Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 40

Third Party Mediation in the Arab-Israeli Conflict

Abstract

This article investigates the links between mediation determinants and mediation

outcomes in the Arab-Israeli conflict between 1948 and 2008. We identify the most

substantive and most researched cases of mediation in the conflict, as well as the most

pertinent theoretical determinants of mediation as they appear in the literature, to present

several hypotheses about the significance of such factors to mediation outcome. Using

bivariate correlation analysis and various multiple regression models, we find that in the

context of this conflict, Arab-Israeli mediation has been most successful when used by

high-status third parties who employed manipulative strategies and focused on limited

objectives, as opposed to pursuing a comprehensive settlement to the conflict or tackling

its core issues.

Dr. Asaf Siniver


Dept. of Political Science and International Studies
University of Birmingham
a.siniver@bham.ac.uk

Dr. Peter Thomas


School of Law and Social Science
University of East London

Draft – please do not cite without permission

1
Introduction

Mediation is one of the oldest forms of conflict resolution and has been used extensively

worldwide by individuals, states and organisations to bring about peaceful resolution to

interstate and intrastate conflicts. While definitions of, and approaches to mediation vary,

it is commonly understood as the intervention of a third party in the dispute of two or

more parties, for the purpose of improving the nature of interaction between the

disputants (Kressel and Pruitt, 1989). Mediation is a distinct form of third party

intervention. It is initiated and performed on a voluntary and non-violent basis, and its

proposals or recommendations are non-binding. As a conflict resolution tool, mediation

has proved to be the most popular form of contemporary conflict resolution, present in

nearly 60 percent of international and intrastate disputes between (Bercovitch and Fretter,

2004: 29), while nearly half of all post Cold War crisis were mediated by third parties

(Beardsley et. al., 2006: 59). However, despite the prevalence of mediation in

contemporary conflicts, the academic literature is surprisingly modest in its attempts to

provide tangible hypotheses about the linkage between certain mediation characteristics

and the likelihood of a successful outcome. Whereas some studies of mediation tend to be

overly generic and lack rigorous testing of their propositions, others are insufficiently

driven by systematic case-study analysis, thus reducing the treatment of cases to

descriptive footnotes.

The Arab-Israeli conflict (AIC) is perhaps the world‘s most protracted conflict

and has been visited by dozens of mediators since its inception in 1948. However despite

the diversity of third parties and their strategies, as well the as changes in global/regional

contexts and the nature of relationships between the disputants, very few studies of

2
mediation have attempted to hypothesise about the relationship between contextual and

procedural factors of mediation and mediation outcomes in the Arab-Israeli conflict (see

for example Kriesberg, 2001; Siniver, 2006; Stein and Lewis, 1996). In its various forms,

the conflict includes a diverse yet comparable population of enduring rivalries (Greig,

2005). These represent the high end of interaction in the international system, and as they

are the most likely to escalate into protracted conflicts, they invariably necessitate the

intervention of third parties. We know that enduring conflicts (that is, repeated military

confrontations between two states over time) represent only 13 per cent of all

international conflicts, yet they account for more than half of all mediation efforts since

1945, and that they are ten times more likely to attract third party mediation. On average

such conflicts last for nearly 38 years and they attract at least 14 attempts at managing

them by third parties, though few of them succeed (Goertz and Diehl, 1993; Bercovitch

and Diehl, 1997). As a prime example of an enduring and intractable conflict then, we

can learn a lot from the experience of the Arab-Israeli conflict about possible links

between mediation determinants and mediation outcomes.

In recent years the literature on mediation and conflict resolution more broadly

has seen a large number of normative studies which use formal models and game-theory

approaches to suggest when and how rational actors can mediate most effectively with

regards to the use of information and resources vis-à-vis the disputing parties (Kydd

2003, 2005; Rauchhaus 2006; Greig and Regan 2008). The reality of conflict however, is

that often mediators and disputants alike do not enjoy the degree of rationality, logic and

perfect information which is assumed by such normative studies. At the same time

empirical approaches and large-N studies are particularly under-represented in the

3
literature. Such studies can be scrutinised for imperfect data gathering or possible bias

selection, but they offer a useful platform to study the conditions for mediation success

and failure as part of a systematic, inductive framework which can offer generalised

observations about effective mediation with real policy relevance (Bercovitch 2005).

Whilst the AIC represents a single case of inquiry, its development over time in terms of

actors, mediation processes and the international context, provides in effect a larger

population of relevant observations which allows us to test theories on the possible

relationships between factors and conditions of mediation and mediation outcomes, with

empirical resonance which goes beyond this particular conflict (Rubin 1981; Bercovitch

1997).

Ever since the first Arab-Israeli war of 1948, dozens of third parties have offered

their services to the disputants, in the hope of bringing a successful resolution to arguably

the most protracted conflict in contemporary history. These third parties have entered the

conflict at its various stages, brandishing a wide range of mediation expertise and

capabilities. Some sought to achieve a lasting comprehensive peace between Israel and its

Arab neighbours, while others concentrated their efforts on more limited objectives, such

as the disengagement of forces or interim political agreements. Even a cursory survey of

the history of third party mediation in the Arab-Israeli conflict points to a ‗dismal record‘

(Eisenberg and Caplan, 1998: 1). If success is judged by the longevity of the agreement,

then this record stands at just five long-term successes: Ralph Bunche‘s mediation of the

1949 armistice agreements which delineated the conflict‘s borders (the Green Line) and

remains a reference point to this day; Henry Kissinger‘s separation of Egyptian and

Israeli forces in the immediate aftermath of the 1973 October War, and further

4
disengagement between Israeli, Egyptian and Syrian forces in 1974-5; Jimmy Carter‘s

mediation at Camp David which led to the 1979 Egyptian-Israeli peace treaty; and the

secret Oslo channel which produced a historic and irreversible agreement between Israel

and the Palestine Liberation Organization (PLO), which, despite subsequent failures of

implementation of some of its terms remains the cornerstone of two decades of Israeli-

Palestinian dialogue (Siniver, 2006). However, if we accept that mediation success is to

be measured simply by the mutual and formal signing of an agreement, rather than by the

ability of the disputants and external actors to maintain it over a long period of time, then

we can add four more cases of successful mediation in the Arab-Israeli conflict. These

include the 1970 Rogers plan, the 1983 Israel-Lebanon peace treaty, and the 1997 Hebron

agreement and the 1998 Wye River memorandum between Israel and the PLO.

Defining mediation success

Despite mediation being the most common method of conflict resolution in the post-Cold

war era, the literature lacks an authoritative empirically-tested and theory-based

conceptualisation of mediation success. Whilst the nature of the relationship between

mediation attributes and success is regularly debated by mediation scholars, there is still

no consensus as to why some mediation cases are successful when others are not. As

Kleiboer correctly observed, ‗the issue of assessing outcomes of international conflict

management is a tricky one‘ (1996: 361). Some scholars see the listing of general

conditions for success as an exercise in futility given the importance of contextual,

political and cultural idiosyncrasies (Meyer, 1960; Simkin, 1971), while others adopt a

5
more prescriptive approach by suggesting which conditions are more conducive to

successful mediation (Mitchell, 1981; Bercovitch, 1997; Berridge, 2002).

