wants (Rothbaum, Weisz, et al., 2000).
acceptance from others, preference for un- The notion that ambivalent attachment
qualified closeness, and experience of love functions similarly in Japan as in the US
as involving union (Bartholomew & is perhaps most challenged by differences
Shaver, 1998; Feeney, 1999; Hazan & in the evaluation and interpretation of
Shaver, 1994). behavior associated with children’s am-
Japanese theorists take a different bivalent attachment. There are many
view. In Japan, dependence (i.e., interde- similarities between Western investiga-
pendence and amae), seeking of accep- tors’ descriptions of children’s ambivalent
tance and commitment, and desire for behaviors and child behaviors widely re-
union are more common and more likely garded as adaptive in Japan. These in-
to be associated with competence (Fiske, clude: exaggerated cute and babyish be-
Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998). The haviors (Main & Cassidy, 1988), extreme
path of relying on others, so often deval- expressions of need for care and atten-
ued in the West, is more often favored and tion, extensive clinging and proximity
is even prescribed in Japan (Azuma et al., seeking, helpless dependency (Cassidy &
1981; Lebra, 1994; Rothbaum, Pott, et al., Berlin, 1994), extreme passivity, blurring
2000). Dependence on others as a way of of boundaries between self and other
meeting one’s needs, and coordinating (Weinfield et al., 1999) and failure to en-
one’s needs with the needs of others, are gage in exploration (Ainsworth, Blehar,
seen as essential to the goal of social har- Waters, & Wall, 1978). These features of
mony that is highly valued in Japan ambivalent attachment would not be seen
(Kitayama, Markus, Masumoto, & Nor- as abnormal or maladaptive in Japan, nor
asakkunkit, 1997; Roland, 1989; Weisz, as deriving from insensitive maternal be-
Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984a,b). Inde- havior, nor as leading to later social in-
pendence in the Western sense is deval- competence (Doi, 1973; Kondo-Ikemura &
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 335
Matsuoka, 1999; Lebra, 1994; Mizuta, ners with various attachment classifica-
Zahn-Waxler, Cole, & Hiruma, 1996).3 tions. We will focus on relationships
In summary, the antecedents (sensitiv- involving a female preoccupied partner
ity) and consequences (social competence) and a male dismissive partner because
of secure attachment and insecure-am- that is one of the most common patterns
bivalent attachment are viewed and of insecure attachment. First, we briefly
are manifested very differently in Japan review the attachment evidence and how
than in the West. Security in Japan arises it meshes with family systems theory.
from extremely close and interdependent Next we discuss cultural differences that
mother-child interactions that, according lead us to believe that the attachment of
to Western theorists, promote insecure at- couples functions differently and has a dif-
tachment; moreover, behaviors associated ferent meaning in Japan. We suggest that
with ambivalent attachment, such as ex- the larger family system, as well as the
treme dependency, are more adaptive in spousal and parent-child relationships,
Japan than in the US. Mother-child en- are likely to function somewhat differ-
meshment, which is viewed as pathologi- ently in Japan, and that attachment and
cal by both attachment and family sys- family systems theorists’ interpretations
tems theorists in the US, is viewed much are much more relevant to the Western
more positively by Japanese theorists. experience.

MARITAL AND FAMILY RELATIONSHIPS: Evidence from Attachment Theory


TOO CLOSE, TOO FAR The marital dyad: There is considerable
In the last section we focused on the evidence that adults with preoccupied at-
mother-child attachment relationship; in tachment styles tend to form romantic re-
this section we focus on the marital at- lationships with partners who have dis-
tachment relationship and the mother- missive attachment styles. (As noted ear-
father-child triad. According to Marvin & lier, avoidant and dismissive refer to the
Stewart (1990), family therapists explain same category, as do preoccupied and am-
the enmeshed family system as follows: bivalent; see endnote 2). Findings indi-
cate that adults with avoidant attach-
ment tend to respond more favorably to,
It is likely that the spouse dyad lacks an
adequate degree of intimacy and/or conflict
and to be more involved with, those with
resolution strategies. The emotional dis- ambivalent attachment, and that rela-
tance between the spouses leads to resent- tionships involving an ambivalent female
ment between them and makes it difficult as and an avoidant male are quite stable
well to work together as parents. Mother although not very happy (Feeney, 1994;
begins to experience her intimacy within her Kirkpatrick & Davis, 1994).
relationship with the child rather than with The central dynamic in pairings of pre-
her husband/parenting partner, and father occupied women and dismissive men is
focuses his energy on his job, hobbies, an- what has been termed “too close, too far”
other child, etc. [p. 81] or pursuing-distancing. In general, preoc-
cupied adults desire extremely close and
Research on partner attachments in intimate relations (Feeney, 1999) and
adulthood provides support for this por- seek near, constant contact (Byng-Hall,
trait of marital relations. This research 1999, p. 628), whereas dismissing adults
examines the frequency, stability and na- need to maintain distance (Feeney, 1999)
ture (e.g., intimacy, patterns of communi- as seen in their unwillingness to engage
cation, and conflict) of pairings of part- in romantic, affectionate, or other attach-

