Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

A reprint from

American Scientist
the magazine of Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society

This reprint is provided for personal and noncommercial use. For any other use, please send a request to Permissions,
American Scientist, P.O. Box 13975, Research Triangle Park, NC, 27709, U.S.A., or by electronic mail to perms@amsci.org.
©Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society and other rightsholders
The 95 Percent Solution
School is not where most Americans learn most of their science

John H. Falk and Lynn D. Dierking

T he scientific research and educa-


tion communities have long had
a goal of advancing the public’s un-
museums, zoos, aquariums, national
parks, community activities such as
4-H and scouting and many other sci-
The unquestioned focus was to in-
crease the quantity of qualified science
teachers and by doing so, the quality
derstanding of science. The vast ma- entifically enriching enterprises. The of teaching. This assumption shaped
jority of the rhetoric and research on sheer quantity and importance of this years of research on the public under-
this issue revolves around the failure science learning landscape lies in plain standing of science, summarized bian-
of school-aged children in the United sight but mostly out of mind. We be- nually by the National Science Board
States to excel at mathematics and sci- lieve that nonschool resources—used in their Science and Engineering Indica-
ence when compared with children in by learners across their lifetimes from tors series. National organizations such
other countries. Most policy solutions childhood onward—actually account as the American Association for the
for this problem involve improving for the vast majority of Americans’ sci- Advancement of Science and the Na-
classroom practices and escalating the ence learning. If this premise is cor- tional Academies of Sciences commis-
investment in schooling, particularly rect, then increased investment in sioned white papers focusing on the
during the precollege years. The as- free-choice (also known as informal) issue, and science-education reform
sumption has been that children do learning resources might be a very efforts were funded by the National
most of their learning in school and cost-effective way to significantly im- Science Foundation and the Depart-
that the best route to long-term public prove public understanding of science. ment of Education.
understanding of science is successful Taking this view, though, requires dis- Over the ensuing years, the content
formal schooling. The “school-first” mantling a widespread misconception and approach to teaching science in
paradigm is so pervasive that few that out-of-school educational experi- schools has varied from year to year
scientists, educators or policy makers ences only support superficial science and from district to district. However,
question it. This despite two impor- learning and the recreational interests the general commitment to science
tant facts: Average Americans spend of a limited percentage of the curious for all has remained a basic tenet of
less than 5 percent of their life in class- public, rather than the learning of real school-based science education. Also
rooms, and an ever-growing body of science by all citizens. fundamentally unchanged over the
evidence demonstrates that most sci- Traditional assumptions about the past 25 years is the assumption by vir-
ence is learned outside of school. source of science knowledge are deep- tually all within the science education
We contend that a major educa- ly held. Historian of science Steven community—scientists, science educa-
tional advantage enjoyed by the U.S. Turner locates the beginning of today’s tors, science learning researchers, edu-
relative to the rest of the world is its Public Understanding of Science move- cation policy makers and the public—
vibrant free-choice science learning ment in the 1980s. Its hallmarks were that if science for all is the goal, then
landscape—a landscape filled with a “new, vigorous efforts to promote pub- schools are the most effective conduit.
vast array of digital resources, edu- lic knowledge of science and to instill However, a range of data are emerg-
cational television and radio, science confidence and support for the scien- ing that suggest other interpretations
tific enterprise.” The major focus of this that at the very least raise important
John H. Falk and Lynn D. Dierking are Sea effort was a widespread reassessment questions about the prevailing para-
Grant professors in free-choice science learn- of the content and goals of school sci- digm that schooling is the primary
ing, College of Science, Oregon State Univer- ence teaching and a shift of curricu- mechanism for public science learn-
sity. Their research focuses on youth, adults, lar reform efforts toward the needs of ing. For example, for more than a
and families in free-choice learning environ- the substantial majority of students decade, performance by U.S. school-
ments such as museums, libraries, and commu- who would not pursue scientific and aged children on international tests
nity organizations. Falk has a joint doctorate
technological careers or postsecond- such as the quadrennial Trends in In-
in biology and education from the University
ary training in technical subjects. This ternational Mathematics and Science
of California, Berkeley. Dierking received her
Ph.D. in science education from the University of reform movement went forward under Study (TIMSS) and the Programme
Florida, Gainesville. Address for Falk/Dierking: the catchy slogan “scientific literacy,” for International Student Assessment
237/235 Weniger Hall, Corvallis, OR 97331. but its other motto, “science for all,” (PISA) has followed a consistent pat-
Email: falkj@science.oregonstate.edu; dierkinl@ better expresses its true political and tern. Elementary-school-aged U.S.
science.oregonstate.edu pedagogical objectives. children perform as well as or better