Before presenting our criteria to evaluate mediation outcomes, it is important to

consider the conditions under which mediation may be accepted in the first place. In their

study of mediation acceptance in civil wars, Greig and Regan claim that, ‗[o]ne way to

think about the acceptance of mediation is that both sides implicitly agree to make

concessions, or at least that they have some concessions to give under certain

circumstances‘ (2008: 760). However whereas the acceptance of mediation is necessary

for its ultimate success, it is of course not sufficient, and whilst many studies explore why

and when third parties choose to mediate, they cannot tell us why or when mediation is

likely to succeed (Regan, 1998; Greig, 2004; Terris and Maoz, 2005). As demonstrated

repeatedly in the Arab-Israeli conflict, in protracted conflicts which experience variation

in intensity and the nature of the disputed issues, the disputants‘ decision to accept the

offer of third party mediation has sometimes more to do with saving face domestically or

gaining favour with the international community, than with a genuine desire to achieve a

settlement (Beardsley, 2010). This also applies to the mediator‘s decision to offer his/her

services based on a cost-benefit analysis. Despite the obvious costs, such as damage to

reputation in the event of failure, in some instances third parties will seek to mediate in

the first place or continue to do so knowing that the chances of success are slim, as means

to maintain a regional or global role or in order to keep the parties talking simply to avoid

the resumption of violence. Third parties are also more likely to offer their services if

they have established relations with at least one of the parties, or they have

strategic/regional interests in the resolution of the conflict, or they possess appropriate

6
expertise or resources to offer effective mediation (Smith and Stam, 2003; Greig, 2005;

Bercovitch and Gartner, 2006).

Finally, any discussion of mediation success must distinguish between mediation as

process and mediation as outcome. Whilst they represent consecutive phases under the

broad umbrella of ‗mediation‘, the factors determining them are somewhat different. The

process of mediation may be determined by factors such as leverage, timing, and

impartiality, whereas studies of the outcome of mediated agreements have highlighted the

importance of external security guarantees, peace dividends, etc., in ensuring the

successful implementation and durability of the agreement (Stedman, Rothchild and

Cousens, 2002; Walter, 2002). The question of why successfully mediated agreements

sometime fail to produce durable peace is important because of the implicit assumption

that a failed mediated outcome is necessarily attributed to a failed mediated process,

whereas in reality this may not be the case. In a prolonged and intractable conflict

conditions are rarely auspicious to dialogue and compromise, and even the conclusion of

an agreement with the good will of the disputants and the expert assistance of the

mediator does not guarantee its durability in a volatile and violent environment – note for

example the successful mediation and subsequent collapse of the 1983 Israel-Lebanon

peace treaty and the 1993 Oslo accords between Israel and the PLO. This empirical

evidence is supported by Gartner and Bercovitch (2006), whose International Conflict

Management dataset suggests that high intensity and protracted conflicts are unlikely to

end in a full-scale agreement, mediated or not, and that any agreement reached is likely to

be limited and short-lived (see also Carment et. al., 2009). In such a hostile environment,

7
achieving a limited ceasefire can be a huge success for the mediator and disputants alike

given the inhospitable conditions for political settlement.

The distinction between mediation process and outcome can be explained with

reference to two contending conceptualisations of success and failure in international

mediation. The first approach offers seemingly objective criteria which look at the

ultimate consequences of the mediation effort. These criteria are often defined broadly to

compensate for the idiosyncratic nature of specific events. This allows the observer

significant latitude for interpretation, and to retain a certain amount of freedom in

analysing patterns of mediation while ignoring the motives of the disputants and the

mediator. This approach is closely linked to the structuralist paradigm of mediation and

rational choice theories, which explain the occurrence and outcomes of mediation by

important signposts such as timing and power (Haass, 1990; Princen, 1992). Accordingly

success here is measured by the occurrence of objective and observable events, such as a

cease-fire, a peace treaty or other tangible political settlements (Touval, 1982; Kriesberg,

1991; Bercovitch and Houston, 1996). This approach of defining success in terms of

political settlements may fail to account, however, for the degree of effectiveness of

mediation in producing a durable agreement (Bercovitch and Simpson, 2010). This is

where the second approach comes in, attempting to bridge the gap between results and

perceptions. While the first approach eschews any discussion of the subjective

interpretations of the disputants or the mediator to the bargaining process, this approach

explains mediation success by focussing on the process of communication as a means of

changing attitudes, largely outside the structures of formal negotiation. This approach is

linked to the social-psychological paradigm of mediation, and accordingly successful

8
mediation is defined here in terms of the (subjective) perceptions of the disputants and

the mediator of their respective efforts to accomplish their aims as they were outlined at

the initiation of the process (Smith, 1985; Hopmann, 1995). In addition to effectiveness,

other criteria to evaluate success here may include fairness, efficiency and satisfaction

(Katz 1999), and while these intangible abstractions are not without their merit, it is clear

that the very personal and subjective nature of such criteria cannot, on its own, provide an

accurate indication of mediation outcomes.

Research design

We adopt the first approach to evaluate mediation success, that is, we take the conclusion

of objective signposts as success regardless of durability, thus keeping the analytical

distinction between process and outcome. We find that out of 24 cases of mediation in the

Arab-Israeli conflict, nine were concluded with some kind of a formal agreement, ranging

from a cease-fire to a peace treaty. As international mediation is not a uniform practice it

would be erroneous to treat the outcome in such a manner, and particularly in a

protracted, high intensity conflict such as the one between Israel and its Arab neighbours.

Limited agreements or even short-lived settlements should not be disregarded as abject

failures of mediation. Our nine cases of successful mediation are not equal in their

historic significance or indeed in their longevity – some have collapsed fairly rapidly

whereas others are still honoured by the parties several decades later. For this reason we

treat mediation success/failure not as a dichotomous outcome but rather across a dynamic

spectrum which takes into account issues such as significance, longevity, the

international context, and the objectives of the disputants and mediator at the initiation of

9
the process. Specifically, we borrow the typology used by Beardsley et. al. (2006) to code

mediation outcomes in our study as follows: 1= complete failure; 2= short-term post-

mediation tension reduction (up to 12 months); 3= sustained post-mediation tension

reduction (lasting more than 12 months); 4= formal agreement but resumption of violence

over previously agreed issues within 5 years; 5= enduring formal agreement, lasting more

than 5 years.

The long history of mediation efforts to bring an end to the Arab-Israeli conflict in all

its form requires us to develop a systematic framework to identify which mediation

factors are consistently linked to a successful outcome. We proceed in three stages:

First, we surveyed the entire population of third party mediation in the conflict

between 1948-2008 and identified the most significant and most researched cases for our

analysis – we found 24 such cases. While individual case studies are notorious for their

lack of conceptual comparability, this meta-analysis of multiple case studies allows us to

arrive at comparable data for all 24 cases. Of course, any systematic analysis of 24 cases,

using potential explanatory variables will necessarily have to be somewhat tentative. The

number of cases is sufficiently large to detect very strong relationships, but insufficient to

determine whether or not weaker (and even moderate) associations are reflecting real

empirical regularities. The risk of Type-1 errors (incorrectly concluding that a

relationship exists) is as real as that of Type-2 errors (incorrectly concluding that there is

no relationship between variables), although the latter is mitigated by the fact that we are

predominantly interested in whether any factors have strong effects on outcomes. The

only solution for this problem consists of increasing the number of cases for analysis, but

this is not feasible as we have covered all substantive mediation efforts in this conflict.

10
We do not include every case of diplomatic activity in this period for

methodological and epistemological reasons. Some cases are not as well researched as

others, and as such could not offer substantial contribution to knowledge or be compared

effectively to other cases. Touval, for example, cites at least five failed cases of

mediation from the early 1950s (1982: 106), however their relative obscurity and

inconsequential impact on the progression of the conflict meant that they were not

followed in subsequent by multiple and authoritative accounts, on par with the cases

selected here. Furthermore, some periods in the conflict have seen an influx of mediators

in a relatively short period of time, representing a broader strategy of mediation rather

than individual efforts with discernible characteristics, such as the long list of American

special envoys who visited the region in the run up to the 1983 Israel-Lebanon peace

treaty, or the various officials who have come to facilitate Israeli-Palestinian talks in the

past decade under the terms of reference of the Road Map framework. In essence then,

our population of cases is comprised of mediation efforts, rather than mediators; naming

every diplomat who ever shuttled between Israeli and Arab capitals will do little to

further our understanding of the linkage between certain mediation determinants and

mediation outcomes. We are concerned here with important chapters in the diplomatic

history of the Arab-Israeli conflict, rather than its anecdotal footnotes.

