Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

Screening for

Communication and
Continuing
Nursing
Cognitive Disorders in
Education
Series
Infants and Toddlers
Frances Scheffler Jeanel Burgess
Donald Vogel Tara Conneally
Rachel Astern Kathy Salerno
Pediatric nurse practitioners (PNPs) have a primary role in providing parent-inclusive well-child physical
and developmental examinations. Although routine physical examinations are well defined, developmental
assessments, including communication and cognition, are not. Currently a number of developmental
screening tests exist; however, none have become established as the “gold standard” for the primary
health care professional as none are convenient or time-efficient to employ. In particular, there is a need
for a screening tool that PNPs can use to evaluate early development in their youngest patients. This
article offers a screening instrument capable of being easily completed through parent interview in a
routine integrated well-child exam. The screening includes questions that probe communication and
cognition in infants and toddlers, and identifies atypical behaviors that are considered by developmental
specialists to be “red flags” and precursors to later communication and cognitive disorders. A brief
description of the nature of communication and cognitive disorders in young children is included.

n many medical settings, pediatric behaviors such as those found in chil-


Frances Scheffler, PhD, is Certified Speech-
Language Pathologist and Assistant Profes-
sor, Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY.

Donald Vogel, AuD, MS, BA, is Certified


I nurse practitioners (PNPs) have a
primary role in providing parent-
inclusive health examinations for
infants and toddlers during well-baby
visits. PNPs are well trained to admin-
dren with autism, with behaviors that
are socially acceptable (Guralnick,
1998; Ward, 1999).
The American Academy of Pedia-
trics (AAP) (2001) recommends
Audiologist and Assistant Professor, ister physical examinations while developmental screenings for every
Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY. obtaining case history information child on a regular basis during well-
from the parent or caregiver. Beyond child examinations, yet it is unknown
Rachel Astern, MS, is a graduate of
Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY. the physical examination, PNPs are if such screenings are widely per-
expected to assess the developmental formed. Although a number of screen-
Jeanel Burgess, MS, is a graduate of (communication and cognition) status ing tools exist (i.e., at least 11 are
Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY. of their young patients (New York available for use with children under 2
State Department of Education, 2004; years of age), none are specifically
Tara Conneally, MS, is a graduate of Sherwood, Brown, Vaunette, & designed for the primary care practi-
Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY. Wardell, 1997). tioner. None are convenient or time-
Early identification of developmen- efficient for the PNP to employ during
Kathy Salerno, MS, is a graduate of
tal delays and disorders is crucial well-baby visits (Nelson, Nygren,
Hunter College – CUNY, New York, NY.
(Guralnick, 1998). Many can be iden- Walker, & Panoscha, 2006). In addi-
Note: The authors reported no actual or tified before the age of 24 months. tion, PNP educational curricula, as for
potential conflict of interest in relation to Delays and disorders that go undiag- physicians and other primary health-
this continuing nursing education article. nosed and untreated put children care providers, may only include cur-
unnecessarily at risk for later poor sory preparation in these areas. PNPs
Sally S. Russell, MN, CMSRN, Education academic, social, and vocational therefore need basic information
Director, Anthony J. Jannetti, Inc., dis- progress (Laing, Law, Levin, & Logan, about early communication and cog-
closed that she is on the Advisory Board 2002; Olswang, Rodriguez, & Timler, nitive disorders, and a reliable screen-
for Roche/Abbott Labs.
1998; Ramey & Ramey, 1998; ing procedure to identify those infants
The Pediatric Nursing Editorial Board Snowling, Adams, Bishop, & Stothard, and toddlers who should be referred
members reported no actual or potential 2001). Conversely, for many affected for further testing.
conflict of interest in relation to this con- children, early diagnosis leads to This article provides a brief
tinuing nursing education article. appropriate treatment that, when initi- overview of typical and atypical com-
ated during infancy and toddlerhood, munication and cognitive develop-
facilitates the emergence of many ment in preschool-aged children (2.5-
The CNE Posttest skills that would otherwise lag behind 5 years of age), describes the most
can be found or fail to develop. Treatment also can prevalent pre-linguistic precursors to
on pages 486-487. include goals to replace atypical communication disorders that appear

PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6 473


in infants and toddlers, and suggests a children who exhibit aggressive cepts such as object permanence,
newly designed efficient screening behaviors (e.g., biting, kicking, means-ends, and recognition of object
tool for PNPs to use during well-baby tantrums) may, in fact, be frustrated functions (Rice, 1983). Memory
visits in the first 24 months of life. The due to communication delays or dis- deficits can be demonstrated as
screening tool relies on caregiver orders. reduced or absent fast-mapping abili-
observations of the child. It does not Two groups of children display ties (i.e., the ability to remember an
require special toys or books and can communication delays or disorders event after one instance of that event)
be completed seamlessly during each without cognitive delays or disorders: (Carey & Bartlett, 1978; Dollaghan,
routine well-child examination. (a) children who are ‘late talkers’ 1985). Deficits in general learning
(Weismer, Murray-Branch, & Miller, skills occur as reduced ability to
Typical and Atypical Language 1994) and (b) children who have acquire new skills or to generalize old
and Cognitive Development in Specific Language Impairment (SLI) skills to new events comparably to
Preschool-aged Children (Leonard, 1998). In infancy and tod- same-age cognitively normal peers
Communication occurs when there dlerhood, these children exhibit simi- (Jepsen & VonThaden, 2002).
is an exchange of ideas between a lar communication and social behav- Preschool children with cognitive
speaker and a listener. It is comprised iors. However, there are some subtle deficits often play and interact similar-
of several processes, including lan- differences. ly to children who are chronologically
guage (the symbolic representation of Late talkers have better compre- younger. Some may have positive
thoughts), speech (oral expression hension of spoken language than chil- social interactions, but do not learn
using speech sounds to form words), dren with SLI. They tend to under- simple tasks as quickly as their peers
and gestures (i.e., facial expressions, stand age-appropriate commands (Nilholme, 1999). Others with cogni-
body language). whereas children with SLI may not. tive deficits may suffer atypical social
Cognition includes non-language Generally, both groups play appropri- skills because of poor ability to assess
mental processes such as attention, ately with toys, although children with social situations, or atypical affect
memory, and problem-solving abili- SLI tend to play on a more immature (e.g., children with autism). Some
ties. As the infant grows and matures, level. Children who are late talkers children with cognitive deficits may
communication and cognition develop develop normal language skills during have age-appropriate language skills
and interact with one another. their early school years. Children with (e.g., children who are high function-
However, communication and cogni- SLI have difficulty with language skills ing autistic). A number of children
tion are comprised of separate sets of throughout their school years and are with cognitive delays (e.g., some chil-
mental functions. It is therefore em- likely to exhibit reading and learning dren with mental retardation) have
phasized that communication impair- disabilities as they mature (Weismer et cognitive deficits but normal lan-
ments are separate and distinct from al., 1994). guage.
cognitive impairments. These two Children who are late talkers and Young children who have suffered
types of impairments are often co- children with SLI have normal hearing, traumatic brain injury (TBI) may
morbid. Impairments (i.e., delays or and age-appropriate nonverbal prob- demonstrate cognitive deficits in the
disorders) in communication or cogni- lem-solving skills. Both groups can presence of normal language. They
tion occur in approximately 8% of all assemble age-appropriate puzzles. may have age-appropriate vocabular-
young children (National Institute of These children have normal health ies and speak in well-formed sen-
Deafness and Other Communicative histories although otitis media with tences. However, they have deficits in
Disorders [NIDCD], 2006). effusion is common. nonverbal problem solving, and show
Communication delays and disor- The speech-language pathologist immature judgment in social situa-
ders. In preschoolers, communication can differentiate between the young tions.
delays and disorders include any child who is a late talker and the one Other children may exhibit com-
atypical comprehension or production who will be SLI through extensive bined language and cognitive delays
of speech sounds (i.e., consonants standardized evaluation procedures. or disorders. Cognitive disorders
and vowels), words, phrases, or sen- However, the differentiation may not occurring with language disorders
tences (American Speech-Language- be possible until after the age of 24-30 include some children with mental
Hearing Association [ASHA], 2006; months: the late talker will begin to retardation, most children with autism,
Kent, 1994). Children with these con- show signs of “catching up” whereas and some children with learning dis-
ditions may ignore or misunderstand the child with SLI will continue to show abilities (Tanner, 2003).
parental instructions and commands. limited signs of language expansion. Communication and cognitive dis-
They may have few words, or exhibit Both groups nevertheless require orders often are present in children
unintelligible speech. They may omit speech-language therapy. For the late with physical impairments. Physical
many speech sounds or produce talker, the therapy will be short term. disabilities that are commonly co-
unusual combinations of speech For the child with SLI, it likely will be morbid with communication or cogni-
sounds. Their vocabulary and gram- long term. For both groups, the thera- tive disorders include hearing impair-
mar develop slowly. They may grunt py focuses on developing language ment, visual impairment, cerebral
or point to comment or request rather skills, including those that will allow palsy, and other neuromuscular
than attempt to produce words. Some positive interactions with peers so that impairments. Hearing impairment
children show disinterest in toys, social isolation is minimized. affects language comprehension,
books, and games that other children Cognitive delays and disorders. vocabulary and grammar develop-
their age enjoy. Atypical social inter- Cognitive delays and disorders include ment, and speech production (Davis,
actions with family members and delays or deficits in problem— solving Elfenbein, Schum, & Bentler, 1986).
peers are common. Children who are skills, memory skills, and general Visual impairments affect concept
considered shy, withdrawn, or prefer learning skills. Deficits in problem- development and may influence
to play alone may be speech and lan- solving skills are exhibited as atypical vocabulary development (Recchia,
guage delayed or disordered. Young or delayed development of basic con- 1997). Cerebral palsy affects the neu-

