Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/11169179

Positive thinking.

Article  in  Nursing standard: official newspaper of the Royal College of Nursing · June 2001
Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

0 8,497

1 author:

May Mccreaddie
RCSI-Bahrain
28 PUBLICATIONS   372 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

May McCreaddie View project

doctoral work View project

All content following this page was uploaded by May Mccreaddie on 19 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

European Journal of Oncology Nursing


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ejon

Ensnared by positivity: A constructivist perspective on ‘being positive’


in cancer care
May McCreaddie*, Sheila Payne, Katherine Froggatt
Nursing and Midwifery, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, FK9 4LA

a b s t r a c t
Keywords: Background: The concept of ‘positive thinking’ emerged in cancer care in the 1990s. The usefulness of this
Positive thinking approach in cancer care is under increasing scrutiny with existing research, definitions and approaches
Being positive
debated. Nurses may wish to judiciously examine the debate in context and consider its relevance in
Positivity
Constructivist
relation to their experience and clinical practice.
Grounded theory Purpose: To offer a constructivist perspective on ‘being positive’ we extract data from a constructivist
Interactions grounded theory study on humour in healthcare interactions in order to identify implications for practice
Humour and future research.
Methods: We offer three areas for consideration. First, we briefly review the emergence of ‘positive
thinking’ within cancer care. Second, we present data from a grounded theory study on humour in
healthcare interactions to highlight the prevalence of this discourse in cancer care and its contested
domains. We conclude with implications for practice and future research.
Findings: Patients actively seek meaningful and therapeutic interactions with healthcare staff and ‘being
positive’ may be part of that process. Being positive has multiple meanings at different time-points for
different people at different stages of their cancer journey. Patients may become ensnared by positivity
through its uncritical acceptance and enactment.
Conclusion: Positive thinking does not exist in isolation but as part of a complex, dynamic, multi-faceted
patient persona enacted to varying degrees in situated healthcare interactions. Nurses need to be aware
of the potential multiplicity of meanings in interactions and be able (and willing) to respond
appropriately.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction Wilkinson and Kitzinger (2000) claim there is an inappropriate


over-reliance on self-report data (e.g. interviews or questionnaires)
Positive psychology e a response to psychology’s previous in previous literature and offer their data of unstructured focus
emphasis on the abnormal e emerged to much acclaim at the turn of groups and interviews analysing spontaneous utterances of ‘posi-
the century (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Notably, this tive thinking’. Their discursive approach views talk as action with
turn to ‘flexible optimism’ took place against the backdrop of the meaning constructed for its local interactional context. Talk is
discussion on ‘positive thinking’ in cancer care in the 1990s (Gray therefore, not necessarily accepted as an accurate depiction of the
and Doan, 1990; Rittenberg, 1995; de Raeve, 1997). ‘Positive speakers’ cognitive processes (i.e, what they say is not necessarily
thinking’, ‘being positive’ or any combination of a ‘positive’ attitude, what they mean). Accordingly, Wilkinson and Kitzinger’s (2000)
thought/belief or behaviour, including psychological constructs analysis suggests that ‘positive thinking’ may operate, in part, as
such as optimism and hope, arguably emerged by stealth via a a conversational idiom or, as a normative way of talking about
plethora of quantitative, cognitive scale-based studies (e.g. Folkman, cancer.
1997; Taylor and Armor, 1996; Greer and Watson, 1987; Taylor, 1983; The value of ‘positive thinking’ in cancer care is therefore, under
Greer et al., 1979). The usefulness of much of the research carried increasing scrutiny irrespective of the relevance of the research
out in this area is debatable. Consequently, Wilkinson and Kitzinger approach adopted and its subsequent interpretation (Ehrenreich,
(2000) offer a different analysis of ‘positive thinking’. 2009; Pistrang and Barker, 1998; McGrath et al., 2006a). Never-
theless, the prevailing view suggests that ‘positive thinking’ may be
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ44 (0) 01786 466349.
at least an ‘artificial pressure’ (McGrath, 2004:5) if not, oppressive
E-mail addresses: may.mccreaddie@stir.ac.uk (M. McCreaddie), s.a.payne@ (de Raeve, 1997). We contend that nurses should judiciously
lancaster.ac.uk (S. Payne), k.froggatt@lancaster.ac.uk (K. Froggatt). examine the debate in context and consider its relevance in relation

1462-3889/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2010.03.002
284 M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290