Second, each of our 24 cases was broken into its constituent elements in relation

to three distinct categories: context, mediator’s capacity and process. Taken together,

there are 24 such independent variables which cover the range of mediation experience in

the AIC. Our contextual factors include the scope of mediated issues (e.g. Jerusalem,

refugees, water, etc.), the degree of power asymmetry between the parties and point in

11
time in the conflict cycle in which mediation took place. As for the mediator‘s capacity to

mediate the process, we measured various permutations of power (leverage), impartiality

and status/rank. The procedural variables covered a range of issues such as mediation

strategies, degrees of visibility of the process as well as its objectives as identified by the

mediator.

The analysis will use bivariate correlation and ordinary least squares (OLS)

regression in order to fit models that best describe the data involving our independent

variables and the dependent variable, Outcome of Mediation. To allow for interceding

reliability we used a group of independent researchers to measure the potency of these

variables in every one of our cases of Arab-Israeli mediation. All of our variables,

including the outcome of every mediation effort are treated as continuous rather than

dichotomous factors. Coding our variables and indeed the outcome as either

present/absent or success/failure would have been a too blunt instrument which would not

account for degrees of success, longevity, significance, etc. Treating all our variables as

continuous allows for a more accurate survey of factors and a more nuanced analysis of

results. Given that we are dealing with events data such as the ranking of issues in terms

of complexity and historical significance, the status ranking of mediators and so on -

which requires coding or ‗translating‘ of qualitative categories into quantitative values

(sometimes with limited sets of categories) – we subject all of our data to inter-coder

reliability (Ruggeri et. al. 2009).

12
Determinants of mediation

There have been several attempts by academics to capture the range of the mediation

experience, and specifically the desired attributes of the ideal mediator (Wall and Lynn,

1993; Wall, Stark and Standifer, 2001). Some of these determinants do not require

rigorous academic scrutiny to be of logical relevance to mediation success. It is self-

evident, for example, that the mediator‘s intelligence, tact, tenacity, and other

interpersonal skills are conducive (though not sufficient) to successful mediation. Other

factors, however, require a more scrupulous examination. As noted above, we identified

24 variables which we believe capture the essence of mediation in the AIC in the past six

decades, divided into the following categories: contextual, mediator’s capacity and

procedural. Similar typologies are common in the literature (Frei, 1976; Touval, 1982;

Bercovitch and Langley, 1993; Kleiboer, 1996), and they all share the assumption that the

outcome of any instance of mediation can be understood in terms of the combination of

the context of the conflict, the qualities of the mediator and the process of mediation.

i. Contextual factors

The literature is rather vague on the effect of contextual variables on the outcome of

mediation. Some suggest that protracted and intense disputes are more difficult to resolve

by mediation (Frei, 1976; Bercovitch, 1991), while others contend that the disputants will

be more likely to accept outside mediation in the face of spiralling violence (Young,

1967; Beardsley et. al., 2005). The context of mediation refers to systemic variables

which mostly determine the degree of manoeuvrability of the mediator before the process

has even begun. Thus in the context of the AIC for example, the nature of the dispute and

13
the issues to be resolved are independent to the identity of the mediator. Moreover, past

relations between the disputants, their balance of power or the historical, political,

religious and ideological idiosyncrasies of the disputed issues may often invite certain

types of mediators while excluding others. In this context, a cursory review of the

diplomatic history of the AIC reveals the predominance of U.S.-led mediation efforts

since the late 1960s and the marginal role of other external actors, such as the United

Nations.

The nature of conflict/disputed issues is considered in the literature to have

significant bearing on the success or failure of mediation. The importance the parties

attach to a disputed issue will have a bearing on the identity of the mediator, the

mediator‘s choice of strategies, and ultimately the likelihood of success. Ott (1972: 616)

for example, argues that the ‗absence of vital national security interests, particularly

questions of territorial control‘ is no less than a necessary condition for mediation

success. In a similar fashion, Randle (1973: 49) suggests that ‗should a dispute affect

vital security interests of the parties, no amount of mediation by a third party is likely to

prevent the outbreak of hostilities.‘ Historically, the AIC has revolved around the

following core issues: Jerusalem, security/borders, refugees and settlements. The entire

range of issues cannot be treated dichotomously however, with these core issues situated

directly opposite ‗softer‘ issues such as redeployment of forces or economic cooperation.

For example, it can be argued that control of water resources in the Arab-Israeli conflict

is just as urgent as the core issues, however over the years it has been seen as being of

secondary importance by mediators and disputants alike, though in the long-term its

resolution is as imperative as that of the core issues. To reflect this complexity in terms of

14
our operationalisation and analysis of the data, as a first step we identify the issues that

existed at the time of a particular mediation event. As a second step, we rate each issue

contained within a specific mediation event on the basis of its complexity and historical

significance, using a scale rating of 0 – 100 (100 representing maximum complexity and

maximum historical significance). Thirdly, to provide an additional layer of context, we

calculate total (mean average) issues score for each specific mediation event. Our

hypothesis will test the notion that mediation success is dependent on how ‗hard‘ or ‗soft‘

the disputed issues are perceived to be by the parties.

Hypothesis A1: Mediations that address the core issues of the AIC (Jerusalem,
security/borders, refugees and settlements) are less likely to be successful than those
addressing issues perceived as ‘softer’ by the parties

The issue of balance of power (or power symmetry) between the disputants is less

contested in the literature, with most observers agree that as long as one party feels that it

has the upper hand, it will not be inclined to engage productively with mediation, and

therefore rough parity is crucial for achieving compromise (Young, 1967; Kressel, 1981;

Touval, 1982). By asymmetrical conflict we mean a conflict where one party has a

territorial or military advantage, or that its history of previous relationship with the

mediator makes its position at the negotiation table more powerful (see for example the

blatant asymmetry in power between Israel and the PLO during the Clinton years).

Asymmetry in power is therefore expected to have a bearing on the process, and perhaps

even the outcome, of mediation (Aggestam 2002; Quinn et. al. 2006).