© 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction


486 American Scientist, Volume 98
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
Figure 1. Recent findings challenge the longstanding belief that the place for science knowledge acquisition is the classroom. International
comparisons of trends in science knowledge over lifetimes suggests that much if not most science knowledge is acquired outside of school.
This raises important questions about where our efforts should be spent if we want to improve public understanding of science. A powerful
example of free-choice exposure to science is the highly praised MythBusters television program, which exemplifies the central aspects of sci-
entific exploration: hypothesis, experiment and measurement. Here cohost Adam Savage takes on the folk knowledge that sneezes are expelled
at 100 miles per hour. A bit of snuff, a high-speed camera, a spirit of inquiry and a calculation of distance over time yields an engaging lesson
in science. And an answer: Sneezes travel about 40 miles per hour. (Photograph courtesy of The Discovery Channel.)

than most children in the world, but For starters, most in the U.S. science so rarely occurs. Indicative of the situ-
the performance of older U.S. children learning community agree that the ation nationwide, a 2007 study of San
has been mediocre at best. Interestingly, quality of school science education is Francisco Bay–area elementary schools
however, for more than 20 years, U.S. better at the secondary level than at the found that 80 percent of K–5 multiple-
adults have consistently outperformed preschool and elementary levels. Re- subject teachers who are responsible
their international counterparts on sci- cent statistics show that only about 4 for teaching science in their classrooms
ence literacy measures, including adults percent of U.S. school teachers of kin- reported spending 60 minutes or less per
from South Korea and Japan, as well dergarten through second grade (K–2) week on science; 16 percent of teachers
as Western European countries such as majored in science or science educa- reported spending no time at all on sci-
Germany and the United Kingdom. If tion as undergraduates, and many ence. Consistent science instruction in
schooling is the primary causative fac- took no college-level science courses U.S. schools only begins at the middle-
tor affecting how well the public un- at all. However, the quality of science school level, when every student takes
derstands science, how do we explain instruction at that level is almost a moot at least one or two science courses, usu-
these findings? point because science instruction itself ally taught by individuals with some

www.americanscientist.org © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction 2010 November–December 487
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
ence and technology as well as piqued
their interest in science and prompted
further inquiries after the visit.
These data were validated by a
“conceptual marker” in the form of a
science specific scientific concept—homeosta-
education sis. Prior to the opening of the new
science center, only 7 percent of the
Los Angeles public could define this
time of day

