Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 5

The Effect of Sugar on Behavior

or Cognition in Children
A Meta-analysis
Mark L. Wolraich, MD; David B. Wilson, PhD; J. Wade White, MD

Objective.\p=m-\Toexamine the effects of sugar on the behavior or cognition of behavioral extension to the vomiting re¬
children by using meta-analytic techniques on reported studies. action to milk proteins.5 Three more re¬
Data Sources.\p=m-\Studieswere identified through a literature search of the cent investigations have used this theory
MEDLINE and PsychINFO databases and the authors' files using sugar, sucrose, to study the effects of placing children
on restricted diets.68 These studies have
and attention deficit disorder as the search terms.
found beneficial effects from placing chil¬
Study Selection.\p=m-\Studieswere required to (1) intervene by having the subjects dren on restricted diets compared with
consume a known quantity of sugar; (2) use a placebo (artificial sweetener) con-
placebo diets,6 or they found worsening
dition; (3) blind the subjects, parents, and research staff to the conditions; and (4) behaviors in children on the restricted
report statistics that could be used to compute the dependent measures effect sizes. diets when they are challenged with of¬
Data Extraction.\p=m-\Variablesincluded publication year, study setting, subject fending foods compared with placebo
type and number, gender, age, sugar and placebo type and dose, prior dietary challenges.7·8 However, those dependent
condition, measurement construct, means and SDs for the sugar and placebo con- measures that found significant effects
were more subjective in nature (paren¬
ditions, and direction of effect.
Data Synthesis.\p=m-\Sixteenreports met the inclusion criteria for a total of 23 tal or clinician ratings of behavior). In
within-subject design studies. The weighted mean effect size and related statistics addition, sugars were not singled out
for individual evaluation.
for each of the 14 measurement constructs revealed that although the range for The second theory suggested that
these means was from -0.14 for direct observations and up to +0.30 for academic some children may experience functional
tests, the 95% confidence interval for all 14 mean effect sizes included 0. reactive hypoglycemia similar to that
Conclusion.\p=m-\The meta-analytic synthesis of the studies to date found that seen in adults.9 Adults with this condi¬
sugar does not affect the behavior or cognitive performance of children. The strong tion are reported to experience glucose
belief of parents may be due to expectancy and common association. However, a levels in the hypoglycémie range while
small effect of sugar or effects on subsets of children cannot be ruled out. on diets high in carbohydrates. Consum¬
(JAMA. 1995;274:1617-1621) ing diets high in proteins seems to pre¬
vent the effect. It was theorized that
children would display increased motor
REFINED sugar first became a nutri¬ tension-fatigue syndrome. Sugar later ap¬ activity at low blood glucose levels. This
tional concern in the United States after peared in the 1970s as a major offending theory became prominent after a study
the Civil War. At that time the main con¬ agent when the lay literature provided a of 271 hyperactive children found a large
cern was that sugar had lower nutritive good deal of coverage to the condition number of children who, during glucose
value.1 The idea that food including sugar called "functional reactive hypoglyce- tolerance tests, had patterns of blood
might have an adverse effect on behavior mia."1 The hypothesis in the relationship glucose similar to the pattern seen in
was first raised in 1922 by Shannon.2 This between sugar and behavior has since adults with functional reactive hypogly¬
concept was further elaborated in 1947 become so strong that it has been used in cemia.10 For children, subsequent study
by Randolph3 in his description of the court cases for a self-defense, referred to has shown that the patterns found by
as the "Twinkie defense," and reference Langseth and Dowd10 are actually nor¬
to it appears in cartoons and on television mal variations in children.11
From the Department of Pediatrics, Vanderbilt Uni- shows.4 Separate from any of the underlying
versity Child Development Center (Dr Wolraich), and The hypothesis in the relationship be¬ etiologies, the interest in sugar as a
the Institute for Public Policy Studies (Dr Wilson),
Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tenn; and US Naval tween sugar and hyperactivity is based single offending agent peaked in the mid
Hospital, Yokosuka, Japan (Dr White). on two theories. The first, a possible 1970s because of several correlational
Reprint requests to Department of Pediatrics, allergic response to refined sugar, was studies. One early cross-sectional study
Vanderbilt University, Child Development Center, 2100
Pierce Ave, 426 MCS, Nashville, TN 37232-3573 (Dr conceived of in the first half of this cen¬ suggested an association between sugar
Wolraich). tury as the tension-fatigue syndrome, a intake and hyperactivity.12 It appeared