474 PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6


Table 1. Common Conditions Co-Morbid with Language/Cognitive Disorders PNPs and Communication/
Cognitive Screenings
Condition Speech/Language/Cognitive Disorder PNPs are among the first profes-
Congenital, permanent hearing loss Language delay, speech disorders sionals to partner with parents. The
PNP can depend on the parent inter-
Otitis Media with Effusion and con- Language delay, speech disorders view as a source of accurate informa-
comitant conductive hearing loss tion about a baby’s behaviors and rou-
Autism Language disorder, cognitive disorder tines. Questioning parents in a case
history format about their child’s
Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Language disorder health is a standard procedure in med-
(often familial) ical practice. It is also a valid proce-
Cranio-facial disorders Speech disorders, language disorders, dure for determining the status of
cognitive disorders, hearing disorders communication and cognitive devel-
opment (Rescorla & Alley, 2001;
Cerebral palsy Speech disorders, language delay Stokes, 1997). As accurate observers
Poor social conditions (poverty, Language delay, language disorder of their child’s development, parents
parental substance abuse) become concerned should expected
behaviors occur rarely or not at all
Mental retardation Cognitive delay, language delay, speech (Billeaud, 2003). Parents notice atyp-
delays ical behaviors, too, and are relieved
when a primary care practitioner
responds to their concerns with advice
(AAP, Committee on Children with
romotor speech system that underlies or delayed emergence these behav- Disabilities, 2001).
coordination of respiration, phonation, iors are indicators of increased likeli- Although an array of language and
resonation, and articulation. Deficits in hood of later communication and cog- cognitive screening tests exist, few
these functions will affect speech intel- nitive delays and disorders. Speech- appear to be used currently in medical
ligibility. Children with speech motor language pathologists routinely ques- practices (Law, Boyle, Harris,
impairments are not necessarily lan- tion parents about the emergence of Harkness, & Nye, 1998; Nelson et al.,
guage impaired or cognitively im- pre-linguistic and early cognitive 2006; Sand et al., 2005; Sices,
paired. behaviors during communication Feudtner, McLaughlin, Drotar, &
Finally, language and cognitive assessments. Questions are based on Williams, 2004; Sturner et al., 1994).
delays and disorders may occur as a lists of commonly known develop- The screening tools available often
result of deprived social conditions. mental milestones. For example, even take 10 minutes or more to administer
Such conditions include children who before first words appear between the and many require the use of specific
are living in poverty (Fazio, Naremore ages of 12 to 15 months, typical toys and books to direct the observa-
& Connell, 1996) or in residence with infants smile, laugh, gurgle, babble, tion. Indeed, primary care practition-
parental drug or alcohol abusers point at desired objects, enjoy looking ers appreciate the need for communi-
(ASHA, 2003). Further, the likelihood at pictures and manipulating toys, and cation and cognitive screening tools,
of deficient communication and cog- begin to recognize some spoken yet they do not routinely include them
nitive development is increased for the words. In addition, very young infants in well-baby visits because the tools
child of a single teen-aged parent or a imitate facial expressions and body are too cumbersome or time-consum-
parent suffering from depression or gestures. These are important precur- ing. Although some screening tools
other mental illness (Coster & sors to the development of normal rely on parent report, none are
Cicchetti, 1993; Keown, Woodward, & language. designed specifically for primary care
Field, 2001; NICHD Early Child Care Conversely, many infants whose settings (Blackwell & Baker, 2002;
Research Network, 1999; Papero, communication skills will be poor later Dale, Bates, Reznick, & Morisset,
2005). In addition, some communica- on exhibit atypical pre-language 1989; Stokes, 1997; Sturner et al.,
tion and cognitive disorders have high behaviors. For example, some infants 1994). Currently, no available screen-
rates of incidence within members of are unusually quiet, show evidence of ing tool is considered to be a ‘gold
families and are therefore considered minimal babbling, vocalize with standard’ (Nelson et al., 2006).
to be genetically based (Shriberg et unusual sounds, or display limited By definition, a screening is a pro-
al., 2005). interest in family members, toys, or cedure that identifies a group as differ-
Table 1 summarizes the most com- books. These behaviors are alerts to ing from the larger population
mon conditions that are often co-mor- the presence of a disorder or a devel- (Nicolosi, Harryman, & Kresheck,
bid with language and cognitive disor- opmental delay. Similarly, other at-risk 2004). In the case of a communica-
ders. Despite these general guidelines, behaviors necessitating appropriate tion and cognitive screening, the tool
not all children with illnesses, injuries, investigation include minimal abilities must distinguish between children
high familial incidence, or deficient liv- to imitate, tendency to ignore people who have delays or disorders from
ing conditions exhibit poor communi- speaking, and failure to comprehend those who do not (Klee et al., 1998).
cation or cognitive skills. simple words and sentences that other A screening does not differentiate one
Screening for delays and disorders babies understand. Speech-language type of disorder from another. Rather,
in infants and toddlers. Typical pathologists routinely inquire about it must identify those children who are
infants and toddlers exhibit identifiable atypical behaviors that a parent or in need of a thorough evaluation, while
pre-linguistic and pre-cognitive be- caregiver may have observed in their screening out those who are develop-
haviors in developmental sequences child. Parent observations are impor- ing normally. In addition, the screen-
that are precursors to typical commu- tant as they are used as guidelines in ing must be brief enough for inclusion
nication and cognition. The absence assessing a child’s abilities. in a general physical examination.

PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6 475


Figure 1. Screening for Communication and Cognition in Infants and Toddlers (SCCIT)

Screening for Communication The tool is intended to quickly screen PNP can suggest the parent seek a full
and Cognition in Infants and infants and toddlers ages 6 to 24 assessment by a speech-language
Toddlers (SCCIT) months for the most common behav- pathologist or other developmental
Presented here is the Screening for ioral precursors to later communica- specialist.
Communication and Cognition in tion and cognitive delays or disorders. Administration of the SCCIT. The
Infants and Toddlers (SCCIT), which Results can aid the PNP and parent in SCCIT is designed to be kept as a
is designed to be used by PNPs during determining if a more thorough evalu- cumulative record posted in each
any or all well-baby examinations. ation is warranted. In that case, the child’s medical file (see Figure 1). The

476 PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6


Table 2. SCCIT Questions, Examples of Tests that Probe with Similar/Related Questions, Rationale for Inclusion, and
Disorder Group(s) Most Likely to Show This Feature

SCCIT Tests with Disorder Group Most Likely to


Criterion Probes Comments/Rationale for Inclusion Show Deficits in this Feature
Is parent con- DDSTa; Rossettib This question allows parents to express general Hearing impairment, physical
cerned about concerns. Parents have been found to reliably disability (e.g., cerebral palsy),
feeding, identify problems in their young children cognitive delay (e.g., mental
speech, hear- (Rosetti, 1990; Stokes, 1997). Mothers report retardation), cognitive disorder
ing? concern over feeding difficulties in their children (e.g., autism)
with cerebral palsy (Motion, Northstone,
Emond, & Stucke, 2002).
Does baby DDST; MCHATc; Loud sounds should produce a startle response Hearing impairment, cognitive
ignore loud REEL-3d; in normally developing infants and in toddlers. disorder (e.g., autism)
sounds? Rossetti
Does baby DDST General feeding questions may be included in Physical deficits (e.g., cerebral
have difficulty some tests (e.g., Does baby drink from a cup?). palsy), cognitive delays (e.g.,
swallowing? Swallowing problems can occur in the first 12 mental retardation)
months of life. Observing feeding, preferably in
the home, is important (Reilly et al., 1996).
Is baby over- MCHAT; REEL-3; Hypersensitivity to auditory stimuli is a symptom Cognitive disorder (e.g., autism,
ly-sensitive to Rossetti of autism and Williams syndrome (Borsel, Curfs, mental retardation)
loud sounds? & Fryns, 1997; Klein, Armstrong, Greer, & Brown,
1990; Koegel, Openden, & Koegel, 2004).