term (including first-time visitors to


3:00 the California Science Center). How-
p.m. ever, because of a popular exhibition
K–12 college experience designed to teach this con-
June
cept—a 50-foot animatronic woman—
s a majority of Science Center visitors
nth
8:00 mo could define the term upon exiting
a.m. September the museum. The ability to correctly
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 explain this one scientific concept has
life span (years)
increased nearly threefold in Los An-
Figure 2. On average, only about 5 percent of an American’s lifetime is spent in the classroom,
geles over the decade following the
and only a small fraction of that is dedicated to science instruction. Emerging data suggest reopening of the Science Center. By
that the best way to increase the public understanding of science is to reach people during the tracking this conceptual marker, we
other 95 percent of their life. can directly attribute the increase in
understanding to visits to the Science
science background. Interestingly, it is walk in the park contribute to people’s Center. These data, along with data
just at the point when school-based sci- knowledge and interest in science and from other science centers and com-
ence instruction begins in earnest that the environment. Adults visit settings parable free-choice science learning
American children start falling behind such as national parks, science centers settings, have shown that the majority
their international peers. Meanwhile, and botanical gardens not only to relax of visitors significantly increase their
what accounts for the high performance and enjoy themselves, but equally to conceptual understanding of science
of American adults? satisfy their intellectual curiosity and on a variety of levels—basic infor-
Although data show that taking col- enhance their understanding of the mation, breadth and depth of under-
lege-level science courses dramatically natural and human-made world. Even standing—immediately following a
improves public science literacy, only more common is the science people visit, and for most of these individu-
about 30 percent of U.S. adults have learn while engaged in efforts to satis- als this understanding persists and
ever taken even one college-level sci- fy their personal need to know. Some- grows for two or more years after the
ence course. Thus, the superior science times the need is fleeting. For example, experience. Similar science learning
literacy of the U.S. general public rela- individuals may choose to watch a outcomes have been found for youth
tive to other countries cannot be easily nature show on television, or invest and after-school program experiences,
explained by schooling either at the time, energy and money in support- and both print and broadcast media
precollege or college levels. Develop- ing their children’s science learning by sources have long since been shown to
ers of the large-scale national science taking them to national parks, science be vital to both children’s and adults’
literacy tests, the results of which are centers and zoos, or encourage their understanding of health, science and
compared internationally, claim that children to participate in a wide vari- environmental issues.
these measures reliably measure the ety of extracurricular experiences such Historically, the majority of atten-
knowledge of representative samples as scouting and summer nature camps. tion paid to out-of-school science learn-
of target populations, so it follows that One specific example of the role that ing, including most academic research,
other factors beyond schooling must out-of-school institutions play in the has been directed to experiences like
explain or at least significantly contrib- support of the public’s science learning visiting a museum, science center, zoo
ute to the U-shaped pattern of Ameri- comes from more than a decade of re- or aquarium, or watching broadcast
cans’ comparative performance on sci- search at the California Science Center media such as NOVA shows and the
ence literacy measures. in Los Angeles. Findings from one part like. Although, as suggested above,
of this series of studies—large-scale, these free-choice science learning ex-
Science in the Wild random telephone surveys—found periences are undoubtedly important
A growing body of evidence supports that more than 60 percent of Los An- contributors to the public’s science
the contention that the public learns geles residents had visited the Science literacy, they represent only the most
science in settings and situations out- Center since it was renovated in 1998, conspicuous part of the free-choice sci-
side of school. A 2009 report by the including residents of all races/ethnici- ence learning landscape. Equally im-
National Research Council, Learn- ties, neighborhoods, incomes and edu- portant but much less discussed and
ing Science in Informal Environments: cation levels. Findings also showed that studied are education situations that
Places, People and Pursuits, describes a a majority of former visitors (95 per- support long-term, more in-depth op-
range of evidence demonstrating that cent) self-reported that the experience portunities for science learning. A wide
even everyday experiences such as a increased their understanding of sci- range of adolescents and adults are