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a Northern Ontario School of Medicine User on 09/28/2013


that hyperactive children who consumed sugar and placebo doses; and prior di¬ pendence in the data are the multiple
more sugar seemed to display more hy¬ etary condition (ie, none, overnight fast, studies reported by a single researcher
peractive behavior. A similar but smaller sugar restricted prior to challenge, post¬ or research group, regardless of whether
effect was found in a second study.13 Since prandial, or other). The dependent mea¬ the studies were reported in a single
the major limitation of cross-sectional sure variables included measurement article or separately.31 However, because
studies is that causal relationships can¬ construct, number of subjects, means the analyses performed in this meta-
not be determined, it is equally possible and SDs for the sugar and placebo con¬ analysis weight each effect size by the
that the behavior caused the increased ditions, and direction of effect. The mea¬ inverse of its variance, which is primar¬
sucrose intake as that the increased su¬ surement constructs were groupings of ily a function of sample size, a combined
crose intake caused the behavior. There¬ the dependent measures that measured effect size based on separate studies
fore, more definitive examination of the similar behavioral or cognitive charac¬ within a report would receive greater
issue has required research with inter¬ teristics. weight in the analyses than the corre¬
vention methods.
Calculation of Effect Sizes
sponding individual effect sizes. There¬
Most intervention research has en¬ fore, treating each subject sample as
tailed controlled double-blind challenge The unbiased effect size estimator (d) independent has little effect on the over¬
studies. In these studies, the children was applied to the data for each depen¬ all mean effect size estimates. Further¬
receive challenges with foods or drinks dent measure reported in the studies. more, it is conventional in a meta-analy-
containing either sugar or an artificial The statistic d is the mean difference sis to treat effect sizes from different
sweetener. The children, their parents, between the sugar condition and the pla¬ subject samples as independent. To ex¬
and the researchers are unaware of the cebo condition divided by the pooled SD, plore the possibility of a dependency, a
composition of the foods or drinks when adjusted for the small sample bias. sensitivity analysis was performed. In¬
given, and the children's behavior and Hedges and Olkin30 showed that the stan¬ cluding only a single average effect size
cognitive performance are closely as¬ dardized mean difference effect size is per author per analysis produced com¬
sessed within the few hours after the upwardly biased for small samples and parable results to treating the 23 sub¬
ingestion. It would be helpful to deter¬ derived an adjustment function to re¬ ject samples as independent.
mine if the cumulative results of these move this bias. The 23 studies yielded On some measures, a positive sugar-
studies identify any effects of sugar on 532 effect sizes. The largest number of placebo difference reflected that sugar
the behavior or cognitive performance effect sizes from a single study was improved behavior in the sugar condi¬
of children. 7828; the smallest number of effect sizes tion (eg, increased performance on aca¬
was l.13 demic tests) or it reflected worsened
METHODS In meta-analysis it is important to behavior (eg, increased score on an ag¬
maintain statistical independence be¬ gression measure). Therefore, the di¬
Literature Search tween the effect sizes included in an rection of effect was standardized across
Studies were identified through the analysis.31 Effect sizes are considered effect sizes so that a positive sign on the
authors' files and a literature search of dependent or correlated if they are based effect size indicated a desirable effect of
the MEDLINE and PsychlNFO data¬ on the same subject sample. Indepen¬ sugar, and a negative sign indicated an
bases using sugar, sucrose, and atten¬ dence among the effect sizes can be undesirable effect of sugar on behavior.
tion deficit disorder as the search terms. achieved in several ways.32 This study Thus, for the aggression measure, a pos¬
Studies were only included if they met used a less conservative approach that itive effect size indicated a decrease
the following criteria: (1) they intervened makes better use of the data by calcu¬ in aggression (ie, a desirable effect of
by having the subjects consume a known lating a single effect size for each theo¬ sugar).
quantity of sugar; (2) they used a pla¬ retically meaningful measurement con¬
cebo (aspartame or saccharin) condition; struct. Multiple effect sizes from a single Analysis
(3) their design had the subjects, par¬ study within a measurement construct A weighted mean d for each of the 14
ents, and research staff blinded to the were averaged. The analyses were per¬ measurement constructs was calculated,
conditions; and (4) they reported means formed for each measurement construct weighting by the inverse variance of d
and SDs or other statistics that could be separately, thus maintaining statistical to reflect the sampling error inherent in
used to compute an effect size for the independence. The 532 effect sizes were each effect size. Confidence intervals
dependent measures. This search pro¬ categorized into one of 14 measurement (CIs) and the homogeneity statistic, Q,
cess identified 16 articles1429 reporting constructs. Three constructs were sub¬ were computed using the formulas pro¬
23 separate studies. All identified stud¬ jective ratings of observers (parents, vided by Hedges and Olkin.30 The ho¬
ies used a within-subject design. teachers, or experimenters). One was mogeneity analysis tests the hypothesis
direct counting of defined behaviors ob¬ that the sample of effect sizes estimates
Coding served. Eight were measures of skills a single underlying population param¬
The variables coded reflected two lev¬ and activities using standardized mea¬ eter for each measurement construct.
els of analysis: that of the study and the sures (neuropsychological tests, actom- Essentially, this meta-analysis can be
dependent measure. Study level vari¬ eter, continuous performance test, paired viewed as 14 different meta-analyses,
ables included the following: publication associate learning, figure matching, one on each measurement construct.
year; study setting (ie, laboratory, school, drawing figures, motor skills, and aca¬ RESULTS
hospital, other); subject type included in demic tests). Aggression was left as a
sample (ie, normal, attention deficit hy- separate construct because it reflected The 16 studies that met the inclusion
peractivity disorder [ADHD], opposi- a different type of behavior, and mood criteria were conducted over a period of
tional defiant disorder [ODD]/aggres- consisted of self-report measures com¬ 12 years from 1982 to 1994. All of the
sive, adversely responsive to sugar based pleted by the subjects about how they studies used a within-subject design. All
on parent report, ADHD and ODD/ag- felt. This reduced the number of effect studies but one used challenges with
gressive, or other); proportion of males sizes to 91 across the 14 measurement sugar and placebo. The one study29 that
in the study sample; mean age and age constructs for the 23 studies. did not used provision of total diets to
group of sample; sugar and placebo type; An additional potential source of de- the subjects for three 3-week intervals

