Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 24

CPR4}q&?

6;gp of Pages 24
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
«^'~ . ^. . CLINICAL
o
o* ScienceDirect psychology
REVIEW
ELSEVIER Clinical Psychology Review xx(2007) xxx- xxx

Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression


among children and adolescents: A cognitive
science and developmental perspective
Rachel H. Jacobs3'*, Mark A. Reineckea'b, Jackie K. Gollana'c, Peter Kanea'd
0Department ofPsychiatry Division ofPsycliology and Behavioral Sciences atNorthwestern University Feinberg School ofMedicine.
United States
bNorthwestern University Feinberg School ofMedicine, 710 N. Lake Shore Drive, Abbott Hall Suite 1205, Chicago. Illinois 60611, United States
cNorthwestern University Feinberg School ofMedicine, 446 E. Ontario St. Suite 7-100. Chicago. Illinois 60611. United States
dNorthwestern University Feinberg School ofMedicine. 446 E. Ontario St. Suite 7-100, Chicago, Illinois 60611. United States
Received 11 September 2007; received in revised form 23 October 2007; accepted 29 October 2007

Abstract

We summarize and integrate research oncognitive vulnerability todepression among children and adolescents. We first review
prospective longitudinal studies ofthe most researched cognitive vulnerability factors (attributional style, dysfunctional attitudes,
and self-perception) anddepression among youth. We next review research oninformation processing biases inyouth. We propose
that theintegration of these two literatures will result ina more adequate testof cognitive vulnerability models. Last, we outline a
program of research addressing methodological, statistical, and scientific limitations in the cognitive vulnerability literature.
© 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Cognitive vulnerability; Information processing; Depression

Contents

1. Introduction 0
1.1. Development of depression 0
1.2. Theoretical hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models 0
1.3. Cognitive development 0
2. Attributional style 0
2.1. Review . 0
3. Dysfunctional attitudes 0
3.1. Review 0

* Corresponding author. Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, 710 N. Lake Shore Drive, Abbott Hall Suite 1205, Chicago,
Illinois 60611, United States. Tel: +1 312 835 1568; fax: +1 312 926 0406.
E-mailaddresses: rlijacobs@northwestern.edu (R.H. Jacobs),m-reinccke@northwestern.edu (MA. Reinecke),j-gollan@northwestem.edu
(J.K. Gollan), p-kanc@northwcstem.edu (P. Kane).

0272-7358/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved,
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006

Pleasecite thisarticleas: Jacobs, R. H., et aL,Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression amongchildrenand adolescents: A
cognitive scienceand developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
! R.H. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

4. Self-perception 0
4.1. Review 0
5. An integrative perspective 0
5.1. Review 0
6. Discussion 0
6.1. Future research 0
Acknowledgements 0
References 0

1. Introduction

Using a developmental and cognitive science framework, we review and integrate recent research on cognitive
vulnerability to depression among children andadolescents. First, we review prospective longitudinal studies assessing
relations between putative cognitive vulnerabilities and the occurrence of depression. We then propose that the
incorporation of experimental paradigms and the assessment of information processing biases among children may
facilitate the testing of alternative cognitive vulnerability models. Last, we outline a program of research addressing
methodological, statistical, and scientific limitations in this literature.

/./. Development ofdepression

Major depressive disorder (MDD) oftenbegins during adolescence, is chronic and recurrent, and frequently places
youth at risk for recurrent MDDduring adulthood. Between 20%and50%percent of adolescents report experiencing
subsyndromal levels of depression (Kessler, Avenevoli, & Merikangas, 2001; Petersen, Compas, Brooks-Gunn, &
Sternmler, 1993). Lifetime prevalence rates of 1.5% (e.g., Costello etal., 1996) and7%(e.g., Kessler etal., 2005) have
been reported for depressive disorders among children and adolescents, respectively. Depression during adolescence
sharesfeatures ofthe depression that occursduringadulthood (e.g., Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley, & Gotlib, 1999; Pine,
Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998). Adolescence, then, represents a critical period of vulnerability. Seventy-five
percent of adultswith MDD experience their first depressive episode during childhoodor adolescence, whereas only
25%experience onsetofMDDin adulthood (Kim-Cohen et al., 2003). Thatsaid,patternsof depressive symptomsmay
differ over the course of development given the cognitive, social, emotional, and biological changes that transpire
(Cicchetti & Toth, 1998; Weiss & Garber, 2003). In addition, the observed genderdifference in depression emerges
in early adolescence (e.g., Angold, Erkanli, Silberg, Eaves, & Costello, 2002; Weissman, Warner, Wickramaratne,
Moreau, & Oltson, 1997). Depression is a common, persistent, and pernicious occurrence in the lives ofyouth. Any
comprehensive model of vulnerability for depression must address these observations.

1.2. Theoretical hypotheses ofcognitive vulnerability models

Several models have been proposed to explain the development and maintenance of depression among youth.
Cognitive vulnerability modelsstand at the forefront ofresearch activity. Beck (1967)definedcognitive vulnerability
as the presenceofmaladaptiveself-schemareflectingthemesofhelplessness and unlovabilitythat becomeactivatedby
negative life events or negative moods. Many cognitive vulnerability theories employ a vulnerability-stress paradigm
(e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1967), whereby cognitive factors interact with environmental
stressors to increase risk for emotional disorders. Indeed, stressfullife experiencespredict depression among children
and adolescents (see Grant et al., 2004a; Grant, Compas, Thurm, McMahon, & Gipson, 2004b). This relationship
appears to be bidirectional, as depressive symptoms also predict increases in objectively assessed stressors among
youth (Grant et al., 2004b). The assessment of stress in the study of cognitive vulnerability is crucial, as exposure to
mild uncontrollable stress during adolescence can impaircognitive functioning (Steinberg, 2004).
Specific criteria for defining cognitive vulnerabihty factors have been put forth. First, a cognitive vulnerability
fector must temporally precede depression andexhibit stability overtime (e.g., Alloy et al., 1999; Ingram, Miranda,
& Segal, 1998). Second, construct validity must be established, in that the cognitive vulnerability fector must
demonstrate predictive validity not better accounted for by an extraneous variable. Third, cognitive vulnerability
Please cite thisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H.,et aL, Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability fordepression among children andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10J016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
RJI. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Reviewxx (2007) xxx-xxx 3

factors arebelieved to be specific to particular disorders. Forexample, if dysfunctional attitudes serveas a vulnerability
fordepression, they must notalso predict conduct disorder (discriminant validity). Fourth, vulnerability isviewed asan
endogenous process thatis conceptualized as latent(e.g., Ingram et al., 1998). Thatis, the vulnerability mustrepresent
an enduring characteristic of the child, andnottheir family, relationships, or environment These general andspecific
theoretical hypotheses guide the scientific exploration of cognitive vulnerability.

1.3. Cognitive development

Adolescence represents a phase wherein cognitive vulnerability may occur andis likely to emerge. The cognitive
developmental 'prerequisites' arepresent and can emerge (Alloy, Abramson, Walshaw, Keyser, &Gerstein, 2006). The
adolescent transition ischaracterized bytheemergence ofa more self-directed andself-regulated mind (Keating, 2004).
Self-regulation is advanced through an executive suite of capabilities (Donald, 2001) as central conceptual structures
coordinate across domains (Keating, 1996). A developing executive control monitors andmanages cognitive resources
during adolescence (Kuhn & Pease, 2006). Thus, across development, increasing executive functioning ability
translates to an increasingly 'top down' mode of cognition.
A parallel coordination of information processing, or 'bottom-up' components allows for the strategic use of
emerging cognitive resources. In such, information processing and cognitive reasoning operate in concert. Indeed,
recentworkreveals thatadvances in levelofreasoning aresupported by 'bottom-up'changesin efficiency andworking
memory (Demetriou, Christou, Spanoudis, & Platsidou, 2002). These processes, in turn, are reciprocally affected
by 'top down' cognition. Moreover, processing speed increases from early childhood through mid-adolescence
(Demetriou et al., 2002). Forexample, adolescents arebetter able to ignore irrelevant stimuli on the Stroop taskthan
children (Demetriou, Efklides, & Platsidou, 1993). Increasing processing speed leads to greater efficiency intop down
processes, including social problem solving. Such research suggests that adolescents possess the cognitive
prerequisites for vulnerabilities to depression. If cognitive vulnerability is a valid model representing the onset of
depression among youth, we expect children to be capable of demonstrating maladaptive cognitions, which, in
interaction with the stress of the adolescent transition, result in depressive disorders.

2. Attributional style

Attributional style is thecognitive vulnerability fector thathasgarnered themost research and theoretical attention.
First proposed as a means of addressing limitations in the original theory of learned helplessness (Seligman, 1975),
Abramson et al. (1978) proposed that depressed individuals attribute negative events to internal, global, and stable
causes. In contrast, viewing the causes of negative events as external, specific, and unstable represents a positive or
adaptive style, andmay buffer or protect an individual from depression. A negative attributional style is hypothesized
to be a proximal contributor to the onset of depression. In further theoretical modification, hopelessness theory
(Abramson, Metalsky, &Alloy, 1989), introduced three separate cognitions thatcontribute to depression—a lowsense
of efficacy following life stress, viewing negative events ashaving negative consequences, andviewing thecauses of
these events as global and stable. These cognitions evolve into hopelessness, which is hypothesized to be a proxi
mal and sufficient cause of depression. Hopelessness depression reflects motivational deficits and dysphoric affect
stemming from a negative attributional style (Abramson, Alloy, & Metalsky, 1988). An elaborated cognitive
vulnerability-transactional stress depression model (Hankin & Abramson, 2001) accounts forthegeneral development
of depression aswell as specific outcomes, such astheemergence of a gender difference in depression. In this model,
negative events contribute to initial elevations of general negative affect Cognitive vulnerabilities suchas a negative
attributional style, then, moderate risk for depression. Depression, in turn, can lead to more negative life events,
creating a pathogenic cycle. The aforementioned models allseek to explicate therelations between attributional style
and depression.

2.1. Review

Twenty-one studies support the prospective effects of attributional style on depressed mood in children and
adolescents (see Table 1). Young children are capable of drawing inferences about events and evidence potential
for demonstrating a stable attributional style. In one study, third graders demonstrated greater pessimism in their
Please cite this article as: Jacobs; R. H., etaL, Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
2 S • Table 1

*• I- CD:,
Prospective cognitive contentstudies of vulnerability to depression amongyouth
4n Study Sample N Age Depression Depression VulnerabiUty Tune Control Life stress Summary of findings
(range) status measure measure frame baseline interaction
and mean depression? tested?
i: Attributionalstyle
••'p."; O ,
Abela (2001) School 382 3rd (8.9) Sx CDI CCSQ, 6 w CLES CASQ x CLES predictedincreases in
and 7th CASQ depressive sx in 7th, but not 3rd graders.
:5v::8-:i (12.9) Attributionsaboutconsequences x CLES
predicteddepression in 3rd and 7th,
Attributionsaboutself x CLES predicted
depression increases in 3rd and7th grade
5$
• .CD.- girls,but not boys
Abela and School 314 3rd (8.8) Sx CDI CCSQ, 6 w X CHAS Gender and SE moderators of'weakest
Payne(2003) and 7th CASQ link' x CHAS. Boys with low SE and girls
If (12.8) with high SE significant interaction
§^
a* 2
predicting increases in HS depression sx
So Abela and School 79 (12.3) Sx CDI CCSQ, 10 w X CLES* CLES x'weakest link' predicted increasesin
~**" a '• Sarin (2002) CASQ weakest link HS depression sx only
§ S Abela, Children of 140 6-14 Sx CDI CASQ, iy X Stress of Genderx weaklinkx parental depression
S '•*.. Skitch, depressed (9.8) CCSQ parental stressor. Childrenwith depressogenic
Adams, and parent onset of inferentialstyles greater elevations in "3
Hankin (2006) depression depression following elevations in parent's
level of depression. Stronger association in
girls
I*
S B*
Bennett and School 95 11-13 Sx CDI CASQ-R 6m X CHS, LES CHS x CASQ predicted CDI, no main
so
iu!-
Bates (1995) (12) effect ofCASQ. More support found against 5
vulnerability models
« Brozina and School 480 8-13 Sx CDI CASQ, 6w X CHAS CASQ x CHAS to predict increase in
Abela (2006) (10.5) CCSQ depression, but only for those with low 8
2'g- depression at tl
Conley et al. School 147 5-10 Sx CDI CASL 3w X DHQ CASI main effect, CASI x DHQx age
•§ a.
^ o (2001) (8.2) CASQ-R slightly higher levels of depression in
older childrenthan for younger (all
findings for positive subscale)
Dixon, and Ahrcns Summer 84 9-12(11) Sx CDI KASTAN-R lm X Own measure Hassles and hasslesx attributionsignificant
§•8. (1992) camp
Garber et al. School, but 240 (11.9) Sx CDI CASQ 5y X LEIA Adolescents becoming more negative in
* (2002) 78% of CASQ over time also reportinghigher
children had depression over time. Initiallevel of
mothers with stressassociated with trajectory
a history of of depression sx. Reciprocal models also
mood supported
disorders
2 "0

if
a. »
< e»
Gibb and
Alloy (2006)
School 448 9-11(9.8) Sx CDI CASQ-R 6m X Peer
victimization,
In 4th gradersCASQ mediates only, 5th
grademediator and moderatoreffects.
CO • CTQ-EA Depression also predicted negative
CD S. changes in CASQ. Best fitting models
include reciprocal relations
Gibb etal. (2006) School 448 9-11(9.8) Sx CDI CASQ-R 6m X Peer Depression predicted negativechange
victimization, in CASQ-R
s & CTQ-EA
1*8 Hamraen Children of 79 (12.5) Dx K-SADS, CASQ 6m X Brown and Main effect for stress, but not CASQ
(3 to
et al. (1988) depressed CDI Harris