488 American Scientist, Volume 98 © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
engaged in hobbies that involve sci- Pew Internet & American Life Project,
ence, including model rocketry, raising 2006 was the tipping point when the In-
ornamental fish, gardening, rock col- ternet exceeded even broadcast media
lecting and star gazing. Hobbyists such as a source of public science informa-
as these often possess deep specialized tion. The medical profession has come
knowledge of science and invest con- to appreciate that the public today is far
siderable amounts of money in equip- more likely to seek medical information
ment, travel, education and training online than from a “live” healthcare
to refine their craft. Equally important professional; as stated earlier, individu-
are the many events in life, often highly als with serious ailments use the Inter-
personal, which demand increased un- net for continued, deep learning about
derstanding of science “right now.” For their illnesses.
example, when individuals are diag-
nosed with leukemia or heart disease, Science on the Side
they and their loved ones invest large Another emerging area of research in-
amounts of time researching websites vestigates science-related hobbies. Re-
and medical reports in order to learn as search conducted by Marni Berendsen,
much as possible about the particular education researcher and project direc-
disease. Similar behaviors arise when tor of the NASA Night Sky Network,
an environmental crisis occurs such as showed that amateur astronomy club
a toxic spill or the discovery of radon members lacking college-level astron-
gas seeping from the rock on which omy training often knew more general
one’s home is built. With an increas- astronomy than did undergraduate
ingly accessible Internet, becoming in- astronomy majors. Research by oth-
formed about such issues is easy, even ers has also shown hobbyists, many
routine. with little formal training, exhibiting
A small but compelling set of data high levels of knowledge and depth
is beginning to emerge showing that of understanding. Such hobbyists of-
the nonstudent public also gathers ten have collegial relationships with Figure 3. Tess, the 50-foot animatronic body
in-depth science knowledge outside experts in the field and some, having simulator, is part of the World of Life per-
of school. Our research shows that put themselves in the right place at the manent gallery at the California Science
Center in Los Angeles. When she arrived, 7
free-choice learning experiences rep- right time, have contributed scientific
percent of Angelenos could define the term
resent the single greatest contributors discoveries. For example, on March homeostasis. That figure had almost tripled
to adult science knowledge; childhood 18–19, 2010, amateur astronomer Nick by a decade later. (Photograph courtesy of the
free-choice learning experiences also Howes was working from his desk- California Science Center.)
significantly contributed to adult sci- top computer in Great Britain using a
ence knowledge. Schooling ranks at the remotely controlled 2-meter telescope for school, as opposed to learning for
bottom of significant sources of adult located in Hawaii and operated by the life. One study found that the number
science knowledge. Specifically, our re- Faulkes Telescope Project. He dialed or level of mathematics courses taken
search shows that science information up the coordinates of a comet he had in school correlated poorly, if at all, with
sources such as books, magazines, dis- been observing, calibrated his camera mathematical performance in out-of-
cussions with experts, and the Internet and snared a set of six photos showing school, everyday-life situations. In an-
represented the primary mechanisms an object moving away from the icy other study of mathematics learning,
the public uses to delve more deeply nucleus of the comet. What he cap- even individuals who did not do well
into a topic. During the recent dramas tured was the breakup of comet C2007 or were not formally trained in school
surrounding the deep-water oil spill in C3, an observation hailed by the In- mathematics demonstrated the ability
the Gulf of Mexico, news websites such ternational Astronomical Union as a to use math successfully in everyday
as CNN and CNBC, information web- “major astronomical discovery.” life—for example, sellers of candy in
sites such as www.theoildrum.com and Investigations of everyday science street markets and shoppers selecting
even the government’s own NOAA literacy have yielded other interesting good deals. Success in technical and
website were humming with activity as data. For example, a series of studies scientific training courses for ship of-
the public tried to get below the super- by Canadian science-education re- ficers was shown to be unrelated to the
ficial headlines of the six o’clock news. searcher Wolff-Michael Roth and col- relevant knowledge required onboard.
These and other data suggest that the leagues found that members of an en- As observed by Roth and his colleagues
importance of school as a source of vironmental activist group working on in their investigation of adults working
science learning is actually declining the revitalization of a local creek and on a local environmental issue, “There
among the public as citizens utilize an its watershed acted and learned using was little that looked like school sci-
ever-broadening range of information knowledge derived from a wide variety ence, and there was little done in school
resources, including most dramatically of resources, virtually none of which science that prepared these adults for
the Internet, which now represents the required or drew from school-based this or any other similar kinds of prob-
major source of science information for sources. Similar research by others re- lematic situations in life.”
all citizens, including young children. inforces that much of what is learned in Although the role of free-choice
According to research conducted by the school actually relates more to learning learning experiences remains contest-

www.americanscientist.org © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction 2010 November–December 489
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
United States Japan China Russia cally and socially advantaged subur-
ban counterparts. However, each and
South Korea Brazil European Union every summer of the study, the inner-
city children fell woefully behind; the
70 suburban 70children continued to70gain 70
in performance while the inner-city
60 children stagnated
60 or even declined
60 in 60
performance.
number of visits to informal science
or cultural institutions (percent)