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


336 / FAMILY PROCESS

ment-related behaviors (Fraley, Davis, & had the most damaging effects. Recent
Shaver, 1998). As the preoccupied partner research indicates that parents’ nega-
escalates the appeal to have dependency tive conflict strategies predict children’s
needs met, the dismissing partner in- negative representations of the mother-
creases his defensive response of distanc- father-child triad (Shamir, Du Rocher-
ing, which leads to cycles of pursuer-dis- Schudlich, & Cummings, 2001).
tancer interaction (Fisher & Crandall, Most findings regarding the association
1997). The more that the ambivalent between marital quality and parent-child
partner focuses on the partner’s lack of relationships (Erel & Burman, 1995) re-
accessibility, the more that the avoidant port a direct relationship between the two
partner restricts attention to the environ- (i.e., negative marital relations are asso-
ment, and vice versa. In this way, the ciated with negative parent-child rela-
pursuer-distancer cycle becomes an ongo- tions). A common interpretation of these
ing feature of the relationship (Bar- findings is that spouses in negative mar-
tholomew & Horwitz, 1991; Pistole, 1994). ital dyads satisfy their needs for closeness
One of the most outstanding features of via their children, and that the quality of
these couples is their propensity for con- the parent-child relationship is generally
flict. The conflict often centers on the pre- poor in such cases.
occupied partner’s expression of discon- These findings are consistent with
tent, deprivation, and abandonment, and those suggested by family-oriented at-
the dismissing partner’s tendency to at- tachment theorists. According to Byng-
tribute the relational problems to the Hall (1999, p. 633):
partner’s discontent (Byng-Hall, 1999;
Feeney, 1999). Compared to secure adults, Following pursuer-distancer escalations
ambivalent and avoidant adults use less between parents, one parent is likely to turn
compromise and more defensive and to a child . . . A child’s mixed feelings about
destructive conflict resolution strategies the parents’ marriage— on the one hand,
(Gaines, Reis, Summers, et al., 1997; Pis- wanting one parent to himself or herself; on
the other, wanting the parents to be to-
tole, 1989). Ambivalent adults report high
gether—means that the child will try to di-
levels of distress, hostility, and anxiety vide the parents when they get too close and
when discussing problems; and avoidant unite them when they seem too far apart . . .
men engage in lower quality interaction A variation on this pattern is a child who
(Simpson, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996). In- ‘captures’ a parent and excludes all others,
terestingly, ambivalent adults had more including the other parent; capturing is
negative perceptions of their partners af- maintained if it resolves the parent’s dis-
ter important discussions regardless of tance conflict.
the quality of the discussion, suggesting
that they enter the discussions with en- Several studies (reviewed in Davies &
trenched negative expectations (Feeney, Cummings, 1994) indicate that children
1998). of insecure marriages (the major form of
The mother-father-child triad: What which is the ambivalent-avoidant pairing)
are the effects of these marital conflicts on often take action in response to parental
child functioning and larger family dy- disputes. These children comfort, defend,
namics? In a meta-analysis, Davies & or distract parents during conflicts, espe-
Cummings (1994) found that marital dis- cially their mothers. Children’s misbe-
cord, more than marital distress or mari- havior, intended to distract the parents, is
tal apathy, led to a variety of child adjust- reinforced if the parents’ interrupt their
ment problems, and that overt conflict conflict with one another to attend to (e.g.,
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 337
discipline) the child, which may explain different meaning and consequences be-
the association between marital conflict cause it is societally sanctioned. A com-
and child problems. mon expression in Japan, “Teishu tassha
Research by family systems theorists de rusu ga ii,” expresses mothers’ wish
indicates that children of conflicting part- that the father stay healthy and cheerful,
ners may sometimes become ill or develop but away from the home. Though Japa-
an emotional problem so as to stabilize nese mothers increasingly want hus-
the marital dyad by bringing the parents bands to share household and childrear-
together to look after the child (e.g., ing responsibilities, this phrase expresses
Minuchin, Rosman, & Baker, 1978). Re- mothers’ adaptation to the demands of
search by Marvin (1992) builds on these their husbands’ work.
findings from family systems theory. Mar- Many Japanese fathers continue to
vin found that, in a group of adolescents spend long hours away from home. A
suffering from psychogenic pain, one or study of middle- to upper-middle-class fa-
both of the parents were “preoccupied” by thers indicated they spent 54 hours a
threatened or real loss, and that their at- week at work not including many evening
tachment behavior was activated by the and weekend hours, as contrasted with 42
child’s illness. Marvin developed an inter- hours in the US (Ishii-Kuntz, 1993). In
vention for these parents, the final “most addition, more than twice as many Japa-
important” step of which was for them to nese (37.4%) as U.S. (14.7%) fathers claim
go out on a date together once a week. they “never” engage in father-child inter-
This intervention was designed to disen- action during weekdays—the correspond-
tangle the marriage from the parent-child ing figures on weekends are 17.1% and
relationship: i.e., to reverse the pattern 5.1% (reported in Ishii-Kuntz, 1993). The
set up by the child’s symptom. At a Japanese fathers’ lack of guilt about their
6-month followup, 90% of the children limited interaction could be seen as fur-
were symptom free. These findings high- ther indication of their dismissiveness;
light Western investigators’ concerns yet, “they seem to think that their hard
about overinvolved parent-child ties and work is appreciated by their family, and
their emphasis on the therapeutic value therefore worth their absences from
of increased marital intimacy. home” (Ishii-Kuntz, 1993, p. 55). Paternal
distance is also a vehicle for maintaining
Cultural Differences one’s role as an authority figure (Ishii-
We believe that there are important Kuntz, 1993). Thus, fathers’ distance has
cultural differences in the patterns of a very different and less negative mean-
marital and family relationships de- ing in Japan than in the US.
scribed above. There are several features of preoccu-
Preoccupied-dismissing relationships: pied/ambivalent attachment that are
Instances in which the “. . . mother begins likely to have more aversive consequences
to experience her intimacy within her re- in the West than in Japan. Ambivalent
lationship with the child rather than with women are most likely to endorse items
her husband, and the father turns his en- such as “I can’t seem to stop from moth-
ergy to his job . . .” are viewed as mal- ering my partner too much” that are
adaptive if not pathological in the West viewed as compulsive caregiving and as
(Marvin & Stewart, 1990, p. 80). The com- intrusive in the West (Kunce & Shaver,
bination of “overinvolved” mother-child 1994). These women prefer unqualified
ties and distance between spouses is closeness, commitment, and affection, and
much more common in Japan, but it has a tend to idealize their partners (Feeney,