Is baby quiet DDST; Rossetti Babies are expected to gurgle, coo, babble, or Language delay, cognitive delay
too often? utter other noises besides crying. Babbling is an (e.g., autism), speech delay (e.g.,
important precursor for speech development. cerebral palsy)

Has baby had Usually includ- It is important to pay attention to the language Speech delay, language delay
3 or more ear ed in case histo- development of children with middle ear fluid
infections ry questions: because although it is not conclusive, some
over the last 6 not included in researchers have found a negative effect on lan-
months? language tests. guage development (Bess, 1985; Hooper,
Ashley, Roberts., Zeisel, & Poe, 2006).

Does baby Quick Screen Abnormal prosody has been identified as a fea- Autism, cerebral palsy, traumatic
have an for Voice ture of autism (Paul, 2005). Deficient respiration, brain injury, some developmental
unusual Evaluatione abnormal laryngeal function can result in voice syndromes
voice? disorders in some infants.
Are there con- DDST; REEL-3; Paul (1991) considers children to have a lan- Language delay, language disor-
cerns about Rossetti guage delay if they produce less than 10 intelli- der, cognitive delay, cerebral
how many gible words by age 18-23 months. Parents have palsy
words baby been found to reliably identify problems in their
says? young children (Stokes, 1997). Thal and Bates
(1988) consider a language delay present when
no two-word combinations are used by 18-24
months.
Does baby fin- MCHAT Repetitive/stereotypic behaviors involving Autism, hearing impairment
ger-fidget for hand/finger mannerisms are often found in chil-
long periods dren with autism (see Prater & Zylstra, 2002 for
of time? a review).
Does baby MCHAT; DDST; Children with autism have been found to dis- Hearing impairment, autism
look at you as REEL-3; Rossetti play less eye gaze directed to people (Wetherby,
you talk? Gaze Prizant, & Hutchinson, 1998). Joint attention is
toward an important for language development (Wetherby
object you et al., 1998).
point to?

PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6 477


Table 2. SCCIT Questions, Examples of Tests that Probe with Similar/Related Questions, Rationale for Inclusion, and
Disorder Group(s) Most Likely to Show This Feature (continued)

SCCIT Tests with Disorder Group Most Likely to


Criterion Probes Comments/Rationale for Inclusion Show Deficits in this Feature
Does baby MCHAT; DDST; Children with autism lack symbolic play Autism, mental retardation
show appro- REEL-3; Rossetti (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988).
priate interest Typically developing children are likely to use
in toys or joint attention, behavior regulation, and social
books? interaction even in the prelinguistic stage (Watt,
Wetherby, & Shumway, 2006; Wetherby et al.,
1988). AAP suggests that a pediatrician be con-
tacted if a child does not point to objects or pic-
tures by 8-12 months of age.
Does baby MCHAT; REEL-3; During the first year of life, babies begin to Hearing impairment, autism,
respond to Rossetti respond to their own name (ASHA, 2007; mental retardation
his/her name? NIDCD, 2000).
Does baby DDST; REEL-3; Most early speech sounds develop directly from Hearing impairment, language
make speech Rossetti early babbling (Stoel-Gammon, 1998). delay, autism
sounds?
Does baby MCHAT; DDST; This represents evidence of early receptive Hearing impairment, language
understand REEL-3; Rossetti language development. disorder, receptive language
you? delay, cognitive delays, cognitive
disorders
Does baby REEL-3; Rossetti This should be well established by nine months
gesture (e.g., of age. Babies who are developing good interac- Cognitive disorders (e.g., autism).
“hi”, “bye”, tive skills will show evidence of interactive ges-
“up”)? tures. Babies with poor interactive skills will not.

Does baby REEL-3; Rossetti This represents evidence of the emergence of Late talkers, language disorders,
say 15 or early expressive language development. cognitive disorders, hearing
more words? impairment, cerebral palsy
Are baby’s REEL-3; Rossetti Mothers and other caregivers should under- Hearing impairment, speech dis-
words under- stand baby’s utterances. A parent’s or caregiv- orders (e.g., cerebral palsy), lan-
stood most of er’s difficulty in understanding baby suggests guage disorders
the time? atypical speech development.
Does baby REEL-3; Rossetti By 24 months of age, baby is expected to have Hearing impairment, speech
say 50 words at least 50-word vocabulary and combine words delays and disorders, language
or more and into short sentences. delays and disorders, cognitive
combine delays and disorders
words into
sentences?

a
DDST: The Denver Developmental Screening Test (Frankenburg & Dodds, 1975).
b
Rosetti: The Rosetti Infant-Toddler Language Scale (Rosetti, 1990).
c
MCHAT: The modified checklist for autism in toddlers (Robbins, Fein, Barton, & Green, 2001)
d
REEL-3: Receptive-Expressive Emergent Language Scale (3rd ed.) (Bzoch, League, & Brown, 2000).
e
Quick Screen for Voice Evaluation (Lee, Stemple, Glaze, & Kelchner, 2004).