to their experience and clinical practice. Thus, we consider it However, for the purposes of this paper, we believe it is necessary at
appropriate and timely to offer a constructivist perspective on this juncture to offer a broad a priori interpretation of ‘positive
‘positive thinking.’ thinking’. Thus, ‘positive thinking’ is a generic phrase used to
describe any derivative encompassing hope, optimism, positive
A constructivist perspective mental attitude, including ‘being positive’ It is taken to mean
a particular attitude, belief, feeling or behaviour that may infer
Constructivism is based upon a philosophy of learning and the optimism but may not represent the ‘realities’ of the individual or of
premise that, by reflecting on our experiences, we construct our their situated context.
own understanding of the world we live in (Vygotsky, 1978). Indi-
viduals actively generate their own understanding and ‘rules’ are Positive thinking e a contested domain
then used to make sense of their experiences. Social constructivists
therefore, explore how individuals make meaning within a social It appears to be broadly accepted that positive thinking is better
context while social constructionists (e.g. Wilkinson and Kitzinger, than negative thinking (Moberly and Watkins, 2008). However,
2000; Edwards and Potter, 1992; Gilbert and Mulkay, 1984) review fairly early in the debate, the idea that positive thinking had a direct
phenomena (like ‘positive thinking’) relative to context. causal link with well-being (or illness) was refuted (Cassileth and
The constructivist paradigm draws upon the sociological Stimnett, 1982) although this is contested (Siegel, 1986). Conse-
perspective of symbolic interactionism (SI) (Blumer, 1969) and SI is quently, there has been a focus on positive thinking as an indirect or
based upon the triumvirate of meaning, thought and language. A mediating influence. Notwithstanding, the theoretical, methodo-
constructivist perspective based upon SI focuses on meaning logical and definitional tensions, the broad body of work in this area
making within a social context and the multiple realities of the reviews positive thinking in terms of coping per se (mental
participants. Those multiple realities are individual interpretations adjustment, reframing) and postulates the potential for this to
(not shared realities) that arise out of interaction and introspection. (indirectly) impact upon the progression or otherwise of the disease
The constructivist perspective also recognizes the proactive and (e.g. Yu et al., 2003). Thus, Shou et al. (2005) suggest that positive
a priori role of individual agency: the capacity to make choices and thinking is more likely to create a perception of a better quality of
act upon them. Thus, participants actively engage in constructing, life rather than a better quality of life per se.
adapting and making sense of their interactions and draw upon However, while positive thinking may be useful it may also
a host of experiences in order to do so. impede important conversations at the end of life in an attempt
We offer three areas for consideration. First, we briefly review to protect loved ones (McGrath et al., 2006b). Moreover, it exists
the emergence of ‘positive thinking’ within cancer care. Second, we as a contested domain among patients, particularly the notion
present data from a grounded theory study on humour in health- that positive thinking is a social norm or moral obligation (Coreil
care interactions to highlight the prevalence of this discourse in et al., 2004; Holland and Lewis, 2000). Although patients are a
cancer care and its contested domains. We conclude with impli- valuable and arguably under-used resource in healthcare as a
cations for practice and future research. means of peer support (Isaksen and Gjengedal, 2000) they can
also project ‘unwanted pressure’ onto their peers via inappro-
‘Positive thinking’ e what is it? priate ‘cheerleading’ or as McGrath (2004) terms it e the ‘ra ra
positives’. Interestingly, nurses and patients have different
One of the difficulties in reviewing existing research on ‘positive understandings of positive thinking: nurses view it as an attribute
thinking’ is the diverse definitions and interpretations of its (e.g. courage) while patients reportedly perceive it as a way to
constitution and meaning(s). Generally, positive thinking is taken attain normality (O’Baugh et al., 2003).
to indicate a particular attitude, belief, mental state or behaviour Finally, several authors raise the issue of the potential for ‘blame’
(e.g. articulating positive thinking). However, with regards to the or the marginalization of individuals for their ‘failure’ to think
latter, we agree with Wilkinson and Kitzinger (2000) that when positively and perhaps even for the resultant worsening of disease
someone says ‘I am being positive’ e what they say is not neces- (de Raeve, 1997; Rittenberg, 1995 Coreil et al., 2004). However,
sarily what they ‘think’. Moreover, we concur with Wilkinson and there is no evidence whatsoever to support the notion that
Kitzinger (2000) that ‘positive thinking’ is a relatively ambiguous psychological coping styles impact upon disease progression
concept. It follows therefore, that ‘positive thinking’ may hold (Petticrew et al., 2002).
multiple meanings depending upon the participants, (individual) We now present data from a constructivist grounded theory
experiences and context. study which explored spontaneous humour in Clinical Nurse
A further difficulty emerges when positive thinking diffuses into Specialist-patient interactions (McCreaddie, 2008).
the considerable research on related issues such as hope (Herth,
1990, 1992), optimism (Scheier and Carver, 1992) and spirituality Method
(Larimore et al., 2002). Inevitably, these issues have their own
semantic tensions to resolve and, correspondingly, their preferred The data upon which this paper is based was drawn from the
research approaches (see Eliot and Olver, 2002). For example, much main study which took place over an 18-month period
of the research quoted does not specifically address the topic of (McCreaddie, 2008). The theory (McCreaddie and Wiggins, 2009)
positive thinking but, rather includes it, or some aspect thereof, on and the methodology (McCreaddie and Payne, in press) are pre-
a trait measurement scale, e.g. optimism. Accordingly, while sented in full elsewhere. We will first, briefly outline the main study
psychological constructs such as optimism, positive mental attitude to provide the reader with appropriate background and context.
or hope are distinguishable from each other, they are often attrib-
uted to, or cited as, part of an amorphous tranche of literature on The main study (methods)
‘positive thinking’ depending upon the perspective being
presented. The main study reviewed the phenomenon of spontaneous
From our constructivist perspective, what is relevant is not our humour in Clinical Nurse Specialist e patient interactions and their
interpretation (or a particular definition), but the co-construction respective peer groups using a constructivist grounded theory
of ‘positive thinking’ by researchers and participants from data. approach (Charmaz, 2006). Grounded theory is particularly useful
M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290 285