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


338 / FAMILY PROCESS

1999). No doubt this is why conflicts with relationships, but the Japanese place less
their romantic partners are so intense value on such relationships. Second,
and why they need to transfer their com- when behaviors associated with the pre-
pulsive caregiving and unqualified close- occupied-dismissing spousal relationship
ness to the infant. In Japan, where ro- occur in Japan, they are less likely to be
mance is expected to abate after marriage accompanied by open conflict. Open con-
and children (evidence reviewed below), flict has very negative effects on the mar-
and where extremely close relationships ital relationship (Gottman, 1991), and
with infants are sanctioned, women with highly deleterious effects on children (Da-
behavior labeled preoccupied in the West vies & Cummings, 1994). These points are
are likely to be better prepared for the elaborated upon, below.
diminished closeness with their romantic Romantic relationships: Here we re-
partners, and to find a socially sanctioned view evidence that secure adult relation-
outlet for their need for emotional and ships in the West are based on very dif-
physical closeness. These findings suggest ferent characteristics than are secure
that behaviors labeled preoccupied and adult relationships in Japan.
dismissing in the West may be more com- In the West, marital relationships are
mon and less maladaptive in Japan. seen as secure when they are based on
Based on a review of Western studies, romance, verbal intimacy, and sexuality.
Davies and Cummings (1994) propose Attachment theorists provide abundant
that child wellbeing is fostered when chil- evidence linking security with romance in
dren have a secure model of the parents’ adulthood. There is much more research
relationship. That is, children’s security on attachment between sexual mates
may depend in part on parents’ attach- than between any other adult partners
ment to one another. Since distance be- (e.g., same-sex friends or blood relatives).
tween spouses is more accepted in Japan The reason for this emphasis is clear:
than the US, Japanese children may view “Bowlby claim[ed] . . . that sexual part-
such relationships as more secure, high- ners assume the role of attachment fig-
lighting the complexity of the link be- ures in adult life,” and, in subsequent re-
tween perceived security of parents’ rela- search by other investigators, “the com-
tionship and child functioning. What mon practice [has been to use] romantic
counts as a secure model of the parents’ relationships as the context for investi-
relationship is likely to vary across cul- gating adult attachment phenomena”
ture. Moreover, in Japan, a secure model (Hazan & Zeifman, 1999, p. 340).
of the parents’ relationship may be rela- In Japan, there is greater valuing of
tively less important than a secure model close relationships that are based on har-
of the family relationship, which may in- mony (i.e., mutual accommodation, ab-
volve grandparents and even ancestors sence of conflict, and enduring loyalty)
(Klass, 2001), as well as the nuclear fam- than on relationships based on romance,
ily (cf. Marvin & Stewart’s, 1990, discus- verbal intimacy, and sexuality (Yama-
sion of the secure family base). gishi & Yamagishi, 1994). Since romance
There are other reasons why Japanese is difficult to sustain overtime (beyond a
spouses who engage in behaviors labeled few years), it is not particularly conducive
preoccupied and dismissing in the West to enduring, committed, and guaranteed
may fare better than their Western coun- relationships. While romantic relation-
terparts. First, in all cultures, preoccu- ships before marriage are highly valued
pied and dismissing parents are unlikely in Japan as well as the US (Bando, 1992),
to experience verbally intimate, romantic it is only in the US that romantic love
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 339
continues to bind mates even after mar- refer to each other as otoosan (father) and
riage and children (DeVos, 1985; Dion & okaasan (mother), reflecting the primacy
Dion, 1993; Iwao, 1993). In Japan, there of their parental roles. In Japan, wives’
is less emphasis on romance after mar- and husbands’ roles and responsibilities
riage and children and more emphasis on center on the family, including grandpar-
loyalty and commitment4 (Rothbaum, ents and ancestors as well as the child,
Pott, et al., 2000). Whereas U.S. marital rather than centering on each other.
partners continue to emphasize physical Western partners’ focus on their dyadic
attraction and eros, in Japanese mar- relationship may explain why they expe-
riages romance is diluted by pragmatic rience so much marital distress shortly
and dependency needs (reviewed in Roth- after the birth of a child (reviewed in Bel-
baum, Pott, et al., 2000). sky & Rovine, 1990).
In Japan, mutual attraction, and other A study of marital partners conducted
needs of the dyad (e.g., for intimacy), are by the Japanese Association of Sex Edu-
not viewed as the most important criteria cation (1987) indicates that Americans,
for preserving a marriage. When asked more so than Japanese, attribute success-
about a couple that wanted to get di- ful marriages to factors having to do with
vorced, 93% of Japanese, but only 39% of the dyad—i.e., partnership, intimacy, fi-
Americans, said that the couple should delity, sexual satisfaction, sharing of con-
stay married just for the sake of the chil- cerns and activities, and romance. This
dren (Soumuchou, 1987). Even if they are same study indicates a much greater per-
estranged, Japanese couples tend to stay centage of American than Japanese part-
together in the same home for the sake of ners who report couple-related activities,
the children (katei nai rikon refers to di- including recreation, going out together
vorce within the family home). In Japan, as a couple, and sexual intercourse. The
stability of marital relations relates more difference between US and Japanese
to complementarity in roles, successful adults mentioning “enjoyment between
rearing of children, and participation in the two” is a striking 70% (see Durrett,
wider kin ties and less to refueling pas- Richards, Otaki, et al., 1986, for similar
sion (Imamura, 1987; Iwao, 1993; Vogel, findings). Taking the focus off the marital
1996). According to Jing Hsu: “the core dyad is likely to reduce the negative con-
relationship in the . . . Japanese family is sequences of a partnership that Western
not that of the husband and wife, as in the investigators label preoccupied-dismis-
Caucasian families, but that between the sive.
parent and the child” (Hsu, Tseng, Ashton, Family life in Japan has changed in
et al., 1987, p. 357). The greater emphasis profound ways over the last 50 years, and
on the mother-child dyad and lesser em- continues to undergo transformation. For
phasis on the marital relationship in Ja- example, the Japanese Government has
pan than in the US makes behaviors as- become increasingly concerned with fa-
sociated with the preoccupied-dismissing thers’ absence and has shown interest in
partnership less problematic in Japan. increasing men’s involvement with their
Romance and parental roles: Clearly children (reported in Ishii-Kuntz, 1993);
defined role relationships contribute to and intimacy between marital partners is
the loyalty and stability of mate relation- more common. Yet, we believe that under-
ships in Japan. Traditionally, the wife’s lying patterns remain intact. While fa-
sphere has been inside the home, espe- thers are more involved in care of children
cially when children are young. As soon as than previously, adolescents in Japan
the first child is born, husbands and wives spend only a third as much time with