18 questions are divided into two sec- considered appropriate when three or visits should be seen by a develop-
tions with indicators for six age levels more alerts (i.e., any combination of mental specialist (e.g., speech-lan-
(i.e., 6 months, 9 months, 12 months, three YES and/or NO responses) are guage pathologist) who could use
15 months, 18 months, 24 months). accrued during a single visit or over more thorough instruments to deter-
Part A includes 9 questions and is for- two or three visits. mine the child’s potential to develop
matted to reveal alerts if YES respons- Rationale for three alerts to indi- normally or atypically.
es are offered during any visit. Part B, cate a referral. The items on this test Item analysis of the SCCIT:
also contains 9 questions and is for- are the most compelling ‘red flags’ Validity and reliability. Each of the
matted to reveal alerts if NO respons- known as indicators for referrals. A questions included on the SCCIT is
es are offered during any visit. Referral child whose parent has expressed evidence-based as reported in pub-
to a speech-language pathologist or concerns that included three problems lished tests or research and was
other qualified specialist should be in one visit or one problem over three selected for its value as a proven indi-

478 PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6


cator of typical or atypical behavior readers to make copies of the SCCIT Dollaghan, C. (1985). Child meets word:
(i.e., content validity). Each question screening form to be included in each “Fast Mapping” in preschool chil-
is a common one asked routinely by child’s medical file. dren. Journal of Speech and Hearing
speech-language pathologists during Research, 28, 449-454.
Fazio, B., Naremore, R., & Connell, P.
case history interviews, general devel- (1996). Tracking children from pover-
opmental screening tests, or tests or References
ty at risk for language impairment: A
questionnaires that probe for particu- American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP).
three year longitudinal study.
lar disorders (see Table 2). Table 2 Committee on Children with
Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disabilities. (2001). Developmental
lists all the questions included in the surveillance and screening of infants
Research, 39, 611-624.
SCCIT and summarizes the sources of Frankenburg, W., & Dodds, J. (1975). The
and young children. Pediatrics, 108,
each, provides a brief explanatory Denver Developmental Screening
192-196.
comment describing the rationale Test – Revised (DDST). Denver, CO:
American Speech Language Hearing Asso-
behind the selection of each question, University of Colorado Medical
ciation (ASHA). (2003). Impact of
Center.
and notes the type of developmental abuse, neglect, and alcohol exposure
Guralnick, M.J. (1998). Effectiveness of
disorder for which the behavior is sig- on children development. Retrieved
early intervention for vulnerable chil-
nificant. In that regard, content validi- July 11, 2006, from ASHA Leader
dren: A developmental perspective.
ty has been established (Schiavetti & OnLine via ASHA Publications
American Journal on Mental Re-
http://www.asha.org/about/publica-
Metz, 2002). tions/leader-online/ archives/2003/q4/
tardation, 102, 319-345.
Although sensitivity and specificity f031118b1.htm
Hooper, S., Ashley, T., Roberts, J., Zeisel,
data for the SCCIT are not currently S., & Poe, M. (2006). The relationship
American Speech Language Hearing Asso-
available, this screening has intrinsic of otitis media in early childhood to
ciation (ASHA). (2006). Speech and
value as it presents a format for PNPs attention dimensions during the
language disorders. Retrieved June
early elementary school years.
to use that includes questions about 20, 2006, from ASHA via ASHA Publi-
Journal of Developmental Behavioral
development that might otherwise be cations http://www.asha.org/public/
Pediatrics, 27, 281-289.
omitted during well-child examina- speech/disorders/
Jepsen, R.H., & VonThaden, K. (2002). The
tions. The behaviors the SCCIT American Speech Language Hearing
effect of cognitive education on the
Association (ASHA). (2007). Typical
probes, if absent or atypical (i.e., any speech and language development.
performance of students with neuro-
combination of three alerts) in any Retrieved October 20, 2007, from
logical developmental disabilities.
child, warrants a referral. Further NeuroRehabilitation, 17, 201-209.
http://www.asha.org/public/speech/d
research is planned to determine sta- Kent, R.D. (1994). Normal development of
evelopment/01.htm
tistical reliability of the SCCIT. language and related behaviors. In R.
Bess, F. (1985). The minimally hearing
D. Kent (Ed.), Reference manual for
impaired child. Ear and Hearing, 6(1),
Conclusion communicative sciences and disor-
43-47.
ders (pp. 21-22). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
The value of routine language and Billeaud, F. (2003). Assessment: Examina-
Keown, L.J., Woodward, L.J., & Field, J.
cognitive screening in infants and tod- tion, interpretation, and reporting. In
(2001). Language development of
F. Billeaud (Ed.), Communication dis-
dlers cannot be disqualified. The PNP orders in infants and toddlers:
pre-school children born to teenage
who screens young patients at each mothers. Infant and Child Develop-
Assessment and intervention (3rd
well-baby visit can be instrumental in ment, 10, 129-145.
ed.). Philadelphia: Elsevier.
noting early signs of later deficits in Klee, T., Carson, D.K., Gavin, W.J., Hall, L.,
Blackwell, P.B., & Baker, B.M. (2002).
language and cognition, and aiding Kent, A., & Reece, S. (1998).
Estimating communication compe-
Concurrent and predictive validity of
parents in determining when further tence of infants and toddlers. Journal
an early language screening pro-
testing may be indicated. Early inter- of Pediatric Health Care, 16, 29-35.
gram. Journal of Speech, Language,
vention can be implemented in a time- Borsel, J.V., Curfs, L.M., & Fryns, J.P.
and Hearing Research, 41, 627-641.
ly manner. Language and cognitive (1997). Hyperacusis in Williams syn-
Klein, A., Armstrong, B., Greer, M., &
drome: A sample survey study.
delays and disorders that go undiag- Genetic Counseling, 8(2), 121-126.
Brown, F. (1990). Hyperacusis and
nosed and untreated put children Bzoch, K., League, R., & Brown, V. (2000).
otitis media in individuals with
unnecessarily at risk for poor academ- Williams Syndrome. Journal of
Receptive-Expressive Emergent Lang-
ic, social, and vocational progress Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55,
uage Scale (3rd ed.), (REEL-3). San
during their school years. Conversely, 339-344.
Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment,
Koegel, R.L., Openden, D., & Koegel, L.K.
for many affected children, treatment Inc.
(2004). A systematic desensitization
initiated during infancy and toddler- Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a
paradigm to treat hypersensitivity to
hood facilitates the emergence of single new word. Papers and Reports
auditory stimuli in children with
skills that would otherwise lag behind on Child Language Development
autism in family contexts. Research
(Stanford University), 15, 17-29.
or fail to develop. Although a number Coster, W., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Research
and practice for persons with severe
of screening tools exist, none emerge disabilities. Special Family and
on the communicative development
as a convenient, time-efficient tool for Disability, 29(2), 122-134.
of maltreated children. Topics in
the pediatric nurse practitioner to Laing, G.J., Law, J., Levin, A., & Logan, S.
Language Disorders, 13, 25-38.
employ during well-baby visits. (2002). Evaluation of a structured test
Dale, P.S., Bates, E., Reznick, J.S., &
and a parent led method for screen-
The brief two-part screening tool Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a
ing for speech and language prob-
included here (see Figure 1) provides parent report instrument of child lan-
lems: Prospective population based
18 questions that probe precursor guage at twenty months. Journal of
study. British Medical Journal, 325,
behaviors to later development of lan- Child Language, 16, 239-249.
1152-1156.
Davis, J.M., Elfenbein, J., Schum, R., &
guage and cognitive skills in infants Bentler, R.A. (1986). Effects of mild
Law J., Boyle J., Harris F, Harkness, A., &
and toddlers. The screening is and moderate hearing impairments
Nye, C. (1998). Screening for primary
designed to highlight the need for a speech and language delay: A sys-
on language, educational, and psy-
referral to a speech-language patholo- tematic review of the literature.
chosocial behavior of children.
gist for further assessment. International Journal of Language
Journal of Speech and Hearing
and Communication Disorders, 33,
The authors give permission to Disorders, 51, 53-62.
21-23.

PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6 479


Lee, L., Stemple, J., Glaze, L., & Kelchner, autism and pervasive developmental How do primary care physicians
L. (2004). Quick screen for voice and disorders (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley manage children with possible devel-
supplementary documents for identi- & Sons. opmental delays? A national survey
fying pediatric voice disorders. Prater, C., & Zylstra, R. (2002). Autism: A with an experimental design.
Language, Speech, and Hearing medical primer. American Family Pediatrics, 113, 274-282.
Services in Schools, 35, 308-319. Physician, 66, 1667-1674. Snowling, M., Adams, J., Bishop, D., &
Leonard, L.B. (1998). Children with specif- Ramey, C.T., & Ramey, S.L. (1998). Stothard, S. (2001). Educational
ic language impairment. Cambridge, Prevention of intellectual disabilities: attainments of school leavers with a
MA: MIT Press. Early interventions to improve cogni- preschool history of speech-lan-
Motion, S., Northstone, K., Emond, A., tive development. Preventive guage impairments. International
Stucke, S., Golding, J. (2002) Early Medicine, 27, 224-232. Journal of Language and Communi-
feeding problems in children with Recchia, S.L. (1997). Play and concept cation Disorders, 36, 173-183.
cerebral palsy: weight and neurode- development in infants and young Stokes, S.F. (1997). Secondary prevention
velopmental outcomes. Develop- children with severe visual impair- of paediatric language disability: A
mental Medicine & Child Neurology, ments: A constructivist view. Journal comparison of parents and nurses as
44, 40–43. of Visual Impairment & Blindness, screening agents. European Journal
National Institute for Deafness and Other 91(4), 401. of Disorders of Communication: The
Communication Disorders (NIDCD) Rescorla, L., & Alley, A. (2001). Validation Journal of the College of Speech and
(2000). Statistics on voice, speech, of the language development survey Language Therapists, London, 32,
and language. Retrieved July 12, (LDS): A parent report tool for identi- 139-158.
2006, from http://www.nidcd.nih.gov/ fying language in toddlers. Journal Sturner, R., Layton, T., Evans, A., Heller, J.,
health/statistics/vsl.asp of Speech, Language, and Hearing Funk, S., & Machon, M. (1994).
National Institute for Deafness and Other Research, 44, 434-445. Preschool speech and language
Communication Disorders (NIDCD) Rice, M.L. (1983). Contemporary accounts screening: a review of currently avail-
(2006). Speech and language devel- of the cognition/language relation- able tests. American Journal of
opmental milestones. Retrieved ship: Implications for speech-lan- Speech Language Pathology, 3, 25-
October 20, 2007, from http://www. guage clinicians. Journal of Speech 36.
nidcd.nih.gov/health/voice/speechan and Hearing Disorders, 48, 347-359. Tanner, D. (2003). Exploring communica-
dlanguage.asp Robbins D., Fein D., Barton M., & Green, tion disorders. Boston: Allyn and
Nelson, H. D., Nygren, P., Walker, M., & J. (2001).The modified checklist for Bacon.
Panoscha, R. (2006). Screening for autism in toddlers [MCHAT]: An ini- Thal, D., & Bates, E. (1988). Language and
speech and language delay in pre- tial study investigating the early gesture in late talkers. Journal of
school children: Systematic evidence detection of autism and pervasive Speech and Hearing Research, 31,
review for the U.S. Preventive developmental disorders. Journal of 115-123.
Services Task Force. [Electronic ver- Autism and Developmental Disorders, Ward, S. (1999). Investigation into the
sion]. Pediatrics, 117, 298-319. 31(2), 149-151. effectiveness of an early intervention
New York State Education Department Rosetti, L. (1990). The Rosetti infant-tod- method for delayed language devel-
Office of the Professions. (2004). dler language scale. East Moline, IL: opment in young children.
What you should know about nurses LinguiSystems. International Journal of Language
and their services. Retrieved June 14, Sand, N., Silverstein, M., Glascoe, F.P., and Communication Disorders, 34,
2006, from http://www.op.nysed.gov Gupta, V.B., Tonniges, T.P., & O’Connor, 243-264.
/nursebroch.htm K.G. (2005). Pediatricians’ reported Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S.
NICHD Early Child Care Research practices regarding developmental (2006). Prelinguistic predictors of lan-
Network. (1999). Chronicity of mater- screening: Do guidelines work? Do guage outcome at 3 years of age.
nal depressive symptoms, maternal they help? Pediatrics, 116, 174-179. Journal of Speech Language and
sensitivity, and child functioning at Schiavetti, N., & Metz, D. (2002). Hearing Research, 49, 1224-1237.
36 months. Developmental Psych- Evaluating research in communica- Weismer, S., Murray-Branch, J., & Miller,
ology, 35, 1297-1310. tive disorders (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn J. (1994). A prospective longitudinal