for exploring phenomena about which little is known (Morse, and the pursuit of a negative case. Data comprised a total of 88
2001). Moreover, a constructivist GT approach aims to develop an participants involving 51 patients, 17 next of kin/volunteers, 14
interpretive theory that ‘assumes emergent, multiple realities; inde- CNSs and 5 other staff.
terminancy; facts and values as inextricably linked, trust as provisional The constant comparison method of data collection and analysis
and social life as processural’ (Charmaz, 2006: 126). was applied. Open, axial and selective coding (Strauss and Corbin,
Twenty CNS-patient interactions e naturally occurring interac- 1998) were undertaken on all data. Specially devised interpreta-
tions audio-recorded independently of the researcher (Silverman, tive and illustrative frameworks comprising the three main
2007) e formed the baseline data corpus in the main study. (motivational) humour theories: superiority, incongruity and
Other data collection methods such as interviews and/or focus release and Hay’s (2001) humour support implicatures: a non-
groups offer a second-hand account and presume that participants laughter based interpretation of humour support based upon
‘know’ the phenomenon under study, e.g. humour. To further a Conversation Analytic study, were applied to data. Aspects of
facilitate the natural, spontaneous emergence of humour, partici- Discursive Psychology (Edwards and Potter 1992) were applied
pants; CNSs and patients e were informed that the study was where appropriate e specifically the baseline data corpus e and an
broadly about communication. Ethical review agreed that being amended form of the Jefferson system (Sacks et al., 1974) which
explicit about the aim of study may compromise findings with the highlights the prosodical features of speech (intonation, breath
proviso that participants must not be harmed by the process sounds, laughter particles) was used to illustrate, rather than
(Chantler and Chantler, 1998). The CNSs also recorded pre- and interpret data. Martin’s (2001) psychological overview of humour
post-interaction audio-diaries responding to questions provided in highlighted contextual aspects at the level of axial coding. There-
a sealed envelope on general pre-interaction information (envi- fore, open, axial and selective coding was applied to all data in
ronment, length of relationship) and post-interaction humour- conjunction with the interpretive and illustrative frameworks
specific information (smiling, humour awareness). and a discursive grounded theory methodology (DGTM) evolved
Written consent was only obtained for audio-recording with (McCreaddie and Payne, in press).
verbal consent for observation (e.g. negative case) following the
provision of information sheets or A4 posters highlighting an opt- The main study (theory)
out provision (05/SO709/6, 06/S0709/7).
Interviews, field notes, observations and focus groups were The main study presented a substantive grounded theory:
added to the baseline data corpus to provide introspective data and reconciling the good patient persona with problematic and non-
different perspectives. Consequently, a second ethics submission problematic humour (McCreaddie and Wiggins, 2009). The theory
was necessary to extend the data collection timeframe and allow differentiates potentially problematic humour from non-problem-
follow-up data and different data to be added (see Fig. 1). Theo- atic humour and notes that how humour is identified and
retical sufficiency (Dey, 1999) was declared on the basis of addressed is central to whether patients concerns are resolved or
decreasing interrogation, increasing abstraction, time in the field not.

Fig. 1. Data collection (whole study).


286 M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290

The theory and data extraction rationale Table 1


First dataset e Janet’s case study data.

A case study; Janet e and data from a breast cancer focus group Data and date Participants Format
are presented. Both datasets comprise female participants only. It CNS-patient interaction CNS and patient, ‘normal’ clinical
should be noted that we have not purposefully chosen to focus on (researcher not present) with one trainee consultation lasting
gender-specific data. Rather, we have extracted this data for the August 2005 radiographer 22 min in clinic
observing
reasons outlined below.
CNS pre- and post- CNS only 6 Semi-structured
We have extracted and presented this data separately for two interaction audio questions contained in
reasons. First, this data was noted to be particularly rich in certain diary August 2005 sealed envelopes
dimensions of the good patient persona: positive coping and dis- CNS Follow-up CNS and Researcher Field note follow-up
placed concern. Second, this data also offers a unique perspective as September 2005 lasting 70 min in clinic
Janet e patient Janet and researcher Audio-recorded, 90 min.
it represents (a) different time-points in the study and in the follow-up In cancer support centre.
participants’ treatment as well as (b) different data sources (CNS- March 2006
patient interaction, interview and focus group). Therefore, we
believe our data is most suited to offering a constructivist
perspective on ‘positive thinking’.
offers her perception of her presentation at the initial CNS-patient
The data interaction.
Pseudonyms are used in the reporting of the following to Extract 1
preserve anonymity. The first dataset is the case study of Janet: Janet follow-up interview 7 months after the initial CNS-patient
a 66-year old lady with low posterior rectal carcinoma treated with interaction:
radiotherapy prior to surgery for stoma formation. Janet had 1 Janet I wasn’t a person that moaned or groaned. I was a good
particular past experiences a propos cancer and these are high- 2 patient. I was a quick healer which helped.
lighted in Fig. 2. [slightly later]
Janet took part in CNS-patient interaction six e a radiotherapy 3 int You said there that you were a good patient,
review e and a follow-up interview seven months post-interaction 4 what do you think a good patient is?
and surgery for stoma formation. Janet’s case study data comprises 5 Janet Well I think a good patient is if you don’t moan, what’s the
(a) the initial CNS-patient interaction, (b) the CNS pre- and post- 6 sense in moaning, trying to get better, try and help
interaction audio diary and (c) Janet’s follow-up interview and (d) yourself.
a follow-up field note of the CNS (see Table 1). A second dataset e First, Janet self identifies as a good patient. Further, Janet
a focus group of three female breast cancer patients undertaken at describes a good patient as someone who is overtly uncomplaining
the end of the study timeframe (17 months) e is also presented (line 1) and independent (line 6) while there may be an element of
(see Table 2). expectation or moral obligation of self-help in the phrase: ‘trying to
get better’. The notion of Janet being ‘a quick healer’ is intriguing.
Does Janet’s quintessential good patient persona; uncomplaining,
Findings independent and positive, assume somatic form? Notably, Janet
repeats this colloquial metaphor several times during the course of
Being positive and being a good patient the follow-up interview. However, there is no causal association
between positive thinking and morbidity. Does Janet ‘believe’ that
We propose that positive thinking, being positive or positive being a good patient or being positive in some way helps her
coping is but one dimension of a good patient persona which physically and/or emotionally or is it something expected of people
patients may present as a means of engaging the CNS in a mean- with cancer e to ‘fight’ their disease.
ingful and therapeutic interaction. For example, in the initial CNS- Interestingly, Temoshok (1983, 1987) posits a Type C coping
patient interaction Janet was perceived (by the researcher) to be style or response where there is a discrepancy between the
very sycophantic and (by the CNS) to be ‘very positive’. The CNS conscious experience and the self-report of emotion. Thus, what
perceived her to have a ‘bubbly personality’ (post-interaction audio an individual thinks or more importantly feels is not necessarily
diary) with a good social network. These perceptions contrasted what they report, e.g. the suppression of anger following
with Janet’s lone attendance at clinic, limited use of humour and a diagnosis.
specifically her (over) use of self-disparaging humour (SDH) as, In the following extract, Janet has been recounting her family
according to scale-based studies, SDH correlates with poor social history of cancer to the CNS: her breast cancer, her sister’s death
networks and loneliness (Hampes, 2005). In this first extract Janet from cancer and her daughter’s breast cancer.