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


340 / FAMILY PROCESS

their fathers as American adolescents same-gender friendships to obtain emo-


(Sengoku, 1994). And while intimacy (sin- tional intimacy. Japanese mothers are
mitsu) is more emphasized in Japan to- more likely to say they would consult with
day, the word is rarely used. A person who friends (42%) than with husbands (18%)
feels intimacy toward another would be when they have concerns about childrear-
unlikely to put the feeling into words; the ing (Hokkaido Private Kindergarten As-
Japanese believe that if feelings are deep, sociation, 1997). While corresponding fig-
you do not need to put them into words. ures are not available from the US, they
Romance and its effect on the child: Be- are likely to differ because of the empha-
cause sexuality and romance are so cen- sis on maintaining the closeness of the
tral to the marital dyad and are so inter- marital dyad (e.g., Gottman, 1999). The
twined with spouses’ attachment in the wider network of supports available in
West, it is not surprising that issues of Japan may reduce pressure on the mari-
sexuality and romance tend to emerge in tal partnership and may render the neg-
other family relationships. This is seen, ative consequences of marital distance
for example, in child-parent attraction, less severe than in the US (cf. McLoyd,
(i.e., oedipal and electra phenomena), Harper, & Copeland, 2001, for a similar
which appear to be relatively common in point regarding ethnic minority families
the US (Watson & Getz, 1990). In Japan, in the US). For these reasons, behaviors
the family is less a locus of romantic re- commonly associated with the preoccu-
lationships. Lebra (1994), a Japanese psy- pied-dismissive pattern may have differ-
chologist, notes that Western investiga- ent implications for family dynamics and
tors are prone to mistakenly interpret child functioning in the two cultures.
commonly occurring, close bodily contact Parental conflict: A major cultural dif-
between Japanese parents and young ference in marital relationships is that, in
children (e.g., co-sleeping and co-bathing) the West as compared to Japan, conflict is
as having a sexual element to it. Simi- far more common. Marvin and Stewart
larly, Allison (2000) describes a Japanese (1990) assume that “there will be frequent
TV show for children depicting maternal conflict within the family regarding how
breasts, which she notes are likely to be proximity and contact will be organized”
viewed as sexual by American, but not by (p. 365), and Byng-Hall (1999) suggests
Japanese, audiences. that conflict among family members and
The Japanese find the association be- expression of anger is “a functional part of
tween sexuality and maternal care diffi- family life” (p. 627). Attachment is seen as
cult to comprehend because, for them, the providing a safe base for family members
sexual and attachment systems are not to engage in conflict (Byng-Hall, 1999).
closely linked; the link is weak in the mar- While Western authors believe that high
ital dyad (i.e., after children are born), levels of conflict are dysfunctional, espe-
and it is irrelevant in other familial rela- cially if the conflict is not resolved, mod-
tionships. As a result, if children do be- erate levels that are resolved are consid-
come triangulated by their parents, and a ered benign or even adaptive. The belief
too-close parent-child relationship devel- in the normality of conflict, which is
ops, that relationship is unlikely to have widely accepted by family systems theo-
sexual overtones. rists (e.g., Nichols & Schwartz, 1998;
Romance and the wider social network: Satir, Stachowiak, & Taschman, 1975), is
The relatively lesser emphasis on the supported by findings from Western stud-
marital dyad in Japan may be associated ies— e.g., limited amounts of disagree-
with the relatively greater reliance on ment and angry exchanges are not harm-
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 341
ful to relationships (cf. Gottman, 1999) protection and care that characterize
and, when conflicts are resolved, they are mothers and children, and marital part-
not harmful to children (reviewed in Da- ners, who are engaged in these patterns.
vies & Cummings, 1994). However, the evidence comparing Ja-
Open conflict in the family is much less pan and the US indicates that many of
common in Japan, and even low levels of the behaviors associated with these inse-
conflict are less accepted. Ninety percent cure attachment styles have different
of Japanese report arguing with spouses meanings (i.e., evaluations and interpre-
less than once a month (Long, 1996). In tations) in the two countries. Japanese
Japan, direct communication is avoided mothers are expected to form extremely
because it leads to open conflict, which is close, even symbiotic relations with their
seen as unhealthy, even when it is not children; they value behavior that, by US
extreme (Hsu et al., 1987; Rothbaum, Pott, standards, is overinvolved, intrusive, and
et al., 2000). “Because one of the ways of overindulgent of dependency needs, and
maintaining group cohesiveness and har- that has been shown to foster insecure
mony is to avoid open conflict, the clear (ambivalent) attachment in Western chil-
and straightforward statement of one’s dren. Japanese children are expected to
own opinions and feelings may risk con- exhibit extreme expressions of need for
frontation and possible conflict, therefore, care and attention, extensive clinging and
less direct and perhaps even somewhat proximity seeking, helpless dependency,
evasive communication are more adaptive passivity, blurring of boundaries between
in [East Asian] cultures” (Hsu et al., 1987, self and other, and other behaviors asso-
p. 357). ciated with ambivalent attachment in the
West. Despite this manifest similarity in
CONCLUSION overt behavior, the underlying meaning of
the behavior is very different.
Summary and Elaboration Similarly, spousal patterns of behavior
There are important points of conver- that are associated with insecure attach-
gence between attachment theory and ment in the West, particularly the wife
family systems theory, but some of the who engages in compulsive, dependency-
dynamics about which they agree may op- fostering caregiving, and the workaholic
erate differently in non-Western cultures. husband who spends little time at home,
Attachment investigators’ description of are more common and are viewed as more
preoccupied mothers and ambivalent chil- normative and healthy in Japan. Japa-
dren contribute in important ways to our nese couples with this profile are less
understanding of enmeshed mother-child likely to be dissatisfied with one another
dyads—a major concern in the family sys- because they are less focused on the mar-
tems literature. Attachment investiga- ital dyad and more focused on the child
tors’ research on partners with preoccu- and family. Moreover, Japanese couples
pied and dismissive styles provides evi- are less likely to exhibit the kind of overt
dence of the kinds of pursuer-distancer conflict that is associated with these cou-
cycles and spousal conflicts that family ples in the West; as a result, Japanese
systems theorists have long considered children are less likely to experience the
important elements in enmeshed rela- problems common among children from
tionships. Attachment theory has much to high-conflict families.
tell us about the antecedents, stability, The larger point is that principles of
and consequences of these attachment attachment and family systems that are
styles, and about the underlying needs for based on research in the West do not nec-