Hypothesis A2: Mediations where rough power symmetry exists between the
parties are more likely to be successful than those where there is clear power
asymmetry between the parties

15
The contextual variable of timing in mediation is perhaps more difficult to

conceptualise. It is not static like the nature of the conflict or the issues at hand – these

rarely change in essence. Here, however, the initiation of the mediation process is

determined by the disputants and the mediator. This creates endless opportunities during

the life of the conflict, leading to the assumption that choosing the right entry point can

sometimes make the difference between success and failure in mediation (Stein and

Lewis, 1996). However there is no consensus in the literature regarding the most

opportune time for mediators to enter the conflict, and how to distinguish points in time

as more or less ‗ripe‘ for mediation (Crocker, Hampson and Aall, 2003). Whilst it has

been suggested that ‗knowing when to use mediation may be more important than how

often it is used‘ (Bercovitch, 1997: 145), assessing when is the most propitious time for

mediation is a difficult task, and various studies have attempted to identify the ripe

moment for mediation (Kleiboer, 1994; Zartman, 1985, 2001; Rubin, 1992; Haass, 1990;

Lyons and Khadiagala, 2008). Much like Abba Eban‘s famous quip, that men and nations

behave wisely once they have exhausted all other alternatives, we argue that mediation is

more likely to succeed when both disputants have decided that the continuing costs of the

status quo are no longer tolerable, or in other words, when the disputants have come to

the conclusion that they will be better off with a settlement than without one. Some

contend that this realisation only comes late in the conflict, and therefore mediators

should enter after perceptions of crisis or emergency have increased disputants‘

incentives to accept mediation (Rubin, 1981). Others propose that mediation needs to be

firmly initiated before such threshold of violence is reached and perceptions of the other

side have hardened (Edmead, 1971). A ripe moment opens for the mediator a window of

16
time which provides the best opportunity to succeed. To assess ripeness, the notions of

conflict cycle and hurting stalemate are often used in the literature. A conflict cycle

suggests that most conflicts are characterised by sequential phases, namely rising of

tensions, confrontation, and the outbreak of hostilities, followed by military clashes. The

de-escalatory phases may include a cease-fire, settlement, rapprochement, and

reconciliation (Bercovitch and Langley, 1993; Crocker et. al., 1999). In the AIC the

notion of (phases of) a conflict cycle is difficult to employ, as reversals in the conflict

cycle, or ‗leaps‘ between escalatory and de-escalatory phases are common. For example,

the 1990s saw the signing of several Israeli-Palestinian agreements, accompanied almost

simultaneously by unprecedented levels of violence and hostility on both sides. This

leaves us with the concept of a hurting stalemate as perhaps the best benchmark to gauge

when mediation should be initiated and when it is likely to succeed. Military defeats,

changes in power relations or the threat of a deadline may convince the disputants that

mediation is the best way out of a worsening situation. Linked to the concept of conflict

ripeness is a process of ‗softening up‘ by the disputants, that is to say that parties have

shifted from intransigence to willingness. This process can follow the introduction of

contextual conflict factors which are more amenable to settlement, or the successful

application of appropriate strategies by the mediator (Greig and Diehl, 2006). The

softening up of the parties on its own however, is an insufficient condition for ripening of

the conflict as a whole, as it is exogenous to factors such as structural constraints or the

willingness of the mediator itself to mediate. In our data, ‗timing‘ refers to the mediator‘s

point of entry into the conflict, namely during an escalatory phase (coded 1-4, 4

17
indicating the highest level of escalation) or de-escalatory phase (coded -1 to -4, -4

representing the reconciliation phase).

Hypothesis A3: Mediations that occur during an escalatory phase in the conflict
cycle are more likely to be successful than those occurring during a de-escalatory
phase

ii. Mediator’s capacity

As noted above, the mediator may assume numerous behaviours, roles and functions

during the mediation process. Wall alone identifies more than a hundred specific

mediator traits (Wall, 1981), while Stulberg (1987) provides a more modest list of nine

mediator‘s roles, ranging from a ‗catalyst‘ to a ‗scapegoat‘. Others provide useful

typologies of mediator roles and strategies (Zartman and Touval, 1985). Whatever the

method used to evaluate these qualities, it is generally understood that the mediator‘s

power (leverage) and perceived impartiality are of utmost relevance to its chances of

success. The vast majority of definitions of mediation include either the mediator‘s active

use of leverage, or its perceived impartiality on the disputed issue. Furthermore, power

and impartiality are intractably linked — impartiality being a potential source of

leverage, while power deriving from other sources may lessen the need for impartiality

(Smith, 1994). Finally, there is some agreement in the literature that the mediator‘s status

has significant bearing on the chances of success.

Power, or leverage in mediation refers to the mediator‘s ability to move a party in

its intended direction (Zartman and Touval, 1996: 455). However by its voluntary, non-

binding and non-violent nature, the mediation process ensures that the ultimate power lies

with the disputants – they have the power to initiate the process, and they hold the right to

terminate it. Explicitly then, a first prerequisite for the initiation, let alone success, of any

18
mediation effort is that the mediator is accepted by the disputing parties. Therefore the

ability to satisfy the expectations of both parties through the use of various mediation

techniques and strategies is paramount. The more powerful the mediator is, the more

resources it can employ to change the disputants‘ attitudes and perceptions. The literature

also suggests that powerful mediators who use active strategies and enjoy a wide range of

resources are more effective at managing intense conflicts, whereas mediators with lower

profile who offer more passive engagement and have limited access to resources can do

better at managing less intractable conflicts (Bercovitch and Gartner, 2006). Whilst some

see the use of manipulative strategies as sitting outside the spectrum of mediation (Kydd,

2006; Skjelsbaek, 1991), in reality the use of deprivation or gratification strategies is

accepted and even expected by the disputants, especially when the third party is a major

power with an interest in the conflict. Moreover, even manipulative mediators often

engage with some sort of consultation and facilitation, and as such their activities should

be viewed across a wide spectrum of options rather than dichotomously (Schrodt and

Gerner, 2004). Great powers can be particularly effective in mediating conflicts, as they

are ‗uniquely positioned to manipulate the bargaining range in a way that ensures both

disputants can be reasonably satisfied with the outcome‘ (Favretto, 2009: 259). On the

other hand, Quinn and his colleagues find that power as an independent variable has little

effect on the successful outcome of mediated crises, but rather acts indirectly through the

identity and status of the mediator (Quinn et. al., 2006). In any case, as mediation is a

voluntary and non-violent process, the use of force by such powers – for example by

joining sides with one of the disputants as means to bring about a settlement – needs then

to be analysed as a more coercive form of third party intervention. While the advantages

19
of a powerful mediator who maintains leverage in the disputed issue are evident, this is

not to suggest that less powerful mediators cannot be successful. Power is essential only

to powerful mediators; less powerful mediators such as individuals or NGOs cannot offer

the disputants material incentives or to deprive them of certain resources, but their

perceived impartiality on the disputed issue is the decisive factor that makes them

acceptable to the disputants.

In considering the power of the mediator, we assess how much leverage over the

disputants the mediator had coming into the process. In cases where mediators are

principally associated with a particular national government then it is likely that the

mediator may have more power over one disputant and not the other, for example. Our

variable seeks to examine the extent to which this discrepancy is important. Accordingly,

we distinguish mediators in terms of possessing power over all disputants, over Israel

only, over Arab disputants only or over none of the disputants.

Hypothesis B1: Mediations where the mediator has leverage over all the
disputants are more likely to be successful than those where the mediator has
leverage over some of the disputants or none of the disputants

Impartiality is perhaps the most contentious aspect of mediation. In essence impartiality

(or bias) is a matter of perception, rather than reality. Whereas some studies argue that

mediation, by definition, requires the mediator to be impartial (Burton & Dukes, 1990;

Skjelsbaek, 1991), others maintain that impartiality is not a necessary prerequisite for

successful mediation and in some cases can even get in the way (Touval, 1982;

Bercovitch, 1986; Princen, 1992; Savun, 2008). Smith and Stam (2003) find that biased

mediators (defined as biased towards war, peace, or one of the disputants) cannot relay

information effectively to the warring parties and are not perceived as honest brokers,

20
whereas Kydd (2003) finds that biased mediators can perform this task effectively.

Rauchhaus (2006), however, contends that both biased mediators and impartial mediators

are expected to serve as effective mediators, though impartial mediators are expected to

perform their task better. Whereas the vast majority of mediation studies tend to treat bias

dichotomously – the mediator is either biased or not towards the conflict and/or the

disputants, Savun (2008) makes the important distinction between a mediator‘s absolute

and relative bias, the latter is derived from the mediator‘s relationship with one disputant

in relative terms to its history or relations with the other disputant. In our study we go one

step further and treat bias as a continuous variable, as it is conceivable that on some

issues the mediator‘s attitudes may change over time or indeed when multi-party

mediation is involved, a more nuanced manifestation of bias collectively may be

exhibited. As noted earlier, the mediator‘s tasks of eliciting information and exercising

influence are often best achieved not when a mediator is impartial, but when it possesses

certain resources that are valued by the disputants (Zartman and Touval, 1985). Perceived

impartiality is thus more crucial in the cases of less powerful mediators. Their mandate to

mediate rests upon the assumption – and expectation – that they will not favour one party

during the process. Conversely, in cases where the mediator is more powerful,

impartiality is not expected by the disputants; as Smith concludes, ‗the relevant

characteristic for power mediators is their power, not their impartiality‘ (1994: 447).

To measure bias, we contend that a biased mediator is one who has closer ties with

one party than the other and the greater the discrepancy in its relations with the parties,

the greater the bias. We measure this variable using a scale of 0-100, with 100 being

maximum discrepancy.

21
Hypothesis B2: Mediations where the mediator has a perceived bias towards one
of the disputants is more likely to be successful than those where the mediator is
perceived to be unbiased towards all the disputants

Finally, the status of the mediator may have a considerable impact on the process and the

chances of success. The status of the mediator includes important issues such as

knowledge of the conflict and understanding of its political and historical roots; expertise

in negotiation; the formal rank of the individual mediator in his or her home state or

organisation; and the credibility which derives from these factors (Pruitt, 2002; Svensson,

2009). At least implicitly, mediator‘s high status is linked to issues of credibility and

trust, and thus has bearing not only on the disputants‘ decision to accept the mediator in

the first place, but also on their incentives to cooperate with the mediator (Kleiboer,

1996; Maoz and Terris 2006).

In terms of conceptualising status as an operational variable we can distinguish two

key facets of status: experience and positional status rank. By experience, we mean the

years of diplomatic experience a mediator has and the number of mediations a mediator

has been involved in. In the AIC it is safe to assume that the office of the president of the

United States garners the highest status rank irrespective of previous diplomatic

experience or mediation expertise. For example Bill Clinton would receive a positional

status rank score higher than that of Dennis Ross, his special envoy, even though Ross

had considerably more experience in dealing with the Israelis and Palestinians. We will

score status on a scale of 0 – 100, with 100 representing maximum status (US

Presidents). For experience, we will use a composite measure comprising years of

previous diplomatic experience and number of previous mediations.

22
Hypothesis B3a: Mediations where the mediator has a high status rank are more
likely to be successful than those where the mediator has a lower status rank

Hypothesis B3b: Mediations where the mediator/ mediation team has a high level
experience are more likely to be successful than those where the mediator/
mediation team has limited experience

iii. Procedural variables

What strategies are available to the mediator? Should the objectives of mediation be

limited to specific issues which are relatively easy to negotiate, or be ambitious and

comprehensive in order to tackle the root causes of the conflict? Can we expect secret

negotiations to be more effective than a process conducted in front of the watchful eyes

of the public and the media? Evidently these are important issues, and scholars of

mediation are split over the significance of these variables to a successful outcome.

The choice of mediation strategies/tactics is linked to the mediator‘s resources, and

thus powerful mediators enjoy a wider arsenal of strategies and tactics than weak

mediators. Carnevale‘s (2002) distinction between strategic and tactical strength in

mediation is useful here. The first refers to the ‗social power that relates to the resources

and relationships that the mediator brings to the conflict‘ (p. 28), whereas the latter

‗refers to what the mediator does at the negotiating table; this involves adroit

manoeuvring, technique, and procedure. (2002, p. 30). We argue that a mediator who is

more strategically equipped will also have more tactical manoeuvrability at the

negotiation table. The literature on mediation offers several typologies of mediation

strategies which in essence offer similar insights into the strategic choices mediators face

at various stages of the process. Perhaps the most widely used typology is the one offered

by Zartman and Touval (1985), who suggest three discrete strategies of mediation: a)

23
communication-facilitation, b) formulation, c) manipulation. As the mediator offers the

parties something they cannot achieve themselves, each strategy aims to overcome a

particular obstacle during the mediation process. Communication strategies are

comparatively passive and procedural in nature, such as the opening of channels of

communication and carrying messages. Formulation strategies invite a more active role

for the mediator in putting forward its own ideas and drafting parameters for reference.

Lastly, the manipulative mediator may change the bargaining process by offering or

denying the disputants certain incentives. However the risk here is that by its sheer power

to push the disputants towards a settlement, the mediator becomes a party to the solution,

or even to the conflict itself. As Kleiboer correctly observes, the distinction between

communicator and formulator is more notional than real, as these roles may overlap

(1996: 375). Beardsley et al (2006) find that the use of a manipulative strategy by the

mediator is more likely to result in a formal agreement, and we build on this assumption

to develop three hypotheses. First, we assign (approximate) percentages to the three

strategies (communicative, formulative and manipulative) for each of the particular

mediation events, to see if a dominant strategy (or the absence of a particular strategy) is

significant. This latter point indicates that not all three strategies will necessarily be used

at every mediation event. Second, in what we term the chess test, we propose that the

opening strategy of the mediator might have a significant effect on the outcome. Finally,

it is likely that mediators alternate between strategies throughout the mediation process,

and we suggest that this may have an impact on the outcome.

Hypothesis C1a: Mediations which involve a manipulative strategy are more


likely to be successful than those where other strategies are used

24
Hypothesis C1b: The opening mediation strategy will be significant as to the
likelihood of achieving success

Hypothesis C1c: The sequence of strategies employed by the mediator will be


significant as to the likelihood of achieving success

Choosing a mediation strategy is often determined by the objectives of the mediation

process. According to Bercovitch (2002) the three underlying objectives of the mediator

include the changing of the physical environment, the changing of perceptions on both

sides of what is at stake, and the changing of the disputants‘ motivation to reach a

settlement. The first two are closely associated with communicative/formulative

strategies of mediation, as these objectives entail issues such as secrecy, agenda setting,

redefinition of issues, etc. However in order to change disputants‘ motivation, the

mediator must have the capacity and willpower to actively manipulate the disputants and

to restructure the dynamics of the bargaining process. In practical terms, the mediator

may opt for a comprehensive settlement, or pursue a more limited objective upon

initiating the mediation process. Although both types of strategies can be applied to

achieve either comprehensive or limited objectives, it is not readily evident whether

limited objectives, for example, are best achieved when communicative/formulative

strategies are used, or whether manipulative strategies are more effective in this instance.

In their study of mediation in the Middle East, Stein and Lewis suggest that

‗comprehensive approaches to mediating this conflict have ultimately failed‘ (1996: 471).

A comprehensive approach is understood here as the attempt to bring about a lasting

peace treaty which officially ends the state of hostility between Israel and the Arab world,

whereas a limited approach may either be of a more gradualist nature (‗step-by-step‘) or

concentrate on a specific issue (water, redeployment of forces, etc).

25
Hypothesis C2: Mediations where the objectives are limited are more likely to be
successful than those where the objectives are comprehensive.

Finally, the complex nature of the mediation process and the disputants‘ desire to save

face may call for secrecy, or ‗sidebar‘ negotiations (Rubin, Pruitt and Kim 1994: 188-

189). Secret or private negotiations offer the mediator and the disputants greater

manoeuvrability and in the event of failure, the option of deniability. However this is not

to suggest that secret negotiations are necessarily more effective than public ones.

According to Zartman and Berman, the answer is somewhere in the middle: ―‘Too great‘

publicity for the negotiations is inefficient, for it draws negotiators to the windows to

address the public rather than each other, and makes for inflexible positions‖, whereas

―‘too little‘ publicity is considered undemocratic, may lead to shady deals, and in the end

is probably inefficient too, since at some point in the process of ratification the press and

other media will comment on the agreement and bring it under scrutiny‖ (1982: 215-

216). The unavoidable conclusion, then, is that ‘the argument in favour of secrecy in

negotiation is right, even if that does not make the argument in favour of informing the

public wrong‘ (1982: 217).

Hypothesis C3: Mediations where the process is secret are more likely to be
successful than those which are public knowledge.

Findings and analysis

An inspection of the Pearson correlations in Table 1 reveals that six variables have a

correlation with mediation outcome ranging from strong in strength (significant at the

0.01 level) to modest in strength (significant at the 0.10 level). In order, beginning with

the strongest correlation these are: C1 Manipulation Strategy; A1 Forces redeployment;

26
B3 Main Mediator Status; C2 Objectives of mediation; B1 Mediator Power coming into

the mediation; and C1 Opening Strategy.

Table 1: Correlation Analysis of Mediation Outcome

Dependent Variable: Mediation Outcome


A: Contextual Factors Pearson’s B: Interpersonal Pearson’s C: Procedural Pearson’s
R Factors R Factors R
A1 Territory 0.060 B1 Mediator 0.355* C1 Strategy -0.011
Power coming in Communication
A1 refugees -0.063 B2 Mediator Bias -0.022 C1 Strategy -0.274
Formulation
A1 Holy Sites -0.113 B3 Main 0.405** C1 Strategy 0.566***
Mediator Status Manipulation
A1 water -0.004 B3 Experience 0.073 C1 Opening -0.352*
Strategy
A1 ceasefire 0.148 B3 Mediations 0.316 C1 Strategy -.221
Sequence
A1 Forces re- 0.531*** B3 days mediating -0.009 C2 Objectives of 0.404*
deployment Mediation
A1 settlements 0.138 B3 Team Status 0.305 C3 Visibility -0.259
A1 complex-history 0.283
average
A2 Balance of power -0.233
A3 Timing -0.163

* Significant at a .10 level; ** Significant at a .05 level; *** Significant at a .01 level

Pictorially, we can see a fairly tight fit between the mediation outcome, strategy of

manipulation and main mediator status in Chart 1 and again a reasonably tight fit between

mediation outcome and objectives of mediation in Chart 2. It appears, therefore, that

there is initial support for at least one hypothesis in each of our categories. In the

contextual category there is initial support for Hypothesis A1 (nature of the conflict), as it

is abundantly clear that none of the ‗core‘ issues of the AIC is, statistically speaking,

positively associated with successful mediation outcomes. In the mediator‘s capacity

category there is initial support for Hypothesis B1 (mediator power coming into the

mediation) and B3a (status rank of the main mediator). In the procedural category there is

27
initial support for C1a (mediator employment of a manipulative strategy), C1b (opening

strategy) and C2 (limited objectives for the mediation event).

28
We begin our OLS regression analysis by testing all six of our statistically significant

factors identified above. We find that the two most significant independent variables in

our correlation analysis – strategy of manipulation and forces redeployment - remain the

most important variables within the multiple regression analysis, and are joined by the

variable C2 Objectives of mediation. The multiple regression model comprising these

three variables (Regression Model 1, see Table 2) is statistically significant. The value of

adjusted R² in this model is 0.459, which tells us that the overall model accounts for 45.9

per cent of the variation in mediation outcome. The model‘s F-ratio is 7.501, which is

significant at p < 0.001. The F-ratio is a measure of how much our model has improved

the prediction of the outcome compared to the level of inaccuracy in the model. If a

model is good, then we expect the improvement in the prediction to be large and the

difference between the model and the observed data to be small. A good model should

have a large F-ratio (greater than one at least). Our result tells us that there is less than a

0.1% chance that an F-ratio this large would happen by chance alone. Therefore, we can

conclude that the regression model results in a significantly better prediction of mediation

outcome than if we used the mean value of mediation outcome. Succinctly, the overall

regression model predicts outcome of mediation significantly well.

The Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) tells us whether the overall model is a good

predictor of the dependent variable, but cannot tell us about the individual contribution of

independent variables in the model. For this we turn to the standardised Beta scores in

Table 3. An examination of the Beta scores reveals that C1 Strategy of Manipulation is

the most significant individual independent variable (standardised beta score 0.411**).

This confirms Hypothesis C1a which contends that mediations which involve a

29
manipulative strategy are more likely to be successful than those where other strategies

are used.

Second in terms of individual statistical significance is A1 Forces re-deployment.

The presence of this issue, but not any of the AIC core issues – Jerusalem, borders,

settlements and refugees – gives support to the claim that greater success in mediation is

more likely when the parties are dealing with issues perceived as being ‗softer‘. The

issue of forces redeployment is present in every single successful mediation outcome

ranked here as a mediation outcome 4 or mediation outcome 5. Finally, C2 Objectives of

mediation is statistically significant. Greater success in terms of mediation outcome is

clearly linked with limited objectives, which supports Hypothesis C2.

Table 2: Regression Model 1


Dependent Variable: Mediation Outcome
Beta (Standardised) t-scores
(Constant) (-0.430)
C1 Strategy of Manipulation 0.411** 2.532
A1 Forces re-deployment 0.345** 2.104
C2 Objectives of Mediation 0.291* 1.863

Adjusted R square 0.459


Number of cases 24
* Significant at a .10 level; ** Significant at a .05 level; *** Significant at a .01 level

From this we are now in position to formulate the least squares regression model in terms

of an equation. The equation is derived whereby each independent variable has its own

co-efficient, and the dependent variable has its own co-efficient. The dependent variable

is then predicted from a combination of all the variables multiplied by their respective

coefficients plus a residual term, where Y is the dependent variable, β1 is the coefficient

30
of the first independent variable (X1), β2 is the coefficient of the of the second

independent variable (X2), βn is the coefficient of the of the nth independent variable

(Xn) and εi is the difference between the predicted and observed value of Y for the ith

subject.

Y = β0 + β1X1 + β2X2 +....+ βnXn + ε i

Table 2 indicates that the constant (β0) is -0.430, which is the Y intercept. Therefore our
equation for the regression model is:

Mediation Outcome = -0.430 + 2.532 Manipulation + 2.104 A1Forces +1.863


C2Objectives + εi

So, to summarise, Regression model 1 is a statistically significant predictor of mediation


outcome from which three independent variables – C1 Strategy of Manipulation, A1
Forces Re-deployment and C2 Objectives of Mediation – are shown to be statistically
significant independent predictors of mediation outcome. This allows us to claim that the
following hypotheses are supported by the regression model:
Hypothesis A1: Mediations that address the core issues of the AIC (Jerusalem,
security/borders, refugees and settlements) are less likely to be successful than
those addressing issues perceived as ‘softer’ by the parties

Hypothesis C1a: Mediations which involve a manipulative strategy are more


likely to be successful than those where other strategies are used.

Hypothesis C2: Mediations where the objectives are limited are more likely to be
successful than those where the objectives are comprehensive.

Perhaps the one slightly surprising result of the multiple regression analysis is the

non-significance of B3 Main Mediator Status, when the initial correlation results

indicated statistical significance at the 0.05 level. Undoubtedly, this is due to the issue of

31
multicollinearity, which increases the probability that a good predictor of the dependent

variable is found to be non-significant and rejected from the model (a type II error). As is

indicated by Chart 2, C1 Strategy of Manipulation is strongly correlated with B3 Main

Mediator Status (r = 0.595***).

It is likely that the variance accounted for by main mediator status is, to a considerable

degree, the same variance that is accounted for by manipulative strategy. This is a logical

deduction, given that it is clear from Chart 2 that in all cases where a strategy of

manipulation is employed the status of the main mediator is ranked at a minimum of 50

per cent, and in all but one of those cases the main mediator status is ranked at a

minimum of 75 per cent. It is clear, too, that causation in this case is one-way. That is to

say, it is high status ranking mediators who possess the capacity to employ a

manipulative strategy when they deem necessary; it is not the strategy of manipulation

32
that results in mediators having a high status rank. In all seven cases where we have

ranked mediation outcome as 4 or 5, a strategy of manipulation has been used.

We thus run a second Regression model where we create a composite variable

comprising B3 Main Mediator Status and C1 Strategy of Manipulation to capture the fact

that the higher the status rank of the mediator the greater the probability they will employ

a manipulative strategy. We call this new variable StatusManipulation. Correlation

analysis indicates that there is a statistical association with our dependent variable

Mediation Outcome, significant at the 0.01 level (r = 0.548***).

When running an OLS regression analysis using the other statistically significant

factors identified in Regression Model 1, we find that Regression Model 2 is statistically

significant. The value of adjusted R² is 0.439, meaning that the overall model accounts

for 43.9 per cent of the variation in mediation outcome. The model‘s F-ratio is 6.994,

which is significant at p < 0.001. So once again we can conclude that Regression Model 2

results in a significantly better prediction of mediation outcome than if we used the mean

value of mediation outcome. An examination of the Beta scores in Table 4 reveals that

StatusManipulation is the most significant individual independent variable (standardised

beta score 0.383**). This indicates that the higher positional status rank of the main

mediator the higher the likelihood they will employ a strategy of manipulation and the

greater the probability of a successful mediation outcome resulting in a formal

agreement. Second in terms of individual importance is A1Forces (0.360**), again

confirming support for hypothesis A1. Finally, C2Objectivies of Mediation remains

statistically significant (0.318*). Greater success in terms of mediation outcome remains

33
clearly linked with limited objectives, which again confirms support for Hypothesis C2.

The regression equation is as follows:

Mediation Outcome = -0.598 + 2.338 StatusManipulation + 2.167 A1Forces + 1.998


C2Objectives + εi

Whilst this second regression model is of interest and is a statistically significant

predictor of the outcome of mediation it is not a strong a predictor of mediation outcome

as Regression Model 1, and explains less of the variance of mediation outcome than

Regression Model 1.

Table 3: Regression model 2


Dependent Variable: Mediation
Outcome
Beta (Standardised) t-scores
(Constant) (-0.598)
Status Manipulation 0.382** 2.338
A1 Forces 0.360** 2.167
C2 Objectives of Mediation 0.318* 1.998
Adjusted R square 0.439
Number of cases 24
* Significant at a .10 level; ** Significant at a .05 level; *** Significant at a .01 level

Conclusion

The most important finding from our bivariate correlation analysis and the various

multiple regression models is that successful mediation outcomes are more probable

when mediators are able to employ a strategy of manipulation, thus supporting the

findings in previous studies (Beardsley et. al. 2006). Our claim that a strategy of

34
manipulation is more likely to be employed if the main mediator is of higher status rank

is also borne out by the statistical analysis. More pertinently to current efforts at

mediating Israeli-Palestinian peace, we found significant support for the claim that

mediations addressing the core issues of the conflict (Jerusalem, borders, refugees and

settlements) are less likely to be successful than those addressing issues perceived as

‗softer‘ by the parties. Similarly, our finding that mediation success is evidently

associated with limited objectives can provide important contextualisation to recent

(failed) efforts at ending the conflict via comprehensive plans, such as the 2002 Arab

League initiative.

These important findings notwithstanding, there remains the caveat that due to the

relatively small number of cases that exist in this conflict, identifying potential

explanatory variables must be regarded as provisional. However given the empirical

validity of some of our hypotheses in the face of the idiosyncratic nature of mediation

over six decades of conflict between several actors and numerous mediators, we

nevertheless maintain that the use of quantitative methodology to explain social

interaction offers important insight which is rarely observable in single-case study

analysis or purely historical studies.

Sources

Aggestam, K. 2002. ‗Mediating Asymmetrical Conflict‘, Mediterranean Politics 7:1, pp.


69-91.

35
Beardsley, K. 2010. ‗Pain, Pressure and Political cover: Explaining Mediation Incidence‘,
Journal of Peace Research 47:4, pp. 395-407.

Beardsley, K. C., D. M. Quinn, B. Biswas, and J. Wilkenfeld. 2006. ‘Mediation Style and
Crisis Outcomes‘, Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50:1, 58-86.

Bercovitch, J. 1986. ‗International mediation: A Study of the Incidence, Strategies and


Conditions of Successful Outcomes‘. Cooperation and Conflict 21:3, 155-168.

Bercovitch, J. 1991. ‗International Mediation and Dispute Settlement: Evaluating the


Conditions for Successful Mediation‘. Journal of Peace Research 7:1, 17-30.

Bercovitch, J. 1997. ‗Mediation in International Conflict: An Overview of Theory, A


Review of Practice‘, in I. W. Zartman and J. L. Rasmussen (eds.), Peacemaking in
International Conflict: Methods & Techniques. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Institute of
Peace Press, 125-153.

Bercovitch, J. 2002. ‗Introduction: Putting Mediation in Context‘, in J. Bercovitch (ed.),


Studies in International Mediation. Basingstoke: Macmillan, 3-24.

Bercovitch, J. 2005. ‗Social Research and the Study of Mediation: Designing and
Implementing Systematic Archival Research‘, International Negotiation 9:3, pp.
415-428.

Bercovitch, J. and P. F. Diehl. 1997. ‗Conflict Management of Enduring Rivalries: The


Frequency, Timing, and Short-term Impact of Mediation‘, International
Interactions 22:4, pp. 299-320.

Bercovitch, J. and J. Fretter. 2004. Regional Guide to International Conflict and


Management from 1945 to 2003. Washington, D.C.:CQ Press.

Bercovitch, J. and A. Houston. 1996. ‗The Study of International Mediation: Theoretical


Issues and Empirical Evidence‘, in J. Bercovitch (ed.), Resolving International
Conflicts. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner, 11-35.

Bercovitch, J. and J. Langley. 1993. ‗The Nature of the Dispute and the Effectiveness of
International Mediation‘. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 37:4, 670-691.

Bercovitch, J. and S. Gartner. 2006. ‗Is There Method in the Madness of Mediation?
Some Lessons for Mediators from Quantitative Studies of Mediation‘, International
Interactions 32:4, pp. 329-354.

Bercovitch, J. and L. Simpson. 2010. ‗International Mediation and the Question of Failed
Peace Agreements: Improving Conflict Management and Implementation‘, Peace
& Change 35:1, pp. 68-102.

36
Berridge, G. R. 2002. Diplomacy: Theory and Practice. Basingstoke: Palgrave.

Burton, J. and Dukes, F. 1990. Conflict: Practices, Settlement, and Resolution. London:
Macmillan.

Carment, D., Y. Samy and S. El Achkar. 2009. ‗Protracted Conflict and Crisis Mediation:
A Contingency Approach‘, in J. Bercovitch and S. S. Gartner (eds.), International
Conflict Mediation: New Approaches and Findings (London: Routledge, 2009), pp.
216-239.

Carnevale, P. J. 2002. ‗Mediating from Strength‘, in J. Bercovitch (ed.), Studies in


International Mediation. Basingstoke: Macmillan, 25-40.

Crocker, C. A., F. O. Hampson, and P. R. All. 2003. ‗Ready for Prime Time: The When,
Who and Why of International Mediation‘, Negotiation Journal, 19: 2, 151-167.

Edmead, F. 1971. Analysis and Prediction in International Mediation. New York:


UNITAR Study.

Eisenberg, L., and N. Caplan. 1998. Negotiating Arab-Israeli Peace: Patterns, Problems,
Possibilities. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Favretto, K. 2009. ‗Should Peacemakers Take Sides? Major Power Mediation, Coercion,
and Bias‘, American Political Science Review 103:2, pp. 248-263.

Fisher, R. J. and L. Keashly. 1991. ‗The Potential Complementarily of Mediation and


Consultation within a Contingency Model of Third Party Intervention‘. Journal of
Peace Research, 28:1, 29-42.

Frei, D. 1976. ‗Conditions Affecting the Effectiveness of International Mediation‘, Peace


Science Society (International) Papers, no. 26, 67-84.

Goertz, G. and P. F. Diehl. 1993. ‗Enduring Rivals: theoretical Constructs and Empirical
Patterns‘, International Interactions 2:2, pp. 57-78.

Greig, M. 2005. ‗Stepping Into the Fray: When Do Mediators Mediate? American
Journal of Political Science 49:2, pp. 249-266.

Greig, M. and P. F. Diehl. 2006. ‗Softening Up: Making Conflicts More Amenable to
Diplomacy‘ , International Interactions 32:4, pp. 355-384.

Haass, R. N. 1990. Conflicts Unending: The United States and Regional Disputes. New
Haven: Yale University Press.

37
Hopmann, P. T. 1995. ‗Two Paradigms of Negotiation: Bargaining and Problem
Solving‘. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, no. 542,
24-47.

Katz, J. 1999. ‘Toward a Comprehensive Model for the Assessment of


Intraorganizational Conflict: developing the frameowrk‘, Journal of Conflict
Management 10:1, pp. 27-44.

Kleiboer, M. 1994. ‗Ripeness of Conflict: A Fruitful Notion?‘. Journal of Peace


Research, 31:1, 109-116.

Kleiboer, M. 1996. ‗Understanding Success and Failure of International Mediation‘.


Journal of Conflict Resolution, 40:2, 360-389.

Kressel, K., and D. Pruitt. 1989. Mediation Research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Kydd, A. 2003. ‗Which Side Are You On? Bias, Credibility, and Mediation‘, American
Journal of Political Science 47:4, pp. 597-611.

Kydd, A. 2006. ‗When Can Mediators Build Trust?‘, American Political Science Review
100:3, pp. 449-462.

Kriesberg, L. 1991. ‗Formal and Quasi-Mediators in International Disputes: An


Explanatory Analysis‘. Journal of Peace Research, 28:1, 19-27.

Maoz, Z. and L. Terris. 2006. ‗Credibility and Strategy in International Mediation‘,


International Interactions 32:4, pp. 409-440.

Meyer, A. 1960. ‗Functions of the Mediator in Collective Bargaining‘. Industrial and


Labour Relations Review 13:161, 156-165.

Mitchell, C. R. 1981. Peacemaking and the Consultant’s Role. Westmead: Gower.

Princen, T. 1992. Intermediaries in International Conflict. Princeton: Princeton


University Press.

Pruitt, D. G. 2002. ‗Mediator Behaviour and Success in Mediation‘, in J. Bercovitch


(ed.), Studies in International Mediation. Basingstoke: Palgrave, 41-54.

Quinn, D., J. Wilkenfeld; K. Smarick, and V. Asal. 2006. ‗Power Play: Mediation in
Symmetric and Asymmetric International Crises‘, International Interactions 32:4,
pp. 441-470.

Ruggeri, A., Gizelis, T-I., and Dorussen, H. 2009. ‗Events Data as Bismarck‘s Sausages?
Intercoder Reliability, Coders Selection and Data Quality‘. Paper presented at the
ISA 50th Annual Convention in New York, 15-18 February.

38
Rubin, J. Z. 1981. Dynamics of Third Party Intervention: Kissinger in the Middle East.
New York: Praeger.

Rubin, J. Z. 1992. ‗International Mediation in Context‘, in J. Bercovitch and J. Z. Rubin,


(eds.), Mediation in International Relations: Multiple Approaches to Conflict
Management. New York: St. Martin's Press, 249- 27.

Rubin, J. Z., D. G. Pruitt, and S. H. Kim. 1994. Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate,
and Settlement. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Schrodt, P. A. and D. Gerner. 2004. ‗An Event Data Analysis of Third-Party Mediation in
the Middle East and the Balkans, Journal of Conflict Resolution, 48:3, pp. 310-330.

Siniver, A. (2006) ‗Power, Impartiality and Timing: Three Hypotheses on Third Party
Mediation in the Middle East‘, Political Studies 54:4, 806-826.

Simkin, W. 1971. Mediation and the Dynamics of Collective Bargaining. Washington,


D.C.: Bureau of National Affairs.

Smith, W. P. 1985. ‗Effectiveness of the Biased Mediator‘. Negotiation Journal, 1:4,


363-372.

Smith, J. D. 1994. ‗Mediator Impartiality: Banishing the Chimera‘. Journal of Peace


Research, 31:4, 445-450.

Smith, A. and A. Stam. 2003. ‗Mediation and Peacekeeping in a Random Walk Model of
Civil and InterstateWar‘, International Studies Review 5:1, pp. 115-135.

Stedman, S. J., D. Rothchild , and E. M. Cousens (eds.). 2002. Ending Civil Wars: The
Implementation of Peace Agreements. Boulder: Lynne Rienner.

Stulberg, J. B. 1987. Taking Charge – Managing Conflict. Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath.

Svensson, I. 2009. ‗Guaranteeing Peace: The Credibility of Third-party Mediators in


Civil Wars‘, in J. Bercovitch and S. S. Gartner (eds.), International Conflict
Mediation: New Approaches and Findings (London: Routledge, 2009), pp. 115-
134.

Terris, L. G. and Z. Maoz. 2005. ‗Rational Mediation: A Theory and a Test‘, Journal of
Peace Research 42:5, pp. 563-583.

Touval, S. 1982. The Peace Brokers: Mediators in the Arab-Israeli Conflict, 1948-1979.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.

39
Wall, J. A., Jr., and A. Lynn. 1993. ‗Mediation: A Current Review‘. Journal of Conflict
Resolution 37:1, 160-194.

Wall, J. A., Jr., J. B. Stark, and R. L. Standifer. 2001. ‗Mediation: A Current Review and
Theory Development‘. Journal of Conflict Resolution 45:3, 370-391.

Walter, B.F. 2002. Committing to Peace: The Successful Settlement of Civil Wars.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Wilkenfeld, J., K. Young, D. Quinn and V. Asal. 2005. Mediating International Crises.
New York: Routledge.

Young, O. 1967. Intermediaries: Third Parties in International Crises. Princeton:


Princeton University Press.

Zartman, I. W. 1985. Ripe for Resolution: Conflict and Intervention in Africa. New York:
Oxford University Press.

Zartman, I. W., and M. R. Berman. 1982. The Practical Negotiator. London: Yale
University Press.

Zartman, I. W. and Touval, S. 1985. ‗International Mediation: Conflict Resolution and


Power Politics‘. Journal of Social Issues, 41:1, 27-45.

Zartman, I. W. and S. Touval. 1996. ‗International Mediation in the Post-Cold War Era‘,
in C. Crocker, O. Hampson and P. Aall (eds.), Managing Global Chaos: Sources of
and Responses to International Conflict. Washington, DC: US Institute of Peace
Press, 445-461.

40

Вам также может понравиться