The authors concluded that much


50 50 in performance between
of the “gap” 50 50
disadvantaged and advantaged chil-
40 dren appeared
40 to be the consequence
40 40
of what happened outside of school.
Interestingly, these authors, and many
30 others who 30 have read this research,
30 30
interpret the findings as evidence that
20 disadvantaged
20 children need to 20
spend 20
more time in school! Of course, an al-
ternative interpretation could be that
10 10 10
what happens in school is not10suffi-
cient to ensure equity among all chil-
0 dren and adults.
0 If, as we’ve argued
0 0
public library zoo/aquarium natural history science/technology all along, school is not where Ameri-
museum museum cans learn much of what they know,
Figure 4. The U.S. public has a lush endowment of free-choice opportunites to learn science, including science, then it follows that
which it uses extensively. The relative patronage of science-oriented institutions shown above what happens outside of school pro-
may explain why the disappointing gap in science proficiency of U.S. youngsters compared to foundly influences learning. Rather
their most advanced peers worldwide disappears as the youngsters become adults. than increasing school time, perhaps
we should be investing in expanding
ed, few would argue that out-of-school ences, most still consider such experi- quality, out-of-school experiences for
experiences support the public’s sci- ences a nicety rather than a necessity, disadvantaged children.
ence interest and attitudes. However, an adjunct to the serious business of
recent research by Robert H. Tai and learning that takes place in classrooms. Nonacademic Academics
associates, utilizing data from the Na- Most policy and funding initiatives Supporting evidence for the impor-
tional Educational Longitudinal Study continue to be directed towards im- tant role that out-of-school experi-
(NELS), pushes the potential impor- proving in-school performance based ences have on children’s learning is
tance of this role far beyond what most on the rarely questioned assumption emerging from a variety of fronts. For
have assumed. Tai’s research group that classroom-based education is the example, a recent meta-analysis of ex-
found that attitudes toward science ca- exclusive route to achieving desired perimental and quasi-experimental
reers, formed primarily during out-of- educational outcomes. evaluation findings for after-school
school time in early adolescence, ap- A major justification for these argu- programs showed that such programs
peared to be the single most important ments is the issue of equity. After all, need not be academically focused in
factor in determining children’s future schooling is the “great leveler,” the order to have academic impact. In fact,
career choices in science. Among a mechanism for eliminating socioeco- because the authors were interested
random sample of 3,359 NELS partici- nomic disparities. If only, the argument in programs with a socio-emotional
pants who finished college, those who goes, schools could all be brought up learning focus, academic-only after-
expected at age 13 to have a science to comparable levels of quality, historic school programs were not included
career, compared to those with other inequalities could be overcome. A re- in the study, and investigators still
career expectations, were two times cent study on the “performance gap” observed gains overall in the grades
more likely to have graduated with in reading between advantaged and children earned. Similarly, a recent
a degree in the life sciences and three disadvantaged children in Baltimore evaluation of Chicago’s After-School
times more likely to have a degree in was designed to highlight just this is- Matters found that programs without
the physical sciences or engineering. sue; however, the results ran counter an explicit academic focus (they fo-
Interestingly, achievement in school to expectations. Data from this major cused instead on career awareness and
mathematics, considered a critical fil- longitudinal study showed that over development) had a positive effect on
ter and a major focus of today’s high- the first five years of schooling, the in- several school-related outcomes, in-
stakes testing, was not as important a school performance gains in reading of cluding graduation rates and atten-
predictor as was interest in the topic. low-income, inner-city Baltimore chil- dance. On a completely different front,
Despite alternative interpretations dren was completely equivalent to that data from the Programme for Inter-
for U.S. adults’ higher science literacy of affluent, suburban Baltimore chil- national Student Assessment showed
scores internationally and the growing dren; in fact in some cases the inner- that a major predictor of high achieve-
body of evidence supporting the criti- city children’s gains were greater than ment on the test was participation in
cal role of free-choice learning experi- those shown by their more economi- out-of-school, free-choice learning

490 American Scientist, Volume 98 © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
Mitch Kezar/Getty Images Galen Rowell/Corbis

Figure 5. The ubiquity of opportunities for informal science learning is often underestimated. Informative interludes range from strolling with
a birdwatching manual to touring the hydrosphere at one of the nation’s great aquariums. Knowledge seekers can enter the boundless Web
or curl up with the iPad app The Elements—sound, scholarly and hugely popular. (Bottom left image from WebMD.com; bottom right image
courtesy of Touch Press.