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a Northern Ontario School of Medicine User on 09/28/2013


Table 1.—Characteristics of the Studies

No. of Age
Source, y Subjects* Subjects Range, y Male, % Preconditions Sugars Placebo
Gross,14 1984 ADHD 50 72 None Sucrose Saccharin
Beharetal,151984 Sugar reactors 21 6-14 100 Overnight fast Sucrose Saccharin
Beharetal,'51984 Sugar reactors 21 100 Overnight fast Glucose Saccharin
Kruesietal,,61987 Sugar reactors 18 2-7 100 None Sucrose Saccharin
Glucose Aspartame
Kruesl et al,16 1987 Normal 2-7 100 None Sucrose Aspartame
Glucose Saccharin
Rosen et al," 1988 Normal 45 5-8 58 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Ferguson et al,'8 1986 Sugar reactors 5-13 None Sucrose Aspartame
Ferguson et al,'8 1986 Normal 18 3-5 50 None Sucrose Aspartame
Manan et al,'31988 Sugar reactors 3-10 80-100 None Sucrose Aspartame
Wolraich et al,20 1985 ADHD 16 7-12 100 Postprandial Sucrose Aspartame
Wolraich et al,20 1985 ADHD 16 8-12 100 Overnight fast Sucrose Aspartame
Milich and Pelham,2' 1986 ADHD 16 6-9 Overnight fast Sucrose Aspartame
Goldman et al,22 1986 Normal 3-7 50 None Sucrose Aspartame
Roshon and Hagen,231989 Normal 12 None Sucrose Aspartame
Bachorowskl et al,2" 1990 Delinquent 14-19 100 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Bachorowski et al,24 1990 Normal 39 14-19 100 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Wender and Solante,25 1991 ADHD/aggressive 5-7 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Wender and Solante,25 1991 Normal 5-7 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Conners,26 1983 Psychiatric ¡npatients 37 6-12 76 None Sucrose Aspartame
Sarvais et al,271990 Normal 20 9-10 50 With breakfast Sucrose Aspartame
Otto et al,28 1982 Prader-WIIII syndrome 10-30 100 With breakfast Sucrose Saccharin
Wolraich et al,29 1994 Sugar reactors 6-10 78 3-wk diet Sucrose Aspartame
Saccharin
Wolraich et al,281994 Normal 25 3-5 48 3-wk diet Sucrose Aspartame
Saccharin