Fig. 2. Case study Janet.


M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290 287

Table 2 12 positive, I do want to have a wee cry. I want to have a cuddle.


Second dataset e Breast Cancer Focus group. Strength and positivity are not necessarily one and the same
Condition Breast cancer attribute whether ‘real’ or enacted. ‘Other people’s perceptions’
Number n ¼ 3, Breast cancer treated with radiotherapy, chemotherapy equating strength with positivity although possibly well-meaning
and surgery. All recently completed treatments. Age range from may be complicit in leading Irene to feel that she has to ‘be’
late 40s e late 50s in professional occupations. positive e or, at least articulate it. Her use of the term ‘pressurised’
Participants Patients only, group meets weekly
is interesting and resonates with other studies which suggest that
Venue Cancer support centre
Format Audio-recorded for 75 min, Open discussion of communication some patients object to inappropriate ‘cheerleading’ (McGrath,
and then humour 2004; Coreil et al., 2004). Nonetheless, Irene appears to be reti-
Date September 2006 cent in articulating ‘the very few times’ she lacks positivity. Surely
an individual who has been diagnosed with a life-threatening
illness facing lengthy therapy, that may not necessarily be cura-
tive, be expected to be less than positive, some, if not all of the
Extract 2 time?
CNS-patient interaction (Janet) seven months previously: Both Janet and Irene attest to ‘being’ positive and articulating
1 CNS But having all that in your family and yourself positivity e the latter to greater or lesser degrees. Whether Janet
2 Janet Well my daughter had it when she was 40 or Irene ‘believe’ being positive is useful and/or expected is not
3 CNS And you coming through it again. It is quite amazing clear. Who comprises the ‘cheerleaders’ that may make Irene feel
4 that you have kept quite so positive. ‘pressurized’ into articulating positivity? Is it her fellow patients
5 Janet Well I think it does help, that’s what I tell everybody. or non-cancer patients e well-meaning friends e who like the
There are two points of note in this extract. First, is the CNSs’ CNS in extract two, affirm positivity. If positivity is not already an
‘naming’ of Janet’s presentation as positive. The CNS described expectation initially, then it may become so via others articu-
Janet in her post-interaction audio diary as ‘very positive’ and lating it. By naming it and making it explicit it may become a fait
confirmed this perception at a field note follow-up interview accompli. A patient may have articulated positivity at some
undertaken one month post CNS-patient interaction: point and by doing so effectively give others permission to do
Extract 3 likewise. However, the expression may become fossilized, like
CNS follow-up from interaction 6, one month post-interaction: Wilkinson and Kitzinger’s (2000) notion of positive thinking as
conversational idiom e emerging at times when the patient is
“Perhaps that’s more of a reflection about me e positive coping.
not feeling positive. Consequently, the patient becomes ensnared
I think that’s how I would respond.”
by positivity e a situation that may, or may not, be of their own
The CNS further elaborates that she would ‘need’ to be positive making.
were she in Janet’s position, using it as a stress moderator to cope Being positive and displaced concern
and keep the dark side in abeyance (McGrath et al., 2006a). Notably, This data strongly suggests that patients have an acute aware-
the CNS stated that she preferred working with older patients as ness of how others may be affected by a cancer diagnosis e
they were ‘easier to jolly along’. The CNS therefore, arguably especially so with regard to close family members and partners.
distances herself from the tragedy (of cancer) rather than engaging Extract 5
with it (Bolton, 2001). Breast cancer group: 17 months into field work:
The second point of note is Janet’s utterance that she thinks 1 Lara The tears were simmering but I just could not (), and
being positive is helpful which she articulates to ‘everybody’. 2 watching your husband and daughter at the same
However, if being positive is helpful is it necessary to articulate this time. I
to others? 3 think about them more than I think about myself.
Being positive: articulating positivity Lara articulates displaced concern: concern for others rather
The following extract from the breast cancer focus group may than herself e how she felt like crying at one point but tried to
give some insight into the relevance of articulating positivity: remain composed so not to distress her husband and daughter,
Extract 4 which would, in turn, add to Lara’s distress. Notably, Janet also
Breast cancer group 17 months into field work: expressed displaced concern for others; patients and staff e several
1 Irene: People keep saying to me, you’re a strong person. times during the initial CNS-patient interaction. However, it was
I don’t see only in the follow-up interview she expressed displaced concern
2 myself as a strong person but people keep saying for her husband and daughter within the context of being positive:
that to me and Extract 6
3 it’s, well it’s like yesterday, I had a long conversation Janet follow-up interview 7 months after the initial CNS-patient
with interaction:
4 somebody, and she said, you’ve got a positive 1 I didn’t want them to be ill or anything. I think if you are
attitude. showing that
5 (Lara) It’s other people’s perception 2 you are trying to get better and you are going to get better, it
6 Irene Yes. makes them
7 int You are saying you are positive? (to Irene) 3 feel better.
8 Irene I am really positive about it most of the time but I Thus, Janet clearly states that being positive can have a somatic
don’t think effect e not necessarily for her e but for her husband and daughter.
9 anybody is positive all the time and you have to give Somewhat ironically, Janet also stated that ‘she had to think of him
yourself that (her husband) e because he doesn’t show his feelings’ so Janet did not
10 time to have your little cry or be angry or be upset. ‘show’ her feelings. Consequently, Janet like Irene in extract four
And I don’t like may become trapped inexorably into articulating positivity to
11 to be pressurised and the very few times when I protect others e particularly those closest to her e from her own
don’t feel distress.
288 M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290