experiences such as visits to science science understanding among popu- and forcefully as the Harvard Family
museums. lations typically underrepresented Research Project, which stated:
As the Baltimore study and oth- in science. The report recommended
er research cited above make clear, that to make informal science rele- The dominant assumption be-
not just summer experiences but all vant to children and youth within a hind much current educational
kinds of free-choice childhood expe- community, the development of pro- policy and practice is that school
riences significantly contribute to a gramming and experiences should is the only place where and when
person’s science literacy; early child- be a collaborative effort between the children learn. This assumption
hood experiences form a particularly informal science organization, local is wrong. Forty years of steadily
critical foundation for all future sci- education institutions, and other en- accumulating research shows that
ence learning. The 2009 report on tities within the community such as out-of-school, or “complementary
learning science in informal environ- science-related industries and busi- learning” opportunities are major
ments from the National Research nesses. predictors of children’s develop-
Council, cited earlier, found that not Similar ideas have recently been ment, learning, and educational
only do free-choice science learn- voiced by a range of organizations, achievement. The research also
ing experiences jump-start a child’s such as the National 4-H Council indicates that economically and
long-term interest in science topics, and the American Youth Policy Fo- otherwise disadvantaged children
they also can significantly improve rum. None has stated it so clearly are less likely than their more-ad-

www.americanscientist.org © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction 2010 November–December 491
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
and the public need to continue to fo-
cus on equity to ensure that this trend
continues.

Serious Fun
However, as the potential beneficial
relationship between science learning
and OST becomes better understood,
there is a temptation to hand these pro-
grams over to schools. This would be
a huge mistake. It is exactly because
free-choice learning is not like school
that it has such value. What is impor-
tant is that children and youth perceive
the free-choice learning experiences that
often occur in typical OST programs as
personally meaningful, engaging and,
dare we say, fun—what educator Da-
vid Alexander calls, “the learning that
lies between play and academics.” The
inclusion of free-choice science learn-
ing experiences in the lives of children
is essential because young children
in particular learn through play. The
prevalence of a play-oriented medium
for educational delivery, which is very
common in the free-choice parts of the
science education landscape, has been
shown to encourage children to interact
with each other, adults and the objects
surrounding them in ways that signifi-
cantly support the development of sci-
ence inquiry skills.
If OST programs are merely devic-
es to extend the school day with more
hours of the same pedagogical experi-
ences, they are unlikely to be successful,
particularly in the long term. In fact, it’s
quite likely that they will do more harm
than good by reinforcing stereotypes of
science and science professionals as dry
and boring and schoollike. Our skepti-
cism and concerns revolve around the
fact that current discussions about in-
Figure 6. A great favorite of young and old: combustion chemistry. “When I talk to my Nobel creasing the scope and quality of OST
colleagues,” said Sir Richard Roberts, winner of the 1993 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medi- programs, though well-intentioned,
cine, “More than half of them got interested in science via fireworks.” (Photographs courtesy almost always focus on how such pro-
of Bryan Jackson and Zambelli Fireworks.) grams can support children and youth’s
achievement in school, rather than how
vantaged peers to have access to forts. According to the Harvard Family such programs should support children
these opportunities. This inequity Research Project’s 2007 Study of Predic- and youth in life.
substantially undermines their tors of Participation in Out-of-School- It seems reasonable to assume that
learning and chances for school Time Activities, participation rates in out-of-school science-learning experi-
success. before- and after-school programs have ences are fundamental to supporting
increased at all levels of family income, and facilitating lifelong science learn-
Fortunately, there are increasing op- with the greatest increase among the ing. We would argue that the current
portunities for youth and families from lowest-income youth. They attribute state of science literacy in America can-
poor and underserved communities this trend to an increasing policy fo- not be explained otherwise. One of the
to engage in out-of-school-time (OST) cus on the benefits of OST, along with major ways that U.S. adults and chil-
science experiences, driven by such extensive funding for the 21st Century dren under the age of 12 differ from
efforts as the NSF Informal Science Community Learning Centers, a pro- their counterparts in other countries is
Education program, which invests in gram of the U.S. Department of Educa- their access to and use of free-choice sci-
community-based science education ef- tion. They suggest that policymakers ence learning opportunities. Compared

492 American Scientist, Volume 98 © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.
with other countries, the U.S. has a
luxurious endowment of such destina-
tions. In the same studies that demon-
strated high correlations between adult
science literacy and levels of school-
ing, utilization of the free-choice science
learning landscape was a strong cor-
relate, as was shown in the Los Angeles
findings discussed earlier in this article.
In other words, utilization of these re-
sources could be a primary or at least
a highly important causal factor in U.S.
adults’ relatively high performance on
international measures of science lit-
eracy and interest.
Similarly, the simplest explanation

Jacques M. Chenet/Corbis
for why American 8-year-olds do so
well compared with their counter-
parts in other countries on the TIMSS
and PISA tests is that young Ameri-
can children have greater exposure to
free-choice science learning opportu-
nities than do children in any other Figure 7. This child at play receives lessons in the physiology of hearing, the physics of sound,
country. Unfortunately, utilization of and the mechanics of biological adaptation, as well as the chance to pretend to be a fox.
these learning opportunities declines
precipitously after age 12 in the U.S.
As has been shown repeatedly, the best Insufficient data exist to conclusively another window. Commissioned position
predictor of student success in school demonstrate that free-choice science paper for the Coalition for After-School Sci-
ence. New York, NY.
is family life. The quality of parent- learning experiences currently contrib-
ing is more important than socioeco- Dorph, R., et al. 2007. The Status of Science
ute more to public understanding of
Education in the Bay Area: Research Brief.
nomic factors, race/ethnicity or qual- science than in-school experiences, but Lawrence Hall of Science, University of
ity of school. Children with parents a growing body of evidence points in California, Berkeley.
who support their learning at home do this direction. There certainly are in- Ferreira, M. 2002. Ameliorating equity in sci-
better than children with parents who sufficient data to refute the claim that ence, mathematics, and engineering: A case
do not. A logical and perhaps more ef- free-choice learning is vitally impor- study of an after-school science program.
fective way for parents to support their tant. Surely the best informed and most Equity and Excellence in Education 35(1):43–49.
children’s learning beyond providing science-literate citizens are those who Fox, S. 2008. The engaged E-patient popula-
homework help is through free-choice enjoy maximal benefits from both in- tion. Washington, D.C.: Pew Internet &
learning experiences. However, as and out-of-school science learning op- American Life Project.
the Baltimore research cited above so portunities. Thus, we would argue for Harvard Family Research Project. 2007. Find-
clearly highlights, the availability and increased efforts to measure the cumu- ings from HFRP’s study of predictors of
opportunities for accessing free-choice participation in out-of-school time activi-
lative and complementary influences
ties: Fact sheet. http://www.hfrp.org/
science learning experiences are not in- of both in- and out-of-school science content/download/1072/48575/file/find-
dependent of income and geography. learning. However, given that at pres- ings_predictor_OSTfactsheet.pdf
By challenging the assumption that ent school-based science education Horrigan, J. 2006. The Internet as a resource
school is the primary place where efforts receive an order of magnitude for news and information about science.
Americans learn science, our goal is not more resources than free-choice learn- Washington, D.C.: Pew Internet & Ameri-
to diminish the importance and value ing options, even a modest change in can Life Project.
of schooling, but rather to suggest that this ratio could make a huge difference. Rahm, J., J. C. Moore and M.-P. Martel-Reny.
what goes on in the other 95 percent of The data suggest it would be a wise 2005. The role of afterschool and community
a citizen’s life may be equally impor- investment. science programs in the lives of urban youth.
School Science and Mathematics 105(6):283–291.
tant, and possibly more important to
increasing science literacy among the Taylor, S. 2008. School science and its controver-
public. Although we are not advocating Bibliography sies; or, whatever happened to scientific liter-
Bell, P., B. Lewenstein, A. W. Shouse, and M. A. acy? Public Understanding of Science 17:55–72.
any diminishment in the efforts to im-
Feder, eds. 2009. Learning Science in Informal
prove and expand school-based science Environments: People, Places, and Pursuits. The
education, we do strongly propose that National Academies Press, Washington. D.C.
it is time to seriously question whether, Bowles, A., and B. Brand. 2009. Learning For relevant Web links, consult this
in the 21st century, schooling should Around the Clock: Benefits of Expanded Learn- ­issue of American Scientist Online:
continue to be viewed as the most im- ing Opportunities for Older Youth. Washing-
http://www.americanscientist.org/
portant and effective mechanism for ton, D.C.: American Youth Policy Forum.
issues/id.87/past.aspx
advancing the public’s scientific interest Dierking. L. D. 2007. Linking after-school pro-
and understanding. grams and STEM learning: A view from

www.americanscientist.org © 2010 Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. Reproduction 2010 November–December 493
with permission only. Contact perms@amsci.org.

Вам также может понравиться