Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 5

Personality and Individual Differences 91 (2016) 154–158

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The mindful personality: Associations between dispositional


mindfulness and the Five Factor Model of personality
Adam W. Hanley ⁎
Educational Psychology and Learning Systems, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: While curiosity about the correlates of mindfulness continues to grow, how mindfulness is related to personality
Received 30 August 2015 factors remains unclear. Indeed, the relationships between dispositional mindfulness (DM) and one of the most
Received in revised form 22 November 2015 common conceptualizations of personality, the Five Factor Model (FFM) have yielded mixed results. It may be
Accepted 27 November 2015
that these mixed findings have resulted from a lack of analytic specificity. This study explored the relationship
Available online xxxx
between DM and the FFM of personality, paying particular attention to the analysis of the mindfulness facets
Keywords:
with respect to the FFM using canonical correlation analysis. The total DM score was found to be signifi-
Mindfulness cantly correlated with each personality factor, with the strongest relationships observed between DM
Awareness and neuroticism (negatively associated) as well as DM and conscientiousness (positively associated).
Personality The canonical correlation analysis provided further evidence of the relationship between DM and the
Five factor personality model FFM at a finer level of specificity. Three clusters of association emerged between the DM facets and the
personality factors: 1) a self-regulation cluster, negatively associated with neuroticism and positively as-
sociated with conscientiousness, 2) a self-awareness cluster positively associated with openness, and
3) the conscientious confusion cluster, demonstrated a mixed relationship between conscientiousness
and the mindful self-regulation cluster.
© 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

1. Introduction 1.1. Dispositional mindfulness

How mindfulness is related to personality remains unclear, despite Dispositional mindfulness in the West is commonly defined as
mounting evidence of the benefits of mindfulness (e.g., Eberth & the tendency to “[pay] attention in a particular way: on purpose,
Sedlmeier, 2012). Better understanding the relationship between dispo- in the present moment, and non-judgmentally” (Kabat-Zinn,
sitional mindfulness and personality is believed to have significant clin- 1994, p. 4). Operationally, five facets are commonly taken to consti-
ical utility, potentially providing important predictive information with tute dispositional mindfulness: observing experience, describing
respect to clinical concerns such as suicide (Tucker et al., 2014). How- internal experiences, acting with awareness, being non-reactive,
ever, the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and one of and being non-judgmental (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, &
the most common conceptualizations of personality, the Five Factor Toney, 2006).
Model (FFM), has yielded mixed, and sometimes conflicting, results Vago and Silbersweig (2012) suggest that mindfulness encour-
(Giluk, 2009). It has been suggested that these mixed findings may re- ages greater self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence
sult from measurement concerns, such as differential construct validity (S-ART), a collection of capacities believed to support greater well-
(Siegling & Petrides, 2014), or insufficiently detailed analyses (Giluk, being. Greater self-awareness is thought to promote greater self-
2009). This study will primarily address the second concern by extend- regulation, which in turn allows for the transcendence of maladap-
ing previous analyses of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire tive behavioral and cognitive patterns (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).
(FFMQ) and the Five Factor Model of personality to more comprehen- Applying the S-ART framework to the FFMQ's mindfulness facets,
sively examine the relationships among each mindfulness facet and fac- suggests that the facets could be conceptually grouped into self-
tor of personality using canonical correlation analysis. aware and self-regulatory clusters. The mindfulness facets of observ-
ing and describing could be taken to constitute the self-awareness
cluster, primarily reflecting attunement with internal and external
experiences. The acting with awareness, non-reacting and non-
⁎ Educational Psychology & Learning Systems, 3210 Stone Building, PO Box 3064453,
judging facets could be taken to constitute the self-regulation clus-
Tallahassee, FL 32306-4453, United States. ter, suggestive of the tendency to respond intentionally to internal
E-mail address: awh10d@my.fsu.edu. and external behavioral cues.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.054
0191-8869/© 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
A.W. Hanley / Personality and Individual Differences 91 (2016) 154–158 155

1.2. Personality and dispositional mindfulness 2. Method

The Five Factor Model (FFM), including 1) neuroticism, 2) extraver- 2.1. Participants and procedures
sion, 3) openness, 4) agreeableness, and 5) conscientiousness, is the
most thoroughly researched conceptualization of personality (John, Participants (N = 458) were recruited online from the college of ed-
Naumann, & Soto, 2008). The only extant meta-analysis investigating ucation subject pool at a large Southeastern university. Student partici-
personality and dispositional mindfulness suggests neuroticism is nega- pants were primarily female (78%) and Caucasian (72%) with a mean
tively associated with DM; conscientiousness and agreeableness are age was 21 (SD = 2.89). The two measures in this study were part of
positively associated with DM; and extraversion and openness demon- a larger research project in which a battery of surveys were adminis-
strate only weak, positive relationships with DM (Giluk, 2009). While tered online in a single session for course credit. The completion rate
these findings provide preliminary evidence of the broad relationships was 89% with a mean completion time of 27 min.
between dispositional mindfulness and the FFM, the conflation of unidi-
mensional and multidimensional mindfulness measures as well as inat- 2.2. Measures
tention to the mindfulness subdomain scores may have resulted in
imprecise results. Indeed, Siegling and Petrides' (2014) investigation Dispositional Mindfulness was measured by the Five Facet Mindful-
of the associations between seven different DM scales and the FFM con- ness Questionnaire (Baer et al., 2006), a well-validated, 39-item mea-
cluded that, “differences in the breadth of these measures could lead to sure yielding a total dispositional mindfulness score (α = .88 in this
divergent patterns of associations and uncertainty about the relation- study) as well as scores for each of the five facets: observing (α =
ships between mindfulness and the FFM” (Siegling & Petrides, 2014, p. .84), describing (α = .87), acting with awareness (α = .92), non-
5). However, continued exploration of the relationship between the reacting (α = .81), and non-judging (α = .93).
mindfulness facets and personality factors has been lacking. Personality was measured by the Big Five Inventory (John &
Two exceptions can be found in Tucker et al.'s (2014) and van den Srivastava, 1999), a well-validated measure using 44 items to assess
Hurk et al.'s (2011) analyses of the relationships between the individual five personality factors: neuroticism (α = .85), conscientiousness
mindfulness facets and the personality factors. Tucker et al. (2014) re- (α = .81), agreeableness (α = .79), extraversion (α = .86), and
ported basic correlational relationships, observing that all the mindful- openness (α = .76).
ness facets appeared related to the elements of personality, with
certain mindfulness facets (i.e., describing, non-judging, non-reacting) 2.3. Bivariate correlation analysis
more closely associated with the personality factors than others (i.e. ob-
serving). The majority of significant associations were positive. Neurot- Basic bivariate correlations between the total dispositional mindful-
icism was the only personality factor evidencing negative associations ness score, the dispositional mindfulness facets and the big five person-
with the mindfulness facets. Van den Hurk et al. (2011) explored the in- ality traits are reported in Table 1.
fluence of meditation practice on the relationship between DM and the
FFM. Using the KIMS, a multidimensional measure of mindfulness that 2.4. Canonical correlation analysis
preceded the FFMQ, van den Hurk et al. (2011) found each of the mind-
fulness facets to mediate the relationship between meditation and per- A canonical correlation analysis was conducted to investigate the
sonality factors. Similar to Tucker et al.'s (2014) findings, specific layers of relationship between the facets of dispositional mindfulness
mindfulness facets (i.e., describing, observing, acting with awareness) (observing, describing, acting with awareness, non-reacting, non-
were stronger mediators than others (i.e., accepting without judgment). judging) and the five personality factors (openness, agreeableness,
Broadly, van den Hurk et al.'s (2011) results, along with Tucker extraversion, conscientiousness, neuroticism). Canonical correlation
et al.'s (2014) findings suggest that exploration at the facet level pro- analysis (CCA) provided an estimation of the multivariate, shared
vides greater explanatory clarity, clarity they may be lost in analyses re- relationship between the dispositional mindfulness facets and the per-
lying solely on total scores. Specifically, both studies highlight the sonality factors. CCA is a useful statistical approach when exploring
relationship between the mindfulness facet of describing and the FFM. groups of variables as it reduces type I error while honoring multiple
However, Tucker et al. (2014) also found non-judging to be highly asso- levels of association between two variable groups. A CCA creates multi-
ciated with the FFM, while van den Hurk et al.'s (2011) analysis empha- ple synthetic variables for both variable groups that maximize the de-
sized the importance of the observing and acting with awareness facets. gree of association between the variable groups, allowing for distinct
A more dedicated exploration of the relationships between the facets of patterns of relationship to emerge that may more accurately capture
mindfulness and personality factors may provide insight into previously the complexity of relationship existing between two variables. The full
reported inconsistencies in the observed relationships between disposi- model was statistically significant (Wilks's λ = .36, F(25,1766.05) =
tional mindfulness and the FFM. Indeed, Giluk (2009) suggests that 22.49, p b .001), explaining 64% of the variance between dispositional
exploring the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and per- mindfulness and the FFM.
sonality at the dimensional level may be more illuminating. This sug- The CCA generated five distinct functions. However, only the first
gestion was more recently echoed with respect to exploring the three functions explained a significant proportion of the variance be-
mindfulness facets in general (Baer, Lykins, & Peters, 2012). Ultimately, tween the two variable sets and were interpretable. The first significant
as the nomological net surrounding mindfulness continues to ex- function was termed the self-regulation cluster, F(25,1766.05) = 22.49,
pand, complimentary analyses of narrowing specificity are also p b .001, and yielded a squared canonical correlation of .46. The second
needed to more fully clarify the relationships between mindfulness significant function was termed the self-awareness cluster, F(16,
and established correlates. This study extends previous investiga- 1454.84) = 13.00, p b .001, and yielded a squared canonical correlation
tions of mindfulness and personality by, specifically attending to of .19. The third significant function was termed the conscientious con-
the analysis of the mindfulness facets with respect to the FFM. fusion cluster, F(9,1161.04) = 10.95, p b .001, and yielded a squared ca-
Most precisely, the relationship between conscientiousness and nonical correlation of .15. Standardized canonical function coefficients,
the facets of mindfulness were attended to, as relatively few inves- structure coefficients, and squared structure coefficients for both func-
tigations of this relationship have been conducted (Giluk, 2009) tions are provided in Table 2.
despite both constructs emphasizing self-regulatory tendencies In function 1, the self-regulation cluster, the relevant dispositional
(Giluk, 2009) and attending to the present moment (Latzman & mindfulness facets were acting with awareness, non-judging and non-
Masuda, 2013). reacting. These three mindfulness facets, representing the mindful
156 A.W. Hanley / Personality and Individual Differences 91 (2016) 154–158

Table 1
Dispositional mindfulness and personality correlation matrix.

Variable Dispositional mindfulness Observing Describing Acting with awareness Non-reacting Non-judging

Neuroticism −.44*** .11* −.18*** −.38*** −.36*** −.42***


Extraversion .22*** .03 .18*** .14** .06 .20***
Openness .31*** .36*** .30*** .08 .15** .05
Agreeableness .31*** .02 .21*** .31*** .01 .26***
Conscientiousness .38*** −.06 .21*** .53*** .06 .27***
*
p b .05.
**
p b .01.
***
p b .001.

self-regulation skills, were most closely associated with the personality Results from the bivariate correlation analysis support previous
traits of neuroticism and conscientiousness. Examination of the struc- analyses investigating the relationships between DM and the FFM
ture coefficient signs suggests that conscientiousness is positively relat- (e.g., Tucker et al., 2014). The total DM score was found to be significant-
ed to the identified mindful self-regulation facets and neuroticism is ly correlated with each personality factor, demonstrating the strongest
inversely related to the facets in the self-regulation cluster. relationships between DM and neuroticism as well as conscientious-
In function 2, the self-awareness cluster, the relevant dispositional ness. With respect to the mindfulness facets, results broadly suggest
mindfulness facets were observing and describing. These two mindful- that each of the DM facets, except observing, are associated with re-
ness facets, representing the mindful self-awareness skills, demonstrat- duced negative emotionality. Results further suggest that the mindful
ed a strong relationship with the personality trait of openness. Unified tendencies to use language in the description internal and external ex-
structure coefficient signs indicate a positive relationship between the periences, to act intentionally, and to remain non-critical of experience
two mindfulness facets constituting the self-awareness cluster and are associated with the conscientious, agreeable, and extraverted per-
openness. sonality factors. The personality factor characterized by openness to ex-
In function 3, the conscientious confusion cluster, the relevant dis- perience appears to be most closely associated with the mindful
positional mindfulness facets were non-reacting and acting with aware- tendencies to observe and describe experience. However, notable con-
ness. These two mindfulness facets reflect the most behavioral elements sistencies as well as inconsistencies in this study's findings compared
of mindful self-regulation and were most closely related to the person- with Tucker et al.'s (2014) results highlight the need for continued ex-
ality trait of conscientiousness. However, examination of the structure ploration of the relationship between the facets of mindfulness and
coefficient signs revealed a differential relationship between the mind- the personality factors, specifically with respect to the acting with
fulness facets and conscientiousness. Acting with awareness was awareness, non-reacting and observing mindfulness facets.
positively associated with conscientiousness, while non-reacting The canonical correlation analysis provided further evidence of the
was inversely related to conscientiousness. relationship between DM and the FFM at multiple levels of specificity,
Broadly, the communality coefficients suggest that across the three suggesting that the DM facets are strongly correlated with the FFM.
significant functions, all five facets of dispositional mindfulness contrib- The strength of this relationship appears to emerge from three clusters
ute significantly to the big five personality traits. Comparatively, neurot- of association. Function one, the self-regulation cluster, accounts for the
icism, conscientiousness and openness appear to be the personality traits majority of the co-variance between the two variable sets. The DM
most related to the dispositional mindfulness facets. Only marginal rela- facets identified in the self-regulation cluster, acting with awareness,
tionships were observed between the personality traits of agreeableness non-reacting and non-judging, reflect the mindful tendencies to adap-
and extraversion and the facets of dispositional mindfulness (Fig. 1). tively regulate both behavior and thoughts. These adaptive regulation
characteristics appear to be linked simultaneously with lower reports
3. Discussion of negative emotionality (i.e., neuroticism) and higher reports of inten-
tional, goal directed behavior (i.e. conscientiousness). It may be that
This study investigated the relationship between dispositional better self-regulation allows for engagement with activities or pursuits
mindfulness as a multidimensional construct and the Five Factor that are more consistent with personal values. Such pursuits are likely
Model of personality using both bivariate correlation analysis and ca- to encourage greater psychological well-being, inconsistent with nega-
nonical correlation analysis. tive emotionality (Wenzel, von Versen, Hirschmüller, & Kubiak, 2015),

Table 2
Canonical solution for dispositional mindfulness and personality, functions 1, 2 and 3.

Variable Function 1 Function 2 Function 3


Self-regulation cluster Self-awareness cluster Conscientious confusion cluster

Coef rs r2s (%) Coef rs r2s (%) Coef rs r2s (%) h2 (%)

Observing .25 .18 3.10 .80 .80 63.36 .08 .29 8.53 75
Describing .05 −.33 10.89 .45 .69 47.61 .12 .12 1.42 60
Acting with awareness −.62 −.78 60.22 .18 .25 6.40 −.95 −.56 31.02 98
Non-reacting −.63 −.45 20.34 −.30 .09 .75 .56 .61 37.33 58
Non-judging −.32 −.65 42.51 −.04 .07 .44 .70 .13 1.74 45
R2c 46.13 18.93 14.83
Neuroticism .78 .90 81.72 .30 .14 1.88 −.62 −.40 15.60 99
Conscientiousness −.46 −.67 45.29 .17 .28 8.07 −.92 −.68 45.97 99
Openness .02 −.08 .64 .88 .94 87.42 .34 .26 6.76 95
Agreeableness .03 −.40 15.84 .12 .36 13.25 −.05 −.19 3.76 33
Extraversion −.02 −.25 6.30 .16 .26 6.60 .20 .17 3.03 16

Note. Structure coefficients (rs) greater than |.45| are underlined. Communality coefficients (h2) greater than 45% are underlined. Coef = standardized canonical function coefficient; rs =
structure coefficient; r2s = squared structure coefficient; h2 = communality coefficients; R2c = squared canonical correlation.
A.W. Hanley / Personality and Individual Differences 91 (2016) 154–158 157

Fig. 1. Bolded lines represent the facets and factors in the self-regulation cluster. Dashed lines represent the facets and factors in the self-awareness cluster. Triple lined boxes represent
the conscientious confusion cluster. Dotted lines indicate the factors not significantly associated with any function. R2c = squared canonical correlation, shared variance between the
mindfulness and personality variables. R2 = shared variance between the mindfulness and personality variables across all functions.

and enhanced self-efficacy beliefs, which have also been linked to psy- this study precludes causal claims. However, as DM is believed to
chological well-being (Deci, Ryan, Schultz, & Niemiec, 2015). be influenced by mindfulness practices (Carmody & Baer, 2008), fu-
Functions two and three also accounted for significant proportions ture studies may wish to explore the impact of mindfulness training
of the variance between the DM facets and the FFM, although less on personality or, more simply, investigate changes in both DM and
than function one. Function two, the self-awareness cluster, suggested the FFM longitudinally.
a relationship between the mindful tendencies to be observant and de- Ultimately, much remains to be done to identify the mindful person-
scriptive of experience and the openness to experience personality fac- ality. However, continued pursuit of this relationship appears rewarding
tor. This relationship makes intuitive sense, with individuals evidencing given the range of benefits associated with mindfulness and the poten-
greater attunement with internal and external phenomena more likely tial clinical applications of more thoroughly understanding the relation-
to experience life with greater “breadth, depth, originality, and com- ship between mindfulness and personality with respect to clinical
plexity” (John et al., 2008), as characterized by the open personality. concerns, such as suicidality (Tucker et al., 2014). Importantly, how
The tendency to attend to the present moment, drawing awareness DM, as an emerging personality construct, fits with more traditional
into the here-and-now, would be expected to allow for a richer engage- personality conceptualizations is beginning to be better understood.
ment with daily life. This study contributes to this literature base, providing further evidence
Where as functions one and two suggest relatively clear, conceptual of the link between DM and the FFM and offering three distinct clusters
channels of association between the DM facets and the FFM, function of relationship providing a more nuanced understanding of mindfulness
three, the conscientious confusion cluster, is a bit more opaque. In this and personality.
cluster, the two primary behavioral regulation DM facets, acting with
awareness and non-reacting, are associated with conscientiousness.
However, acting with awareness is positively associated with conscien- References
tiousness while non-reacting is inversely associated. It may be that non-
Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L., & Peters, J. R. (2012). Mindfulness and self-compassion as predic-
reacting, as “the tendency to allow thoughts and feelings to come and tors of psychological wellbeing in long-term meditators and matched nonmeditators.
go, without getting caught up in or carried away by them” (Baer et al., The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(3), 230–238.
2008, p. 300) undermines the goal directedness that characterizes Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report
assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45.
conscientiousness. In other words, holding onto thoughts and feel- Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., ... Williams, J. M.
ings as planning tools or motivational guides may support the (2008). Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating
“task- and goal-directed behavior” (John et al., 2008) characterizing and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15, 329–342.
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of
the conscientious personality. However, the unexpected nature of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-
this finding supports previous calls for a more directed investigation based stress reduction program. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 23–33.
of the relationship between DM and conscientiousness (Giluk, Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Schultz, P. P., & Niemiec, C. P. (2015). Being aware and functioning
fully: Mindfulness and interest-taking within self-determination theory. Handbook of
2009). Moreover, this finding highlights the importance of investi-
mindfulness: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 112–129). New York: Guilford.
gating the relationship between DM and conscientiousness at the Eberth, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2012). The effects of mindfulness meditation: A meta-analysis.
facet level. Mindfulness.
Giluk, T. L. (2009). Mindfulness, big five personality, and affect: A meta-analysis.
Continued study of the relationship between DM and the FFM is also
Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 805–811.
encouraged given specific limitations in the current study. Fundamen- Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world?
tally, the W.E.I.R.D. sample (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010), com- Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.
posed disproportionately of females, may limit generalizability. As such, van den Hurk, P. A., Wingens, T., Giommi, F., Barendregt, H. P., Speckens, A. E., & van Schie,
H. T. (2011). On the relationship between the practice of mindfulness meditation and
investigating the relationship between DM and the FFM in more diverse personality—An exploratory analysis of the mediating role of mindfulness skills.
populations is an important next step. Also, the correlational nature of Mindfulness, 2(3), 194–200.
158 A.W. Hanley / Personality and Individual Differences 91 (2016) 154–158

John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big-five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, Tucker, R. P., O'Keefe, V. M., Cole, A. B., Rhoades-Kerswill, S., Hollingsworth, D. W., Helle, A.
and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personal- C., ... Wingate, L. R. (2014). Mindfulness tempers the impact of personality on suicidal
ity: Theory and research. 2. (pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press. ideation. Personality and Individual Differences, 68, 229–233.
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative big five Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-
trait taxonomy. Handbook of personality: Theory and research, 3, 114–158. transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mecha-
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday nisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(296), 1–30.
life. New York: Hyperion Books. Wenzel, M., von Versen, C., Hirschmüller, S., & Kubiak, T. (2015). Curb your
Latzman, R. D., & Masuda, A. (2013). Examining mindfulness and psychological inflexibil- neuroticism—Mindfulness mediates the link between neuroticism and subjec-
ity within the framework of big five personality. Personality and Individual Differences, tive well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 68–75.
55(2), 129–134.
Siegling, A. B., & Petrides, K. V. (2014). Measures of trait mindfulness: Convergent validity,
shared dimensionality, and linkages to the Five-Factor Model. Frontiers in Psychology,
5, 1–8.

Вам также может понравиться