However much as patients wish to protect their loved ones from Lara, Irene and Sarah were adamant despite their displaced
their own plight there was also a sense that being positive could concern and possible feelings of being pressurised, that partners
potentially became a somewhat muddled entity for the individual should be part of the process e unlike Janet. Janet attended clinic on
concerned. her own. In contrast, Lara described how her partner had been
Extract 7 involved in her chemotherapy and did, according to her, provide
Breast cancer group: 17 months into field work: ‘support’. Nonetheless Lara noted e like Janet in extract six e that
1 Sarah I think you are conscious because the people close to there were times when those closest to the patient, in this instance
you, they her partner and daughter, were the very same people of whom she
2 feel it, but they feel helpless because there is nothing was protective of for fear of causing distress (extract five). They
they can do. were therefore, the individuals she was least likely to burden with
3 So you are aware of that. So you have to try and be her concerns.
positive and And finally, being positive and healthcare workers
4 show a bright side and it’s all going to be alright. Patients may not burden nearest and dearest with their
5 int So how do you do that? concerns and emotions. Can patients therefore, discuss their
6 Sarah Just by saying those words. That’s the kind of person concerns with nurses or, like Janet’s CNS, do patients perceive that
that I am e healthcare workers also prefer them to display positivity?
7 I’ve been saying that to my husband all along, it will Extract 8:
be alright. Breast cancer group: 17 months into field work:
8 Because I believe it. It suits me to believe that it’s true 1 Sarah I think medical people like you to be positive because
for myself. it makes
In the above extract Sarah attests to an awareness of the 2 their job easier doesn’t it?
powerlessness of loved ones, of their concern and concomitant 3 Irene well less consulting time .hha
positivity with a view to possible cure (line four). Of note is her use Irene cites the pervasive element of time-workload pressures
of a deontic modality in line 3: ‘you have to try and be positive’ e as if and does so with superiority humour and post-construction stance
there is no other option but an obligation to be positive. Her laughter (Haakana, 2002). Sarah’s view appears to resonate with
interpretation is that positivity is required and she articulates this Janet’s CNS who liked older patients who were ‘easier’ to ‘jolly
along with the notion that cure is therefore, a tangible endpoint. along.’ Both, arguably, suggest an awareness of ‘medical peoples’
Note how Sarah uses the future progressive tense in line four in predicament: busy clinics filled with people with cancer all with
reference to herself (going to be alright), but the simple future tense varying degrees of fears and anxieties. Patients are therefore
of the verb when articulating this to her husband in line seven (will attendant to the idea that as much as their cancer is, for them,
be alright): the latter verb tense being more definitive than the hugely significant, e for healthcare staff working in that area, it is
former. Similarly, Irene softens the epistemic modality ‘I believe it’ simply an everyday occurrence.
to ‘it suits me to believe’ (line eight). Sarah may therefore, be
expressing a view that positive thinking may lead to cure Discussion
(Wilkinson and Kitzinger, 1993). Moreover, it arguably constructs
belief as if it is a matter of choice. Clearly it is not, more so in this The data presented here corroborates much of the literature
instance, where a variety of variables (e.g. stage of cancer) are more about positive thinking e it exists as a front to protect loved ones
likely to determine outcome. (McGrath et al., 2006a: Janet, Sarah, Lara), may be unwelcome/
There is patently a difference between an individual’s ‘need’ for oppressive (McGrath et al., 2006b; McGrath, 2004; de Raeve, 1997;
positivity and others ‘expectation’ of positivity. Sarah arguably Lara), a normative way of talking about cancer (Wilkinson and
demonstrates need and expectation colliding and being obfuscated Kitzinger, 2000; Janet) and/or an attribute that is positively
by the emotions, uncertainty and multiple meanings for her and appraised by nurses (O’Baugh et al., 2003, CNS).
her husband. There is a large body of literature on positive experiences
Many healthcare consultations involve partners or others, e.g. following cancer care (e.g. post-traumatic growth, Foley et al.,
seven out of the twenty CNS-patient interaction involved at least 2006; Steel et al., 2008; Hefferon et al., 2009) and some may
one other party. Given patients’ apparent difficulty in expressing argue that this contradicts our ‘negative’ perspective on ‘positive
emotions in the presence of loved ones, the interviewer was moved thinking’. We do not dispute the potential for a diagnosis of cancer
to ask the women in the breast cancer group whether they felt that, to engender positive experiences. However, ‘positive experiences’
sometimes, the involvement of others in those circumstances may per se are arguably very different from our examination of ‘positive
be unhelpful. This provoked a strident retort: thinking’ within the context of dynamic, situated healthcare
Extract 8 interactions. What this paper does is demonstrate multiple mean-
Breast cancer group: 17 months into field work: ings at different time-points for different people at different stages
Lara They should be involved e it’s like labour! (laughter) of their cancer journey. It draws upon several timeframes, data
Lara humorously invokes the analogy of labour to justify partner sources, perspectives and a robust analysis demonstrating how
involvement, simultaneously dismissing the researcher’s sugges- patients reflect upon their experiences (Janet’s family cancer
tion. Lara’s use of labour as a simile is however, somewhat incon- history), interpret meaning making (Sarah and Irene, extract 8), and
gruous. First, the act of labour, or rather conception, is a joint create ‘rules’ such as articulating positive thinking (Sara, extract 7).
undertaking whereas cancer is not, although arguably the support Patients are cognizant of the difficulties healthcare staff working in
expected from a partner is similar, which is presumably what Lara is this area face and thus, what they say is not necessarily what they
referring to. Second, labour usually involves a positive, even joyous mean. In taking a constructivist approach this paper demonstrates
outcome, whereas cancer treatment brings relief at best. Labour/ that patients actively engage with healthcare staff to effect a more
pregnancy and cancer treatment however, are generally unfamiliar, therapeutic interaction and being positive may be (a negative) part
uncertain, protracted and medicalised events and consequently of that process.
they may therefore, remain enduring experiences. As such, it may be The inscrutable concept of mind-body dualism and positive
reasonable to expect partners to be part of the process. thinking is also highlighted here. Healthcare staff, partners,
M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290 289