EL mothers
Hankin, School 270 14-18 Sx BDI, CASQ-R 5w X APES CASQ x APES predicted increasesin
Abramson, (16.2) HDSQ-R depression sx. Gendermoderated this
If- and Siler interaction for BDI, so held for boys
(2001) but not girls. Held for HS depression
9 5: sx for both genders,HS did not mediate
a* 2 relations
89 Hilsman and School 439 (11.4) Sx CES-D, CASQ Approx X Grade deficit Depressivesx predicted by interactions of
If Garber(1995) Depressive
Adjective
2w stressor negative explanatorystyle with stressors

M Checklist

™ SS.
Lewinsohn
et al. (1994)
School 1508 (16.5) Dx K-SADS,
CES-D
Items from
KASTAN-R
1y X Measured, but
no interactions
Attributions predictedcurrent, past, and
first episode depression. OR for current
I
s. 5. 5*
assessed highest
Lewinsohn School 1507 (16.6) Dx K-SADS, CASQ 1y X Brown and CASQ x LE predictMDD onset, but at
et al. (2001) CES-D Harris higher levels of stress
Mccarty, School 331 (12.0) Sx MFQ CASQ-R 1y X Support for 'scar' models. Baseline
Vander Stoep, depression predictiveofmore
If and McCauley negative attributional style
© s»
to- *t (2007)
2f Nolen-Hceksema, School 168 (8-11) Sx CDI CASQ iy LEQ CASQ predicted depression changes at
<?* a Girgus, and Seligman some time points over the year. Interaction
•§ a.
rt o et al. (1986) with life stress was significant
to a
Nolen-Hoeksema School 352 3rd and Sx CDI CASQ 5y LEQ As children grew older, pessimistic
etal. (1992) 4th explanatory style significant predictor of
pera laterdepression. Among younger children,
graders
ON & life events only. Modest support for
diathesis-stress. Some evidence of scar
hypothesis
Panak and School 521 3rd-5th Sx CDI CASQ iy Peerrejection Interaction betweenstress (increase in peer
Garber(1992) graders (a developmental rejection) and CASQ predicted depression
life stress
assessment)
(continued on next page)
-a
8
Table 1 (continued)
*(7. j&
CD
<
CD
O Study Sample N Age Depression Depression VulnerabiUty Time Control Life stress Summary of findings
CD
eo
n (range) status measure measure frame baseline interaction
i' s and mean depression? tested?
8
ft Prinsteiu and School 158 15-17 Sx CDI CASQ 17 m Peerrejection (a Peer rejection*CASQ predictive of dep.
g O
p. CD Aikins (2004) (16.31) developmental for girls. Main effect for attributional
3 life stress style overall
ft*
5" assessment)
cr
IO
Prinstein, Chea, School 159 15-17 Sx CDI Own 17m Peer Higher levels of critical self-referent
K
and Guyer (16.31) victimization attributions associated with prospective
(2005) increases in

1 CD
depressive symptoms under conditions
8 P- of high levelsof boys' peervictimization
Robinson, School 371 (12.0) Sx CDI CASQ 15 m Developed for Main effect stressors, perceived self-worth.
II Garber, and study based on CASQ main and interaction with stressors, mm
Q a. Hilsman (1995) Compas (1987)
a" 3 way interaction with self-worth
8' CD major events
~*
<;
*5 Q. and hassles,
transition to
8 junior high
Schwartz and School 397 14-18 Sx BDI ASQ 1.5 m X LEQ Attributions predicted depression
a o
o
Koenig (1996) (16.0)
r? fl
§ a-. Southall and School 115 14-19 Sx BDI CASQ 14 w X LES 3 way significant interaction and SEx
s 3 Roberts (2002) (16.5) CASQ in non-symptomatic at baseline
13 2 Spence, Sheffield, School 733 12-14 Sx BDI CASQ-R 1y X NLE CASQ-R main effect >9
© cr
o
and Donovan (12.91)
CJ B-
o- (2002)
R- rr
^ Toner and School 112 (12.5) Sx "the I PASS-1 2y A post-hoc combinedgenerality attribution • ir:ni
o 8» Heaven (2005) feel" d peer-social (both positiveandnegativeevents to
o 9r
J2 on ILQ attributional stable/global factors) predicted depression
C\ •u
Cn W and BDS style scale
(A
o
T3 !9.
fi o
to
o
13 Dysfunctional attitude.V
o
-J Abela and Children of 140 6-14 Sx CDI CDAS Every X CHAS CDAS x CHAS significant for children with
O
o
B
00
Skitch (2007) depressed (10.0) 6wfor low self-esteem,also significant for children
o o
o er parents iy with low CDAS and high self-esteem
0\
Abela and School 184 (12.8) Sx CDI CDAS 6w X CHAS CDAS x CHAS predictedincreased
it Sullivan (2003)
B depression, in those with high, but not low
6 levels of self-esteem
o.
0
Q.
Lewinsohn School 1507 (16.6) Dx K-SADS, DAS iy Brown and DAS xNLE (p=.086) trend at high levels
O
et al. (2001) CES-D Harris of stress CASQ x LE predict MDD onset
8
o
at higher levels of stress, little effect vs.
S
(7 low levels of stress
>
R.H. Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychology Reviewxx (2007) xxx-xxx

SI
_r 5
a 3

§ eS
BHIF,

° B DISC-IV Conduct
CO
Disorde Module
•a -a «
g Oi n

9* « s

(8
o
•O u

•8 8

X X X X X X x
X X
S 2
9- S fes
< CS < VO
cs

,-y
2
J? u so
5! 1"
TO CL. a Hi 2 jj U& £h
J3 Q .3 * Q 33 c/i S ca K 55 S W 04 K co X

<u

la_
§1
Q 5 3 S?6
i4 O CQ a

Cd O " ~t O -~. ^ -a -r-


—< —« 00
OJ 00 w -O T3 ^J
8£*> ST! 22.^ ^ T3 T3 J 00 ^
o —
t*> to »w oh n (3 v^

o o O
o o o
J3 J3

tf S
to O o

Please citethisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H., et aL, Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
2 *o Table 1 (continued)
if Study Sample N Age Depression Depression Vulnerability Time Control Life stress Summary of findings
(range) status measure measure frame baseline interaction
::B'.:"''.?:
and mean depression? tested?
p :B.'
Kistner et al. School 108, 4th and Sx CDI,RADS Harter's SPPC 7y Perceivedacceptance, but not actual
(1999) 68 at 5th
O. O : acceptance, predicted depression 7 years
follow-up graders at later
start
c?,5 Kistner et al. School 667 (9.4) Sx CDI Harter's SPPC 6 m Inaccurate self-perceptions predicted
(2006)
increases in depressive symptoms and
depressive symptoms predicted
;:'rp: decreased accuracy. Bias didnotpredict
change in depression, rather depression
predictednegative bias
Kistner, David-Ferdon, School 641 3rd-5th Sx CDI Harter's SPPC 6 m Self-perception didnot predict changes in
Lopez, and Dunkl graders depression
••S-b: (2007)
•§: t
Lewinsohn School (16.5)
iff et al. (1994)
1508 Dx K-SADS,
CES-D
Items from
Harter's SPPC
iy Measured, but Social self-perception predicted current
no interactions depression andprevious depression, butnot
ft assessed first onset
Measelle, Ablow, Cowan, Community 97 (4.6) Sx BPI BPI
S H),
3y Depression-anxiety subscale significantly
and Cowan (1998)
negatively correlated with competence and
motivation in Kindergarten and 1stgrade
McGrath and School 248 (9.5) Sx CDI.TRF Harter's SPPC 3y Negative self-perceptions and
Repetti (2002)
underestimations not associated with
change in depression
Ohannessian School 75 (11.8) Sx CES-D Harter's SSPC ly Genderpredicts depression until
et al. (1999)
self-perception ofathletic competence
is added as covariate
5^ Tram and Cole School 468 13-17 Sx CDL PNID, Harter's SPPC Approx X APES Self-perceived competence predicted
(2000) (14.5) TRID 6m change in depressive symptoms
Note. Age ranges and means reported when available. Grades reported when age data was not available. Sx =symptoms; Dx =diagnosis; CDI =Children's Depression Inventory; CCSQ =
MB Children's Cognitive Style Questionnaire; CASQ « Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire; CASQ-R =Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire-Revised; CLES =The Children's
O-to;
Negative Life Events Scale; CHAS and CHS =Hassles Scale for Children; weakest =weakest link; LES =Life Events Scale; CASI =The Children's Attributional Style Interview; DHQ =
Poo
Daily Hassles Questionnaire; KASTAN-R =Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire-Revised; LEIA =Life Events Interview for Adolescents; CTQ-EA « Childhood Trauma
Questionnaire —Emotional Abuse Subscale; K-SADS •» Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children; Brown and Harris =Brown and Harris; 1978; BDI =
H Beck Depression Inventory; HDSQ-R =Hopelessness Depressive Symptoms Questionnaire-Revised; APES =Adolescent Perceived Events Scale; CES-D =The Center for Epidemiological
si: Studies Depression Scale for Children; MFQ =The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; LEQ =Life Events Questionnaire; NLE » Negative Life Events assessed as modified version ofLife
Event Record; ILQ - Illinois Loneliness Questionnaire; BDS =Birleson Depression Scale; PASS-1 =Peer-social Attributional Style Scale; ASQ =Attributional Style Questionnaire; CDAS =
Children's Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale; BHIF =Baltimore How I Feel; DISC-IV =NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV; CAPS =Child and Adolescent Perfectionism
Scale; CRSQ =Children's Response Styles Questionnaire; RADS =Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale; PSI =The Problem-Solving Inventory; SSLES =Secondary School Students' Life
Events Scale; Harter's SPPC =Harter's Self-perception Profile for Children; PNMC =Peer Nominations Measure ofCompetence; TRS =Teacher's Rating Scale ofChild's Actual Behavior;
PRS =Parental Rating Scale; PNMC =Peer Nominations ofMultiple Competencies; BPI =Berkeley Puppet Interview; PNID =Peer Nomination Index ofDepression; TRID =Teacher's Rating
Index of Depression.
R.H. Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychologyReview xx (2007) jccc-jccc 9