*ADHD indicates attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and ellipses, sex not given in article.

Table 2.—Weighted Mean Effect Size (d) by Measurement Construct and Related Statistics for 23 Within-Subject Sugar Challenge Studies
95% Confidence
Interval Range of d
No. of No. of I
Measurement Construct* Mean d Lower Upper Effect Sizest Subjects^ Lower Upper
Neuropsychological tests 0.04 -0.20 0.27 135 -0.02 0.24 0.50
Teacher ratings 0.03 0.25 161 -0.24
Experimenter ratings 0.05 -0.17 0.27 -0.01 0.08 0.05
Direct observation -0.14 0.05 13 229 -0.62 0.03 5.43
Parent ratings 0.03 -0.20 0.25 -0.09 0.10 0.27
Actometer 0.04 0.27 142 -0.50
Aggression -0.10 -0.38 0.17 105 -0.51 0.11 2.17
Continuous performance tests -0.04 -0.25 169 -1.20
Paired associate learning 0.02 -0.21 0.24 149 -0.19 0.16 0.62
Academic test 0.30 84 -0.24 2177§
Figure matching 0.04 -0.32 0.39 -0.08 0.16 0.27
Drawing figures 0.04 -0.45 0.53 32 0.02 0.06 0.00
Mood -0.02 -0.35 0.32 68 -0.18 0.21 0.85
Motor skills 0.20 -0.03 0.43 158 -0.42 2.43 33.76§
"The direction of the effect size for each construct is such that a positive value reflects a more desirable outcome on the measure and a negative value reflects a less desirable
outcome. Therefore, the negative effect size for aggression indicates slightly more aggression for the sugar condition,
tlf there were multiple effect sizes for a measurement construct within a study, the average of these effect sizes was used.
iThe total number of subjects across studies contributing to the mean d.
§P<.05; distribution heterogeneous.

with diet high in sucrose. In some


one per kilogram of body weight ranging Although the range for these means is
cases the reports used more than one from 1.25 to 5.6 g, while eight used fixed from -0.14 for direct observation to +0.30
subject group so that there were 23 sepa¬ doses from 35 to 78 g. Thirteen used for academic tests, the 95% CI for all 14
rate within-subject design studies. Sev¬ aspartame as the placebo, two used sac¬ mean effect sizes includes 0, as can more
enteen of the studies had a laboratory charin, and six used both saccharin and easily be seen in Figure 1. An examina¬
setting, three took place in a school set¬ aspartame. The types of subjects, the tion ofthe homogeneity statistic, Q, shows
ting, two in a psychiatric inpatient set¬ age distribution, and the preconditions that all but two ofthese distributions are
ting, and one in another setting. All of are presented in Table 1. homogeneous; thus each set ofeffect sizes
the studies used sucrose challenges. In The weighted mean effect size (d) and shows no more variation than expected
addition, three studied glucose and two related statistics for each of the 14 mea¬ from sampling error and their mean can
studied fructose. Most (15) used doses surement constructs are shown in Table 2. be presumed to estimate a single under-