relatives and society have expectations of people with cancer and Conflict of interest statement
these may sublimate the feelings of the individual. Sontag (1991)
properly suggested that disease is a physiological entity that is None of the authors have any conflict of interests relating to the
not engendered, or affected by, psychological factors. Consequently development and submission of this paper.
it is improper (whether intentional or not) to propagate a fallacy
that may make a difficult situation worse. Patients may well initiate
Acknowledgements
positivity but they can eventually become ensnared by it. In turn,
nurses may recognize the emotional impact of cancer, but under-
The patients and staff who took part in the research. The reviewers
state it (Kendall, 2007). Thus, while patients are unwilling to
for their constructive comments. This paper was developed and
burden their nearest and dearest with their concerns, they are also
prepared as part of a scholarship funded by the Cancer Experiences
uncertain or dissuaded from doing so with (or by) healthcare
Collaborative (CECo) at the International Observatory on End of Life
workers.
Studies, Lancaster University.
Finally, ‘positive thinking’ in conjunction with displaced
concern, similar to Wilkinson and Kitzinger’s (2000) data, is pre-
sented within an all-female dataset. Women are positioned as References
a competent nurturer of others (O’Grady, 2005). Women may also
face greater expectation to cope leading to reduced emotional Blumer, H., 1969. Symbolic Interactionism: Perspective and Method. University of
expression and ‘self-silencing’ (Ussher and Sandoval, 2008) and less California Press, London.
Bolton, S., 2001. Changing faces: nurses as emotional jugglers. Sociology of Health
‘self-silencing’ evidently leads to better coping (Kayser et al., 1999). and Illness 23 (1), 85e100.
However, Emslie et al (2009) suggests that both men and women Cassileth, B.R., Stimnett, J.L., 1982. Psychological Problems. In: Cassileth, B.R.,
control emotion and there are, as Moynihan (2002) notes meth- Cassileth, P.A. (Eds.), Clinical Care of the Terminal Patient. Lea & Febiger,, New
York, pp. 108e118.
odological challenges in studying gender and cancer. ‘Positive Chantler, C., Chantler, S., 1998. Deception: difficulties and initiatives. British Medical
thinking’ may well be a gendered concept. Nevertheless, it is Journal 316, 1731e1734.
a concept that needs further exploration. Charmaz, K., 2006. Constructing Grounded Theory: a Practical Guide through
Qualitative Analysis. London. Sage Publications.
ConnectedÓ (2009). http://www.connected.nhs.uk Retrieved on 25th August 2009.
Coreil, J., Wilke, J., Pintado, I., 2004. Cultural Models of illness and recovery in breast
Limitations cancer support groups. Qualitative Health Research 14, 905e923.
de Raeve, L., 1997. Positive thinking and moral oppression in cancer care. European
Journal of Cancer Care 6, 249e256.
There are two particular limitations with this study as a conse- Dey, I., 1999. Grounding Grounded Theory: guidelines for qualitative inquiry.
quence of the data extraction rationale and the constructivist Academic Press, London.
approach adopted. First, this study extracted data from the main Edwards, D., Potter, J., 1992. Discursive Psychology. Sage Publications, London.
Ehrenreich, B., 2009. Smile or Die: How Positive Thinking Fooled America and the
study to review a particular aspect that emerged. While positive World. Granta, London.
thinking cannot be viewed in isolation it is possible that being Eliot, J., Olver, I., 2002. The discursive properties of “Hope”: a qualitative analysis of
selective about the data discussed gives a de-contextualised view of cancer patients’ speech. Qualitative Health Res 12, 173e193.
Emslie, C., Browne, S., MacLeod, U., Rozmovits, L., Mitchell, E., Ziebland, S., 2009.
the issue. Second, all the participants are female and this gender ‘Getting through’ not ‘going under’: A qualitative study of gender and spousal