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


342 / FAMILY PROCESS

essarily apply in cultures with different Kitayama, 1991; Sampson, 1988; Spence,
values and experiences. As noted above, 1985; Weisz et al., 1984a,b).
behaviors that in the West comprise a This Western bias is evidenced by find-
maladaptive pattern known as enmesh- ings that Japanese-American families are
ment, are seen as more normative and evaluated as less competent and less
more adaptive in Japan. The Japanese healthy than Caucasian families, even
phenomenon of ittaikan, which refers to when the evaluations are by Japanese-
extremely close relationships character- American as well as Caucasian raters.
ized by an absence of boundary between Hsu et al. (1987) found that Caucasians
self and other, is seen as a sign of matu- are rated more favorably than Japanese
rity. The same phenomenon would be on a variety of measures including empa-
seen as a sign of regression and would be thy, expressiveness, invasiveness, clarity,
considered pathological in the West. Be- and closeness, as well as on overall
haviors that are relatively rare and mal- health-pathology. We believe that reli-
adaptive in one culture may be prevalent ance on Western-based assumptions about
and accepted in another culture, if those what is normative and healthy contrib-
behaviors are serving very different func- utes to biased interpretations in which
tions in the two contexts. Behaviors are other cultures and ethnic groups are
embedded within a larger network of re- likely to be evaluated negatively. Even
lationships (extended family, community, Japanese raters who are raised in the US
ancestors) and social institutions (educa- are prone to these biases, especially if
tional, religious, economic, political, etc.) they rely on Western measures.
that support a particular experience of
closeness. Qualifications
Some attachment and family systems Throughout this article, we have em-
theorists are sensitive to the cultural is- phasized cultural differences; yet, we are
sues we are emphasizing here. Byng-Hall equally concerned about intra-cultural
(1999) and Marvin and Stewart (1990) do differences. Research comparing Chinese-
not make claims of universality and they American and European Americans in
mention ways in which the phenomena the US (Rothbaum, Morelli, Pott, & Liu-
they describe might manifest themselves Constant, 2000) indicates within-culture
differently in other cultures or subcul- differences that in important respects
tures. They note that dynamics which parallel the cultural differences we high-
Western theorists typically attribute to light here:
the marital couple could instead involve a European-Americans’ practices and be-
parent-grandparent dyad or the extended liefs related to family relationships were
family. As eloquently stated by Marvin & best organized by the theme of romance,
Stewart (1990, pp. 63– 64): “attachment and Chinese-American practices and be-
caregiver relations exist within a network liefs were best organized by the theme of
of ordered relations and cannot fully be harmony. For example, European-Ameri-
understood except in that context.” Still, cans parents were much more likely than
the history of Western science indicates their Chinese-American counterparts to
that presumptions about universality are emphasize spousal exclusivity and inti-
common when evidence is gathered macy and to be permissive regarding nu-
within Western cultures, and when other dity and sexuality in their children. In-
cultures are not examined (Berscheid, tracultural differences in Japan also war-
1995; Bruner, 1990; Gergen, Gulerce, rant examination: socioeconomic, rural-
Lock, & Misra, 1996; Markus and urban, gender and age-related differences
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 343
are likely to provide valuable contrasts tion of these similarities in family pat-
(Azuma et al., 1981; Hendry, 1995; Iwao, terns associated with school phobia leads
1993; White, 1993). For example, certain to the detection of yet other differences:
stereotypes about Japanese mothers, Japanese clinicians maintain that school
such as their unfailingly responsiveness phobia is manifested by “good children
and warmth, may mask important cul- who want to go to school,” (Kameguchi &
tural variation in maternal acceptance vs. Murphy-Shigematsu, 2001, p. 66), they
rejection (Behrens, 2001). suggest that “mothers’ overinvolvement
Generational differences, especially in in their children’s lives may be a way of
Japan which is undergoing dramatic freeing the fathers to work,” (p. 67), and
Westernization, also warrant mention. they partially attribute the children’s re-
We suspect that, in 21st century Japan, fusal to their natural desire to have their
there are more negative feelings about dependency needs indulged by their
dependence and interdependence than mothers—“a psychological concept re-
even 10 years ago, particularly in urban ferred to in Japanese as amae” (p. 67).
areas. The Japanese Ministry of Educa- Western experts are less likely to explain
tion now explicitly stresses the impor- school refusal in these ways (indeed,
tance of children’s individuality, auton- there is no exact counterpart to amae in
omy, and expressivity, in an intentional the West). This example highlights the
effort to foster more Western ways of complex interplay between cultural dif-
thinking and behaving (Holloway, 1997).5 ferences and commonalities: the search
Despite our emphasis on cultural differ- for differences highlights underlying com-
ences, we acknowledge that impressive monalities and the examination of those
intercultural similarities have been docu- commonalities highlights new differences.
mented by attachment investigators (van
IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999) and by re- Therapeutic Implications
searchers interested in parental conflict The findings reviewed here suggest
(Cummings & Davies, 2002). Beneath that, when working with Japanese fami-
the cultural differences that we have em- lies, therapists should: (a) not assume
phasized lie important similarities be- that extremely close and/or dependent
tween Japan and the West in both attach- mother-child relations are pathological or
ment and family systems dynamics. For sexualized, nor that the marital couple’s
example, there are clinical reports that failure to find time alone together weak-
Japanese children who suffer from school ens the overall family, or is seen as a
phobia have family patterns like those de- problem by either partner; (b) be espe-
scribed in the West— extremely high lev- cially respectful of role-prescribed behav-
els of mother-child closeness and avoid- iors endorsed in the wider culture; and (c)
ance by fathers (Kameguchi & Murphy- rely on nonverbal forms of communication
Shigematsu, 2001). While we believe that rather than require family members to be
this pattern is more common and more verbally explicit.
adaptive in Japan than in the West, the To maximize his or her effectiveness,
clinical reports suggest that, even in Ja- the therapist working with Japanese fam-
pan, the most severe instances of this ilies should not maintain the distant, per-
family pattern may be associated with sonally disengaged stance demanded by
problems that are similar to those it en- both Western science and Western psy-
genders in the West. chotherapy (Bankart, Koshikawa, Ne-
The dialectic of similarity and differ- date, & Haruki, 1992; Roland, 1989).
ence does not end there. Close examina- Rather, the therapist should be willing to