attributions than seventh graders, were more likely to catastrophize, and viewed themselves as flawed following
negative life events (Abela & Payne, 2003). Consistentwith prediction, the interactionof life events and attributional
style appears to be a more potent predictor of depression as children age (Abela, 2001; Conley, Haines, Hilt, &
Metalsky, 2001; Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, & Seligman, 1992).Adolescents who demonstrate a positive attributional
style in sixth grade continue along the same lineartrajectory (in the positive direction) over time (Garber et al., 2002).
Similarly, adolescents who manifest a negative attributional style in sixth grade continue on a trajectory to a more
negative attributionalstyle. Taken together, findings indicatethat children and adolescents are capable ofdeveloping
the attributions that have been linked with depression. Longitudinal evidence suggests that as early as middle
childhood, negative attributions may place children on a trajectory toward an increasingly negative and pernicious
attributional style.
The nature and strengthofassociations betweenattributional style and depressionmay vary with development in
function and/or content. We note Cole and TAimer's (Cole & Turner, 1993; Turner & Cole, 1994) suggestion that
attributional style mediates, rather than moderates, associations between life stress and depression in early- to mid-
childhood. From this perspective, young children's negative attributions stem from adverse life events and are
internalized as negative attributional styles, creating vulnerability to depression. During adolescence, a more stable
attributional style interacts with life stress to produce depressive symptoms (Cole & Turner, 1993; Turner & Cole,
1994). Recent evidence supports this model. Attributional stylemediated and moderated relations between life events
and depression among fifth graders; whereas only a mediational role was presentin fourth graders (Gibb & Alloy,
2006). Congruent with Cole and Turner's (1993; Turner & Cole, 1994) model, these observations suggest a
developmental shift in the relations between cognition, life events, and mood.
On the otherhand, we note the 'weakest link* hypothesis, whichproposes that an individual'smostdepressogenic
inference leads to vulnerability to depression (Abela & Sarin, 2002). As such, an individual's most depressogenic
inference represents their degree of vulnerability. This model is descriptive in that individual attributions (global,
stable, and self) appear relatively independent among youth (Abela, 2001). In contrast, stability of the weakest link
among youth is moderate (test retest reliability=0.38) across six weeks. The weakest link model allows for the
possibilitythat attributional errors can vary from settingto setting.In an investigationofthis hypothesis among seventh
graders, the interaction of life stress by weakest link predicted increases in hopelessness depression symptoms (Abela
& Sarin, 2002). Recent tests ofthis model are also supportive (Abela & Payne, 2003; Abela et al., 2006). As such, the
weakest link hypothesis represents a refinement of attributional models of cognitive vulnerability, but would be
strengthenedthrough: 1) explication ofwhy a child would develop one weak link, as opposed to another, and 2) how a
child's weakest link may vary over time and across settings.
Developmental psychopathology models suggest that the cognitive, social, environmental, and self-regulatory
factors associated with risk to depressiontransactionally influence one another. The integrationofreciprocal modelsto
the study of attributional style represents an important conceptual advance. Latent factor growth modeling of
longitudinal data illustrates the parallelincreases in negative attributional style and depression severity. Adolescents
with initially higherand increasing levelsof depressive symptoms also demonstrate increasingly negative attributions
(Garber, Keiley, & Martin, 2002). Attributional styleand depression maybe mutually dependent In a comparison of
mediation, moderation, and reciprocal models, a reciprocal model gained the most support, wherein initial levels of
depressive symptoms predicted residual change in levels of stress and attributional style over the follow-up (Gibb &
Alloy, 2006). Evidence alsosupports depression as leading to negative attributional style(Bennett & Bates, 1995; Gibb
et al., 2006; McCarty et al., 2007; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1992). In sum, reciprocal models allow for dynamic
modeling within a developmental psychopathology perspective.
Studies reviewed thus for support relations between attributions and depressive symptoms among youth.
Attributions are related to past episodes and predict current and first episodes of depression (Lewinsohn, Clarke,
Seeley, & Rohde, 1994). The interaction of life events and attributional style predict onset of MDD, but only at
high levels of stress (Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001). In the only study of children, evidence did not support
attributional style as a vulnerability to MDD onset (Hammen, Adrian, & Hiroto, 1988). An attributional weakest link
does, however, predict depressive symptoms among children of depressed parents (Abela et al., 2006). Such findings
are preliminary and warrant replication.
Turning to measurement, we note several developments. Almost every study reviewed uses the Children's
Attributional Style Questionnaire (CASQ; Seligman etal.,1984; CASQ-R; Kaslow, &Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Many
researchers, however, call for better instruments, acknowledging the psychometric weaknesses of the CASQ. The
Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R. H., etal., Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression'among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and devetopmehtei perspective; Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:I0.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
10 RH. Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

CASQ measures attributional style in line with the reformulated theory of learned helplessness, rather than hope
lessnesstheory. One new measure,the AdolescentCognitiveStyleQuestionnaire(ACSQ; Hankin & Abramson, 2002),
assesses the entire negative attributional construct proposedby hopelessness theory. Moreover, the CASQ has poor
internalconsistency(coefficient alphas=0.4-0.6; Gladstone & Kaslow, 1995).Accordingto guidelines (e.g.,Nunnally
& Bernstein, 1994), internal consistencies below 0.7 may lead to increases in Type II error. Recently developed
measures includethe Children's Cognitive StyleQuestionnaire (CCSQ; Abela,2001)and the Children'sAttributional
StyleInterview (CASI; Conleyet al., 2001)allowfor more reliable assessment ofattributional style. Futureresearch
can rely on these more psychometrically sound measures, thereby increasing the likelihood of accurately detecting
developmental differences in attributional style.
It isnoteworthy, however, thatdespite measurement limitations, thebulkof thecurrent evidence remains supportive
ofattributional styleas a cognitive vulnerability. Evidence is more consistent among adolescent, as opposed to child,
samples. Research in this area will be facilitated by the recent development of structured interviews assessing the
attributional style of young children (Conley et. al, 2001).

3. Dysfunctional attitudes

Evidence supporting dysfunctional attitudes as a vulnerability to depression derives from a setofstudies. Following
theory, which places dysfunctional attitudes at thecenter oftheetiology of depression (Beck 1967,1983), prospective
studies with adults support the role of dysfunctional attitudes in the development of depression (Alloy et al., 1999;
Joiner, Metalsky, Lew, & Klocek, 1999; Kwon & Oei, 1994). Results from the Temple-Wisconsin Cognitive
Vulnerability to Depression project (CVD; Alloy et al., 2006) indicate that first onset of a depressive disorder is
significantly more likely among individuals with high levels ofdysfunctional attitudes and anegative attributional style
man among individuals with low levels of these cognitions. In another example, increases in depression symptoms
result from the interaction of dysfunctional attitudes and a negative university admissions life stressor among high
school seniors (Abela &D'Alessandro, 2002). These datasupport dysfunctional attitudes as a cognitive vulnerability to
depression among adults.

3.1. Review

Six studies among youth (Abela & Sullivan, 2003; Lewinsohn et al., 1994; Lewinsohn et al., 2001; McCreary,
Joiner, Schmidt, & Ialongo, 2004) prospectively assess the effects of dysfunctional attitudes on depression (see
Table 1). In all studies, dysfunctional attitudes, either alone or in interaction with life stress, predict depression. The
interactionofdysfunctional attitudesand life stresspredictsa diagnosisofdepressionat the level ofa trend (Lewinsohn
et al., 2001).This exampleis a conservative test; however, as the effects ofimportantcovariatessuch as co-morbidnon-
mood disorders and family psychiatric history are controlled. 'Pessimism' (which includesitems from Weissman and
Beck's (1978) scale) predicts first onset ofdepression(Lewinsohnet al., 1994).Higher levels ofdysfunctionalattitudes
are observed among girls who met clinical cutoff scores for depression over the course of two years (Marcotte,
Levesque, & Fortin, 2006). Overall, evidencesupports dysfunctional attitudes in the prediction ofMDD among youth.
However, it is not clear that dysfunctional attitudes serve as a cognitive vulnerability strictly to first onset
depression, since 'pessimism' is also associated with current and past depression (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). More
perniciousand stable maladaptive cognitionsmay result fromrepeatedactivationduring episodes ofdepressedmood.
Dysfunctional attitudes may increase vulnerability to first onset of major depression during adolescence, as well as
increase vulnerability to future episodes. Parallel to findings within the attributional style literature, dysfunctional
attitudes may transactionally relate to mood.
Dysfunctional attitudes have not been explored in children until recently. This work is facilitated by the
development ofthe Children's DysfunctionalAttitudes Scale (CDAS;D'Allessandro, & Abela, 2000). A test ofBeck's
cognitivediathesis-stress theory ofdepressionrevealeda significant interactionofdysfunctionalattitudesby hassles,
but only among children with high self-esteem (Abela & Sullivan, 2003). The self-esteem effect ran contrary to
hypotheses.Anotherstudy identifieda significantinteraction- thistime in line with hypotheses- amongchildrenwith
high levels of dysfunctional attitudes and low levels of self-esteemin the prediction of moderately severe depression
(Abela& Skitch, 2007). Contraryto hypothesis, however, this interaction also appliedto childrenwith low levelsof
dysfunctional attitudesand high self-esteem. Age did not modifytheserelations despitechildrenas young as six being
Please cite thisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H., et al, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability fordepression amongchildren andadolescents: A
cognitive scienceand developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review(2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
R.H.Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychology Reviewxx (2007) xxx-xxx 11

included, suggesting that young children can experience the deleterious effects of dysfunctional attitudes. In sum,
studies with children yield complex results, which may bedue todevelopmental ormeasurement issues. Self-esteem in
particular appears to impact the way in which dysfunctional attitudes impact children at risk for depression. Within
adolescent samples, dysfunctional attitudes appear to represent a vulnerability factor; however, it has yet to be
demonstrated that the reverse relation is not also true.

4. Self-perception

A competency-based model asserts that negative events in a child's life lead to maladaptive self-cognitions that
predispose a child todepression (Cole, 1990). Cole's model posits thatnegative self-perceptions regarding competence
may serve as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression. This reasoning is congruent with cognitive models of
depression in emphasizing self-schemata asproximal todepression onset (e.g., Abramson etal., 1978,1988). Negative
self-perceptions arebelieved to result from thenegative competency evaluations of significant others, such as parents
and teachers. A child's self-perception of competency may interact with others' appraisals to influence depression.
Studies testing Cole's model represent a majority ofexisting research inthis area. We examine theexisting literature to
evaluate self-perception as a cognitive vulnerability to depression in youth.
Definitional issues are important when reviewing this literature. Self-concept and self-esteem represent broad
constructs andencompass a range of components including cognitive processes, personality style, affective processes,
andmotivational domains. Harter's (1985) scale of self-perceived competence is oneof the most frequently employed
measures of self-esteem among youth. Germane to research on vulnerability for depression are scales assessing
perceptions of personal competence, rather than omnibus measures of self-worth. Accordingly, we do not review
studies that include only Harter's general self-worth scale. This stems from a desire to incorporate a wide range of
possible cognitive vulnerabilities, yet still restrict ourreview to cognitive, as opposed to personality, phenomena.

4.1. Review

Table 1presents prospective studies that explore self-perception anddepression. Evidence supporting negative self-
perception as a proximal vulnerability to depressive symptoms is found in eight (Cole, Jacquez, & Maschman, 2001;
Cole, Martin, & Powers, 1997; Cole, Martin, Powers, & Truglio, 1996; Hilsman & Garber, 1995; Kistner, Balthazor,
Risi, & Burton, 1999; Measelle et al., 1998;Ohannessian, Lemer, Lemer, & von Eye, 1999; Tram& Cole, 2000)of the
fourteen studies. Mixed evidence is found in threestudies (Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski, & Fier, 1999; Hoffman,
Cole, Martin, Tram, & Seroczynski, 2000; Kistner, David-Ferdon, Repper, & Joiner, 2006), with an additional three
studies (Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski, & Hoffman, 1998; Lewinsohn et al., 1994; McGrath & Repetti, 2002)
presenting evidence thatdepression predicts self-perception. Thus, theempirical base forself-perception asa cognitive
vulnerability in youth is decidedly mixed.
Evidence that self-perception results from depression, or thatthe relation may be reciprocal, is drawn from a few
well-designed studies. Among fourth graders followed prospectively through sixthgrade, depression symptoms predict
change in children's negative self-evaluations (McGrath & Repetti, 2002). Reciprocal relations arealso illustrated in a
study of third and fifth graders (Kistner et al., 2006). Similarly, inaccurate self-perception predicts increases in
depressive symptoms anddepressive symptoms, in turn, predict decreased accuracy in self-perception (Kistner et al.,
2006). Academic overestimation predicts depression at many grade levels, but the reverse relation yields stronger
effects (Cole, 1999). Underestimated competency predicts increases in depression within few grade levels; however,
thereverse relation is found in allgrades (Cole, 1998). Partial support forreciprocal models is also found by Hoffman
et al. (2000). These studies support the reciprocal, or transactional, relations between self-perception anddepression.
Yet another possibility isthat relations shift across development with self-perception leading todepression earlier in
development, while later indevelopment the opposite relation may emerge. Adevelopmental task ofmiddle childhood
isthe construction ofa personal sense ofone'sown competencies (Garber, 1984). Children may become increasingly
capable of drawing realistic judgments about their competence asthey grow. From a developmental psychopathology
framework, it is plausible that normative developmental stressors influence emerging perception regarding
competence, thereby increasing risk of depressive symptoms (e.g., Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). As such, inaccurate or
negative self-perception may serve as a mediator of the relation between life stress and depression. Tram and Cole
(2000) assessed these effects among ninth graders. Support was consistent for a mediational model whereby negative
Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R. H., et aL, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science anddevelopmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
12 RJI. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

