Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Differentiating the contributions of emotional intelligence and resilience on T


adolescent male scholastic performance
Kat Dropperta, Luke Downeya, Justine Lomasa, Emily R. Bunnetta, Nathan Simmonsb,

Andrew Wheatonb, Chris Nieldb, Con Stougha,
a
Emotional Intelligence Research Unit, Swinburne University, Australia
b
Anglican Church Grammar School, Brisbane, Australia

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Recent research is indicating predictive validity for emotional intelligence (EI) in a range of important scholastic
Emotional intelligence variables including bullying, well-being and mental health. There have been some suggestions that emotional
Resiliency intelligence and resiliency in school children are also related, however, there is little research on these variables.
Scholastic performance Therefore, the present study attempted to bridge this gap by examining the relationship between EI, resiliency
and scholastic performance. Specifically, the study examined whether resiliency mediated the relationship be-
tween EI and scholastic performance. One hundred and fifty-three year-10 males completed the Adolescent
Swinburne Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT-A) and the Resiliency Scale for Children and Adolescents (RSCA).
In addition, current academic achievement was collected for all school subjects. Regression Analyses revealed
that several dimensions of the SUEIT-A and the RSCA predicted individual subject grades. Reactivity, a subscale
of Resiliency, mediated the relationship between the EI subscales, Understand Emotions of Others and Emotions
Direct Cognition, and two measures of scholastic performance (i.e., GPA and Science grades). Given these results,
future research should examine whether improving emotional intelligence and resiliency leads to improvements
in scholastic performance.

1. Introduction (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002).


Inconsistent research findings have been reported on whether re-
Research has sought to understand the underlying psychological siliency predicts scholastic performance (Ayyash-Abdo, Sanchez-Ruiz,
mechanisms that contribute to scholastic performance, as scholastic & Barbari, 2016; Mavroveli & Sánchez-Ruiz, 2011). Additionally, there
success has been shown to predict a number of important life outcomes have only been a few studies examining the predictive validity of in-
including, higher employment rates (Hunter & Schmidt, 1996), en- dividual components of EI on subject grades (e.g., Downey et al., 2008).
gagement in further tertiary studies (Lizzio, Wilson, & Simons, 2002),
greater mental health (Shiner, Masten, & Roberts, 2003), and life suc- 2. Emotional Intelligence (EI)
cess (Finn, Fish, & Scott, 2008). Scholastic performance has been linked
to factors such as supportive networks, stress management, achieve- EI has been modeled as either a distinct group of mental abilities,
ment motivation (McMillan & Reed, 1994) and mood management that is, as a specific intelligence (Ability EI) or as a combination of
(Ciarrochi, Chan, & Bajgar, 2001). These factors are conceptually re- positive traits (trait EI), such as, self-esteem, optimism and hapiness
lated to resiliency, which characterizes an individual's ability to adapt (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2008; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). The
and recover quickly from challenging circumstances, changes in cir- current study employs a self-report scale based on the four branch
cumstances or misfortune (Connor & Davidson, 2003; Pulley, 2001) and Ability EI model originally posited by Salovey and Mayer (1990), the
emotional intelligence (EI; Billings, Downey, Lomas, Lloyd, & Stough, Adolescent Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT-A;
2014; Downey, Mountstephen, Lloyd, Hansen, & Stough, 2008; Luebbers, Downey, & Stough, 2007). The scale used has been shown
Downey, Lomas, Billings, Hansen, & Stough, 2014; Gumora & Arsenio, repeatedly to provide reliable and valid measurements in adolescents,
2002). EI has been defined as a set of abilities regarding identification, predicting several key scholastic outcomes in adolescents previously
discrimination, regulation, management, control and use of emotions (such as scholastic success, coping and bullying behaviours (Downey


Corresponding author at: Swinburne University, PO Box 218 Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia.
E-mail address: cstough@swin.edu.au (C. Stough).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.03.023
Received 22 November 2018; Received in revised form 13 March 2019; Accepted 18 March 2019
0191-8869/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

et al., 2008; Downey et al., 2014; Downey, Johnston, Hansen, Birney, & From an interactionalist framework, Resiliency is considered to be
Stough, 2010; Lomas, Stough, Hansen, & Downey, 2012; Luebbers complex and reflects the interplay between an individual and their
et al., 2007). This model conceptualizes EI as a complete, integrated set environment (Prince-Embury, Saklofske, & Vesely, 2015). It has been
of abilities that develop hierarchically (Mayer, Roberts, & Barsade, posited that utilising internal and external protective factors relating to
2008). Resiliency (Miller & Daniel, 2007) can result in a more successful out-
Adolescents with higher EI report greater awareness and manage- come under high-risk conditions (Egeland, Carlson, & Sroufe, 1993).
ment of their own emotions (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2004, 2008), External protective factors, such as, social support, can reduce the im-
mood management skills, and satisfaction with social support pact of negative effects of the individual's misfortune (Miller & Daniel,
(Ciarrochi et al., 2001). They also experience lower rates of bullying 2007), through decreasing the perceived salience of the stressor and
(Lomas et al., 2012) and depression (Mavroveli, Petrides, Rieffe, & increasing one's perceived ability to cope with the situation (Pressman
Bakker, 2007). Several models of EI have effectively predicted scho- & Cohen, 2005). Internal factors including positive self-esteem (Miller &
lastic performance in youth (Billings et al., 2014; Petrides, Daniel, 2007), strong motivation to achieve, belief in an internal locus
Frederickson, & Furnham, 2004). For instance, Parker et al. (2004) of control (Rouse, 2001), optimism (Pressman & Cohen, 2005), and
found that students who obtained better grades also reported higher EI. stress tolerance (Hartley, 2011) have also been associated with lower
In particular, EI dimensions that measure managing and controlling perceived stress and greater ability to cope. Consequently, the devel-
emotions have been shown to make a unique and significant con- opment of these skills is seen as an integral component to improving
tribution to scholastic performance, over and above the influence of resiliency.
cognitive contributors (Gumora & Arsenio, 2002). There is also little A distinction between resiliency, resilience and resilient outcomes is
agreement in the literature about the influence of EI on specific aca- required to ensure that these terms are not used interchangeably in the
demic subjects, however, this may reflect differences in course content literature which have often been the case (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker,
across schools and countries. Australian students with higher scores on 2000). Resilience refers to the interplay between a person and their
the emotional management and control scale (EMC) produced higher environment that results in a resilient outcome, whereas, resiliency
Mathematics and Science grades (Downey et al., 2008). The authors refers to a set of personal qualities that influence their ability to ex-
suggested that this may be caused by the ability of students higher in perience a resilient outcome. Based on developmental theory by Masten
EMC to manage anxiety and other negative emotions which may disrupt (2001), the Resiliency Scales for Children and Adolescents.
working memory- a key process involved in performing mathematical (RSCA) was developed by integrating empirically relevant traits to
skills. In contrast, Petrides et al. (2004) found that EI predicted English measure the core constructs underlying resiliency in adolescents and
but not Mathematics or Science grades in British students. Several children (Prince-Embury & Courville, 2008). The scale encompasses the
theoretical models may be proposed in attempt to explain the re- three related global concepts of adolescent resiliency, which collec-
lationship between EI and scholastic performance. For instance, support tively comprise 10 more specific facets; Sense of Mastery (optimism,
networks, which are enhanced through superior EI, can positively in- self-efficacy and ability), Sense of Relatedness (comfort, support, trust
fluence scholastic performance (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Gumora & and tolerance) and Reactivity (sensitivity, recovery and impairment).
Arsenio, 2002). Additionally, dimensions of EI involving emotional self- High resiliency is characterised by high Mastery and Relatedness, and
awareness and emotional management can reduce the negative impact low Reactivity.
of stress on cognition (Pekrun, Elliot, & Maier, 2009). As scholastic A moderate correlation between EI and resiliency has been pre-
performance and learning can directly and indirectly be negatively in- viously observed (Connor & Slear, 2009). It has been theorised that
fluenced by stress or anxiety (Boekaerts, 1993) yet be enhanced by individuals with greater emotional self-awareness and emotional
positive emotions (Fredrickson, 1998) and relationships (Gumora & management are able to adapt to emotional demands of stressful en-
Arsenio, 2002), an understanding of how EI abilities can directly and counters and buffer the effects of aversive events (Salovey, Bedell,
indirectly reduce stress is important. Detweiler, & Mayer, 1999). Previous assessment of resiliency or theo-
In addition, there are a range of other plausible mechanisms relating retically related constructs to resiliency have suggested some overlap
EI and scholastic performance. Positive engagement with peers and between the abilities encompassed by EI and resiliency exist (Salovey,
teachers offers an explanation of how EI and scholastic performance Stroud, Woolery, & Epel, 2002). Ciarrochi et al. (2001) observed that
may be related (Gumora & Arsenio, 2002). Students with poor ability to participants with higher EI were better able to induce positive moods in
recognise and express their own emotions, are more likely to feel themselves, manage their moods more effectively, and were happier in
withdrawn and excluded, increasing the likelihood of poor school both positive and negative circumstances. Additionally, emotional
performance resulting in classroom disengagement (Izard, 1989; management was associated with more frequent use of adaptive beha-
Petrides et al., 2004). Recognising and managing one's own and others' viours such as problem–solving and self-pep talks (Salovey et al., 2002).
emotions, have been shown to improve educational support networks These skills may be necessary for developing concepts relating to re-
(Gumora & Arsenio). Furthermore, it has been suggested that analytical siliency including optimism, self-efficacy and adaptability. Those who
skills are more important than intuition on performance in exams. can manage their moods have greater access to social support
Benjamin, Cavell, and Shallenberger (1984), found students who com- (Ciarrochi et al., 2001), and are more willing to seek help when feeling
pleted a multiple choice answer on an exam that used logic, rather than overwhelmed (Ciarrochi & Deane, 2001) which may improve related-
‘trusting their gut’, performed better academically. The extent to which ness. Studies have also observed that higher resiliency and higher
logic or emotions are used to problem-solve is measured on many academic test scores are positively related (Ayyash-Abdo et al., 2016;
measures of EI. It has been suggested that decision making based on Kotzé & Kleynhans, 2013; Putwain, Nicholson, Connors, & Woods,
more factual information rather than gut-feelings or emotions may lead 2013). Resiliency has been found to predict scholastic performance
to better performance in mathematics and some science-based subjects. even when controlling for gender, age and hope (Ayyash-Abdo et al.,
These results may explain why Science grades are associated with in- 2016). The negative influence of Reactivity on cognitive capacity may
dividual differences in EI (Downey et al., 2008). offer a theoretical explanation as to why resiliency and scholastic per-
formance could be related. Hyper-Reactivity to environmental stimuli
3. Resiliency have been associated with poorer scholastic performance (Krupic &
Corr, 2014), higher evaluative pressure, greater cognitive impairment,
Resiliency refers to a person's ability to adapt and recover quickly and lower GPA (Putwain et al., 2013). Scholastic performance may be
from change, hardship or misfortune (Pulley, 2001) and to confront negatively impacted if attention is diverted away from the educational
significant adversity in life (Campbell-Sills, Cohan, & Stein, 2006). materials or the task (Pekrun et al., 2009). High reactivity is considered

76
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

a risk factor for poor scholastic performance as intense emotional re- 4.2. Procedure
actions have been shown to impair cognitive functioning (Putwain
et al., 2013) and supportive relationships influential to better scholastic All parents and participants provided informed consent prior to
performance (Valiente, Swanson, & Eisenberg, 2012). participation and the study was approved by the institutional ethics
Given the theoretical overlap between resiliency and emotional committee. Participants completed the questionnaire during class.
intelligence, it is somewhat surprising that there is no research directly Students' overall GPA for the academic year was obtained.
comparing the relationship between EI, resiliency and scholastic per-
formance. Several studies have measured EI and attempted to connect
4.3. Measures
individual differences in EI with processes related to resiliency. For
instance, Petrides et al. (2004) found that students with poorer emo-
4.3.1. Emotional intelligence and resiliency
tional skills were more likely to feel excluded and not connected to
The 57-item SUEIT-A (Luebbers et al., 2007) was employed to assess
others (similar to the construct of relatedness measured by resiliency
four EI subscales.
scales such as the RSCA), which increased the likelihood of poorer
The 64-item, self-report RSCA was used to measure the three un-
scholastic performance. Superior emotional management is an im-
derlying systems of Resiliency to gain a greater understanding of spe-
portant predictor of scholastic and social engagement, even in the
cific strengths and vulnerabilities the participant faces in the afore-
presence of constructs relating to reactivity. Valiente et al. (2012) found
mentioned three global and ten specific scales. The RSCA displays
that students with good emotional control were more motivated to
strong internal reliability, convergent and concurrent validity (see
perform, engage in learning activities, and were able to develop strong
Prince-Embury et al., 2015 for more details).
relationships with others, even when experiencing moderate levels of
negative emotionality. Both of these studies attempt to at least a the-
oretical level, connect EI, resiliency (relatedness) and scholastic per- 4.3.2. Grade Point Average (GPA)
formance. Unfortunately, there has not been a consistent approach to mea-
The present study will examine the relationship between EI, re- suring AP in previous research. This makes comparisons between stu-
siliency and scholastic performance in an adolescent population. We dies difficult. AP has been quantified using a wide range of measures
aim to provide further clarity as to how EI and resiliency subscales are including: Grade Point Averages (GPA; Ayyash-Abdo et al., 2016);
correlated using two tests with established reliability, predictive and scores on American College entrance exams such as the Stanford
construct validity (Luebbers et al., 2007; Prince-Embury et al., 2015). It Achievement Test (SAT; Peters, Kranzler & Rossen, 2009); final sec-
is hypothesised that: 1) higher EI scores will be associated with higher ondary education scores such as the General Certificate of Secondary
GPA and subject grades; 2) higher Mastery and Relatedness scores will Education (GSCE; Petrides et al., 2004); and individual subject grades.
predict better GPA and subject grades and lower Reactivity will be To add further confusion, some individual subject grades reflect student
associated with high GPA and subject grades; 3) EI woll predict re- performance in final year exams (Petrides et al., 2004) whilst other
siliency, with higher EI subscale scores correlating positively with subject grades are calculated over several assessment types and periods
Mastery and Relatedness scores and negatively with Reactivity scores; of time (Kotzé & Kleynhans, 2013). Therefore, there is no gold standard
and 4) resiliency will partially mediate the relationship between EI and to use when assessing the relationship between AP and other variables,
scholastic performance (partial mediation is hypothesised given that EI which makes comparison between research difficult. In this study GPA
and resiliency are expected to share some but not all of the variance was measured as the average of four individual subjects that were
with scholastic performance). available at the time of this study (Science, English, Religious Education
and History). We also included performance in each subject to examine
whether some subjects were differentially related to the main psycho-
4. Methods logical variables.

4.1. Participants
4.4. Statistical analysis
The sample consisted of 153 Year 10 students from the Anglican
A mediation analysis was used to investigate whether EI enhanced
Church Grammar School which is an Australian boys' school located in
scholastic performance accounting for resiliency. Fig. 1 depicts the
Brisbane, Australia. Ages ranged from 14 to 16 years (M = 14.64,
mediation model that was tested.
SD = 0.56). All participants provided written informed consent. Full
The Bootstrapping Method, developed by Preacher and Hayes
written information about the study was sent home for each parent in
(2008), was used in this study to measure the mediation effects of re-
advance of their child's participation. All parents were afforded the
siliency on EI and scholastic performance. This model tests the sig-
opportunity to exclude their child from participation. The study was
nificance of the indirect effect (mediator) on the dependent and in-
approved by the institutional human research ethics committee.
dependent variables. If zero does not appear between the intervals, it
can be understood that mediation has occurred.

Resiliency
‘a’path ‘b’ path

Emotional
Intelligence GPA
C’ Path (C Path)
Direct Effect (Total Effect)
Indirect Effect, 95% CI

Fig. 1. Proposed mediation model.

77
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

Table 1 scholastic performance measure, this hypothesis was only partially


Means, standard deviations and reliability statistics of variables. supported. Similarly, EMC, UOE and ERE were observed to significantly
Variable M SD α predict variance in all global subscales of resiliency. Nevertheless, EDC
predicted variance in Reactivity only. Hence, hypothesis three was
Emotional Management & Control 61.97 10.56 0.88 partially supported.
Emotions Direct Cognition 28.27 5.15 0.77
Understanding Emotions of Others 75.26 9.19 0.88
Emotional Recognition & Expression 34.60 5.34 0.72 5.3. Resiliency as a mediator
Mastery 79.66 10.60 0.92
Reactivity 40.28 11.07 0.92
Reactivity partially or fully significantly mediated the relationship
Relatedness 80.14 10.94 0.95
GPA 10.90 1.57
between four models: UEO and GPA, UEO and Science, EDC and GPA,
English 10.39 1.60 EDC and Science. Consequently, the hypothesis that resiliency would
History 11.06 1.80 mediate the relationship between EI and scholastic performance was
Religious Education 11.22 1.74 partially supported because only reactivity mediated this relationship.
Science 10.58 1.91
As demonstrated in Fig. 2, UEO significantly predicted Reactivity and
N = 153. Reactivity in turn significantly predicted GPA and Science grades. Re-
sults indicated UEO was no longer a significant predictor of GPA or
5. Results Science after controlling for Reactivity, consistent with full mediation.
Approximately 6.8% of the variation in GPA, and 6.4% of variation in
5.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations Science was accounted for by the predictors UEO and Reactivity. The
indirect coefficient was significant in both analyses. UEO was asso-
The SUEIT-A mean scores appear slightly higher than those pre- ciated with approximately 0.012 points higher GPA scores and 0.017
viously published, however, this may be expected, given the greater points higher Science scores as mediated by Reactivity. EDC was a
mean age of the current study (Downey et al., 2010; Lomas et al., significant predictor of Reactivity, and Reactivity was a significant
2012). The mean scores for the RSCA were in the normal range (Prince- predictor of GPA and Science (see Fig. 2). EDC was no longer a sig-
Embury, 2006). Reliability was considered acceptable as all Cronbach's nificant predictor of GPA after controlling for Reactivity, consistent
alphas were above 0.7 (Tabacknick & Fidell, 2007). The relevant with full mediation. In comparison, EDC was still a significant predictor
means, standard deviations, and internal reliabilities are presented in of Science when controlling for Reactivity, indicating partial mediation
Table 1. occurred. Approximately 7.1% of the variation in GPA, and 8.7% of
variation in Science was accounted for by EDC and Reactivity. EDC was
associated with 0.012 points lower GPA scores and 0.015 points lower
5.2. Correlations and regression analyses Science scores as mediated by Reactivity.

Inter-correlations between resiliency, EI and academic grades are 6. Discussion


displayed in Table 2. As all three variables are required to be correlated
for mediation to occur, Emotional Recognition and Expression (ERE) The current study examined the inter-relationships between EI, re-
was not included in further analysis as it was not correlated with any siliency, and scholastic performance in adolescent males. EI sig-
academic measures. Grade Point Average (GPA) was only correlated nificantly predicted some subscales of resiliency (measured by the
with Emotional Management and Control (EMC), Understanding Emo- RSCA). These results support previous research evidencing a relation-
tions of Others (UOE) and Reactivity. A series of independent standard ship between EI and resiliency (Armstrong, Galligan, & Critchley, 2011;
multiple regression analyses were subsequently run to test hypotheses Connor & Slear, 2009; Magnano, Craparo, & Paolillo, 2016). Armstrong
one to three. et al., (2011) found that participants with higher EMC and emotional
GPA was only significantly correlated with EDC, UEO and EMC expression displayed higher resiliency. However, the ability to manage
(Table 3). The direction of the effect was positive suggesting that an other's emotions did not predict resiliency. ERE, EMC, UEO predicted
increase in levels of Emotions Direct Cognition (EDC), UEO and EMC Reactivity. However, EMC was observed to predict between two and ten
predicted higher GPAs. As three of the four EI subscales predicted some times the amount of variance in Reactivity scores than did the other EI
variance in GPA, hypothesis one was partially supported. Reactivity subscales. This finding may be expected given that EMC encompasses
predicted variance in all subjects and GPA. Mastery predicted variance the ability to manage and control intense emotions and hence reduce
in Science. However, as Relatedness did not predict performance in any the frequency and duration of strong, cognitively impairing emotional

Table 2
Pearson correlations.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. ERE 1
2. EDC 0.11 1
3. UEO 0.58⁎⁎ −0.07 1
4. EMC 0.49⁎⁎ −0.15 0.60⁎⁎ 1
5. Mastery 0.47⁎⁎ −0.12 0.49⁎⁎ 0.64⁎⁎ 1
6. Reactivity −0.33⁎⁎ 0.19⁎ −0.40⁎⁎ −0.65⁎⁎ −0.62⁎⁎ 1
7. Relatedness 0.52⁎⁎ −0.05 0.45⁎⁎ 0.51⁎⁎ 0.81⁎⁎ −0.47⁎⁎ 1
8. General GPA 0.06 −0.17⁎ 0.20⁎ 0.29⁎⁎ 0.15 −0.23⁎⁎ 0.05
9. English 0.12 −0.12 0.26⁎⁎ 0.23⁎⁎ 0.15 −0.26⁎⁎ 0.08
10. History 0.01 −0.07 0.16⁎ 0.24⁎⁎ 0.12 −0.20⁎ 0.03
11. RE 0.02 −0.03 0.13 0.25⁎⁎ 0.04 −0.17⁎ 00
12. Science 0.04 −0.21⁎⁎ 0.17⁎ 0.26⁎⁎ 0.17⁎ −0.24⁎⁎ 0.03


p < .05.
⁎⁎
p < .01.

78
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

Table 3
Variance in scholastic performance and resiliency explained by EI (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001).
ERE EDC UEO EMC Mastery Reactivity Relatedness

GPA * 2.9% 4.1% 8.4% * 5.5% *


English * * 6.9% 5.3% * 6.6% *
History * * 2.7% 5.9% * 3.8% *
RE * * * 6.5% * 2.7% *
Science * 4.6% 3.0% 6.9% 2.9% 5.8% *
Mastery 22.3% * 24.4% 39.5% – – –
Reactivity 10.9% 3.7% 16.3% 42.5% – – –
Relatedness 26.8% * 19.9% 26.5% – – –

N = 153, * = no significant variance explained, − = no comparison made.

Significant Mediation
Reactivity
Reactivity
-.03* .-.49*** -.04*
-.49***

UOE GPA UEO Science

.02 (.03)* .02 (.04*)


Indirect effect, b =. 01 95% CI (.01, .03) Indirect effect, b =.02 95% CI (.01, .03)

Reactivity Reactivity

.41* -.03* .41* -.04*

EDC GPA EDC Science

-.04 (-.05*) -.07* (-.08**)


Indirect effect, b =-.01 95% CI (-.03, -.04) Indirect effect, b =-.01 95% CI (-.03, -.005)

Fig. 2. Mediation models of the effects of resiliency on EI and scholastic performance.

responses, as those depicted by the Reactivity scale. Additionally, et al., 2014).


adolescents who scored higher on EDC were more likely to have long Reactivity was negatively associated with poorer scholastic perfor-
and intense emotional responses. The relationship between ERE and mance across all domains and Mastery was associated with higher
Reactivity supports previous research (Salovey et al., 1999). Science grades, suggesting that adolescents who are more easily upset,
Relatedness and Mastery were significantly predicted by ERE, UEO took longer to recover from being upset and who felt that they made
and EMC. This suggests emotional awareness and management can more mistakes when upset, performed worse academically. Conversely,
increase feeling supported by, trusting of, comfortable with and ac- adolescents who were more optimistic, willing to learn from mistakes
cepting of others, as well as feelings of self-efficacy, optimism and and have greater belief in their ability to problem-solve, received higher
adaptability. This finding supports previous research that has demon- Science grades. Reactivity was the most consistent Resiliency predictor
strated that UEO and EMC are associated with supportive relationships of academic success in the current study. One reason may be that stu-
with peers, teachers and family (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Gumora & dents who experience intense and enduring emotional states more
Arsenio, 2002). Furthermore, previous research has demonstrated that frequently perform worse academically as their attention and cognitive
greater ERE is related to feeling socially connected (Izard, 1989). Fur- resources are diverted away from learning and problem-solving pro-
thermore, EDC, UEO and EMC were related to GPA scores. Performance cesses (Pekrun et al., 2009). Alternatively, students who are less re-
across English, History, RE and Science were also predicted by specific active may receive greater support (Gumora & Arsenio, 2002). Mastery
EI subscales. These findings replicate those of Downey et al. (2008) and only predicted performance in Science grades, despite previous re-
Downey et al. (2014). Although the exact reasons why EI and scholastic search having observed a significant relationship between Mastery and
performance are related are not fully known, there are a number of other academic domains (e.g., Ayyash-Abdo et al., 2016; Pekrun et al.,
plausible theoretical mechanisms. For instance, some authors have 2009; Phan, 2010). These results may reflect the use of varied opera-
suggested that EI contributes to the development and maintenance of tional definitions of Mastery.
support networks (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Gumora & Arsenio, 2002), Despite numerous studies discussing how perception of support
which in turn reduces the cognitively impairing effects of stress on from others can act as a buffer against stress (Ciarrochi & Deane, 2001;
scholastic performance (Boekaerts, 1993; Pressman & Cohen, 2005). McDowell & Serovich, 2007; Pressman & Cohen, 2005) and improve
Subjects, such as Science, which demand greater working memory ca- scholastic performance (Gumora & Arsenio, 2002), Relatedness was not
pacity and concentration (Owens, Stevenson & Hadwin, 2012), can be correlated with any subject grades. However, our results support the
influenced by emotions that enhance or detract from problem-solving research by Ayyash-Abdo et al., (2016) who did not observe a re-
capabilities (Valiente et al., 2012). Conversely, adolescents who more lationship between Relatedness and GPA.
frequently used emotions to guide decision-making, received lower
GPAs and Science grades. This supports the abovementioned findings of 6.1. Resiliency as a mediator
Benjamin et al. (1984). However, it is inconsistent with previous studies
that did not observe a relationship between EDC and scholastic per- Reactivity mediated the relationship between some EI subscales and
formance in year seven to ten students (Downey et al., 2008; Downey performance on some subjects, suggesting that sensitivity for, recovery

79
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

from and impairment by emotional reactivity, is an underlying me- Importantly, developing EI skills and resiliency could be incorporated
chanism between some EI components and performance in Science and into individual or group intervention programs, given the current
GPA. Overall, students with a greater ability to manage and control findings and those of previous research that evidence advantages of
their own and others emotions and use logic rather than emotions to having advanced skills in these areas. For example, no one factor solely
problem-solve, are less likely to be reactive and in turn, perform better influences performance in scholastic environments, and the complex
overall (GPA) and specifically in Science subjects. interaction between internal and external factors needs to be con-
The relations between UEO and GPA, and UEO and Science were sidered. As academic achievement has been associated with a range of
fully mediated by Reactivity in the current study, indicating that ado- short and long-term life outcomes (Finn et al., 2008; Hunter & Schmidt,
lescent males who report a greater ability to understand other's emo- 1996; Lizzio et al., 2002), the need for developing efficacious academic
tions, are less likely to experience or be impaired by intense emotional intervention strategies that incorporate a range of relevant internal and
reactions, which may lead to greater performance in Science and GPA. external factors is important.
Furthermore, the relationship between understanding the emotions of
others and higher performance in Science and overall grades, is ex- Acknowledgements
plained by Reactivity. This could reflect previous studies that showed
that those who have a greater ability to understand other's emotions This work was funded by the Anglican Church Grammar School,
receive greater emotional or academic support from friends, family, and Brisbane, Australia.
teachers (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Gumora & Arsenio, 2002) which in turn
improves scholastic performance. The quality of these relationships References
may be improved by a reduced tendency to respond to a situation in a
heightened state (Valiente et al., 2012). Reactivity only predicted a Armstrong, A., Galligan, R., & Critchley, C. R. (2011). Emotional intelligence and psy-
small amount of variation in the current study which may suggest that chological resilience to negative life events. Personality and Individual Differences,
51(3), 331–336.
the relationship is significantly influenced by other mediating factors, Ayyash-Abdo, H., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., & Barbari, M. L. (2016). Resiliency predicts aca-
such as support received from others, that are related to but also dis- demic performance of Lebanese adolescents over demographic variables and hope.
tinct from Reactivity. Learning and Individual Differences, 48, 9–16.
Benjamin, L. T., Cavell, T. A., & Shallenberger, W. R. (1984). Staying with initial answers
The relationships between EDC and GPA and Science, were fully and on objective tests: Is it a myth? Teaching of Psychology, 11, 133–141.
partially mediated by Reactivity, respectively. Perhaps adolescent Billings, C. E. W., Downey, L. A., Lomas, J. E., Lloyd, J., & Stough, C. (2014). Emotional
males who more frequently use emotional knowledge or trust their intelligence and scholastic achievement in pre- adolescent children. Personality and
Individual Differences, 65, 14–18.
emotional instincts to problem-solve, are more influenced by their
Boekaerts, M. (1993). Being concerned with well-being and with learning. Educational
emotions and are more susceptible to intense emotional reactions Psychologist, 28(2), 149–167.
leading to poorer scholastic performance (Pekrun et al., 2009). Re- Campbell-Sills, L., Cohan, S. L., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Relationship of resilience to per-
sonality, coping, and psychiatric symptoms in young adults. Behaviour Research and
activity did not significantly mediate the relationship between EMC and
Therapy, 44(4), 585–599.
scholastic performance, suggesting that regardless of the tendency to Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A. Y. C., & Bajgar, J. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence in
experience and be impacted by intense emotional responses, higher adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(7), 1105–1119.
scholastic performance is a consequence of higher EMC. These results Ciarrochi, J. V., & Deane, F. P. (2001). Emotional competence and willingness to seek
help from professional and nonprofessional sources. British Journal of Guidance and
support the observation by Valiente et al. (2012) that an ability to Counselling, 29(2), 232–246.
control emotions positively influences engagement in academic activ- Connor, K. M., & Davidson, J. R. T. (2003). Development of a new resilience scale: The
ities, even in the presence of negative emotional responses. As EMC Connor‐Davidson Resilience Scale (CD‐RISC). Depression & Anxiety, 18, 76–82.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.10113.
explains over 40% of observed variance in Reactivity, there is some Connor, B., & Slear, S. (2009). Emotional intelligence and anxiety; emotional intelligence
evidence to suggest that the constructs are measuring similar concepts. and resiliency. International Journal of Learning, 16(1), 249–260.
Downey, L., Johnston, P., Hansen, K., Birney, J., & Stough, C. (2010). Investigating the
mediating effects of emotional intelligence and coping on problem behaviours in
6.2. Limitations adolescents. Australian Journal of Psychology, 62(1), 20–29.
Downey, L., Mountstephen, J., Lloyd, J., Hansen, K., & Stough, C. (2008). Emotional
Clearly the results of the current study are based on only adolescent intelligence and scholastic achievement in Australian adolescents. Australian Journal
of Psychology, 60(1), 10–17.
males. Whilst males comprise an important cohort of school aged Downey, L. A., Lomas, J., Billings, C., Hansen, K., & Stough, C. (2014). Scholastic success.
children they do have their own issues which may be difficult to gen- Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 29(1), 40–53.
eralize to female adolescents. It is currently unknown if self-reported EI Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resilience as process. Development and
Psychopathology, 5(4), 517–528.
and resiliency differ between genders (Ciarrochi et al., 2001; Mavroveli
Finn, J. D., Fish, R. M., & Scott, L. A. (2008). Educational sequelae of high school mis-
et al., 2007; McGloin & Widom, 2001), and so caution must be taken to behavior. The Journal of Educational Research, 101, 259–274.
ensure that these findings are only generalized to male adolescents. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology,
Similarly, the results can only be generalized to adolescents within the 2(3), 300–319.
Greenberg, M. T., & Snell, J. L. (1997). Brain development and emotional development:
age range sampled. Adolescence is a time of significant physical, neu- The role of teaching in organising the frontal lobe. In P. Salovey, & D. J. Sluyter
rological and emotional changes (Greenberg & Snell, 1997). Future (Eds.). Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications. New
research should investigate if different results are obtained with dif- York: Basic Books.
Gumora, G., & Arsenio, W. F. (2002). Emotionally, emotion regulation, and school per-
ferent age group. Finally, common method variance can sometimes be formance in middle school children. Journal of School Psychology, 40(5), 395–413.
observed in studies that utilize similar responding formats in psycho- Hartley, M. (2011). Examining the relationships between resilience, mental health, and
logical questionnaires. Future studies could replicate our results using academic persistence in undergraduate college students. Journal of American College
Health, 59(7), 596–604.
different methods for example the use of an ability based EI measure or Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1996). Intelligence and job performance: Economic and
rater versions of the questionnaires. social implications. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2, 447–472.
Izard, C. E. (1989). The structure and functions of emotions: Implications for cognition,
motivation, and personality. In I. S. Cohen (Ed.). The G. Stanley Hall lecture series (pp.
6.3. Implications and future research 39–73). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Kotzé, M., & Kleynhans, R. (2013). Psychological well-being and resilience as predictors
An interest in the influence of social-emotional capabilities on a of first-year students' academic performance. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 23(1),
51–59.
range of personal, social and academic outcomes has led to a range of EI
Krupic, D., & Corr, P. (2014). Individual differences in emotional elicitation in university
and Resiliency intervention strategies being developed and im- examinations: A quasi-experimental study. Personality and Individual Differences, 71,
plemented in schools. The results from the current study could be ef- 176–180.
fectively utilised to inform academic intervention strategies. Lizzio, A., Wilson, K., & Simons, R. (2002). University students' perceptions of the

80
K. Droppert, et al. Personality and Individual Differences 145 (2019) 75–81

learning environment and academic outcomes: Implications for theory and practice. Peters, C., Kranzler, J. H., & Rossen, E. (2009). Validity of the Mayer—Salovey— Caruso
Studies in Higher Education, 27, 27–52. Emotional Intelligence Test: Youth Version—Research Edition. Canadian Journal of
Lomas, J., Stough, C., Hansen, K., & Downey, L. (2012). Brief report: Emotional in- School Psychology, 24(1), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/0829573508329822.
telligence, victimisation and bullying in adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 35(1), Petrides, K., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric in-
207–211. vestigation with reference to establish trait taxonomies. European Journal of
Luebbers, S., Downey, L. A., & Stough, C. (2007). The development of an adolescent Personality, 15, 425–488.
measure of EI. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 999–1009. Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Furnham, A. (2004). The role of trait emotional in-
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical telligence in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personality and
evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71(3), 543–562. Individual Differences, 36(2), 277–293.
Magnano, P., Craparo, G., & Paolillo, A. (2016). Resilience and emotional intelligence: Phan, H. P. (2010). Empirical model and analysis of mastery and performance-approach
Which role in achievement motivation. International Journal of Psychological Research, goals: A developmental approach. Educational Psychology, 30(5), 547–564.
9(1), 9–20. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing
Masten, A. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behaviour Research
Psychologist, 56, 227–238. Methods, 40, 879–891.
Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence, Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological
psychological well-being and peer-rated social competence in adolescence. British Bulletin, 131(6), 925–971.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 263–275. Prince-Embury, S. (2006). Resiliency scales for adolescents: Profiles of personal strengths. San
Mavroveli, S., & Sánchez-Ruiz, M. J. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence influences on Antonio, TX: Harcourt.
academic achievement and school behaviour. British Journal of Educational Prince-Embury, S., & Courville, T. (2008). Comparison of one-, two-, and three-factor
Psychology, 81(1), 112–134. models of personal resiliency using the resiliency scales for children and adolescents.
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abilities: Emotional in- Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 23(1), 11–25.
telligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507. Prince-Embury, S., Saklofske, D., & Vesely, A. (2015). Measuring constructs. In G. Boyle,
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional D. Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.). Measuring constructs. Book measures of personality
Intelligence Test: MSCEIT. Item booklet. MHS. and social psychological construct. San Diego: Elsevier.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, Pulley, M. L. (2001). Building resiliency how to thrive in times of change. Greensboro, N.C.:
and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 197–215. Greensboro, N.C: Center for Creative Leadership.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2008). Emotional intelligence: New ability or Putwain, D. W., Nicholson, L. J., Connors, L., & Woods, K. (2013). Resilient children are
eclectic traits? The American Psychologist, 63(6), 503. less test anxious and perform better in tests at the end of primary schooling. Learning
McDowell, T. L., & Serovich, J. M. (2007). The effect of perceived and actual social and Individual Differences, 28, 41–46.
support on the mental health of HIV-positive persons. AIDS Care - Psychological and Rouse, K. A. G. (2001). Resilient students' goals and motivation. Journal of Adolescence,
Socio-Medical Aspects of AIDS/HIV, 19(10), 1223–1229. 24(4), 461–472.
McGloin, J. M., & Widom, C. S. (2001). Resilience among abused and neglected children Salovey, P., Bedell, B. T., Detweiler, J. B., & Mayer, J. D. (1999). Coping intelligently:
grown up. Development and Psychopathology, 13(4), 1021–1038. Emotional intelligence and the coping process. In C. R. Snyder (Ed.). Coping: The
McMillan, J. H., & Reed, D. F. (1994). At-risk students and resiliency: Factors contributing psychology of what works (pp. 141–164). New York: Oxford University Press.
to academic success. The Clearing House, 67(3), 137. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and
Miller, D., & Daniel, B. (2007). Competent to cope, worthy of happiness?: How the duality Personality, 9(3), 185–211.
of self-esteem can inform a resilience-based classroom environment. School Salovey, P., Stroud, L., Woolery, A., & Epel, E. (2002). Perceived emotional intelligence,
Psychology International, 28(5), 605–622. stress reactivity, and symptom reports: Further explorations using the trait meta-
Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Hadwin, J. A., & Norgate, R. (2012). Anxiety and depression in mood scale. Psychology & Health, 17(5), 611–627.
academic performance: An exploration of the mediating factors of worry and working Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Roberts, J. M. (2003). Childhood personality foreshadows
memory. School Psychology International, 33(4), 433–449.. adult personality and life outcomes two decades later. Journal of Personality, 71,
Parker, J. D. A., Creque, R. E., Barnhart, D. L., Harris, J. I., Majeski, S. A., Wood, L. M., & 1145–1170.
Hogan, M. J. (2004). Academic achievement in high school: Does emotional in- Tabacknick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Pearson.
telligence matter? Personality and Individual Differences, 37(7), 1321–1330. Valiente, C., Swanson, J., & Eisenberg, N. (2012). Linking students' emotions and aca-
Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement demic achievement: When and why emotions matter. Child Development Perspectives,
emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal 6(2), 129–135.
of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 115–135.

81

Вам также может понравиться