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


344 / FAMILY PROCESS

forge a close relationship with family generally, we use the terms preoccu-
members and to allow clients to assume pied and dismissive.
3
highly dependent and passive behavior We are not suggesting that most Japa-
that would be regarded as counter thera- nese are insecurely attached. Two
peutic in the U.S. (Bankart et al., 1992; characteristics of insecure-ambiva-
Hsu et al., 1987; Roland, 1989). In addi- lent subjects that are not at all com-
tion, Japanese therapists may be most mon among Japanese are uncertainty
effective when they recruit respected about caregivers’ availability because
members of the family members’ social of caregiver inconsistency, and diffi-
network to help resolve conflicts (cf. Hsu culty regulating negative affect. Our
et al., 1987). These and other differences point is that Japanese children and
between best therapeutic practice with children classified as insecure-ambiv-
US and Japanese families will become alent in the US share several salient
more obvious as we further investigate qualities. Perhaps this is why many
attachment dynamics and family systems Japanese are classified as ambiv-
in Japan. We suspect, and other authors alently attached (Miyake, Chen, &
have speculated (e.g., Hsu et al., 1987; Campos, 1985; Takahashi, 1990; but
Rothbaum, Morelli, et al., 2000), that see Durrett, Otaki, & Richards, 1984,
many of the differences to which we are for findings more similar to those ob-
alluding pertain to best practice with tained in the US). For a detailed dis-
other East Asian families as well. cussion of Japanese-US differences in
the meaning and manifestation of at-
ENDNOTES tachment behaviors, see Rothbaum,
1
Weisz, et al. (2000).
Marvin and Stewart (1990) also link 4
Interestingly, US dyadic relationships
these behaviors to the insecure con- are depicted as empty when they in-
trolling category. However, they refer clude commitment but not passion or
to “one” classificatory group and, as intimacy (Sternberg, 1986). By con-
the evidence we review indicates, trast, “Unconditional loyalty and
most of these behaviors pertain to compassion is central to Japanese
the insecure ambivalent category. morality just as love is central to US
2
The terms ambivalent and preoccupied morality” (Miyanaga, 1991). Compan-
refer to fundamentally the same ionable forms of love are more com-
adult attachment category, as do the mon in Japan— close, long-lasting
terms avoidant and dismissive. In friendships characterized by endur-
past research on adults, the terms ing commitment (Rothbaum, Pott, et
ambivalent and avoidant have typi- al., 2000). To the extent that ro-
cally been used when the attachment mance is emphasized, it is more
classification is based on self-report viewed as a mental and physical va-
measures, and the terms preoccupied cation than as an expression of libidi-
and dismissive have typically been nal impulses (Hendry, 1995; Lebra,
used when the attachment classifi- 1994; Iwao, 1993). Saying that a
cation is based on the Adult At- marital relationship is “like air”
tachment Interview. When discussing (smooth, relaxed, and harmonious) is a
particular findings, our use of terms compliment in Japan (Iwao, 1993, p.
is consistent with these differences 95). It is the familiarity and ease of the
in methods. However, when referring relationship, not its novelty and pas-
to adult attachment categories more sion, that sustains it (Iwao, 1993).
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 345
This is probably partly responsible for close relationships. New York: Guilford
the much lower divorce rate in Japan. Press.
5
Yet it is important not to overstate the Behrens, K. (1999). Japanese and American
generational differences. For exam- mother-child relationship: Comparison of
attuning behaviors during play. Unpub-
ple, self-expression in present day Ja-
lished manuscript, University of California,
pan may mean different things than
Berkeley.
in the US. Most Japanese parents Behrens, K. (2001). Working models of parent-
and educators who advocate self-ex- ing and parental efficacy: The case of Japa-
pression seem to regard it as some- nese mothers. Unpublished manuscript,
thing the child should be able to do University of California, Berkeley.
when prompted by his teacher or Belsky, J. (1999a). Modern evolutionary the-
other adults, or in the peer context. ory and patterns of attachment (pp. 141–
The Japanese view self-expression as 161). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.),
more a matter of clearly answering a Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
question rather than as spontaneously and clinical applications. New York: Guil-
expressing a personal feeling; even in ford Press.
Belsky, J. (1999b). Interactional and contex-
21st century Japan, the latter form of
tual determinants of attachment security
self-expression is not often valued, es-
(pp. 249 –264). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver
pecially in the presence of adults. (eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, re-
search, and clinical applications. New York:
REFERENCES Guilford Press.
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment:
& Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A Theory and evidence (pp. 372– 402). In M.
psychological study of the strange situation. Rutter & D. Hay (eds.). Development
Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ- through life: A handbook for clinicians. Ox-
ates. ford: Blackwell.
Allison, A. (2000). Permitted and prohibited Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1990). Patterns of
desires: Mothers, comics and censorship in marital change across the transition to par-
Japan. Berkeley: University of California enthood. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
Press. ily 52: 5–19.
Azuma, Kashiwagi, K., & Hess, R. (1981). The Berscheid, E. (1995). Help wanted: A grand
influence of attitude and behavior upon the theorist of interpersonal relationships, soci-
child’s intellectual development. Tokyo: Uni- ologist or anthropologist preferred. Journal
versity of Tokyo Press. of Social and Personal Relationships 12:
Bando, M. (1992). Data bank of Japanese 529 –533.
women [in Japanese]. Tokyo: Ministry of Fi- Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of
nance. affectional bonds. London: Tavistock Publi-
Bankart, P. Koshikawa, F., Nedate, K., & Ha- cations.
ruki, Y. (1992). When West meets East: Con- Bretherton, I. (1995). A communication per-
tributions of Eastern traditions to the future spective on attachment relationships and in-
of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy 29: 141– ternal working models (pp. 310 –329). In E.
149. Waters, B. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K. Kondo-
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L.M. (1991). At- Ikemura (eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and
tachment styles among young adults: A test cognitive perspectives on secure-base behav-
of a four-category model. Journal of Person- ior and working models. Monographs of the
ality and Social Psychology 61: 226 –244. Society for Research in Child Development
Bartholomew, K., & Shaver, P.R. (1998). 60 [Serial No. 244].
Methods of assessing adult attachment: Do Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cam-
they change? (pp. 25– 45). In J.A. Simpson & bridge: Harvard University Press.
W.S. Rholes (eds.), Attachment theory and Byng-Hall J. (1999). Family and couple ther-