events predicted changes inself-perception, and self-perception predicted changes indepressive symptoms. Incontrast,
little evidence for moderation emerged. Inthis regard, self-perception may represent a mechanism, whereby salient life
events affect mood.
Few studies examining relations between self-perception and mood have included simultaneous assessment of life
events. This issurprising and may contribute todiscrepant findings, asadverse events may benecessary to activate a
child's latent negative self-perception inaparticular domain. Inone study, a 'grade deficit stressor' was employed ina
sample ofsixth graders wherein children were asked todefine their own level ofacceptable grades (Hilsman &Garber,
1995). Children with negative self-perception of their academic achievement expressed more depressive symptoms
after receiving unacceptable grades than did students without negative self-perception. The inclusion of life stress
assessment inthe study of self-perception must beincorporated in future research to adequately test the role ofself-
perception as a cognitivevulnerability fector.
We propose that the presence ofreciprocal, ortransactional, relations between self-perception and depression may
account forthemixed state oftheliterature. Findings may also differ depending onwhich aspects ofself-perception are
studied, at which points of development assessments are conducted, and what levels of baseline depression are
included. Temporal and causal precedence have not yetbeen well-established. The literature offers support for self-
perception as contributing to depression, butoffers parallel support for self-perception asresulting from depression.
Self-perception does not predict onset of first-episode MDD among youth (Lewinsohn et al., 1994). Seff-perception
does, however, relate to current and past depression. Thus, it is currently unclear whether Cole's (1990) model
maps well onto a clinical diagnosis of depression. The possibility remains mat self-perception may simply represent
a concomitant ofdepressed mood. We also believe, inline with Cole (1990), that early indevelopment, self-perception
may serve as a mechanism, whereby life events affect levels of depression. That is, negative self-perception may
mediate thisrelationship. Later in development, self-perception may come to moderate thisassociation.
Methodological difficulties may also contribute to discrepancies between studies. Harter's (1985) measure,
although systematically constructed from a strong conceptual base, may contribute to divergent findings. Some
subscales demonstrate minimally acceptable reliability (Harter, 1985), warranting psychometric refinement Harter's
(1985) multifeceted scale may also lead to confusion among younger children. Researchers may wish to use the
Pictorial Scale ofPerceived Competence and Acceptance for Young Children (PSPCSA; Harter, 2002; Harter &Pike,
1984), which does not rely entirely onlanguage comprehension and allows for assessment ofyoung children. Lastly, it
ispossible that the construct ofself-perception istoo conceptually broad. Theoretical coherence inthis area of study
may lead to more congruent findings. In sum, research on self-perception among youth has not conclusively
establishedwhether it represents a cognitivevulnerability fector.

5. An integrative perspective

Overall, this body of literature implicates several cognitive factors in vulnerability for depression among youth.
As we have seen, research to date is not definitive and has produced complex results. Over-reliance on self-report
measures is a notable weakness. It is clear that children are capable of experiencing the type of cognitions under
discussion; however, whether or nota child canreliably hold these cognitions consciously in mind andreport them is
currently debatable. Similarly, the questionable reliability of extant measures is problematic.
Due tothese challenges, wepropose thatincorporating informationprocessing paradigms into thestudy ofcognitive
vulnerability allows for a more developmentally sensitive and adequate test Indeed, we note that Clark and Beck
(1999) define the cognitive model "as an information processing theory... understood in terms of the structures,
processes, and products involved in the representation and transformation of information" (pp. 109-110). According
to the cognitive model, biases in information processing are core aspects of depression. Information processing
paradigms, such asthe Stroop and dot-probe tasks, measure cognitive processes outside ofthe child's awareness. Such
measures may offer more objective tests of cognitive operations, while also allowing forexperimental manipulation.
Lower order phenomena, such as attentional biases, may represent themechanisms thatlead to cognitive vulnerability
products. Ifthis is thecase, thedevelopmental study ofthese processes would allow researchers to explore theorigins
of maladaptive cognitions. Moreover, information processing biases may represent markers for broader cognitive
vulnerability factors. Studies incorporating both self-report cognitive content and information processing para
digms allow researchers to refine cognitive vulnerability models, fostering theoretical andempirical advancements in
psychopathology research.

Please citethisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H.,et aL, Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:I0.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
R.H.Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychology Reviewxx (2007)xxx-xxx 13

On the other hand, information processing paradigms often suffer from questionable reliability and validity.
Relations between theoretical constructs and information processing paradigms are oftenunclear(Vasey, Dalgleish, &
Silverman, 2003). Convergent validity withother measures hypothesized to tapsimilarunderlying concepts is lacking.
Similarly, little is known about the reliability of information processing measures (Vasey et al., 2003). This low
reliability canreduce statistical power (e.g., Nicewander &Price, 1983). Moreover, asVasey etal.(2003) point out,the
clinical utility of these measures in children is notestablished. Nevertheless, information processing paradigms offer a
valuable framework for conceptualizing and studying cognition in psychopathology (Vasey et al., 2003). While
experimental paradigms have pitfalls, they do nothave the same pitfalls asself-report methods. It is likely thathigher
order andlower order cognition reciprocally relate inthedevelopment ofspecific cognitive vulnerabilities. Aswe have
seen, topdown cognition constrains the manner in which information is processed at lower levels of thesystem, and
vice versa (e.g., Dalgleish, 2002). The consequences of these two processes in interaction may be greater than either
cognitive process inisolation. Integration ofthese methods would allow for theassessment of such interactive effects.
Anintegrative cognitive approach tovulnerability for depression represents a critical step inestablishing a cognitive
science base to guide empirically supported treatments (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 2007; Matthews, 2006). In order to
advance this integrative perspective, webriefly review the information processing literature testing emotional stimuli in
children and adolescents. The information processing studies we review are not longitudinal. However, we explore
cross-sectional findings and discuss how these paradigms can be applied to the study of cognitive vulnerability.
Samples include children and adolescents with a diagnosis of depression, varying levels of depressive symptoms,
recovered depressed, and children of depressed mothers. We do not review the procedures involved in information
paradigms, butrefer the reader to Matthews and MacLeod (1994) and Garber and Kaminski (2000).

5.1. Review

Table 2presents studies ofinformation processing biases and depression among children and adolescents. Level of
depression isassociated with greater recall ofnegative information relative topositive information inyouth (Bishop,
Dalgleish, &Yule, 2004; Cole &Jordan, 1995; Drummond, Dritschel, Astell, O'Carroll, &Dalgleish, 2006; Rudolph,
Hammen, &Burge, 1997; Taylor &Ingram, 1999; Zupan, Hammen, &Jaenicke, 1987; for null results see Dalgleish
et al., 2003; and Hammen & Zupan, 1984). This association is also found among children and adolescents with a
diagnosis ofMDD (Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Taghavi, Yule, &Dalgleish, 2000). Depressed youth demonstrate higher
rates ofrehearsal ofnegative memories (Kuyken &Howell, 2000). They recall positive information less well (Whitman
& Leitenberg, 1990) and reveal significantly fewer positive autobiographical memories (Drummond et al., 2006).
Lower rates ofpositive adjective endorsement also occur among psychiatric inpatient youth (Gencoz, Voelz, Gencoz,
Pettit, &Joiner, 2001). Furthermore, depressed youth rate more negative words as self-descriptive (Timbremont &
Braet, 2004). Youth atrisk for depression demonstrate memory biases for negative self-descriptions (Hammen, 1988).
These self-descriptions can interact with life stress to result in onset or exacerbation of depression (Hammen &
Goodman-Brown, 1990). Insum, information processing paradigms reveal abias toward negative stimuli among youth
with symptoms or a diagnosis ofdepression.
Information processing paradigms also highlight overgeneral memory biases among depressed children and
adolescents (Kuyken, Howell, & Dalgleish, 2006; Park, Goodyer, & Teasdale, 2002). Rumination appears to
exacerbate this effect (Park etal., 2002). There isalso evidence that depression impairs the memory ofnegative events.
Children and adolescents with clinically significant levels of depression (as measured by the CDI) show impaired
memory for negative events (Hughes, Worchel, Stanton, Stanton, &Hall, 1990). Depression severity is also related to
less specific negative memories among adolescents in residential treatment (Swales, Williams, & Wood, 2001).
Deficits inmemory for fearful feces are exhibited among children ofdepressed parents (Pine etal., 2004). Incontrast
no such effect is found in reaction times to the detection of threatening versus non-threatening faces (Hadwin et al.,
2003). Lastly, depressed youth present with significantly longer reaction times to negative emotional working memory
tasks compared to neutral tasks (Ladouceur et al., 2005). Thus, depression is related to memory biases in youth, but
these relations are complex and worthyof additional study.
Research regarding relations between deployment ofattention and mood issimilarly complex. More attention is
given to negative stimuli by depressed youth than the non-depressed (Kyte, Goodyer, &Sahakian, 2005), and slower
response rates on an attention cuing task are found among childrenofparents with depression (Perez-Edgar, Fox, Cohn,
&Kovacs, 2006). However, studies with attentional dot-probe tasks for words do not support attentional biases in
Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R.H., etal., Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
Table 2
Information processingamong youth
q\ a Study N Age Depression Sample Cognitive measure Summary of findings
g S.
in & measure

2. £•
s &
Bishop etal. 121 (5-11) DSRS High and low non-clinicallydepressed Emotional stories Higher depression showed enhanced recallofnegative stories
p. o
(2004) children as assessed by DSRS) recall task relative to positive storiescomparedto low depressed group.
from school
S-8'
Cole and 394 (9-15) CDI School Incidental recall Higher depression recall fewer positiveself-referential words, only
€5 o"
8 Jordan significant in grade 8.
P M
(1995)
Dalgleish 80 (9-18) DRSS 15 diagnosed depressed, 22 diagnosed Dot probe, Stroop, Depressed estimatednegativeevents equallylikely to happento
etal. (1997) anxious, 43 controls subjective them as others.
a v probability, and
word memory
f i1 Dalgleish et al. 71 (9-18) MFQ 24 recovered depressed (K-SADS) and 47 Subjective Recovered depressed ratenegative events as less likely to occur, all 'KV>
(1998) school control Probability estimated negative more likely to happen to others than themselves.
S" g Questionnaire
ll Dalgleish etal. 67
(2003)
(7-18) DSRS MDD, PTSD, GAD patients and school Dot probe, Stroop,
subjective
No depression biases in comparison to other groups, but depressed
group was significantly higher than otherson anxiety self-report
control
probability, and
r s, word memory
<$ Drummond 70 (7-11) CDI School sample with CDI cutoff AMT Young dysphoric children recallmore specific negative and fewer
et al. (2006) specific positive. Older dysphoric difficulty in retrieving specific a '.. ::::• •
S. 5. negative memories. In general, children's specific negative recall i ~ ..'.•
5 •

improveswith age. Dysphoric bad significantly fewer positive >>
i"'."ir]i.
AMs.
°. ffi a*
Ci 3
• er
Gencoz et al. 58 (9-17) CDI Psychiatric inpatients with various chart SRET Lowerrates of positiveadjectiveendorsement and recall associated «

(2001) diagnoses, 44% non-bipolarmood with depression, but not anxiety. 8


-a ' •'':'!•.
Hadwin et al. 38 in (6-10) CDI School Visual search for No effects of depression on searchtime. a
5s» : •(.•'.•%•
• »i
(2003) exp 1 threatening and non-
<=> s-
S-8 threateningfaces
Hammen 79 (8-16) CDI, 16 children of depressed mothers, 10 of Self-Schema Task When controlling for baseline CDI, trendtowardself-schema scores
•ei "•
p' o (1988) K-SADS bipolar, 18 ofmedically ill mothers, 35 of as predictorof affective diagnosis.
to P
o is normal
O R
Hammen and 64 (8-16) K-SADS 12 children of depressed mothers, 12 of Self-Schema Task Significant association between onsetor exacerbation of depression
P<ff Goodman- bipolar, 14 of medically ill, 26 of normal and the experienceof stressors relevant to child's self-schema.
§8- Brown Particularly marked effect for children of depressed mothers.
I" (1990)
Hammen and 61 (7-12) CDI School Incidental Recall, Non-depressed higherproportion of positive self-descriptive words
Zupan Self-Schema Task, (but not significant), no evidenceofnegative biasin depressed.
(1984)
Hughes et al. 322 (10-13) CDI, School with cutoff Recall and Significant interaction between group status andvalence of story
(1990) PNID recognition events for recognition. Depression symptoms impair memory for
negative story events.
if
<•• Q
Joorman et al.
(2007)
41 (9-14) CDI-S 21 girls of mothers with recurrent MDD, 20 Dot probewith faces
girls ofmothers with no history of Axis 1
High risk daughters selectively attendedto negative facial
expressions.Control daughters selectively attended to positive facial
expressions
Kelvin et al. 102 14.0 MFQ Community sample defined as at AMALSS with prime Adolescents with high emotionality endorsed morenegativeself-
(1999) risk by level of emotionality descriptorsafter a dysphoric, but not neutral,mood induction.
p. o
Kuyken and 65 (12-18) BDI-H DSM-IV diagnosis of MDD recruited from AMT Depressed more likely to retrievememories from observer
Howell community perspective andmorerecenttime period. Also rehearsed negative
(2000) memories and rated as more personally important.
•&& Kuyken et al. 62 (12-18) BDI-II DSM-IV diagnosis from SCID recruited AMT MDD and no trauma demonstrated overgeneral memory bias.
B £ (2006) from community
If Kyte et aL
(2005)
79 15.3 MFQ 30 patientsdiagnosedwith first onset
MDD (K-SADS) and school control
CANTAB, WCS,
affective Go-No-Go,
Depressed greater attention to sad stimuli, moreimpulsivewhen
making decisions.
decision making
Ladouceur 75 (8-16) CDI, BDI 16 MDD diagnosed(K-SADS) from Emotional n-back MDD andco-morbid significantly longer reaction timeson negative
i et al. (2005) inpatient and outpatient clinics: 17 anxiety, (working memory emotional backgrounds compared to neutral.
24 co-morbid depression and anxiety, 18 task)
low-risk normal control recruited from
community
:^S Martin et al. 63 3rd and CDI Children with low and high peer-rated Emotional Stroop with Forunpopularchildren,greater friendship valuingand greater a'
(2003) 6th popularity negative social words negative social word Stroop interference predictedincreasesin
*:8 graders depressive symptoms across 6 months.
Murray et al.
(2001)
95 5.0 None 55 children of mothers with depression
(SADS-L) and 40 control
Verbal and nonverbal When dealt a losing hand, exposed childrenmade more negative
expressions card game expressions compared to non-exposed children.
i
5. 5- (9-18) CDI, MFQ 19 DSM-IV/ICD-10 depressed, CDI, MFQ Stroop No significant differences.
:*•••}:•
Neshat-Doost
et aL (1997)
64
cutoff, 19 mixed anxious/depressed and 26
I
si controls recruited from clinics
I
Neshat-Doost 38 (10-17) CDI, DSRS 19 patients with ICD-10 diagnosis and Recallandrecognition Depressed grouprecalled significandymore negativeadjectives, this
et aL (1998) MFQ cutoff score on CDI, DSRS, and MFQ memory tasks depressionrelated bias became strongerwith age. No bias in
If and 19 school controls recognition task. T5
© s>
j^. ••*»• Neshat-Doost 55 (9-17) DSRS 19 with DSM-IV MDD diagnosis, 24 Attentionaldot-probe No support for attentional bias.
^ .2 et al. (2000) school controls task
Park et al. 155 (12-17) MFQ, 96 clinically referred adolescents with AMT Adolescentswith current MDD more categoric overgeneral
"« (2002) HDRS MDD, 26 non-depressed psychiatric, memories than controls,but not more thannon-depressed.
and 33 community controls (K-SADS)
Park, Goodyer, 134 (12-17) MFQ, 75 first onset MDD, 26 non-depressed AMT with prime Rumination increased overgeneral memoriesto negativecues
and HDRS psychiatric assessed (K-SADS-PL) and in MDD patients.
It Teasdale 33 community controls
(2004)
Perez-Edgar 33 (6-10) None 16 children of parents with child onset Posner task under Children of parents with COD were slower in theirresponserates
et al. (2006) depression (COD) diagnosed (SCID) versus neutral and affective compared with control children, subtle deficits in selective attention.
community control conditions
Pine et al. 152 (9-19) PARIS 19 children of depressed parents (SCID) Face memory task MDD offspring significantdeficits in memory selectively for fearful
(2004) and 133 anxiety or healthy faces, but not happy or angry faces. MDD not associated with
face-memory accuracy.
(continued on nextpage) £
o -a. Table 2 (continued)
tl
a.
Study JV Age Depression Sample Cognitive measure Summary of findings
J? 9. (range) measure
ut H
O mean

a
B
to

8 Possel et al. 92 (13-15) SBB-DES School children divided into groups based SRET No significantdifferences on SRET.
•|8
rl
o (2006) on depression measure
P. o
Prieto, Cole, 50 (8-12) CDI 15 clinic-depressed, 18 clinic-non- Self-Schema Task Significandy more positive words recalled by non-depressed children.
a. IS
18 and Tageson depressed, and 17 control children This relationdid not hold after effects of word recognition
n r?
(1992)
•JT §
controlled.
B ca Reid et al. 133 (8-14) CDI School Self-Schema Task, Psychopathology (externalizing andinternalizing) was associated with
(2006) Attentional dot probe, hypervigilance for threat cues,negative interpretations of social
•a
X Attribution vignettes situations, internal attributions, perception of hostile intent, and recall : /: '. .':;!
of negative self-descriptions. None of these relations were specific
1
•a
(9

to depression. | •'**•-"•:•'- '


8 P- &
Rudolph et al. 81 (8-12) CDI School Story task with recall Depressed children significandy greater recall of negative maternal s • ';'-;'. :

1I (1997) andlevel of processing attributes thannon-depressed on recall from storytask.Non-


a*
n
• --in!
. ...; ••-)
,_.-,_
'
!
i
,

Q
a"
5: task depressed demonstrated greater recall of positive attributes on levelof Q
2f I processingtask. -s»
1. S. Swales et al. 46 14.2 BDI Residential patients with ICD-10 AMT More depressed less specific,but many patients recalling the same o .' : * ';* : <
*J3 ex
a*
•- •,'• •.•-,••

S y (2001) psychiatricdiagnosis and school control traumatic memory (parasuicide) over and over. o' i!; v
Q
5 8 Tagbavi et aL 67 (9-18) DSRS 24 anxious, 19 mixed anxious-depressed Attentional dot-probe No attentionalbias in mixed anxious-depressed children.
,u
o
*•>
*J
(1999) primary diagnosis DSM-IV, 24 normal task <i
^ 8 O
school controls
^2 1.
p. Taylor and 86 (8-12) CDI 40 offspring of depressed mothers SRET with prime Primed at risk children demonstrateda less positive self-concept and
a
A
n
«
<
a
Ingram (SCID) high-risk and 46 low-risk a higher proportion ofnegative endorsed words recalled. K3
>}
13 2 (1999) I.V.U.!)-...
o fT %
o
e- Timbremont 44 (8-16) CDI 19 depressed, 15 never depressed, and 10 SRETwith prime Remitted depressed rated more negative words as self-descriptive. a
«
O"
g- E
and Braet remitted depressed (CDI cutoff) 8 : >V".i '-• '
; • <•!,
«3 (2004) in residential facility •fc
o
o 3»
•i Vrielynck, 60 (9-13) MDI-C 15 depressed children, 25 never depressed AMT Depressed children gave fewerspecificmemories. a
<3
o
8- Deplus, and 11.5 clinical, and 20 control
&> •o
<i*
'i Philippot
-8
si £•
o
(2007)
to
o
13
Whitman and 52 4th-6th CDI 26 with score at least 1 SD above normative Word association task Evidence that depressedrecall positiveless well.
o
•>J g Leitenberg grade mean on CDI and 26 school controls
»~
B
o (JO (1990)
b
o & Zupan, 81 (8-16) CDI 20 children of women with depression Self-schema incidental Depressed demonstrated recall of negative self-descriptive adjectives.
0\ c
Hammen, (K-SADS), 21 not depressed memory task
9"
0 and Jaenicke
g (1987)
p.
to
a.
o
Note. Ageranges and means reported when available. Grades reported when age data was not available. AMT =autobiographical memory task; SRET =self-referent encoding task; AMALSS =
& Assessment ofMood Activated Latent Self-schema; CANTAB =Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, WCS =Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; DSRS =The Depression Self-Rating
Scale; K-SADS =Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children; CDI =Children's Depression Inventory; SCID =Structured Clinical Interview for theDiagnostic and
a
Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders; SADS-L = Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia — Long; MFQ =The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; ICD-10 =International Statistical
> Classification of Diseases and RelatedHealth Problems 10th Revision; DSM-IV = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of MentalDisorders — 4th edition.
R.H.Jacobs et al. / ClinicalPsychology Reviewxx (2007) xxx-xxx 17

depressed youth (Dalgleish et al.,2003; Neshat-Doost et al.,2000; Taghavi, Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Yule, & Dalgleish,
1999). Children with MDD are more easily distracted by negative pictures than neutral pictures, whereas control
children are more distracted by positive pictures (Ladouceur et al., 2005). Moreover, recent work suggests that
daughters of depressed mothers selectively attend to negative facial expressions, whereas control daughters selectively
attend to positive fecial expressions (Joorman, Talbot, & Gotlib, 2007). In this study, a dot-probe task was used, but
fecesinsteadofwords were selectedas stimuli.Ofnote, fecialexpressionparadigmsresult in more consistentfindings
than verbal within the adult literature, as do studies using longerstimulusdurations (e.g., Gotlib et al., 2004). In sum,
relations between depression and attention in youth are not yet well understood.
This body of literature affirms that symptomatically depressed youth demonstrate memory and attention biases.
However, thesestudies are overwhelmingly cross-sectional anddo not explicate whether thesebiases contribute to the
etiology or maintenance of depression. Results from longitudinal studies with adults suggest that information
processing paradigms may reveal key processing biases underlying depression. Cognitive biases predict change in
depressive symptoms in community samples (Rude, Wenzlaff, Gibbs, Vane, & Whitney, 2002). Pregnant mothers
recalling more negative words ona self-referential encoding taskdemonstrate more symptoms of depression following
childbirth (Bellow & Hill, 1991). Among adults with MDD, greater recall of positive words on the self-referential
encoding taskuniquely predicts a decrease indepression symptoms (Johnson, Joorman, &Gotlib, 2007). A tendency to
shift attention toward negative information following anemotional prime interacts withlife stress to predict increases in
symptoms of depression seven weeks later (Beevers & Carver, 2003). Thus, there is good evidence that information
processing paradigms are useful in predicting depression longitudinally among adults.
Among theyouth-focused information processing research, only one study followed participants longitudinally and
tested relations with depression (Hammen, 1988). A child's memory bias toward negative self-descriptions predicted
affective diagnosis across sixmonths (Hammen, 1988). However, this relation was only observed atthelevel ofa trend.
In a related literature, Martin et al. (2003) observed higher levels of Stroop interference among children whoattached
greater importance to friendships and whose peers rated them as having few friends. This interference predicts
increases in depressive symptoms over a six month period. Martin and colleagues propose that the Stroop paradigm
may be more sensitive to children's social concerns than traditional paper-and-pencil measures. A limited body of
evidence suggests that information processing paradigms may predict depression across time. Clearly, more research is
necessary to establish the prospective relations between these lower order processes and depression among youth.
The relations between information processing and cognitive content measures are relatively unknown among youth
as well. Inan adult example, individuals athigh cognitive risk for depression inthe CVD exhibited more self-referent
information processing biases than individuals with low cognitive risk (Alloy, Abramson, Murray, Whitehouse, &
Hogan, 1997). Similarly, among high cognitive risk participants, the self-referent information processing task battery
(SRIP; Alloy et al., 1997) partially mediated cognitive risk effects (Steinberg, Oelrich, Alloy, & Abramson, 2004).
Within the same sample, the negative SRIP composite interacted with cognitive risk to predict first onset, but not
recurrence ofdepression. While there is some evidence that these constructs relate among adults, how these cognitive
processes relate among youth is currently unknown. A recent study (Reid, Salmon, & Lovibond, 2006), however,
interviewed children about their attributional style and gathered data on attention allocation and memory recall.
Information processing and cognitive content biases were congruent and associated with psychopathology, although
not to depression specifically. The integrative study of information processing biases and cognitive content vul
nerabilities may result in theoretical refinement of cognitive models of depression among youth.
We conclude that information processing paradigms offer a useful tool for investigating cognitive vulnerability for
depression among youth. However, the information processing studies reviewed assess youth with a range of
internalizing diagnoses. Many ofthese studies did not propose specific hypotheses inrelation to differing diagnoses,
clouding the theoretical utility of results. Moreover, a notable distinction between the cognitive content and
information processing literature is the use ofemotional priming. Emotional priming - the experimental induction of
mood for the purposes oftapping latent cognition - isused successfully within the information processing literature,
but has not yet been incorporated into prospective longitudinal studies ofcognitive vulnerability among youth. On the
other hand, information processing studies rarely investigate the effects oflife stress onthe accuracy ofinformation
processing. Emotional priming prior to measuring cognitive content via questionnaires allows for amore powerful and
adequate test ofcognitive vulnerability models, as the induction ofa negative mood state may allow individuals to
report latent schema (e.g., Persons &Miranda, 1992). Integrating the assessment ofstress, orimplementing astressor in
the lab, would similarly result inan effective assessment ofcognitive biases within information processing paradigms.
Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R.H.; etaL; Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
18 RH. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

Consequently, acombination ofinformation processing paradigms and self-reportquestionnaires permits researchers to


evaluate whether these higher and lower level cognitions do, in fact, influence one another in the development of
psychopathology.

6. Discussion

Several cognitive models ofvulnerability for depression have been proposed during recent years, and have attracted
empirical attention. Derived from research on cognitive concomitants ofdepression among adults, they propose thatthe
establishmentofmaladaptive schema, negative attributional style, and impaired self-perception may place youth atrisk
for major depression. Although evidence supporting each ofthese models has emerged, conceptual challenges remain,
and no single model adequately accounts for the full range offectors implicated inrisk for depression.
We envision significant advances in our understanding of cognitive vulnerability for depression through the
incorporation ofinformation processing paradigms into existing models. Capturing the relations between vulnerabi
lity fectors and depression may be facilitated through the study ofyouth within acognitive-developmental framework.
Interdisciplinary collaboration between developmental and clinical scientists will allow for broad conceptual
integration.
The use of emotional priming strategies and methodologies borrowed from research in experimental cognitive
psychology may shed light on cognitive vulnerability fectors among young children. According to Beck etal. (1979),
schemata "may be latent but can be activated by specific circumstances which are analogous to experiences initially
responsible for embedding the negative attitude" (p. 16). Germane toa discussion oflatent schemata, the mood-state
hypothesis proposes that cognitive vulnerabilities are accessible only during negative mood states (Persons &Miranda,
1992). This hypothesis is based on an associative-network model in which mood states cue related thoughts
(e.g., Bower, 1987). Persons and Miranda (1992) argue that, because depressogenic cognitions develop when one
experiences negative affect, cognition and affect are linked in memory. When not experiencing a negative mood,
cognitions related to negative affect may be inactive. Adolescents may experience more negative life events than
children (e.g., Ge et al., 1994), offering opportunities for latent cognitive schemata to become activated. The lower
average rates ofstress inearly childhood raise the possibility thatcognitive vulnerability fectors may bepresent, but not
activated. I£ despite emotional priming, cognitive vulnerability factors donot appear tobeassociated with depressive
symptoms among children as they are with adults, a developmental difference would be identified. Given the
possibility that lower order and higher order cognition appear to develop in tandem andreciprocally influence one
another, the inclusion of information processing paradigms will clarity the role of lower order and higher order
cognitive vulnerabilities. In sum, latent measurement of cognitive vulnerability through information processing
paradigms may allow fora broader understanding ofcognition inpsychopathology andaddress theoretical hypotheses
regarding cognitive vulnerability models.
Have the theoretical hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models gained strong support in studies of youth (e.g.,
Alloy et al., 1999; Ingram et al., 1998)? First, the stability of cognitive vulnerability factors among children and
adolescents remains unclear. Datasuggest that a degree of stability exists withinadolescent samples, whereas among
pre-pubertal youth, onlyshort term stability of cognitive content have beenreported. We note thatJust et al. (2001)
question the assumption thatcognitive vulnerability represents an immutable trait We propose thatin the downward
extension of cognitive vulnerability models to youth, allowance mustbe made for the variable nature of developing
cognition. Second, findings to date have not demonstrated temporal precedence of proposed cognitive vulnerability
fectors. Future research must assess prior episodes and follow young samples to make the detection of these
phenomena feasible and likely. Third, research suggests that a number of cognitive fectors may be implicated in
vulnerability fordepression. These factors have rarely beenexamined simultaneously within thesame sample. Little is
known, men, about howtheymay interact in placing youth atriskfordepression. Literatures surrounding each of the
putative riskfactors haveevolved independently. Synthesis, through integrative research paradigms is needed. Fourth,
some evidence has emerged for the specificity criterion (e.g., Gencoz et aL, 2001; Robinson et al., 1995). Given the
centrality of this hypothesis to cognitive models of psychopathology, additional research is needed. Last, the
endogenous and latent nature of cognitive vulnerability in youth is likely. Cognitive vulnerability factors appear to
reside within the child. However, an increased attention to ecologically valid life stress assessment, as well as the
incorporation of information processing paradigms and emotional priming into research design, allows for more
thorough investigation. In sum,the central hypotheses of cognitive vulnerability models haveyet to be put to the test.
Please citethisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H.,et aL, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability fordepression among children andadolescents: A
cognitive scienceand developmental perspective, ClinicalPsychology Review(2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
R.H. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx 19

We believe cognitive vulnerability research incorporating information processing paradigms will more adequately
address these theoretical hypotheses.

6.1. Future research

Several lines ofresearch will advance study and are detailed in Table 3. Framing the study ofcognitive vulnerability
within the broader domain of cognitive development will be essential. Age is, at best, a crude marker of ontogenetic
change (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Specific markers ofcognitivedevelopmentmust be realized. Sensitivity must be given
to how depressionand cognitive vulnerabilitymanifestin relationto the developmentof cognitive capacities and the
attainments of childhood and adolescence (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002). In addition to the integration of information
processing paradigms, the inclusion of neuropsychological measurement will be informative. Tracking normative
trajectories of cognitive and brain maturity will embed research on the etiology of mood disorders within a
developmental context. For example, a widely used tool, the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery
(CANTAB; see http://www.camcog.com), assesses executive functioning (for an example see Kyte, Goodyer, &
Sahakian, 2005). Such measurement allows for analysis of specific links between changes in depression and the
developmentofcognitive abilities, hi sum, these lines ofinquiry move scientists and clinicians toward a cognitive and
developmental base for the study and treatment of psychopathology.
In addition, integration with research in developmental biology and functional neuroimaging will advance our
understanding of the development of psychopathology among youth. For example, Cortisol and psychophysiology
assessment within studies of cognitive vulnerability may promote the identification of possible phenotypic and
endophenotypic markers ofrisk (e.g., Gottesman & Gould, 2003). As Bearden and Freimer (2006) note, the study of
heritable traits that can be reliably measured is fruitful in the study of psychiatric disorders. Identifying specific
processes and behaviors, or 'behavioral endophenotypes', advances the study of psychopathology (Cacioppo et al.,
2007; Prathikanti & Weinberger, 2005). In line with this argument, information processingparadigms may allow for
pre-morbid identification of vulnerability at younger ages. Linking these literatures is imperative work for psy
chopathology researchers.
The study ofcognitive models ofvulnerabihty for depressionhas illuminated mechanisms and factors important for
adolescent health. Much work, however, remains. This area is complex whereby individual, group, developmental,
biological, and environmental fectors impact cognition and depression. What is clear from existing cognitive
vulnerability research is that adolescence represents a phase wherein cognitive vulnerability is likely to become
apparent. However, giventhe presentdifficulty establishing temporalprimacyofcognitivediathesesto depression, we
recommend research with younger populations (e.g., ages 7-12 years) that incorporates information processing
paradigms. A developmental framework permits assessment of the child, accounting for their stageof growth within

Table 3
Recommendations for further research

Assessment • Community children followed from middle childhoodthroughentry to middle schooL


• Careful assessment of prior depression episodes to distinguish vulnerability from scar.
• Attentionto pubertal maturation, cognitivedevelopment, latent and explicitmeasures ofcognitivevulnerability and stress.
• Onset of clinical disorder assessed every six months.
• Youth followedthrough remission andrelapse to establish whethervulnerabilities apply to firstonset only.
• Study ofrumination and problem solving as possiblecognitivevulnerabilities.
• Integration of emotional priming and information processing paradigms.
Analyses • Mapnormal andabnormal trajectories through growth mixturemodeling to increase the likelihood thatreciprocal effects
between cognition and mood will be captured.
• Analyseswhich account forinitiallevel of depression, asmodelsmay only apply to asymptomatic at baseline sub-samples.
• Integrative and transactional models(e.g. Hankin & Abramson, 2001)assessing the rolesofvulnerabilities in relation to one
another.
Interdisciplinary • Establish developmental trajectories and'normal benchmarks' ofcognitive processes, allowing cognitive vulnerability factors
collaboration to be distinguished from normative developmentaldifferences.
• Cortisolassessmentallowing for integration with biological underpinnings ofdepression.
• Establish a taxonomy systemwith established agenorms for stress (Grant et aL, 2004a) to facilitate developmental
frameworks andallow forcomparison of stressors across samples.

Please citethisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H.,et aL,Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability fordepression among children andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
20 RJI. Jacobs et al / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

the dynamic, continuous, and reciprocal interactions ofthe environment Such investigation firmly places cognitive
vulnerability research within the developmental psychopathology perspective. Not only will this work advance the
scientific study ofpsychopathology, but itwill produce implications for treatment and prevention ofdisorder among
youth (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 2007; Matthews, 2006).

Acknowledgements

Preparation of this manuscript was supported bya F31 (MH075308) to Rachel H. Jacobs.

References

Abela, J. R. Z. (2001). The hopelessness theory ofdepression: Atest ofthe diathesis-stress and causal mediation components in third and seventh
grade children. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 29,241-254.
Abela, J. R.Z., &D'Alessandro, D. U. (2002). Beck's cognitive theory ofdepression: A test ofthe diathesis-stress and causal mediation components.
British Journal ofClinical Psychology, 41,111-128.
Abela, J. R. Z., &Payne, A.V. (2003). A test ofthe integration ofthe hopelessness and self-esteem theories ofdepression in schoolchildren.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27,519-535.
Abela, J. R.Z., &Sarin, S. (2002). Cognitive vulnerability to hopelessness depression: A chain isonly as strong as its weakest link. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 26,811-829.
Abela, J. R. Z., & Skitch, S. A. (2007). Dysfunctional attitudes, self-esteem, and hassles: Cognitive vulnerability to depression in children of
affectivelyill parents. Behaviour Researcli and Therapy, 45,1127-1140.
Abela, J. R.Z.,Skitch, S.A., Adams, P., &Hankin, B. L.(2006). The timing of parent and child depression: A hopelessness theory perspective.
Journal ofClinical ChildandAdolescent Psychology, 35,253-263.
Abela, J. R. Z., & Sullivan, C. (2003). A test of Beck's cognitive diathesis-stress theory of depression inearly adolescents. Journal ofEarly
Adolescence, 23,384-404.
Abramson, L.Y., Alloy, L.B.,&Metalsky, G. I.(1988). The cognitive diathesis-stress theories ofdepression: Toward an adequate evaluation ofthe
theories' validities. In L. B. Alloy(Ed.), Cognitive processes indepression (pp. 3-30).NewYork: Guilford Press.
Abramson, L.Y.,Metalsky, G. L,&Alloy, L.B.(1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype ofdepression. PsychologicalReview, 96,
358-372.
Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness inhumans: Critique and reformulation. Journal ofAbnormal
Psychology, 87,49-74.
Alloy, L.B., Abramson, L.Y.,Murray, L.A.,Whitehouse, W. G., &Hogan, M. E. (1997). Self-referent information-processing inindividuals athigh
and low cognitive risk for depression. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 539-568.
Alloy, L.B.,Abramson, L.Y.,Walshaw, P. D., Keyser, J., &Gerstein, R.K. (2006). Acognitive vulnerability-stress perspective onbipolar spectrum
disorders in a normative adolescent brain, cognitive, and emotional development contextDevelopment andPsychopathology, 18,1055-1103.
Alloy, L.Y, Abramson, L.Y, Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Tashman, N. A.,Sternberg, D. L., etal. (1999). Depressogenic cognitive styles:
Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behavior Research and Therapy, 37,
503-531.
Angold, A.,Erkanli, A.,Silberg, J., Eaves, L.,&Costello, E.J. (2002). Depression scale scores in8-17-year-olds: Effects ofage and gender. Journal
ofChild Psycliology and Psycliiatry, 43,1052-1063.
Bearden, C. E.,& Freimer, N. B. (2006). Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: Ready for primetime? "Rends in Genetics, 22,306-313.
Beck,A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes andtreatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In P. J. Clayton & J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of depression: Old
controversies and new approaches(pp. 265-290). New York: Raven Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. R, & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy ofdepression. New York: GuilfordPress.
Beevers, C G., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Attentional bias and mood persistence as prospective predictors of dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 27, 619-637.
Bellow, M.,&Hill, A. B.(1991). Schematic processing andtheprediction ofdepression following childbirth. Personality andIndividualDifferences,
12,943-949.
Bennett, D. S., & Bates, J. E. (1995). Prospective models of depressive symptoms in early adolescence: Attributional style, stress, and support
Journal ofEarly Adolescence, 15,299-315.
Bishop, S. J., Dalgleish, T., & Yule,W. (2004). Memory foremotional stories in highandlow depressed children. Memory, 12,214-230.
Bower,G. H. (1987). Commentaryon mood and memory.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25,443-455.
Brozina, K., & Abela,J. R. Z. (2006). Symptoms ofdepression andanxiety inchildren: Specificity of thehopelessness theory. Journal of Clinical
Child and Adolescent Psycliology, 35,515-527.
Brown,G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Socialorigins ofdepression: A studyofpsychiatric disorder in women. New York:Wiley.
Cacioppo, J.T., AmaraL, D.G., Blanchard, J. J.,Cameron, J. L., Carter, C. S., Crews, D.,et aL(2007). Socialneuroscience progress andimplications
for mental health. Perspectives on Psycliological Science, 2,99-123.
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 70,6-20.

Please cite thisarticle as: Jacobs, R. H.,et aL,Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability fordepression amongchildren andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmentalperspective, ClinicalPsychology Review(2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006 ,
RJt. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx 21

Cicchetti, D., &Toth, S.L. (1998). The development ofdepression in children and adolescents. American Psycliologist, 53,221-241.
Clark, D. A.,&Beck, A.T. (1999). Scientificfoundations ofcognitive theory ofdepression. New York: John Wiley.
Cole, D. A.(1990). Relation ofsocial and academic competence to depressive symptoms in childhood. Journal ofAbnormalPsychology, 99,422-429.
Cole, D. A., Jacquez, F. M., &Maschman, T. L. (2001). Social origins ofdepressive cognitions: Alongitudinal study ofself-perceived competence in
children. Cognitive Therapy andResearch, 25,377-395.
Cole, D. A., &Jordan, A. E. (1995). Competence and memory: Integrating psychosocial and cognitive correlates ofchild depression. Child
Development, 66,459-473.
Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. A.,Seroczynski, A.D., &Fier, J. (1999). Children's over- and underestimation ofacademic competence: A
longitudinal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development, 70,459-473.
Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. A., Seroczynski, A. D., &Hoffman, K. (1998). Are negative cognitive errors predictive or reflective of
depressive symptoms in children: A longitudinal study. Journal ofAbnormal Psycliology, 107,481-496.
Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., &Powers, B. (1997). Acompetency-based model ofchild depression: Alongitudinal study ofpeer, parent, teacher, and
self-evaluations. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry, 38, 505-514.
Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Powers, B., &Truglio, R. (1996). Modeling causal relations between academic and social competence and depression: A
multitrait-multimethod longitudinal study ofchildren. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 105,258-270.
Cole, D. A., &Turner, J. E. (1993). Models ofcognitivemediation and moderation in childdepression. JournalofAbnormalPsycliology, 102,271-281.
Conley, C.S., Haines, B. A., Hilt, L. M., &Metalsky, G. I. (2001). The Children's Attributional Style Interview: Developmental tests ofcognitive
diathesis-stress theories of depression. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 20,445-463.
Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Bums, B. J., Erkanli, A., StangL D. K., &Tweed, D. L. (1996). The Great Smoky Mountains Study ofYouth: Functional
impairment and serious emotional disturbance. Archives ofGeneral Psychiatry, 53,1137-1143.
D'Alessandro, D. U. &Abela, J. R. Z. (2000). The ChUdren's Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. Unpublished manuscript, McGill University.
Dalgleish, T. (2002). Information processing approaches to emotion. In R. J. Davidson K. R. Scherer &H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
Affective Sciences (pp. 661-673). New York: Oxford University Press.
Dalgleish, T., Neshat-Doost, H., Taghavi, R., Moradi, A., Yule, W., Canterbury, R., et al. (1998). Information processing in recovered depressed
children and adolescents. Journal ofChild Psychology andPsychiatry, 39,1031-1035.
Dalgleish, X, Taghavi, R., Neshat-Doost, H., Moradi, A., Canterbury, R., &Yule, W. (2003). Patterns ofprocessing bias for emotional ^formation
across clinical disorders: Acomparison ofattention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized
anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal ofClinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32,10-21.
Dalgleish, T., Taghavi, R., Neshat-Doost, H., Moradi, A., Yule, W., &Canterbury, R. (1997). Information processing in clinically depressed and
anxious children and adolescents. Journal ofChild Psychology andPsychiatry, 38,535-541.
Demetriou, A., Christou, C, Spanoudis, G., &Platsidou, M. (2002). The development ofmental processing: Efficiency, working memory, and
thinking. Monographs ofthe Societyfor Research in Child Development, 67 (1, Serial No. 268).
Demetriou, A., Efklides, A., &Platsidou, M. (1993). The architecture and dynamics ofdeveloping mind: Experiential structurahsm as aframe for
unifying cognitive development theories. Monographs ofthe Societyfor Researcli in Child Development, 58 (5/6, Serial No. 234).
Dixon, J. F., &Ahrens, A. H. (1992). Stress and attributional style as predictors of self-reported depression in children. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 16, 623-634.
Donald, M. (2001). Amind sorare: The development ofhuman consciousness. New York: Norton.
Drummond, L. E., DritscheL B., Astell, A., O, CarrolL R. E., &Dalgleish, T. (2006). Effects ofage, dysphoria, and emoUon-focusmg on
autobiographical memory specificity in children. Cognition and Emotion, 20,488-505.
Garber, J. (1984). Classification ofchildhood psychopathology: Adevelopmental perspective. Child Development, 55,30-48.
Garber, J., &Kaminski, K. M. (2000). Laboratory and performance-based measures ofdepression in children and adolescents. Journal ofClinical
ChildPsychology, 29,509-525. m . .
Garber, J., Keiley, M. K., &Martin, N. C. (2002). Developmental trajectories ofadolescents' depressive symptoms: Predictors ofchange. Journal of
Consulting andClinical Psycliology, 70,79-95. . «.,!,„:„„
Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., &Simons, R. L. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during
adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30,467-483. ^mBinm»tu
Gencoz,T.,Voelz,Z.R., Gencoz, F.,Pettit, J.W..& Joiner, T.E. (2001). Specificity ofinformation processmg stylesto depressive symptomsmyouth
psycM&tncinpaliiMs. Journal afAbnormal Child Psycliology, 29,255-262. Aj„,a„0„t
Gibb, B. E., &Alloy, L. B. (2006). Aprospective test ofthe hopelessness theory ofdepression in children. Journal ofClinical Child andAdolescent
GibC aE.! AlloyfL2B"walshaw, P. D.,Comer, J. S., Shen,G. H., &Villari, A.(2006). Predictorsofattributionalstylechangein children.Journal
ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 34,425-439. , „r^»„,w
Gladstone, T. R., &Kaslow, N. J. (1995). Depression and attributions in children and adolescents: Ameta-analytic review. Journal ofAbnormal
GotUM^S^^^
indepression and social phobia. Journal ofAbnormal Psycliology, 77J,386-398. ... , T , f
Gottesman, LL, &Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of
Gr^T^m^ilstoh^^, A. F., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., &Halper, J. A. (2004a). Stressors and child and adolescent
psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective effects. Journal ofClinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33,412-425.
gZTITc™??**,* E., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., &Gipson, P. Y. (2004b). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology:
Measurement issues and prospective effects. Journal ofClinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33,412-425.
Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R. H., et aL, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vubembmty for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Reviexv (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
22 RH. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx

Hadwin, J. A., Donnelly, N., French, C. C, Richards, A., Watts, A., &Daley, D. (2003). The influence ofchildren's self-report trait anxiety and
depression on visual search for emotional feces. Journal ofChild Psycliology and Psychiatry, 44,432-444.
Hammen, C(1988). Self-cognitions, stressfulevents, and the prediction ofdepression in children ofdepressed mothers. Journal ofAbnormal Child
Psychology, 6,347-360. .
Hammen, C,Adrian, G,&Hiroto, D. (1988). Alongitudinal test ofthe attributional vulnerability model in children at risk for depression. British
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27,37-46.
Hammen, C, &Goodman-Brown, T. (1990). Self-schemas and vulnerability to specific life stress in children at risk for depression. Cognitive
Therapy andResearch, 74,215-227. .
Hammen, C., &Zupan, B. A.(1984). Self-schemas, depression, and the processing ofpersonal information in children. Journal ofExperimental
Child Psychology, 37, 598-608. .
Hankin, B. L., &Abramson, L. Y. (2001). Development ofgender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerabihty-transacUonal
stress theory. Psychological Bulletin, 127,773-796.
Hankin, B. L., &Abramson, L. Y. (2002). Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression in adolescence: Reliability, validity, and gender
differences. Journal ofClinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31,491-504.
Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y, &Siler, M. (2001). Aprospective test ofthe hopelessness theory ofdepression in adolescents. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 25,607-632.
Harter, S.(1985). Manualfor the Self-Perception Profilefor Children. Denver CO: University ofDenver.
Harter, S. (2002). The Pictorial Scale ofPerceived Competence and Social Acceptance for Young ChUdren. University ofDenver, Dept of
Psychology. Availablefrom Susan Harter. PhD. University ofDenver. Department ofPsycliology 2155 SRace Street, Denver, CO 80208-0204;
sharter@psy.du.edu.
Harter, S., &Pike, R. (1984). The Pictorial Scale ofPerceived Competence and Social Acceptance for young children. Child Development, 55,
1969-1982. .
Hilsman, R., &Garber, J. (1995). A test ofthe cognitive diathesis-stress model ofdepression in children: Academic stressors, attributional style,
perceived competence, and controL Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 69,370-380.
Hoffman, K. B., Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Tram, J., &Seroczynski, A. D. (2000). Are the discrepancies between self- and others' appraisals of
competence predictive or reflective ofdepressive symptoms in children and adolescents: Alongitudinal study, Part IL Journal ofAbnormal
Psychology, 109,651-662. .
Hughes, J., Worchel, F., Stanton, S., Stanton, H., &HalL B. (1990). Selective memory for positive and negative story content mchildren with high
self- and peer-ratings of symptoms ofdepression. Scliool Psychology Quarterly, 5,265-279.
Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., &Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press.
Johnson, S. L., Joorman, J., &Gotlib, LH. (2007). Does processing ofemotional stimuli predict symptomatic improvement and diagnostic recovery
from majordepression? Emotion, 7,201-206.
Joiner, T. E., Metasky, G. I., Lew, A., &Klocek, J. (1999). Testing the causal mediation component ofBeck's theory ofdepression: Evidence for
specific mediation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23,401-412.
Joorman, J., Talbot, L., &Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Biased processing ofemotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal ofAbnormal
Psychology,116,135-143.
Just, N., Abramson, L. Y, &Alloy, L. B. (2001). Remitteddepression studies as tests ofthe cognitivevulnerabilityhypotheses ofdepression onset A
critique and conceptual analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 21,63-83.
Kaslow, N. J., &Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Children's Attributional Style Questionnaire —revised (CASQ-R). Unpublished manuscript, Emory
University, Atlanta. .
Keating, D. P. (1996). Central conceptual structures: Seeking developmental integration. Monograplis ofthe Society for Research in Child
Development, 61,276-282.
Keating, D. P. (2004). Cognitive and brain development Handbook ofadolescent psycliology (2nd ed.). (pp. 45-84). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Kessler, R. C,AvenevoU, S., &Merikangas, K. R. (2001). Mood disorders in children and adolescents: An epidemiologic perspective. Biological
Psychiatry, 49,1002-1014.
Kessler, R. C,Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., &Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distnbudons of
DSM-IV disorders inthe National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives ofGeneral Psycliiatry, 62,593-602.
Kelvin, R. G., Goodyer, I. M., Teasdale, J. D., &Brechin, D. (1999). Latentnegative self-schemaand high emotionality in welladolescents at risk for
psychopathology. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 40,959-968.
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. R,Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., &Poulton, R. (2003). Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder
Developmental follow-back ofaprospective-longitudinal cohort Archives ofGeneral Psycliiatry, 60,709-717.
Kistner, J., Balthazor, M., Risi, S., &Burton, C. (1999). Predicting dysphoria in adolescence from actual and perceived peeracceptance in childhood.
Journal ofClinical ChildPsychology, 2«, 94-104.
Kistner, J. A., David-Ferdon, C. F., Lopez, C. M., &Dunkel, S. B. (2007). Ethnic and sex differences in children's depressive symptoms. Journal of
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psycliology, 36,171-181.
Kistner, J. A., David-Ferdon, C. F., Repper, K. K., &Joiner, T. E. (2006). Bias and accuracy ofchildren's perceptions ofpeeracceptance: Prospective
associations withdepressive symptoms. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 34,349-361.
Kuhn, D., &Pease, M. (2006). Do children and adults learn differendy? Journal ofCognition and Development, 7,309-312.
Kuyken, W., &HowelL R. (2000). Facets ofautobiographical memory in adolescents with major depressive disorder and never-depressed controls.
Cognition and Emotion, 20,466M87.
Kuyken, W, Howell, R., &Dalgleish, T. (2006). Overgeneral autobiographical memory in depressed adolescents with versus without, areported
historyof trauma. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 115,387-396.

Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R. H., etaL, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psychology Review (2007),doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006
R.H. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psychology Review xx (2007) xxx-xxx 23

Kwon, S. M., &Oei, T. P. S. (1994). The roles of two levels of cognition inthe development, maintenance, and treatment of depression. Clinical
Psychology Review, 14,331-358.
Kyte, Z.A.,Goodyer, I. M., &Sahakian, B.J. (2005). Selected executive skills inadolescents with recent first episode major depression. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46,995-1005.
Ladouceur, C. D.,Dahl, R. E.,Williamson, D.E.,Birmaher, B., Ryan, N. D.,&Casey, B.J.(2005). Altered emotional processing in pediatric anxiety,
depression, and comorbid anxiety-depression. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 33,165-177.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Allen, N. B., Seeley, J. R., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). First onset versus recurrence of depression: Differential processes of
psychosocialrisk. JournalofAbnormalPsychology, 108,483-489.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Clarke, G.N., Seeley, J. R.,&Rohde, P. (1994). Major depression incommunity adolescents: Ageatonset, episode duration, and
timetorecurrence. Journal oftheAmerican Academy ofChild andAdolescent Psycliiatry, 33, 809-818.
Lewinsohn, P. M, Joiner, T. E., & Rohde, P. (2001). Evaluation of cognitive diathesis-stress models in predicting major depressive disorder in
adolescents.Journal ofAbnormalPsychology, 110,203-215.
Marcotte, D., Levesque, N., & Fortin, L. (2006). Variations of cognitive distortions and school performance indepressed and non-depressed high
school adolescents: A two-year longitudinal study. Cognitive Therapy andResearch, 30, 211-225.
Martin, J. M.,Cole, D. A., Clausen, A., Logan, J., & Strosher, H. L. W. (2003). Moderators of the relation between popularity and depressive
symptoms in children: Processing strength andfriendship value. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 31,471-483.
Matthews, A. (2006). Towards anexperimental cognitive science of CBT. Behavior Therapy, 37,314-318.
Matthews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Reviews ofPsychology, 45,25-50.
McCarty, C. A.,Vander Stoep, A.,&McCauley, E. (2007). Cognitive features associated with depressive symptoms inadolescence: Directionality
and specificity. Journal ofClinical ChildandAdolescent Psychology, 36,147-158.
McCreary, B.X, Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, N. B.,&Ialongo, N. S.(2004). The structure and correlates of perfectionism inAfrican American children.
Journal ofClinicalChild and AdolescentPsychology, 33,313-324.
McGrath, E. P., &Repetti, R.L.(2002). A longitudinal study ofchildren's depressive symptoms, self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the
self. Journal ofAbnormalPsycliology, 111, 77-87.
Measelle, J. R.,Ablow, J. C, Cowan, P. A.,&Cowan, C.P. (1998). Assessing young children's views oftheir academic, social, and emotional lives:
An evaluation of theself-perception scales of the Berkeley Puppet Interview. Child Development, 69,1556-1576.
Murray, L,, Woolgar, M., Cooper, P., & Hipwell, A. (2001). Cognitive vulnerability to depression in 5-year-old children of depressed mothers.
Journal ofChildPsychologyand Psycliiatry, 42,891-899.
Neshat-Doost, H. X, Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R.,Yule, W., &Dalgleish, X (2000). Lack ofattentional bias for emotional information inclinically
depressed children and adolescents onthe dot probe task. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 363-368.
Neshat-Doost, H. X, Taghavi, M. R., Moradi, A. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, X (1997). The performance of clinically depressed children and
adolescents on themodified Stroop paradigm. Personality andIndividual Differences, 23, 753-759.
Neshat-Doost, H. X,Taghavi, M. R., Moradi, A.R., Yule, W., &Dalgleish, X (1998). Memory for emotional trait adjectives inclinically depressed
youth. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 107,642-650.
Nicewander, W. A.,&Price, J. M. (1983). Reliability ofmeasurement and the power ofstatistical tests: Some new results. PsychologicalBulletin, 94,
524-533.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., &Seligman, M. E. P. (1986). Learned helplessness inchildren: A longitudinal study ofdepression, achievement,
andexplanatory style. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 51,435-442.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year
longitudinal study.JournalofAbnormalPsychology, 101,405-422.
Nunnaliy, J. C, & Bernstein, L H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). NewYork: McGraw-Hill.
Ohannessian, C. M., Lemer, R.M., Lemer, J. V., &von Eye, A.(1999). Does self-competence predict gender differences in adolescentdepression and
anxiety?Journalof Adolescence, 22,397-411.
Panak, W. F., &Garber, J. (1992). Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction ofdepression in children. Development and
Psychopathology, 4,145—165.
Park, R. J., Goodyer, I. M., &Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Categoric overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescents with major depressive disorder.
Psychological Medicine, J2,267-276.
Park, R. J., Goodyer, I. M., &Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Effects of induced rumination and distraction on mood and overgeneral autobiographical
memory inadolescent major depressive disorder and controls. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,996-1006.
Perez-Edgar, K., Fox, N. A., Cohn, J. F., &Kovacs, M. (2006). Behavioral and electrophysiological markers ofselective attention in children of
parents with a history of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 60,1131-1138.
Persons, J. B., & Miranda, J. (1992). Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: Reconciling negative evidence. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 16,485-502.
Petersen, A.C, Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., &Stemmler, M. (1993). Depression in adolescence. American Psychologist, 48,155-168.
Pine, D. S., Lissek, S., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S., Moulton, J. L., Guardino, M., etaL (2004). Face memory and emotion: Associations with major
depression inchildren and adolescents. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,1199-1208.
Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., &Ma, Y.(1998). The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with
anxiety anddepressive disorders. Archives of General Psycliiatry, 55,56-64.
Possel, P., Seemann, S., Ahrens, S., &Hautzinger, M. (2006). Testing the causal mediation component of Dodge's Social Information Processing
Model of Social Competence and Depression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35,849-859.
Prathikanti, S., &Weinberger, D. R. (2005). Psychiatric genetics —the new era: Genetic research and some clinical implications. British Medical
Bulletin, 73,107-122.

Please cite this article as: Jacobs, R.H., etaL, Empirical evidence ofcognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective, Clinical Psycliology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006 __
24 RH. Jacobs et al. / Clinical Psycliology Reviewxx (2007) xxx-xxx

Prieto, S.L., Cole, D. A.,&Tageson, C. W. (1992). Depressive self-schemas inclinic and nonclinic children. Cognitive Tlterapy and Research, 16.
Prinstein, M. J., &Aikins, J. W. (2004). Cognitive moderators of the longitudinal association between peer rejection and adolescent depressive
symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32,147-158.
Prinstein, M. I, Chea, C. S.L.,&Guyer, A.E. (2005). Peervictimization, cue interpretation, and internalizing symptoms: Preliminary concurrentand
longitudinal findings for children and adolescents. Journal ofClinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34,11-24.
Reid, S.C, Salmon, K., &Lovibond, P. F. (2006). Cognitive biases inchildhood anxiety, depression, and aggression: Are they pervasive orspecific?
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30,551-549.
Robinson, N.S.,Garber, J., &Hilsman, R.(1995). Cognitions and stress: Direct and moderating effects ondepressive versus externalizing symptoms
during thejunior highschool transition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104,453-463.
Rude, S. S., Wenzlaff, R. M., Gibbs, B.,Vane, J., & Whitney, X (2002). Depressive negative processing biases predict subsequent depression.
Cognition and Emotion, 16,423-440.
Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C, &Burge, D. (1997). A cognitive-interpersonal approach to depressive symptoms inpreadolescent children. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psycliology, 25,33-45.
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L.A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and psychopathology, 12,265-296.
Schwartz, J. A., & Koenig, L. J. (1996). Response styles and negative affect among adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Researcli, 20,13-36.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, anddeath. San Francisco: Freeman.
Seligman, M. E. P., Peterson, C, Kaslow, N. J., Tenenbaum, R. L., Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1984). Attributional style and depressive
symptoms among children. JournalofAbnormal Psychology, 93,235-241.
Spence, S. H., Sheffield, J., &Donovan, C.(2002). Problem-solving orientation and attributional style: Moderators of the impact of negative life
events on thedevelopment of depressive symptoms inadolescence? Journal ofClinical Child Psychology, 31, 219-229.
Southall, D., &Roberts, J. E.(2002). Attributional style and self-esteem invulnerability toadolescent depressive symptoms following lifestress: A
14-weekprospective study.Cognitive Therapy andResearcli, 26,563-579.
Steinberg, L. (2004). Risktaking inadolescence: What changes, and why? Academy ofSciences Annals ofthe New York, 1021,51-58.
Steinberg, J. A.,Oelrich, C, Alloy, L. B.,&Abramson, L.Y. (2004). Negative self-referent information processing asamoderator vs.Mediator of
negative cognitive styles in predicting prospective onsets of depression. Manuscript in preparation, Temple University.
Swales, M.A.,Williams, J.M.G.,&Wood, P. (2001). Specificity ofautobiographical memory and mood disturbance inadolescents. Cognition and
Emotion, 15,321-331.
Taghavi, R.,Neshat-Doost, H. X, Moradi, A. R.,Yule, W., &Dalgleish, X (1999). Biases invisual attention inchildren and adolescents with clinical
anxiety andmixed depression-anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27,215—223.
Taylor, L., & Ingram, R. E. (1999). Cognitive reactivity and depressotypic information processing in children of depressed mothers. Journal of
Abnormal Psycliology, 108,202-210.
Timbremont, B.,&Braet, C.(2004). Cognitivevulnerability inremitteddepressed children and adolesceats.BeliaviourResearclt andTherapy, 42,423-437.
Toner, M. A., & Heaven, P. C. L. (2005). Peer-social attributional predictors of socio-emotional adjustment in early adolescence; a two-year
longitudinalstudy. Personalityand Individual Differences, 38,579-590.
Tram, J. M., & Cole, D. A. (2000). Self-perceived competence and the relation between life events and depressive symptoms in adolescence:
Mediator or moderator?Journal ofAbnormalPsycliology, 109,753-760.
Turner, J.E.,&Cole, D.A.(1994). Developmentaldifferences incognitive diatheses for childdepression.JournalofAbnormalChildPsycliology,22,15-32.
Vasey, M. W., Dalgleish, X, & Silverman, W. K. (2003). Research oninformation-processing factors in child and adolescent psychopathology: A
critical commentary. JournalofClinicalChildand Adolescent Psychology, 32,81 -93.
Vrielynck, N., Deplus, S., & Philippot, P. (2007). Overgeneral autobiographical memory and depressive disorder in children. Journal of Clinical
Child and AdolescentPsycliology, 36,95-105.
Weiss, B., & Garber, J.(2003). Developmental differences in thephenomenology of depression. Development andPsychopathology, 15,403-430.
Weissman, A. N.,& Beck, A. X (1978). Development andvalidation ofthe Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented atthe annual meeting of
the Associationfor the AdvancementofBehavior Hierapy, Chicago.
Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Moreau, D., & Olfson, M. (1997). Offspring of depressed parents: 10 years later. Arcftives of
GeneralPsychiatry, 54,932-940.
Whitman, P. B., & Leitenberg, H.(1990). Negatively biased recall in children with self-reported symptoms ofdepression. Journal ofAbnormal Cliild
Psychology, 18,15-27.
Zupan, B. A., Hammen, C, & Jaenicke, C. (1987). The effectsofcurrent moodandprior depressive history on self-schematic processing in children.
JournalofExperimentalChild Psychology, 43, 149-158.

Please cite this article as:Jacobs, R. H., et aL,Empirical evidenceofcognitive vulnerability fordepression amongchildren andadolescents: A
cognitive science and developmental perspective,ClinicalPsychologyReview (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006

Вам также может понравиться