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a Northern Ontario School of Medicine User on 09/28/2013


sumed thus far.38·34 Under the random
effects model, the width of the CIs in¬
creases substantially and the mean d
moves toward 0 (0.13 and 0.09). These
different assumptions lead to the same
conclusion: a null finding of the effect of
sugar on these two measurement con¬
structs.
The homogeneity of the effect size
distributions for each measurement con¬
struct indicates that the observed dif¬
ferences between the effect sizes within
each measurement construct cannot be
attributed to methodological or substan¬
tive differences between the studies. In
other words, the observed differences
in effect size magnitude across studies
does not exceed what would be expected
from chance variation. Unfortunately,
Figure 1.—Weighted mean effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) by measurement construct. given the small number of studies, the
statistical power ofthe homogeneity test
is low. Therefore, several exploratory
analyses of breakouts of the larger ef¬
fect size distributions by substantive
variables, eg, subject type and age group,
were performed. These analyses failed
to identify any important modifiers of
the effect of sugar on these measure¬
ment constructs.
Of interest was the possibility that
sugar might have a larger effect on some
behaviors than others. The general in¬
ability to reject the null hypothesis for
any of the measurement constructs cau¬
tions against any such interpretation. Ad¬
ditionally, because each mean effect size
reported in Table 2 is based on a differ¬
ent subset of the 23 studies, any differ¬
ences in the mean effect sizes may also
reflect differences between these stud¬
ies other than the outcome measured.
A threat to the validity of the above
findings is the possibility that studies
showing stronger effects were somehow
missed by the search process. Concep¬
tually, this threat is the opposite of the
file-drawer problem, which asks how
many studies with null findings would
need to exist in "file drawers" to reduce
the observed mean effect size to a non¬
significant or nonmeaningful level.35,36
Figure 2.—Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) by study for three measurement constructs. Here we need to ask how many studies
with nonnull findings would need to ex¬
lying population effect size. Choosing a skills—both of these effect sizes are ist to raise the observed mean to a sig¬
more liberal of less than .10 does not greater than 2 SDs from the mean). If nificant or meaningful level. One ap¬
change the findings. we assume that these outliers are the proach to this issue is to modify Orwin's
For purposes of illustration, Figure 2 occasional statistically rare observation fail-safe formula35 to derive the num¬
presents the effect size and associated from a common underlying population ber of additional studies of a given mean
95% CIs for each study within three and recalculate the mean d without these effect size (eg, 0.60) needed to raise the
measurement constructs. Shown in this values, the two distributions become ho¬ observed mean to a specified level (eg,
Figure are one homogeneous distribu¬ mogeneous and the mean d is substan¬ 0.40). This formula assumes that the ad¬
tion, direct observation, and the two het¬ tially reduced, becoming negative in one ditional studies have a mean sample size
erogeneous distributions, motor skills case ( -0.05 and 0.01). Taking a different equal to the mean sample size of the
and academic tests. line of reasoning and assuming that the observed studies. Applied to these data,
Examining the two heterogeneous dis¬ outliers were produced by real differ¬ between three additional studies using
tributions more closely shows that both ences between the studies suggest the academic tests and 35 additional studies
include an outlier (an effect size of 1.86 need to apply a random effects model, using direct observation measures are
for academic tests and 2.43 for motor rather than the fixed effects model as- needed with a mean d of 0.60 to raise the

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a Northern Ontario School of Medicine User on 09/28/2013


overall mean value of d to 0.40 (these or cognitive performance of children. considerably higher than those found
would be negative values for the direct There is a high degree of homogeneity for the sugar studies.
observation studies). This represents a and all the studies show effect sizes Parents' beliefs and expectancies can
60% to 200% increase in the number of whose CIs include 0, despite the fact have a significant effect on how they
studies per measurement category. The that there was a great deal of variation perceive behavior and how they inter¬
measurement category of academic tests in the types and ages of children stud¬ act with their children. For example,
was the only category with less than a ied, dose, and preexisting diet condi¬ excited states in children are common
100% increase in the number of studies tions. However, the current number of with birthday parties and holidays where
needed (these "reverse" fail-safe Ns are studies doesn't eliminate the possibility large sugar intake is likely. Since sugar
presented in Table 2). The target popu¬ of a small effect. is commonly ingested and enjoyed by
lation mean d of 0.40 was chosen as a The question remains as to why the children, variation in their behavior may
small but meaningful difference between results of controlled studies differ so be mistakenly correlated with its con¬
the sugar and placebo conditions. The much from the impression of parents. A sumption. In addition, all of the lay pub¬
0.60 value was chosen as the magnitude possible explanation is expectancy. A licity suggesting an association between
of the additional unobserved effect sizes study by Hoover and Milich37 tested a sugar and behavior may prime parents
because it represents a moderate effect group of parents and children where the to expect the adverse behavior. This
size. Furthermore, less than 5% of the parents believed that their children were combination could appear very convinc¬
532 effect sizes computed for this study adversely affected by sugar. They chal¬ ing to parents, thus perpetuating their
had an absolute value greater than 0.60, lenged all the children with a drink beliefs.
indicating that an effect size of 0.60 is sweetened with artificial sweetener (pla¬ Although the current evidence gives
rare in this population of studies. Al¬ cebo), but for half the parents they told no support for the effect of sugar on
though it is conceivable that several mod¬ them the drink was sweetened with behavior, there are not currently enough
erate-to-large effect size studies within sugar. They then asked the parents to studies to reach a definitive conclusion.
a given measurement construct have observe and rate their children's behav¬ With the small number of studies the
been unintentionally omitted, it seems ior, and the researchers observed and power to detect a small effect of sugar
unlikely. recorded the parents' behaviors in in¬ is low. Furthermore, additional studies
teracting with their children. The effect would be needed to test for differential
COMMENT sizes (d) for the parent ratings and be¬ effects of sugar on restricted subsets of
This meta-analysis of the reported havioral coding of the parents ranged children.
studies to date found that sugar (mainly from -0.57 to 1.11 with seven of the 11 We are grateful to Mark Lipsey, PhD, for his ad¬
sucrose) does not affect the behavior measures greater than 0.42, which are vice and feedback with the study and manuscript.

References
1. Deutsch RM. The New Nuts Among the Berries. 1986;86:500-504. normal children. Pediatrics. 1991;88:960-966.
Palo Alto, Calif; Bull Publishing Co; 1977:34, 235-238. 14. Gross M. Effect of sucrose on hyperkinetic chil- 26. Conners CK. The Effects of Sucrose, Fructose,
2. Shannon WR. Neuropathologic manifestations dren. Pediatrics. 1984;74:876-878. and Aspartame on Behavior, Cognitive Performance,
in infants and children as a result of anaphylactic 15. Behar D, Rapoport J, Adams C. Sugar chal- and Brain Function in Hospitalized Children: Fi-
reaction to foods contained in their dietary. AJDC. lenge testing with children considered behaviorally nal Report to the Sugar Association. Washington,
1922;24:89-94. sugar reactive. Nutr Behav. 1984;1:277-288. DC: The Sugar Association; 1983.
3. Randolph TG. Allergy as a causative factor of 16. Kruesi MJ, Rapoport JL, Cummings M, et al. 27. Sarvais S, Schachar R, Zlotkin S, et al. Aspartame:
fatigue, irritability and behavior problems in chil- Effects of sugar and aspartame on aggression and effects on learning, behavior and mood. Pediatrics.
dren. J Pediatr. 1947;31:560-572. activity in children. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144: 1990;86:75-83.
4. Wurtman JJ. Carbohydrate craving, mood 1487-1490. 28. Otto PL, Sulzbacher SI, Worthington-Roberts
changes, and obesity. J Clin Psychiatry. 1988;49: 17. Rosen LE, Bender ME, Sorrell S, et al. Effects BS. Sucrose-induced behavior changes of persons
37-39. of sugar (sucrose) on children's behavior. J Consult with Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J Ment Defic.
5. Speer F. The allergic tension-fatigue syndrome. Clin Psychol. 1988;56:583-589. 1982;86:335-341.
Pediatr Clin North Am. 1954;1:1029-1037. 18. Ferguson HB, Stoddart C, Simeon J. Double 29. Wolraich ML, Lindgren S, Stumbo P, et al.
6. Egger J, Graham PJ, Carter CM, et al. Con- blind challenge studies of behavioral and cognitive Effects of diets high in sucrose or aspartame on the
trolled trial of oligoantigenic treatment in the hy- effects of sucrose-aspartame ingestion in normal behavioral and cognitive performance of children.
perkinetic syndrome. Lancet. 1985;1:540-545. children. Nutr Rev. 1986;44:144-150. N Engl J Med. 1994;330:301-307.
7. Kaplan BJ, McNicol RD, Conte RA, et al. Di- 19. Mahan LK, Chase M, Furukawa C, et al. Sugar 30. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for
etary replacement in preschool aged hyperactive 'allergy' and children's behavior. Ann Allergy. 1988; Meta-analysis. Orlando, Fla: Academic Press; 1985.
boys. Pediatrics. 1989;83:7-17. 61:453-458. 31. Hedges LV. Directions for future methodol-
8. Boris M, Mandel FS. Foods and additives are 20. Wolraich ML, Milich R, Stumbo P, et al. Effects ogy. In: Wachter KW, Straf, ML, eds. The Future
common causes of the attention deficit hyperactive of sucrose ingestion on the behavior of hyperactive of Meta-analysis. New York, NY: Russell Sage
disorder in children. Ann Allergy. 1994;72:462-468. boys. J Pediatr. 1985;106:675-682. Foundation; 1990.
9. Cott A. Treatment of learning disabilities. In: 21. Milich R, Pelham WE. Effects of sugar inges- 32. Durlak JA, Lipsey MW. A practitioner's guide
William RJ, Kalita DK, eds. A Physician's Hand- tion on the classroom playgroup behavior of atten- to meta-analysis. Am J Community Psychol. 1991;
book on Orthomolecular Medicine. New York, NY: tion deficit disordered boys. J Consult Clin Psy- 19:291-332.
Pergamon Press; 1977:90-91. chol. 1986;54:714-718. 33. Hedges LV. A random effect model for effect
10. Langseth L, Dowd J. Glucose tolerance and 22. Goldman JA, Lerman RH, Contois JH, et al. sizes. Psychol Bull. 1983;93:388-395.
hyperkinesis. Fed J Cosmetic Toxicol. 1978;16:120\x=req-\ Behavioral effects of sucrose on preschool children. 34. Shadish WR, Haddock CK. Combining estimates
133. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1986;14:565-577. of effect size. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, eds. The
11. Josefsberg Z, Vilunski E, Haukugh A, et al. 23. Roshon MS, Hagen RL. Sugar consumption, Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York, NY:
Glucose and insulin responses to an oral glucose locomotion, task, orientation, and learning in pre- Russell Sage Foundation; 1994.
load in normal children and adolescents in Israel. school children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1989;17: 35. Orwin RG. A fail-safe N for effect size in meta\x=req-\
Isr J Med Sci. 1976;12:189-194. 349-357. analysis. J Educ Stat. 1983;8:157-159.
12. Prinz RJ, Roberts WA, Hantman E. Dietary 24. Bachorowski J, Newman J, Nichols SL, et al. 36. Rosenthal R. The 'file drawer problem' and
correlates of hyperactive behavior in children. Sucrose and delinquency: behavioral assessment. tolerance for null results. Psychol Bull. 1979;85:
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1980;48:760-769. Pediatrics. 1990;86:244-253. 185-193.
13. Wolraich ML, Stumbo P, Milich R, et al. Di- 25. Wender E, Solanto M. Effects of sugar on ag- 37. Hoover DW, Milich R. Effects of sugar inges-
etary characteristics of hyperactive and control boys gressive and inattentive behavior in children with tion expectancies on mother-child interactions.
and their behavioral correlates. J Am Diet Assoc. attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity and J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1994;22:501-515.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a Northern Ontario School of Medicine User on 09/28/2013

Вам также может понравиться