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


346 / FAMILY PROCESS

apy: Toward greater security (pp. 625– 645). and the cultural context of love and inti-
In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.), Hand- macy, Journal of Social Issues 49: 53– 69.
book of attachment: Theory, research, and Durrett, M.E., Otaki, M., & Richards, P.
clinical applications. New York: Guilford (1984). Attachment and the mother’s per-
Press. ception of support from the father. Interna-
Carlson, V., & Harwood, R. (in press). Attach- tional Journal of Behavioral Development 7:
ment, culture, and the caregiving system: 162–176.
The cultural patterning of everyday experi- Durrett, M.E., Richards, P., Otaki, M., Penne-
ences among Anglo and Puerto Rican moth- baker, J.W., & Nyquist, L. (1986). Mother’s
er-infant pairs. Infant Mental Health Jour- involvement with her infant and her percep-
nal. tion of spousal support, Japan and America.
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment Journal of Marriage and the Family 48:
and the self in six-year-olds. Child Develop- 187–194.
ment 59: 121–131. Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelated-
Cassidy, J. (1999). The nature of child’s ties ness of marital relations and parent-child
(pp. 3–20). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver relations: A meta-analytical review. Psycho-
(eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, re- logical Bulletin 118: 108 –132.
search, and clinical applications. New York: Feeney, J.A. (1994). Attachment style, com-
Guilford Press. munication patterns, and satisfaction across
Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. (1994). The insecure/ the life cycle of marriage. Personal Relation-
ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory ships 2: 143–159.
and research. Child Development 65: 971– Feeney, J.A. (1998). Adult attachment and re-
991. lationship-centered anxiety: Responses to
Cassidy J., & Shaver, P.R. (eds.). (1999). physical and emotional distancing (pp. 189 –
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, 218). In J.A. Simpson & W.A., Rholes (eds.),
and clinical applications. New York: Guil- Attachment theory and close relationships.
ford Press. New York: Guilford Press.
Christensen, A., & Jacobson, N.S. (2000). Rec- Feeney, J.A. (1999). Adult romantic attach-
oncilable differences. New York: Guilford ment and couple relationships (pp.
Press. 355–377). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.),
Clancy, P.M. (1986). The acquisition of com- Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
municative style in Japanese (pp. 213–250). and clinical applications. New York: Guil-
In B.B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (eds.), Lan- ford Press.
guage of socialization across cultures. New Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common
York: Cambridge University Press. themes and cultural variations in Japanese
Cummings, E.M., & Davies, P.T. (2002). Ef- and American mothers’ speech to infants.
fects of marital discord on children: Recent Child Development 64: 637– 656.
advances and emerging themes in process- Fisher, J.V., & Crandell, L.E. (1997). Complex
oriented research. Journal of Child Psychol- attachment: Patterns of relating in the cou-
ogy and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines ple. Sexual and Marital Therapy 12: 211–
43: 31– 63. 224.
Davies, P.T., & Cummings, E.M. (1994). Mar- Fiske, A.P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., &
ital conflict and child adjustment: An emo- Nisbett, R.E., (1998). The cultural matrix of
tional security hypothesis. Psychological social psychology (pp. 915–981). In D. Gil-
Bulletin 116: 387– 411. bert, S.T. Fiske, G. Lindzey (eds.), The
DeVos, G. (1985). Dimensions of the self in handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol.
Japanese culture (pp. 141–184). In A. 2). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Marsella, G. DeVos, & F. Hsu (eds.), Culture Fogarty, T.F. (1976). Marital crisis (pp. 325–
and self: Asian and Western Perspectives. 334). In P.J. Guerin (ed.), Family therapy:
New York: Tavistock Publications. Theory and practice. New York: Gardner
Dion, K.K., & Dion, K.L. (1993). Individualis- Press.
tic and collectivistic perspectives on gender Gaines, S.O., Reis, H.T., Summers, S., Rus-
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 347
bult, C.E., Cox, C.L., Wexler, M.O., search, and clinical applications. New York:
Marelich, W.D., & Kurland, G.J. (1997). Im- Guilford Press.
pact of attachment style on reactions to ac- Hokkaido Private Kindergarten Association
commodative dilemmas in close relation- (1997). Unpublished survey. Hokkaido, Ja-
ships. Personal Relationships 4: 93–113. pan.
George, C., & Solomon, J. (1999). The caregiv- Holloway, S. (April, 1997). Images of close re-
ing behavioral system (pp. 649 – 670). In J. lationships in Japanese Preschools. Paper
Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.), Handbook of presented at the Society for Research in
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical Child Development, Washington DC.
applications. New York: Guilford Press. Hsu, J., Tseng, W., Ashton, G., McDermott,
Gergen, K.J., Gulerce, A., Lock, A., & Misra, J.F., & Char, W. (1987). Symposium on cul-
G. (1996). Psychological science in cultural tural aspects of family psychiatry. Interna-
context. American Psychologist 51: 496 –503. tional Journal of Family Psychiatry 8: 349 –
Goldberg, S., Blokland, K., Cayetano, P., & 361.
Benoit, D. (1998). Across the transmission Ishii-Kuntz, M. (1993). Japanese fathers:
gap: Adult attachment and response to in- Work demands and family roles (pp. 45– 67).
fant emotion. Paper presented at the Water- In J.C. Hood (ed.), Men, work and family.
loo Conference on Child Development, Uni- Newbury Park CA: Sage Publications.
versity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada. Imamura, A. (1987). Urban Japanese house-
Goldberg, W.A., & Easterbrooks, M.A. (1984). wives. Honolulu: University of Hawaii
Press.
The role of marital quality in toddler devel-
Isabella, R. (1994). Origins of maternal role
opment. Developmental Psychology 20: 504 –
satisfaction and its influence upon maternal
514.
interactive behavior and infant-mother at-
Gottman, J.M. (1991). Predicting the longitu-
tachment. Infant Behavior and Development
dinal course of marriages. Journal of Mari-
17: 181–187.
tal and Family Therapy 17: 3–7.
Iwao, S. (1993). The Japanese woman: Tradi-
Gottman, J.M. (1999). The marriage clinic: A
tional image and changing reality. Cam-
scientifically-based marital therapy. New
bridge: Harvard University Press.
York: W.W. Norton.
Japanese Association of Sex Education (1987).
Haft, W.L., & Slade, A. (1989). Affect attun-
Kekkon wo meguru nichibei hikaku chousa
ement and maternal attachment: A pilot houkoku sho. [Report of cross-cultural com-
study. Infant Mental Health Journal 10: parison on marriage between Japan and the
157–172. United States of America.] Tokyo, Japan.
Haley, J. (1976). Problem-solving therapy. San Kameguchi, K., & Murphy-Shigematsu, S.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. (2001). Family psychology and family ther-
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1994). Attachment as apy in Japan. American Psychologist 56:
an organization framework for research on 65–70.
close relationships. Psychological Inquiry 5: Keller, H., Voelker, S., & Zach, U. (1997). At-
1–23. tachment in cultural context. Newsletter: In-
Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1999). Pair bonds as ternational Society for the Study of Behav-
attachment: Evaluating the evidence (pp. ioural Development 31: 1–3.
336 –354). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver Kirkpatrick, L.A., & Davis, K.E. (1994). At-
(eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, re- tachment style, gender, and relationship
search, and clinical applications. New York: stability: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Guilford Press. Personality and Social Psychology 66: 502–
Hendry, J. (1995). Understanding Japanese 512.
society. London: Routledge. Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Masumoto, H., &
Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment inter- Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and
view: Historical and current perspectives collective processes in the construction of
(pp. 395– 433). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver the self: Self-enhancement in the United
(eds. Handbook of attachment: Theory, re- States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