imbalance may be significant. support after diagnosis with colorectal cancer. Social Science & Medicine 68 (6),
1169e1175.
Foley, K.L., Farmer, D.F., Petronis, V.M., Smith, R.G., McGraw, S., Smith, K., Carver, C.S.,
Avis, N., 2006. A qualitative exploration of the cancer experience among
Implications for research and practice long-term survivors: comparisons by cancer type, ethnicity, gender, and age.
Psychooncology 15 (3), 248e258.
Folkman, S., 1997. Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Social
Nurses should be aware of the potential multiplicity of mean-
Science and Medicine 45, 1207e1221.
ings in interactions and be able (and willing) to respond appro- Gilbert, N., Mulkay, M., 1984. Opening Pandora’s Box: A sociological analysis of
priately. In our view current oncology communication skills scientists’ discourse. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Gray, R.E., Doan, B.D., 1990. Heroic self-healing and cancer: clinical issues for the
training in the UK (ConnectedÓ) may benefit from adopting a more
health professions. Journal of Palliative Care 6, 32e41.
constructivist-orientated approach. Greer, S., Morris, T., Pettingale, K.W., 1979. Psychological response to breast cancer:
Longitudinal studies on positive thinking that follow patients effect on outcome. Lancet 2, 785e787.
through their experiences of cancer and its treatment journey Greer, S., Watson, M., 1987. Mental adjustment to cancer: its measurement and
prognostic importance. Cancer Surveys 6, 439e453.
would make a constructive contribution to the existing evidence Haakana, M., 2002. Laughter in medical interaction: from quantification to analysis,
base. It would also be useful to investigate how positive thinking and back. Journal of Socio linguistics 6 (2), 207e235.
operates across genders for e staff, patients, relatives, individually Hampes, W.P., 2005. Correlations between humor styles and loneliness. Psycho-
logical Reports 96, 747e750.
and collectively in support groups. Hay, J., 2001. The pragmatics of humor support. Humor 4 (1), 55e82.
Hefferon, K., Grealy, M., Mutrie, N., 2009. Post-traumatic growth and life threat-
ening physical illness: a systematic review of the qualitative literature. British
Journal of Health Psychology 14 (2), 343e378.
Conclusion Herth, K., 1990. Fostering hope in terminally ill people. Journal of Advanced Nursing
15, 1250e1259.
Positive thinking or being positive does not exist in isolation but Herth, K., 1992. Abbreviated instrument to measure hope: development and
psychometric evaluation. Journal of Advanced Nursing 17, 1251e1259.
as part of a complex, dynamic, multi-faceted patient persona Holland, J.C., Lewis, S., 2000. The Human Side of Hope: Living with Hope, Coping
enacted to varying degrees in situated healthcare interactions. with Uncertainty. Harper Collins Inc, New York.
Nurses need to be aware of the potential multiplicity of meanings in Isaksen, A.S., Gjengedal, E., 2000. The significance of fellow patients for the patient
with cancer: what can nurses do? Cancer Nursing 23 (5), 382e391.
interactions and be able (and willing) to respond appropriately.
Kayser, K., Sormanti, M., Strainchamps, E., 1999. Women coping with cancer.
When patients actually say what they mean rather than what they Psychology of Women Quarterly 23 (4), 725e739.
think ‘we’ want to hear we will have truly made progress. In short, Kendall, S., 2007. Witnessing tragedy: nurses’ perceptions of caring for patients
it is not for patients to make our job ‘easier’ but for us to make their with cancer. International Journal of Nursing Practice 13, 111e120.
Larimore, W., Parker, M., Crowther, M., 2002. Should clinicians incorporate positive
cancer journey, an understanding, supportive and therapeutic spirituality into their practices? What does the evidence say? Annals of
experience. Behavioral Medicine 24 (1), 69e73.
290 M. McCreaddie et al. / European Journal of Oncology Nursing 14 (2010) 283e290