Nicolosi, L., Harryman, E., & Kresheck, J. & Bacon. study of language development in
(2004). Terminology of communica- Sherwood, G.D., Brown, M., Vaunette, F., late talkers. Journal of Speech and
tion disorders. Philadelphia: & Wardell, D. (1997). Defining nurse Hearing Research, 37, 852-867.
Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. practitioner scope of practice: Wetherby, A., Cain, D., Yonclas, D., &
Nilholm, C. (1999). The zone of proximal Expanding primary care services. Walker, V. (1988). Analysis of inten-
development: A comparison of chil- [Electronic version]. Internet Journal tional communication of normal chil-
dren with Down syndrome and typi- of Advanced Nursing Practice, 1(2). dren from the prelinguistic to the
cal children. Journal of Intellectual & Shriberg, L., Lewis, B., Tomblin, J., multiword stage. Journal of Speech
Developmental Disability, 24, 265- McSweeney, J., Karlsson, H., & and Hearing Research, 31, 240-252.
279. Scheer, A. (2005). Toward diagnostic Wetherby, A., Prizant. B., & Hutchinson, T.
Olswang, L.B., Rodriguez, B., & Timler, G. and phenotype markers for geneti- (1998). Communicative, social/affec-
(1998). Recommending intervention cally transmitted speech delay. tive, and symbolic profiles of young
for toddlers with specific language Journal of Speech, Language, Hear- children with autism and pervasive
learning difficulties: We may not ing Research, 48, 834-852. developmental disorders. American
have all the answers, but we know a Sices, L., Feudtner, C., McLaughlin, J., Journal of Speech-Language Path-
lot. American Journal of Speech- Drotar, D., & Williams, M. (2004). ology, 7, 79-91.
Language Pathology, 7, 23-32.
Papero, A.L. (2005). Is early, high-quality
daycare an asset for the children of
low-income, depressed mothers?
Developmental Review, 25, 181-211.
Paul, R. (1991). Profiles of toddlers with
slow expressive language develop-
Earn CNE credits by taking the
ment. Topics in Language Disorders,
11(4), 1–13. Posttest found on pages 486-487.
Paul, R. (2005). Assessing communication
in autism. In F. Volkmar, A. Klin, R.
Paul, & D. Cohen (Eds.) Handbook of

480 PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6


Infants and Toddlers
The purpose of this continuing nursing education series is to Objectives:
increase the pediatric nurse’s understanding of selected issues relat- 1. Discuss the importance of a developmental/family-
ed to the care of infants, toddlers, and their families. centered approach in the care of infants and toddlers.
Infancy and toddlerhood are both periods of rapid development.
Nurses often are critically situated to assess and monitor early devel- 2. Describe a screening tool to assess communication
opment and to intervene when necessary. Separation is a primary and cognitive disorders in infants and toddlers.
developmental issue for infants and toddlers who are hospitalized, 3. Compare and contrast parents’ and staff’s responses
one that maximizing parental presence may help to alleviate. Keeping to a survey evaluating family-centered care on an
updated on methods to identify and address concerns increases pedi- infant-toddler unit.
atric nurses’ ability to promote development in infants and toddlers 4. Identify opportunities for pediatric nurses to keep cur-
who interact with the healthcare system. rent on infant-toddler issues.
This continuing nursing education series consists of two articles
that address infant-toddler issues. The first article describes a screen-
ing tool nurses can use to assess communication and cognitive dis-
orders in infants and toddlers. The second article presents findings This offering for 1.5 contact hours is provided by Anthony J.
from a study that evaluated parent and staff perceptions of family- Jannetti, Inc.
centered care on an infant-toddler unit. Anthony J. Jannetti, Inc is accredited as a provider of continuing
nursing education by the American Nurses Credentialing Center's
ASSIGNMENT Commission on Accreditation (ANCC-COA).
Scheffler, F., Vogel, D., Astern, R., Burgess, J., Conneally, T., & Anthony J. Jannetti, Inc. is an approved provider of continuing
Salerno, K. (2007). Screening for communication and cognitive education by the California Board of Registered Nursing, CEP No.
disorders in infants and toddlers. Pediatric Nursing, 33(6), 473- 5387.
480. Articles accepted for publication in the continuing education
Neal, A., Frost, M., Kuhn, J., Green, A., Gance-Cleveland, B., & series are refereed manuscripts that are reviewed in the standard
Kersten, R. (2007). Family centered care within an infant-tod- Pediatric Nursing review process with other articles appearing in the
dler unit. Pediatric Nursing, 33(6), 481-485. journal.
This test was reviewed and edited by Judy A. Rollins, PhD, RN,
Pediatric Nursing associate editor, and Veronica D. Feeg, PhD, RN,
Earn 1.5 Contact Hours FAAN, Pediatric Nursing editor.

Questions
1. Language may best be defined 5. Infants and toddlers can be 8. What similarities did Neal et al.
as: screened for delays and disorders identify between parent and staff
a. oral expression using consonants in communication and cognition concerns?
and vowels to form words. by questions that probe for: a. Parents and staff were positive
b. symbolic representation of a. the absence of typical behaviors about family centered care.
thoughts. at expected ages. b. Parents and staff were congruent
c. problem solving skills. b. the presence of atypical in concern about pain
d. fast-mapping skills. behaviors. assessment.
e. None of the above c. concerns raised by parents about c. Parents and staff were congruent
their child’s development. in concern over privacy.
2. Speech may best be defined as: d. All of the above d. None of the above
a. oral expression using consonants e. None of the above
and vowels to form words. 9. Which statement best describes
b. symbolic representation of 6. Which of the following is NOT the major differences between
thoughts. true of family centered care? parent and staff concerns identi-
c. facial expressions. a. Recognizing parents as fied in the study?
d. body gestures. information sources a. Families were overall more
e. None of the above b. Limiting parent involvement in negative than staff.
care b. Staff were overall more negative
3. Preschool-aged children who c. Respecting the diversity of than families.
have delays in communication: families c. Families were more concerned
a. may ignore or misunderstand d. Building on family strengths with the environment.
parental instructions. d. Staff were more concerned about
b. may show slow vocabulary 7. Which of the following best de- pain assessment.
growth compared to same-age scribes why family centered care
peers. is important? 10. What can hospitals do to improve
c. may be very shy. a. Families are responsible for family centered care?
d. may be overly aggressive (bite or hospital bills. a. Assess the current state of care
kick). b. Consumers demand family b. Involve staff and parents in plans
e. All of the above centered care. c. Educate staff involved in care
c. Families are a constant source of d. All of the above
4. Behaviors in infants and toddlers support.
that are considered precursors to d. None of the above
communication and cognitive
delays and disorders include:
a. ignoring loud sounds.
b. minimal babbling.
c. minimal interest in toys and
objects.
d. lack of joint attention.
e. All of the above

486 PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6


Infants and Toddlers
Answer Form: Infants and Toddlers *PED J0708
Check the box next to the correct answer.
1.  A 2.  A 3.  A 4.  A 5.  A 6.  A 7.  A 8.  A 9.  A 10.  A
B B B B B  B  B  B  B  B
C C C C C  C  C  C  C  C
D D D D D  D  D  D  D  D
E E E E E
This test may be copied for use by others.

COMPLETE THE FOLLOWING:


POSTTEST INSTRUCTIONS
Name: ______________________________________________________________ 1. Select the best answer and check the
corresponding box on the answer
Address: ____________________________________________________________ form. Retain the test questions as your
record.
____________________________________________________________ 2. Complete the information requested in
the space provided.
City: ___________________________State: _______________Zip:____________ 3. Detach the answer form or a copy of
the answer form and mail to: Pediatric
Strongly Strongly Nursing, CNE Series, Jannetti
Evaluation disagree agree Publications Inc.; East Holly Avenue
Box 56; Pitman, NJ 08071-0056 with a
1. The objectives relate to the overall 1 2 3 4 5
purpose/goals of the education activity.
check or money order payable to
2. The activity met the stated objectives. Jannetti Publications Inc. for $10.00
a. Discuss the importance of a developmental/ 1 2 3 4 5 (subscriber) or $15.00 (nonsubscriber).
family-centered approach in the care of infants 4. Test returns must be postmarked by
and toddlers. December 15, 2009. If you pass the test
b. Describe a screening tool to assess com- 1 2 3 4 5 (70% or better), a certificate for 1.5 con-
munication and cognitive disorders in infants tact hours will be awarded by Anthony
and toddlers. J. Jannetti, Inc.
c. Compare and contrast parents’ and staff’s 1 2 3 4 5
responses to a survey evaluating family-
centered care on an infant-toddler unit. Please allow 6–8 weeks for processing.
d. Identify opportunities for pediatric nurses to 1 2 3 4 5 For recertification purposes, the date that
keep current on infant-toddler issues. contact hours are awarded will reflect the
3. Home study format was appropriate. 1 2 3 4 5 date of processing.
4. The content was relevant to my practice. 1 2 3 4 5
5. The content met my needs. 1 2 3 4 5
6. How much time was used to complete reading
assignment and posttest: Test Scoring, CNE Awarding/Recording fees:
a. Less than 1 hour _______ b. 1-2 hours _______ PN Subscriber ■ $10.00
c. 2-3 hours _______ d. 3 hours or more _______ Nonsubscriber ■ $15.00
Comments _____________________________________________________________
Expir. Date _______________________
Signature ______________________________________________________________

Plan to Attend!
24th Annual
Pediatric Nursing
Conference
June 26-28, 2008
Las Vegas Hilton Hotel
Las Vegas, NV

PEDIATRIC NURSING/November-December 2007/Vol. 33/No. 6 487

Вам также может понравиться