348 / FAMILY PROCESS

of Personality and Social Psychology 72: tion, and motivation. Psychological Review
1245–1267. 98: 244 –253.
Klass, D. (2001). Continuing bonds in the res- Marvin, R.S. (1992). Attachment- and family
olution of grief in Japan and North America. systems-based intervention in developmen-
American Behavioral Scientist 44: 742–763. tal psychopathology. Development and Psy-
Kobak, R., Ferenz-Gilles, R., Everhart, E., & chopathology 4: 697–711.
Seabrook, L. (1994). Maternal attachment Marvin, R.S., & Stewart, R.B. (1990). A family
strategies and emotion regulation with ado- systems framework for the study of attach-
lescent offspring. Journal of Research on ment (pp. 51– 86). In M.T. Greenberg, D.
Adolescence 4: 553–566. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (eds.), Attach-
Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development ment in the preschool years. Chicago: Uni-
in children with different attachment histo- versity of Chicago Press.
ries: The first three years. Child Develop- McLoyd, V.C., Harper, C.I., & Copeland, N.
ment 72: 474 – 490. (2001). Ethnic minority status, interparen-
Kondo-Ikemura, R., & Matsuoka, Y. (1999, tal conflict and child development (pp. 98 –
April). The characteristics of attachment 125). In J. Grych & F. Fincham (eds.). Inter-
styles in Japanese students. Paper pre- parental conflict and child development.
sented at the biennial meeting of the Society New York: Cambridge University Press.
for Research in Child Development, Albu- Minuchin, P. (1985). Families and individual
querque NM. development: Provocations from the field of
family therapy. Child Development 56: 289 –
Kunce, L., & Shaver, P. (1994). An attach-
302.
ment-theoretical approach to caregiving in
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family ther-
romantic relationships (pp. 205–237). In K.
apy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Bartholomew & D. Perlman (eds.), Attach-
Minuchin, S., & Nichols, M.P. (1993). Family
ment processes in adulthood. London: Jes-
healing: Tales of hope and renewal from
sica Kingsley.
family therapy. New York: Free Press.
Lebra, T.S. (1994). Mother and child in Japa-
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B.K., & Baker, L.
nese socialization: A Japan-U.S. comparison
(1978). Psychosomatic families: Anorexia
(pp. 259 –274). In P. Greenfield & R. Cocking
nervosa in context. Cambridge: Harvard
(eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child
University Press.
development. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erl- Miyake, K., Chen, S., & Campos, J. (1985).
baum Associates. Infant temperament, mother’s mode of in-
Long, S.O. (1996). Nurturing and femininity teraction, and attachment in Japan: An in-
(pp. 156 –176). In A.E. Imamura (ed.), Re- terim report (pp. 276 –297). In I. Bretherton
imagining Japanese women. Berkeley: Uni- & E. Waters (eds.), Growing points of at-
versity of California Press. tachment theory and research. Monographs
Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Categories of of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
response to reunion with the parent at age 6: ment 50 [Serial No. 209].
Predictable from infant attachment classifi- Miyake, K., & Yamazaki, K. (1995). Self-con-
cations and stable over a 1-month period. scious emotions, child rearing, and child
Developmental Psychology 24: 1–12. psychopathology in Japanese culture (pp.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). 488 –504). In J.P. Tangney & K.W. Fischer
Security in infancy, childhood, and adult- (eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychol-
hood: A move to the level of representation ogy of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and
(pp. 66 –104). In I. Bretherton & E. Waters pride. New York: Guilford Press.
(eds.), Growing points of attachment theory Miyanaga, K. (1991). The cutting edge: Emerg-
and research. Monographs of the Society for ing individualism in Japan. New Bruns-
Research in Child Development 50: [1–2, Se- wick: Transaction Publishers.
rial No. 209]. Mizuta, I., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P., & Hi-
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture ruma, N. (1996). A cross-cultural study of
and the self: Implications for cognition, emo- preschoolers attachment: Security in Japa-
ROTHBAUM, ROSEN, UJIIE, & UCHIDA / 349
nese and U.S. dyads. International Journal (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An
of Behavioral Development 19: 141–159. attachment perspective. Journal of Person-
Nichols, M.P., & Schwartz, R.C. (1998). Fam- ality and Social Psychiatry 71: 899 –914.
ily therapy: Concepts and methods. Boston: Soumuchou, S.T.H. (1987). Nippon no kodomo
Allyn & Bacon. to hahaoya. [Children and mothers in Ja-
Pistole, M.C. (1989). Attachment in adult ro- pan.] Tokyo: Okurashou Insatsu Kyoku.
mantic relationships: Style of conflict reso- Spence, J.T. (1985). Achievement American
lution and relationship satisfaction. Journal style: The rewards and cost of individual-
of Social and Personal Relationships 6: 505– ism. American Psychologist 40: 1285–1295.
510. Sternberg, R.J. (1986). A triangular theory of
Pistole, M.C. (1994). Adult attachment styles: love. Psychological Review 93: 119 –135.
Some thoughts on closeness-distance strug- Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1990). Attachment
gles. Family Process 33: 147–159. within family systems: An overview. Infant
Roland, A. (1989). In search of self in India and Mental Health Journal 11: 218 –227.
Japan. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Takahashi, K. (1990). Are the key assump-
Press. tions of the “Strange Situation” procedure
Rothbaum, F., Morelli, G., Pott, M., & Liu- universal? A view from Japanese research.
Constant, Y. (2000). Immigrant-Chinese and Human Development 33: 23–30.
Euro-American parents’ physical closeness Tamura, T., & Lau, A. (1992). Connectedness
with young children: Themes of family relat- versus separateness: Applicability of family
edness. Journal of Family Psychology 14: therapy to Japanese families. Family Pro-
334 –348. cess 31: 319 –340.
Rothbaum, F., Pott, M.L., Azuma, J., Miyake, van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans-Kranen-
K., & Weisz, J. (2000). The development of burg, J. (1997). Intergenerational transmis-
close relationships in Japan and the U.S.: sion of attachment: A move to the contextual
Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative level (pp. 135–170). In L. Atkinson & K.J.
tension. Child Development 71: 1121–1142. Zucker (eds.), Attachment and psychopathol-
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., & Miyaki, ogy. New York: Guilford Press.
K. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Sagi, A. (1999).
in the United States and Japan. American Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Uni-
Psychologist 55: 1093–1104. versal and contextual dimensions (pp. 713–
Sampson, E.E. (1988). The debate on individ- 734). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (eds.),
ualism: Indigenous psychologies of the indi- Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
vidual and their role in personal and societal and clinical applications. New York: Guil-
functioning. American Psychologist 43: 15–22. ford Press.
Satir, V., Stachowiak, J., & Taschman, H. Vogel, E. (1963, 1991). Japan’s new middle
(1975). Helping families to change. New class. Berkeley: University of California
York: Jason Aronson. Press.
Scarf, M. (1995). Intimate worlds. New York: Vogel, S. (1996). Urban middle-class Japanese
Random House. family life, 1958 –1996: A personal and
Sengoku, T. (1994). Koukousei to kazoku ni evolving perspective (pp. 173–200). In D.
kansuru syousa: Nichibei koukousei hikaku Schwalb & B. Schwalb (eds.), Japanese
houkakusho. [Survey on high school stu- childrearing: Two generations of scholar-
dents and their families: Report on compar- ship. New York: Guilford Press.
isons between Japan and the US.] Tokyo: Watson, M., & Getz, K. (1990). The relation-
Japan Youth Research Institute. ship between oedipal behaviors and chil-
Shamir, H., Du Rocher-Schudlich, T., & Cum- dren’s family role concepts. Merrill Palmer
mings, E.M. (2001). Marital conflict, parent- Quarterly 36: 487–505.
ing styles, and children’s representations of Weinfield, N.S., Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., &
family relationships. Parenting: Science and Carlson, E.A. (1999). The nature of individ-
Practice 1: 123–152. ual differences in infant-caregiver attach-
Simpson, J.A., Rholes, W.S., & Phillips, D. ment (pp. 68 – 88). In J. Cassidy & P.R.

Fam. Proc., Vol. 41, Fall, 2002


350 / FAMILY PROCESS

Shaver (eds.), Handbook of attachment: The- White, M. (1993). The material child: Coming
ory, research, and clinical applications. New of age in Japan and America. New York:
York: Guilford Press. Free Press.
Weisz, J.R., Rothbaum, F.M., & Blackburn, Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust
T.C. (1984a). Standing out and standing in: and commitment in the United States and
The psychology of control in America and Japan. Motivation and Emotion 18: 129 –
Japan. American Psychologist 39: 955–969. 166.
Weisz, J.R., Rothbaum, F.M., & Blackburn,
T.C. (1984b). Swaping recipes for control. Manuscript received July 9, 2001; final revi-
American Psychologist 39: 974 –975. sion submitted and accepted May 2, 2002.

Вам также может понравиться