Martin, R.A., 2001. Humor, laughter, and physical health: methodological issues and Steel, J.L., Gamblin, T.C., Carr, B.I., 2008. Measuring post-traumatic growth in people
research findings. Psychological Bulletin 127 (4), 504e519. diagnosed with hepatobiliary cancer: directions for future research. Oncology
McCreaddie, M., 2008. Reconciling the Good Patient persona with problematic and Nursing Forum 35 (4), 643e650.
non-problematic humour: a grounded theory. Unpublished Doctoral Disserta- Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., 1992. Effects of optimism on psychological and physical
tion, University of Strathclyde. well-being: theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and
McCreaddie, M., Payne, S. Evolving Grounded Theory Methodology: towards Research 16 (2), 201e228.
a discursive approach. International Journal of Nursing Studies, in press, doi:10. Schou, I., Ekeberg, Ø, Ruland, C.M., 2005. The mediating role of appraisal and coping
1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.11.6. in the relationship between optimismepessimism and quality of life. Psy-
McCreaddie, M., Wiggins, S., 2009. Reconciling the good patient persona with chooncology 14, 718e727.
problematic and non-problematic humour: a grounded theory. International Seligman, M.E.P., Csikszentmihalyi, M., 2000. Positive psychology: an introduction.
Journal of Nursing Studies 46 (8), 1071e1091. American Psychologist 55, 5e14.
McGrath, C., Montgomery, K., White, K., Kerridge, I.H., 2006a. A narrative account of Siegel, B.S., 1986. Love, Medicine and Miracles. Harper & Row, New York.
the impact of positive thinking on discussions about death and dying. Support Silverman, D., 2007. A Very Short, Fairly Interesting and Reasonably Cheap Book
Care Cancer 14, 1246e1251. about qualitative research. Sage Publications, London.
McGrath, C., Jordens, C.F.C., Montgomery, K., Kerridge, I.H., 2006b. ’Right’ way to ’do’ Sontag, S., 1991. Illness as Metaphor; AIDS and Its Metaphors. Penguin.,
illness? Thinking critically about positive thinking. Internal Medicine Journal 36 Harmondsworth.
(10), 665e669. Strauss, A., Corbin, J., 1998. Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures
McGrath, P., 2004. The burden of the ‘RA RA’ positive: survivors’ and hospice for Developing Grounded Theory, second ed. Sage Publications, London.
patients’ reflections on maintaining a positive attitude to serious illness. Taylor, S.E., 1983. Adjustment to threatening events: a theory of cognitive adapta-
Support Care Cancer 12, 25e33. tion. American Psychologist 58, 1161e1173.
Moberly, N.J., Watkins, E.R., 2008. Ruminative self-focus, negative life events, and Taylor, S.E., Armor, D.A., 1996. Positive illusions and coping with adversity. Journal
negative affect. Behav Res Ther 46 (9), 1034e1039. of Personality 64, 873e898.
Morse, J.M., 2001. Situating Grounded Theory within Qualitative Inquiry. In: Temoshok, L., 1983. Emotion, Adaptation, and Disease: a Multidimensional Theory.
Schreiber, R.S., Stern, P.N. (Eds.), Using Grounded Theory in Nursing. Springer In: Temoshok, L., Van Dyke, C., Zegans, L.S. (Eds.), Emotions in Health and
Publishing Company, New York, pp. 1e16. Illness: Theoretical and Research Foundations. Grune and Stratton, New York.
Moynihan, C., 2002. Men, women, gender and cancer. European Journal of Cancer Temoshok, L., 1987. Personality, coping style, emotion and cancer: towards an
Care 11 (3), 166e172. integrative model. Cancer Surveys 6 (3), 545e567.
O’Baugh, J., Wilkes, L.M., Luke, S., George, A., 2003. ’Being positive’: perceptions of Ussher, J.M., Sandoval, M., 2008. Gender differences in the construction and expe-
patients with cancer and their nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing 44, 262e270. rience of cancer care: the consequences of the gendered positioning of carers.
O’Grady, H., 2005. Women’s relationship with herself: gender, Foucault, therapy. Psychology & Health 23 (8), 945e963.
Routledge, London. Vygotsky, L.S., 1978. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological
Petticrew, M., Bell, R., Hunter, D., 2002. Influence of psychological coping on Processes. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Massachusetts.
survival and recurrence in people with cancer: systematic review. British Wilkinson, S., Kitzinger, C., 1993. Whose breast is it anyway? A Feminist consider-
Medical Journal 325 (7372), 1066. ation of Advice and ‘Treatment’ for breast cancer. Women’s Studies Interna-
Pistrang, N., Barker, C., 1998. Partners and fellow patients: two sources of emotional tional Forum 16 (3), 229e238.
support for women with breast cancer. American Journal of Community Wilkinson, S., Kitzinger, C., 2000. Thinking differently about thinking positive:
Psychology 26 (3), 439e456. a discursive approach to cancer patients’ talk. Social Science and Medicine 50
Rittenberg, C.N., 1995. Positive thinking: an unfair burden for cancer patients? (6), 797e811.
Support Care Cancer 3, 37e39. Yu, C.L.M., Fielding, R., Chan, C.L.W., 2003. The mediating role of optimism on post-
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A., Jefferson, G., 1974. A simplest systematics for the organi- radiation quality of life in nasopharyngeal carcinoma. Quality of Life Research
sation of turn-taking in conversation. Language 50 (4), 696e735. 12 (1), 41e51.

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться