Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/321887719

The Belief in God: Why People Believe and Why They Don’t

Article  in  Current Directions in Psychological Science · December 2018


DOI: 10.1177/0963721418754491

CITATIONS READS

6 9,766

3 authors:

Brett Mercier Stephanie Kramer


University of California, Irvine Pew Research Center
8 PUBLICATIONS   15 CITATIONS    8 PUBLICATIONS   51 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Azim Shariff
University of British Columbia - Vancouver
99 PUBLICATIONS   4,467 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Religion View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Azim Shariff on 02 August 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


754491
research-article2018
CDPXXX10.1177/0963721418754491Mercier et al.Belief in God

ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Current Directions in Psychological

Belief in God: Why People Believe, Science


1­–6
© The Author(s) 2018
and Why They Don’t Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0963721418754491
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418754491
www.psychologicalscience.org/CDPS

Brett Mercier1, Stephanie R. Kramer2, and Azim F. Shariff1


1
Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, and 2Department of Psychology,
University of Oregon

Abstract
Belief in a god or gods is a central feature in the lives of billions of people and a topic of perennial interest within
psychology. However, research over the past half decade has achieved a new level of understanding regarding both
the ultimate and proximate causes of belief in God. Ultimate causes—the evolutionary influences on a trait—shed light
on the adaptive value of belief in God and the reasons why a tendency toward this belief exists in humans. Proximate
causes—the immediate influences on the expression of a trait—explain variation and changes in belief. We review this
research and discuss remaining barriers to a fuller understanding of belief in God.

Keywords
belief, God, evolution, religion

At one point in the prehistory of our species, no one types of causation (Scott-Phillips, Dickins, & West, 2011).
believed in a god. Today, an estimated 80% of humans For example, a question posed at one level of explanation
are theists (P. Zuckerman, 2007). Why did the belief in cannot be answered at another level of explanation. An
gods become so broadly and powerfully held? Religion ultimate question about why cooperation exists requires
has fascinated psychologists since James (1902), but a an explanation for the evolution of cooperation, such as
recent body of research has sharply focused on the that it provides a selective advantage by increasing the
reasons people believe in gods—defined here as fitness of one’s kin. It cannot be answered with a proxi-
“supernatural beings believed to have created or [to] mate explanation for cooperation, such as concerns for
govern all reality, intervene in human affairs, and praise and blame (Scott-Phillips et al., 2011). This ultimate-
enforce or support human morality” (Botero et al., 2014, proximate distinction thus provides a useful organizing
p. 16784). Here, we separate these reasons along Mayr’s framework for recent research on the belief in God.
(1961) ultimate versus proximate categorization of
causal explanations. Ultimate explanations focus on
Ultimate Reasons for the Cognitive
why a behavior evolved—its functional origins as an
adaptation or evolutionary by-product. Proximate expla- Features Making Belief in God Intuitive
nations focus on the immediate factors influencing how Features of the human mind have arisen as evolved
and when a behavior is performed. For example, the adaptations to environmental challenges throughout our
proximate explanation for a bird undertaking its yearly prehistory. However, many authors have argued that
migration is the experience of changing daily hours of these cognitive adaptations have, as a by-product, made
sunlight, but the ultimate explanation is that because humans prone to the conception of supernatural agents
of the scarce winter food supply, better survival and (Norenzayan et al., 2016). An early, oft-cited example
reproductive opportunities were afforded to those who of such a cognitive adaptation is our hypersensitivity to
migrated to warmer climates.
Ultimate and proximate causes thus provide comple-
Corresponding Author:
mentary—rather than competing—explanations for a
Azim F. Shariff, University of California, Irvine, Department of
behavior. Although not without critics (see Vromen, 2017, Psychology and Social Behavior, 4558 Social and Behavioral Sciences
for a discussion), the ultimate-proximate distinction has Gateway, Irvine, CA 92697
proved useful for preventing confusion about different E-mail: azim.shariff@uci.edu
2 Mercier et al.

cues of humanlike agency (Guthrie, 1993). Because failing survive and spread their beliefs, making these beliefs
to notice potentially dangerous agents in our ancestral envi- more common. Analysis of historical societies has found
ronment was costlier than making false alarms, our agency- that these beliefs were especially likely to spread where
detection system evolved to be tilted toward overperception. the need for cooperation was high, such as in societies
But the by-product of being adaptively tuned to overper- with rights to movable property, high political complex-
ceive agency is that humans are biased toward perceiving ity, or resource scarcity (Botero et al., 2014).
agents—such as gods—behind natural phenomena.
Alongside this hypersensitive agency-detection
Proximate Reasons for Who Believes
device, theorists have described biases for overperceiv-
ing human-relevant purposes behind events and and When
objects—adaptive because of the necessity of decipher- While ultimate explanations tell us why specific cogni-
ing intentionality and the usefulness of understanding tive biases evolved, proximate explanations show how
tools but leading in turn to a tendency to make attribu- and when these biases contribute to belief in God.
tions of divine purpose (Kelemen, 2004). In addition, Understanding these cognitive, motivational, and
because of the importance of understanding mental social factors that influence belief helps explain the
states, humans evolved separate systems for thinking great variation in religiosity among our species—not
about social stimuli and physical, nonsocial objects just across countries (84% of people in the Philippines
(Bloom, 2007). Although this was adaptive for our report being certain about God’s existence, compared
social cognition, a by-product was that it made it easy with 4% in Japan; Smith, 2012) but across time—which
for humans to imagine the existence of disembodied can in turn inform long-standing debates about
supernatural agents that nonetheless had an active secularization.
mental existence (Forstmann & Burgmer, 2015).
These by-products of innately occurring cognitive
features provided the psychological raw materials from
Cognitive factors
which supernatural beliefs were culturally shaped into One method of examining the proximate cognitive fac-
shared God beliefs (Gervais, Willard, Norenzayan, & tors influencing belief in God is to compare individuals
Henrich, 2011). Unlike the gods of most modern reli- who differ on cognitive factors, such as the tendency
gions, the supernatural agents that early societies to rely on evolved cognitive intuitions. People with an
believed in tended to be nonmoralistic, with limited analytical thinking style—that is, people more likely to
powers and knowledge (Roes & Raymond, 2003). To override their intuitions in decision making—are less
explain the transition, cultural evolutionary theorists likely to be believers (Pennycook, Ross, Koehler, &
have argued that, in a process roughly analogous to Fugelsang, 2016). In addition to overriding intuitions,
genetic evolution, selective pressures made certain cul- the extent to which people experience these intu-
tural beliefs more likely to survive and spread than itions in the first place also plays a role. Because
others. As human group sizes increased, it became dif- attributing mental states to unseen agents facilitates
ficult to track which members were cooperative con- belief in God, people who less easily perceive and
tributors and which were defecting free riders, straining understand the mental states of others (such as indi-
the mechanisms maintaining group cohesion. Beliefs viduals on the autism spectrum and men relative to
in omniscient supernatural watchers capable of doling women) show lower levels of belief (Norenzayan,
out punishments and rewards helped solve this problem Gervais, & Trzesniewski, 2012).
by deterring free riding, giving groups adopting these Though still controversial, convergent research sug-
beliefs an advantage over other groups (Norenzayan gests that more intelligent individuals are less likely to
et al., 2016; though see the commentaries to that article believe in God (Kanazawa, 2010; Lynn, Harvey, & Nyborg,
for criticisms and alternative perspectives). In a recent 2009). Although partially explained by its overlap with
cross-cultural study demonstrating this religiously analytic thinking, the relationship between belief in God
inspired prosociality, researchers found that the more and intelligence also has compelling motivation-based
individuals believed their gods were punitive and explanations. For example, M. Zuckerman, Silberman,
knowledgeable about humans, the more money these and Hall (2013) argue that more intelligent individuals
individuals shared with anonymous coreligionists in an may have less need for the psychological benefits that
economic game (Purzycki et al., 2016). In another study, religion provides (such as a sense that the world is
subtle reminders of God made people more charitable, controllable; see below) because they can more ably
but only when these reminders were of God’s punitive generate these benefits themselves. Intelligent people
aspects (Yilmaz & Bahçekapili, 2016). are also more likely to be nonconformists and thus feel
Thus, increased cooperation made groups who more comfortable deviating from the (typically) reli-
believed in watchful, moralizing gods more likely to gious majority.
Belief in God 3

Motivational reasons Social factors


In addition to studying the factors that make people Finally—but critically—declines in belief are acceler-
more or less cognitively receptive to belief in God, ated by feedback loops with changing cultural norms.
research has also examined the factors that motivate In religious societies, people are socialized to believe
(or demotivate) people to believe. For instance, in God through communities that value and encourage
Sedikides and Gebauer (2010) present a meta-analysis belief (Sherkat, 2003). This socialization, particularly
showing that high self-enhancers—people with a strong the religious behaviors of one’s community, appears to
desire to see themselves positively—have higher levels be one of the strongest determinants of belief. Accord-
of intrinsic religiosity, especially in societies that place ing to Henrich (2009), humans evolved to be acutely
greater value on religion. They argue that elements of sensitive to credibility-enhancing displays in their ten-
religion—such as a believed association with God— dency to adopt beliefs. For example, claims that blue
create feelings of positive self-regard, motivating high mushrooms are not poisonous are more credible when
self-enhancers to adopt stronger religious beliefs. How- the claimant eats the mushrooms (Henrich, 2009). Con-
ever, more research is needed to confirm this causal sistent with this theory, research has shown that the
direction. more frequently children observe others not just pro-
Other research has used experiments to provide a fessing belief in God but engaging in religious credibility-
causal test of motivational factors influencing belief. enhancing displays (such as volunteering for religious
Epley and colleagues show not only that chronically organizations), the more likely these children are to
lonely people are less likely to believe in God but also believe in God as adults (Lanman & Buhrmester, 2017).
that randomly assigning people to situations that Thus, once triggered, generational declines in belief in
increase loneliness (such as informing people that they God might gain momentum through a positive feedback
will probably be alone later in life) increases reports effect. Children of each generation are raised witness-
of belief (Epley, Akalis, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2008). ing fewer displays of religious commitment than the
Compensatory-control theory posits that people last, making them less likely to believe in God and less
strive to believe their world is predictable and control- likely to expose their own children to displays of com-
lable (Kay, Gaucher, Napier, Callan, & Laurin, 2008). mitment (Willard & Cingl, 2017; see Fig. 1).
On the basis of this theory, Kay and colleagues (2008)
argue that belief in God is motivated by a need for
perceived control, which they demonstrated by showing
Limitations With the Literature
that experimentally threatening people’s sense of con- Though the past 5 years have seen remarkable progress
trol can increase belief in God. Experimentally increas- in the psychology underlying belief in God, a more
ing mortality salience has also been shown to increase complete understanding of the phenomenon has been
belief (Vail, Arndt, & Abdollahi, 2012), which may hampered by significant limitations in interpreting what
explain why religiosity tends to increase when people is by far the primary source of data about beliefs in
get older, become terminally ill, or experience a natural God: self-reports.
disaster (Bentzen, 2013; Jong, 2013). The ability of the The first issue derives from a Western and Abrahamic
belief in God—and religion more generally—to palliate bias in the psychology of religion. Efforts to conduct
the effect of negative life events has been offered as research outside the monotheistic, Judeo-Christian reli-
one explanation for religiosity’s association with greater gious traditions in North America and Europe are limited
well-being (Whitehead & Bergeman, 2011). not just by inconvenience and political restrictions (e.g.,
Together, these cognitive and motivational factors in China and several Muslim-majority countries) but also
help explain where and when belief in God has by conceptual differences in the meaning of belief. That
declined around the world. For example, some research- is, methods of measuring belief in God that assume a
ers have argued that the declines in belief over the 20th traditional Western conception of God may not be
century can be explained by the corresponding appropriate for understanding the beliefs of people with
increases in IQ over this same period (Lynn et  al., different conceptions of God (Höllinger & Eder, 2016).
2009). Likewise, religiosity has tended to decline when These cultural variations in the meaning of belief are
strong and predictable political systems dampen the rarely assessed (for a notable exception, see Bluemke,
uncertainty and adversity in a society. The religiosity Jong, Grevenstein, Mikloušić, & Halberstadt, 2016). How
of the United States, which is aberrantly high among similar is an American Christian’s belief in a personal
rich countries, has been attributed to the sense of eco- God to the Vedic theistic beliefs in India or to ancestor
nomic insecurity caused by its (relatively) laissez faire worship in China? Although someone from each country
economic policies (Norris & Inglehart, 2004). may answer affirmatively to straightforward questions
4
Proximate Factors

Cognitive
Ultimate Factors
Analytical thinking style (–): Analytical thinkers are less
likely to be influenced by the cognitive intuitions
contributing to belief in God.
Evolved Cognitive Biases
Hypersensitive agency detection: Over- Mentalizing (+): People more capable of mind reading
perception of agency leads to the perception are more likely to perceive the existence of God.
that God is causing natural events.
Intelligence (–): People with higher intelligence are
Promiscuous teleology: Overperception of less likely to believe in God.
purpose leads to attributions of divine purpose
in the world.

Mind–body dualism: Perceiving minds as Motivational


separate from bodies makes it easy to Loneliness (+): When lonely, people are more likely to
conceive of disembodied supernatural perceive agents, including God.
agents.
Belief Mortality salience (+): Belief increases when death is salient.
in Self-enhancement (+): In places where religion is
valued, a desire to see oneself positively motivates belief
Cultural Evolution
God in God.

Belief in punitive, monitoring gods provided Control (–): Belief in a controlling God increases in
a survival advantage by increasing in-group uncontrollable situations, allowing people to regain a
cooperation, making these beliefs more sense of control.
common.

These beliefs were most likely to spread Social


when cooperation was important, such as in
Socialization (+): People are more likely to believe in
societies with few resources, rights to
God when this belief is valued and encouraged by their
movable property, and high political
community.
complexity.
Credibility-enhancing displays (+): Witnessing credible
displays of religious commitment increases the likelihood
of believing in God.

Fig. 1.  An overview of ultimate and proximate factors contributing to belief in God. On the right-hand side of the figure, a plus sign denotes that the factor increases the likeli-
hood of belief, whereas a minus sign denotes that the factor decreases the likelihood of belief.
Belief in God 5

about belief in “God” (if only to conform to Western extent, nature, or future of how belief in God factors
surveyors’ expectations), it remains unclear whether into our psychology and society.
these homogenous responses disguise psychologically
important differences. Recommended Reading
Even in Western contexts, the continued evolution Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K.,
of religion has led to widening conceptions of God, McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016).
which may not cleanly correspond to traditional beliefs. (See References). An in-depth review of the cultural evo-
For example, 30% of Europeans, including many self- lution of religion plus an assortment of commentaries that
described “atheists,” report believing in a “spirit God or discuss additional and alternative perspectives.
vital life force” (Bréchon, 2007, p. 469). Should this Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). (See References). A review
quasideism be defined as belief in God? Clearly, tradi- of the causes and consequences of secularization around
tional methods are not fully capturing the scope and the world.
Sedikides, C. (2010). Why does religiosity persist? Personality
meaning of belief in these contexts.
and Social Psychology Review, 14, 3–6. The introduction
The other major challenge to the integrity of self- to the 2010 Personality and Social Psychology Review spe-
report-based research on belief in God is that reports cial issue on religion, which provides a summary and
may reflect self-presentation concerns as much as genu- overview of the approaches contained within the issue.
ine belief. Around the world, people show considerable Zuckerman, M., Silberman, J., & Hall, J. A. (2013). (See
prejudice against nonbelievers (Gervais et  al., 2017), References). A meta-analysis, comprehensive review,
and majorities consider belief necessary for morality and theoretical discussion of the research examining the
(Pew Research Center, 2014). Thus, people may be relationship between intelligence and religiosity.
inclined to present themselves as more religious than
they truly are. Evidence of exaggeration in church Action Editor
attendance supports this concern—as evidenced by Randall W. Engle served as action editor for this article.
discrepancies between direct self-reports and head
counts (Hadaway, Marler, & Chaves, 1993) or time-use Declaration of Conflicting Interests
diaries (Brenner, 2011). And though it is harder to con- The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest
firm the veracity of people’s internal beliefs than their with respect to the authorship or the publication of this
behavior, research supports the idea that people are article.
indeed overreporting their belief in God. Survey tech-
niques that decrease socially desirable responding, such References
as responding anonymously online rather than directly Bentzen, J. S. (2013). Origins of religiousness: The role of natu-
to a live surveyor (Cox, Jones, & Navarro-Rivera, 2014) ral disasters (Discussion Paper No. 13-02). Copenhagen,
or using the unmatched-count technique (Gervais & Denmark: Department of Economics, University of
Najle, 2018), suggest that rates of belief may be sub- Copenhagen.
stantially lower than traditional methods have assumed. Bloom, P. (2007). Religion is natural. Developmental Science,
The potential invalidity of self-reported belief calls 10, 147–151.
into question the interpretation of many findings within Bluemke, M., Jong, J., Grevenstein, D., Mikloušić, I., &
the social scientific literature on religion. Consider Halberstadt, J. (2016). Measuring cross-cultural supernatu-
ral beliefs with self- and peer-reports. PLOS ONE, 11(10),
research that finds relationships between God beliefs
Article e0164291. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164291
and any construct that might be misreported because Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin,
of social pressure (e.g., happiness, health, prosociality). M. C., & Gray, R. D. (2014). The ecology of religious
Any correlation between belief in God and (for exam- beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
ple) happiness may be the result not of a genuine USA, 111, 16784–16789.
relationship between the two but of a third variable: Bréchon, P. (2007). Cross-national comparisons of individual
individual differences in people’s tendency to overre- religiosity. In J. A. Beckford & N. J. Demerath, III (Eds.),
port levels of both. Furthermore, experimental studies— The SAGE handbook of the sociology of religion (pp. 463–
including those discussed above—that show changes 489). London, England: SAGE.
in self-reported belief in God could very well be reveal- Brenner, P. S. (2011). Identity importance and the overreport-
ing changes in people’s willingness to admit their belief ing of religious service attendance: Multiple imputation of
religious attendance using the American Time Use Study
rather than changes in belief itself.
and the General Social Survey. Journal for the Scientific
These methodological questions should weigh heav- Study of Religion, 50, 103–115.
ily on the field. The past 5 years have seen an expanding Cox, D., Jones, R. P., & Navarro-Rivera, J. (2014). I know what
body of research on belief in God; researchers should you did last Sunday: Measuring social desirability bias
devote some of the next 5 to tackling these critical chal- in self-reported religious behavior, belief, and identity.
lenges. Until we do, we will not fully understand the Public Religion Research Institute, 2, 57–58.
6 Mercier et al.

Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating and commentaries]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39,
social connection through inferential reproduction: Article e1. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14001356
Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion
greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, 114–120. and politics worldwide. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Forstmann, M., & Burgmer, P. (2015). Adults are intuitive University Press.
mind-body dualists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Pennycook, G., Ross, R. M., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A.
General, 144, 222–235. (2016). Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than
Gervais, W. M., & Najle, M. B. (2018). How many atheists are religious believers: Four empirical studies and a meta-
there? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 3–10. analysis. PLOS ONE, 11(4), Article e0153039. doi:10.1371/
Gervais, W. M., Willard, A., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. journal.pone.0153039
(2011). The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate Pew Research Center. (2014, March 13). Worldwide, many
intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain reli- see belief in God as essential to morality. Retrieved from
gious belief. Religion, 41, 389–410. www.pewglobal.org/2014/03/13/worldwide-many-see-
Gervais, W. M., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R. T., van Elk, M., belief-in-god-as-essential-to-morality/
Buchtel, E. E., Aveyard, M., . . . Bulbulia, J. (2017). Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E.,
Global evidence of extreme intuitive moral prejudice McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., . . . Henrich, J. (2016).
against atheists. Nature Human Behavior, 1, Article 0151. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expan-
doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0151 sion of human sociality. Nature, 530, 327–330.
Guthrie, S. E. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of Roes, F. L., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in moralizing gods.
religion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Evolution & Human Behavior, 24, 126–135.
Hadaway, C. K., Marler, P. L., & Chaves, M. (1993). What the Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. (2011).
polls don’t show: A closer look at U.S. church attendance. Evolutionary theory and the ultimate–proximate distinc-
American Sociological Review, 58, 741–752. tion in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on
Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, coopera- Psychological Science, 6, 38–47.
tion and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2010). Religiosity as self-
implications for cultural evolution. Evolution & Human enhancement: A meta-analysis of the relation between
Behavior, 30, 244–260. socially desirable responding and religiosity. Personality
Höllinger, F., & Eder, A. (2016). Functional equivalence and Social Psychology Review, 14, 17–36.
and validity of religiousness indicators in cross-cultural Sherkat, D. E. (2003). Religious socialization: Sources of
comparative surveys. Methodological Innovations, 9. influence and influences of agency. In M. Dillion (Ed.),
doi:10.1177/2059799115622756 Handbook of the sociology of religion (pp. 151–163). New
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A study York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
in human nature. New York, NY: Penguin. Smith, T. W. (2012). Beliefs about God across time and coun-
Jong, J. (2013). On faith and the fear of fatality: A review tries. NORC at the University of Chicago. Retrieved from
of recent research on deities and death. Journal for the http://gss.norc.org/Documents/reports/cross-national-
Cognitive Science of Religion, 1, 193–214. reports/Godissp.pdf
Kanazawa, S. (2010). Why liberals and atheists are more intel- Vail, K. E., III, Arndt, J., & Abdollahi, A. (2012). Exploring
ligent. Social Psychology Quarterly, 73, 33–57. the existential function of religion and supernatural agent
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. beliefs among Christians, Muslims, atheists, and agnostics.
(2008). God and the government: Testing a compensatory Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1288–1300.
control mechanism for the support of external systems. Vromen, J. (2017). Ultimate and proximate explanations
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 18–35. of strong reciprocity. History and Philosophy of the Life
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning Sciences, 39(3), Article 25. doi:10.1007/s40656-017-0151-4
about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Whitehead, B. R., & Bergeman, C. S. (2011). Coping with daily
Science, 15, 295–301. stress: Differential role of spiritual experience on daily
Lanman, J. A., & Buhrmester, M. D. (2017). Religious actions positive and negative affect. Journals of Gerontology, Series
speak louder than words: Exposure to credibility-enhanc- B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 67, 456–459.
ing displays predicts theism. Religion, Brain & Behavior, Willard, A. K., & Cingl, L. (2017). Testing theories of secu-
7, 3–16. larization and religious belief in the Czech Republic and
Lynn, R., Harvey, J., & Nyborg, H. (2009). Average intelligence Slovakia. Evolution & Human Behavior 38, 604–615.
predicts atheism rates across 137 nations. Intelligence, Yilmaz, O., & Bahçekapili, H. G. (2016). Supernatural and
37, 11–15. secular monitors promote human cooperation only if they
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134, remind of punishment. Evolution & Human Behavior, 37,
1501–1506. 79–84.
Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012). Zuckerman, M., Silberman, J., & Hall, J. A. (2013). The rela-
Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal God. tion between intelligence and religiosity: A meta-analysis
PLOS ONE, 7(5), Article e36880. doi:10.1371/journal.pone and some proposed explanations. Personality and Social
.0036880 Psychology Review, 17, 325–354.
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Zuckerman, P. (2007). Atheism: Contemporary numbers and pat-
McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). terns. In M. Martin (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to athe-
The cultural evolution of prosocial religions [Target article ism (pp. 47–65). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться