Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 4

European Journal of Neurology 2003, 10: 445–448

SHORT COMMUNICATION

Subcortical aphasia: three different language disorder syndromes?


D. C. Kuljic-Obradovic
Sveti Sava Hospital, Nemanjina, Belgrade, Yugoslavia

Keywords: The study analyses clinical presentation of language functions of 32 patients with
aphasia, paraventricular subcortical aphasia induced by stroke. The patients have been divided into three groups
white matter, striato- according to neuroanatomic localization of the lesion, defined by CT and MRI
capsular region, subcorti- examination (striato-capsular aphasia, aphasia associated with white matter paraven-
cal, thalamus tricular lesions and thalamic aphasia). The following batteries and tests were used: the
neurologic examination, CT scan, MRI, Doppler ultrasound, Mini Mental State
Received 8 July 2002 Examination, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE), Boston Naming Test
Accepted 3 March 2003 (BNT), Token Test and Verbal Fluency Test. Clinical presentation of subcortical
aphasias is characterized with preserved repetition, however, some groups differ by
certain specific features of language impairment. Striato-capsular aphasia and aphasia
associated with white matter paraventricular lesions are characterized with lack of
speech fluency, occurrence of literary paraphasias, mainly preserved comprehension
and naming. Thalamic aphasia, however, is characterized with fluent output, impaired
comprehension and naming with predominant verbal paraphasias. The specific features
of language impairment suggest that subcortical structures contribute to language
organization. Considering the results of language tests we presume that the most
prominent feature in striato-capsular aphasia is phonetic impairment of language,
opposite to thalamic aphasia where lexical-sematic processing seems to be affected.

group of subcortical aphasias showed certain specific


Introduction
disorders of speech and language functions.
Contradictory opinions about the role of some sub- Striato-capsular aphasia in spontaneous speech is
cortical structure in language organization, pathogen- characterized with impairment of articulation and pros-
esis, clinical manifestations and classifications of ody. Comprehension is mainly preserved, whilst nomi-
subcortical aphasia are mostly common in referential nation is usually affected with occurrence of semantic
literature. Authors, sometimes even exclude complete paraphrasias (Benson and Ardila, 1996; Demonet, 1997).
existence of this entity. Clinical presentation of aphasia caused by lesions of
Different authors usually highlight the importance of paraventricular white matter has not been studied in
the lesions in striato-capsular region (head of the greater detail, but some studies suggested that produc-
caudate nucleus, putamen, anterior limb of the internal tive components of language are affected primary
capsule), thalamus and paraventricular white matter. (Staudt et al., 2001).
Accordingly, subcortical aphasias are classified into The reduction of spontaneous verbal output, severe
three groups: striato-capsular aphasia, thalamic aphasia anomia, defective comprehension and occurrence of
and aphasia associated with white matter paraven- verbal paraphrasias are main features of thalamic
tricular lesions. aphasia (Benson and Ardila, 1996; Neau and Bogous-
Whilst the majority of authors accentuate variability slavsky, 1996; Demonet, 1997; Nadeau and Crosson,
of clinical manifestations amongst and within the 1997).
groups of subcortical aphasias, others point out that
certain groups are characterized with clearly identified
Materials and methods
symptoms (D’Esposito and Alexander, 1995).
Preservation of repetitive speech is widely recognized Thirty-two aphasic patients, 18 males and 14 females,
as the common feature, suggesting similarity of sub- age 47–72 years (mean 57.9), 29 predominantly right-
cortical and trascortical aphasias (Benson and Ardila, handed and three ambidextrous with left-sided vascular
1996; Kreisler et al., 2000). On the other hand, each lesions of subcortical structures admitted in ÔSveti SavaÕ
Hospital for cerebrovascular diseases, were tested 4–
8 days after the onset of the disease. The native lan-
Correspondence: Dragana C. Kuljic-Obradovic, Sveti Sava Hospital,
guage for all patients was Serbian. The patients were
Nemanjina 2, 11000 Belgrade, Yugoslavia (fax: + 381(11) 642–014; divided into three groups: (i) patients with lesion in the
e-mail: ukisrki@eunet.yu). striato-capsular region 15, (ii) patients with lesion of

Ó 2003 EFNS 445


446 D. C. Kuljic-Obradovic

paraventricular white matter eight and (iii) patients patients with Mini Mental State Examination score
with thalamic lesions nine. Patients have given their below 24, (v) patients with carotid occlusion seen on
informed consent. Doppler ultrasound and (vi) patients with brain atro-
Clinical neurologic examination performed on the phy, multiple lesions, or other cerebral tissue lesions
onset of the diseases presented: right hemiplegia (five than acute stroke, seen on CT/MRI.
patients), mild right hemiparesis (25 patients), right
sited cutaneous sensory loss (two patients), thalamic
Statistics
pain (one patient) and right sited deep sensory loss
(three patients). The neurological findings were con- Statistical analysis of the data was bases on Kruskal–
sistent with radiological localization of the lesions, Wallis chi-square test that was used for non-parametric
although three patients with thalamic lesions suffered variance analysis (ANOVA).
from right hemiparesis without sensory loss. Loss of
sensory function may have been so mild and gradual as
Results
to pass unnoticed. Adequate hearing was confirmed by
tuning-fork tests. Mini Mental State Examination was The used batteries and tests provided results to assess
used for evaluation of cognitive function. specific functions of language in the studied groups of
Both, brain CT on Shimadzu X-ray SCT 4800 TF/TE subcortical aphasias.
(Shimadzu Corporation, Kyoto, Japan) sliced 2–5 mm Conversational speech results in patients with striato-
and MRI on Siemens Magnetom Plus Impact (1 Tesla) capsular aphasias and aphasia associated with white
(Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlargen, Germany), sliced matter paraventricular lesions showed a similar profile
3–6 mm, without contrast, have been performed 1– of language impairment. Aphasia is characterized with
4 days after the onset of the disease. The lesions were lack of fluency, short phase length (three to four
estimated visually by two experienced reviewers. Three words), affected articulation and preserved repetition.
patients had haemorrhages (thalamus), one had haem- Nomination and comprehension impairments were not
orrhagic infarct (thalamus) and 28 had non-hem- severe.
orrhagic infarcts. The striato-capsular lesions were On the other hand, thalamic aphasia is characterized
approximately 7–25 mm, the lesions of paraventricular with preserved fluency, repetitive speech and grammar,
white matter 20–35 mm and thalamic lesions 10–35 mm but with frequent and severe damage of nomination
measured by MRI. and comprehension.
Doppler ultrasound has also been performed, dem- Comprehension tests failed to indicate statistically
onstrating normal finding in 28 patients, atheromatous significant differences amongst the groups. The most
plaques and 30% stenoses of the left internal carotid severe aberrations were recorded in the group of tha-
artery in one patient and slower flow of vertebro-basilar lamic aphasia (Table 1).
arteries in three patients. Preservation of repetition was illustrated with aver-
Diagnosis of aphasia was carried out using the fol- age values remaining above the cutoff score in all three
lowing batteries and tests: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia groups.
Examination (BDAE), Boston Naming Test (BNT), Naming tests indicated statistically significant differ-
Token Test (Original Version) and Verbal Fluency Test. ences amongst the groups. In the group of thalamic
Conversational speech was analysed by BDAE rating aphasia the impairment of naming was the most severe.
scale profile of speech characteristics. Comprehension In all three groups high values of SD were noticed
was analysed by subtests: word discrimination, body- (Table 2).
part identification, commands, complex ideational ma- Speech fluency test based on animal naming failed,
terial, contained in BDAE and token test-original ver- whilst FAS did show statistically significant difference
sion. Repetition was controlled with BDAE subtests: amongst the groups. FAS test results were lower than in
repetition of words and sentences. Naming was also animal naming test, which is particularly marked in
analysed with BDAE subtests (Responsive Naming and striato-cortical aphasia: mean value of animal naming
Visual Confrontation naming) and Boston Naming 11.93, FAS 5.64, whilst the difference was the least in
Test (BNT). Speech fluency test based on animal na- thalamic aphasias: 11.3 vs. 5.87.
ming and FAS were performed, too. Paraphasias was Paraphasias usually occurred in the group of tha-
also studied with BDAE tests. lamic aphasia (mean verbal 9.78, neologisms 2.22, lit-
The exclusion criteria were: (i) Patients with history erary 1.78), and less commonly in the group of striato-
of previous neurological disorders, (ii) patients with capsular aphasias (literary paraphasias 3,6) and the
diseases of relevance for the study (e.g. reduced vision, least common in the group of aphasias with lesion of
deafness), (iii) patients with lack of cooperation, (iv) paraventricular white matter.

Ó 2003 EFNS European Journal of Neurology 10, 445–448


Subcortical aphasia 447

Table 1 Comprehension scores of patients with subcortical aphasia

Thalamic Striato-capsular White-matter


aphasia aphasia lesions aphasia

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD P-value


a
Word discrimination 52.83 23.08 63.27 10.27 54.69 11.95 >0.05
Body-part identificationa 15.00 6.10 17.70 4.00 16.19 3.78 >0.05
Commanda 10.00 4.50 12.40 4.79 11.88 5.00 >0.05
Complex ideational materiala 6.67 3.50 9.13 3.44 7.75 2.05 >0.05
Total answerb 47.00 8.37 48.20 16.35 44.50 15.15 >0.05
a
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE); bToken Test (TT).

Table 2 Naming scores of patients with subcortical aphasia

Thalamic Striato-capsular White-matter


aphasia aphasia lesions aphasia

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD P-value

Responsive naminga 22.12 4.09 24.40 9.66 24.00 9.12 <0.05


Visual naminga 77.89 30.67 96.87 22.68 93.38 7.17 <0.10
Spontan answerb 28.88 8.54 37.33 18.36 36.63 5.06 >0.05
Total answerb 40.25 7.87 45.60 15.01 44.75 1.12 >0.05
a
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE); bBoston naming Test (BNT).

Aphasia associated with white matter paraventricular


Discussion
lesions possesses similar characteristics as striato-cap-
Subcortical aphasia induced by lesions of studied, left- sular aphasia, but with longer phrase length (mean four
sided, localizations (striato-capsular region, paraven- words), what is correlated with other study (Staudt
tricular white matter, thalamus) showed unique aphasia et al., 2001).
syndrome. Comprehension is the least affected in cases of stri-
The most consistent common finding was preserva- ato-capsular aphasias and the most impaired in tha-
tion of repetition in all three groups, coinciding with lamic aphasia, although we did not find statistically
reports of other authors (Kreisler et al., 2000; Benson significant differences amongst the groups. Our findings
and Ardila, 1996; Neau and Bogousslavsky, 1996; are coinciding with reports of many authors (Benson
Nadeau and Crosson, 1997; Demonet, 1997), suggesting and Ardila, 1996; Demonet, 1997; Nadeau and Cros-
preservation of Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas and their son, 1997), but opposing to some (Neau and Bogous-
links (Ociæ and Stefanova, 1996). slavsky, 1996).
On the other hand, clearly defined characteristics and The most striking difference (statistically significant)
differences in severity of affection of analyzed verbal amongst the groups of subcortical aphasia was noted in
functions in certain groups of subcortical aphasia were naming. It was showed that the naming ability was the
showed. most severely affected in the group of thalamic aphasias
Conversational speech profile suggests that striato- and the least impaired in striato-capsular ones. All
capsular aphasia is characterized with lack of fluency, patients with thalamic aphasia have their naming
usually preserved comprehension, rare occurrence of function affected, especially responsive naming and
paraphasias, mostly literary. As opposed to the above, visual naming, coinciding with referential studies
thalamic aphasia is characterized with fluent speech, (Megas et al., 1992; Benson and Ardila, 1996; Neau and
long sentences, preserved melodic line, articulation Bogousslavsky, 1996; Demonet, 1997; Nadeau and
agility and grammar. Comprehension is, however, more Crosson, 1997; Raymer et al., 1997). Only few authors
severely affected. Naming is a great deal impaired, failed to identify naming dysfunction in cases of tha-
paraphasias are common, usually verbal. The obtained lamic infarction (Bogousslavsky et al., 1988).
findings are supported in the reports of many authors Nevertheless, analysing our results on comprehen-
(Benson and Ardila, 1996; Nadeau and Crosson, 1997; sion and naming test one can perceive that the values
Wallesch et al., 1997), however, in the others they are of SD are high in all groups, resulting from variability
opposed (Megas et al., 1992; Neau and Bogousslavsky, of clinical presentation within the groups. This fact
1996). is correlating with the most of referential reports

Ó 2003 EFNS European Journal of Neurology 10, 445–448


448 D. C. Kuljic-Obradovic

(Benson and Ardila, 1996; Nadeau and Crosson, 1997; increasingly sophisticated questions about the func-
Wallesch et al., 1997). Previous suggests that features tional neuroanatomy of language.
of language disorders should be discussed as symptom We recognize that our study has certain limitation.
clusters, not as general clinical-anatomical correlates. Audiometric test have not been performed and tuning
Therefore, farther investigation should consider the fork tests were not a good substitute.
question of more precise and define localization of
subcortical lesions associated with speech and lan-
Acknowledgements
guage disorders.
On fluency tests, lower achievement on FAS test than The author thanks Professor M. Dimitrijevic for his
in the animal naming test was particularly striking in review of statistical analyses and Mrs V. Kostic for her
the group of striato-capsular aphasias, whilst in the assistance with preparation of the manuscript.
thalamic aphasia group the difference was the least
prominent, as in other study (Schmand et al., 2000).
References
Occurrence of paraphasias was most commonly
recorded in the group of thalamic aphasia, and least in Benson DF, Ardila A (1996). Aphasia, a Clinical Perspective.
Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 166–179.
patients with lesions of the paraventricular white mat-
Bogousslavsky J, Regli F, Uske A (1988). Thalamic infarcts:
ter. The most literary paraphasias in patients with str- clinical syndromes, etiology, and prognosis. Neurology
iato-capsular aphasia were recorded. Our findings do 38:837–848.
not correlate with the report of Demonet who found D’Esposito M, Alexander MP (1995). Subcortical aphasia:
higher percentage of semantic paraphasias and absence distinct profiles following left putaminal hemorrhage.
Neurology 45:38–41.
of literary paraphasias (Demonet, 1997).
Demonet JF (1997). Subcortical aphasia(s): a controversial
Paraphasias were commonly recorded in the group of and promising topic. Brain Language 58:410–417.
thalamic aphasia with verbal paraphasias, as most Grabowski TJ, Anderson SW, Cooper GE (2002). Disorders
common, whilst neologisms and literary paraphasias of cognitive function. Continuum. Lippincott Williams &
were less frequent; as substantiated in numerous papers Wilkins, Philadelphia, pp. 29–33.
Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Barker PB et al. (2002). Subcortical
(Megas et al., 1992; Benson and Ardila, 1996; Neau and
aphasia and neglect in acute stroke: the role of cortical
Bogousslavsky, 1996; Demonet, 1997) and opposed to hypoperfusion. Brain 125:1094–1104.
other (Kirshner H & Kistler, 1982). Kirshner H, Kistler K (1982). Aphasia after right thalamic
Considering the results mentioned above, one could hemorrhage. Arch Neurol 39:667–669.
presume that the most prominent feature in striate- Kreisler A, Godefroy O, Delmairg C et al. (2000). The
anatomy of aphasia revisited. Neurology 54:1117–1123.
capsular aphasia is phonetic disintegration (Taubner
Megas J, van Loon J, Goffin J, Gybels J (1992). Subcortical
et al., 1999), opposite to thalamic aphasia where lexical- aphasia from a thalamic abscess. J Neurol, Neurosurg
sematic processing seems to be affected. Psychiatry 55:319–321.
Patterns of neural connectivity and clinical observa- Nadeau S, Crosson B (1997). Subcortical aphasia. Brain Lang
tions testify that cortical regions operate in concert with 58:355–402.
Neau JP, Bogousslavsky J (1996). The syndrome of posterior
subcortical structures which specifically contribute to
choroidal artery territory infarction. Ann Neurol 39:779–
language organization (Grabowski et al., 2002). Dif- 788.
ferent parts of subcortical structures are involved in Ociæ G, Stefanova E (1996). Neurolingvistièka interpretacija
specific neuronal networks, cortico-subcortical circuits afazija. Klinie`ka I Eksperimentalna Neurologija 1:91–100.
and complex, feedback loops. The lesions of certain Raymer AM, Moberg PJ, Crosson B, Nadeau SE, Gonzales-
Rothi LJ (1997). Lexical-semantic deficits in two patients
subcortical structures disrupt cognitive processing in a
with dominant thalamic infarction. Neuropsychologia
definite way, suggesting their important and specific 35:211–219.
role in language organization and other cognitive Schmand B, de Bie RM, Koning-Haanstra M, de Smet JS,
function. Some authors suppose that many of the def- Speelman JD, van Zomeren AH (2000). Unilateral pallid-
icits following subcortical lesions may represent dis- otomy in PD A controlled study of cognitive and behavioral
effects. Neurology 54:1058–1064.
connection syndromes (Taubner et al., 1999).
Staudt M, Grodd W, Neimann G (2001). Early left periven-
A completely different concept is that aphasia tricular brain lesions induce right hemispheric organization
because of acute subcortical stroke can be largely of speech. Neurology 57:122–125.
explained by cortical hypoperfusion (Hillis et al., 2002). Taubner RW, Raymer AM, Heilman KM (1999). Frontal-
In summary, true language disorders can be observed opercular aphasia. Brain Lang 70:240–266.
Wallesch CW, Johansen-Horbach H, Bartels C, Herrmann M
in patients with lesions in specific subcortical areas. The
(1997). Mechanisms of and misconceptions about subcor-
question of the role of those structures on language tical aphasia. Brain Lang 58:403–409.
organization is still unsolved, but further neuroimaging
studies can provide opportunity to examine and answer

Ó 2003 EFNS European Journal of Neurology 10, 445–448

Вам также может понравиться